Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 36

Accepted Manuscript

The Long-Term Benefits of Dog Ownership in Families with Children with Autism
Sophie S. Hall, Hannah F. Wright, Annette Hames, PAWS Team
PII:

S1558-7878(16)30034-X

DOI:

10.1016/j.jveb.2016.04.003

Reference:

JVEB 965

To appear in:

Journal of Veterinary Behavior

Received Date: 17 November 2015


Revised Date:

29 March 2016

Accepted Date: 12 April 2016

Please cite this article as: Hall, S.S., Wright, H.F., Hames, A., PAWS Team, The Long-Term Benefits
of Dog Ownership in Families with Children with Autism, Journal of Veterinary Behavior (2016), doi:
10.1016/j.jveb.2016.04.003.
This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to
our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo
copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please
note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all
legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1

The Long-Term Benefits of Dog Ownership in Families with Children with Autism

Authors: Sophie S. Hall. Hannah F. Wright. Annette Hames. PAWS Team. Daniel S. Mills.

Corresponding Author: S. S. Hall

University of Lincoln

Joseph Banks Laboratories

School of Life Sciences

Lincoln,

Lincolnshire,

M
AN
U

SC

RI
PT

LN6 7DL

11

Email: shall@lincoln.ac.uk

12

Tel: 0781739077

13

H. F. Wright: University of Lincoln, Lincoln, UK; hwright@lincoln.ac.uk

14

A. Hames: University of Lincoln, Lincoln, UK; aehames@hotmail.com

15

D. S. Mills; University of Lincoln, Lincoln, UK; dmills@lincoln.ac.uk

16

PAWS Team; Dogs for Good, Banbury, UK; peter.gorbing@dogsforthedisabled.org

18

19

20

EP

AC
C

17

TE
D

10

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Abstract

There is growing interest in Animal Assisted Therapy in the treatment of Autism Spectrum

Disorders. Despite the potential promise for pet dog ownership to improve the lives of those

affected by autism, there is limited research in this area. This study is the first to explore the

long-term effects of acquiring a pet dog. Using standardized self-report measures, families

who had acquired a pet dog (intervention group; n = 22) showed significantly improved

family functioning in comparison to control group families (n = 15, with no dog). Both

groups showed reductions in domains of parenting stress. These reductions were more

evident in the intervention group; 20% of parents moved from clinically high to normal stress

M
AN
U

SC

RI
PT

10

levels. In the domain of parent-child dysfunctional interactions reductions were only

11

observed in the intervention group. A significant positive relationship was observed between

12

parenting stress of the childs main carer and their attachment to the dog.

16

17

18

19

20

21

EP

15

Keywords: Pet dogs; Autism Spectrum Disorder; Family Functioning; Parenting Stress.

AC
C

14

TE
D

13

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Introduction

Neurodevelopmental impairments, including autism, form the largest group of disabled

children in the UK and USA (Blackburn et al. 2012; Perou et al., 2013). Autism spectrum

disorder (ASD) is a heterogeneous condition defined by the DSM-5 as a person experiencing

persistent difficulties in verbal and non-verbal interactions, which result in functional

limitations (e.g., in a social and educational context). These problems must have been evident

in early childhood, cause significant impairment in functioning and not be explainable by

intellectual disorders or developmental delays (DSM-IV, APA 2013). Parents to children

with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) often have reduced quality of life, with high anxiety

10

and stress related problems (Dunn et al., 2001), in comparison to other parents (Lach et al.,

11

2009). There is growing recognition that caregiver and family-based factors influence the

12

effectiveness of ASD treatments (Fisman et al., 2000; Tunali & Power, 2002). Research

13

suggests that supporting the childs main carer directly benefits the child, improving behavior

14

management (e.g. Brereton & Tonge 2005; Tonge et al., 2006; Green et al. 2010) and sibling

15

adjustment (Quintero, 2010), suggesting that the development of effective interventions that

16

support the wider family unit may also bring direct benefits to the child with ASD.

SC

M
AN
U

TE
D

EP

17

RI
PT

There is growing scientific and clinical interest in the value of placing trained autism
assistance dogs in the homes of children with ASD. Studies have shown that autism

19

assistance dogs increase child safety, outdoor access, and enhance communication and social

20

interaction with other people (Burrows et al., 2008; Redefer & Goodman, 1989). Parental

21

reports suggest that the presence of an assistance dog in the home results in reduced child

22

anxiety, and this is supported by studies showing decreased cortisol awakening response in

23

children with autism following placement of the dog, which increase again following removal

24

of the animal (Viau et al., 2010). It is possible that the calming effect of the dog on the child

25

and the ability for the family to engage in activities outside the home also benefits wider

AC
C

18

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
family members, including the childs main carer. Furthermore, as the dog is likely to be

primarily cared for by the childs main caregiver the presence of the dog may bring direct

benefits to the caregiver, in terms of a therapeutic stress reducing effect (e.g., Allen et al.,

1991, 2001) and by providing the opportunity to get outside of the home environment by

taking the dog for a walk, which may increase the opportunity for social interactions

(McNicholas & Collis, 2000). If such mechanisms improve quality of life for the carer then

this may have a consequently positive effect on the childs ASD behaviors.

RI
PT

The hypothesis that the presence of a dog in the family may bring wider benefits to

the family members, such as reduced stress through mechanisms such as improved family

10

behaviors, increased me time, greater social interactions when engaging in dog walking,

11

and reduced stress through the therapeutic contact with dog, may all be achieved from a pet

12

dog as opposed to a trained assistance dog. Only recently have studies begun to look at how

13

pets, without any specific training, may offer similar benefits to children with ASD and their

14

families. These studies report increase improved pro-social behaviors (Bystrm & Lundqvist

15

Persson, 2015; Grandgeorge et al., 2012), a reduction in restrictive behavior patterns

16

(Bystrm & Lundqvist Persson, 2015) and improved child interactions and bonding

17

experiences (Carlisle, 2014) with the addition of a pet (not necessarily a dog). Only one

18

known study has reported the effects of acquiring a pet dog to the main carer to a child with

19

ASD. Wright et al. (2015a, 2015b) measured family functioning and parenting stress in

20

families with a child with autism during the first year of dog ownership (intervention group;

21

n=42) in comparison to families who did not acquire a pet dog during this time (control

22

group; n=28). Significant improvements in family functioning (reduced family weaknesses,

23

increased strengths) were identified in the intervention group (n=42; dog owners) compared

24

to the control group (n=28; non-dog owners). The intervention group also showed significant

25

improvements in the parenting stress (total stress, parental distress and difficult child

AC
C

EP

TE
D

M
AN
U

SC

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
domains) in comparison to the control group (Wright et al., 2015b). These findings are

compatible with evidence that suggests that pets can provide a pivotal role in family

functioning (Cain, 1983; Walsh, 2009) and that pets can help reduce depressive moods

(Krause-Parello, 2012), stress (Allen et al., 1991), and offer comfort in times of need

(McConnell et al., 2011).

RI
PT

These investigations suggest that acquiring a pet dog can bring a range of benefits to
families affected by ASD during the first year of dog ownership, but the durability of the

benefits reported remain unknown. Given that acquiring a dog is a life-long commitment, it is

essential that realistic expectations are set for any potential long-term benefits. Therefore, the

10

aim of this study was to evaluate the longer term effects of dog ownership in the families who

11

were studied by Wright et al. (2015a, 2015b), approximately 2.5 years after initially acquiring

12

a pet dog, using the same outcome measures employed in the original studies. When

13

considering the long term effects of dog ownership, it is also useful to consider the

14

attachment bond that has formed between the dog and the main parent carer over time, since

15

this might affect the value derived from the relationship. Therefore, a secondary aim of the

16

study was to explore relationships between family functioning and parenting stress and pet

17

attachment.

EP

Method

AC
C

18

TE
D

M
AN
U

SC

19

Participants

20

Participants were recruited for the original studies on a voluntary basis via Dogs for the

21

Disableds PAWS (Parents Autism Workshops and Support) network (Dog for the Disabled

22

2013; since re-named Dogs for Good) and advertisements through the National Autistic

23

Society (see Wright et al., 2015a, 2015b for further details). Participants were asked to take

24

part in the study if their child had a confirmed diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder and was

25

aged between 3-16 years. Because of the heterogeneous nature of ASD we did not have

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
exclusion criteria relating to the condition for participation, which allowed us to obtain a

sample that reflected the disparity of characteristics of families in the general population. All

children had received a clinical diagnosis through Children and Adolescent Mental Health

Services (CAMHS), and this diagnostic process was confirmed by the parents. These

families/parents had received no specialized service dog training for children with ASD.

RI
PT

Contact was made with parents, who had completed the scales at the last data collection

point, and whom we knew had not requested to be withdrawn from any future studies. There

were 42 sets of parents in the intervention group and 24 in the control group. In the

SC

intervention group 22 (52.4%) of these families chose to participate in the long-term follow

11

up; 7 families were not contactable via phone or email; 13 withdrew from the study (reasons:

12

1 re-homed dog, 2 family stressors; 10 chose not to be involved/ did not provide an

13

explanation). The average age of the dog originally acquired by these families was 3.35

14

months (mean) 4.65 (standard deviation) (range: 2 24 months); 13 of the dogs were

15

female and 9 were male; 15 were purebreds (2 cocker spaniels, 2 Cavalier King Charles

16

spaniels, 2 retrievers, 2 miniature schnauzers, 2 Labradors, 1 Jack Russell terrier, 1 West

17

Highland white terrier, 1 fox terrier, 1 Border collie, 1 Bernese mountain dog) and 7 were

18

cross-breeds (3 spaniels poodles, 3 Labradors poodles, 1 Labrador whippet). In the

19

control group 15 (53.6%) families participated; 9 families withdrew (reasons: 3 obtained a

20

dog, 2 were not contactable, 4 chose not to be involved). Across the intervention and control

21

group data was collected from 37 families.

22

AC
C

EP

TE
D

M
AN
U

10

Family functioning (Brief FAM-III-General Scale; FAM-III-GS). Of the 37

23

participants who responded to the FAM-III GS one participant in the control group declined

24

to answer some of the questions. The remaining data set comprised 36 participants, 22 in the

25

intervention group and 14 in the control group (see Table 1). The time elapsed since baseline

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1

measures were taken (pre-intervention; up to 17 weeks before acquiring a dog for the

intervention group and matched time points for the control group) was 2.61 years 0.05

(Mean Standard Error Mean), since post-intervention measures were taken it was 2.37

years 0.06, and 1.91 years 0.05 since follow-up measures were recorded.
Parenting stress (Parenting Stress Index-Short Form; PSI-SF). Of the 37

RI
PT

participants who completed the PSI-SF, three participants (2 = intervention; 1 = control) were

removed from analysis for low scores (10 or below) on the defensive responding scale,

indicating that that their responses may be biased to present a favourable impression, in

accordance with the PSI manual (Abidin, 1995). The time elapsed since baseline measures

10

were taken (pre-intervention) was 2.71 years 0.07 (Mean Standard Error Mean), since

11

post-intervention measures were taken it was 2.51 years 0.07, and 2.00 years 0.07 since

12

follow-up measures were recorded. Demographics for the sample retained in the analysis are

13

provided in Table 1.

M
AN
U

SC

[Insert Table 1]

TE
D

14

Apparatus and Materials

16

With the aim of being able to make direct comparisons with original studies conducted by

17

Wright et al. (2015a, 2015b) we replicated the tests used in these studies.
Family functioning. To measure family functioning we used the Brief FAM-III,

AC
C

18

EP

15

19

General Scale (Skinner et al., 1995). We used the General Scale (14 items), which is designed

20

to measure basic family functioning, is suitable for use when measuring family functioning

21

over time (Skinner et al., 1995) and is effective at discriminating between problem and non-

22

problem families (see Skinner et al., 1983). Example items include We feel loved in our

23

family and We can rely on family members to do their part. Each item is scored on a four-

24

point scale, from 0 = strongly agree, to 3 = strongly disagree. Seven items are reverse scored.

25

Reported alpha coefficients for the General Scale are high, = 0.93 (Skinner et al., 2000).

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1

Parenting stress. To measure parenting stress we administered the Parenting Stress


Index (III edition), Short Form (PSI-SF; Abidin, 1995). The PSI-SF is an abbreviated version

of the original 120 item form; comprised of 36 questions, it takes approximately 10 minutes

to complete. The 36 questions measure three domains of stress; 12 items measure Parental

Distress (PD) (e.g. I often have the feeling that I cannot handle things very well), 12 items

Parent-Child Dysfunctional Interaction (P-CDI) (e.g. My child rarely does things for me that

make me feel very good), and 12 items measure perception of Difficult Child (DC) (e.g.

My child seems to cry or fuss more often than other children). These three sub-scales (PD,

P-CDI, DC) combine to provide a score of Total Stress (TS). Items are scored on a five-point

10

scale from Strongly Agree to Strongly Disagree. The PSI has been well used with parents

11

to children with autism (e.g. Hastings, 2003; Hoffman et al., 2009; ZaidmanZait et al. 2010).

12

The PSI short form was designed for use by clinicians working under time restrictions in

13

primary health care settings and for research purposes. As documented by Abidin (1995) the

14

short form scales have satisfactory test-retest reliability scores (TS: 0.84, PD: 0.85, P-CDI:

15

0.68, DC: 0.78) and good internal reliability coefficients () (TS: 0.91, PD: 0.87, P-CDI:

16

0.80, DC: 0.85).

SC

M
AN
U

TE
D

EP

17

RI
PT

Attachment to the dog. We used the widely used Lexington Attachment to Pets
Scale (LAPS; Johnson et al., 1992) to measure the affectionate bond between the main parent

19

carer and the dog. This 23 item scale includes items such as Quite often I confide in my pet

20

and I love my pet because it never judges me. Each question is scored on a four point scale

21

from 0 = strongly disagree, to 3 = strongly agree, with two items being reverse scored. The

22

questions comprise one scale with a high coefficient = 0.93 (Johnson et al., 1992).

23

Procedure

24

Parents provided written informed consent. All testing procedures complied with BPS Ethics

25

Code of Conduct (2009) and were approved by the Life Sciences University of Lincoln

AC
C

18

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Ethics committee. Parents were contacted via the telephone at an agreed date and time. The

researcher read the questions and response options to the parents and recorded the parents

responses. Forms were scored anonymously; the researcher who collected the data did not

have access to the parents responses from the earlier studies. No time restriction was set on

answering the questions; the interview typically lasted for 40-45 minutes. Participants were

assured of their anonymity and encouraged to answer as honestly as possible.

Data Analysis

To assess whether scores on family functioning and parenting stress data were significantly

different at long-term follow up between the intervention and control group, and to assess

M
AN
U

SC

RI
PT

whether scores on these scales changed over time we conducted analysis of co-variance

11

(ANCOVA). The within subjects factor was time (post-intervention, follow-up, and long term

12

follow-up). The between subjects factor was group (intervention, control). We used baseline

13

scores as co-variate to control for any differences between the groups due to sampling

14

characteristics prior to the start of the intervention. To explore whether the owners

15

attachment to the dog was related to scores of family functioning and parenting we conducted

16

a Pearsons correlation between scores on family functioning (FAM-III-GS) and parenting

17

stress (PSI-SF) with scores on pet attachment (LAPS).

EP

We recognised that the reduction in sample size over the study period may alter the

AC
C

18

TE
D

10

19

statistical ability to detect effects at the group level, and lead to an inappropriate failure to

20

reject the null hypothesis. To guard against this we calculated effect size (Cohens d; Cohen

21

1969) and power using G*Power (Faul et al., 2007) (Table 2).

22

[Insert Table 2]

23

To assess whether there was statistical evidence to support a continued improvement in

24

parenting stress associated with dog ownership we conducted a chi square analysis to

25

investigate if a significant number of parents had moved from clinically high (in the 90th

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
percentile, as defined by the PSI manual; Abidin, 1995) to clinically normal stress levels,

between follow-up and long-term follow-up. To explore for potential relationships between

parenting stress and family functioning we conducted a Pearsons correlation between scores

on the FAM-III-GS at Long-term follow-up and scores of parenting stress. To assess whether

the owners attachment to the dog was related to scores of family functioning we conducted a

Pearsons correlation between scores on the FAM-III-GS or PSI scores (total and 3

constituent elements) at long-term follow-up with scores on the Lexington Attachment to Pets

Scale (LAPS).
Results

M
AN
U

SC

RI
PT

Family Functioning

11

For the ANCOVA comparing the intervention and control group in family functioning, a

12

significant effect of group (F(1, 32) = 4.71, p = 0.037, p = 0.125; see Table 3 and Figure 1)

13

revealed lower scores in the intervention group compared to the control group, reflecting

14

decreased family difficulties and increased family strengths. The analysis showed no

15

significant effect of time (F(2, 66) = 0.583, p = 0.561) on scores of family functioning. No

16

significant time group effect was observed (F(2, 66) = 0.074, p = 0.928). Inspection of the

17

graphed data (Figure 1) suggested that different changes may have occurred within the two

18

groups over the sampled time points, and that the control group may show decreased family

19

difficulties between follow-up and long-term follow-up. To assess the significance of this

20

observation we conducted paired samples t-tests separately for the intervention and control

21

group. For the intervention group a significant reduction in family difficulties from baseline

22

to long-term follow-up (t(21) = 2.69, p = 0.01) a nearly significant difference from post-

23

intervention to long-term follow-up (t(21) = 1.84, p =0 .07) and no significant difference

24

from follow-up to long-term follow-up (t(21) = 1.50, p = 0.17). In the control group no

25

significant differences were observed (baseline to long-term follow-up: t(13) = 0.34, p = 0.73,

AC
C

EP

TE
D

10

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1

post-intervention to long-term follow-up: t(13) = 1.80, p = 0.08, Follow-up to Long-term

follow-up t(13) = 1.41, p = 0.17). The overall effect size for group, accounting for baseline

scores, was large (0.8) and was consistent with that obtained in the initial study (Table 2).
[Insert Figure 1 and Table 3]

RI
PT

Relationship with dog attachment. Not all participants chose to complete the LAPS
(those that did not complete the LAPS were under personal time constraints); therefore

analysis was conducted on a population of 17 parents. The analysis revealed no significant

correlation between LAPS and FAM-III scores (r = 0.072, n = 17, p = 0.785).

Parenting Stress

M
AN
U

SC

To assess whether there was a difference in PSI scores between the intervention and control

11

group over time an ANCOVA was conducted. There were no significant effects of group or

12

time (all ps > 0.05), buta comparison of effect sizes of group showed similar, albeit non-

13

significant, trends at long-term follow-up to that achieved in the original studies (Table 2).

14

The largest effects in the original studies were observed in the domains of Total Stress and

15

Difficult Child (both of large effect). At long-term follow-up these effect sizes were

16

classified as medium to large effect. The smallest effect sizes in the original studies were

17

associated with scores on Parent Distress and Parent Child Dysfunctional Interactions (of

18

medium to small effect). In this study these effect sizes were small, to approaching medium

19

in size. To further address the aim of evaluating the nature of the long-term effects of

20

acquiring a pet dog in these families, we explored mean scores within the PSI sub-scales.

21

Comparison of mean scores also revealed some potentially interesting individual trends

22

(Table 3 & Figure 1). The difference in percent decrease between the two groups was greater

23

in the intervention group by 35% for Parental Distress, 97% for Parent-Child Dysfunctional

24

Interactions, 50% for Difficult Child and 59% for Total Stress. To further investigate these

25

trends and to assess changes that may have occurred within the groups over the duration of

AC
C

EP

TE
D

10

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
the intervention we conducted paired sample t-tests separately for the intervention and control

group. Significant changes were observed in the Intervention group in the domains of Parent

Distress (decrease from Baseline to Long-term follow-up; t(13) = 2.30, p = 0.04), Parent-

Child Dysfunctional Interactions (decrease from baseline to long-term follow-up; t(19) =

2.07, p = 0.05), Difficult child (decrease from baseline to long-term follow-up; t(19) = 2.46, p

= 0.03, and from post-intervention and long-term follow-up t(19) = 3.00, p = 0.03 and Total

stress (decrease from baseline to long-term follow-up; t(19) = 2.85, p = 0.01). Significant

changes were seen in the control groups for the domains of Parent Distress (decrease from

baseline to long-term follow-up; t(19) = 2.06, p = 0.05), Difficult Child (decrease from

10

baseline to long-term follow-up; t(19) = 2.48, p = .02) and Total Stress (decrease from

11

baseline to long-term follow-up; t(19) = 2.71, p = .02). No significant changes were observed

12

in Parent-Child Dysfunctional Interactions. As seen in Table 3, there was a tendency for a

13

decrease in parenting stress in the intervention group compared to the control group,

14

although most comparisons across time were not significant. Effect sizes were reduced in the

15

smaller population sampled in this study in comparison the original population (Table 2), and

16

were of medium (Total Stress, Difficult Child) or small (Parent-Child Dysfunctional

17

Interactions, Parental Distress) size. These effect sizes reflect those obtained in the original

18

sample.

SC

M
AN
U

TE
D

EP

AC
C

19

RI
PT

In the intervention group 20% of parents (4/20) moved from high to normal levels of

20

parenting stress, in the control group no parents showed a clinical but non-significant

21

decrease in total stress, and one parent moved from clinically normal to clinically high stress

22

( (1, N = 34) = 3.17, p = 0.07).

23

Relationship with dog attachment. There was a significant positive correlation

24

between scores on LAPS and Difficult Child (r = 0.570, n = 17, p = 0.01), higher scores on

25

difficult child were associated with higher scores of pet attachment. No significant

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1

correlations were observed between LAPS and Parental Distress, Parent-Child Dysfunctional

Interactions and Total Stress (ps > 0.05).


Discussion

Our long-term follow-up study shows that the short-term benefits to family functioning

associated with acquiring a pet dog (Wright et al., 2015a, 2015b) appear to be largely

maintained 2.5 years after acquiring the dog, in families where obtaining a dog proved to be a

successful addition to the family and the dog was not relinquished. We observed some trends

towards a greater reduction of parenting stress in the dog owning group compared to the non-

dog owning group, but these effects were not statistically significant. Additionally, we

SC

RI
PT

identified a relationship between attachment to the dog and parenting stress. We first briefly

11

discuss the findings relating to the effects in family functioning and parenting stress before

12

considering potential mechanisms, limitations and implications of this work.

13

Family Functioning

14

Significantly reduced family difficulties (increased family strengths) were observed in

15

families who acquired a pet dog compared to families who did not acquire a pet dog,

16

indicating that improved family functioning was maintained 2.5 years later (see Wright et al.

17

2015a). The family functioning assessment was completed by the main parent carer to the

18

child with ASD, and therefore represents the main parent carers perception of family

19

functioning. Visual inspection of the graphed data and comparison of the means suggests that

20

families in the control group experienced a fluctuation in family functioning over the study

21

period, whereas families in the intervention group experienced steady, gradual reductions in

22

family difficulties. Given that the Brief-Fam-III General Scale measures functioning of the

23

entire family rather than that of an individual the lack of a significant relationship between

24

parent attachment to the dog and family functioning is not surprising.

25

Parenting Stress

AC
C

EP

TE
D

M
AN
U

10

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Parenting stress scores revealed trends to a greater reduction in all domains of parenting

stress (total stress, parental distress, parent-child dysfunctional interactions and difficult

child) in the intervention group compared to the control group. However, these between

group effects were not statistically significant in our population and may be explained by the

generally low statistical power achieved. From baseline to long-term follow-up both groups

showed a significant decline in parenting stress in the domains of total stress, parental

distress, and difficult child. It could be suggested that over time parents may adapt to the

demands having caring for a child with ASD, regardless of whether or not they own a pet

dog. It is worth noting that only the intervention group showed a significant decline in parent-

10

child dysfunctional interactions during this time. Of potential importance is that whereas 20%

11

of parents in the intervention group moved from clinically high to clinically normal stress

12

levels, no clinical-level improvements were observed in the control group, and one parent

13

moved from clinically normal stress levels to clinically high stress levels. Overall, the data

14

indicate that the stress associated with parenting a child with autism reduces over time, and

15

these reductions may be facilitated with the acquisition of a pet dog. These findings generally

16

support the short-term effects of dog acquisition reported by Wright et al. (2015b). However,

17

whilst Wright et al. (2015b) reported statistically significant changes to parenting stress

18

during the early stages of acquiring a dog, the scores at long-term follow-up were not

19

statistically different between the intervention and control group. Whilst it should be

20

considered that the greatest benefits of dog ownership on parenting stress may only be

21

evident in the initial stages after obtaining a dog, as we have previously mentioned the lack of

22

statistically significant results may also be the result of reduced statistical power. It should

23

also be considered that as the dog-parent relationship matures other factors mediate the

24

strength of the protective value of dog ownership against parenting stress. One such factor to

AC
C

EP

TE
D

M
AN
U

SC

RI
PT

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1

consider is the attachment bond between the parent and the dog. Indeed, we observed an

interesting relationship between scores of pet attachment and parenting stress.

A significant positive correlation emerged between scores of difficult child and


LAPS. This indicates that parents who perceive their child as being difficult have greater

attachment to their dog. It is not possible to determine the causal nature of these relationships,

but the relationship between parenting stress and pet attachment supports evidence indicating

that adults turn to their pets in times of need (Kurdek, 2009). It is noticeable that the domain

of parenting stress which relates most directly to the childs behaviors is that which

demonstrates a relationship with dog attachment. This is consistent with suggestions that

SC

RI
PT

there is a link between the type of bond formed between owners and pets as there is between

11

parent and child (Archer, 1997). Indeed, similarly to children, pets are seen as dependents but

12

also a source of amusement and play (Berryman et al., 1985); when the positive aspects of the

13

parent and child relationship break down (as evidenced in scores of difficult child) it appears

14

that the dog-owner bond increases. It is possible that this increase derives from humans need

15

for feeling related (a basic psychological need to be in a close relationship) (Deci & Ryan,

16

2000); as the care-giving relationship between child and parent is increasingly difficult the

17

parent seeks this similar caring relationship through attachment to their dog (see Stenseng,

18

2013). It has been proposed that greater attachment to a pet creates dissociation (the lack of

19

integration of various parts of an experience such as thoughts, feelings, or images into the

20

stream of consciousness; Carlson et al., 1993) (see Brown & Katcher, 1997, 2001).

21

Dissociation could help protect parents from stress, but it could also reduce diligent

22

parenting. However, our data show no evidence for parent-dissociation; greater attachment to

23

a dog was associated with greater perceptions of child difficulty. If pet attachment produced

24

dissociation we would expect greater attachment to the dog to relate to reduced child

25

difficulties. We are careful to point out these results are based on a small sample of parents,

AC
C

EP

TE
D

M
AN
U

10

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
nonetheless, they represent an important foundation for future research questions which could

have important implications for both the effectiveness of pet-dogs to improve the quality of

life those affected by ASD and for the welfare of dogs in these families.

Possible Mechanisms

The purpose of this research was to establish proof of concept for pet dog ownership as a

method to improve the lives of those affected by autism; in this case the main parent carer to

a child with ASD. As such, the measurements employed make it difficult to determine the

exact mechanisms behind the observed improvements Previous research suggests that animal

companionship can reduce stress and anxiety in typical and atypical populations (e.g.,

SC

RI
PT

OHaire, 2010, 2013; Smyth & Slevin 2010; Viau et al., 2010), and such reductions in the

11

parent and/or the child may indirectly affect both parenting stress and family functioning.

12

Other possible considerations are that the dog may increase social support, and that dog

13

ownership is associated with greater social interactions (McNicholas & Collis, 2000) and

14

feelings of support (McConnell et al., 2011). The dog may provide an excuse for increased

15

social interactions in the family, as a common talking point and interest (Walsh, 2009) or

16

through chance encounters when dog walking (McNicholas & Collis, 2000). Additionally, the

17

act of dog walking alone provides time away from it all which could be an important

18

contributor to improving wellbeing. Indeed, when collecting the data informal

19

communications with the parents suggested that the dog walk represents a valuable

20

opportunity for freedom to many parents. As well as the mental break, the psychological

21

benefits of the cardiovascular effects of exercise on stress are well documented (e.g., Fetzner

22

& Asmundson, 2014; Salmon, 2001). Future research could focus on the relationship between

23

what parents do with their dogs and whether the activities in which they engage affect family

24

functioning and relationships. A final point to mention is that in our original studies (Wright

25

et al., 2015a, 2015b) we considered the possibility that the acquisition of something new in

AC
C

EP

TE
D

M
AN
U

10

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
the family altered family interactions which improved stress and family functioning, but that

the improvements may not be specific to the dog and could be observed with, for example,

the addition of a new baby. Given that the average age of the dog acquired by families was 3

months it could also be that the cute and funny stage associated with owning a puppy

improved wellbeing, but that this would not be maintained as the dog grew older. If this was

the case then we would expect deteriorations in family functioning and parenting stress when

the novelty of the dog has reduced at long-term follow-up, not the sustained improvements

that we observed. Although, given that studies report the unique effects of dogs, as a species,

in reducing stress (e.g. Allen et al., 1991), we cannot conclude this at present, and future

SC

RI
PT

studies would need to consider the effects of other pets on the family.

11

Implications and Limitations

12

This is the first study to report long-term benefits of dog ownership in families with children

13

with ASD, specifically the benefits to the main parent carer to the child.

14

It extends pervious work in this field to show that pet dogs, as well as trained assistance dogs

15

can improve family normalcy (Burgoyne et al., 2014) and demonstrates that the positive

16

effects observed in family functioning during the early stages of pet dog ownership

17

(Grandgeorge et al., 2012; Wright et al., 2015a) are maintained 2.5 years later. This study

18

provides important information to parents and clinicians when considering pet ownership as

19

an effective family intervention and directly responds to the need to provide up-to-date

20

information on how to help parents improve family functioning using a flexible, integrative

21

approach (Solomon & Chung, 2012). Specifically, the data indicate that acquiring a dog can

22

bring considerable improvements to family functioning and shows some support for

23

maintaining long-term improvements to parenting stress levels. Family based factors have

24

been identified as being important to consider in the development of autism therapies

25

(Herring et al., 2006; Higgins et al., 2005). Indeed, high parent stress is associated with

AC
C

EP

TE
D

M
AN
U

10

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
increased severity in disruptive child behaviors, which in turn elevates parenting stress

(Baker et al., 2003), anxiety and depression (Hastings & Brown, 2002; Sofronoff & Farbotko,

2002). Additionally increasing family functioning is thought to benefit children with autism

(Herring et al., 2006; Rao & Beidel, 2009). It is difficult to make direct comparisons with

existing family interventions (e.g., Robbins et al., 1991; Shields & Simpson, 2004) due to a

range of protocol differences (i.e., child age, baseline scores, measure of stress used,

type/lack of control group). Nonetheless, our findings provide promise for future research to

compare changes in stress and family functioning between those acquiring a dog, those in

another family-based intervention, and those in a control group.

SC

RI
PT

We acknowledge that our findings are based on a relatively small sample, that was

11

self-selected (parents responded to advertisements), but they provide important support for

12

the value of further investment using larger samples. Given that longitudinal studies with

13

participants samples that have neurodevelopmental, social and behavioral disorders are

14

typically problematic in maintaining participation (e.g., Cotter et al., 2005) our greater than

15

50% recruitment is a satisfactory percentage to achieve and comparable to those achieved in a

16

study of a 3 year follow up concerning families with a child with autism (51%: Manti et al.,

17

2011). Due to the nature of the intervention, it was not possible to randomly assign a pet dog to

18

a family or to utilize Propensity Score Matching on the sample (see Wright et al., 2015c). These

19

results are only relevant to families who wish to acquire a pet dog and believe that a pet dog

20

may be a suitable addition to their family, which is a non-random population. We recognise

21

that for some families the challenging conditions of living with a child with autism may cause

22

significant welfare implications for the dog and the child,so acquiring a pet dog may not be an

23

effective strategy for all families living with a child with autism. . It is vital that we inform

24

scientific and clinical practice about the longer-term effects of acquiring a pet dog to these

25

families, as publications of the immediate effects, only, may lead to unrealistic expectations.

AC
C

EP

TE
D

M
AN
U

10

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1

Despite the potential for pet dogs as an effective family intervention we would like to
note that given the highly individualised nature of autism, dog ownership may benefit some

families more than others and acquiring a dog should only be taken after a due consideration

(Carlisle, 2014). Further research is required to help us understand which families are most

likely to benefit from dog ownership. We did not make our own independent evaluation of

the type and severity of the autistic symptoms of the children involved and instead we relied

on parental report. This was the procedure in the original studies (Wright et al., 2015a,

2015b). Given that parents had to give us 40-45 minutes of their time, with no tangible

reward, there is little incentive for parents to be dishonest in reporting their childs diagnosis,

SC

RI
PT

but future studies should consider the need for this additional control.

11

Conclusion

12

This long-term follow-up study highlights the potential benefits of pet dog ownership in

13

bringing relatively long-term improvements to the lives of families living with a child with

14

autism, particularly the main parent carer. These enduring improvements apply primarily to

15

reducing family difficulties, although there is some evidence of promise for dog ownership

16

reducing parenting stress long-term. These conclusions pertain only to families who are

17

willing to acquire a dog and the relationship is satisfactory. This research has significant

18

implications for veterinary, clinical and family practice and highlights the necessity for

19

further high quality research in this area.

20

Ethical Statement

21

All testing procedures complied with BPS Ethics Code of Conduct (2009) and were carried

22

out in accordance with The Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association, Declaration of

23

Helsinki.

AC
C

EP

TE
D

M
AN
U

10

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Acknowledgements

The project received funding from the Morris Animal Foundation; Grant Code / Title:

D14HA-008 / Long Term Effects of Pet Dogs on Families with Children with Autism. The

funders were not involved in the design of the study, data collection, data analysis and

preparation of the report.

Conflict of Interest

The authors report no conflict of interest.

Authorship Statement

The idea for the paper was conceived by Mills and the PAWS team. The experiments were

10

designed by Mills and the PAWS team. The experiments were performed by Hall, Wright,

11

Hames and the PAWS team.

12

The data were analyzed by Hall. The paper was written by Hall, Wright and Mills.

13

References

14

Abidin, R., 1995. Parenting Stress Index Manual (3rd ed.). Odessa, FL: Psychological

SC

M
AN
U

TE
D

EP

Assessment Resources.

AC
C

15

RI
PT

16

Allen, K. M., Blascovich, J., Tomaka, J., & Kelsey, R. M., 1991. Presence of human friends

17

and pet dogs as moderators of autonomic responses to stress in women. J. Pers. Soc.

18

19
20
21
22

Psychol. 61, 582.

Allen, K., Shykoff, B. E., & Izzo, J. L., 2001. Pet ownership, but not ACE inhibitor therapy,
blunts home blood pressure responses to mental stress. Hypertens. 38, 815-820.
American Psychiatric Association. 2013. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1

Archer, J., 1997. Why do people love their pets? Evol. Human Behav. 18, 237-259.

Baker, B. L., McIntyre, L. L., Blacher, J., Crnic, K., Edelbrock, C., & Low, C., 2003. Preschool children with and without developmental delay: Behaviour problems and

parenting stress over time. J. Intellect. Disabil. Res. 47, 217230.

Berryman, J. C., Howells, K., & Lloyd-Evans, M., 1985. Pet owner attitudes to pets and
people: A psychological study. Vet. Rec. 117, 659-661.

Blackburn, C., Read, J., & Spencer, N., 2012. Children with Neurodevelopmental

SC

RI
PT

Disabilities. Annual report of the Chief Medical Officer 2012, Our Children Deserve

Better: Prevention Pays. Accessed online

M
AN
U

10

https://www.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/252659

11

/33571_2901304_CMO_Chapter_9.pdf, last accessed 21/10/2014.

12

16

17
18

TE
D

15

training programme. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.


British Psychological Society, 2009. Ethics Code of Conduct. Ethics Committee of the

EP

14

Brereton, A. & Tonge, B., 2005. Pre-schoolers with autism: A parent education and skills

British Psychological Society: ISBN: 978-1-85433-495-4

AC
C

13

Brown, S. E., & Katcher, A. H., 1997. The contribution of attachment to pets and attachment
to nature to dissociation and absorption. Dissociation. 10, 125-129.

19

Brown, S. E., & Katcher, A. H., 2001. Pet attachment and dissociation. Soc. Anim. 9, 25-42.

20

Burgoyne, L., Dowling, L., Fitzgerald, A., Connolly, M., Browne, J. P., & Perry, I. J., 2014.

21

Parents perspectives on the value of assistance dogs for children with autism

22

spectrum disorder: a cross-sectional study. BMJ Open. 4, e004786.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1

Burrows, K. E., Adams, C. L., & Spiers, J., 2008. Sentinels of safety: Service dogs ensure

safety and enhance freedom and well-being for families with autistic children. Qual.

Health Res. 18, 1642-1649.


Bystrm, K. M., & Lundqvist Persson, C. A., 2015. The meaning of companion animals for

RI
PT

4
5

children and adolescents with autism: The parents' perspective. Anthrozoos. 28, 263-

275.

Cain, A., 1983. A study of pets in the family system. In A. Katcher and A. Beck (Eds.) New

SC

Perspectives on our Lives with Companion Animals (pp. 7281). Philadelphia, PA:

University of Pennsylvania Press.

10
11

12

M
AN
U

Carlisle, G. K., 2014. Pet dog ownership decisions for parents of children with autism
spectrum disorder. J. Paediatr. Nurs. 29, 114-123.

Carlson, E. B., Putnam, F. W., Ross, C. A., Torem, M., Coons, P., & Dill, D. L., 1993.
Validity of the dissociative experience scale in screening for multiple personality

14

disorder: A multicenter study. Am. J. Psychiatry. 150, 1030-1036.

17
18
19

20
21

Press.

EP

16

Cohen, J., 1969. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York: Academic

AC
C

15

TE
D

13

Cotter, R. B., Burke, J. D., Stouthamer-Loeber, M., & Loeber, R., 2005. Contacting
participants for follow-up: how much effort is required to retain participants in
longitudinal studies? Eval. Program Plann. 28, 15-21.

Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M., 2000. The what and why of goal pursuits: Human needs and
the self-determination of behaviour. Psychological Inquiry. 11, 227-268.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

3
4

Dogs for the Disabled, 2013. Parents autism workshops and support
http://www.paws.dogsforthedisabled.org/ (accessed 25 September 2013).
Dunn, M. E., Burbine, T., Bowers, C. A., & Tantleff-Dunn, S., 2001. Moderators of stress in
parents of children with autism. Community Ment. Health J. 37, 3952.

RI
PT

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A., 2007. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical
power analysis program for the social, behavioural and biomedical science. Behav.

Res. Methods. 39, 175-191

10

11
12

13

posttraumatic stress disorder: a randomized controlled trial. Cogn. Behav. Ther.


(ahead-of-print), 1-13.

M
AN
U

Fetzner, M. G., & Asmundson, G. J., 2014. Aerobic exercise reduces symptoms of

Fisman, S. Wolf, L. Ellison, D. and Freeman, T. 2000. A longitudinal study of siblings of


children with chronic disabilities. Can. J. Psychiatry. 45, 369375.

TE
D

SC

Grandgeorge, M., Tordjman, S., Lazartigues, A., Lemonnier, E., Deleau, M., & Hausberger,
M., 2012. Does pet arrival trigger prosocial behaviors in individuals with autism?

15

PloS One 7(8): e41739.

17
18

19

Green, J. Charman, T. McConachie, H. Aldred, C. Slonims, V. Howlin, P. et al., 2010.

AC
C

16

EP

14

Parent-mediated communication-focused treatment in children with autism (PACT): a


randomised controlled trial. The Lancet. 375(9732): 2152-2160.

Hastings, R. P., 2003. Child behaviour problems and partner mental health as correlates of

20

stress in mothers and fathers of children with autism. J. Intellect. Disabil. Res. 47,

21

231-237.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1
2

Hastings, R. P., & Brown, T., 2002. Behavior problems of children with autism, parental selfefficacy, and mental health. Am. J. Ment. Retard. 107, 222232.
Herring, S., Gray, K., Taffe, J., Tonge, B., Sweeney, D., & Einfeld, S. (2006). Behaviour and
emotional problems in toddlers with pervasive developmental disorders and

developmental delay: associations with parental mental health and family

functioning. J. Intellect. Disabil. Res. 50, 874-882.

Higgins, D. J., Bailey, S. R., & Pearce, J. C., 2005. Factors associated with functioning style

SC

RI
PT

and coping strategies of families with a child with an autism spectrum

disorder. Autism. 9, 125-137.

10

M
AN
U

Hoffman, C. D., Sweeney, D. P., Hodge, D., Lopez-Wagner, M. C., & Looney, L., 2009.

11

Parenting stress and closeness mothers of typically developing children and mothers

12

of children with autism. Focus Autism Other Dev. Disabil. 24, 178-187.
Johnson, T. P., Garrity, T. F., & Stallones, L., 1992. Psychometric evaluation of the

14

Lexington attachment to pets scale (LAPS). Anthrozos. 5, 160-175.

TE
D

13

Krause-Parello, C. A., 2012. Pet ownership and older women: the relationships among

16

loneliness, pet attachment support, human social support, and depressed

17

mood. Geriatr. Nurs. 33, 194-203.

19

AC
C

18

EP

15

Kurdek, L. A. 2009. Pet dogs as attachment figures for adult owners. J. Fam. Psychol. 23,
439-446.

20

Lach, L. M., Kohen, D. E., Garner, R. E., Brehaut, J. C., Miller, A. R., Klassen, A. F., &

21

Rosenbaum, P.L., 2009. The health and psychosocial functioning of caregivers of

22

children with neurodevelopmental disorders. Disabil. Rehab. 31, 741-752.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1

Manti, E., Scholte, E. M., & Van Berckelaer-Onnes, I. A., 2011. Development of children

with autism spectrum disorders in special needs education schools in the Netherlands:

a three-year follow-up study. Eur. J. Spec. Needs Educ. 26, 411-427.

McConnell, A. R., Brown, C. M., Shoda, T. M., Stayton, L. E., & Martin, C. E., 2011.
Friends with benefits: On the positive consequences of pet ownership. J. Pers. Soc.

Psychol. 101, 1239.

9
10
11
12
13

of the effect. Br. J. Psychol. 91, 61-70.

SC

McNicholas, J., & Collis, G. M., 2000. Dogs as catalysts for social interactions: Robustness

OHaire, M. E., 2013. Animal-assisted intervention for autism spectrum disorder: A


systematic literature review. J. Autism Develop. Disord. 43, 1606-1622.

M
AN
U

RI
PT

O'Haire, M., 2010. Companion animals and human health: Benefits, challenges, and the road
ahead. J. Vet. Behav. 5, 226-234.

Perou, R., Bitsko, R. H., Blumberg, S. J., Pastor, P., Ghandour, R. M., Gfroerer, J. C., et al.,
2013. Mental health surveillance among children-United States, 2005-2011. MMWR

15

Surveillance Summary. 62(Suppl 2), 1-35.

TE
D

14

16

Quintero, N., 2010. Sibling adjustment and maternal well-being: An examination of families

18

with and without a child with an autism spectrum disorder. Focus Autism Other Dev.

20
21
22

23
24

AC
C

19

EP

17

Disabil. 25, 37-46.

Rao, P. A., & Beidel, D. C., 2009. The impact of children with high-functioning autism on
parental stress, sibling adjustment, and family functioning. Behav. Modification. 33,
437-451.
Redefer, L. A., & Goodman, J. F., 1989. Brief report: Pet-facilitated therapy with autistic
children. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 19, 461-467.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1

Robbins, F. R., Dunlap, G., Plienis, & A. J., 1991. Family characteristics, family training, and

the progress of young children with autism. J. Early Interv. 15, 173-184.

Salmon, P., 2001. Effects of physical exercise on anxiety, depression, and sensitivity to

RI
PT

stress: a unifying theory. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 21, 33-61.


Shields, J., & Simpson, A., 2004. The NAS Early Bird Programme: preschool support for
parents of children with autistic spectrum disorder. Good Autism Practice. 5, 49-60.
Skinner, H. A., Steinhauer, P. D., & Santa-Barbara, J., 1983. The family assessment

SC

measure. Canadian Journal of Community Mental Health (Revue canadienne de sant

mentale communautaire). 2, 91-103.

11

12
13

14

Skinner, H. A., Steinhauer, P. D., & Santa-Barbara, J., 1995. FAM-III: Family Assessment
Measure III. Multi-Health Systems. Toronto, NY.

Skinner, H. Steinhauer, P. and Sitarenios, G., 2000. Family Assessment Measure (FAM) and

TE
D

10

M
AN
U

process model of family functioning. J. Fam. Ther. 22, 190-210.


Smyth, C., & Slevin, E. (2010). Experiences of family life with an autism assistance dog:
Placing specially trained dogs in families that have a child with autism can bring

16

many benefits. Claire Smyth and Eamonn Slevin evaluated parents views from a

18
19
20
21

AC
C

17

EP

15

study in Ireland. Learn. Disabil. Pract. 13, 12-17.

Sofronoff, K., & Farbotko, M., 2002. The effectiveness of parent management training to
increase self-efficacy in parents of children with Asperger syndrome. Autism. 6, 271
286.
Solomon, A. H., & Chung, B., 2012. Understanding autism: How family therapists can

22

support parents of children with autism spectrum disorders. Fam. Process. 51, 250-

23

264.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1
2
3

Stenseng, F., 2013. Attaching Person-Pet Attachment to Positive Psychology: In Response to


Andreassen, Stenvold, & Rudmin. Psychol. Soc. 5, 24-27.
Tonge, B. J. Brereton, A. Kiomall, M. Mackinnon, A. King, N. and Rinehart, N. J., 2006.
Effects on parental mental health of an education and skills training program for

parents of young children with autism: A randomised controlled trial. J. Am. Acad.

Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 45, 561-569.

Tunali, B., & Power, T., 2002. Coping by redefinition: cognitive appraisals in mothers of
children with autism and children without autism. J Autism Devel. Disord. 32, 2534.

SC

RI
PT

Viau, R., Arsenault-Lapierre, G., Fecteau, S., Champagne, N., Walker, C. D., & Lupien, S.,

10

2010. Effect of service dogs on salivary cortisol secretion in autistic children.

11

Psychoneuroendocrinology. 35, 1187-1193.

13

14

Walsh, F., 2009. HumanAnimal bonds II: The role of pets in family systems and family
therapy. Fam. Process. 48, 481-499.

TE
D

12

M
AN
U

Wright, H., Hall, S., Hames A., Hardiman, J., Mills R., & Mills, D., 2015a. Pet dogs improve
family functioning and reduce anxiety in children with Autism Spectrum Disorders

16

Anthrozoos. In press.

18
19

20

Wright, H., Hall, S., Hames A., Hardiman, J., Mills R., & Mills, D., 2015b. Acquiring a pet

AC
C

17

EP

15

dog significantly reduces stress of primary carers for children with Autism Spectrum
Disorder: A prospective case control study. J. Autism Devel. Disord. 1-10.

Wright, H. F., Hall, S., & Mills, D. S., 2015c. Additional evidence is needed to recommend

21

acquiring a dog to families of children with Autism Spectrum Disorder: A response to

22

Crossman and Kazdin. J. Autism Devel. Disord. 1-4.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1

ZaidmanZait, A., Mirenda, P., Zumbo, B. D., Wellington, S., Dua, V., & Kalynchuk, K.,
2010. An item response theory analysis of the Parenting Stress IndexShort Form with

parents of children with autism spectrum disorders. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 51,

1269-1277.

RI
PT

5
6

SC

M
AN
U

10

11

TE
D

12

13

EP

14

AC
C

15

16

Table 1

17

Summary of Participant Demographics in the Intervention and Control Group

Demographic Item*

Family Functioning Scale

Parenting Stress Index

Intervention

Control

Intervention

Control

(n=22)

(n=14)

(n=20**)

(n=14)

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
(All data is % except for child age)
Parents
85.7

80

85.7

Household status (One parent) 36.4

28.6

40

28.6

Children
Childs age (years) 11.63 0.06

28.6

No siblings 18.2

7.1

One sibling 54.5

50

Childs Diagnosis
Autism diagnosis 18.2
Autism spectrum diagnosis 27.3

TE
D

Aspergers / High functioning 50


Autistic traits 4.5
Childs Language Ability

EP

None verbal 4.5

AC
C

Single words / gestures 18.3


Simple phrases / sentences 4.5
Full sentences 72.7

Medication taken for autistic

22.7%

25.0

28.6

20

14.3

60

50

20

35.7

14.3

20

21.4

71.4

20

64.3

14.3

55

14.3

7.1

7.2

20

7.2

35.7

28.6

57.1

70

57.1

28.6%

25%

28.6%

42.80

M
AN
U

Two or more siblings 27.20

11.85 .2.92 11.5 4.21

SC

Gender (Female) 22.7

11.21 1.19

RI
PT

Gender (Female) 81.8

symptoms
1
2
3
4

*Demographic Item: Within categories, mutually exclusive; ** Total number of participants = 37, see text for reasons of removal from
analysis

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1
2
3
4

RI
PT

5
6
7

SC

8
9

M
AN
U

10
11
12
13

TE
D

14
15
16

EP

17

19
20

AC
C

18

21

Table 2

22

Effect Size and Power of the Main Effect of Group

23

d = Effect sizes are Cohens d calculated using G*Power: Small = 0.2, Medium = 0.5, Large = 0.8

24

*Original Data: This refers to the power of the data collected in the first studies conducted by Wright et al. (2015a, 2015b).

25

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1
2
3

Original Data*
Power

0.8

0 .99

0.8

0.99

Total Stress

0.8

0.99

0 .5

0 .80

Parental Distress

0.6

0.99

0.3

0.38

0.8

0.99

0.6

0.92

0.2

0.34

0.1

0.08

Family Functioning

Difficult Child
Parent-Child Dysfunctional Interaction
4

9
10
11
12

EP

AC
C

TE
D

M
AN
U

PSI-SF

Power

RI
PT

d
FAM-III-GS

Long-Term Follow-up

SC

Scale

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Table 3
Changes between baseline and long-term follow-up for the intervention and control group
Intervention Group

Control Group

Baseline

Scale

Long-term

MeanSEM
Diff

RI
PT

MeanSEM
Baseline

Follow-up

Diff

Follow-up

FAM-III-GS
9.221.15

2.96

PSI-SF

12.281.82

11.711.68

0.57

35.642.19

33.352.29

2.29

SC

Family Functioning 12.181.33

Long-term

35.701.65

32.151.89

3.55

Parent-CDI

36.301.64

33.101.61

3.20

31.421.34

31.351.17

0.07

Difficult Child

47.301.21

43.201.92

4.10

46.282.03

44.212.25

2.07

Total Stress

119.303.57

108.404.69

10.9

113.353.69

108.924.19

4.43

M
AN
U

Parent Distress

AC
C

EP

TE
D

Parent-CDI = Parent-child dysfunctional interactions, Diff= Difference between the two scores.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Family Difficulties (Functioning)
Dog Owners (Intervention):
Non-Dog Owners (Control):

15
14

*BL: Baseline
PI: Post-Intervention
FU: Follow-up
LTF: Long-term Follow up

13
12

RI
PT

11
10
9
8
PI

FU

LTF

SC

BL

Difficult Child

M
AN
U

Parent Distress
36

48

34

46

32

44
42

BL

PI

TE
D

30

FU

LTF

BL

EP

Parent-Child Dysfunctional Interactions

34
32

AC
C

36

PI

FU

LTF

Total Stress

120
115
110

30

105
BL

PI

FU

LTF

BL

PI

FU

LTF

Figure 1. The longitudinal effects of acquiring a pet dog on family functioning and parenting
stress (PD, DC, P-CDI, TS).
*The time elapsed since measures of family functioning were taken was: pre-intervention =
2.61 years 0.05; post-intervention = 2.37 years 0.06; follow-up = 1.91 years 0.05. The

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

AC
C

EP

TE
D

M
AN
U

SC

RI
PT

time elapsed since measures of parenting stress were taken was: pre-intervention 2.71 years
0.07; post-intervention measures = 2.51 years 0.07; follow-up = 2.00 years 0.07.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

RI
PT
SC
M
AN
U
TE
D
EP

A long-term follow up of families with children with autism who acquired a pet dog
Improvements to family functioning was maintained 2.5 years later
20% of parents who acquired a pet dog moved from clinically high levels of parenting
stress to normal levels
Parenting stress was related to attachment to the dog

AC
C

Вам также может понравиться