Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 9

Archives of Sexual Behavior, Vol. 35, No. 3, June 2006, pp.

279286 (
C 2006)

DOI: 10.1007/s10508-006-9032-8

Pubertal Onset and Sexual Orientation in an Adolescent


National Probability Sample

Ritch C. Savin-Williams, Ph.D.1,3 and Geoffrey L. Ream, Ph.D.2

Received January 26, 2005; revisions received June 15, 2005 and November 7, 2005; accepted November 7, 2005
Published online: 27 June 2006

Using 6-year longitudinal data from the National Longitudinal Survey of Adolescent Health (Add
Health) and multiple measures of puberty as it occurs and of sexual orientation (romantic attraction,
sexual identity), the present study attempted to replicate previous research which reported that
homosexuals and heterosexuals differed in their age of pubertal onset. The study hypotheses were not
confirmed for either males or females: on most pubertal measures, same-sex oriented groupings did
not differ from heterosexuals. The only significant findings regarding homosexual males indicated
that they were more likely to report having a later rather than an earlier onset of puberty, and the
significant findings regarding homosexual females were contradictorythey tended to have an earlier
onset of puberty. These findings are attributed to methodological improvements in the present study
that reduced retrospective bias, used multiple indicators of sexual orientation and puberty timing, and
assessed less eroticized measures of puberty.

KEY WORDS: puberty; sexual orientation; adolescence; gay.

INTRODUCTION distinguish sexual orientation groupings. Thus, homo-


sexual individuals will be more similar to the opposite
The onset of puberty during the second decade of than their own sex in their onset of pubertal maturation.
life signals significant biosocial status differentiation as Homosexual males should be female-typical (an earlier
individuals move from biologic and social childhood age of puberty relative to heterosexual males) because
toward adulthood. Although individuals vary in their relevant structures in the nervous system (e.g., sites
pubertal timing based primarily on their genetic predis- in the anterior hypothalamus) have been feminized or
positions and severe challenges in nutrition and health demasculinized relative to heterosexual men (Bogaert,
that alter the expression of genes, group differences in Friesen, & Klentrou, 2002, p. 78). Although data sup-
the timing of pubertal onset are relatively rare (Archibald, porting a sexual orientation group difference in timing of
Graber, & Brooks-Gunn, 2003; Eveleth & Tanner, 1990; puberty have been forthcoming for males, they have not
Katchadourian, 1977, 1990). Exceptions include ethnic/ for females (Bogaert et al., 2002; Mustanski, Chivers, &
racial (not addressed here) and sex differences, with Bailey, 2002), perhaps because females follow a different
females beginning earlier. trajectory with regard to processes that affect their sexual
Previous research hypothesized that the genetic, orientation and pubertal timing (Bogaert et al., 2002;
neuroanatomic, endocrine, and morphologic differences Pattatucci & Hamer, 1995; Tenhula & Bailey, 1998).
that distinguish the sexes in age of pubertal onset also A number of methodological problems have plagued
investigations of pubertal timing among sexual orientation
1 Department of Human Development, Cornell University, Ithaca, New groups: small sample sizes, volunteer sample composi-
York. tion, absence of heterosexual control groups, restricted
2 Medical and Health Research Association of New York City and
definitions of sexuality and puberty, and retrospective
National Development and Research Institutes, New York, New York.
3 To whom correspondence should be addressed at Department of Human reporting bias. The last may be a particularly impor-
Development, Cornell University, Ithaca, New York 14853; e-mail: tant explanatory factor accounting for sexual orientation
rcs15@cornell.edu. differences. Three investigations based on the pubertal

279
0004-0002/06/0600-0279/0 
C 2006 Springer Science+Business Media, Inc.
280 Savin-Williams and Ream

onset reports of adolescent/young adult male samples Hypotheses


have not found a significant sexual orientation group
difference (Cohen, 1999; Savin-Williams, 1995; Schmidt, The present study replicated several of the method-
1997). These participants were considerably closer in age ological advancements of Bogaert et al. (large national
to pubertal onset (less than 10 years) than previous adult probability sample, control group, three sexuality do-
samples one to five decades removed from adolescence. mains) and corrected several of its limitations: subjects
The impact of retrospective biases on pubertal onset were adolescent participants assessed with multiple mea-
reports is unknown but likely not negligible (Gilger, sures of sexuality and puberty over a 6-year period of time.
Geary, & Eisele, 1991). The potential for retrospective Two hypotheses that have been investigated in previous
reporting bias might be greater for males because, unlike reports were proposed here as well: homosexual males
menses for females, male puberty lacks a discrete event will have an earlier and homosexual females will have a
that designates pubertal onset with precision (Mustanski later onset of puberty than will heterosexual members of
et al., 2002). their sex.
A second methodological problem with previous
investigations has been their reliance on a singular
assessment of puberty or on a questionable criterion of METHOD
puberty (e.g., age of first masturbation, first ejaculation,
first sexual behavior, first sexual feeling). Compared to Participants
heterosexual males, Bell, Weinberg, and Hammersmith
(1981) found that homosexual males had an earlier onset Data for this study were drawn from the on-going
of self-reported first ejaculation. However, when asked National Longitudinal Survey of Adolescent Health (Add
about the source of first ejaculation, unlike heterosexuals, Health) (Udry & Bearman, 1998a, 1998b). Add Health
homosexual males were more likely to report active (mas- was designed to assess the health status of U.S. adoles-
turbation, sexual relations) rather than passive (nocturnal cents and the contextual determinants of it. The sampling
emissions) sourcesthe latter elicited a later age of frame was school-based, with a nationwide sample of
ejaculatory onset. Thus, particular measures of pubertal 80 high schools with a 70% response rate. Comparable
age (e.g., onset of ejaculation and sexual behavior) might replacement schools were then selected for schools that
be assessing sexual libido or sensitivity to ones own declined to participate. If the recruited high school was
biologic development rather than pubertal onset. A related not for all grades 7 through 12, younger students were
problem concerns global measures of pubertal timing: included from middle and junior high schools that fed
How old were you when you reached puberty? By into the sample high schools. A total of 132 public and
puberty, I mean when your voice changed or when you private schools, including high schools and junior high
began growing pubic hair? (Bogaert et al., 2002). Not and middle schools that fed into them, in a total of 80
only was this assessment of pubertal onset asked of men communities participated.
on average 25 years after it occurred, the two events In selecting students to participate in the in-home
that defined pubertal onset themselves are discrepant interviews, the within-school sample was split into sex
on average by 3 years (age 15 and 12, respectively) in by grade strata and a random sample was taken within
terms of when they first become manifest (Katchadourian, each stratum. Roughly 17 students per stratum per school
1990). pair were selected for a total of 12,105 students in the
Given the methodological shortcomings in the empir- core sample. At this point in time, three waves have been
ical literature, the most comprehensive data are provided collected. The total Wave 1 in-home interview sample,
by Bogaert et al. (2002). In their retrospective study of a including core sample and special-purpose over-samples,
large national probability sample, they addressed several numbered 20,747 individuals (M age = 15.8 years). At
methodological problems: a heterosexual control group Wave 2, one year later, 14,738 participants who had not
and multiple measures of sexual orientation (attraction, been in the 12th grade at Wave 1 and who were not part of
behavior, and identity) were included, volunteer bias was the special disabled sample were re-interviewed (M age =
reduced, and the sample size was relatively large. Consis- 16.7 years). Wave 3 data were follow-up interviews with
tent with previous reports, they found that homosexual/ 15,170 of the original Wave 1 respondents located by field
bisexual men reported a pubertal onset significantly earlier interviewers between August 2001 and April 2002 when
(about 6 months) than did heterosexual men; women did participants were between the ages of 18 and 26 years
not vary significantly by sexual orientation in reported age (M age = 21.7 years). To be included in this studys
of pubertal onset. analyses, cases had to have valid data for all three waves
Puberty and Sexual Orientation 281

plus a valid grand sample weight value (n = 10,828), in- mostly homosexual (gay), but somewhat attracted to
dicating a positive probability, however small, of inclusion people of the opposite sex, 100% homosexual (gay),
in a national probability sample of American adolescents and not sexually attracted to either males or females.
and young adults. In order to create a classification scheme that both
followed logically from previous research and maximized
statistical power, these categories were collapsed to create
Measures a dichotomy between exclusive heterosexuality (the first
rating) and bisexuality/homosexuality (the third through
Participants listened to recorded interview questions fifth ratings). Individuals missing data or reporting mostly
read to them via headphones plugged into a laptop heterosexual or no sexual attraction were excluded.
computer and then entered responses (audio computer- Significance of results was not affected when other
assisted self-interviews). recoding schemes were tried: using all five categories
of attraction, collapsing the five into three categories,
Sexual Orientation and dichotomizing the first versus the fourth and fifth
(excluding the second and third).
Because self-attribution of sexual orientation in the
Add Health data set varies over time and across measures
Pubertal Development
(Savin-Williams & Ream, 2006), to test for any possible
relationship between sexual and pubertal status to emerge,
The Add Health data set asked about various markers
sexual orientation was assessed over three time periods
of pubertal development during Waves 1 and 2 that
and with two measures: romantic attraction at Waves
reflected relative age of pubertal onset. For purposes
1, 2, and 3 and sexual identity at Wave 3. Thus, even
of this investigation, they were considered individually
though pubertal onset and relative timing was assessed at
rather than as a composite measure to assess whether some
and between Waves 1 and 2, participants sexual status
indicators were more useful in this context than others.
was determined during both of these time periods as
well as how they would eventually identify their sexual
orientation 5 and 6 years later as young adults. Developed for Age

Participants were asked, How advanced is your


Romantic Attraction
physical development compared to other boys/girls of
your age? Response options were I look younger
Based on responses to the questions in Waves 1 and
than most, I look younger than some, I look about
2, Have you ever had a romantic attraction to a female?
average, I look older than some, and I look older
and Have you ever had a romantic attraction to a male?
than most. Despite its subjective, self-report nature, this
and in Wave 3, Since (time of Wave 2 interview), have
assessment has proven to be a modestly reliable measure
you had a romantic attraction to a (male/female)? par-
of maturation level relative to peers (Schlossberger,
ticipants attraction was classified as either opposite-sex,
Turner, & Irwin, 1992).
both-sex, or same-sex. For purposes of consistency with
previous research, which does not distinguish between
developmental precursors of bisexual and exclusively Axillary Hair
same-sex attraction, and for ease of analysis, attraction
was dichotomized into same-sex (including bisexual Males were asked, How much hair is under your
and exclusively same-sex) and exclusively opposite-sex. arms now? Which sentence best describes you? A 5-
Individuals reporting no sexual attraction were excluded. point response scale followed: I have no hair, I have a
little hair, I have some hair, but not a lot; it has spread
out since it first started growing and is thicker, I have a
Sexual Identity
lot of hair that is thick, and I have a whole lot of hair
that is very thick, as much hair as a grown man.
At Wave 3, participants were given six possible
responses to the statement Please choose the description
that best fits how you think about yourself: 100% Facial Hair
heterosexual (straight), mostly heterosexual (straight),
but somewhat attracted to people of your own sex, Males were asked, How thick is the hair on your
bisexualthat is, attracted to men and women equally, face? Which sentence best describes you? Response
282 Savin-Williams and Ream

options included, I have a few scattered hairs, but the Analytic Strategy
growth is not thick, The hair is somewhat thick, but you
can still see a lot of skin under it, The hair is thick; you The first set of analyses examined the relationship
cant see much skin under it, and The hair is very thick, between sexual orientation and indicators of the timing of
like a grown mans facial hair. pubertal maturation onset. Participants were selected for
specific analyses based on whether they had valid data for
measures of pubertal maturation. Although females who
Voice Change had experienced menses could enter a valid age at first
menstrual period, the questionnaire did not ask for specific
Males were asked, Is your voice lower now than it ages for other pubertal onset milestones. For males
was when you were in grade school? The five possible arm/body hair and voice and females body curvature
responses were, No, it is about the same as when I was and breast development, the questionnaire asked for the
in grade school, Yes, it is a little lower than when I was current level at both Wave 1 and Wave 2. Participants who
in grade school, Yes, it is somewhat lower than when reported completely childlike appearance at Wave 1 and
I was in grade school, Yes, it is a whole lot lower than something other than a completely childlike appearance
when I was in grade school, and Yes, it is a whole lot at Wave 2 were considered to have undergone pubertal
lower than when I was in grade school; it is as low as an maturation onset between the two waves. Their age at
adult mans voice. Wave 1 was entered as a predictor of sexual orientation for
analyses of pubertal maturation timing. Analyses of each
indicator necessarily involved only the subset of cases that
Breast Development experienced pubertal onset according to those indicators.
The disadvantage to this was that the valid number of
Females were asked, As a girl grows up, her breasts cases available was severely reduced from the thousands
develop and get bigger. Which sentence best describes to the hundreds, leaving a sample size comparable to
you? The five possible responses were, My breasts are that used in other studies. The explicit advantage for this
about the same size as when I was in grade school, My approach, however, was that these questions asked only
breasts are a little bigger than when I was in grade school, for observations of present levels of physical indicators
My breasts are somewhat bigger than when I was in grade and were perhaps less prone to social desirability and
school, My breasts are a lot bigger than when I was in retrospective bias effects that affect responses to questions
grade school, and My breasts are a whole lot bigger about development relative to peers.
than when I was in grade school, they are as developed as The second set of analyses examined relative pu-
a grown womans breasts. bertal developmental progress, or how developed par-
ticipants were for their age. Though not longitudinal
Body Curvature in nature, these analyses consider romantic attraction
at all three waves and sexual identity at Wave 3 as
Females were asked, As a girl grows up, her body four different indicators of sexual orientation; the seven
becomes more curved. Which sentence best describes indicators of pubertal maturation timing measured at
you? Response options included, My body is about as two waves constituted 14 distinct operationalizations of
curvy as when I was in grade school, My body is a pubertal maturation status during adolescence. In survey-
little more curvy than when I was in grade school, My adjusted analyses, pubertal maturation indicatorsthat
body is somewhat more curvy than when I was in grade were roughly normally distributedwere regressed over
school, My body is a lot more curvy than when I was in sexual orientation with age as a control. Survey-adjusted
grade school, and My body is a whole lot more curvy analyses accounted for the stratified random nature of the
than when I was in grade school. sample, weighted the data so that sample statistics were
as close as possible to population parameters, and made
tests of statistical significance more realistically strict.
Menses Although this approach may draw statistical power to
some degree, valid N for all analyses was in the thousands;
Females were asked to report their age in response to they would have been unrealistically overpowered unless
the question, How old were you when you had your very some adjustments were made. Individuals who indicated
first menstrual period? Valid cases included only those a lower degree of maturation at Wave 2 than at Wave 1
who had reported an age of first menses. were excluded from analyses.
Puberty and Sexual Orientation 283

Table I. Proportions and Means of Youth Reporting Indicators of Pubertal Maturation

11 12 13 14 15 16

Male
Axillary hair (%) 0.0 13.3 40.5 26.3 12.1 7.8
Facial hair (%) 0.0 3.4 23.9 26.8 26.5 19.4
Voice change (%) 0.0 7.2 27.1 32.8 17.7 15.1
Adv. age (Wave 1)a 3.30 2.72 3.04 3.13 3.25 3.27
Adv. age (Wave 2)a 3.23 2.90 3.17 3.19 3.23 3.23
Female
Breast development (%) 0.7 7.6 24.6 26.1 20.3 20.8
Body curvature (%) 0.4 14.1 26.3 25.5 20.7 13.0
Menses (Wave 1) (%) 28.9 33.9 25.2 9.0 2.5 0.6
Menses (Wave 2) (%) 27.9 32.3 26.7 9.8 2.7 0.6
Adv. age (Wave 1)a 3.17 3.08 3.30 3.42 3.38 3.31
Adv. age (Wave 2)a 3.48 3.33 3.37 3.44 3.30 3.15

a Scaled from 1 = I look younger than most to 5 = I look older than most.

RESULTS later onset of puberty than will heterosexual members


of their sex, logistic regression models were run in which
Descriptive Statistics each indicator of pubertal maturation was entered, in its
own model, as an independent variable predicting each
Table I summarizes the onset of puberty as derived indicator of same-sex sexuality, each in its own model.
from Waves 1 and 2, separate by age grouping and pubertal In interpreting the results presented in Table II, it is
indicator. Except for 12-year-old boys, the average youth important to take into account that each coefficient reports
(sexuality unspecified) reported being slightly ahead of the meaningful result from a separate logistic regression
peers in pubertal development. The youths self-reported model. In all but the menses analyses, only participants
pubertal indicator medians were consistent with national in the 1116 age range were included in order to restrain
norms (Archibald et al., 2003; Coleman & Coleman, the scope of analyses to ages in which puberty normally
2002). occurs. All models except those with the advanced-
for-age reports contained only the single independent
variable of the pubertal maturation indicator and the single
Tests of Hypotheses dependent variable of the same-sex affiliation indicator.
In models with the advanced-for-age reports, age was
To test the two hypotheses with respect to pubertal entered as a control because of a relationship between
maturation timing, that same-sex attracted males will have perceived pubertal status and age that previous research
an earlier and same-sex attracted females will have a has established (Schlossberger et al., 1992).

Table II. Logistic Regression Analyses Predicting Sexual Orientation from Pubertal Maturation Timing Indicators

W1 attraction W2 attraction W3 attraction W3 identity

B SM N B SM N B SM N B SM N

Male
Axillary hair 0.125 15 164 0.116 13 164 0.240 8 169 0.117 9 173
Facial hair 0.197 46 750 0.056 29 691 0.111 38 656 0.171 40 674
Voice change 0.166 19 293 0.181 26 280 0.065 18 312 0.255 16 327
Female
Breast development 1.391 10 147 0.287 8 152 0.536 15 167 0.389 14 172
Body curvature 0.154 10 181 0.010 10 187 0.074 22 202 0.079 21 209
W1 Menses 0.031 456 7393 0.002 297 5177 0.027 881 6560 0.026 951 6675
W2 Menses 0.023 318 5698 0.181 299 5361 0.013 663 5118 0.047 715 5205

Note. SM, sexual minorities in analysis; N, total cases in analysis.


p < .01.
284 Savin-Williams and Ream

Entering all indicators at once was neither possible Table III. Linear Regressions Predicting Pubertal Maturation Indi-
nor desirable for several reasons. Hypotheses did not spec- cators from Sexual Orientation Indicators, Controlling for Age
ify on which pubertal maturation indicator differences Attraction Identity
would be manifested and this study was not concerned
Wave 1 Wave 2 Wave 3 Wave 3
with whether any indicators were better than others, which
is the research question that a model with all indicators Male
entered simultaneously would evaluate. Further, in order Axillary hair
to be included in an analysis with all indicators simultane- W1 0.007 0.089 0.173 0.147
ously, a participant would have to experience all indicators W2 0.054 0.016 0.190 0.023
Facial hair
in the same year-long time frame of the first two waves of
W1 0.072 0.003 0.026 0.013
the study. Given that these indicators do not normatively W2 0.021 0.014 0.080 0.034
occur at the same time, this would leave a subject pool Voice change
that would be neither sufficiently large nor representative W1 0.159 0.407 0.025 0.004
for meaningful analyses. W2 0.223 0.134 0.021 0.106
Comparison with peers
Even with one indicator at a time in the analyses,
W1 0.003 0.359 0.121 0.087
there remained justifiable concern about sample size. W2 0.091 0.132 0.053 0.261
Because some physical manifestations of puberty were Female
only experienced by a few participants, analyses involv- Breast development
ing those manifestations have a small total number of W1 0.115 0.085 0.175 0.206
W2 0.017 0.138 0.161 0.229
individuals with a same-sex orientation. These analyses
Body curvature
have low statistical power and a significant possibility W1 0.053 0.149 0.188 0.105
of Type II error. They should, therefore, be interpreted W2 0.066 0.093 0.135 0.062
with caution. They were left in to treat the broadest Comparison with peers
range of indicators possible and to address the concern W1 0.191 0.197 0.084 0.184
W2 0.093 0.014 0.161 0.292
that significant findings may be based only on having
a large sample. So that the reader can evaluate the Note. Maturation was the dependent variable and age (coefficients not
potential for nonsignificant findings due to lack of power shown) and sexual orientation were the independent variables.
p < .05, p < .01.
or statistically significant but meaningless findings due
to overwhelming statistical power, the sample number is
provided in Table II. and females were expected to report less developed
Results for males did not confirm the hypothesis. for their agereflecting later, male-typical maturation
A finding that confirmed hypotheses would have a timing. Few analyses were statistically significant and all
significant positive coefficient, indicating that those who that were significant were in the opposite direction of
were more mature for their age were more likely to have hypotheses.
same-sex attraction. The only significant finding for males
suggested that those who reported being less developed
for their age (Wave 2 attraction) were more likely to report DISCUSSION
same-sex attraction.
Findings for females were mixed. A coefficient for The study hypotheses were not confirmed for either
one of the objective physical indicators (breast develop- males or females: on most pubertal measures, same-
ment for Wave 1 attraction) was significant in the direction sex oriented groupings did not differ significantly from
of confirming the hypothesis, indicating that females heterosexuals. The first set of analyses that prospectively
who developed later were more likely to report same- captured pubertal maturation as it happened mostly failed
sex attraction. Not supporting the female hypothesis were to predict sexual orientation from pubertal maturation.
findings that same-sex attracted females had an earlier and The only significant results were in the opposite direction
not later menstrual onset (Wave 2 attraction) and were of hypotheses. The overpowered second set of analyses
more, not less, developed for their age (Wave 3 attraction found several statistically significant instances of sexual
and identity). orientation emerging as a predictor of maturation over
Table III describes results of testing hypotheses and above age; yet, all were in the opposite direction
with respect to relative pubertal maturation. Males were of hypotheses. Same-sex attracted males tended to report
expected to report being more developed for their age delayed, not accelerated, pubertal development, and same-
reflecting earlier, female-typical, maturation timing sex attracted females reported being farther along, not
Puberty and Sexual Orientation 285

less far along, for their age than opposite-sex attracted between-group mean differences based on sexual orienta-
females. tion exists.
These nonfindings were unlikely to have emerged Several study limitations caution against any attempt
from a problem with the data; the ages at which study to represent these findings as the final word. First, due to
participants reported pubertal onset were consistent with the relatively small number of same-sex attracted youth,
national norms. Neither was it likely that findings arose statistical power was reduced. Second, pubertal measures
from lack of power, as even pubertal indicators with were self-reports rather than actual assessments and thus
several thousand valid cases, including several hundred could be subject to systematic bias by sexual orientation.
sexual minorities, did not significantly distinguish sexual Third, it was unfortunate that the Add Health data set
orientations. Thus, results raised doubts about pubertal did not include the typical measure of pubertal onset
maturation as a correlate of sexual orientation. Given that (pubic hair) or a sexualized measure (e.g., first orgasm) to
previous studies on females have rarely found significant test the significance of primary versus secondary pubertal
sexual orientation differences in age of pubertal onset, indicators.
the present findings were consistent with the empirical Our failure to find sexual orientation differences in
literature. Although results have been inconsistent when pubertal onset age was indicative that at least in this one
the sample was composed of adolescent males, our regard, a biological variable might not be a primary agent
findings were in contrast to previous research with adult separating sexual orientation groups. Thus, if homosexual
samples (Bogaert et al., 2002) and with a recent review of individuals are cross-sexed as indicated by recent reviews
the literature (Mustanski et al., 2002). (Mustanski et al., 2002; Rahman & Wilson, 2003), then it
This review also offered the most parsimonious is not true for all biologic indicators.
explanation of our findingsmethodological. Mustanski
et al. (2002) pointed out that pubertal timing results were
suspect because past findings have been retrospective and ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
based on a singular event. When research relies on subjects
recalling their age of pubescence considerably beyond its This research used data from Add Health, a pro-
occurrence (retrospective bias) and on singular measures gram project designed by J. Richard Udry, Peter S.
of puberty, findings may be compromised. In addition, Bearman, and Kathleen Mullan Harris, and funded by
previous research has tended to assess highly eroticized a grant P01-HD31921 from the National Institute of
aspects of male pubertal development (penile and testic- Child Health and Human Development, with cooperative
ular development, pubic hair onset, first ejaculation, first funding from 17 other agencies. Special acknowledgment
sexual arousal, onset of sexual behavior); gay males in is due Ronald R. Rindfuss and Barbara Entwisle for
those studies might have been more sensitive to these assistance in the original design. Persons interested
sexualized aspects of male development and thus more in obtaining data files from Add Health should con-
likely to remember or report an early first development. tact Add Health, Carolina Population Center, 123 W.
The present studys greater reliance on secondary rather Franklin Street, Chapel Hill, North Carolina 27516-2524
than primary pubertal development might have blunted (www.cpc.unc.edu/addhealth/contract.html)
this sexual orientation difference in recall.
Although a studys nonfindings would normally
not be a compelling contribution to the scientific lit- REFERENCES
erature, what may prove most valuable are not the
findings themselves, but the methods used to obtain them. Archibald, A. B., Graber, J. A., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2003). Pubertal
The present study used a nationally representative sample process and physiological growth in adolescence. In G. R. Adams
and employed both cross-sectional and prospective time & M. D. Berzonsky (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of adolescence
(pp. 2447). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
frames. The only youth included in our analyses were Bell, A. P., Weinberg, M. S., & Hammersmith, S. K. (1981). Sexual
those who observed themselves to have gone through preference: Its development in men and women. Bloomington:
puberty between the first and second data collections; Indiana University Press.
Bogaert, A. F., Friesen, C., & Klentrou, P. (2002). Age of puberty and
their data were arguably more convincing because they sexual orientation in a national probability sample. Archives of
represented the pubertal experience as it was happening Sexual Behavior, 31, 7381.
rather than its recall years later. Prospective longitudinal Cohen, K. M. (1999). The biology of male sexual orientation: Rela-
tionship among homoeroticism, childhood sex-atypical behavior,
design with heterosexual control groups built into the spatial ability, handedness, and pubertal timing. Unpublished
sampling frame will help resolve the extent to which doctoral dissertation, University of Detroit Mercy, Detroit, MI.
286 Savin-Williams and Ream

Coleman, L., & Coleman, J. (2002). The measurement of puberty: A Savin-Williams, R. C. (1995). An exploratory study of pu-
review. Journal of Adolescence, 25, 535550. bertal maturation timing and self-esteem among gay and
Eveleth, P., & Tanner, J. M. (1990). Worldwide variation in human bisexual male youths. Developmental Psychology, 31, 56
growth (2ne ed.). New York: Cambridge University Press. 64.
Gilger, J. W., Geary, D. C., & Eisele, L. M. (1991). Reliability and Savin-Williams, R. C., & Ream, G. L. (2006). Prevalence and stability
validity of retrospective self-reports of the age of pubertal onset of sexual orientation during adolescence and young adulthood.
using twin, sibling, and college student data. Adolescence, 26, Manuscript submitted for publication.
4153. Schlossberger, N. M., Turner, R. A., & Irwin, C. E. (1992). Validity of
Katchadourian, H. (1977). The biology of adolescence. San Francisco, self-report of pubertal maturation in early adolescents. Journal of
CA: Freeman. Adolescent Health, 13, 109113.
Katchadourian, H. (1990). Sexuality. In S. S. Feldman & G. R. Elliott Schmidt, G. (1997, December 21). Hamburg student study 19661981
(Eds.), At the threshold: The developing adolescent (pp. 330351). 1996. Retrieved from sexnet@listserve.acns.nwu.edu.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Tenhula, W. N., & Bailey, J. M. (1998). Female sexual orientation
Mustanski, B. S., Chivers, M. L., & Bailey, J. M. (2002). A critical and pubertal onset. Developmental Neuropsychology, 14, 369
review of recent biological research on human sexual orientation. 383.
Annual Review of Sex Research, 13, 89140. Udry, J. R., & Bearman, P. S. (1998a). The national longitudinal
Pattatucci, A. M. L., & Hamer, D. H. (1995). Development and study of adolescent health. Retrieved April 30, 2003, from
familiality of sexual orientation in females. Behavior Genetics, http://www.cpc.unc.edu/projects/addhealth.
25, 407420. Udry, J. R., & Bearman, P. S. (1998b). New methods for new research on
Rahman, Q., & Wilson, G. D. (2003). Born gay? The psychobiology of adolescent sexual behavior. In R. Jessor (Ed.), New perspectives
human sexual orientation. Personality and Individual Differences, on adolescent risk behavior (pp. 241269). New York: Cambridge
34, 13371382. University Press.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Вам также может понравиться