Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 18

Environmental Science & Policy 8 (2005) 637–654

www.elsevier.com/locate/envsci

Landscape visualisation and climate change: the potential


for influencing perceptions and behaviour
Stephen R.J. Sheppard
Collaborative for Advanced Landscape Planning (CALP), Department of Forest Resources and Landscape Architecture,
University of British Columbia, 2045-2424 Main Mall, Vancouver, BC, Canada V6T 1Z4

Abstract

The urgent need to mitigate and adapt to climate change is becoming more widely understood in scientific and policy circles, but public
awareness lags behind. The potential of visual communication to accelerate social learning and motivate implementation of the substantial
policy, technological, and life-style changes needed, has begun to be recognised. In particular, realistic landscape visualisations may offer
special advantages in rapidly advancing peoples’ awareness of climate change and possibly affecting behaviour and policy, by bringing certain
possible consequences of climate change home to people in a compelling manner. However, few such applications are yet in use, the
theoretical basis for the effectiveness of visualisations in this role has not been clearly established, and there are ethical concerns elicited by
adopting a persuasive approach which deliberately engages the emotions with visual imagery. These questions and policy implications are
discussed in the context of a theoretical framework on the effects of landscape visualisation on a spectrum of responses to climate change
information, drawing in part on evidence from other applications of landscape visualisation. The author concludes that the persuasive use of
visualisations, together with other approaches, may be effective, is justified, and could be vital in helping communicate climate change
effectively, given ethical standards based on disclosure, drama, and defensibility.
# 2005 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Visualisation; Climate change; Visual communications; Carbon consciousness; Behavioural response

1. Introduction (2001) reported that the majority of their study participants


were not concerned about climate change. Many authors have
This paper addresses the potential role of realistic addressed the substantial gap between the possession of
visualisation tools in rapidly increasing peoples’ awareness environmental knowledge or behavioural intent and actual
of climate change and possibly affecting behaviour and pro-environmental behaviour (e.g. Kollmuss and Agyeman,
policy, together with associated ethical dilemmas. 2002). In some countries, organisations such as the UK
Climate Impacts Programme (UKCIP, 2000a) have been set
1.1. The need for increased public awareness and policy up to address this gap through education and communication.
response on climate change Tickell (2002) has summarised the difficulties in com-
municating climate change (discussed further below), and
Evidence of climate change and its impacts on society and warned that it may require a calamity to induce people and
the environment have become generally accepted by scientists governments to take the necessary radical actions. The
(Pearce et al., 1996; IPCC, 2001), along with the importance principal question therefore becomes: what are the most
of mitigating and adapting to climate change (Fawcett et al., effective ways to stimulate climate change mitigation and
2002; Adger, 2003). However, public awareness and policy adaptation behaviours in society before crises occur, and
changes are lagging behind. Boardman and Palmer (2003) reduce harm to the environment and society? The main
found low levels of ‘carbon consciousness’ among European hypothesis examined in this paper is that certain kinds of
consumers and businesses, and Lorenzoni and Langford visual communication (i.e. realistic landscape visualisations)
which attempt to look into the future and which engage the
E-mail address: Stephen.Sheppard@ubc.ca. emotions, may substantially enhance awareness-building on

1462-9011/$ – see front matter # 2005 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.envsci.2005.08.002
638 S.R.J. Sheppard / Environmental Science & Policy 8 (2005) 637–654

various complexities and implications of climate change, and Community-based social marketing using such techniques
may help motivate behavioural change at the individual to has proved effective in bringing about behaviour change
societal levels (Sheppard, 2004). Since few such visualisa- (McKenzie-Mohr and Smith, 1999; Kollmuss and Agyeman,
tions addressing climate change have yet been produced or 2002).
subjected to research evaluation, this paper attempts a Among the various forms of visual imagery available, 3D
proactive, systematic analysis of their potential, informed by visualisations present special characteristics which help
early precedents. It is recognised that many more abstract, demonstrate consequences of environmental change in a
statistical, or iconic aspects of climate and related landscape compelling manner. Scientific information is commonly
change may not be readily communicated with realistic communicated by forms of data visualisation such as charts,
landscape visualisation, and other more conventional tools diagrams, maps, graphics, and 3D computer models (Cox,
may be needed in addition. 1990; MacEachren and Ganter, 1990). These 2D and 3D
As background to the discussion of visualisation representations are often somewhat abstract or simplified
applications to climate change issues, this section provides (Fig. 1). Landscape visualisation, sometimes referred to as
a brief rationale for using visual imagery, and landscape visual simulation or landscape modelling (Ervin and
visualisation in particular, in promoting environmental Hasbrouck, 2001), represents actual places and on-the-
awareness and behaviour. ground conditions in 3D perspective views (Fig. 2), often
with fairly high realism (Sheppard and Salter, 2004). These
1.2. The rationale for using visual imagery and specific forms of virtual reality are now typically computer-
visualisation to raise environmental awareness and generated in three or four dimensions, and can convey
influence behaviour detailed information on the assumed future appearance of
environments (sky, terrain, buildings, and vegetation). This
Human responses to environments and visual displays amounts to a unique form of visual communication,
can be broadly categorised as cognitive (related to knowl- conveying information in the dominant form to which the
edge and understanding), affective (related to feelings, human species is genetically adapted (i.e. visual land-
attitudes, and emotions), behavioural (related to changes in scapes), but capable of showing future worlds as they would
behaviour of the viewer), and physiological (biological or be seen if the viewer were actually there. Historically,
physical effects on the observer’s body) (Zube et al., 1982). landscape visualisations have been used primarily in urban
The latter type of response is not considered further in this design and impact assessment (Porter, 1979; Sheppard,
paper. Perception is defined here as the process of seeing or 1989; Bosselmann, 1998). GIS-based methods for landscape
otherwise perceiving phenomena, leading to particular visualisation (Appleton et al., 2002) are now being applied
responses or states which include both cognitive and more broadly to depict alternative future scenarios for
affective outcomes. The focus of the paper is primarily visioning, public input, and decision-making (Tress and
on individual perceptions, individual behavioural responses, Tress, 2002; Steinitz et al., 2003; Sheppard and Meitner,
and organisational behaviour (i.e. policy). 2005).
The ability of visual imagery to communicate messages Nicholson-Cole (2005) describes the promise of land-
quickly and powerfully has long been recognised in fields of scape visualisation in conveying strong messages quickly,
human activity ranging from commercial advertising, to the condensing complex information, engaging people in issues
media, to political campaigns: seeing is believing. The of environmental change, and motivating personal action.
cognitive advantages of visual information over written or However, we lack comprehensive knowledge and integrative
verbal information have been widely documented (e.g. theories to validate, explain, and predict a broad range of
Tufte, 1990), for example, when mentally visualising responses to landscape visualisations (Appleyard, 1977;
conditions that cannot be seen directly in the real world Sheppard, 2001). These techniques also harbour risks and
(e.g. famines in remote countries, or the design of a proposed key limitations, and there are a number of ethical and
building). Visual stimuli can also trigger innate and instant
reflexes and feelings which can be persistent even in the face
of new information (Slovic et al., 2002); Zajonc (1984) has
termed this the ‘‘primacy of affect’’ over cognition. Visual
imagery can therefore be a powerful tool to reach people’s
emotional side, as witnessed in the more dramatic imagery
from the news media. Imagery (visual or otherwise) which
triggers affective responses has been shown to improve
cognition in some cases (Winn, 1997), and to influence
people’s decision-making (Slovic et al., 2002). Clearly, the Fig. 1. Example of a conceptual or abstract 3D visualisation (showing
terrain and elevation). Credit: John Lewis, CALP, UBC. Reproduced from
intent of some visual imagery is not just to inform or engage Sheppard, Lewis, and Akai, 2004, Landscape Visualisation: An Extension
emotionally, but also to influence people’s behaviour, as in Guide for First Nations and Rural Communities, courtesy of Sustainable
advertising and public service health announcements. Forest Management Network, Canada.
S.R.J. Sheppard / Environmental Science & Policy 8 (2005) 637–654 639

Fig. 2. Visualisations of landscapes can now be modelled in detail and realistically rendered, as in this depiction of two forest management alternatives after 38
years of plan implementation. Credit: Ken Fairhurst, Mike Meitner, Ryan Gandy, and Duncan Cavens, CALP, UBC. Courtesy of CANFOR.

professional issues raised by attempts to influence behaviour influences will be required to effect societal change on
using visual imagery. It is therefore important to consider climate change; the attempt made here is to articulate the
carefully available evidence and theoretical arguments for additional or value-added contributions of visualisation
the effectiveness and ethics of using visualisation on climate within the toolset. In the rest of this paper, the term
change issues, in order to avoid false expectations, critique ‘visualisation’ refers to landscape visualisation, unless
the inevitable use of visualisations of climate change, and otherwise noted.
guide further research.

1.3. Scope and structure of paper 2. A theoretical framework for the impact of
visualisation on awareness, attitudes, and
These issues are discussed in the context of an initial behaviour in relation to climate change
theoretical framework on the possible effects of landscape
visualisations on human responses to climate change; thus, This section addresses the issue of whether landscape
Section 2 addresses the question: ‘‘Can landscape visualisa- visualisation can be expected to impact awareness,
tion deliver on improving awareness and stimulating action individual behaviour, and policy. We briefly consider
on climate change?’’ Section 3 examines dilemmas relating theoretical concepts, available evidence, and experience
to such use, essentially addressing the question: ‘‘Should we from professional practice, relating to people’s responses to
use landscape visualisations to engage the emotions and general environmental issues presented via landscape
influence behaviour, and if so, how?’’ Possible approaches visualisation. Then, we consider how this knowledge on
are described involving ethical standards for use of visualisation can be applied to the question of people’s
visualisations in a persuasive role on climate change, responses to climate change.
resulting policy implications, and research priorities.
Visualisation can have multiple roles in society: in 2.1. Human responses to landscape visualisations
entertainment, planning and decision-support, education,
and research on public perceptions. In this paper, the focus is Some of the potential benefits promised by landscape
on visualisations as awareness-building tools and potential visualisation techniques in the arena of awareness-building,
change agents in fostering action by both the general public behaviour change, and environmental decision-making can
and policy-makers, although with more emphasis on be summarised as in Table 1. These benefits include both
individual behaviour. In the context of climate change, self-evident capabilities (e.g. the ability to depict alternative
the most urgent applications address the need for future scenarios side by side) and theorised influences on
behavioural change to mitigate climate change, though people’s responses (e.g. increased engagement and per-
the ideas discussed in this paper apply equally to adaptation ceived salience). However, despite the widespread use of
priorities. landscape visualisations in planning and design, findings on
While the focus is on landscape visualisation, it is responses to them are generally not scientifically docu-
understood that in reality a combination of techniques and mented or comprehensive (Sheppard, 2001; Lange, 2001;
640 S.R.J. Sheppard / Environmental Science & Policy 8 (2005) 637–654

Table 1
Potentially beneficial attributes of landscape visualisation for promoting environmental awareness and actiona
Integration of science The combination of the predictive capabilities of modelling and GIS with the intuitive and experientially rich
and intuition media of photography and realistic representation, with meaningful socio-cultural associations for communities
that may help strengthen more informed perspectives in decision-making
Engagement of lay-people The attractiveness of virtual reality and its novel applications to conventional planning problems may be helpful
in getting multiple stakeholders to engage in public processes (Sheppard and Meitner, 2005; Nicholson-Cole, 2005)
Personal salience The ability to localize and ground the information by detailed depiction of recognisable and well-known sites as they
would be seen by local residents or users, as opposed to a detached plan or aerial view or an expert’s conceptualisation
Presentation of choices The ability to present alternative futures side-by-side and over time, posing ‘what-if’ questions (Steinitz et al., 2003)
for the future in the search for preferred or acceptable environmental solutions over the long term
Flexibility of tool Digital visualisation techniques can be modified or customized to emphasize important information or condense
complex details, to fit the presentation to the needs and capabilities of the user (Sheppard, 2005)
a
Potential disadvantages of landscape visualisation are discussed in the text in Section 3.1.

Sheppard and Salter, 2004), and there is clear potential for (MacEachren and Ganter, 1990; Winn, 1997; Lewis, 2000;
offsetting disadvantages (discussed below). Salter, 2005).
Most recent visualisation evaluation studies focus on Daniel and Meitner (2001) have described several studies
direct self-report evaluation of the tools, and here there is which show that visualisations can arouse positive or
considerable evidence of their usability or perceived negative emotional reactions in observers. Few studies have
communications effectiveness (Bengtsson et al., 1997; Al- attempted to measure the intensity of emotional reaction,
Kodmany, 2000; Appleton and Lovett, 2003; Sheppard and though practical experience suggests public reactions to
Meitner, 2005), including the ability to engage lay-people visualisations can sometimes be vehement. A few studies
(Lewis and Sheppard, in press) (Fig. 3). Some research has have evaluated the ability of visualisations to match the
evaluated effects on cognition, but mostly with somewhat affective and related experiential and evaluative responses
abstract forms of visualisation (MacEachren and Ganter, obtained from real world environments (e.g. Bosselmann
1990; Al-Kodmany, 2000), which can be helpful in and Craik, 1987; Sheppard, 1989, 2001; Meitner and Daniel,
explaining concepts, ecological processes, and overall 1997; Bishop and Rohrmann, 2003), with mixed results.
environmental conditions not easily expressed in text or Zube et al. (1982) and Bishop and Rohrmann (2003)
data tables. Winn (1997) has argued that more complex, nonetheless believe that the greater the realism, the more
interactive 3D virtual reality displays may provide similar the responses will be to real life.
qualitatively and quantitatively superior forms of learning Very few studies have been carried out on the behavioural
on environmental science and global change issues. impacts of landscape visualisation, either during the
Researchers have found that visualisation can demonstrate exposure to the visualisation material or afterwards. Some
cognitive advantages over other media (e.g. Furness et al., experiments have evaluated behavioural actions such as
1998; Danahy, 2001), though MacEachren (2001) notes that pathway choices in a virtual environment (Bishop et al.,
learning may be hampered by the virtual devices used to 2001), or tracked behaviour in collaborative immersive
attract and sustain attention. We should also be careful to settings during planning workshops (Campbell and Salter,
discriminate between learning quickly (widely reported in 2004). Orland (1992, p. 259) has speculated that visualisa-
practice) and learning correctly, which is seldom measured tions may be used ‘‘in a more persuasive mode to motivate

Fig. 3. The impact of imagery: substantially increased dialogue was obtained with members of a First Nation community on resource management issues when
using realistic landscape visualisations, compared with using simple GIS maps. Credit: John Lewis, CALP, UBC. Courtesy of Cheam Band, BC. Reproduced by
permission of Sustainable Forest Management Network.
S.R.J. Sheppard / Environmental Science & Policy 8 (2005) 637–654 641

people to do something about the impact being represented,’’ too broadly (Bosselmann and Craik, 1987). There would
as happens with photography, film, and television media. however seem to be advantages in engagement and
McKenzie-Mohr and Smith (1999) provide numerous cognition, with some evidence of emotional arousal and
examples of visual information which, with other commu- the potential to affect behaviour on environmental issues.
nity-based social marketing techniques, can influence
sustainable behaviour or adaptation if they are vivid, 2.2. Applications of landscape visualisation to the
personal, and concrete. problem of climate change
Interactions between cognitive, affective, and beha-
vioural responses may help explain visual triggers to So, can visualisation, in combination with other
behavioural responses. Slovic et al. (2002, p. 398) state that information sources/media, affect human responses to
‘‘many theorists have given affect a direct and primary role climate change? To address this question systematically,
in motivating behaviour.’’ They review evidence that more we need to consider the specific context of society’s
vivid and sensational narratives associated with feelings perceptions of climate change, before developing a
rather than cognition have more influence on risk conceptual framework of how visualisation may relate to
behaviours. This suggests that introducing compelling these perceptions.
visual evidence that is readily translatable into personal
risks or implications can be more successful in strengthening 2.2.1. Perceptions of climate change
motivations to change behaviour than cognitive information The potential for visualisation in relation to climate
alone. Maiteny (2002) argues that behavioural change to change can be considered in terms of:
support sustainability can only take place when the
individual has gone through a deep-rooted personal ! perceptual characteristics and difficulties relating to
transformation, as in experiential learning processes. climate change itself;
Sheppard (2004) and Nicholson-Cole (2005) speculate that ! potential and observed responses of people and agencies
the strength and symbolic emotive content of visualisation to climate change, and observed gaps between awareness
imagery may motivate people to act in a more sustainable and behaviour;
manner. However, it must be recognised that both the forms ! early precedents for responses to imagery and visualisa-
of and causal influences on environmentally significant tion of climate change-related phenomena.
behaviour are very complex, varying with pre-determined
attitudes, individual capability, context, etc. (Stern, 2000), The characteristics of climate change make it difficult to
and therefore the effectiveness of any form of external see directly. Carbon dioxide and even the carbon source
intervention such as visualisations, on their own, may be itself is effectively invisible. There is potential to recognise
limited by other factors. some quite tangible visual or landscape-related effects of
There is also the important issue of how the use of climate change, such as sea-level rise, flooding, or drought (a
visualisation could influence relevant policy, either directly typology of such effects is provided below). However,
through presentations to key decision-makers and policy- Tickell (2002) has described the problems of communicat-
makers or indirectly through public opinion and collective ing change which occurs over long time periods and which
individual actions. Again, however, there is little scientific carries uncertain and uncomfortable future consequences.
information on such policy responses in the visualisation Winn (1997) and Nicholson-Cole (2005) have described the
literature. There is some evidence from perception studies scale, complexity, and abstraction of climate change as
using visualisation which indicate differing levels of challenges to communication. This is exacerbated by
acceptability of various resource management scenarios problems of media coverage, ranging from down-playing
(e.g. Sheppard and Meitner, 2005; Ribe, 2005), although the the issue (Monbiot, 2004) to ‘‘inappropriate use of model
practical linkages between judgements of unacceptability in predictions, inaccurate representations of physical pro-
research and actual policy impacts in the real world have not cesses, false associations of unrelated phenomena, and use
been widely demonstrated. Professional practice does of emotion-laden language’’ (Furness et al., 1998, p. 19). It is
provide anecdotal examples suggesting sometimes profound also difficult to relate global or national figures on climate
effects of visualisation on government behaviour: for change to local scales (Furness et al., 1998), and there are as
example, computer visualisations of potential housing yet few reliable predictions for local climate effects or
development under the Official Community Plan in Maple spatially explicit consequences at high levels of resolution,
Ridge, BC, when shown to members of the city council, other than for sea-level rise. It is therefore difficult for
caused such surprise and negative reaction that the council individuals to relate their actions (good or bad) to the larger
initiated a process to develop sustainable community plans context of climate change (Sheppard, 2004; Nicholson-Cole,
with a very different footprint. 2005).
Overall, many dimensions of visualisation use and Information has begun to flow on the nature of
subsequent human response (especially behavioural) are conceptualisations of and human responses to climate
not well understood, and we should be wary of generalizing change. Several researchers report evidence of persistent
642 S.R.J. Sheppard / Environmental Science & Policy 8 (2005) 637–654

misconceptions of climate change, e.g. confusing clean air


or ozone-layer issues with climate change (e.g. Furness
et al., 1998; Boardman and Palmer, 2003). Kollmuss and
Agyeman (2002) and Lorenzoni and Langford (2001)
describe various types of barrier or gaps between percep-
tions/intentions and behaviour conducive to climate change
mitigation, including gaps in cognition and awareness
(ignorance), gaps between knowledge and action, and gaps
between intent and action. Lorenzoni and Langford (2001)
identify different perspectives that people adopt when
confronted by climate change issues, including denial,
doubt, disinterest, and engagement. Other responses include
confusion, pessimism, guilt, frustration, and, more rarely,
motivation to change behaviour (Kollmuss and Agyeman,
2002; Maiteny, 2002). Nicholson-Cole (2005) found that a
sample of Norfolk residents expressed their feelings about
climate change in a somewhat negative but abstract, distant
sense, though personal experiences and impacts on local
environments were a common feature of people’s visual
conceptions of climate change. Her results highlight the
influence of popular visual media on the mental imagery of
climate change that respondents held (Nicholson-Cole,
2005), and suggest, in particular, that respondents were most
emotionally affected by national and local imagery, rather
than global. Gaps in behavioural motivation seemed to be
related to difficulties in personalising climate change
(Nicholson-Cole, 2005); respondents felt that they needed
to understand the personal implications of climate change
and be regularly reminded if they were to act. However, it
should be noted that these findings represent self-report data
on behavioural intent, and not actual measured behaviour.
In the context of climate change, visual communications
are beginning to be used to accelerate social learning, and
the possibility of their motivating the substantial policy,
technological, and life-style changes needed has begun to be
recognised. Cohen (1997) used GIS and remote sensing
imagery with other information to communicate climate
change scenarios to Canadian stakeholders, who reported Fig. 4. Computer visualisations of simulated summer-averaged sea-ice
that the scenarios made a difference in their visions of the thickness at the South Pole over a 150 year period, where white represents
future and potential interventions in policy debates. the thickest ice and the red line depicts the maximum extent of 1cm thick
Scientific agencies provide animated 3D visualisations of sea-ice. Credit: Copyright, # 2002, University Corporation for Atmo-
modelled climate change phenomena seen from space, such spheric Research, http://www.vets.ucar.edu/vg/seaice/index.shtml. Scienti-
fic credits: Mark Holland, CGD/NCAR; Cecilia Bitz, U. Washington; Bruce
as changes in sea ice over time (Fig. 4), on the web; similar Briegleb, CGD/NCAR; Elizabeth Hunke, LANL; Bill Lipscomb, LANL;
visualisations of the ‘ozone hole’ are commonly credited Richard Moritz, U. Washington; Julie Schramm, CGD/NCAR. Visualization
with helping to spur legislation to ban fluorocarbons. New credits: Fred Clare, NCAR/SCD; John Clyne, NCAR/SCD; Tim Scheitlin,
books on climate change highlight the impacts of climate NCAR/SCD. (For interpretation of the references to colour in this figure
change as illustrated with vivid landscape photography of legend, the reader is referred to the web version of the article.)
changing iconic landscapes such as snow-capped volcanoes
and glaciers (Lynas, 2004). There has been much debate
over the recent film entitled ‘‘The Day After Tomorrow’’ change, with significantly more concern, anxiety, and
which dramatically depicts fictionalized climate change motivation to change behaviour, though the latter effect
events (http://www.nydailynews.com/entertainment/story/ was short-lived. It appeared, however, that the disaster film
189422p-163828c.html). Lowe et al. (2005) found that genre, overly dramatic special effects, and the failure to
the film, which included spectacular ‘realistic’ visualisations distinguish fact from fiction amid the high realism, led to
of weather extremes and sea-level rise threatening the low credibility of the film itself, making inferences for more
characters, did result in changes in attitude towards climate scientifically based visualisation difficult to draw.
S.R.J. Sheppard / Environmental Science & Policy 8 (2005) 637–654 643

Few visualisations of climate change effects on the hypothetical mitigation strategy to reduce fossil fuel usage,
landscape have yet been produced to scientific standards, seen from within a real community. Fig. 6 provides
and even fewer have yielded results on responses. The examples of 3D modelling with semi-realistic visualisation
feasibility of using realistic landscape visualisations to software developed for the Royal Commission on Environ-
depict climate change is not in question: it can be done quite mental Pollution, to depict a hypothetical British landscape
simply using limited 2D perspective imagery with photo- in existing conditions and with alternate mitigation
imaging software to depict specific possible consequences of strategies, here seen in birds-eye view. Dockerty et al.
climate change, or by more sophisticated 3D/4D modelling (2005) describe one of the first studies to visualise
of future scenarios with multiple attributes and visualisation scientifically modelled effects of climate change in an
inputs. The ease of manipulating images with convincing actual rural landscape in Norfolk, at the detailed site level.
photo-realism is demonstrated in Fig. 5, which depicts a Here, local scenarios visualised were developed from the

Fig. 5. Photo-realistic landscape visualisations comparing existing conditions and a hypothetical low-carbon future in southern England. Credit: Cecilia
Achiam, CALP, UBC.
644 S.R.J. Sheppard / Environmental Science & Policy 8 (2005) 637–654

four basic socio-economic scenarios described by Nakice-


novic and Swart (2000), by applying an agricultural land use
model and locally specific decision-rules and assumptions,
to develop high-resolution 3D visualisations showing
changes in the farming landscape (Fig. 7). However, no
studies of formally collected responses to these visualisa-
tions have yet been published.
In other visualisation-based research on responses to
long-range scenarios for forest management planning
(Sheppard and Meitner, 2005), rural community members
questioned the failure to build in climate change factors to
such models, raising concerns over modelling credibility.
Schroth et al. (2005) used interactive 3D aerial views of
semi-abstract 3D visualisations of the Entlebuch Biosphere
Reserve in Switzerland, to assess qualitatively responses to
retreating snow cover on winter recreation under climate
change modelling assumptions based on topographic
elevation (Fig. 8). When shown to community stakeholders,
the visualisations caused surprise and disquiet, which led
quickly to a new and different discussion of summer
recreation opportunities (Schroth et al., 2005), suggesting
that the medium can stimulate a rapid adaptation response.

2.2.2. Towards a theoretical framework for visualisation


of climate change
Based on a synthesis and simplification of the available
evidence and theoretical arguments discussed above, Fig. 9
presents a conceptual diagram in the form of a spectrum of
possible individual responses to visual information on
climate change, representing different levels of carbon
consciousness and commitment to action on climate change
issues. We can envisage the range of perceptual and
behavioural responses to visual information in the form of an
ordered categorisation moving from a low state of awareness
through to action, which corresponds generally with a
progression from cognitive processing of information,
through emotional responses, to behaviour change. It is
not assumed that each stage on the spectrum is a necessary
precursor for response types further along the spectrum, or
that over time any individual will necessarily move through
the range of responses in this specific linear sequence. For
example, emotional effects may actually precede cognitive
effects from a given presentation, or occur at the same time.
The diagram is presented as a simple way of structuring our
thinking and emerging research programmes on the range of
response types and effects we may look for as the result of
Fig. 6. Hypothetical British landscape visualised in existing conditions and applying visualisations to issues of climate change. It is
with alternate future scenarios including mitigation strategies featuring recognised that there are many other models of the
wind-turbines and biofuels plantations, and varying levels of development.
The two future scenarios represent two major energy choices (S1 – energy
motivations and controlling factors on pro-environmental
demands stabilized at 1998 levels; S4 – energy demands at half the 1998 behaviour, which may not be reflected in this simple
levels*), affecting the need for large-scale renewable or nuclear energy framework.
plants that adversely impact on landscape and seascape character, and the The responses illustrated here can be related to other
capacity of small-scale generation to cope with local energy demands. systems of categorising types of respondents, e.g. Lorenzoni
Credit: Images from ‘‘Visualizing renewable Energy in the Landscape of
2050’’: Copyright of The Countryside Agency. Images by ethos-uk.com.
and Langford’s (2001) classification of the deniers,
*S4 scenario uses energy projections from the Royal Commission for uninterested, doubters, and engaged; or to Kollmuss and
Environmental Pollution. Agyeman’s (2002) analysis of barriers to pro-environmental
S.R.J. Sheppard / Environmental Science & Policy 8 (2005) 637–654 645

Fig. 8. Semi-realistic 3D visualisations of forecasted snow conditions and


existing ski runs in the Sorenberg area of Switzerland, under current
conditions and after 50 years with climate change, with safe snow elevations
for skiing depicted in white. Credit: Olaf Schroth/VisuLands 2005, #Geo-
data: RAWI Lucerne.

effective than other forms of communication. The response


spectrum suggests that the early stages along the spectrum
may be more straightforward to achieve with visualisation
than the upper stages: the ability of pictures to convey
information is well accepted, whereas the ability to impact
behaviour is much less certain.
In order to bridge the various kinds of perceptual or
behavioural gap shown in Fig. 9, it would seem that different
forms of visualisation may be required, as suggested in
Fig. 10. In improving understanding of climate change, we
Fig. 7. Model-derived 3D visualisations of existing conditions in a Norfolk
agricultural area, with two scenarios under climate change in 2020: a low
may expect that more conceptual 3D visualisations may be
mitigation, high carbon emissions scenario (A2) and a local stewardship appropriate, through simplification and focus on the most
scenario with lower carbon emissions (B2). Credit: Katy Appleton, Uni- cogent information such as overall CO2 cycles, shifts in
versity of East Anglia. Reproduced from Dockerty et al., 2005, courtesy of biogeoclimatic zones, or cumulative effects. Cognitively
Computers, Environment and Urban Systems. effective landscape visualisation might focus on augmenting
reality to make the invisible visible, making the abstract
behaviour. The gaps between the stages along the spectrum tangible, collapsing long time scales into short periods, and
represent some of the main gaps between knowledge and easily switching between different scales (Winn, 1997;
action. However, for visualisation to be judged effective in Furness et al., 1998). Clarity of message and of depicted
addressing climate change, viewers do not need to move along conditions appears important in effective cognition (Shep-
the entire length of the spectrum; if visualisation succeeds at pard, 1989; Nicholson-Cole, 2005). While realism may not
any one of these levels, e.g. in reducing ignorance or denial, be necessary for (and may in some cases detract from)
then this should be beneficial, particularly if it is more cognition, Furness et al. (1998, p. 13) believe that, in most
646 S.R.J. Sheppard / Environmental Science & Policy 8 (2005) 637–654

environmental psychology literature that familiar land-


scapes tend to be associated with stronger and more
positive affective responses (e.g. Kaplan and Herbert,
1988), and as noted above, people seem most affected by
personal implications of climate change (McKenzie-
Mohr and Smith, 1999; Nicholson-Cole, 2005).
! Immediacy: near-term conditions (Lorenzoni and Lang-
ford, 2001) or possibly longer term conditions made to
seem nearer term through speeding up time, combined
with meaningful future considerations such as their
neighbourhood as seen by the viewers’ grand-children.
! Containing images of people, animals, or other symbols
with strong affective content (Nicholson-Cole, 2005).
! Demonstrating the future consequences of people’s
actions or inactions (Furness et al., 1998).

In theory, behaviour change may require emotional and


cognitive attributes described above, but intensified to be
vivid, memorable, and transformative (McKenzie-Mohr and
Smith, 1999; Maiteny, 2002): ‘‘a compelling virtual
environment will likely heighten (people’s) motivation to
act responsibly’’ (Furness et al., 1998, p. 28). Both Winn
(1997) and Furness et al. (1998) argue that vivid and
compelling visualisations can be obtained through a range of
techniques, notably:

! immersion in a virtual environment: large images and


panoramic ‘wrap-around’ displays can increase the sense
of presence (Furness et al., 1998), engagement (Apple-
yard, 1977), and intensity of experience (Sheppard et al.,
2001);
! dynamic or animated imagery that increases enthusiasm
and engagement (e.g. Dykes, 2000) and/or provides
freedom of virtual movement for the viewer (Orland and
Uusitalo, 2001);
Fig. 9. A theoretical spectrum of human responses to visual information on
! interactivity with the displayed data in real-time, to
climate change.
increase engagement (Orland and Uusitalo, 2001; Camp-
bell and Salter, 2004; Schroth et al., 2005).
cases, ‘‘. . . realistic representation will lead to more direct
and more robust knowledge construction.’’ Experience has Beyond media attributes, the content of the climate
already shown that the novelty or interest in computer change message also appears critical if behaviour is to be
visualisations can attract people and engage them in influenced. McKenzie-Mohr and Smith (1999) argue that
collaborative learning processes (Winn, 1997; Campbell messages which emphasize environmental losses due to
and Salter, 2004; Sheppard and Meitner, 2005). inaction are consistently more persuasive than those which
In order to reach the emotional side of viewers, the simply emphasize benefits of action; such threats should be
following additional attributes of visualisation would appear combined with positive implications of action to overcome
to be important: the barrier of helplessness which might otherwise afflict
viewers (Kollmuss and Agyeman, 2002). Nicholson-Cole
! Realism, in the sense of photo-realistic or ‘lifelike’ (2005) stresses the importance of tailoring the visualisation
imagery in re-creating experiential qualities (Appleyard, material to the target audience in light of the variability in
1977) and making abstract concepts ‘concrete’’ (McKen- human responses: this can be interpreted as identifying the
zie-Mohr and Smith, 1999). most applicable stage on the response spectrum for the target
! Depicting personally relevant environments, such as local audience in selecting the appropriate visualisation stimulus.
and recognisable neighbourhoods (Nicholson-Cole, A more systematic discussion of approaches to the content
2005), or iconic, well-known landscape symbols to which of visualisations addressing aspects of climate change is
people can relate (Sheppard, 2004). It is well known in the provided in Section 3.2.
S.R.J. Sheppard / Environmental Science & Policy 8 (2005) 637–654 647

Fig. 10. Theoretical effects of different types of landscape visualisations in stimulating perceptions and behaviour in response to climate change.

In summary, therefore, there is strong evidence of the 3. Ethical and policy issues of using visualisation to
cognitive effectiveness of visualising global change, and a influence perceptions and behaviour on climate
strong likelihood of the ability to engender emotional change
responses. It also appears theoretically possible that the use
of landscape visualisation could affect behaviour with regard In this section we turn our attention to the ethical
to climate change, if the imagery provides: (1) disclosure: a questions: ‘Should we use visualisations for the purpose of
window into the future which is personally meaningful and persuasion, and if so, how?’ Is there a ‘right’ way to do this?
tangible, making the global both local and personal, showing
possible negative and positive outcomes; (2) drama: a vivid 3.1. Risks and dilemmas in using visualisation of
and compelling presentation with emotional content. How- climate change
ever, the behavioural part of the theoretical response spectrum
(Fig. 10) is mostly in doubt and in need of testing. Developing Attempts deliberately to harness the power of visualisations
theories on the potential of visualisations to influence attitudes to influence behaviour, what Luymes (2001) describes as the
and behaviour must also recognise that their effectiveness rhetoric of visual simulation, have radical policy implications
would be dependent on many factors including the intended in terms of visualisation use and the role of the presenter. The
purpose of the exercise (i.e. nature of response sought), socio- conventional role of visualisation as an informative tool in
cultural and environmental context, the type of audience, and decision support is associated with the supposed neutral role of
the contribution of other forms of information. science in not imposing value judgements on the public.
648 S.R.J. Sheppard / Environmental Science & Policy 8 (2005) 637–654

Arguments for a more deliberate attempt to use misconceptions. There is the risk of aesthetic responses
visualisation to influence the public or impact government to visual conditions over-riding more important but non-
policy emphasize the need to forestall an actual crisis in the visible implications of climate change. Both Luymes
environment as an over-riding imperative. A persuasive (2001) and Orland and Uusitalo (2001) express concerns
approach should not necessarily be equated with inaccurate about the high credibility and low transparency of
or distorted visualisations: visualisations properly prepared realistic immersive forms of virtual reality, which foster
can simply disclose the truth, which may itself be sometimes false assumptions of authority, and which
persuasive. In fact, it could be argued that visualisations encourage suspension of disbelief in the virtual worlds
which disclose possible futures are less misleading than not depicted. These problems can be increased when
using them, and there may be an obligation to disclose the deliberate efforts are made to engage the emotions
truth to those who cannot or will not see. Luymes (2001) has (Slovic et al., 2002).
advocated the use of powerful visualisation tools to shape 2. The risk of disbelief: Lack of credibility (Sheppard, 1989)
public values on sustainability. History is replete with of the visualisation imagery or the underlying modelling/
examples of the inability of cultures to foresee the impact of assumptions would seriously damage the effectiveness of
their actions or inaction: e.g. the use of DDT, fluorocarbons, the visualisation exercise. Sheppard and Meitner (2005)
and indeed carbon emissions. Precedents do exist for other and Schroth et al. (2005) record participants’ concerns
kinds of long-term predictions and disclosure of con- about apparent or potential manipulation of visualisation
sequences, including environmental impact assessment, imagery to support a particular message. Realistic
multiple-rotation forest modelling, and biodiversity model- visualisations of possible climate change effects
ling in response to climate change (Berry et al., 2002). There (Fig. 11) which are presented without corresponding
is also an argument that scientifically produced visualisa- scientific explanations may not be taken seriously. Even
tions are needed to counter the misinformation propagated with such information, the necessary disclosure of
by entertainment media such as the recent film ‘‘The Day massive uncertainty (Webster, 2003) in climate change
After Tomorrow,’’ while utilizing a similar medium. More and land use projections could itself trigger lack of
generally, Michaelis (2003) has argued for a government confidence in the scientists and their models.
strategy to change public behaviour on greenhouse gas 3. The risk of confusion: The enormous complexity of
emissions through a process of leadership, dialogue, and climate change and associated policy options (Keeney
facilitation: there would appear to be strong role for and McDaniels, 2001) could simply confuse people over
visualisation in helping project ‘‘visions for a sustainable the number of visualised scenarios, contingencies,
way of living’’ (Michaelis, 2003, p. S143). associated risks, and consequent choices.
This author concludes that the persuasive use of 4. The risk of overkill: Even visual information can become
visualisations, together with other tools and approaches, too much, either through information overload (Orford
is justified if they can be effective, and may even be vital in et al., 1999) or sheer habituation to what becomes
communicating climate change urgently. We should take the mundane (McKenzie-Mohr and Smith, 1999). There may
perceptual leap and seek to engage the public’s emotions.
This seems worth doing even if behaviour does not change
immediately, by engaging people to think more seriously
about the future and showing them they have a choice among
alternative futures. This at least would remove the first
barrier of ignorance on the response spectrum of carbon
consciousness and action (Fig. 9).
Once the decision is taken that a policy of persuasive use
of powerful visual imagery can be justified in the fight
against climate change, a second tier of issues becomes
evident. What risks do we take in such usage, how can they
be minimized, and are the resulting risks worth taking?
There are many process decisions and constraints in the
production of any realistic simulated landscape imagery, and
many different problems can result (Sheppard, 1989). There
is space here only for a brief discussion of some of these, as
follows:

1. The risk of biased responses: How can unintentional bias


Fig. 11. Examples of photo-simulations of hypothetical sea-level rise in
or deliberate attempts to mislead be prevented? Holland in 2020, used in public communications. Credit: Stone/Getty
(McQuillan, 1998; Orland et al., 2001). Winn (1997) Images; courtesy of The Guardian ‘‘The Drowned World’’ September
points out that virtual reality media may create their own 11, 2004; and Glen Taylor.
S.R.J. Sheppard / Environmental Science & Policy 8 (2005) 637–654 649

be different desired levels of stimulus and response Sheppard (2001, 2005) has proposed a code of ethics for
intensity for different purposes, e.g. multiple iterative visualisation which identifies the following principles or
exposures to promote better learning versus single sharp criteria that may be relevant to climate change applications:
exposures for shock value. Winn (1997) has raised the accuracy of visualisation relative to expected conditions;
fear of the virtual reality medium itself being ‘over- representativeness of views in space and time; visual clarity
hyped’ and losing effectiveness. of presentations; interest and engagement of the audience/
5. The risk of upsetting people: There are differing views on users; legitimacy or accountability of the visualisation,
the desirability and effectiveness of emotional content including transparency of data and of the production
(such as fear or unease), leading to negative or defensive process, and delivery of visualisations by a trusted source
reactions to the issue (Nicholson-Cole, 2005), versus (Sheppard and Meitner, 2005; Nicholson-Cole, 2005); and
judicious use to trigger behavioural change (McKenzie- accessibility of the visualisations to the public and potential
Mohr and Smith, 1999). Furness et al. (1998) note that users.
students who become personally engaged in serious Other ethical and policy issues of particular concern to
consideration of global change may become very climate change are likely to include:
distressed, raising conflicts between the goal of
‘truthfulness’ and the desire not to upset people. ! Representing uncertainty, e.g. through presentation
6. The risk of perpetuating the problem by encouraging devices (Dockerty et al., 2005) or through multiple
acceptance of climate change: What if the visualisation alternative visualisations per given scenario as a form of
imagery is valid but simply too subtle to influence sensitivity testing (Appleyard, 1977; Sheppard, 2001,
attitudes, or depicts positive imagery that may result from 2004).
some aspect of a given climate change scenario? ! Down-scaling from global, regional, or even local climate
Dockerty et al. (2005) have generated landscape change scenarios: we need robust ways of developing
visualisations for Norfolk (Fig. 7) showing what some decision rules for visualising the detailed landscape
may consider to be attractive fields of sunflowers which conditions necessary for personal engagement and local
would replace the existing agricultural crops. recognition, but based on very broad land use or
environmental scenarios with limited resolution data-sets
3.2. Ethical standards and policy implications (e.g. UKCIP, 2000b; Dockerty et al., 2005).
! Presentation of non-visible conditions (in the visualisa-
Given the issues just raised, how should we embark upon tion and with other media) to mediate and augment the
a policy of deliberate use of visualisations to influence visible aspects of climate change, through a blend
attitudes and behaviour on climate change? The following (Fig. 12) of realism (in this case, individual trees on
sections briefly explore three topics key to the ethical the hillside) and abstraction (e.g. false colour).
implementation of visualisations of climate change. ! Permissible types of drama, including dramatic content
such as forest die-back or the effects of extreme storms,
3.2.1. Defensible methodologies for preparing dramatic viewing conditions such as animated ‘fly-bys’
visualisations and unusual lighting, or dramatic display formats such as
An obvious starting point would be to develop guidelines stereo vision, big screens, and panoramic angles of view.
for uses of visualisation related to climate change,
appropriate to the needs of public motivation and One defensible policy on permissible drama would
decision-making. This would address the principal missing perhaps be that a defined ‘reality’ remains the guide: for
ingredient beyond disclosure and drama for effective example, animated large-screen panoramic imagery of a
visualisation of climate change: defensibility. If there is future landscape may be both dramatic and legitimate if it
no overall scientific or logical underpinning of the conveys the expected appearance at the actual image size
visualisations, they are unlikely to change peoples’ minds and angle of view with which it would be seen in the
or convince policy-makers suspicious of new media. neighbourhood, but not if it exaggerated the effects of
However, there is little guidance on how much climate change, gave a magnified view of the landscape
manipulation or selectivity should be allowed to create features affected, or selectively left out key elements.
the appropriate kinds of drama while avoiding the pitfalls However, the type of decision or response required does
described in Section 3.1. What accuracy or response validity need to be taken into account, and cognitive awareness, as
standards can be applied when visualisations represent suggested in Fig. 10, may require the very types of omission
predictions of unknowable futures? Should accuracy be or abstraction that could be misleading with the experiential
measured against likelihood or feasibility of occurrence, or response. Ensuring that multiple media are used, retaining
as veracity to the modelled outcomes? To what degree would the cognitive information along with the realistic experi-
existing conventions developed for typical uses of landscape ential media (e.g. 3D landscape models augmented with
visualisation in design and planning apply to the special draped polygons and labels) may reduce the risk, together
circumstances of climate change mitigation and adaptation? with disclosing any manipulation of suppression of data. The
650 S.R.J. Sheppard / Environmental Science & Policy 8 (2005) 637–654

Fig. 12. Non-visible conditions (in this case tree species changing over time on a BC mountainside) can be displayed with colour-coding in detailed landscape
visualisations to express important environmental changes due to policy implementation or climate change, even though the actual landscape appearance might
not change significantly. Credit: Jon Salter, CALP, UBC.

aims of the exercise (e.g. fostering sustainable behaviour) emissions. Mechanisms for developing and analyzing
should also be made explicit at the outset. Allowing the additional visualisation scenarios which can be derived
sceptical user to navigate and interrogate the visualisation independently from the global scenarios might therefore be
imagery and underlying databases, and to choose their own strategically advisable.
viewing conditions or view sequence, may help in assuring There may be other reasons to develop independent
defensibility (Furness et al., 1998; Sheppard and Salter, scenarios for visualisation purposes. These could address
2004). Another strategy for building defensibility would be specific local environmental or cultural issues which would
to ensure effective stakeholder participation in the devel- be more meaningful to local planners and communities, and
opment of socio-economic scenarios, the application of require less work to link to much larger multi-dimensional
decision-rules for visualising the scenarios, and even the global or regional scenarios and modelling systems. The
process of generating the visualisations (Sheppard and challenge would lie in still having these visual scenarios be
Salter, 2004). credible. One approach would involve local stakeholders in
the scenario generation process, though this may become
3.2.2. The content of visualisations of climate change constrained by the participants’ inability or unwillingness to
scenarios consider the more radical alternatives posed by climate
The selection of appropriate global, regional, and local change (Berry, 2005). Another possibility is to assume
scenarios to visualise would be a key strategic decision. While certain likely conditions, based on prior scientific analysis,
the IPCC (Nakicenovic and Swart, 2000) and various levels of without detailed linkages of visualisation to complex
national and regional governments have developed generic modelling systems. For example, there is a strong likelihood
alternative scenarios for future climate change and adaptation in the dry Okanagan basin of BC that increasing drought
(e.g. UKCIP, 2000b), systematic mechanisms for down- conditions will continue or get worse under regional climate
scaling these to local landscapes are needed. This usually predictions (Cohen et al., 2004), leading to more widespread
means accepting the limited range and assumptions inherent forest die-back; even without specific modelling, visualisa-
in the internationally or nationally selected generic scenarios, tion that shows the possibility of 80% die-off of forest trees
although none of the seminal IPCC scenarios currently at some point in the lifetime of current middle-aged
contemplates substantial short-term reductions in carbon residents of the area, generated with the advice of recognised
emissions to reduce future climate change risks (UKCIP, experts and with appropriate caveats on the general
2002). Visualisation of these scenarios alone might be uncertainties, may not be too speculative. In such
mistaken by the public as expressing the full range of possible circumstances, there is a limited set of visual possibilities:
solutions, rather than what some may see as a limited and more of the trees turn brown or all the trees turn brown. It
politically influenced subset. Such scenarios do not articulate may be sufficient to say: ‘we are not sure which of these
estimated long-term benefits of a radical short-term approach futures will occur, but you should be prepared for all of
to climate change mitigation, relative to a slower response them,’ in order to stimulate adaptation behaviour. Much
which effectively endorses additional discretionary carbon more thought needs to be given to this issue, and where to
S.R.J. Sheppard / Environmental Science & Policy 8 (2005) 637–654 651

Table 2
An initial typology of selected climate change impacts in terms of their potential to be readily and realistically visualised
Climate change impact type Capability to be visualised realisticallya
Often readily apparent Visually subtle or invisible
or imageable to lay-viewer (requiring augmented realism)
at landscape level at landscape level
Sea-level rise (coastline) H
Permanent flooding H
Seasonal flooding H
Changes in seasonal patterns/timing H
Increased storm severity H
Ice retreat H
Snow depth reduction H
Soil erosion from concentrated precipitation H H
Stream character change (drying up, loss of riparian vegetation, etc.) H
Lake level drop H
Drought induced vegetation stress H
Drought induced vegetation die-back H
Vegetation succession/invasion from biome shift H H
Desiccation of forest floor H
Increased forest disease epidemics H
Increased vegetation fire risk H
Fire events H
Loss of rare plant species H
Loss of wildlife species (through extreme climatic events or long-term shifts) H
Crop failures H
Farm abandonment H
Cultural event disruption (e.g. seasonally disrupted traditional ceremonies) H
Urban landscaping stress H
Urban brown-outs H
a
Some climate change impacts vary widely in their visibility, and are noted in both columns. Some impacts may or may not be realistically imageable
depending on whether time-lapse visualisations are used.

draw the line between defensible (though not necessarily ! the root causes of climate change and sources of carbon
quantifiable) depictions of possible futures, and less entering the ecosystem cycle, e.g. oil wells and coal
scientific, alarmist scenarios. mines;
A range of climate change impacts would lend ! mechanisms of GHG emission into the atmosphere, e.g.
themselves to disclosure in visualisations of a future local powerplants, vehicles, and houses;
landscape, depending on the geographic conditions of the ! indirect impact through adaptation, e.g. raised dykes for
area. Table 2 provides an initial typology of climate change flood abatement, changed agricultural land-use, or
effects, with an initial categorisation of how readily they logging to reduce fire-risk;
could be expressed in realistic landscape visualisations: an ! mitigation measures to reduce climate change and CO2
initial indication of their potential for visual drama or emissions, e.g. proposed low energy transportation
meaningful communication in that medium. One use of systems, dispersed renewable energy sources, houses
such a classification would be to identify those phenomena with energy-generating technology, etc. (Fig. 5).
which are not well suited to portrayal by landscape
visualisations and where other media need to be used. 3.3. Research priorities
Issues in attempting to represent future climate-influenced
conditions in visualisations include conditions or events Research programmes to support such usage of visualisa-
which have occurred before but which now occur with tion would need to address:
different frequency or timing, and the importance of
threshold conditions (e.g. tree death and dam-failure) that 1. Empirical testing of visualisation to support or adapt
are more visibly demonstrated than more subtle and gradual the theoretical framework of effects on the response
changes. spectrum, and especially, effects on behaviour. This
In the context of motivating behavioural change at the should combine more psychological research on
policy level, the potential content of visualisation imagery climate change perception and behaviour (e.g. Lor-
need not be limited to the direct impacts of climate change enzoni and Langford, 2001) with classical environ-
on the local environment and community (as shown in mental perception research methods using controlled
Table 2 and Fig. 8). It might also be effective cognitively and visual landscape imagery as stimuli (e.g. Daniel and
affectively to visualise: Meitner, 2001).
652 S.R.J. Sheppard / Environmental Science & Policy 8 (2005) 637–654

2. Exploration of ethical issues through policy debate and ethical concerns including unrecognised bias in using such
initial case studies developed especially to illuminate tools. Despite these issues, the rhetoric of visualisation
these difficult questions. (Luymes, 2001), applied deliberately and defensibly as a
3. Scientific monitoring of real-world projects or processes persuasive force, may be vital in helping to accelerate social
of long-term planning, resource modelling, and climate learning, engage the emotions, influence individual beha-
change adaptation, where these address alternative viour, and project the rationale for policy change. The
futures as mediated by landscape visualisation-based hypothetical mechanism is realistically and dramatically to
tools or related visual imagery. This would track actual bring home the impacts of future climate change scenarios to
consequences of the use of visualisations on learning and people in their back yard or in other meaningful and iconic
behaviour. landscapes: ‘making climate change personal.’
A theoretical framework is needed for examining
Such a combined programme would help to identify some responses to visualisations of climate change, addressing
of the underlying triggers for attitudinal or behavioural their ability to influence people’s hearts, minds, and
change on climate change policy and life-styles, as well as behaviour. One possible framework has been outlined in
risks and benefits of particular visualisation approaches. It this paper. However, even if the theorised effects of
would also be instructive to evaluate the effect of visualisation on behaviour are not established and the
visualisations relative to other policy and communication framework presented here is not borne out, there may be
tools. advantages in using visualisation to stimulate awareness and
Visualisation research could be built into ongoing studies deliberative discourse. We should test carefully every
focused on people’s reactions to possible climate change potentially powerful weapon in the fight against climate
strategies and mitigation approaches, e.g. alternative change, especially those which promise rapid results.
technologies for house construction and energy sources Visualisation tools are potentially too powerful either to
(Fawcett et al., 2002). With an appropriate research design, be ignored or used without careful consideration. The hope
landscape visualisations (e.g. Fig. 5) being used as is that the dilemmas of whether and how to use visualisations
surrogates for real world conditions in determining proactively as agents of learning and behavioural change,
preferences or acceptability of possible climate change can be resolved through development of ethical standards
strategies could provide additional research results on the incorporating the ‘3 Ds’ of visualising climate change:
performance/effects of alternative visualisation methods. disclosure, drama, and defensibility.
Research programmes which are using landscape visualisa-
tion stimuli for perception research on planning issues could
also be retuned to address climate change issues. Acknowledgments

I would like to acknowledge the initial inspiration for this


4. Conclusions paper and the subsequent review from Dr. David Little of
Arthur D. Little Limited. Dr. Russ Parsons also provided
There is an alarming gap between awareness and action on helpful comments on early drafts. I would like to thank
climate change. Fawcett et al. (2002) admit that ‘‘Appropriate Green College and the Environmental Change Institute at
policy on energy use and carbon emissions would only have a Oxford University, and members of The Zuckerman Institute
small part to play in the social revolution which would be for Connective Environmental Research (ZICER) at Uni-
needed to achieve voluntary acceptance of reduced con- versity of East Anglia, for providing venues and/or vital
sumption levels’’ (p. 80). Emerging techniques of landscape information contributing to the development of this paper.
visualisation promise considerable improvements over other Thanks also to Cecilia Achiam for her graphics and
media in communicating some climate change issues, guidance, and to the many CALP members who have
fostering social learning, and possibly in influencing popular supported me. The preparation of the manuscript was
perceptions and behaviour on climate change. There is supported in part by sabbatical funding from the Sustainable
considerable evidence of the effectiveness of visualisation as a Forest Management Network, Canada.
planning tool and its ability to enhance cognition, but less
research has taken place on responses to realistic landscape
visualisations, especially their behavioural impacts. However, References
experience in practice suggests that landscape visualisations
can sometimes have substantial effects on policy, and there is Adger, W.N., 2003. Social capital, collective action, and adaptation to
evidence with other visual media of behavioural effects climate change. Econ. Geogr. 79 (4), 387–404.
through engaging emotional responses. Al-Kodmany, K., 2000. GIS in the urban landscape: reconfiguring neigh-
bourhood planning and design processes. Landsc. Res. 25 (1), 5–28.
New immersive and interactive systems offer attractive Appleton, K., Lovett, A., Sunnenberg, G., Dockerty, T., 2002. Rural land-
possibilities for engagement, awareness building, and scape visualisation from GIS databases: a comparison of approaches,
reaching the emotions of the viewer; however, there are options, and problems. Comput. Environ. Urban Syst. 26, 201–211.
S.R.J. Sheppard / Environmental Science & Policy 8 (2005) 637–654 653

Appleton, K., Lovett, A., 2003. GIS-based visualisation of rural landscapes: IPCC (Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change), 2001. Climate Change
defining ‘sufficient’ realism for environmental decision-making. 2001: Impacts, Adaptation and Vulnerability. Summary for Policy-
Landsc. Urban Plann. 65, 117–131. makers. World Meteorological Organisation, Geneva.
Appleyard, D., 1977. Understanding professional media: issues, theory, and a Kaplan, R., Herbert, E.J., 1988. Familiarity and preference: a cross-cultural
research agenda. In: Altman, I., Wohlwill, J.F. (Eds.), Human Behavior analysis. In: Nasar, J.L. (Ed.), Environmental Aesthetics: Theory,
and Environment, vol. 1. Plenum Press, New York, NY, pp. 43–88. Research, and Applications. Cambridge University Press, New York,
Bengtsson, P., Johansson, C.R., Akselsson, K.R., 1997. Planning working NY, pp. 379–389.
environment and production by using paper drawings and computer Keeney, R.L., McDaniels, T.L., 2001. A framework to guide thinking about
animation. Ergonomics 40, 334–347. climate change policies. Risk Anal. 21 (6), 989–1000.
Berry, P.M., 2005. Personal communication. Kollmuss, A., Agyeman, J., 2002. Mind the gap: why do people act
Berry, P.M., Dawson, T.P., Harrison, P.A., Pearson, R.G., 2002. Modelling environmentally and what are the barriers to pro-environment
potential impacts of climate change on the bioclimatic envelope of behaviour? Environ. Educ. Res. 8 (3), 239–260.
species in Britain and Ireland. Global Ecol. Biogeogr. 11 (6), 453–462. Lange, E., 2001. The limits of realism: perceptions of virtual landscapes.
Bishop, I.D., Rohrmann, B., 2003. Subjective responses to simulated and Landsc. Urban Plann. 54, 163–182.
real environments: a comparison. Landsc. Urban Plann. 65, 261–277. Lewis, J.L., 2000. Ancient values, new technology: emerging methods for
Bishop, I.D., Ye, W.S., Karadaglis, C., 2001. Experiential approaches to integrating cultural values in forest management. Unpublished M.Sc.
perception response in virtual worlds. Landsc. Urban Plann. 54, 115– Thesis. Faculty of Forestry, UBC, Vancouver, BC.
123. Lewis, J.L., Sheppard, S.R.J. Culture and communication: can landscape
Boardman, B., Palmer, J., 2003. 4CE—Consumer Choice and Carbon visualisation improve forest management consultation with indigenous
Consciousness: Electricity Disclosure in Europe. 4CE Final Report. communities? Landsc. Urban Plann., in press.
Environmental Change Institute, Oxford, UK, 81 pp. Lorenzoni, I., Langford, I.H., 2001. Climate change now and in the future: a
Bosselmann, P., 1998. Representation of Places: Reality and Realism in City mixed methodological study of public perceptions in Norwich (UK).
Design. University of California Press, Berkeley, CA. CSERGE Working Paper ECM 01-05. CSERGE, Norwich, UK.
Bosselmann, P., Craik, K.H., 1987. Perceptual simulations of environments. Lowe, T., Brown, K., Dessai, S., de Franca Doria, M., Haynes, K., Vincent,
In: Bechtel, R.B., Marcus, R.W., Michelson, W. (Eds.), Methods in K., March 2005. Does tomorrow ever come? Disaster narrative and public
Environmental and Behaviour Research. Van Nostrand Reinhold, New perceptions of climate change. Tyndall Centre for Climate Change
York, NY (Chapter 5). Research Working Paper 72. University of East Anglia, Norwich.
Campbell, D.C., Salter, J., 2004. The Digital Workshop: Exploring the Luymes, D., 2001. The Rhetoric of Visual Simulation in Forest Design:
Effectiveness of Interactive Visualisations and Real-Time Data Analysis Some Research Directions. International Union of Forest Research
in Enhancing Participation in Planning Processes. Report Submitted to Organizations (IUFRO) Research Series. CABI International, Wall-
Forestry Innovation Investment Forestry Research Programme, Deliver- ingford, UK, pp. 191–204 (Chapter 13).
able #5. Collaborative for Advanced Landscape Planning, UBC, Van- Lynas, M., 2004. High Tide: News from a Warming World. Flamingo,
couver, BC, 32 pp. Oxford, UK.
Cohen, S.J., 1997. Scientist–stakeholder collaboration in integrated assess- MacEachren, A.M., 2001. Cartography and GIS: extending collaborative
ment of climate change: lessons from a case study of Northwest Canada. tools to support virtual teams. Prog. Hum. Geogr. 25 (3), 431–444.
Environ. Model. Assess. 2, 281–293. MacEachren, A.M., Ganter, J.H., 1990. A pattern identification approach to
Cohen, S.J., Nielsen, D., Welbourn, R. (Eds.), 2004. Expanding the Dia- cartographic visualization. Cartographica 27 (2), 64–81.
logue on Climate Change and Water Management in the Okanagan Maiteny, P.T., 2002. Mind in the gap: summary of research exploring ‘inner’
Basin, BC. Final Report, Environment Canada, Agriculture and Agri- influences on pro-sustainability learning and behaviour. Environ. Educ.
Food Canada and University of British Columbia. Project A463/433. Res. 8 (3), 299–306.
Submitted to the Adaptation Liaison Office, Climate Change Impacts McKenzie-Mohr, D., Smith, W., 1999. Fostering Sustainable Behavior: An
and Adaptation Directorate, Natural Resources Canada, Ottawa, 300 pp. Introduction to Community-Based Social Marketing. New Society
Cox, D.J., 1990. The art of scientific visualization. Acad. Comput. 46, 20– Publishers, Gabriola Island, Canada.
56. McQuillan, A.G., 1998. Honesty and foresight in computer visualizations. J.
Danahy, J.W., 2001. Technology for dynamic viewing and peripheral vision Forestry 96 (6), 15–16.
in landscape visualization. Landsc. Urban Plann. 54, 125–137. Meitner, M., Daniel, T., 1997. The effects of animation and interactivity on
Daniel, T.C., Meitner, M.J., 2001. Representational validity of landscape the validity of human responses to forest data visualizations. In: Orland,
visualizations: the effect of graphical realism on perceived scenic beauty B. (Ed.), Proceedings of Data Visualization’97, St. Louis, MO.
of forest vistas. J. Environ. Psychol. 21 (1), 61–72. Michaelis, L., 2003. Sustainable consumption and greenhouse gas mitiga-
Dockerty, T., Lovett, A., Sunnenberg, G., Appleton, K., Parry, M., 2005. tion. Climate Policy 3S1, S135–S146.
Visualising the potential impacts of climate change on rural landscapes. Monbiot, G., 2004. Beware the fossil fools. In: The Guardian, Tuesday,
Comput. Environ. Urban Syst. 29 (3), 297–320. April 27, accessed June 8, http://www.guardian.co.uk/comment/story/
Dykes, J., 2000. An approach to virtual environments for visualization using 0,3604,1204194,00.html.
linked geo-referenced panoramic imagery. Comput. Environ. Urban Nakicenovic, N., Swart, R. (Eds.), 2000. Emissions Scenarios 2000: Special
Syst. 24, 127–152. Report of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change. Cambridge
Ervin, S.M., Hasbrouck, H.H., 2001. Landscape Modeling: Digital Tech- University Press, Cambridge, UK.
niques for Landscape Visualization. McGraw-Hill, New York, NY. Nicholson-Cole, S.A., 2005. Representing climate change futures: a critique
Fawcett, T., Hurst, A., Boardman, B., 2002. Carbon UK. Industrial Sustain- on the use of images for visual communication. Comput. Environ. Urban
able Development Group, Environmental Change Institute, University Syst. 29 (3), 255–273.
of Oxford, Oxford, UK, 65 pp. Orford, S., Harris, R., Dorling, D., 1999. Geography: information visualisa-
Furness III, T.A., Winn, W., Yu, R., 1998. The impact of three dimensional tion in the social sciences. Soc. Sci. Comput. Rev. 17 (3), 289–304.
immersive virtual environments on modern pedagogy: global change, Orland, B., 1992. Evaluating regional changes on the basis of local
VR and learning. In: Proceedings of Workshops in Seattle, Washington, expectations: a visualisation dilemma. Landsc. Urban Plann. 21,
and Loughborough, England in May and June 1997. Human Interface 257–259.
Technology Lab, University of Washington, Seattle, WA available Orland, B., Budthimedhee, K., Uusitalo, J., 2001. Considering virtual
online at http://www.hitl.washington.edu/publications/r-97-32/, rtf file worlds as representations of landscape realities and as tools for land-
accessed June 1, 2004. scape planning. Landsc. Urban Plann. 54 (1–4), 139–148.
654 S.R.J. Sheppard / Environmental Science & Policy 8 (2005) 637–654

Orland, B., Uusitalo, J., 2001. Immersion in a virtual forest: some implica- Steinitz, C., Rojo, H.M.A., Bassett, S., Flaxman, M., Goode, T., Maddock
tions. In: Sheppard, S.R.J., Harshaw, H.W. (Eds.), Forests and Land- III, T., Mouat, D., Peiser, R., Shearer, A., 2003. Alternative Futures for
scapes: Linking Sustainability, Ecology, and Aesthetics. International Changing Landscapes: The Upper San Pedro River Basin in Arizona and
Union of Forest Research Organizations (IUFRO) Research Series.- Sonora. Island Press, Washington, DC.
CABI International, Wallingford, UK, (Chapter 14), pp. 205–224. Stern, P.C., 2000. Toward a coherent theory of environmentally significant
Pearce, D.W., Cline, W.R., Achanta, A.N., Frankenhauser, S., Pachauri, behaviour. J. Soc. Issues 56 (3), 407–424.
R.K., Tol, R.S., Vellinga, P., 1996. The social costs of climate change: Tickell, C., 2002. Communicating climate change. Science 297, 737
greenhouse damage and the benefits of control. In: Bruce, J.P., Lee, H., (Editorial).
Haites, E.F. (Eds.), Climate Change 1995: Economic and Social Tress, B., Tress, G., 2002. Scenario visualisation for participatory landscape
Dimensions of Climate Change. Cambridge University Press, Cam- planning: a study from Denmark. Landsc. Urban Plann. 982, 1–18.
bridge, UK. Tufte, E.R., 1990. Envisioning Information. Graphics Press, Cheshire, CT.
Porter, T., 1979. How Architects Visualize. Van Nostrand Reinhold, New UKCIP (Climate Impacts Programme), Mackenzie Hedger, M., Gawith,
York, NY. M., Brown, I., Connell, R., Downing, T.E. (Eds.), 2000a. Climate
Ribe, R., 2005. Aesthetic perceptions of green-tree retention harvests in Change: Assessing the Impacts—Identifying Responses: The First 3
vista views: The interaction of cut level, retention pattern and harvest Years of the UK Climate Impacts Programme. UKCIP, Oxford, UK, 123
shape. Landsc. Urban Plann. 73 (4), 277–293. pp.
Salter, J.D., 2005. Designing and testing a prototypical landscape informa- UKCIP (Climate Impacts Programme), 2000b. Socio-Economic Scenarios
tion interface for lay-people. Unpublished Masters Thesis. Faculty of for Climate Change Impact Assessment: A Guide to their Use in the UK
Forestry, University of British Columbia, Vancouver. Climate Impacts Programme. UKCIP, Oxford, UK, 123 pp.
Schroth, O., Lange, E., Schmid, W.A., 2005. From information to participa- UKCIP (Climate Impacts Programme), 2002. Climate Change Scenarios for
tion: applying interactive features in landscape visualizations. Paper the United Kingdom: The UKCIP02 Scientific Report. UKCIP, Oxford,
presented at Our Shared Landscape Conference, Ascona, Switzerland, UK, 112 pp.
May 2–6. Webster, M., 2003. Communicating climate change uncertainty to policy-
Sheppard, S.R.J., 1989. Visual Simulation: A User’s Guide for Architects, makers and the public: and editorial comment. Climatic Change 61, 1–8.
Engineers, and Planners. Van Nostrand Reinhold, New York. Winn, W., 1997. The Impact of Three-Dimensional Immersive Virtual
Sheppard, S.R.J., 2001. Guidance for crystal ball gazers: developing a code Environments on Modern Pedagogy. HITL Technical Report R-97-
of ethics for landscape visualization. Landsc. Urban Plann. 54 (1–4), 15. Human Interface Technology Laboratory, University of Washington,
183–199. Seattle, WA.
Sheppard, S.R.J., 2004. Making Unsustainability Obvious to the Public. Zajonc, R.B., 1984. On the primacy of affect. Am. Psychol. 39 (2), 117–123.
Green College Environmental Seminar Presentation, Oxford, 4 Febru- Zube, E.H., Sell, J.L., Taylor, J.G., 1982. Landscape perception: research,
ary. application, and theory. Landsc. Plann. 9, 1–33.
Sheppard, S.R.J., 2005. Validity, reliability, and ethics in visualization. In:
Bishop, I., Lange, E. (Eds.), Visualization in Landscape and Environ- Stephen Sheppard teaches in sustainable landscape planning, aesthetics,
mental Planning. Taylor and Francis, London, (Chapter 5), pp. 79–97. and visualisation in the Faculty of Forestry and Landscape Architecture
Sheppard, S.R.J., Cavens, D., Salter, J., Meitner, M., 2001. Seeing the big programme at the University of British Columbia. He received a BA/MA in
picture: the benefits of digital landscape immersion. In: Proceedings of agricultural and forest sciences at Oxford, an MSc in forestry at UBC, and a
the LANDTECH ASLA/CSLA Annual Meeting, 24 September 2001, PhD in environmental planning at UC, Berkeley. He directs the Collabora-
Montreal, Canada. (CD-ROM). tive for Advanced Landscape Planning (CALP), an interdisciplinary
Sheppard, S.R.J., Meitner, M.J., 2005. Using multi-criteria analysis and research group using perception-testing and immersive/interactive visuali-
visualisation for sustainable forest management planning with stake- sation to support public awareness and collaborative planning on sustain-
holder groups. Forest Ecol. Manage. 207 (1–2), 171–187. ability issues. He has over 25 years’ experience in environmental
Sheppard, S.R.J., Salter, J., 2004. The role of visualization in forest assessment and public participation internationally. He has written or co-
planning. In: Encyclopedia of Forest Sciences, Academic Press/Else- written two books on visual simulation, and co-edited ‘‘Forests and Land-
vier, Oxford, UK, pp. 486–498. scapes: Linking Ecology, Sustainability, and Aesthetics,’’ vol. 6, in the
Slovic, P., Finucane, M., Peters, E., MacGregor, D.G., 2002. The affect IUFRO Research Series. He is currently contributing to the BC chapter of
heuristic. In: Gilovich, T., Griffin, D., Kahneman, D. (Eds.), Heuristics Canada’s National Assessment of climate change impacts and adaptation.
and Biases: The Psychology of Intuitive Judgement. Cambridge Uni- Current research interests lie in perceptions of climate change, the
versity Press, New York, NY, (Chapter 23), pp. 397–420. aesthetics of sustainability, and visualisation theory and ethics.

Вам также может понравиться