Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 13

informs ®

Vol. 26, No. 4, July–August 2007, pp. 460–472


issn 0732-2399  eissn 1526-548X  07  2604  0460 doi 10.1287/mksc.1060.0253
© 2007 INFORMS

The Influence of Product Variety on


Brand Perception and Choice
Jonah Berger, Michaela Draganska, Itamar Simonson
Graduate School of Business, Stanford University, Memorial Way, Stanford, California 94305
{jberger@wharton.upenn.edu, draganska_michaela@gsb.stanford.edu, simonson_itamar@gsb.stanford.edu}

W e propose that the variety a brand offers often serves as a quality cue and thus influences which brand
consumers choose. Specifically, brands that offer a greater variety of options that appear compatible and
require similar skills tend to be perceived as having greater category expertise or core competency in the cate-
gory, which, in turn, enhances their perceived quality and purchase likelihood. Six studies support this propo-
sition and demonstrate that compared to brands which offer fewer products, (a) brands which offer increased
compatible variety are perceived as having higher quality; (b) this effect is mediated by product variety’s impact
on perceived expertise; (c) the higher perceived quality produces a greater choice share of the higher vari-
ety brand, even among consumers who select options that multiple brands offer and (d) product variety also
impacts post-experience perceptions of taste. The findings suggest that in addition to directly affecting brand
choice share through influencing the fit with consumer preferences, product line length can also indirectly affect
brand choice through influencing perceived brand quality.
Key words: variety; consumer choice; quality cues; product line length
History: This paper was received November 9, 2005, and was with the authors 4 months for 2 revisions;
processed by Ravi Dhar.

A basic assumption concerning the depth of a associated with higher quality will influence subse-
brand’s product assortment is that offering consumers quent (perceived) experience with the chosen product.
more options is superior to offering fewer options, If these propositions are found to be true, it would
for the simple reason that a greater variety of options suggest that when determining optimal product line
can cater to a wider range of tastes (e.g., Lancaster length, marketers should consider both the direct and
1990). This basic assumption has been challenged by indirect ways in which additional variety can influ-
recent research that has raised doubts about the wis- ence brand choice.
dom of offering consumers many options to choose We begin with a discussion of prior research on the
from (e.g., Iyengar and Lepper 2000, Schwartz 2004). effect of the number of considered options on choice,
For example, Iyengar and Lepper demonstrated that followed by an analysis of the role of variety in brand
consumers who were presented with a set of 24 evaluation and purchase. We then describe six stud-
jams were significantly less likely to purchase one of ies that were designed to test our predictions and
these options compared to consumers presented with the conditions under which greater product variety
just 6 jams. Other researchers (e.g., Dhar 1996, 1997; is expected to enhance brand perceptions. We con-
Greenleaf and Lehmann 1995; Tversky and Shafir clude with a discussion of the theoretical and practical
1992) have shown that offering more options can implications of this research.
generate decision conflict and preference uncertainty,
leading to decision deferral. The Impact of Brand Product Variety
Although these studies have provided important on Brand Evaluation and Choice
insights, we propose that the variety a brand offers Consider a consumer shopping at their local grocery
also often serves as a quality cue and thus influences store who decides that they would like to buy some
which brand consumers choose. In particular, we sug- ice cream. They are confronted with an onslaught of
gest that a brand offering greater variety of compati- options, with flavors ranging from Vanilla to Rum
ble options, that is, options that require similar skills, Raisin and brands from Haagen Dazs to Breyers. On
is perceived as having greater category expertise and, this particular day, Haagen Dazs offers more than a
consequently, is more likely to be selected. Further- dozen different flavors while Breyers only offers a
more, because true quality is often ambiguous, an couple. Assuming the consumer has no prior brand
initial belief that brands offering more options are preference, which brand will they choose and how
460
Berger et al.: The Influence of Product Variety on Brand Perception and Choice
Marketing Science 26(4), pp. 460–472, © 2007 INFORMS 461

will the variety the brand offered influence their deci- quality cue may play a key role particularly when
sion? If they know they want Rum Raisin and only detailed attribute information is unavailable or under
Breyers offers it, the choice is easy. But what if they low involvement (see, e.g., Kassarjian 1978). Further-
decide they want a flavor offered by both brands (e.g., more, it may play a role both when consumers eval-
Vanilla)? Could the amount of variety each brand uate each brand separately, such as when consumers
offers influence their perception of those brands and, consider a brand’s end-of-aisle display or a brand
hence, their brand choice? that is sold through exclusive distributors, and when
These questions apply broadly because most pur- two or more brands’ offerings are directly compared
chase situations involve brands that offer different (see, e.g., Nowlis and Simonson 1997).
levels of product variety, with the level of variety The notion that consumers rely on cues to assess
easily observed by the consumer. A good deal of quality is well established and has been relied on
research has assumed that, other things being equal in both marketing (e.g., Allison and Uhl 1964, Olson
(e.g., costs, shelf space), offering more options is bet- 1977, Mayzlin 2006, Purohit and Srivastava 2001) and
ter (e.g., Kekre and Srinivasan 1990, Lancaster 1990). economics (e.g., Klein and Leffler 1981, Nelson 1974).
Supporting the notion that consumers like variety, in For example, in the absence of other diagnostic infor-
certain product categories a reduction in assortment mation, consumers tend to rely on price as an indica-
has been shown to lead to reduced sales (Borle et al. tor of quality, particularly for experience goods. The
2005). reasons for using quality cues such as price, brand
At the same time, offering greater product variety is name, and manufacturer’s reputation as proxies for
usually associated with higher costs (e.g., Draganska quality appear rather straightforward. In contrast, the
and Jain 2005, Lancaster 1979); a firm’s production notion that consumers infer quality from the number
costs often increase with the length of their prod- of options offered by a brand is less obvious.
uct line. Furthermore, recent research has shown that We propose that offering greater variety with
more options can generate decision conflict, confu- finer distinctions among items in the product line—
sion, and frustration, leading to choice deferral or chocolates with different cocoa content levels, or
even no choice at all (e.g., Chernev 2003a, 2003b; yogurts representing both standard and more unusual
Dhar 1996, 1997; Greenleaf and Lehmann 1995; Iyen- flavors—is likely to convey category expertise. To be
gar and Lepper 2000). Paradoxically, people choosing precise, a firm that offers finer distinctions within a
from larger variety enjoyed the decision-making pro- product line, as indicated by its wider variety, com-
cess more, but they also felt greater frustration and municates that it has core competency in the cate-
difficulty with choice and were less likely to make a gory (Prahalad and Hamel 1990) and has invested
purchase (Iyengar and Lepper 2000). in learning the category details and dimensions on
In many situations, however, the relevant question which consumers’ tastes vary. Given the investment
is not whether a choice will be made, but which involved in developing such category expertise and
brand consumers will select. When we go to the store the additional costs associated with offering greater
in search of yogurt or need to buy chocolate for a variety, the firm has more to lose if buyers are
friend’s birthday, we have often already decided to subsequently disappointed by actual product qual-
make a choice but may be uncertain about which ity. In addition, consumers likely employ a heuristic
brand to purchase. How would the variety a brand whereby breadth of the product line tends to come at
offers influence which brand consumers choose? The the expense of depth. Therefore, as long as the com-
literature on “too much choice” might indicate that position of the brand’s product line sends a consis-
consumers would avoid high-variety brands in antic- tent message, e.g., variations of gourmet chocolates
ipation of the difficulty of choosing from a large set as opposed to both gourmet chocolates and gourmet
of options. However, typical studies pertaining to the cheeses, we expect greater variety within the specific
role of decision conflict (e.g., Dhar 1997, Tversky and category to convey higher quality and, correspond-
Shafir 1992) and “too much choice” (e.g., Iyengar ingly, affect brand choice.1
and Lepper 2000) have not addressed that question As noted earlier, a positive effect of greater vari-
directly and have focused instead on whether a choice ety on brand choice may simply reflect the fact that
is made. Thus our focus is on a common situation in more options can satisfy more varied tastes. That is,
which the relevant issue is not whether a choice will a fit with preferences account indicates that offering
be made, but which brand will be selected (given that greater variety increases brand choice through sales
the decision to buy has already been made).
In this context, we propose that the variety a brand 1
Although there are many expensive brands that offer few options
offers can act as an important quality cue, affecting and are seen as exclusive and high quality, their perceived quality
the inferences consumers make about the brand and typically derives from their high price and features, rather than the
thus influencing which brand consumers choose. This level of variety offered.
Berger et al.: The Influence of Product Variety on Brand Perception and Choice
462 Marketing Science 26(4), pp. 460–472, © 2007 INFORMS

of unique options, or those that are offered only by Study 1: Product Variety and
the high-variety brand. If, however, consumers tend Brand Choice
to perceive brands that offer greater product vari- Our first study provides an initial test of the impact of
ety more favorably, then they may select from high- product variety on brand choice and perceived brand
variety brands even when choosing shared options, quality. Participants selected a chocolate from one of
or items that are offered by both a high-variety and a two chocolate brands that differed in the total number
low-variety brand. This prediction is expected to hold
of options they offered. They then tasted the selected
when the product variety applies to a compatible set
chocolate and rated the quality of both brands. Impor-
of options that conveys depth in a focused area, such
tantly, a subset of the options (the most popular
as variations of dark chocolate, mountain bicycles, or
chocolates based on a pilot study) were offered by
bird-watching binoculars.2 It is not expected to hold,
both brands (“shared options”). We expected that par-
and might even be reversed, when the broader variety
ticipants would be more likely to select the shared
contains options of different types, such as a product
options from the larger variety brand, even though
assortment that contains both ice skates and running
the same chocolates were offered by the smaller vari-
shoes.
ety brand.
Product perceptions influenced by compatible prod-
uct variety are also expected to influence actual prod- Method
uct experience, particularly when the experience is A table was set up in front of the university book-
ambiguous. Consistent with work regarding influ- store, offering a “Free Chocolate Tasting.” The thirty-
ences on perceived sensory experiences (e.g., Levin three participants were presented with two chocolate
and Gaeth 1988, Nowlis and Shiv 2005), we can expect brands side-by-side and asked to select and taste
that the higher perceived quality of brands offering a chocolate from the display. They were given a
greater variety might lead consumers to believe that
description of each brand and told that “the entire
the high variety brand actually tastes or performs bet-
set of chocolates offered by each brand appears on
ter. If such an effect occurs, it would imply that offer-
the table in front of you.” Brand names (Au Duc
ing high brand variety can have a long-term effect
de Praslin and Arnaud Soubeyran) and descriptions
by generating not only higher trial rates, but also a
were adopted from Chernev (2003a) and were rotated
higher likelihood of repeat purchase.
across participants to control for name/description-
Our predictions regarding the effect of product
specific effects. Because no brand-name effects were
variety on brand perception and choice were tested
found, the data reported below were pooled across
in six studies. The first study focuses on the effect of
brand presentations.
product set size on brand choice, holding the effec-
Chocolates were arranged in rows of five. One
tive choice set constant. The next two studies employ
brand offered 30 chocolates (larger variety) while the
a between-subjects design to test more directly the
other offered ten chocolates (smaller variety). The
effect of product variety on perceived brand quality
smaller variety brand offered the ten most popu-
(Study 2) and taste (Study 3). Study 4 examines our
lar chocolates, whereas the larger variety included
proposed causal path, investigating whether (a) vari-
20 additional, less popular items. Thus, participants
ety’s influence on brand choice is mediated by qual-
ity perceptions and (b) variety’s effect on perceived who selected one of the ten popular chocolates tasted
quality is mediated by the influence of variety on per- the same chocolate regardless of which brand they
ceived category expertise. The last two studies inves- selected it from.
tigate key boundary conditions. Study 5 tests whether Participants were asked to “write down the option
the impact of product variety is still observed when you would be most likely to buy and the name of
the available option set represents only part of the the brand offering this option.” After tasting their
brand’s product line. Study 6 examines the moderat- selected option, they turned over the page and com-
ing role of the option set compatibility. pleted the dependent measures. Specifically, they
were told the experimenter was “interested in your
2
perceptions of both brands of chocolates (not the spe-
Compatibility differs from assortment alignability (Gourville and
Soman 2005) because it focuses on the inferences made from the cific chocolate you tasted).” For each brand, they rated
consistency of the set rather than the difficulty of choice. Alignabil- brand quality (1 = Low Quality, 7 = High Quality), as
ity suggests that consumers may be less likely to choose brands well as likelihood of purchase (“If you were purchas-
whose options vary simultaneously along multiple noncompen- ing chocolates, how likely would you be to buy each
satory dimensions (e.g., a car with a sunroof versus a car with a
of the brands,” 1 = Not Likely, 7 = Very Likely).
leather interior) because it makes choice between the options more
difficult. But regardless of whether the act of choice is easy or dif-
ficult, we suggest that if the offered option set is compatible, con-
Results
sumers may perceive the brand more favorably and be more likely Not surprisingly, participants were more likely to
to choose it relative to other brands. select an option from the brand offering more variety
Berger et al.: The Influence of Product Variety on Brand Perception and Choice
Marketing Science 26(4), pp. 460–472, © 2007 INFORMS 463

(79%) than the brand offering smaller variety (21%), Method


 2 1 N = 33 = 1094, p < 001. The key test, however, The study was again conducted in front of the uni-
focused on the 73% of all participants who chose one versity bookstore. Respondents (N = 50) were invited
of the shared options. As expected, more participants to participate in a “Free Chocolate Tasting” and were
selected shared options from the brand that offered randomly assigned to one of two conditions. In the
larger variety, 71% versus 29%,  2 1 N = 24 = 417, smaller variety condition, the brand offered 10 choco-
p < 004. Finally, quality ratings were higher for the lates arranged on a table in two rows of five, whereas
brand offering greater variety (Mlarger variety = 650 ver- the larger variety group saw 30 chocolates arranged in
sus Msmaller variety = 556, t33 = 301, p < 001), and six rows of five. Each item was labelled (e.g., “English
participants reported being more likely to purchase Walnut Cluster”). In the smaller variety condition,
the larger variety brand (Mlarger variety = 603 versus three groups of ten chocolates were rotated such that
Msmaller variety = 485, t33 = 374, p < 0001). every chocolate in the larger variety also appeared in
one of the smaller variety rotations.
Discussion Participants were informed that researchers were
The results of Study 1 demonstrate the impact of interested in consumer perceptions of the chocolate
product variety on brand choice. Even when focusing brand, and that the set of chocolates on the table
on identical options offered by both brands, partici- represented all the chocolates that a particular brand
pants were more likely to choose a chocolate when offered. They were told that they could taste any
it was offered by the high-variety brand. The results chocolate, after which they were asked to complete a
also provide a preliminary indication that greater short survey regarding their evaluations of the brand.
product variety is associated with higher perceived Participants were first asked to “look at the names
quality. However, this finding of Study 1 might have of the chocolates and the chocolates themselves and
rival explanations. In particular, the measured differ- write down which one you would buy for your-
ences in quality perceptions could have been due to self.” After selecting the chocolate, they were invited
dissonance reduction and preference for consistency: to taste that chocolate and were then asked to turn
participants tended to choose from the brand offer- the page and complete a number of dependent mea-
ing greater variety and justified their selection by rat- sures. The first two measures referred to product qual-
ing their choice as having higher quality. This finding ity, including (a) “quality of this chocolate brand”
might also be explained based on research regarding (1 = Low Quality, 7 = High Quality), and (b) the pos-
focus-of-comparison effects (e.g., Dhar and Simonson itivity of their brand perceptions (1 = Not At All Pos-
1992). Further, though we had no reason to believe itive, 7 = Very Positive). The two brand perceptions
that Study 1 suffered from demand effects, the use measures were highly correlated (r = 072) and were
of a within-subject test does raise that possibility. To averaged to form a brand perception index.
deal with these alternative accounts, Study 2 uses a Next, participants answered questions about the
between-subjects design where participants see only choice process itself (adopted from Iyengar and Lep-
one brand, offering either a smaller or larger set of per 2000). Specifically, participants rated the degree to
options. which the choice process was difficult (“Did you find
it difficult to make your selection of which chocolate
Study 2: Variety, Quality Perceptions, to pick?”) and frustrating (“How frustrated did you
feel when making the choice?”) on 7-point scales (1 =
and Choice Experience Not At All, 7 = Extremely).
As indicated, the proposition that greater product
variety enhances a brand’s choice probability may ap- Results
pear inconsistent with evidence that larger choice sets Consistent with prior research, when the brand
create conflict and frustration and may thus diminish offered more options, choosing an option was
purchase likelihood (e.g., Iyengar and Lepper 2000). rated as more difficult (Mlarger variety = 416 versus
However, we propose that greater choice difficulty Msmaller variety = 308, t48 = 205, p < 005) and more
notwithstanding, in the typical case in which the rele- frustrating (Mlarger variety = 316 versus Msmaller variety =
vant question is which brand will be selected, higher 220, t48 = 218, p < 005). However, as pre-
product variety enhances a brand’s perceived qual- dicted, when the brand offered greater variety it
ity and thus also its purchase likelihood. Accordingly, was perceived as having higher quality chocolates
in Study 2 we simultaneously examine the impact of (Mlarger variety = 572 versus Msmaller variety = 480, t48 =
product variety on the choice experience, perceived 304, p < 005).
quality, and purchase likelihood. That is, Study 2 tests
whether offering more variety can enhance quality Discussion
perceptions even though a larger set makes choosing The finding that the variety a brand offers positively
from the set more difficult and frustrating. influences quality perceptions, even in a case where
Berger et al.: The Influence of Product Variety on Brand Perception and Choice
464 Marketing Science 26(4), pp. 460–472, © 2007 INFORMS

it makes actual choice more difficult, underscores the the two groups were shown either 30 chocolates or
effect of product variety on perceived brand qual- 13 chocolates, but only (the same) ten items were actu-
ity. The results of Study 2 also suggest that quality ally available for choice. Thus the variety the brand
inferences based on variety can occur in situations offered differed between conditions, but the actual
of single-brand evaluation, such as shopping on a options they could select from did not.
brand’s website. Furthermore, increasing the variety
a brand offered enhanced quality evaluations of that Method
brand even after participants were given the oppor- Participants were 90 university staff and students,
tunity to experience actual quality. Thus, the demoti- who were paid $20 for completing a “Chocolate Tast-
vating impact of large choice sets notwithstanding, it ing survey” and additional studies. They were told
seems that offering increased variety can enhance the that the experimenters were “doing a market research
quality perceptions of a brand. study for a brand of chocolates that is consider-
This conclusion, however, does not address the pos- ing expanding into the U.S. market” and they were
sibility that participants perceived the large variety shown a display containing the set of options offered
brand as offering higher quality because they were by that brand. Participants were randomly assigned
more likely to identify a chocolate that matched their to one of two chocolate display conditions. In the
specific taste from the larger set of options. We exam- smaller variety condition, the brand display included
ine this rival account in Study 3 by isolating the effect 13 chocolates, arranged in two rows of five and one
of perceived product variety, such that the effective row of three. In the larger variety condition, the brand
set of options from which participants can make a offered 30 chocolates, arranged in six rows of five
selection is held constant across conditions. In addi- chocolates. Chocolate were labelled, and a sign indi-
tion, Study 3 tests whether greater variety can affect cated that participants could “select from the top 2
not just perceived quality, but also purchase likeli- rows only.” Thus participants in both groups had the
hood and post-consumption perceptions of taste. same effective choice set of ten options.
Participants received the chocolate they selected,
and after eating that chocolate, completed the depen-
Study 3: Product Variety and dent measures. Specifically, they rated the taste of
Sensory Experience their selected option (“How tasty was the chocolate
Brand quality can be a somewhat ambiguous term, you sampled,” 1 = Not at all, 7 = Extremely) and their
whereas the taste of a food product has a more con- purchase likelihood (“How likely would you be to
crete meaning that involves one of the basic senses. purchase this brand,” 1 = Not at all, 7 = Extremely).
It is thus interesting to examine whether greater vari-
ety can also impact perceived taste after consumers Results
get an actual taste of the product. It is also note- As predicted, despite the fact that participants chose
worthy that real-world sensory experiences typically from the same effective set in both conditions, the
involve separate “between-subjects” evaluations. For presented product variety offered by the brand pos-
example, although consumers may consider different itively affected the chocolate’s taste ratings, t88 =
brands at the store or different entrees on a restau- 245, p < 001. Participants rated the tasted choco-
rant menu, they often select just one and do not late as more tasty when the brand offered larger
have the benefit of simultaneously experiencing mul- (M = 628), as opposed to smaller (M = 578) variety.
tiple options. We therefore examine, using a between- Furthermore, consistent with the earlier studies, par-
subjects design, whether selecting an option from a ticipants who tasted one of the available ten items
brand that offers a large product assortment produces indicated a higher purchase likelihood when these
more positive (perceived) sensory experiences. chocolates were part of the larger chocolate assort-
In addition, though we controlled for brand qual- ment (Mlarger variety = 587 versus Msmaller variety = 472,
ity in Study 2 and rotated the actual stimuli used, t88 = 343, p < 001).
one could argue that the observed effect of vari-
ety was due to the greater likelihood that the high Discussion
variety brand offered participants a more preferred The results of Study 3 are noteworthy in two respects.
option. To control for this rival explanation, Study 3 First, the positive effect of the brand’s product variety
used a design that ensured that the available option was observed even though participants chose from
set was identical in the “larger” and “smaller” set. the exact same option set in both the “smaller” and
That is, in a between-subjects design we restricted “larger” variety conditions. Second, when the brand
the options participants could select, such that both offered greater variety, participants found the same
those shown the small set and those exposed to the chocolates to be tastier and indicated a higher likeli-
large set had the same effective choice set. Specifically, hood of purchasing that brand. These results suggest
Berger et al.: The Influence of Product Variety on Brand Perception and Choice
Marketing Science 26(4), pp. 460–472, © 2007 INFORMS 465

that a brand offering a larger variety is more likely to shown “the entire product line offered by an exist-
be sampled and, after actual experience, consumers ing company or a foreign company that is getting
are likely to perceive it as superior even on a concrete ready to enter the U.S. market with one of their
dimension such as taste. product lines.” Participants first received information
The finding that variety affected brand perceptions about the assortment offered by a brand of binocu-
after tasting the chocolate is also important because lars (Bushnell), and after completing the dependent
the impact of quality cues tends to be weaker in measures, were given information about a chocolate
the presence of other cues. For example, the signif- brand (Au Duc de Praslin). In the smaller [larger]
icance of price as a quality cue has been shown to variety condition, the binocular brand offered 4 [16]
be greatly diminished when additional information pairs of binoculars and the chocolate brand offered 10
(e.g., brand name) about the product is available (e.g., [30] chocolates.
Olson 1977). Conversely, the variety cue “withstood” Brand descriptions were adapted from websites of
a source of quality information that is likely to be actual products in the category, and the two condi-
more meaningful than extrinsic cues such as a brand tions differed only in the amount of variety the brand
name—the actual taste of the product. offered. For the binoculars brand, participants read
that “Bushnell offers durable and affordable binocu-
lars, in compact to full-sized, with fully coated optics.
Study 4: Variety, Perceived Expertise, InstaFocus® system for fast focus on moving tar-
and Quality gets. Nonslip rubbergrip pads for secure grip in all
The first three studies provide convergent evidence weather conditions. Available in 4 [16] different mod-
that the product variety offered by a brand can influ- els, it’s easy to see why Bushnell is a good choice.”
ence quality perceptions and brand choice. How- For the chocolate brand, participants read “Created
ever, none of these studies has allowed us to directly in the 17th century, this chocolate confectionary was
investigate the causal path by which these effects named after the Duc De Praslin. Since then Duc De
occur. In particular, our earlier analysis indicates that Praslin chocolates have been a legendary symbol of
(a) variety can impact brand choice through influenc- ‘art de vivre.’ These delectable chocolates are a mix-
ing brand quality perceptions, which is (b) due to ture of distinction, frivolity, and improvisation. Avail-
the effect of variety on perceived category expertise. able in 10 [30] different varieties, these chocolates are
Accordingly, Study 4 tests the effect of product variety the true taste of a historic creative culture” (adapted
in two categories, focusing on the mechanisms under- from Chernev 2003a). Participants were then pre-
lying these effects. sented with an array filled with images of the brand’s
Participants received information about a few product offerings: rows of four options in the binocu-
brands (first binoculars and then chocolates) and were lar category and rows of five options in the chocolate
asked to evaluate each brand on a number of dimen- category.
After reading the description and viewing the as-
sions, including product quality and the brand’s
sortment, participants rated the brand on a number
category expertise. The only difference between con-
of dimensions with question order randomized to
ditions was the amount of variety offered by the
control for order effects. They rated product qual-
brand. They also then chose between this target brand
ity: “the likely quality of the company’s binoculars/
and an alternative brand. We expected that (a) brands
chocolates” (1 = Very Low Quality, 7 = Very High
would be seen as having higher category expertise
Quality). Participants also rated the category expertise
when they offered greater variety, (b) expertise would
of the brand (“How much expertise do you think the
mediate the influence of variety offered on quality
company has in the product category,” “How much
perceptions, and (c) quality perceptions would medi-
knowledge do you think the company has regard-
ate the influence of variety offered on brand choice. To ing the product category,” “How committed do you
test for a possible halo effect, we also included items think the company is to success in the U.S. binoc-
(price and exclusivity) that were not expected to be ulars/chocolates market,” “How committed do you
influenced by product variety. think the company is to the product category,” and
“How invested do you think the company is in the
Method
product category,” all on 7-point scales).3 As expected,
Respondents (N = 76) completed a “Brand Percep- the five items were highly correlated ( > 083) and
tion Study” over the Web in exchange for a $5 Ama- were averaged to form an expertise index.
zon.com gift certificate. They were randomly assigned
to either the smaller or larger variety condition in 3
Perceptions of a brand’s commitment to, investment in, and exper-
each category. They were told that the experimenters tise in the category are closely linked because the development
were “interested in how consumers evaluate com- of expertise requires an investment in and commitment to the
panies and their brands,” and that they would be category.
Berger et al.: The Influence of Product Variety on Brand Perception and Choice
466 Marketing Science 26(4), pp. 460–472, © 2007 INFORMS

As indicated, two additional measures were in- the brand offered greater variety (binoculars, t74 =
cluded to test whether ratings were driven by a halo 256, p < 001; chocolates, t74 = 293, p < 001). Vari-
effect, whereby the larger variety brand would be ety also impacted positively perceived category exper-
rated more favorably on all dimensions, including tise: when the brand offered larger variety, it was
those not implied by our theoretical analysis. Specifi- perceived as having greater expertise (t74 = 434,
cally, although it is conceivable that a brand offering p < 0001 and t74 = 337, p < 0001). Finally, prod-
a greater variety would be assumed to be more exclu- uct variety did not affect either perceived price (p’s >
sive and/or cost more than a brand offering fewer 014) or perceived exclusivity (chocolates, p > 025),4
items, that relation is likely to be weaker. Accordingly, indicating that a halo effect did not play a signifi-
respondents also rated the brand in terms of price cant role.
(“What do you think the price of the brand is relative
to other binoculars/chocolates on the market,” 1 = Mediational Analyses. We next performed two
Much Cheaper, 7 = Much More Expensive) and exclu- mediational analyses (Baron and Kenney 1986) to test
sivity (“How exclusive is the brand relative to the our hypothesized causal path. Specifically, we tested
typical U.S. binocular/chocolate brand,” 1 = Much (a) whether quality perceptions mediated the influ-
Less Exclusive, 7 = Much More Exclusive). Finally, in ence of variety on brand choice, and (b) whether per-
each category, respondents were told to imagine that ceived brand category expertise mediated the impact
they were looking to buy a certain type of binoc- of variety on quality perceptions.
ulars/chocolate that was offered by both the tar- In our first set of analyses, all four conditions for
get brand (Bushnell/Au Duc de Praslin) and another mediation were met in both categories, suggesting
brand (Minolta/See’s), and asked which brand they that quality perceptions fully mediated the influence
would choose. of product variety on brand choice (see Figure 1).
Variety offered was correlated with brand choice
Results (binoculars  = 019, p < 010 and chocolates,  = 024,
p < 005, Step 1) and was also correlated with quality
Assortment Effects on Choice, Quality Percep- perceptions ( = 029 and  = 032, ps < 001). How-
tions, and Category Expertise. Table 1 summarizes ever, when both variety offered and quality percep-
the results. As predicted, the product variety offered tions were simultaneously included in a regression
influenced brand choice; participants were more predicting brand choice, quality perceptions was a
likely to choose the target brand over the alterna- significant predictor ( = 043 and  = 025, ps < 005)
tive brand when the former offered greater variety. but variety offered was not ( = 007 and  = 016,
Specifically, 65% of the respondents indicated they
ns). Finally Sobel tests (Sobel 1982) revealed signif-
would purchase the target binocular brand when
icant effects for both categories (binoculars z = 268
it offered larger variety, compared to 46% when it
ps < 001; chocolates z = 439, ps < 001), indicating
offered smaller variety,  2 1 N = 76 = 269, p < 010.
that the effect of variety offered on brand choice was
Similarly, 72% chose the target chocolate brand when
fully mediated by quality perceptions.
it offered larger variety, compared to 49% who chose
All four requirements were also met in our second
it when it offered smaller variety,  2 1 N = 76 = 426,
set of analyses, indicating that perceived expertise
p < 004.
did, in fact, fully mediate the relationship between
As expected, product variety influenced quality per-
variety and quality perceptions (see Figure 1). Vari-
ceptions and perceived category expertise, whereas it
ety offered was correlated with quality perceptions
did not influence price or exclusivity. In both cate-
(binoculars,  = 029, and chocolates,  = 032, ps <
gories, brand quality perceptions were higher when
001) and was also correlated with perceived exper-
tise ( = 045 and  = 037, ps < 0001). However,
Table 1 Study 4: Influence of Variety Offered on Choice Share, when both variety offered and perceived expertise
Perceptions of Quality, and Category Expertise were simultaneously included in a regression pre-
Choice share dicting quality perceptions, perceived expertise was
(%) Quality perceptions Category expertise a significant predictor ( = 077 and  = 061, ps <
Binoculars
0001), whereas product variety was not ( = 008 and
Larger variety 65 5.62 (0.98) 6.09 (0.78)  = 010, ns). Both Sobel tests also revealed significant
Smaller variety 46 5.00 (1.12) 5.16 (1.04) effects (z’s > 297, p’s < 001), indicating that the effect
Chocolates
Larger variety 72 5.85 (0.81) 5.90 (0.62) 4
Smaller variety 49 5.08 (1.40) 5.26 (1.00) Variety did have a marginally significant effect on perceived exclu-
sivity in the binocular category, t74 = 183, p = 007, but the effect
Note. Cell values reflect choice share and means (standard deviations) on the was actually in the opposite direction; perceived exclusivity was
other dependent variables. higher in the low variety condition (M = 481 versus 4.31).
Berger et al.: The Influence of Product Variety on Brand Perception and Choice
Marketing Science 26(4), pp. 460–472, © 2007 INFORMS 467

Figure 1 Study 4: Mediational Analyses

Bin: 0.77***
Category Choc: 0.61*** Quality
expertise perceptions

Bin: 0.45*** Bin: 0.29**/0.08 Bin: 0.43***


Choc: 0.37*** Choc: 0.32**/0.10 Choc: 0.25*

Variety Brand
offered choice
0.19*/0.07
Binoculars
Chocolates 0.24*/0.16
Notes. The first coefficient on a given path represents the direct effect without the mediator in the model. The second coefficient represents the direct effect
when the mediator is included in the model. ∗ p < 005, ∗∗ p < 001, ∗∗∗ p < 0001, one-tailed.

of variety offered on quality perceptions was signifi- should be harder for consumers to infer brand quality
cantly decreased by the introduction of the mediator from the variety offered, and the impact of variety on
(i.e., expertise). brand choice share should be reduced or eliminated.
Study 5 examines this prediction.
Discussion Similar to Study 1, participants were shown choco-
Study 4 again demonstrated that offering greater lates sold by two chocolate brands, offering smaller
variety positively influences brand choice share and or larger variety, and asked to select a chocolate from
perceived brand quality. There was also no corre- one of the brands. We also manipulated the abil-
sponding effect of variety on measures we did not ity to infer brand quality differences from variety:
expect it to influence: price and exclusivity. Further- half the participants were informed that the choco-
more, the results support our analysis regarding the lates displayed were the complete product assort-
mechanism underlying these effects, showing that the ment each brand offered (“complete” assortment
impact of brand variety on choice is mediated by condition), while the other half were told the dis-
quality perceptions, and that the impact of variety on played products were the chocolates that were avail-
quality perceptions is mediated by brand expertise. able at the time (“partial” assortment condition). We
expected that the ability to infer quality differences
Study 5: The Moderating Role of from variety would be greater in the former condi-
the Ability to Infer Quality tion, leading to stronger effect of assortment size on
Though Studies 1–4 demonstrate a robust positive brand choice.
effect of product variety on brand choice and per-
ceived quality, one would not expect this effect to Method
hold under all conditions, and it might even reverse in Respondents (N = 70) were paid three dollars to par-
some situations. Thus Studies 5 and 6 examine some ticipate in a choice study. They sat next to a round
important boundary conditions for this effect. table on which the product offerings of two chocolate
The proposition that variety influences choice brands were displayed. Brand names, descriptions,
through its impact on perceived quality implies that and counterbalancing were identical to those used in
the ability to infer quality differences from the offered Study 1.
variety is essential for the variety-brand quality and Participants were randomly assigned to condition.
hence variety-brand choice link. The distinction can In the complete assortment [partial assortment] con-
be made between produced variety, i.e., the set of dition they were told, “All [Some] of the chocolates
options produced by the brand, and offered variety, offered by each brand appear on the table in front
or the particular set of produced options that happens of you. For each brand, the selection provided is the
to be available in a given shopping context. In the first full range [some] of chocolates they offer, that is, all
four studies, participants were told that all the options the different options they make [the particular options
made by the brand were in front of them, and thus shown are those that happened to be available in the box
offered variety equaled produced variety and partici- size we could purchase]” (italics added).
pants could form inferences about brand quality from They were then asked to “write down which choco-
the variety offered. In contrast, if the offered variety late you would buy for yourself.” After tasting the
is less informative about produced variety, then it selected chocolate, they turned the page and rated
Berger et al.: The Influence of Product Variety on Brand Perception and Choice
468 Marketing Science 26(4), pp. 460–472, © 2007 INFORMS

the quality of both brands (1 = Low Quality, 9 = High Figure 2 Study 4: Influence of Perceived Assortment on Brand Choice
Quality). 100
After completing the study, participants were
Smaller variety
thanked for their participation and offered the follow-

Brand choice share (%)


Larger variety
ing gift options: “As a compensation for participating 75

in the study, you can choose to receive either three


dollars or a box containing four preselected chocolates 50
from Au Duc de Praslin or a box containing four pre-
selected chocolates from Arnaud Soubeyran. Which
would you prefer?” Boxes of chocolates were visible 25
behind the chocolate display for each brand, and par-
ticipants indicated their decision.
0
Complete assortment Partial assortment
Results
Assortment Effects on Quality Perceptions and As predicted, the influence of perceived assortment
Choice. We expected that participants would per- on brand choice was fully mediated by difference
ceive smaller quality differences between the high and in quality perceptions between the brands. Perceived
low variety brands in the partial than in the com- assortment was correlated with brand choice ( =
plete assortment condition. A 2 (Perceived Assort- 026, p < 003) and was also correlated with differ-
ment: complete versus partial) × 2 (Variety Offered: ence in quality perceptions between the brands ( =
Smaller versus Larger) repeated measures ANOVA 028, p < 002). However, when both perceived assort-
was performed on brand quality perceptions. Consis- ment and difference in quality perceptions between
tent with the earlier studies, there was a main effect of the brands were simultaneously included in a regres-
Variety Offered (F 1 67 = 544, p < 005), indicating sion predicting brand choice, difference in quality per-
that participants rated the chocolate brand offering ceptions was a significant predictor ( = 040, p <
greater variety as having higher quality (Mlarger variety = 0001), whereas perceived assortment was not ( =
696 versus Msmaller variety = 648). This effect, however, 015, p > 020). The Sobel test also revealed a signifi-
was qualified by a significant Perceived Assortment × cant effect, z = 199, p = 005, indicating that the effect
Variety Offered interaction (F 1 67 = 614, p < 005). of perceived assortment on brand choice was fully
As expected, participants perceived a significant qual- mediated by the difference in quality perceptions.
ity difference between the brands in the “com-
plete” assortment condition (Mlarger variety = 711 versus Actual Purchase. We also tested the effect of
Msmaller variety = 611 F 1 67 = 1258, p < 0001) but not brand variety in the partial and complete conditions.
in the “partial” assortment condition, (Mlarger variety = Because most participants (76%) preferred the three
684 versus Msmaller variety = 684, F < 1). dollars to chocolates, the effective sample sizes were
We next examined whether the reduced ability to small. However, selections of those who did choose
infer quality differences affected brand choice (Fig- chocolates paralleled the above brand choice results.
ure 2). As expected, participants were more likely to That is, participants who selected chocolates over
select the brand that offered more options ( 2 1 N = money tended to purchase chocolates from the brand
70 = 1463, p < 001), but this effect was moderated offering greater variety ( 2 1 N = 14 = 457, p <
by perceived assortment ( 2 1 N = 70 = 450, p < 005). All of the chocolate purchasers in the complete
005). Specifically, although participants in the com- assortment condition chose the brand offering greater
plete assortment condition were significantly more variety, compared to only 57% of those in the partial
likely to select an option from the brand that offered assortment condition ( 2 1 N = 14 = 382, p = 005).
greater variety (84%) than the brand that offered less
Discussion
variety (26%),  2 1 N = 37 = 1689, p < 001, the
Consistent with our prediction that the effect of vari-
effect was less pronounced and not statistically signif-
ety on brand choice is in part driven by quality
icant in the partial assortment condition (61% versus
inferences, the results of Study 5 demonstrate the
39%,  2 1 N = 33 = 148, p > 02).
moderating impact of the ability to infer quality dif-
Mediational Analysis. Participants were more ferences on the variety-brand choice link. The product
likely to choose the greater variety brand in the variety offered only influenced which brand partici-
complete assortment condition, but we also exam- pants chose when they could use it to infer produced
ine whether, as we hypothesized, this differences was variety and thus form brand quality inferences. Fur-
driven by changes in the difference between quality thermore, difference in quality perceptions between
perceptions of the two brands. the brands fully mediated the relationship between
Berger et al.: The Influence of Product Variety on Brand Perception and Choice
Marketing Science 26(4), pp. 460–472, © 2007 INFORMS 469

perceived assortment and brand choice. Finally, the The brands differed in both the number of options
results suggest that inferences based on the product they offered and whether the variety was compat-
variety offered by a brand can affect actual purchase ible. Specifically, one brand offered two road bikes
behavior with real consequences. (made with different frame materials); a second brand
offered the same two road bikes as well as five addi-
tional road bikes (compatible high variety); a third
Study 6: The Moderating Role of brand (incompatible high variety) offered the same
Option Set Compatibility two road bikes plus five bikes for different use types,
Study 5 identified one boundary condition regarding including mountain bikes and city cruisers. Respon-
dents were then asked to guess how the magazine
the positive effect of product variety on brand per-
rated the quality of each brand (1 = Low Quality,
ception, indicating that this effect applies only when
7 = High Quality). On the next page, they also rated
the observed set of options is informative with respect
each brand on the same expertise items used in Study
to the entire variety produced by the brand. Study 6 4 ( = 088, averaged to form an index).
examines a second boundary condition that relates
to the composition of the product variety. Specifi- Results
cally, offering a greater variety of compatible options Responses were analyzed using a 3 (Variety Offered:
that reflect a depth of expertise in a particular area Low versus High Compatible versus High Incom-
(e.g., dark chocolate, mountain bicycles) is expected patible) repeated measures ANOVA. We first exam-
to have a positive effect on perceived quality. Con- ined responses on the expertise measure. As expected,
versely, offering a wide variety of what might be the variety offered influenced perceptions of exper-
seen as incompatible options, such as both Japanese tise (F 2 20 = 1298, p < 0001); however, relative to
and Chinese food, may signal lack of focus and raise the low variety condition M = 462 offering greater
doubts about the quality of the brand’s product line. variety increased perceptions of expertise only when
That is, the basic notion of specialization and exper- it was compatible (M = 615 F 1 21 = 1098, p <
tise implies doing a few things very well, without try- 0005). When the variety was incompatible, it slightly
decreased brand perceptions (M = 429 F < 05; p >
ing to cover the entire spectrum. Although a company
02).
may excel in a wide range of related product types—
Offering increased variety only increased percep-
road and mountain bikes, Japanese and Chinese food,
tions of expertise when it was compatible, so we
or ice skates and running shoes—consumers are likely expect offering more options should only increase
to employ a heuristic whereby breadth tends to come quality perceptions under those conditions. Consis-
at the expense of depth, especially when there is lim- tent with this prediction, there was an effect of
ited information about quality. product variety on quality perceptions (F 2 20 =
To test whether the positive effect of product vari- 1578, p < 0001), but greater variety only enhanced
ety on perceived brand quality is eliminated when the quality perceptions when the set of options was
variety is less focused, we gave participants informa- focused (M = 627 versus 4.82, F 1 21 = 1331,
tion about the product offerings of different bicycle p < 0005). When the options offered were unfo-
brands and asked them to rate each brand’s quality cused, offering more variety slightly decreased qual-
and category expertise. The brands differed in both ity perceptions (M = 405 versus 4.82, F 1 21 = 321,
the number of options they offered and whether the p = 009).
variety was compatible. We expect that greater prod-
uct variety will enhance perceived quality and exper- Discussion
Results of Study 6 demonstrate an important bound-
tise only when the set of options offered by the brand
ary condition: The positive effect of greater product
is compatible.
variety is observed only when the options are com-
patible, representing category expertise. A follow-up
Method study extended these findings using a between-
Sixty-four consumers, who are members of a Web- subject design. Participants in that study rated the
based subject pool, completed a “Brand Perception quality of a restaurant that offered either low vari-
Study” in exchange for a chance to win a $20 online ety (i.e., a few Thai food options), greater compatible
gift certificate. They were given information about variety (the same options plus five other Thai food
different bicycle brands and rated each one. Respon- options), or greater incompatible variety (i.e., a few
dents were told that a number of bike brands had Thai options plus five non-Thai options, such as egg
recently been rated by a cycling magazine and that rolls). Replicating the results of Study 6, participants
the experimenters were interested in whether they perceived the brand more favorably (both in terms of
could predict the overall quality rating given to these expertise and quality) only when the options included
brands. in the expanded set were compatible.
Berger et al.: The Influence of Product Variety on Brand Perception and Choice
470 Marketing Science 26(4), pp. 460–472, © 2007 INFORMS

General Discussion particularly high cocoa content chocolate, are likely to


The number of product variants offered by a firm in have greater impact on perceived quality than more
a category is a key marketing mix variable. Recent mundane product variants. Even if few consumers
research has provided evidence for both the bene- actually choose such items, their presence can create
fits and downsides of offering greater product vari- a certain aura and enhance the brand’s perceived cat-
ety. On the one hand, more options allow the firm egory expertise (also see Chernev 2005; Gourville and
to offer a better match to varied customer tastes and Soman 2005 for ways in which assortment structure
segments. However, offering more options can also can influence choice).
be frustrating and demotivating (e.g., Chernev 2003a, Although we have focused on the impact of the
2003b; Iyengar and Lepper 2000), leading to deci- number of options, the structure and presentation
sion deferral. The present research suggests a separate of the offered product assortment can also affect
route through which the variety offered may influence the brand’s perceived quality by influencing the per-
which brand consumers choose: brand quality infer- ceived product variety (Broniarczyk et al. 1998, Hoch
ences. The present research is particularly relevant et al. 1999, Kahn and Wansink 2004). Research sug-
to common situations in which the relevant question gests, for example, that large varieties may be harder
to see when they are disorganized, whereas disor-
is which brand a consumer will choose, rather than
ganization may benefit smaller assortments because
whether a choice from a given set of options will be
it obscures the fact that few options are available
made. In this section, we review the key findings and
(Kahn and Wansink 2004). Consequently, brands that
discuss their theoretical and practical implications.
offer large assortments might be perceived as higher
Summary of Findings quality when their assortments are organized, but
the opposite might occur for brands that offer fewer
The present research demonstrates a robust effect
options.
of compatible product variety on brand quality per-
Future research might investigate additional ways
ceptions and brand choice, even when the effective
in which the variety of options from which an indi-
option set is held constant. The evidence indicates
vidual makes a selection affects the perception of the
that product variety influences perceived brand qual-
chosen option. In particular, in addition to influencing
ity both when evaluating a single brand (Studies 2–4)
perceived quality, the variety offered might affect the
and when choosing between brands (Study 1 and 5).
perceived fit between the option consumers select and
This effect is observed despite the fact that variety their tastes. Specifically, holding the “true” fit between
makes the act of choice itself more difficult and frus- an option and an individual’s (often malleable) tastes
trating (Study 2). In addition, the effect of product constant, the mere fact that an option was selected
variety on perceived quality and on a concrete dimen- from a large set of varied options may enhance
sion such as taste persists even after consumers expe- the perceived match between that option and the
rience the product (Studies 1–3 and 5), suggesting that consumer’s preferences. For example, a strawberry-
product variety can also enhance repeat purchase rate. banana yogurt might be seen as providing a better
The influence of product variety on brand choice mir- fit to one’s ideal yogurt preference when that option
rors its effect on quality perceptions, which, in turn, is is selected from a set of 60 yogurts than when it is
mediated by the effect of variety on perceived brand selected from a set of just three yogurts.
category expertise (Study 4). Future research might also examine the implica-
We identified two boundary conditions regarding tions of the present findings for recent research that
the effects of product variety. First, consistent with has emphasized the disadvantages of offering large
our analysis, the positive effects of variety occur only choice sets. This issue can be viewed as reflecting pro-
when the observed set of options is informative with cesses that arise at different stages in the decision
respect to the entire variety offered by the brand process (e.g., Kahn and Lehmann 1991, Sood et al.
(Study 5). Second, the composition of the product 2004). Consumers often decide both which brand,
variety, in particular, the degree to which it is seen as or assortment, to choose as well as which option
compatible and reflecting specialization, is a key mod- to select from that brand, and these decisions can
erator of the impact of variety on perceived quality be made sequentially or simultaneously.5 Too many
(Study 6). In fact, unfocused variety may sometimes
backfire and negatively affect perceptions of expertise 5
Of course, one can decompose the decision process based on other
in a well-defined product category. dimensions as well. For example, consumers may first select a
subtype (e.g., plasma HDTV) and then make a specific choice based
Theoretical and Practical Implications on other attributes. However, in the context of this and many other
consumer studies, the brand cue is particularly important, because
Other aspects of the composition of the set of options it is reasonable to make inferences about a brand based on its offer-
a brand offers might also moderate the effect of prod- ings, and it thus provides an effective way to decompose the deci-
uct variety. For example, unique, exotic flavors, or a sion process.
Berger et al.: The Influence of Product Variety on Brand Perception and Choice
Marketing Science 26(4), pp. 460–472, © 2007 INFORMS 471

options can lead to choice deferral at either stage of size of a brand’s assortment can be a potent quality
the process, and although the variety offered may cue. Thus, marketers will be well advised to consider
make the choice from a given assortment more dif- and try to measure both the direct and indirect con-
ficult, our findings suggest it can also help simplify tribution of individual products to consumers’ per-
the choice between assortments or brands (see also ception of the product line and likelihood of brand
Chernev 2006). Especially in categories where con- choice.
sumers have little prior knowledge, they may use
the variety brands offer as a heuristic to resolve
brand choice. Thus, although larger product variety References
may enhance choice difficulty, the reliance on sim- Allison, R. I., K. P. Uhl. 1964. Influence of beer brand identification
ple heuristics, such as “choose the brand offering the on taste perception. J. Marketing Res. 1(3) 36–39.
greatest variety of options,” is likely to increase as Baron, R. M., D. A. Kenny. 1986. The moderator-mediator variable
distinction in social psychological research.: Conceptual, strate-
well, consistent with the concept of effort-accuracy gic, and statistical considerations. J. Personality Soc. Psych. 51(6)
tradeoffs (e.g., Bettman et al. 1998). Consequently, a 1173–1182.
brand’s decision to offer greater product variety may Bettman, J. R., M. F. Luce, J. W. Payne. 1998. Constructive consumer
have the dual impact of both increasing the overall choice processes. J. Consumer Res. 25(3) 187–217.
effort necessary to choose and providing a heuristic Borle, S., P. Boatwright, J. B. Kadane, J. C. Nunes, G. Shmueli. 2005.
cue that simplifies at least the choice between brands. The effect of product assortment changes on customer reten-
tion. Marketing Sci. 24(4) 616–622.
Though there are many cases in which consumers
Broniarczyk, S. M., W. D. Hoyer, L. McAlister. 1998. Consumers’
are unwilling to defer choice, for the cases in which perceptions of the assortment offered in a grocery category:
deferral is an option, it would be interesting to exam- The impact of item reduction. J. Marketing Res. 35(May)
ine the conditions under which offering more variety 166–176.
has an overall positive effect. The present research Chernev, A. 2003a. Product assortment and individual decision pro-
cesses. J. Personality Soc. Psych. 85(1) 151–162.
did not force consumers to make a choice, but it also
Chernev, A. 2003b. When more is less and less is more: The role
did not explicitly provide the option to defer choice. of ideal point availability and assortment in consumer choice.
Future research might examine how the variety a J. Consumer Res. 30(September) 170–183.
brand offers influences the overall choice likelihood. Chernev, A. 2005. Feature complementarity and assortment in
One might expect that offering variety may be like a choice. J. Consumer Res. 31(March) 748–759.
common dilemma, where it is better for each individ- Chernev, A. 2006. Decision focus and consumer choice among
ual brand to offer more options, but each additional assortments. J. Consumer Res. 33(June) 50–59.
offering hurts overall customer satisfaction with their Dhar, R. 1996. The effect of decision strategy on the decision to
defer choice. J. Behav. Decision Making 9(December) 265–281.
choice from the category and increases the likelihood
Dhar, R. 1997. Consumer preference for a no-choice option. J. Con-
of choice deferral. sumer Res. 24(September) 215–231.
Though prior research has discussed how supply Dhar, R., I. Simonson. 1992. The effect of the focus of comparison on
and demand considerations influence the optimal consumer preferences. J. Marketing Res. 29(November) 430–440.
product line breadth (e.g., Draganska and Jain 2005, Draganska, M., D. Jain. 2005. Product line length as a competitive
Kekre and Srinivasan 1990), our research suggests tool. J. Econom. Management Strategy 14(1) 1–28.
marketers should also consider the potential benefits Gourville, J., D. Soman. 2005. Overchoice and assortment type:
When and why variety backfires. Marketing Sci. 24(3) 382–395.
of greater variety with respect to consumers’ brand
Greenleaf, E. A., D. R. Lehmann. 1995. Reasons for substantial delay
perceptions. A typical line extension decision involves in consumer decision making. J. Consumer Res. 22(September)
estimating market share gained from serving more 186–199.
consumers and comparing it to the production costs Hoch, S. J., E. T. Bradlow, B. Wansink. 1999. The variety of an assort-
(e.g., product design, development, and manufactur- ment. Marketing Sci. 18(4) 527–546.
ing) as well as possible cannibalization of existing Iyengar, S. S., M. R. Lepper. 2000. When choice is demotivating:
sales. Such a decision is usually focused on the partic- Can one desire too much of a good thing? J. Personality Soc.
Psych. 79 995–1006.
ular option to be added: Does the market share gained
Kahn, B., D. R. Lehmann. 1991. Modeling choice among assort-
from offering that option outweigh the costs of pro- ments. J. Retailing 67(Fall) 274–299.
ducing it? Our findings, however, suggest that it is Kahn, B., B. Wansink. 2004. The influence of assortment structure
also important to consider the broader impact of offer- on perceived variety and consumption quantities. J. Consumer
ing greater product variety; offering more options can Res. 30(March) 519–533.
enhance the perceived quality of the entire line and Kassarjian, H. 1978. Presidential address 1977: Anthropomorphism
and parsimony. Adv. Consumer Res. Association for Consumer
the brand, more generally.
Research.
In summary, the present research adds to the exist-
Kekre, S., K. Srinivasan. 1990. Broader product line: A necessity to
ing evidence regarding the impact of product assort- achieve success? Management Sci. 36 1216–1231.
ments on consumer preferences (e.g., Simonson 1999), Klein, B., K. B. Leffler. 1981. The role of market forces in assuring
but going beyond prior research, it indicates that the contractual performance. J. Political Econom. 89(Fall) 615–639.
Berger et al.: The Influence of Product Variety on Brand Perception and Choice
472 Marketing Science 26(4), pp. 460–472, © 2007 INFORMS

Lancaster, K. 1979. Variety, Equity, and Efficiency. Columbia Univer- Prahalad, C. K., G. Hamel. 1990. The core competence of the cor-
sity Press, New York. poration. Harvard Bus. Rev. 90(May–June) 79–91.
Lancaster, K. 1990. The economics of product variety: A survey. Purohit, D., J. Srivastava. 2001. Effect of manufacturer reputation,
Marketing Sci. 9 189–206. retailer reputation, and product warranty on consumer judg-
Levin, I. P., G. J. Gaeth. 1988. How consumers are affected by the ments of product quality. J. Consumer Psych. 10(3) 123–134.
framing of attribute information before and after consuming Schwartz, B. 2004. The Paradox of Choice. Why More is Less. Harper
the product. J. Consumer Res. 15(3) 374–378. Collins, New York.
Mayzlin, D. 2006. Promotional chat on the internet. Marketing Sci. Schwarz, N., G. L. Clore. 1983. Mood, misattribution, and judg-
25(2) 155–163.
ments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of
Nelson, P. 1974. Advertising as information. J. Political Econom. 81 affective states. J. Personality Soc. Psych. 45 513–523.
729–754.
Simonson, I. 1999. The effect of product assortment on buyer pref-
Nowlis, S., B. Shiv. 2005. Influence of consumer distractions on erences. J. Retailing 75 347–370.
the effectiveness of food sampling programs. J. Marketing Res.
42(May) 157–168. Sobel, M. E. 1982. Asymptotic intervals for indirect effects in struc-
tural equations models. S. Leinhart, ed. Sociological Methodol-
Nowlis, S., I. Simonson. 1997. Attribute–task compatibility as a
ogy. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco, 290–312.
determinant of consumer preference reversals. J. Marketing Res.
34(May) 205–218. Sood, S., Y. Rottenstreich, L. Brenner. 2004. On decisions that lead
Olson, J. C. 1977. Price as an informational cue: Effects on product to decisions: Direct and derived evaluations of preference. J.
evaluations. A. G. Woodside, J. N. Sheth, P. D. Bennet, eds. Con- Consumer Res. 31(June) 17–25.
sumer and Industrial Buying Behavior. Elsevier, North-Holland, Tversky, A., E. Shafir. 1992. Choice under conflict: The dynamics of
New York, 267–286. deferred decision. Psych. Sci. 3 358–361.

Вам также может понравиться