Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 16

J Comp Physiol A (1996) 179:169 184 9 Springer-Verlag 1996

R. M. Johnston 9 A. Bekoff

Patterns of muscle activity during different behaviors in chicks:


implications for neural control

Accepted: 6 January 1996

A b s t r a c t The large behavioral repertoire that spans the participate in a synergy to be modified in tandem or on
embryonic and postembryonic stages of development an individual basis. Furthermore, the common syner-
make chicks an ideal system for identifying patterns of gies are established early during prenatal development
muscle activity that are common to different behaviors in chicks.
and those that are behavior-specific. The main goal of
this work was to identify the similar and dissimilar Key words Electromyogram - Kinematics 9
aspects of the recruitment patterns and the regulation Development. Chicken. Locomotion
of muscle activity during three distinct postembryonic
behaviors: walking, swimming and airstepping. We Abbreviations alF anterior iliofibularius 9
identified two synergies that were common to each of CF caudilioflexorius - CNS central nervous
these behaviors. The synergies were not disrupted by system 9 EMG electromyogram 9
the absence of FT1 activity in airstepping. Within each F T femorotibialis 9 GL gastrocnemius lateralis 9
synergy the recruitment time, recruitment order and P1 protraction 1 9 P2 protraction 2 9 plF posterior
duration of activity were not rigid, but varied according iliofibularis 9 R retraction 9 SA sartorius -
to the context-specific resistance that the leg encoun- TA tibialis anterior 9 plF posterior iliofibularis 9
tered. Unlike the other muscles, FT2 activity was not ms millisecond
recruited as part of the same synergy in each behavior.
When weight-bearing contact with the substrate did
not occur, as in swimming and airstepping, as well as in
walking in chicks with deafferented legs, FT2 activity Introduction
was not recruited as part of either synergy, but was
recruited during the time between them. Although not Animals display a wide variety of behaviors using many
identical, embryonic motility and hatching motor pat- of the same muscles (e.g. walking, airstepping, scratch-
tern both show the two synergies described for the ing and paw shake in cats, Giuliani and Smith 1985;
postembryonic behaviors. Like the latter behaviors, the Koshland and Smith 1989; Pratt and Loeb 1991;
synergies tolerated the absence of activity from specific embryonic motility, hatching and walking in chicks,
muscles. Thus, we suggest that the CNS produces dif- Bekoff 1986, Bekoff et al. 1987; walking, searching,
ferent behaviors using many of the same muscles by rocking and grooming in stick insects, B/issler and
organizing the patterned activity around two common Wegner 1983; swimming, jumping and kicking in lo-
synergies while permitting the different muscles that custs, Pfliiger and Burrows 1978; flight and walking in
locusts, Ramirez and Pearson 1988; and different
modes of walking in lobsters, Ayers and Davis 1977;
Ayers and Clarac 1978). In some cases, different behav-
iors show the same general recruitment patterns for
R. M. Johnston (N:~I) 1 . A. Bekoff
Department of Environmental, Population and Organismic Biology muscle activity (i.e. synergies) and only subtle differ-
and The Center for Neuroscience, University of Colorado, ences in the timing and duration of motor activity
Boulder, CO 80309-0334, USA distinguish one behavior from another (Gruner and
Present address: Altman 1980; Giuliani and Smith 1985; Bekoff 1988). In
1ARL Division of Neurobiology, University of Arizona, contrast, in other cases extensive changes in the timing
Tucson, AZ 85721, USA of muscle activity, which produces different synergies,
170 R. M. Johnston, A. Bekoff: Patterns of muscle activity during different behaviors in chicks

can distinguish one behavior from another. For bated at the University of Colorado, Boulder. Chicks were housed
e x a m p l e , in stick insects, specific m u s c l e s t h a t e x t e n d in heated and lighted bins with food and water ad libitum. The
f r o m t h e b a s a l p a r t o f the leg to the w i n g in t h e s a m e results presented here were collected from 10 different chicks per
behavior with 10 separate measurements per chick. This sample size
s e g m e n t , a r e a c t i v a t e d as a g o n i s t s d u r i n g w a l k i n g , b u t was determined to be appropriate for estimating population para-
r e c r u i t e d as a n t a g o n i s t s d u r i n g flight ( B a s s l e r 1989). meters and reducing the Type l error rate for a two-level nested
In cats, specific m u s c l e s o f the h i n d l i m b a r e r e c r u i t e d experimental design (Sokal and Rohlf 1995; Machlis et al. 1985).
d u r i n g the s a m e s y n e r g y d u r i n g w a l k i n g , b u t in Chicks were selected for this study based on their ability to initiate
walking in response to the sound made by lightly tapping a finger on
different s y n e r g i e s d u r i n g p a w s h a k e ( S m i t h 1986).
a table.
T h u s , b y r e o r g a i n z i n g the s y n a p t i c i n p u t s c o n v e r g i n g
o n t o the m o t o r n e u r o n s the n e r v o u s c a n p r o d u c e
a v a r i e t y o f m o t o r p a t t e r n s u s i n g m a n y o f the s a m e
muscles. Electromyograms
V a r i o u s s o u r c e s of m o d u l a t o r y i n p u t s h a v e b e e n
s h o w n to influence specific f e a t u r e s o f t h e m o t o r p a t - Electomyogram (EMG) recordings were used to provide a measure
t e r n s in different b e h a v i o r s . S e l e c t e d e x a m p l e s i n c l u d e of the output of the motor circuits within the central nervous system.
n e u r o m o d u l a t o r y influences t h a t d i c t a t e the e x p r e s s i o n Each chick was anesthetized with halothane and maintained on
o f v a r i o u s m o t o r p a t t e r n s in the s t o m a t o g a s t r i c s y s t e m a respirator throughout the preparation procedure. Bipolar hook
electrodes made from enamel-coated stainless steel wires (75 ~m
o f d e c a p o d c r u s t a c e a n s ( M e y r a n d et al. 1991; W e i m a n n diameter) were implanted through a small incision in the overlying
et al. 1991); b r a i n s t e m m e c h a n i s m s i n v o l v e d in c h o i c e skin into the major muscles of the right leg parallel to the long axis of
o f e l e c t r o m o t o r b e h a v i o r in fishes ( K e l l e r a n d H e i l i g e n - their fibers. The electrode wires were threaded underneath the skin
b e r g 1989); s p i n a l m e c h a n i s m s r e s p o n s i b l e for s e l e c t i o n to the mid-lumbar region and attached to the long recording leads.
The leads were light-weight and flexible and allowed the chick to
of different t y p e s o f s c r a t c h reflexes in t u r t l e s (Stein a n d
move about freely. A one cm length of shielding wire attached to the
C u r r i e 1989); a n d the i n f l u e n c e o f s e n s o r y afferents recordings leads was reinforced with solder and fashioned into the
r e s p o n s i b l e for the i n i t i a t i o n o f h a t c h i n g in c h i c k s shape of a thin, smooth tongue. This was inserted between the
(Bekoff a n d K a u e r 1982; B e k o f f a n d S a b i c h i 1987). rostral region of the synsacrum (Fig. 1D) and the overlying skin and
Thus, establishing the relationships among behaviors secured in place with cyanoacrylate adhesive. The anchored tongue
provided a stable base for connecting the electrodes to the recording
has facilitated our understanding of how behaviors are
leads. This provided no apparent discomfort to the animals or
organized by the CNS and by modulatory inputs. irritation to the surrounding tissues.
C h i c k e n s p r o v i d e a n i d e a l s y s t e m in w h i c h to e x a m - To monitor intralimb coordination, recordings were made
ine the r e l a t i o n s h i p s a m o n g b e h a v i o r s . D u r i n g the e m - from 6 major muscles of the right hindlimb (Fig. 1A): gastrocnemius
bryonic and postembryonic stages of development, lateralis (GL-ankle extensor), tibialis anterior (TA: ankle
flexor), femorotibialis (FT: knee extensor), caudilioflexorius (CF:
chicks produce a variety of behaviors involving the hip extensor-knee flexor), sartorius (SA: hip flexor-knee extensor)
legs, such as different t y p e s o f e m b r y o n i c m o t i l i t y , and iliofibularius (IF: knee flexor-hip extensor) (muscle nomen-
h a t c h i n g , w a l k i n g , b a c k w a r d w a l k i n g , s w i m m i n g , air- clature and functions are based on Bekoff and Kauer 1984). To
stepping, jumping, foot shaking and head scratching. monitor interlimb coordination, the left GL was implanted as
P r e v i o u s w o r k e s t a b l i s h e d the r e l a t i o n s h i p s b e t w e e n well. Following recovery from the anesthetic, chicks were housed
individually in heated and lighted bins with food and water ad
h a t c h i n g a n d w a l k i n g b e h a v i o r s ( B e k o f f et al. 1987). libitum. The chicks were allowed to recover for 24 h before record-
H o w e v e r , less is k n o w n a b o u t the r e l a t i o n s h i p s a m o n g ings began.
different p o s t e m b r y o n i c b e h a v i o r s . T h e g o a l o f this Chicks were enticed to walk along a runway by lightly tapping on
s t u d y w a s to i d e n t i f y t h e c o m m o n a n d d i s t i n c t f e a t u r e s the opposite end of the runway. As discussed previously (Johnston
o f m o t o r a c t i v i t y d u r i n g t h r e e different p o s t e m b r y o n i c and Bekoff 1992), we did not restrict the speed at which the chick
moved along the runway and refer to the entire range of this mode of
b e h a v i o r s : w a l k i n g , s w i m m i n g a n d a i r s t e p p i n g . By se- locomotion as walking. However, we did omit any walking episodes
lecting b e h a v i o r s f r o m the s a m e d e v e l o p m e n t a l stage, where the chick deviated from a straight path and the beginning and
b u t w h i c h o c c u r r e d in different c o n t e x t s , we h o p e d to ending steps. For analysis we selected steps from the middle portion
r e v e a l the e x t e n t to w h i c h the r e c r u i t m e n t p a t t e r n s a n d of an episode where there were no apparent changes in walking
speed. Swimming behavior was observed when a chick was placed in
r e g u l a t i o n o f m u s c l e a c t i v i t y c o u l d be m o d i f i e d . W e a Plexiglas tank of warm water (approximately 35C). Chicks are
show that not only are common recruitment patterns buoyant and capable of propelling themselves through the water
seen a m o n g these t h r e e b e h a v i o r s , b u t t h a t these re- quite well. However, to avoid potential differences in muscle activity
cruitment patterns are shared with embryonic behav- associated with turning behavior, we recorded swimming in place by
iors as well. holding onto the recording leads near the synsacrum while taking
care not to support the chick in any way. To elicit airstepping
behavior each chick was suspended with its legs pendent by holding
the recording leads close to the synsacrum. The chicks showed no
Materials and methods discomfort in any of these experimental situations. EMG activity
was amplified, filtered, and stored on a Vetter eight channel recorder
for subsequent analysis. Following each recording, the chicks were
Animals euthanised with carbon dioxide as recommended by the Panel on
Euthanasia of the American Veterinary Medicine Association
Experiments were performed on 1-to- 3 day old chicks (Gallus gallus) (1993). Post-mortem dissections were used to confirm the precise
hatched from fertile eggs obtained from a local supplier and incu- location of each electrode.
R. M. Johnston, A. Bekoff: Patterns of muscle activity during different behaviors in chicks 171

B cP C
k,,,dllta~ ~.A~-L . J.L,I L,a..~ i, r CP
1

1
L
i
BD i
BD
i i
Time (ms) o o.s l'.o ;.s
Normalized cycle period

D S

,"\ flexion

<
\k// \\J l
I 'ankl extension

Time (ms)

Fig. 1 A Muscles of the hindlimb examined using electromyogra- generated which was used to synchronize the numeric display to the
phy (see text for further explanation and abbreviations). B An concurrent E M G recording.
example of an E M G record showing the variables used to quantify In each behavior, five chicks were used for synchronizing the
the motor activity. CP, cycle period with respect to the reference E M G recordings with the movement kinematics. In addition to
muscle; L, latency to onset of activity with respect to the reference implanting EMG electrodes in select muscles, the hindlimb was
muscle; BD, burst duration. C Diagram representing the average prepared for video recordings as described previously (Johnston and
patterned activity normalized to cycle period: latency/cycle period Bekoff 1992). Basically, the skin overlying the knee was secured to
and duration/cycle period. The onset of each bar represents the mean the underlying cartilage of the femur and the hip, knee and ankle
phase value for a muscle and the length of each bar represents the joints where marked with small, black dots (Fig. 1D). Video record-
mean normal duration value for a muscle. Abscissa represents nor- ings were made as each animal performed each behavior. Kinematic
malized cycle period. Two normalized cycles are shown. D Drawing data were digitized once every 16.7 ms using Peak Performance
of the right hindlimb and pelvic skeleton of a post-hatching chick. software. E M G activity was collected and stored as described above.
Dots on the skeleton represent ink marks placed on the skin overlying The numeric synchronizer display allowed us to correlate on a one-
bony landmarks of the lateral surface of the hindlimb: anterior and to-one basis each video field with a DC voltage trace on the E M G
posterior synsacrum (s), the proximal and distal ends of the femur recordings. The latency to bursting activity in a given muscle was
(/), distal end of the tibiotarsus (tb) and the midpoint of the tar- quantified with respect to onset of extension (and flexion) move-
sometatarsal (tm) segment. These points were digitized to generate ments at each joint (Fig. 1D). An increase in joint angle was defined
a stick figure representation of the leg as shown in the ('enter panel. as extension and a decrease in joint angle was defined as flexion. In
Changes in joint angle calculated from the stick figures were plotted addition, we described the position of the leg during each behavior
versus time. An increase in joint angle was defined as extension and based on retraction and protraction stages (for complete details on
a decrease in joint angle was defined as flexion defining movement stages and kinematic details refer to Johnston
and Bekoff 1992). Briefly, in each behavior a cycle contained
one retraction period and two protraction periods. Retraction
(R) was defined from the onset of hip extension to the onset of
E M G activity synchronized with movement kinematics hip flexion, which marked the beginning of the first protraction
period. Protraction 1 (P1) lasted until the onset of knee extension
A Video/EMG synchronizer was used to directly correlate muscle which corresponded to the beginning of the second period of pro-
activity and the overt motion of the hindlimb. The synchronizing traction. Protraction 2 (P2) continued until the leg began the next
device, when supplied with a video signal, generated a numeric retraction period. In walking, retraction roughly corresponded to
display that was updated prior to the start of each new video field the stance phase and protraction corresponded to the swing phase of
(i.e. every 16.7 ms). In addition, a variable amplitude DC voltage was the cycle.
172 R.M. Johnston, A. Bekoff: Patterns of muscle activity during different behaviors in chicks

Analyses and variable definitions the assumption test of homogeneity of slopes in analysis of
covariance to determine whether specific slopes differed statistically
Quantitative analysis of EMG recordings were used to describe and from one another.
compare the motor activity of walking, swimming and airstepping.
Data were analyzed using Run Technologies software and values for
cycle periods, burst durations, latencies, normalized durations and Results
phase values were calculated (Fig. 1B, C). A cycle was defined from
the onset of bursting activity in GL to the onset of the next activity
period in GL; cycle period was defined as the duration between Interlimb coordination
contiguous cycles. Within each cycle the burst duration of a given
muscle was defined from the onset of activity to the offset of activity. Despite the common pattern of alternating movements
To quantify whether the onsets in different muscles occurred syn- between right and left legs, these three behaviors
chronously, the latency to bursting activity with respect to the
beginning of the cycle (GL onset) was quantified. Burst durations showed differences in interlimb coordination (Fig. 2).
and latency to burst onset were also expressed as a proportion In walking (Fig. 2A), activity in the right GL overlap-
of their cycle period to define normalized duration and phase, ped activity in the left GL during a significant and large
respectively. portion of the cycle (25%). The onset of activity in the
Except where indicated, all means are based on N = 10 for each left GL overlapped with the ongoing activity in the
behavior: 10 animals per behavior with 10 separate values per
animal. A critical level of ~ < 0.05 and two-tailed probabilities were right GL; similarly, the onset of activity in the right GL
used to determine statistical significance throughout this study. We overlapped with the ongoing activity in the left GL. In
compared sample means within and among behaviors using analysis contrast to walking, neither swimming nor airstepping
of variance in conjunction with Tukey's HSD post hoc test (Systat, showed significant periods of overlapping activity be-
1992). Percentages and proportional data were transformed prior to
testing. In addition, all assumptions for the statistical tests used in
tween the right and left GL muscles. In swimming
this study were met. To determine whether average burst durations (Fig. 2B), the extensor activity in one leg terminated as
show similar degrees of variability, the ratio of their variances (i.e. a c t i v i t y i n t h e c o n t r a l a t e r a l leg w a s i n i t i a t e d . T h e g a p
mean square) for specific pairs of muscles were statistically com- (3%) between contralateral activity in swimming was
pared based on an F distribution (Sokal and Rohlf 1995). Regression n o n s i g n i f i c a n t . H o w e v e r , in a i r s t e p p i n g (Fig. 2C), t h e r e
analysis was used to describe the relationship between burst dura-
tion and cycle period. Although both of these variables are depen- w a s a s i g n i f i c a n t g a p b e t w e e n G L a c t i v i t y in t h e r i g h t
dent (i.e. they each vary), we calculated least square regressions a n d left legs ( 9 % o f t h e cycle).
rather than Model II regressions (Sokal and Rohlf 1995) to facilitate
comparisons with other studies. However, analyses based on the
Model II approach did not result in different conclusions. In the Cycle periods
regressions, the slope, /3, described the linear relationship between
the burst duration and cycle period; the coefficient of determination,
r 2, described the proportion of the variation in these data that was The frequency distributions show that walking occur-
accounted for by the regression. In addition, we took advantage of red over a much broader range of cycle periods than

Fig. 2A-C Summary of A Walking


quantified EMG activity for
right GL and left GL in walking Right GL
A, swimming B and airstepping
C. Burst durations and latency Left GL
to burst onset have been
expressed as a proportion of I I I I I
a cycle. The onset of each bar
represents the mean phase value
for a muscle and the length of
each bar represents the mean B Swimming
normal duration value for
Right GL
a muscle. Abscissa represents
normalized cycle period. Two Left GL
cycles in each behavior are
shown. N = 10 for right GL and I I I I I
left GL in each behavior (10
animals per behavior; 10 values
per animal). Phase left GL with
respect to the right GL
(mean + s.d.: walking
C Airstepping
0.44 0.02; swimming Right GL
0.48 + 0.05; and airstepping
0.51 -I- 0.06). In all cases, Left GL
standard errors were less than
0.02 and omitted for clarity I I | I I
0 0.5 1.0 1.5 2.0
Normalized cycle period
R. M. Johnston, A. Bekoff: Patterns of muscle activity during different behaviors in chicks 173

Fig. 3A-C Frequency A Walking B Swimming C Airstepping


distributions for cycle periods in
walking A, swimming B and 220 ]/
airstepping C. Cycle period was
defined from the onset of activity
in the right GL to the next onset
of activity in right GL. 120 t
Distributions were based on
pooled data for GL activity (600
values per behavior). The mean Z 80
cycle period __+standard
deviation is shown below the
abscissa and was based on 40
a sample size of N = 10 (10
animals per behavior; 10 values
per animal)
100 300
|
500 700 100 300 500 700 ~0
I -.l-
Cycle period (ms) Cycle period (ms) Cycle period (ms)

either swimming or airstepping (Fig. 3). However, In contrast to the preceding example, the majority of
a portion of the range (175-350 ms) was shared by these the muscles showed more subtle differences in their
three behaviors. The mean cycle periods for walking bursting activity among these three behaviors. On
(362 _+ 131 ms) and swimming (308 + 55 ms) did not average, normalized durations of muscle activity differ-
differ significantly from one another, but were signifi- ed among walking, swimming and airstepping; these
cantly greater than the mean cycle period for airstep-
ping (196 + 34 ms).
A Airstepping
2 2 2 2 2 2
Intralimb coordination: patterns of muscle activity

In these three behaviors, the muscles examined showed


either one or two periods of activity (i.e. bursts) per
cycle. The muscles that burst once per cycle were: GL,
TA, CF, SA, pIF and aIF (Fig. 1A). Previous work on
the IF muscle in chicks showed that the distinct 'a' and
'p' bursts originate from either the anterior or posterior
regions of IF, respectively, and that each region re-
ceived separate innervation (Settles et al. 1987). Thus,
we considered the anterior IF (aIF) and the posterior
IF (pIF) as separate muscles. FT was the only muscle B Walking
we examined that was active twice per cycle. With 1 2 1 2 1 2 1 2
respect to the beginning of a cycle, we designated the
first burst in FT as FT1 and the second burst as FT2.
Our attempts to isolate each of these bursts to regions
within FT have been unsuccessful so far. Although
there were no apparent morphological compartments
for FT, it is possible that the recruitment of motor units
could be functionally compartmentalized.
The most obvious difference in the motor patterns
among these behaviors was the absence of FT1 activity
in airstepping (Figs. 4, 5C). The activity of FT1 was 200 ms
similar in walking and swimming, but completely ab- Fig. 4A, B Examples of E M G recordings showing FT and GL ac-
sent during airstepping. An example of FT activity tivity during airstepping A and walking B from the same animal. The
during airstepping and walking in the same animal is numbers 1 and 2 indicate the first (FTI) and second burst (FT2) of
shown in Fig. 5. During airstepping, only FT2 activity FT. A In airstepping, only the second burst in FT (FT2) was present.
However, when the same animal was placed on the substrate and
was present (Fig. 4A). However, when the same animal initiated walking B, both the first and second bursts were present.
was allowed to walk, both FT1 and FT2 bursts were The phase of FT2 activity with respect to GL was similar for walking
present (Fig. 4B). and airstepping
174 R . M . Johnston A. Bekoff: Patterns of muscle activity during different behaviors in chicks

Fig. 5A-C Summary of A Walking


normalized EMG activity for P1 P2 R PI P2 R
walking A, swimming B and GL
airstepping C. The onset oj each pIF t I , I I
bar represents the mean phase CF
FT If_m__ ! 1 ~ 1 ~
value for a muscle and the length TA
of each bar represents the mean SA
normal duration value for aIF
a muscle. The abscissa } I I I I
I I I t I
represents normalized cycle
-0.5 0 0.5 1.0 1.5
period. Two cycles for each
behavior are shown. The
numbers 1 and 2 identify the first
(FT1) and second (FT2) bursts of
FT. Note that FTI activity is B Swimming P2 R PI P2 R Pl
absent in airstepping. N = 10 for GL I I []
each muscle within each pIF
behavior: 10 chicks per muscle CF I I
with 10 measurements per FT I all III I
muscle. Also shown are the three TA I
stages of the leg movements, SA
a/F i
retraction (R), protraction 1 (P1) I I 1 I I
and protraction 2 (P2). These I 1 I I I

three stages were quantified -0.5 0 0.5 1.0 1.5


from digitized, video taped
records of each behavior that
were synchronized with the
motor activity. In all cases,
standard errors were less than C Airstepping P2 R P1 P2 R P1
0.02 and not shown for clarity GL I ii.s_
prF I
CF
i I I
TA
SA
alFi 1I i
i i i i i

-0.5 0 0.5 1.0 t.5


Normalized cycle period

differences may be related to the extent of resistance plF, CF, TA and SA each occupied 40-50% of a cycle
encountered during these movements. In walking, (Fig. 5B; Table 1). We described swimming as having
muscles active during the stance phase (GL, pIF and symmetrical activity durations. Note that for swim-
CF), where the leg movements encountered resistance ming the activity in FT and aIF were the exceptions to
due to contact with the ground and weight-bearing, this generalization as discussed below.
showed long normalized durations (Fig. 5A; Table 1) In contrast to both walking and swimming, the legs
(note that FT1 was the exception to this generalization encountered very little resistance from the medium
as discussed below). This was in contrast to the shorter during airstepping. Several of the muscles showed nor-
normalized durations (and delayed onset) for muscles malized burst durations which occupied 40-50% of the
active during the swing phase where the leg moved cycle: GL, TA and SA (Fig. 5; Table 1). In contrast, the
unimpeded through the air (TA, SA, aIF and FT2) normalized duration of pIF was much shorter (Table
(Fig. 5A; Tables 1, 2). We use the term asymmetry to 1). The remaining two muscles examined in airstepping,
describe this type of difference in burst durations (i.e. FT and aIF, showed an interesting parallel with the
long vs. short). Thus, a cycle of walking was character- both walking and swimming. In none of the three
ized by asymmetrical durations of activity between behaviors did FT and aIF fall into the generalized
groups of muscles which corresponded with the high trend where normalized durations were correlated with
resistance (stance or retraction) and the low resistance the extent of resistance. Although walking, swimming
(swing or protraction) stages of a walking cycle. and airstepping encounter different degrees of resist-
Neither swimming nor airstepping had a stance ance during the hindlimb movements, the normalized
phase. The legs encountered roughly uniform resistance durations of FT1, FT2 and aIF changed little among
from the water throughout swimming and from the air these behaviors (Table I; Fig. 5).
throughout airstepping. In contrast to the asymmetry In general, the timing of motor activity differed in
described during walking, the average durations of GL, these behaviors. In contrast to either swimming or
R. M. Johnston, A. Bekoff: Patterns of muscle activity during different behaviors in chicks 175

Table 1 Mean normalized


durations for each muscle in GL pie CF FT1 FT2 TA SA aIF
several chick behaviors
Walking 0.69 0.61 0.63 0.41 0.30 0.33 0.41 0.16
Swimming 0.45 0.42 0.45 0.30 0.37 0.43 0.52 0.17
Airstepping 0.40 0.24 0.35 a 0.38 0.42 0.50 0.13
Embryonic 0.40 a 0.34 0.39 a 0.34 0.43 0.36
motility
Hatching 0.48 0.38 0.42 0.47 a 0.32 0.17 0.25
0.17
Hatching- 0.42 0.35 0.37 0.33 a 0.43 0.48 0.13
deafferented legs
Walking- 0.47 0.49 0.38 0.35 0.32 0.37 0.51 0.20
deafferented legs

aIndicates the absence of muscle activity. Normal duration is defined as burst duration/cycle duration.
N = 10 for each muscle within each behavior. Data for embryonic motility (Bradley and Bekoff 1990),
hatching, deafferented hatching and deafferented walking (Bekoffet al. 1987) were recalculated based on
a sample size of N = 10:10 chicks per muscle with 10 measurements per muscle. Standard error of the
means were all < 0.02. For these data variation within each animal was similar to variation among
animals

Table 2 Mean phase values for


each muscle in several chick GL pIF CF FT1 FT2 TA SA aIF
behaviors
Walking 0.00 0.10 0.02 0.21 0.80 0.71 0.70 0.70
Swimming 0.00 0.04 - 0.01 0.09 0.70 0.44 0.47 0.51
Airstepping 0.00 0.08 0.01 ~ 0.78 0.42 0.47 0.38
Embryonic 0.00 a 0.04 - 0.05 a 0.51 0.45 0.50
motility
Hatching 0.00 0.03 0.03 - 0.02 a 0.48 0.43 0.53
0.63
Hatching- 0.00 0.02 0.00 -- 0.02 " 0.35 0.32 0.50
deafferented legs
Walking- 0.00 0.02 -- 0.04 0.00 0.67 0.51 0.45 0.51
deafferented legs

a Indicates the absence muscle activity. Phase is defined as latency to burst onset with respect to the onset
of the cycle/cycle duration. N = 10 for each muscle within each behavior. Data for embryonic motility
(Bradley and Bekoff 1990), hatching, deafferented hatching and deafferented walking (Bekoff et al. 1987)
were recalculated based on a sample size of N = 10:10 chicks per muscle with 10 measurements per
muscle. Because SA shows bursts twice per cycle in hatching, two phase values are given. Standard error
of the means were all < 0.02. For these data variation within each animal was similar to variation
among animals

airstepping, the recruitment of TA, SA and aIF activity G L - C F - p I F - F T 1 s y n e r g y b e g a n . B a s e d o n its r e c r u i t -


w a s d e l a y e d in w a l k i n g (Fig. 5). H o w e v e r , w i t h o n e m e n t t i m e w i t h r e s p e c t to the o t h e r m u s c l e s , we d o n o t
e x c e p t i o n , t h e m u s c l e s t h a t w e r e r e c r u i t e d t o g e t h e r (i.e. i n c l u d e F T 2 a c t i v i t y as p a r t o f e i t h e r s y n e r g y in s w i m -
s y n e r g i s t s ) in o n e b e h a v i o r c o n t i n u e d to be r e c r u i t e d ming and airstepping. In these two behaviors, FT2
t o g e t h e r in t h e o t h e r b e h a v i o r s . W e i d e n t i f i e d t w o o c c u r r e d b e t w e e n t h e t w o c o m m o n synergies.
s y n e r g i e s t h a t w e r e c o m m o n to e a c h b e h a v i o r : the
GL-CF-pIF-FT1 synergy and the TA-SA-aIF synergy
(Fig. 5). W i t h i n e a c h o f t h e t w o s y n e r g i e s t h e r e c r u i t - Muscle activity synchronized with movement
m e n t o r d e r o f the m u s c l e s w a s n o t rigid. F o r e x a m p l e , kinematics
a I F w a s r e c r u i t e d b e f o r e S A in a i r s t e p p i n g , b u t after S A
in s w i m m i n g (Fig. 5). U n l i k e the o t h e r muscles, F T 2 For most of the muscles examined, each behavior
a c t i v i t y w a s n o t r e c r u i t e d as p a r t o f t h e s a m e s y n e r g y in showed similar relationships between muscle activity
t h e s e b e h a v i o r s . I n w a l k i n g , F T 2 was r e c r u i t e d as p a r t a n d c h a n g e s in j o i n t a n g l e (Fig. 6). F o r e x a m p l e , p I F
o f t h e T A - S A - a I F s y n e r g y . H o w e v e r , in s w i m m i n g a n d was active during knee flexion and CF was active
a i r s t e p p i n g , F T 2 a c t i v i t y w a s r e c r u i t e d well after t h e s e d u r i n g h i p e x t e n s i o n in e a c h b e h a v i o r . A l t h o u g h t h e
m u s c l e s h a d a l r e a d y b e e n r e c r u i t e d (Fig. 5). I n a d d i - muscle activity occurred during either joint flexion or
tion, F T 2 w a s r e c r u i t e d well b e f o r e a c t i v i t y in the e x t e n s i o n m o v e m e n t s in e a c h b e h a v i o r , the d u r a t i o n o f
176 R . M . Johnston, A. Bekoff: Patterns of muscle activity during different behaviors in chicks

Walking Swimming Airstepping


Hip CF/-

Knee
/
20J
160.
SAV
l r ,

--&

~a
<
e-, I
40.
all=
180-
Ankle

\
\ GL~
~a / \
<
T /

0
o Time (ms) 60o 0 Time (ms) ~o o Time (ms) 6oo

Fig. 6 E M G recordings synchronized with movement kinematics. as the joints extended. Protraction 1 (P1) occurred
The onset and duration of activity in several muscles are plotted when the leg was drawn towards the body as the joints
with the corresponding changes in angular joint displacements. The
non-continuous, solid lines represent periods of activity in individual flexed. Protraction 2 (P2) stage identified when the leg
muscles. The dashed lines represent changes in joint angles. Exten- moved anteriorly. Figure 5 shows the relationships
sion was defined as an increase in joint angle; flexion was defined as between periods of muscle activity and the retraction
a decrease in joint angle. In walking, the shaded area denotes the and protraction stages of the leg movements (also see
stance period. The data shown were from a different animal in each
behavior and were typical of all other analyzed records. Synchro-
Table 3). Many muscles were active during more than
nized results were quantified from 5 different animals per behavior one stage of the leg movements. In addition, the dura-
with 10 samples per muscle tion of muscle activity in a particular stage of the
movement could differ among the three behaviors. For
example, in walking GL was active during almost all of
activity and/or phasing could differ (e.g. FT2). Figure stage R and only a small portion of stage P1; in swim-
6 also shows that the onset of joint movement did not ming, GL was active for approximately half of stage
always coincide with the onset of muscle activity at that R and half of stage P1; and in airstepping, GL was
joint. For example, in each behavior, ankle extension active for more than half of stage R and roughly half of
preceded the onset of activity in the ankle extensor, GL stage P1.
(Fig. 6). Furthermore, GL activity stopped at the For many of the muscles, the movement stage in
transition from ankle extension to ankle flexion in both which muscle activity was initiated remained much the
walking and swimming. In airstepping, GL activity same in each behavior (e.g. GL began in stage R and
continued well into ankle flexion. Even so, on average TA began in stage P1 in each behavior) (Fig. 5). How-
GL and TA bursts did not show any overlapping peri- ever, FT2 initiated activity during P1 in walking and
ods of activity in airstepping (Fig. 5C). P2 during both swimming and airstepping. The stage in
In addition to correlating muscle activity with which the muscle activity was terminated was more
flexion and extension movements at individual joints, variable than that seen for initiation of activity. For
we also related the motor patterns to retraction and example, CF activity ended during stage R in walking
protraction movements of the entire leg (defined in and during stage P1 in swimming and airstepping
Materials and methods; also see Johnston and Bekoff (Fig. 5). On average, in these three behaviors, 5 out of
1992, for more details on the kinematic profiles of the 8 muscles terminated activity in different movement
legs during walking, swimming and airstepping). In stages, but only 1 out of 8 of the muscles initiated
general, during retraction (R) the leg moved posteriorly activity during a different movement stage. In each
R. M. Johnston, A. Bekoff: Patterns of muscle activity during different behaviors in chicks 177

Table 3 Average onset (with respect to GL) and duration of retrac- Standard error of the means were all < 0.01. N = 10 (10 chicks per
tion and protraction movements of the hindlimb (expressed as behavior with 10 measurements per chick
proportion of the cycle) during walking, swimming and airstepping.

Onset Duration

Retraction Protraction 1 Protraction 2 Retraction Protraction 1 Protraction 2

Walking - 0.03 0.66 0.87 0.69 0.21 0.10


Swimming - 0.19 0.29 0.61 0.48 0.32 0.20
Airstepping - 0.15 0.32 0.50 0.47 0.18 0.35

A B C muscles aIF and SA were coactive, it is likely that SA


flexed the hip and aIF flexed the knee (Figs. 6, 7B).
When activity in aIF stopped, sustained activity in SA
could continue to flex the hip and initiate knee exten-
sion (Fig. 7C). Therefore, presumably, the initiation of
knee extension did not require the recruitment of a new
muscle; knee extension was initiated by the changing
role of SA throughout the movement.

I
Variability in muscle activity
Fig. 7 A - C Proposed muscle interactions between SA and alF. Analyses based on the arithmetic means are but one
A Schematic illustration showing the anatomical positions of each
muscle9 The leg profile is shown in dark lines and the muscles are
way to describe patterned activity. Analyses of the
shown in stippled lines. B and C Diagrams showing the movements scatter in the data provide a useful tool to examine the
of the leg during the activity of SA and aIF. The dotted lines variability in the patterned activity. To account for
represent the new position of the leg profile variability in the duration of muscle activity, we plotted
burst durations against their corresponding cycle peri-
ods for each muscle in each behavior and used regres-
behavior, the GL-CF-plF-FT1 synergy occurred dur- sion analysis to quantify the relationships (Fig. 8). Dur-
ing R and the TA-SA-aIF synergy occurred during P1. ing walking, the duration of muscles active during the
However, FT2 activity occurred during P1 in walking, stance phase increased as cycle periods increased (GL,
but during P2 in both swimming and airstepping pIF, CF and FT1). The slopes describing the relation-
(Fig. 5). ships for these muscles were all greater than 0.50. In
Analyses of the synchronized data also revealed that addition, changes in cycle period accounted for 66% to
the effects of activity in a given muscle could be 89% of the variability in the duration of muscle activity
changed by the concurrent or previous activity of an- (based on r2). In contrast, the changes in the duration of
other muscle. Two examples demonstrating such inter- muscles active during the swing phase of walking were
active relationships were CF and FT, and SA and aIF. only weakly associated with changes in cycle period
Based on their anatomical positions, activity in FT (slopes less than 0.20), if at all (TA, SA, aIF and FT2)
would be expected to extend the knee and activity in (Fig. 8). Changes in cycle period accounted for less of
CF would be expected to extend the hip and flex the the variability in the duration of muscles active during
knee (Fig. 1A). During the time when these two muscles the swing phase in walking.
were coactive, knee extension was not observed (Fig. 6). In comparison to walking, the majority of the rela-
One possible explanation for this observation was that tionships between duration and cycle period changed
activity in the uniarticular FT could be playing two during swimming or airstepping. For example, in both
roles. First, activity in FT opposed knee flexion due to of these behaviors the relationships for the antagonistic
activity in CF. Second, as FT contracted, it lengthened ankle muscles, GL and TA, had slopes that were rever-
CF thereby promoting continued hip extension by CF sed in comparison to walking (Fig. 8). In swimming or
activity, a movement FT could not accomplish on its airstepping, the regression slope of GL was less than
own. Figure 7 illustrates the interaction between the that for TA; in walking the regression slope of GL was
muscles SA and aIF. Activity in SA would be expected greater than TA. Other muscles, such as pIF and CF,
to flex the hip and extend the knee (Figs. 1, 7A) while showed smaller or non-significant slopes in comparison
knee flexion was associated with the activity in the to walking. Although the regression slope for FT2 was
anterior portion of IF (aIF). During the time when the greater in swimming or airstepping, the amount of
178 R. M. Johnston, A. Bekoff: Patterns of muscle activity during different behaviors in chicks

Fig~ 8 Burst duration versus Walking Swimming Airstepping


cycle period graphs for the
muscles examined during 600 13= 0.67 400 13= 0.12 400 13= 0.17
walking, swimming and r 2 = 0.08
airstepping. Regressions are not
shown for FT1 for airstepping GL ! I'i: /
because FT1 activity is absent
during airstepping. Each figure
contains 100 points compiled
i oS
from 10 animals per muscle with 600 13= 0.81 400 13= 0.50 400 6=ns
I0 values per animal, fl indicates
the slope for each regression and
~ I r2=O:~Y r2=034
r 2 is the coefficient of pIF
determination. All slopes given
showed P < 0.05. ~o o 0
Nonsignificant slopes (ns)
(P > 0.05) are not drawn. 600 ] 13= 0.63 400 1 13= 0.23 400 13----as
Regressions of each individual
animal showed the same
CF
I ':0:0
relationships as the combined .~:'- "' "
data set
~o o 0

FT1
6001.0,1
/ r2 = 0.74 411.=068
.j
r2 = 0,58

i
~o

400 13= 0.50


60011 r2
0"3613=0"15
= // r2 = 0"33. 400] 6= 0.32

FT2 -

~o
~
'7 0
. 9 .~
" .,,~ :
~I

600 13= us 400 6 = 0.65


=, [ r2 = 0 . 3 2 . ~ , , r2 = 0.42

TA Y
9 ~ ~l ~

~0 0

600 g = 0.19 6= ns
g r 2 = 0.23 i r'=Ot=
SA .:.. ,~.....,.~ ~ .',o

= o

600 1 13= 0.05 4o0 i 13=0.18 4ool 6=ns


"~ ] r2=0"14
aIF

0 ~ 0i
100 700 150 550 0 400
Cycle period (ms) Cycle period (ms) Cycle period Ires)

variability that was accounted for by the regressions tion and cycle period. Despite these many differences,
(based on r 2) remained low. The amount of variation in the relationship between FT1 duration and cycle peri-
SA or aIF duration remained low in swimming, al- od was similar in walking and swimming (FT1 was not
though the magnitude of the slopes increased in com- active during airstepping). Although the magnitude of
parison to walking. In contrast, airstepping showed the slopes differed, they both showed moderate slopes
non-significant relationships between SA or aIF dura- and accounted for much of the variability in the dura-
R. M. Johnston, A. Bekoff: Patterns of muscle activity during different behaviors in chicks 179

Table 4 Summary of the


statistical comparisons of Statistical results Variance (ms z)
duration variability for specific
pairs of muscles within walking W S A W S A
(W), swimming (S) and
airstepping (A) based on GL v. TA = = = GL 2622 420 183
variance ratios CF v. SA > = = CF 4639 528 237
pIF v. FT2 > = = pIF 7270 1217 107
plF v. SA > = = SA 1048 725 347
alF v. FT2 = > > TA 1015 592 342
FT1 1832 1858 a

alF 185 256 12


FT2 616 2793 110

aIndicates that comparisons were not possible because FT1 activity was absent in airstepping.
Non-significant differences are represented by the symbol, = . Significant differences where the
variance of the first muscle was greater than the second are represented by the symbol, > . For example,
the variance in duration between GL and TA as not significantly different. The variance for CF duration
was significantly greater than that for SA in walking. Significance was determined based on a critical
level of 0.05 and two-tailed probabilities: F . o 5 / 2 1 9 , 9 ] = 4.03. N = 10 for each muscle within each behavior

tion of FTI. In walking, more than 30% of the variabil- during three distinct postembryonic behaviors: walk-
ity in duration was accounted for by changes in cycle ing, swimming and airstepping. Although the behav-
period for 62.5% (5 out of 8) of the muscles examined. ioral context differs greatly for these behaviors, we
Regression analysis accounted for at least 30% of dura- identified many similarities in the patterns of muscle
tion variability in 50% (4 out of 8) of the muscles activity. Each behavior showed similar relationships
examined in swimming, but only 14% (1 out of 7) in between muscle activity and joint motion for many of
airstepping. Unlike the other two behaviors, in airstep- the muscles examined in this study (Fig. 6). For
ping the majority of the muscles (4 out of 7) examined example, CF was active during hip extension in each
showed non-significant regression relationships be- behavior. These trends were maintained under the dif-
tween duration and cycle period. ferent behavioral conditions associated with walking,
We also examined the extent to which walking, swimming and airstepping and over a wide range of
swimming and airstepping showed common trends in cycle periods (Fig. 3). However, as seen in Fig. 6, the
variation by statistically comparing the duration mean synchronized results also revealed that some muscles,
squares (i.e. variance) for specific pairs of muscles that such as FT2, displayed different relationships with the
acted primarily at the same joint, but during opposing joint angle profiles during different behaviors. In air-
stages of the movement cycle (i.e. retraction and pro- stepping, FT2 activity was seen only at the end of knee
traction). This analysis showed that the extent of vari- extension. In both walking and swimming, activity in
ation for the antagonistic ankle muscles, GL and TA, FT2 occurred throughout knee extension. We propose
were similar in each behavior (Table 4). However, in that the delay in onset of FT2 in airstepping can be
walking, most of the muscles active during leg retrac- accounted for by examining the interactions between
tion had burst durations that were more variable than two other muscles, SA and alF (Figs. 6, 7). During the
the muscles active during the leg protraction move- time when SA and alF were coactive, aIF flexed the
ments (e.g. CF-SA) (Table 4). This was in contrast to knee and SA flexed the hip. When activity in aIF
both swimming and airstepping where each showed no stopped, activity in SA not only continued to flex the
detectable differences in burst duration variability for hip, but now initiated knee extension. In walking and
the majority of the muscles (e.g. CF-SA). In fact, the swimming, aIF activity stopped midway through the
same statistical conclusions were present for swimming SA burst duration (Fig. 5A, B). However, in airstep-
and airstepping for all of the comparisons. ping, alF stopped near the beginning of SA activity
(Fig. 5C). This suggests that much of knee extension
during airstepping resulted from the bifunctional
muscle, SA; as SA shortened, FT2 may be recruited to
continue knee extension movements. Thus, the differ-
Discussion ences in the timing of these and other muscles may
indicate that the recruitment of muscles is task depen-
Relationships between joint angle, dent as suggested for hindlimb behaviors in cats (Pratt
muscle activity and behavior and Loeb 1991).
The relationships between GL activity and the
The primary purpose of this study was to identify the movement profiles in airstepping also differed from
c o m m o n and unique features of leg muscle activity walking and swimming. In contrast to the other two
180 R. M. Johnston, A. Bekoff:Patterns of muscle activity during differentbehaviors in chicks
behaviors, in airstepping GL activity continued past mind us that behavior results from the combined effects
the transition from extension to flexion and into ankle of many muscles whose relationships change through-
flexion (Fig. 6). It has been suggested that an out of out the movement. Thus, behavior is the product of
phase relationship between the direction of joint interactive and interdependent strategies used to gener-
motion and muscle action forcibly stretches the muscle, ate coordinated movements.
thus enhancing force production needed to satisfy the
dynamic requirements of rapid oscillatory movements
(Abraham and Loeb 1985; Smith et al. 1985). In airstep- Variation in patterned activity
ping, if early ankle flexion was in part due to the effects
of intersegmental dynamics, then continued activity of The resistance that the leg movements encountered
the ankle extensor, GL, could aid in slowing or oppos- from the environment differed among these three be-
ing these forces. Experiments addressing intersegmen- haviors. During walking, the retraction or stance stage
tal dynamics using kinetic analyses are needed to vali- impeded the leg movements and delayed the onset of
date these suggestions. the protraction or swing stage where the legs encounter
In walking, our results showed that knee flexion only minor resistance from the air. In swimming, the
occurred during the stance phase even though the knee legs received uniform resistance from the water envi-
extensor, FT1, was active. The observed knee flexion ronment during both the retraction and protraction
could be passive as the leg yields to the weight of the stages of the movements. The context of airstepping
animal during the stance phase and the concurrent also provided uniform, although much less, resistance
knee extensor activity could be required to stabilize the to the leg movements. (For a discussion of leg move-
knee joint. However, because of anatomical specializa- ment kinematic in these three behaviors, see Johnston
tions (Young 1981; Raikow 1985), knee flexion is the and Bekoff 1992). Although not the only source of
only means by which the chick can continue forward afferent modulatory input, the extent of resistance pres-
movement to change its center of mass relative to its ent during walking, swimming and airstepping, was
foot placement during walking (Gatesy 1989). There- correlated with the changes in the duration of muscle
fore, it would appear that in chicks knee flexion during activity. However, when normalized for differences in
stance does not result only from a passive yielding of cycle periods, FT and aIF activity showed only small
the joint, but results from muscle activity in pIF. One changes in duration in these behaviors (Table 1; Fig. 5).
possible advantage for recruitment of the uniarticular This suggests that the effects of the various sources of
muscle, FT, with its biarticular antagonist, pIF, during modulatory inputs, including resistance from weight
the stance phase may be to enhance the work that the support, water or air and other influences unique to
uniarticular muscle can do in addition to contributing each behavior, were functionally similar for the muscles
to stabilizing the knee joint during the stance phase in aIF and FT. In contrast, activity in the remaining
walking (van Ingen Schenau 1987, 1990; van Ingen muscles, GL, CF, plF, TA and SA, differed among
Schenau et al. 1992). Because both pIF and FT1 are these behaviors. In walking, where resistance related to
active during knee flexion in swimming, the need to weight support was present, these muscles showed an
stabilize the knee or augment muscle work may not be asymmetry in burst durations (Fig. 5; Table 1). On
restricted to behaviors where contact occurs with the average, the durations of muscles active during the
substrate, such as walking. In addition, this muscle stance phase were longer than the durations of muscles
synergy may participate in other functional roles im- active during the swing phase. In swimming or air-
portant to the performance of the behavior. Given the stepping, where the legs experience more uniform
above information, it is possible that the absence of resistance, the activity of these muscles were more sym-
FT1 activity during airstepping reflects that the leg metrical. Asymmetrical burst durations for stance and
movements are unable to generate force that would swing muscles are a common feature of walking in
result in forward movement of the animal. Although many animals (Cracraft 1971; Grillner 1981; Jacobson
this hypothesis needs to be formally tested, these data and Hollyday 1982; Gatesy 1989). Changing from
show that patterned muscle activity and coordinated asymmetrical to symmetrical bursting activity has been
leg movements continue uninterrupted even when one described for walking and swimming in turtles (Len-
element of a motor synergy is absent. nard and Stein 1977; Williams 1981) and in rats
Another possible role played by FT was to enhance (Gruner and Altman 1980). These data support pre-
hip extension. During the time when FT1 and CF were vious conclusions that the asymmetries characteristic
coactive, FT1 activity not only opposed knee flexion of walking were the result of sensory modulation from
due to activity in CF, but also lengthened CF thereby the legs that biased the output of the premotor circuit
promoting continued hip extension by CF activity elements underlying the muscle activity (Bekoff et al.
(Fig. 7). This suggestion reflects that the effects of activ- 1987). In addition, even though resistance-related
ity in a given muscle can be changed by the concurrent modulation affects the activity of aIF and SA different-
or previous activity of other muscles. These examples ly in these behaviors, the result of their concerted action
relating muscle activity to movement kinematics re- was maintained. We propose that the premotor ele-
R. M. Johnston, A. Bekoff:Patterns of muscleactivity during differentbehaviors in chicks 181

ments underlying the production of these behaviors Relationships with embryonic behaviors in chicks;
accommodates modulation that affects muscle activity, implications for neural control
such as the absence of a burst (e.g. FT1 in airstepping)
or changes in the onset and duration of activity, with- Embryonic motility and hatching share several features
out disrupting the integrity of the motor patterns or leg of the motor activity described for the postembryonic
movements. Although our analysis is far removed from behaviors of walking, swimming and airstepping.
the actual pattern generating elements, the flexibility in Although not identical, the two common synergies
muscle activity is consistent with ideas of plasticity that described for the postembryonic behaviors were also
have been well characterized in the stomatogastric sys- present in these embryonic behaviors (Figs. 5, 9A, B).
tem of decapod crustaceans (Harris-Warrick and Joh- The differences in duration and timing of their activity
nson 1989; Harris-Warrick and Marder 1991; Meyrand between embryonic behaviors and walking has been
et al. 1991; Weimann et al. 1991; Meyrand and Simmers correlated with differences in afferent modulation
1992). (Bekoff 1976; Bekoff et al. 1987; Bradley and Bekoff
We identified two motor synergies common to each 1990). These similarities in synergies suggest that the
behavior. With only one exception (FT2), muscles that mechanisms that underlie the organization of muscle
were recruited together in walking were also recruited activity in postembryonic behaviors are established
together in swimming and airstepping. Within each early in development during the embryonic period.
synergy, changes in the timing and duration of activity Because hatching, a behavior that is normally produc-
were correlated with the presence or absence of weight- ed only during a specific developmental period, can be
bearing by the legs. Although FT2 activity was recruit- in re-elicited in posthatching chicks it has been sugges-
ed during one of the common synergies in walking, it ted that the neural elements that produce this stage-
did not participate in either synergy in swimming or in specific behavior are retained into the postnatal period
airstepping. In the latter behaviors, FT2 activity occur- (Bekoff and Kauer 1984). Thus, although muscle activ-
red between the two common synergies. Another prom- ity is an indirect measure of the neural networks that
inent difference in motor activity among these behav- are involved in producing behaviors, similarities in
iors was the absence of FT1 activity during airstepping. motor activity during the different behaviors shown in
Despite its absence, the activity of the other muscles this study may reflect elements of the pattern generat-
that were part of the synergy showed patterned activity ing circuits that are common to these embryonic and
that was similar to activity produced in walking and postembryonic behaviors in chicks (Bekoff 1986; Bekoff
swimming. et al. 1987). These ideas are consistent with direct evid-
It has been proposed that the CNS simplifies control ence from other systems that elements of circuits are
of a multisegmental, multimuscular limb by combining reused during different developmental stages (Levine
muscles into groups based on common periods of activ- and Weeks 1989; Weeks and Levine 1990) and that the
ity (i.e. synergies). Different behaviors may show differ- same circuit can produce different behavioral patterns
ent synergies in response to the unique afferent modu- (Getting and Dekin 1985; Getting 1989; Hooper and
lation associated with the movements (Koshland and Moulins 1989; Cazalets et al. 1990; Dickinson et al.
Smith 1989; Smith et al. 1985; Pratt and Loeb 1991). In 1990; Harris-Warrick and Marder 1991; Weimann
contrast, it has been proposed that the CNS is organ- et al. 1991). Based on features of motor activity other
ized to control each muscle individually (MacPherson studies have also made similar suggestions that differ-
1991). Based on our results, these two proposals are not ent behaviors share pattern generating circuitry (Ayers
mutually exclusive. Comparisons of burst duration and Davis 1977; Ayers and Clarac 1978; Stein et al.
versus cycle period regressions among walking, swim- 1986; Mortin and Stein 1989). Of course, we do not
ming and airstepping indicate that muscles that partici- exclude the possibility that different circuits and or
pate in the same synergy can be differentially affected mechanisms exist that produce similar patterns of mo-
by afferent inputs (Fig. 8). These data support previous tor activity in different behaviors and developmental
work suggesting that each muscle can be influenced stages.
independently by sources of modulation (Grillner Although many of the muscles show similar patterns,
and Wall6n 1985; Hoy et al. 1985; Smith et al. 1985; the activity of FT2 and aIF in prenatal behaviors differ
Hoy and Zernicke 1986; Smith 1986). In addition, al- in comparison to the motor profiles in the postnatal
though there are apparent differences in afferent behaviors. FT2 activity is absent in both embryonic
modulatory inputs during these three behaviors, the motility and hatching, but present in all of the postem-
majority of the muscles continue to participate in bryonic behaviors that we have examined. In addition,
synergies common to each of these behaviors; changes unlike the posthatching behaviors where aIF duration
in synergies appear to be restricted to muscles that act is brief and is recruited at different times within the
at the knee (i.e. FT2) during the protraction phase of synergy, the duration of aIF is long and maintains
the leg movements. Similar trends have been seen in a similar onset phase in both embryonic motility and
studies of various cyclical leg behaviors in cats (Smith hatching (Tables 1, 2; Figs. 5, 9). However, data from
1986). chicks with deafferented legs suggests that phasic
182 R. M. Johnston, A. Bekoff: Patterns of muscle activity during different behaviors in chicks

Fig. 9A-D Summary of A Embryonic Motility


normalized EMG activity
characterizing embryonic GL
motility A, hatching B and pIF
CF
hatching and walking in chicks FT
with deafferented legs (D, E). TA
The numbers 1 and 2 identify the SA
first (FTI) and second (FT2) alF
bursts of FT. Note that FT2 [ I I I I

activity is absent in both A, -0.5 0 0.5 1.0 1.5


B and C. Note that pIF activity
for embryonic motility is not B Hatching
shown because the data were not
yet available. However, data GL
suggests that both bursts of IF
are present in embryos
(Landmesser and O'Donovan FT
TA
1984). Data for leg motility in SA m m B m
9-day embryos (Bradley and alF m l
Bekoff 1990) and deafferented
hatching and walking (Bekoffet -~J.5 o15 11o 115
al. 1987) were recalculated based
on a sample size of N = 10 for
each muscle in each behavior: l0
chicks per muscle with 10
measurements per muscle in C Deafferented Hatching
each behavior. Abscissa GL
represents normalized cycle pIF
period. Two cycles for each CF
behavior are shown. Standard FT
errors were less than 0.02 and TA
SA
not shown for clarity alF m

-o.5 o 0'.5 1'.o

D Deafferented Walking

GL
plF
CF
FT z t
TA
SA
alF m
I

-0.5 o o'.5 llO 1.5


Normalizedcycle period

sensory information from the legs during hatching con- but prolonged during walking (Fig. 9D). This difference
tributes to the prolonged a I F duration. In the absence in response to afferent input during different develop-
of such input, the duration of aIF during hatching leg mental stages may reflect the involvement of unique
movements is similar to that seen in the posthatching a I F p r e m o t o r elements during each stage or differences
behaviors (Figs. 5, 9). Thus, the apparent differences in in the n e u r o m o d u l a t o r y state of the nervous system.
aIF duration between pre- and posthatching behaviors Among the chick behaviors discussed so far, m a n y of
most likely reflects afferent m o d u l a t i o n of p r e m o t o r the differences in patterned activity are associated with
elements. We do not know yet the extent to which changes in either burst of the uniarticular knee exten-
phasic sensory information influences a I F duration sor, FT: FT1 and FT2. O u r results show that either
during embryonic motility. However, it is interesting FT1 or F T 2 activity could be absent in certain behav-
that afferent input from the legs affects the duration of iors, without disrupting the activity of the other mem-
a I F differently during different stages of development. bers of the synergies. However, in behaviors where
In the absence of phasic sensory inputs from the legs, input associated with the demands of a stance phase
the duration of a I F is brief during hatching (Fig. 9C), were diminished, such as in swimming, airstepping and
R. M. Johnston, A. Bekoff: Patterns of muscle activity during different behaviors in chicks 183

deafferented walking (Figs. 5, 9), FT2 activity did not Bradley NS, Bekoff A (1990) Development of coordinated move-
participate in the common synergies; instead FT2 was ment in chicks: I. Temporal analysis of hindlimb muscle syner-
recruited during the time between the two synergies in gies at embryonic days 9 and 10. Dev Psychobiol 23:763-782
Cazalets J-R, Nagy F, Moulins M (1990) Suppressive control
these behaviors (Fig. 5). The narrow range for recruit- of the crustacean pyloric network by a pair of identified inter-
ment phase of FT2 in all of the behaviors (Table 2) may neurons. I. Modulation of the motor pattern. J Neurosci 10:
reflect a less mutable feature of the underlying circuits 448457
generating leg movements in chicks. We suggest that to Cracraft J (1971) The functional morphology of the hind limb of the
domestic pigeon, Columba livia. Bull Am Mus Nat Hist 144:
produce different behaviors in chicks, the CNS organ-
171-268
izes leg motor output around common synergies; with- Dickinson P, Mecsas C, Marder E (1990) Neuropeptide fusion of
in these common synergies, the muscle activity can be two motor-pattern generator circuits. Nature 344:155 258
modified in tandem and on an individual basis. In Gatesy SM (1989) Archosaur neuromuscular and locomotor evolu-
addition, we suggest that the basic recruitment patterns tion. PhD thesis. Harvard University
Getting PA (1989) Emerging principles governing the operation of
and behavior continue even when activity from specific neural networks. Annu Rev Neurosci 12:185 204
muscles is absent completely or no longer part of the Getting PA, Dekin MS (1985) A model system for integration within
synergy. rhythmic motor systems. In: Selverston AI (ed) Model neural
networks and behavior. Plenum, New York, pp 3-20
Giuliani CA, Smith JL (1985) Development and characteristics of
Acknowledgements We thank WC Lemon, DL Nicholl, MR Wood airstepping in chronic spinal cats. J Neurosci 5:127~%1282
and the anonymous referees for their valuable comments on the Grillner S (1981) Control of locomotion in bipeds, tetrapods and
manuscript. The experiments reported here comply with The Prin- fish. In: Brooks VB (ed) The Handbook of Physiology, section 1.
ciples of Animal Care, publication No. 86 26, revised 1985 of the The Nervous System, vol. II. Am Physiol Soc, Bethesda,
National Institute of Health. This work was supported by NIH Maryland, pp 1179 1236
grant NS20310 to AB. Grillner S, Wall6n P (1985) Central pattern generators for locomo-
tion, with special reference to vertebrates. Annu Rev Neurosci 8:
233.-261
Gruner JA, Altman J (1980) Swimming in the rat: analysis of loco-
motor performance in comparison to stepping. Exp Brain Res
References 40:374 382
Harris-Warrick RM, Johnson BR (1989) Motor pattern networks:
Abraham GV, Loeb G (1985) The distal hindlimb musculature of the flexible foundations for rhythmical pattern production. In:Crew
cat. Patterns of normal use. Exp Brain Res 58:580 593 T J, Kelley DB (eds) Perspectives in neural systems and behavior.
American Veterinary Medicine Association (1993) Report of the A.R. Liss, New York, pp 55 71
AVMA panel on euthanasia. J Am Vet Med Assoc 202:229 249 Harris-Warrick RM, Marder E (1991) Modulation of neural net-
Ayers JL, Clarac F (1978) Neuromuscular strategies underlying works for behavior. Annu Rev Neurosci 14:39 57
different behavioral acts in a multifunctional crustacean leg Hooper SL, Moulins M (1989) Switching of a neuron from one
joint. J Comp Physiol 128:81 94 network to another by sensory-induced changes in membrane
Ayers JL, Davis WJ (1977) Neural control of locomotion in the properties. Science 244:1587-1589
lobster, Homarus americanus. J Comp Physiol 115:1 27 Hoy MG, Zernicke RF (1986) The role of intersegmental dynamics
B/issler U (1989) Muscle and reflex partitioning in insects? Behav during rapid limb oscillations. J Biomech 19:867 877
Brain Sci 12:646-647 Hoy MG, Zernicke RF, Smith JL (1985) Contrasting roles of inertial
B~ssler U, Wegner U (1983) Motor output of the denervated thor- and muscle moments at the knee and ankle during paw-shake
acic ventral nerve cord in the stick insect Carausius morosus. response. J Neurophysiol 54:1282 1294
J Exp Biol 105:127 145 Jacobson RD, Hollyday MA (1982) A behavioral and electromyo-
Bekoff A (1976) Ontogeny of leg motor output in the chick embryo: graphic study of walking in the chick. J Neurophysiol 48:
a neural analysis. Brain Res 106:271 291 238 256
Bekoff A (1986) Ontogeny of chicken motor behaviors: evidence for Johnston RM, BekoffAC (1992) Constrained and flexible features of
multi-use limb pattern generating circuitry. In: Grillner S, Stein rhythmical hindlimb movements in chicks: kinematic profiles of
PSG, Stuart DG, Forssberg H, Herman RM (eds) Neurobiology walking, swimming and airstepping. J Exp Biol 171:43-66
of vertebrate locomotion. Macmillian, Hampshire, England, Keller C, Heiligenberg W (1989) From distributed sensory process-
pp 433453 ing to discrete motor representations in the diencephalon of the
Bekoff A (1988) Embryonic motor output and movement patterns: electric fish, Eigenmannia. J Comp Physiol A 164:565-576
relationships to postnatal behavior. In: Smotherman WP, Koshiand GF, Smith JL (1989) Mutable and immutable features of
Robinson SR (eds) Behavior of the fetus. Telford Press New paw-shake responses after hindlimb deafferentation in the cat.
Jersey, pp 191 206 J Neurophysiol 62:162 173
Bekoff A, Kauer JA (1982) Neural control of hatching: role of neck Landmesser LT, O'Donavan MJ (1984) Activation patterns of em-
position in turning on hatching leg movements in post-hatching bryonic chick hindlimb muscles recorded in ovo and in an
chicks. J Comp Physiol 145:497 504 isolated spinal cord preparation. J Physiol 347:189 204
Bekoff A, Kauer JA (1984) Neural control of hatching: fate of the Lennard PR, Stein PSG (1977) Swimming movements elicited by
pattern generator for the leg movements of hatching in post- electrical stimulation of turtle spinal cord. I. Low-spinal and
hatching chicks. J Neurosci 4:2659 2666 intact preparations. J Neurophysiol 40:768 778
Bekoff A, Sabichi AL (1987) Sensory control of the initiation of Levine RB, Weeks JC (1989) Reorganization of neural circuits and
hatching in chicks: effects of local anesthetic injected into the behavior during insect metamorphosis. In: Crew TJ, Kelley DB
neck. Dev Psychobiol 20:489495 (eds) Perspectives in neural systems and behavior. A.R. Liss,
Bekoff A, Nusbaum MP, Sabichi AL, Clifford M (1987) Neural New York, pp 195 228
control of limb coordination: comparison of hatching and walk- Machlis L, Dodd PWD, Fentress JC (1985) The pooling fallacy:
ing motor output patterns in normal and deafl'erented chicks. problems arising when individuals contribute more than one
J Neurosci 7:2320 2330 observation to a data set. Z Tierpsychol 68:201 214
184 R.M. Johnston, A. Bekoff: Patterns of muscle activity during different behaviors in chicks

MacPherson JM (1991) How flexible are muscle synergies? In: Sokal RR, Rohlf FJ (1995) Biometry, 3rd edn. W.H. Freeman,
Humphrey DR, Freund HJ (eds) Motor control: Concepts and New York
issues. John Wiley, New York, pp 33-47 Stein PSG, Currie SN (1989) Motor program selection in the verte-
Meyrand P, Simmers J (1992) An unstable brain for stable behav- brate spinal cord: three forms of the scratch reflex in the turtle.
iors. In: Proceedings of the Third International Congress of In: Erber J, Menzel R, Pfliiger H-J, Todt D (eds) Neural mecha-
Neuroethology. McGill University Press, Montreal, p 64 nisms of behavior: Proceedings of the Second International
Meyrand P, Simmers J, Moulins M (1991) Construction of a Congress of Neuroethology. Georg Thieme, Stuttgart,
pattern-generating circuit with neurons of different networks. pp 178 179
Nature 351:60 63 Stein PSG, Mortin LI, Robertson GA (1986) The forms of a task and
Mortin LI, Stein PSG (1989) Spinal cord segments containing key their blends. In: Grillner S, Stein PSG, Stuart DG, Forssberg H,
elements of the central pattern generators for three forms of Herman RM (eds) Neurobiology of vertebrate locomotion.
scratch reflex in the turtle. J Neurosci 9:2285 2296 Macmillian, Hampshire, England, pp 185-19
Pfli,iger H-J, Burrows M (1978) Locust use the same basic motor Systat (1992) SYSTAT: Statistics, Version 5.2 Edition. SYSTAT,
patterns in swimming as in jumping and in kicking. J Exp Biol Evanston, IL
75:81 93 van Ingen Schenau GJ (1987) The unique action of bi-articular
Pratt CA, Loeb GE (1991) Functional complex muscles of the cat muscles in complex movements. J Anat 155: 1- 5
hindlimb. I. Patterns of activation across sartorius. Exp Brain van Ingen Schenau GJ (1990) On the action of biarticular muscles,
Res 85:243 256 a review. Neth J Zool 40:521 540
Raikow RJ (1985) Locomotor system. In: King AS, McLelland van Ingen Schenau GJ, Boots PJM, de Groot G, Snackers RJ, van
J (eds) Form and function in birds, vol. 3. Academic Press, Woensel W W L M (1992) The constrained control of force and
London, pp 57 147 position in multi-joint movements. Neuroscience 46:197 207
Ramirez JM, Pearson KG (1988) Generation of motor patterns of Weeks JC, Levine RB (1990) Postembryonic neuronal plasticity and
walking and flight in motoneurons supplying bifunctional its hormonal control during insect metamorphosis. Annu Rev
muscles in the locust. J Neurobiol 19:257 282 Neurosci 13:183 194
Settles HE, Hanken J, Bekoff A (1987) Functional compartments of Weimann JM, Meyrand P, Marder E (1991) Neurons that form
the chick iliofibularis (biceps) muscle. Am Zool 27: 23A multiple pattern generators: identification and multiple activity
Smith JL (1986) Hindlimb locomotion in the spinal cat: synergistic patterns of gastric/pyloric neurons in the crab stomatogastric
patterns, limb dynamics and novel blends. In: Grillner S, Stein system. J Neurophysiol 65:111-122
PSG, Stuart DG, Forssberg H, Herman RM (eds) Neurobiology Williams TL (1981) Experimental analysis of the gait and frequency
of vertebrate locomotion. Macmillian, Hampshire, England, of locomotion in the tortoise, with a simple mathematical de-
pp 185 199 scription. J Physiol (Lond) 310:307 320
Smith JL, Hoy MG, Koshland GF, Phillips DM, Zernicke RF (1985) Young JZ (1981) The life of vertebrates. Third edition. Oxford New
Intralimb coordination of the paw-shake response: a novel York, pp 327 334
mixed synergy. J Neurophysiol 54:1271-1281

Вам также может понравиться