Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 27

Household Store Brand Proneness: A Framework

PAUL S. RICHARDSON
Loyola University Chicago

ARUN K. JAIN University at Buffalo ALAN DICK


University at Buffalo

Store brands play an important role in retail grocery strategy. Yet, little recent research has examined factors thought to influence the selection of store brand.s. This paper augments prior research by building an integratedframework within which to view private brand proneness. Factors found to in@ence store brand proneness included familiarity with store brands, the extent to which consumers rely on extrinsic cues such as price and packaging to judge product quality, intolerance for ambiguity, perceived quality variation between national andstore brand products, perceived risk. perceived valuefor money, income and family size. The results suggest that consumer.s negative perceptions of store brands are driven primarily by the poor quality image of these products. Implications for marketing strategy are presented.

INTRODUCTION According to the RI, the volume market share of store brand grocery items has increased from, 15.3% in 1988 to 19.7% in, 1993. Sales of private label cereals alone have increased from $150 million in 1988 to over $400 million in 1993 (Bums, 1995). As a group, private labels have higher unit market share than the top national brands in 77 out of 250 supermarket product categories (Quelch and Harding, 1996). This increase in store brand market share partly reflects retailers recognition that store brands represent an important strategic asset for the firm. For example, unlike national brands which may be purchased at virtually any chain, store brands are proprietary to the chains themselves. Large U.S. retailers are now realizing that effective marketing of store brands can increase store loyalty, chain prof-

Paul S. Richardson, Loyola University Chicago, School of Business Administration, 820 North Michigan Avenue, Chicago, IL 6061 I. Arun K. Jain, University at Buffalo, School of Management, Samuel P. Capen Professor of Marketing Research, Amherst, NY 14260-4000. Alan Dick, University at Buffalo, School of Management, Amherst, NY 14260.4COO.
Journal of Retailing, Volume 72(2), pp. 159-185, ISSN: 0022-4359 Copyright 8 1996 by New York University. All rights of reproduction

in any form reserved.

159

160

Journal of Retailing Vol. 72, No. 2 1996

itability, and product turnover (Liesse, 1993). Hence, it is important to better understand the decision making processes underlying private brand purchase. Existing field studies of store brands have been exploratory in nature. Typically these studies have concentrated on developing customer typologies (e.g., Becherer and Richard, 1978; Bettman, 1974) or focused on testing correlational relationships between private brand attitude and socioeconomic or other explanatory factors (Coe, 1971; Frank and Boyd, 1965; Murphy, 1978). One weakness of these field studies is that possible interrelationships among variables have been ignored or examined in an ad hoc nature. Simil~ly, experimental studies have generally been limited to a single factor with treatments manipulated using artificial laboratory stimuli (Fugate, 1978; Sundel, 1974). Thus, the purpose of this paper is to present a framework of the factors that might influence store brand proneness, The framework is based on findings in various areas of research and thus serves an integrative role. Additionally, two new concepts, not previously linked to private brand proneness (extrinsic cue reliance and intolerance of ~biguity) are integrated into the model. Also, to provide insight about the areas of greatest importance to managers, an examination of the relative effect sizes of all of the constructs influencing private brand proneness is conducted. We first present a brief literature review concerning store brand grocery items. Following the literature review, we discuss the ~onstm~ts of the proposed model and present our research findings. We conclude with a discussion of the implications of the study, study limitations, and directions for future research.

LITE~TURE

REVIEW

Research on store brand grocery items has taken two approaches. We first review studies which have examined correlates of private brand proneness. Next, we describe experimental investigations of private brand attitude.

Correlates of Private Brand Proneness

Exploratory investigations of correlates of store brand purchase dominate the empirical literature concerning private label brands. For example, Frank and Boyd (1965), found some evidence that store brand buyers are better educated, older, and have lower incomes than national brand buyers. Burger and Schott (1972) and Cunningham, Hardy and Imperia (1982) likewise found that store brand buyers have greater education. Contradicting the findings of Frank and Boyd, Coe (197 1) and Murphy (1978) found that store brand buyers belong to higher rather than lower income classes. Therefore, the results concerning socioeconomic correlates of private brand proneness are somewhat mixed. Other work has focused on identifying personality and perceptual factors thought to relate to private brand proneness. Myers (1966) found that store brand buyers tend to be more enthusiastic, sensitive, and submissive than national brand buyers, Interestingly, in

Household

Store Brand Proneness

161

another test of personality characteristics on private brand attitude, Becherer and Richard {1978) report that store brand buyers show greater levels of independence and rely less on the behavioral norms of others. Finally, Bettman (1974) tested correlates of private brand proneness using an information processing approach. Bettman found that the store brand buyer can be distinguished from the national brand buyer on the basis of perceived quality of the respective brands, perceived risk associated with store brand purchase, and familiarity with store brands. In related research, marketers have tabulated and compared perceptions of store brands relative to national brands and generic grocery items using a variety of dependent measures. Invariably these studies have found that consumers perceive store brands to be inferior to national brands but superior to generic grocery items on attributes such as overall quality, taste, aroma, and reliability (Bell&i, Kruckeberg, Hamilton and Martin, 1981; Cunningham et al., 1982; Hawes, Hutchens and ~~opoulos, 1982). Given the Private Label Manufacturers Associations claim that store brands are similar to national brands in terms of intrinsic quality, these results suggest that there is a large difference between consumers perceptions and reality.

Experimental Investigations of Store Brands

Two early experiments concerning store brands were conducted by Sundel(l974) and Fugate (1979). Sundel had consumers taste and then rate national and private label brands of canned corn and fresh bread. No difference in evaluations were found for the bread; for the corn the national brand was judged superior. The mixed findings can probably be attributed to the fact that consumers were used to buying regional or local brands of bread. These are typically just as fresh as the national brands and constitute a large percentage of sales in this product category. Fugate manipulated store brand packaging and tested whether manufacturer disclosure on the packaging would influence store brand evaluation of cake mixes and ketchup. He found that manufac~rer disclosure (i.e., writing on a store brand package that the store brand was produced by a national manufacturer) had a positive effect on store brand evaluation, especially when the manufacturer disclosed was well known to subjects and this information was prominently displayed. In a more recent study, Richardson, Dick and Jain (1994) examined the impact of extrinsic cues (e.g., price, packaging, etc.) on evaluation of private brand grocery products. They found that consumers taste test evaluations of store brand grocery products were much higher when the store brand products were repackaged as national brands and presented with national brand prices. Additionally, they found that when national brand products were tasted, consumer ratings were considerably lower when the national brand products were represented to be store brands. This study is important because it suggests that a major part of the evaluation of store brand products is based on the extrinsic cues of the product. In fact, in this study, extrinsic cues played a much more important role in determining consumers evaluations than did actual product ingredients.

162

Journal of Retailing Vol. 72, No. 2 1996

The literature to date has identified a number of factors correlated with store brand proneness. For example, we know that socioeconomic factors, perceptual factors, and the extrinsic cues identified with these products all seem to influence private brand proneness. However, what is lacking is an attempt to integrate these disparate findings. For example, the experimental studies point out the potential importance of extrinsic cues, however they fail to provide a link between extrinsic cue reliance and the perceptual factors identified in the literature (e.g., perceived risk, perceived quality, perceived value for money, etc.). Since the extrinsic cues associated with store brand grocery products (e.g., cheaper prices, simpler packaging, lack of brand image,etc.) tend to suggest that these products are of lower quality than national brands, it would seem reasonable to argue that extrinsic reliance might influence other perceptual factors associated with store brands and that such perceptions might partly determine private brand proneness. Additionally, since extrinsic cue reliance may differ among individuals, attempting to determine factors thought to influence extrinsic cue reliance would likely be helpful in formulating marketing strategy. Consequently, a framework which integrates these various findings would likely lead to greater understanding and additional insights for marketers. The purpose of this paper is to introduce and test such a framework. We turn now to a discussion of the proposed model. In so doing, we attempt to synthesize, offer additional insights, and resolve some of the contradictions pertaining to the store brand literature described above.

PROPOSED

MODEL OF STORE BRAND PRONENESS

A flow diagram of the proposed mode1 is presented in Figure 1. The premise of the proposed model is that the perceived value for money offered by store brands, the perceived quality variation between national and store brand grocery items, the perceived risk associated with store brand purchase, the degree to which consumers rely on extrinsic cues such as price and brand name in quality assessment, consumer familiarity with private label brands, intolerance of ambiguity, and a variety of socioeconomic variables contribute either directly or indirectly in explaining individual differences in private brand proneness. In the following section we describe each of the constructs of the proposed model and offer theoretical support for the hypothesized relationships.

Private Brand Proneness

We define private brand proneness as the degree to which consumers are inclined to actually purchase store brand grocery items. In this sense, our definition of this construct is consistent with that of all previous studies that have examined this construct. In the literature, private brand proneness has been measured either by calculating the percentage of grocery expenditures devoted to private label lines through the use of diary panel data (Cunningham, 1961; Frank and Boyd, 1965; Rao, 1969) or by relying on consumers self reports

Household

Store Brand Proneness

163

Pl4>0 !36>O

v I Extrinsic Cue

Perceived Quality Variation OQV)

P4<0

q Perceived Value for Money (PVM) p, ,o ) Private Brand Proneness (PBP)

Perceived

Figure I.

Hypothesized

Private Brand Proneness

Framework

regarding the extent to which private label brands are selected (Bettman, 1974; Burgher and Schott, 1972; Murphy, 1978). Most of the literature concerning store brands is quite old. For example, we know of no recent study that has taken advantage of scanner data availability to measure this construct. Other measures of private brand attitude include stated brand preference (Becherer and Richard, 1978; Coe, 1971) or attribute ratings collected using paper and pencil measures (Bellizzi et al., 1981; Cunningham et al., 1982). In our model, we posit that consumers propensity to purchase store brands depends on a variety of constructs. The first of these constructs is perceived value for money.

Perceived

Value for Money

Value for money implies consideration of quality not in absolute terms but in relation to the price of a particular brand (i.e., utility per dollar). Thus, a lower priced product that has desirable features (e.g., natural ingredients, real cream, extra virgin olive oil) may be viewed as offering greater value for money than another brand sold at the same price but comprised of less appealing attributes. An emphasis on value for money is an integral part of many retailers promotion efforts (Davies, Gilligan and Sutton, 1986; Martell, 1986; McGoldrick, 1984; Patti and Fisk, 1982; Simmons and Meredith, 1984). This promotion strategy encourages consumers to consider the quality of store brand grocery items not in absolute terms but in relation to their lower price levels. Through these promotion efforts, retailers appeal to two different segments of the market: (1) those consumers who believe that store brands are lower priced and of rel-

164

Journal

of Retailing Vol. 72, No. 2 1996

atively good quality, and (2) those consumers who believe that store brands are lower priced but of relatively poor quality. The former segment of the market derives the full utility associated with the price differential. The latter segment gains less utility but may still buy store brands if the savings are greater than any perceived costs associated with tolerating inferior store brand ingredients. Other things being equal, greater value for money perceptions of store brands will lead to higher levels of store brand purchase. Hence, we hypothesize that: HI: The greater the perceived value for money o#ered by store brands, the greater consumers private brand proneness.

Perceived Risk

The perceived risks associated with using store brands are an important determinant of consumers propensity to favorably evaluate and purchase these products (Bettman, 1974; Livesey and Lennon, 1978). Bettman (1974), for example, found that uncertainty regarding store brand quaIity and perceptions of danger associated with store brand purchase are key variables that dis~minate private prone from national brand buyers, Livesey and Lennon (1978) further speculate that social risk inhibits the selection of particular kinds of store brand grocery items according to the usage situation. For example, these researchers argue that English consumers serve national brand tea to guests in social settings but consume less expensive store brand tea when such behavior cannot be observed by significant acquaintances. Consistent with these findings, we hypothesize that: H2: H3: The greater the perceived risk associated with using store brands, the lower consumers private brand proneness. The greater the perceived risk associated with using store brands, the less favorable the value for m.oney perceptions of these products.

Perceived Quality Variation

Quality perceptions are a critical element in purchase decisions. In the case of consumer non-durables, product quality is judged in terms of product pe~o~ance and the consistency of performance over time with respect to intrinsic attributes. Store brands are generally evaluated inferior to national brands on a variety of intrinsic attributes such as taste, texture, aroma, reliability of ingredients, nutritional value, and overall quality (Bellizzi et al., 1981; Cunningham et al., 1982). These perceptions persist despite the fact that store brand ingredients are subject to the same stringent FDA guidelines as those of national manuf~turers. Thus, even if store brand quality were as good as that of national brands (as the PLMA asserts), perceptions of store brand inferiority will likely serve to decrease favorable value for money perceptions. Hence, it is hypothesized that:

Household

Store Brand Proneness

165

II4:

The greater the perceived qualiry variation between national and store brand grocery items, the less favorable the value for money perceptions of store brands.

The perceived quality variation between national and store brand grocery items is also likely to directly influence the perceived risk associated with buying these products. For example, perceptions of high quality variation may lead to the belief that store brands lack desired att~butes, will fail to perform as expected, will be less reliable, or may meet with social or family disapproval. Hence, we hypothesize that: HS: The greater the perceived quality variation between national andprivate label brands, the greater the perceived risk associated with using private label brands.

Extrinsic Cue Reliance

Extrinsic cues are product related attributes such as brand name, packaging, and price which are not part of the physical product (Olson, 1972). Extrinsic cues form the image of the product and reflect marketing strategies independent of the physical characteristics of the product (e.g., ingredients, texture, color). Consumers make extensive use of extrinsic cues in brand evaluation (See Purwar, 1982; Rao and Monroe, 1989 for a review of this literature). For example, Jacoby, Olson and Haddock (1971), and Wheatley and Chu (1977) found strong brand image effects on product evaluation. Extrinsic cue effects on product evaluation have also been found for packaging and labeling (McDaniel and Baker, 1977) and store image contextual cues (Andrews and Valenzi, 1970; Park and Winter, 1979). Unfavorable perceptions of store brand quality may be fostered by consumers reliance on extrinsic cues when making quality judgments (Jacoby et al., 1971; Olson, 1972; Olson and Jacoby, 1973; Vale& and Andrews, 1971). Store brands are frequently presented in unimaginative, inexpensive looking packages, have little promotions support on which a more favorable image may be built, and are priced below national brands. To the extent that consumers rely on such surrogate cues in quality assessment, it is likely that consumers may perceive large differences in quality between national and competing store brands as a result. Hence, it is hypothesized that: H6: The greater the re&znce on extrinsic cues in qua~i~ assessment, the higher the perceived quality variation between national and store brand grocery items.

Extrinsic cue reliance may also aggravate the perceived risk associated with buying store brands. For example, the lack of advertising and absence of a well known brand name typically associated with store brands may heighten unce~~nty regarding store brand quality. Similarly, lower store brand prices may signal inferior or unreliable ingredients to consumers. In addition, the less prestigious image of private label brands may heighten perceived

166

Journal

of Retailing Vol. 72, No. 2 1996

social risk and constrain the usage of store brands to particular situations such as when these products cannot be seen by significant acquaintances. Hence we hypothesize that: H7: The greater the reliance on extrinsic cues in quality assessment, higher the perceived risk associated with store brand purchase. the

Familiarity

Familiarity denotes brand comprehension, product knowledge, or skill in judging the criteria needed to evaluate products (Howard and Sheth, 1969). Wolinsky (1987) argues that differences in consumer familiarity with grocery items makes it possible for manufacturers to adopt mixed brand strategies in which identical or similar ingredients are marketed in both national and store brand packaging. The national brand is targeted to those consumers who have less product knowledge and consequently evaluate grocery items using easy to process extrinsic cues such as price or brand name. The store brand, on the other hand, is targeted to those consumers whose skill and expertise makes possible reliance on a wider range of cues including those intrinsic to the product (Wolinsky, 1987). These findings are consistent with cue utilization theory which predicts that consumer familiarity acts to decrease reliance on price and brand name because of consumers ability to synthesize a greater range of cues in quality assessment (Raju, 1977; Valenzi and Eldridge, 1973; Wheatley, Walton and Chiu, 1977). Hence, it is hypothesized that: HS: Greater familiarity with store brands results in less reliance extrinsic cues in the quality assessment process. on

Consumers familiar with store brands consider these products with a greater level of information and confidence. As a result, Bettman (1974) posits that store brand familiarity serves to increase private brand proneness by decreasing the perceived risk and perceived quality variation associated with these brands. When familiarity is high, the perceived danger of selecting store brands decreases and the certainty that store brands offer an acceptable level of quality increases. Consistent with Bettman, we hypothesize that: H9: HlO: Hll: Greater familiarity proneness. with store brands results in higher private brand risk

Greater familiarity with store brands results in lower perceived associated with using these products. Greaterfamiliarity with store brands results in less perceived variation between national and store brand grocery items.

quality

Intolerance

of Ambiguity

Individuals differ in the manner in which they react to intrusive stimuli. Intolerants of ambiguity are those individuals who shun rather than integrate intrusive elements (Pinson, Jain and Malhotra, 1980). Intolerants of ambiguity experience discomfort when confronted

Household Store Brand Proneness

167

with novel situations or products (Blake, Zenhausen, Perloff and Heslin, 1973; Budner, 1962). Such individuals are resistant to change because of their inability to synthesize additional information into a pre-existing belief structure (Shaffer and Hendrick, 1974). Intolerants of ambiguity choose familiar over unfamiliar stimuli and seek highly structured, objective, and defined approaches to decision resolution. Such individuals also tend to be more conventional in their approach to problem solving (Budner, 1962). Since intolerants of ambiguity are reluctant to include ambiguous information into pre-existing belief structures, these individuals tend to reduce situations to a right way or wrong way dichotomy. Specifically, according to Smock (1955), intolerants of ambiguity are likely to respond only to the known or most familiar elements in the choice situation. Store brands are likely to be viewed as more ambiguous than national brands since these products lack a brand name that identifies the producer of these products. Given this ambiguity, we expect that intolerance of ambiguity has a negative effect on private brand proneness. Formally, we hypothesize that: H12: The greater consumers intolerance of ambiguity, the lower the level of private brand proneness.

We further expect that intolerance of ambiguity increases consumers propensity to believe that store brands offer poor value for money. Intolerance of ambiguity has been defined as the tendency to perceive (i.e., interpret) ambiguous situations as sources of threat (Budner, 1962). Since the intrinsic quality of store brands may be unknown to consumers or ambiguous, consumers intolerant of ambiguity may be more likely to be skeptical of private brand quality and assume that these products offer poor value for money. Hence, we hypothesize that: H13: The greater consumers intolerance of ambiguity, the less favorable the value for money perceptions of private label brands.

In the cue utilization literature, intolerance of ambiguity has been identified as an important construct that affects reliance on extrinsic cues such as price and brand name. Consumers intolerant of ambiguity are more likely to seek familiar, more easily identifiable, and less ambiguous cues when making choices among competing brands in the marketplace (Etgar and Malhotra, 1981). Such consumers are likely to reject cues that are difficult to judge or which lend themselves to multiple interpretations. Extrinsic cues such as brand name, packaging, and price are more easily recognized, processed, and interpreted than are intrinsic cues (purwar, 1982). Hence, we hypothesize that: H14: The greater consumers intolerance of ambiguity, the greater the reliance on extrinsic cues in quality assessment.

Socioeconomic

Variables

Most investigations of private brand proneness have sought to investigate the degree to which a variety of socioeconomic variables can explain this construct. The rationale behind

168

Journal

of Retailing Vol. 72, No. 2 1996

this literature is that since store brands are less expensive than national brands, store brands should logically appeal to individuals in distinct socioeconomic group(s). For example, Frank and Boyd (1965) used a battery of 14 socioeconomic variables for a sample of 491 households and cross-sectional data for 32 products and reported that private brand proneness is negatively associated with income, but positively associated with education and the number of persons in the family. However, taken together, these variables explained only 7% of variance in the dependent measure. In another study, Myers (1966) employed a battery of 15 socioeconomic and 8 personality variables for a sample of 347 female shoppers and cross-sectional data for 27 products. Myers found that socioeconomic variables had virtually no predictive power in distinguishing the private brand prone from the non-private prone consumer. Other studies by Bettman (1974), Burgher and Schott (1972), and Fugate (1979) likewise found socioeconomic variables to be ineffectual in discriminating the private brand prone from the national brand prone consumer. Significant results have been reported in only two other studies. Coe (1971), using a sample of 100 housewives and data for 25 products, reported that middle income housewives are significantly more private brand prone than are lower income consumers. Similarly, Murphy (1978), using a sample of 309 female shoppers and data for 3 products, reported that consumers in high income classes are significantly more prone to purchase store brands than are consumers in lower or middle income classes. The conflicting findings reported above may simply be an artifact of the different products, sample sizes, and dependent measures used in the various investigations. Given that researchers have not explored the impact of socioeconomic variables on private brand proneness for almost twenty years, it seems timely to take another look at this topic. A further examination of the impact of socioeconomic variables on private brand proneness may reveal whether relationships have changed over time. In addition, including socioeconomic variables in the proposed framework affords the opportunity to test the importance of these variables relative to perceptual factors in determining private brand proneness. We test the effect of four socioeconomic variables: (1) income, (2) education, (3) age of the primary grocery shopper of the household, and (4) family size. Traditionally, private label grocery products have been merchandised on the basis of price. The purchase of private label rather than national brands results in significant savings to households. By purchasing store brands, households may stretch their grocery budgets and fight inflation. Lower income households have a greater incentive to purchase store brands because of financial pressure (Frank and Boyd, 1965). Hence, it is hypothesized that: HE Greater household income results in lower private brand proneness.

In regards to education, the direction of the relationship with private brand proneness is less obvious. On the one hand, education may act as a surrogate measure of income. Other things being equal, more highly educated individuals may possess greater income and thereby enjoy more liberty in brand choice. As a result, national brands may be preferred despite their higher prices. The result would be a negative association between education and private brand proneness. On the other hand, the selection of store brands takes a certain amount of cognitive resources. More highly educated individuals may be better able to dis-

Household Store Brand Proneness

169

criminate between national and store brand grocery items and be better able to process product related cues. Less highly educated individuals, on the other hand, may be less able to deal with the ambiguity of store brands or accurately process ingredient or other information regarding intrinsic product attributes. If this is the case, education may be positively associated with private brand proneness. In the absence of strong theoretical support for a directional effect of education on private brand proneness, we hypothesize: H16:
Education influences private brand proneness.

The age of the primary grocery shopper of the household is also likely to influence private brand proneness. Other things being equal, older shoppers have greater shopping expertise than younger shoppers. Whereas younger shoppers may rely on simple heuristics when selecting brands, older shoppers are likely to have developed more sophisticated choice processes in brand choice. For example, younger, inexperienced shoppers may be more inclined to rely on brand name or price when selecting products and limit their purchases to well established national brands. Older, experienced shoppers, on the other hand, may have developed the expertise needed to evaluate brands using harder to process intrinsic cues and consider store brands as viable alternatives to national brands for a wider range of products. Hence, we hypothesize: H17:
The older the primary grocery shopper of the household, the private brand proneness of the household. the greater

Of the four socioeconomic variables included in the study, the influence of family size may offer the easiest prediction. Regardless of income or education, the greater the size of the family, the fewer the resources that are available to make ends meet. Consequently, it is reasonable to expect that the greater the size of the household, the higher the proportion of the grocery budget devoted to private label rather than national brands. This may explain Frank and Boyds finding of a positive relationship between family size and private brand proneness. We hypothesize: H18:
The larger proneness. the size of the household, the greater the private brand

METHODOLOGY

We tested the model in Figure 1 though a field investigation in a large northeastern metropolitan area. Shoppers in a mall were randomly intercepted and requested to participate in a study being sponsored by a prominent local institution of higher education. Subjects were asked to complete a questionnaire and return it within a week in an attached business reply envelope to qualify for participation in a cash giveaway. The analysis described here is based upon data from 582 subjects from whom complete model-related information was available. This represents a response rate of 19.7%.

170

Journal

of

Retailing

Vol.

72,

No.

2 1996

Measurement

Standard demographic questions were used to obtain information regarding household income (yl), education (yz), age of the primary grocery shopper of the household (yg), and size of the household (~4). Intolerance of ambiguity (INT) was measured using Budners (1962), 16-item scale. A higher score suggests greater intolerance of ambiguity &). A Likert-type question asking respondents their familiarity (y6) with store brand grocery items served as the indicator of familiarity (FAM). Extrinsic cue reliance (ECR) in brand selection was measured through four Likert-type questions asking about reliance on brand name (xl), advertising (x2), packaging (x3), and price (x4) in brand evaluation. To measure perceived quality variation (PQV), respondents were asked to indicate their agreement/disagreement with statements regarding differences in quality (x=J, reliability (x6), and nutritional value (x7) between nationally advertised and store brand grocery items. The two indicators of perceived risk (PRS) measured were social risk (xs) and functional risk associated with private brand purchase (x9). Perceived value for money (PVM) was measured by asking the respondents whether store brands offer good value for money (xlo) or are a waste of money (x, 1). In measuring the ultimate dependent variable (private brand proneness), an attempt was made to create a measure that reflected consumers overall likelihood of purchasing store brand grocery products. Consequently, we consulted with store brand managers to identify products which had both national and private brand versions widely available in the local market, and which would be commonly included in a typical grocery basket. The managers identified the 28 product categories shown in Table 1. A survey of the local grocery chains confirmed that store brand counterparts to the national brands were available for the selected products. To measure private brand proneness, subjects in the survey were asked whether they regularly bought each product, and if so the frequency with which the product was a store brand: never (I), rarely (2), sometimes (3), often (4), or always (5). A private brand proneness index (PBP) was constructed by summing responses across the 28 products and adjusting the total score for the number of products subjects actually bought (x12). With the exception of private brand proneness, all other items were measured using a sixpoint Likert-type scale anchored by definitely disagree-definitely agree. A sample item used to operationalize each construct and Cronbach alpha are presented in Table 2.

TABLE Products Comprising


Bacon Bottled Juices Butter/Margarine Canned Tuna Canned Vegetables Canned Fruits Canned Soups Cereal (Hot or Cold) Cheeses Chip Dip Cookies Crackers Eggs Frozen Vegetables Frozen Orange Juice Ice Cream Jams/jellies/Preserves Ketchup/Mustard Liquid Laundry Detergent Mayonnaise/Salad Dressing

the Grocery

Basket
Pancake Syrup Paper Towels POP Potato Chips Relish/Pickles Spaghetti/Pasta Sauce Spaghetti/Pasta Vegetable Oil

Household

Store Brand Proneness

171

TABLE 2 Sample Items Used in Scales


Total # of

Construct
Intolerance Familiarity of Ambiguity

/terns 16 1 An expert who

Typical Statement doesnt come up with a definite

Coefficient Alpha
0.56 -

answer probably doesnt know too much.

I am
The

very familiar with the various store brand gro-

cery items available in the market place. Extrinsic Cue Reliance 4 more famous the brand name of a grocery 0.76

item, the better the quality. Perceived Quality Variation 3 There is a great deal of difference between cery items. Perceived Risk 2 The purchase of store brand grocery items is risky because the quality of store brands in inferior. Perceived Value for Money 2 Store money. Note: *Represents coefficientof correlation. brand grocery items offer great value for 0.36* 0.39* in overall quality 0.81

nationally advertised and store brand gro-

Partial least Squares (PLS) Models with latent Variables: A Brief Exposition The model of household private brand proneness was specified, and its parameters were estimated using Herman Wolds (1975,1982) Partial Least Squares (PLS) approach to structural equation modeling. The PLS model can be best understood through its arrow scheme. Figure 2 shows an example. The arrow scheme involves manifest or directly observed variables (x,, x2, x3, yl, y2, y3, zl, z2, z3) and latent or indirectly observed variables (LX,Ly, L,). The latent variables are a weighted linear combination of their respective manifest variables. Thus, for example, Lx is a weighted linear combination of xl, x2, and x3. These are referred to as outer relations. The relations among the latent variables or inner relations are the causal-predictive core of the model. In PLS (Bookstein, 1980), the latent variables (Lx, L,,, L,) capture their respective manifest variables (x-ness, y-ness and z-ness) in the context of the joint linear determination of the dependent latent variable (z-ness) by the independent latent variable(s) (x-ness and y-ness). It will be noticed that the arrow scheme does not hypothesize any direct relationship between the manifest variables. In PLS, the modeler specifies structural relationships between the latent variables and not between the discrete observed variables (Papademetrion and Hopple, 1982). The PLS approach has been successfully applied in a variety of contexts and disciplines. These include psychology, sociology, political science, economics, chemistry and biology (Papademetrion and Hopple, 1982). In marketing, PLS has been employed to study consumer exit and voice caused by dissatisfaction (Fomell and Bookstein, 1982) and user interaction with decision support systems (Zinkhan, Jochimethaler and Kinnear, 1987). An excellent review of the history and use of PLS is provided by Geladi (1987).

172

Journal of Retailing

Vol. 72, No. 2 1996

figure

2.

An Example of a PLS Model with Latent Variables

Model Description

The PLS model consists of two sets of equations: the structural and measurement equations. The structural equations specify the relationships among the latent variables and may be written as follows:

u = (m x 1) is a columu vector of unobserved dependent variables; r; =(nx 1)IS a column vector of unobserved independent (predictor) variables; p=t m x m) is a matrix of de~ndent variable ~~f~~ients; T = (RIx n) is a matrix of coefficients for the independent variables; and, E=( m x 1) is a column vector of residuals. The measurement equations specifying relations between the manifest and latent variables are:
y = ATpi? _._ (2)

Household

Store Brand Proneness

173

x = h&+6

--

(3)

where, y = (p x 1) is a column vector of observed dependent x = (q x 1) is a column vector of observed independent 4, = (p x m) is a matrix of regression coefficients variables; variables;

of y on n ; _ +X = (q x n) is a matrix of regression coefficients of x on 5 ; _ _E= Ip x 1) is a column vector of errors of criterion measurement; 6 = (q x 1) is a column vector of errors of predictor measurement.

and,

For convenience all variables are assumed to be standardized. Linear regression equations are used to model the relations between the variables. The latent variables are estimated as weighted aggregates of their respective indicators. The PLS algorithm (Lohmiiller, 1989) uses an iterative procedure to estimate weights for the indicators and regression coefficients. The method is described in detail by H. Wold (1975). The assessment of convergent and discriminant validity in the PLS context may be undertaken using the approaches developed by Fomell and Larcker (198 1) and Fomell, Tellis and Zinkhan (1982). Significance of the parameters can be tested by using Tukeys jackknife technique.

RESULTS

Validity

Testing

Before testing for the significance of relationships establish the convergent and discriminant validity of ment model. To assess convergent and discriminant and Fomell, et al (1982) have proposed using average (2), the variance shared by the construct rJj estimated puted as follows:

in our structural model, we need to the various constructs in the measurevalidity, Fomell and Larcker (1981) shared variance (p,,). From equation by different measures yij can be com-

where, 1 = measures of
Xi, = rlj;

loadings of yy on ~j; and,

j = 1, . ... m.

174

Journal

of Retailing

Vol.

72, No.

2 1996

The same method may be employed to compute pvCfor &. According to Fornell et aI. (1982) a reasonable condition for satisfying convergence is that pvCfor a construct should exceed OS. The p, for the model ranged between 0.59 (ECR) and 0.73 (PQV). Thus, there is a reasonable degree of convergence of the constructs, i.e., different indicators of each construct are in agreement. An acceptable test of discriminant validity (Fomell et al., 1982) is for the variance shared between any two constructs to be less than the variance shared between a construct and its measures. In ali cases, this condition was met. Thus, the various ~ons~cts differ from each other offering disc~minant validity for the constructs used in this investigation

TABLE 3 Measurement Model Parameter Estimates Construct and


Observed Age Yl Family Income Y2 Education Y3 Size of Family Y4 Intolerance Ys Familiarity (FAM) Yb Extrinsic Cue Reliance (ECR) xt x2
x3 x4

Variabels

Loadings

Error Variance

PVC 1.00

1 .I30

O.Ood 1 .oo

1 .oo

o.ooa 1 .oo

1 .oo

O.OOd 1.00

1 .oo of Ambiguity (INT) 1 .oo

0.003 1 .oo o.ooa 1 .oo

1.00

o.ooa 0.59

0.77 0.80 0.75 0.75 (PQV) 0.87 0.89 0.80

0.41 0.37 0.43 0.44 0.73 0.24 0.21 0.35 0.69

Perceived Quality Variation


x5

x6
x7

Perceived Risk (PRS) X8


x9

0.76 0.90

0.43 0.20 0.68

Perceived Value for Money (WM) Xl0 x11 Private Brand Proneness (PBP)
x12

0.89 0.76

0.21 0.42 7 .OO

1.00

o.ooa

Notes: a. Fixed parameters

Household

Store Brand

Proneness

175

Structural Coefficients Structural coefficients for the basic model are presented in Table 4. The t-values are from the jackknife parameter estimates and jackknife standard errors (Fenwick, 1979; Gray and Schucany, 1972). Three of the hypothesized direct paths to private brand proneness-those beginning with respondents intolerance of ambiguity (H12), education (H16), and age (H17)-were not found to be statistically significant. To gain a more accurate understanding of the relationships, the basic model was modified by deleting these three paths. The results of the revised model are shown in Table 4 and Figure 3. Fifteen structural coefficients are statistically significant at the 0.01 level or better. PLS provides standardized estimates and hence coefficients between constructs can be interpreted in the same way as one would interpret standardized regression coefficients in classical ordinary least squares regression. We find that perceived value for money is a significant predictor of household private brand proneness (Hl: ppvM,pBp = 0.144). Thus, households that perceive greater value for money in private label brands exhibit a higher propensity to buy these products. Higher perceived risk associated with buying private
TABLE 4

Structural Parameter Estimates and t-Values for Hypothesized and Revised Models
Hypothesized Standardized Parameter/ ~e~ationsb;~ H,: H,: H,: H,: H,: H,: H,: H,: H,: H,: H,,: H,,: H,J: H,,: H,,: H,,: H,,: PVM -+ PBP PRS I) Pm PRS -+ PVM tQV -+ PVM PQV -+ PRS ECR -+ PQV ECR -+ PRS FAM-+ECR FAM -+ PBP FAM -+ PRS FAM -+ PQV INT -+ PEP INT -+ PVM INT -+ ECR FIN -+ PRP EDU -+ PBP FSZ -+PBP
Stfuctufa/

Mode/ Standardized Structural

Revised Mode/

Coefficients 0.145 -0.092 -0.286 -0.329 0.336 0.568 0.224 -0.266 0.401 -0.271 -0.231 0.012 -0.117 0.251 -0.114 -0.002 -0.037 0.089

&-Value 28.27 -10.97 46.33 -46.40 40.58 131.52 33.92 -45.12 70.43 -29.64 -54.28 0.37 -24.64 35.41 -21.14 -0.18 -0.25 10.97

RJ

Coefficients 0.144 -0.084 -0.286 -0.329 0.336 0.568 0.224 -0.266 0.403 -0.217 -0.231 -0.117 0.251 -0.121

t-Value la.42 -9.92 -46.33 -46.40 40.58 131.52 33.92 -45.12 74.91 -29.64 -54.28 -24.64 35.41 -31.71

H, ?: AGE -+ PBP

0.097

12.06

Structural

Equations CR*)

Extrinsic Cue Reliance (ECR) Perceived Quality Variation (PQV) Perceived Risk (PRS) Perceived Value for Money (PVM) Private Brand Proneness (PEP)

0:145
0.451 0.391 0.334 0.302

0.145 0.451 0.391 0.334 0.300

176

Journal

of Retailing Vol. 72, No. 2 1996

Intolerance for Ambiguity (INT)

i3,3<-0.117

Private Brand Value for W

Money (PVM)

Proneness (PBP)

PAM)

Figure 3.

Revised Model withy Estimated

Parameter

Coeficents

label brands results in poorer value for money perceptions (H3: PPRS,PVM = -0.286) and, . p~~~~~~~ ultimately, decreases private brand proneness (H 2. = -0.084). The greater the perceived quality variation between national and store brand grocery items, the less favorable the value for money perceptions of store brands (H4: I3 PQV,PvM = -0.329), and the greater the perceived risks associated with store brand purchase (H5: PPQV,PRS = 0.336). Households that are inclined to rely on extrinsic cues in quality assessment perceive greater variation between national and store brand quality (H6: PECR,PQV= 0.568) and greater risk in store brand purchase (H7: PECR,PRs = 0.224). However, the good news for retailers is that greater familiarity with private label brands results in less reliance on extrinsic cues in quality assessment (HS: PFAM,ECR = - 0 _ 266) greater private brand proneness (H9: PFAM, PBP = 0.403), less perceived risk (HlO: PFAM,PRS = -0.217), and less perceived quality variation between national and store brand offerings (Hll: PFA~,P~V = -0.23 1). Importantly, greater intolerance of ambiguity results in less favorable value for money perceptions (HI31 PINT,PVM = -0.117) and greater propensity to rely on extrinsic cues when judging characterisproduct quality (H14: PrNT,ECn = 0.25 1). In terms of the socio-demographic tics, more affluent households exhibit lower propensity to buy private label brands (HE: &IN,PBP = -0.121). Finally, consistent with our predictions, the larger the households size, the greater the private brand proneness (H18: f3FSZ,PBP = 0.097).

Interpreting

the Importance

of Predictor

Variables

Variables exert influence through both simple direct and complex indirect causal chains (Schmidt, 1979; Davis, 1985). For example, in Figure 3, perceived quality variation

Household

Store Brand Proneness

177

between national and private label brands (PQV) is hypothesized to have a direct influence on value for money perceptions of these products (&.Qv,PvM). Concurrently, it also exerts influence through its effect on the perceived risk associated with buying store brands (BPQv,PRS). Perceived risk has a direct influence on perceived value for money #&s,~~M). The indirect effect is the product of indirect path coefficients (e.g., &~v,pRs*&~~,pv~) while the total effect is the sum of direct and indirect effects: PPQv,Pvh4* = PPQV,PVM
+

@PQV,PRS x P ~Rs,Pvd

Where PPQ,PvMLrepresents the total effect of PQV on PVM.


In some cases there will be more than one indirect path. For example, while FAM has a direct path to PRS, there are three indirect paths: FAM + ECR + PRS; FAM + PQV + PRS; and FAM + ECR + PQV + PRS. In such cases, all of the indirect effects are summed and added to the direct effect to obtain the total effect. It follows that indirect effects may play an important role in understanding the nature of the influence exerted by variables. As Noonan (1982) has pointed out, the question of statistical significance is not relevant for the total effects. These follow as a corollary of the inner structure of the model, and hence the issue of statistical significance enters only when deciding which direct paths should be retained in the model. Table 5 presents each of the total effects for the final model. A comparison of direct path coefficients from Table 4 indicates that the most important single construct predicting private brand proneness is brand familiarity (PFAM = .403). Relatively speaking, perceived value for money (PPvM = -.144), family income (PFIN = -.144), family size ( PFsz = .089), and perceived risk (p PRs = -0.0X4), in that order, have substantially lower influence on private brand proneness. An examination of total effects indicates that the extent of reliance on extrinsic cues (ECR) has strong indirect effects on various constructs. For example, although it has no direct effect on perceived value for money, the indirect effect is -0.315. Furthermore, the direct effect of ECR on perceived risk (PRS), 0.224,

TABLE5 Total Path Coeffkients


Dependent Constructs
INT FAM

for theRevised
PQV
-

Model*

Independent Constructs INT FAM


-

ECR

PRS

PVM
-

FIN
-

FSZ
-

ECR

PQV
PRS PVM FIN FSIZ PBP Notes:

0.251 0.143 0.104 -0.180


-0.031

-0,266 -0.382 -0.405


0.243 0.472

_ 0.568 0.415
-0.315 -0.063

_ 0.336
-0.425 -0.085

_ -0.286

_ -0.121

_ 0.097

-0.125 0.144

Values in the table refer to the total effect of the column variable on the row variable. For example the total effect of perceived risk on private brand proneness is -0.125. This is obtained by adding the direct effect of PRS on PEP C-.084) to its indirect effect through PVM (-0.286 x 0.144 = -0.041).

178

Journal of Retailing

Vol. 72, No. 2 1996

while ifipressive, only tells part of the story. When factoring in not only its direct effect, but also its indirect effect through PQV, its total effect is nearly doubled to .415. Finally, while perceived quality variation between national and store brands and the extent of reliance on extrinsic cues have no hypothesized direct effects on private brand proneness, they exert noteworthy negative influences on private brand proneness through their indirect effects.

DISCUSSION

Previous research has shown correlations between private brand proneness and a number of other socioeconomic, perceptual, and individual difference variables. This research has attempted to integrate these disparate findings into a cohesive framework to better understand the interrelationships between the variables thought to influence private brand proneness. By taking such an approach, it is posited that greater insight can be gained in the management of retailers private brands. An examination of the relative importance of the factors influencing private brand proneness reveals that familiarity with retailers private label brands is critical. The large relative importance of familiarity suggests that consumers who are familiar with private label products are likely to view them as high quality, low risk products, producing good value for the money. Consumers who lack experience with such brands are likely to view them with skepticism and consider them to be risky choices. Such perceptions may lead consumers to discount store brand quality and lower perceived value for money. Lack of familiarity may also serve to increase reliance on extrinsic cues such as brand name, packaging, and price in quality assessment-areas in which retailers private brands suffer from deficiencies relative to their national brand counterparts. Our findings suggest that an important first step in building greater consumer acceptance of retailers private brands is to increase consumer familiarity with them. Familiarity with retailers private label brands could be increased through in store taste tests, blind comparisons with national brands, distribution of free samples, or issuing store brand coupons to buyers of competing national brands at the checkout counter. Also of importance is consumers reliance on extrinsic cues in quality assessment. This reliance exerts strong negative effects on consumers attitudes towards store brands. Extrinsic cue reliance greatly heightens perceptions of quality variation between national and store brands and increases perceptions of risk associated with using these products. The PLMA asserts that store brand ingredients are as good if not better than those of national brands. However, in the absence of information concerning intrinsic quality, extrinsic cues serve as surrogate indicators of quality. Cues such as price, brand name, and packaging are easily recognizable and interpreted. Our results suggest that simple improvements in the extrinsic cues associated with store brands may go a long way towards increasing consumer acceptance of private label brands. European retailers understand this and have been successful in increasing store brand market share through dramatic improvements in package design, labeling, advertising, and branding strategies (Hester, 1988). This has contributed to the market success of companies such as Carrefour

Household Store Brand Proneness

179

which commands respectable market shares for its store brands (Ody, 1987). Given that store brands offer higher margins, investments in creating a more favorable image for private label brands may offer large returns and increase retailers competitiveness vis-a-vis national manufacturers. Consistent with perceptual studies of store brands (Bellizi et al., 1981; Cunningham et al., 1982; Myers, 1966), we find that misgivings regarding store brand quality are a major problem facing these products. Our results suggest that perceived quality variation is derived in large part from extrinsic cue driven inferences. However, for retailers to improve their competitive position in the market, it is important that they also pay close attention to maintaining high levels of intrinsic product quality. It may not be enough to have good quality. What may be needed is comparable qualityyuality which matches or even exceeds that of leading national brands. This will require that retailers be aware of consumer requirements and benchmark store brand quality against the category leader to achieve superiority. To the extent that there are geographic differences in consumers needs, retailers may be able to take advantage of their local dominance in particular markets. The success of Wegmans cola (McCarthy, 1992) can be partially attributed to managements success in benchmarking their product against the category leader in the customer segment and communicating the benefits of this store brand product to consumers. We find that promotional strategies emphasizing value for money may have a positive and significant effect on private brand proneness. However, it is clear from our results that such a promotional strategy may not be very effective if consumers perceive quality variation or risk associated with store brand purchase. This is consistent with the findings of Hoch and Banerji (1993) who found that high quality and consistency are much more important than price in determining market share for private label products. In addition, it may be that lower store brand prices themselves hurt value for money perceptions: That is, by lowering store brand prices, retailers may simply be signaling poorer store brand quality. This calls into question the wisdom of relying on a low price strategy to move store brands. Instead, a better focus may be to position store brands on the basis of quality. An emphasis on quality might be coupled with attempts to lessen the perceived risk associated with buying store brands. Perceived risk could be minimized by conducting in-store taste tests, publicizing the results of independent testing agencies, offering money back guarantees, and conducting image building campaigns to favorably portray the private label buyer as smart and quality conscious. The successful French hypermarket Carrefour aggressively promotes comparative information about store brands vis-a-vis national brands and maintains a mid-range pricing policy which compares favorably to the category leader. Their private label brands are typically not the cheapest version of the product. This has enabled them to gain as much as 25% of their turnover from private labels. We concur with Bettman (1974) that an information processing approach to private brand purchasing behavior holds promise. We find that intolerants of ambiguity, for example, process product related cues differently and rely on easier to process extrinsic cues such as price and brand name. Since private label brands lack easily decipherable brand related information, intolerants of ambiguity are inclined to perceive less value for money associated with store brands. This finding further suggests that retailers may draw a larger

180

journal of Retailing

Vol. 72, No. 2 1996

store brand franchise by improving ciated with private label brands.

the packaging,

labeling, and promotional

support asso-

LIMITATIONS Our study provides some interesting insights into factors which influence consumer private brand proneness. The proportion of variance explained by our model, although favorable when compared to past studies, is modest. The findings presented here should be validated using a new sample. We adopted a self-report measure of private brand proneness for twenty-eight products. Although this measure is an improvement over single-item measures typically used in previous research, it would be useful to employ a behavioral measure of this construct. Using panel data, one could track the actual number of private label brands purchased and use this information to develop a better picture of the effects of the variables hypothesized to influence consumers decisions to buy store brands. Lack of such data and resource constraints prevented us from employing such an approach. The measure of private brand proneness employed in this investigation asked consumers to rate the frequency with which they purchase a store brand for various products. Subjects may have different interpretations of what rarely or sometimes means. This could contribute to random noise in the measures, thus decreasing our ability to detect significant relationships. Next, it should be recognized that several of the predictor concepts (e.g., perceived risk, perceived value for the money, etc.) might take on different values for a given consumer in different product categories. The measures of these predictor variables were general (i.e., not product specific). The key dependent variable (private brand proneness) encompasses many different packaged grocery products and it is possible that consumers perceived risk, perceived value for money, etc., differ across these different product categories. Consequently, our global variable measures could not capture these differences. This is likely to have added to random error in the model and decreased the efficiency of our parameter estimates making it more difficult to find significant results. There is, however, no reason to believe that this introduced any systematic bias in the estimates. Furthermore, it could be argued that consumers decisions to buy a particular store brand may depend on the type of product under consideration. For example, a given household may be more prone to buy store brands of low rather than high involvement products. In addition, buying motives may play a role in purchase decisions of store brands. For example, consumers may be more prone to select store brands for think type rather than feel type products. Thus, consumers may prefer national brands for high involvement-feel type products such as toothpaste but consider store brands for low involvement-think type products such as cooking oil. To offer insight into product type differences, future researchers might consider analyzing private brand proneness separately for each of the four quadrants of the FCB Grid (Vaughn, 1986). We suggest that additional items should be developed for each of our constructs to improve their reliability and content validity and to provide more rigorous tests of their dimensionality. Two of our scales had only two items and the scale for familiarity had only

household Store Brand Proneness

181

one item. It could be argued that this inadequate sampling of the construct domains resulted in underestimation of the strengths of the hypothesized relationships. The low alpha value of the intolerance of ~bi~i~ scale (a = S65) should be contrasted with 0.49 reported by Budner (1962). He defended the low Cronbachs alpha on the grounds that the scale is free from artifacts such as acquiescence and social desirability which, because they are consistent, tend to maximize reliability estimates. There is also the nature of the construct, itself, the de~nition of which posits a complex, multidimension~ construct. Since increasing complexty of a trait increases the probability that individuals will exhibit unique patterns of the component elements, it is generally true that the more complex the construct, and the more complex the measure, the lower will the reliability of the estimate be. Although Budners intoler~ce of ~biguity scale is most commonly employed in behavioral research, future researchers might consider employing scales developed by Norton (1975) or McDonald (1970) which are longer but exhibit higher reliability. Finally, this study has ignored cultural differences which might partially account for the greater success of private label products in Europe. Future research could attempt to understand the role culture plays in this process.

CONCLUSfON

Store brands are products owned and branded by retailers. These products have been enormously successful in Europe where chains like Carrefour (France), Migros (Switzerland), Esselunga (Italy), and Sainsburys (England) have achieved significant market shares for their private label brands. Store brands help retailers increase store traffic and customer loyalty by offering exclusive lines under labels not found in competing stores. They offer higher margins, increase control over shelf space, and give retailers greater bargaining power in the channel of distribution. North American retailers are gradually recognizing the potential power of brand name ownership as witnessed by the success of Loblaws in Canada and Kroger in the U.S. These chains generate a quarter of their revenue from the sales of their private label brands. Despite the importance of store brands, little is known about what motivates consumers to buy private label brands. We have proposed and estimated a model of household private brand proneness. Our study findings have important managerial implications. Future research should attempt to incorporate improved measures of the various predictor constructs employed in this study (e.g., incorporating product-specific measures of the perceptual variables to reduce error variance and using more items in the scales for perceived risk, perceived value for the money and familiarity to improve their content validity and reliability). In addition, using behavioral as opposed to self-report measures of private brand proneness would increase confidence in the findings. Partial support for this project was provided by a research grant from a major Ac~owl~~en~ international supermarket chain which wishes to remain anonymous. Their support is gratefully acknowledged. The authors wish to acknowledge the research assistance of Mr. Amit Bhatnagar. The authors thank the editor and three anonymous JR reviewers for their invaluable help in revising the manuscript.

182

Journal of Retailing

Vol. 72, No. 2 1996

REFERENCES
Andrews, I. R. and Enos R. Valenzi. (1970). The Relationship Between Product Purchase Price and Blind Rated Quality, Journal of Marketing, 1: 36-39. Becherer, Richard C. and Lawrence M. Richard. (1978). Self-Monitoring as a Moderating Variable in Consumer Behavior, Journal of ConsumerResearch, S(December): 159-162. Bellizzi, Joseph A., Harry F. Kmckeberg, John R, Hamilton and Warren S. Martin. (1981). Consumer Perceptions of National, Private, and Generic Brands, Journal of Retailing, 57(Winter): 56-70. Bettman, James R. (1974). Relationship of Information-Processing Attitude Structures to Private Brand Purchasing Behavior, Journal of Applied Psychology, 59( 1): 79-83. Blake, B.F., R. Zenhausem, R. Perloff and R. Heslin. (1973). The Effect of Intolerance of Ambiguity upon Product Perceptions, Journal of Applied Psychology, 58: 239-243. Bookstein, F.L. (1980). Data Analysis by Partial Least Squares. In K. Kmenta and J.B. Ramsey (eds.), Evaluation of Econometric Methods. New York: Academic Press. Budner, S. (1962). Intolerance of Ambiguity as a Personality Variable, Journal of Personality, 30: 29-50. Burger, P.C. and B. Schott. (1972). Can Private Brand Buyers Be Identified?, Journal of Marketing Research, 9: 219-222. Bums, Greg. (1995). A Froot Loop by Any Other Name, Business Week, June 26,72,76. Coe, Barbara Davis. (1971). Private Versus National Preference Among Lower and Middle-Income Consumers, Journal of Retailing, 4(Fall): 61-72. Cunningham, Isabella CM., Andrew P. Hardy and Giovanna Imperia. (1982). Generic Brands Versus National Brands and Store Brands, Journal of Advertising Research, 22(0ctober/ November): 25-32. Cunningham, Ross M. (1961). Customer Loyalty to Store and Brand, Harvard Business Review, 39: 127-137. Davis, James A. (1985). The Logic of Causal Order. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Davies, K., CT. Gilligan and C.J. Sutton. (1986). The Development of Own Label Product Strategies in Grocery and DIY Retailing in the United Kingdom, International Journal of Retailing, l( 1): 6-19. Etgar, Michael and Naresh K. Malhotra. (198 1). Determinants of Price Dependency: Personal and Perceptual Factors, Journal of Consumer Research, 8(September): 217-226. Fenwick, I. (1979). Techniques in Market Measurement: The Jacknife, Journal of Marketing Research, 16(August): 410-414. Fomell, Claes and Fred L. Bookstein. (1982). Two Structural Equation Models: LISREL and PLS Applied to Consumer Exit-Voice Theory, Journal of Marketing Research, 19(November): 440452. Fomell, Claes and David F. Larcker. (1981). Evaluating Structural Equation Models with Unobservable Variables and Measurement Error, Journal of Marketing Research, 18(February): 39-50. Fomell, Claes, G.J. Tellis and G.M. Zinkhan. (1982). Validity Assessment: A Structural Equations Approach Using Partial Least Squares. Pp. 405-409 in B.J. Walker, et al. (eds.), An Assessment of Marketing Thought and Practice. Chicago, IL: American Marketing Association. Frank, R.E. and H.W. Boyd. (1965). Are Private-Brand Prone Grocery Customers Really Different? Journal of Marketing Research, 2(4): 27-35. Fugate, Douglas Lee. (1979). An Exploratory Investigation of the Effects of Manufacturer Disclosure on Consumer Perception of Private Brand Grocery Product Attributes. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Missouri-Columbia.

Household Store Brand Proneness

183

Geladi, P. (1987). History and Uses of Partial Least Squares (PLS). Pp. 5-13 in M. Martens (ed.), Data Approximation by PLS Methods. Oslo: Norwegian Computing Center. Gray, H.L. and W.R. Schucany. (1972). The Generalized Jacknife Statistic. New York: Marcel Dekker. Hawes, Jon M., Stephen P. Hutchens and John Thanopoulos. (1982). Quality and Value Perceptions of Arkansas Consumers for National, Private, and Generic Brand Grocery Products, Arkansas Business and Economic Review, 15: 4-10. Hester, Thomas. (1988). Packaging and Design: Shelf Deceit, Marketing, (May): 47-48. Hoch, Stephen J. and Shumeet Banerji. (1993). When Do Private Labels Succeed? Sloan Management Review, 34(Summer): 57-67. Howard, John A. and Jagdish N. Sheth. (1969). The Theory of Buyer Behavior. New York: John Wiley and Sons. Jacoby, Jacob, Jerry Olson and Rafael Haddock. (1971). Price, Brand Name and Product Composition Characteristics as Determinants of Perceived Quality, Journal of Applied Psychology, %(December): 57-59. Liesse, Julie. (1993). Private Label Nightmare, Advertising Age, April 12, 1,4-5. Livesey F. and P. Lennon. (1978). Factors Affecting Consumers Choice Between Manufacturer Brands And Retailer Own Brands, European Journal of Marketing, 12(2): 158-170. Lohmiiller, Jan-Bemd. (1989). Latent Variuble Path Modeling with Partial Least Squares. Heidelberg: Pysica-Verlag. Martell, Deborah. (1986). Own Labels: Problem Child or Infant Prodigy? Quarterly Review of Marketing, ll(Summer): 7-12. McCarthy, Michel J. (1992). Soft Drink Giants Sit Up and Take Notice as Sales of Store Brands Show More Fizz The Wall Street Journal, March 6, B 1. McDaniel, Carl and R.C. Baker. (1977). Convenience Food Packaging and the Perception of Product Quality, Journal ofMarketing, 4l(October): 57-58. McDonald, A.P. Jr. (1970). Revised Scale for Ambiguity Tolerance: Reliability and Validity, Psychological Reports, 26: 791-798. McGoldrick, Peter J. (1984). Grocery Generics-An Extension of the Private Label Concept, European Journal of Marketing, 18( 1): 5-24. Murphy, Patrick E. (1978). The Effect of Social Class on Brand and Price Consciousness for Supermarket Products, Journal ofRetailing, 54(Summer): 33-42, 89. Myers, John G. (1966). Determinants of Brand Imagery and Attitude with Special Reference to Private Brands. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Northwestern University. (1967). Determinants of Private Brand Attitude, Journal of Marketing Research, 4: 73-81. Noonan, Richard D. (1982). School Environments and School Outcomes: An Empirical Comparative Study Using the IEA Data. Pp. 169-202 in Manfred Niessen and Jules Perschar (eds.), Comparative Research on Education: Overview, Strategy and Applications in Eastern and Western Europe. Oxford: Pergamon Press. Noonan, Richard D. and Herman Wold. (1985). Partial Least Squares Path Analysis. Pp. 37693775 in Torsten Husen and T. Neville Postlethwaite (ed.), International Encyclopedia of Education, Research and Studies. Oxford: Pergamon Press. Norton, R.W. (1975). Measurement of Ambiguity Tolerance, Journal of Personality Assessment, 39: 607-619. Ody, Penelope. (1987). The Growth in Private Brands, Retail & Distribution Management, lS(May/June): 9-l 1. Olson, Jerry C. (1972). Cue Utilization in the Quality Perception Process: A Cognitive Model and an Empirical Test. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Purdue University.

184

Journal of Retailing Vol. 72, No. 2 1996

Olson, Jerry C. and Jacob Jacoby. (1973). Cue Utilization in the Quality Perception Process. Pp. 167-179 in M. Venkatesan (ed.), Proceedings 3rd Annual Conference. Chicago, IL: Association for Consumer Research. Papademetrion, Demetrious and Gerald W. Hopple. (1982). Causal Modeling in International Migration Research: A Methodological Prolegomenon, Quality and Quantity, 16: 369-402. Park, C. Whan and Frederick W. Winter. (1979). Product Quality Judgement: Information Processing Approach, Journal of Marketing Research Society, Zl(July): 2 1 l-217. Patti, Charles H. and Raymond P. Fisk. (1982). National Advertising, Brands and Channel Control: An Historical Perspective with Contemporary Options, Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, lO(Winter/Spring): 90-108. Pinson, Christian R.A., Arun K. Jain and Naresh Malhotra. (1980). Identifying Cognitive Style Determinants of Retail Patronage. Pp. 358-379 in Michael J. Baker and Michael A. Saren (eds.), Marketing in the Eighties. Edinburgh: Proceedings, European Academy for Advanced Research in Marketing and the Marketing Education Group of the U.K. Purwar, Prem C. (1982). The Role of Price Cue in Product Quality Perception: A Comprehensive Model and an Empirical Investigation. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, State University of New York at Buffalo. Quelch, John and David Harding. (1996). Brands versus Private Labels: Fighting to Win, Harvard Business Review, 74( 1): 99- 111. Raju, P.S. (1977). Product Familiarity, Brand Name and Price Influence on Product Evaluation. Pp. 64-7 1 in William D. Perreault, Jr. (ed.), Advnnces in Consumer Research, Vol. 4. Atlanta, GA: Association for Consumer Research. Rao, Akshay R. and Kent B. Monroe. (1989). The Effect of Price, Brand Name, and Store Name on Buyers Perceptions of Product Quality: An Integrative Review Journal of Marketing Research, ZL(August): 351-357. Rao, Tannira R. (1969). Are Some Consumers More Prone to Purchase Private Brands? Journal of Marketing Research, S(November): 447-550. Richardson, Paul S., Alan S. Dick and Arun K. Jain. (1994). Extrinsic and Intrinsic Cue Effects on Perceptions of Store Brand Quality, Journal of Marketing, 58(0ctober): 28-36. Schaffer, D.R. and C. Hendrick. (1974). Dogmatism and Tolerance for Ambiguity as Determinants Journal of Personality and Social of Differential Reactions to Cognitive Inconsistency, Psychology, 29: 60 l-608. Schmidt, P. (1979). On Decomposition of Effects in Lognitudinal Data. In Proceedings, Conference on Systems under Indirect Observations: Causality, Structure, and Prediction, Center de Rencontres: Castigny, Switzerland, October, 18-20. Simmons, Martin and Bill Meredith. (1984). Own Labels Profile and Purpose, Journal of the Market Research Society, 26(January): 3-27. Smock, C.D. (1955). The Influence of Psychological Stress on Intolerance of Ambiguity, Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 50: 177- 182. Sundel, Harvey H. (1974). An Experimental Analysis of Consumer Attitudes Towards Grocery Products Under Manufacturers Brands, Nationally Distributed Private Brands, and Locally Distributed Private Brands. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Saint Louis University, St. Louis, Missouri. Valenzi, Enos R. and Larry Eldridge. (1973). Effects of Price Information, Composition Differences, Expertise and Rating Scales on Product-Quality Ratings. Pp. 8, 829-830 in Proceedings, 81st Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association. Vaughn, Richard. (1980). How Advertising Works: A Planning Model, Journal of Advertising Research, (October): 27-33.

Household

Store Brand Proneness

185

Wheatley, John J. and John S.Y. Chiu. (1977). The Effect of Price, Store Image and Product and Respondent Characteristics on Perceptions of Quality, Journal ofMarketing Research, 14(May): 181-186. Wheatley, John J., Richard G. Walton and John S. Y. Chiu. (1977). The Influence of Prior Product Experience, Price, and Brand on Quality Perceptions. Pp. 72-77 in William D. Perreault, Jr. (ed.), Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 4. Atlanta, GA: Association for Consumer Research. Weld, H. (1975). Path Models with Latent Variables: The NIPALS Approach. Pp. 307-357 in H.M. Blalock, et al. (eds.), Quantitative Sociology: International Perspectives on Mathematical and Statistical Modeling. New York: Academic Press. (1982). Soft Modeling: The Basic Design and Some Extensions. Pp. 1-54 in J.G. Joreskog and H. Weld (eds.), System Under Indirect Observation, Causality, Structure, Prediction, Vol. 2 (2 volume set). Wolinsky, Asher. (1987). Brand Names and Price Discrimination, Journal of Industrial Economics, 35(March): 255-268. Zinkhan, Geroge, Eric Jochimsthaler and Thomas Kinnear. (1987). Individual Differences and Marketing Decision Support System Usage and Satisfaction, Journal ofMarketing Research, 24: 208-214.

Вам также может понравиться