Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 6

Social Development

Although not every student with a learning disability has problems with parents,
teachers, or peers, social problems nevertheless are more prevalent among this group
than among students having no academic difficulties. Intervention requires attending to
the environmental as well as student aspects of the problem because social difficulties
never exist in a vacuum.
Normal Patterns of Prosocial Development
In order for children to behave in a socially acceptable manner, they must be able to
judge what is right and wrong. As with other areas of development, moral judgments
change with age.
Piaget noted that children less than age 7 or 8 often don't understand why, something is
right or wrong; rather, they accept an external source such as a parent or older child as
an authority. Thus they tend to be very rigid about interpreting rules. If David takes
April's toy, he is wrong because adults say so. The fact that he is taking the toy out of
retaliation for April's taking his toy isn't considered. Likewise a lie is a lie, whether or not
it was told in an attempt to spare someone's feelings. Children at this age are concrete
in judging the magnitude of a misdeed. If you accidentally drop a tray of six glasses and
they break this is deserving of a more severe punishment than if you dropped and broke
just one glass. And children base their judgments of a lie on how far it departs from the
truth. It's worse to say you saw a cow in the street when it really was a dog, than
pretending to have better grades than you do in school.
In children over age 8, Piaget noted a gradual shift toward conforming to the behavioral
rules of the group and greater self-reliance on following these rules. The parent is no
longer considered the final authority. Motives are sometimes considered, and
sometimes not.
By age 11 or 12, the child begins to understand that rules come from mutual consent
and that rules make life easier for people. Lying begins to be regarded as wrong
because if everyone told lies there could be little trusthow could families classrooms,
and the world operate if people couldn't be believed? The 12-year-old connects the
magnitude of punishments to the perpetrator's motives and justification. In late
adolescence, young people begin to fully understand that rules preserve society and
that we are obliged to follow them for that reason. From Kohlberg's research with adults,
we have learned that a small percentage eventually come to recognize that not all laws
are just, and will act to change these laws to better society.
This developmental progression in moral reasoning is extremely important for teachers
and parents to understand. An understanding of a child's level of moral reasoning helps
us deal with behavioral infractions "at their level" and encourages growth in their social
maturity.
Prosocial Development and the Student with Learning Disabilities
Just as students with LD are academically delayed, their ability to make social
judgments also may be like that of younger children. A 9-year-old, for example, may still
rely heavily on authority figures for cues about how to act. This makes it almost
impossible to function in school where numerous situations arise that demand
immediate decisions and actions, on many of which the child's parents or teachers have
never made a ruling.
Consider the dilemma of Oliver, a 10-year-old impulsive child who must decide whether
telling a lie to avoid hurting someone is more correct than telling the truth. He has
trouble focusing on several aspects of the problem at once and delaying a response
until alternatives and consequences have been considered. Among other things, Oliver
must look at the situation from another's perspective, evaluate verbal and nonverbal
cues, and choose which words to use, all skills with which students who are learning
disabled have trouble. That Oliver might make the wrong decision and get negative
feedback is understandable.
Teachers need to understand the students' stage of moral reasoning and help them
progress to higher moral reasoning levels. Their responses to misbehavior must fit the
children's moral reasoning levels, as in simply "stating the rule" to Eron who is too
aggressive in his bear hugs, or to Shira who overstays her welcome with friends
because of her excessive teasing and roughhousing. Both children are old enough to
know better, but reasoning with them about the other children's perspective would not
have worked. For young children, behavior modification techniques and simply stating
what is right and wrong work well. These children choose their behaviors based on
adults' rules and stated consequences. Older elementary school children, very aware of
social obligations to their groups, benefit from explicit reminders about these
expectations and from instruction to model their peers' behavior. Older youngsters, who
recognize the benefits of society's laws, may respond to negotiated agreements, such
as not talking out in class, or setting goals for grades and contracting for positive
consequences (e.g., more free time) if they live up to their end of the agreement.
Parents are powerful influences on their children's moral reasoning and social
development. Research has found that those children who are more competent socially
with their peers are the ones who experience more attachment and emotional security
in their homes, and less family dysfunction. Children learn from their parents through
identification (wanting to be like them), modeling (imitating them), and direct training
through parental teaching and discipline.
Identification and Modeling
Parents are high-status people in their children's lives, and children learn by simply
observing what their parents do. If a father is always willing to lend a helping hand in the
neighborhood, his son sees this model and, wanting to be like dad, is helpful to younger
children. A daughter may be tempted to lie, but resists because she knows her parents
would be disappointed in her if she did so. These processes of identification and
modeling are fostered by parents' warmth and support, which in turn encourage
prosocial traits in their children such as kindness, honesty, generosity, obedience to the
rules, resistance to cheating and lying, and consideration of the rights and welfare of
others.
Bandura's classic modeling studies showed that parents need to do more than model
good behaviors. They must first of all begin with positive attitudes toward their children,
and then draw the children's attention to the modeled behavior, help them remember
through rehearsal, encourage them to practice the behavior, and give incentives for
adopting the behavior. Most parents do this unconsciously and inconspicuously. But
many children with LD don't pick up on these messages. Therefore, parents have to be
far more intentional about these teachings. To complicate matters, their children often
don't understand the emotions the model is expressing and have difficulty attending to,
interpreting, and retaining the modeled behavior and verbal explanations.
Consequently, parents may need to explain more, highlight important aspects, help the
child verbally rehearse and role-play, and provide support when information-processing
or motivational problems present obstacles.
Unfortunately, many children with LD come from homes where warm and supportive
modeling of prosocial behaviors is in short supply. Their family systems can be more
chaotic, disturbed, less structured, less emotionally stable, less cohesive, more rigid,
and more disengaged than other families. Parents often react more negatively to their
children with learning disabilities, are more harsh and remote, communicate lower
expectations, and provide less affection and care giving. More affection is shown to the
sibling who has higher verbal ability, perseveres in school tasks, and is less worrisome.
Parents may view the successes of the child with LD less positively (caused by luck
rather than ability) and the failures more negatively (caused by inability rather than bad
luck) than do parents of nondisabled children. Parents frequently expect less of their
children with LD than the children expect of themselves.
An additional difficulty for children with LD is that some parents may themselves be poor
models. Many evaluate their own social skills as poorly as they do their children's.
Because learning disorders often show a hereditary pattern, children and parents can
share similar cognitive problems and inappropriate behavioral styles. Therefore, parents
may need to evaluate and modify their own behavior before being able to be of
maximum help to their children.
Direct Training
Direct training, through teaching and discipline, is another way in which parents can
influence their children's attitudes and behaviors. Clear expectations offer a consistent
family structure that has a stabilizing influence on children's development. Threats of
punishment or power plays are less effective than explanations about why certain
behaviors are better than others, and positively reinforcing these behaviors.
Baumrind's classic research on child-rearing practices among middle-income parents
identified three general patterns: authoritative, authoritarian, and
permissive. Authoritative parentswere controlling and demanding. They had high
demands for obedience, academic achievement, and sharing household tasks. At the
same time, they were warm, open to discussion and negotiation with their children
about why they didn't want to comply, and responded positively to their children's
independent behavior. Authoritarian parents were more punitive and rejecting. They,
too, had high standards for behavior, but these were absolute. Power, unaccompanied
by reasoning and communication, was used to compel compliance. They valued
obedience for its own sake and there was little give-and-take with their children. By
contrast, permissive parents were very accepting of all their children's impulses and
didn't enforce rules or standards for behavior and achievement.
Authoritative parents tended to produce children who were socially responsible,
independent, friendly to peers, cooperative, achievement-oriented, dominant, and
purposeful. In contrast, children of authoritarian parents tended to be discontent,
withdrawn, and distrustful. Permissive parents raised children who were the least self-
reliant, self-controlled, and explorative.
Baumrind identified several parent practices and attitudes that help develop children
whose behavior is socially responsible, assertive, cooperative, purposeful. confident,
altruistic, creative, cognitively challenging, and independent:
1. Modeling socially responsible and self-assertive behavior.
2. Firmly enforcing policies that reward socially responsible behavior and punish
deviant behavior, and that are accompanied by explanations consistent with the
parents' principles.
3. Accepting, but not overprotective or passive, parental attitudes; approval is
contingent on the child's behavior.
4. High demands for achievement and conformity with parental policies,
accompanied by openness to the child's rationale and encouragement of
independent judgment.
5. Provision of a complex, stimulating environment that offers challenge and
excitement as well as security.
Many of Baumrind's authoritative parents' behaviors correlated with greater autonomy in
their youngsters: parents being accepting, empathic, supportive, self-aware, curious,
appreciative of individual's differences yet building connectedness within the family, and
using family discussions to solve problems. Parents who tell children why, not just what,
to do help give children a rationale for the future when they face not only a similar
situation but also new problems.
Authoritative parenting helps to moderate peer influence in that these children are more
strongly influenced by high-achieving friends and less influenced by drug-using friends.
These parents directly influence their adolescents' friendships through such means as
giving advice on which teenagers to steer clear of, inviting over prosocial friends, and
encouraging activities that draw socially appropriate peers. The authoritative parent
knows to "pick the battles" carefully, overlooking harping on the child's sloppy dress
habits, for example, in favor of promoting the value of studying. These teenagers have
lower drug use than do teenagers whose parents use lower levels or too controlling
levels of management.
Research shows that overprotective parents tend to delay their children's development.
Mothers in the Collaborative Perinatal Project who were overdemonstrative to their 8-
month-old infantsfrequently fondling and caressing them, using terms of endearment,
talking only about their child's good qualities while glossing over or explaining away less
desirable behaviorsseven years later more often had children who had learning
difficulties or were hyperactive.
A great deal of the child's ability to identify, model and learn from his or her parents
depends on the interaction of the child's unique temperament with that of family
members. Unfortunately, at times the disorganization of children with LD and their
families becomes a vicious cycle; the child's behavior causes disruption in the family,
which in turn is increasingly unable to provide the structure, model and discipline the
child needs.
Parent-Child Temperament Match
Although many children are born with difficult temperaments, a good fit between parent
and child can go a long way toward modifying a child's hard-to-live-with temperament.
Conversely, a poor fit can make a difficult situation much worse, or create a difficult
situation when the child's temperament is just fine. For example, when parents expect
concentration for long periods of time, they put undue stress on a distractible child.
When parents of a highly active child insist that he or she sit still all through dinner,
rather than being permitted to help serve and clear the table, they make life harder for
everyone. When parents abruptly interrupt a goal-directed, persistent child in the midst
of a project to engage in a family activity, that child will be in no mood to be cooperative
or pleassant. When parents don't value the hard work of a very capable child, they
diminish his or her incentive to achieve. And when parents expect unreasonable
academic achievement of a child with LD, they make a tough situation even worse.
One expert has likened a child's temperament to the fault lines that can lead to
earthquakes. Environmental events, particularly parenting styles, are the strain. And just
as earthquakes result from the match between faults and strains, so behavior difficulties
can arise from a mismatch between certain child temperaments and certain styles of
parenting. One student's story is a good illustration of such a mismatch. His
temperament and life experiences are like those frequently associated with children with
learning disabilities. When these children grow up in homes that are hostile or critical
and parental expectations are beyond the children's capabilities, then the outcome can
be bleak. But if the children have warm, supportive homes where the environment is
arranged to help them rather than imposing unreasonable pressures and standards,
they have a better chance of avoiding such secondary problems as poor self-image and
depression.
It is also true that the same home environment affects different children in different
ways. A number of factors make the home environment unique for each child:
differences in parental behavior and affection toward different children the child's
perception of differences in treatment, birth order and gender differences, experiences
outside the family, peer interactions, and so forth. It may happen that a parent-child
match was perfect for all the other siblings in terms of shaping their personalities and
facilitating cognitive gains but, because of the challenges posed by a child with a
learning disability, the match needs to be reworked for this child.

Вам также может понравиться