Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 14

Music2702_02 11/16/09 6:00 PM Page 89

Absolute Pitch Ability 89

A D ISTRIBUTION OF A BSOLUTE P ITCH A BILITY AS R EVEALED


BY C OMPUTERIZED T ESTING

PATRICK B ERMUDEZ AND R OBERT J. Z ATORRE Gore, & Marks, 2005; Zatorre, 2003). The strongest
Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, forms of the ability are far from universal and not all
Montreal, Canada & BRAMS Laboratory, Montreal, those who are exposed to equivalent musical tuition (to
Canada the extent that this can ever be determined) develop the
ability. Sufficiently early and intensive music training
BEHAVIORAL ASSESSMENT OF ABSOLUTE PITCH ( AP ) seems to be an important factor but it likely interacts
ability over the better part of the last century has strongly with certain biological/cognitive dispositions and nor-
suggested that a variety of proficiency levels exists and mal developmental stage in forming propitious circum-
can only be more comprehensively described with the stances that favor its emergence.
use of rigorous testing providing precise and unbiased To date, there exists no standard method for the
reaction times for all responses. This study describes the behavioral determination of absolute pitch ability, and
design, implementation and validation of a computerized many studies have simply relied on participant self-
test of absolute pitch and resulting data for 51 musicians, report. Among those studies that have verified AP
27 of whom self-reported as AP possessors. The test was explicitly, a variety of approaches have been espoused,
sensitive to previously reported differences in accuracy most having required the identification (verbal, writ-
and timing for C major diatonic versus non-diatonic ten, or other) of a given tone in the absence of a refer-
notes and showed a range of performance, from perfect ence tone (Takeuchi & Hulse, 1993; Ward, 1999).
to random, including a substantial number of interme- Stimuli have most commonly been piano notes (real
diate levels of proficiency. We discuss the implications and synthesized), sine waves, and multiharmonic syn-
of detecting such a distribution of behavior as well as thetic tones, and there has been little consistency in
the effect of test design and scoring strategies on that terms of administration and analysis parameters such
distribution. as stimulus length, number of trials, reaction time col-
lection, scoring strategy, performance criteria, etc. As
Received November 28, 2008, accepted June 24, 2009.
there seems to be a gamut of AP ability, ranging from
Key words: absolute pitch, behavior, computerized test, excellent performance with pure tones over a wide fre-
distribution, scoring quency spectrum (showing ability that is least stimulus
bound, perhaps corresponding to Bachems genuine
AP; Bachem, 1937) to moderate performance within a
more narrow range of notes from a preferred instru-

A
BSOLUTE PITCH (AP) HAS LONG BEEN OF INTEREST ment, it is most desirable to have control over stimulus
to scientists studying music cognition (Bachem, type, accuracy and response latency as possible means
1937; Ward, 1999). Simply and typically defined, for distinguishing different subtypes of AP (Baharloo,
it is the ability to identify the names of musical pitches Johnston, Service, Gitschier, & Freimer, 1998; Miyazaki,
without reference to a standard (Bachem, 1937; Ward, 1989, 1990; Takeuchi & Hulse, 1993). The use of piano
1999). Part of its scientific attraction stems from the tones alone risks what Ward and Burns (1982) have
fact that it is a rare ability whose overt manifestation referred to as the testing of absolute piano, meaning
requires complex cognitive function, and yet it is read- generally that timbre and other cues characteristic of an
ily amenable to operationalization. The reasons for the instrument can be used in addition to fundamental fre-
uncommon occurrence of AP are ill-understood and quency to help identify notes. Although such a phe-
the necessary and sufficient conditions for its develop- nomenon is in itself interesting, it may be best
ment remain the subject of considerable speculation considered a more stimulus-bound variety of absolute
and investigation (Baharloo, Service, Risch, Gitschier, & pitch. Probable differences in performance and strategy
Freimer, 2000; Gregersen, Kowalsky, Kohn, & Marvin, among AP musicians have been recognized for some
2000; Levitin & Rogers, 2005; Miyazaki, 2004; Ross, time (Bachem, 1937) yet limited efforts have been made

Music Perception VOLUME 27, ISSUE 2, PP. 89101, ISSN 0730-7829, ELECTRONIC ISSN 1533-8312 2009 BY THE REGENTS OF THE UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA . ALL
RIGHTS RESERVED. PLEASE DIRECT ALL REQUESTS FOR PERMISSION TO PHOTOCOPY OR REPRODUCE ARTICLE CONTENT THROUGH THE UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA PRESS S
RIGHTS AND PERMISSIONS WEBSITE , HTTP :// WWW. UCPRESSJOURNALS . COM / REPRINTINFO. ASP. DOI:10.1525/MP.2009.27.2.89
Music2702_02 11/16/09 6:00 PM Page 90

90 Patrick Bermudez & Robert J. Zatorre

to describe and quantify this variability and take it into a participants responses and a measure of average devi-
account in experimental contexts. It seems therefore ation also provides intuitively accessible estimates of
that relying on participants self-report may only be the average resolution of responses; that is, the ability
adequate during initial stages of recruitment. to achieve a sufficiently narrow pitch-class distinction
As alluded to above, choice of stimulus, administra- so as to allow reliable recognition and labelling at the
tion parameters, and scoring strategies all have con- semitone level. Of further concern is the possibility
tributed to considerable variability among reported that the strictest and traditional scoring method (only
results (Miyazaki, 1989; Takeuchi & Hulse, 1993). crediting exactly correct responses) may exaggerate
Relatively more discussion has been dedicated to possi- the occasionally described bimodal distribution of AP
ble effects of stimulus type than to administration performance among musicians. In other words, it is
details; specifically the importance of collecting accu- sometimes suggested (e.g. Athos et al., 2007; Carroll,
rate reaction times and the possible effects of various 1975; Miyazaki, 1988) that there are two clearly resolv-
scoring criteria on the perceived grouping of partici- able populations, those who can and those who cannot
pants performance. Response latency is one dimension perform an AP task, with very little (if any) overlap
of performance that holds the potential to resolve par- between the two distributions. If one ignores large parts
ticipants who are indistinguishable from each other of the variance in performance by only considering
solely on the basis of a percent correct or other similar exactly correct responses (or even 1 semitone), one
score. As has been observed by other researchers can easily imagine the effect on a pair of gamma-like
(Miyazaki, 1989, 1990; Takeuchi & Hulse, 1991), we distributions; namely, that they may be rendered more
might expect that participants with a weaker form of narrow, with artificially sharp boundaries thereby
AP or Bachem-style pseudo-AP ultimately could accentuating the bimodal impression. Assigning scores
achieve far better than chance performance but would to semitone errors serves to palliate this problem some-
require more time to respond on average, likely due to what but also unnecessarily blurs a potentially fine
the use of alternative strategies including relative pitch distinction in ability between the participant who
judgements from a limited number of internalized performs perfectly and the one who performs with a
pitch class representations (e.g., a stable memory for a lower-resolution in a two semitone band centered on
frequent tuning note such as A = 440 Hz). the correct answer. As suggested in the previous para-
By far the most common scoring of a typical test of graph, this information is recovered with a trivially
absolute pitch, where a participant is presented a note simple measure of mean deviation from the correct
and asked for a verbal, written or other identification of response.
that note, has been to assign a point for an exactly cor- Athos et al. (2007) used an Internet-based test and
rect chroma identification and, in fewer studies, a survey to collect data from well over 2000 self-selected
point, 3/4-point, or 1/2-point for semitone deviations. participants, 44% of whom were categorized as being
Given that there is no community-wide operational possessors of a particularly strong form of AP. Their
definition or scoring convention and often no clear dis- distribution of scores indicates some degree of bimodal-
continuity between purported sub-populations among ity, suggesting that there are two largely distinct popu-
musicians, these are necessarily arbitrary criteria. One lations accounting for the worst and best performers.
might reasonably conjecture that the stricter of the The authors drew two conclusions based on this distri-
approaches, where only exact responses are considered, bution: that their data resolve the question of whether
risks ignoring relatively high performers who consis- AP participants lie at the extreme of a continuum of
tently approximate the correct answer but are not quite ability or form a distinct population (clearly the latter,
resolving the semitone, and therefore not being awarded in their opinion), and that AP could have its origins in
the crucial points, or participants who perform well but one or a few genes. Additionally, they document a finding
with a consistent shift of a semitone or more due to consistent with the anecdotally described age-related
their learning history (e.g., with a mistuned instru- shift in perception that leads some participants to
ment) or some paracusis. An information transfer perceive notes as sharper than they once did (Ward,
approach (Miller, 1956; Pollack, 1952; Ward & Burns, 1999). They also speculate on a possible perceptual
1982) that can recover the consistency of information magnet effect for the pitch class A, following mainly
transmitted by the participant (expressed in terms of from the finding that the class G#, in particular, often is
bits of information) is somewhat appropriate for dealing mislabelled as A (which we read as consistent with
with the latter and a simple measure of average deviation some of the results obtained by Van Krevelen, 1951,
sufficient for the former. Examining the distribution of who recorded mean recognition frequencies for G# and
Music2702_02 11/16/09 6:00 PM Page 91

Absolute Pitch Ability 91

A# that were higher and lower than standard, respec- Certain predictions follow from AP literature. We
tively). The study highlighted the value of web-based obviously expect significant differences between self-
data collection as a technique complementary to those reported AP and non-AP (NAP) possessors in various
normally used in this area of investigation. Several measures of accuracy and reaction time for chroma
notes of caution should be voiced, however. Any claims identification (higher and shorter, respectively, in the AP
about the population must be constrained and tempered group). We also expect that diatonic notes of the C
by knowledge of the limitations inherent to the sampling major scale will be identified more accurately and more
methodology used, which are essentially those of most quickly than the accidentals of C major (Miyazaki, 1990;
unconstrained survey techniques (Kaplan & Saccuzzo, Takeuchi & Hulse, 1991). Additionally, intermediate
1993). For instance, participants were self-selected and performers might show different response patterns and
motivated to participate out of personal interest, from reaction times as compared to either high-performing
which one might anticipate an over-representation of AP or randomly performing NAP musicians, perhaps
AP possessors given the advertised aims of the study reflecting alternative strategies and/or weaker, lower res-
(as suggested by the conspicuously high 44% of respon- olution chroma representations. This paper presents a
dents showing particularly strong AP-1 performance), detailed description of a computerized test of absolute
thereby accentuating one of the two supposed modes. pitch, data collected to date, and future directions. We
Secondly, the scoring method used, where exactly correct also will describe the reliability of the test and explore
answers were awarded one point and semitone devia- the advantages of collecting precise reaction times as
tions 3/4 of a point (0 for all other responses including well as alternative descriptions of performance that do
any requiring more than 4 seconds), risks exaggerating not rely on traditional scoring methods.
the separation between the best and worst performers
by discarding intermediate performance. Given these Method
and other related issues, it seems imprudent to declare
the matter resolved. In what concerns the genetic claim, Participants
even if AP is reduced to its most bare and requisite fea-
tures, there is no necessity that it should be attributed Participants primarily were recruited through the music
to a single gene. There are examples of traits and diseases faculties of two local universities. The posted advertise-
with simple genetic origins yet complex manifestations, ments simply stated that we were seeking musicians both
but also of simple traits being governed by complex with and without absolute pitch, with at least 10 years
and large collections of genes (NLM, 2008). More of training/practice to participate in behavioral testing
fundamentally, a bimodal distribution of measured and a neuroimaging experiment. All participants gave
behavior does not necessarily map to simple underly- informed consent and all procedures were approved
ing biology and could just as easily suggest a poly- by the Research Ethics Committee of the Montreal
genic or multifactorial inheritance model in which an Neurological Institute, McGill University. Sample con-
interaction of several genes and environment conspire stitution was as follows: 51 musicians, 39 females and
to produce a variety of phenotypic manifestations 12 males, 27 of which self-reported as AP possessors.
(Benjamin, 2008), i.e., levels of proficiency. Put another They had an average age of 23.1 years (SE = 0.52), average
way, the effect we are measuring could be one among age of commencement of playing and/or training of
many manifestations of more general underlying factors 6.1 years (SE = 0.32), and average of 16.4 years of total
(genetic/biological/cognitive). Indeed, there is a line of playing and training experience (SE = 0.63).
parsimonious reasoning that informs us of the unlike-
lihood of this ability being so completely isolated from Stimuli
the rest of brain function, perfectly self-encapsulated
and having no relation whatsoever to any other aspects Fundamental frequencies of tones corresponded to
of cognition (Bermudez, 2008; Bermudez & Zatorre, notes ranging from C3 to B5 (three octaves) and were
2009; Chin, 2003; Costa-Giomi, Gilmour, Siddell, & based on an A = 440 Hz equal temperament tuning,
Lefebvre, 2001; Moreno Sala & Costa-Giomi, 2008). We with a 21/12:1 frequency ratio between adjacent semi-
might also observe, due to a metric of limited dimen- tones. Each note was presented three times, once at
sionality and resolution, similar behavior accomplished each of three different intensities (-1, -4 and -7 dB) in
by means of different neural infrastructure that could, order to eliminate the use of intensity cues to aid judge-
in turn, be attributable to different biological/genetic ments. This summed to 108 trials. The notes were syn-
factors. thetic tones generated by summing a series of sinusoid
Music2702_02 11/16/09 6:00 PM Page 92

92 Patrick Bermudez & Robert J. Zatorre

(a) (b) resets to the center of the circle at the beginning of each
C C# b
B D C -> B trial, thereby making each of the 12 possible responses
Bb D equidistant from the trial starting point which, together
A#
with the high temporal resolution, is critical for the col-
A D#
Eb lection of reliable and unbiased reaction time data. Both
Ab the stimulus type and response method also avoid con-
E
G# founds that might result from the different instruments
G
Gb F #
F of concentration among musicians.

FIGURE 1. Response screens presented to the participant during the Scoring


test. Panel (a) shows the chroma response screen, with its 12 possible
responses equidistant from the center cursor position, and panel (b) shows
Percent correct and mean absolute deviation (MAD)
the octave response screen with each band of color (here shown in grey-
scale) corresponding to the C to B range of a single octave. were used as measures of accuracy. In the percent correct
score, only exactly correct responses are counted (0 semi-
tone deviation). MAD was computed by discarding the
waveforms including a fundamental frequency and a octave information, taking the absolute value of the
harmonic series (f, 2f, 3f, etc.) where the difference in semitone deviation from the correct answer (ranging
amplitude between adjacent harmonics was on the from -6 to +6 semitones) and then the mean of these
order of 12 dB. Given a 16-bit sampling depth, this values across all trials for a given participant. Given a
yielded approximately 9 harmonics. All notes had a 1 s possible range of 0 to 6 semitones deviation, a perfectly
duration including 50 ms linear onset and offset ramps. random response pattern and therefore flat distribution
yields a MAD of 3, whereas a perfectly correct perform-
Procedure ance yields a MAD of 0. Reaction times are reported in
ms (after being iteratively cleaned by removing values
Participants were seated at a computer workstation and lying beyond 3 standard deviations of the mean) and as
given headphones through which to listen to the stimuli log-transformed values (base 10).
at an intensity of approximately 75 dB SPL. The 108 trials
were presented in a randomized order and, upon hear- Results
ing each note, the participant saw the response screen
depicted in Figure 1a.1 After selecting a note name, the Participants
participant was presented with a second response
screen (Figure 1b) from which s/he made an octave Data concerning age of commencement of training/
judgment for the note identified with the previous practice and total years of training/practice were not
screen (color bands represent C to B of one octave). available for 3 participants in each group. AP and NAP
Among general instructions given to the participant, it groups did not differ in age (M = 23.33 years SE 0.71
was emphasized that this octave judgement does not vs. M = 23.14 years SE 0.64, respectively; t(49) = 0.85,
require the participant to select the exact note on the p = .40) or years of training/practice (M = 17.04 years
grand staff but rather simply click anywhere in the col- SE 0.79 vs. M = 16.09 years SE 0.80, respectively; t(43) =
ored band representing the octave to which the note 0.84, p = 0.41). The AP group had a significantly lower
belongs. Participants also were told that they should age of commencement of training/practice than the
respond as accurately and quickly as possible but were NAP group (M = 5.46 years SE 0.35 vs. M = 6.95 years
left to use whichever strategy was most useful to them. SE 0.49, respectively; t(43) = 2.52, p = .02).
Trials were self-paced with no timeout and pauses were
introduced at the 1/3 and 2/3 marks through trials to Split-Half Reliability
provide a short resting period. The timing of responses
was measured with an error inferior to 10 ms. Although For each participant, the 108 randomly ordered trials
somewhat lacking in ethological validity, the chroma were split into two halves of 54 trials by selecting the odd
response method chosen ensures that the mouse pointer and even numbered trials separately. Correlations
between the two halves for the mean absolute deviation
1
An earlier version of the response screen, used to collect the present and log-transformed reaction times (logRT) were r(49) =
data, used only sharps rather than enharmonic notation. .99, p < .001, and r(49) = .98, p < .001, respectively.
Music2702_02 11/16/09 6:00 PM Page 93

Absolute Pitch Ability 93

4 100

80

60

40

20

Mean Absolute Deviation (semitones)


3 0
6 5 4 3 2 1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6

100

80

60

2 40

20

100
0
6 5 4 3 2 1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6
80

60

40

1 20

0
6 5 4 3 2 1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6

0 20 40 60 80 100 120
Percent Correct
AP Subjects NAP Subjects

FIGURE 2. Percent correct versus mean absolute deviation for pitch class identification. Participants are grouped according to self-report and the
dotted-line box delimits the 95% confidence interval around chance performance. The inset histograms show examples of individual performance with
semitone deviation ranging 6 to +6 on the x-axis and number of responses on the y-axis.

Self-Report vs. Performance-Based Classification the accuracy, M = 78% SE 3.16 vs. M = 70% SE 3.35;
F(1, 49) = 2.83, p < .099, or log reaction times of octave
As can be seen in Figure 2, one participant who reported identification, M = 3.24 SE 0.03 vs. M = 3.23 SE
possessing AP performed more than 2 standard devia- 0.03; F(1, 49) = 0.14, p = .71. A repeated-measures
tions below the AP mean with a MAD of 1.94 semitones. ANOVA contrasting accuracy in identifying notes corre-
Conversely, another participant who self-reported as a sponding to white keys versus notes corresponding to
non-possessor achieved a MAD of 0.43 semitones, black keys and two levels of group (AP and NAP) yielded
more than 2 standard deviations above the NAP mean. a marginally significant interaction, F(1, 49) = 3.72, p =
These clear outliers therefore were reclassified according .06. Tukeys HSD posthoc tests revealed this interaction
to their performance. Results reported henceforth use to be due to significantly greater accuracy in identifying
this revised grouping. white notes among AP but not NAP participants
(Figure 4). The same was true for a similar contrast of
AP vs. NAP log reaction times for black and white notes, with a sig-
nificant interaction of group and note type, F(1, 49) =
For the identification of chroma, AP participants showed 23.12, p < .001, due to significantly faster reaction times
a greater number of correct responses, M = 77% SE 2.87 for white note identification among AP but not NAP
vs. M = 15% SE 3.04; F(1, 49) = 217.78, p < .001 participants (Figure 5). Additionally, accuracy and log
(Figures 3 and 4), smaller mean absolute deviation from reaction time performance were best for the pitch class
the correct chroma, M = 0.38 SE 0.10 vs. M = 2.48 A over all other notes in the AP group, F(1, 49) = 21.24,
SE 0.10; F(1, 49) = 221.24, p < .001, quicker reaction p < .001; F(1, 49) = 25.54, p < .001, respectively, as was
times, M = 3346 ms SE 526 ms vs. M = 7586 ms SE accuracy, F(1, 49) = 11.67, p = .001, but not log reaction
558 ms; F(1, 49) = 30.59, p < .001, and log reaction times, time in the NAP group. Age of commencement of train-
M = 3.45 SE 0.034 vs. M = 3.76 SE 0.04; F(1, 49) = ing was significantly correlated with MAD, r(43) = .46,
39.21, p < .001 (Figure 5) as compared to NAP partici- p = .01, percent correct chroma, r(43) = .44, p = .002,
pants. AP and NAP participants did not differ in either and log reaction time, r(42) = .40, p = .007. MAD also
Music2702_02 11/16/09 6:00 PM Page 94

94 Patrick Bermudez & Robert J. Zatorre

70

60

50
Percent of Total Responses

40

30

20

10

0
14 13 12 11 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14
Semitone Deviation
NAP AP

FIGURE 3. Histogram of semitone deviations from the correct response. AP participants show the expected narrow modes around the exactly cor-
rect responses as well as the correct pitch class with an octave deviation (+ 12 and 12 semitones). The NAP musicians, on the whole, are not respond-
ing completely arbitrarily, with a very broad mode centered on correct identifications.

100

80

60
Percent Correct

40

20

C C# D D# E F F# G G# A A# B
Pitch Class
AP NAP

FIGURE 4. Percent correct identification for each pitch class by group (AP and NAP). The bands of grey correspond to the black keys of a piano or
non-diatonic notes of the C major scale. The pattern reveals how AP musicians identified diatonic notes more accurately on average.
Music2702_02 11/16/09 6:00 PM Page 95

Absolute Pitch Ability 95

4.0

Log Reaction Time 3.8

3.6

3.4

3.2
C C# D D# E F F# G G# A A# B
Pitch Class
AP NAP

FIGURE 5. The logRT for each pitch class by group (AP and NAP). The bands of grey correspond to the black keys of a piano or non-diatonic notes
of the C major scale. The pattern reveals how AP musicians identified diatonic notes more quickly on average.

was strongly correlated with log reaction time, r(49) = Discussion


.63, p < .0001 (Figure 6). Figure 7 plots percent correct
score against an index that multiplies mean absolute A number of issues associated with the behavioral
deviation by the log reaction times, thereby penalizing assessment of AP have obfuscated the distribution and
particularly time consuming strategies. variety of ability among both self-declared possessors

4
Mean Absolute Deviation (semitones)

2.8 3.0 3.2 3.4 3.6 3.8 4.0 4.2 4.4


Log Reaction Time
AP Subjects NAP Subjects

FIGURE 6. The logRT is plotted against mean absolute deviation time. Across all participants, the better performers clearly require less time to
respond. There is a trend towards longer reaction times among intermediate performers.
Music2702_02 11/16/09 6:00 PM Page 96

96 Patrick Bermudez & Robert J. Zatorre

55

Mean Absolute Deviation (semitones)


50

X Log Reaction Time


45

40

35

30

25
0 20 40 60 80 100 120
Percent Correct
AP Subjects NAP Subjects

FIGURE 7. Percent correct pitch class identification versus an index that multiplies mean absolute deviation by the logRT. This index effectively
weighs the MAD score with logRT so that more time consuming strategies are penalized. Despite this, a range of performance, including many inter-
mediate scores, is still obvious.

and non-possessors. The main design considerations the striking differences in distributions of responses
for our test of absolute pitch were intended to address a between the AP and NAP groups (Figure 3) that dif-
few long-standing concerns in the community. For fered in all the predicted ways. However, Figure 2 shows
instance, (1) we used multiharmonic synthetic stimuli how we begin to distinguish a third group of musicians
because they are equally unfamiliar to all listeners who typically do not consider themselves to have AP
(unlike specific instrument tones) and avoid problems but perform at a level intermediate to that of the clear
associated with the use of pure tones (loudness varia- AP musicians (mean deviation < one semitone) and
tion, poor performance for intermediate and weak AP NAP musicians with an essentially random response
possessors, poor ethological validity), (2) we collected (mean deviation ~ three semitones). These participants
both pitch class and octave judgements that allow us to also show a non-significant trend towards longer reac-
calculate the exact distance between the presented note tion times (Figure 6) that would be consistent with the
and the participants response and describe pitch- use of more time consuming alternative strategies in
height in addition to pitch-class judgements, (3) we attempting to identify chroma. Greater numbers of
collected precise reaction times for both pitch class and participants and trials in future work will be required to
octave responses, allowing us to describe intermediate tease apart those participants who are using a relative
performers who may be using alternate strategies to pitch strategy from one or a few dependable notes versus
those of the strongest performers, (4) we used a graph- those who simply have broad, low-resolution chroma
ical pitch class identification method that makes all 12 representations centered on the correct pitch. The more
possible responses equally accessible and unbiased in conventional measure of total correct chroma responses
terms of timing and interface familiarity, thereby cir- does not discriminate as clearly between the intermediate
cumventing difficulties associated with keyboard-based and weak performers (placing them closer to each other
and other response methods, and (5) the wealth of col- than mean deviation; Figure 2), thereby highlighting
lected data allows us to derive indices of AP ability that one of the limitations of this particular scoring strategy.
do not neglect performance variance that is beyond one It is also notable that the split-half reliability of the test
semitone from the correct response, as has often been is sufficiently strong so as to suggest that it is not only
the case in the literature. internally reliable but also that a shortened version
The data collected are indicative of the sensitivity of would be suitably accurate for screening purposes. Two
the test, as it begins to make fine discriminations in participants, one claiming to possess AP and the other
levels of absolute pitch proficiency. There are, of course, not, performed in ways that were clearly discordant with
Music2702_02 11/16/09 6:00 PM Page 97

Absolute Pitch Ability 97

their self-report which highlights the need to verify AP including an important number of intermediate-level
performance explicitly although, on the whole, partici- performers (Figure 2). This is particularly clear if one
pants qualitative assessments of their abilities were considers a more comprehensive and intuitive descrip-
usually in reasonable agreement with test results. tor such as mean absolute deviation (MAD) from the
correct pitch class, a measure that ignores the octave
and therefore lies between 0 and 6 semitones and yields
Sampling and Distribution
an average of 3 for randomly distributed responses. The
result is a distribution of behavior across participants
Our recruitment effort advertised the need for musi-
that one would hesitate to describe as bimodal, though
cians both with and without absolute pitch. Although
one might construe mini-clusters among the very best
participants ultimately would report as either pos-
(deviation < 0.1) and worst (deviation ~ 3) performers.
sessing or not possessing AP in a forced-choice ques-
Most studies have awarded 1 point for exactly correct
tion (with certainty ratings), some non-possessors
pitch class identifications and a fraction of a point for
claimed that, under limited circumstances, they were
semitone deviations. Such approaches further exacerbate
able to label pitches with some degree of accuracy. A
the lack of distinction between those performing ran-
few of these and others who reported as either NAP
domly, with a MAD of approximately 3, and those with
or weak AP scored between 1.0 and 2.3 semitones
an average deviation of 2 semitones thus effectively
deviation, as can be seen in Figure 2. It is therefore
strengthening an apparent cluster of random performers.
interesting to note that, using a recruitment strategy
In our study, we cannot know for certain how participants
that welcomed a variety of ability, we have a consid-
responses might have differed had they been attempting
erable number of participants showing an intermedi-
to respond inside a typical 2 to 4 s time limit, though we
ate level of performance. There is some noticeable
can reasonably expect a higher error rate than is the case
clustering among the strongest (mean deviation ~ 0)
when the procedure is self-paced. Eight high-performing
and the weakest performers (mean deviation ~ 3) but
participants in our sample, scoring a mean absolute
also an important range of performance falling between
deviation of less than one, had mean reaction times rang-
these extremes. The trend remains clear even when
ing from 4 to 6 s and therefore many of their responses
considering the gross and strict measure of percent
would have earned a score of 0 under similar schemes
correct chroma identification. This is not entirely con-
(assuming that they were, in fact and as instructed,
sistent with the occasional claim of a clearly bimodal
performing as quickly as they could).
distribution among musicians (Athos et al., 2007;
Let us further concretize the effects of scoring with an
Carroll, 1975; Miyazaki, 1988), though there is wide-
example: two of our participants who performed at
spread acknowledgement that there exists a gamut of
chance according to percent correct chroma (each at
AP ability (Takeuchi & Hulse, 1993; Ward, 1999). The
8.3%) had very different mean absolute deviations of
appearance or not of a bimodal distribution seems to
1.44 and 2.81 semitones with mean reaction times of
depend in part on the way participants are recruited,
4905 and 9124 ms, respectively. The first of these par-
the test is administered, and the data are reduced,
ticipants more quickly gave a response that was, on
analyzed, and presented. Therefore, this remains a con-
average, much more accurate than the second partici-
troversial point and sources of discrepancy in results
pant. According to criteria used in many studies, these
may include sampling, testing materials, and scoring
participants would not be resolved (although they
strategies. Conjecture as to what differences in biology
would likely both be categorized as non-possessors).
may underlie performance in different regions of this
Assigning a partial or full point for semitone devia-
distribution is a very large topic unto itself (Baharloo
tions, as is often done in the AP literature to account for
et al., 2000; Bermudez, Lerch, Evans, & Zatorre, 2008;
semitone shifts/imprecision (see introduction), dimin-
Gregersen et al., 2000; Miyazaki, 2004; Ross et al., 2005;
ishes this effect but also can obfuscate important and
Zatorre, 2003).
subtle distinctions among high performing partici-
pants that are recovered by the mean absolute deviation
Scoring Strategy or other similar indices. (An index of overall performance
that combines accuracy and response latency information
Even when using the most stringent scoring strategy, might also be a useful descriptor; see y-axis of Figure 7.)
which only credits perfectly correct pitch class identifica- It can safely be suggested that a scoring scheme that
tion, a range of behavior from perfect to random perform- only considers perfectly correct responses (or, at most,
ance was observed across our sample of 51 musicians, semitone deviations) is likely to artificially accentuate
Music2702_02 11/16/09 6:00 PM Page 98

98 Patrick Bermudez & Robert J. Zatorre

differences between the top performers and others, and random performers. Among NAP participants, there
sharpen the boundaries of a transition from strong to was a non-significant trend towards longer response
weak performers by discarding important performance times with lower MAD, perhaps indicating use of the
variance. It follows from our results that if one allows more time-consuming strategies among a subset of
for a wider range of strategies in performing an AP task intermediate performers. Figure 7, which plots percent
and takes into consideration the full variability of accu- correct score against an index that multiplies mean
racy, with less arbitrary descriptors as well as precise absolute deviation by the log reaction times (thereby
reaction times, the apparent boundaries between differ- penalizing particularly time consuming strategies) still
ent groups of musicians may not be quite so manifest. yields a broad range of scores. This spectrum of ability
That said, one could construe from our data two small is behaviorally and (likely) biologically interesting.
clusters, one of 11 participants with nearly perfect per-
formance and another of 13 participants with essentially Pitch Class Dependence
random performance (though these are more apparent
on the percent correct axis than the MAD axis; Figure 2). Several previously observed findings are replicated in
Ascertaining how these groups differ from each other, the data presented here. We are able to discern a pattern
and how each differs from intermediate performers, is where the notes corresponding to the C Ionian scale
essential in the ongoing elucidation of the ability and (non-accidental or white piano keys) were identified
relating its characteristics to underlying neurological significantly more accurately and more quickly than
infrastructure and genetics. non-diatonic notes (accidental or black piano keys;
If we were to expect AP to manifest in a binary fash- Miyazaki, 1988, 1989, 1990; Schellenberg, 2001;
ion, we would observe a sharply bimodal distribution Takeuchi & Hulse, 1991; Figures 4 and 5). Additionally,
of scores; no such effect is visible. If we were to expect a planned comparison revealed that the pitch class A,
all intermediate performers to use a combination of AP the most typical tuning reference, was identified more
for a subset of notes and relative pitch calculation to fill accurately and more quickly compared to the collapsed
in the gaps, we should observe a bimodal distribution performance across all other chroma. It was also more
for an index score that penalizes slower responses; accurately identified in the NAP group, likely driven by
Figure 7 does not support this interpretation. If we use the intermediate performers who may have used A as a
a score that artificially penalizes anything but the frequent reference for relative judgements of other
strongest performers, such as the strict percent correct pitches. These differ from the findings of Miyazaki
pitch class identification seen along the x-axis of Figure (1990) showing that pitch classes C and G were most
2, we should accentuate any bimodal tendency; it is still quickly and accurately identified, presumably as a
not clearly so. In sum, even though scoring strategies reflection of their highest-ranking harmonic roles in a
that discard important proportions of performance C Ionian context. The cohort of musicians in the work
variance are likely to exaggerate and, in some cases, per- of Miyazaki (1990) was highly homogeneous in terms
haps even create the impression of bimodality in a dis- of instrument of concentration (all keyboard players).
tribution of responses, applying these harsh scoring Our sample was broadly representative in that respect,
approaches to our data still does not create easily including keyboard, string, brass, percussion, and other
resolvable subsets of participants. We can therefore say instrumentalists. Various instruments across these cat-
with some confidence that there is true heterogeneity of egories favor different keys in which notes occur with
AP competence in our sample. Surely an important different frequencies. Perhaps the only commonality
contributor to this is the fact that we allowed our par- across them is the frequent use of A as a tuning refer-
ticipants an unlimited amount of time in which to give ence. Also of note, the ratio of correct white to black
their responses, allowing for the use of circuitous notes decreases as AP proficiency increases. In other
strategies such as relying on absolute pitch for one or a words, the very strongest performers are more likely to
few notes and relative judgements to determine the rest. label all chroma equally well (an obvious consequence
Though this ability is often deprecated as pseudo-AP, of making very few or no errors) whereas weaker per-
it is nonetheless of interest. In order for the relative formers are more likely to identify white notes dispro-
judgements to be accurate, the representation of the portionately well. This is one of many lines of evidence,
absolute note(s) must be sufficiently accurate and of along with the unusually high prevalence of AP among
high-resolution (narrow-band). All participants per- the blind (Bachem, 1940; Hamilton, Pascual-Leone, &
forming with a mean deviation of less than one semi- Schlaug, 2004; Welch, 1988), East Asian music students
tone responded within 6 s, as did about half of the (Gregersen, Kowalsky, Kohn, & Marvin, 1999; Miyazaki,
Music2702_02 11/16/09 6:00 PM Page 99

Absolute Pitch Ability 99

1988, 2004), and musical savants (Heaton, 2003; Heaton, Conclusion


Hermelin, & Pring, 1998; Lenhoff, Perales, & Hickok,
2001; Young & Nettelbeck, 1995), and broad perform- Although AP has been defined operationally within
ance distributions in our data and those of many other some studies, typically by establishing an arbitrary per-
researchers (Bachem, 1937; Takeuchi & Hulse, 1993; formance cut-off for an adhoc test, there are no com-
Ward, 1999), contributing to the largely accepted notion munity-wide conventions. That portion of disparity in
that most forms of AP are at least partly experience the literature that is not attributable to theoretical con-
dependent, with the typically more frequent exposure siderations is counter-productive. A certain amount of
to white notes during the early, formative years of music the discrepancy in the AP literature is doubtless due to
education having a lasting impact on adult AP perform- inconsistency in testing materials, methods, and crite-
ance. This, of course, is in addition to the patently obvi- ria, suggesting that there would be clear benefit from
ous and principled need for learning given that chroma standardization. If we hope to accurately describe the
do not exist in nature; that is, they are not constituent AP ability, including possible subtypes and their rela-
parts of the frequency continuum but rather impositions tive incidences in musical populations, we should aim
of our scalar constructs. Therefore, in performing an to standardize our stimuli and administration parame-
absolute pitch task such as ours, one must have formed ters. Doing so would allow us to test large numbers of
representations for the pitch classes to which the per- participants across many institutions and better
ceived pitch is compared and subsequently labelled. As describe and ascertain the heterogeneity of AP. Also of
has been suggested by Takeuchi and Hulse (1991), part interest is how we would be afforded the opportunity to
of the diatonic vs. non-diatonic effect may be due to compile norms that would then allow for statistically
familiarity with the note names themselves, though no meaningful descriptions and categorizations of per-
work to date has disentangled the relative contributions formance and, in turn, provide a means for opera-
of pitch and note name/notation processing requirements tionally defining AP for a larger research community.
and familiarity. We stand to incur the usual advantages associated with
AP and NAP participants did not differ in their establishing a carefully designed standardized screening
accuracy or speed of octave identification (Miyazaki, and behavioral assessment battery, and convention
1989). Though it might seem from Figure 3 as though would permit comparisons between studies with a level
AP participants are committing more octave errors of confidence that has not been possible to date, there-
given the peaks at 12 and +12 semitones deviation, by hastening collective progress in the endeavor to
this a misleading impression resulting from the fact characterize and understand absolute pitch.
that AP participants are able to make octave errors as Ours is a traditional note-naming test that is (and
a direct consequence of being able to correctly iden- can only be) targeted to a population with considerable
tify chroma and thereby accumulate responses in the knowledge, experience, and facility with conventional
same histogram bin. In contrast, the distribution of Occidental note-naming schemes. This was a purpose-
responses among NAP musicians is largely unimodal ful design decision that suited our immediate purposes
and a reflection of a coarser pitch height judgement. as well as those of the vast majority of experimentalists
AP participants are in fact non-significantly more who specifically recruit highly trained musicians when
accurate in identifying octaves which we suspect is investigating AP. Relying on conventional musical
due to the ability to accurately resolve notes close to knowledge allows for the design of a simple, portable,
octave transitions (e.g., B3 vs. C4, with only a semi- convenient, and discriminating test for AP ability
tone between them). Indeed, AP participants were among trained musicians. The obvious disadvantage is
significantly more accurate in identifying the octave that it cannot identify any form of absolute pitch that
assignments of B and C, t(49) = 2.22, p < .0001, but manifests itself outside the musical domain or inde-
not for the midway point in the octave F#, t(49) = 0.88, pendently of musical instruction. Ross and colleagues
p = .37. AP possessors have more recourse than tone (Ross, Olson, Marks, & Gore, 2004) have described a
height in labelling octaves, being able to precisely locate method appropriate for the identification of such indi-
a note on a grand staff, yet, despite this, they do commit viduals. A subset of their tasks generally take the form
octave errors that may in part be due to differences in of having a participant hear a tone (pure tone, iterated
octave number and notation conventions between instru- ripple noise, or other) and, after a variable delay filled
ments, lack of familiarity with the timbre of stimuli, and with distractor tones, then use a variable frequency
overtone confusion (Takeuchi & Hulse, 1993; Ward & tone generator to reproduce the presented tone. Ross
Burns, 1982). et al. (2005) have additionally posited a discrete biological
Music2702_02 11/16/09 6:00 PM Page 100

100 Patrick Bermudez & Robert J. Zatorre

mechanism that would account for the abilities of those in order to more comprehensively describe absolute pitch
participants showing the strongest form of AP. and provide the means for identifying possible subpop-
Our test has proved sensitive to key features of ulations, as well as deriving behavioral indices that can
absolute pitch proficiency. This is evinced by the detec- be regressed onto various biological metrics. This may
tion of such effects as greater accuracy and speed of help in deciphering the behavioral characteristics and
pitch class identification for white versus black notes neural underpinnings of this unusual ability which serves
(C major diatonic vs. non-diatonic notes), most accu- as an interesting model for several aspects of cerebral
rate and fastest identification for the pitch class A, and function.
the description of a wide variety of absolute pitch pro-
ficiency. Having provided a validation of our AP test, Author Note
we are now proceeding to the next step in development:
we intend to produce a distributable version for the PC We thank Marc Bouffard, Pierre Ahad, and Joyce Chen
that will be available to other centers and researchers to for their contributions and consultations. This work
provide a reliable and precise method for behaviorally was funded by the International Foundation for Music
assessing absolute pitch ability in musicians. The test will Research (IFMR), the Canadian Institutes of Health
be distributed through the BRAMS Laboratory (Montral) Research (CIHR), and the Natural Sciences and
upon request. It is our hope that this will constitute a Engineering Research Council of Canada
modest step toward the above mentioned benefits and Correspondence concerning this article should be
avoid experimenters unnecessary development costs for addressed to Patrick Bermudez, Department of Cognitive
proprietary tests. Overall, the aim is to gather multidi- Neuroscience, Montreal Neurological Institute, 3801
mensional information (accuracy, latency, per-note University, room 276, Montral, Qubec, Canada H3A
patterns of response, etc.) across many research centers 2B4. E-MAIL: patrick@bic.mni.mcgill.ca

References

ATHOS , E. A., L EVINSON , B., K ISTLER , A., Z EMANSKY, J., C ARROLL , J. B. (1975). Speed and accuracy of absolute pitch
B OSTROM , A., F REIMER , N., ET AL . (2007). Dichotomy and judgments: Some latter day results. Educational Testing
perceptual distortions in absolute pitch ability. Proceedings of Service Research Bulletin, 6, 1-77.
the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 104, 14795-14800. C HIN , C. S. (2003). The development of absolute pitch: A
B ACHEM , A. (1937). Various types of absolute pitch. Journal of theory concerning the roles of music training at an early
the Acoustical Society of America, 9, 146-151. developmental age and individual cognitive style. Psychology
B ACHEM , A. (1940). The genesis of absolute pitch. Journal of the of Music, 31, 155-171.
Acoustical Society of America, 11, 434-439. C OSTA-G IOMI , E., G ILMOUR , R., S IDDELL , J., & L EFEBVRE ,
BAHARLOO, S., JOHNSTON, P. A., SERVICE, S. K., GITSCHIER, J., & E. (2001). Absolute pitch, early musical instruction, and
FREIMER, N. B. (1998). Absolute pitch: An approach for identi- spatial abilities. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,
fication of genetic and nongenetic components. American 930, 394-396.
Journal of Human Genetics, 62, 224-231. G REGERSEN , P., KOWALSKY, E., KOHN , N., & M ARVIN , E.
BAHARLOO, S., SERVICE, S. K., RISCH, N., GITSCHIER, J., & FREIMER, (2000). Early childhood music education and predisposition
N. B. (2000). Familial aggregation of absolute pitch. American to absolute pitch: teasing apart genes and environment.
Journal of Human Genetics, 67, 755-758. American Journal of Human Genetics A, 98, 280-282.
B ENJAMIN , P. A. (2008). Transmission and population genetics, GREGERSEN, P. K., KOWALSKY, E., KOHN , N., & MARVIN , E. W.
(2nd ed.). Basingstoke, Hampshire: W. H. Freeman. (1999). Absolute pitch: Prevalence, ethnic variation, and
B ERMUDEZ , P. (2008). The neural correlates of absolute pitch. estimation of the genetic component. American Journal of
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, McGill University, Montreal. Human Genetics, 65, 911-913.
B ERMUDEZ , P., L ERCH , J. P., E VANS , A. C., & Z ATORRE , R. J. H AMILTON , R. H., PASCUAL -L EONE , A., & S CHLAUG , G. (2004).
(2008). Neuroanatomical correlates of musicianship as Absolute pitch in blind musicians. Neuroreport, 15, 803-
revealed by cortical thickness and voxel-based morphometry. 806.
Cerebral Cortex, 19, 1583-1596. H EATON , P. (2003). Pitch memory, labelling and disembedding
B ERMUDEZ , P., & Z ATORRE , R. J. (2009). The absolute pitch in autism. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44,
mind continues to reveal itself. Journal of Biology, 8, 75. 543-551.
Music2702_02 11/16/09 6:00 PM Page 101

Absolute Pitch Ability 101

H EATON , P., H ERMELIN , B., & P RING , L. (1998). Autism and R OSS , D. A., G ORE , J. C., & M ARKS , L. E. (2005). Absolute
pitch processing: A precursor for savant musical ability. Music pitch: Music and beyond. Epilepsy and Behavior, 7, 578-601.
Perception, 15, 291-305. ROSS , D. A., O LSON , I. R., M ARKS , L. E., & G ORE , J. C. (2004).
K APLAN , R. M., & S ACCUZZO, D. P. (1993). Psychological A nonmusical paradigm for identifying absolute pitch possess-
testing: Principles, applications, and issues (3rd ed.). Pacific ors. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 116, 1793-1799.
Grove, California: Books/Cole. S CHELLENBERG , E. G. (2001). Asymmetries in the discrimination
L ENHOFF, H. M., P ERALES , O., & H ICKOK , G. (2001). Absolute of musical intervals: Going out-of-tune is more noticeable than
pitch in Williams syndrome. Music Perception, 18, 491-503. going in-tune. Music Perception, 19, 223-248.
L EVITIN , D. J., & R OGERS , S. E. (2005). Absolute pitch: TAKEUCHI , A. H., & H ULSE , S. H. (1991). Absolute-pitch judg-
Perception, coding, and controversies. Trends in Cognitive ments of black-key and white-key pitches. Music Perception, 9,
Sciences, 9, 26-33. 27-46.
M ILLER , G. A. (1956). The magical number 7, plus or minus TAKEUCHI , A. H., & H ULSE , S. H. (1993). Absolute pitch.
2Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 345-361.
Psychological Review, 63, 81-97. VAN K REVELEN , A. (1951). The ability to make absolute judg-
M IYAZAKI , K. (1988). Musical pitch identification by absolute ments of pitch. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 42, 207-215.
pitch possessors. Perception and Psychophysics, 44, 501-512. WARD, W. D. (1999). Absolute pitch. In D. Deutsch (Ed.), The
M IYAZAKI , K. (1989). Absolute pitch identificationEffects of psychology of music (2nd ed., pp. 265-298). San Diego:
timber and pitch region. Music Perception, 7, 1-14. Academic Press.
M IYAZAKI , K. (1990). The speed of musical pitch identification WARD, W. D., & B URNS , E. M. (1982). Absolute pitch. In D.
by absolute-pitch possessors. Music Perception, 8, 177-188. Deutsch (Ed.), The psychology of music (1st ed., pp. 431-451).
M IYAZAKI , K. (2004). How well do we understand absolute San Diego: Academic Press.
pitch? Acoustical Science and Technology, 25, 426-432. W ELCH , G. F. (1988). Observations on the incidence of
M ORENO S ALA , M. T., & C OSTA-G IOMI , E. (2008). Cognitive absolute pitch (AP) ability in the early blind. Psychology of
and spatial abilities related to young childrens development of Music, 16, 77-80.
absolute pitch perception.Unpublished manuscript. YOUNG , R. L., & N ETTELBECK , T. (1995). The abilities of a
NLM. (2008). Genes and disease. Retrieved June 2008 from http:// musical savant and his family. Journal of Autism and Develop-
www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/bv.fcgi?rid=gnd mental Disorders, 25, 231-248.
P OLLACK , I. (1952). The information of elementary auditory ZATORRE, R. J. (2003). Absolute pitch: A model for understanding
displays. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 24, the influence of genes and development on neural and
745-749. cognitive function. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 692-695.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Вам также может понравиться