Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 53

A study on the coping behaviours of wives of alcoholics.

Rao TS, Kuruvilla K. Source


Department of Psychiatry, J.S.S. Medical College and Hospital, Kamanuja Road, Mysore - 570 004.

Abstract
A study was conducted on 30 wives of alcoholics using Orford-Guthrie's 'coping with drinking' questionnaire. Tlie commonest coping behaviour reported was discord, avoidance, indulgence and fearful withdrawal while marital breakdown, taking special action, assertion and sexual withdrawal were least frequent. There was no significant correlation between the coping behaviours and the variables like duration of marriage, duration of husband's alcoholism, socio-economic and educational status. Implications of these findings are discussed and a cross cultural comparison is made

Patterns and determinants of coping behaviour of wives of alcoholics.


Chandrasekaran R, Chitraleka V. Source
R. CHANDRASEKARAN, M.D., Professor and Head, Department of Psychiatry, JIPMER, Pondicherry-605 006.

Abstract
One hundred wives of alcoholics with a confirmed diagnosis of alcohol dependence syndrome according to DCR 10 were studied with a "coping with drinking questionnaire". "Avoidance" was the most commonly endorsed coping behaviour. There was a significant correlation between all the coping components and alcohol related problems. No correlation was observed between neuroticism scores and coping behaviour. It is evident from the study that both personality and situational variables play a role in determining the coping behaviour of the wives of alcoholics.

KEYWORDS: Coping, determinants, wives of alcoholics

Marital stability in alcoholic-spouse relationships as a function of drinking pattern and location. Abstract The relation between alcohol consumption and marital stability was assessed longitudinally in two groups of male alcoholics: in-home (n = 4) and out-of-home (n = 4) drinkers. Through the use of univariate and bivariate time-series analyses, the study identified a causal relation between alcohol consumption and marital stability and a significant impact of drinking location on obtained relations. Findings are discussed in terms of Steinglass's suggestion that alcohol can have adaptive consequences for the marriage and family life of alcoholics. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

Caregiver Burden and Alcohol Use in a Community Sample


Kathleen M. Rospenda, PhD, Lisa M. Minich, MA, Lauren A. Milner, MS, and Judith A Richman, PhD
Author information Copyright and License information The publisher's final edited version of this article is available at J Addict Dis See other articles in PMC that cite the published article.

Go to:

Abstract
Little attention has been paid to the relationship between caregiver burden and alcohol use. It is important to examine the particular aspects of caregiver burden which most influence alcohol use. A mail survey was conducted utilizing a representative sample of 998 employed Chicago residents who provided informal care for at least one person. Ordinary least squares regression models were computed to examine the relationship between caregiver burden and drinking outcomes. Findings suggest that caregivers who experience social and emotional burden related to caregiving are at risk for problematic alcohol use and warrant attention from health and mental health service professionals. Keywords: Caregiving, burden, alcohol abuse, social support Go to:

Introduction
Many individuals in the United States find themselves in the role of an informal (unpaid) caregiver during the course of their lives. Parents provide care for their children, adult children may care for their elderly parents, and one spouse may care for another after an illness or injury. Healthcare innovations, which have resulted in longer life expectancies, combined with rising healthcare costs and pressures to keep individuals living in the community rather than institutionalized, have resulted in a rise in caregiving for the elderly, those with severe mental illnesses, those with chronic medical conditions, and those with developmental disabilities.14 This suggests that increasing numbers of people are faced with issues related to caregiver burden. While caregiver burden has been linked to an array of deleterious physical and mental health consequences, little attention has been paid to its effects on drinking behaviors. Thus, this paper focuses on the consequences of caregiver burden on alcohol use and abuse.
Caregiver burden

Caregiver burden can be conceptualized as both the tasks that need to be done in the course of caregiving and the way in which the caregiver appraises the performance of these tasks.5 Caregiving tasks take many different forms. For example, caregivers may

assist care recipients with activities of daily living, prepare meals, perform housekeeping tasks, run errands, or manage finances. Caregivers may also be responsible for providing care after a debilitating illness or for monitoring for a person suffering from dementia. A number of external factors, including caregiver personality, social support network, status in the family, and other responsibilities impact how an individual appraises their caregiving responsibilities.6,7 For example, a large body of literature has found that caregivers with a strong social support network report less burden than those who lack social support,810 regardless of the number or type of caregiving tasks. A positive interpretation of the caregiving role has been shown to be similarly protective.11
Negative effects of caregiving

Considerable work has been done to document the negative effects caregivers suffer as a result of providing care, particularly for those who are caring for adults or for children with disabilities. Caregivers tend to report worse physical health, including insomnia, headache, and weight loss,1215and are more likely than non-caregivers to put off seeking needed medical care.1617 Caregivers also report higher rates of depression and anxiety than non-caregivers.1719 Finally, caregivers tend to report poorer quality of life than noncaregivers.2021 These findings have been consistent across many different groups of caregivers, including those caring for disabled or chronically ill children, those caring for chronically ill adults, and those caring for older adults suffering from dementia. However, as elaborated later in the paper, alcohol-related outcomes have been relatively neglected in this literature. While it is clear that increased caregiver burden increases negative health and psychological outcomes experienced by caregivers, little work has been done to determine which facets of caregiver burden are most predictive of negative caregiver outcomes. Caregiver burden was initially conceptualized as a unidimensional variable derived from a variety of items.22 This approach was found to be insufficient, however, and as the field moved forward both objective burden (tasks included in the caregiving role) and subjective burden (distress experienced in relation to those tasks) were measured.2324 Subsequent instruments designed to measure caregiver burden further refined the idea of objective versus subjective burden, incorporating subscales that identified different types of objective and subjective burden.25 For example, the Caregiver Burden Inventory (CBI), used in this study, includes five burden sub-scales: timedependence burden, developmental burden, physical burden, social burden, and emotional burden.26 Instruments such as the CBI provide for the opportunity to further identify the components of caregiving that are most risky to caregiver well-being.
Caregiving and alcohol use

Caregiver burden is important in part because a caregiver who is exhausted, depressed, or physically ill may be unable to provide the quality of care needed to their care recipient. While the relationship between caregiver burden and mental and physical health have been studied in great detail, the relationship between caregiver burden and alcohol use has largely been ignored. Alcohol use, particularly alcohol use that meets

criteria for abuse or dependency, is a cause for concern among caregivers, as both their health and the health of their care recipient is at risk, particularly if they are responsible for assisting their care recipient with activities of daily living. For example, caregiver alcohol use has been linked to elder abuse.2728 Some well-known correlates of alcohol use in nationally representative adult samples are exhibited by a large proportion of caregivers. For example, depression and anxiety are predictors of increased alcohol use.2930 Social isolation, which is experienced by some caregivers,3132 is also predictive of increased alcohol use.33 For these reasons, the relationship (if any) between caregiver burden and alcohol use deserves attention. While relatively sparse, the research on caregiving and alcohol use suggests that a significant number of caregivers consume alcohol, and that some caregivers use alcohol as a result of stress derived from caregiving. A 1994 study by Connell found that 34.1% of spousal caregivers reported using alcohol as a coping strategy, and that 2.3% of spousal caregivers reported using alcohol as a coping strategy on a frequent basis.34 A 2006 study by Heflinger and Brannan found that about one-third of caregivers for youth with substance abuse problems or mental health problems had used alcohol within the past 30 days.35 Gallant and Connell (1997) found similar results in a study of spousal caregivers, with 30.3% reporting some alcohol use and 3.5% reporting increased alcohol use since assuming caregiving responsibilities.17 Saad et al. (1995) found that approximately 10% of caregivers in their sample used alcohol to reduce stress.36 McKibben, Walsh, Rinki, Koin, and Gallegher-Thompson (1999) found that female dementia caregivers are more likely to use alcohol than their peers.37 While these studies show that alcohol use is a concern among caregivers, they do not consider how different components of caregiver burden influences alcohol use. In a study on role transition and alcohol use, Richman, Rospenda, and Kelley (1994) reported increased problem drinking in a sample of new parents following the birth of their children, particularly among those who reported reduced social support. While parents of healthy children are not typically considered in the caregiving literature, their findings suggest that caregiving of any kind may result in increased alcohol use, highlighting the need for further inquiry into this area.38 This study seeks to address this gap in the research by examining the effects of different types of caregiver burden on drinking outcomes. Understanding more about what types of burden can lead to problematic alcohol use can suggest ways to help prevent problematic drinking among caregivers. This information can also be useful for health and mental health service providers who treat caregivers.
Caregiver Burden Inventory subscales as predictors of alcohol use

Negative effects associated with caregiving have been shown to be consistent across many different types of caregivers and when caregiver burden is conceptualized in a number of different ways. However, to the best of our knowledge, no study to date has considered the extent to which different facets of caregiver burden are associated with problematic alcohol use. We used Novak and Guests 1989 Caregiver Burden Inventory, a

survey designed to measure five components of caregiver burden listed above.26 Each burden subscale and its expected relationship to alcohol use is described below.
Time-dependence burden

The time-dependence burden subscale measures the perceived impact caregiving has on the caregivers time. Examples of items in this subscale include, My care recipient needs my help to perform daily tasks, and I have to watch my care recipient constantly when I am with them. Role theory suggests that individuals who have multiple roles are less likely to drink because the increased demands associated with multiple roles leave less time for drinking.3940 For example, Hajema and Knibbe (1998) found that the acquisition of a spouse or parent role was associated with a decrease in alcohol consumption.41 We hypothesize that those who report high time-dependence burden will report less alcohol use than those with low time-dependence burden (H1).
Developmental burden

The developmental burden subscale measures the extent to which caregivers feel offtime or out-of-synch compared to the rest of their peers. Examples of items in this subscale include, I feel that I am missing out on life because of caregiving, and My social life has suffered because of caregiving. Novak and Guest (1989) suggested that caregivers may feel considerable anxiety and stress as they compare their situation with others in their peer group who are free of caregiving responsibilities.26 Mjelde-Mossey, Barak and Knight (2004) found that among caregivers, those who utilized selfcontrolling and distancing coping techniques were more likely to consume alcohol than other caregivers.42 Self-controlling involves not sharing with others how difficult a situation is; distancing involves cognitively and emotionally detaching from a stressor. Both of these behaviors suggest that caregivers who use these coping techniques do not feel as if they can share their experiences with others, increasing a sense of detachment from peers. Thus, we hypothesize that caregivers who report greater developmental burden will exhibit more drinking behavior than those who report low developmental burden (H2).
Physical burden

The physical burden subscale measures caregivers feelings of fatigue due to caregiving. Examples of items in this subscale include, Im not sleeping enough because of caregiving, andCaregiving has made me physically sick. There is evidence that suggests that people who are in poor health do not drink. For example, Green, Polen, and Perrin (2003) found that in both men and women, good physical health was predictive of greater alcohol consumption.43 We hypothesize that caregivers who report high physical burden will drink less than those who report low physical burden (H3).
Social burden

The social burden subscale measures caregivers feelings of conflict resulting from their caregiving roles in their work and family lives. Examples of items in this subscale include,Ive had problems with my spouse/partner because of caregiving

responsibilities, and I dont do as good a job at work as I used to because of my caregiving responsibilities. Social support has been shown repeatedly to help mediate the harmful effects of burden on caregivers health. Zarit, Reever, and Bach-Peterson (1980) found that burden was less severe for caregivers with a strong social support network;22 George and Gwyther (1986) found that just the perception of a strong support network was sufficient to protect against some caregiving burden.44 We hypothesize that caregivers who report considerable strain in other relationships because of their caregiving role will drink more than those who do not feel that their caregiving interferes with their other social roles (H4).
Emotional burden

The emotional burden subscale measures negative feelings caregivers have for their care recipient. Examples of items in this subscale include, I resent my care recipient, and I feel uncomfortable when I have friends over because of caregiving. It has been posited that individuals use alcohol to escape from or avoid uncomfortable situations or negative feelings.45 Cooper, Frone, Russell, and Mudar (1995) found support for the idea that drinking can be used to regulate negative emotions in both adolescents and adults.46 We hypothesize that caregivers who report high levels of emotional burden will drink more than those who report low levels of emotional burden (H5). Go to:

Method
Participants

Data for this study derive from a mail survey conducted by the first and fourth authors to assess how people balance their caregiving responsibilities with their work, as well as how family and work responsibilities affect peoples well-being in a sample of employed adults (age 18 and older) who were fluent in English or Spanish. The sample was identified by purchasing randomly selected phone numbers for block groups within the City of Chicago and screening for eligible participants. In the case of multiple eligible respondents in the same household, the Troldahl-Carter-Bryant method of respondent selection was used to select the respondent.4748 Eligibility criteria included being at least 18 years of age, having been employed at least 20 hours per week at some time in the past 12 months prior to the survey, currently performing unpaid caregiving for children or an adult, and fluency in English or Spanish. Of the 35,000 sample numbers contacted, 22,281 (71.6%) were working residential numbers. Contact was made at 15,464 (69.4%) numbers, and individuals at 10,011 (64.8%) numbers cooperated with the screener. Of those, 2,114 completed the screener and were found to be eligible for the study. Verbal informed consent was obtained from potential respondents for inclusion in the study. The questionnaire was mailed to the 2,114 participants who agreed to be sent a mail questionnaire. Completed questionnaires were returned by 998 (47.2%) participants. Of

those who completed the survey, 16.1% were Latino, 37.1% were African American, 42.5% were White, and 4.3% were Asian/Pacific Islander or of other race/ethnicity. The study was approved by the university Institutional Review Board. Telephone prescreens were conducted from November 2006 to August 2007. The first batch of surveys was mailed in December 2006; the cutoff date for accepting returned surveys was December 2007. A $30 American Express card was sent with the mail questionnaire to those who agreed to complete the survey. A reminder postcard was sent to individuals who did not respond to the initial mailing Phone screens and surveys were administered in English or Spanish. Special care was taken to include men and Hispanic participants.
Measures

Caregiving responsibilities

Caregivers indicated the number of care recipients for whom they provided informal care in each of the following relationship categories: child(ren) under age 18, child(ren) over age 18, spouse/ partner, parent(s), brother(s)/ sister(s), aunt(s)/ uncle(s), grandparent(s), friend(s), and other(s).
Caregiver burden

Respondents completed a modified version of the Caregiver Burden Index (CBI), a 19item measure used to assess five types of caregiver burden: time-dependence ( = 0.85), developmental ( = 0.85), physical ( = 0.86), social ( = 0.73), and emotional ( = 0.77). Examples of items from each of these subscales are listed in the introduction above. Responses for each of these measures was given on a 4-point scale from 1 = Not at all true to 4 = Extremely true.26 Due to length limitations, the item with the lowest factor loading value for each subscale was not included in this study. Thus, each subscale score was derived from summing participants responses on 4 items, with the exception of the physical burden subscale, which derived its value from 3 items.
Drinking Frequency and Quantity

Respondents were asked to estimate the number of days they drank any kind of alcoholic beverage, and typical number of drinks consumed on drinking days in the past 30 days.
Excessive drinking

Binge drinking was assessed by the number of days respondents had 5 or more drinks containing alcohol for men, or 4 or more drinks containing alcohol for women on one occasion in the past 12 months. Drinking to intoxication was assessed by one item: About how often in the past 12 months did you drink enough to feel drunk, that is, where drinking noticeably affected your thinking, talking, or behavior? Responses to each of these measures were given on an 8-point scale from 0= Never to 7= 5 times a week or more.49

Problem drinking

The 10-item Brief Michigan Alcohol Screening Test (BMAST)50 was used to measure problematic alcohol use in the past-year. The BMAST correlates strongly with the full version of the MAST,50 and is an effective screening tool for alcohol problems among current drinkers.5152Respondents answered yes (coded 15) or no (coded 0) for each item. Items were summed to create a composite index of problem drinking. Demographic variables included age, gender (1 = female, 0 = male), marital status (1= married, 0= widowed/divorced/separated/never married), race/ ethnicity (dummy coded groups for White, African American, Hispanic, and Asian/ Pacific Islander/other), household income, and average number of hours worked per week. Age was measured continuously in years, average number of hours worked per week was measured continuously in hours, and income was an ordinal scale ranging from 0 (less than $10,000) to 7 (greater than $90,000). Go to:

Results
Ordinary least squares regression models were computed to examine the relationship between drinking outcomes and each caregiver burden subscale. Pairwise deletion of missing data resulted in sample sizes that varied slightly by model, ranging from 776 to 802. Models were tested in two steps, with the demographic control variables entered in step 1 and burden subscale scores entered individually at step 2. Descriptive statistics of the sample are presented in Table 1, both overall and by presence of any alcohol consumption in the past 12 months. Chi-square analyses, Kruskal-Wallis tests and one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) were performed to examine differences between participants who had and had not had at least one alcoholic beverage in the past 12 months. Compared to non-drinkers, those who reported drinking in the past 12 months were more likely to be younger, male, white, and married (p < .05). Drinkers were also more likely to report higher household income and higher levels of educational attainment (p < .05). Drinkers reported higher scores on the physical and social scales of the Caregiver Burden Index (p < .05). Those who did not drink were more likely to be African American (p < .05). Descriptive statistics for the alcohol use variables are found in Table 2.

Table 1

Demographic characteristics of all responders and of responders who reported having at least one alcoholic drink in the past year.

Table 2 Means and standard deviations for alcohol use variables. Results of the significant regression analyses are found in Table 3 and Table 4. Hypotheses 1, 2, and 3 were not supported, as time-dependence burden, physical burden, and developmental burden did not predict alcohol use. Those results are not presented here, but available from the first author by request. Hypotheses 4 and 5, that high levels of social burden and emotional burden would predict more alcohol use, were supported. Social burden was predictive of the average number of drinks per day in the past 30 days (p < .01), the frequency of drinking to intoxication in the past 12 months (p < .01), and scores on the Brief MAST (p < .05). Emotional burden was predictive of the number of days on which alcohol was consumed in the past 30 days (p < .05), the frequency of drinking to intoxication in the past 12 months (p < .01), and scores on the Brief MAST (p < .01). In all of these cases, higher scores on the burden scales were predictive of increased drinking behavior and problem drinking.

Table 3 Social burden regression analyses.

Table 4 Emotional burden regression analyses.

Go to:

Discussion
In a group of employed caregivers, higher scores on two types of caregiver burden, social burden and emotional burden, were predictive of increased drinking behaviors and problem drinking, while higher scores on the time-dependence, developmental, and physical burden subscales were not predictive of drinking. The social burden subscale emphasizes perceptions of how caregiving has impacted family relationships, with higher scores coinciding with relationships strained by caregiving. The emotional burden subscale includes items that measure caregiver attitude toward the care recipient, including feeling embarrassed by or resentful toward the care recipient. Higher scores on this subscale coincided with more negative feelings toward the care recipient. While these findings provide no support for our first three hypotheses, that increased amount of time spent on caregiving and physical illness resulting from caregiving activities would be predictive of less alcohol use (H1 and H3) and that reporting feeling out of touch with ones peers would be predictive of greater alcohol use (H2), they did provide support for our fourth and fifth hypotheses, that caregiving can result in increased drinking behaviors when caregiving impedes the caregivers social interactions with family or friends or when the caregiver has negative feelings about caregiving. These findings are similar to previous work on the mitigating effect of social support on caregiver burden and the expectation that alcohol use will help regulate negative thoughts. These findings have a practical application for health and mental health service providers who work with caregivers. Previous research has documented the physical and mental health risks associated with being a caregiver. Increased alcohol use should be included in this catalog of risks, and those who work with caregivers should be particularly attentive regarding reports of strained family relationships or negative feelings towards their care recipient(s), as alcohol use puts both the caregiver and the care recipient at risk. Interventions designed to improve caregiver social support and to reduce negative caregiver reactions to care recipient behaviors may be particularly useful. For example, The New York University Silberstein Aging and Dementia Research Center has pioneered an intervention designed to enhance social support for spousal caregivers of individuals with Alzheimers disease. This intervention has improved many aspects of well-being for both the caregiver and the care recipient. Participating in the intervention delayed nursing home placement for the care recipient53 reduced caregiver depression,5455 reduced negative caregiver reactions to the problem behavior of the care recipient,56and improved caregiver satisfaction with social support, which in turn predicted better mental health outcomes for the caregivers.5758 This paper differs from previous work on caregiving, caregiver burden, and alcohol use in two ways. First, unlike much of the work in the caregiving field, this study did not focus on one subset of caregivers (e.g., spousal caregiver of a stroke victim or parental caregiver of a child with sickle cell disease). All caregivers, including parents caring for healthy children under the age of 18, were included in our analyses. We felt that this was

appropriate for several reasons. First, we were interested in the subjective experience of caregiving and the relationship between that experience and alcohol use rather than in the experiences of just one particular group of caregivers. Caregiving includes a wide range of events and occurs in many different types of situations; few caregivers share the same experience. Further, the caregiving experience is tempered by many factors, including caregiver personality, support network, and physical resources. It is impossible to define the caregiving experience simply by the characteristics of the care recipient. For these reasons, we wanted to survey a wide range of caregivers. Secondly, we measured five well-defined sub-types of caregiver burden with a multi-item questionnaire rather than using just a few items to create one or two burden variables (either a composite variable or an objective burden variable and a subjective burden variable.) Because caregiving is a complex task that involves a wide range of activities, it is reasonable to assume that caregiving burden can manifest itself in multiple ways. We used this more complex method of measuring caregiver burden to begin to understand which types of burden put caregivers at particular risk for increased alcohol use, something that to our knowledge has not been done when examining caregiver burden as a predictor of alcohol use. These findings provide a preliminary model to guide future work on the relationship between caregiving and alcohol. Previous research has demonstrated that, for certain people, serving as a caregiver is associated with greater alcohol use.17,3435 While this study did not examine alcohol consumption before and after assuming a caregiving role, it does suggest that some types of caregiver burden are more likely to result in increased drinking behavior and problem drinking. More work is necessary to understand how caregiver burden increases alcohol consumption. Although it might be argued that experiences of burden are strongly linked to anxiety and depression, we did not directly assess those relationships in this paper. The link between caregiver burden, anxiety and depression, and drinking behavior also deserves attention in future research. It is possible that personality traits such as neuroticism may contribute to the association between caregiver burden and problem alcohol use. Neuroticism is a reliable predictor of problematic alcohol use in previous research.59 While we were unable to control for neuroticism in the present study, we do not believe neuroticism would have a significant impact on our findings. Previous research examining the role of personality vulnerability on drinking outcomes found that, after controlling for neuroticism, the association between perceived workplace harassment and deleterious drinking behavior was attenuated but was still stignificant.60 Therefore, we are fairly confident that neuroticism would not play a critical role in explaining the relationship between caregiver burden and problem alcohol use. Other limitations should be considered when interpreting these results. The sample is not nationally representative; rather, it was derived from employed caregivers in the Chicago metropolitan area. As our analyses included only one wave of data, our findings cannot be interpreted as causal. Finally, while our regression models were statistically significant and the addition of the burden variables to the control variables resulted in a

statistically significant R-squared change, these models only accounted for relatively small percentages of the overall variance, and our results should be interpreted conservatively. In summary, caregivers who report higher levels of social and emotional burden were also more likely to engage in more frequent drinking behaviors and to report higher scores on the Brief MAST. Those who provide services to caregivers should be aware of this relationship and consider increased alcohol use a potential risk faced by caregivers. The results reported here are preliminary and suggest that additional work to better understand the connection between caregiving and alcohol use is necessary. Go to:

Acknowledgements
This paper was made possible by grant number R01AA015766 from the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA). Its contents are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of NIAAA. The data were collected by the Survey Research Laboratory at the University of Illinois at Chicago. Go to:

References
1. Federal Agency Forum on Aging-Related Statistics. Older Americans 2008: Key Indicators of Well-Being. Washington, DC: Government Printing Office; 2008. Mar, Federal Agency Forum on Aging-Related Statistics. 2. Spillman BC, Black KJ. Staying the Course: Trends in Family Caregiving. Washington, DC: The Urban Institute; 2005. 3. Stone RG, Cafferata L, Sangl J. Caregivers of the frail elderly: A national profile. The Gerontologist.1987;27:616626. [PubMed] 4. Talley RC, Crews JE. Framing the public health of caregiving. American Journal of Public Health.2007;97:224228. [PMC free article] [PubMed] 5. Hoenig J, Hamilton MW. The schizophrenic patient in the community and his effect on the household. International Journal of Social Psychiatry. 1966;12:165 176. [PubMed] 6. Chenier MC. Review and analysis of caregiver burden and nursing home placement. Geriatric Nursing. 1997;18:121126. [PubMed] 7. Lawton MP, Kleban MH, Moss M, Rovine M, Glicksman A. Measuring caregiver appraisal. Journal of Gerontology. 1989;44:6171. [PubMed]

8. Goodman CC, Potts MK, Pasztor EM. Caregiving grandmothers with vs .without child welfare system involvement: Effects of expressed need, formal services, and informal social support on caregiver burden. Children and Youth Services Review. 2007;29:428 441. 9. Miller B, Townsend A, Carpenter E, Montgomery RVJ, Stull D, Young RF. Social support and caregiver distress: A replication analysis. Journal of Gerontology. 2001;56B:S249S256. [PubMed] 10. Vrabec NJ. Literature review of social support and caregiver burden, 1980 1995. Journal of Nursing Scholarship. 1997;29:383388. [PubMed] 11. Pinquart M, Sorensen S. Associations of stressors and uplifts of caregiving with caregiver burden and depressive mood: a meta-analysis. Journal of Gerontology. 2003;58B:P112P128. [PubMed] 12. Ho SC, Chan A, Woo J, Chong P, Sham A. Impact of caregiving on health and quality of life: a comparative population-based study of caregivers for elderly persons and noncaregivers. Journal of Gerontology: Medical Sciences. 2009 e-pub ahead of print. [PubMed] 13. Hughes SL, Giobbie-Hurder A, Weaver FM, Kubal JD, Henderson W. Relationship between caregiver burden and health-related quality of life. The Gerontologist. 1999;39:534545. [PubMed] 14. Polen MR, Green CA. Caregiving, alcohol use, and mental health symptoms among HMO members.2001 [PubMed] 15. Vitaliano PP, Zhang J, Scanlan JM. Is caregiving hazardous to ones physical health? A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin. 2003;129:946972. [PubMed] 16. Burton LC, Newsom JT, Schulz R, Hirsch CH, German PS. Preventive health behaviors among spousal caregivers. Preventive Medicine. 1997;26:162169. [PubMed] 17. Gallant MP, Connell CM. Predictors of decreased self-care among spouse caregivers of older adults with dementing illnesses. Journal of Health and Aging. 1997;9:373 395. [PubMed] 18. Dekel R, Solomon Z, Bleich A. Emotional distress and marital adjustment of caregivers: contribution of level of impairment and appraised burden. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping. 2005;18:7182. 19. Grunfeld E, Coyle D, Whelan T, Clinch J, Reyno L, Earle CC, Willan A, Viola R, Coristine M, Janz T, Glossop R. Family caregiver burden: Results of a longitudinal study of breast cancer patients and their principal caregivers. Canadian Medical Association Journal. 2004;170:17951801. [PMC free article][PubMed]

20. Glozman JM. Quality of life of caregivers. Neuropsychology Review. 2004;14:183 196. [PubMed] 21. McCullagh E, Brigstocke G, Donaldson N, Kalra L. Determinants of caregiving burden and quality of life in caregivers of stroke patients. Stroke. 2005;36:2181 2186. [PubMed] 22. Zarit SH, Reever KE, Bach-Peterson J. Relatives of the impaired elderly: correlates of feelings of burden. Gerontologist. 1980;20:649655. [PubMed] 23. Montgomery RJ, Gonyea JG, Hooyman NR. Caregiving and the experience of subjective and objective burden. Family Relations. 1985:1925. 24. Test MA, Stein LI. Alternative to mental hospital treatment. Archives of General Psychiatry.1980;37:409412. [PubMed] 25. Kosberg JI, Cairl RE. The cost of care index: a case management tool for screening informal care providers. Gerontologist. 1986;26:273278. [PubMed] 26. Novak M, Guest C. Application of a multidimensional caregiver burden inventory. The Gerontologist. 1989;29:798803. [PubMed] 27. Conlin MM. Silent suffering: a case study of elder abuse and neglect. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 1995;43:13031308. [PubMed] 28. Cooney C, Howard R. Abuse of patients with dementia by carers. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 1995;10:735741. 29. Conner KR, Pinquart M, Gamble SA. Meta-analysis of depression and substance use among individuals with alcohol use disorders. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment. 2009 E-pub ahead of print. [PubMed] 30. Kushner MG, Abrams K, Borchardt C. The relationship between anxiety disorders and alcohol use disorders: a review of major perspective and findings. Clinical Psychology Review. 2000;20:149171.[PubMed] 31. Manne S. Coping and social support. In: Nezu A, Nezu C, Geller P, editors. Comprehensive Handbook of Psychology, Volume 9: Health Psychology. New York: John Wiley & Sons; 2003. pp. 5174. 32. Quittner AL, Glueckauf RL, Jackson DN. Chronic parenting stress: moderating versus mediating effects of social support. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1990;59:12661278. [PubMed] 33. Seezman M, Seezman AZ, Budros A. Powerlessness, work, and community: a longitudinal study of alienation and alcohol use. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1988;29:185198. [PubMed]

34. Connell CM. Impact of spouse caregiving on health behaviors and physical and mental health status. The American Journal of Alzheimers Care and Related Disorders & Research. 1994;9:2636. 35. Heflinger CA, Brannan AM. Differences in the experience of caregiver strain between families caring for youth with substance use disorders and families of youth with mental health problems. Journal of Child and Adolescent Substance Abuse. 2006;15:83104. 36. Saad K, Hartman J, Kurian M, Graham C, Wilcock G, Ballard C. Copings by the carers of dementia sufferers. Age and Ageing. 1995;24:495498. [PubMed] 37. McKibben CL, Walsh W, Rinki M, Koin D, Gallegher-Thompson D. Lifestyle and health behaviors among female family dementia caregivers: A comparison of wives and daughters. Aging and Mental Health. 1999;32:165172. 38. Richman JA, Rospenda KM, Kelley MA. Gender roles and alcohol abuse across the transition to parenthood. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1995;56:553557. [PubMed] 39. Wilsnack RW, Cheloha R. Womens roles and problem drinking across the lifespan. Social Problems.1987;34:231248. 40. Wilsnack SC, Wilsnack RW. Epidemiology of womens drinking. Journal of Substance Abuse.1991;3:133157. [PubMed] 41. Hajema KJ, Knibbe RA. Changes in social roles as predictors of changes in drinking behavior.Addiction. 1998;93:17171727. [PubMed] 42. Mjelde-Mossey LA, Barak MEM, Knight BG. Coping behaviors as predictors of drinking practices among primary in-home dementia caregivers. The Journal of Applied Gerontology. 2004;23:295308. 43. Green CA, Polen MR, Perrin NA. Structural models of gender, alcohol consumption, and health.Substance Use & Misuse. 2003;38:97125. [PubMed] 44. George LK, Gwyther LP. Caregiver well-being: a multidimensional examination of family caregivers of demented adults. The Gerontologist. 1986;26:253259. [PubMed] 45. Folkman S, Lazarus RS. Ways of Coping Questionnaire. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists; 1988. 46. Cooper ML, Frone MR, Russell M, Mudar P. Drinking to regulate positive and negative emotions: a motivational model of alcohol use. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1995;69:9901005.[PubMed] 47. Bryant B. Respondent selection in a time of changing household composition. Journal of Marketing Research. 1975;12:129135. 48. Troldahl C, Carter R. Random selection of respondents within households in phone surveys. Journal of Marketing Research. 1964;1:7176.

49. Wilsnack SC, Klassen AD, Schur BE, Wilsnack RW. Predicting onset and chronicity of women's problem drinking: A five-year longitudinal analysis. American Journal of Public Health. 1991;81:305318. [PMC free article] [PubMed] 50. Pokorny AD, Miller A, Kaplan HB. The Brief MAST: A shortened version of the Michigan Alcoholism Screening Test. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1972;129:342 345. [PubMed] 51. Allen JP, Maisto SA, Connors GJ. Self-report screening tests for alcohol problems in primary care.Archives of Internal Medicine. 1995;155:17261730. [PubMed] 52. Ross HE, Gavin DR, Skinner HA. Diagnostic validity of the MAST and the alcohol dependence scale in the assessment of DSM-III alcohol disorders. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1990;51:506513.[PubMed] 53. Mittelman MS, Ferris SH, Shulman F, Steinberg G, Levin B. A family intervention to delay nursing home placement of patients with Alzheimers disease: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American Medical Association. 1996;276:1725 1731. [PubMed] 54. Mittelman MS, Ferris SH, Shulman E, Steinberg G, Ambinder A, Mackell JA, Cohen J. A comprehensive support program: Effect on depression in spouse-caregivers of AD patients. The Gerontologist. 1995;35:792802. [PubMed] 55. Mittelman MS, Roth DL, Coon DW, Haley WE. Sustained benefit of supportive intervention for depressive symptoms in Alzheimers caregivers. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2004;161:850856.[PubMed] 56. Mittelman MS, Roth DL, Haley WE, Zarit SH. Effects of caregiver intervention on negative caregiver appraisals of behavior problems in patients with Alzheimers disease: Results of a randomized trial.Journal of Gerontology, Psychological Sciences. 2004;59B:P27P34. [PubMed] 57. Roth DL, Mittelman MS, Clay OJ, Madan A, Haley WE. Changes in social support as mediators of the impact of a psychosocial intervention for spouse caregivers of persons with Alzheimers disease.Psychology and Aging. 2005;20:906915. [PubMed] 58. Drentea P, Clay OJ, Roth DL, Mittelman MS. Predictors of improvement in social support: five year effects of a structured intervention for caregivers of spouses with Alzheimers disease. Social Science and Medicine. 2006;63:957967. [PubMed] 59. Sher KJ, Trull TJ. Personality and disinhibitory psychopathology: Alcoholism and antisocial personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1994;103:92 101. [PubMed] 60. Wislar JS, Richman JA, Fendrich M, Flaherty JA. Sexual harassment, generalized workplace abuse and drinking outcomes: The role of personality vulnerability. Journal of Drug Issues. 2002;32:10711088.

For better or for worse? The effects of alcohol use on marital functioning
Michael P. Marshal*
Author information Copyright and License information The publisher's final edited version of this article is available at Clin Psychol Rev See other articles in PMC that cite the published article.

Go to:

Abstract
Two competing hypotheses propose opposite effects for the relation between alcohol use and marital functioning. One hypothesis conceptualizes alcohol use as maladaptive and proposes that it serves as a chronic stressor that causes marital dysfunction and subsequent dissolution. An opposing hypothesis proposes that alcohol use is adaptive and serves to temporarily relieve stressors that cause marital dysfunction, stabilizing the marital relationship, and perhaps preventing dissolution. Sixty studies were reviewed that tested the relation between alcohol use and one of three marital functioning domains (satisfaction, interaction, and violence). Results provide overwhelming support for the notion that alcohol use is maladaptive, and that it is associated with dissatisfaction, negative marital interaction patterns, and higher levels of marital violence. A small subset of studies found that light drinking patterns are associated with adaptive marital functioning; however, more research is necessary to replicate these effects and identify specific conditions under which they occur. 2003 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Keywords: Alcohol drinking patterns, Alcoholism, Marriage, Marital relations, Marital satisfaction Go to:

1. Introduction
The term alcoholic marriage (McCrady & Epstein, 1995a, 1995b; Paolino & McCrady, 1977) is often used to describe a partnership in which one or both of the partners have a history of alcoholism that interferes with successful, day-to-day marital functioning. Although alcoholic marriages are fairly common in the United States, with estimated prevalence rates ranging anywhere from 10% to 45% (Clark & Hilton, 1991), the commencement and subsequent trajectory of the alcoholic marriage is anything but common. For example, a recent review of what has been termed alcohol and the marriage effect concluded that (a) alcoholics are less likely to get married than are nonalcoholics, (b) if they do get married, it is often earlier or later in the young-adult developmental cycle than nonalcoholics, (c) alcoholics are likely to reduce alcohol use

once they are married, and (d) they are more likely to get divorced than are nonalcoholics (Leonard & Rothbard, 1999). This last finding, that alcoholics are more likely to get divorced than are nonalcoholics, is consistent with the most intuitive and popular hypothesis regarding this relationship, that alcohol use serves as a chronic stressor, and has a deleterious influence on marital functioning (for reviews, see Gotlib & McCabe, 1990; Halford, Bouma, Kelly, & Young, 1999; OFarrell & Rotunda, 1997). Indeed, social exchange theory (see McCrady, 1982) posits that alcohol use causes stressful family interactions, the negative effects of which are dampened by subsequent alcohol use, thereby serving as a negative reinforcer. As alcohol use increases, then, so do negative family interactions, marital violence, and marital dissatisfaction, all of which serve to perpetuate the dissolution process (Gottman, 1994). Another popular hypothesis, originating from a family systems approach to alcoholism, suggests that alcohol use serves an adaptive function in the marital relationship rather than a maladaptive one (seeSteinglass & Robertson, 1983, for review). The basic tenets of this theory contend that family interaction patterns serve to maintain alcohol use, by rewarding drinking behaviors through subsequent family interactions that serve as positive reinforcers. The seminal work of Steinglass and colleagues provides some qualitative evidence in support of this process. For example, they observed alcoholic family interaction (marital dyads, with and without children) in the hospital, laboratory, and at home, and concluded that alcohol use temporarily relieves daily stress or tension in the family system through increases in affective expression and problem-solving ability, helping to maintain relatively stable and adaptive marital and family relationships (Steinglass, 1971, 1979a, 1979b; Steinglass, Davis, & Berenson, 1977). This hypothesis is provocative not only because it suggests that drinking behavior is maintained by family interactions, but that a relatively successful marital relationship might be maintained by alcohol use. These contradictory hypothesesone predicting maladaptive marital outcomes and the other predicting adaptive marital outcomesare important because they have guided several decades of research on the effects of alcohol use on marital functioning. This research has posed and tested three main questions: (a) Do alcoholic marriages differ from nonalcoholic marriages in their quality of marital functioning? (b) Is alcohol consumption related to marital functioning? (c) What theoretical and methodological factors moderate the relation between alcohol consumption and marital functioning? The primary goal of this review was to evaluate the nature of the relation between alcohol use and marital functioning (adaptive versus maladaptive) using these questions. The secondary goal of this review was to evaluate and summarize the methodological strengths and weaknesses of this literature and the extent to which they inform us about the internal and external validity of the findings, and our ability to make causal inferences regarding the relationship between alcohol use and marital functioning. Marital functioning outcome variables used with intact couples are typically operationalized as marital satisfaction, interaction, and violence. Although some

researchers have provided fairly extensive reviews within one marital functioning domain (e.g., Jacob & Seilhamer, 1987; Leonard, 1993; McCrady & Epstein, 1995a, 1995b), a comprehensive review that includes all three marital functioning domains has not been written. There are two main reasons why a review of this nature is important. First, a comprehensive review of the empirical literature that has evaluated the effects of alcohol use on multiple indices marital functioning is a necessary first step towards understanding their role in the association between alcohol and marital success and/or marital dissolution and divorce. Second, it can highlight similarities and differences in the methods, results, and conclusions of these studies across three frequently used marital functioning domains, which have been studied relatively independently of one another in the alcohol use literature. Indeed, one of the strengths of an integrative review is that it can compare and contrast the conclusions from studies that have inherently different design methodologies. For example, marital interaction research involves the microanalysis of moment-to-moment behaviors of each spouse during a dyadic interaction task in the laboratory, whereas violence research typically evaluates the effects of concurrent, self-reported predictors and moderators of verbal and physical aggression. Because evaluating the adaptive and maladaptive associations between alcohol use and marital functioning involves integrating the conclusions of these disparate methodologies, a qualitative rather than quantitative review of this literature was conducted. Several search methods were employed to identify studies in the literature that tested the relation between alcohol and marital functioning in intact, married couples. The primary search engine/database employed was PsychInfo, which identified well over 1000 articles and book chapters using several variations of the keywords alcohol and marriage. Alcohol was also used in combination with several marital functioning keywords such as satisfaction, interaction, communication, conflict, violence, and aggression to identify relevant studies. These abstracts were then evaluated to determine whether the study tested the relation between alcohol and marital functioning. The second stage of the search procedure involved identifying key alcohol and marital functioning measures and using the Social Science Citation Index to identify articles that cited them. These abstracts were also evaluated to determine if they met criteria for the current review. Finally, articles were located using the bibliographies of all articles that tested the relation between alcohol and marital functioning. Altogether, 60 peer-reviewed articles and book chapters were identified using these procedures, which tested 70 outcomes (24 satisfaction, 16 interaction, and 30 violence) using 44 independent samples. Because the majority of these studies were quasi-experimental, an evaluation of the internal and external validity of the results and our ability to make causal inferences about this relation rely heavily on study design characteristics including: sampling procedures, sample size, demographic characteristics, inclusion/exclusion criteria, data analytic strategies, and the construct validity of the alcohol and marital functioning variables. A summary of these characteristics and their implications as they pertain to this literature, therefore, will be discussed prior to summarizing the results. A detailed description of the methodological characteristics of

all studies cited in this review is presented in Table 1. The information contained in this table was used as a vehicle for describing and summarizing the methodological characteristics of each subset of studies, and as a result, is divided into three parts. Table 1A contains all studies that tested the relationship between alcohol and marital satisfaction, Table 1B contains all marital interaction studies, and Table 1C contains all marital violence studies. Each part is alphabetically ordered by the last name of the first author.

Table 1 Methodological characteristics of studies that tested the effects of alcohol use on marital satisfaction, interaction, and violence Go to:

2. Study design characteristics


2.1. Sampling procedures

2.1.1. Sample type

Participants across studies were recruited from several sources that can be broadly categorized into treatment- and community-based samples. Treatment samples were often recruited from university-based counseling clinics, hospitals, and/or community mental health clinics. Couples in treatment-based samples are thought to have more severe and acute alcohol and/or marital problems than non-treatment-based samples do, as well as higher rates of associated psychiatric comorbidity. Community samples are normally recruited through local newspaper advertisements. Because they are typically not characterized by acute distress, these couples are often seen as having less severe problems than couples recruited from treatment programs. As a result, the source of recruitment has implications for the internal and external validity of the findings. For example, the effects of alcohol use observed in treatment-based samples might be inflated due to symptoms associated with comorbid psychopathology rather than due to the effects of alcohol use, and may not generalize to nontreatment populations. Fifteen of the 44 independent samples were treatment samples, 26 were community samples, and 3 samples were recruited from treatment and community sources. Marital violence studies have the highest proportion of treatment-based samples (40%), followed by interaction studies (31%), and satisfaction studies (25%).

2.1.2. Inclusion/exclusion criteria

Specificity of the relation between alcohol and marital functioning is influenced by the sample recruitment procedures. Researchers that exclude couples or individuals with concurrent psychological diagnoses such as major depression, antisocial personality disorder, or drug abuse and dependence (as opposed to alcohol abuse and dependence) can make more confident statements about the relation between alcohol and marital functioning than researchers who do not. This is especially true of spousal alcoholism studies because there is a high rate of psychiatric comorbidity within alcoholism samples (Helzer & Pryzbeck, 1988; Kessler, 1995), and as a result some speculated that the relation between spousal alcoholism and marital functioning may be spurious (Gelles, 1993; Leonard, 1993; Leonard & Rothbard, 1999). Indeed, marital functioning is affected by depression and other forms of psychopathology that may preclude, mask, or exacerbate the observed relations between alcohol and marital functioning. Despite problems presented by presence of concurrent psychiatric disorders in alcoholics, only 20 of the 60 studies excluded such participants (see Table 1). For studies in which alcoholic couples were compared with nonalcoholic couples, the internal validity of the findings and the ability to make causal inferences are also influenced by the nature and source of the comparison groups (see the Comparison Group column in Table 1). Nonequivalent comparison groups threaten the conclusion that differences between alcoholic and nonalcoholic couples are due to alcohol rather than other demographic or clinical factors. While the majority of the studies that compare alcoholic and nonalcoholic groups test for differences between groups, some compare their results to national survey samples (e.g., OFarrell & Choquette, 1991) or do not control for demographic differences that they do find. These problems will be discussed in more detail below.
2.2. Sample size

Sample sizes across these 60 studies ranged from 8 to 11,870 and are highly positively skewed, with a median sample size of 96. Study design and procedures heavily influence the resources that are required to recruit and pay participants. Studies that assess behavioral interactions between partners typically utilize more resources because they require (a) that participants visit the laboratory, (b) larger monetary incentives to encourage participation by the couples, and (c) several experimenters to observe and code each interaction sequence. These research demands are reflected in the smaller sample sizes used in marital interaction research (mean/median = 72/82) relative to the marital satisfaction samples (mean/ median = 170/80) and marital violence samples (mean/median = 1166/343). In such studies, the statistical conclusion validity of the results is threatened (see Shadish, Cook, & Campbell, 2002), primarily by the fact that they do not have enough power to detect small (but meaningful) effects. Problems that arise as a result of small sample sizes in the marital interaction literature are discussed in more detail below.
2.3. Sample age and duration of marriages

Mean age of participants in each study in this review ranges from 23 to 45, with a mean age across studies of 36. Some differences are observed between sets of studies. For example, the average age in the set of marital violence studies is 32, whereas the average age in the set of marital interaction studies is 40. Nevertheless, only 5 of the 44 independent samples reported mean sample ages under 30, and only one interaction study had a mean sample age under 30. This bias limits the external validity of the results, because the age of the participants is likely to have an effect on the relation between alcohol and marital functioning. Young adults are likely to have different alcohol use and marital functioning trajectories than older adults. For example, the 2week prevalence of heavy drinking declines after age 21 (Johnston, OMalley, & Bachman, 1997), and marital satisfaction has a tendency to decline during the first several years of the relationship (Kurdek, 1998). As couples get older, then, their drinking habits change and their relationship becomes increasingly stable. Because of these differences, some caution is warranted when generalizing results from these studies to younger populations.
2.4. Ethnicity

Thirty-three of the 60 studies in Table 1 (55%) reported descriptive information on sample ethnicity or race. The proportion of Caucasian participants across these studies ranged from 28.9% to 100%, and the average was 76.6%. Some differences across subsets of studies are noteworthy. For example, only 3 out of the 16 marital interaction studies (18.8%) reported descriptive information about ethnicity and their average proportion of Caucasian participants was 91%. Interestingly, marital violence studies were most likely to contain information regarding their participants ethnicity status (67.9%), and also had the most diverse representation (percentage of Caucasian: 28.9 100%) compared to satisfaction studies (60100%) and interaction studies (81100%), and as a result had the lowest average proportion of Caucasians (48.2%). While some studies in this literature made explicit efforts to incorporate ethnicity into their research questions (e.g., Neff, Holamon, & Schluter, 1995), others noted discrepancies between experimental and control groups in ethnic composition but did not attempt to control for them (e.g.,Schiavi, Stimmel, Mandeli, & White, 1995). Doing so is important due to ethnic group differences in the prevalence of alcohol use disorders (Helzer, Burnam, & McEvoy, 1991), the trajectories of drinking behavior over time and their relationship to marital status (Curran, Muthen, & Harford, 1998), and changes in alcohol related problems over time (Caetano & Clark, 1998). It should be noted here that other important indices of cultural influences on the relationship between alcohol use and marital functioning were either underreported (e.g., religious beliefs) or too heterogeneous in their operationalization across studies (e.g., socioeconomic status) to facilitate meaningful conclusions or discussion.
2.5. Independent variables

Alcohol use is a broad term purposely employed throughout this review to subsume a wide range of alcohol use behaviors and consequences that are most commonly used in

this literature. Indeed, particular attention should be paid to the alcohol use constructs used within and across studies, because the operational definition of these constructs play a strong role in the interpretation of their effects on marital functioning and in the conclusions that are drawn from this review. For this reason, a brief overview of the four most common conceptual and operational definitions of alcohol use behaviors used in this literature will be provided below. It should be noted that the majority of these studies assessed the effects of husbands alcohol use and abuse on wives marital functioning outcomes. This bias influenced this review in two ways. First, because it is historically not studied as often in this literature, the effects of alcohol use on ones own marital functioning was not reviewed. Second, terminology used in this review was such that alcoholics in alcoholic marriages will refer to the husbands unless otherwise stated. The effects of wives alcohol use and abuse on marital functioning and the effects of concordant use will be given special consideration and summarized towards the end of this review.
2.5.1. Alcoholism diagnosis

The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual for Mental Disorders (DSM; American Psychiatric Association) is the most widely used alcoholism diagnostic tool in the United States. Individuals who meet DSM alcohol dependence and/or abuse criteria are considered alcoholics. Dependence primarily refers to a cluster of symptoms that involve physiological and psychological tolerance of alcohol and withdrawal symptoms in the absence of alcohol. Alcohol abuse is primarily defined as the amount of negative personal, social, and work/ school related consequences as a result of alcohol consumption. Despite these relatively concrete definitions, this literature has been criticized for the lack of consistency across studies in the conceptual and operational definitions of alcoholism (Leonard, 1993; McCrady & Epstein, 1995a, 1995b).
2.5.2. Problem use

Problem use is a broad term used to describe behaviors and symptoms of drinkers that are characteristic of formally diagnosed alcoholics. Problem use is often assessed using diagnostic screening instruments such as the Michigan Alcoholism Screening Test (MAST; Selzer, 1971) or the Alcohol Dependency Scale (ADS; Skinner & Allen, 1982). In addition, the term problem use is used to describe negative alcohol-related consequences that occur as a result of alcohol consumption (e.g., been in a fight or have had legal problems as a result of alcohol use). While some studies in this review employ diagnostic screening measures like the MAST to identify alcoholic and nonalcoholic groups, others use it as a continuous problem use variable.
2.5.3. Alcohol consumption

Measures of alcohol consumption refer to the quantity and frequency of alcohol consumed in a given period. Typical measures of alcohol consumption will assess how often an individual drinks (frequency) or how much an individual drinks in one sitting (quantity). Quantityfrequency indices are often employed by studies in the current review and are used to calculate average daily or weekly estimates of a drinkers volume

of alcohol consumption. Heavy use is a term that is also used in the alcohol literature, and although there are no standard measures, it is often operationalized as having five or more drinks in one sitting or getting drunk or very high on alcohol.
2.5.4. Alcohol typologies

Although there is an extensive history of research on the development of alcoholic subtypes (see Babor, Hesslebrock, Meyer, & Shoemaker, 1994, for a review), the marital functioning literature most often focuses on three subtypes defined primarily by either drinking pattern or comorbid high-risk behavior such as aggression or antisociality. For example, some researchers distinguish between steady and binge drinkers (Epstein, Kahler, McCrady, Lewis, & Lewis, 1995). Steady drinkers are more likely to drink a smaller volume of alcohol and do so more frequently, whereas binge drinkers are likely to consume heavy amounts of alcohol less frequently. Although this is a conceptual rather than empirical typology, it is consistent with some empirically derived alcoholic subtypes (Epstein et al., 1995), and has been useful in identifying subgroups in the marital functioning literature. Go to:

3. Dependent variables and summary of results


Marital satisfaction, interaction, and violence are the three primary constructs used to operationalize marital functioning in the alcohol use literature. In each of the following sections, the marital functioning construct will be described and the strengths and weaknesses of the common assessment measures will be discussed. The remainder of each section is organized such that each subset of studies will be evaluated based on these three questions: (a) Do alcoholic marriages differ from nonalcoholic marriages in their quality of marital functioning? (b) Is alcohol consumption related to marital functioning? (c) What theoretical and methodological factors moderate the relation between alcohol consumption and marital functioning? Ten studies are represented in more than one part of Table 1because they assessed more than one marital functioning construct (see Outcome column in table). Five samples were used in more than one study, therefore are represented two or more times in this review.
3.1. The effects of alcohol use on marital satisfaction

Marital functioning is often assessed using intraindividual measures of marital satisfaction. Marital satisfaction is a common way to distinguish between maritally distressed and nondistressed couples and has been an integral part of marital research for many decades (e.g., Terman, 1938). Common marital satisfaction items might ask a respondent how happy they are in the marriage, whether they feel supported by their partner, how often they argue, and whether they have ever regret getting married. Eighteen of the 24 satisfaction studies in Table 1A assessed marital satisfaction with the Short Marital Adjustment Test (MAT; Locke & Wallace, 1959) and/or the Dyadic Adjustment Scale (DAS; Spanier, 1976). The MAT provides a total marital satisfaction score that ranges from 2 to 158. Scores lower than 100 are considered to be in the

clinically distressed range. The DAS also provides an overall marital adjustment score similar to the MAT, ranging from 0 to 151. The DAS also includes several subscale scores (affection, cohesion, consensus, and satisfaction) that are not used often in this literature.
3.1.1. Differences between alcoholic and nonalcoholic couples in marital satisfaction

Eight studies tested differences in marital satisfaction between spouses of alcoholics and spouses of nonalcoholics. OFarrell and Birchler (1987) conducted a study that is prototypic of this design. They compared 28 alcoholic couples with 28 maritally conflicted couples and 28 normal control couples. Alcoholic couples were recruited from an alcohol treatment program. All of the husbands in the alcoholic group reported clinically significant levels of alcohol use and/or abuse on the MAST (Selzer, 1971). Maritally conflicted couples were recruited from a marital therapy treatment program, and reported clinically significant levels of marital dissatisfaction on the MAT. Normal control couples were recruited from newspaper advertisements requesting the participation of happily married couples, and met study criteria if they scored above 100 on the MAT, indicating healthy levels of marital satisfaction. The results indicated that wives of alcoholics reported marital satisfaction levels similar to the wives in conflicted marriages, both of which were significantly lower than levels reported by wives of happily married couples. Seven other studies reported similar findings (Escallier-Nicola, Botwin, & Tarter, 1994; Jacob & Leonard, 1992;Leonard & Jacob, 1997; McLeod, 1993; Perodeau & Kohn, 1989; Schiavi et al., 1995; Tubman, 1991) and suggest that spousal alcoholism is associated with marital dissatisfaction.
3.1.2. Relationship between alcohol consumption and marital satisfaction

Another common way to assess the relation between alcohol and marital functioning is to test whether continuous measures of alcohol consumption and alcohol problems are correlated with marital satisfaction. Several studies tested this question, generating several interesting trends. First, studies with relatively large sample sizes (Dumka & Roosa, 1993, 1995; Leonard & Roberts, 1998b; Leonard & Senchak, 1993; Zweben, 1986) reported significant negative correlations between alcohol problems or heavy use and marital satisfaction. These results are consistent with the results from studies comparing alcoholic and control groups and suggest that heavy/problem alcohol use plays a maladaptive role in marital relationships. In addition, two of these studies found the relation between problem use and marital satisfaction to be mediated by stress associated with alcohol problems (Dumka & Roosa, 1993; Zweben, 1986). Several other studies reported marginal (Jacob, Dunn, & Leonard, 1983) or nonsignificant correlations between spouses alcohol use/abuse and total scores on the DAS (Halford & Osgarby, 1993; Katz, Arias, Beach, Brody, & Roman, 1995). Nonsignificant results can be explained a couple of different ways. First, these studies have relatively small sample sizes compared to those that found significant correlations, and suggest that they do not have enough power to detect the effect. Second, not only were these samples small, but two of these studies (Halford & Osgarby, 1993; Katz et al.,

1995) did not over sample alcoholic participants, perhaps not capturing enough variability in alcohol use and abuse. Indeed, these studies used alcoholism screening tools rather than diagnostic tools such as the DSM, and only identified about one third of their sample as possible alcoholics.
3.2. Moderators of the relationship between alcohol use and marital satisfaction

Another explanation for null effects found by some studies is that the relation between alcohol use and abuse and marital satisfaction is different for different subgroups of alcoholic couples. Indeed, there is evidence to suggest that the type of drinker moderates the relation between alcohol use and marital satisfaction. Dunn, Jacob, Hummon, and Seilhamer (1987) found that marital satisfaction was positively related to husbands consumption when the husband drank primarily at home, and negatively related to consumption when the husband drank primarily out of the home. In addition, Jacob et al. (1983), who reported null effects or weak positive correlations between consumption and satisfaction for their total sample, found that total scores on the MAT and several subscales of the DAS were positively correlated with husbands alcohol consumption for steady drinkers and uncorrelated with consumption for binge drinkers. A recent study of drinking partnerships corroborated this general trend. Roberts and Leonard (1998)performed a cluster analysis on several husband and wife drinking variables and identified several drinking partnership clusters that included husband heavy, heavy out-of-home, light social, light intimate, and frequent intimate drinking couples. Results found that the wives levels of marital satisfaction as measure by the MAT were significantly lower for those in the husband heavy and heavy out-ofhome groups, but were equivalent across all other groups. In all, these studies suggest that light or moderate levels of alcohol use can be adaptive and help maintain a healthy level of marital satisfaction, and heavy or problem use is maladaptive. These results are consistent with those that found alcohol use to be mediated by stress, in that light patterns of use are much less likely to be accompanied by the negative consequences often experienced as a result of heavy use. Interestingly, one study found that wives marital satisfaction was positively associated with their alcoholic husbands report of alcohol severity (Epstein, McCrady, & Hirsh, 1997). This finding is provocative because it supports the hypothesis that alcohol use may sometimes be adaptive; however, it is inconsistent with other studies because it suggests that severe consequences of alcohol use (a composite scale of three negative alcohol consequence measures) in an alcoholic sample are associated with higher levels of satisfaction. Reasons for such inconsistency are unclear, but it may be that this positive effect is an artifact of the study recruitment procedure. That is, participants in this study consisted of couples that presented at a clinic seeking conjoint treatment for the males alcohol problems (Epstein et al., 1997). It may be that the wives level of marital satisfaction was associated more with feelings of relief that they were seeking help rather than the severity of recent negative alcohol consequences.

In sum, the majority of these studies suggest that alcohol use is maladaptive, and that marital satisfaction suffers as a result. A fairly robust trend suggests that spouses of alcoholics have lower levels of marital satisfaction than do spouses of nonalcoholics. In addition, problem use and heavy use are negatively correlated with spouses marital satisfaction, and two studies suggested that this relation is mediated by stress due to negative alcohol-related consequences. There is some limited evidence to suggest that light or moderate patterns of use are adaptive. For example, within alcoholic samples marital satisfaction is positively correlated with spouses alcohol consumption for inhome and steady drinkers, while it is uncorrelated or negatively correlated to alcohol consumption for binge and out-of-home drinkers.
3.3. The effects of alcohol use on marital interactions

One putative mediator of the relation between alcohol use and marital satisfaction is marital interaction patterns. Marital interactions have been used for decades to distinguish between distressed and nondistressed couples (for reviews, see Schapp, 1984; Weiss & Heyman, 1990, 1997) and are typically assessed using the Marital Interaction Coding System (MICS; Weiss, Hops, & Patterson, 1973). Affective dimensions of marital interaction (as opposed to behavioral dimensions) identified using the MICS and other measures have provided the most consistent findings (Weiss & Heyman, 1990), indicating that maritally distressed couples manifest higher levels of negativity (criticism, hostility, complaining, excuses, withdrawal, etc.) and lower levels of positivity (empathy, congeniality, smiling, humor, approval, etc.) than nondistressed couples do. Some behavioral indices are also quite predictive, for example, indicating that nondistressed couples manifest more problem-solving behavior (providing problem descriptions, problem solutions) than distressed couples do.
3.3.1. Differences between alcoholic and nonalcoholic couples in marital interactions

One of the primary questions in the marital interaction literature asks whether alcoholic couples exhibit different levels of positive, negative, and problem-solving behavior than do distressed couples without alcohol problems, and nondistressed, nonalcoholic couples. Jacob and Krahn (1988) conducted a study prototypic of this design by evaluating group differences in marital interaction patterns with a sample of alcoholic couples, couples with a depressed partner, and normal control couples. Prior to the interaction task, all couples completed the Areas of Change Questionnaire (ACQ; Margolin, Talovic, & Weinstein, 1983), a 34-item measure of each members current complaints regarding their marital relationship. Couples then participated in an interaction protocol that required them to engage in several 10-minute, dyadic, conflict resolution interaction tasks in which they were instructed to discuss a behavior they would like to see their partners change. Interactions were then coded using the MICS and coders who were blind to the nature of the study. Results showed that alcoholic and depressed couples engaged in less positive behavior and more negative behavior than did normal control couples. Four other studies using similar methods and coding procedures found similar results, indicating that alcoholic couples were characterized by more negativity (Billings, Kessler, Gomberg, & Weiner, 1979; Haber & Jacob, 1997;Jacob,

Ritchey, Cvitkovic, & Blane, 1981), less positivity (Billings et al., 1979; Haber & Jacob, 1997;Jacob et al., 1981; OFarrell & Birchler, 1987), and lower levels of problem solving (Jacob et al., 1981) than nonalcoholic couples were. Moreover, alcoholic couples were comparable to distressed, nonalcoholic couples on levels of positivity (OFarrell & Birchler, 1987) and negativity (Billings et al., 1979). In addition to using frequency of positive, negative, and problem-solving behaviors to assess differences between alcoholic and nonalcoholic couples, sequential interactions between partners during an interaction task have been used (see Gottman & Roy, 1990, for a review). Sequential interaction methods are useful because they can provide information about how positive and/or negative interactions escalate or deescalate over time. Escalation can occur when partners engage in positive and negative reciprocity, such that positive interaction loops or negative interaction loops are formed. Jacob and Leonard (1992) are the only researchers to date to analyze differences in sequential interaction patterns between alcoholic and nonalcoholic couples. Using the same sample and data as Jacob and Krahn (1988), Jacob and Leonard (1992) found that alcoholic husbands were less likely to respond positively and more likely to respond negatively to their wives problem-solving efforts than were nonalcoholic husbands. Interestingly, alcoholic husbands were less likely to respond to wives negativity with their own negativity (i.e., negative reciprocity) than were nonalcoholic couples, suggesting that alcoholics behavior was more adaptive than nonalcoholic controls behavior. Overall, these results suggest that alcohol couples are less positive, more negative, and engage in less problem-solving behavior than alcoholic couples do, and alcoholic couples exhibit patterns of behavior that are similar to maritally distressed couples. This does not identify alcohol consumption, per se, as the cause of these behavior patterns in alcoholic couples, but suggests that chronic alcohol problems and/or alcohol-related negative consequences associated with high levels of consumption are related to maladaptive behavioral interaction patterns.
3.3.2. Relationship between alcohol consumption and marital interactions

Indeed, another common question that has been tested is whether acute alcohol intoxication changes the behavioral patterns of alcoholic couples and whether patterns change relative to nonalcoholic couples. Do alcoholics become more like nondistressed, happily married couples under the influence of alcohol, or do they remain similar to maritally distressed couples? Jacob and Krahn (1988) tested the effects of alcohol consumption by making alcoholic beverages available to their participants prior to one of the interaction tasks, and then compared couples behavior in the drink condition to the no-drink condition. Results indicated that alcoholic and control couples engaged in more negative and more positive behaviors in the drink condition than in the no-drink condition. Three other studies found similar results, indicating that in drinking conditions couples were more positive and congenial (Frankenstein, Hay, & Nathan, 1985; Haber & Jacob, 1997) and engaged in more problem-solving behaviors (Leonard & Roberts, 1998a) than in the no-drink condition. Some studies found that couples were also more negative (Leonard & Roberts, 1998a) in the drink than no-drink condition,

and one study found that this is especially true for alcoholic couples, who experienced increases in negativity during the drinking condition while the nonalcoholic control couples did not (Jacob et al., 1981). At first glance, these results are consistent with the notion that alcohol consumption serves an adaptive function, because it increases problem-solving and positive behavior between spouses. In addition, although consumption also increases negative behavior, this pattern of results is consistent with the notion that alcohol encourages the expression of a larger range of emotions, both positive and negative, which serve to temporarily relieve ongoing tensions between the alcoholic and the spouse (Steinglass et al., 1977). There is some evidence in the general marital interaction literature to support this notion. For example, some suggest that negativity might not always be negative (Weiss & Heyman, 1997), and that some negative behaviors are associated with positive outcomes (Sher & Weiss, 1991). In addition, consistent with Steinglass claims, some behaviors might produce maladaptive short-term effects but be adaptive over the long term (Gottman & Krokoff, 1989). However, this interpretation should be employed with caution for several reasons. First, there is strong support for the notion that high levels of expressed emotion are associated with negative relationship and/or mental health outcomes. For example, high levels of expressed emotion, especially expressed negative emotion, are often indicative of maladaptive interaction patterns between family members (Hooley, 1987) and are associated with high relapse rates in married alcoholics (OFarrell, Hooley, Fals-Stewart, & Cutter, 1998). Second, alcoholic couples are often starting with lower baseline levels of positive interactions, and although this hypothesis has not been directly tested, it suggests that increases in positive interactions due to alcohol consumption might simply raise them to normal levels. These positive behaviors are accompanied by increases in negative behaviors during drinking, which presumably started at levels above baseline relative to controls. It may be that the extremely high levels of negative interactions during drinking negate the positive effects of experiencing normal levels of positive interactions during drinking. Third, a somewhat different picture is presented when sequential interaction patterns are analyzed in drink and no-drink conditions. For example, husbands tendency to increase problem-solving behaviors in response to wives positivity decreased significantly in the drink condition as compared to the no-drink condition (Jacob & Leonard, 1992). The same was true for wives responses to husbands positivity. That is, in the drink condition, wives were less likely to increase problem-solving behaviors, and less likely to repress negativity in response to husbands positivity than they were in the no-drink condition. Finally, there are some reasons to question the specific operational definition of problem-solving behaviora behavior that purportedly increases in the drink conditionand whether it serves an adaptive or maladaptive role in marital interaction. For example, although it is described as behaviors that are relatively neutral in affect, it involves a variety of behaviors such as problem descriptions, positive solutions, negative solutions, compromising, and questions (Jacob & Krahn, 1988; Jacob & Leonard, 1992; Leonard & Roberts, 1998a). In other words, the problem-solving construct as coded by the MICS aggregates these behaviors regardless of their substantive

contribution towards an appropriate solution. Such behavior may or may not reflect positive change (Leonard & Roberts, 1998a). For example, Leonard and Roberts (1998a) found that aggressive men showed higher levels of problem solving than did nonaggressive men, and Leonard and Senchak (1993) found that mens problem-solving behaviors were prospectively predictive of marital violence. Indeed, these data suggest that problem-solving behaviors are positively correlated with aggression in men, and therefore cannot always be interpreted as facilitative.
3.3.3. Moderators of the relationship between alcohol use and marital interactions

Differences in marital interaction behaviors between different subtypes of alcoholics have not been tested as often in the marital interaction literature. Jacob and Leonard (1988) found no significant differences between steady and episodic drinking couples in levels of positivity, negativity, and problem solving. They did find that in the no-drink condition, episodic and steady alcoholic couples had similar levels of problem solving, whereas in the drink condition, steady alcoholics engaged in more problem solving than did episodic alcoholics. Moreover, wives of episodic alcoholics in the drink condition showed lower levels of negativity than in the no-drink condition (Jacob & Leonard, 1988). However, they exhibited higher levels of negative reciprocity in the drink than in the no-drink condition (Leonard & Jacob, 1997). Furthermore, Murphy and OFarrell (1997) found that husbands and wives in aggressive alcoholic relationships were more likely to manifest negativity than were husbands and wives in nonaggressive alcoholic relationships. In addition, they also showed that aggressive couples were more likely to engage in negative reciprocity than nonaggressive couples were. Negative reciprocity is considered a precursor to negative escalation loops that lead to aggression (Leonard & Roberts, 1998a). These results are important because they are consistent with theories that propose that the relation between alcohol abuse and marital dissolution is mediated by marital conflict and marital violence resulting from escalating conflict (Leonard, 1993; OLeary, 1988). Finally, a recent study evaluated the differences between high and low antisocial alcoholic husbands (Jacob, Leonard, & Haber, 2001) and found that for high antisocial alcoholics, couples were more negative in the drink condition than they were in the no-drink condition, and that the wives negativity increased in response to the husbands negativity. These effects were not found for the low antisocial alcoholic group. Because antisocial behavior is one of the most common constructs used to differentiate between different subtypes of alcoholics, this study has made an important step toward the external validity of these interaction effects. In sum, overall patterns suggest that alcoholic couples were characterized by more negativity and less positivity than were nonalcoholic couples. In addition, although alcohol consumption increased positive interactions between spouses, it also increased negative interactions, and decreased positive or adaptive responses to partners positive behavior. These patterns indicate that alcoholic couples are more similar to distressed nonalcoholic couples than they are to nondistressed, nonalcoholic couples, and suggest that alcohol has a maladaptive rather than adaptive influence on marital functioning. In addition, although only a few studies have assessed the moderating effects of alcoholic

subtypes on the relation between alcohol use and abuse and marital interaction, results suggest that some differences between groups emerge, and that the maladaptive effects of alcohol are stronger for episodic and aggressive alcoholics than for steady and nonaggressive alcoholics. Several methodological weaknesses limit our ability to draw strong conclusions about the nature of the relation between alcohol use and marital interaction. First, the most disconcerting problem in this literature is the relatively small samples that were employed (see Table 1B). While quantitative research suggests that researchers need at least 126 total participants to have enough power to detect a moderate effect size (Cohen, 1988), many more participants are needed if two- or three-way interactions are being tested. Sample sizes in the marital interaction literature range from 8 to 135, with the mean sample size of 73, making it difficult to detect main effects, let alone two- and three-way interactions. This is problematic because most of the studies that assess differences between alcoholic and nonalcoholic couples (six in all) typically test main effects for group (alcoholic, nonalcoholic), member (husband, wife), condition (drink, no-drink), and all of the interactions between these variables. It is not surprising, then, that across three outcome variables (positivity, negativity, and problem solving) only three studies detected a three-way interaction (Haber & Jacob, 1997; Jacob & Krahn, 1988; Leonard & Roberts, 1998a), and not coincidentally, these studies had the largest samples (ranging from 107 to 135). A similar pattern can be found when testing two-way interactions. Most two-way interactions were nonsignificant, and the majority of those that were significant had samples sizes above 100 (Haber & Jacob, 1997; Jacob & Krahn, 1988). Second, despite notable efforts to evaluate and validate the psychometric properties of MICSs (see Jacob & Krahn, 1987), inconsistent coding and operational definitions of the various behavioral interaction categories across studies limits our ability to draw strong, substantive conclusions regarding the effects of alcohol use on marital interaction. Although general behavioral categories such as positivity, negativity, and problem solving were identified, they were operationalized in many different ways. For example, positivity was operationalized as positive verbal behavior, positive nonverbal behavior, congeniality, responsibility acceptance, and/or facilitative/enhancing behavior. These different operational definitions probably contributed to unreliability and measurement error across studies. Because unreliability reduces power (see Aiken & West, 1991), these measurement problems likely contributed to an inability to detect important effects. Third, only one marital interaction study employed a placebo group to test whether the effects of alcohol consumption on marital interaction were due to inebriation or whether they were due to the expectations couples have regarding the effects of alcohol consumption (Leonard & Roberts, 1998a). Although this study found that there was no placebo effect, therefore no reason to believe that interaction behavior was due to alcohol expectancies, there is reason to believe that this finding would not generalize to other studies. For example, participants in this study were targeted as aggressive and nonaggressive husbands. Alcoholic participants, on the other hand, might have different alcohol expectancies. A review of alcohol expectancy research supported the conclusion that heavy drinkers perceive the effects of

alcohol to be less negative than nonheavy drinkers, and that alcohol expectancies in general vary depending on drinking patterns (Leigh, 1989). Finally, some evidence suggests that discussing alcohol-related topics during an interaction task influences interaction behavior; however, no systematic effort to assess the effects of alcohol-related discussions has been conducted. This is important because for some couples, interactions about alcohol might occur more frequently than interactions about other topics, especially for couples in which alcohol use and abuse plays a major role in day-to-day life. For example, Halford & Osgarby (1993)reported that over 80% of men and women in their sample reported frequent disagreements about alcohol consumption. Several pieces of evidence suggest that talking about alcohol might provoke different kinds of interactions than talking about another conflict topic or about a benign topic. First, one study explicitly tested differences in behavior between alcohol- and non-alcohol-related discussions during couples marital interaction tasks (Becker & Miller, 1976) and found that alcoholic and nonalcoholic husbands talked more during alcohol-related than non-alcohol-related discussions, and that their wives looked at them more than they looked at their wives during alcohol-related discussions. Second, Halford and Osgarby (1993) found that marital disagreements about alcohol were strongly related to decreased levels of marital satisfaction and to increased number of steps toward divorce. Third,Weiss and Heyman (1997) suggest that distressed couples are more likely to discuss hot topics than are nondistressed couples, and that differences seen in the laboratory between distressed and nondistressed couples might very well be due to the nature of the topic rather than their group status. This suggests that we might see larger effects (i.e., larger differences between alcoholic and nonalcoholic couples), and therefore more consistent effects across studies, if alcoholic couples discussed alcoholrelated problems.
3.4. The effects of alcohol use on marital violence

Several decades of research on the relation between alcohol use and marital violence have generated dozens of studies and at least three comprehensive reviews (Hotaling & Sugarman, 1986; Leonard, 1993;Leonard & Roberts, 1998a). The goal of this section, therefore, is not to reinvent the wheel and write a review similar in scope and objective to previous reviews. Rather, the goal of this section is to provide a review of published studies that have tested the relation between spouses alcohol use and abuse and marital violence to assess whether alcohol use is associated with adaptive (i.e., no violence) or maladaptive levels of marital violence. Kantor and Straus (1987) have operationally defined violence as an act carried out with the intention or perceived intention of causing physical pain or injury to another person (p. 218). The most popular measure of marital violence used in the alcohol use literature is the Conflict Tactics Scale (CTS; Straus, 1979), which has demonstrated high internal validity and construct validity, and has been used in at least two national surveys (Straus & Gelles, 1986). A common conceptual distinction made by the CTS and used often in this literature is among verbal aggression (e.g., yelling, cursing), moderate physical aggression (e.g., throwing something at partner, pushing or grabbing partner), and severe physical

aggression (e.g., hit, kicked, or threatened partner with a weapon). The CTS was used in over half of the marital violence articles reviewed in this article. Other studies typically use single-item or multi-item violence measures of unidentified source (Coleman, Weinman, & Hsi, 1980; Halford & Osgarby, 1993; Hutchinson, 1999; Leonard & Blane, 1992;Rosenbaum & OLeary, 1981), and none of these reported reliability or validity data. Still, others operationalized marital violence by whether individuals were participating in a domestic violence treatment program; however, they did not provide specific entrance criteria (e.g., Hurlbert, Munoz, & Whittaker, 1991).
3.4.1. Differences between alcoholic and nonalcoholic couples in marital violence

A common study design used in this literature is to compare alcoholic and nonalcoholic couples on levels of violence. OFarrell and colleagues took this approach (OFarrell & Choquette, 1991; OFarrell & Murphy, 1995) and found that alcoholic couples reported higher rates of violence than the national norms (OFarrell & Choquette, 1991) and higher rates than a normal control group (OFarrell & Murphy, 1995; Leonard, Bromet, Parkinson, Day, & Ryan, 1985). Three other studies found similar results, indicating that men who were more frequently drunk were more likely to be verbally and physically violent toward their partners (Coleman & Straus, 1983; Hutchinson, 1999; Kantor & Straus, 1989).
3.4.2. Relationship between alcohol consumption and marital violence

Another common approach in the violence literature is to compare violent and nonviolent couples on levels of alcohol use and abuse.Leonard and Roberts (1998a) conducted a study prototypic of this design. They recruited 60 aggressive and 75 nonaggressive men from a previous research program and from the community using newspaper advertisements. Couples were considered aggressive if they engaged in two or more aggressive episodes that did not include physical violence or one or more episodes of husband-to-wife aggression that included a serious aggressive act (a slap or greater). Their results indicated that the aggressive husbands had higher levels of alcohol dependence and consumed more alcohol than did the nonaggressive group. Ten of the 11 violentnonviolent group studies in Table 1C showed similar results. Compared to nonviolent husbands, violent husbands reported higher levels of problem drinking (Hurlbert et al., 1991; Julian & McKenry, 1993; Murphy & OFarrell, 1994; Rosenbaum & OLeary, 1981;Van Hasselt, Morrison, & Bellack, 1985), more frequent alcohol consumption (Coleman et al., 1980), higher quantities of alcohol consumption (Barnett & Fagan, 1993; Leonard & Roberts, 1998a;Rodriguez, Lasch, Chandra, & Lee, 2001), and an earlier onset of problem drinking (Murphy & OFarrell, 1994, 1997). Finally, two studies found similar results using multivariate regression analyses, such that alcohol problems and alcohol use were associated with marital violence prospectively (Leonard & Quigley, 1999; Leonard & Senchak, 1993).
3.4.3. Moderators of the relationship between alcohol use and marital violence

Several studies suggest that the relation between alcohol consumption and marital aggression is moderated by interpersonal and intrapersonal factors. For example, there

is consistent evidence to suggest that alcohol use/abuse is positively correlated with marital aggression for couples with low levels of marital satisfaction and unrelated for those with high levels of satisfaction (Leonard & Blane, 1992; Leonard & Senchak, 1993;Margolin, John, & Foo, 1998). Similar effects are found for other moderators such that alcohol use and abuse is positively related to marital aggression at high levels of marital conflict (Quigley & Leonard, 1999), negative life events (Margolin et al., 1998), and negative affect (Leonard & Blane, 1992), and unrelated at low levels. Interestingly, one study showed that problem alcohol use is associated with violence at low levels of drug use, but not at high levels (Miller et al., 1990). Husbands hostile personality traits also increased the relation between alcohol use/abuse and marital aggression (Heyman, OLeary, & Jouriles, 1995), particularly when satisfaction levels were low (Leonard & Blane, 1992) or when the husband believed that aggression was permissible (Kantor & Straus, 1987), and this was especially true when the alcoholic is drinking (Leonard & Senchak, 1993). Finally, there is some evidence to suggest that aggressive and nonaggressive alcoholic husbands have different drinking styles that might influence whether alcoholics engage in aggressive behavior. For example, nonaggressive alcoholics are more likely than aggressive alcoholics to have steady drinking patterns, and are less likely to drink outside of the home (Murphy & OFarrell, 1994). These drinking patterns might serve as protective factors because they can be associated with higher levels of marital satisfaction and increases in husband problem-solving behavior while drinking (Jacob et al., 1983; Jacob & Leonard, 1988). The moderating effects of level of use are also observed when comparing different levels of diagnosis. For example, men with alcohol dependence were more likely to report marital violence than men diagnosed with misuse or with no diagnosis. Moreover, alcoholics who report recent alcoholism symptoms were more likely to report marital violence than recovered alcoholics and participants with no diagnosis (Leonard et al., 1985). The effects of acute binge or very heavy drinking on marital violence, however, might be different than their effects on marital satisfaction or interaction. For example, two studies show that alcohol use might be curvilinearly related to violence, such that when husbands engage in very heavy or binge drinking (Coleman & Straus, 1983; Hutchinson, 1999; Neff et al., 1995), marital violence actually decreases. Reasons for this are unclear, but some suggest that the alcoholic husband is too inebriated to engage in violent behavior. Overall, there is very little evidence to suggest that alcohol use is associated with adaptive levels of marital violence. On the contrary, for certain couples, alcohol use is associated with higher levels of violence that are detrimental to successful marital functioning. These conclusions should be interpreted with some caution due to several methodological limitations. First, of primary concern is the overreliance on treatmentbased samples (Leonard, 1993). Couples in treatment-based samples are considered inherently different from couples recruited through other sources because they are typically characterized by more severe and acute psychiatric and psychosocial problems. Indeed, 40% of the violence studies in Table 1C (and all but one of the studies that compared violent and nonviolent control groups) were recruited from treatment sources, raising concerns that the differences between groups were not due to alcohol, but to

other characteristics of the sample that might be correlated with marital violence. Second, this concern is exacerbated by the fact that very few (4 of 22 independent samples) studies that test the relation between alcohol use and marital violence excluded couples with other concurrent, comorbid psychiatric diagnoses, or statistically controlled for such diagnoses. Doing so would increase confidence that the effects reported are specific to alcoholism rather than symptoms related to comorbid psychopathology. Third, in the current review, the recruitment source is confounded with the methods and statistical techniques employed to assess the relationship between alcohol use and marital violence. All of the studies that test moderators of this relation employed community-based samples and tested direct and moderating effects of alcohol use without distinguishing between violent and nonviolent groups. Thus, little is known about the moderating effects of inter- and intrapersonal variables in treatment-based samples. Ironically, couples in treatment-based samples would benefit from efforts to identify modifiable moderators (e.g., alcohol expectancies) that would serve to reduce the effects of alcohol use on violent interactions. Finally, an important methodological weakness of the violence literature in particular, and the spousal alcoholism literature in general, is that the majority of the studies assess the relationship between the husbands alcohol use and marital functioning. The next section, therefore, will summarize the effects of wives alcohol use on marital functioning. Go to:

4. The effects of wives alcohol use on marital functioning


Gender differences in the effects of alcohol use on marital functioning are suspected due to gender differences in the etiology, psychiatric comorbidity, course, and consequences of alcohol use and alcoholism (see Halford et al., 1999; Wilsnack & Wilsnack, 1997). Although gender differences in alcoholism are receiving increased attention, little is known about differences in the context of the marital relationship (McCrady, 1990; Roberts & Leonard, 1997). Indeed, very few studies have evaluated the association between wives alcohol use and marital functioning, and even fewer have evaluated the association between concordant alcohol use and marital functioning. For this reason, the following review should be considered preliminary and interpreted with caution. Because marital satisfaction is an intrapersonal measure of marital functioning, the effects of wives alcoholism on husbands marital satisfaction were of primary interest. These effects have been tested in a couple of different ways. Two studies compared husbands with nonalcoholic wives to husbands with heavy drinking wives and found no differences in levels of marital satisfaction (McLeod, 1993;Perodeau & Kohn, 1989). In addition, female alcoholics and their husbands reported higher levels of marital satisfaction than male alcoholics and their wives (Noel, cCrady, Stout, & Fisher-Nelson, 1991). These results stand in stark contrast with the robust, maladaptive effects of husbands alcoholism, and suggest that wives alcohol use might be less maladaptive than husbands alcohol use; however, they are limited by a few methodological shortcomings. For instance, the use of single-item marital satisfaction variables

(McLeod, 1993) and single-item heavy alcohol use items (McLeod, 1993; Perodeau & Kohn, 1989) raises questions about the reliability and validity of the measures. Interestingly, Leonard and Roberts (1998b) reported that wives daily consumption was positively correlated with a composite score of husbands and wives marital satisfaction; however, wives problem use (heavy use and alcohol dependence) was negatively correlated with the composite score of couples satisfaction, and with the husbands own marital satisfaction. This pattern is consistent with the relation between husbands alcohol use and wives marital satisfaction reviewed earlier (e.g., Jacob & Krahn, 1988), suggesting that patterns of light use might serve an adaptive function in the marriage, and patterns of problem use might serve a maladaptive function. Three studies assessed the relation between alcoholism and marital interactions in female alcoholic couples and reported inconsistent results. Noel et al. (1991) reported that female alcoholics engaged in higher levels of positivity and lower levels of negativity toward their spouses than did male alcoholics. Another study found that maritally distressed, female alcoholics and their spouses showed levels of positive verbal behaviors that were similar to normal control couples and higher than distressed nonalcoholic couples during a conflict resolution interaction task; however, spouses of alcoholics engaged in more negative verbal behavior (e.g., criticism) than did normal control spouses (Kelly, Halford, & Young, 2002). Others suggest that female alcoholic couples were more negative and less positive than male alcoholic couples and normal control couples were (Haber & Jacob, 1997) and that female alcoholic couples manifested similar levels of negativity than those of male alcoholic couples and controls in the drink condition. The reasons for this discrepancy and the conclusions that can be drawn are unclear. One explanation may be that when comparing across individuallevel behavior rather than couple-level behavior gender differences emerge. That is, it may be that higher levels of positivity exhibited by female alcoholics (Noel et al., 1991) are characteristic of females in general, rather than female alcoholics. Indeed, women tend to manifest higher levels of positivity regardless of group status (Haber & Jacob, 1997; Jacob & Krahn, 1988; Jacob et al., 1981; Murphy & OFarrell, 1994; OFarrell & Birchler, 1987). Nevertheless, more research with female alcoholics is necessary before conclusions can be drawn about the adaptive versus maladaptive effects of wives alcoholism on marital interaction. The data on the relation between females alcohol use and marital violence, on the other hand, are a little clearer. In general, studies show that indicators of wives heavy use (frequency of times drunk, heavy drinking) are positively correlated with premarital aggression (Leonard & Senchak, 1993) and marital aggression (Kantor & Straus, 1989; Leonard & Roberts, 1998b). In addition, women with alcohol problems were more likely to experience marital violence than were women without alcohol problems (Miller, Downs, & Gondoli, 1989; Neff et al., 1995). Once again, these data suggest that (heavy) alcohol use plays a maladaptive role in marital relationships, being associated with high levels of moderate and severe marital violence. Interestingly, there is some evidence to suggest that wives alcohol use can curb marital violence and that it might serve an adaptive role as well. Leonard and Roberts (1998b) found that wives average daily

consumption was negatively related to marital aggression, which was consistent with the relation between wives daily consumption and marital satisfaction in the same sample. If this average daily consumption measure captures light or steady use patterns, then these results would be consistent with those that suggest light drinking patterns might facilitate healthy relationship functioning. Go to:

5. Concordant drinking and marital functioning


Evaluating the role of alcohol use in marriages in which both partners drink is particularly important because husband and wife drinking patterns might influence each other, and because concordant drinking might be an epiphenomenon of other, more global patterns partner selection and influence such as assortative mating (see Leonard & Das Eiden, 1999). Nevertheless, only seven studies assessed the relation between husbands and wives concordant drinking and their effects on marital functioning. Simple descriptive data using a national probability sample indicated that 8% of the couples that experienced marital violence reported alcohol consumption by both partners prior to the event (Kantor & Straus, 1989). Haber and Jacob (1997) described two different hypotheses that predict adaptive versus maladaptive outcomes in concordant drinking couples. Adaptive outcomes might be observed in concordant couples because attitudes and behaviors regarding alcohol are similar and thus do not serve to divide them. Maladaptive marital outcomes might be observed, on the other hand, if stressors that are associated with alcohol use are, in effect, doubled, and are manifested through poor marital interaction patterns between spouses. Their results supported the second hypothesis, showing that concordant couples were more negative than male alcoholic and normal control couples and less positive and congenial than normal control couples. In addition, while female alcoholic couples with a nonalcoholic husband decreased their negativity in the drink condition, concordant couples increased their negativity in the drink condition. Similar findings were reported with respect to the effects of concordant drinking on wives marital satisfaction. For example, wives in concordant couples reported lower levels of marital satisfaction than did wives of alcoholic husbands (McLeod, 1993; Perodeau & Kohn, 1989) and normal controls (McLeod, 1993). Husbands in concordant drinking couples, on the other hand, either reported negligible differences between groups (McLeod, 1993) or reported higher levels of satisfaction than alcoholic men who were married to nonalcoholic women (Perodeau & Kohn, 1989). Results from a recent study on husband-to-wife violence are consistent with these findings. Quigley and Leonard (2000) found an interaction between husbands and wives alcohol consumption, such that violence was lower if they both drank heavily than it was if the husband drank heavily and the wife did not. Violence levels were lowest when both were light drinkers. Two other studies found similar results, showing higher levels of violence (Leadley, Clark, & Caetano, 2000) and lower levels of marital satisfaction (Mudar, Leonard, & Soltysinski, 2001) in discordant couples than in

nondiscordant couples, and these results were consistent across heavy drinking, frequency of intoxication, and drug use variables (Mudar et al., 2001). Another plausible hypothesis that is not often discussed in the concordant literature is that both adaptive and maladaptive processes are operating concurrently. For example, it could be that one spouse uses alcohol as a mechanism to cope with the negative consequences of his or her partners alcoholism. There is some limited evidence for this hypothesis. For example, one study found that the wives number of steps toward divorce as measured by the Marital Status Inventory (MSI; Weiss & Cerreto, 1980) was positively correlated with the husbands alcohol consumption, however it was negatively correlated with her alcohol consumption (Halford & Osgarby, 1993). Perhaps alcohol use in this sample of wives served as a buffer to the stressors associated with being in a dysfunctional, alcoholic marriage. In sum, these studies present a relatively mixed picture, such that the association between concordant alcohol use and marital functioning can sometime be adaptive and sometimes maladaptive and that there is some evidence that these processes might be dependent on one another. Reasons for the unreliability of effects across studies are unclear, but may be due to methodological shortcomings such as small sample sizes, poorly defined variables, and a lack of data that elucidate under which conditions these effects occur. For example, these effects might be dependent on levels of other contextual factors such as where the couples drink, who they drink with including whether they drink with each other (see Leonard & Mudar, in press), and whether there is a presence of other maladaptive behaviors or disorders, for example, depression, aggression, and antisocial behavior. Much more research on the effects of concordant alcoholism on marital functioning should be conducted before drawing definitive conclusions. Go to:

6. Summary and conclusions


Several decades of research on the effects of alcohol use on marital functioning have been guided primarily by two theoretical hypotheses. The first states that alcohol use is a maladaptive stressor that causes problems in the marital relationship and contributes to marital dysfunction and subsequent dissolution. The second states that alcohol use is an adaptive mechanism that facilitates affective expression and relieves ongoing daily tension between partners, in effect, maintaining the relationship. Three questions have been posed and tested in this literature to test these competing hypotheses: (a) Do alcoholic marriages differ from nonalcoholic marriages in their quality of marital functioning? (b) Is alcohol consumption related to marital functioning? (c) What theoretical and methodological factors moderate the relation between alcohol consumption and marital functioning? This review suggests that there is an overwhelming amount of evidence for the conclusion that spousal alcoholism is maladaptive, and that heavy and problematic alcohol use is associated with lower levels of marital satisfaction, higher levels of maladaptive marital interaction patterns, and higher levels of marital violence. The fact

that these results are observed across several marital functioning domains and several study design characteristics bolsters the robustness and reliability of this effect. For example, studies using intraindividual measures of marital satisfaction (e.g., MAT; Locke & Wallace, 1959) were consistent with those using unbiased observers ratings of marital interaction (e.g., MICS; Weiss, Hops, & Patterson, 1973). In addition, the findings from samples that were recruited from courtordered violence treatment programs (Barnett & Fagan, 1993) were consistent with those from samples recruited through newspaper advertisements in the local paper (Jacob & Krahn, 1988). Finally, maladaptive outcomes are observed in male-alcoholic couples, femalealcoholic couples, and concordant couples. Interestingly, there is also some limited evidence to suggest that alcohol use can serve an adaptive function in the marital relationship. This evidence was also robust in the sense that it was also observed in all three marital functioning domains (e.g., Frankenstein et al., 1985; Jacob et al., 1983; Leonard & Roberts, 1998b), within (Haber & Jacob, 1997) and across (Jacob & Krahn, 1988) alcoholic and nonalcoholic subgroups, during acute intoxication induced in the laboratory (Frankenstein et al., 1985) and using self-reported pencil-and-paper measures (Roberts & Leonard, 1998), and in distinctly different demographic populations, for example, in a community sample of newlywed couples (Leonard & Roberts, 1998b) versus a treatment sample of relatively older alcoholics who had been married an average of 17 years (Jacob et al., 1983). Although these results should be considered tentative until further research can contribute to the reliability of the results, they provide some limited empirical evidence to support Steinglass theory. The effects of alcohol use on marital functioning is moderated by the level of consumption. A fairly clear and consistent pattern emerges when comparing adaptive and maladaptive effects, such that when adaptive results were found, they were almost invariably in the context of light to moderate drinking patterns. In contrast, maladaptive results are found when comparing alcoholic couples to nonalcoholic couples, or when testing the relation between heavy-use patterns and marital functioning. Future research should attempt to test these moderating effects, by testing curvilinear effects of alcohol consumption or by testing the interaction between alcohol consumption and alcoholrelated typologies. Empirical support for these moderating effects will help determine whether they are strong enough to warrant further exploration. Nevertheless, this curvilinear hypothesis is consistent with other literatures that have evaluated the positive and negative effects of alcohol use. For example, several decades of research have shown that moderate levels of alcohol use, as opposed to abstinence or heavy use, are associated with cardiovascular health and decreased mortality rates associated with coronary heart disease (see Klatsky, 1994). Given these possible health benefits of moderate alcohol use, and the association between physical health and well-being and positive marital functioning (Burman & Margolin, 1992), it may be possible that personal health and well-being mediates the relationship between moderate levels of alcohol use and interpersonal functioning.

Furthermore, a long line of research has shown that moderate levels of alcohol use are associated with stress-dampening effects, whereas light and heavy patterns of use are associated with behavioral activation and stress induction (see Greeley & Oei, 1999). The adaptive effects found in this review could be preliminary evidence to suggest that the typical stressors that married couples endure as a function of a normal marital relationship are dampened by moderate alcohol use, thereby showing positive effects on measures of marital functioning. It is important to note that the nature of this curve might depend on the marital functioning variable. For example, descriptive data suggests that at very high levels of use, violence decreases (Coleman & Straus, 1983; Hutchinson, 1999; Neff et al., 1995), perhaps because the alcoholic is incapacitated. Given these potentially different trends, it will be important for future research to compare and contrast the nature of the alcohol effect across marital functioning outcomes, and important for researchers and theorist to assume that variability in this effect exists, despite the message unintentionally implied by the broad conclusions drawn in this review, that the negative alcohol effects are somewhat homogeneous across marital functioning domains.
6.1. Evidence for causal inference

A secondary goal of this review is to evaluate evidence to support a causal relation between spouses alcohol use and abuse and marital functioning. Decisions about whether alcohol use and abuse has an adaptive or maladaptive effect on marital functioning implies that alcohol use and abuse causes changein marital functioning. While this hypothesis is relatively intuitive, alternative hypotheses are plausible. For example, some suggest that marital functioning causes alcohol problems, not the other way around (Jacob, 1992), and other suggest that alcohol use and abuse are correlated with marital functioning, but not its cause (Gelles, 1993). There are three commonly used criteria for establishing causal inference between two variables: (a) an observed relation between variables, (b) evidence that the relation is not spurious, and (c) temporal precedence of the cause to the effect (see Shadish et al., 2002). The results from the preceding review provide strong evidence to support criteria a, that there is a fairly consistent statistical relationship between alcohol use and marital functioning across studies. The jury is still out regarding whether the alcohol effect is spurious (criteria b). As noted previously, the majority of studies do not employ exclusion criteria and/or covariates in their statistical models that would increase confidence that the effects observed are specific to alcohol use and alcoholism; however, given that the results from these less rigorous studies were more or less consistent with those that employed more rigorous designs, the difference may only be a matter of degree. Finally, while criteria c, establishing temporal precedence of the cause to the effect, is important to inferring causality, only 13% of the satisfaction studies (3/24) and 24% (7/29) of the violence studies evaluated the prospective effects of alcohol use. Although none of the interaction studies measured alcohol use prospectively, many of the interaction studies administered alcohol to their participants and tested the acute effects of alcohol use on marital interaction patterns rather than the effects of chronic alcohol problems. This experimental design might be the strongest and clearest demonstration

of temporal precedence in the marital functioning literature, and shows that when alcohol is administered to alcoholic couples prior to an interaction task, behavioral interactions change. Nevertheless, these methodological limitations across studies show that despite the relatively consistent finding that alcohol use is associated with marital functioning, more research is needed to make strong causal statements about this effect.
6.2. Implications for clinical intervention

Decades of research on the treatment of alcohol problems in couples have generated several effective therapeutic models to help treat alcoholic couples (for examples, see McCrady & Epstein, 1995a, 1995b;OFarrell & Rotunda, 1997). These models and the intervention procedures are highly consistent with the results of this empirical review, by assuming that alcohol use and alcoholism are maladaptive, and suggesting that measures of marital satisfaction, interaction, and violence are primary assessment domains that serve as gauges for the status of an alcoholic marriage. Because a thorough review and critique of these intervention procedures is beyond the scope of this review, only a few relevant points will be made. First, some researchers highlight the problems encountered during treatment when alcohol problems are discussed, stating that the first few sessions focus on decreasing alcohol-related feelings and interactions and increasing positive exchanges (OFarrell & Rotunda, 1997, p. 563). Other intervention techniques use alcohol-related topics as vehicles (McCrady & Epstein, 1995a, 1995b) for addressing communication deficits and teaching adaptive communication skills. The marital interaction literature reviewed in this article, however, provides us with very little information about how alcohol discussions might influence communication patterns. It may be that alcohol discussion tasks are even more negative and less positive than discussions about relatively benign topics. In addition, the effects of acute alcohol intoxication on subsequent interaction patterns might be more salient and negative during alcohol-related discussions. Since alcohol-related discussions are primary components of clinical interventions, they might benefit from marital interaction research with alcoholic couples that systematically assesses the effects of alcohol-related discussions on marital interaction patterns. Second, some caution is warranted for alcohol treatment strategies that encourage alcoholics to temporarily reduce alcohol intake to moderate levels as a step towards abstinence, because different types and levels of drinking have different marital functioning consequences. Two things might happen if a drinker were to decrease from heavy drinking patterns to moderate drinking patterns. First, some studies show that marital violence is higher at moderate levels than at heavy levels of drinking (Coleman & Straus, 1983; Hutchinson, 1999), raising concerns that this transition might increase marital problems. Second, other studies show that moderate levels of drinking are sometimes associated with higher levels of marital satisfaction (Dunn et al., 1987; Jacob et al., 1983; Leonard & Roberts, 1998b), which could mislead the couple into believing that their problems have been solved and that continuation in therapy is unnecessary. Clearly, some intervention strategies can use moderation effectively as a step towards

abstinence and are justified in proceeding with caution in the event that these unwanted marital side effects might be elicited.
6.3. Limitations of current research and future directions

Despite the strong evidence in support of the maladaptive hypothesis, and the many strengths of this literature overall, many of these studies are characterized by methodological limitations that threaten the internal and external validity of their results. Many of these limitations applied to specific subsets of studies and were already discussed in relative detail, including small sample sizes, poor measurement quality, over reliance on treatment samples, failure to employ placebo groups to control for expectancy effects, and a failure to systematically evaluate the effects of alcohol related topics during marital interaction tasks. Other limitations of this literature transcend specific study design or marital outcome subgroups and include a striking dearth of studies that evaluate the effects of wives alcohol use and concordant alcohol use on marital functioning, the relationship between alcohol use and ones own marital functioning, relatively few systematic attempts to rule out alternative hypotheses regarding the specificity and directionality of the alcohol effect, and an oversampling of older, Caucasian couples who are in well-established marriages. Recent research by Leonard and colleagues is a notable exception, however, and has made special efforts to assess the longitudinal effects of alcohol use on marital violence in young newlywed couples with a higher than average proportion of non-Caucasian participants (see Leonard & Roberts, 1996, for a review), to evaluate the role of wives drinking on marital outcomes, and to investigate the role of ethnicity in alcohol involvement and the transition to marriage (Mudar, Kearns, & Leonard, 2002). Furthermore, only a handful of satisfaction and violence studies have systematically identified and tested mediators and moderators of the alcohol effect (Dumka & Roosa, 1993; Kantor & Straus, 1987;Leonard & Blane, 1992; Leonard & Senchak, 1993; Margolin et al., 1998; Quigley & Leonard, 1999;Zweben, 1986). Future research should use these studies as a point of departure for identifying empirically supported risk and protective factors that can be targeted in future prevention and intervention programs and for developing theoretical models of risk and resilience to alcohol problems that contribute specifically to marital success and/or dissolution processes. Potential mediators should be explored for both adaptive and maladaptive processes. For example, the adaptive effects of alcohol use may be mediated by positive interpersonal/social expectancies associated with alcohol use, increases in positive affect, decreases in perceived stress, and perhaps from the physical health benefits associated with moderate alcohol use. The maladaptive effects of heavy or problematic alcohol use and alcoholism may be explained by the common stressors associated with the negative consequences of heavy use (e.g., problems at work, problems with social network members), cognitive/intellectual impairments while drinking heavily that could impair communication skills (Leonard & Roberts, 1998a) or preoccupation with alcohol use behavior that might cause an alcoholic to neglect his or her interpersonal relationships. Moreover, it is important to characterize the causal relationships among the various marital functioning domains.

For example, alcohol use may be associated with interpersonal violence because the alcoholic is disinhibited, but it may also be due to low satisfaction levels and an inability to communicate effectively with a partner. A few researchers have begun to elucidate these relationships (e.g., Quigley & Leonard, 1999). Another serious limitation of the current research is that very few studies assess effects of alcohol use prospectively (Leonard & Rothbard, 1999). Longitudinal studies are important for several reasons. First, establishing temporal precedence of alcohol use and abuse increases our confidence that there is a causal relation between alcohol and marital functioning. Second, longitudinal studies would allow researchers to test the reciprocal effects of this relation. That is, a host of research and theory in the literature suggests that poor marital functioning is likely to cause drinking problems rather than the other way around (see Halford et al., 1999; Jacob, 1992), and it is likely that they are influencing each other over time. Third, longitudinal studies would provide the opportunity to observe the effects of alcohol use and marital functioning on subsequent dissolution. While there are several longitudinal studies that assess the effects of the transition into and out of marriage on rates of alcohol use and abuse (see Leonard & Rothbard, 1999, for a review), and over 100 longitudinal studies of marital quality and stability (Karney & Bradbury, 1995), no studies have observed alcohol and marital functioning variables over time and tested how they contribute to marital dissolution. Indeed, several studies in this review found that alcohol use and abuse was related to dissolution potential in married couples (Halford & Osgarby, 1993; Katz et al., 1995; OFarrell & Birchler, 1987; Perodeau & Kohn, 1989) as measured by the MSI. In addition, one of the most comprehensive theoretical and empirical models of dissolution suggests that marital satisfaction, interaction, and violence are central components of the dissolution process (Gottman, 1994). Given the strong association between alcohol use and these marital processes, longitudinal studies that evaluate their course over time and their contribution to dissolution are paramount.
6.4. A heuristic model of alcohol use and marital functioning

Despite the advantages of using the adaptive and maladaptive hypotheses as heuristics for guiding past research (and as an interesting storyline in this review), most researchers and clinicians would agree that they grossly oversimplify what are considered highly complex phenomena. For example, the role that alcohol use plays in interpersonal aggression alone has been observed for hundreds of years, has been the topic of dozens of theoretical and/or empirical reviews, and almost as many explanatory models that include, but are not limited to cognitive, behavioral, systemic, expectancy, personality, biological/pharmaceutical, and social/contextual theories (see Kantor & Straus, 1986; Pernanen, 1976, for reviews). It is no surprise, then, that comprehensive theoretical models of risk that attempt to explain all of the possible mechanisms by which alcohol use contributes to marital success or failure are elusive. Nevertheless, researchers and clinician may benefit from a conceptual guide that can be used to replicate and build upon the studies evaluated in this review. The heuristic model

proposed in Fig. 1 is meant to be a modest step toward this end. The central purpose of the model is to provide a pragmatic framework that is broad enough to encompass the multiple factors that may play a role in the alcoholic marriage, but also detailed enough that it can serve as a mechanism for articulating and testing specific hypotheses regarding the longitudinal, bidirectional, moderated, and mediated pathways of risk and protection that are accumulating in this literature. It is not meant to be exhaustive; rather, it is meant to serve as a foundation and springboard for future studies, and as a point of departure for more elaborate, comprehensive theoretical models of risk. For example, the majority of the studies evaluated in this review tested the direct effects of alcohol use on marital satisfaction (path a) or interaction and violence (path b). Other studies went one step further by testing interactions between alcohol use and risk or protective factors represented by paths c and d (Kantor & Straus, 1987; Leonard & Blane, 1992; Leonard & Senchak, 1993; Margolin et al., 1998; Quigley & Leonard, 1999; Zweben, 1986), or by testing mediated effects represented by paths e and f (Dumka & Roosa, 1993, 1995). However, no studies have attempted to identify multiple mediated pathways of risk from alcohol use to divorce, or pathways that evaluate the relationships among the various marital functioning domains in the context of alcohol use and divorce (paths g, h, i, and j). Such pathways are important because they can serve as building blocks for larger, more comprehensive theoretical models, and as targets for intervention and prevention studies that aim to reduce marital problems, drinking problems, or both. Future studies that (a) evaluate risk and protective mechanisms via mediator and moderator models delineated in Fig. 1, (b) account for covariates that might threaten the specificity of the alcohol effect (path k), (c) attempt to model the bidirectional effects between alcohol use and marital functioning, and (d) attempt to incorporate multiple marital functioning domains over time to evaluate their relative and interactive effects on marital success and dissolution will make generous contributions to the literature.

Fig. 1 A heuristic model of alcohol use and marital functioning. Go to:

Acknowledgements
This review was supported in part by grants from the National Institute on Mental Health (MH12010) and the National Institute of Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (F31AA13217). The author would like to thank Drs. Laurie Chassin, Manuel Barrera, Ken Leonard, Susan Somerville, Nancy Russo, Larry Dumka, Frank Lotrich, Tad Gorske, Christopher Martin, Mary Amanda Dew, and Joshua Miller for their helpful guidance and feedback.

Go to:

References
1. Aiken L, West S. Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage; 1991. 2. Babor TF, Hesselbrock V, Meyer RE, Shoemaker W. Types of alcoholics: Evidence from clinical, experimental, and genetic research. New York: New York Academy of Sciences; 1994. 3. Barnett OW, Fagan RW. Alcohol use in male spouse abusers, and their female partners. Journal of Family Violence. 1993;8:125. 4. Becker JV, Miller PM. Verbal and nonverbal marital interaction patterns of alcoholics and nonalcoholics. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1976;37:1616 1624. [PubMed] 5. Billings AG, Kessler M, Gomberg CA, Weiner S. Marital conflict resolution of alcoholic and nonalcoholic couples during drinking and nondrinking sessions. Journal of Studies on Alcohol.1979;40:183195. [PubMed] 6. Burman B, Margolin G. Analysis of the association between marital relationships and health problems: An interactional perspective. Psychological Bulletin. 1992;112:3963. [PubMed] 7. Caetano R, Clark CL. Trends in alcohol consumption patterns among Whites, Blacks and Hispanics: 1984 and 1995. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1998;59:659668. [PubMed] 8. Cohen J. Statistical Power for the Behavioral Sciences. 2nd ed. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1988. 9. Clark WB, Hilton ME. Alcohol in America. New York: State University of New York; 1991. 10. Coleman DH, Straus MA. Alcohol abuse and family violence. In: Gottheil E, Druley KA, Skolada TE, Waxman HM, editors. Alcohol, drug abuse and aggression. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas; 1983. pp. 104124. 11. Coleman KH, Weinman ML, Hsi BP. Factors affecting conjugal violence. Journal of Psychology. 1980;105:197202. [PubMed] 12. Curran PJ, Muthen BO, Harford TC. The influence of changes in marital status on developmental trajectories of alcohol use in young adults. Journal of Studies on Alcohol.1998;59:647658. [PubMed] 13. Dumka LE, Roosa MW. Factors mediating problem drinking and mothers personal adjustment.Journal of Family Psychology. 1993;7:333343. 14. Dumka LE, Roosa MW. The role of stress and family relationships in mediating problem drinking and fathers personal adjustment. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1995;56:528537.[PubMed] 15. Dunn NJ, Jacob T, Hummon N, Seilhamer RA. Marital stability in alcoholicspouse relationships as a function of drinking pattern and location. Journal of Abnormal Psychology.1987;96:99107. [PubMed]

16. Epstein EE, Kahler CW, McCrady BS, Lewis KD, Lewis S. An empirical classification of drinking patterns among alcoholics: Binge, episodic, sporadic, and steady. Addictive Behaviors.1995;20:2341. [PubMed] 17. Epstein EE, McCrady BS, Hirsh LS. Marital functioning in early versus late-onset alcoholic couples. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 1997;21:547 556. [PubMed] 18. Escallier-Nicola E, Botwin DE, Tarter RE. The influence of psychosocial variables on male adolescents at risk for alcoholism. Journal of Child and Adolescent Substance Abuse. 1994;3:18. 19. Frankenstein W, Hay WM, Nathan PE. Effects of intoxication on alcoholics marital communication and problem solving. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1985;46:16. [PubMed] 20. Gelles RJ. Alcohol and other drugs are associated with violenceThey are not its cause. In: Gelles RJ, Loseke DR, editors. Current controversies on family violence. Newbury Park, CA: Sage; 1993. pp. 182196. 21. Gotlib IH, McCabe SB. Marriage and psychopathology. In: Fincham FD, Bradbury TN, editors.The psychology of marriage: Basic issues and applications. New York: Guilford Press; 1990. pp. 226257. 22. Gottman JM. What predicts divorce?: The relationship between marital processes and marital outcomes. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1994. 23. Gottman JM, Krokoff LJ. Marital interaction and satisfaction: A longitudinal view. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1989;57:4752. [PubMed] 24. Gottman JM, Roy AK. Sequential analysis: A guide for behavioral researchers. New York: Cambridge University; 1990. 25. Greeley J, Oei T. Alcohol and tension reduction. In: Leonard KE, Blane HT, editors.Psychological theories of drinking and alcoholism. 2nd ed. New York: Guilford Press; 1999. pp. 1453. 26. Haber JR, Jacob T. Marital interactions of male versus female alcoholics. Family Process.1997;36:385402. [PubMed] 27. Halford WK, Bouma R, Kelly A, Young R., McD Individual psychopathology and marital distress: Analyzing the association and implications for therapy. Behavior Modification.1999;23:179216. [PubMed] 28. Halford WK, Osgarby SM. Alcohol abuse in clients presenting with marital problems. Journal of Family Psychology. 1993;6:245254. 29. Helzer JE, Burnam LN, McEvoy LT. Alcohol abuse and dependence. In: Robins LN, Reiger DA, editors. Psychiatric disorders in America. The epidemiologic catchment area study. New York: Free Press; 1991. pp. 81115. 30. Helzer JE, Pryzbeck JR. The co-occurrence of alcoholism with other psychiatric disorders in the general population and its impact on treatment. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1988;49:219224. [PubMed] 31. Heyman RE, OLeary KD, Jouriles EN. Alcohol and aggressive personality styles: Potentiators of serious physical aggression against wives? Journal of Family Psychology. 1995;9:4457.

32. Hooley JM. The nature and origins of expressed emotion. In: Hahlweg K, Goldstein MJ, editors.Understanding major mental disorder: The contribution of family interaction research. New York: Family Process; 1987. pp. 176194. 33. Hotaling GT, Sugarman DB. An analysis of risk markers in husband to wife violence: The current state of knowledge. Violence and Victims. 1986;1:101 124. [PubMed] 34. Hurlbert DF, Munoz CJ, Whittaker KE. Etiological characteristics of abusive husbands. Military Medicine. 1991;156:670675. [PubMed] 35. Hutchinson IW. Alcohol, fear, and woman abuse. Sex Roles. 1999;40:893920. 36. Jacob T. Family studies of alcoholism. Journal of Family Psychology. 1992;5:319 338. 37. Jacob T, Dunn NJ, Leonard KE. Patterns of alcohol abuse and family stability. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 1983;7:382 385. [PubMed] 38. Jacob T, Dunn NJ, Leonard KE, Davis P. Alcohol-related impairments in male alcoholics and the psychiatric symptoms of their spouses: An attempt to replicate. American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse. 1985;11:55 67. [PubMed] 39. Jacob T, Krahn GL. The classification of behavioral observation codes in studies of family interaction. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1987;49:677687. 40. Jacob T, Krahn GL. Marital interactions of alcoholic couples: Comparison with depressed and nondistressed couples. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1988;56:7379.[PubMed] 41. Jacob T, Leonard KE. Alcoholic-spouse interaction as a function of alcoholism subtype and alcohol consumption interaction. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1988;97:231237.[PubMed] 42. Jacob T, Leonard KE. Sequential analysis of marital interactions involving alcoholic, depressed, and nondistressed men. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1992;101:647656. [PubMed] 43. Jacob T, Leonard KE, Haber JR. Family interactions of alcoholics as related to alcoholism type and drinking condition. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2001;25:835843.[PubMed] 44. Jacob T, Ritchey D, Cvitkovic JF, Blane HT. Communication styles of alcoholic and nonalcoholic families when drinking and not drinking. Journal of Studies on Alcohol.1981;42:466482. [PubMed] 45. Jacob T, Seilhamer RA. Alcoholism and family interaction. In: Jacob T, editor. Family interaction and psychopathology. New York: Plenum; 1987. pp. 535580. 46. Johnston L, OMalley P, Bachman J. National survey results from the Monitoring the Future Study, 19751995: College students and young adults. Vol. 2. Rockville, MD: National Institute on Drug Abuse; 1997. 47. Julian TW, McKenry PC. Mediators of male violence toward female intimates. Journal of Family Violence. 1993;8:3956.

48. Kantor GK, Straus MA. The drunken bum theory of wife beating. Social Problems.1987;34:213230. 49. Kantor GK, Straus MA. Substance abuse as a precipitant of wife abuse victimizations. American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse. 1989;15:173 189. [PubMed] 50. Karney BR, Bradbury TN. The longitudinal course of marital quality and stability: A review of theory, method, and research. Psychological Bulletin. 1995;118:3 34. [PubMed] 51. Katz J, Arias I, Beach SRH, Brody G, Roman P. Excuses, excuses: Accounting for the effects of partner violence on marital satisfaction and stability. Violence and Victims. 1995;10:315326.[PubMed] 52. Kelly AB, Halford WK, Young RM. Couple communication and female problem drinking: A behavioral observation study. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2002;16:269271. [PubMed] 53. Kessler RC. The national comorbidity survey: Preliminary results and future directions.International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research. 1995;5:139 151. 54. Klatsky AL. Epidemiology of coronary heart disease: Influence of alcohol. Alcohol: Clinical and Experimental Research. 1994;18:8896. [PubMed] 55. Kurdek LA. Developmental changes in marital satisfaction: A 6-year prospective longitudinal study of newlywed couples. In: Bradbury TN, editor. The developmental course of marital dysfunction. New York: Cambridge University; 1998. pp. 180204. 56. Leadley K, Clark CL, Caetano R. Couples drinking patterns, intimate partner violence, and alcohol-related partnership problems. Journal of Substance Abuse. 2000;11:253263. [PubMed] 57. Leigh BC. In search of the seven dwarves: Issues of measurement and meaning in alcohol expectancy research. Psychological Bulletin. 1989;105:361 373. [PubMed] 58. Leonard KE. Drinking patterns and intoxication in marital violence: Review, critique, and future directions for research. In: Martin SE, editor. Alcohol and interpersonal violence: Fostering multidisciplinary perspectives. Monographs of the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. Pt 3. Vol. 24. Bethesda, MD: National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism; 1993. pp. 253280. 59. Leonard KE, Blane HT. Alcohol and marital aggression in a national sample of young men.Journal of Interpersonal Violence. 1992;7:1930. 60. Leonard KE, Bromet EJ, Parkinson DK, Day NL, Ryan CM. Patterns of alcohol use and physically aggressive behavior in men. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1985;46:279282.[PubMed] 61. Leonard KE, Das Eiden R. Husbands and wifes drinking: Unilateral or bilateral influences among newlyweds in a general population sample. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1999;(Suppl 13):130138. [PubMed] 62. Leonard KE, Jacob T. Sequential interactions among episodic and steady alcoholics and their wives. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 1997;11:1825.

63. Leonard KE, Mudar P. Peer and partner drinking and the transition to marriage: A longitudinal examination of selection and influence processes. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2003 June;17(2):115125. [PubMed] 64. Leonard KE, Quigley BM. Drinking and marital aggression in newlyweds: An event-based analysis of drinking and the occurrence of husband marital aggression. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1999;60:537545. [PubMed] 65. Leonard KE, Roberts LJ. Alcohol in the early years of marriage. Alcohol, Health, and Research World. 1996;20:192196. 66. Leonard KE, Roberts LJ. The effects of alcohol on the marital interactions of aggressive and nonaggressive husbands and their wives. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1998a;107:602615.[PubMed] 67. Leonard KE, Roberts LJ. Marital aggression, quality and stability in the first years of marriage: Findings from the Buffalo newlywed study. In: Bradbury TN, editor. The developmental course of marital dysfunction. New York: Cambridge University; 1998b. pp. 4473. 68. Leonard KE, Rothbard JC. Alcohol and the marriage effect. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1999;(Suppl 13):139146. [PubMed] 69. Leonard KE, Senchak M. Alcohol and premarital aggression among newlywed couples. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1993;(Suppl 11):96108. [PubMed] 70. Leonard KE, Senchak M. Prospective prediction of husband marital aggression within newlywed couples. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1996;105:369 380. [PubMed] 71. Locke HJ, Wallace KM. Short marital-adjustment and prediction tests: Their reliability and validity. Marriage and Family Living. 1959;21:251255. 72. Margolin G, John RS, Foo L. Interactive and unique risk factors for husbands emotional and physical abuse of their wives. Journal of Family Violence. 1998;13:315344. 73. Margolin G, Talovic S, Weinstein CD. Areas of Change Questionnaire: A practical approach to marital assessment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1983;51:920931. 74. McCrady BS. Marital dysfunction: Alcoholism and marriage. In: Pattison EM, Kaufman E, editors. Encyclopedic Handbook of Alcoholism. New York: Gardner Press; 1982. pp. 673685. 75. McCrady BS. The marital relationship and alcoholism treatment. In: Collins RL, Leonard KE, Searles JS, editors. Alcohol and the family: Research and clinical perspectives. New York: Guilford Press; 1990. pp. 338355. 76. McCrady BS, Epstein EE. Directions for research on alcoholic relationships: Marital- and individual-based models of heterogeneity. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 1995a;9:157166. 77. McCrady BS, Epstein EE. Marital therapy in the treatment of alcohol problems. In: Jacobson NS, Gurman AS, editors. Clinical handbook of couple therapy. New York: Guilford Press; 1995b. pp. 369393.

78. McLeod JD. Spouse concordance for alcohol dependence and heavy drinking: Evidence from a community sample. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 1993;6:11461155.[PubMed] 79. Miller BA, Downs WR, Gondoli DM. Spousal violence among alcoholic women as compared to a random household sample of women. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1989;50:533540.[PubMed] 80. Miller BA, Nochajski TH, Leonard KE, Blane HT, Gondoli DM, Bowers PM. Spousal violence and alcohol/drug problems among parolees and their spouses. Women and Criminal Justice.1990;1:5572. 81. Mudar P, Kearns JN, Leonard KE. The transition to marriage and changes in alcohol involvement among black couples and white couples. Journal of Studies on alcohol.2002;63:568576. [PubMed] 82. Mudar P, Leonard KE, Soltysinski K. Discrepant substance use and marital functioning in newlywed couples. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2001;69:130134. [PubMed] 83. Murphy CM, OFarrell TJ. Factors associated with marital aggression in male alcoholics.Journal of Family Psychology. 1994;8:321335. 84. Murphy CM, OFarrell TJ. Couple communication patterns of maritally aggressive and nonaggressive male alcoholics. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1997;58:83 90. [PubMed] 85. Neff JA, Holamon B, Schluter TD. Spousal violence among Anglos, Blacks, and Mexican Americans: The role of demographic variables, psychosocial predictors, and alcohol consumption. Journal of Family Violence. 1995;10:121. 86. Noel NE, McCrady BS, Stout RL, Fisher-Nelson H. Gender differences in marital functioning of male and female alcoholics. Family Dynamics of Addiction Quarterly. 1991;1:3138. 87. OFarrell TJ, Birchler GR. Marital relationships of alcoholic, conflicted and nonconflicted couples. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy. 1987;13:259274. 88. OFarrell TJ, Choquette K. Marital violence in the year before and after spouseinvolved alcoholism treatment. Family Dynamics of Addiction Quarterly. 1991;1:3240. 89. OFarrell TJ, Hooley J, Fals-Stewart W, Cutter HSG. Expressed emotion and relapse in alcoholic patients. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1998;66:744752. [PubMed] 90. OFarrell TJ, Murphy CM. Marital violence before and after alcoholism treatment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1995;63:256 262. [PubMed] 91. OFarrell TJ, Rotunda RJ. Couples interventions and alcohol abuse. In: Halford WK, Markman HJ, editors. Clinical handbook of marriage and couples interventions. New York: Wiley; 1997. pp. 556588. 92. OLeary KD. Physical aggression between spouses: A social learning perspective. In: Van Hasselt VB, Morrison RL, Bellack AS, Hersen M, editors. Handbook of family violence. New York: Plenum; 1988. pp. 3155.

93. Pan HS, Neidig PH, OLeary KD. Predicting mild and severe husband-to-wife physical aggression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1994;5:975 981. [PubMed] 94. Paolino TJ, McCrady BS. The alcoholic marriage: Alternative perspectives. New York: Grune & Stratton; 1977. 95. Pernanen K. Alcohol and crimes of violence. In: Kissin B, Begleiter H, editors. The biology of alcoholism: Social aspects of alcoholism. New York: Plenum; 1976. pp. 344351. 96. Perodeau GM, Kohn PM. Sex differences in the marital functioning of treated alcoholics. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 1989;23:111. [PubMed] 97. Quigley BM, Leonard KE. Husband alcohol expectancies, drinking, and marital conflict styles as predictors of severe marital violence among newlyweds. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors.1999;13:4959. 98. Quigley BM, Leonard KE. Alcohol and the continuation of early marital aggression. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2000;24:1003 1010. [PubMed] 99. Roberts LJ, Leonard KE. Gender differences and similarities in the alcohol and marriage relationship. In: Wilsnack RW, Wilsnack SC, editors. Gender and alcohol: Individual and social perspectives. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers Center of Alcohol Studies; 1997. pp. 289311. 100. Roberts LJ, Leonard KE. An empirical typology of drinking partnerships and their relationship to marital functioning and drinking consequences. Journal of Marriage and the Family.1998;60:515526. 101. Rodriguez E, Lasche KE, Chandra P, Lee J. Family violence, employment status, welfare benefits, and alcohol drinking in the United States: What is the relation? Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 2001;55:172 178. [PMC free article] [PubMed] 102. Rosenbaum A, OLeary KD. Marital violence: Characteristics of abusive couples. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1981;49:6371. [PubMed] 103. Schapp C. A comparison of the interaction of distressed and nondistressed married couples in a laboratory situation: Literature survey, methodological issues, and empirical investigation. In: Halweg K, Jacobson NS, editors. Marital interaction: Analysis and modification. New York: Guilford Press; 1984. pp. 133 158. 104. Schiavi RC, Stimmel BB, Mandeli J, White D. Chronic alcoholism and male sexual function.American Journal of Psychiatry. 1995;152:1045 1051. [PubMed] 105. Selzer ML. The Michigan Alcoholism Screening Test: The quest for a new diagnostic instrument.American Journal of Psychiatry. 1971;127:89 94. [PubMed] 106. Shadish WR, Cook TD, Campbell DT. Experimental and quasiexperimental design for generalized causal inference. Boston: HoughtonMifflin; 2002.

107. Sher TG, Weiss RL. Negativity in marital communication: Wheres the beef? Behavioral Assessment. 1991;13:15. 108. Skinner HA, Allen BA. Alcohol dependence syndrome: Measurement and validation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1982;91:199209. [PubMed] 109. Spanier GB. Measuring dyadic adjustment: New scales for assessing the quality of marriage and similar dyads. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1976;38:1528. 110. Steinglass P. A systems approach to alcoholism: A model and its clinical implications. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1971;24:401408. [PubMed] 111.Steinglass P. The alcoholic family in the interaction laboratory. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 1979a;167:428436. [PubMed] 112. Steinglass P. The home observation assessment method (HOAM): Realtime naturalistic observation of families in their homes. Family Process. 1979b;18:337354. [PubMed] 113. Steinglass P, Davis DI, Berenson D. Observations of conjointly hospitalized alcohol couples during sobriety and intoxication: Implications for theory and therapy. Family Process.1977;16:116. [PubMed] 114. Steinglass P, Robertson A. The alcoholic family. In: Kissen B, Begleiter H, editors. The pathogenesis of alcoholism. New York: Plenum; 1983. pp. 243307. 115. Straus MA. Measuring intrafamily conflict and violence: The Conflict Tactics Scales. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1979;41:7588. 116. Straus MA, Gelles R. Societal change and change in family violence from 1975 to 1985 as revealed by two national surveys. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1986;48:785793. 117. Terman LM. Psychological factors of marital happiness. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1938. 118. Tubman JG. A pilot study of family life among school-age children of problem drinking men: Child, mother, and family comparisons. Family Dynamics of Addiction Quarterly. 1991;1:1020. 119. Van Hasselt VB, Morrison RL, Bellack AS. Alcohol use in wife abusers and their spouses.Addictive Behaviors. 1985;10:127135. [PubMed] 120. Weiss RL, Cerreto MC. The Marital Status Inventory: Development of a measure of dissolution potential. American Journal of Family Therapy. 1980;8:8085. 121. Weiss RL, Heyman RE. Observation of marital interaction. In: Fincham FD, Bradbury TN, editors. The psychology of marriage. New York: Guilford; 1990. pp. 87117. 122. Weiss RL, Heyman RE. A clinical research overview of couples interactions. In: Halford WK, Markman HJ, editors. Clinical handbook of marriage and couples interventions. New York: Wiley; 1997. pp. 1341. 123. Weiss RL, Hops W, Patterson GR. A framework for conceptualizing marital conflict, a technology for altering it, and some data for evaluating it. In: Hamerlynck LA, Handy LC, Mash EJ, editors. Behavior change: Methodology, concepts, and practice. Champaign, IL: Research Press; 1973. pp. 309342.

124. Wilsnack RW, Wilsnack SC. Gender and alcohol: Individual and social perspectives. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers Center of Alcohol Studies; 1997. 125. Zweben A. Problem drinking and marital adjustment. Journal of Studies on Alcohol.1986;47:167172. [PubMed]

Вам также может понравиться