Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 7

Cocaine Addiction: Psychology and

Neurophysiology
FRANK H. GAWIN
presumed to pose less severe treatment problems. The perception
Cocaine was considered incapable of producing depen- that cocaine produced purely a psychological problem wrongly
dence in 1980 but was recently proclaimed the drug of implied that it was appropriate to limit treatment to psychological
greatest national health concern. Recent clinical and pre- therapies and suggested there was no role for neurophysiological
clinical investigations demonstrate that cocaine produces interventions with pharmacological agents. These drawbacks of
unique abuse and withdrawal patterns that differ from prior "classic" tenets in the drug abuse field has recently led the
those of other major abused drugs and suggest that World Health Organization to discard these terms in favor of the
long-term cocaine abuse produces neurophysiological al- more physiologically precise term "neuroadaptation" (1) and has led
terations in specific systems in the central nervous system the American Psychiatric Association to define drug dependence by

Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on October 5, 2012


that regulate the capacity to experience pleasure. It will be using behavioral criteria, rather than only physiological criteria (2).
necessary to develop clinically pertinent research models The epidemic ofcocaine abuse may therefore provide one positive
before these findings can be considered definitive, but legacy-forced recognition ofthe complexity ofsubstance abuse and
these evolving ideas have already led to applications of new conceptualizations of and research on addiction. In this article,
promising experimental treatments for cocaine abuse. I review the recent concordance of clinical and preclinical findings
about long-term cocaine exposure that suggest cocaine causes a
neurophysiological addiction. Corresponding advances in the treat-
ment of cocaine abuse are noted, but these are discussed fully
A DDICTION TO COCAINE DOES NOT CAUSE GROSS PHYSIO- elsewhere (3, 4). Further, this article will describe the methodolog-
logical withdrawal symptoms. However, the addictive con- ical limitations of current preclinical and clinical research to illustrate
sequences of cocaine use have forced reconsideration of the where conceptual or technical methodological advances are now a
importance attributed to "classic" drug abuse constructs, such as prerequisite to systematic advancement of research, theory, and
physiological withdrawal, tolerance, and physical dependence, in treatment of the addictions.
drug and alcohol addiction. These constructs are based on easily
quantitated gross physiological alterations in parameters such as
heart rate or blood pressure and on easily observed symptoms of
physical illness. Their central position in drug abuse theory and The Clinical Characteristics of Cocaine
research, although questioned, has been unchanged for decades; Addiction
drug dependence has thus largely been considered an avoidance of
physical discomfort on withdrawal. We are in the largest cocaine abuse epidemic in history. In the
Pharmacological agents have been developed that reverse the United States alone, one to three million cocaine abusers are
gross physiological withdrawal symptoms produced by benzodiaz- estimated to be in need of treatment (5), or up to six times the
epines, alcohol, and opiates. However, these agents, fully assessed in number of heroin addicts. In the 1890s, cocaine use also surged and
recent clinical treatment trials, have not solved the problem of was considered safe by clinicians as illustrious as Sigmund Freud (6).
addiction to these drugs. The facts that cocaine produces no gross Use declined after its dangers became well known (7). This recurred
physiological withdrawal symptoms and that, in withdrawal from in the 1920s. In the early 1950s and late 1960s, abuse of the similar
other abused drugs, agents reversing such symptoms show limited stimulants, amphetamine and methamphetamine, resulted in the
efficacy, demonstrate that subjective experiences or symptoms other same pattern (8). In 1980, cocaine was again considered a safe,
than physiological discomfort are crucial in addiction to cocaine and nonaddicting euphoriant (9). Historical descriptions of cocaine
to other substances of abuse. dependence were dismissed as exaggerations similar to marijuana
Clinical and preclinical investigators are now exploring how reports from earlier eras. Clinical research on cocaine abuse that used
psychological symptoms in drug withdrawal, particularly unpleasant systematic and objective techniques did not exist, and this led to the
mood states and cravings for drug euphoria, maintain chronic drug miscomprehension that cocaine dependence did not exist. Cocaine
addiction. Such symptoms may be mediated by neurophysiological use then exploded. Almost one of two Americans between 25 and
adaptations that are attributable to repeated perturbations of the 30 years of age has tried cocaine. A powerful new route of
central nervous system (CNS) by abused drugs. In cases where these administration has appeared; cocaine smoked as the free base, also
adaptations occur in brain systems that regulate only processes called "crack," is as addictive as intravenous injection, without the
lacking physical expression, as appears to be the case with cocaine, stigma or infectious dangers of injection (10).
addiction may have been erroneously considered as purely "psycho- Individuals who are in treatment for cocaine abuse report that 2
logical" rather than "physiological" and had erroneously been to 4 years intervene between the initial exposure to cocaine and the
development of addiction (11); this interval has delayed reports of
The author is in the Department of Psychiatry, Stimulant Abuse Treatment and cocaine's adverse effects at the beginning of epidemics and, com-
Research, University of California, Los Angeles, CA 90024. bined with reports of apparently controlled initial use, promotes
1580 SCIENCE, VOL. 251
illusions of safety. Early in the course of cocaine epidemics, the use and abuse exist (3).
public, scientists, respected texts, and astute clinicians have all fallen As cocaine addiction develops, a transition to high-dose, long-
victim to this illusion, thus enabling the repeated emergence of duration bingeing occurs, in which the intensely pleasurable effects
cocaine abuse. are experienced alone, and increasingly apparent negative contin-
Psychological effects of cocaine: initial use. In both reports from gencies go unrecognized (3, 6, 10, 15). Users readminister cocaine
individuals addicted to cocaine and human laboratory investigations every 10 to 30 min. Such compulsive, uncontrolled binge use begins
(in which cocaine is administered to nonaddicted cocaine users), when availability and dosage escalate (for example, as a result of
cocaine initially induces profound subjective well-being together increased funds or improved supply sources) or when a switch to
with alertness (6, 7, 12, 13). The fundamental effect of cocaine is the rapid, higher intensity administration routes occurs (from intranasal
magnification of the intensity of almost all normal pleasures. The use to smoking or intravenous injection) (3, 15-17). During such
environment takes on intensified but nondistorted qualities. Emo- binges, there are numerous periods of extreme euphoria, and vivid
tions and sexual feelings are enhanced (6, 13). Self-confidence and memories are formed that are later contrasted with current dyspho-
self-perceptions of mastery increase, so anxiety is initially decreased. rias to produce cocaine craving (3, 16). Several days of abstinence
Social inhibitions are reduced, and interpersonal communication is often separate binges; users average one to seven binges per week,
facilitated. Satiation of appetite occurs, so pleasures associated with each lasting from 4 to 24 hours (3, 16). Unlike the case for alcohol
eating are not enhanced (11, 12). or opiate users, the absence of a daily use pattern in a cocaine user
Human laboratory studies of cocaine's effects are more reliable does not indicate decreased impairment, and it may indicate the
than retrospective self-reports by cocaine users, but they are serious- opposite (3).
ly limited as aids in understanding cocaine abuse because there are Continuous, rapid, cocaine self-administration also occurs in
multiple disparities between the methods used in such experiments animal experiments in which unlimited access to intravenous cocaine

Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on October 5, 2012


and the characteristics of actual street cocaine use. Maximum is provided (12). The animals die within 14 days. If access is limited,
individual laboratory doses are 10% of the maximum doses typically an animal will press a lever thousands of times for a single cocaine
reported by street abusers (12). The maximum duration of admin- dose (12). Human cocaine addicts report that virtually all thoughts
istration in human experiments is 5 hours (14), compared to street are focused on cocaine during binges; nourishment, sleep, money,
binges lasting as long as 200 hours. Cocaine abusers usually loved ones, responsibility, and survival lose all significance (3, 16).
coadminister other drugs (alcohol, heroin, and marijuana) to de- Supplies of cocaine are drawn on until they are exhausted. Limita-
crease unpleasant components of the cocaine experience, such as tions on drug access, including the high price of cocaine and legal
anxiety, that admix with euphoria. The effects ofcoadministration of limitations on distribution, regulate human cocaine use and may
these drugs, of their relative dosages, and of varying the time prevent human cocaine use from more frequently mimicking animal
patterns of their administration have not been studied. Further, free-access experiments in producing death. A low-intensity parallel
because administration of cocaine to a cocaine abuser in need of to early, controlled use in humans has not been described in animal
treatment has been considered unethical, the subjects in such studies self-administration studies, probably because most self-administra-
are not in need of treatment for cocaine abuse, have less severe tion experiments in animals utilize large boluses and intravenous
histories of cocaine abuse, and may often instead have substantial administration, bypassing an initial phase of low-intensity use and
histories of abuse of other substances. These facts raise the possibil- the binge transition (3).
ities that any neuroadaptation pertinent to cocaine dependence is
not present, or that neuroadaptation to other abused substances
might be present, or both. Finally, the effects of nonpharmacological Cocaine Abstinence Symptoms
factors that interact with the effects of cocaine have been largely
ignored. Variations in the environmental context of cocaine admin- A triphasic pattern of symptoms characterizes the response to
istration, such as familiarity with the administration environment, abstinence from cocaine (16); this pattern dispels the perception that
the spectrum and pleasantness of available activities, degree of cocaine use produces no withdrawal. This abstinence sequence,
interpersonal isolation, degree of perceived safety, presence or initially described on the basis of clinical observations in 30 outpa-
difficulty of tasks, perceived medical risk, and many others may all tients, has been verified in one large-scale outpatient study and some
affect cocaine's use and effects but have not been studied. Such (but not all) assessments in hospitalized cocaine abusers, as well as in
studies are fundamental. For example, it is unknown whether the animal models and positron emission tomographic (PET) studies in
distraction that is required for reporting the subjective effects of humans (18). The symptoms of cocaine abstinence are more com-
cocaine to investigators might substantially alter the experience of plex and subtle than those previously associated with drug with-
cocaine euphoria. drawal, and researchers cannot be certain of the characteristics of
Psychological effects ofcocaine: addiction and dependence. On the basis cocaine abstinence until methodological obstacles, delineated below,
of National Institute of Drug Abuse estimates, only about 10 to can be surmounted. Cocaine abstinence is shown schematically in
15% of those who initially try cocaine intranasally become abusers. Fig. 1.
Some individuals who experiment with cocaine cease use immedi- Phase one-crash. Immediately after cessation of a cocaine binge,
ately, typically describing overwhelming anxiety rather than eupho- there is a "crash" of mood and energy (3, 10, 16). Cocaine craving,
ria as cocaine's main effect. Some who do experience intensified depression, agitation, and anxiety then rapidly intensify, and, in over
euphoria cease use because of extreme expense or unavailability. half of abusers seeking treatment, suspiciousness and paranoia are
Others, before addiction is fully developed, fear imminent loss of prominent (14, 19). During the next 1 to 4 hours, cocaine craving
self-control over cocaine use and are able to cease use. decreases and is supplanted by mounting exhaustion and craving for
Individual differences in the progression to addiction could sleep; further use is then often strongly rejected, unlike in the
provide crucial insights for prevention and treatment. However, parallel situation after several hours of opiate, sedative, or alcohol
reliable predictors of later heavy abuse have not been found in light withdrawal (3, 16). Abusers administer marijuana, sedatives, opi-
cocaine users who have not yet developed addiction; a cocaine ates, or alcohol to induce sleep, and after sleep begins, they
"addictive" personality has not been identified; and no objective, experience intense hypersomnolence, with electroencephalographic
systematic assessments of the natural longitudinal course of cocaine changes characteristic of sleep deprivation (20). During brief awak-
29 MARCH 1991 ARTICLES 1581
enings, hyperphagia occurs. The hypersomnolence can last several in animal models and anxiety inventory ratings in humans have
days, after which mood normalizes. confirmed the slightly delayed emergence of anxiety during cocaine
The crash has been confused with withdrawal (8, 10), but the withdrawal (18).
crash parallels an alcohol hangover, not the withdrawal associated Cocaine abusers desiring to cease use are cognizant, when in
with chronic opiate or alcohol administration (3), and although withdrawal, of the adverse consequences of continued cocaine use.
crash symptoms prolong cocaine binges, they do not maintain They are usually able to transiently withstand this anhedonic
long-term cocaine abuse. dysphoria, until they are presented with a conditioned cue. The cue
Phase two-withdrawal. In addiction to cocaine, continued use is superimposes a second, more transient dimension of craving
induced by a protracted dysphoric syndrome-including decreased (evoked craving) on anhedonic craving, and, most often, cocaine use
activation, anxiety, lack of motivation, and boredom, with markedly then resumes. Conditioned cocaine craving is described as pulsatile,
diminished intensity of normal pleasurable experiences (anhe- lasting only minutes or hours. Cocaine and the amphetamines are
donia)-that emerges shortly (0.5 to 4 days) after the crash. This the most potent reinforcing agents known, and as such they produce
hedonically limited existence, when contrasted to memories of intense classical and operant conditioning (12). Such cravings
cocaine-induced euphoria, induces severe cocaine cravings, resump- appear after the appearance of varied, idiosyncratic objects or events
tion of use, and unceasing cycles of recurrent binges (3, 16). Because that were temporally paired with prior cocaine intoxications, and
it is directly related to craving and resumption of use, this with- these appearances are experienced as partial memories of cocaine
drawal phase parallels withdrawal from other abused substances, euphoria. Cues can include mood states (positive as well as nega-
except that there is an absence of gross physiological changes (16). tive), specific persons, locations, events or times of year, mild
These symptoms are far more subtle than those of the crash and alcohol intoxication, interpersonal strife previously soothed by
went unrecognized by early observers. They are not constant or cocaine euphoria, or abuse objects (for example, money, white
severe enough to meet the criteria for major psychiatric mood powder, glass pipes, mirrors, syringes, and single-edged razor

Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on October 5, 2012


disorders. blades) (3, 16, 22).
The existence of a withdrawal phase is indicated by broad clinical Initiation of a cocaine binge thus depends on an interaction
and preclinical observation and consensus (3, 15, 18). However, between drug availability, environmental stimuli (conditioned cues),
precise and objective quantitation of anhedonia and similar dyspho- and the withdrawal status of the dependent abuser. The time course
ric symptoms is problematic. Available psychiatric and psychological of binge resumption fluctuates within and between cocaine-depen-
rating scales are not sensitive to these subtle mood components. For dent individuals, to an extent much greater than in opiate or alcohol
example, no scale or technique assessing anhedonia has been vali- withdrawal, and depends on the weight of these three factors.
dated in human substance abusers (21). Further, quantitation of Manipulation of these factors has thus become the focus of initial
anhedonia requires interactive assessment with a pleasurable probe medical and societal interventions to control cocaine addiction.
because anhedonia is the absence of an appropriate mood response Recent treatment advances focus on decreasing withdrawal dyspho-
to a pleasurable event. In addition, there are extensive individual ria pharmacologically or on extinguishing conditioned craving by
differences in the perception of what stimuli are pleasurable: there using graded exposure to cocaine, and public policy advances in law
are few or no events that can serve as plausible human research tools enforcement could decrease availability of cocaine.
and that uniformly induce pleasure. In animals, however, elegant Although the interplay of these factors has been well described in
methods involving behavioral or electrophysiological responses to cocaine-dependent humans, the interactions of these factors have
electrical, pharmacological, or behavioral stimulation have been not been studied in preclinical experiments in animals. Excellent
developed that allow study of reward responses (reviewed below), models exist for each of the factors. Investigators could model
and complementary preclinical data exist to support the hypothesis availability of cocaine by changing the parameters of work (change
that impairment of reward responses occurs after chronic cocaine in pattern or amount of lever-pressing required for a stimulant dose)
abuse. or by producing impediments to access (delivery of aversive stimuli).
There are established and validated measurement techniques for Conditioned associations, created by pairing of experimental events
anxiety, unlike for anhedonia. Experimental cocaine administration with cocaine or amphetamine administrations, have been studied,
isolated from these other factors, quite extensively. Finally, with-
drawal status could be modeled by regulation of the chronicity,
Crash
r
Phase Phas 2
YKhdmal
Phase 3
Enco administration route, and pulse pattern of cocaine administration,
9 houm
to 4 das to 10wees kcl teflk
and craving can be modeled by assessment of avidity of responding
(point of response cessation after reward ceases) and frequency of
choosing alternative nondrug reinforcers (food, sex, social interac-
tion), after long-term cocaine treatment in animals that precisely
duplicates human use patterns.
Thus, the cocaine disease process could be precisely reconstructed
in animals, particularly regarding administration patterns and aver-
sive stimuli, but animal models designed to mimic human cocaine
administration characteristics have not been used. This is ironic
because the unavailability of precise animal models that impedes
rapid advances in other psychiatric and medical disorders is avoid-
able in studies of cocaine abuse. For example, potential pharmaco-
Fig. 1. Cocaine abstinence phases. Duration and intensity of symptoms vary therapies targeted at conditioned cocaine craving
have not appeared
on the basis of binge characteristics and diagnosis. Binges range in duration in clinical or preclinical research. Such treatments are nonetheless
from under 4 hours to 6 or more days. High cocaine craving in phase 1 plausible and could be screened by assessment of the effect of
usually lasts less than 6 hours and is followed by a period of noncraving with pharmacological agents on cocaine-seeking behavior that has been
similar duration in the next subphase (middle phase 1). Substantial craving conditioned cues in animals treated with cocaine for long
then returns only after a lag of 0.5 to 5 days, during phase 2. [Reprinted linked to
from (16) with permission, Arch. Gen. Psychiatry] periods of time in a binge pattern like that seen in humans. Similarly,
1582 SCIENCE, VOL. 251
the relative importance of conditioned craving and withdrawal tems. Cocaine also affects neurotransmission in histamine, acetyl-
craving at varied stages and degrees of abstinence, which is currently choline, and phenethylamine pathways.
limited to clinical conjecture, could be modeled. Differential and None of these neurotransmitter actions are solely responsible for
specific treatment effects are also likely to appear according to the cocaine euphoria because each is also produced by other pharmaco-
stages and degrees of abstinence at the time a treatment technique is logical agents that do not produce euphoria, are not self-adminis-
applied (for example, isolation from or exposure to cocaine avail- tered by animals, and are not abused by humans (3). Thus, although
ability, and pharmacotherapy at the time withdrawal anhedonia is the rewarding properties of cocaine clearly require activation of
present). Optimal intervention timing and interactions between dopaminergic systems, the molecular mechanisms involved and
treatments could be precisely modeled and rapidly evaluated. Final- whether activation is due only to direct cocaine effects on dopam-
ly, public policy could be based on experimental findings modeled in inergic neurons or to simultaneous collateral actions on other
animals, but it is not. For example, the effect of decreasing the neurotransmitters is uncertain (3, 28). Several lines of evidence
availability of cocaine on increasing work for cocaine (the human support the hypothesis that a simultaneous interaction between
equivalent to this work is often criminal activity) could predict how catecholamine and serotonergic systems is fundamental to cocaine
the crime rate would change after an increase in cocaine prices. At reward and euphoria. Cocaine and other abused stimulants, such as
present, vast government expenditures occur (on the basis of the the amphetamines and methylphenidate, have numerous structural
presumption that increased price would decrease use and crime), in and neuropharmacological dissimilarities, but all produce, by means
the absence of guidance from such plausible experiments. of neurotransmitter reuptake inhibition, activation of dopaminergic
If cocaine abusers sustain abstinence, anhedonic symptoms lift and noradrenergic pathways associated with mood and activation of
within 2 to 12 weeks (16). Animal studies show that behavioral serotonergic systems associated with mood and arousal. Manipula-
depression occurs after stimulant withdrawal for a similar time tions of serotonergic systems modulate the reward potency of both

Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on October 5, 2012


period (23). On the basis of clinical observations, symptom severity cocaine and amphetamine in animals, and agents that activate
and duration depend on the intensity of the preceding months of dopaminergic or noradrenergic systems, or both, but are devoid of
chronic abuse and on the presence of predisposing psychiatric strong serotonergic activity (benztropine, trihexyphenidyl, nomifen-
disorders, which amplify withdrawal symptoms (16). Conversely, in sine, L-dopa/carbidopa) are neither intensively abused by humans
infrequent cocaine users without psychiatric disorders, withdrawal or, when administered systemically rather than directly into the
may not occur. High-intensity binge cocaine use and coinciding CNS, avidly self-administered by animals (29). Pure serotonin
neuroadaptation may be required before withdrawal occurs. agonists and antagonists lacking acute dopaminergic activity, includ-
Phase three-extinction. After resolution of withdrawal anhedonia, ing those with selective activity at specific serotonin receptor
intermittent, conditioned cocaine craving can still emerge (16) subtypes, also do not produce reward or abuse. Hence, the author
months or even years after the last cocaine use. Lasting cocaine proposes that augmentation of the effects of dopamine reuptake
abstinence depends on experiencing intermittent, conditioned crav- blockade probably occurs by way of other inputs (serotonergic or
ing without relapse. The previous consistent pairing of cues with others) to dopaminergic reward pathways to produce sufficient
cocaine euphoria then does not occur, and craving is gradually dopaminergic activation to cause stimulant euphoria. For example,
extinguished (16, 22). Relapse caused by classically conditioned serotonergic synapses exist on the cell body of dopaminergic
craving and withdrawal has been described in opiate and nicotine neurons in the midbrain (AlO) that may, in turn, regulate activation
withdrawal. In cocaine dependence, however, conditioned craving thresholds in reward cells. Cocaine-induced serotonin reuptake
may be more intense than in other addictive disorders. This is not inhibition could magnify cocaine-induced perturbations in the
surprising because cocaine is such a powerful reinforcer in animal dopaminergic reward system to induce euphoria. If so, blockade or
models and because of the relation between strength of reinforce- perturbation of serotonin or other augmenting systems might block
ment and magnitude of classical conditioning. cocaine effects while minimally influencing normal reward transmis-
sion and, unlike the dopamine receptor-blocking neuroleptics,
averting exacerbation of anhedonia.

Short-Term Neurochemical Actions of


Cocaine
Cocaine and the amphetamines are thought to produce pleasure
Neuroadaptation to Long-Term Cocaine
or reward by increasing neurotransmission in mesolimbic or meso-
Abuse
cortical dopaminergic tracts, or both, in the brain (24). In animals, The response of the nervous system to persistent, drug-induced
electrical self-stimulation of these pathways produces reward-seek- neurochemical perturbation is a compensatory adaptation in the
ing behavior that mirrors that of cocaine self-administration. Both perturbed systems. Dysregulation follows adaptation when the drug
electrical self-stimulation of these pathways and cocaine increase is not present. Despite this, until recently it had been assumed that
extracellular dopamine concentration in brain nuclei that control neuroadaptation does not occur in cocaine abuse. Gawin, El-
reward behavior. Lesions in these pathways block cocaine effects, linwood, and Kleber have hypothesized that high-dose cocaine use
and dopamine receptor blockers attenuate cocaine effects (25). over long periods of time generates sustained neurophysiological
Whether cocaine also directly influences efferent or afferent connec- changes in brain systems that regulate only psychological processes,
tions is less clear. particularly hedonic responsivity or pleasure (3, 16). Changes in
Cocaine inhibits reuptake of the neurotransmitters norepineph- these neurophysiological systems produce a true physiological ad-
rine and serotonin, as well as of dopamine. It binds to the dopamine diction and withdrawal, but one whose clinical expression appears
transporter labeled by mazindol and to imipramine binding sites on solely psychological (3).
serotonergic neurons (26). It is a local anesthetic (26) and also Intracranial electrical self-stimulation (ICSS) of brain reward sites
increases catecholamine receptor sensitivity (27). In contrast to in animals provides a model for human pleasure. Chronic cocaine
abused substances like heroin, cocaine does not directly activate and amphetamine administration appears to decrease ICSS reward
enkephalinergic receptors, but may indirectly influence these sys- indices and to increase the threshold voltage required to elicit ICSS
29 MARCH 1991 ARTICLES 1583
in dopaminergic reward areas such as the nucleus accumbens (24). resolved only by studies of long-term cocaine administration in
These ICSS decrements imply that brain reward regions affected by animals in which these variables are systematically regulated.
cocaine are subsensitive or down-regulated. They are consistent with Despite such limitations, neurochemical studies based on periph-
clinical observations of protracted anhedonia in abstinent cocaine eral indices of dopaminergic functioning and electroencephalo-
abusers and with behavioral depression after cocaine use in animals. graphic studies in street abusers suggest that neuroadaptation occurs
The neurophysiological dysregulations that would be consistent in street cocaine abuse (18, 20, 33). Both homovanillic acid (HVA),
with these ICSS data have not been clearly established, but could the principle metabolite of dopamine in humans, and the neurohor-
take place at several sites. Central dopaminergic, a-adrenergic, and mone prolactin (inhibited by tuberoinfundibular dopamine release)
3-adrenergic receptor supersensitivity has been demonstrated after have been assessed. Basal prolactin and HVA concentrations in
long-term cocaine administration in animals (30). Receptor alter- cocaine abusers are either unchanged or reflect decreased dopami-
ations could produce ICSS decrements in animals and anhedonia in nergic functioning (34). However, three studies now indicate that
humans (30). It has only recently been possible to differentiate transiently increased dopaminergic functioning is associated with
presynaptic and postsynaptic receptor changes and Dl, D2, D3, and resurgence of craving for cocaine (18, 33). This is consistent with
D4 dopamine receptors. A primary effect ofdecreased dopaminergic microdialysis studies in animals indicating that conditioned cues and
reward transmission would produce a secondary increase in postsyn- nondrug stimuli that indicate craving (exposure of male rodents,
aptic receptor sensitivity. Disproportionate D2 autoreceptor super- without access, to sexually receptive females) are associated with
sensitivity (that is, greater functional autoreceptor supersensitivity increased dopamine release, of substantially less magnitude than that
than postsynaptic D2 receptor supersensitivity) would decrease caused by cocaine, in the nucleus accumbens (35). It is possible that
dopaminergic neurotransmission. Such changes have been demon- such fluctuations in dopaminergic activity occur broadly enough to
strated in animals with continuous but not intermittent long-term be reflected in less sensitive peripheral assessments in humans.
administration of cocaine and amphetamine (31). Alternatively, In vivo imaging of human brain functioning can directly measure

Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on October 5, 2012


other feedback mechanisms could influence firing and activation metabolism and neurophysiology in cocaine abusers (18). The
thresholds, or other parameters of transmitter release, in dopami- spatial and temporal resolution of PET scanning, single photon
nergic reward nuclei. These include possible neurotoxic degenera- emission computerized tomography, magnetic resonance spectros-
tion of dopaminergic reward neurons (3, 32). Dl receptor-mediat- copy, and methods of assessing regional cerebral blood flow appear
ed feedback innervation to the cell bodies of reward neurons, and to be insufficient for precise demonstration of alterations in reward
multiple possible feedback loops involving serotonergic, noradren- function in human abusers. For example, diminished reward func-
ergic, enkephalinergic, and GABaergic synapses (GABA, -y-ami- tion could occur only in the nucleus accumbens, which is beyond the
nobutyric acid), among others. Pharmacological interventions at spatial resolution of current methods; and the period of craving that
each of these sites influence cocaine administration in preliminary follows a conditioned cue may be associated with very brief activa-
preclinical or clinical reports. The predominant pharmacological tion or deactivation of reward pathways that may not be detectable
treatment thus far investigated in therapy for cocaine addition (the because of the relatively lengthy acquisition time of these imaging
tricyclic antidepressants) has multiple effects that are opposite to techniques. Simple practical problems also exist. Because of the
those of long-term cocaine use: applied on a long-term basis, the unpredictable time course of cocaine abstinence symptoms, imaging
antidepressants increase ICSS reward sensitivity, neuronal firing cannot be scheduled to coincide with specific clinical states, and
rates, and synaptic dopamine concentrations in dopaminergic re- keeping equipment and personnel at ready is implausible.
ward neurons (3, 30). They may also induce dopaminergic autore-
ceptor subsensitivity, and they may have antianhedonic effects in
unipolar depression (3, 30). Recovery and Treatment
The appropriateness of generalization from long-term cocaine
administration studies in animals to human cocaine withdrawal Data from treatment programs using different therapeutic ap-
anhedonia is uncertain. Almost all animal research on long-term proaches indicate that outpatient cocaine abuse treatment can be
administration of cocaine or other stimulants has used drug admin- successful. From 30 to 90% of abusers remaining in outpatient
istration paradigms that do not reflect human abuse patterns. For treatment programs cease cocaine use (36). Samples vary widely in
example, these animal studies usually utilize daily intraperitoneal degree of abuse severity, psychosocial resources, route of adminis-
administration, producing a slowly increasing single peak in dopa- tration, and magnitude and pattern of cocaine and other drug
minergic facilitation each day, and minimal sleep disruption. This exposure, so they are not readily comparable. Further, rigorous
does not coincide with the multidose binges, perturbed diurnal design is usually lacking, so little evidence exists to suggest that any
cyclicity, and multiple-day cocaine-free intervals characteristic of one treatment approach is superior (3). Also, despite superficial
human abusers seeking abstinence. Long-term cocaine administra- differences, strong commonalities characterize all current treatment
tion in animal studies varies in duration from 5 days to 3 months strategies. Initial efforts focus on retaining the abuser in treatment
and may not have consequences similar to multiple years of human and disrupting the cycles of binges; subsequent efforts focus on
abuse. Further, it is unclear how species differences in life-span relapse prevention (3).
should be interpreted in extrapolation of these studies to humans. There is controversy regarding the relative therapeutic value of,
Neurochemical studies in cocaine abusers seeking treatment are and indications for, most components of cocaine abuse treatment.
also limited. Several investigations have assessed peripheral neuro- These include, but are not limited to, hospital versus outpatient
endocrine and neurotransmitter metabolite indices of central cocaine abuse treatment; types of treatment techniques; use, selec-
dopaminergic functioning in treatment-seeking cocaine abusers. tion, dosage, or duration of adjunctive medication; lengths of
Such studies are potentially confounded by multiple sources: con- isolation from and rapidity of latter immersion in the environment
current medical or psychiatric disorders, the intensity and time in which cocaine is available; and most other variables in treatment
course of antecedent cocaine use, other drugs abused, questionable design. These areas require assessment in controlled, random-
reliability of details of self-reports, diet, duration of abstinence, and assignment treatment studies.
genetic heterogeneity, among many others. The complexities pre- Recent double-blind comparisons of medication treatment and
sented by differences in antecedent history can, at present, be abstinence initiation in outpatients have demonstrated that hetero-
1584 SCIENCE, VOL. 251
4.0 Fig. 2. Desipramine fa- ponents. Initial clinical investigations of how the "normal euphoria
cilitation of initial co- of a healthy person" (7) becomes transformed into the source of
2 caine abstinence. End-
(D 3.0 __
t
point analysis of
"mad craving" (6) have resulted in the promise and initial actualiza-
reported cocaine use tion of advances in pharmacological and psychotherapeutic treat-
(grams) during treat- ment of cocaine addiction. These advances have been founded in
=2.0 ment of outpatient co- basic animal research, thus underscoring the value of basic research
directed at unraveling the neurophysiological mysteries of human
caine abusers with desi-
pramine hydrochloride experiences of pleasure and pain.
o 1.0
0
(2.5 mg per kilogram of
body weight per day, n
= 24, solid line), lithium
REFERENCES AND NOTES
0 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 carbonate (17.5 mg/kg,
ime after treatment onset (week) or 1. World Health Organization (WHO) Expert Committee on Addiction-Producing
nplacebo = 24,line),
= 24, (ndashed dotted Drugs, Bull. WHO 39, 225 (1981).
line). [Reprinted from Gawin et al. (37) with permission, Arch. Gen. 2. B. J. Rounsaville, R. Spitzer, J. B. Williams, Am.J. Psychiatry 143, 463 (1986).
Psychiatry] 3. F. Gawin and E. H. Ellinwood, N. Engi. J. Med. 318, 1173 (1988); Treatment of
Psychiatric Disorders: A Task Force Report of the American Psychiatric Association
(American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC, 1989).
cyclic antidepressants (desipramine and imipramine), which do not 4. F. H. Gawin, D. Allen, B. Humblestone, Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 46, 322 (1989).
produce dependence or addictive use patterns, diminish cocaine use 5. E. H. Adams and N. J. Kozel, in NatI. Inst. Drug Abuse (NIDA) Res. Monogr. 61,
(1985), pp. 35-49.
and craving and improve initial treatment outcome in the first 6. The psychic effect of cocaine consists of exhilaration and lasting euphoria ...
months of treatment (Figs. 2 and 3) (4, 37). Multiple uncontrolled which does not differ from the normal euphoria ofa healthy person.... Absolutely
no craving for cocaine appears after the first, or repeated, taking of the drug" [S.
reports also exist ofdecreases in craving induced by other agents that Freud, Centralbl. Gesammt Ther. 2, 289(1884), pp. 300-302].

Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on October 5, 2012


are hypothesized to normalize reward function, and multiple dou- 7. By Lewin in 1887: "They give all that they possess, in order to indulge their mad
ble-blind trials of similar pharmacological treatment for cocaine craving" [L. Lewin, Phantastica (Stilke, Berlin, 1924), p. 80]; H. W. Meier, Der
Kokainismus (Verlag, Leipzig, Germany, 1926).
craving are in progress. Efforts to block the effects of cocaine appear 8. E. H. Ellinwood, in Drug Use: Epidemiological and Sociological Approaches, F.
less promising. Dopamine blocking agents may block cocaine Josephson and E. Carroll, Eds. (Hemisphere, Washington, DC, 1974), pp.
303-329.
effects, but apparently do so only at doses that so markedly amplify 9. 'Chronic cocaine abuse does not usually appear as a medical problem" [L.
anhedonia that such treatment is counter-therapeutic (28, 38). Grinspoon, and J. B. Bakalar, in Comprehensive Textbook of Psychiatry, H. I.
Reports of other agents that may decrease self-administration of Kaplan, A. M. Freedman, B. J. Sadock, Eds. (Williams & Wilkins, Baltimore,
1980)]; National Commission on Marijuana and Drug Abuse, Drug Use in
cocaine in animals have been difficult to interpret. Buprenorphine, America: Problems in Perspective (Government Printing Office, Washington, DC,
for example, was reported to diminish cocaine administration in 1973).
primates by presumed decreases in reward potency (39). However, 10. "Cocaine is, from a public health perspective ... the drug of greatest national
health concern" [J. Grabowski, NIDA Res. Monogr. 50 (1984), p. viii]; J. F. Jekel
increases in reward also diminish frequency of administration in et al., Lancet i, 459 (1986); F. H. Gawin and H. D. Kleber, Arch. Gen. Psychiatry
animals, and rodent studies are consistent with buprenorphine 43, 107 (1986); R. K. Siegel,]. Psychoact. Drugs 14, 321 (1982).
11. F. H. Gawin and H. D. Kleber, NIDA Res. Monogr. 61 (1985), p. 182.
augmentation, rather than blockage, of the effects of cocaine (40). 12. C. Johanson and M. Fischman, Pharmacol. Rev. 41, 3 (1989).
The feasibility of systematic, controlled psychotherapy research 13. C. Van Dyke et al., Int. J. Psychiatry Med. 12, 1 (1982); E. H. Ellinwood, in
for cocaine abuse is demonstrated by promising assessments of Psychopharmacology in the Practice ofMedicine, M. E. Jarvik, Ed. (Appleton-Century-
Crofts, New York, 1977), pp. 467-479.
systematic desensitization of cues eliciting conditioned cocaine 14. M. Scherer, K. Kumor, E. Cone, J. Jaffe, Arch. Gen. Psychiatry, 45, 673 (1988).
craving, but only preliminary clinical trials have appeared (43). 15. E. H. Ellinwood and W. M. Petrie, in DrugAbuse: Clinical and Basic Aspects, S. N.
In-depth studies of the outcome of systematic desensitization are in Pradham, Ed. (Mosby, New York, 1977), pp. 248-262.
16. F. H. Gawin and H. D. Kleber, Arch. Gen. Psychiatry, 43, 107 (1986).
progress, as are studies contrasting psychotherapies with and with- 17. P. H. Connell, in Drugs and Youth, J. R. Wittenborn et al., Eds. (Thomas,
out pharmacotherapies that are similar to prior studies of severe Springfield, IL, 1970).
18. H. Kalsa, D. Anglin, and F. Gawin (manuscript in preparation) describe consistent
depression treatment in non-drug users. findings in the first large-scale investigation of withdrawal symptoms in 240
outpatients. In smaller hospitalized populations, S. Martin, V. Yeragani, R. Lohdi,
and M. P. Galloway [Biol. Psychiatry 26, 356 (1989)]; S. Hollanderet al. 33, 1611
[Psychiatry. Res. (1990)]; N. D. Volkow et al. [Am. J. Psychiatry 147, 719
Conclusions (1990)]; and N. D. Volkow, N. Mullani, K. Gould, S. Adler, and K. Krajewski
[Br. J. Psychiatry 152, 641 (1988)] have reported findings consistent with prior
Cocaine addiction has evolved within one decade from a virtually outpatient evaluations, but W. Weddington et al. [Arch. Gen. Psychiatry, 47, 861
(1990)] have not. W. Wolverton and M. S. Kleven [Psychopharmacology 94, 288
nonexistent problem to a complexly regulated disorder with inter- (1988)]; D. M. Woods and H. Lal [Life Sci. 41,1431 (1987)]; and M. E. Carrol
woven environmental, psychological, and neurophysiological com- and S. T. Lac [ibid. 40, 2183 (1987)] have also described consistent findings on
cocaine discontinuation in animals.
19. S. A. Satel and F. H. Gawin, Am. J. Psychiatry, in press.
8 Fig. 3. Effect of chronic 20. R. Watson, E. Hartmann, J. J. Schildkraut, ibid. 129, 263 (1972).
desipramine treatment 21. Only the Fawcett anhedonia scale has been validated as an index for assessing
on cocaine-induced co- anhedonia in nonpsychotic populations, but in alcohol abusers (J. Fawcett,
caine craving. Intrave- personal communication) and cocaine abusers (F. H. Gawin, unpublished obser-
am nous cocaine (0.5 mg vations), construct validity for this instrument appears to be absent.
22. A. J. Giannini and C. Sangdahl, Int. J. Psychiatry Med. 15, 415 (1985); C. P.
per kilogram of body O'Brien et al.,J. Clin. Psychiatry 49 (suppl.), 17 (1988).
0 weight) or cocaine place- 23. H. Utena, Prog. Brain Res. 21, 1902 (1962).
bo infused in five sub- 24. Reviewed in F. Gawin and E. H. Ellinwood, New Engl.J. Med. 318, 1173 (1988).
c0) jects after long-term See also A. Markou, C. Hubner, and G. Koob [Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 14, 660
0, treatment with desipra- (1988)], and L. Kokkinidis and B. D. McCarter [Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 36,
mine hydrochloride 463 (1990)].
0 (150 mg/day for greater 25. R. Wise,J. Abnormal Psychol. 97, 118 (1988); G. F. Koob and F. E. Bloom,
than 10 days) or placebo Science 242, 715 (1988).
26. M. C. Ritz, R. J. Lamb, S. R. Goldberg, M. J. Kuhar, Science 237, 1219 (1987);
maintenance. (C, place- M. Reith, H. Sershen, D. L. Allen, A. Lajtha, Mol. Pharmacol. 23, 600 (1983); R.
bo/placebo; *, placebo/ Jones, NIDA Res. Monogr. 50 (1984), p. 182.
0 100 200 desipramine; U, cocaine/ 27. M. Memo, S. Pradham, I. Hanbaur, Neuropharmacology 20, 1145 (1981).
Time after infusion (min) placebo; and 0, cocaine/ 28. C. Spyraki, H. C. Fibiger, A. C. Phillips, Brain Res. 253, 195 (1982); F. H. Gawin,
desipramine) Psychopharmacology 90, 142 (1986).
29 MARCH 1991 ARTICLES 1585
29. M. E. Carrol, S. Lac, M. Asencio, R. Kragh, Psychopharmacology 100, 293 (1990); IL, 10-15 May 1987; J. Mendelson et al.,Am.J. Psychiatry 145, 553 (1988); N.
Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 35, 237 (1990); J. G. Modell, R. Tandon, T. P. S. Pilotte, L. G. Sharpe, E. M. Dax, Brain Res. 512, 107 (1990); C. M. Schwarz,
Beresford,J. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 9, 347 (1989). K. Breen, F. Leone, Am.J. Psychiatry 147, 777 (1990).
30. F. Gawin and H. Kleber, Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 42, 903 (1984). 35. P. W. Kalivas and P. Duffy, Synapse 5, 48 (1990); J. R. Blackburn, A. G. Phillips,
31. L. P. Dwoskin, J. Perns, R. Yasuda, K. Philpot, and N. Zahniser [Life Sci. 42, 255 A. Jakuboic, H. C. Fibiger, Behav. Neurosci. 103, 15 (1989).
(1988)]; and D. J. Henry, M. Greene, and F. White U. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 251, 36. F. H. Gawin and H. D. Kleber, in Cocaine: Clinical and Biobehavioral Aspects, S.
901 (1989)] document autoreceptor subsensitivity with intermittent administra- Fisher, B. Raskin, R. Uhlenhuth, Eds. (Oxford Univ. Press, New York, 1986), pp.
tion of cocaine, but see M. Reith, M. Benuck, and A. Lathja [ibid. 243, 281 169-87; C. P. O'Brien et al., NIDA Res. Monogr. 95 (1989), p. 78.
(1987)]; and Kokkinidis and McCarter [in (24)] for a demonstration of the 37. A. Gianninni, D. Malone, M. Gianninni, W. Price, R. Loiselle,. Clin. Pharmacol.
importance of the dosing pattern in cocaine neuroadaptation. Increased effects of 26, 211 (1986); N. Nunes, F. Quitkin, D. Klein, in preparation; F. H. Gawin et
single-dose cocaine administration after repeated cocaine administrations, or al., Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 46, 117 (1989); S. L. McElroy et al., NIDA Res.
sensitization, can also occur. The relevance of sensitization for human cocaine Monogr. 95 (1989), p. 57.
addiction and craving is questionable. The animal literature contains multiple 38. M. Sherer, K. Kumor, J. Jaffe, Psychiatry Res. 27, 117 (1989).
reports describing the occurrence of sensitization with regularly repeated (for 39. N. K. Mello, J. H. Mendelson, M. P. Bree, S. E. Lukas, Science 245, 859
example, daily) small cocaine doses. The only similar clinical phenomenon is (1989).
cocaine-induced paranoia, which occurs only in a subpopulation of users (S. A. 40. E. E. Brown et al., Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 16, 251 (1990).
Satel and F. H. Gawin, Am. J. Psychiatry, in press) and which does not directly 41. A. Childress, R. Ehrman, A. McLellan, C. O'Brien, NIDA Res. Monogr. 81 (1988),
contribute to cocaine addiction. p. 74.
32. R. J. Wyatt, R. Fawcett, F. Karoum, paper presented at the 26th Annual Meeting 44. Supported by NIDA grants P50-04060, R18-DA06082, and R01-DA06289, by
of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, San Juan, Puerto Rico, the Friends of Medical Research and by the Foundation for the International
7-11 December 1987; M. Trulson, S. Babb, J. C. Joe, J. D. Raese, Exp. Neurol. Exchange of Scholars. I thank D. Allen, R. Byck, E. Ellinwood, H. Fibiger, B.
94, 744 (1986). Humblestone, J. Jaffe, J. Jekel, H. Kleber, G. Koob, P. Jatlow, C. O'Brien, N.
33. F. H. Gawin and H. D. Kleber, Br.J. Psychiatry 147, 569 (1985). Nunes, R. Schuster, R. Weiss, and R. Wise for stimulating and illuminating
34. C. Dackis and M. Gold, Neurosci. Behav. Res. 9, 469 (1985); I. Extein et al., paper discussions. The opinions expressed are, however, solely the responsibility of the
presented at the 140th Meeting of the American Psychiatric Association, Chicago, author.

Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on October 5, 2012

" Whatever it was, it was around during the ice-age. "


1586 SCIENCE, VOL. 251

Вам также может понравиться