Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 10

EDUCATION & THERAPIES

It’s Time
We Had A Talk
-- About Talk
MARION BLANK, PHD Intervention and the Law of Unintended Consequences
Dr. Marion Blank is the Director Few topics in autism spectrum disorders (ASD) rival the attention devoted to
of A Light on Literacy in the language. For good reason, vast amounts of time, effort and money are directed at
Developmental Neuropsychiatry getting children to talk. This leads programs to be judged, and to judge themselves,
Program at Columbia University. by their success in reaching this goal. You see the process at work in statements such
She is an internationally recognized
as the following, “Research now shows that between 75-95% of children who receive
developmental psychologist with
extensive experience in designing intensive behavioral intervention will speak by the age of five.”
language and reading programs (Dawson, 2008, p. 775).
for children with a wide range of There is no question about the desirability of getting children to talk. However,
learning disabilities. when this is the cornerstone of intervention, unintended—and unexamined—
consequences often ensue. The following two vignettes reflect the types of problems
that can occur from a tunnel vision focus on “talk.”

 A parent of a six-year old child with autism related this experience:


“I took Damien to a new place the other day. He immediately left my side, walked into a
bathroom, glanced at the toilet seat and with no one in sight, said, ‘That is O.’ He was
labeling the shape with one of the letter names that he loves. It’s so weird. I thought
that all I wanted was for him to talk. Now he’s doing that, but it’s all wrong.”

 A teacher of children with ASD was talking to a colleague:


“ While I was talking with his mother, somehow Sam got into her car and turned on the
engine. I was frightened out of my mind. I banged on the window and pointed to the
door lock near the window. I kept telling him to pull it up and I kept gesturing to show
him how to do it. Nothing worked. Fortunately the police came quickly and got the door
open. After it was all over, I kept thinking. We’ve been teaching him language for years.
And he can’t even understand simple, basic language that could save his life.”

Both vignettes show “breakdowns” in their size, shape, color and so on. In
MARY BETH CULL
language--breakdowns that are so familiar commenting about the toilet seat, he
Mary Beth Cull joined Dr. Blank three that they are accepted as an unquestioned was simply applying what he had been
years ago to become a member of “given” of the children’s deficiencies. taught.
the Developmental Neuropsychiatry Certainly, their deficiencies play a role. But
Program. She has been trained in when they are seen as the only source, we  the second child had a similar curriculum.
behaviorist approaches with children risk overlooking other possible causes. But that curriculum rarely, if ever,
on the spectrum and has now One such cause is the effect of included any “real world” situations
extended the work to include more intervention itself. For example, remotely comparable to one he was in.
cognitive/linguistic principles.
 the first child had, for years, been taught In his case, he was simply not applying
to look at objects and speak about what he had not been taught.
118 THE AUTISM FILE | www.autismfile.com | info@autismfile.com REPRINTED WITH PERMISSION © THE AUTISM FILE ISSUE 30 2009
While it is disturbing to consider the idea
that well-intentioned efforts may go awry,
such outcomes are far from unique (Dubner
& Levitt, 2008). The field of medicine has
even coined the term iatrogenic disease to
identify the unintentional illnesses caused
by physician intervention. No comparable
term exists in education. However, it is vital
to determine whether a similar process may
be at work.

Tackling the Behemoth of Language


Given the history of intervention efforts in
ASD, “unintended consequences” ought
not be surprising. The rapid rise in the
numbers of affected children led to a sense
of urgency that worked against careful
planning. The imperative was to create, as
rapidly as possible, programs to turn things
around. Since a significant percentage of the
children were non-verbal or minimally verbal,
the goal of getting them to “speak” rose to
the fore as an unquestioned objective. could be drawn from any of the available two settings leads the child to not provide
At the same time, there was little programs. However, because Skinnerian- the expected response on the rare occasions
systematic discussion as to what the children based behaviorist programs (Skinner, 1957) when it actually is requested. Should this
should be speaking about. Instead, the dominate the field and are often cited as occur, it is easy to attribute the “failure” to
profound nature of the children’s limitations THE state-of-the art treatment for ASD, we the child’s limited powers of generalization
seemed to dominate the agenda, leading to will use an illustration from this approach. (even though generalization should not be
the mindset of “What can we get children to To this end, let’s consider the common expected).
say when they have little or no speech?” goal of getting a child to offer his or In the other outcome, the high rate of
In answering that question, an (unstated) her name when asked. In aiming for this repetition leads the child to conclude that
rule took hold: make requests that can behavior, a teacher may, over months, a name is what adults want (as occurred
be met with single word responses. devote innumerable trials to this skill in the toilet seat example). So instead of
Operationally, this meant having the children (i.e., having a child respond consistently withholding the response, the child offers
produce high numbers of nouns (such as and accurately when asked “What is your it freely, even when no one has requested
apple, cookie, dog, house, car) and, to name?”). Taken at face value, this activity the information. Ironically, in the school
a lesser degree, adjectives (such as red, can easily be justified as representing a setting, this “inappropriate behavior” might
round, big). There seemed to be little else useful language/social skill. be hailed as a success since it shows that
one could reasonably ask them to say. So When the behavior is placed in context the child is spontaneously applying a skill
curricula were developed geared to labeling (i.e., the setting where it might be called without being prodded to do so.
of objects, colors, sizes, shapes, letters and upon), a different picture emerges. For a Intervention programs are replete with
the like (Lovaas, 1987; Schopler, Reichler, & start, in “real life,” this request is rarely these sorts of difficulties. If we liken the
Lansing, 1980). made. Typically it happens only when there situation to a piano, it’s as if the instrument
When speech was totally absent, is a stranger who lacks the information. is being played as a set of individual,
essentially the same curriculum was used, Further, when the request is made, it discordant notes with the end result being
albeit restricted to receptive language. happens only once! After receiving an cacophony. In no way does this mean
Instead of asking the children to name answer, any repetition of the request would that well-structured music is impossible.
objects, they were asked to select objects be viewed as “peculiar.” On all counts, However, it can occur only when the keys
based on the noun or adjective that the training the child receives is almost are played in carefully orchestrated patterns.
was supplied (e.g., “point to car”). The the diametric opposite. Not only is there Unfortunately, the individual notes
augmentation with Picture Exchange extensive repetition, but the repeated technique is essentially how language has
Communication System (PECS) (Bondy & request is made by the same person and that been taught to the children. Because of
Frost, 1994) fits with this approach. person is far from being a stranger. the wide diversity among children with
To gain a sense of the problems Under these circumstances, the instruction ASD, some can benefit from the input
intervention can cause, it’s instructive to is likely to result in two possible outcomes. (Cohen, Amerine-Dickens & Smith, 2006).
review an actual teaching unit. Examples In one, the lack of similarity between the When their language skill is great enough

ISSUE 30 2009 REPRINTED WITH PERMISSION © THE AUTISM FILE info@autismfile.com | www.autismfile.com | THE AUTISM FILE 119
EDUCATION & THERAPIES

(i.e., those “higher on the spectrum”) they


can “go beyond the information given.” ...language does not simply emerge at a particular
Their inherent skills allow them to take
the disconnected elements and generate
time in development; it emerges only when there
an impressive language system—using is an established base of other skills upon which
processes like those employed by typical
children (Sherer & Schreibman, 2005). But language can build.
they are in the minority. Most are confined
to the dissected, distorted elements they to objects or experiences—if they did not The abilities involved:
have been given, resulting in language that already have a clear notion of what those Temporal Sequencing
is “all wrong.” objects or experiences were. Put simply, In any list of the pre-verbal skills underlying
children did not realize that a toy car was language, “temporal sequencing” has to be
Taking a New Path a car because they learned the word ‘car’. central. While the term may be unfamiliar,
For better outcomes to be achieved, it’s vital Instead, through what Piaget termed the behaviors it encompasses are not. They
that we consider, in far greater depth than their “sensori-motor occur every time we either
has been the case till now, the language that operations,” infants knew register or produce a
we teach children with ASD. As an entrée what a car was. The word behavior involving two or
into this topic, we are going to focus on four “car” was simply a label more elements that follow
areas. They are the: for naming what had each other in time (from
1. cognitive precursors of language already been learned. The walking down steps, to
2. key structural components of language label could be helpful opening a box, to driving
3. distinction between language and in allowing them to a car).
communication communicate about a In the realm of
4. bridge between language of school and car when they wanted to language, one set of skills
home discuss it with others. But is particularly critical. It
the language label did is the ability to register
Because these categories are rarely nothing for their conceptual level. independent, sequenced elements and
discussed, the reaction might well be “Why This “chicken-egg” debate about language perceive them as representing continuous,
these?” As we hope to show, despite their and concepts led to young children being integrated input. (Back about 100 years ago,
having been relegated to the background, perceived very differently. Now, prior to some people took advantage of this capacity
the four areas are central to intervention. So, language, they were seen as active, thinking when they showed audiences individual
knowing that some of the territory may be individuals who had a far richer mental life images in rapid succession. Although each
unfamiliar, let’s begin our journey. than had previously been envisaged. Further, frame was a still image, when shown at the
the pre-verbal skills they engaged in were right speed, there was the illusion of motion.
Cognitive Precursors of Language viewed as critical to their acquiring language. And so the motion picture industry was
Although our sights are on language, we In other words, language does not simply born.)
start with events that take place prior to emerge at a particular time in development; From the outset, temporal sequencing
the development of language. This shift in it emerges only when there is an established pervades language. It starts from day one
perspective mirrors a major shift that has base of other skills upon which language can when the infant begins to attend to the flow
taken place in cognitive psychology. build. Not unexpectedly, the study of the of sounds that make up speech (Ervin-Tripp,
For much of the twentieth century, precursors to language has become a major 1966). For example, it has been shown that
the dominant view was that language enterprise. phonemes (i.e., the sounds that make up
significantly affects our ability to form This perspective, however, has had a words) are differentiated from one another
concepts. In other words, having words minimal effect in the field of ASD. Work by extraordinarily short transition periods
enables us to think at a “higher level” (Sapir, such as that associated with Relationship within the set of sounds that are heard. So a
1958 [1929]; Vygotsky, 1986 [1934]). As Development Intervention (RDI) (Gutstein, sound like “ga” is different from a sound like
indicated by the oft-stated command, “Use 2001) and Developmental, Individual- “ba” only because of millisecond differences
your words,” this view pervades intervention Difference, Relationship-Based (DIR) – in intervals within the sounds themselves. It
efforts. Floortime (Greenspan & Wieder, 2006) does has also been shown that for some children,
In the 1970’s, however, this idea was highlight the emotional base of language the inability to register these differences is
turned on its head when the work of a Swiss (i.e., creating the emotional bonds that lead the source of their language problems (Tallal,
developmental theorist, Jean Piaget, began a child to communicate). But these efforts Miller & Fitch, 1993). This phenomenon
to receive increasing attention (Piaget, do not involve the pre-verbal cognitive skills (which is only one of the many sequencing
1952 [1936]; 1954 [1937]). Piaget made that a child needs in order to benefit from properties in language) has served as the
the obvious, albeit neglected, point that language instruction. What might these skills basis for the development of the Fast
children would not know the words to apply be? Forward program (Tallal et al, 1997).
120 THE AUTISM FILE | www.autismfile.com | info@autismfile.com REPRINTED WITH PERMISSION © THE AUTISM FILE ISSUE 30 2009
A delightful example of sequencing in activities should be offered to stir the skills the work can then move on to language
typical development appears in what has into being. However, this option is not likely itself. As noted earlier, much language work
been termed “language in the crib” (Weir, to be productive. Regardless of the source, is directed towards single word labeling
1962). It refers to countless numbers of (whether disinclination and/or disability), (e.g., concepts such as colors, numbers,
hours infants and toddlers spend in long the children are not likely to process the shapes, weather, parts of the body, holidays,
stretches of sustained babbling—when information. A failure in processing is greetings).
totally on their own—with no one in sight. precisely why they are not learning language While the concepts are part of language,
While the typical infant is wired to to begin with. So the offering of auditory the use of single words fails to capture the
endlessly practice and process the sounds of information will not be useful. properties of actual language (i.e., where
words, the child with ASD is doing anything In the future, this situation will hopefully words are assembled together to form
but that. Hence, the absence of babbling. change as we expand our views on what can sentences). Sentences are not simply chains
It is not clear the extent to which this is and should be taught. For example, music, of words that are attached one to the other.
a disinclination and/or a disability (i.e., an area that the children typically love, is In order to be meaningful, the words must
whether the child will not attend vs. whether grounded in auditory sequencing. It would combine according to set patterns (Pinker,
he cannot attend). Regardless of the source, be important to study whether activities 1994). That’s why, even though they
the limited behavior has major consequences in this sphere could be crafted to foster contain the same words, The boy is sitting is
for the children’s language functioning. It the requisite temporal processing skills a sentence whereas Boy is the sitting is not.
means that the children miss out on the (Trevarthen, 2000).
countless hours of sound production that Even without these possibilities, there The abilities involved: The “little” words
are central to the development of speech. are tools for structuring visual information In creating the patterns of sentences, a
This component likely plays a key role in the so that it involves temporal processing. A select, albeit relatively ignored group of
finding that, of the children with ASD who sample activity might be the following: the words plays a critical role. Some samples
speak, many are restricted to one and two child sees a board with a set of variously from this select group are “the,” “is,”
word utterances. They have not developed shaded boxes. The adult then taps, in “were,” “these,” etc. They go by a variety
the sequencing abilities required for sequence, on any set of two to four boxes. of names, including the “little” words
producing actual sentences. The child’s task is to reproduce the tapping (because they rarely go beyond a single
The effects of limited sequencing sequence. syllable and many have only two letters as in
extend beyond the children’s own speech “is,” “of,” “he,” “to,” “if,” etc.). In contrast
production. It is likely that they are also not to the concepts such as color and size that
processing more than one or two words that come from a domain in linguistics known as
others say to them (i.e., the sounds they semantics, these little words come from the
hear)—regardless of the actual length of domain of syntax.
the utterances in which those one or two At first glance, the lack of attention
words may be embedded. Often, the extent This disarmingly simple task contains to these words seems reasonable since
of the children’s difficulties is hidden by the a range of properties that give it power. they appear to do little in the way of
familiarity of the contexts of everyday life. Specifically, just as in language, conveying meaning. Indeed, among the
For example, when given commands such  the input is composed of several variety of terms used to characterize them
as “Go upstairs and get your shoes,” the elements. is the phrase “non-content words,” clearly
only word the child may be processing is  the particular elements that are selected suggesting that they are devoid of meaning.
“shoes.” All the rest may be part of a routine follow a set order. Nevertheless, these seemingly minor words
that the child has become accustomed to.  the order in which the elements are are critical. For example, consider what
Yet the overall impression is that the child presented is the order that the child must the word “to” does in the following set of
is processing complex chunks of language. process and reproduce. sentences.
At this point, the precise details of what is  memory demands are critical since the The girl walked the dog.
taking place are not critical. What is critical information that the child is to reproduce The girl walked to the dog.
is the need to recognize the powerful role is no longer present.
temporal factors play in processing and Amazingly, young children are very aware
producing language. A wide range of activities can be created of the existence of these words and they
along these lines. The tasks are relatively devote large swaths of time to their mastery.
Structuring the teaching: Mirroring intricate and may not be of interest to all Their skill was shown in a landmark study
Characteristics of the Auditory Modality readers. For those who would like to delve at Harvard University in the 1970s that was
How might pre-linguistic temporal further into this area, details are provided in aimed at documenting the stages toddlers
processing be fostered? In typical an appendix at the end of the article. go through in learning to speak (Brown,
development, the auditory modality is the 1973).
major vehicle for promoting the requisite Structural Components of Language After spending the first stage expressing
skills. So it might seem that auditory With mastery of core pre-linguistic skills, simple relationships such as “big dog” and
ISSUE 30 2009 REPRINTED WITH PERMISSION © THE AUTISM FILE info@autismfile.com | www.autismfile.com | THE AUTISM FILE 121
“Mommy fix,” they move into the next since they are the ones that perform actions.
stage, where they do something quite By contrast, with a few exceptions (such
extraordinary. While still tiny tots, they as planes fly and balloons pop), inanimate
spend months figuring out how to insert, beings do not meet this criterion.
into their two- and three-word utterances, Significantly, the familiar concepts in
the “little” words and part words such as intervention efforts (i.e., shape, size, color,
the, a, -ing (as in sitting), -ed (as in looked), etc.) typically refer to inanimate beings
in, on, -s (as in cookies), my and that. or features that lack animate properties.
Though no one has, or could have, told Concepts that “do nothing” have minimal
them, the toddlers sense the power of the value for helping the children unlock
“little” words in the use of language. What the language puzzle. Consequently,
gives them this power? The answer is to be their emphasis in instruction needs to
found in the central role that nouns and be challenged. (These familiar concepts,
verbs play in language. Edward Sapir, a the present through is, are, have, etc., the however, do have a role to play in math—but
noted linguist in the early twentieth century, past through was, were, did, had, etc. and the language of math is a discussion for
described the situation in the following the future through is going to, will, etc.). another day.)
terms: When these “time related” terms are absent, Further, the relevant noun-verb concepts
children cannot perceive the distinctions must be embedded in sentences, and the
What, then, are the absolutely essential between sentences such as “the boy is sentences must contain the relevant non-
concepts in speech…? ... We must have eating,” “the boy was eating,” “the boy is content words. In order to enable children to
objects,..(and) actions... No language wholly going to eat.” (Uccelli et al, 2006). develop a reasonable mastery of this area,
fails to distinguish noun and verb... It is The “little” words have awesome power. the instruction should lead them to produce
different with the other parts of speech. Not Unfortunately, intervention programs do not sentences that (a) are eight to ten words
one of them is imperatively required for the recognize this basic fact of language life. in length and (b) reflect a wide range of
life of language. Typically, those words are seen as not only sentence types. Given the limited speech of
Sapir, 1921, pp 93, 119 insignificant, but as sources of confusion for so many of the children, this goal may seem
the child. So in the belief that it “simplifies” wildly unrealistic. Fortunately, this is not the
The “little” words play a key role in the language, they are specifically excluded. case.
achieving the noun-verb distinction because That’s why you hear phrases such as A notable characteristic of many children
they attach themselves differently to the “touch same,” or “show me sitting” rather with ASD is echolalia. While the production
two groups of words. Nouns attract words than “touch the one that is the same,” or of this behavior is often inappropriate, the
like the, that, those, these (as in the girl, that “show me the kid who is sitting.” Despite presence of this behavior is quite another
car, those balls, these flowers). By contrast, the well-intentioned efforts, in omitting matter. It is a sign that the children are (a)
verbs attract words like is, -ing, are, did, these words for the avowed purpose of processing and producing verbal information
have, were, will (as in is going, are sitting, “simplifying” language, the programs are and (b) capable of verbal imitation. Through
did run, were playing, will eat). Essentially actually consigning the children to a world these skills, it is possible to extend their
when a child hears “the” or “these,” he where meaningful language is essentially sentence span (by having the adult produce
or she senses that the word that follows unattainable. well-formed sentences that the child
is likely to be a noun; similarly “did” or imitates) (Speidel & Nelson, 1989).
“were” signals the likelihood of a verb. This Structuring the teaching: Because of the children’s language
selective attachment of the “little” words Teaching sentences via imitation limitations, the linguistic demands must be
helps children make the vital differentiation The noun-verb issue clearly has significant carefully arranged. At any point, the number
between nouns and verbs. implications for the content of intervention. of words should be just above the level
When these words are not mastered, If the distinction between the two groups that the children can comfortably produce.
children lack the tools to distinguish among of words is to be mastered, nouns and verbs Then, with sufficient repetition, they can
such varied forms as “What is the boy must be taught—and the teaching must not systematically be led to longer utterances
doing?” “Where is the boy?” “What does present them as individual, disconnected (e.g., a child who can produce three word
the boy want?” “What was the boy doing?” elements. Wherever possible, they should be combinations can be moved to handling four
“Who is with the boy?” All the questions meaningfully linked in ways that reveal their words). Over time, with repeated experience,
merge into the single word “boy” –since that properties. the children’s span can steadily increase,
is THE word that the child understands. The To achieve this linkage, the nouns must until it is up to sentences of eight to ten
end result is that the children cannot make be capable of performing the actions words.
sense out of the language they hear and the represented by the verbs to which they are Accurate imitation does not imply that
language they are expected to respond to. attached (as in birds fly, kids run, bugs crawl, the children fully comprehend the meaning
The “little” words also serve a major role fish swim, etc.). This, in turn, means that the of what they are saying. But, considerable
in the representation of time (e.g., reflecting nouns have to represent animate beings— processing is required to incorporate, retain
122 THE AUTISM FILE | www.autismfile.com | info@autismfile.com REPRINTED WITH PERMISSION © THE AUTISM FILE ISSUE 30 2009
and reproduce well-formed utterances. That forward (i.e., initiate) and/or (ii) respond skills that included: (a) being able to hold
processing, even in the absence of complete to a topic that has been put forward (i.e., back crying, despite intensely negative
comprehension, serves the child well in respond). Over the course of any interaction, feelings; (b) knowing how to frame his
expanding his or her language base. As in each individual can take on both roles; that intense feelings into a well-organized set
the previous section, illustrative material of initiator (i.e., the one who introduces of words; (c) recognizing that his message
is offered in the appendix for readers an idea) and that of responder (i.e., the was potentially “dangerous” and, therefore,
interested in more details of teaching one who responds to the idea). (Blank & it was wise to whisper it; and (d) reckoning
sentence structure. Franklin, 1980) that the whispering may not have been
Although not typically described in these enough, so that leaving the scene was a wise
The Distinction between terms, the goals of many programs are to backup move.
Language and Communication develop the children’s initiating abilities. This The language/social skills amalgam
The skills considered thus far (i.e., temporal objective is also at the heart of deeply felt of effective initiation is subtle and
sequencing, sentence structure) are aimed wishes parents express for their children “to immeasurable. While social skills programs
at getting the children to recognize and be able to say what is on their minds.” have been developed, none begins to
deal with some of the key building blocks of The steady pressure to have children approximate the complexities for effective
language. We have not yet touched upon the verbalize their requests (e.g., “you have to initiation. There is, understandably, little
way the skills, once acquired, are used (i.e., tell me what you want”) is deemed to be evidence that the programs achieve
the way they function in the child’s life). a step towards this end (Koegel & Koegel, substantial gains (Bellini et al, 2007).
A, if not, THE chief function of language 2006). And, the children do often begin In contrast to the many complexities
is communication where (a) messages are to verbalize requests without being asked underlying initiation, the responder role is
produced for others and (b) messages to do so. Despite its widespread use, in far easier. With the topic selected, most of
from others are taken in and understood. our experience, there is little evidence the work has been done. The path of the
Oftentimes, language and communication to suggest that the expression of verbal discussion is clear, leaving the responder
are used as if they are synonymous. That’s requests transfers to any other forms of only with the task of coming up with an
why “speaking” (i.e., the production of initiation. acceptable response. The constraints
language) has so readily been accepted as a Further, from what is known about imposed by the initiation mean that the
criterion of a program’s success. But as the communication, this transfer is not range of acceptable responses is quite
toilet seat example shows, a child can speak to be expected. Effective initiation is narrow. For example, if someone asks,
and it can be “all wrong.” extraordinarily complex. In raising a topic, “Would you like to have a sandwich for
The problem in these cases is not one has to be aware of the social role of the lunch?” reasonable responses vary from
language; the child’s speech represented partner, know the topics that are suitable “No” to “Yes” to “What kind?” Not much
a reasonable sentence. What is awry is the for discussion with that person, and figure else is possible.
communication. The child spoke to no one out how to phrase the topic in ways that are Essentially, the responder has only to
nor was there any intention of sending the socially acceptable. These are the factors listen to the idea that has been put forth
comment to another. Further, the topic was that lead one to speak in a totally different and, guided by the limitations that have
inappropriate. What the child’s comment manner to a boss as opposed to a friend. been set, come up with an acceptable
shows us is the disturbing and anomalous The relevant skills are not based in language response. This, by the way, represents the
world of “language without communication” —but in social understanding. The social basic design of all classrooms. The adult
(Blank, 1980). domain is even more problematic for the (i.e., the teacher) initiates the topic, and the
children than is language (Goldstein & child’s role is to respond to whatever idea
The abilities involved: Brooks, 2007) and so, apart from request has been put forth (Blank & Klig, 1982).
The roles of initiator and responder language, effective initiation is often beyond Significantly, when children can effectively
A central difference between language and the reach of children with ASD. carry out the responder role, their chances
communication is the fact that language is The ease with which typical children, from for inclusion in a regular classroom are
a system within an individual, while verbal early in life, handle this area misleads us as greatly enhanced.
communication is a system that operates to the complexity of the skills involved. Their
between individuals. Communication has all skill is evidenced in the following initiation Structuring the teaching: Developing
the complexity of language and, in addition, produced by a two-year-old for his mother. Effective Responding
it has all the complexity of interpersonal The child had just “endured” yet one more Efforts to foster communication are more
interaction. So, in verbal communication, in application of an unpleasant ointment to feasible when geared to helping children
addition to using language, the participants treat a skin condition. When the treatment master the responder role. There is no
have the responsibility of tailoring the ended, he carefully walked behind the chair difficulty in placing them in that role since
language so they stay “in sync” with one where his mother was seated and whispered, that is the role they are assigned in most
another. “Me hate you.” Then he scurried away. intervention (i.e., questions are steadily
This responsibility can be accomplished In this incident, which took less than five asked and they are expected to respond).
in two ways. A person can (i) put a topic seconds, the toddler showed an array of Where the difficulty rests is in leading
ISSUE 30 2009 REPRINTED WITH PERMISSION © THE AUTISM FILE info@autismfile.com | www.autismfile.com | THE AUTISM FILE 123
EDUCATION & THERAPIES

them to become effective responders. To “given” (that is, their presence is implicit and The Bridge between Language
respond effectively, the child must be able there is no reason to make it explicit). Many at School and at Home
to understand precisely what information question-answer combinations have this While intervention programs vary, on
is being demanded by a question that has structure. one issue, there is common ground. All
been posed. Experience shows that this is a When restructured, this feature of share the belief that the children cannot
major challenge for children with ASD. They language can be key in helping children master language as it appears in “everyday
often do not understand what the question with ASD connect questions and answers. life.” They agree that the language has
means. Essentially, the communication is set up so to be carefully structured so as to make
When faced with lack of comprehension, that the “implicit” is made “explicit” (i.e., in it accessible to the children. Fortunately,
the children adopt a range of strategies. A place of omitting the words that are taken programs have the power to achieve this
common one is: “Answer any question with for granted, the responses include those goal since they can steadily control the
a label (noun).” From the child’s vantage words). By doing this, the children are given content that the children experience. As
point, that strategy is reasonable. It “works” a tool for “seeing” the connections between a result, the children are presented with a
for a range of questions such as, “What is speakers that have, for so long, eluded simpler, clearer world.
that?” and “Which one do you want?” It also them.
fits with the emphasis placed on labeling. For example, imagine a boy eating and The abilities involved:
The strategy, however, fails them when the question “What is the boy doing?” In The complexities of the “natural” world
they face questions such as “What is the the normal course of events, the answer Where does this leave the children when
boy doing?” For example, imagine that in would be the single word “eating.” In the they face other settings, in particular,
response to that question, a child responds, expanded mode, the answer would be “The the home? Along with the school, this is
“Boy.” The therapist then is likely to repeat boy is eating.” In other words, (as indicated the most powerful social context in the
the question, often with an emphasis on by the underlined words) the question and children’s lives. However, except for the
the word that he or she feels the child answer have been shaped so that they rare instances where a parent totally adopts
“missed.” “NO! What is the boy DOING?” explicitly share as many words as possible. the language of intervention (e.g., Maurice,
The child senses that something is awry but This method can be applied to a relatively 1993) the home fails to offer the simplified
hasn’t a clue as to what it is. So the child wide range of questions as indicated in the language characteristic of intervention.
tries a different response. S/he may recall following examples. (In all cases, relevant This situation is only to be expected.
that color has been an important feature play materials such as dolls and toy animals A typical household automatically uses
of teaching, and so then offers the next are provided so that the questions have not “everyday language”, and it is extraordinarily
response of “blue.” only structure but clear referents.) difficult to re-package that language so
The therapist, again lacking a specific that it mirrors the simplified language of
effective means for re-directing the child, Question: Who are these? intervention. But this does not mean that
repeats the original question – often with Answer: These are boys. the children must live with an unbridgeable
changes in emphasis. This may work – or at chasm between the language of the two
least, seem to work, when the child finally Question: Where is the cat? major settings in their lives. While it is
comes up with an action that represents the Answer: Here is the cat unrealistic to get parents to match their
“correct” response. However, the end result language to that of the school, options exist
of interactions like this is a strengthening Question: What are they doing? for reasonable patterns of co-existence.
of the child’s feelings that verbal mastery is Answer: They are running.
forever beyond reach. Structuring interaction in the home:
One source of the children’s difficulties Question: What were the birds doing? Reducing questions
is a failure to realize how questions Answer: The birds were resting. In bridging the chasm, it is important to
and answers relate. Their confusion is see the world through the child’s eyes. For
understandable. In most cases, although Question: Which one can fly? anyone with language difficulties, verbal
questions and answers link, those links rarely Answer: This one can fly. interaction can be a source of considerable
see the light of day. For example, imagine stress. For example, when questions are
the question “What time is it?” In response, asked, even when the precise words are
the answer is likely to be a specific time such As in the teaching of sentence structure not understood, the children still know
as “Four o’clock.” discussed above, the combinations are also that a response is expected. To lessen the
In the absence of a question, the words designed to foster noun-verb connections. pressure, they try to avoid the interaction
“four o’clock” by themselves would be Indeed, the answers to the questions use by “tuning out.” In other words, well-
meaningless. The reason they are not is the same forms that the child has learned intentioned interaction actually compounds
that the “real” answer is “The time is four to use in imitation. In this way, the various their problems because it leads to an active
o’clock.” The responder has elected not components of the teaching offer the avoidance.
to say all those words; instead they have children the consistency that is so necessary Just think of the times when you were
been omitted because they are taken as a for effective learning. in a classroom where questions were being
124 THE AUTISM FILE | www.autismfile.com | info@autismfile.com REPRINTED WITH PERMISSION © THE AUTISM FILE ISSUE 30 2009
see if you want more….” In other words, the
In bridging the chasm, it is important to see the comments are far more extensive than they
world through the child’s eyes. For anyone with need be, but they are still tied to the reality
that is clear to the child.
language difficulties, verbal interaction can be a After a few weeks, the child begins
to feel secure about the lessening of
source of considerable stress. questions. They also begin to realize that
they can be surrounded by language
asked and you were not prepared to answer. more usual response is a wrong response without experiencing failure. This can be
Probably the major mental activity you or no response. In other words, questions a powerful force in reframing the parent-
were engaged in were prayers containing are breeding grounds for experiences child interaction. On its own, it will not
the message, “Please don’t let her call on in repeating failure and a sense of advance the child’s language. But it makes
me.” Those feelings are a fraction of the incompetence. the child more available for attending to and
intensity that children with severe language The simplest solution is to reduce processing language. It thereby helps create
processing problems face in the exchanges questions to the lowest possible number. a more positive attitude towards language
they encounter. This recommendation is diametrically that can advance the work the child does
Ways must be found to minimize the opposed to the common advice to ask as in the intervention. As such, it is one of the
children’s avoidance and discomfort. many questions as possible. Hence, it is not most effective methods for bridging the
Questions, of course, represent one of surprising for parents to be shocked when home-school divide.
the most challenging aspects of verbal they first hear this suggestion. But, they are
interaction. Certainly, not every question equally surprised after a few weeks when A Concluding Comment
elicits a negative reaction. For example, if they see their children’s language blossom. This article, as with so much of our
you ask children—even those with severe It is not all unusual to get an increase in work, arose as a result of discussions
language problems—“Would you like the children’s verbal productions when with parents. A major part of our work is
some ice cream?” the reaction is likely to the pressure of unnecessary questions is aimed at teaching literacy to children with
be pleasure, not pain. But those “nice” removed. ASD—even those who cannot speak (Blank,
questions are few and far between. The lessening of questions does not mean 2006; Maidman, 2006). (As with the topics
For children with language difficulties, the lessening of language. It is important for covered here, this is another skill that has
the much more common experience is to the parent to sustain the interaction with been overlooked because of the restricted
be flooded with questions that demand the child by switching from a questioning views of language/communication that have
answers--answers that are hard to come mode to a commenting mode. The most held sway in intervention.)
by. “What is in that picture?” “What color effective comments are those elaborating Before asking the parents to put forth
is the ball?” “Why don’t you get a puzzle?” information that is immediately at hand. the considerable effort that the program
“What did you do in school today?” “Where For example, if a child is having dinner, the requires, we regularly ask, “Why do you
did you put your school bag?” and on and parent can offer comments such as, “We want your child to learn to read and
on. Even many “non-question” interchanges have something that you really like tonight. write?” Surprisingly, (or at least, it used to
are configured so that they are questions I know you like chicken, but we are not be surprising until we got so used to the
in disguise. While they lack the “wh” words having chicken. We have something else that response) the parents can rarely offer an
such as “who,” “what,” and “where,” they you like. It is meatballs. I am going to give answer. In one way, their “non-response”
nevertheless require a verbal response you one of these to start and then we will made sense. Like all parents, they deemed
(e.g., as when children are told, “Use your
words.”)
Interestingly, children who are competent
in language do not get asked nearly this
many questions. They are able to hold their
own in conversations and readily offer
comments without being asked. The end
result, ironically, is that the more limited a
child’s language skills, the more likely he or
she is to be flooded with questions.
Parents are even encouraged in this
practice –by being told that questions
“get the children to think.” If one actually
plots the percent of questions that are
answered appropriately, the typical picture
yields a number that is quite low. The
ISSUE 30 2009 REPRINTED WITH PERMISSION © THE AUTISM FILE info@autismfile.com | www.autismfile.com | THE AUTISM FILE 125
literacy to be a wonderful skill (which it is) References
and, as with any wonderful skill, they felt no Bellini, S., Peters, J. K., Benner, L., & Hopf, A. Maidman, A. (2006). Language Via Literacy: Who
need to have thought about what it can do (2007). A Meta-Analysis of School-Based Social Would Have Believed It? The Autism Perspective
in the lives of their children. It was a “given” Skills Interventions for Children With Autism (TAP Magazine), Fall issue, 34-36.
Spectrum Disorders. Remedial and Special
to be sought without question. Education, 28, 153-162.
Maurice, C. (1993). Let Me Hear Your Voice: A
Family’s Triumph Over Autism. New York: Ballantine
As we pursued matters with the parents, Blank, M. (1980). A Communication Model for Books.
it became clear that similar processes were Assessing and Treating Language Disorders. In Piaget, J. (1952). The Origins of Intelligence in
at work in their views of spoken language. R.M. Knights & D.J. Bakker (Eds.) Treatment of Children. New York: International Universities Press.
Hyperactive and Learning Disordered Children.
All the children were in intense language (pp.307-319). Baltimore: University Park Press.
(Original work published 1936).
intervention programs—often supported by Blank, M. (2006). Language Via Literacy: The
Piaget, J. (1954). The Construction of Reality in
the Child. New York: Basic Books. (Original work
outside therapies that placed heavy burdens Children Who Made This Journey Possible. The published 1937).
on the families’ time and finances. Yet, the Autism Perspective (TAP magazine), Fall issue,
Pinker, S. (1994). The Language Instinct: How the
37-38.
parents were unable to specify the precise Mind Creates Language. New York: William Morrow
Blank, M. & Franklin, E. (1980). Dialogue with
skills that all the time and effort were Preschoolers: A Cognitively Based System of
and Company.
designed to teach. Assessment. Journal Applied Psycholinguistics, 1, Sapir, E. (1921). Language: An Introduction to the
Study of Speech. New York: Harcourt, Brace and
Upon reflection, it became clear that the 127-150.
Company.
parents could be in no situation other than Blank, M. & Klig, S. (1982). The Child and the
School Experience. In C. B. Kopp & J. B. Krakow Sapir, E. (1929). The Status of Linguistics as a
the one they displayed. Why? Because the (Eds.) The Child: Development in a Social Context Science. Language, 5, 207-214. Reprinted in D.
field itself had never defined what language (pp. 456-513). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. G. Mandelbaum (Ed.) Culture, Language and
Personality. (1958). Berkeley, CA: University of
was all about. Much like the parents, the Bondy, A., & Frost, L. (1994). The picture exchange California Press.
field has assumed language is a “good” and communication system. Focus on Autistic Behavior,
9, 1–19. Schopler, E., Reichler, R. J., & Lansing, M. D.
any and all efforts to teach that “good” are (1980). Individualized Assesment and Treatment
Brown, R. (1973). A First Language: the Early for Autistic and Developmentally Disabled Children.
worthwhile and need little justification. It is Stages. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press. Baltimore: University Park Press.
difficult to believe that nationwide efforts Cohen, H., Amerine-Dickens, M., & Smith, T. (2006). Sherer, M. R., & Schreibman, L. (2005). Individual
in language intervention costing millions Early Intensive Behavioral Treatment: Replication of Behavioral Profiles and Predictors of Treatment
of dollars have been put into place without the UCLA Model in a Community Setting. Journal Effectiveness for Children with Autism. Journal of
of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 27, Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 525-538.
thinking through exactly what was being 145-155.
taught and the value of that teaching. Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal Behavior. Acton, MA:
Dawson, G. (2008). Early Behavioral Intervention, Copley Publishing Group.
Our goal in writing this article has been to Brain Plasticity, and the Prevention of
Autism Spectrum Disorder. Development and Speidel, G. E. & Nelson, K. E. (1989). A Fresh Look
start a discussion that has for too long been at Imitation in Language Learning. In G. E. Speidel
Psychopathology, 20, 775-803.
a “non-topic.” As these issues are addressed, & K. E. Nelson (Eds.) The Many Faces of Imitation
Dehaene, S. (1997). The Number Sense: How the in Language Learning (pp. 1-21). New York:
professionals can begin both to define the Mind Creates Mathematics. New York: Oxford Springer-Verlag.
precise skills they are addressing and their University Press.
Tallal, P., Miller, S., & Fitch, R. H. (1993).
success in enabling the children to attain Dubner, S.J. & Levitt, S. D. Unintended Neurobiological basis of speech: A case for the
these skills. In this process, parents can Consequences, New York Times Magazine, January preeminence of temporal processing. Annals of the
20, 2008. New York Academy of Sciences 682, 27–47.
become much more knowledgeable about
Ervin-Tripp, S. M. (1966). Language development. Tallal, P., Saunders, G., Miller, S., Jenkins, W.M.,
the factors that make a program effective. In L. Hoffman (Ed.), Review of child development Protopapas, A., & Merzenich, M.M. (1997). Rapid
They are already amazingly powerful research (vol 2, pp.55-105). New York: Russell Sage training-driven improvement in language ability
advocates for their children. Armed with Foundation. in autistic and other PDD children. Society for
greater knowledge, their advocacy can be Goldstein, S. & Brooks, R. B. (2007). Social Learning Neuroscience, 23, 490.
Problems. In S. Goldstein & R. B. Brooks (Eds.), Trevarthen, C. (2000). Autism as a
strengthened to become more meaningful Understanding and managing children’s classroom neurodevelopmental disorder affecting
and effective. behavior: Creating sustainable, resilient classrooms communication and learning in early childhood:
Given the heated issues that surround (2nd Ed.). (pp. 166-185). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley prenatal origins, post-natal course and effective
& Sons Inc. educational support. Prostaglandins Leukotrienes &
ASD, it is easy for the ideas we have put
Greenspan, S. I. & Wieder, S. (2006). Engaging Essential Fatty Acids, 63, 41-46.
forth to be viewed as yet more fuel for the Autism: Helping Children Relate, Communicate and Uccelli, P., Hemphill, L., Pan, B. A., & Snow,
many fires already burning in this highly Think with the DIR Floortime Approach. Perseus C. (2006). Conversing with Toddlers about the
contentious field. That is, in no way, our Distribution: Jackson, TN. Nonpresent: Precursors to Narrative Development
intention. Rather, we believe that we are Gutstein, S.E. (2001). Autism/Asperger’s: Solving in Two Genres. In L. Balter & C. S. Tamis-LeMonda
the Relationship Puzzle. Future Horizons Press: (Eds.), Child Psychology: A Handbook of
now at a point where it is possible and Arlington, Texas. Contemporary Issues. (pp. 215-240). CRC Press.
fruitful to combine the experiences that Koegel, R. L. & Koegel, L. K. (2006). Pivotal Vygotsky, L. (1986). Thought and Language,
have been gained from programs to date Response Treatments for Autism: Communication, Revised Edition. A. Kozulin (Ed.) Cambridge, MA:
with advances in the behavioral sciences to Social and Academic Development. Baltimore, MD: MIT Press. (Original work published 1934).
Brookes Publishing Company. Weir, R. H. (1962). Language in the Crib. The
address the long neglected question “What
Lovaas, O. I. (1987). Behavioral treatment and Hague: Mouton.
are the skills that we should be teaching to normal educational and intellectual functioning in
best advance the children’s development of young autistic children. Journal of Consulting and
language and communication?” Clinical Psychology, 55, 3–9.

126 THE AUTISM FILE | www.autismfile.com | info@autismfile.com REPRINTED WITH PERMISSION © THE AUTISM FILE ISSUE 30 2009
Appendix who lacks the appropriate memory skills, the experience is of
A Pre-Verbal Activity Designed to Foster Temporal disconnected, unrelated elements—with the element that is most
Sequencing salient or familiar being the only item that “comes across.” So if
Because the children are typically attracted to the visual world, the material is to mirror auditory perception, the activity must be
they are likely to be responsive to activities in this realm. The visual set up so that each element is retained in memory as the next one
world, however, focuses on space more than on time. To make appears.
that modality useful, we need to reconfigure the material so that The sample activity offered in the section on temporal
it (a) incorporates key features of the auditory-temporal world, sequencing offers these features. Comparable activities can be
while (b) still retaining the features of visual input that the children developed using a variety of other input. For example, the adult
find attractive. To meet that goal, the activities should contain the could create and the child could reproduce
following properties.  a series of actions on objects rather than symbols (e.g., turning
First, the input must be composed of several elements. Single over one object, putting another in a container, tapping
units, whether of pictures or objects, are not sufficient. At the another object, etc.);
same time, the “several elements” must not exceed the perceptual
 a sequence of actions on one’s body (e.g., touching the nose, a
limits that allow it to be perceived as an entity. For example, it
shoulder and a knee), and
is relatively easy to perceive the organization where the pattern
involves sequences of two elements as in the following:  sets of visual objects (e.g., sets of colored blocks that are
shown and then hidden from view).
♣ ♣ ♣ ♣
It is far more difficult in a problem where the sequence has many A Verbal Activity Designed to Foster Sentence Production
more elements such as the following: The key to effective sentence production is noun-verb
combinations. Further, those combinations must be paired with
✽♣✽♣✽♣ the appropriate “little” words (e.g., as in Kids can jump. The kid
is jumping. Etc.). These pairings mean that the children, at the
Work in early mathematics suggests that children—without
outset, must deal with sentences of three to five words in length
applying language—can deal effectively with units up to four—
(the two word noun-verb combinations plus whatever additional
that is, they can perceive four as different from two or three.
little words attach to them). The training the children have received
However, in the absence of language, they cannot perceive four as
in temporal processing will have created the base they need to deal
different from five or six (Dehaene, 1997). So, in terms of number
with sequences of this length.
of elements, the goal in mirroring pre-linguistic processing should
Some relatively simple sentence types that fit the criteria of
be to build up a child’s skill to work up to four units.
sentences that teach “noun-verb” connections are:
Second, the elements must appear in a set order and the material
must be set up so that order must be processed. Though reading here (there) is (are) a (an) X(s),
will not be the vehicle used in the initial work with children, an (as in Here is a cat. There are dogs. Etc.)
example from that area is useful to illustrate the significance of the X(s) (he, she, it, they) can Y;
order. (as in The cats can jump. They can sleep. Etc.)
Consider, for example, the words evil-live, stop-pots, time- the X (he, she, it, they) is/are Ying.
item. They are perceived as totally different entities even though (as in The girl is eating. They are running. Etc.)
the words in each pair contain identical elements. We have become
so accustomed to the significance of sequencing that we are not, As the children master these forms, new forms can be added—so
for a moment, drawn to the fact that the individual letters are that their repertoires steadily build in length and complexity.
identical. The new forms enable a child to become familiar with additional
Similarly, in line with the examples above, via instruction, the concepts that are integral to meaningful messages such as
children could be helped to recognize that the following two sets negation (e.g., The car is not going), past tense (e.g., The boy was
are not equivalent even though they contain identical symbols. running), and future tense (e.g., The girl is going to walk).
With the forms selected, the next step is to determine how to
♣ lead the children to process the information. Despite its widespread
use, it is important to avoid question asking. As discussed above,
♣ the children have great difficulties in differentiating among
Third, memory demands are a pervasive feature of spoken various questions. So questions will simply generate high rates
language. For example, in a sentence such as The boy is sitting, of error. Further, they are almost certain to answer any question
the word The has come and gone before the word boy makes its (whether correctly or not) with one or two word responses,
appearance. The same process occurs with each of the words in the thereby omitting the very words they need to learn. The sentence
sentence. At the same time, when a sentence has been completed, imitation techniques outlined above in the section on the Structural
the experience of the listener with appropriate memory skills is Components of Language offer a more productive approach to
one of hearing a single, coherent idea. Conversely, for the listener developing this vital area of language.

ISSUE 30 2009 REPRINTED WITH PERMISSION © THE AUTISM FILE info@autismfile.com | www.autismfile.com | THE AUTISM FILE 127

Вам также может понравиться