Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 10

Journal of Environmental Psychology 48 (2016) 65e74

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Environmental Psychology


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jep

Intergenerational association of environmental concern: Evidence of


parents' and children's concern
 , Jose
Luis V. Casalo n Escario*
-Julia
n, s/n, 22001, Huesca, Spain
Faculty of Business and Public Management, University of Zaragoza, Spain, Plaza de la Constitucio

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: To understand how young people develop environmental concern, this article investigates the rela-
Received 11 February 2016 tionship between parents' environmental concern and those of their children. Using 2006 survey data
Received in revised form from the Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA), featuring nationally representative
30 August 2016
samples from 16 countries, this study provides evidence that parents' environmental concern has an
Accepted 3 September 2016
important inuence on children's environmental concern. The inuence of parents in this realm reects
Available online 7 September 2016
the parent equivalent socialization hypothesis and applies for both boys and girls. However, girls are
more sensitive to the intergenerational association of their mothers and fathers. Informative campaigns
Keywords:
Intergenerational association
about the environment in schools also contribute to increase children's environmental concern. These
Environmental concern results offer interesting implications for both research and practice.
Sex 2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Parents

1. Introduction (Kaiser, Oerke, & Bogner, 2007; Kaiser, Wo lng, & Fuhrer, 1999;
Levine & Strube, 2012; Ritter, Borchardt, Vaccaro, Pereira, &
Human activities have signicant effects in terms of degrading Almeida, 2015; Rodrguez-Barreiro et al., 2013). Second, some
the environment and altering ecosystems, and increasing aware- studies focus on explaining differences in environmental attitudes,
ness acknowledges both these environmental problems and human often by identifying predictors of attitudes and then inferring new
responsibility for them (Dunlap, Van Liere, Mertig, & Jones, 2000; ways to inuence them (Boeve-de Pauw, 2011; Boeve-de Pauw &
Rodrguez-Barreiro et al., 2013). To protect the natural environ- van Petegem, 2010; Duarte, Escario, & Sanagustn, 2016; Shen &
ment from further degradation, a key goal is to change people's Saijo, 2008; Zelezny, Chua, & Aldrich, 2000). Whether explicitly or
environmental attitudes, dened as psychological evaluations of implicitly, such studies predict that attitudes determine behavior,
the natural environment expressed with some degree of favor or so improving environmental attitudes improves environmental
disfavor (Milfont & Duckitt, 2010), as well as their behaviors behaviors. Building on this analysis, we explicitly investigate the
(Boeve-de Pauw & van Petegem, 2010). Analyses of environmental role of intergenerational associations as determinants of the envi-
attitudes are widespread in research, because of their likely inu- ronmental attitudes of children. With this exploratory study, we
ence on environmental behaviors, as predicted by the theory of test empirically whether parents' environmental attitudes, along
planned behavior (Ajzen, 1991). However, the effects of attitudes with other contextual variables, can predict their children's envi-
tend to be indirect, such that they inuence behavioral intentions, ronmental attitudes. We use nationally representative survey data
which in turn inuence behavior. across several countries and also consider the possible effect of sex
The growing research into environmental attitudes in recent patterns on this intergenerational association, such that we inves-
decades features two broad perspectives. First, some authors tigate whether the related socialization process depends on the sex
investigate possible causal relationships between pro- of either the parent or the child.
environmental attitudes and pro-environmental behaviors In turn, the present study offers an initial investigation of the
relationship between parents' environmental concern and the
environmental concern of their children using a homogeneous,
nationally representative, international sample. The data came
n Pblica, Plaza de la
* Corresponding author. Facultad de Empresa y Gestio from the Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA)
Constitucio  n, s/n, 22001, Huesca, Spain.
E-mail addresses: lcasalo@unizar.es (L.V. Casalo), jescario@unizar.es 2006 survey, developed by the Organization for Economic Co-
(J.-J. Escario). operation and Development (OECD). We focus on environmental

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2016.09.001
0272-4944/ 2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
66 , J.-J. Escario / Journal of Environmental Psychology 48 (2016) 65e74
L.V. Casalo

concern because the PISA 2006 contains detailed information about inuenced by their parents' opinions and behaviors. If intergener-
environmental concern, but not about attitudes, among children ational transmission occurs, some similarity arises in the values,
and one of their parents. In addition, environmental concern is attitudes, and behaviors expressed by parents and their children
generally considered a close concept to attitude (Rhead, Elliot, & (Grnhj & Thgersen, 2009). Younger people have less stable
Upham, 2015). Subsequent PISA reports do not include informa- beliefs and are more susceptible to others' messages (Hess, 1994;
tion about parents' environmental concern, so the 2006 surveys are Sears, 1986); the strongest inuence comes from their close refer-
the most recent versions that contain this specic information. ence groups, such as their immediate family (Sancho, Miguel, &
We extend the scarce research focused on the transmission of Aldas, 2011). As a reference group, parents generally have more
environmental attitudes from parents to children (Grnhj & experience with and knowledge about environmental issues, so
Thgersen, 2009; Leppa nen, Haahla, Lensu, & Kuitunen, 2012; they can inuence their children's beliefs and behaviors, largely
Meeusen, 2014) by considering the specic element of environ- through their own routines. This inuence might reect an inter-
mental concern and using a homogeneous and representative nalization of information from relevant sources (e.g., observing
survey data for several countries; we also investigate how the sex of parents' behaviors, communication within the family) or a need to
the parents and their children affect this relationship. Specically, comply with parents' expectations (Belanche, Casalo , & Flavi
an,
we evaluate (1) the inuence of parents' environmental concern on 2012). Grnhj and Thgersen (2009) afrm that specic pro-
the environmental concern of their children, (2) whether boys and environmental attitudes and behaviors are positively correlated
girls exhibit different levels of environmental concern, (3) whether between parents and children. Therefore, we propose:
one sex is more sensitive to the inuences of mothers and fathers,
H1. Parents' environmental concern is positively associated with
(4) whether the inuences of fathers and mothers differ depending
children's environmental concern.
on the sex of the child, and (5) the inuence of school character-
istics and family socioeconomic characteristics on environmental
concern. With this expansive exploration, we clarify how children's
environmental concern builds and how to promote it, which may 2.3. Role of parents' and children's sex
help increase children's willingness to contribute to environmental
solutions (Dunlap & Jones, 2002). Previous studies suggest that although men usually show a
greater knowledge about environmental problems (e.g. Schahn &
2. Background and hypotheses Holzer, 1990), women exhibit stronger environmental concerns, at-
titudes, and behaviors than men (e.g., Arcury & Christianson, 1990;
2.1. Environmental concern Arnocky & Stroink, 2010; Maineri, Barnett, Valdero, Unipan, &
Oskamp, 1997). This distinction may be explained due to sex differ-
According to the impressionable years hypothesis, people are ences in terms of personality traits, roles, and socialization (Zelezny
highly susceptible to attitude changes during their adolescence and et al., 2000), including the greater levels of emotional empathy
early adulthood, but this susceptibility drops precipitously there- among women (Arnocky & Stroink, 2010). Sex differences may also
after and remains low throughout the rest of the person's life appear in personality traits, starting in early childhood (Else-Quest,
(Krosnick & Alwin, 1989). If this hypothesis holds, research on Hyde, Goldsmith, & Van Hulle, 2006) and persisting throughout
attitude development seemingly should focus primarily on ado- adolescence (Wilgenbusch & Merrell, 1999) and adulthood
lescents and youth (Meeusen, 2014), for whom family has signi- (Feingold, 1994). Specically, women appear to be more tender-
cant inuences on attitude development and formation and minded and agreeable and less risk-taking than men (Byrnes,
education (Noller & Callan, 1991). Therefore, many researchers Miller, & Schafer, 1999; Feingold, 1994; Schmitt, Realo, Voracek, &
analyze the intergenerational transmission of various attitudes and Allik, 2008). Other values linked to femininity include caring for
behaviors, including political party preferences, religious afliation others and quality of life (Belanche, Casalo  , & Guinaliu, 2015), and
(Duriez & Soenens, 2009; Jennings, Stoker, & Bowers, 2009), edu- women are also socialized to value the needs of others (Zelezny et al.,
cation (Mendez, 2015; Remmerswaal, Muris, & Huijding, 2015), 2000), such that they often show more helping behavior and
tobacco consumption (Escario & Wilkinson, 2015), and well-being altruism (Rand, Brescoll, Everett, Capraro, & Barcelo, 2016).
(Carlsson, Lampi, Li, & Martinsson, 2014). Compared with men, women exhibit a stronger ethic of care and are
Despite such interest in both intergenerational transmission and more socialized to be other-oriented and socially responsible, which
environmental attitudes, few studies consider their combination, may motivate them to be more worried about environmental issues
that is, the intergenerational transmission of environmental atti- and develop greater environmental concern than men (Zelezny et al.,
tudes (cf. Grnhj & Thgersen, 2009; Leppa nen et al., 2012; 2000). Taking all these factors into account, we propose:
Meeusen, 2014). We seek to address this gap by investigating
H2. Children's environmental concern is greater among girls than
environmental concern, which in Meeusens (2014) terminology
among boys.
represents a postmaterialist attitude, in the sense that this is a
concern that moves beyond simply economic or physical security In addition, men and women may process information differ-
issues. Dunlap and Jones (2002) regard environmental concern as a ently (e.g. Wolin, 2003). In this line, in a meta-analysis of 148
broad concept, reecting the degree to which people are aware of studies, Eagly and Carli (1981) found that women are slightly more
environmental problems, support efforts to solve the problems, and persuadable than men are. This may be explained by the fact that
are willing to contribute personally to the solution. Despite some whereas men tend to be more assertive (Schmitt et al., 2008),
different perspectives on environmental concerns and attitudes in women consider others' opinions and social information to a
previous literature, environmental concern generally is regarded as greater extent (Sun & Zhang, 2006; Venkatesh & Morris, 2000),
attitudinal (Rhead et al., 2015; Vining & Ebreo, 1992), which makes potentially because women are more oriented toward social re-
it an appropriate variable for the current study. lationships and motivated by afliation needs (Belanche et al.,
2012; Eagly & Carli, 1981; Venkatesh & Morris, 2000). As
2.2. Intergenerational association of environmental concern Guadagno and Cialdini (2002) note, while men often attempt to
demonstrate one's independence from others (for example,
Children's concern about environmental issues could be through assertiveness in their interactions); women are more
, J.-J. Escario / Journal of Environmental Psychology 48 (2016) 65e74
L.V. Casalo 67

communally oriented, so that their conversations are meant to (Duarte et al., 2016) and second generation immigrants (Coertjens
achieve closeness and consensus. Taking all these into account in et al., 2010) are more pro-environmental than natives.
our research context, it is reasonable to expect that girls' concern As perhaps the most relevant reference group for children
may be more affected by their parents' concern. Therefore, we (Sancho et al., 2011), the family's sociodemographic characteristics
propose: (e.g., socioeconomic status, cultural possessions, education level)
also might affect environmental concerns (Coertjens et al., 2010;
H3. The association between parents' environmental concern and
Duarte et al., 2016). Specically, while the relationship between
children's environmental concern is greater for girls than for boys.
socioeconomic status and environmental concern offers mixed
We focus on this moderating effect in more detail. Following results (Shen & Saijo, 2008), previous studies suggest that educa-
gender schema theory (Bem, 1984), we posit that the parentechild tion level increases pro-environmental orientation (e.g. Scott &
relationship might be affected by the sex of both parent and child. Willits, 1994).
The inuence of the father and the mother thus may differ, Finally, previous literature (e.g., Ballantyne, Fien, & Packer,
depending on the sex of the child. Different predictions about these 2001) shows that informative campaigns at school also may posi-
varying inuences are available in prior literature. For example, in tively affect children's attitudes toward environmental issues.
the parents' equivalent socialization hypothesis, both parents exert Finally, school management (public vs. private) may also inuence
the same inuence on sons and daughters, but the gender-specic environmental attitudes and previous studies (Duarte et al., 2016)
socialization hypothesis indicates that socialization differs already note that, relative to private schools, students in public
depending on the sex of both the parent and the child (Kulik, 2002). schools show a more pro-environmental attitude.
In this latter case, either parents have more inuence on a child of These variables all might relate to children's environmental
the same sex or they have more inuence on a child of the opposite concern, beyond the effects of the parents' environmental concern,
sex. Other proposals also suggest that mothers exert the dominant so we postulate:
inuence (Acock & Bengtson, 1978). For this study, we adopt the
H5. Socioeconomic and school characteristics are associated with
gender-specic socialization hypothesis and propose that parents
children's environmental concern.
have more inuence on a child of the same sex. If a teenager per-
ceives similarities with the information source (i.e., father or
mother), he or she may consider the opinions of that source as 3. Materials and methods
more congruent with his or her own personal values (Casalo ,
Flavi
an, & Guinalu, 2013), which would prompt greater interper- 3.1. Participants
sonal inuence. In studies that demonstrate the importance of the
source of information (Dou, Walden, Lee, & Lee, 2012; Vermeulen & The participants for this study (n 95,008) were children aged
Seegers, 2009), sex congruency between the sender and the 15 years who participated in the PISA 2006 data survey. The data
receiver generally appears to increase perceived trust in the sender came from children in 16 countries for which parent questionnaires
(Jordan, 2015), which then enhances persuasion, internalization of from the same survey also were available: Bulgaria (n 4498),
the message, and intentions to follow the sender's advice (Casalo , Colombia (n 4478), Croatia (n 5213), Denmark (n 4532),
Flavi
an, & Guinalu, 2011). Therefore, taking sex-oriented congru- Germany (n 4891), Hong Kong (n 4645), Iceland (n 3789),
ence between the parent and the child into account, we propose: Italy (n 21,773), Korea (n 5176), Luxembourg (n 4567),
Macao-China (n 4760), New Zealand (n 4823), Poland
H4a. The association between a mother's environmental concern
(n 5547), Portugal (n 5109), Qatar (n 6265), and Turkey
and her daughter's environmental concern is greater than the as-
(n 4942). The sample includes 47,810 female children (50.33%)
sociation between a father's environmental concern and his
and 47,198 male children (49.67%). In terms of immigration status,
daughter's environmental concern.
the breakdown among the 92,412 participants who answered the
H4b. The association between a father's environmental concern pertinent question was as follows: native (85.67%, n 79,174),
and his son's environmental concern is greater than the association native rst-generation (7.97%, n 7366), and immigrant (6.35%,
between a mother's environmental concern and her son's envi- n 5872). The parents of these children had relatively similar
ronmental concern. educational levels, such that about one-quarter had earned uni-
versity degrees (25.06% for mothers, 26.62% for fathers). The 87,756
students who indicated the types of schools they attended indi-
2.4. Role of socio-economic and school characteristics cated the following distribution: private (4.72%, n 4144), private
and dependent on government support (14.17%, n 12,438), and
Because most people grow up in families, and school is an public (81%, n 71,114).
important part of children's lives, these social contexts cannot be The survey also contains information about individual, family,
neglected when evaluating environmental concern (Coertjens, and socioeconomic characteristics, which we can link to the school
Boeve-de Pauw, de Maeyer, & van Petegem, 2010). We investigate characteristics. The two-stage stratied sample design required a
socioeconomic characteristics of the family (socioeconomic index, minimum response rate of 80% of selected children in participating
parents' education) and the child (native vs. immigrant), as well as schools (OECD, 2009, pp. 64e83). The nal PISA 2006 subsample
school characteristics (information provided at school, school for the selected countries, after excluding observations for which
management [public vs. private]), as potentially relevant to the the survey did not provide information about the sex of the parent,
concern formation process. consisted of 70,041 children.
First, previous studies suggest that environmental concerns and
attitudes are affected by individual socioeconomic characteristics 3.2. Measures
(Shen & Saijo, 2008) and whether the person lives in her or his
native country or is an immigrant (Coertjens et al., 2010). In this The dependent variable, an individual environmental concern
respect, although previous studies suggested that immigrants ex- index (ECI), was measured with six items (see Table 1) included in
press similar environmental attitudes as compared to native resi- the student questionnaire for PISA 2006 (p. 19, Q24). Children
dents (e.g. Hunter, 2000), recent ndings shows that both rst indicated whether they considered six topics as being of serious
68 , J.-J. Escario / Journal of Environmental Psychology 48 (2016) 65e74
L.V. Casalo

Table 1
Environmental concern.

Do you see the environmental issues as a This is not a serious This is a serious concern only This is a serious concern for other people This is a serious concern for me
serious concern for yourself and/or others? concern to anyone % for people in other countries % in my country but not me personally % personally as well as others %

Students
a) Air pollution 1.55 4.66 13.75 80.04
b) Energy shortage 2.52 11.38 24.54 61.56
c) Extinction of plants and animals 3.63 12.43 23.27 60.67
d) Clearing of forests for other land use 2.84 14.51 24.12 58.53
e) Water shortages 2.03 15.87 14.13 67.97
f) Nuclear waste 4.40 20.98 25.26 49.35
Parents
a) Air pollution 0.55 1.69 6.31 91.45
b) Energy shortage 1.18 5.05 10.61 83.15
c) Extinction of plants and animals 1.75 6.74 13.49 78.02
d) Clearing of forests for other land use 1.42 8.77 12.23 77.57
e) Water shortages 0.92 7.90 7.39 83.79
f) Nuclear waste 1.59 10.13 10.89 77.38

concern: air pollution, energy shortages, extinction of plants and Table 2


animals, clearing of forests for other land use, water shortages, and Cronbach's alpha and mean of the environmental concern index by sex.

nuclear waste. Their possible responses, matched to a 0e3 scale, Sample M SD n Cronbach's alpha
were This is not a serious concern for anyone; This is a serious All 14.67 3.34 90,379 0.80
concern only for people in other countries; This is a serious Girls 14.98 3.20 45,513 0.78
concern for other people in my country but not for me personally; Boys 14.36 3.65 44,866 0.82
and This is a serious concern for me personally as well as others.
Each respondent's ECI thus was the sum of the points obtained
across all six responses, from 0 (the child believes each topic is not a extends beyond an ordinary linear regression that cannot account
serious concern for anyone) to 18 (the child considers all six topics for nested individual observations within groups. That is, an ordi-
as serious concerns for the self and others). The Cronbach's alpha nary linear regression assumes that individual observations are
values for the ECI in Table 2 reveal acceptable values above the independent, instead of acknowledging that responses from
threshold of 0.7 in all cases (Cronbach, 1970). members of the same group might be more similar than would be
To measure the ECI for parents, the parent questionnaire of the expected by chance. This correlation biases the standard errors
PISA 2006 survey included the same six topics, so we computed toward zero, leading to overestimated t-ratios and thus erroneous,
their ECI in the same way. Unfortunately though, each parent overly optimistic conclusions about the statistical signicance of
questionnaire was answered by only one parent; we do have not the independent variables.
information for both parents. Ideally, we would estimate the model with data about the par-
To evaluate intergenerational association of environmental ents' ECI in the past to avoid the potential for reverse causal effects.
concern, we also must control for the broad set of physical, family, However, the lack of such information in the surveys left us with no
and school characteristics that we outlined previously. Table 3 choice but to relate the environmental concern of parents and their
contains the covariates for these variables, including their means children using contemporaneous data, with the reasonable
and standard deviations. assumption that most inuences ow from parents to children. The
statistical analyses were implemented with Stata software.

3.3. Statistical analyses


4. Results
Our empirical model for the ECI of child i attending school s in
country c (ECIisc) relies on a linear function of the parent's ECI In Table 1 we present the frequencies that correspond to the six
(PECIisc), together with a group of control variables related to in- items used to compute the ECI. In general, children show a
dividual, family, and school characteristics (Xisc): moderately high degree of concern about the environment, such
that about 80% (n 74,312 of 92,843) were seriously concerned

ECIisc f(PECIisc, Xisc) usc uc uisc dPECIisc bXisc usc uc uisc,

where uisc represents the unobservable inuences on the ECI of with air pollution, and 68% (n 63,023 of 92,723) were seriously
child i attending school s in country c. The hierarchical data concerned with water shortages. However, less than half (49%,
prompted us to introduce school-specic (usc) and country-specic n 45,509 of 92,214) noted concern about nuclear waste. Parents
(uc) intercepts. To estimate the effect of the covariates, we used a reported higher levels of concern on all topics, such that more than
multivariate, multilevel regression in which specic intercepts are 77% (n 57,994 of 74,943) were seriously concerned about all six
included as random intercepts (Boeve-de Pauw & van Petegem, environment topics. Similar to their children, air pollution was the
2010; Rabe-Hesketh & Skrondal, 2005). This multilevel regression top focus of parents: More than 91% (n 68,724 of 75,146) reported
can handle non-independence due to group membership and thus it as a serious concern. According to the descriptive analyses in
, J.-J. Escario / Journal of Environmental Psychology 48 (2016) 65e74
L.V. Casalo 69

Table 3
Variable denitions and descriptive statistics.

Variable Denition M (SD)

Concern Environmental concern index (ECI) for children 14.67


(3.44)
Concern of Parent ECI for parent (mother or father) who answered the survey 16.38
(2.72)
Concern of Mother ECI for the mother 16.48
(2.630)
Concern of Father ECI for the father 16.35
(2.76)
Sex Equal to 1 if the child is a boy and 0 if a girl 0.50
(0.50)
Native (Omitted category) Equal to 1 if the child is a native student (born in the country of assessment or with at least one parent born in the country) 0.86
and 0 otherwise (0.35)
Native rst-G Equal to 1 if the child is a rst-generation resident (born in the country of assessment but whose parent(s) were born in 0.08
another country) and 0 otherwise (0.27)
Immigrant (Non-native) Equal to 1 if the child is not native (born outside the country of assessment and whose parents were also born in another 0.06
country) and 0 otherwise (0.24)
Socio Economic Statusa (ESCS) Socioeconomic index provided by PISA 2006 survey and labeled Economic, Social and Cultural Status (ESCS). 0.18
(1.08)
Cultural Possessions (CULTPOSS) Cultural possession index provided by PISA 2006 survey and labeled Cultural Possessions (CULTPOSS). 1.80
1.34)
b
Primary Studies Mother (Omitted Equal to 1 if the mother has ISCED0 or ISCED1 level and 0 otherwise 0.16
category) (0.37)
Secondary Studies Motherb Equal to 1 if the mother has ISCED2, ISCED3B or ISCED3C level and 0 otherwise 0.29
(0.46)
Postsecondary Studies Motherb Equal to 1 if the mother has ISCED3A or ISCED4 level and 0 otherwise 0.29
(0.45)
University Studies Motherb Equal to 1 if the mother has ISCED5A, ISCED5B or ISCED6 level and 0 otherwise 0.25
(0.43)
Primary Studies Fatherb (Omitted Equal to 1 if the mother has ISCED0 or ISCED1 level and 0 otherwise 0.15
category) (0.35)
Secondary Studies Fatherb Equal to 1 if the mother has ISCED2, ISCED3B or ISCED3C level and 0 otherwise 0.29
(0.46)
Postsecondary Studies Fatherb Equal to 1 if the mother has ISCED3A or ISCED4 level and 0 otherwise 0.30
(0.46)
University Studies Fatherb Equal to 1 if the mother has ISCED5A, ISCED5B or ISCED6 level and 0 otherwise 0.27
(0.44)
Private (Omitted category) Equal to 1 if the school is private and 0 otherwise 0.05
(0.21)
Private Dependent Equal to 1 if the school is private government-dependent and 0 otherwise 0.14
(0.35)
Public Equal to 1 if the school is public and 0 otherwise 0.81
(0.39)
InfoSchoolAir Equal to 1 if school is the main source of information about air pollution for the child and 0 otherwise. 0.71
(0.45)
InfoSchoolForest Equal to 1 if school is the main source of information about clearing of forests and other land uses for the child and 0.64
0 otherwise. (0.48)
InfoSchoolWater Equal to 1 if school is the main source of information about water shortages for the child and 0 otherwise. 0.58
(0.49)
InfoSchoolNuclear Equal to 1 if school is the main source of information about nuclear waste for the child and 0 otherwise. 0.52
(0.50)
a
For technical aspects, see the PISA 2006 Technical Report (OECD, 2009, p. 346).
b
PISA 2006 used the International Standard Classication of Education (ISCED) developed by UNESCO to facilitate comparisons of education statistics and indicators across
countries, on the basis of uniform and internationally agreed denitions. Using this denition (http://www.uis.unesco.org/Library/Documents/isced97-en.pdf), we specied
the parent education variables, following the codication in the table as, Primary Studies (primary education or less), Secondary Studies (secondary education), Post-secondary
Studies (post-secondary, non-tertiary education), and University Studies (tertiary education).

Tables 2 and 3, children reported an approximately 10% lower ECI When we divided the samples by sex, as in the second and third
(M 14.67, SD 3.44) than their parents (M 16.38, SD 2.72). columns, we found support for H3. Concretely, the estimated co-
Furthermore, among parents, the ECI was slightly higher for efcients for the intergenerational effects were 0.17 (95% con-
mothers (M 16.48, SD 2.63) than for fathers (M 16.35, dence interval [CI]: 0.16, 0.19) for boys and 0.25 (95% CI: 0.23, 0.26)
SD 2.76). for girls. That is, the effect for girls was approximately 43% higher
The estimated results in Table 4 depict how the random in- than that for boys. Both CIs exclude the coefcient value obtained
tercepts for schools and countries have signicant variances. The for the other sex, in further support for H3.
results in the rst column show that parents' environmental Next, we reestimated the model for the full sample, the female
concern was positively and signicantly associated with their sample, and the male sample, while introducing parents' environ-
children's concern. A child's ECI increased by approximately 0.21 mental concern separately for mothers and fathers (Table 5). The
for each unit increase in her or his parents' ECI. These estimates intergenerational association was nearly identical for both parents
provide support for H1. With regard to H2, boys expressed less when we considered the full sample, but we found some small
concern, with a negative, signicant coefcient of 0.42, in support differences across sexes. Specically, girls' environmental concern
of our prediction. was more strongly inuenced by their mothers and fathers
70 , J.-J. Escario / Journal of Environmental Psychology 48 (2016) 65e74
L.V. Casalo

Table 4
Multilevel estimates for intergenerational transmissions of environmental concern.

Variable All (n 52,840) Girls (n 27,676) Boys (n 25,164)

B SE B p B SE B p B SE B p

Concern of Parent 0.21 *** (0.01) 0.00 0.24 *** (0.01) 0.00 0.17 *** (0.01) 0.00
Sex 0.42 *** (0.03) 0.00 0.00 (.) . 0.00 (.) .
Native (Omitted category) e e e e e e e e e
Native rst-G. 0.03 (0.06) 0.60 0.02 (0.07) 0.81 0.09 (0.09) 0.31
Immigrant 0.18 *** (0.06) 0.00 0.09 (0.08) 0.26 0.25 *** (0.09) 0.01
Socio Economic Status 0.08 *** (0.02) 0.00 0.11 *** (0.03) 0.00 0.06 (0.04) 0.13
Cultural Possessions 0.23 *** (0.02) 0.00 0.23 *** (0.02) 0.00 0.22 *** (0.03) 0.00
Secondary Studies Mother 0.05 (0.05) 0.28 0.03 (0.06) 0.58 0.07 (0.07) 0.37
Primary Studies Mother (Omitted category) e e e e e e e e e
Postsecondary Studies Mother 0.13 ** (0.05) 0.01 0.09 (0.07) 0.19 0.18 ** (0.08) 0.03
University Studies Mother 0.10 (0.06) 0.11 0.07 (0.08) 0.35 0.12 (0.09) 0.21
Primary Studies Father (Omitted category) e e e e e e e e e
Secondary Studies Father 0.01 (0.05) 0.86 0.08 (0.06) 0.21 0.05 (0.07) 0.48
Postsecondary Studies Father 0.02 (0.05) 0.73 0.11 * (0.07) 0.09 0.08 (0.08) 0.33
University Studies Father 0.04 (0.06) 0.48 0.06 (0.08) 0.45 0.14 (0.10) 0.15
Private (Omitted category) e e e e e e e e e
PrivateDependent 0.08 (0.09) 0.36 0.13 (0.11) 0.22 0.00 (0.13) 0.98
Public 0.29 *** (0.08) 0.00 0.28 *** (0.10) 0.01 0.29 ** (0.12) 0.02
InfoSchoolAir 0.18 *** (0.03) 0.00 0.15 *** (0.04) 0.00 0.21 *** (0.05) 0.00
InfoSchoolForest 0.10 *** (0.03) 0.00 0.05 (0.04) 0.24 0.15 *** (0.05) 0.00
InfoSchoolWater 0.19 *** (0.03) 0.00 0.17 *** (0.04) 0.00 0.21 *** (0.05) 0.00
InfoSchoolNuclear 0.24 *** (0.03) 0.00 0.27 *** (0.04) 0.00 0.19 *** (0.05) 0.00
Intercept 10.86 *** (0.34) 0.00 10.22 *** (0.34) 0.00 11.06 *** (0.38) 0.00
Variance random Intercept
School level 0.14 *** (0.02) 0.14 *** (0.21) 0.18 *** (0.03)
Country level 1.35 *** (0.51) 1.28 *** (0.49) 1.46 *** (0.56)

Notes: The variable denitions are available in Table 3.


*p < 0.1. **p < 0.05. ***p < 0.01.

Table 5
Multilevel estimates for intergenerational transmissions of environmental concern (father and mother concern separately).

Variable All (n 52,840) Girls (n 27,676) Boys (n 25,164)

B SE B p B SE B p B SE B p

Concern of Mother 0.21 *** (0.01) 0.00 0.25 *** (0.01) 0.00 0.17 *** (0.01) 0.00
Concern of Father 0.21 *** (0.01) 0.00 0.24 *** (0.01) 0.00 0.17 *** (0.01) 0.00
Sex 0.42 *** (0.03) 0.00 0.00 (.) . 0.00 (.) .
Native (Omitted category) e e e e e e e e e
Native rst-G. 0.03 (0.06) 0.61 0.02 (0.07) 0.83 0.09 (0.09) 0.32
Immigrant 0.18 *** (0.06) 0.00 0.09 (0.08) 0.24 0.25 *** (0.09) 0.01
Socio Economic Status 0.08 *** (0.02) 0.00 0.11 *** (0.03) 0.00 0.06 (0.04) 0.13
Cultural Possessions 0.23 *** (0.02) 0.00 0.23 *** (0.02) 0.00 0.22 *** (0.03) 0.00
Primary Studies Mother (Omitted category) e e e e e e e e e
Secondary Studies Mother 0.05 (0.05) 0.28 0.04 (0.06) 0.52 0.06 (0.07) 0.41
Postsecondary Studies Mother 0.13 ** (0.05) 0.01 0.10 (0.07) 0.15 0.17 ** (0.08) 0.04
University Studies Mother 0.10 (0.06) 0.11 0.09 (0.08) 0.28 0.11 (0.10) 0.26
Primary Studies Father (Omitted category) e e e e e e e e e
Secondary Studies Father 0.01 (0.05) 0.86 0.08 (0.06) 0.19 0.06 (0.07) 0.45
Postsecondary Studies Father 0.02 (0.05) 0.73 0.12 * (0.07) 0.07 0.09 (0.08) 0.29
University Studies Father 0.04 (0.06) 0.49 0.07 (0.08) 0.37 0.15 (0.10) 0.13
Private (Omitted category) e e e e e e e e e
PrivateDependent 0.08 (0.09) 0.36 0.14 (0.11) 0.22 0.00 (0.13) 0.98
Public 0.29 *** (0.08) 0.00 0.28 *** (0.10) 0.01 0.29 ** (0.12) 0.02
InfoSchoolAir 0.18 *** (0.03) 0.00 0.15 *** (0.04) 0.00 0.21 *** (0.05) 0.00
InfoSchoolForest 0.10 *** (0.03) 0.00 0.05 (0.04) 0.24 0.15 *** (0.05) 0.00
InfoSchoolWater 0.19 *** (0.03) 0.00 0.17 *** (0.04) 0.00 0.21 *** (0.05) 0.00
InfoSchoolNuclear 0.24 *** (0.03) 0.00 0.27 *** (0.04) 0.00 0.19 *** (0.05) 0.00
Intercept 10.86 *** (0.34) 0.00 10.22 *** (0.34) 0.00 11.06 *** (0.37) 0.00
Variance random Intercept
School level 0.14 *** (0.11) 0.14 *** (0.02) 0.18 *** (0.03)
Country level 1.35 *** (0.51) 1.28 *** 0.49 1.46 *** (0.55)

Notes: The variable denitions are available in Table 3.


*p < 0.1. **p < 0.05. ***p < 0.01.

(coefcients of 0.25 and 0.24, respectively) than boys' environ- inuenced by their mothers than by their fathers, and boys' envi-
mental concern (coefcients of 0.170 and 0.173, respectively). ronmental concern was slightly more affected by their fathers than
However, girls' environmental concern also was slightly more by their mothers. These results are in line with H4a and H4b; the
, J.-J. Escario / Journal of Environmental Psychology 48 (2016) 65e74
L.V. Casalo 71

intergenerational inuences on both girls and boys were greater for Finally, the coefcient estimates for the parentechildren envi-
the parent of the same sex. However, the differences were not ronmental concern association by country revealed results consis-
statistically signicant. tent with those in Tables 4 and 5 All estimates provided positive,
To address our last hypothesis, we included socioeconomic and signicant coefcients, with the exception of Denmark. Moreover,
school characteristics in the model. The socioeconomic status and most of countries indicated coefcients near the estimate obtained
cultural possessions variables were signicant for both subsamples. using the full sample (0.21). In addition, the intergenerational as-
In the former case, the association was negative, such that a higher sociation of environmental concern was very similar for mothers
score on the socioeconomic index entailed a lower ECI score. In and fathers. These consistent results offer support for the robust-
contrast, cultural possessions revealed a positive link, such that a ness of our ndings for the full sample.
higher score on this index was associated with a higher ECI score.
The inuence of the socioeconomic index was also greater for girls 5. Discussion
than for boys (i.e., e0.11 and 0.06, respectively; see Tables 4 and
5), but the inuence of the cultural possessions index was about Various studies analyze the intergenerational transmission of
the same for both subsamples (0.23 and 0.22, respectively). We attitudes and behaviors, but without really considering the trans-
found no signicant differences in children's environmental mission of environmental attitudes from parents to children (cf.
concern between those studying in private government-dependent Grnhj & Thgersen, 2009; Leppa nen et al., 2012; Meeusen, 2014).
versus private schools, but students in public schools revealed We focus on the relationship between parents' environmental
slightly higher ECI values, with a positive, signicant coefcient in concern and the environmental concern of their children by con-
all estimations. Finally, the four variables we included to measure structing an environmental concern index, based on PISA 2006 data
the effect of environmental information provided at school were survey across 16 countries. We also use a multivariate, multilevel
signicant, with positive coefcients. regression approach with two random intercepts for the school and
In Table 6, we reproduce some analyses by country. The means country levels. This approach enables us to obtain more robust
and standard deviations of the ECI reveal some differences. In standard errors as the hypothesis of independent observations
particular, countries with lower ECI levels included Iceland, among schools and countries may not hold true (Rabe-Hesketh &
Denmark, New Zealand, and Qatar (<14), whereas Turkey, Skrondal, 2005).
Colombia, and Portugal all indicated higher levels (>16). It is dif- These data showed that parents reported higher environmental
cult to explain these differences precisely, though Inglehart (1995) concern, with an ECI that is around 10% higher than the ECI of their
posits that the degree of pollution in the country and the shift from adolescent children. Previous literature has suggested that young
materialist to postmaterialist values have important effects on people hold more positive attitudes toward environmental issues
environmental concern. but are less inclined to follow up with behavior than older

Table 6
Analyses by country.

Country Descriptive Estimates model Table 4 Estimates model Table 5

M SD Concern of B SE B p Concern of B SE B p

Bulgaria 15.55 2.98 Parent 0.16 *** (0.02) 0.00 Mother 0.16 *** 0.02 0.00
Father 0.17 *** (0.02 0.00
Colombia 16.39 2.74 Parent 0.36 *** (0.03) 0.00 Mother 0.36 *** (0.03) 0.00
Father 0.37 *** (0.03) 0.00
Croatia 15.77 2.75 Parent 0.20 *** (0.03) 0.00 Mother 0.20 *** (0.03) 0.00
Father 0.20 *** (0.03) 0.00
Denmark 12.99 3.69 Parent 0.02 (0.03) 0.49 Mother 0.02 (0.03) 0.50
Father 0.02 (0.03) 0.65
Germany 14.62 3.21 Parent 0.17 *** (0.03) 0.00 Mother 0.17 *** (0.03) 0.00
Father 0.17 *** (0.03) 0.00
Hong(Kong 14.18 3.43 Parent 0.24 *** (0.01) 0.00 Mother 0.24 *** (0.01) 0.00
Father 0.23 *** (0.02) 0.00
Iceland 12.41 4.12 Parent 0.08 *** (0.03) 0.00 Mother 0.08 *** (0.03) 0.00
Father 0.07 ** (0.03) 0.01
Italy 14.69 3.09 Parent 0.24 *** (0.01) 0.00 Mother 0.24 *** (0.01) 0.00
Father 0.24 *** (0.01) 0.00
Korea 14.80 2.64 Parent 0.16 *** (0.02) 0.00 Mother 0.16 *** (0.02) 0.00
Father 0.16 *** (0.02) 0.00
Luxembourg 14.17 3.79 Parent 0.18 *** (0.03) 0.00 Mother 0.18 *** (0.03) 0.00
Father 0.17 *** (0.03) 0.00
Macao(China 14.54 3.68 Parent 0.19 *** (0.02) 0.00 Mother 0.20 *** (0.02) 0.00
Father 0.19 *** (0.02) 0.00
New Zealand 13.15 3.79 Parent 0.20 *** (0.03) 0.00 Mother 0.20 *** (0.03) 0.00
Father 0.20 *** (0.03) 0.00
Polanda 14.39 3.41 Parent n. a. n. a. (n. a.) n. a. Mother n. a. n. a. (n. a.) n. a.
Father n. a. n. a. (n. a.) n. a.
Portugal 16.23 2.54 Parent 0.20 *** (0.02) 0.00 Mother 0.20 *** (0.02) 0.00
Father 0.20 *** (0.02) 0.00
Qatar 13.44 4.04 Parent 0.26 *** (0.02) 0.00 Mother 0.25 *** (0.02) 0.00
Father 0.27 *** (0.02) 0.00
Turkey 16.85 2.46 Parent 0.33 *** (0.02) 0.00 Mother 0.33 *** (0.02) 0.00
Father 0.33 *** (0.02) 0.00

*p < 0.1. **p < 0.05. ***p < 0.01.


a
No information was available about the environmental concern of parents of Polish children.
72 , J.-J. Escario / Journal of Environmental Psychology 48 (2016) 65e74
L.V. Casalo

generations are; some researchers instead have found that young ndings offer some support for both the gender-specic socializ-
people exhibit less pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors ation hypothesis and predictions of parents' equivalent socializ-
(Grnhj & Thgersen, 2009). Our results are in line with this latter ation. For girls, the effect of the mother is slightly stronger, and for
nding. The participants for this study were 15 years of age, a stage boys, the effect of the father is slightly stronger. But at the same
at which attitudes rarely are well-developed and beliefs tend to be time, the effects of both parents are quantitatively similar for girls
less stable (Hess, 1994; Sears, 1986). Therefore, their environmental and for boys, but different across boys and girls (being higher for
attitudes and concerns may still be forming and subject to many girls). The quantitative differences related to the effects of both
inuences. In addition, our ECI might be interpreted as a proxy for parents for a child of the same sex thus are not signicant.
behavioral intentions, such that it would be closer to a behavior School socialization is also an important factor for the formation
than an attitude measure. Even though it is attitudinal in nature, of children's environmental concern. That is, informative cam-
concern includes an affective component (Rhead et al., 2015) that paigns about environmental problems can increase environmental
may increase willingness to contribute to the solution of environ- concern among children, in line with Duarte et al.s (2016) nding
mental problems (Dunlap & Jones, 2002). The reports of lower that school variables can signicantly explain differences in an
environmental concern thus are consistent with young people's environmental attitude index. Policy makers might exploit the
relatively minimal environmental behaviors (Grnhj & Thgersen, synergies revealed by our study, especially considering that school-
2009). based informative campaigns will have lasting effects by increasing
The positive but moderate association between environmental the environmental concern of both current students and subse-
concern of children and parents suggests that parents are impor- quent generations, when these students spread their concern to
tant socialization agents. This association is slightly lower than a their children through intergenerational socialization processes
quarter and it is quite similar to others that have been found in the (Meeusen, 2014). Environmental campaigns at school could also
literature (Meeusen, 2014). Consequently, we conrm H1, which is reach improved results by integrating the information into a more
in line with the impressionable years theory (Krosnick & Alwin, general community effort that includes teachers and parents as
1989). Younger people have less stable beliefs and are more sus- additional campaign targets (Barle s, Escario, & Galbe, 2014). On the
ceptible to others' inuences, including family (Dekovic, Wissink, & other hand, we observe that students in public schools reported
Meijer, 2004). Therefore, policy makers can inuence children with higher levels of environmental concern. Although we may assume
informative campaigns targeted either directly to them or to their that public schools could be more involved in environmental topics
parents, who positively affect the environmental concern of their or implement public policies regarding environmental issues more
children through intergenerational socialization. However, as pre- promptly, understanding the role of school type in developing
vious studies note, this association may differ across families, environmental concern and attitudes deserves more attention in
depending on aspects such as the degree of communication within future research. Several limitations also emerge from this study,
the family. Parents with similar environmental concern might in- largely due to data issues. First, we used cross-sectional survey data
uence their children with varying intensities, depending on obtained from 15-year-old children, with the assumption that the
whether they are talkative and persuasive or not (Meeusen, 2014). environmental concern relationship features some causal effect
Moreover, cultural or educational characteristics that parents and from parents' environmental concern to their children, even
their children already share could inuence their environmental though some reverse causal inuence could also be possible
concern in the same direction, increasing the family-level similar- (Vaughan, Gack, Solorazano, & Ray, 2003). Longitudinal data could
ity. A deeper understanding of which factors motivate stronger clarify these causal inuences. Second, the PISA 2006 survey we
intergenerational transmissions thus would be an interesting used lacks some important information, such as parents' existing
extension to our study. environmental attitudes and behaviors, how much time parents
We also provide new evidence of sex differences in environ- spend with their children, and how much they discuss or exchange
mental concern, such that girls are more concerned than boys ideas. Such information would offer a deeper understanding of the
(Boeve-de Pauw & van Petegem, 2010; Coertjens et al., 2010; Duarte relationship of parents' and children's environmental concern
et al., 2016). This consistent result suggests several explanations. As (Meeusen, 2014). These PISA 2006 data, obtained from a survey
Arnocky and Stroink (2010) note, sex differences in environmental developed by the OECD (2009), were also developed for a purpose
concern are fully mediated by differences in emotional empathy, other than our study; we did not participate in the development of
such that women are more empathic than men. Similarly, Belanche appropriate measurement scales for the variables we considered.
et al. (2015) nd evidence that women are more caring for others Third, it might be interesting to analyze the inuence of ethnicity,
and about the quality of life. It is reasonable to predict that more nationality, or culture in more detail. We considered the cultural
caring people are also more concerned about environmental issues. possession index provided by PISA, but further research might
The intergenerational association effect is also greater for girls evaluate the inuence of cultural attitudes, norms, and practices
than for boys. The nding that girls are more inuenced by parents' across the different countries (for example, socialization of women
concern is in line with literature that predicts sex differences in is not xed as it may vary culturally or by country) or whether
personality traits (Schmitt et al., 2008; Sun & Zhang, 2006), such parenting across diverse cultural groups affects the intergenera-
that women tend to be more oriented to social relationships and tional transmission of environmental concern. However, as the rst
open to others' opinions. Female children then might be more study to investigate the relationship between parents' environ-
prone to discuss and talk about issues with their parents, leading mental concern and the environmental concern of their children,
them to gain better understanding of their parents' environmental using homogeneous, nationally representative survey data for 16
concern, with a stronger resulting inuence than occurs for boys. countries, we provide an initial exploration of these topics. More-
This nding also resonates with the previously introduced notion over, we acknowledge the clustered nature of these data and
that communication patterns within the family affect the inter- therefore took intraclass correlations within schools and countries
generational transmission of environmental concern, such that into account, using a hierarchical multilevel regression approach.
families with boys might communicate differently than families
with girls. 6. Conclusion
However, the parental sex patterns related to the intergenera-
tional association of environmental concern are less clear. Our This exploratory study, using nationally representative data
, J.-J. Escario / Journal of Environmental Psychology 48 (2016) 65e74
L.V. Casalo 73

surveys across several countries, reveals that the environmental Dekovic, M., Wissink, I. B., & Meijer, A. M. (2004). The role of family and peer re-
lations in adolescent antisocial behaviour: Comparison of four ethnic groups.
concern of parents relates positively to the environmental concern
Journal of Adolescence, 27(5), 497e514. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
of their children. This association largely afrms the parent j.adolescence.2004.06.010.
equivalent socialization hypothesis, in that it holds for both boys Dou, X., Walden, J. A., Lee, S., & Lee, J. Y. (2012). Does source matter? Examining
and girls. However, girls are more sensitive to intergenerational source effects in online product reviews. Computers in Human Behavior, 28(5),
1555e1563. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2012.03.015.
transmissions from their mothers and fathers than are boys. Duarte, R., Escario, J. J., & Sanagustn, M. V. (2016). The inuence of the family, the
Informative campaigns developed in schools contribute to increase school, and the group on the environmental attitudes of European students.
the environmental concern of children. These two ndings suggest Environmental Education Research. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
13504622.2015.1074660 (in press).
that it is possible to amplify the effects of informative campaigns by Dunlap, D., & Jones, J. (2002). Environmental concern: Conceptual and measure-
including parents as targets. Doing so may increase their environ- ment issues. In D. Dunlap, & M. Michelson (Eds.), Handbook of environmental
mental concern, which then should inuence the environmental sociology (pp. 482e542). London: Greenwood Press.
Dunlap, R. E., Van Liere, K. D., Mertig, A. G., & Jones, R. E. (2000). Measuring
concern of their children. Using data with a more exible structure endorsement of the new ecological paradigm: A revised NEP scale. Journal of
and richer information, further research might control for addi- Social Issues, 56(3), 425e442. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.125.3.367.
tional factors to clarify and conrm the causal mechanisms of the Duriez, B., & Soenens, B. (2009). The intergenerational transmission of racism: The
role of right-wing authoritarianism and social dominance orientation. Journal of
relationship of parents' and children's environmental concern. Research in Personality, 43(5), 906e909. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
j.jrp.2009.05.014.
Acknowledgments Eagly, A. H., & Carli, L. L. (1981). Sex of researchers and sex-typed communications
as determinants of sex differences in inuenceability: A meta-analysis of social
inuence studies. Psychological Bulletin, 90(1), 1e20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
This research did not receive any specic grants from funding 0033-2909.90.1.1.
agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-prot sectors. Else-Quest, N. M., Hyde, J. S., Goldsmith, H. H., & Van Hulle, C. A. (2006). Gender
differences in temperament: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 132(1),
33e72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.33.
References Escario, J. J., & Wilkinson, A. V. (2015). The intergenerational transmission of
smoking across three cohabitant generations: A count data approach. Journal of
Acock, A. C., & Bengtson, V. L. (1978). On the relative inuence of mothers and fa- Community Health, 40(5), 912e919. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10900-015-
thers: A covariance analysis of political and religious socialization. Journal of 0013-5.
Marriage and Family, 40(3), 519e530. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/350932. Feingold, A. (1994). Gender differences in personality: A meta-analysis. Psycholog-
Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human ical Bulletin, 116(3), 429e456. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.116.3.429.
Decision Processes, 50(2), 179e211. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0749-5978(91) Grnhj, A., & Thgersen, J. (2009). Like father, like son? intergenerational trans-
90020-T. mission of values, attitudes, and behaviours in the environmental domain.
Arcury, T. A., & Christianson, E. H. (1990). Environmental worldview in response to Journal of Environmental Psychology, 29(4), 414e421. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
environmental problems. Environment and Behavior, 22, 387e407. http:// j.jenvp.2009.05.002.
dx.doi.org/10.1177/0013916590223004. Guadagno, R. E., & Cialdini, R. B. (2002). Online Persuasion: An examination of
Arnocky, S., & Stroink, M. (2010). Gender differences in environmentalism: The gender differences in computer-mediated interpersonal inuence. Group Dy-
mediating role of emotional empathy. Current Research in Social Psychology, namics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 6(1), 38e51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//
16(9), 1e14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2009.12.008. 1089-2699.6.1.38.
Ballantyne, R., Fien, J., & Packer, J. (2001). Program effectiveness in facilitating Hess, T. M. (1994). Social cognition in adulthood: Aging-related changes in
intergenerational inuence in environmental education: Lessons from the eld. knowledge and processing mechanisms. Developmental Review, 14(4), 373e412.
Journal of Environmental Education, 32(4), 8e14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/drev.1994.1015.
00958960109598657. Hunter, L. M. (2000). A comparison of the environmental attitudes, concern, and
s, M. J., Escario, J. J., & Galbe, J. (2014). Predictors of driving under the inuence
Barle behaviors of native-born and foreign-born U.S. Residents. Environment & Pop-
of alcohol among Spanish adolescents [Predictores de la conduccio n bajo los ulation, 21(6), 565e580. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02436772.
efectos del alcohol entre los adolescentes espan ~ oles]. Adicciones, 26(2), 96e105. Inglehart, R. (1995). Public support for environmental protection: Objective prob-
http://dx.doi.org/10.20882/adicciones.11. lems and subjective values in 43 societies. PS: Political Science & Politics, 28(01),
Belanche, D., Casalo , L. V., & Flavi
an, C. (2012). Understanding the inuence of social 57e72. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/420583.
information sources on e-government adoption. Information Research-an In- Jennings, M. K., Stoker, L., & Bowers, J. (2009). Politics across generations: Family
ternational Electronic Journal, 17(3), 531. http://InformationR.net/ir/17-3/ transmission reexamined. Journal of Politics, 71(3), 782e799. http://dx.doi.org/
paper531.html. 10.1017/S0022381609090719.
Belanche, D., Casalo , L. V., & Guinaliu, M. (2015). The effect of culture in forming e- n de la conanza en equipos de trabajo
Jordan, P. (2015). El papel del lder en la gestio
loyalty intentions: A cross-cultural analysis between Argentina and Spain. virtuales. Unpublished doctoral thesis. Spain: University of Zaragoza.
BRQeBusiness Research Quarterly, 18(4), 275e292. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ Kaiser, F. G., Oerke, B., & Bogner, F. X. (2007). Behavior-based environmental atti-
j.brq.2015.02.003. tude: Development of an instrument for adolescents. Journal of Environmental
Bem, S. L. (1984). Androgyny and gender schema theory: A conceptual and Psychology, 27(3), 242e251. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2007.06.004.
empirical integration. In T. B. Sonderegger (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on moti- Kaiser, F. G., Wo lng, S., & Fuhrer, U. (1999). Environmental attitude and ecological
vation (pp. 179e226). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. behaviour. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 19(1), 1e19. http://dx.doi.org/
Boeve-de Pauw, J. (2011). Valuing the invaluable: Effects of individual, school and 10.1006/jevp.1998.0107.
cultural factors on the environmental values of children. Thesis. Instituut voor Krosnick, J. A., & Alwin, D. F. (1989). Aging and susceptibility to attitude change.
Onderwijs-en Informatiewetenschappen: Universiteit Antwerpen. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(3), 416e425. http://dx.doi.org/10.
Boeve-de Pauw, J., & van Petegem, P. (2010). A cross-national perspective on youth 1037/0022-3514.57.3.416.
environmental attitudes. Environmentalist, 30(2), 133e144. http://dx.doi.org/ Kulik, L. (2002). Like-sex versus opposite-sex effects in transmission of gender role
10.1007/s10669-009-9253-1. ideology from parents to adolescents in Israel. Journal of Youth and Adolescence,
Byrnes, J. P., Miller, D. C., & Schafer, W. D. (1999). Gender differences in risk taking: A 31(6), 451e457. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1020263120774.
meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 125(3), 367e383. http://dx.doi.org/ Leppanen, J. M., Haahla, A. E., Lensu, A. M., & Kuitunen, M. T. (2012). Parent-child
10.1037/0033-2909.125.3.367. similarity in environmental attitudes: A pairwise comparison. Journal of Envi-
Carlsson, F., Lampi, E., Li, W., & Martinsson, P. (2014). Subjective well-being among ronmental Education, 43(3), 162e176.
preadolescents and their parents e Evidence of intergenerational transmission Levine, D. S., & Strube, M. J. (2012). Environmental attitudes, knowledge, intentions
of well-being from urban China. Journal of Socio-Economics, 48, 11e18. http://dx. and behaviors among college students. Journal of Social Psychology, 152(3),
doi.org/10.1016/j.socec.2013.10.003. 308e326. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2011.604363.
Casalo, L. V., Flavi
an, C., & Guinalu, M. (2011). Understanding the intention to follow Maineri, T., Barnett, E., Valdero, T., Unipan, J., & Oskamp, S. (1997). Green buying:
the advice obtained in an online travel community. Computers in Human The inuence of environmental concern on consumer buying. Journal of Social
Behavior, 27(2), 622e633. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2010.04.013. Psychology, 137, 189e204. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224549709595430.
Casalo, L. V., Flavi an, C., & Guinalu, M. (2013). New members' integration: Key Meeusen, C. (2014). The intergenerational transmission of environmental concern:
factor of success in online travel communities. Journal of Business Research, The inuence of parents and communication patterns within the family. Journal
66(6), 706e710. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2011.09.007. of Environmental Education, 45(2), 77e90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
Coertjens, L., Boeve-de Pauw, J., de Maeyer, S., & van Petegem, P. (2010). Do schools 00958964.2013.846290.
make a difference in their students' environmental attitudes and awareness? Mendez, I. (2015). The effect of the intergenerational transmission of noncognitive
Evidence from PISA 2006. International Journal of Science and Mathematics Ed- skills on student performance. Economics of Education Review, 46, 78e97. http://
ucation, 8(3), 497e522. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10763-010-9200-0. dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.econedurev.2015.03.001.
Cronbach, L. (1970). Essentials of psychological testing. New York: Harper and Row. Milfont, T. L., & Duckitt, J. (2010). The environmental attitudes inventory: A valid
74 , J.-J. Escario / Journal of Environmental Psychology 48 (2016) 65e74
L.V. Casalo

and reliable measure to assess the structure of environmental attitudes. Journal 10.1037/0022-3514.94.1.168.
of Environmental Psychology, 30(1), 80e94. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10669- Scott, D., & Willits, F. (1994). Environmental attitude and behavior: A Pennsylvania
008-9192-2. survey. Environment and Behavior, 26, 239e260. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
Noller, P., & Callan, V. (1991). The adolescent in the family. London: Routledge. 001391659402600206.
OECD. (2009). PISA 2006 technical report. Retrieved from http://www.oecd.org/pisa/ Sears, D. O. (1986). College sophomores in the laboratory: Inuences of a narrow
pisaproducts/42025182.pdf. data base on social psychology's view of human nature. Journal of Personality
Rabe-Hesketh, S., & Skrondal, A. (2005). Multilevel and longitudinal modeling using and Social Psychology, 51(3), 515e530. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-
Stata. College Station, TX: Stata Press. 3514.51.3.515.
Rand, D. G., Brescoll, V. L., Everett, J. A. C., Capraro, V., & Barcelo, H. (2016). Social Shen, J., & Saijo, T. (2008). Reexamining the relations between socio-demographic
heuristics and social Roles: Intuition favors altruism for women but not for characteristics and individual environmental concern: Evidence from
men. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(4), 389e396. http://dx.doi. Shanghai data. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 28(1), 42e50. http://
org/10.1037/xge0000154. dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2007.10.003.
Remmerswaal, D., Muris, P., & Huijding, J. (2015). Transmission of cognitive bias and Sun, H., & Zhang, P. (2006). The role of moderating factors in user technology
fear from parents to children: An experimental study. Journal of Clinical Child acceptance. International Journal of Human Computer Studies, 64(2), 53e78.
and Adolescent Psychology, 45(5), 642e654. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijhcs.2005.04.013.
15374416.2014.987378. Vaughan, C., Gack, J., Solorazano, H., & Ray, R. (2003). The effect of environmental
Rhead, R., Elliot, M., & Upham, P. (2015). Assessing the structure of UK environ- education on schoolchildren, their parents, and community members: A study
mental concern and its association with pro-environmental behaviour. Journal of intergenerational and intercommunity learning. Journal of Environmental
of Environmental Psychology, 43, 175e183. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp. Education, 34(3), 12e21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00958960309603489.
2015.06.002. Venkatesh, V., & Morris, M. G. (2000). Why don't men ever stop to ask for di-
Ritter, A. M., Borchardt, M., Vaccaro, G. L. R., Pereira, G. M., & Almeida, F. (2015). rections? Gender, social inuence, and their role in technology acceptance and
Motivations for promoting the consumption of green products in an emerging usage behavior. MIS Quarterly: Management Information Systems, 24(1),
country: Exploring attitudes of Brazilian consumers. Journal of Cleaner Pro- 115e136. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/3250981.
duction, 106(1), 507e520. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jclepro.2014.11.066. Vermeulen, I. E., & Seegers, D. (2009). Tried and tested: The impact of online hotel
Rodrguez-Barreiro, L. M., Ferna ndez-Manzanal, R., Serra, L. M., Carrasquer, J., reviews on consumer consideration. Tourism Management, 30(1), 123e127.
Murillo, M. B., Morales, M. J., et al. (2013). Approach to a causal model between http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tourman.2008.04.008.
attitudes and environmental behaviour. A graduate case study. Journal of Vining, J., & Ebreo, A. (1992). Predicting recycling behavior from global and specic
Cleaner Production, 48, 116e125. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jclepro.2012.09.029. environmental attitudes and changes in recycling opportunities. Journal of
Sancho, F. M., Miguel, M. J., & Alda s, J. (2011). Factors inuencing youth alcohol Applied Social Psychology, 22(20), 1580e1607. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-
consumption intention: An approach from consumer socialization theory. 1816.1992.tb01758.x.
Journal of Social Marketing, 1(3), 192e210. http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/ Wilgenbusch, T., & Merrell, K. W. (1999). Gender differences in self-concept among
20426761111170704. children and adolescents: A meta-analysis of multidimensional studies. School
Schahn, J., & Holzer, E. (1990). Studies of individual environmental concern: The Psychology Quarterly, 14(2), 101e120. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0089000.
role of knowledge, gender and background variables. Environment and Behavior, Wolin, L. D. (2003). Gender issues in advertisingdan oversight synthesis of
22, 767e786. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0013916590226003. research: 1970e2002. Journal of Advertising Research, 43(1), 111e129.
Schmitt, D. P., Realo, A., Voracek, M., & Allik, J. (2008). Why can't a man be more like Zelezny, L. C., Chua, P., & Aldrich, C. (2000). New ways of thinking about environ-
a woman? Sex differences in big ve personality traits across 55 cultures. mentalism: Elaborating on gender differences in environmentalism. Journal of
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(1), 168e182. http://dx.doi.org/ Social Issues, 56(3), 443e457. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/0022-4537.00177.

Вам также может понравиться