Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 32

Measuring Mental Illness Stigma

by Bruce Q. Link, Lawrence H. Yang, Jo C. Phelan, and Pamela Y. Collins

The effectiveness of efforts designed to address mental illness stigma will rest on our ability to understand stigma processes, the factors that produce and
sustain such processes, and the mechanisms that lead
from stigmatization to harmful consequences.
Critical to such an understanding is our capacity to
observe and measure the essential components of
stigma processes. This article is designed to assist
researchers in selecting or creating measures that can
address critical research questions regarding stigma.
Our conceptualization of stigma processes leads us to
consider components of labeling, stereotyping, cognitive separating, emotional reactions, status loss, and
discrimination. We review 123 empirical articles published between January 1995 and June 2003 that
have sought to assess mental illness stigma and use
these articles to provide a profile of current measurement in this area. From the articles we identify commonly used and promising measures and describe
those measures in more detail so that readers can
decide whether the described measures might be
appropriate for their studies. We end by identifying
gaps in stigma measurement in terms of concepts
covered and populations assessed.
Keywords: Stigma, discrimination, public attitudes, measurement, social rejection.
Schizophrenia Bulletin, 30(3):511-541, 2004.
The Surgeon General's 1999 report on mental health
notes that there is a strong consensus that "our society
no longer can afford to view mental health as separate
and unequal to general health" (p. vii, Executive
Summary, U.S. Department of Health and Human
Services 1999) and that stigma "deprives people of their
dignity and interferes with their full participation in
society" (p. 6, 1999). From the vantage point of the
Surgeon General's report, if we are to improve mental
health and mental health care, we must address stigma.

Send reprint requests to Dr. B.G. Link, Department of Epidemiology,


Columbia University, 722 West 168th Street, Room 1609, New York,
NY 10032; e-mail: BGLl@Columbia.edu.

511

Downloaded from http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016

In keeping with this idea, a diverse set of initiatives


have emerged in the United States and around the world
focused on efforts to combat stigma and discrimination.
But the effectiveness of any such efforts will rest on our
ability to understand stigma processes, the factors that
produce and sustain such processes, and the mechanisms that lead from stigmatization to harmful consequences.
Essential to the scientific understanding of stigma
is our capacity to observe and measure it. The central
purpose of this article is to assist researchers studying
the stigma of mental illness in selecting and creating
the measures they need to conduct their work.
Additionally, we seek to stimulate future research in
neglected areas by identifying gaps in stigma measurement that need further attention. Specifically, we
review 123 articles focused on the stigma of mental illness that were published between January 1995 and
June 2003 and examine the measures employed in
them. This review will inform readers about the
breadth of methods employed in studying stigma, the
kinds of study populations that have been assessed, the
geographic locations of the study sites, and the coverage of stigma concepts that the investigations have
achieved. The next step in our presentation is a detailed
description of promising and frequently used measures
identified in the review. Specifically, we describe the
origin and content of selected measures, discuss evidence for reliability and validity, indicate particular
strengths or shortcomings, and provide key citations
regarding their use. Following this review of measures,
we address three additional topics critical to the measurement of stigma: (1) the use of vignettes, (2) the use
of behavioral measures in experimental and nonexperimental studies, and (3) qualitative assessment as a substitute for or complement to quantitative measures.

Abstract

Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 30, No. 3, 2004

B.G. Linketal.

As a starting point for our review, we return to the


stigma concept and in particular to three theoretical
frameworks relevant to that concept. We do so as a basis
for judging whether current measurement corresponds
closely to theoretical concepts and covers the full range
of stigma-relevant conceptualization. The conceptualizations we return to were all developed by considering not
just mental illnesses but multiple circumstances in which
stigma arises. In reviewing these conceptual frameworks, the reader should be aware that there is debate
about the definition and the utility of the stigma concept
and that none of the conceptualizations should be
viewed as definitive. At the same time, we believe that
close attention to these and other (Fine and Asch 1988;
Crocker et al. 1998; Sayce 2003) conceptual considerations will help refine and expand measurement in this
area.
Goffman. Goffman (1963) is widely cited for his insightful exposition of the stigma concept, and any current
researcher planning to pose and answer research questions
regarding stigma is well advised to return to it. The influence of Goffman's work is evident in the fact that
researchers draw on it with such frequency when seeking
definitions of stigma. Although different definitions can
be taken from Goffman, a very common one is that of an
"attribute that is deeply discrediting" and that reduces the
bearer "from a whole and usual person to a tainted, discounted one" ([Goffman 1963], p. 3). Elsewhere,
Goffman defines stigma as the relationship between an
"attribute and a stereotype" (p. 4). Many of Goffman's
ideas have been carried forward to more recent work on
the conceptualization and measurement of stigma. His
distinctions between the "discredited" and the "discreditable" are evident in subsequent conceptualizations of the
"visibility" dimension of stigma (Jones et al. 1984) and in
attention paid to "secrecy" as a potential coping mechanism for people whose stigmatizing circumstance is discreditable (Conrad and Schneider and Conrad 1980; Link
et al. 1989). Similarly, Goffman's concepts of the "own"
(those similarly stigmatized) and the "wise" (those who
know about and accept the stigma) are evident in efforts
to measure withdrawal as a stigma coping mechanism
(Linketal. 1989).

Link and Phelan's Components of Stigma. Link and


Phelan (2001) developed a conceptualization in response
to criticisms that the stigma concept locates the "problem"
in the individual and tends to focus on cognitive processing of information rather than on the discrimination and
exclusion that a stigmatized person experiences (Sayce
1998). Link and Phelan construct a definition that links
component concepts under a broad umbrella concept they
call stigma:
In our conceptualization, stigma exists when the
following interrelated components converge. In the
first component, people distinguish and label human
differences. In the second, dominant cultural beliefs
link labeled persons to undesirable characteristics
to negative stereotypes. In the third, labeled persons
are placed in distinct categories so as to accomplish
some degree of separation of "us" from "them." In

Jones and Colleagues' Dimensions of Stigma.


Following on Goffman's insights, a second conceptual
framework was developed by Jones et al. (1984) in their

book Social Stigma: The Psychology of Marked


Relationships. Jones et al. use the term "mark" as a
descriptor that encompasses the range of conditions con-

512

Downloaded from http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016

sidered deviant by a society that might initiate the stigmatizing process. Stigma takes place when the mark links the
identified person via attributional processes to undesirable
characteristics that discredit him or her in the eyes of others.
Jones et al. proceed to identify six dimensions of
stigma. Concealability
indicates how obvious or
detectable the characteristic is to others. Concealability
varies depending on the nature of the stigmatizing mark
such that those who are able to conceal their condition
(e.g., people with mental illness) often do so. Course
indicates whether the stigmatizing condition is
reversible over time, with irreversible conditions tending to elicit more negative attitudes from others.
Disruptiveness indicates the extent to which a mark
strains or obstructs interpersonal interactions. For
example, interaction with people with mental illness is
sometimes experienced as disruptive by others because
of a fear of unexpected behavior by individuals with
mental disorders. Aesthetics reflects what is attractive
or pleasing to one's perceptions; when related to stigma,
this dimension concerns the extent to which a mark elicits an instinctive and affective reaction of disgust.
Origin refers to how the condition came into being. In
particular, perceived responsibility for the condition
carries great influence in whether others will respond
with unfavorable views and/or punishment toward the
identified offender. The final dimension, peril, refers to
feelings of danger or threat that the mark induces in
others. Threat in this sense can either refer to a fear of
actual physical danger (e.g., from a communicable disease such as leprosy) or exposure to uncomfortable feelings of vulnerability (e.g., uneasiness or guilt resulting
from watching a disabled person negotiate a flight of
stairs).

Conceptualizing Stigma

Measuring Mental Illness Stigma

Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 30, No. 3, 2004

the fourth, labeled persons experience status loss and


discrimination that lead to unequal outcomes.
Stigmatization is entirely contingent on access to
social, economic and political power that allows the
identification of differentness, the construction of
stereotypes, the separation of labeled persons into
distinct categories and the full execution of disapproval, rejection, exclusion and discrimination. Thus
we apply the term stigma when elements of labeling,
stereotyping, separation, status loss and discrimination co-occur in a power situation that allows them to
unfold. (Link and Phelan 2001, p. 367)
Like Jones et al.'s dimensions, Link and Phelan's
stigma components are useful in identifying the domain of
content that stigma measures should include. Although a
more detailed explication of these components is available
elsewhere (Link and Phelan 2001), we attend to them briefly
here. In addition, we expand the conceptualization to
include a component for emotional responses. It should be
noted that each component is a matter of degreelabels can
be more or less socially salient, they can link to many
stereotypes or just a few, the Linking can be relatively strong
or relatively weak, people can be more or less set apart,
emotional responses can vary from overwhelming to relatively trivial, and the extent of status loss and discrimination
can be relatively great or relatively small. Thus, stigma is a
"matter of degree" and will vary across stigmatizing circumstances such as schizophrenia, depression, obesity, HTV status, short stature, diabetes, and cancer.
Labeling. The vast majority of human differences,
(e.g., finger length, preferred vegetables) are not considered to be socially relevant. However, some differences,
such as skin color and sexual preferences, are currently
awarded a high degree of social salience. Both the selection of salient characteristics and the creation of labels for
them are social achievements that need to be understood
as essential components of stigma.
Stereotyping. In this component, the labeled difference is linked to undesirable characteristics either in the
minds of other persons or the labeled person him- or herself. For example, it may be assumed that a person who
has been hospitalized for mental illness represents a violence risk.
Separating. A third aspect of the stigma process
occurs when social labels connote a separation of "us"
from "them." For example, members of certain ethnic or
national groups (Morone 1997), people with mental illness, or people with a different sexual orientation may be
considered fundamentally different kinds of people from
"us." When this separation is particularly thorough, members of a stigmatized group may accept stereotypes about
themselves and view themselves as fundamentally different from and inferior to other people.

From the vantage point of the person who is stigmatized, emotions of embarrassment, shame, fear, alienation,
or anger are possible. Thomas Scheff (1998) has, for
example, argued that the emotion of shame is central to
stigma and that shaming processes can have powerful and
hurtful consequences for stigmatized persons. For all of
these reasons, we believe that emotional responses and
reactions need to be included in the broad conceptualization of stigma.
Status loss and discrimination. When people are
labeled, set apart, and linked to undesirable characteristics, a rationale is constructed for devaluing, rejecting,
and excluding them. This occurs in several ways. The
most obvious is individual discriminationfor example,
when a person rejects a job application or refuses to rent
an apartment to a person with a mental illness. However,
there are also more subtle mechanisms through which
labeling and stereotyping lead to negative outcomes. One
of these is structural discrimination, in which institutional
practices work to the disadvantage of stigmatized groups,
even in the absence of purposeful discrimination by individuals. For example, schizophrenia receives low levels
of funding for research and treatment relative to other illnesses, and treatment facilities for schizophrenia tend to

513

Downloaded from http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016

Emotional reactions. Underrepresented in the Link


and Phelan (2001) formulations of stigma are the emotional responses it entails. We believe that this underrepresentation needs to be corrected, because emotional
responses are critical to understanding the behavior of
both stigmatizers and people who are recipients of stigmatizing reactions.
From the vantage point of a stigmatizer, the identification of human differences, the linking of those differences
to undesirable attributes, and the separation of the identified
person into a separate "them" group is likely to be associated with emotions of anger, irritation, anxiety, pity, and
fear. The emotions engendered are likely to be important
for at least two reasons. First, an emotional response is
something that can be detected by the person who is stigmatized, thereby providing an important statement about a
stigmatizer's response to them as a person. For example, a
person who feels some combination of pity and anxiety in
the presence of a person with mental illness might modulate his or her voice, speaking softly and in an unnaturally
calm tone, signaling to the person with mental illness that
he or she is being approached from a standpoint of differentness. Second, emotional responses may shape subsequent behavior toward the stigmatized person or group
through processes identified by attribution theory (Weiner
1986). In this formulation, attributions about why a negative event occurred, especially the stigmatized person's perceived control over the cause, has great influence over how
others will think of and behave toward that person.

Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 30, No. 3, 2004

B.G. Link et al.

cal studies and 1 literature review. Thus, in total, our


review included 109 empirical studies and 14 literature
reviews. We do not claim that this review is exhaustive;
one particular weakness of our sampling strategy is that
we may have missed qualitative studies published in
books. Nonetheless, the data set represents a broad
assessment of current stigma measures in use.
Methodology Employed in the Reviewed Studies.
Table 1 shows the number and overall percentage of
each of several design types employed in the 109 studies: (1) survey (nonexperimental), (2) experiment, (3)
qualitative, and (4) literature review. Surveys and
experiments were further classified as "with vignette"
or "without vignette," and qualitative studies were further classified as "with content analysis" or "with interviews or particpant observation." As the table shows,
most of the research involved nonexperimental survey
research (60.1%), with another sizable component
involving survey research with a vignette component
(7.3%). Qualitative research in this review of journals
was relatively rare (13.8%), and nonvignette experiments or quasi-experiments were almost nonexistent
(0.8%).
Study Population in Reviewed Studies. Studies were
coded as to the type of research participant included in
them. They were deemed general population studies if
they employed samples of college students and/or surveys of the general population of adults. Children and
adolescents were subjects 17 years old or younger,
whereas professional groups consisted of either health
providers (e.g., mental health professionals, general
practitioners, medical students) or other professional
groups who interact with people with mental illness in
an official capacity (e.g., police officers, case managers).
People with psychiatric illness were individuals who had
been diagnosed with mental illness or who exhibited
psychiatric symptoms but who have not been formally
diagnosed, and families of people with mental illness
awere individuals related by blood or marriage to people
with psychiatric illnesses.
Of the 109 empirical studies, the most common type
of study population was some segment of the general
population (n - 51; 46.8%), followed by people with
mental illnesses (n = 24; 22.0%), professional groups
(n = 20; 18.3%), and family members of people with
mental illnesses (n = 11; 10.1%). Of interest, only 4
studies (3.7%) assessed stigma in samples of children
and adolescents
a striking fact given the relevance of
this group to understanding the development of attitudes,
beliefs, and behaviors.

Characterization of Current
Measurement
In this article we evaluate current measurement practices
in research on mental illness stigma based on a search
(MEDLINE and PSYCHLIT) completed in July 2003. In
both search engines, the search word "stigma" included
the key terms of "prejudice," "stereotyping," "public
opinion," "attitude to health," or "attitude." Likewise,
the search words "mental disorder" included the key
terms of "diagnosis," "drug therapy," "psychology,"
"education," "epidemiology," "etiology," "genetics,"
"therapy," and "history of." The search spanned from
January 1995 to June 2003. In MEDLINE, 523 articles
were identified with both search terms in the title or
abstract; PSYCHLIT yielded an additional 161 articles.
To be included in the data base, an article had (1) to be
about an identifiable aspect of stigma that pertained
specifically to psychiatric disorders, (2) to be an empirical study or literature review, and (3) to have an English
text and abstract. A total of 95 empirical studies and 13
literature reviews were identified. Examining the reference lists of identified articles and conferring with
researchers in the field yielded an additional 14 empiri-

514

Downloaded from http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016

be located in isolated settings or disadvantaged neighborhoods. Finally, once cultural stereotypes are in place,
they can also have negative consequences that operate
through the stigmatized person him- or herself via
processes specified in modified labeling theory (Link et
al. 1989), stereotype threat (Steele 1997), and stigma
consciousness (Pinel 1999). Thus, there are a variety of
wayssome working through nonlabeled individuals,
some working through labeled individuals, and some
working through societal institutions, some direct and
obvious and others notthrough which labeling, stereotyping, and separating result in poor life outcomes for
stigmatized persons.
Dependence of stigma on power. A unique feature
of Link and Phelan's (2001) conceptualization is the
idea that stigma is entirely dependent on social, economic, and political power. Groups with less power
(e.g., psychiatric patients) may label, stereotype, and
cognitively separate themselves from groups with more
power (e.g., psychiatrists). But in these cases, stigma as
Link and Phelan define it does not exist, because the
potentially stigmatizing groups do not have the social,
cultural, economic, and political power to imbue their
cognitions with serious discriminatory consequences.
Without a reference to power differences, stigma
becomes a much broader and less useful concept that
might be applied to lawyers, politicians, Wall Street
traders, and white people.

Measuring Mental Illness Stigma

Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 30, No. 3, 2004

Table 1. Summary of methodologies


Name
Survey (nonexperimental)

With vignette
Without vignette
Experiment

With vignette

Qualitative

With content analysis

With interviews or
participant observation
Literature review

74

60.1

7.3

65

52.8

The investigators explicitly manipulate an independent


variable and randomly assign the manipulated variable
to study groups.
Experiment with use of a vignette depicting someone
with mental illness.
Experiment without use of a vignette.

20

16.2

19

15.4

0.8

The investigators used any one of a broad band of


qualitative methodologies (e.g., participant observation,
focus groups, life history interviews) in the study's design
and data analysis.
Qualitative study with the use of source material such as
newspapers, books, or television programs as the primary
data for analysis.
Qualitative study with the use of interview or participant
observation as the main study design.

17

13.8

2.4

14

11.4

14

11.4

One or more standardized questions were asked of each


respondent. Responses could be either forced choice
or open-ended. There was no manipulation of the
independent variable by the investigator.
Survey (nonexperimental) with use of a vignette depicting
someone with mental illness.
Survey (nonexperimental) without use of a vignette.

The article was not an empirical study but a summary


of stigma research.

Note.Total n = 123; percentages do not add up to 100 percent because of use of more than one methodology by studies.

Location of Reviewed Studies. Studies were also

voices and had delusional thinking) as a stimulus to gauge


the subjects' attitudes or behaviors. Note that the construct
of behavior is not a component of stigma according to
Link and Phelan, but rather a stimulus that may elicit
stigma processes such as labeling and stereotyping. The
stigma components of labeling, stereotyping, cognitive
separating, emotional reactions, status loss/discrimination (expectations), status loss/discrimination (experiences), structural discrimination,
and behavioral
responses to stigma (described above) are briefly defined
in table 2.
Studies were identified as having measured a
stigma component if one or more of the questions
employed addressed it. As table 2 shows, with one
exception, each of the stigma components was assessed
by more than 10 percent of the studies. We find this
instructive, as it underscores the need for a multicomponent conceptualization of stigma. The single exception is "structural discrimination," which was coded as
present in only two studies. This may be because issues
of structural discrimination are discussed outside of the
framework of stigma and therefore did not make it into
our review. Nevertheless, we see the underrepresentation of this aspect as a dramatic shortcoming in the lit-

coded to reflect the country in which they were conducted or, in the case of literature reviews, written. The
most common location was North America (United
States or Canada; n - 62 or 50.4%). The next most frequent locations were Europe (United Kingdom,
Germany, Greece, Ireland, Italy, Austria, and Sweden; n
= 31 or 25.2%), followed by Asia (Hong Kong,
Singapore, Japan, China, and India; n = 12 or 9.7%), and
Eurasia (Australia and New Zealand; n = 12 or 9.7%).
Relatively few studies or literature reviews were carried
out in the Middle East (Israel and Turkey; n - 4 or 3.2%)
or Africa (Ethiopia; n = 2 or 1.6%). One reason for this
could be that we restricted our review to English language journals. Even so, given the relevance of crosscultural research to understanding stigma processes,
information from Asia and especially Africa seems to be
dramatically underrepresented.
Stigma Components Assessed in Reviewed Studies.
The studies were categorized by the stigma components
identified by Link and Phelan (2001). Behavior was coded
if the study introduced the actual behaviors indicative of
mental illness (e.g., a vignette of a person who heard

515

Downloaded from http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016

Without vignette

Definition

Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 30, No. 3, 2004

B.G. Link et al.

Table 2. Summary of stigma components


Const ruct/stigmacomponent

Definition
The study introduces the actual behaviors indicative of
the presence of mental illness as a stimulus.

28

25.7

Labeling

The study includes the assigning of social significance


to particular characteristics as a variable of study.

20

18.3

Stereotyping

The study incorporates how labeled differences are


linked with negative attributes.

68

62.4

Cognitive separating

The study measures when social labels imply a


fundamental difference ("them") compared with those
without the label ("us").

18

16.5

Emotional reactions

The study measures either the affective reactions of the


stigmatizer toward people with mental illness or the
emotional response of the stigmatized people themselves.

27

24.8

Status loss/discrimination
(expectations)

The study includes expectations or beliefs of how


persons with mental illness are reduced in social status
or face discriminatory treatment from others.

64

58.7

Status loss/discrimination
(experiences)

The study measures actual experiences of how persons with


mental illness are reduced in social status or face
discriminatory treatment from others.

15

13.8

Structural discrimination

The study assesses how institutional practices


disadvantage persons with mental illness.

1.8

Behavioral responses to stigma

The study measures how individuals with mental illness


act in response to societal discrimination, such as
utilizing coping or avoidance strategies.

17

15.6

Note.Total n = 109 (excluding literature reviews); percentages do not add up to 100% because of use of more than one stigma construct by investigations.

readers to find articles that have assessed aspects of


stigma that particularly interest them or that use methodologies they are considering for future studies. For example, an article included in the cell for "Survey
vignette'V'Stereotyping" used a nonexperimental survey
with a vignette to assess whether and to what extent
labeled differences are linked to negative characteristics.

erature on stigma, as the processes involved are likely


major contributors to unequal outcomes for people with
mental illnesses (see Corrigan et al., this issue, for an
in-depth discussion of structural discrimination). The
table is also revealing in that, as expected, measurement of stereotyping (62.4%) and expectations of status loss and discrimination (58.7%) are much more
common than are experiences of status loss and discrimination (13.8%) and behavioral responses (15.6%)
to stigma. Given that the conceptualizations of Link
and Phelan (2001) underemphasized emotional
responses, we were pleasantly surprised to find that
nearly a quarter of the studies assessed this component
nevertheless.
Methodology of Studies Grouped by Stigma
Component Assessed. Tables 3 and 4 are provided to
help readers find examples that show how stigma measures have been used in the literature. The numbers in the
tables refer to citations in the numbered reference list. In
table 3, articles are cross-classified by the methodology
used in the study and by the Link and Phelan stigma
component that is assessed. The table allows interested

516

Study Population Grouped by Stigma Component.


Table 4 classifies the articles by study population and
stigma component. For example, an article included in the
cell for "Families of people with mental illness'V'status
loss/discrimination (experiences)" discusses a study conducted with families of individuals with mental illness
that assesses actual experiences of status loss or discriminatory treatment.

Some Orienting Questions To Be


Asked When Selecting Measures
Among the questions that need to be asked when selecting
appropriate measures are the following:

Downloaded from http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016

Behavior

42,78
25,47,61,68,72,74,
76,87,92, 102, 104,
114, 120, 121
10, 18, 19,22,25,26,
29,31,38,42,47,61,
62.67.68.71.72.74.

86, 101, 111

36,86

2,12,36,52,86,95,
101, 111

Labeling

Cognitive
separating

Stereotyping

12

Behavioral
responses to
stigma

11,33,34,59,62,63,
65,75,85, 113,
115, 122
11, 13, 15,40,58,59,
79,82,85,88,103,
106, 115, 122

3, 20, 21, 23, 28, 51,


53, 54, 57, 66, 80, 99
100

3,28,51

53, 54, 57, 77, 80, 99,


100

1,5,6, 14,20,23,28,

23,28,49,51,53,54,57
66,77,80,99, 100
1 ,5,6, 14,21,23,49,
53,57,77, 100

3 ,5,6, 14,20,21,

Experimental
vignette

Method

45

45

45

Experimental
no vignette

Note.Numbers represent identification numbers for stigma articles found in reference section from 1995 to 2003.

101

Status loss/
discrimination
(experiences)

8-10, 15, 19, 25, 26,


29, 38, 40, 42, 47, 58,
59,61,63,68,72,74,
76, 78, 79, 87-89,

Status loss/
7, 36, 52, 86
discrimination
(expectations)

91-94,96, 102-104,
106-110, 112, 114,
115,120-122

8-10,15,26,31,35,
63, 68, 75, 78, 85, 89,
103-105, 108, 110,
114, 115, 120, 121

Emotional
reactions

76, 78, 83-85, 88, 89,


91,92,94,96, 102104, 109, 110, 116,
120, 121, 123

67, 104, 114

Survey
no vignette

2,7,52,86,95, 101,
111

Survey
vignette

Behavior

Stigma
component

Table 3. Methodology grouped by stigma component

Downloaded from http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016

118

98

Qualitative
content analysis

11,32,70

11,70, 101,113

9,32,70,86,87, 104

9,32,70, 104

70, 86,97,101,104

70, 86,87, 104

32, 70,86, 101

86, 101,104

Qualitativeinterviews or
participant
observation

F
C

5'

ig Mental 111ness Stigm;

Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 30, No. 3, 2004

B.G. Link et al.

Table 4. Study populations grouped by stigma component


Study Population
Stigma
component

Children and
adolescents

General
population

Professional
groups (health
providers/other)

86, 101

2, 3, 5-7, 14, 20,


21,23,28,49,51,
53, 57, 77, 80, 99,
100, 111,114

Labeling

1,86, 101

14,21,42 32 70
5,6, 14,21,23,
42, 49, 53, 57, 77,
78, 100, 111

Cognitive
separating

74,86

25,36,45,61,72,
76,87,92, 114,
120, 121

Stereotyping

14, 18,20,22,42,
1, 74, 86, 101 2,5,6, 10,12,14,
47,52,54,68,71,
19,20,23,25,26,
72,94-96, 102,
28,29,31,36,38,
95-97, 123
42,45,53,57,61,
104
72, 76-78, 80, 83,
84,89,91,92,94,
96,99, 100, 110,
111, 116,120,
121

14,20,21,52,54,
66, 95, 104

Families of
people with
mental Illness

67,95

32

47,68,72, 102,
104

70

70

22, 38, 62, 67, 88,


95-97, 123

70,85,96, 103,
109

Emotional
reactions

3,8, 10,26,28,
31,51,78,89,
108, 110,114,
121

9, 15,68,104

32,35,63,115

70,75,85, 103,
105

Status loss/
74,86
discrimination
(expectations)

3,7,8,10,19,20,
21,23,25,26,28,
29, 36, 38, 42, 45,
51,53,57,61,72,
76, 78, 80, 87, 89,
91,92,94,96,99,
100, 108, 110,
112, 114, 120,
121

9,15,20,21,42,
47, 52, 54, 66, 68,
72,94,96, 102,
104

32, 38, 58, 59, 63,


79, 88, 93, 96,
106, 107, 115,
122

40,70,96, 103,
109

Status loss/
discrimination
(experiences)

34

11,33,59,62,63,
82, 115, 122

65, 70, 75, 82, 85,


113

11, 13,32,58,59,
79,88, 106,
115, 122

40, 70, 82, 85,


103

Behavioral
responses to
stigma

101

12

15

Note.Numbers represent identification numbers for stigma articles found in reference section from 1995 to 2003.

1. What is the research question regarding stigma, and


what are the variables one must measure to
answer the question posed?
2. Is an appropriate measure currently available? If not,
can an existing measure be modified to suit
research needs?

3. Is a candidate measure appropriate for the population I


wish to study? Is the measure likely to be suitable to the social circumstances, culture, age
group, and so forth of the population? Are the
words and phrases used to refer to or describe
people with mental illnesses appropriately sensi-

518

Downloaded from http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016

Behavior

People with
mental illness

Measuring Mental Illness Stigma

Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 30, No. 3, 2004

ness as a group. To our knowledge, Phillips (1963) was


the first to employ a social distance scale in the context of
a vignette experiment designed to assess responses to a
described individual. Since that time, variants of the scale
have been used with great frequency in research on
stigma but particularly in the context of studies employing
vignettes.
Social distance scales tend to show good to excellent
internal-consistency reliability ranging from 0.75 to
greater than 0.90. Evidence regarding the validity of social
distance scales is available mainly with regard to construct
validity (Cronbach and Meehl 1955). Most studies show
that older persons, persons with relatively low educational
levels, and persons who have never known anyone with a
mental illness are more likely to desire social distance than
their younger, more educated counterparts who have had
more extensive contact with people with mental illnesses.
Moreover, individuals who believe that people with mental
illnesses are dangerous are much more likely to desire
social distance from a person who is described as having a
mental illness (Link et al. 1987, 1999; Martin et al. 2000).
These patterns of association would fit expectations of
results if social distance scales measured what they claim
to measure.
There are two main limitations to the validity of
social distance scales. The first of these is social desirability bias. Over the years, public education and anti-stigma
campaigns have made it clear to the public that rejecting a
person simply because he or she has seen a psychiatrist or
been treated in a mental hospital is unenlightened and
wrong. Not wanting to appear heartless or ignorant, people might deny social distancing responses in order to
appear enlightened and caring. To the extent that social
desirability bias is operative, assessments of reported
social distance underestimate the extent of true social distancing sentiments. Furthermore, to the extent that measures of social distancing responses are contaminated by
social desirability bias, one cannot know whether predictors of reported social distance are due to willingness to
report social distance or to true social distancing
responses.
Another limitation arises if one seeks to infer behavioral responses from reported intentions. Although behavioral intentions like social distance items are often good
predictors of behavior, situational circumstances, competing attitudes, and other such factors can intervene so as to
make the association far from perfect. If the target concept is in fact a specific behavior or a set of specific
behaviors, then the relevant behaviors need to be measured directly.

The following review of commonly used measures


will help investigators begin to answer these questions.
The review is organized by considering measures applicable to (1) adult members of the general public, (2) children from the general public, (3) consumers of mental
health services, and (4) family members of consumers.
Following descriptions of measures that fall into these
categories, we also include separate sections focused on
(1) the use of vignettes, (2) measures used in laboratory
experiments, and (3) qualitative approaches in assessing
the stigma of mental illness.

Measures Applicable To Adult


Members of the General Population
Social Distance. One of the most commonly used measures we encountered was that of social distance.
Measures of social distance seek to assess a respondent's
willingness to interact with a target person in different
types of relationships. Measures frequently include items
that differ in the closeness of the association a respondent
is asked to accept or decline.
The concept was initially defined by one of the leaders of the Chicago School, Robert Park (1924), as "the
grades and degrees of understanding and intimacy which
characterize pre-social and social relations generally" (p.
339). The first social distance scale (Bogardus 1925) was
used to describe social distance by race/ethnicity. The
scale's first published use in the area of the mental illnesses came in the context of Cumming and Cumming's
(1957) classic effort to change public attitudes in a town
in Saskatchewan, Canada. Shortly thereafter, Whatley
(1959) administered an eight-item agree-disagree socialdistance scale to 2,001 persons in 17 parishes (counties)
in Louisiana. Much like its original use in the area of
race/ethnicity, Cumming and Cumming and Whatley used
the scale to assess attitudes toward people with mental ill-

Semantic Differential and Related Measures. The


semantic differential is a measurement technique that pro-

519

Downloaded from http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016

tive and respectful? If not, can the measure be


modified to make it appropriate?
4. Is the candidate measure suitable to the methods to be
used? Can it be administered by phone, by paper
and pencil, within the context of a vignette, in an
experimental context, and so on?
5. What is the evidence regarding the measure's reliability
and validity? Is the measure likely to be valid for
its intended use? What is the evidence concerning contamination of the measure by biases such
as social desirability bias?
6. How feasible is the measurement task in comparison to
what might be required for other measures? How
much time will its administration require, and
how much will it cost?

Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 30, No. 3, 2004

B.G. Link et al.

should replicate at least some of the previously used


adjective pairs. We caution, though, that some of the
descriptive phrases used in the original applications are
dated (e.g., "mental patient," "insane person"), and
researchers should consider their appropriateness before
adopting them.
A shortcoming of this measurement approach, shared
by many of those we review, is its vulnerability to socialdesirability bias. However, the consistent differences
observed between ratings of mental illness-related concepts and other concepts suggests that social desirability
bias is not extreme. This may be in part because, when
respondents are asked to rate concepts such as "average
woman" or "average man," they are encouraged to make
implicit comparisons to the mental illness-related concepts that reduce the tendency to rate the mental illness
concepts positively. That is, respondents may experience
the following thought process: "If I'm going to give
'average woman' a '6' on 'warm/cold,' I can't give 'mental patient' more than a '4.'"
Opinions About Mental Illness. This scale was developed in the early 1960s by Cohen and Struening (1962;
Struening and Cohen 1963) and has been used extensively ever since. Cohen and Struening (1962) sought
the "adequate conception and objective measurement of
attitudes toward mental illness" (p. 349) through a multidimensional scale. The Opinions About Mental Illness
Scale (OMI) was developed in two large psychiatric hospitals using the responses of 1,194 hospital workers. By
creating new items and by including items from the
Custodial Mental Illness Ideology Scale, the California
F-Scale, and Nunnally's work on popular conceptions of
mental health, Cohen and Struening constructed a 70item instrument. When items were factor-analyzed, five
dimensions were identified: (1) authoritarianismthat
obedience to authority is critical and that people with
mental illness are an inferior class requiring coercive
handling; (2) benevolencea kindly, paternalistic view
of people with mental illnesses supported by humanism
and religion rather than science; (3) mental hygiene ideologythe idea that mental illness is an illness like any
other and that a rational, professional approach to people
with mental illnesses is crucial for adequate treatment;
(4) social restrictivenessthat the activities of people
with mental illnesses should be restricted in domains
such as marriage, voting, childbearing, jobs, and parenting; and (5) interpersonal etiologythe idea that mental
illnesses arise from interpersonal experiences, particularly the lack of a loving home environment. In a subsequent paper (Struening and Cohen 1963), the original 70
items were reduced to 51 by retaining only items with
mental illness content. Evidence suggesting similar fac-

520

Downloaded from http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016

vides a direct assessment of stereotypingthat is, the


tendency to link a label like "mental patient" with negative attributes. It was developed by Charles E. Osgood et
al. (1957) as a general technique for measuring the psychological meaning that concepts have for people. Jum
Nunnally (Nunnally and Kinross 1958; Nunnally 1961)
and later Olmsted and Durham (1976) and Crisp et al.
(2000) applied the semantic differential to the question
of public conceptions of people with mental illnesses
and the professionals who treat them. The semantic differential presents respondents with labels, or concepts
(e.g., "mental patient"), and asks them to evaluate the
extent to which those labels are associated with various
characteristics. Specifically, respondents are asked to
rate the concept on a number of 7-point rating scales,
each bounded by a pair of polar adjectives such as
"insinceresincere" and "coldwarm." In addition to
the concepts of interest (e.g., "mental patient"), respondents rate one or more additional concepts (e.g., "average person" or "me") using the identical response scales
to provide a context or point of comparison for evaluations of the target concepts. See Nunnally (1961),
Olmsted and Durham (1976), and Crisp et al. (2000) for
the stimulus concepts and adjective pairs they employed.
Nunnally provides instructions and sample pages from
his instrument.
The semantic differential has several features to
recommend it. First, it provides a direct measure of
stereotyping. Regarding reliability, Olmsted and
Durham (1976) found very high correlations among ratings on their 14 adjective pairs (r - 0.95 to 0.99
depending on the stimulus concept). Regarding validity,
although neither Nunnally (1961) nor Olmsted and
Durham (1976) reported the statistical significance of
the differences, evidence for construct validity is provided by the finding that concepts with mental illness
labels (e.g., neurotic man, insane woman, mental
patient) were rated substantially more negatively (e.g.,
more worthless, dirty, dangerous, cold, unpredictable,
and insincere) than those without such labels.
Furthermore, Nunnally found that a "neurotic" individual was rated less negatively on the evaluative dimension but also as less potent than an "insane" individual
or a "mental patient," and Olmsted and Durham found
that an "ex-mental patient" was evaluated more positively than a "mental patient." Similar differences in
ratings applied to mentally ill and non-mentally ill persons were found by Crisp et al. (2000). Finally, because
the semantic differential is a measurement approach
rather than a specific measure, it allows for flexibility in
applying it to the concepts and evaluative dimensions of
interest to the researcher. If researchers want to maintain comparability to other studies, however, they

Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 30, No. 3, 2004

Measuring Mental Illness Stigma

developed. The social policy of deinstitutionalization and


the increased salience of genetic factors in the etiology of
mental illnesses are examples of domains that might be
represented in a new formulation of the OMI.
Community Attitudes Toward the Mentally 111. Partly
because the OMI did not cover issues of deinstitutionalization and the community treatment of people with mental illness, Taylor et al. (1979) and Taylor and Dear (1981)
created the Community Attitudes Toward the Mentally 111
(CAMI). The measure they developed used the OMI as a
conceptual basis, seeking to regenerate three of five OMI
factors (authoritarianism, benevolence, and social restrictiveness) and to create a fourth factor assessing community mental health ideology. The scale includes 40 items,
for each one of the proposed factors. The four a priori
scales ranged in internal consistency reliability from 0.68
for authoritarianism to 0.88 for community mental health
ideology, with benevolence (0.76) and social restrictiveness (0.80) lying in between. Taylor and Dear also show
that the scales correlate highly with each other and as
expected with demographic variables such as age, gender,
occupational status, and household income. The major
strength of the CAMI is its exploration of attitudes toward
community mental health treatment facilities.
Deinstitutionalization represents a major new development in the care and management of people with mental
illnesses, and it is very important to have this area
included in attitudinal assessments. The wording of the 40
items in the CAMI and the response format is available in
Taylor and Dear (1981). Citations to the use of the measure should also include an earlier paper by Taylor et al.
(1979), although this article does not include the item
wording.
Attributional Measures. Measurement focused on a
subject's emotional reactions (e.g., pity, anger), a subject's
behavioral intentions, and the perceived controllability of
a stigmatizing condition stems from attribution theory
(Weiner 1986). According to attribution theory, the target's perceived responsibility for the stigmatizing circumstance predicts either anger and punishing actions (if
believed to be controllable) or pity and helping behaviors
toward the target (if believed to be uncontrollable).
Causes that are seen as changeable over time (unstable)
generate conceptions that recovery from the condition is
possible, whereas causes that are seen as unchanging (stable) elicit beliefs that the condition is immutable.
Corrigan (2000) provided a conceptual review of two
social cognitive models based on Weiner's work. He first
expanded upon Weiner et al.'s (1988) approach by suggesting specific ways in which attribution theory's causal
pathway of cognition-emotion-behavior could be inte-

521

Downloaded from http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016

tor structures among personnel of three newly sampled


hospitals was obtained, and explicit scoring procedures
for the five factors were presented. One of the factors,
mental hygiene ideology, showed less than adequate
internal consistency in the three hospitals, with coefficients ranging from 0.29 to 0.39. However, authoritarianism (0.77-0.80), benevolence (0.70-0.73), restrictiveness (0.71-0.77), and interpersonal etiology (0.65-0.66)
had adequate internal consistency. It is this 51-item version of the OMI that has been and continues to be used
in research today (Struening and Cohen 1963).
The OMI has several important strengths that have
undoubtedly contributed to its extensive use for such a
long time and in so many different cultural settings. One
such strength becomes apparent when its items are
closely scrutinized and compared with items in other
scales in this area of research. The items tend to have a
poignancy and complexity aimed at supplying a stimulus
that affects the respondent and provides something
potent to react to. For example, one item reads, "Even
though patients in mental hospitals behave in funny
ways, it is wrong to laugh about them," and another,
"All patients in mental hospitals should be prevented
from having children by a painless operation."
A second advantage of the OMI is its breadth of
coverage of salient issues. It includes items assessing
many of the components that Jones et al. and Link and
Phelan identify. The linking of labels (mental hospital
patient) to stereotypes is prominent, for example, in
items such as "People who are mentally ill let their emotions control them: normal people think things out" and
"People who were once patients in mental hospitals are
no more dangerous than the average citizen." The notion
of separation into "us" and "them" is evident in items
such as "A heart patient has just one thing wrong with
him, while a mentally ill person is completely different
from other people" and "There is something about mental patients that makes it easy to tell them from normal
people." In addition, status loss ('To become a patient in
a mental hospital is to become a failure in life") and
inclination to discriminate ("Anyone who is in a hospital
for mental illness should not be allowed to vote") are
also prominent in the OMI, as are items reflecting Jones
et al.'s (1984) dimensions of origins, course, peril, and
disruptiveness.
A third, as yet unrealized, advantage of this measure
is its long history and thus the possibility of assessing
changes in attitudes over time. Although it was never part
of a nationwide study, the OMI has been used in so many
populations for so long that useful comparisons could be
made that would allow an opportunity to assess whether
and to what extent attitudes have changed. A disadvantage
of the OMI is that new issues have arisen since it was

Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 30, No. 3, 2004

B.G. Link et al.

form real-world helping behaviors (e.g., to sign a petition


protesting unfair depiction of mental illness in the media).
Finally, Corrigan et al. (this issue) conducted an experiment that showed that contact with a person with mental
illness led to significant changes in perceived personal
responsibility and dangerousness and that these benefits
remained at follow-up 1 week later. Hence, it appears that
subscales measured by Corrigan's AQ relate to each other
and to a highly related construct (i.e., previous exposure
to people with mental illness) in ways that attribution theory would lead us to expect.
Another instrument that assesses causal attributions
is the Revised Causal Dimension Scale (CDSII,
McAuley et al. 1992; CDS, Russell 1982). The CDSII
has a locus of causality scale, which determines whether
the cause is located within or is external to the person
making the attribution, and a stability dimension, which
refers to whether the cause is fixed or changeable over
time. Three items with anchoring statements, rated on a
nine-point Likert scale, compose each of the four attributional domains in the CDSII: Locus of causality
(internality), stability, external control, and personal
control. Alpha for each subscale ranged from 0.60 to
0.92 across four studies, with the following averages:
locus of causality, 0.67; stability, 0.67; personal control,
0.79; and external control, 0.82. When used to measure
stigma, the CDSII has been modified to assess an
observer's attributions about a stigmatized individual
(Boisvert and Faust 1999). A copy of the AQ can be
found in Corrigan (2003), while the CDSII can be found
in McAuley et al. (1992).
Key Informant Questionnaire and EMIC. The structured Key Informant Questionnaire (Wig et al. 1980;
Alem et al. 1999) assesses the way mental disorders are
perceived in the community, how people react to disorders, and whom people turn to for treatment when faced
with a mental disorder. It was administered to key informants (people whose perceptions were based on a wide
range of problems in the community and whose views
would be important in decision making) in India, Sudan,
and the Philippines. Seven vignettes depicting mental
retardation, epilepsy, acute psychosis, mania, depressive
psychosis, schizophrenia, major depression, and neurotic
depression were presented to respondents. Subjects were
asked to rate the gravity (seriousness or harmfulness),
prognosis, marriage prospects (likelihood that the condition would impair chances of marriage), ability to live at
home, and ability to work/study of the individual in each
vignette on a three-point (e.g., not seriousmost serious)
scale.
The pattern of findings in the two studies that have
used this measure provides evidence for the construct

522

Downloaded from http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016

grated with anti-stigma change strategies. Second,


Corrigan proposed that signals of mental illness (e.g.,
"that person muttering to himself is crazy") yield stereotypes ("crazy people are unpredictable") that lead to
behavioral reactions, including discrimination ("I am not
going to let that person talk to me"). In his formulation,
stereotypes of people with mental illness are cognitive
knowledge structures that mediate behavioral reactions.
Weiner et al.'s (1988) original attribution measure
included eight questions about ten illnesses. These eight
questions consisted of: (1) three questions using ninepoint scales to assess the responsibility, blame, and
changeability of each illness; and; (2) five questions about
the subject's liking, pity, anger, charitable donations, and
personal assistance toward each of the ten conditions.
Three separate indexescontrollability, positive emotions, and a helping variablewere created by adding
individual item scores together, and internal consistency
was reported as good for each of these indexes (see
Weiner et al. 1988 for details).
The Attribution Questionnaire (AQ) was developed
by Corrigan (2003) to measure key constructs defined in
his social cognitive models. He used Weiner et al.'s
(1988) measure and 11 questions from Reisenzein (1986)
that measured controllability, sympathy, anger, and helping behavior. Corrigan's AQ consists of 21 items measuring six constructs, using a nine-point response scale (1 =
not at all, 9 = very much). In Corrigan's study, the AQ
was administered following a vignette to assess personal
responsibility (three items, alpha = 0.70), pity (three
items, alpha = 0.74), anger (three items, alpha = 0.89),
fear (four items, alpha = 0.96), helping/avaidant behavior
(four items, alpha = 0.88), and coercion-segregation (four
items, alpha = 0.89).
The literature shows some evidence for construct
validity of the attribution questionnaires. Weiner et al.'s
measure (1988) showed the expected finding that subjects rated causes of five mental-behavioral stigmas
(Vietnam War syndrome, AIDS, child abuse, drug abuse,
and obesity) as more controllable than five physically
based stigmas (Alzheimer's disease, blindness, cancer,
heart disease, and paraplegia). Subjects also endorsed
less liking, pity, and assistance and more anger toward
the mental-behavioral stigmas than the physically based
ones.
Corrigan's (2003) measures also show evidence of
construct validity by correlating with relevant concepts in
an anticipated manner. For example, Corrigan has shown
that perceived controllability is related to avoidance, withholding help, and the endorsement of support for coercive
treatment (Corrigan 2003). In another study, Corrigan et
al. (1999) found that subjects who rated people with mental illness as less blameworthy were more likely to per-

Measuring Mental Illness Stigma

Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 30, No. 3, 2004

as individuals whose opinions need not be taken seriously.


The scale also includes items that assess perceived discrimination by most people in jobs, friendships, and
romantic relationships. The items were originally administered in a paper-and-pencil questionnaire in a six-point
Likert format.
The scale has been used mainly among people in
treatment for mental illnesses but can be administered to
members of the general public. In fact, information from
the general public is crucial to testing modified labeling
theory. The theory predicts that perceived devaluation-discrimination should have no impact on social or psychological functioning in people who have never been officially labeled with mental illness. Absent the label, the
individual may believe that others will reject psychiatric
patients, but this belief has no personal relevance.
Consistent with this prediction, when the scale is administered to persons who have never been officially labeled,
there are no significant associations between scale scores
and earnings, unemployment, social network ties, or psychological demoralization (Link 1987).

Emotional Reaction To Mental Illness Scale.


Angermeyer and Matschinger (1996) developed a scale to
measure emotional reactions toward people with mental
illnesses. In two representative surveys of adults in
Germany, the investigators administered two vignette
descriptions (one describing schizophrenia and the other
depression). In the first survey (1990), the measure consisted of 18 five-point Likert scale items, with each item
assessing a single emotional response. Factor analysis
yielded three dimensions: (1) aggressive emotions (e.g.,
anger, irritation); (2) prosocial reactions (desire to help,
sympathy); and (3) feelings of anxiety (uneasiness, fear).
The final version of the instrument included the four items
that loaded highest on each factor. This instrument's key
strengths are its assessment of affective experiences of the
stigmatizer, which have previously been underassessed;
its demonstrated reliability; and its validity in demonstrating a predicted pattern of relationships with the construct
of previous contact with mentally ill people. A copy of the
original measure can be found in Angermeyer and
Matschinger (1996).

Measures Applicable To Children in


the General Population
Our data base included four papers based on child or adolescent research participants, one of which developed
measures tailored specifically to children. Adler and Wahl
(1998) assessed third graders' conceptions of people with
mental illness with two methods. First, children were
shown pictures of a man labeled as "a mentally ill person," "a physically disabled person," or "a regular grownup." The children were asked to tell stories about the man,
and the stories were rated on a number of known negative
attributions about people with mental illnesses (e.g., dangerous, unpredictable). Second, children were asked a
series of questions about what the man would be likely to
do, such as "dress nicely," "have a lot of friends," or "yell
a lot." While no reliability data were reported, some evidence for the validity of the coded-story approach is
available in the expected finding of more globally negative statements about the person with mental illness.
Concerning the direct questions, results did not differ for
the mentally ill man, the physically disabled man, and the
regular grown-up. This may indicate that third graders
have a general idea that people with mental illness are not
as good as other people but have not developed specific
negative stereotypes. The 18 direct questions can be found
in Adler and Wahl (1998). Readers interested in children's
views of mental illness are also directed to Wahl's (2002)
review of this literature, which describes additional measures adapted for use with children.

Perceived Devaluation-DiscriminationGeneral
Public Link (1987) constructed a perceived devaluationdiscrimination measure to test hypotheses associated with
the "modified labeling theory." The measure assesses a
respondent's perception of what most other people
believea key feature of modified labeling theory. Link
(1987; Link et al. 1989, 1991, 1997, 2001) developed a
12-item perceived devaluation-discrimination measure
that asks respondents the extent to which they agree or
disagree with statements indicating that most people
devalue current or former psychiatric patients by seeing
them as failures, as less intelligent than other persons, or

523

Downloaded from http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016

validity of the vignette section and scales. A weakness of


this measure is the absence of reliability data for the
vignette questions. However, one notable strength of these
vignettes and attached questions is their apparent adaptability to different cultures; "skeleton" vignettes describing basic features of each illness can be fleshed out to
accommodate different cultural settings using local language and expressions.
A similar instrument called the EMIC (Chowdhury et
al. 2000) was developed and used in India. The EMIC
uses vignettes and a scale measuring perceived stigma
based on Goffman's (1963) conception of "spoiled identity." Internal consistency of the EMIC stigma scale was
reported as 0.66 and 0.76 when administered to the general public and health care professionals, respectively. The
original Key Informant Questionnaire can be found in
Wig et al. (1980), and the EMIC is presented in
Chowdhury et al. (2000).

Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 30, No. 3, 2004

B.G. Link et al.

Mental Health Consumers' Experience of Stigma.


The most comprehensive measure of mental health consumers' experience of stigma is Wahl's (1999) selfadministered questionnaire. The questionnaire includes
nine statements concerning stigma experiences such as
worrying that others will view one unfavorably, being
treated as less competent, being advised to lower one's
expectations, and hearing others say unfavorable things
about oneself. There are 12 items concerning discrimination experiences such as being denied a job, a passport,
educational opportunities, housing, or health insurance
coverage when consumer status was revealed, as well as
avoiding indicating consumer status on written applications for fear of discrimination. Items are presented as
statements that are responded to as occurring "never,"
"seldom," "sometimes," "often," or "very often." Factor
analysis indicated that, of four factors accounting for 50
percent of the variance, the first two factors represented
stigma and discrimination, respectively (Mann and Wahl
2003). Evidence for validity of the measure is provided
by Zaveri et al. (2003), who reported that (using an
appropriately modified version of the measure), people
with epilepsy report experiences of stigma and discrimination, but to a lesser extent than people with mental illnesses.
Dickerson et al. (2002) adapted the instrument for a
sample of outpatients with schizophrenia by supplementing the term "consumers" with the terms "persons with
mental illness," "persons who have a psychiatric disorder," and "persons who use psychiatric services," because
pilot interviews indicated that many respondents were not
familiar with the use of the term "consumers" to refer to
consumers of mental health services.
Measures for Consumers Associated with Modified
Labeling Theory. Previously, we described Link's
(1987) perceived devaluation-discrimination scale.
According to modified labeling theory, one's perception
of how most people treat a person who is officially
labeled as having a mental illness becomes personally relevant when a person develops a mental illness and is officially labeled. Moreover, anticipating status loss and discrimination, a person may seek to avoid such negative
outcomes by adopting coping orientations such as keeping
a history of treatment a secret, educating others so as to
ward off stereotypes, or withdrawing to a group of people
who have been similarly labeled or who know about the
label and can be trusted. While such coping orientations
can protect a person from rejection, they may also carry
costs. To evaluate whether people do indeed adopt such

524

Downloaded from http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016

coping orientations and to assess the consequences of


doing so, Link et al. (1989) developed measures of the
coping orientations of secrecy, education, and withdrawal.
More recently, Link et al. (2002) expanded the assessment
of coping orientations to include distancing and challenging as well as stigma-related feelings, of being misunderstood by others and different and ashamed (see below).
The significance of these new measures for modified
labeling theory is that they provide a more fully elaborated set of empirical assessments that can be used to test
that theory.
Measures of rejection and perceptions of rejection. Devaluation-discrimination is the 12-item measure whose content and scoring are described in the section on measures for the general public. In the measure's
use among consumers of mental health services, internal-consistency reliability ranges from 0.82 (Link et al.
1991) to 0.86 (Link et al. 2001). Evidence for validity
comes mainly from construct validity. Theory predicts
that the measure will be related to a wide variety of outcomes among people with mental illnesses, and to date
evidence exists linking the measure to earned income
(Link 1987), employment status (Link 1987), social support networks (Link et al. 1989; Perlick et al. 2001),
demoralization (Link 1987), quality of life (Rosenfield
1997), depressive symptoms (Link et al. 1997), help
seeking (Sirey et al. 2001), and self -esteem (Link et al.
2001).
Link et al. (1997) also constructed a 12-item measure
(alpha = 0.80) of rejection experiences, for use among
dually diagnosed persons with serious mental illness and
substance abuse. The scale was a strong predictor of
depressive symptoms in this longitudinal study. However,
subsequent to the creation of this scale, Wahl (1999)
developed a more complete set of items measuring rejection/discrimination experiences (see above), and we now
recommend the use of that scale over the one developed
by Link et al.
Measures of coping orientations. Secrecy assesses
the extent to which participants endorse concealment as a
means of avoiding rejection. An early scale constructed
by Link et al. (1989) included five items (alpha = 0.71).
More recently, Link et al. (2002) revised and expanded
the scale to include nine items (alpha = 0.84) with new
items such as, "You encourage other members of your
family to keep your mental illness a secret."
Withdrawal assesses the extent to which people
endorse withdrawal or avoidance as a means of self-protection from potential rejection. The idea stems from the
work of Goffman (1963) and focuses on the tendency to
limit social interaction to those who know about and tend
to accept one's stigmatized circumstance. An original
seven-item version developed by Link et al. (1989)

Measures Applicable To
Patients/Consumers

Measuring Mental Illness Stigma

Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 30, No. 3, 20O4

525

make them feel set apart, different from other people, and
ashamed. Once again, this scale correlates as expected
with other scales: perceived devaluation-discrimination
0.48, rejection experiences 0.28, and withdrawal 0.48. In
addition, feeling different and ashamed is more strongly
correlated with self-esteem (0.50) and depressive symptoms (0.51) than any other stigma scale mentioned above.
These correlations are consistent with the possibility that
other stigma variables have their effect on self-esteem and
feelings of depression through feelings of being different
and ashamed.
The items in some of the measures associated with
modified labeling theory (perceived devaluation-discrimination, secrecy, withdrawal, and education) are published
in the American Sociological Review (Link et al. 1989).
However, all of the items for all of the newly developed
and revised scales are available in the spring 2002 issue of
Psychiatric Rehabilitation Skills (Link et al. 2002).
Measurement Biases in Self-Report Measures of
Stigma Components. As with other measurement
approaches, there are potential biases associated with
using self-report measures of stigma experiences or
stigma feelings. For example, Major et al. (2002) have
indicated that measures of neuroticism can be associated
with the perception of being stigmatized and with measures of well-being, thereby bringing into question any
causal link between stigma and well-being. Similarly, a
person who is unemployed, isolated, or beset by low selfesteem may seek to explain his or her disadvantaged status by invoking stigma. In such a scenario, levels of measured stigma do not cause bad outcomes but are instead
consequences of those outcomes. There is no single prescription for avoiding such biases. Instead, one needs to
carefully consider these possibilities as threats to validity
and seek ways of addressing them. For example, one
might include a measure of neuroticism (or other such
confound) in one's measurement protocol (Major et al.
2002), employ multiple approaches to assessing stigma
rather than relying on self-report alone, or craft measurement and design features that help address potential confounds such as these (e.g., Link 1987).

Stigma Experienced by Family


Caregivers
Greenberg et al. (1993) present new measures of subjective burden experienced by parents of people with mental
illness, including stigma. The stigma measure was
adapted from earlier work by Freeman and Simmons
(1963) and consists of the mean of seven items asking the
extent to which family members avoid having family and

Downloaded from http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016

(alpha - 0.67) was recently modified and expanded to a


nine-item scale (alpha = 0.70) (Link et al. 2002).
Educating measures participants' orientation to educating others as a means of reducing the possibility of
rejection. The idea stems from the work of Schneider and
Conrad (1980) and their ideas about "preventive telling."
An earlier five-item version developed by Link et al.
(1989) (alpha = 0.71) was recently revised to yield a new
three-item scale (alpha = 0.67).
Challenging measures people's orientations to confronting prejudice and discrimination. Link et al. (2002)
created a five-item scale (alpha = 0.72) to assess the
extent to which participants are likely to point out stigmatizing behavior when it occurs, disagree with people who
make stigmatizing statements, and so on.
Distancing is a newly created three-item scale (Link
et al. 2002) (alpha = 0.63) assessing the extent to which
people cope with stigma by cognitively distancing themselves from the stigmatized group, by indicating that their
problems are very different from those of other people
with mental illness and that they have little in common
with such people.
Evidence for the validity of the coping orientation
scales varies from scale to scale. Face validity is evident
for most of the items, as they appear to assess the construct the scale purports to measure. Some evidence for
the construct validity of the scales comes in a pattern of
expected correlations between the various measures, as
reported in Link et al. (2002). Additional evidence for the
construct validity of the withdrawal scale exists in Link et
al. (1989) and subsequently Perlick et al.'s (2001) findings
that withdrawal is associated with a constricted social
support network outside the household.
Stigma-related Feelings. A missing component in
the measurement of stigma-related processes associated
with modified labeling theory has been an assessment of
the feelings that stigma creates in the people exposed to it.
As a beginning effort to fill this gap, Link et al. (2002)
introduced two new scalesfeeling misunderstood and
feeling different and ashamed.
Feeling misunderstood assesses the extent to which
people feel that their experience of mental illness has
been misunderstood by others (alpha = 0.62). Example
items are as follows: "Most people cannot understand
what it is like to be a patient in a mental hospital" and
"Most people have no idea what it is like to experience a
serious mental illness." The scale correlates with other
scales created by Link et al. (2002) in expected ways:
0.34 with perceived devaluation-discrimination, 0.38 with
rejection experiences, and 0.24 with withdrawal.
Feeling different and ashamed is a four-item scale
(alpha 0.70) measuring the extent to which people's
experiences of mental illness and mental hospitalization

Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 30, No. 3, 2004

B.G. Link et al.

Use of Vignettes in Research on Stigma


One of the most common methodological approaches
employed in the study of the stigma of mental illness is
the vignette. The approach was first used in this context
by Shirley Star, in a nationwide study of public attitudes
toward mental illnesses (Star 1955). Star constructed
vignettes depicting paranoid schizophrenia, simple schizophrenia, alcoholism, anxiety neurosis, juvenile character
disorder, and compulsive phobia and administered them to
over 3,000 residents of the United States in 1950. After
each vignette, Star inserted queries about the described
person to ascertain the respondent's judgment as to how
serious the condition was and whether or not it was some
kind of mental illness.
An important turning point in the use of vignettes
came when Derek Phillips (1963) employed the Star
vignettes in a survey experiment. Phillips varied five
vignette disorders (four from Star and a "normal"

526

Downloaded from http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016

man) and five help sources (no help source, clergy,


physician, psychiatrist, and mental hospital) in a classic Greco-Latin Square experimental design. Phillips'
study showed that help source significantly influenced
the desire for social distance, thereby indicating that
rejection might be a consequence of seeking mental
health treatment. From a measurement and methods
point of view, Phillips' innovation ensured the future
use of vignettes by integrating their use with the
experimental method. Subsequently, researchers have
randomly varied symptoms, behaviors, labels, causal
attributions, social statuses (gender, race/ethnicity,
education), and other characteristics in vignette studies.
But the Star vignettes per se are rarely used any more.
DSM-III (and subsequently DSM-III-R and DSM-IV)
brought new and far more explicit criteria for diagnosing
mental disorders that rendered the Star vignettes out-ofdate as representations of mental illnesses as conceptualized by mental health professionals. Researchers at
Indiana and Columbia Universities collaborated to produce the Mac Arthur Mental Health Module of the 1996
General Social Survey, which included four DSM-IV
vignettes: alcoholism, major depression, schizophrenia,
and cocaine abuse. An additional vignette describing a
"troubled person" who did not meet criteria for mental
disorder was constructed to serve as a point of comparison for the vignettes depicting disorders. The vignettes
were then administered to a nationally representative
sample of 1,444 respondents in an in-person interview.
The exact wording of the vignettes employed in the survey is available in Link et al. (1999) and Pescosolido et
al. (2000). Vignettes depicting disorders according to the
International Classification of Diseases have also been
developed by Angermeyer and Matschingerner (1997) to
study public attitudes in Germany and by Jorm et al.
(1997) to study mental health literacy in Australia.
A vignette, then, is a form of stimulus that
researchers can ask people to react to. Following a
vignette description, many, many types of measures can
be applied. Examples of questions investigators have
asked following vignettes can be accessed by reviewing
recent vignette-based studies (table 3). In addition, a
report by Pescosolido et al. (2000) contains all of the
vignette-based questions employed in the 1996 General
Social Survey.
Ever since Star and Phillips, vignettes have enjoyed
a very prominent position in research on the stigma of
mental illness. There are two major reasons for this
popularity. First, vignettes allow the researcher to present a more elaborate stimulus to respondents than is
afforded in measurement approaches that simply ask
people about "mental illness," a "psychiatric hospital

friends over or avoided telling others about their child's


illness for fear of what others may think of them (alpha =
0.74). Evidence for validity comes from the fact that the
scale is significantly correlated with co-residence with the
ill child (r = 0.27), greater psychiatric symptomatology of
the child (r = 0.26), and a 27-item measure of "objective
burden" consisting of stressful life events for the family
(r = 0.23).
Szmukler et al.'s (1996) Experience of Caregiving
Inventory (ECI) includes a five-item stigma scale (alpha
= 0.82) assessing the frequency of distress over covering
up the relative's illness, feeling unable to tell anyone
about the illness or have visitors to one's home, feeling
stigma, and worrying about how to explain the illness to
others. Martens and Addington (2001) found that the
stigma scale of the ECI was strongly negatively correlated (-0.49; p < 0.01) with the psychological well-being
of a sample of relatives of people with mental illness as
assessed with the General Health Questionnaire (Bech
1993). Item wording can be found in Szmukler et al.
(1996).
Most recently, Struening et al. (2001) developed a
seven-item scale answered on a four-point scale (strongly
agree to strongly disagree) to estimate the extent to which
family caregivers believe that most people devalue families that include a person with a serious mental illness.
The seven-item scale has an alpha of 0.71 in a sample of
family members of people with schizophrenia and
schizoaffective disorders (n = 180) and 0.77 in a sample
{n = 281) of family members of people with bipolar I and
II disorders. Item wording can be found in Struening et al.
(2001).

Measuring Mental Illness Stigma

Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 30, No. 3, 2004

of themselves "stigma" measures, and they are not, as a


result, reflected in a listing of such measures. They are
brought into the domain of stigma research when labeling, stereotyping, and/or being set apart are shown to
produce the good or the bad outcome assessed. This can
occur either through direct discrimination, through structural discrimination, or via processes that operate
through the stigmatized person him- or herself (Link and
Phelan 2001). The task for stigma researchers, then, is to
select outcome measures from the vast array of possible
good or bad outcomes extant in our society. A good
selection requires a creative match of an outcome measure to the theory being tested. In the remainder of this
section, we review behavioral measures that have been
incorporated into experimental studies and then turn to
non-experimental studies that have measured discrimination.
Laboratory Experiments and Behavioral Measures
Indicating Discrimination. As shown in table 1, most of
the empirical studies we located employed nonexperimental survey methods. However, the laboratory experiment,
in which the behavior of research participants is observed
and measured, has an important tradition in stigma
research, especially in the field of psychology. This
research has examined phenomena such as the disruptive
effect of labeling on social interactions and the impact of
labels on the tendency to discriminate, punish, or help.
Researchers interested in the stigma attached to mental illness may also draw on methods and measures employed
in experimental studies of other stigmatized characteristics such as minority racial status and low socioeconomic
status.
Experiments have demonstrated the pernicious
effects that the introduction of a mental illness label can
have on social interaction. In one study, Sibicky and
Dovidio (1986) randomly assigned college students to be
unwittingly labeled as a psychotherapy client or as an
introductory psychology student. These "target" students
then interacted with a "perceiver" student, to whom the
label had been disclosed. When the target was labeled as a
psychotherapy client, blind observers rated the perceiver
as behaving more negatively (i.e., unenthusiastically, cruelly, artificially) toward the target and rated the target as
behaving in a less socially desirable manner (see also
Farina et al. 1971). Other experiments have measured the
effect of labeling on discrimination in housing (Page
1977) and punishment via "shocks" in learning situations
(the shocks were not actually administered).
Experimental studies addressing other forms of
stigma and discrimination can also be profitably examined
by mental illness-stigma researchers. For example,
according to Steele and Aronson's (1995) concept of

Measurement of Stigma-Relevant
Behavior
The final component of the Link and Phelan (2001) conceptualization of stigma is status loss and discrimination. Clearly, assessing either status loss or discrimination involves the measurement of behavior. Although
most of the measures described above imply that discrimination is a likely consequence of the attitude,
belief, or behavioral intention expressed, very few aim
to assess discriminatory behavior directly. Why? The
reason is that when we seek to measure discrimination,
our intent is to determine whether and to what extent
people with mental illnesses are denied access to the
good things our society affords and are differentially
exposed to the bad things it confers. The measures we
use to assess discrimination can include employment
status; social network ties; access to medical treatment;
hiring decisions; influence in group situations; or being
shunned, put down, or ignored. None of these are in and

527

Downloaded from http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016

patient," or a "mental health consumer." Second,


vignettes allow the use of random assignment and bring
the power of the experimental method to hypothesis
testing. Furthermore, because vignettes can be used in
survey research, vignette experiments can be administered to randomly selected general population samples
and therein achieve somewhat better external validity
than is typical of many laboratory experiments that
employ college students as subjects.
These advantages have ensured and will continue to
ensure the use of vignettes in future research. At the
same time, it is important to recognize that vignettes are
hypothetical and abstracted from "real life" experience.
For example, a member of the public rarely encounters a
vignette-type situation in which a person systematically
displays all the requisite symptoms for a diagnosis of
schizophrenia. Moreover, the respondent is not in the
presence of a real person, is not gleaning information
from appearance and other nonverbal cues, and cannot
assess the described person's responses to initial gestures that might affect reactions in "real" situations. In
the absence of information of this sort, people rely on
cognitive schemas or scripts to answer questions about
vignettes that may not map on to their actual behavior in
real situations. Although vignettes have drawbacks, we
believe the best approach to developing future knowledge in this area will be built on information derived
from multiple methodological approaches with different
strengths and weaknesses. Vignette-based research can
be one important component of such a multimethod
approach.

Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 30, No. 3, 2004

B.G. Link et al.

528

Downloaded from http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016

Observational Studies and Behavioral Measures


Indicating Discrimination. Observational studies do
not enjoy the power of the experimental method but are
better able to directly measure real-world outcomes. One
nonexperimental approach involves asking consumers of
mental health services about their experiences of discrimination. In Wahl's measure (described above) consumers
are asked how often discrimination experiences (being
denied a job, an educational opportunity, etc.) occurred
when the person's consumer status was known. From
such a measure, one can learn about self-reported discrimination in a variety of important life domains. Note
that this strategy asks the consumer to make the connection between the label (their psychiatric treatment) and
the discriminatory behavior. In a different approach,
study participants are asked about employment status,
earned income, or social network ties without any reference to the involvement of labels or stereotypes (Link
1982, 1987; Link et al. 1989; Perlick et al. 2001).
Because people with mental illnesses tend to be disadvantaged in areas like these, the question becomes
whether stigma processes play a role in creating this disadvantage. To gauge the potential importance of stigma
processes, independent measures of labeling and stereotyping are related to variation in outcome variables while
controlling for other possible influences on those outcomes. An excellent example of this approach is a study
by Druss et al. (2000). Their question was whether persons with a label of schizophrenia would receive equal
access to optimum treatment approaches for heart
attacks. They not only found that people with schizophrenia were less likely to experience optimum treatment but
that this difference could not be explained by a comprehensive set of measures assessing the availability of the
procedures in question or the physical status of the persons studied.
Still another approach to assessing differential treatment that may indicate discrimination is to include behavioral measures in surveys of knowledge, attitudes, beliefs,
and behavioral intentions. For example, in a study by
Corrigan et al. (1999), after completing the Psychiatric
Disability Attribution Questionnaire (PDAQ), research
participants were given the opportunity to sign a petition
objecting to stereotypic portrayals of people with mental
illnesses in the media. Participants were provided with
copies of the petition and instructed to sign it if they
wished and deposit it in a box at the front of the room. In
another study, Penn and Nowlin-Drummond (2001) used
a behavioral measure (willingness to participate in a seminar with mental health consumers and providing a contact
number) to help assess the impact of psychiatric labels.
In sum, as with experimental studies, there exist several examples of approaches to assessing behaviors

"stereotype threat," people in stigmatized groups are


familiar with the stereotypes that might be applied to
them, and in situations in which these stereotypes are
salient, performance on tasks relevant to the stereotype
can be impaired. For example, focusing on the stereotype
that African-Americans are less intelligent than members
of other groups, Steele and Aronson found that,
controlling for initial differences on SAT scores,
African-American college students performed worse than
white students on a test when participants were led to
believe that the test measured intellectual ability. In contrast, when the same test was not labeled as being diagnostic of ability, African-Americans scored as well as
whites.
Another body of workthat pertaining to "expectations states" and "status construction"is also highly relevant to the study of stigma, particularly the component
of "status loss" (Link and Phelan 2001). This research has
shown how status hierarchies based on characteristics
such as gender or race are constructed and maintained
and can be alteredthrough social interactions. Studies
have shown that status characteristics affect estimation of
a person's overall social worth as well as specific performance capacities, even when the status characteristic is
irrelevant to the task at hand. These expectations in turn
lead to behaviors such as who listens and who speaks
more, and whose ideas are accepted by others (Berger et
al. 1980, 1998). Moreover, an arbitrarily selected characteristic (e.g., whether one prefers paintings by Klee or
Kandinsky) can be made a status characteristic by treating
a person with that characteristic with more or less deference, and this status can be "taught" to third-party
observers (Ridgeway and Erikson 2000).
Thus, there exist many strategies for assessing
behavior within the experimental context. One issue
regarding these measures is that they come in response
to temporary stigma conditions or in designs that make
one person think the other is stigmatized. Responses
might be different if the person in question was someone who had been in the stigmatized circumstance for
some time. In addition, most of the behaviors assessed
are short-lived discriminatory consequences that do not
have far-reaching effects outside the experimental context. Inducing truly serious consequences for study participants would not be ethical. Nevertheless, several of
the studies involve dimensions that are directly connected to real-world outcomes such as assessments of
achievement, power, and influence. Stigma researchers
can use the examples provided above both as direct
models for the measurement of outcomes and as more
abstract schemas that can assist in creating new behavioral measures for research on the stigma of mental illness.

Measuring Mental Illness Stigma

Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 30, No. 3, 2004

indicative of differential treatment of people with mental


disorders that can be used to study the discrimination
component of the stigma process.

Applying Qualitative Methods To the


Study of Stigma
Quantitative assessments using measures such as those
described above have contributed significantly to our
understanding of attitudes, behaviors, emotions, and
beliefs that form the expression of and response to
stigma. Measuring constructs quantitatively may not
fully reflect the intricacies of the lived experience of
stigma (Schneider 1988; Kleinman et al. 1995), however. Studies using qualitative methods sometimes
directly reflect such experience and thereby deepen our
understanding of stigma processes. Thus, qualitative
inquiry provides another set of methodologies that can
offer rich insights into the subjective experience of
stigma and the complexity of social systems that produce stigma.
Seventeen of the identified papers in this review
report the results of qualitative studies. In this section we
discuss, first, the uses of qualitative research and its relevance for the study of stigma. Second, we examine
lessons learned from classic studies of stigma that used
qualitative methods. Third, we describe selected qualitative studies, focusing on the concepts of stigma addressed
and highlighting specific studies that have deepened our
understanding of stigma.

Classic Qualitative Studies of Stigma. Qualitative


research studies dramatically influenced the early social
science literature on mental illnesses. Major studies
focused on the social structure and social climate of psychiatric hospitals (Stanton and Schwartz 1954; Caudhill
1958; Goffman 1961), family identification of psychosis
(Yarrow et al. 1955), social reactions to symptoms
(Lemert 1962), and involuntary commitment proceedings
(Scheff 1964). While most of these studies were not
specifically about stigma, the processes identified by our
current conceptualization (see above) were very prominent in them. They all addressed the social arrangements
used to manage and control people with mental illnesses.
The endeavor invariably involved issues of stigma as
people with mental illnesses experienced labeling, faced
powerful stereotypes, were set distinctly apart, experienced status loss and discrimination, and were assigned
little personal power in many of these processes. One
finds in these studies many of the elements we think
about today when we discuss stigma and its consequences.

Uses of Qualitative Research and Relevance for the


Study of Stigma. Bryman and Burgess (1999) define
qualitative research broadly as "[a] strategy of social
research that deploys several methods and displays a preference for the interpretation of social phenomena from the
point of view of the meanings employed by the people
being studied" (Introduction, p. x). Qualitative studies
enable researchers to obtain an insider's view of the situation under investigation (Maxwell 1996). Data collection
that may occur through in-depth interviews (semi-structured or unstructured), chart review, participant observation, life history, oral history, documentary research,
diaries, film, video, or photography captures the complexities of life circumstances. Data analysis prioritizes depth
of understanding (Patton 1990; Bryman and Burgess
1999).
The study of stigma lends itself to qualitative methods of investigation. These research methods permit the
investigator to understand how stigma is constructed in
social interaction and how people interpret their experiences and their behavior (Bryman 1999). These meth-

Review of Current Selected Qualitative Studies. As in


the classic studies described above, several of the qualitative studies in the current review do not focus primarily
on stigma related to mental illness. As researchers apply
qualitative methods to study the experiences and attitudes
of people with mental illness, families, or health care
providers, references to stigma or shame frequently

529

Downloaded from http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016

ods are essential for appreciating the subtle, damaging


effects of stigma. Structural discrimination, for example, can only be understood when the institutions
through which it operates are well characterized, the
history of the relationship with the stigmatized group
understood, the policies of the institution examined,
and the attitudes of its leaders explored. Out of this indepth exploration may come confirmation of previous
theories as well as new findings that contribute to new
theory development (Maxwell 1996; Strauss and
Corbin 1998).
Qualitative research may also be exploratory or
may form the basis of further study (Yang and Fox
1999; Bryman and Burgess 1999). Some studies in the
current selection illustrate the use of qualitative methods in conjunction with quantitative methods for three
purposes identified by Hammarsley (1996): (1)
methodological triangulation (each method serves to
validates the findings of the other), (2) facilitation (one
method serves as research groundwork for the other),
or (3) complementarity (qualitative and quantitative
methods are used to explore different aspects of one
question).

Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 30, No. 3, 2004

B.G. Linketal.

to being identified as a psychiatric patient, social isolation, and fear. Significantly, this study suggests that the
hazards of living in low-income, high-crime environments, and having few economic resources compounded the stress related to stigma. These individuals
experienced social exclusion because of stigma related
to their mental illness and because of poverty secondary to their disability. Thus, stigma related to mental illness does not operate in isolation, but in concert
with other challenges in the lives of psychiatric
patients.
Qualitative studies in the literature on stigma of
mental illness vary widely in their treatment of
stigmafrom relatively superficial reports of the experience of "stigma" or the fear of being stigmatized, to
deeper exploration of certain components (usually status loss, discrimination, stereotyping, and labeling). Of
note, these studies are few in number in the current
review (table 1). The underutilization of qualitative
methodologies is significant given that certain aspects
of stigma can best be explored through the use of qualitative research.

Gaps in Measurement
Our review is intended not only to help researchers
locate suitable measures but also to identify gaps in the
use or availability of measures. Our review revealed
several prominent areas of inquiry that appear to be
understudied.
Structural Discrimination. Earlier in this article, we
referred to the concept of structural discrimination,
which we conceptualize as institutional practices that
work to the disadvantage of stigmatized groups and
that allow extensive disparities in outcomes even when
direct person-to-person enactment of discrimination is
absent. We found this form of discrimination almost
entirely unaddressed in the log stigma papers we
reviewed. While issues of structural discrimination
(e.g., insurance parity for people with severe mental illnesses) are discussed in the literature, they have not
been integrated into the literature on stigma. Any complete accounting of the processes that disadvantage
people with mental illnesses must incorporate such
phenomena. One way to do this is through ethnography. Classic studies such as Goffman's (1961) Asylums,
Caudhill's (1958) The Psychiatric Hospital as a Small
Society, and Estroff's (1981) Making It Crazy are
excellent examples of how this can be achieved. At the
same time, quantitative researchers interested in stigma
should attend to this by investigating, for example, the

530

Downloaded from http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016

emerge. We encounter descriptions of stigma in studies


that investigate community views toward seeking mental
health care (Fuller et al. 2000), attitudes toward the treatment of depression (Priest et al. 1996), the experience of
help seeking (Kai and Crosland 2001), determinants of
disclosure of mental health problems (Williams and Healy
2001), and experiences working with people with
psychiatric diagnoses (Lyons and Ziviani 1995), as well
as in the experiences of hospitalization (Sayre 2000)
and the community life of persons diagnosed with mental illness (Bedini 2000).
Themes of status denial and discrimination, avoidance of labeling, and stereotyping predominate. Of the
17 studies, 9 focus primarily on the stigmatized. They
describe the perspectives of persons with a psychiatric
diagnosis or mild mental health problem or the perspectives of family members of people with mental illness (Mohr, 1998; Scheff 1998; Bedini 2000; Sayre
2000; Williams and Healy 2001; Kai and Crosland
2001; Treasure et al. 2001; Muhlbauer 2002; Dalgin
and Gilbride 2003). Seven studies convey the perceptions and experiences of the potential stigmatizer
(Lyons and Ziviani 1995; Priest et al. 1996; Bailey
1998; Seeker et al. 1999; Fuller et al. 2000; Shor and
Sykes 2002; Pinfold et al. 2003). Williams and Taylor
(1995) conducted a content analysis of newspaper articles, examining the portrayal of mental health issues in
the media. Semistructured and unstructured interviews
were the most frequently used methods of qualitative
data collection. Of these 17 studies, we highlight 2 that
illustrate how stigma functions in a particular context
of mental health service provision.
Sayre (2000) studied the mechanisms that patients
use to protect against status loss and stigma when faced
with psychiatric hospitalization. The analysis of interviews, field notes, and informal interviews reveals that
most patients opt to reject the psychiatric explanation
of their problems and the negative social implications
of psychiatric hospitalization. Themes that emphasize
the social process of managing self-worth emerge in
explanations of the reasons for hospitalization. Sayre
describes six attribution accounts (problem, disease,
crisis, punishment, ordination, violation) that enable
patients to manage their self-worth and protect against
status loss. By attributing hospitalization to external
stressful events (i.e., crisis), patients avoided acknowledging that they had a mental illness and were thus
somewhat protected from the negative stereotypes and
stigma associated with mental illness.
The second study (Kai and Crosland 2001) investigated the experiences and perceptions of health care for
34 respondents with chronic mental illness. Stigma
issues that emerged focused on victimization secondary

Measuring Mental Illness Stigma

Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 30, No. 3, 20O4

influenced by the work of Amerigo Farina and his


experimental paradigm for the study of mental illness
stigma (e.g., Farina et al. 1968, 1971). Second, experimental studies concerning stigmatizing conditions
other than mental illnesses would seem to be on the
rise as reflected in the excellent work of social psychologists (see Crocker et al. 1998 for a review). As we
hope our section on experimental studies makes clear,
there is a great opportunity to adapt strategies and
apply theories from these approaches.

spatial location of board-and-care homes for people with


schizophrenia, pay scale differentials for professionals
whose work involves the chronic mental illnesses as
opposed to much milder conditions treated in private
practice settings, historical differences in research
funding, differences in insurance coverage for mental
illnesses as opposed to other illnesses, and the social
isolation of treatment facilities for people with severe
mental illnesses. These institutional arrangements need
to be understood from a stigma perspective both as
consequences of stigma and as factors that create and
reinforce stigma on the individual level.
Assessment of the Emotional Responses of Patients/
Consumers. Our review found only four studies
assessing the emotional responses of patients/consumers and few systematic measures for this domain of
inquiry (see Link et al. 2002 for existing examples).
This is odd and unfortunate given the centrality of feelings of shame, humiliation, and embarrassment in the
area of stigma. We are aware of two projects seeking to
expand this domain of measurement, but at the time of
this writing neither haved been published.
Assessment of Children's Knowledge, Attitudes,
Beliefs, and Behaviors and Children's Experience
of Stigma. We found only 4 studies out of the 109 we
reviewed that focused on children or adolescents. This
is remarkable given the salience of the issue for understanding the mechanisms through which our society
perpetuates stereotypes. One of the studies is both
clear and disturbing in indicating that children are
exposed to stereotypes through the cartoons they
watch (Wilson et al. 2000), and another suggests that
developmental issues are likely central by indicating
that third graders have learned that mental illnesses are
"bad" but have yet to develop specific stereotypes
(Adler and Wahl 1998). These important studies break
new ground, but we need to understand much more
about when children develop conceptions of mental
illnesses, what forms their perceptions, and what kinds
of educational experiences might move conceptions in
a positive direction.
Equally as striking, we found no studies assessing
children's experience of stigma; all the studies of
patients/consumers were studies of adults. Moreover,
none of the measures discussed were aimed at assessing children's experience of stigma.

References (Data Base)


1. Adler, A.K., and Wahl, O.F. Children's beliefs about
people labeled mentally ill. American Journal of
Orthopsychiatry, 68(2):321-326, 1998.
2. Alem, A.; Jacobsson, L.; Araya, M.; Kebede, D.;
and Kullgren, G. How are mental disorders seen and
where is help sought in a rural Ethiopian community?
A key informant study in Butajira, Ethiopia. Acta
Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 397(Suppl):40-47, 1999.
3. Angermeyer, M.C., and Matschinger, H. The effect of
personal experience with mental illness on the attitude

Use of Experimental Approaches. Our review also


revealed a shortage of nonvignette experimental studies
addressing the stigma of mental illness. This is surprising for two reasons. First, the field was powerfully

531

Downloaded from http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016

Cross-Cultural Approaches. Although we identified


a number of studies conducted in non-Western societies, many of these applied concepts and instruments
that were developed in the West. Studies that identify
how stigma is constructed and acted upon in nonWestern cultures would contribute greatly to our understanding of these processes in the United States. Such
studies would help us understand how stigma components of labeling, stereotyping, separating, emotional
reactions, status loss, and discrimination are similar
and different in other cultures. This kind of information
could help illuminate how we might change stigma
processes in our own culture.
We began our review by referring to the Surgeon
General's Report on Mental Health and to the prominent position he assigned to stigma in that report.
Proceeding from his statement is the idea that improvement in the nation's mental health and mental health
care will depend in some large part on whether stigma
processes are identified and effectively addressed. One
way our review contributes to this goal is by providing
information about the measurement of stigma that can
guide efforts to identify stigma processes and evaluate
efforts to address those processes. We hope that
improved measurement of stigma will play a direct role
in shaping the kinds of broad policies that are developed to improve the lives of people with mental illnesses and their families.

Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 30, No. 3, 2004

B.G. Linketal.

toward individuals suffering from mental disorders. Social


Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 31(6):321326,
1996.
4. Angermeyer, M.C., and Matschinger, H. Social distance
toward the mentally ill: Results of representative surveys
in the Federal Republic of Germany. Psychological
Medicine, 27(1):131-141, 1997.

16. Byrne, P. Psychiatric stigma: Past, passing and to

come. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine,


90:618-621, 1997.
17. Carey, S.J. Police officers' knowledge of, and attitudes
toward, mental illness in southwest Scotland. Scottish
Medical Journal, 46(2):41^2, 2001.
18. Chou, K.L.; Mak, K.Y.; Chung, P.K.; and Ho, K.
Attitudes toward mental patients in Hong Kong.

5. Angermeyer, M.C., and Matschinger, H. Lay beliefs


about mental disorders: A comparison between the western and the eastern parts of Germany. Social Psychiatry
and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 34(5):275-281, 1999.

International

of Law and

Social

Psychiatry,

20. Chung, K.F.; Chen, E.Y.; and Liu, C.S. University students' attitudes toward mental patients and psychiatric
treatment. International Journal of Social Psychiatry,
47(2):63-72,2001.

Psychiatry,

24(4-5):469-486, 2001.

21. Clinton, M. Collaborative education and social stereotypes. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Mental
Health Nursing, 8:100-103, 1999.

8. Arikan, K., and Uysal, O. Emotional reactions to the


mentally ill are positively influenced by personal acquaintance. Israel Journal of Psychiatry and Related Sciences,
36(2): 100-104, 1999.

22. Cormack, S., and Furnham, A. Psychiatric labelling,


sex role stereotypes and beliefs about the mentally ill. Int

9. Bailey, S.R. An exploration of critical care nurses' and


doctors' attitudes toward psychiatric patients. Australian
Journal of Advanced Nursing, 15(3):8-14, 1998.

J Soc Psychiatrylnternational

Journal of Social

Psychiatry, 44(4):235-247, 1998.


23. Corrigan, P.W. Empowerment and serious mental illness: Treatment partnerships and community opportunities. Psychiatric Quarterly, 73(3):217-228, 2002.

10. Bedini, L.A. 'Just sit down so we can talk': Perceived


stigma and community recreation pursuits of people with

disabilities. Therapeutic Recreation Journal (First


Quarter):55-68, 2000.

24. Corrigan, P.W.; Edwards, A.B.; Green, A.; Diwan,


S.L.; and Penn, D.L. Prejudice, social distance, and familiarity with mental illness. Schizophrenia
Bulletin,
27(2):219-225, 2001.

11. Benkert, O.; Graf-Morgenstern, M.; Hillert, A.;


Sandmann, J.; Ehmig, S.C.; Weissbecker, H.; Kepplinger,
H.M.; and Sobota, K. Public opinion on psychotropic
drugs: An analysis of the factors influencing acceptance or

25. Corrigan, P.W.; Green, A.; Lundin, R.; Kubiak, M.A.;


and Penn, D.L. Familiarity with and social distance from
people who have serious mental illness. Psychiatric
Services, 52(7):953-958, 2001.

rejection. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease,


185(3):151-158, 1997.
12. Ben-Noun, L. Characterization of patients refusing
professional psychiatric treatment in a primary care clinic.

26. Corrigan, P.W., and Penn, D.L. Lessons from social


psychology on discrediting psychiatric stigma. American
Psychologist, 54(9):765-776, 1999.

Israel Journal of Psychiatry and Related Sciences,


33(3): 167-174, 1996.
13. Boisvert, CM., and Faust, D. Effects of the label
"schizophrenia" on causal attributions of violence.

27. Corrigan, P.W.; River, L.P.; Lundin, R.K.; Penn, D.L.;


Uphoff-Wasowski, K.; Campion, J.; Mathisen, J.;
Gagnon, C ; Bergman, M.; Goldstein, H.; and Kubiak,
M.A. Three strategies for changing attributions about

Schizophrenia Bulletin, 25(3):479-491, 1999.


14. Britt, T.W. The stigma of psychological problems in a
work environment: Evidence from the screening of service members returning from Bosnia. Journal of Applied
Social Psychology, 30(8): 1599-1618, 2000.

severe mental illness. Schizophrenia

Bulletin,

27(2): 187-195, 2001.


28. Corrigan, P.W.; River, L.P.; Lundin, R.K.; Wasowski,
K.U.; Campion, J.; Mathisen, J.; Goldstein, H.; Gagnon,
C ; Bergman, M.; and Kubiak, M.A. Predictors of partici-

15. Brown, K. Reducing the stigma of mental illness.


Journal of Menial Health Counseling, 24(l):81-87, 2002.

532

Downloaded from http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016

7. Angermeyer, M.C., and Schulze, B. Reinforcing stereotypes: How the focus on forensic cases in news reporting
may influence public attitudes toward the mentally ill.

Journal

of

19. Chowdhury, A.N.; Sanyal, D.; Dutta, S.K.; Banerjee,


S.; De, R.; Bhattacharya, K.; Palit, S.; Bhattacharya, P.;
Mondal, R.K.; and Weiss, M.G. Stigma and mental illness: Pilot study of laypersons and health care providers
with the EMIC in rural West Bengal, India. International
Medical Journal, 7(4):257-260, 2000.

6. Angermeyer, M.C.; Matschinger, H.; and Riedel-Heller,


S.G. Whom to ask for help in case of a mental disorder?
Preferences of the lay public. Social Psychiatry and
Psychiatric Epidemiology, 34(4):202-210, 1999.

International

Journal

42(3):213-219, 1996.

Measuring Mental Illness Stigma

Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 30, No. 3, 2004

41. Haghihat, R. A unitary theory of stigmatisation.


British Journal of Psychiatry, 178:207-215, 2001.

pation in campaigns against mental illness stigma.


Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 187(6):378-380,
1999.
29. Coverdale, J.; Nairn, R.; and Claasen, D. Depictions
of mental illness in print media: A prospective national

sample. Australian and New Zealand Journal of

42. Hasui, C ; Sakamoto, S.; Suguira, B.; and Kitamura,


T. Stigmatization of mental illness in Japan: Images and
frequency of encounters with diagnostic categories of
mental illness among medical and non-medical university

Psychiatry, 36(5):697-700, 2002.

students.

30. Crisp, A.H.; Gelder, M.G.; Rix, S.; Meltzer, H.I.; and
Rowlands, O.J. Stigmatisation of people with mental illnesses. British Journal of Psychiatry, 177:47, 2000.

28(Summer):253-266, 2000.

of

Psychiatry

and

Law,

43. Hayward, P., and Bright, J.A. Stigma and mental illness: A review and critique. Journal of Mental Health,
6(4):345-354, 1997.

31. Crossley, M.L., and Crossley, N. 'Patient' voices,


social movements and the habitus; how psychiatric sur52(10): 1477-1489, 2001.
32. Dalgin, R.S., and Gilbride, D. Perspectives of people
with psychiatric disabilities on employment disclosure.
Psychiatric Rehabilitation Journal, 26(3):306-310, 2003.

45. Holmes, E.P.; Corrigan, P.W.; Williams, P.; Canar, J.;


and Kubiak, M.A. Changing attitudes about schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 25(3):447^56, 1999.

33. Dickerson, F.B.; Sommerville, J.; Origoni, A.E.;


Ringel, N.B.; and Parente, F. Experiences of stigma
among outpatients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia
Bulletin, 28(1): 143-155, 2002.

46. Hutchinson, G., and Bhugra, D. Social anthropology


and stigma. In: Skultans, V., and Cox, J., eds.
Anthropological Approaches to Psychological Medicine.
Bristol, PA: Jessica Kingsley Publishers, 2000. pp.
233-257.

34. Druss, B.G.; Marcus, S.C.; Rosenheck, R.A.; Olfson,


M.; Tanielian, T.; and Pincus, H.A. Understanding disability in mental and general medical conditions. American
Journal of Psychiatry, 157(9): 1485-1491, 2000.

47. Johnson, M.C.; Morrissey, J.P.; Calloway, M.O.;


Fried, B.J.; Blank, M.; and Starrett, B.E. Rural mental
health leaders' perceptions of stigma and community
issues. Journal of Rural Health, 13(l):59-70, 1997.

35. Edlund, M.J.; Wang, P.S.; Berglund, P.A.; Katz, S.J.;


Lin, E.; and Kessler, R.C. Dropping out of mental health
treatment: Patterns and predictors among epidemiological
survey respondents in the United States and Ontario.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 159(5):845-851, 2002.

48. Jorm, A.F. Mental health literacy. Public knowledge


and beliefs about mental disorders. British Journal of
Psychiatry, 177:396-401, 2000.
49. Jorm, A.F.; Korten, A.E.; Jacomb, P.A.; Christensen,
H.; Rodgers, B.; and Pollitt, P. "Mental health literacy":
A survey of the public's ability to recognise mental disorders and their beliefs about the effectiveness of treatment. Medical Journal of Australia, 166(4): 182-186,
1997.

36. Fan, C. A comparison of attitudes toward mental illness and knowledge of mental health services between
Australian and Asian students. Community Mental Health
Journal, 35(l):47-56, 1999.
37. Farina, A. Stigma. In: Mueser, K.T., and Tarrier, N.,
eds. Handbook of Social Functioning in Schizophrenia.
Needham Heights, MA: Allyn and Bacon, 1998. pp.
247-279.

50. Kai, J., and Crosland, A. Perspectives of people with


enduring mental ill health from a community-based quali-

tative study. British Journal of General Practice,

38. Freidl, M.; Lang, T.; and Scherer, M. How psychiatric


patients perceive the public's stereotype of mental illness.

51(470):730-736, 2001.
51. Kim, Y., and Berrios, G.E. Impact of the term schizophrenia on the culture of ideograph: The Japanese experience. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 27(2): 181-185, 2001.

Epidemiology,

38(5):269-275, 2003.
39. Fuller, J.; Edwards, J.; Procter, N.; and Moss, J. How
definition of mental health problems can influence help
seeking in rural and remote communities. Australian
Journal of Rural Health, 8(3): 148-153, 2000.

52. Kimhi, R.; Barak, Y.; Gutman, J.; Melamed, Y; Zohar,


M.; and Barak, I. Police attitudes toward mental illness
and psychiatric patients in Israel. Journal of the American
Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 26(4):625-629,
1998.
53. Kirmayer, L.J.; Fletcher, CM.; and Boothroyd, L.J.
Inuit attitudes toward deviant behavior: A vignette study.

40. Greenberg, J.S.; Kim, H.W.; and Greenley, J.R.


Factors associated with subjective burden in siblings of
adults with severe mental illness. American Journal of
Orthopsychiatry, 67(2):231-241, 1997.

533

Downloaded from http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016

44. Hinshaw, S.P., and Cicchetti, D. Stigma and mental


disorder: Conceptions of illness, public attitudes, personal
disclosure, and social policy. Development
and
Psychopathology, 12(4):555-598, 2000.

vivors 'speak out.' Social Science & Medicine,

Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric

Journal

Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 30, No. 3, 2004

B.G. Link et al.

66. Meissen, G.; Powell, T.J.; Wituk, S.A.; Girrens, K.;


and Arteaga, S. Attitudes of AA contact persons toward
group participation by persons with a mental illness.
Psychiatric Services, 50(8):1079-1081, 1999.

Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 185(2):78-86,


1997.
54. Kua, J.H.; Parker, G.; Lee, C ; and Jorm, A.F. Beliefs
about outcomes for mental disorders: A comparative study
of primary health practitioners and psychiatrists in
Singapore. Singapore Medical Journal, 41(ll):542-547,
2000.

67. Mino, Y.; Yasuda, N.; Tsuda, T; and Shimodera, S.


Effects of a one-hour educational program on medical students' attitudes to mental illness. Psychiatry and Clinical
Neurosciences, 55(5):501-507, 2001.

55. Lawrie, S.M. Newspaper coverage of psychiatric and


physical illness. Psychiatric Bulletin, 24:104-106, 2000.

68. Mohr, W.K. A tale of two centuries: Voices of the past


and present. Archives of Psychiatric
Nursing,
12(6):301-310, 1998.

56. Link, B.G., and Phelan, J.C. Conceptualizing stigma.


Annual Review of Sociology, 27:363-385, 2001.

58. Link, B.G.; Struening, E.L.; Neese-Todd, S.;


Asmussen, S.; and Phelan, J.C. Stigma as a barrier to
recovery: The consequences of stigma for the self-esteem
of people with mental illnesses. Psychiatric Services,
52(12):1621-1626, 2001.

71. Murray, M.G., and Steffen, J.J. Attitudes of case


managers toward people with serious mental illness.
Community Mental Health Journal, 35(6):505-514,
1999.
72. Nairn, R. Does the use of psychiatrists as sources of
information improve media depictions of mental illness?
A pilot study. Australian and New Zealand Journal of
Psychiatry, 33:583-589, 1999.

59. Link, B.G.; Struening, E.L.; Rahav, M ; Phelan, J.C;


and Nuttbrock, L. On stigma and its consequences:
Evidence from a longitudinal study of men with dual
diagnoses of mental illness and substance abuse. Journal
of Health and Social Behavior, 38(2): 177-190, 1997.
60. Lyons, M., and Ziviani, J. Stereotypes, stigma, and
mental illness: Llearning from fieldwork experiences.
American
Journal
of Occupational
Therapy,
49(10): 1002-1008, 1995.

73. Ng, P., and Chan, K.F. Sex differences in opinion


toward mental illness of secondary school students in
Hong Kong. International Journal of Social Psychiatry,
46(2):79-88, 2000.
74. Ostman, M., and Kjellin, L. Stigma by association:
Psychological factors in relatives of people with mental
illness. British Journal of Psychiatry, 181:494-498,
2002.

61. Madianos, M.G.; Economou, M.; Hatjiandreou, M.;


Papageorgiou, A.; and Rogakou, E. Changes in public
attitudes toward mental illness in the Athens area
(1979/1980-1994). Acta Psychiatrica
Scandinavica,
99(l):73-78, 1999.

75. Papadopoulos, C ; Leavey, G.; and Vincent, C. Factors


influencing stigma: A comparison of Greek-Cypriot and
English attitudes toward mental illness in north London.
Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric
Epidemiology,
37(9):430_434, 2002.

62. Markowitz, F.E. The effects of stigma on the psychological well-being and life satisfaction of persons with
mental illness. Journal of Health and Social Behavior,
39(4):335-347, 1998.
63. Markowitz, F.E. Modeling processes in recovery from
mental illness: Relationships among symptoms, life satisfaction, and self-concept. Journal of Health and Social
Behavior, 42(l):64-79, 2001.

76. Penn, D.L.; Kommana, S.; Mansfield, M.; and Link,


B.G. Dispelling the stigma of schizophrenia: II. The
impact of information on dangerousness. Schizophrenia
Bulletin, 25(3):437^46, 1999.

64. Marsh, D.T. Personal accounts of consumer/survivors:


Insights and implications. Journal of Clinical Psychology,
56(11):1447-1457, 2000.

77. Penn, D.L., and Nowlin-Drummond, A. Politically


correct labels and schizophrenia: Aa rose by any other
name? Schizophrenia Bulletin, 27(2): 197-203, 2001.

65. Martens, L., and Addington, J. The psychological


well-being of family members of individuals with schizophrenia. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology,
36(3): 128-133, 2001.

78. Perlick, D.A.; Rosenheck, R.A.; Clarkin, J.F.; Sirey,


J.A.; Salahi, J.; Struening, E.L.; and Link, B.G. Stigma as
a barrier to recovery: Adverse effects of perceived stigma

534

Downloaded from http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016

69. Muhlbauer, S.K. Experience of stigma by families


with mentally ill members. Journal of the American
Psychiatric Nurses Association, 8:76-83, 2002.
70. Mukherjee, R.; Fialho, A.; Wijetunge, A.; Checinski,
K.; and Surgenor, T. The stigmatisation of psychiatric illness: The attitudes of medical students and doctors in a
London teaching hospital. Psychiatric
Bulletin,
26:178-181,2002.

57. Link, B.G.; Phelan, J.C; Bresnahan, M ; Stueve, A.;


and Pescosolido, B.A. Public conceptions of mental illness: Labels, causes, dangerousness, and social distance.
American Journal of Public Health, 89(9): 1328-1333,
1999.

Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 30, No. 3, 2004

Measuring Mental Illness Stigma

on social adaptation of persons diagnosed with bipolar


affective disorder. Psychiatric Services, 52(12):
1627-1632,2001.

90. Reda, S. Public perceptions of former psychiatric


patients in England. Psychiatric Services, 47(11):
1253-1255, 1996.

79. Pescosolido, B.A.; Monahan, J.; Link, B.G.;


Stueve, A.; and Kikuzawa, S. The public's view of the
competence, dangerousness, and need for legal coercion of persons with mental health problems.
American Journal of Public Health, 89(9): 1339-1345,
1999.

91. Reda, S. Public perception of discharged psychiatric


patients: A community survey. International Journal of
Social Psychiatry, 42(3):220-229, 1996.
92. Rosen, A.; Walter, G.; Politis, T; and Shortland, M.
From shunned to shining: Doctors, madness and psychiatry in Australian and New Zealand cinema. Medical
Journal of Australia, 167(11-12): 640-644, 1997.

80. Phelan, J.C. Genetic bases of mental illnessa cure


for stigma? Trends in Neurosciences, 25(8):43O-j4-31,
2002.
81. Phelan, J.C; Bromet, E.J.; and Link, B.G. Psychiatric
illness and family stigma. Schizophrenia
Bulletin,
24(1):115-126, 1998.
82. Phelan, J.C, and Link, B.G. The growing belief that
people with mental illnesses are violent: The role of the
dangerousness criterion for civil commitment. Social
Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 33(Suppl
1):S7-12, 1998.

95. Ryan, C.S.; Robinson, D.R.; and Hausmann, L.R.


Stereotyping among providers and consumers of public
mental health services. The role of perceived group variability. Behavior Modification, 25(3):406-442, 2001.

83. Phelan, J.C; Link, B.G.; Stueve, A.; and Pescosolido,


B.A. Public conceptions of mental illness in 1950 and
1996: What is mental illness and is it to be feared?
Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 41:188-207,
2000.

96. Ryan, T. Perceived risks associated with mental illness: Beyond homicide and suicide. Social Science &
Medicine, 46(2):287-297,1998.

84. Phillips, M.R. Stigma and expressed emotion. British


Journal of Psychiatry, 181:288-293, 2002.

97. Sayre, J. The patient's diagnosis: Explanatory models


of mental illness. Qualitative Health
Research,
10(l):71-83, 2000.

85. Pinfold, V.; Toulmin, H.; Thomicroft, G.; Huxley, P.;


Farmer, P.; and Graham, T. Reducing psychiatric stigma
and discrimination: Evaluation of educational interventions in U.K. secondary schools. British Journal of
Psychiatry, 182:342-346, 2003.

98. Scheff, T.J. Shame in the labeling of mental illness.


In: Gilbert, P., and Andrews, B., eds. Shame:
Interpersonal Behavior, Psychopathology, and Culture.
New York, NY: Oxford, 1998. pp. 191-205.

86. Priest, R.G.; Vize, C ; Roberts, A.; Roberts, M.; and


Tylee, A. Lay people's attitudes to treatment of depression: Results of opinion poll for Defeat Depression
Campaign just before its launch. British Medical Journal,
313:858-859, 1996.

99. Schnittker, J. Gender and reactions to psychological


problems: An examination of social tolerance and perceived dangerousness. Journal of Health and Social
Behavior, 41(2):224-240, 2000.

87. Prince, P.N., and Prince, C.R. Perceived stigma and


community integration among clients of assertive community treatment. Psychiatric Rehabilitation
Journal,
25(4):323-331,2002.

100. Schwartz, S.; Weiss, L.; and Lennon, M.C. Labeling


effects of a controversial psychiatric diagnosis: A vignette
experiment of late luteal phase dysphoric disorder. Women
& Health, 30(3):63-75, 2000.

88. Read, J., and Law, A. The relationship of causal


beliefs and contact with users of mental health services to
attitudes to the 'mentally ill.' International Journal of
Social Psychiatry, 45(3):216-229, 1999.

101. Seeker, J.; Armstrong, C ; and Hill, M. Young people's understanding of mental illness. Health Education
Research, 14{6):729-739, 1999.
102. Sevigny, R.; Yang, W.; Zhang, P.; Marleau, J.D.;
Yang, Z.; Su, L.; Li, G.; Xu, D.; Wang, Y; and Wang, H.
Attitudes toward the mentally ill in a sample of professionals working in a psychiatric hospital in Beijing
(China). International Journal of Social Psychiatry,
45(l):41-55, 1999.

89. R e a u m e , G. Lunatic to patient to person:


Nnomenclature in psychiatric history and the influence of p a t i e n t s ' activism in North America.

International

Journal of Law and

Psychiatry,

25(4):405-426, 2002.

535

Downloaded from http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016

93. Rosenfield, S. Labeling mental illness: The effects of


received services and perceived stigma on life
satisfaction. American Sociological Review, 62(August):
660-672, 1997.
94. Roth, D.; Antony, M.M.; Kerr, K.L.; and Downie, F.
Attitudes toward mental illness in medical students: Does
personal and professional experience with mental illness
make a difference? Medical Education, 34(3):234-236,
2000.

Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 30, No. 3, 2004

B.G. Linketal.

nervosa and psychosis. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric


Epidemiology, 36(7):343-347, 2001.
114. Vezzoli, R.; Archiati, L.; Buizza, C ; Pasqualetti, P.;
Rossi, G.; and Pioli, R. Attitude toward psychiatric
patients: A pilot study in a northern Italian town.
European Psychiatry, 16(8):451^58, 2001.

104. Shor, R., and Sykes, I.J. Introducing structured dialogue with people with mental illness into the training of
social work students. Psychiatric Rehabilitation Journal,
26(l):63-69, 2002.

115. Wahl, O.F. Mental health consumers' experience of


stigma. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 25(3):467^78, 1999.
116. Whaley, A.L. Ethnic and racial differences in perceptions of dangerousness of persons with mental illness.
Psychiatric Services, 48(10): 1328-1330, 1997.
117. Williams, B., and Healy, D. Disclosure of minor
mental health problems: An exploratory theoretical
study. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 35(1): 108116,
2001.
118. Williams, M., and Taylor, J. Mental illness: Media
perpetuation of stigma. Contemporary Nurse, 4:4146,
1995.
119. Wilson, C ; Nairn, R.; Coverdale, J.; and Panapa, A.
How mental illness is portrayed in children's television. A
prospective study. British Journal of Psychiatry,
176:440-443, 2000.

105. Silver, T. Families of persons with mental illness and


substance use related disorders: Cconsiderations for service delivery. Administration and Policy in Mental
Health, 26(5):361-367, 1999.
106. Sirey, J.A.; Bruce, M.L.; Alexopoulos, G.S.;
Perlick, D.A.; Friedman, S.J.; and Meyers, B.S. Stigma
as a barrier to recovery: Perceived stigma and patientrated severity of illness as predictors of antidepressant drug adherence. Psychiatric Services,
52(12):1615-1620, 2001.
107. Sirey, J.A.; Bruce, M.L.; Alexopoulos, G.S.;
Perlick, D.A.; Raue, P.; Friedman, S.J.; and Meyers,
B.S. Perceived stigma as a predictor of treatment discontinuation in young and older outpatients with
depression. American Journal of Psychiatry,
158(3):479-481,2001.

120. Wolff, G.; Pathare, S.; Craig, T.; and Leff, J.


Community knowledge of mental illness and reaction to
mentally ill people. British Journal of Psychiatry,
168(2): 191-198, 1996.
121. Wolff, G.; Pathare, S.; Craig, T.; and Leff, J.
Community attitudes to mental illness. British Journal of
Psychiatry, 168(2): 183-190, 1996.

108. Starr, S.; Campbell, L.R.; and Herrick, C.A. Factors


affecting use of the mental health system by rural children. Issues in Mental Health Nursing, 23(3):291-304,
2002.
109. Struening, E.L.; Perlick, D.A.; Link, B.G.; Hellman,
F.; Herman, D.; and Sirey, J.A. Stigma as a barrier to
recovery: The extent to which caregivers believe most
people devalue consumers and their families. Psychiatric
Services, 52(12): 1633-1638, 2001.
110. Stuart, H., and Arboleda-Florez, J. Community attitudes toward people with schizophrenia. Canadian
Journal of Psychiatry, 46(3):245-252, 2001.
111. Sugiura, T.; Sakamoto, S.; Kijima, N.; Kitamura, F.;
and Kitamura, T. Stigmatizing perception of mental illness by Japanese students: Comparison of different psychiatric disorders. Journal of Nervous and Mental
Disease, 188(4):239-242, 2000.

122. Wright, E.R.; Gronfein, W.P.; and Owens, T.J.


Deinstitutionalization, social rejection, and the selfesteem of former mental patients. Journal of Health and
Social Behavior, 41(l):68-90, 2000.
123. Yanos, P.T.; Rosenfield, S.; and Horwitz, A.V.
Negative and supportive social interactions and quality of
life among persons diagnosed with severe mental illness.
Community Mental Health Journal, 37(5):405-419,
2001.

References (Text)
Adler, A.K., and Wahl, O.F. Children's beliefs about people labeled mentally ill. American Journal of
Orthopsychiatry, 68(2):321-326, 1998.

112. Tak-fai Lau, J., and Cheung, C. Discriminatory attitudes to people with intellectual disability or mental
health difficulty. International
Social Work,
42(4) :431-444, 1999.

Alem, A.; Jacobsson, L.; Araya, M.; Kebede, D.; and


Kullgren, G. How are mental disorders seen and where is
help sought in a rural Ethiopian community? A key informant study in Butajira, Ethiopia. Ada Psychiatrica
Scandinavica, 397(Suppl):40-47, 1999.

113. Treasure, J.; Murphy, T.; Szmukler, G.; Todd, G.;


Gavan, K.; and Joyce, J. The experience of caregiving for
severe mental illness: A comparison between anorexia

536

Downloaded from http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016

103. Shibre, T.; Negash, A.; Kullgren, G.; Kebede, D.;


Alem, A.; Fekadu, A.; Fekadu, D.; Madhin, G.; and
Jacobsson, L. Perception of stigma among family members of individuals with schizophrenia and major affective
disorders in rural Ethiopia. Social Psychiatry and
Psychiatric Epidemiology, 36(6):299-303, 2001.

Measuring Mental Illness Stigma

Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 30, No. 3, 2004

Angermeyer, M.C., and Matschinger, H. The effect of personal experience with mental illness on the attitude toward
individuals suffering from mental disorders. Social Psychiatry
and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 31(6):321-326, 1996.
Angermeyer, M.C., and Matschinger, H. Social distance
toward the mentally ill: Results of representative surveys
in the Federal Republic of Germany. Psychological
Medicine, 27(1): 131-141, 1997.
Bailey, S.R. An exploration of critical care nurses' and
doctors' attitudes toward psychiatric patients. Australian
Journal of Advanced Nursing, 15(3):8-14, 1998.

Bedini, L.A. 'Just sit down so we can talk': Perceived


stigma and community recreation pursuits of people with

disabilities. Therapeutic Recreation Journal (First

Crisp, A.H.; Gelder, M.G.; Rix, S.; Meltzer, H.I.; and


Rowlands, O.J. Stigmatisation of people with mental illnesses. British Journal of Psychiatry, \11:A-1, 2000.

Quarter):55-68, 2000.
Berger, J.; Ridgeway, C.L.; Fisek, M.H.; and Norman,
R.Z. The legitimation and delegitimation of power and

Crocker, J.; Major, B.; and Steele, C. Social stigma. In:


Gilbert, D.T.; Fiske, S.T.; and Lindzey, G., eds. The
Handbook of Social Psychology. Boston, MA: McGraw
Hill, 1998. pp. 35-69.
Cronbach, L.J., and Meehl, P.E. Construct validity in psychological tests. Psychological Bulletin, 52:281-302,
1955.

Review,

63:379-405, 1998.
Berger, J.; Rosenholz, J.S.; and Zelditch, M. Status organizing processes. Annual Review of Sociology, 6:479-508,
1980.
Bogardus, E.S. Social distance and its origins. Journal of
Applied Sociology, 9:216-226, 1925.

Cumming, J., and Cumming, E. Closed


Ranks.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1957.
Dalgin, R.S., and Gilbride, D. Perspectives of people
with psychiatric disabilities on employment disclosure.

Boisvert, CM. and Faust, D. Effects of the label "schizophrenia" on causal attributions of violence. Schizophrenia
Bulletin, 25(3):479-491, 1999.

Psychiatric Rehabilitation Journal, 26(3):306-310,

Bryman, A. The debate about quantitative and qualitative


research. In: Bryman, A., and Burgess, R.G., eds.
Qualitative Research. London, U.K.: Sage, 1999. pp.
35-69.

2003.
Dickerson, F.B.; Sommerville, J.; Origoni, A.E.; Ringel,
N.B.; and Parente, F. Experiences of stigma among outpatients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia
Bulletin,
28(1): 143-155, 2002.
Druss, B.G.; Marcus, S.C.; Rosenheck, R.A.; Olfson, M.;
Tanielian, T; and Pincus, H.A. Understanding disability
in mental and general medical conditions. American
Journal of Psychiatry, 157(9): 1485-1491, 2000.

Bryman, A. and Burgess, R.G. Qualitative research


methodologya review. In: Bryman, A., and Burgess,
R.G., eds. Qualitative Research. London, U.K.: Sage,
1999. pp. ixxlvi.
Caudhill, W. The Psychiatric Hospital as a Small Society.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1958.

Estroff, S. Making It Crazy: An Ethnography of


Psychiatric Clients in an American Community. Berkeley,
CA: University of California Press, 1981.

Chowdhury, A.N.; Sanyal, D.; Dutta, S.K.; Banerjee, S.;


De, R.; Bhattacharya, K.; Palit, S.; Bhattacharya, P.;
Mondal, R.K.; and Weiss, M.G. Stigma and mental illness: Pilot study of laypersons and health care providers
with the EMIC in rural West Bengal, India. International
Medical Journal, 7(4):257-260, 2000.
Cohen, J., and E. L. Struening, E.L. Opinions about mental illness in the personnel of two large mental hospitals.
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 64:349-360,
1962.

Farina, A.; Allen, J.G.; and Saul, B. The role of the stigmatized in affecting social relationships. Journal of
Personality, 36:169-182, 1968.
Farina, A.; Gliha, D.; Boudreau, L.; Allen, J.; and
Sherman, M. Mental illness and the impact of believing
others know about it. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
77:1-5, 1971.

537

Downloaded from http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016

Corrigan, P.W.; River, L.P.; Lundin, R.K.; Wasowski,


K.U.; Campion, J.; Mathisen, J.; Goldstein, H.; Gagnon,
C ; Bergman, M.; and Kubiak, M.A. Predictors of participation in campaigns against mental illness stigma.
Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 187(6):378380,
1999.

Bech, P. Rating Scales for Psychopathology, Health Status


and Quality of Life. New York, NY: Springer, 1993.

prestige orders. American Sociological

Corrigan, P.W. Mental health stigma as social attribution:


implications for research methods and attitude change.
Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 7(l):48-67,
2000.
Corrigan, P.W. An attribution model of public discrimination toward persons with mental illness. Journal of Health
and Social Behavior, 44:162-179, 2003.
Corrigan, P.W.; Markowitz, F.E.; and Watson, A.C.
Structural levels of mental illness stigma and discrimination. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 30(3):481-491, 2004.

Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 30, No. 3, 2004

B.G. Link et al.

tions of rejection. American Sociological Review,

Fine, M., and Asch, A.. Disability beyond stigma: Social


interaction, discrimination, and activism. Journal of
Social Issues, 44:3-22, 1988.

52:96-112, 1987.
Link, B.G.; Cullen, F T ; Frank, J.; and Wozniak, J. The
social rejection of ex-mental patients: Understanding why

Freeman, H.G., and Simmons, O.G. The Mental Patient


Comes Home. New York, NY: John Wiley, 1963.
Fuller, J.; Edwards, J.; Procter, N.; and Moss, J. How definition of mental health problems can influence help seeking in rural and remote communities. Australian Journal
of Rural Health, 8(3): 148-153, 2000.

labels matter. American Journal of

Sociology,

92:1461-1500, 1987.
Link, B.G.; Cullen, F.T.; Struening, E.L.; Shrout, P.E.; and
Dohrenwend, B.P. A modified labeling theory approach in
the area of mental disorders: An empirical assessment.
American Sociological Review, 54:100-123, 1989.

Goffman, E. Asylums: Essays on the Social Situation of


Mental Patients and Other Inmates. Garden City, NY:
Anchor Books, 1961.

Goffman, E. Stigma: Notes on the Management of


Spoiled Identity. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall,
1963.
Greenberg, J.S.; Greenley, J.R.; McKee, D.; Brown, R.;
and Griffin-Francell, C. Mothers caring for an adult child
with schizophrenia: The effects of subjective burden on
maternal health. Family Relations, 42:205-211, 1993.
Hammarsley, M. The relationship between qualitative and
quantitative research: Paradigm loyalty versus methodological eclecticism. In: Richardson, J.T.E., ed. Handbook
of Research Methods for Psychology and the Social
Sciences. Leicester, UK: BPS Books.
Jones, E.E.; Farina, A.; Hastorf, A.H.; Marcus, H.; Miller,
D.T.; and Scott, R.A. Social Stigma: The Psychology of
Marked Relationships. New York, NY: Freeman and
Company, 1984.

Link, B.G.; Struening, E. L.; Neese-Todd, S.; Asmussen,


S.; and Phelan, J.C. On describing and seeking to change
the experience of stigma. Psychiatric
Rehabilitation
Skills, 6(2):20l-231,2002.

Jorm, A.F.; Korten, A.E.; Jacomb, P.A.; Christensen, H.;


Rodgers, B.; and Pollitt, P. "Mental health literacy": A
survey of the public's ability to recognise mental disorders and their beliefs about the effectiveness of treatment. Medical Journal of Australia, 166(4): 182-186,
1997.

Link, B.G.; Struening, E.L.; Rahav, M.; Phelan, J.C; and


Nuttbrock, L. On stigma and its consequences: Evidence
from a longitudinal study of men with dual diagnoses of
mental illness and substance abuse. Journal of Health and
Social Behavior, 38(2):177-190, 1997.

Kai, J., and Crosland, A. Perspectives of people with


enduring mental ill health from a community-based quali-

tative study. British Journal of General Practice,

Lyons, M., and Ziviani, J. Stereotypes, stigma, and mental


illness: Learning from fieldwork experiences. American
Journal of Occupational Therapy, 49(10): 1002-1008,
1995.

51(470):730-736, 2001.
Kleinman, A.; Wang, W.Z.; Li, S.C.; Cheng, X.M.; Dai,
X.Y.; Li, K.T.; and Kleinman, J. The social course of
epilepsy: Chronic illness as social experience in interior
China. Social Science & Medicine, 40(10): 1319-1330,
1995.

Major, B.; Quinton, W.J.; and McCoy, S.K. Antecedents


and consequences of attributions to discrimination:
Theoretical and empirical advances. In: Zanna, M.P.,
(Eed). Advances in Experimental Social Psychology,
Vol. 34. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, 2002. pp.
251-330.

Lemert, E.M. Paranoia and the dynamics of exclusion.


Sociometry, 25(March):2-20, 1962.
Link, B.G. Mental patient status, work, and income: An
examination of the effects of psychiatric label. American
Sociological Review,, 47:202-215, 1982.

Martens, L., and Addington, J. The psychological wellbeing of family members of individuals with schizophrenia. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology,
36(3): 128-133, 2001.

Link, B.G. Understanding labeling effects in the area of


mental disorders: An assessment of the effects of expecta-

538

Downloaded from http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016

Link, B.G.;, Mirotznik, J.; and, Cullen, F.T. The effectiveness of stigma coping orientations: Can negative
consequences of mental illness labeling be avoided?
Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 32:302-320,
1991.
Link, B.G., and Phelan, J.C. Conceptualizing stigma.
Annual Review of Sociology, 27:363-385, 2001.
Link, B.G.; Phelan, J.C; Bresnahan, M.; Stueve, A.; and
Pescosolido, B.A. Public conceptions of mental illness:
Labels, causes, dangerousness, and social distance.
American Journal of Public Health,, 89(9): 1328-1333,
1999.
Link, B.G.; Struening, E.L.; Neese-Todd, S.; Asmussen,
S.; and Phelan, J.C. Stigma as a barrier to recovery: The
consequences of stigma for the self-esteem of people with
mental illnesses. Psychiatric Services, 52(12): 1621-1626,
2001.

Measuring Mental Illness Stigma

Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 30, No. 3, 2004

Martin, J.; Pescosolido, B.; and Tuch, S. Of fear and


loathing: The role of "disturbing behavior," labels, and
causal attributions in shaping public attitudes toward people with mental illness. Journal of Health and Social
Behavior, 41:208-223, 2000.

Pescosolido, B.A.; Martin, J.K.; Link, B.G.; Kikuzawa,


S.; Burgos, G.; Swindle, R.; and Phelan, J. America's
Views of Mental Health and Illness at Century's End:
Continuity and Change. Bloomington, IN: Indiana
Consortium for Mental Health Services Research,
2000.

Maxwell, J.A. Qualitative Research Design: An


Interactive
1996.

Approach.

Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage,

Phillips, D.L. Rejection: A possible consequence of seeking help for mental disorders. American Sociological
Review, 28:963-972, 1963.
Pinel, E.C. Stigma consciousness: The psychological
legacy of social stereotypes. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 76(1):114-128, 1999.

McAuley, E.; Duncan, T.E.; and Russell, D.W. Measuring


causal attributions: The revised Causal Dimension Scale
(CDSII). Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
18(5):566-573, 1992.

Morone, J.A. Enemies of the people: The moral dimension to public health. Journal of Health Politics, Policy
and Law, 22:993-1020, 1997.
Muhlbauer, S.A. Navigating the storm of mental illness:
Phases in the family's journey. Qualitative Health
Research, 12(8): 1076-1092, 2002.
Nunnally, J.C. Popular Conceptions of Mental Health:
Their Development and Change. New York, NY: Holt,
Rinehart, and Winston, 1961.
Nunnally, J.C, and Kittross, J.M. Public attitudes toward
mental health professions. American
Psychologist,
13(Sept):589-594, 1958.

Ridgeway, C.L., and Erikson, K.G. Creating and spread-

ing status beliefs. American Journal of Sociology,


106:579-615,2000.
Rosenfield, S. Labeling mental illness: The effects of
received services and perceived stigma on life
satisfaction. American Sociological
Review,

Olmsted, D.W., and Durham, K. Stability of mental health


attitudes: A semantic differential study. Journal of Health
and Social Behavior, 17:35-44, 1976.
Osgood, C.E.; Suci, G.J.; and Tannenbaum, P.H. The
Measurement of Meaning. Urbana, IL: University of
Illinois Press, 1957.

62(August):660-672, 1997.
Russell, D. The Causal Dimension Scale: A measure of
how individuals perceive causes. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 42:1137-1145, 1982.
Sayce, L. Stigma, discrimination and social exclusion:
What's in a word? Journal of Mental Health, 7:331-343,
1998.
Sayce, L. Beyond good intentions: Making anti-discrimination strategies work. Disability & Society, 18:625-642,
2003.
Sayre, J. The patient's diagnosis: Explanatory models of
mental illness. Qualitative Health Research, 10(l):71-83,
2000.
Scheff, T.J. The societal reaction to deviance: Ascriptive
elements in the psychiatric screening of mental patients in
a Midwestern state. Social Problems, 11 (Spring):
401^13, 1964.
Scheff, T.J. Shame in the labeling of mental illness. In:
Gilbert, P., and Andrews, B., eds. Shame: Interpersonal

Page, S. Effects of the mental illness label in attempts to


obtain accommodations. Canadian Journal of Behavioral
Science, 9:85-90, 1977.
Park, R.E. The concept of social distance. Journal of
Applied Sociology, 8:339-344, 1924.
Patton, M.Q. Qualitative Evaluation and
Methods. Newbury Park, CA: Sage, 1990.

Research

Penn, D.L., and Nowlin-Drummond, A. Politically correct


labels and schizophrenia: A rose by any other name?
Schizophrenia Bulletin, 27(2): 197-203, 2001.
Perlick, D.A.; Rosenheck, R.A.; Clarkin, J.F.; Sirey, J.A.;
Salahi, J.; Struening, E.L.; and Link, B.G. Stigma as a
barrier to recovery: Adverse effects of perceived stigma
on social adaptation of persons diagnosed with bipolar
affective disorder. Psychiatric Services, 52(12):
1627-1632,2001.

539

Downloaded from http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016

Pinfold, V.; Toulmin, H.; Thornicroft, G.; Huxley, P.;


Farmer, P.; and Graham, T. Reducing psychiatric stigma
and discrimination: Evaluation of educational interventions in U.K. secondary schools. British Journal of
Psychiatry, 182:342-346, 2003.
Priest, R.G.; Vize, C ; Roberts, A.; Roberts, M.; and
Tylee, A. Lay people's attitudes to treatment of depression: Results of opinion poll for Defeat Depression
Campaign just before its launch. British Medical Journal,
313:858-859, 1996.
Reisenzein, R. A structural equation analysis of Weiner's
attribution-affect model of helping behavior. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 42:1123-1133, 1986.

Mohr, W.K. A tale of two centuries: Voices of the past and


present. Archives of Psychiatric Nursing, 12(6):301310,
1998.

Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 30, No. 3, 2004

B.G. Link et al.

Behavior, Psychopathology, and Culture. New York, NY:


Oxford, 1998.
Schneider, J.W. Disability as moral experience: Epilepsy
and self in routine relationships. Journal of Social Issues,
44(l):63-78, 1988.

Szmukler, G.I.; Burgess, P.; Herrman, H.; Benson, A.;


Colusa, S.; and Bloch, S. Caring for relatives with serious
mental illness: The development of the Experience of
Caregiving Inventory. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric
Epidemiology, 31(3-4):137-148, 1996.

Schneider, J.W., and Conrad, P. In the closet with illnessepilepsy, stigma potential and information control.
Social Problems, 28:32^4, 1980.

Taylor, S.M., and Dear, M.J. Scaling community attitudes


toward the mentally ill. Schizophrenia
Bulletin,
7(2):225-240, 1981.

Shor, R., and Sykes, I.J. Introducing structured dialogue


with people with mental illness into the training of social

work students. Psychiatric Rehabilitation Journal,

and psychosis. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric

26(l):63-69, 2002.

Epidemiology, 36(7):343-347, 2001.


U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. Mental
Health: A Report of the Surgeon GeneralExecutive
Summary. Rockville, MD: U.S. Department of Health and
Human Services, Substance Abuse and Mental Health
Services Administration, Center for Mental Health
Services, National Institutes of Health, National Institute
of Mental Health, 1999.

Sibicky, M., and Dovidio, J. Stigma of psychotherapy:


Stereotypes, interpersonal reactions, and the self-fulfilling
prophecy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
33:148-154, 1986.
Sirey, J.A.; Bruce, M.L.; Alexopoulos, G.S.; Perlick,
D.A.; Friedman, S.J.; and Meyers, B.S. Stigma as a barrier to recovery: Perceived stigma and patient-rated
severity of illness as predictors of antidepressant drug
adherence. Psychiatric Services, 52(12): 16151620,
2001.

Wahl, O.F. Mental health consumers' experience of


stigma. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 25(3):467^78, 1999.
Wahl, O.F. Children's views of mental illness: A review of
the literature. Psychiatric Rehabilitation Skills, 5(2):
134-158,2002.
Weiner, B. An Attributional Theory of Motivation and
Emotion. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag, 1986.
Weiner, B.; Perry, R.P.; and Magnusson, J. An attributional analysis of reactions to stigmas. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 55(5):738-748, 1988.
Whatley, C D . Social attitudes toward discharged mental patients. Social Problems, 6:313-320, 1959.
Wig, N.N.; Suleiman, M.A.; Routledge, R.; Murthy,
R.S.; Ladrio-Ignacio, L.; Ibrahim, H.H.A.; Harding,
T.W. Community reactions to mental disorders: A key
informant study in three developing nations. Acta
Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 61:111-126, 1980.
Williams, B., and Healy, D. Disclosure of minor mental health problems: An exploratory theoretical study.
Journal of Advanced Nursing, 35(1): 108-116, 2001.

Stanton, A.A., and Schwartz, M.S. The Mental Hospital.


New York, NY: Basic Books, 1954.
Star, S. "The Public's Ideas About Mental Illness."
Paper presented at the annual meeting of the National
Association for Mental Health, Indianapolis, IN,
1955.
Steele, CM. Stereotyping and its threat are real. American
Psychologist, 53(6):680-681, 1997.
Steele, CM., and Aronson, J. Stereotype vulnerability and
the intellectual test performance of African Americans.

Journal of Personality

and Social

Psychology,

69:797-811, 1995.
Strauss, A., and Corbin, J. Basics of Qualitative Research:
Techniques and Procedures for Developing Grounded
Theory. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, 1998.
Struening, E. L., and Cohen, J. Factorial invariance and
other psychometric characteristics of five opinions about
mental illness factors. Educational and Psychological
Measurement, 23(2): 289-298, 1963.

Williams, M., and Taylor, J. Mental illness: Media perpetuation of stigma. Contemporary Nurse, 4:4146,
1995.
Wilson, C ; Nairn, R.; Coverdale, J.; and Panapa, A.
How mental illness is portrayed in children's television. A prospective study. British Journal of
Psychiatry, 176:440-443, 2000.

Struening, E.L.; Perlick, D.A.; Link, B.G.; Hellman, F.;


Herman, D.; and Sirey, J.A. Stigma as a barrier to recovery: The extent to which caregivers believe most people
devalue consumers and their families. Psychiatric
Services, 52(12): 1633-1638, 2001.

540

Downloaded from http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016

Taylor, S.M.; Dear, M.J.; and Hall, G.B. Attitudes toward


the mentally ill and reactions to mental health facilities.
Social Science & Medicine, 13D:281-29O, 1979.
Treasure, J.; Murphy, T.; Szmukler, G.; Todd, G.; Gavan,
K.; and Joyce, J. The experience of caregiving for severe
mental illness: A comparison between anorexia nervosa

Seeker, J.; Armstrong, C ; and Hill, M. Young people's


understanding of mental illness. Health Education
Research, 14(6):729-739, 1999.

Measuring Mental Illness Stigma

Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 30, No. 3, 2004

previous drafts. We thank Richard Carpiano for his careful review of the manuscript and for assistance in constructing the data base of stigma articles. Special thanks to
Emeline Otey for comments on previous versions and her
extensive support for this undertaking.

Yang, L., Fox, K. Ethnography in health research and


practices. Ambulatory Child Health, 5(4):339-349,
1999.
Yarrow, M.R.; Schwartz, C.G.; Murphy, H.S.; and
Deasy, L.C. The psychological meaning of mental illness in the family. Journal of Social Issues, 11:12-24,
1955.

The Authors

Acknowledgments
Support for this article was provided through Purchase
Order No. 263-MD-20281 from the National Institute of
Mental Health, National Institutes of Health, Department
of Health and Human Services; as well as grants 5T32
MH 13043 to Dr. Link, NIMH K02 MH 65330 to Dr.
Phelan, and NIMH K01 MHO 1691 to Dr. Collins. We
thank Matthias Angermeyer, Patrick Corrigan, David
Penn, Elmer Struening, and Otto Wahl for comments on

541

Downloaded from http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016

Bruce G. Link, Ph.D., is Professor of Epidemiology and


Sociomedical Sciences, Columbia University, New York,
NY; and Research Scientist, New York State Psychiatric
Institute, New York, NY. Lawrence H. Yang, Ph.D., is
Post-Doctoral Fellow, Columbia University. Jo C. Phelan,
Ph.D., is Associate Professor of Sociomedical Sciences,
Columbia University. Pamela Y. Collins, M.D., M.P.H., is
Assistant Professor of Epidemiology, Columbia
University; and Research Scientist, New York State
Psychiatric Institute.

Downloaded from http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on May 1, 2016

Вам также может понравиться