Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 24

Theories of mathematical learning and

understanding
BACKGROUND FOR THE STUDY


http://ponce.inter.edu/cai/tesis/lmrivera/cap2.htm
According to Romberg (Grouws, 1992), there is no general agreement on the definition
of learning, how learning takes place and what constitutes reasonable evidence that learning has
taken place. Some say it is observable changes in behavior, others that it means acquiring new
knowledge, and other say that it is the creating of a disequilibrium.

Psychologists have made different philosophic assumptions about the nature of the
learning process. Those who hold that learning is determined by the forming connections
between the environment stimuli and useful responses are called associationist. A representative
of this view, E.B. Thorndike (1922), recommended that in mathematics, for example, students
perform much drill and practice on correct procedures and facts to strengthen correct mental
bonds. Associationists also argued that curricula should be structured to keep related concepts
well separated, so that students did not form incorrect ties.
By 1943, the behaviorists were maintaining that a real science of education could be built only
on direct observation. Absent from the research and discourse of behaviorists were "thinking",
"meaning" or other such unobservable and possibly nonexistent phenomena. Though
behaviorists, led by B.F. Skinner, denied the theory of "mental bonds" that associationist had put
forth, their prescriptions for mathematics teaching were similar: drill and practice, with
reinforcement by reward for desirable behavior in the form of correct answers and punishment
for undesired behavior. The behaviorists brought to the educational scene programmed learning
curricula and new standardized testing techniques. In the study of teaching "process-product"
researchers searched for types of teaching behavior that led to greater student achievement.

During that same time there existed other views of knowledge and learning. In 1916
Dewey said that "It is that reconstruction or reorganization of experience which adds to the
meaning of experience, and which increases ability to direct the course of subsequent
experience" (p.89). In another occasion Dewey (1938) wrote that "I use the word understanding
rather than knowledge because ...knowledge to so many people means 'information.' Information
is knowledge about things (it is static), and there is no guarantee that understanding-the spring of
intelligent action-will follow from it"(p.48). Brownell (1935) maintained that although incidental
learning could help counteract the practice of teaching mathematics as an isolated subject, it did
not provide an organization in which "the meaningful concepts and intelligent skills requisite to
real arithmetical ability" could be developed. Brownell wrote about a theory of instruction in
which making "sense" of what was learned was the central issue in arithmetic instruction. Piaget
and his coworkers who interviewed hundreds of children, proposed that in learning, children pass
through developmental stages and that the use of active methods which gives scope to
spontaneous research by the child help him rediscover or reconstruct what is to be learned "not
simply imparted to him" (Piaget, 1973, p.23).

Piaget's research and theory, is called developmental constructivism (Romberg, 1969),
and maintains that children acquire number concepts and operations by construction from the
inside and not by internalization. Piaget (1968) pointed out that every normal student is capable
of good mathematical reasoning if attention (and care) is directed to activities of his interest, and
if by this method the emotional inhibitions that too often give him a feeling of inferiority in
lessons in mathematics are removed.

In contrast to Piaget's explanation of construction, Vygotsky (1986) presented an
alternate theory where imbalance and not equilibrium is considered normal.

Jean Piaget's Theory of Learning

According to Jean Piaget (1979), human intellectual development progresses
chronologically through four sequential stages. The order in which the stages occur have been
found to be largely invariant, however the ages at which people enter each higher order stage
vary according to each person's hereditary and environmental characteristics.
Piaget defined intelligence as the ability to adapt to the environment. Adaptation takes place
through assimilation and through accommodation, with the two processes interacting throughout
life in different ways, according to the stage of mental development.

In assimilation, the individual absorbs new information, fitting features of the
environment into internal cognitive structures. In accommodation, the individual modifies those
internal cognitive structures to conform to the new information and meet the demands of the
environment. A balance is maintained through equilibration, as the individual organizes the
demands of the environment in terms of previously existing cognitive structures. A child moves
from one stage of cognitive development to another through the process of equilibration, through
understanding the underlying concept so that the understanding can be applied to new situations.
Equilibration is a balance between assimilation and accommodation.

The stages of cognitive development that Piaget distinguished are four: (Piaget, 1968)
Sensorimotor (0-2 years of age) - children begin to use imitation, memory and thought. They
begin to recognize that objects do not cease to exist when they are hidden from view. They move
from reflex actions to goal-directed activity.
Preoperational (2-7 years) - Children gradually develop language and the ability to think in
symbolic form. They are able to think operations through logically in one direction and they have
difficulty seeing another persons point of view.
Concrete operational (7-11 years) - Children are able to solve concrete (hands-on) problems in
logical fashion. They understand the laws of conservation and are able to classify and seriate.
They also understand reversibility.
Formal operational (11-15 years of age) - Children are able to solve abstract problems in logical
fashion. Their thinking becomes more scientific, they develop concerns about social issues and
about identity.

Piaget suggested that when children do not understand or have difficulty with a certain
concept, it is due to a too-rapid passage from the qualitative structure of the problem (by simple
logical reasoning -e.g. a ball existing physically) to the quantitative or mathematical formulation
(in the sense of differences, similarity, weight, number, etc.). Conditions that can help the child
in his search for understanding according to Piaget is the use of active methods that permit the
child to explore spontaneously and require that "new truths" be learned, rediscovered or at least
reconstructed by the student not simply told to him (Piaget,1968). He pointed out that the role of
the teacher is that of facilitator and organizer who creates situations and activities that present a
problem to the student. The teacher must also provide counterexamples that lead children to
reflect on and reconsider hasty solutions. Piaget argued that a student who achieves a certain
knowledge through free investigation and spontaneous effort will later be able to retain it. He
will have acquired a methodology that serves him for the rest of his life and will stimulate his
curiosity without risk of exhausting it.

A third type of knowledge that Piaget suggests is social or conventional knowledge. He
said that it is always through the external educational action of family surroundings that the
young child learns language, which Piaget (1973) called is an "expression of collective values."
Piaget pointed out that without external social transmission (which is also educational) the
continuity of collective language remains practically impossible.
There are three types of feelings or emotional tendencies, according to Piaget, that affect the
ethical life of the child, that are first found in his mental constitution. In the first place is the need
for love, which plays a basic role in development in various forms from the cradle to
adolescence. There is a feeling of fear of those who are bigger and stronger than himself, which
plays an important role in his conduct. The third is mixed, composed of affection and fear at the
same time. It is the feeling of respect that is very important in the formation or exercise of moral
conscience.

Noddings (1990a) points out certain characteristics that constructivist teachers must have
an ethical commitment to inquiry in order to aid students in their investigations, and the
receptivity and responsiveness of an ethic of care which involves sharing and listening to
students, taking interest in their purposes as well as in those of the teachers' truth.

A constructivist view of knowledge implies that knowledge is continuously created and
reconstructed so that there can be no template for constructivist teaching (Peterson & Knapp,
1993). Since this point of view holds that learning involves student's constructing their own
knowledge, this leads to a redefinition of the teachers' role to one of facilitator. This also leads to
teaching that emphasizes the importance of listening to and valuing students' perception, even
when their understanding differs from conventional knowledge (Cochran, Barson & Davis,
1970).

Mathematical understanding and number sense

Cognitive scientists and mathematics educators who favor the cognitive science approach have
moved well beyond Piaget in describing the way the mind operates. There has been a shift from
an organic language of Piaget to a language "highly colored" of computers, (Noddings 1990b)
with words such as networks, connections, paths, frames, etc.

As a cognitive position, constructivism maintains that all knowledge is constructed, as
Piaget's theories hold. Not only are intellectual processes themselves constructive but are
themselves products of continued construction. It can be said that the construction and
subsequent elaboration of new understandings is stimulated when established structures of
interpretation do not permit or accept a new situation or idea. This clash (not understanding)
produces a disequilibrium that lead to mental activity and the modification of previously held
ideas to account for the new experience (Simon & Schifter, 1991).
Hiebert and Carpenter (1992) propose a framework for considering understanding from
the constructivist
perspective which would shed light on analyzing a "range of issues related to understanding
mathematics." They make a
distinction between the external and internal representation of mathematical ideas, pointing out
that, to think and
communicate mathematical ideas, people need to represent them in some way. Communication
requires that the representations be external, taking the form of spoken language, written
symbols, drawings or concrete objects.
Mathematical ideas becomes tangible when people can express them. By learning to express
their ideas to one another,
students can begin to appreciate the nuance of meaning that natural language often masks, but
that the precise language
of mathematics attempts to distinguish (Lo, Wheatley, & Smith, 1994; Silver, Kilpatrick &
Schlesinger, 1990; Lesh,
Post & Behr, 1987).

The Curriculum and Evaluation Standards for School Mathematics of the National
Council of Teachers of Mathematics (1989) point out in the Standard on Communication that
understanding mathematics can be defined as the ability to represent a mathematical idea in
multiple ways and to make connections among different representations. In order to think about
mathematical ideas these need to be represented internally but these mental representations are
not observable. This has led cognitive science to consider mental representations as a field of
study (Ashcraft, 1982; Greeno, 1991; Hiebert & Carpenter, 1992) .

Connections between external representations of mathematical ideas can be constructed
by the learner (Hiebert & Carpenter, 1992) between different fortes of the same idea or between
related mathematical ideas. These connections are often based on relationships of similarity or of
differences. Connection within the same representation are formed by detecting patterns and
regularities.

The relationship between internal representations of ideas constructs a network of
knowledge. Understanding then is the way information is represented, so that a mathematical
idea, procedure or fact is understood if it is part of an internal network. Networks of mental
representations are developed gradually as new information is connected to the network or new
ties are constructed between previously disconnected information. Understanding grows as the
networks become larger and more organized and can be limited if connections are weak or do not
exist becoming useless (Hiebert & Carpenter, 1992).

Although the image of adding to existing networks is appealing in its simplicity, it may
turn out that the image is too simple. Studies have suggested that students in the act of building
understanding reveal a much more chaotic process (Hiebert, Wearne & Taber, 1991). There have
been a number of studies in which the process of learning and understanding are of central
interest: Cobb, Wood, Yackel, Nichols, Wheatley, Trigatti & Perlwitz (1991); Carpenter,
Fennema, Peterson, Chiang & Loef (1989); Doyle (1988); Baroody, (1985); Hiebert & Wearne
(1988).

Children are natural learners and the environment both social and physical offers them
many opportunities to acquire notions of quantity. Even in very poor or diverse cultures, races or
classes, children have the opportunity to acquire quantitative notions (Gelman 1980; Ginsburg,
Posner & Russel1,1981; Ginsburg & Russell, 1981).

Each healthy human brain, no matter the age, sex, race or culture, comes equipped with a
set of unique features: the ability to detect patterns and to make approximations, a capacity for
various types of memory, the ability to self-correct and learn from experience and external data
and self reflection, and an great capacity to create (Came & Caine 1994). Because of this
predisposition of the brain, children and adults constantly search for ways to make sense and
make connections. This can be translated into a search for common patterns and relationships as
Hiebert and Carpenter (1992) propose.

Caine and Caine (1994) argue that brain research confirms that multiple complex and
concrete experiences are essential for meaningful learning and teaching. They add that the brain
is designed as a "pattern detector" and that the function of educators should be to provide
students with the kind of experiences that enable them to perceive "the patterns that connect."

Children from a very young age are sensitive to quantity. They perceive differences in
number; they see correlation among different numbers of events; their actions contain quantity
and they use words referring to basic mathematical events (Gelman, 1980; Ginsburg, 1989).
Various studies (Ginsburg & Baron, 1993; Starkey & Cooper, 1980; Van de Walle & Watkins,
1993) have pointed out the importance of taking into account children's informal mathematical
connections as building block for formal mathematics. Ginsburg (1989) suggests that students
need to learn that it is acceptable, "even desirable", for them to connect conventional arithmetic
with their own informal knowledge, intuition and invented procedures.
In a study by Carpenter, Ansell, Franke, Fennema and Weisbeck (1993), the results suggest that
children can solve a wide range of problems, including problems involving multiplication and
division, much earlier than is generally presumed. With only a few exceptions, children's
strategies could be characterized as representing or modeling the action or relationships
described in the problem. These researchers conclude that young children's problem-solving
abilities have been seriously underestimated. They suggest that if from an early age children are
motivated to approach problem solving as an effort to make sense out of problem situations, they
may come to believe that learning and doing mathematics involves solving problems in a way
that always makes sense.

The use of manipulatives in mathematics

For years mathematics educators have advocated using a variety of forms to represent
mathematical ideas for students. Physical three-dimensional objects are often suggested as
especially useful. Despite the intuitive appeal of using materials, investigations of the
effectiveness of the use of concrete materials have yielded mixed results (Bednarz & Janvier,
1988; Bughardt, 1992; Evans, 1991; Hestad, 1991; Hiebert, Wearne, & Taber, 1991; Simon,
1991; Thompson,J., 1992).

P. Thompson (1994) suggests that the apparent contradictions in studies using
manipulatives are probably due to aspects of instruction and students' engagement to which the
studies did not attend. Evidently, just using concrete materials is not enough to guarantee success
according to Baroody (1989). The total instructional environment must be looked into to
understand the effective use of concrete materials. In a project by Wesson (1992) for grades 1
and 2, which emphasized exploratory activities with manipulatives, the results suggested that
while a much wider range of content than in standard books or tests was covered, there was no
loss of arithmetic skills.

Children understand when using concrete materials if the materials are presented in a way
that helps them connect with existing networks or construct relationships that prompt a
reorganization of networks. It is important to consider then, the internal networks that students
already carry with them and the classroom activities that promote construction of relationships
between internal representations (Hiebert et al, 1991). Manipulatives then can play a role in
students' construction of meaningful ideas. Clements and McMillan (1996) and others suggest
they should be used before formal instruction, such as teaching algorithms. Clements and
McMillan propose that concrete knowledge can be of two type: "sensory-concrete" which is
demonstrated when students use sensory materials to make sense of an idea; and "integrated
concrete" which is built through learning. Integrated concrete thinking derives its strength from
the combination of many separate ideas in an interconnected structure of knowledge. When
children have this type of interconnected knowledge, the physical objects, the actions they
perform on the objects, and the abstractions they make are all interrelated in a strong mental
structure.
Ross and Kurtz (1993) offers the following suggestions when planning a lesson involving the use
of manipulatives. He suggests that the mathematics teacher should be certain that:

1. manipulatives have been chosen to support the lesson's objectives;
2. significant plans have been made to orient students to the manipulatives and
corresponding classroom procedures;
3. the lesson involves the active participation of each student;
4. the lesson plan includes procedures for evaluation that reflect an emphasis on the
development of reasoning skills.

Invented strategies and number sense

In the last few years there have been studies about the idea of students' constructing their
own mathematical knowledge rather than receiving it in finished form from the teacher or a
textbook (Carpenter, Ansell, Franke, Fennema, Weisbeck, 1993; Markovits & Sowder, 1994). A
crucial aspect of students' constructive processes is their inventiveness (Piaget, 1973). Children
continually invent ways of dealing with the world. Many of the errors they make can be
interpreted as a result of inventions (Ginsburg & Baron, 1993; Peterson, 1991). Similarly, in
school mathematics, students rely many times on invented strategies to solve a variety of
problems (Carpenter, Hiebert, & Moser, 1981; Carraher & Schliemann, 1985; Ginsburg, 1989).
Kamii and Lewis (1993) and Madell (1985) have reported successful work in programs where
children are not taught algorithms, but are encouraged to invent their own procedures for the
basic operations. Treffers (1991) suggests a similar program in the Netherlands and Baker &
Baker (1991) in Australia.

Various studies have been made in the area of invented strategies. Cook and Dossey's
(1982) findings show that children learn number facts easily and quickly and recall them better
when using a strategy approach than when using a learned algorithm, drill or practice approach.
Browne (1906); Howe and Ceci (1979); Kouba (1989); Rathmill (1978);
Sowder and Wheeler (1989) have done studies on strategies used for calculation. Carpenter and
Moser (1984) found that children in the United States ordinarily invent a series of abbreviated
and abstract strategies to solve addition and subtraction problems during their first four years in
school. Romberg and Collis (1987) found that even though some children are limited by their
capacity to handle information, most are able to solve a variety of problems by inventing
strategies that have not been taught. English (1991) observed that in a study of young children's
combinatoric strategies, a series of six increasingly sophisticated solution strategies were
identified. A significant number of children independently adopted more efficient procedures as
they progressed on the task.

A study by Markovits and Sowder (1994) examined the effect of an intervention in the
instruction of seventh grade students for the purpose of developing number sense. Instruction
was designed to provide diverse opportunities for exploring numbers, number relationships, and
number operations and to discover rules and invented algorithms. Measures taken several months
later revealed that after instruction students seem more likely to use strategies that reflected
number sense and that this was a long-term change. Rathmill (1994) suggests that planning for
instruction that promotes the development of children's thinking and reasoning about
mathematics not only helps them make sense of the content they are studying, but also helps
them learn ways of thinking that later will enable them to make sense of new content. Lampert
(1986) proposed that "a sense-making" atmosphere is necessary and that arithmetic should make
sense in terms of children's own experience. Reynolds'(1993) study suggests that children's
imaging activity is at the heart of their sense making and problem solving. Silver, Shapiro and
Deutsch (1993) found that students' performance was adversely affected by their dissociation of
sense making from the solution of school mathematics problems and their difficulty in providing
written accounts of their thinking and reasoning.

In the Everybody Counts document from the National Research Council (1989) the major
objective of elementary school mathematics is the development of "number sense". The
Curriculum and Evaluation Standards for School Mathematics (NCTM 1989) also includes
number sense as a major theme throughout its recommendations. Greeno (1991) interprets
number sense as "a set of capabilities for constructing and reasoning with mental models." This
perspective he argues, provides reasons that support considering various aspects of number sense
as features of students' general condition of knowing in the area of numbers and quantities, rather
than skills that should be given specific instruction. The term number sense refers to several
important but elusive capabilities according to Greeno, capabilities including flexible mental
computation, numerical estimation and quantitative judgment. Flexible mental computation
according to Greeno involves recognition of equivalence among objects that are decomposed and
recombined in different ways.

Reys et al. (1991) describe number sense in the following manner:
Number sense refers to an intuitive feeling for numbers
and their various uses and interpretations; an
appreciation for various levels of accuracy when
figuring; the ability to detect arithmetical errors,
and a common-sense approach to using numbers... Above
all, number sense is characterized by a desire to make
sense of numerical situations (pp.3-4)

Sowder and Schappelle (1994) suggest that there are common elements found in
classrooms that help children acquire good number sense:
1. Sense-making is emphasized in all aspects of mathematical learning and instruction.
2. The classroom climate is conducive to sensemaking. open discussions about
mathematics occurs both in small groups and with the whole class.
3. Mathematics is viewed as the shared learning of an intellectual practice. This is more
than simply the acquisition of skills and information. Children learn how to make and
defend mathematical conjectures, how to reason mathematically and what it means to
solve a problem.

Mental Computation

Mental computation according to Trafton (1986) refers to nonstandard algorithms for
computing exact answers. It is also referred to as the process of calculating an exact arithmetic
result without the aid of an external computational or recording aid. (Hope, 1986; Reys, 1986). It
is recognized as both important and useful in everyday living as well as valuable in promoting
and monitoring higher-level mathematical thinking (Reys et al., 1995). It has been recognized in
the Curriculum and Evaluation Standards for School Mathematics (NCTM, 1989) that increased
attention should be given to mental computation. A National Statement on Mathematics for
Australian Schools (Australian Education Council and the Curriculum Corporation, 1991) was
released in 1991 recommending substantial change in emphasis among mental, written and
calculator methods of computation and between approximate and exact solutions. A major
objective is to redirect the computational curriculum in schools to reflect a balance in the
emphasis on methods of solution. Before the Statement, the curriculum was divided as: 75%
written computation, 25% Calculator, Estimation, Mental Computation. With the new statement
it would be 25% for each method of computation.

According to Boulware (1950) mental arithmetic has its origin during the second quarter
of the nineteenth century. The idea of building a broader foundation of meaning and
understanding in arithmetic gave rise to Mental Arithmetic as it was known in the middle of the
nineteenth century with Warren Colburn (1841) considered as pioneer in the field of mental
arithmetic. Before his time, arithmetic had reached a point of extreme abstraction according to
Boulware. The second half of the century witnessed the decline in interest and understanding of
the purpose of mental arithmetic. With the coming of more writing paper, cheap pencils, with the
rise of industry and its accompanying needs for persons skilled in computation, the practical or
computational phase of arithmetic took on importance around the turn of the century. The
emphasis in arithmetic at that time was the teaching of isolated facts, followed by drill upon
these facts. High among the purposes stated for the study of arithmetic many authors of the time
placed speed, memory and accuracy by mechanical rules. There was an emphasis in arithmetic
on drill for perfection and automatic response at the expense of meaning and understanding. In
1950, a dissertation by Boulware is representative of the quest for the development of "meaning"
in mental computation stirred by Brownwell (1935), who urged that meaning and seeing sense in
what is being learned should be the central focus of arithmetic instruction. Boulware's
conception of mental computation is as follows: Mental arithmetic deals with number as a
unified, consistent system, and not as an aggregate of unrelated facts. [It] consists of methods of
dealing with numerical situations whereby a clear concept of the number system may be
conceived and utilized in quantitative thinking. It proceeds to the analysis of number
combinations by processes of meaningful experiences with concrete numbers, reflective thinking
in number situations, seeing relationships, and discovery of new facts as an outgrowth of known
facts (pp.7-8). In 1960, in an article by Sister Josefina there seems to begin interest in mental
computation and in the 1978 NCTM yearbook on computational skills there appears an article by
Trafton (1978) where the need for including proficiency with estimation and mental arithmetic as
goals for the study of computation is presented. A good number of studies and articles about
mental computation appeared in the period of the 1980s (e.g. Reys, R.E., 1984, 1985; Reys, B. J.,
1985a, 1985b; Madell, 1985; Hope, 1985, 1986, 1987; Reys & Reys, 1986; Langford, 1986;
Markovits and Sowder, 1988; Baroody, 1984, 1985, 1986, 1987, 1989 and others) leading up to
the statement of the inclusion of mental computation as an area where increased attention is
needed in school mathematics by the NCTM (1989). With the increase of studies in cognitive
skills and number sense (e.g.Simon, 1979; Resnick, 1986; Silver, 1987; Schoenfeld, 1987;
Greeno, 1980; Sowder, 1988) and more recent studies mentioned in this chapter, mental
computation is suggested to be related to number sense, needed for computational estimation
skills and considered a higher order thinking skill.

In a study by Reys, Reys and Hope (1993) they argued that the low mental computation
performance reported in this study most likely reflected students' lack of opportunity to use
mental techniques they constructed based on their own mathematical knowledge. The study of
Reys, Reys, Nohda and Emori (1995) assessed attitude and computational preferences and
mental computation performance of Japanese students in grades 2, 4, 6, and 8. A wide range of
performance on mental computation was found with respect to all types of numbers and
operations at each grade level. The mode of presentation (visual or oral) was found to
significantly affect performance levels, with visual items generally producing higher
performance. The strategies used to do mental computation were limited, with most subjects
using frequently a mental version of a learned algorithm.

In a study by G.W. Thompson (1991) about the effect of systematic instruction in mental
computation upon fourth grade students' arithmetic, problem-solving and computation ability, a
significant difference favored the group taught mental computation, with girls improving more
than boys.

According to Markovits and Sowder (1994) it would seem reasonable that if children
were encouraged to explore numbers and relations through discussions of their own and their
peers' invented strategies for mental computation, their intuitive understanding of numbers and
number relations would be used and strengthened. Okamoto (1993) found that children's
understanding of the whole number system seemed to be a good predictor of their performance
on word problems.
Cross-cultural research has identified a variety of mental computation strategies generated by
students, (e.g. Hope & Sherill, 1987; Markovits & Sowder, 1988) and the difference in mental
computation in an out of school and in-school context (e.g. Ginsburg, Posner, & Russell, 1981;
Pettito & Ginsburg, 1982). Sribner (1984) points out that individuals develop invented
procedures suited to the particular requirements of their particular occupations.

In a study on individuals who are highly skilled in mental arithmetic (Stevens 1993),
forty-two different mental strategies were observed. Efficient, inefficient and unique strategies
were identified for each of five groups (grade 8). Dowker (1992) describes in a study the
strategies of 44 academic mathematicians on a set of computational estimation problems
involving multiplication and division of a simple nature. Computational estimation was defined
as making reasonable guesses as to approximate answers to arithmetic problems, without or
before actually doing the calculation. Observing people's estimation strategies, Dowker suggests,
may provide information not only about estimation itself, but also about people's more general
understanding of mathematical concepts and relationships. From this perspective Dowker
concludes that estimation is related to number sense. Sowder (1992) who agrees with this
position points out that computational estimation requires a certain facility with mental
computation.

In a study by Beishuizen (1993), he investigated the extent to which an instructional
approach in which students use of the hundreds board supported their acquisition of mental
computation strategies. In the course of his analysis, he found it necessary to distinguish between
two types of strategies for adding and subtracting quantities expressed as two digit numerals as
follows:

1. 1010 strategy - 49 + 33 -> 40 + 30 -> 9 + 3 = 12

70 + 12 = 82

2. N10 strategy - 49 + 33 -> 49 + 30 -> 79 + 3 = 82

Beishuizen's analysis indicates that N10 strategies are more powerful, but that many
weaker students used only 1010 strategies. The study's findings also suggest that instruction
involving the hundreds board can have a positive influence on a student's acquisition of N10
strategies. Fuson and Briars (1990) and others have also identified these strategies.

Hope (1987) points out that because most written computational algorithms seem to
require a different type of reasoning than mental algorithms, an early emphasis on written
algorithms may discourage the development of the ability to calculate mentally. Lee (1991)
recommends that perhaps it is time to investigate changing our traditional algorithms for addition
and subtraction to left-to-right procedures.

According to Reys et al. (1995) there have been many studies that suggest the benefit of
developing mental computation strategies. Mental computation has also been highlighted in the
Curriculum and Evaluation Standards for School Mathematics (NCTM, 1989).
Mental computation can be viewed from the behaviorist perspective as a basic skill that can be
taught and practiced. But it can also be viewed from the constructivist view in which the process
of inventing the strategy is as important as using it. In this way it can be considered a higher-
order thinking skill (Reys et al., 1995).

Addition, subtraction and teaching strategies

The Curriculum and Evaluation Standards for School Mathematics (NCTM 1989)
recognizes that addition and subtraction computations remain an important part of the school
mathematics curriculum and recommends that an emphasis be shifted to understanding of
concepts. Siegler (1988) indicated how important it is for children to have at least one accurate
method of computation. In a study by Engelhardt and Usnick (1991) while no significant
difference between second grade groups using or not using manipulatives was found, significant
differences in the subtraction algorithm favored those taught addition with manipulatives. Usnick
and Brown (1992) found no significant differences in achievement between the traditional
sequence for teaching double-digit addition, involving nonregrouping and then regrouping, and
the alternative, in which regrouping was introduced before non-regrouping examples in second
graders.

Ohlsson, Ernst, and Rees (1992) used a computerized model to measure the relative
difficulty of two different methods of subtraction, with either a conceptual or a procedural
representation. The results of the use of the model suggested that regrouping is more difficult to
learn than an alternative augmented method, particularly in a conceptual representation, a result
that contradicts current practice in American schools. Dominick's (1991) study with third grade
students suggested that students' confusion with the borrowing algorithm centered around a
misunderstanding of what was being traded. Evans (1991) found that groups taught with pictorial
representations or by rote learned to borrow in significantly less time than did a group using
concrete materials in grades 2 and 3.

Sutton and Urbatch (1991) recommended the use of base-ten blocks, beans and bean
sticks or beans and bean cups to serve as manipulatives to use for trading games and with the
"transition board". (A modified version of the base ten board). They also emphasized that
attempting to teach addition and subtraction without initially preparing the student with trading
games could be counterproductive and result in lack of understanding due to lack of preparation.
In a study which analyzed individual children's learning of multidigit addition in small groups in
the second grade, results suggested that rarely did a child spontaneously link the block trades
with written regrouping (Burghardt, 1992). Fuson and Briars (1990) and P.W. Thompson (1992)
found that the base ten blocks could be a helpful support for children's thinking, but many
children do not seem spontaneously to use their knowledge of blocks to monitor their written
multidigit addition and subtraction. The Fuson and Briars study suggested that frequent solving
of multidigit addition or subtraction problem accompanied by children's thinking about the
blocks and evaluating their written marks procedure, might be a powerful means to reduce the
occasional trading errors made by children. The study also suggested that counting methods that
use fingers, are not necessarily crutches that later interfere with more complex tasks.

Fuson and Fuson (1992) found that in all of the groups studied, children were accurate
and fast at counting up for subtraction as at counting on for addition. This contrasts with the
usual finding that subtraction is much more difficult than addition over the whole range of
development of addition and subtraction solution strategies. Sequence counting on and counting
up according to Fuson and Fuson are abbreviated counting strategies in which the number words
represent the addends and the sum. In both strategies the counting begins by saying the number
word of the first addend. For example: 7 + 5, a child would say 7 pause 8, 9, 10, 11, 12 (up to
five numbers, the last number of the sequence is the answer) and 12 - 5 would be, 5, pause 6, 7,
8, 9, 10, 11, 12, seven numbers were counted, which is the answer. Thornton's (1990) study
provides evidence that children who were given an opportunity to learn a counting up meaning
for subtraction as well as counting down (counting back from minuend), preferred the counting
up meaning.

In a series of studies by Bright, Harvey and Wheeler (1985) they defined an instructional
game as a game for which a set of instructional objectives has been determined. These
instructional objectives may be cognitive or affective and are determined by the persons planning
the instruction, before the game is played by the students who receive the instruction in it. The
results of the studies suggest that:
1. games can be effective for more than drill and practice and for more than low level
learning of skills and concepts,
2. games can be used along with other instructional methods to teach higher level content
such as problem solving,
3. games should probably be used relatively soon before or after instruction planned by the
teacher for the same material,
4. the use of more challenge, fantasy or curiosity might enhance the effectiveness of
instructional games.

Hestad (1991) found that the use of a card game was effective for third grade students in
introducing new mathematical concepts and maintaining skills.

In a study by Cobb (1995) the use of the hundreds board by second graders' in a classroom
where instruction was broadly compatible with recent reform recommendations (NCTM, 1989,
1991) was investigated. The role played by the use of the hundreds board over a 10-week period
in supporting the conceptual development of four second graders was studied.
Particular attention was given to the transition from counting on to counting by tens and
ones. The hundreds board
is a ten-by-ten grid from either 0 to 99 or 1 to 100. The results indicated that the children's' use of
the hundreds board did not support the construction of increasingly sophisticated concepts of ten.
However, children's use of the hundred board did appear to support their ability to reflect on their
mathematical activity once they had made this conceptual advance. The utility of the hundreds
table in teaching computation has been also recognized by Beishuzen (1993);Hope, Leutzinger,
Reys and Reys (1988); Thornton, Jones and Neal (1995) and Van de Walle and Watkins (1993) .

Teachers' Pedagogical Beliefs about Mathematics Teaching Learning and Assessment

We can learn more about how invisible components in the teaching and learning situation
can contribute to or detract from the quality of the mathematical learning that takes place by
focusing on the culture according to Nickson (1992). It is important, he points out, in exploring
the mathematics classroom from the perspective of the culture, it generates, to remember that we
are concerned with the people in the setting and what they bring to it. Nickson adds that we must
increase our sensitivity to the importance of their hidden knowledge, beliefs, and values for
mathematics education.

One of the major shifts in thinking in relation to teaching and learning of mathematics in
recent years has been with respect to the adoption of differing views about the nature of
mathematics as a discipline. The view of mathematics that has informed and historically
transfixed most mathematics curriculum has been, according to Lakatos, (1976) one of
considering that mathematics as consisting of "immutable truths and unquestionable certainty".
Such a view does not take into account how mathematics changes and grows and is waiting to be
discovered (Nickson, 1992). Brown and Cooney (1982) note that the intensity of the teachers'
beliefs is very important in the classroom culture. The traditional detachment of mathematics
content from shared activity and experience, so that it remains at an abstract and formal level,
constructs barriers around the subject, according to Nickson, that sets it apart from others areas
of social behavior. The message conveyed is that is has to be accepted unquestioningly and from
which no deviation is permitted. The classroom culture will mirror this unquestioning
acceptance. The visibility and acceptance of what is done or not done in mathematics are factors
in stopping teachers from engaging in activities that they may instinctively feel are appropriate
but might challenge the supposedly inviolable essence of mathematics as they themselves were
taught.

In investigating the relationship between what teachers believe about how children learn
mathematics and how those teachers teach mathematics, A. Thompson (1992) points out that
studies have examined the congruence between teachers' beliefs and their practice and findings
have not been consistent. Researchers such as Grant (1984) and Shirk (1973) have reported a
high degree of agreement between teachers' professed views of mathematics teaching and their
instructional practice, where as others have reported sharp contrasts (e.g. Carter, 1992; Cooney,
1988; Shaw, 1989; Thompson, 1984).
It has been argued (Nickson, 1988; Ball, 1993) that bringing teachers into the arena of research
activity can be an important step in increasing their understanding of research processes and
results and their relation to classroom practice. Each mathematics classroom will vary according
to the actors within it. The unique culture of each classroom is the product of what teachers bring
to it in terms of knowledge, beliefs, and values, and how these affect the social interactions
within that context. The daily experiences of students in mathematics classes of teachers with
positive attitudes were found to be substantially different from those of students in classrooms of
teachers with negative attitudes in a study by Karp (1991). Overall, teachers with negative
attitudes toward mathematics employed methods that fostered dependency and provided
instruction which was based on rules and memorization, relied on an algorithmic presentation,
concentrated on correct answers and neglected cognitive thought processes and mathematical
reasoning, whereas teachers with positive attitudes were found to encourage student initiative
and independence. Swetman (1991) found no significant relationship between teachers'
mathematics anxiety and students' attitude toward mathematics in grades 3 to 6. Attitude toward
mathematics however, became more negative as grade increased in teachers and students.

Teacher influence on student achievement

At the time of a study by Good and Grouws, (1977), comparatively few studies had
included observational measures that detail how the teacher functions as an independent variable
in order to influence student achievement. Teacher effectiveness (as operationally defined in
their study) appeared to be associated strongly with the following clusters: student initiated
behavior; whole class instruction, general clarity of instruction, and availability of information as
needed, a non-evaluative and relaxed learning environment which is task focused; higher
achievement expectations; classrooms that are relatively free of major behavioral disorders.
Brophy's (1986) study found that most investigative efforts had focused on curricular content and
students' learning without careful consideration of teachers' instructional practices. Loef (1991)
found that more successful teachers (in grade 1) represented differences among addition and
subtraction problems on the basis of the action in the problem and the location of the unknown,
and they organized their knowledge on the basis of the level of the children's understanding of
the problem in context. Hiebert and Carpenter (1992) note that it seems evident that procedures
and concepts should not be taught as isolated bits of information, but it is less clear what
connections are most important or what kind of instruction is most effective for promoting these
connections.

Teachers' influence on class content

Even though some researchers have concluded that textbooks determine the content
addresses in classrooms (Barr, 1988; Barr & Dreeben; 1983) others provide evidence to
challenge that assertion (Freeman .& Porter, 1989; Stodolsky,1989) as Sosniak and Stodolsky
(1993) have pointed out. In mathematics, Barr (1988) found that seven out of nine fourth-grade
teachers used their textbooks by moving lesson by lesson through the book. In contrast, Freeman
and Porter (1989) and Stodolsky (1989) found most mathematics teachers to be selective in their
use of textbook lessons, problem sets, and topics, although topics not included in the texts were
only occasionally added to the instructional program.
Research suggests that teachers are "gatekeepers" (Thornton, 1991) who make their own
decisions about which parts of a textbook to use and which ways to use them (Barr & Sadow,
1989) and such decisions may not necessarily lead according Brophy (1982) to close adherence
to the textbook material.
Sosniak and Stodolsky (1993) found in a study of four fourth-grade teachers that the
influence of textbooks on teachers' thinking and on instruction was somewhat less than the
literature indicates. Their results suggest that patterns of textbook use and thinking about these
materials were not necessarily consistent across subjects even for a single teacher, and that the
conditions of elementary teachers' work encouraged selective and variable use of textbook
materials.
In a study by Stigler, Fuson, Ham, and Myong (1986), an analysis is made of addition and
subtraction word problems in American and Soviet elementary mathematics textbooks. The data
suggests that American children entering first grade can solve the simple kinds of addition and
subtraction word problems on which American texts spend so much time.
Another study on text books is one by Ashcraft and Christy (1995) in which they study the
frequency of arithmetic facts in elementary texts. The study tabulated the frequency with which
simple addition and multiplication facts occur in elementary school arithmetic texts for grades 1-
6. The results indicated a "small-facts bias" in both addition and multiplication. "Large" facts,
with operands larger than 5, occurred up to half as frequently as those with operands in the 2-5
range. As was also found in an earlier tabulation for grades K-3, facts with operands of 0 and 1
occurred relatively infrequently, except for patterns like 1+2 and 1x3 which had a high
frequency. The small facts bias in the presentation of basic arithmetic, at least to the degree
observed, probably works against a basic pedagogical goal, mastery of simple facts. It may also
provide a partial explanation of the widely reported problem size or problem difficulty effect,
that children's and adults' responses to large basic facts are both slower and more error prone
than their solutions to smaller facts.

In a study by Porter (1989) elementary school mathematics is used as a context for
considering what could be learned from careful descriptions of classroom content. Teachers log
and interviews show that large numbers of mathematics topics are taught for exposure with no
expectation of student mastery: much of what is taught in one grade is taught in the next, skills
typically receive 10 times the emphasis compared to either conceptual understanding or
application, and depending on school and teacher assignments, mathematics instruction a student
receives may be doubled or halved. Porter argues that "ultimately teachers must decide what is
best for their students and within the limits of their own knowledge, time and energy." (p.15)
Teaching practices and their effects
Koehler and Grouws (In Grouws, 1992) have suggested that teachers' behavior is influenced by
their knowledge of: the mathematics content being taught, how students might learn or
understand that particular content and of the methods of teaching of that particular content. Also
influencing teachers' behaviors are teachers' attitudes and beliefs about teaching and
mathematics.
Bush (1991) in a study about factors related to changes in elementary student's anxiety
found that mathematics anxiety tended to decrease as teachers in grades 4-6 spent more time in
small group instruction, had more years of experience, and took more post-bachelor's
mathematics courses. According to a study by Tangretti (1994), findings indicated that the
elementary teachers that participated in the study were not adequately prepared to meet NCTM
expectations. Their teaching focus was found to be an algorithmic approach with emphasis on
numeration and computation. Lack of confidence in content areas beyond arithmetic were
reported as contributing to the lack of preparedness of elementary teachers to implement
innovative curriculum. Wood, Cobb and Yackel (1991) report that after participating in a study,
changes occurred in a teacher's (second grade) beliefs about the nature of mathematics (from
rules and procedures to meaningful activity), about learning (from passivity to interacting) and
about teaching (from transmitting information to guiding students' development of knowledge).
A similar result was reported in a study by Zilliox (1991). In-service elementary school teachers
felt they were teaching more and better mathematics lessons, were more comfortable with
student use of hands-on materials and with managing small groups, and had a different sense of
student capabilities and different expectations for student behavior after participating in the
study.
A study by Carpenter, Fennema, Peterson, Chiang, and Loef (1989) investigated
teachers' use of knowledge from research on children's mathematical thinking and how their
students' achievement is influenced as a result. Although instructional practices were not
prescribed, the teachers that participated in the treatment activities taught problem solving
significantly more and number facts significantly less than did control teachers. Treatment
teachers encouraged students to use a variety of problem-solving strategies, and they listened to
processes their students used significantly more than did the control group teachers and knew
more about individual students problem-solving processes.

Teachers and assessment issues

A view of learners as passive absorbers of facts, skills, and algorithms provided by
teachers is a basis for much of the most current use of measures (Stenmark, 1991). Standard
achievement tests according to Kamii and Lewis (1991) measure students' abilities to recall and
apply facts and routines presented during instruction. Some items require only the memorization
of detail; other items, although designed to assess higher-level learning outcomes, often require
little more than the ability to recall a formula and to make the appropriate substitutions to get the
answer (Lambdin, 1993). Test items of this type are consistent with the view of learning as a
passive, receptive process, a process which is additive and incremental. The
practice of scoring answers to items of this type (right or wrong) is consistent with the view that
"bits" of knowledge or skills are either present or absent in the learner at the time of testing.
Under this approach, diagnosis is simply a matter of identifying the missing pieces of knowledge
in the student,thereby creating a need for remedial teaching. In the constructivist view of
teaching, the student is a participant in building his own understanding. The learner does not
absorb new ideas and data but rather constructs his own version and relates it to existing
information (Wilson, 1992). In order to help the student construct firm connections in the sense
of the constructivist theory, the teacher can contribute by facilitating time for auto-evaluating,
reflection on processes and ideas, auto-monitoring procedures like journals, portfolios,
rechecking work (Sanford, 1993; Stenmark, 1989, 1991; NCTM, 1995). These are all
metacognitive processes that can be strengthened through these practices. Metacognition refers
to one's knowledge of one's own cognitive processes and products, and of the cognition of others.
It also refers to self-monitoring, regulation and evaluation of the cognitive activity (Silver &
Marshall, 1990). According to Beyer (1988), metacognition involves thinking about how one
thinks as well as thinking to make meaning. For assessment and monitoring of student learning,
an implication of the constructivist view is that teachers must measure understanding and models
that individual students construct for themselves during the learning process (Webb & Romberg,
1992) . Accordingly, achievement could be better defined and measured not in terms of number
facts and procedures that the student can reproduce, but in terms of best estimates of his or her
level of understanding of key concepts and interrelated underlying principles (Wilson, 1992) .

A survey to investigate teacher awareness of alternative assessment of students in
mathematics of (n=126) public school teachers in primary (K-2), elementary (3-5), middle school
(6-8) and high school (9-12) showed that significant differences in awareness of alternative
assessment practices exists among the four levels (Drury, 1994). In another study, (Watts, 1993)
which is a description of the implementation of the Curriculum Standards for School
Mathematics (NCTM, 1989) in grades K-3, it was found that teachers used in their tests,
knowledge level items significantly more frequently than higher level items, and items with
manipulative materials significantly more than items without manipulatives. Alternative
assessment formats were considered significantly more difficult to use.

In a study on the influence of district standardized testing on mathematics instruction for
grades 3 and 8 (Kolitch, 1993) it was reported that in two school districts, the curriculum was
aligned to test content; in a third district with an innovative mathematics program, the district test
had little influence on mathematics instruction, but the program was in jeopardy because of
decreasing computation scores. Kamii and Lewis (May 1991) also report a similar finding of
achievement testing in primary mathematics as perpetuating lower-order thinking. According to
an achievement test, traditional instruction produced results as good as or better than, a
constructivist program in second grade. Such tests were created within a framework of
mathematics which Kamii and Lewis argue does not measure understanding.

[Pgina Anterior]
Table of Contents

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
LIST OF TABLES
Chapter

I. INTRODUCTION
Need for the Study
Purpose for the Study
Procedures of the Study
Plan of the Report

II. Background for the Study
Theories of Mathematical Thinking
Jean Piaget's Theory of Learning
Mathematical Understanding and Number Sense
The Use of Manipulatives in Mathematics Teaching
Invented Strategies and Number Sense
Mental Computation
Addition, subtraction and teaching strategies
Teachers' Pedagogical Belief about Mathematics Teaching, Learning and
Assessment
Teachers' Influence on Student Achievement
Teachers' Influence on Class Content
Teaching Practices and Their Affects
Teachers and Assessment Issues

III. Preparation and Trial of the Materials
The setting for the study
Subjects
The Instruments and Procedures
The Pre and Post Test
The Interviews and Questionnaires
The Instructional Material
Theoretical Basis and Description
Materials Development

IV. RESULTS

V. SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS AND
RECOMMENDATIONS
Summary of the Project
Conclusions
Recommendations

BIBLIOGRAPHY

Some articles are a vailable in PDF format. If this software is not a vailable in your computer,
free download in the following link.http://www.adobe.com/products/acrobat/readstep.html This
software is vailable in the campus computers.

Estos documentos estn en formato .PDF, de no tener este Software, lo podrn bajar Gratis en la
Internet en:
http://www.adobe.com/products/acrobat/readstep.html


De interesar evaluar el contenido de los apndices escriba a:
Dra. Luz M. Rivera

Вам также может понравиться