Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 6

SPECIAL ARTICLE

The enigma of facial beauty: Esthetics,


proportions, deformity, and controversy
Farhad B. Naini,a James P. Moss,b and Daljit S. Gillc
London, United Kingdom
The study of the face and the ability to alter its form have fascinated mankind for thousands of years. The
clinical ability to alter dentofacial form, whether through orthodontics, facial growth modification, or surgery,
requires an understanding of facial beauty, including the evaluation of facial esthetics, proportions, and
symmetry. The purposes of this article were to give a brief contemporary overview of our concepts of facial
beauty and esthetics and to consider a long-standing and controversial debate on the treatment of patients
with dentofacial deformities. (Am J Orthod Dentofacial Orthop 2006;130:277-82)

rthodontists have a special interest in facial


beauty, although, over time, the relative importance of esthetics has waxed and waned in
relation to other considerations. In part of his ongoing
review of orthodontic history, Wahl1 wrote, Now it
appears that facial esthetics is again in the forefront as
we realize why patients come to us in the first place.1
Perceptions of facial beauty are multifactorial, with
genetic, environmental, and cultural foundations.2 Beauty
can be defined as a combination of qualities that give
pleasure to the senses or to the mind. Esthetics is the
study of beauty and, to a lesser extent, its opposite, the
ugly. The 18th century philosopher Alexander Baumgarten, who established esthetics as a distinct field of
philosophy, coined the term, which is derived from the
Greek word for sensory perception (aisthesis).3
The first question to consider is difficult to answer:
is the origin of the human perception of facial beauty
dependent on each persons sense perception, or is this
sense common to all people? Is facial beauty a quality
of the observed face, or does the sensory enjoyment of
the observers also depend on their own ideas, feelings,
and judgments, which themselves have a direct relationship to sensory enjoyment. The philosopher David
Hume (1741) said, Beauty in things exists in the mind
which contemplates them,4 and the writer Margaret
a

Consultant, Director of Research and Graduate Education, Orthodontic Department, St Georges Hospital and Medical School, Kingston Hospital,
London, United Kingdom.
b
Professor, Orthodontic Department, Royal London Hospital, London, United
Kingdom.
c
Consultant, Orthodontic Department, Eastman Dental Hospital, London,
United Kingdom.
Reprint requests to: Dr Farhad B. Naini, Consultant, Orthodontic Department,
St Georges Hospital and Medical School, Blackshaw Rd, London, SW17 0QT,
United Kingdom; e-mail, farhad@naini.freeserve.co.uk.
Submitted, April 2005; revised and accepted, September 2005.
0889-5406/$32.00
Copyright 2006 by the American Association of Orthodontists.
doi:10.1016/j.ajodo.2005.09.027

Wolfe Hungerford (1878) famously said, Beauty is in


the eye of the beholder.5
Both of these quotations, and their respective philosophical ideologies, assume that the sense is subjective to each person. However, the 18th century
philosopher Francis Hutcheson said, Esthetic judgements are perceptual and take their authority from a
sense that is common to all who make them, and The
origin of our perceptions of beauty and harmony is
justly called a sense because it involves no intellectual
element, no reflection on principles and causes.6 The
philosopher Immanuel Kant (1790) reiterated this view,
saying that, The beautiful is that which pleases universally without a concept.7
Therefore, if a beautiful face pleases universally,
then some part of our sense perception must be common to all men and women. After all, when we describe
a face as beautiful, we do not merely mean that it
pleases us. We are describing the face, not our judgment. We will often point to features of the face to back
up our statement.
A paradox therefore emerges. Obviously one cannot make a judgment about the beauty of a face one has
never seen. Therefore, facial beauty is related to some
quality of the observed face that tends to be universally
accepted. However, each persons ideas and feelings,
like a conditioned response, also have a direct relationship to his or her judgment, hence the difference in the
extent of rating a face as beautiful depending on the
eye of the beholder.
This philosophical question and its related argument continue to be debated. However, beauty undoubtedly has a strong influence on human life. According to Shakespeare, Beauty itself doth of itself
persuade the eyes of men without an orator.8
The philosopher Pascal commented, Had Cleopatras nose been shorter, the whole face of the world
277

278 Naini, Moss, and Gill

would have changed!9 From Homers Helen of Troy,


whom the poet Christopher Marlowe described as having
a face that launched a thousand ships,10 to Queen
Nefertiti, whose name literally means the Perfect One,
to modern models and actors, facial beauty has always
been the most valued aspect of human beauty.
The next question therefore is how do we know that
a face is beautiful? What guides and validates our
judgment?
Our perception of facial beauty might have its
foundation in our heredity, environment, or perhaps
both. Langlois et al11 found that infants as young as 3
months of age can distinguish between attractive and
unattractive faces, showing signs of preference for the
former. It is unlikely that by 3 months of age an infant
will have been subjected to or responded to any cultural
or environmental influences; therefore, this is evidence
to support a genetic theory. The evolutionary basis is
that facial beauty, including facial symmetry and secondary sexual characteristics, is a requirement for
sexual selection, leading to reproduction.12
A study by Martin13 found that both white and
black American men preferred black female faces with
white features, whereas black African men showed a
preference for black female faces with Negroid features. This lends support to environmental or cultural
reasons for the human perception of facial beauty.
However, Perrett et al14 found that both white and
Japanese men and women ranked female faces as most
attractive when youthful facial features, such as large
eyes, high cheekbones, and narrow jaws, were evident.
Esthetic judgments therefore seem to be similar across
various cultural backgrounds. A meta-analysis by Langlois et al15 seemed to confirm that there is crosscultural agreement about facial attractiveness.
Studies by Sir Francis Galton,16 Charles Darwins
cousin, provide evidence to support averageness as the
ideal,17 with composite facial photographs of subjects
gaining higher attractiveness ratings than individual
facial photographs.18 However, Perrett et al14 showed
that, contrary to this averageness hypothesis, the mean
shape of a group of attractive faces is preferred to the
mean shape of the sample from which the faces were
selected. In addition, attractive composite faces were
made more attractive by exaggerating the shape differences from the sample mean. Therefore, an average
face shape is attractive but might not be optimally
attractive,19 and highly attractive facial configurations
are not necessarily average.20
FACIAL PROPORTIONS

The assessment of facial beauty is essentially subjective.21 In the 16th century, the artist Albrecht Drer

American Journal of Orthodontics and Dentofacial Orthopedics


September 2006

said, I know not what beauty is, but I know that it


affects many things in life. Drer explained that,
although the concept of facial beauty was immersed in
subjectivity, the assessment of facial proportions could
be undertaken objectively.22 He maintained that disproportionate human faces were unesthetic, whereas proportionate features were acceptable if not always beautiful.
Therefore, the appropriate goal for the orthodontists
clinical examination is to find facial disproportions.23 The
inevitable question is, therefore, where did the evidence
for ideal facial proportions originate?
In representing the human form, painters and sculptors in the past developed many canons or guidelines.
These were mainly based on the good eye of the
respective artist, which is anecdotal evidence. However, many of these guidelines are still used by clinicians today, albeit somewhat modified from the originals.
A major preoccupation of Greek sculptors was the
idealization of human proportions. In the fifth century
BC, one of the most famous, Polycleitus, wrote a
theoretical work that discussed ideal mathematical
proportions for the parts of the human body. In his
Canon, he also described the importance of the concept
of symmetry in the human form, called symmetria. In
the first century BC, the Roman architect Marcus
Vitruvius Pollio wrote his famous work, The Ten Books
on Architecture. In Book 2, Chapter 2, Vitruvius
defined symmetry as a proper agreement between the
members of the work itself, and relation between the
different parts and the whole general scheme, in accordance with a certain part selected as standard. Thus in
the human body there is a kind of symmetrical harmony
. . . and so it is with perfect buildings.24 Although the
relationship between bilateral facial symmetry and
beauty remains to be clarified,25 evidence seems to
suggest that mild facial asymmetry is compatible with
facial beauty.26 Vitruvius also described the facial
trisection, emphasizing that the ideal face can be
divided vertically into 3 distinct thirds, a concept still
used today in planning facial surgery.
The great Renaissance artist and thinker Leonardo
da Vinci emphasized the importance of harmony between art and science. Leonardo defined proportion as
the ratio between the respective parts and the whole.27
The figure of Vitruvian man (Fig 1), which Leonardo
based on guidelines described by Vitruvius, shows the
importance of proportions in the human form. He also
studied the proportions of the human head (Fig 2). The
distance from the hairline to the inferior aspect of the
chin is one-tenth of a mans height. The distance from
the top of the head to the inferior aspect of the chin is
one-eighth of a mans height. These have important

American Journal of Orthodontics and Dentofacial Orthopedics


Volume 130, Number 3

Fig 1. Leonardo da Vincis Vitruvian man, ca 1490. This


famous figure shows that proportionate human form fits
perfectly in perfect geometric shapes circle and square,
with navel at center. Vertical facial trisection is shown.
Vertical face height (hairline to inferior aspect of chin) is
one tenth of standing height. Interestingly, this is equal
to length of hand (courtesy of Gallerie dellAccademia,
Venice).

clinical implications. If a patients vertical facial proportions are to be altered with surgery, the treatment
plan must take into account the proportion of the
patients total face height to his or her standing height
and stature. The use of absolute numeric values of
facial measurements rather than facial proportions can
be misleading, because the vertical facial height of a
patient who is 6 feet tall is different from that of a
patient 5 feet tall.
Drer, in 1525, maintained the importance of studying facial proportions, criticizing artists of his day, and
saying, They have not learnt Geometry, without which
no one can either be or become an absolute artist.22
Therefore, the guidelines used by clinicians today are
mainly based on those initially described in art. The

Naini, Moss, and Gill 279

Fig 2. Leonardo da Vincis Male head in profile with


proportions, ca 1490. Vitruvian anterior vertical facial
thirds are evident: hairline to eyebrows, eyebrows to
base of nose, base of nose to below chin. Lower facial
third is again divided into upper third (upper lip) and
lower two-thirds. Ear is one third of facial height. What
later came to be described as Frankfort plane and its
perpendicular, facial vertical from soft-tissue nasion,
are also shown (courtesy of Gallerie dellAccademia,
Venice).

orthodontic specialty, however, has been at the forefront in the assessment of the facial soft-tissue profile,28
mainly because of the use of lateral cephalometric
radiographs that provide many analyses.29-35
Clinicians therefore can assess facial esthetics more
objectively by diagnosing and helping to correct facial
disproportions.
Since the establishment of our specialty over 100
years ago, orthodontic theory and practice have been
based on the Angle paradigm.36 The goal of treatment
was to produce perfect occlusion of all the teeth, and
facial beauty was thought to follow. Although this
concept was discredited with the introduction and
growth of cephalometrics, the basic idea that the
dentofacial skeleton determined the goals of treatment
remained intact.37
It is only recently that the concept of the soft-tissue

280 Naini, Moss, and Gill

paradigm, focusing the diagnosis and treatment of


dentofacial problems on the soft tissues of the face
rather than on dentoskeletal structures, has emerged in
orthodontics and orthognathic surgery.23 The dentoskeletal structures of the face are like the scaffold
over which the soft tissues drape. However, it is the
soft-tissue proportions, not the skeletal proportions,
that are the goals of treatment.38 Proffit et al23 have led
the way in the emergence of this paradigm shift,
placing greater emphasis on the clinical examination of
the patient and our assessment of the soft-tissue
changes that occur with each dentoskeletal change and
with age, thus allowing for greater accuracy in treatment planning.
FACIAL PROPORTIONS AND THE
GOLDEN PROPORTION

An often-quoted but rarely substantiated concept is


that of the golden proportion.39 The mathematician
Euclid (ca 325-265 BC) described this in The Elements,
his treatise on mathematics. The origin of this concept
is unknown, having been attributed to both Pythagoras
and Plato. In his edition of Euclids Elements, the
mathematician Luca Pacioli (1509) renamed the golden
proportion the divine proportion because he thought
the concept could not be rationalized. Pacioli also
published a treatise entitled De Divina Proportione (On
Divine Proportion) for which Leonardo da Vinci drew
figures of symmetrical and proportionate faces and
bodies. Later in that century (1597), the first known
calculation of the golden proportion as a decimal was
given by Maestlin in a letter to his former pupil,
Kepler.40 The number is 0.618 for the length of the
longer segment of a line of length 1 when it is divided
in the golden proportion. The ratio of the shorter
section to the longer section of the line is equal to the
ratio of the longer section to the whole line. The point
at which the line is divided is known as the golden
section and is represented by the symbol (phi)
derived from the name of the Greek sculptor Phidias
who incorporated it into the architectural design of the
Parthenon.41
There have been attempts to correlate ideal facial
proportions with the golden proportion.42 However, in
a 3-dimensional study analyzing the faces of professional models, the authors found that they did not fit the
golden proportion and, interestingly, that they had
various malocclusions and a wide range of cephalometric values.43 In another study of the esthetic improvement
of patients having orthognathic surgery, the authors found
that, whereas most subjects were considered more attractive after treatment than before, the proportions were
equally likely to move away from or toward the golden

American Journal of Orthodontics and Dentofacial Orthopedics


September 2006

proportion.44 Therefore, more research evidence is


required to substantiate the true significance of this
fascinating concept in the clinical assessment of facial
esthetics.
FACIAL DEFORMITY AND THE
CONTROVERSIAL DEBATE

The final question concerns patients with facial


anomalies of varying magnitudes and whether they
should be treated, especially when the deformity is not
part of an active disease process.
A persons self-perception of facial appearance is of
utmost importance. It has been said, Nothing has so
marked influence on the direction of a mans mind as
his appearance, and not his appearance itself so much
as his conviction that it is attractive or unattractive.45
There is, of course, considerable individual variation in
peoples abilities to adapt to their facial deformities,
whatever the severity, with some remaining comparatively unaffected and others having significant difficulties that affect their quality of life. It has even been
argued that facial deformity might be a social disability,
because it impacts not only the person affected, but also
contributes to the opinions other people form of them.46
Although these opinions can change as interpersonal
relationships develop, a persons first impression on
others might well affect his or her self-esteem and
quality of life.47
Research seems to indicate that mild to moderate
facial deformities actually cause a patient greater psychological distress than severe facial deformities.48
This could be because other peoples reactions towards
milder deformities are less predictable, whereas more
severe deformities tend to evoke more consistent reactions, albeit negative, allowing the patient to develop
better coping strategies. The variability in peoples
reactions to milder facial deformities also causes considerable patient distress. Most patients seeking orthodontic treatment or facial esthetic surgery fit into the
mild or moderate category in terms of facial deformities, as opposed to craniofacial malformation syndromes or severe facial trauma or disease.
There is a long-standing and controversial debate
about the ability to alter facial appearance. The debate
centers on whether a patient with a facial deformity
should learn to live with his or her facial appearance.
This argument states that a psychologically healthy
patient should be able to adjust to the social environment. The supporters of this view also state that
education of the public and changes in public attitudes
toward visible facial differences are the correct way
forward. The argument against this case states that the
negative effects of facial deformity on a patients

Naini, Moss, and Gill 281

American Journal of Orthodontics and Dentofacial Orthopedics


Volume 130, Number 3

psychosocial well being are enough to warrant clinical


intervention. The 2 sides seem unable to reach common
ground.
We believe that there is merit in both sides of the
argument, and that they are not mutually exclusive.
There is no doubt that attitudes in society toward all
types of deformity have changed, and public education
is undoubtedly the most important factor, with charities
such as Changing Faces49 helping to both educate the
public and change public opinion regarding facial
deformities. Modern societys media-fuelled obsession
with the so-called perfect appearance is at best unhelpful and at worst detrimental. People compare themselves to those who are unrealistically portrayed by the
media to represent cultural ideals of beauty, and, if their
own appearances do not measure up, this might result
in body-image dissatisfaction, possibly leading to altered self-recognition and conditions such as eating
disorders and body dysmorphic disorder.50 Therefore,
although it is unrealistic to expect the medias obsession with appearance to significantly change, greater
responsibility on the medias part would be commended. In addition, television documentaries, perhaps
interviewing patients and families of patients with
dentofacial anomalies, and following patients through
their daily lives as they describe their challenges, would
undoubtedly be useful for educating the general public.
Such education might help lessen societys intolerance
for what is considered an abnormal facial appearance
and contribute to transforming the publics perception
of those with facial deformities.
However, it would be simplistic and possibly unethical to withhold treatment on the basis that the
problem is with attitudes in society alone. If treatment
can significantly improve a patients quality of life,
then it is for the patient to make the ultimate informed
decision of whether treatment is the correct way forward.
CONCLUSIONS

The aim of this article was to briefly describe our


contemporary understanding of facial beauty and to
debate the challenges faced by patients and clinicians in
the treatment of dentofacial deformities. There is no
doubt that the philosophical debate and the research
will continue, because our clinical practice should
always be based on a sound knowledge of theory. One
may only conclude that patients requiring alterations in
facial appearance remain a considerable clinical challenge.

REFERENCES
1. Wahl N. Orthodontics in 3 millenia. Chapter 1: antiquity to the
mid-19th century. Am J Orthod Dentofacial Orthop 2005;127:
255-9.
2. Naini FB, Moss JP. Three-dimensional assessment of the relative
contribution of genetics and environment to various facial
parameters with the twin method. Am J Orthod Dentofacial
Orthop 2004;126:655-65.
3. Baumgarten AG. Aesthetica. Paris: Lherne; 1989.
4. Hume D. Of Tragedy in essays, literary, moral and political.
London: Oxford University Press; 1963.
5. Hungerford MW. Molly Bawn. London: Smith, Elder & Co;
1890.
6. Hutcheson F. An inquiry into the original of our ideas of beauty
and virtue. Indianapolis: Liberty Fund; 2004.
7. Kant I. The critique of judgement. London: Oxford University
Press; 1973.
8. Shakespeare W. The rape of Lucrece. London: New Penguin
Shakespeare; 1971.
9. Pascal B. Pensees. London: Penguin Classics; 1966.
10. Marlowe C. Doctor Faustus. New York: Dover Publications;
1995.
11. Langlois JH, Roggman LA, Casey RJ, Ritter JM, Rieser-Danner
LA, Jenkins VY. Infant preferences for attractive faces: rudiments of a stereotype? Dev Psychol 1987;23:363-9.
12. Jones S. Almost like a whale: the origin of species updated.
London: Doubleday; 1999.
13. Martin JG. Racial ethnocentrism and judgment of beauty. J Soc
Psychol 1964;63:59-63.
14. Perrett DI, May KA, Yoshikawa S. Facial shape and judgements
of female attractiveness. Nature 1994;368:239-42.
15. Langlois JH, Kalanakis LE, Rubenstein AJ, Larson AD, Hallam
MJ, Smoot MT. Maxims or myths of beauty: a meta-analytic and
theoretical overview. Psychol Bull 2000;126:390-423.
16. Galton F. Composite portraits, made by combining those of
many different persons in a single resultant figure. J Anthropol
Inst Gr Brit Ir 1879;8:132-44.
17. Edler RJ. Background considerations to facial aesthetics.
J Orthod 2001;28:159-68.
18. Langlois JH, Roggman LA. Attractive faces are only average.
Psychol Sci 1990;1:115-21.
19. Arvystas M. Orthodontic management of agenesis and other
complexities: an interdisciplinary approach to functional esthetics. New York: Martin Dunitz Ltd; 2003.
20. Alley TR, Cunningham MR. Averaged faces are attractive, but
very attractive faces are not average. Psychol Sci 1991;2:123-5.
21. Peck H, Peck S. A concept of facial esthetics. Angle Orthod
1970;40:284-317.
22. Drer A. The art of measurement. San Francisco: Alan Wofsy
Fine Arts; 1981.
23. Proffit WR, White RP, Sarver DM. Contemporary treatment of
dentofacial deformity. St Louis: Mosby; 2003.
24. Howe TN. Vitruvius: the ten books on architecture. London:
Cambridge University Press; 1999.
25. Rhodes G, Sumich A, Byatt G. Are average facial configurations
attractive only because of their symmetry? Psychol Sci 1999;10:
52-8.
26. Zaidel DW, Cohen JA. The face, beauty, and symmetry: perceiving asymmetry in beautiful faces. Int J Neurosci 2005;115:
1165-73.
27. Pedretti C. Leonardo da Vinci: notebook of a genius. Milan:
Powerhouse Publishing; 2001.

282 Naini, Moss, and Gill

28. Downs WB. Analysis of the dentofacial profile. Angle Orthod


1956;26:191-211.
29. Steiner CC. Cephalometrics for you and me. Am J Orthod
1953;39:729-55.
30. Ricketts RM. A foundation for cephalometric communication.
Am J Orthod 1960;46:330-57.
31. Merrifield LL. The profile line as an aid in critically evaluating
facial esthetics. Am J Orthod 1966;52:804-22.
32. Holdaway RA. A soft tissue cephalometric analysis and its
application in orthodontic treatment planning. Part 1. Am J
Orthod 1983;84:1-28.
33. Holdaway RA. A soft tissue cephalometric analysis and its
application in orthodontic treatment planning. Part 2. Am J
Orthod 1984;85:279-93.
34. Bass NM. The aesthetic analysis of the face. Eur J Orthod
1991;13:343-50.
35. Bergman RT. Cephalometric soft tissue facial analysis. Am J
Orthod Dentofacial Orthop 1999;116:373-89.
36. Ackerman JL. Orthodontics: art, science or trans-science? Angle
Orthod 1974;44:243-50.
37. Park YC, Burstone CJ. Soft tissue profile: the fallacies of hard
tissue standards in treatment planning. Am J Orthod Dentofacial
Orthop 1986;90:52-62.
38. Sarver DM. Esthetic orthodontics and orthognathic surgery. St
Louis: Mosby; 1998.
39. Ricketts RM. Divine proportion in facial esthetics. Clin Plast
Surg 1982;9:401-22.

American Journal of Orthodontics and Dentofacial Orthopedics


September 2006

40. Herz-Fischler R. A mathematical history of the golden number.


New York: Dover Publications; 1998.
41. Green CD. All that glitters: a review of psychological research on
the aesthetics of the golden section. Perception 1995;24:937-68.
42. Ricketts RM. The biologic significance of the divine proportion
and Fibonacci series. Am J Orthod 1982;81:351-70.
43. Moss JP, Linney AD, Lowey MN. The use of three-dimensional
techniques in facial esthetics. Semin Orthod 1995;1:94-104.
44. Baker BW, Woods MG. The role of the divine proportion in the
esthetic improvement of patients undergoing combined orthodontic/orthognathic surgical treatment. Int J Adult Orthod
Orthognath Surg 2001;16:108-20.
45. Tolstoy LN. Childhood, boyhood and youth. London: Penguin
Books; 1970.
46. Macgregor F. After plastic surgery: adaptation and adjustment.
New York: Praeger; 1979.
47. Rumsey N, Clarke A, White P, Wyn-Williams M, Garlick W.
Altered body image: appearance-related concerns of people with
visible disfigurement. J Adv Nurs 2004;48:443-53.
48. Macgregor F. Social and psychological implications of dentofacial disfigurement. Angle Orthod 1970;40:231-3.
49. Coutinho W. Changing faces: facing disfigurement with confidence. London: Changing Faces; 2001.
50. Sarwer DB, Wadden TA, Pertschuk MJ, Whitaker LA. Body
image dissatisfaction and body dysmorphic disorder in 100
cosmetic surgery patients. Plast Reconstr Surg 1998;101:1644-9.

Вам также может понравиться