Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 2

Newswise

http://www.newswise.com/articles/view/597860?print-article

Channels: Behavior/Psychology, Cognition and Learning, Vision Keywords: Vision Contact Information Available for logged-in reporters only Description We rely on our visual system more heavily than previously thought in determining the causality of events. A team of researchers has shown that, in making judgments about causality, we dont always need to use cognitive reasoning. In some cases, our visual brainthe brain areas that process what the eyes sensecan make these judgments rapidly and automatically. Citations Current Biology

Released: 1/8/2013 10:35 AM EST Embargo expired: 1/10/2013 12:00 PM EST Source: New York University Newswise We rely on our visual system more heavily than previously thought in determining the causality of events. A team of researchers has shown that, in making judgments about causality, we dont always need to use cognitive reasoning. In some cases, our visual brainthe brain areas that process what the eyes sensecan make these judgments rapidly and automatically. The study appears in the latest issue of the journal Current Biology. Our study reveals that causality can be computed at an early level in the visual system, said Martin Rolfs, who conducted much of the research as a post-doctoral fellow in NYUs Department of Psychology. This finding ends a long-standing debate over how some visual events are processed: we show that our eyes can quickly make assessments about cause-and-effectwithout the help of our cognitive systems.

1 de 2

12/01/2013 11:22 a.m.

Newswise

http://www.newswise.com/articles/view/597860?print-article

Rolfs is currently a research group leader at the Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience and the Department of Psychology of Berlins Humboldt University. The studys other co-authors were Michael Dambacher, post-doctoral researcher at the universities of Potsdam and Konstanz, and Patrick Cavanagh, professor at Universit Paris Descartes. We frequently make rapid judgments of causality (The ball knocked the glass off the table), animacy (Look out, that thing is alive!), or intention (He meant to help her). These judgments are complex enough that many believe that substantial cognitive reasoning is requiredwe need our brains to tell us what our eyes have seen. However, some judgments are so rapid and effortless that they feel perceptual we can make them using only our visual systems, with no thinking required. It is not yet clear which judgments require significant cognitive processing and which may be mediated solely by our visual system. In the Current Biology study, the researchers investigated one of these causality judgmentsin an effort to better understand the division of labor between visual and cognitive processes. Their experiments centered on isolating how we perceive causalityi.e., where one event apparently triggers the next. The perception of causality generally involves two components, one that is stimulus based and one that is inference based. First, to see causal structure between two events, these events need to follow each other with little delay and typically require contactfor instance, a glass immediately falling off a table after being knocked over. This is the stimulus-based component of perceptual causality. The second component is an inference by which two events are merged into one: rather than seeing one object stopping and a second one starting on its own, there is a continuity of action that is transferred from the first object to the secondjust as in billiards where one ball transfers its motion to another ball. To test how the brain determines causality, the researchers used an adaptation procedure that is often employed to uncover neural mechanisms through visual after-effectschanges in what observers see. The visual system has been shown to quickly change sensitivity to stimuli that are continuously presented: after staring at a red spot continuously, a white wall will appear to have a greenish spot; after seeing a texture moving up continuously, a stationary wall will appear to move down. Using this adaptation approach in a series of experiments, the researchers found that repeated exposure to the causal events collisions test events appeared less causal. Conversely, adapting to non-causal events had little effect. These findings indicate that certain causal judgments show the classic properties of visual processing (i.e., adaptation) and appear to be determined in the visual system without input from cognition. Notably, their experimental results showed that the after-effects moved when the eyes moved, just as the green after-image from adapting to red moves when we move our eyes. Only visual and no cognitive processes would show this specificity to eye-centered reference frame. The finding, the researchers concluded, provides strong evidence that in some cases, the understanding of action causality, animacy, and intention is encoded on a perceptual level rather than on a cognitive one. The study was supported by grants from the EU Seventh Framework Programme (Marie Curie IOF 235625), the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (RO 3579/2-1, FOR868/1), the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research (FKZ 01GQ1001A), and the Agence Nationale de la Recherche Chaire dExcellence. ###
2013 N e w sw i se , I n c. 434-296-9417 215 5th S t. S W , S u i te 100, Ch a r l o tte svi l l e VA 22903 Te r m s o f S e r vi ce Co n ta ct Us

P r i va cy N o ti ce

2 de 2

12/01/2013 11:22 a.m.

Вам также может понравиться