Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 8

Food Quality and Preference 21 (2010) 148155

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Food Quality and Preference


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/foodqual

Sensory characterization of dry-cured ham using free-choice proling


M. Dolors Gurdia, Ana P.S. Aguiar, Anna Claret, Jacint Arnau, Luis Guerrero *
IRTA-Centro de Tecnologia de los Alimentos, Finca Camps i Armet, E-17121 Monells, Spain

a r t i c l e

i n f o

a b s t r a c t
In this study, free-choice proling was carried out to study how consumers perceived and described the sensory properties of dry-cured ham. One hundred and nine consumers from three different Spanish regions evaluated the sensory characteristics of four different commercial dry-cured hams aged with ageing times of 6, 9, 12 and 16 months. Data were analysed by means of Generalized Procrustes Analysis. In order to understand the sensory consumers vocabulary better a Quantitative Descriptive Analysis with seven trained assessors was also performed. In general, results showed that consumers used simple terms to describe the sensory characteristics of the samples, paying special attention to the appearance, especially, the colour and fat content of the dry-curedham. There was some disagreement in the case of salty taste and texture descriptors, but, apart from that a consensus in the use of sensory attributes was observed. Data from trained assessors proved to be a useful method for understanding and validating consumers vocabulary. 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Article history: Received 19 February 2009 Received in revised form 19 August 2009 Accepted 19 August 2009 Available online 23 August 2009 Keywords: Free-choice proling Consumer perception Dry-cured ham Sensory properties

1. Introduction The sensory quality of dry-cured ham results from the interactions between the characteristics of the fresh matter and the biochemical changes occurring during the processing (Arnau, Guerrero, & Srraga, 1998; Buscailhon, Gandermer, & Monin, 1994; Parolari, Virgili, & Schivazappa, 1994; Vestegaard, Schivazappa, & Virgili, 2000). The extent of these changes depends on the initial composition of the raw material and the technological process (Buscailhon, Berdagu, & Monin, 1993) and the ageing duration (Gurdia, Guerrero, Gou, Monfort, & Arnau, 1999). In addition to these factors affecting sensory properties, there are further aspects such as habits of consumption (Mili, Mahlau, & Furitsch, 1996) and socio-cultural characteristics related to the geographical region (Cilla et al., 2006) that also affect consumers quality perception of dry-cured ham. Sensory descriptive tests are among the most sophisticated tools used by sensory scientists (Lawless & Heymann, 1998) and involve the discrimination and description of both the qualitative and quantitative sensory components (Meilgaard, Civille, & Carr, 1991). Several different methods exist within descriptive analysis that reect different sensory approaches (Lawless & Heymann, 1998). Quantitative Descriptive Analysis (QDA) is a technique frequently used in sensory characterization of food. In this respect, several authors (Cilla, Martnez, Beltrn, & Roncals, 2006; Garca-Gonzlez et al., 2006; Guerrero, Gou, Alonso, & Arnau, 1996; Rousset & Martin, 2001) have used trained panels to
* Corresponding author. Tel.: +34 972 630052; fax: +34 972 630373. E-mail address: lluis.guerrero@irta.es (L. Guerrero). 0950-3293/$ - see front matter 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.foodqual.2009.08.014

describe the sensory properties of different types of dry-cured ham. However, QDA is very time-consuming due to the extensive training that assessors must undergo. Furthermore, Piggott, Sheen, and Apostolidou (1990) pointed out that trained and experienced assessors normally tend to generate complex and scientically oriented terms in sensory research which may be difcult for nonspecialized people to understand. Free-choice proling (FCP) is a sensory methodology that differs from other descriptive methods because it is not necessary to use a common vocabulary of attributes to describe the samples, nor are the panellists expected to agree on their interpretation of the terms used. By means of FCP, each participant produces their own descriptive proles of the products, without having to explain the exact meaning (Williams & Langron, 1984). This is based on the assumption that panellists do not differ in their perceptions, but merely in the way in which they describe them. FCP is similar to traditional proling in that assessors must be able to detect differences between the samples, verbally describe the perceived attributes and quantify them (Oreskovich, Klein, & Sutherland, 1991). The FCP strategy can yield important insights into consumer differentiation of products and establish relationships between consumer preferences and sensory characteristics (Jack & Piggott, 1992). The analysis of the data collected from FCP is normally carried out by means of Generalized Procrustes Analysis (Gower, 1975; Langrom, 1983). This statistical technique allows for the rationalization of the spatial congurations derived from individual proles. The result is a consensus conguration revealing the interrelationships between the samples for the panel as a whole (Williams & Langron, 1984). Regarding the number of attributes generated, this is limited only by the perceptual and descriptive

M.D. Gurdia et al. / Food Quality and Preference 21 (2010) 148155

149

skills of the consumer (Oreskovich et al., 1991) and, according to Deliza, MacFie, and Hedderley (2005), terms used by untrained assessors may vary, based on their individual experience and familiarity with the product. To summarize, FCP offers the possibility of assisting the demands of marketing and product development teams who require information on target consumers perception of products, rather than the more technical descriptions of the products typically provided by trained sensory panels (Elmore & Heymann, 1999; Murray, Delahunty, & Baxter, 2001). The aim of this study was to evaluate how consumers representing three different Spanish regions perceive and describe sensory characteristics of four different types of commercial dry-cured ham using FCP. Furthermore and, in order to surmount the inconvenience related to the interpretation of the consumers vocabulary, a QDA with trained assessors was used to facilitate comprehension of the terms generated by consumers.

oxygen scavenger (Tyvek LH3000, ATCO) and then stored at 2 C 2 C for a maximum period of 7 days. 2.2. Physicochemical description of samples Moisture and fat content of six slices per ham were analysed by near infrared transmittance (NIT) spectroscopy with an INFRATEC 1265 Meat Analyzer (Tecator AB, Sweden). NaCl content was determined by the CharpentierVolhard method (ISO, 1970). Physicochemical composition of dry-cured hams used in the study is shown in Table 1.
TM

2.3. Descriptive sensory analysis 2.3.1. Free-choice proling (FCP) A group of 109 consumers aged between 21 and 68, recruited in the Centre (Madrid) (n = 34), South (Sevilla) (n = 37) and North (Girona) (n = 38) of Spain, participated in the study. The recruitment was randomly made by phone, using a lter questionnaire specically elaborated for this study, including demographic and socio-economic information (gender, age, education level, and working situation) and frequency of consumption of different food products including dry-cured ham. Only consumers who declared eating ham at least once every two weeks, and with no previous experience in sensory analysis, were selected. To avoid the effect of storage time on the sensory properties of dry-cured ham, sensory evaluation of the samples was carried out in all three cities in the same week. Two FCP sessions with 1520 consumers each were carried out per city. Participants were received in a conference room and placed at individual tables under white lighting (700 lx 150 lx). First of all, consumers were briefed on the concept of the methodology and the procedure. The aim of this was to allow for development of the vocabulary to describe the four types of dry-cured ham. Each consumer was given one slice of each of the four dry-cured hams. They were instructed to include only objective attributes and not to use hedonic terms. During this task, the consumers were rst asked to describe the appearance and the odour of the samples. After, they were requested to taste the sample and to describe the avour and texture characteristics writing their personal perception in their own words. Then, each consumer once again received one slice of each type and scored the four types of dry-cured ham using his/her own previously generated descriptors. In this case, the same hams were presented but with different codes and in a different order. All samples were assessed in the same tasting session balancing the rst-order and carry-over effects (Mace, Bratchell, Greenhoff, & Vallis, 1989). Sensory attributes were evaluated using an unstructured 100 mm line scale anchored with the words absence and intense at each end. In all sessions, slices of dry-cured ham were evaluated at room temperature (20 C) and presented on white plastic plates covered with a food grade PVC lm (oxygen permeability; 20,000 cm3/m2/

2. Materials and methods 2.1. Dry-cured ham sample preparation Four types of commercial Spanish dry-cured ham from white pig breeds with aging times 6, 9, 12 and 16 months (samples A, B, C and D, respectively) were selected to provide a wide range of sensory variability and to stimulate the generation of attributes by consumers. Three dry-cured hams of each type were boned, cut at the head of the femur level and then sliced (1.5 0.2 mmthick) perpendicularly to the femur axis in the distal part direction (Fig. 1) with a vertical slicer Kolossal 350 BVK (Marconi, Italy). So that the panellists were not distracted by testing cuts from different areas of the dry-cured ham (Guerrero, Gurdia, & Arnau, 2004, 2005), the slices evaluated by each consumer were from the same anatomical position for each one of the four dry-cured hams assessed. Slices were placed inside polypropylene trays, packed in bags (50 lm polyamide/100 lm polyethylene multilayer; oxygen permeability: 7 cm3/m2/24 h at 4 C and 80% RH; CO2 permeability: 1150 cm3/m2/24 h at 23% and 75% RH; water-vapour permeability: 1.5 g/m2/24 h; SACOLIVA, S.L., Castellar del Valls, Spain) under modied atmosphere (99.8 0.1% N2 and 0.1 0.1% O2) with an

Table 1 Least square means and RMSE for moisture, fat content and NaCl and each type of dry-cured ham (n = 3). p Samples* A Moisture (%) Fat content (%) NaCl (%)
20 mm 25 mm
a,b

RMSE B 48.17a 12.36b 5.10b C 50.71a 7.13b 5.63a D 40.65b 19.68a 4.79b 3.035 4.001 0.231

<0.0001 0.0002 <0.0001

51.34a 6.83b 6.05a

Fig. 1. Schematic view of the dry-cured ham sampling: (a) sample for physicochemical analysis; (b) sample for sensory analysis (QDA) and (c) sample for consumer study.

Within a row, least square means with different superscripts differs signicantly (p < 0.05). RMSE: root mean standard error. * A = 6, B = 9, C = 12 and D = 18 months of ageing time, respectively.

150

M.D. Gurdia et al. / Food Quality and Preference 21 (2010) 148155

24 h; water-vapour transmission rate 2000 g/m2/24 h; Macopal, S.L., Lli de Vall, Spain) to prevent drying. Samples were coded with three-random numbers and, for each consumer, all the sample slices came from the same anatomical position. Mineral water and unsalted crisp-bread were provided to rinse consumers mouths between samples. 2.3.2. Quantitative Descriptive Analysis (QDA) Seven selected and trained assessors (ASTM, 1981; ISO 8586-1, 1993; ISO 8586-2, 1994) undertook the sensory analysis on 1.5 mm slices from the same dry-cured hams (n = 12, three dry-cured hams and four ageing times) previously evaluated by the consumers. The generation of the descriptors had been carried out in open discussion in two previous sessions. The descriptors retained were: sweetness (basic taste sensation elicited by sugar), saltiness (basic taste sensation elicited by NaCl), bitterness (basic taste sensation elicited by caffeine and L-tryptophan), metallic (avour similar to a solution of FeSO47H2O), piquantness (stinging sensation in the mouth and throat), matured avour (set of complex nuances characteristic of dry-cured meat products, not described by other avour attributes), aged (avour related to aged fat which is characteristic of long aged Spanish dry-cured ham partially skinned according to the typical V shape; Gou, Arnau, & Gurdia, 2000), adhesiveness (textural property rated by the degree to which the surface of the ham slice adheres to the palate when compressed with the tongue), hardness (amount of pressure required to completely compress the sample), crumbliness (textural property characterized by ease with which a sample can be separated into smaller particles during chewing), pastiness (feeling of paste detected in hams with a high proteolytic index), brousness (textural property characterized by the perception of the amount of muscle bres detected during chewing). The references used to illustrate the maximum intensity of hardness, crumbliness and pastiness were those described by Guerrero, Gou, and Arnau (1999). A non-structured scoring scale (Amerine, Pangborn, & Roessler, 1965) was used, where 0 meant absence of the descriptor and 10 meant high intensity of the descriptor. Sensory evaluation was undertaken in three sessions and a complete block design was used (Steel & Torrie, 19601983), where each taster assessed the four different types of dry-cured ham in each session. Samples were coded with three-random numbers and were presented to the assessors balancing the rst-order and the carry-over effects as much as possible according to Mace et al. (1989). All hams were evaluated in slices from the same anatomical area. 2.4. Data analysis 2.4.1. Free-choice proling data analysis Data from the FCP was submitted to Generalized Procrustes Analysis, and a consensus matrix was obtained by using mathematical data operations according to Arnold and Williams (1986). The average of all transformed congurations that shows the minimum overall deviation was obtained as a consensus space. It summarizes the information on the samples and replaces the panel mean (Langrom, Williams, & Collins, 1984). Similar terms were grouped together in order to simplify the map obtained. Only agreed descriptors with correlation coefcients higher than 0.6 in at least one of the rst two dimensions and mentioned by more than eight consumers were used to visualize the relationships between samples and attributes. The frequency of descriptors used by consumers in each city was analysed using Simple Correspondence Analysis (Greenacre & Belsius, 1994), including gender, age, education level, working situation and consumption frequency of each consumer as supplementary variables.

Statistical analyses were carried out using XLSTAT software (Addinsoft, France). 2.4.2. Quantitative descriptive data analysis Data from the Quantitative Descriptive Analysis was analysed by means of the ANOVA test using the GLM procedure of SAS (2001). The ANOVA test was performed over the mean score (seven assessors) for each dry-cured ham because each dry-cured ham is the experimental unit (when performing sensory analysis all the assessors evaluate the same ham). The model included the type of dry-cured ham (ageing time) and the taste session as xed effects. The interaction assessor type of dry-cured ham was tested and dropped from the model since it was not signicant (p > 0.05). Differences between treatments were tested by the Tukey test. Furthermore, a Principal Component Analysis was carried out on the mean score for each dry-cured ham. 2.4.3. Physicochemical data analysis Data from physicochemical analysis were submitted to oneway ANOVA (SAS, 2001) including the type of dry-cured ham (ageing time) as a xed effect. Mean comparisons were carried out using the Tukey test. 3. Results and discussion Demographic, socio-economic characteristics and frequency of dry-cured ham consumption of the consumers involved in the present study are shown in Table 2. About 44.0% of the consumers were men and 56.0% women and most of them were aged between 20 and 50 (80.7%). The nal age distribution of the consumers who participated in the present study showed a bias when compared with the Spanish population (33.41% of which are older than 50; INE, 2007). This bias may be explained by the additional selection criteria used of consumption frequency eat ham at least once every two weeks. Normally, people aged over 5055 tend to reduce salt and fat consumption in order to prevent cardiovascular risk factors (Black, 2000) and both are present in dry-cured ham. A large percentage of consumers had a medium education level (45.9%) similar to the Spanish average (48.03%, INE, 2001) and, 67.9% of them were employees. In this case, the ofcial data (INE, 2001) in Spain shows that employees represented 55.58% of the

Table 2 Demographic, socio-economic characteristics and frequency of dry-cured ham consumption of the participants (% of respondents, n = 109). Socio-demographic characteristic Gender Male Female Age 2035 years 3650 years >50 years Education level Primary school Secondary school University Working situation Employed Unemployed or retired Student Consumption frequency >3 times a week 2 or 3 times a week Once a week <Once a week % of respondents 44.0 56.0 39.4 41.3 20.2 20.2 45.9 33.9 67.9 28.4 3.7 22.9 47.7 18.3 11.0

M.D. Gurdia et al. / Food Quality and Preference 21 (2010) 148155

151

population aged over 16 and in this sense our sample was also slightly biased. Almost half of the respondents (47.7%) stated that they consumed dry-cured ham two or three times a week, 22.9% more than three times a week, 18.3% once a week and 11.0% less than once a week. These results may be explained by the high Spanish consumption per capita (4.6 kg per year, MAPA, 2005) and also because the consumption of dry-cured ham (higher than once every two weeks) was one of the criteria of recruitment. These results are in agreement with those obtained by Cilla et al. (2006), Resano, Sanjun, and Albisu (2007) and Morales, Guerrero, Claret, Gurdia, and Gou (2008). The results obtained that each consumer in FCP used 10 attributes, on average, to describe differences between samples. The number of attributes ranged from 4 to 20 (Fig. 2). Most of the terms used by consumers (39.7%) referred to appearance, 26.5% to avour, 17.9% to texture and 15.9% to odour. The high number of consumers words dealing with appearance (n = 431) agrees with Guerrero, Aguiar, Gurdia, Claret, and Arnau (2007) which stated that this is the most important factor for Spanish consumers in dry-cured ham purchase decisions. Furthermore, Kster (2003) pointed out that visual attributes are easier to describe than the oronasal senses, because vision and hearing are an inborn mechanism, whereas the other senses rely largely on learning. Table 3 shows the frequency of occurrence for the most frequent attributes mentioned by consumers. Colour, intensity of dry-cured ham odour, saltiness, intensity of dry-cured ham avour and fat content were mentioned by more than 59% of the respondents. On taking into account the consumer response, it can be stated that the aforementioned descriptors probably represent the most important sensory attributes in dry-cured ham or, according to Kster (2003), the easiest to elicit. In a similar way, Resano et al. (2007) observed that 90% of consumers from the central Spanish region considered colour, fat and salty taste as very or quite important in dry-cured ham selection. Also, Morales et al. (2008) found that more than 70% of consumers from Catalonia (Northeast part of Spain) pointed out that colour, salty taste, aged avour and texture were the most important parameters affecting the purchase of dry-cured ham. Fig. 3 shows the graphic results of GPA over the consumer sensory proles. The variance accounting for the rst two components was 83.7%. Similar descriptors used by different assessors and located in the same area in the rst two dimensions have been grouped. In these cases the size of the descriptor is proportional to the number of consumers who agree on their use (Issanchou, Schlich, & Lesschaeve, 1992). The results obtained revealed that

Table 3 Frequency of occurrence for the most frequently elicited descriptors. Attribute Colour (dark, red) Odour intensity (dry-cured ham odour) Saltiness, amount of salt Flavour intensity (dry-cured ham avour) Fat content Matured Marbling Texture Dry Hard (tough) White spots Tender Juiciness Brightness Appearance Rancid Soft Fat colour Uniform Sinews Slice size Slice shape Thickness
a

Original words (in Spanish) Color, color oscuro, rojo Olor, olor a jamn Salado, cantidad de sal Sabor, intensidad, a jamn Grasa, tocino Curado, curacin Veteado, vetas Textura Seco, sequedad Duro, dureza Pintas blancas, puntos blancos Tierno, blando Jugoso, jugosidad Brillo, brillante Aspecto, presencia Rancio Suave Color de la grasa Uniformidad, homogeneidad Nervios Tamao de la loncha Forma de la loncha Grosor, espesor

Frequencya (%) 87.2 75.2 72.5 66.1 59.6 34.9 31.2 22.0 20.2 20.2 19.3 19.3 15.6 13.8 13.8 11.9 11.0 11.0 9.2 9.2 9.2 8.3 8.3

Percentage of respondents (n = 109).

sample D (with the highest ageing time) located in the negative area of the rst dimension was mainly dened by colour, dry-cured ham odour and avour and fat content and, to a lesser degree (the smaller letter size) by marbling, saltiness, texture, matured avour, juiciness and appearance. Conversely, sample A (with the least ageing time) located in the positive direction of the rst axis was characterized by hardness, dryness, saltiness and atypical avour/ odour attributes. Ageing is referred to as one of the main factors responsible for developing sensory characteristics of dry-cured ham due to biochemical reactions that occur throughout this process (Flores, Ingram, Bett, Toldr, & Spanier, 1997). In this study, another consideration to explain the differences observed between samples A and D is their different physicochemical composition, particularly in fat and moisture content (Table 1). Dry-cured hams B and C show intermediate characteristics within this rst dimension of the consensus space. However, the second dimension con-

20

number of consumers

15

10

0 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20

number of atributes

undefined

appearance

odour

taste

texture

Fig. 2. Frequency of attributes used by consumers to describe dry-cured ham samples.

152
1

M.D. Gurdia et al. / Food Quality and Preference 21 (2010) 148155

B
odour tender
soft

Dimension 2 (26.5%)

0.5
flavour

fat

D
shape

marbling
appearance

saltiness

juiceness

atypical flavour/odour hard

0 rancid

saltiness
brightness

odour colour
fibrous/hard

texture cured

A
dry

-0.5

flavour
white spots
cured

saltiness

C
-1 -1 -0.8 -0.6 -0.4 -0.2

saltiness

tender

0.2

0.4

0.6

0.8

Dimension 1 (57.2%)
Fig. 3. Consensus space obtained using Generalised Procrustes Analysis over the sensory consumer prole of dry-cured ham (the letters represent the dry-cured ham samples). The size of each descriptor is proportional to the number of assessors who agree on their use. Only those descriptors having a correlation coefcient higher or equal 0.06 with one of the two rst dimensions of the consensus space are shown.

trasts sample B in the positive direction, with sample C in the negative. Sample C is described as having white spots, matured, saltiness and tenderness attributes. Some discrepancies were observed among consumers regarding perception of saltiness in this study. Conversely, Parolari (1994) did not observe these differences in a similar investigation performed on Italian Parma hams. Saltiness was located in the four areas dened by the rst two dimensions, which means that they were not used in the same way by the participants. Saltiness is a basic taste and a well-known sensory attribute, so agreement among consumers would be expected. This apparent contradiction detected in saltiness can be explained by the fact that salt content (NaCl) in Spanish dry-cured ham is very heterogeneous, particularly when there are important differences in moisture content between muscles (Arnau, Guerrero, Casademont, & Gou, 1995). Since several consumers only tasted a portion of the slices provided, this could have contributed to the discrepancies in saltiness perception among them. Another possible explanation for this contradictory result is that dry-cured ham is a product with high salt content that could lead to an adaptation in consumer perception. OMahony (1986) explains that when a stimulus is experienced (i.e., a product is tasted), some residue remains in the mouth. The taste system adapts to such residual stimuli rendering it less sensitive to such stimuli, and when tasted subsequently the high salt content will taste less intense. In order to reduce any desensitization due to adaptation, unsalted crisp-bread and mineral water were provided to rinse the mouth at the beginning of the session and between samples. However, it is probable that all the consumers did not follow these recommendations fully. Conversely, the trained assessors always tasted the central part of each slice containing a portion of semimembranosus and biceps femoris muscles in order to block differences in salt content between them. Furthermore, trained panellists are aware of the fact that the taste system adapts to residual stimuli when dry-cured ham is tasted and they follow the abovementioned instructions. In our opinion these are the main reasons for explaining why the problems observed in the consumer study were not observed in the QDA study. Hardness, brousness and tenderness attributes also showed a slight disagreement (Fig. 3). Some consumers described sample A as hard in the positive direction of the rst dimension. However, brousness, hardness and tenderness descriptors were also placed

in the negative area of this rst dimension, close to sample D. Furthermore, tenderness was also explained by the second dimension and related with sample C in the negative direction of this axis. Again, this discrepancy could be due to the slice portion tasted by each consumer, since external muscles (semimembranosus) are usually much harder than internal ones (biceps femoris) (RuizRamrez, Arnau, Serra, & Gou, 2006). The meaning of the term texture (located in the negative area of the rst dimension) is not easily interpreted, as it is of a multidimensional concept and therefore a number of confusing attributes come together. In this sense, Szczesniak (2002) stated that consumers have difculty in describing texture due to its complex nature, since there are no single and specic receptors for it, in contrast to other sensory attributes, such as colour and taste. Therefore, the apparent discrepancy observed in our results could be due to the fact that consumers have different ways of describing their perceptions, depending on individual experiences, preferences and familiarity with the product (Deliza et al., 2005). In addition, they may also differ considerably in their ability to express themselves (Lachnit, Busch-Stocksch, Kunert, & Krahl, 2003). Dry-cured ham samples were also sensory evaluated by a trained panel to clarify and understand the consumer vocabulary better. Least square means corresponding to the sensory descriptors are shown in Table 4 (in this table, n = 3 means that three dry-cured hams of each ageing time were sensory evaluated). The total number of different descriptors generated by the trained panel was slightly higher than those from the consumers. Conversely, Moskowitz (1983) stated that trained panellists use fewer descriptors than those untrained, because it seems that the effort expended in accurate description replaces the effort used to capture all attributes. Ten of the selected descriptors by the expert panel were discriminant (p < 0.05) between dry-cured hams. Samples A and D were different in eight of the twelve descriptors used, however, B and C only differed in ve of them (Table 4). Fig. 4 shows the graphic results of PCA over the sensory descriptors from the trained panel and dry-cured ham samples. The results obtained with GPA (Fig. 3) and PCA (Fig. 4) are complementary to each other and sample location was similar among consumers and trained assessors. Guerrero, Gou, and Arnau (1997) also obtained similar results when they compared sensory proles from expert and semi-trained assessors. In this study, samples A and D were differentiated by the rst dimension, whereas B and C were separated by the second dimension. The comparison

Table 4 Least square means and RMSE for each sensory descriptor and for each type of drycured ham obtained from the sensory analysis (n = 3). Attributes Samples* A Flavour Sweetness Saltiness Bitterness Metallic Piquantness Matured avour Aged avour Texture Adhesiveness Hardness Crumbliness Pastiness Fibrousness 0.3c 4.9a 1.3 3.1a 2.1a 2.4b 0.3c 1.1b 4.0a 4.2b 1.2ab 3.4 B 0.8bc 3.0b 1.0 2.2ab 1.4ab 3.0b 0.7c 1.8a 3.0b 4.7ab 1.8a 2.9 C 1.1ab 4.2a 1.2 2.0bc 1.7ab 4.7a 2.3b 1.0ab 4.0a 5.1a 0.5c 3.4 D 1.9a 2.4b 0.9 1.0c 0.9b 5.6a 4.2a 1.6ab 3.2b 5.3a 1.1bc 2.8 0.665 1.168 0.848 0.975 1.242 1.168 1.110 0.999 0.615 0.855 0.879 0.854 RMSE

a,b,c Within a row, least square means with different superscripts differs signicantly (p < 0.05). RMSE: root mean standard error. * A = 6, B = 9, C = 12 and D = 18 months of ageing time, respectively.

M.D. Gurdia et al. / Food Quality and Preference 21 (2010) 148155


1 B adhesiveness 0.5 D A 0 D D crumbliness sweetness -0.5 matured flavour aged flavour C C hardness C
brightness

153

B pastiness

B
0.5

Dimension 2 (36.3 %)

juiciness white spots tender rancid fat colour

metallic A

MADRID
size

PC2 (23.5%)

GIRONA

WOMEN

A piquantness saltiness fibrousness bitterness

fat flavour F1 odour F2 cured tough texture dry saltiness MEN colour F3 soft marbling

nerves uniform

SEVILLA

aspect

-0.5

-1 -1 -0.5 0 0.5 1

-1 -1 -0.5 0 0.5 1

Dimension 1 (63.7 %)
Fig. 5. Graphical distribution of dry-cured samples (A, B, C and D) and descriptors for each city obtained using Simple Correspondence Analysis over the contingency table of the generic descriptors of the consumer sensory prole, including gender (men and women) and consumption frequency (F1 = more than three times a week; F2 = two or three times a week; F3 = once a week or less) as supplementary variables.

PC1 (56.1%)
Fig. 4. Principal Component Analysis of the sensory prole of four dry-cured ham samples (A, B, C and D).

between Figs. 3 and 4 allows us to ascertain the sensory attributes behind consumer vocabulary. Dry-cured ham sample A, dened as bitter, piquant and metallic in PCA was described as having an atypical avour/odour in GPA. Atypical avour/odour is a compilation of several words such as feed, male, acid and different offodour or off-avour attributes used by consumers that had similar location on the consensus space. Hardness and brousness as referred mainly to sample A by the trained panel, could mean hard and dry texture in consumer words. The crumbliest dry-cured ham sample D is characterized by juiciness, texture, tender and soft in the GPA consensus space. Likewise, odour, avour and matured terms mentioned by consumers (GPA) could be interpreted as sweetness, matured and aged avour in the trained panel vocabulary (PCA). Samples D were also the dry-cured hams with the highest fat content (Table 1), which accordingly have been related to higher sweetness and juiciness (Gurdia, Guerrero, Gispert, Garnier, & de Vries, 1999) and matured avour (Bolzoni, Barbieri, & Virgili, 1996; Flores et al., 1997). As expected, consumers in general used a large number of unspecic descriptors (colour, taste, odour, appearance and texture) compared to the trained assessors. Some attributes used by the trained panel i.e., metallic, aged, pastiness, crumbliness, adhesiveness were not elicited by more than three consumers, probably due to the fact that most consumers were either not familiar with these attributes or did not know their meaning. On the other hand, consumers showed great ability in describing appearance, highlighting several features like colour, fat content, marbling, brightness, white spot presence, size and shape. In contrast, Hersleth, Berggren, Westad, and Martens (2005) found in a study on bread that Norwegian consumers were more efcient in describing texture attributes than other sensory characteristics. These different results could be due to the fact that consumer sensory perception tends to be specic for a product, since it provides a different degree of importance to different sensory attributes depending on the particularities of each food product, and texture seems to be a more important aspect in bread than ham. It is important to notice as well that these differences may be due to the existence of a different awareness of descriptive terms between Norwegian and Spanish consumers and the lack of defective texture in the drycured ham samples tasted. Fig. 5 shows the results obtained by means of Simple Correspondence Analysis over the contingency table of the different

descriptors elicited by consumers for each city and gender. Since age, education level and work situation (supplementary variables) did not show signicant differences, these variables were not represented in Fig. 5. Attributes such as brightness, uniform, colour or appearance were most frequently mentioned in Sevilla; juiciness, hardness, matured avour, white spots and slice size in Madrid; and fat colour, rancid avour, sinews, dry and tenderness in Girona. This last result may be explained by some differences in habits of meat products consumption at home between geographical areas (Mili et al., 1996). According to Askegaard and Madsen (1998) Europe cannot be regarded as a homogeneous sensory culture since important differences exist in consumption patterns, behaviour and attitudes not only between countries but also between regions within the same country. In Spain, dry-cured ham consumption patterns differ clearly between regions. If we analyze it by geographical areas, the region of greatest consumption is Catalonia, the Basque Country and Madrid, which represent 70% of the consumption, followed by Andaluca and Extremadura (in the Southwest), and then Cantabria, Navarra, La Rioja, Galicia and Asturias (Comunidad de Madrid, 2006). Regarding consumers gender and consumption frequency, signicant differences were found. Men and more frequent consumers (F1 and F2: 2 or more times a week) described dry-cured ham samples by means of colour, marbling, brightness, hardness, matured avour and juiciness, whereas women and less frequent consumers (F3: once a week or less) tended to use more recurrent descriptors such as sinews, uniformity, fat colour, rancidity, white spots and saltiness. Differences between genders in sensory evaluation of food products have been described in scientic literature and may be mainly explained by the different sensitivity of men and women (Chauban, 1989; Prodi et al., 2004; Ullrich & Tepper, 2000) or by a different attitude towards different food related aspects that are frequent between genders (Aaron, Mela, & Evans, 1994; Dennison & Sheperd, 1995; Guerrero et al., 1999; Gurdia, Guerrero, Gelabert, Gou, & Arnau, 2006; Khknen, Tourila, & Rita, 1996; Sheperd, 1988).

4. Conclusions This study indicates the applicability of FCP methodology in order to nd out how consumers describe dry-cured ham sensorially.

154

M.D. Gurdia et al. / Food Quality and Preference 21 (2010) 148155 Gou, P., Arnau, J., & Gurdia, M. D. (2000). Efecto del tipo de pulido (con corteza o corte en V) en algunas caractersticas sensoriales del jamn curado. In II Symposium Mundial del Jamn Curado, Barcelona. Gower, J. C. (1975). Generalized procrustes analysis. Psychometrika, 40, 3355. Greenacre, M., & Belsius, J. (1994). Correspondence analysis in the social sciences. Amsterdam: Academic Press. Gurdia, M. D., Guerrero, L., Gelabert, J., Gou, P., & Arnau, J. (2006). Consumer attitude towards sodium reduction in meat products and acceptability of fermented sausages with reduced sodium content. Meat Science, 73, 484490. Gurdia, M. D., Guerrero, L., Gispert, M., Garnier, J. P., & de Vries, A. (1999). Caracterizacin sensorial y reolgica de 9 lneas genticas de porcino. Ciudad de Mxico: SENSIBER. Gurdia, M. D., Guerrero, L., Gou, P., Monfort, J. M., & Arnau, J. (1999). Efecto del tiempo de maduracin sobre las caractersticas sensoriales del jamn curado, sobre su aceptabilidad y su preferencia. Ciudad de Mxico: SENSIBER. Guerrero, L., Gurdia, M. D., & Arnau, J. (2005). Methodological proposal about sensorial analysis on dry-cured ham: Criteria to take into consideration and systems to validate them. In Proceedings III dry cured ham world congress on science, technology and marketing 1820 May (pp. 187196), Teruel, Spain. Guerrero, L., Aguiar, A. P. S., Gurdia, M. D., Claret, A., & Arnau, J. (2007). Criterios de eleccin del jamn curado para los consumidores espaoles. In Proceedings IV dry cured ham world congress on science, technology and marketing 1820 April (pp. 431432), Salamanca, Spain. Guerrero, L., Gou, P., Alonso, P., & Arnau, J. (1996). Study of the physicochemical and sensorial characteristics of dry-cured hams in three pig genetic types. Journal of the Science of Food and Agriculture, 70(4), 526530. Guerrero, L., Gou, P., & Arnau, J. (1997). Descriptive analysis of toasted almonds: A comparison between expert and semi-trained assessors. Journal of Sensory Studies, 12, 3954. Guerrero, L., Gou, P., & Arnau, J. (1999). The inuence of meat pH on mechanical and sensory textural properties of dry-cured ham. Meat Science, 52, 267273. Guerrero, L., Gurdia, M. D., & Arnau, J. (2004). Anlisis sensorial de carne y productos crnicos. In J. Briz & R. Garcia (Eds.), Anlisis sensorial de productos alimentarios. Metodologa y aplicacin al mercado espaol (pp. 195218). Madrid: Ministerio de Agricultura, Pesca y Alimentacin. Hersleth, M., Berggren, R., Westad, F., & Martens, M. (2005). Perception of bread: A comparison of consumers and trained assessors. Journal of Food Science, 70(2), S95S101. INE (2001). INEbase, Demografa y poblacin. Cifras de poblacin y Censos demogrcos. Tasas de actividad, paro y escolaridad segn sexo por edad. INE (2007). INEbase, Demografa y poblacin. Cifras de poblacin y Censos demogrcos. ISO (1970). Determination of choride content. R.1841. International Standards Organization. ISO 8586-1 (1993). Sensory analysis. General guidance for the selection, training and monitoring of assessors. Part 1: Selected assessors. Geneva: International Organization for Standardization. ISO 8586-2 (1994). Sensory analysis. General guidance for the selection, training and monitoring of assessors. Part 2: Experts. Geneva: International Organization for Standardization. Issanchou, S., Schlich, P., & Lesschaeve, I. (1992). Carte sensorielle deaux gazeuses. Une application du vocabulaire libre et de lanalyses Procustenne. Cahier Scientique, 109, 181186. Jack, F. R., & Piggott, J. R. (1992). Free choice proling in consumer research. Food Quality and Preference, 3, 129134. Khknen, P., Tourila, H., & Rita, H. (1996). How information enhances acceptability of a low-fat spread. Food Quality and Preference, 7(2), 8794. Kster, E. P. (2003). The psychology of food choice: Some often encountered fallacies. Food Quality and Preference, 14, 359373. Lachnit, M., Busch-Stocksch, M., Kunert, J., & Krahl, T. (2003). Suitability of free choice proling for assessment of orange-based carbonated soft-drinks. Food Quality and Preference, 14, 257263. Langrom, S. P. (1983). The application of Procrustes statistics to sensory proling. In Williams & R. K. Atkim (Eds.), Sensory quality in foods and beverages. Denition, measurement and control. Chichester: Ellis Horwood. Langrom, S. P., Williams, A. A., & Collins, A. K. (1984). A comparison of the consensus conguration from a generalized procrustes analysis with the untransformed panel mean in sensory prole analysis. Lebensm.-Wiss Technol., 17, 296298. Lawless, H. T., & Heymann, H. (1998). Sensory evaluation of food: Principals and practices. New York: Chapman and Hall. Mace, H. J., Bratchell, N., Greenhoff, H., & Vallis, L. V. (1989). Designs to balance the effect of order of presentation and rst-order carry-over effects in hall test. Journal of Sensory Studies, 4, 129149. MAPA (2005). Datos del Anuario de Estadstica Agraria del Ministerio de Agricultura, Pesca y Alimentacin hasta el ao 2002. Available from <http://www.mapya.es/ es/estadistica/infoestad.html>. Meilgaard, M. C., Civille, G. V., & Carr, B. T. (1991). Sensory evaluation techniques (2nd ed.). Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press. Mili, S., Mahlau, M., & Furitsch, H. P. (1996). Comportamiento del consumidor espaol frente a la demanda de productos crnicos (I). Eurocarne, 43, 19. Morales, R., Guerrero, L., Claret, A., Gurdia, M. D., & Gou, P. (2008). Beliefs and attitudes of butchers and consumers towards dry-cured ham. Meat Science, 80, 10051012. Moskowitz, H. (1983). Descriptive analysis of perceptions. In Product testing and sensory evaluation of foods. Connecticut: Food and Nutrition Press, Trumbull.

Consumers paid more attention to the appearance of the product. Most of the attributes selected by participants were elementary, thus reecting their simplicity when compared with those obtained from the trained panel. Consumers showed a high level of agreement in the use of sensory attributes, except for saltiness and texture descriptors. The little agreement in these key drycured ham attributes could be a limitation in FCP applied to ham. Our results indicate that the consumer sensory description of dry-cured ham depends on the geographical region where the consumer lives. Trained assessors and consumers describe sensory attributes of dry-cured ham in a similar way and, in both cases, they are able to differentiate the ageing times of dry-cured ham. The sensory descriptive prole obtained by means of trained assessors proves to be a useful tool to understand consumers vocabulary regarding dry-cured ham better. Acknowledgments The authors would like to acknowledge the Spanish Ministry of Education and Science/INIA (Project RTA 2006-00060-00-00) for supporting this research work.

References
Aaron, J. I., Mela, D. J., & Evans, R. E. (1994). The inuences of attitudes, beliefs and label information on perception of reduce-fat spread. Appetite, 22(1), 2528. Amerine, M., Pangborn, R., & Roessler, E. (1965). In principles of sensory evaluation of food. New York: Academic Press. Arnau, J., Guerrero, L., Casademont, G., & Gou, P. (1995). Physical and chemical changes in different zones of normal and PSE dry cured ham during processing. Food Chemistry, 52(1), 6369. Arnau, J., Guerrero, L., & Srraga, C. (1998). The effect of green ham pH and NaCl concentration on cathepsin activities and the sensory characteristics of drycured hams. Journal of Science and food and Agriculture, 77, 387392. Arnold, G. M., & Williams, A. A. (1986). The use of generalized procrustes analysis. In J. P. Piggott (Ed.), Statistical procedures in food research (pp. 233253). London: Elsevier Applied Science. Askegaard, S., & Madsen, T. K. (1998). The local and the global: Exploring traits of homogeneity and heterogeneity in European food cultures. International Business Review, 7, 549568. ASTM (1981). American society for testing and materials. guidelines for the selection and training of sensory panel members. ASTM STP 758. Philadelphia: ASTM. p. 33. Black, H. R. (2000). Cardiovascular risk factors. In N. D. Wong, H. R. Black, & J. M. Gardin (Eds.), Preventive cardiology (pp. 2335). Washington: McGraw-Hill. Bolzoni, L., Barbieri, G., & Virgili, R. (1996). Changes in volatile compounds of Parma ham during maturation. Meat Science, 43, 301310. Buscailhon, S., Berdagu, J. L., & Monin, G. (1993). Time related changes in volatile compounds of lean tissue during processing of French dry-cured ham. Journal of the Science of Food and Agriculture, 63(1), 6975. Buscailhon, S., Gandermer, G., & Monin, G. (1994). Time-related changes in intramuscular lipids of French dry-cured ham. Meat Science, 37, 245255. Chauban, J. (1989). Relationships between sour and salt taste perception and selected subject attributes. Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 89(5), 652658. Cilla, I., Martnez, L., Beltrn, J. A., & Roncals, P. (2006). Dry-cured ham quality and acceptability as affected by the preservation system used for retail sale. Meat Science, 73, 581589. Cilla, I., Martnez, L., Guerrero, L., Gurdia, M. D., Arnau, J., Altarriba, J., et al. (2006). Consumer beliefs and attitudes towards dry-cured ham and protected designation of origin Teruel ham in two Spanish regions differing in product knowledge. Food Science and Technology International, 12(3), 359371. Comunidad de Madrid (2006). El jamn ibrico: produccin y consumo. Documentos tcnicos de Salud Pblica, nm. 105. Deliza, R., MacFie, H., & Hedderley, D. (2005). The consumer sensory perception of passion-fruit juice using free-choice proling. Journal of Sensory Studies, 20, 1727. Dennison, C. M., & Sheperd, R. (1995). Adolescent food choice: An application of the theory of planned behaviour. Journal of Human Nutrition and Dietetics, 8, 923. Elmore, J. E., & Heymann, H. (1999). Perceptual maps of photographs of carbonated beverages created by traditional and free-choice proling. Food Quality and Preference, 10, 219227. Flores, M., Ingram, D. A., Bett, K. L., Toldr, F., & Spanier, A. M. (1997). Sensory characteristics of Spanish Serrano dry-cured ham. Journal of Sensory Studies, 12, 169179. Garca-Gonzvez, D. L., Roncales, P., Cilla, I., del Ro, S., Poma, J. P., & Aparicio, R. (2006). Interlaboratory evaluation of dry-cured hams (from France and Spain) by assessors from two different nationalities. Meat Science, 73, 521528.

M.D. Gurdia et al. / Food Quality and Preference 21 (2010) 148155 Murray, J. M., Delahunty, C. M., & Baxter, I. A. (2001). Descriptive sensory analysis: Past, present and future. Food Research International, 34, 461471. OMahony, M. (1986). Sensory adaptation. Journal of Sensory Studies, 1, 237258. Oreskovich, D. C., Klein, B. P., & Sutherland, J. W. (1991). Procrustes analysis and its application to free choice and other sensory proling. In B. P. Klein (Ed.), Sensory science theory and application in foods (pp. 353394). New York: Marcel Dekker. Parolari, G. (1994). Taste quality of Italian raw ham in a free-choice prole study. Food Quality and Preference, 5, 129. Parolari, G., Virgili, R., & Schivazappa, C. (1994). Relationship between Cahepsin B activity and compositional parameters in dry-cured hams of normal and defective texture. Meat Science, 38, 117122. Piggott, J. R., Sheen, M. R., & Apostolidou, S. G. (1990). Consumers perception of whiskies and other alcoholic beverages. Food Quality and Preference, 2, 177185. Prodi, D. A., Drayra, D., Forabosco, P., Palmas, M. A., Maestrale, G. B., Piras, D., et al. (2004). Bitter taste study in a Sardinian genetic isolate supports the association of phenylthiocarbamide sensitivity to the TAS2R38 bitter receptor gene. Chemical Senses, 29(8), 647702. Resano, H., Sanjun, A. I., & Albisu, L. M. (2007). Consumers acceptability of cured ham in Spain and the inuence of information. Food Quality and Preference, 18, 10641076.

155

Rousset, S., & Martin, J. F. (2001). An effective hedonic analysis tool: Weak/strong points. Journal of Sensory Studies, 16, 643661. Ruiz-Ramrez, J., Arnau, J., Serra, X., & Gou, P. (2006). Effect of pH24 and NaCl and proteolysis index on the relationship between water content and texture parameters in biceps femoris and semimembranosus muscles in dry-cured ham. Meat Science, 72(2), 185194. SAS (2001). SAS users guide. Cary NC: SAS Institute Inc.. Sheperd, R. (1988). Consumer attitudes and food acceptance. In D. M. H. Thompson (Ed.), Applied science, food acceptability (pp. 253266). London. Steel, R. G. D., & Torrie, J. H. (19601983). Principles and procedures of statistics. New York: Mc Graw-Hill. Szczesniak, A. S. (2002). Texture is a sensory property. Food Quality and Preference, 13, 215225. Ullrich, N., & Tepper, J. B. (2000). Food preferences are inuenced by gender and genetic taste sensitivity to 6-n-propylthiouracil (PROP). IFT annual meeting, June 1014, Dallas, Texas, USA. Vestegaard, C. S., Schivazappa, C., & Virgili, R. (2000). Lipolysis in dry-cured ham maturation. Meat Science, 55, 15. Williams, A. A., & Langron, S. P. (1984). The use of free choice proling for the evaluation of commercial ports. Journal of Science and Food Agriculture, 35, 558568.

Вам также может понравиться