Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 19

Blackwell Publishing LtdOxford, UKADBAddiction Biology1355-6215 2006 The Author(s).

Journal compilation 2006 Society for the Study of Addiction


2006
00
Original Article
Excessive ethanol drinking in P rats
Richard L. Bell et al.

REVIEW doi:10.1111/j.1369-1600.2006.00029.x

The alcohol-preferring P rat and animal models of


excessive alcohol drinking
Richard L. Bell1, Zachary A. Rodd1, Lawrence Lumeng2,3, James M. Murphy1 &
William J. McBride1,3
Departments of Psychiatry1, Medicine2 and Biochemistry3, Indiana University School of Medicine, USA

ABSTRACT

The alcohol-preferring, P, rat was developed by selective breeding to study ethanol drinking behavior and its conse-
quences. Characterization of this line indicates the P rat meets all of the criteria put forth for a valid animal model of
alcoholism, and displays, relative to their alcohol-non-preferring, NP, counterparts, a number of phenotypic traits asso-
ciated with alcohol abuse and alcoholism. Behaviorally, compared with NP rats, P rats are less sensitive to the sedative
and aversive effects of ethanol and more sensitive to the stimulatory effects of ethanol. Neurochemically, research with
the P line indicates the endogenous dopaminergic, serotonergic, GABAergic, opiodergic, and peptidergic systems may
be involved in a predisposition for alcohol abuse and alcoholism. Paralleling the clinical literature, genetically selected
P rats display levels of ethanol intake during adolescence comparable to that seen during adulthood. Binge drinking has
been associated with an increased risk for health and other problems associated with ethanol abuse. A model of binge-
like drinking during the dark cycle indicates that P rats will consume 6 g/kg/day of ethanol in as little as three 1-hour
access periods/day, which approximates the 24-hour intake of P rats with free-choice access to a single concentration
of ethanol. The alcohol deprivation effect (ADE) is a transient increase in ethanol intake above baseline values upon re-
exposure to ethanol access after an extended period of deprivation. The ADE has been proposed to be an animal model
of relapse behavior, with the adult P rat displaying a robust ADE after prolonged abstinence. Overall, these findings indi-
cate that the P rat can be effectively used in models assessing alcohol-preference, a genetic predisposition for alcohol
abuse and/or alcoholism, and excessive drinking using protocols of binge-like or relapse-like drinking.

Keywords Genetic, selection, ethanol, reward, reinforcement, conditioning.

Correspondence to: Richard L. Bell, Indiana University School of Medicine, Institute of Psychiatric Research, 791 Union Drive, Indianapolis, IN 46202,
USA. E-mail: ribell@iupui.edu

REVIEW

INTRODUCTION These concerns stemmed from the fact that, in general,


experimental manipulations [e.g. fluid deprivation
Animal models have been successfully used in developing (Sandi, Borrell & Guaza 1990), schedule-induced poly-
treatments for a number of medical and psychiatric dis- dipsia (Meisch 1976), scheduled availability (Holloway,
orders (e.g. McKinney 2001; Griffin 2002). An animal Bird & Devenport 1984), sucrose-fading (Samson 1986)
model has the advantage of allowing the experimenter to and forced induction of dependence (Deutsch & Eisner
control characteristics of the animals genetic back- 1977; also cf. Roberts et al. 2000)] are required to induce
ground, environment and prior drug exposure. In addi- appreciable levels of ethanol self-administration in com-
tion, an animal model allows for the examination of mon stock rats. Nevertheless, certain criteria for an ani-
neurobehavioral, neurochemical and neurophysiological mal model of alcoholism have been put forth (Lester &
correlates with the behavioral, physiological or neurolog- Freed 1973; Cicero 1979). Briefly, these criteria are as fol-
ical state that is modeled. These correlates in turn lows: (1) the animal should orally self-administer etha-
facilitate the development of pharmacological and/or nol; (2) the amount of ethanol consumed should result in
behavioral treatments for the disorder in question. There pharmacologically relevant blood ethanol levels; (3) eth-
have been reservations as to whether a valid animal anol should be consumed for its post-ingestive pharma-
model of alcoholism could be developed (Cicero 1979). cological effects, and not strictly for its caloric value or

2006 The Authors. Journal compilation 2006 Society for the Study of Addiction Addiction Biology, 11, 270288
Excessive ethanol drinking in P rats 271

taste; (4) ethanol should be positively reinforcing, in ethanol and ethanol functions as a reinforcer (Murphy
other words, the animals must be willing to work for eth- et al. 1989; Files et al. 1998; Samson et al. 1998; McKin-
anol; (5) chronic ethanol consumption should lead to the zie et al. 2000; Melendez et al. 2002; Rodd-Henricks et al.
expression of metabolic and functional tolerance; and (6) 2002a,b; Rodd et al. 2003). Whereas ethanol-nave P
chronic consumption of ethanol leads to dependence, as and NP rats display similar levels of ethanol clearance (Li
indicated by withdrawal symptoms after access to etha- & Lumeng 1977; Lumeng et al. 1982), P rats, given
nol is terminated. More recently, a seventh criterion has chronic free-choice (free-choice indicating that food and
been proposed (cf. McBride & Li 1998). These authors water are concurrently available with ethanol; i.e. the
suggested that because alcoholics generally go through animals are not deprived in any way) access to ethanol,
episodes of abstinence and relapse (Burish et al. 1981; drink sufficient amounts to develop metabolic tolerance
Hilbrom 1990; McMillen 1997), an animal model of (15% increase in ethanol elimination rate: Lumeng & Li
alcoholism should also display characteristics associated 1986) and tolerance to the motor impairing/ataxic
with relapse. One characteristic that should be displayed (Gatto et al. 1987) and aversive (Stewart et al. 1991)
is a loss of control when access to ethanol is reinstated fol- effects of ethanol. Furthermore, P rats develop depen-
lowing an extended period of ethanol abstinence. The dence with 24-hour free-choice drinking, as indicated by
alcohol deprivation effect (ADE) is a transient increase in physical signs upon ethanol withdrawal (Waller et al.
ethanol consumption and/or preference for ethanol over 1982; Kampov-Polevoy et al. 2000). Regarding relapse-
basal levels (average intake just prior to the deprivation like behavior, P rats will display an ADE after extended
interval) displayed by animals when given free-choice periods (28 weeks) of deprivation (Rodd-Henricks et al.
access to ethanol after a period of ethanol deprivation 2000b, 2001). Overall, the data indicate that the P line
(Sinclair & Senter 1967), and has been proposed to model satisfies the criteria proposed by Cicero (1979) and Lester
relapse-like behavior (e.g. Rodd et al. 2004b). & Freed (1973) for an animal model of alcoholism,
including the additional criterion for the display of
The alcohol-preferring P rat
relapse-like behavior (McBride & Li 1998). Table 1 lists
Through bidirectional selective breeding, lines of rats the proposed criteria for an animal model of alcoholism
that display high or low ethanol-drinking phenotypes and how the P line of rat fulfills these criteria.
based solely on selection history can be developed. Bidi-
Behavioral phenotypes
rectional selection, from a heterogeneous foundational
stock, is accomplished through systematic mating of Studies of human subjects with a family history of alco-
non-siblings from the same extreme of the normal distri- holism suggest that there is an association between a low
bution over successive generations, which results in level of response (or sensitivity) to ethanol and risk for the
divergent lines that exhibit the extremes of ethanol pref- development of alcoholism (Schuckit 1986, 1994). For
erence. A major advantage of selective breeding is that example, after an ethanol challenge, young adult family
the high ethanol-consuming animals display this behav- history positive (for alcoholism) males (Schuckit 1985;
ior without environmental manipulations to induce eth- Schuckit & Gold 1988) and females (Lex et al. 1988) dis-
anol preference. play less body sway than family history negative controls.
The alcohol-preferring P and alcohol-non-preferring With regard to sensitivity, compared with NP rats, P rats
NP lines of rats were developed by mass selection from a are less sensitive to the ataxic (Bell et al. 2001), hypoth-
Wistar foundation stock (Lumeng, Hawkins & Li 1977). ermic (Stewart et al. 1992), and aversive, as measured by
Regarding criteria for an animal model of alcoholism, P conditioned taste aversion (Froehlich et al. 1988) and
rats drink greater than 5 g of ethanol/kg body weight/ conditioned place aversion (Stewart et al. 1996), effects of
day; whereas NP rats will drink less than 1 g/kg/day ethanol. The development of tolerance to the sedative-
under these same conditions (Li et al. 1987b). Moreover, hypnotic and motor-impairing effects of ethanol may per-
P rats attain pharmacologically relevant blood alcohol mit the alcoholic to drink greater amounts of alcohol over
concentrations (BACs, 50200 mg%) under 24-hour time (Kalant, LeBlanc & Gibbins 1971). Therefore, an
and limited access conditions (Murphy et al. 1986; Rodd- association between ethanol preference and the develop-
Henricks et al. 2001; Bell et al. 2006). P rats appear to ment of ethanol tolerance has been proposed (Tabakoff &
self-administer ethanol for its post-ingestive effects, as Ritzman 1979; Erwin, McClearn &Kuse 1980; Spuhler &
indicated by the findings that these rats will self- Deitrich 1984; L & Kiianmaa 1988). Regarding toler-
administer ethanol both intragastrically (Waller et al. ance, P rats develop tolerance quicker than NP rats to the
1984; Murphy et al. 1988) and directly into the ventral ataxic (Bell et al. 2001) and hypnotic (Kurtz et al. 1996)
tegmental area (VTA: Gatto et al. 1994; Rodd et al. effects of ethanol. Additionally, chronic drinking by P rats
2005a,b,c). P rats will perform an operant response for results in metabolic tolerance (Lumeng & Li 1986), func-
access to ethanol, indicating P rats will work for access to tional tolerance to the ataxic effects of ethanol (Gatto

2006 The Authors. Journal compilation 2006 Society for the Study of Addiction Addiction Biology, 11, 270288
272 Richard L. Bell et al.

Table 1 Comparisons of the self-administration behavior of selectively bred P rats and proposed criteria for an animal model of alco-
holism (Lester & Freed 1973; Cicero 1979; McBride & Li 1998).

Animal model criteria Characteristics of P rats

Must orally self-administer ethanol under Adolescent (McKinzie et al. 1998; Bell et al. 2003, 2006), and adult (Murphy et al.
free-choice conditions. 1986; Rodd-Henricks et al. 2000b, 2001; Bell et al. 2006) P rats orally self-administer
greater than 5 g/kg/day ethanol under free-choice conditions.
Ethanol self-administration should lead to Oral self-administration of ethanol by P rats results in average BACs of 50200 mg%
pharmacologically relevant blood alcohol (Murphy et al. 1986; Rodd-Henricks et al. 2001; Bell et al. 2006)
concentrations (BACs).
Ethanol should be positively reinforcing, P rats will operantly respond for ethanol, even with high workload (fixed-ratio)
such that the animal will work for ethanol requirements (Murphy et al. 1989; Files et al. 1998; Samson et al. 1998, Ciccocioppo,
access. Angeletti & Weiss 2001; Rodd et al. 2003), up to 30 or more bar presses for a single
0.1 ml ethanol dipper presentation (Ciccocioppo et al. 2001; Rodd et al. 2003).
Ethanol should be self-administered for its P rats will operantly respond to self-administer ethanol intragastrically (Waller et al.
post-ingestive, pharmacological effects, 1984; Murphy et al. 1988) or directly into the ventral tegmental area (VTA: Gatto et al.
and not solely for its taste or for calories. 1994; Rodd et al. 2005a,b,c).
Chronic ethanol consumption should lead P rats drink sufficient amounts of ethanol to develop metabolic tolerance (Lumeng & Li
to metabolic and functional tolerance. 1986) and tolerance to the motor impairing (Gatto et al. 1987) and aversive effects of
ethanol (Stewart et al. 1991).
Chronic ethanol consumption and P rats develop dependence with 24-hour free-choice drinking, as indicated by physical
termination of ethanol access should signs upon ethanol withdrawal (Waller et al. 1982; Kampov-Polevoy et al. 2000).
lead to physical signs of withdrawal.
After chronic consumption and a P rats will display an alcohol deprivation effect (ADE) after chronic consumption of
prolonged deprivation period, the animal ethanol and long periods (28 weeks) of deprivation (Rodd-Henricks et al. 2000b,
should display relapse-like behavior. 2001).

et al. 1987), and increases the threshold for the aversive also been shown in P, compared with NP, rats following
effects of ethanol (Stewart et al. 1991). low-to-moderate dose (1.0 g/kg) ethanol (Bell et al.
It has been hypothesized that the stimulatory or 2001); (2) high novelty-seeking behavior, defined as
behaviorally activating effects of ethanol and other drugs behavioral activation in the presence of an unknown
may be a manifestation of the reinforcing effects of stimulus (Cloninger 1996), which appears to be predic-
abused drugs (Wise & Bozarth 1987). Regarding the tive of current and future alcohol abuse in humans
stimulatory effects of ethanol, adolescent (Rodd et al. (Andrucci et al. 1989), similarly, in response to a novel
2004) and adult (Waller et al. 1986) P rats display olfactory stimulus, P rats display greater increases in
greater locomotor activation after low doses of ethanol, activity than NP rats (Nowak et al. 2000); (3) greater
compared with NP rats. Furthermore, ethanol self- increases in heart rate after ethanol consumption than
administration results in increased locomotor activity family history negative subjects, and that non-alcoholic
(Bell et al. 2002; Melendez et al. 2002) and heart rate family history positive individuals, compared with family
(Bell et al. 2002), compared with P rats self-administer- history negative individuals, display greater increases in
ing water or saccharin. Using electroencephalographic heart rate on the ascending side of the blood alcohol
and event-related potential (ERP)-associated procedures, curve (Conrod, Pihl & Ditto 1995; Conrod et al. 1997;
the P rat displays greater stimulation under basal condi- Conrod, Pihl & Vassileva 1998), regarding basic research,
tions or following an ethanol challenge than NP rats P rats display increased heart rate when consuming eth-
(Morzorati et al. 1988; Ehlers et al. 1991; Slawecki et al. anol, and this increase is greater and longer than that
1999). seen after consuming other palatable solutions (e.g. sac-
In general, the P line of rat displays many behavioral charin, Bell et al. 2002), although studies comparing this
and physiological characteristics found in alcoholics and with NP and/or control lines have yet to be carried out;
children of alcoholics, compared with non-alcoholics and and (4) regarding ethanol reinforcement, contextual
children of non-alcoholics. For example, family history cues associated with drug (self-) administration and
positive, for alcoholism, individuals display (1) lower sen- mental representation of these cues can elicit auto-
sitivity to ethanol-induced motor impairment (Schuckit nomic activation (OBrien et al. 1992; Childress et al.
1985; Lex et al. 1988; Schuckit & Gold 1988), which has 1993; Rajan et al. 1998), and alcoholics display greater

2006 The Authors. Journal compilation 2006 Society for the Study of Addiction Addiction Biology, 11, 270288
Excessive ethanol drinking in P rats 273

autonomic activation in the presence of these cues, com- Moreover, the ethanol self-administration behavior of
pared with controls (e.g. Drummond, Cooper & Glautier the P rat reveals some important parallels between an
1990), similarly, P rats display increased heart rate in an animal model of alcoholism and criteria for a diagnosis of
environment associated with ethanol self-administra- alcohol abuse or alcoholism (Table 2), as put forth by the
tion, in the absence of ethanol (Bell et al. 2002, 2004a), Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders
and P rats with prior home-cage access to ethanol display (Fourth Edition, DSM-IV: American Psychiatric Associa-
this effect to a greater extent than those without prior tion 1994). However, it should be kept in mind that the
home-cage access (Bell et al. unpublished observations). analogy is done with a very broad interpretation, and the
Regarding alcoholic typologies, P rats display many of the animal model cannot relate to the societal consequences
characteristics associated with Cloningers Type 2 alco- implied in the DSM-IV criteria. For example, under
holic subtype (Cloninger 1987). For example, P rats relapse conditions, the P rat does exhibit loss of control
display (1) a genetic predisposition for high ethanol over drinking, as illustrated by alcohol intakes reaching
consumption (cf. McBride & Li 1998; Murphy et al. 2002; 46 g/kg within the first 2 hours after ethanol was
Bell et al. 2005); (2) display adult levels of ethanol con- returned, but this may not be the same as that implied in
sumption during adolescence (discussed below, McKinzie the DSM-IV criterion of inability to control alcohol use.
et al. 1999; Bell et al. 2003, 2006); and (3) display the In addition, the DSM-IV criterion of excessive amount of
impulsivity/sensation-seeking discussed above (Nowak time obtaining or using alcohol may not be the same as
et al. 2000). However, P rats do not appear to be more P rats spending a lot of the time in an operant session bar-
aggressive than their NP counterparts or Wistar controls pressing for ethanol. Still, it is important to try to draw
(Bell et al. unpublished observations), a characteristic parallels between human and animal studies, even with
often associated with the Cloninger Type 2 alcoholic the foreknowledge that there are weaknesses in such
subtype. parallels.

Table 2 Comparisons of the self-administration behavior of selectively bred P rats and diagnostic criteria for alcohol abuse or depen-
dence (American Psychiatric Association 1994).

DSM-IV Characteristics of P rats

Tolerance is displayed by either Chronic oral self-administration of ethanol leads to metabolic tolerance
(a) increased amounts of ethanol needed to (Lumeng & Li 1986), tolerance to the aversive effects of alcohol (Gatto et al.
achieve desired effect or 1987; Stewart et al. 1991) and greater amounts consumed (Stewart et al.
(b) diminished effect with continued level of us 1991).
Alcohol is consumed to either Oral self-administration of ethanol is increased during and/or after withdrawal
(a) avoid expression of withdrawal symptoms or of ethanol access (Rodd-Henricks et al. 2000b, 2001).
(b) to relieve withdrawal symptoms
Alcohol is consumed either Chronic ethanol drinking leads to increased amounts consumed over time
(a) in greater quantities or (Stewart et al. 1991; Bell et al. 2003, 2006).
(b) over a longer period than desired
Inability to control alcohol use Reinstatement of ethanol consumption and the expression of an alcohol
deprivation effect (Rodd-Henricks et al. 2000b, 2001).
Excessive amount of time is spent in Will operantly respond for ethanol, even with high workload (fixed-ratio)
(a) obtaining, requirements (Murphy et al. 1989; Files et al. 1998; Samson et al. 1998;
(b) using or Ciccocioppo et al. 2001; Rodd et al. 2003), up to 30 or more bar presses for a
(c) recovering from the effects of alcohol single 0.1 ml ethanol dipper presentation (Ciccocioppo et al. 2001; Rodd et al.
2003).
Alcohol use leads to forsaking important Under limited access (Murphy et al. 1986; Nowak et al. 2000) or relapse-like
(a) social, (Bell et al. unpublished observations; Rodd et al. unpublished observations)
(b) occupational and/or conditions, will self-administer ethanol until impaired/intoxicated. Also, will
(c) recreational activities maintain their ethanol preference and volume of intake when nutritive and
non-nutritive, highly palatable solutions are concurrently available (Li et al.
1987a; Lankford et al. 1991).
Alcohol use is continued despite knowledge of Not able to test
persisting problems associated with alcohol use

DSM-IV, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders Fourth Edition.

2006 The Authors. Journal compilation 2006 Society for the Study of Addiction Addiction Biology, 11, 270288
274 Richard L. Bell et al.

Neurochemical phenotypes In many instances, the innate neurochemical differ-


ences seen between ethanol-nave P and NP rats parallel
Regarding neurochemical correlates, clinical and basic findings from both the basic and clinical literature. For
research indicate that the central nervous systems sero- example (1) lower levels of CNS serotonin are also present
tonergic (5-HT, cf. Lovinger 1999), dopaminergic (DA, cf. in high alcohol-drinking (HAD: Gongwer et al. 1989) and
Heinz 2002), GABAergic and glutamatergic (cf. Davis & Sardinian alcohol-preferring (sP: Casu et al. 2004; De
Wu 2001), cholinergic (Ach, cf. Soderpalm et al. 2000), Montis et al. 2004) rats compared with their low alcohol-
opiodergic (cf. Herz 1997), peptidergic [e.g. neuropeptide drinking counterparts (low alcohol-drinking, LAD, and
Y (NPY), cf. Heilig & Thorsell 2002] and hypothalamic- Sardinian alcohol-non-preferring, sNP, respectively); (2)
pituitary-adrenal (HPA, cf. Gianoulakis et al. 1995; Keith lower levels of DA metabolites or DA D2 receptors are seen
et al. 1995; Rasmussen et al. 2002) systems are involved in HAD (Gongwer et al. 1989) and sP (Stefanini et al.
in alcohol abuse and alcoholism. Ethanol-nave P rats 1992), compared with LAD and sNP rats, respectively;
have lower levels of 5-HT and 5-HIAA in cortical and (3) increased levels of GABAergic terminals in the accum-
limbic regions than NP rats (adult: Murphy et al. 1982, bens are seen in HAD, compared with LAD rats (Hwang
1987; weanling: Strother et al. 2005). Additionally, et al. 1990); (4) increased mu-receptor levels are seen in
there are fewer 5-HT immunostained fibers in the ante- high alcohol-consuming Alko Alcohol (AA) rats com-
rior frontal cortex, accumbens and part of the ventral pared with their low alcohol-consuming Alko Non-alco-
hippocampus of ethanol-nave P rats, compared with NP hol (ANA) counterparts (Soini et al. 1998, 1999); and (5)
rats (Zhou et al. 1991a,b), with there being fewer 5-HT lower levels of NPY content or NPY receptors are seen in
immunostained neurons in the dorsal and median raphe, AA (Caberlotto et al. 2001) and HAD (Hwang et al. 1999)
as well (Zhou et al. 1991c). Ethanol-nave P rats have rats compared with ANA and LAD rats, respectively.
lower levels of DA and its metabolites (DOPAC and HVA) These results highlight promising common differences
in limbic regions, compared with NP rats (adult: Murphy associated with disparate ethanol drinking behaviors
et al. 1982, 1987; weanling: Strother et al. 2005). Zhou that should be considered for future studies. Regarding
et al. (1995) found fewer DA neuronal projections from the effects of ethanol, Katner & Weiss (2001) carried out
the VTA to the accumbens in ethanol-nave P rats rela- an elegant study from which they reported a significant
tive to NP rats. Moreover, oral self-administration of eth- positive association between increases in extracellular DA
anol, contingent on operant responding, was found to in the accumbens after an ethanol challenge and a pro-
increase extracellular DA levels to a greater extent in the pensity for high alcohol consumption using multiple lines
accumbens of P compared with Wistar rats (Weiss et al. of selectively bred high alcohol-consuming, AA and HAD,
1993), suggesting that the VTA-DA system of the P rat and low alcohol-consuming, ANA and LAD, lines of rats.
may be more sensitive to the reinforcing actions of etha- Additionally, as noted above, these authors had reported
nol than that of the Wistar rat. Ethanol-nave P rats also a similar association using P and Wistar rats previously
have reduced numbers of D2 receptors in the VTA and (Weiss et al. 1993). The clinical literature indicates that
accumbens compared with NP rats (adult: McBride et al. specific genetic polymorphisms of (1) the serotonin trans-
1993; periadolescent: Strother et al. 2003). Regarding porter (SERT: Kweon et al. 2005); (2) the DA D2 receptor
GABA, higher densities of GABAergic terminals were (Konishi et al. 2004; Conner et al. 2005; Johann et al.
identified in the accumbens of ethanol-naive P rats, com- 2005) and dopamine transporter (DAT: Khnke et al.
pared with NP rats (Hwang et al. 1990). Pertaining to 2005); (3) the GABA-A-alpha 2 receptor subtype (Eden-
peptides, McBride et al. (1998) reported higher densities berg et al. 2004; Lappalainen et al. 2005); and (4) NPY
of mu-opioid receptors in central nervous system (CNS) (Lappalainen et al. 2002; Mottagui-Tabar et al. 2005) are
limbic areas of ethanol-nave P relative to NP rats, associated with the development of alcoholism.
including the accumbens shell and core. Lower levels of
The P rat and animal models of excessive
NPY were found in the amygdala, hippocampus, and
ethanol consumption
frontal cortex of ethanol-nave P, compared with NP, rats
(Ehlers et al. 1998). Similarly, lower levels of corticotro- As can be seen from the above, substantial progress has
pin-releasing factor (CRF) were found in the amygdala, been made in determining neurobehavioral, neurophys-
hypothalamus, prefrontal cortex, and cingulate cortex of iological and neurochemical phenotypes associated with
ethanol-nave P, compared with NP, rats (Ehlers et al. a genetic propensity to abuse ethanol. To a great extent,
1992). Overall, the neurochemical differences observed this progress has been made with animal models of eth-
between the P and NP rat indicate that multiple neu- anol preference (of which the alcohol-preferring, P, ver-
rotransmitter and neuromodulatory systems may con- sus alcohol-non-preferring, NP, pair of rat lines is but one
tribute to the disparate alcohol drinking behaviors of of several selectively bred high versus low alcohol-
these lines. consuming pairs of rat lines). Nonetheless, use of the P rat

2006 The Authors. Journal compilation 2006 Society for the Study of Addiction Addiction Biology, 11, 270288
Excessive ethanol drinking in P rats 275

line has also proved advantageous in examining binge- from risky drinking (reaching a BAC between 50 and
like and relapse-like drinking. Binge drinking during high 80 mg%) and a bender (2 or more days of sustained
school and college is becoming more prevalent and is also heavy drinking).
a strong predictor of future ethanol-related problems in Given the criteria for a valid animal model of alcohol-
both men and women in North America (Presley, Meil- ism, discussed above, along with the definitions of binge,
man & Lyerla 1994; Wechsler et al. 2000; Wechsler & and bender, drinking in clinical populations, we propose
Nelson 2001; Bloomfield et al. 2003) and Europe (Gill that an animal model of excessive ethanol drinking
2002; Bloomfield et al. 2003; Kuntsche, Rehm & Gmel should exhibit certain criteria (Table 3). Excessive etha-
2004; Gmel et al. 2006). The deleterious effects of etha- nol consumption should produce repeated, sustainable
nol abuse on societal health are staggering (cf. Brienza & BACs in the range of 100 mg% or higher. Excessive etha-
Stein 2002; Room, Babor & Rehm 2005). Average vol- nol drinking should be demonstrated under free-choice
ume of ethanol consumed is generally the major factor conditions in which water, or another palatable solution,
contributing to the onset and development of these med- is available in addition to the ethanol. Ethanol should be
ical conditions (cf. Rehm et al. 2003; Room et al. 2005). consumed for its post-ingestive CNS effects and not solely
However, pattern of drinking, specifically irregular heavy due to taste factors or caloric value; in addition, excessive
usage (binge-like drinking), appears to play as large a ethanol consumption should not be a result of fluid dep-
role, if not larger, in the onset and development of some rivation. An excessive ethanol drinking protocol should
diseases (e.g. cardiovascular disease: Rehm et al. 2003; produce signs of intoxication (e.g. motor impairment),
Room et al. 2005). and should result in the development of tolerance to
The National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcohol- ethanol-induced effects (e.g. motor impairment/sedative
ism (NIAAA) National Advisory Council (2004) has effects), and signs of physical dependence. Some of these
adopted the following definition of binge drinking: A criteria, e.g. sustainable BACs greater than 100 mg%,
binge is a pattern of drinking that brings BAC to have been proposed by others, as well (e.g. Rhodes et al.
80 mg% or above. For the typical adult, this pattern cor- 2005).
responds to consuming five or more drinks (male), or four Previous work from our laboratory indicated that P
or more drinks (female), in about 2 hours. Binge drinking rats consume most of their ethanol in bouts during the
is clearly dangerous for the drinker and for society. In the dark cycle (Murphy et al. 1986). A bout is defined as a
above definition, a drink refers to half an ounce of alco- cluster of drinking behavior that is sufficiently aggre-
hol (e.g. one 12-oz beer, one 5-oz glass of wine, or one gated such that the organism must stop ongoing behavior
1.5-oz shot of distilled spirits). Binge drinking is distinct in order to carry out this cluster of drinking behavior.

Table 3 Criteria for an animal model of excessive ethanol consumption.

Proposed criteria Characteristics of P rats

Excessive ethanol consumption should produce P rats achieve BACs of 100 mg% or greater under
repeated, sustainable blood alcohol 1. Binge-like conditions: drinking-in-the-dark multiple scheduled access
concentrations (BACs) of 100 mg% or greater. (Fig. 2).
2. Relapse-like conditions: expression of an alcohol deprivation effect, with
home-cage (Rodd-Henricks et al. 2000b, 2001) or operant
self-administration (Rodd et al. 2003).
Excessive ethanol consumption is displayed P rats display excessive ethanol consumption under binge-like (Fig. 2) and
under voluntary free-choice conditions. relapse-like conditions (Fig. 4, Rodd-Henricks et al. 2000b, 2001), with food and
water available ad libitum.
Ethanol is consumed for its post-ingestive central P rats consume ethanol for its post-ingestive effects (Waller et al. 1984; Murphy
nervous system effects and not solely due to et al. 1988); excessive ethanol consumption does not require addition of
taste, caloric value, or fluid deprivation. sweeteners, or food or fluid deprivation (Rodd-Henricks et al. 2001; Figs 2 and 4).
Signs of intoxication (e.g. ataxia) are displayed Anecdotally, P rats display a loss of righting reflex after binge-like (Bell et al.
following an episode of excessive ethanol unpublished observations) and relapse-like (Rodd et al. unpublished
consumption. observations) ethanol consumption.
Tolerance to ethanol-induced effects develops Has not been tested.
following chronic excessive ethanol drinking.
Signs of dependence are displayed after chronic Has not been tested.
excessive ethanol consumption and withdrawal.

2006 The Authors. Journal compilation 2006 Society for the Study of Addiction Addiction Biology, 11, 270288
276 Richard L. Bell et al.

For example, a brief interruption of feeding behavior to rats (Wolfgramm & Heyne 1995; Holter et al. 1998) and
obtain fluid in order to soften the food, with an immediate adult (Rodd-Henricks et al. 2001) and periadolescent
return to feeding behavior, would not be considered a (Bell et al. 2003) selectively bred P rats. In one study of
bout, at least not in the present context. However, a binge-like drinking, adult male P rats were given concur-
period of fluid consumption upon waking, generally sub- rent, free-choice access to 15% and 30% ethanol. Ethanol
stantial across species, to replenish fluids lost during sleep access was given 5 days per week, with 24-hour access
would be considered a bout of drinking behavior, within during the first 2 weeks and two 1-hour access sessions
this context. The circadian microstructure of ethanol self- for the next 4 weeks. Animals were housed on a reverse
administration by P rats has not been evaluated. There- light-dark cycle (lights off 09:00 hour), with the first 1-
fore, in a recent study, our laboratory examined free- hour access period starting at lights out (09:00 hour),
choice home-cage ethanol (15%) drinking behavior of and the second 1-hour access period starting at
male and female, periadolescent and adult P rats. It was 13:00 hour. On the first ethanol access day of the seventh
found that, in general, male and female P rats will con- week, animals were given their usual 09:00 hour
sume ethanol in discrete bouts across the dark cycle dur- through 10:00 hour access period, and sacrificed at
ing which amounts of 1 g/kg/bout ethanol or greater are 10:00 hour to evaluate trunk BACs. According to the
self-administered (Bell et al. 2006). In Fig. 1, the individ- record of ethanol licking behavior, greater than 95% of
ual licking patterns for eight adult female P rats on their the ethanol was consumed in the first 12 minutes of this
30th day of ethanol access are depicted. These individual 1 hour access session (Fig. 2, lower panel). As seen in
records indicate that (1) there is some heterogeneity in Fig. 2 (upper panel), there was a strong correlation
ethanol licking behavior (e.g. the estimated licking between amount of ethanol consumed and BAC
requirement to self-administer 1 g/kg of ethanol per bout, achieved: r = 0.81, P < 0.001 (Bell et al. unpublished
see below, differs markedly between some animals) findings).
among adult female P rats, despite their sharing a com- In another study using the DID-MSA procedure, adult
mon genetically selected phenotype of ethanol preference male P rats were given concurrent access to 15% and
over water; (2) the animals consumed ethanol in discrete 30% ethanol, with ad lib access to water and, except
bouts with the number of licks displayed often exceeding where indicated, food. Rats were housed on a 12-hour
the number required to self-administer 1 g/kg of ethanol reverse dark-light cycle (lights off 09:00 hour). For the
per bout (estimated number of licks to self-administer binge-like group, ethanol was made available 5 days a
1 g/kg/bout = total licks on the ethanol bottle per day week, with three 1-hour access periods spaced 2 hours
divided by the total g/kg ethanol self-administered per apart, starting 1 hour into the dark cycle (10:00 hour).
day: horizontal bar in graphs); (3) these bouts often On the first ethanol access day of the 9th week, continu-
occurred in time spans of 6 minutes or less, which sug- ous access animals (24-hour/day) were sacrificed at
gests appreciable (90 mg% or higher) blood ethanol con- 10:00 hour (after their first hour in the dark), and the
centrations are achieved repeatedly (cf. Murphy et al. binge-like animals were sacrificed at 11:00 hour (after
2002); and (4) total time per day spent consuming etha- their first hour of ethanol access), to evaluate trunk
nol was 1 hour or less for most animals. Additionally, the BACs. Half of the continuous animals had their food
individual licking patterns for adult male P rats revealed removed at dark onset (09:00 hour), and half of the DID-
these same characteristics (Bell et al. 2006). MSA animals had their food removed at the beginning of
their 1-hour ethanol access period (10:00 hour). As seen
An animal model of binge-like drinking
in Fig. 3 (upper panels), there was a strong correlation
Given the P rats propensity for bout-like drinking of eth- between amount of ethanol consumed and BAC ac-
anol during the dark cycle, we have examined their drink- hieved: r = 0.82, P = 0.003; r = 0.92, P < 0.001, for the
ing behavior when given multiple 1-hour access periods continuous (A) and binge-like animals (B), respectively
across the dark cycle (Bell et al. 2004b), and this proce- (Bell et al. unpublished findings). Note that the 1 hour of
dure [drinking-in-the-dark multiple scheduled access food restriction essentially doubled the BACs achieved
(DID-MSA)] has been used to assess the consequences of without significantly affecting overall ethanol intake (i.e.
binge-like drinking in these rats (e.g. proteomic changes the two continuous groups, essentially, drank the same
following binge-like access to ethanol: Kimpel et al. amount of ethanol, with a similar effect seen for the
2005). Toward this end, our laboratory has implemented binge-like animals). The reduced BACs seen in binge-like
the use of concurrent access to multiple concentrations of rats in Fig. 3, compared with Fig. 2 (upper panel), may
ethanol in studies examining excessive ethanol consump- have been due to lower food intake in binge-like rats of
tion. This addition to our protocols resulted from findings Fig. 2 (these animals had the beginning of their first hour
that the concurrent availability of multiple concentra- of ethanol access coincide with lights off, whereas the
tions of ethanol increases ethanol intake in adult outbred binge-like rats in Fig. 3, had the beginning of their first

2006 The Authors. Journal compilation 2006 Society for the Study of Addiction Addiction Biology, 11, 270288
Excessive ethanol drinking in P rats 277

Figure 1 For descriptive purposes, individual ethanol licking patterns for eight adult female P rats on the 30th day of ethanol access are pre-
sented. It is noteworthy that the ethanol licking behavior of female P rats is a heterogeneous phenomenon, with different rats displaying dif-
ferent patterns of drinking and/or different estimated number of licks [total licks per day/total ethanol (g/kg) consumed per day] to self-
administer 1 g/kg of ethanol per bout. By the 30th day of ethanol access, adult female P rats were self-administering an estimated 1 g/kg body
weight of ethanol in bouts taking less than 6 minutes to complete (each vertical bar represents a 6-minute bin), with the total time spent con-
suming ethanol per day being less than 1 hour for many animals. The horizontal lines indicate the estimated number of licks required to self-
administer 1 g/kg/bout of ethanol. The number in the upper right corner refers to the subject ID

2006 The Authors. Journal compilation 2006 Society for the Study of Addiction Addiction Biology, 11, 270288
278 Richard L. Bell et al.

three separate sessions (Fig. 3), then total ethanol intake


per day exceeds one of the original criteria for selective
breeding of the P line of rat (i.e. greater than 5 g/kg is self-
administered each day). These findings indicate that the
P rat can be effectively used in an animal model of binge-
like drinking. This model resembles human binge drink-
ing in that in any one ethanol access session, the P rat
attains BACs of at least 80 mg% within a 2-hour time
frame. However, this model does not resemble human
binge drinking, which does not usually occur in 5-day
patterns with three or four 1-hour sessions each day.
Nonetheless, excessive ethanol drinking is attained with
this limited access procedure, and this offers a model for
studying the neurobiological basis and consequences of
excessive ethanol consumption.

An animal model of relapse-like drinking

As indicated above, the NIAAA National Advisory Coun-


cil (2004) has adopted definitions of binge and bender
drinking. For binge drinking, an individual consumes
enough ethanol in a 2-hour period for their BAC to reach
and/or exceed 80 mg%. For bender drinking, the individ-
ual engages in sustained heavy drinking for at least
2 days. Although a formal BAC level was not indicated
when defining bender drinking, for the purpose of an
animal model, sustainable BACs in excess of 100 mg%
Figure 2 Blood alcohol concentrations (BACs) achieved and should be repeatedly achieved across days. For many indi-
amount of ethanol consumed by adult male P rats during the first
viduals recovering from alcoholism, relapse is a monu-
hour of drinking on the seventh Monday of ethanol access, using a
mental problem (cf. Chiauzzi 1991; Weiss et al. 2001;
drinking-in-the-dark multiple scheduled access (DID-MSA) proce-
dure. Animals had concurrent access to 15% and 30% ethanol 5 days Barrick & Connors 2002; Jaffe 2002). In general, a
per week, with 24-hour access given the first 2 weeks, and two 1- relapse is characterized by sustained heavy drinking
hour sessions per day, the first at the beginning of the dark cycle and across days, which is the definition of a bender. The ADE
the second 3 hours later, for the remainder of the experiment. The has been used to assess relapse-like behavior in P rats (e.g.
regression analysis of the amount of ethanol consumed and BAC lev- Rodd-Henricks et al. 2000b, 2001) and has been pro-
els achieved revealed a significant correlation (r = 0.81, P < 0.001), posed to be an animal model of alcohol relapse (e.g. Rodd
with the average amount consumed being 2.6 g/kg, and average BAC
et al. 2004). The ADE is defined as a temporary increase
achieved being 120 mg%, top panel. Evaluation of the ethanol licking
behavior revealed that practically all of the ethanol was consumed
in the ratio of ethanol/total fluid intake and volume
within the first 12 minutes, bottom panel of ethanol intake over baseline drinking conditions
(baseline = average ethanol consumption across the
3 days before the deprivation interval), when ethanol is
hour of ethanol access start 1 hour after lights off). reinstated following a period of alcohol deprivation (Sin-
Therefore, when using this model and the beginning of clair & Senter 1967). The ADE can be observed following
the first hour of access coincides with lights off, P rats short (12-hour or less; Sinclair & Li 1989) or long (up to
achieve BACs exceeding criteria for binge-like drinking 75 days; Sinclair, Walker & Jordan 1973) deprivation
(NIAAA National Advisory Council 2004). intervals. The ADE phenomenon has been reported in
The utility of the DID-MSA procedure stems from the humans (Mello & Mendelson 1972; Burish et al. 1981),
fact that P rats can self-administer approximately 2 g/kg/ mice (Salimov et al. 1993), monkeys (Sinclair 1971; Kor-
1 hour session repeatedly. Depending upon the feeding net, Goosen & Van Ree 1990) and rats (Sinclair & Senter
behavior of the rats (i.e. is the first hour of access initiated 1967; Heyser, Schulteis & Koob 1997). However, in stud-
at lights off, or is short-term food restriction imple- ies with outbred animals (i.e. not selectively bred for
mented), BACs exceeding 120 mg% are readily attained. an ethanol preference), the ethanol intakes before and
Note that these BACs may not reflect peak BACs, which after deprivation do not necessarily reflect high intakes,
arguably may occur prior to the end of the 1 hour sched- such that blood ethanol levels achieved may not be
uled drinking session. When ethanol is made available for incapacitating.

2006 The Authors. Journal compilation 2006 Society for the Study of Addiction Addiction Biology, 11, 270288
Excessive ethanol drinking in P rats 279

Figure 3 Blood alcohol concentrations (BACs) achieved and amount of ethanol consumed by adult male P rats given either three 1-hour
free-choice access periods (the binge-like group), starting 1 hour into the dark cycle, and spaced 2 hours apart across the dark cycle or con-
tinuous ethanol access.The binge-like access animals had concurrent access to 15% and 30% ethanol 5 days per week, whereas the continuous
access animals had concurrent access to 15% and 30% ethanol 24 hours/day 7 days per week. The upper panels (a and b) depict the amount
of ethanol consumed and BACs achieved after the first hour in the dark for the continuous animals (a) and after the first hour of access for
the binge-like animals (b) on the ninth Monday of ethanol access. To assess the effects of 1-hour food restriction (open circles) half of each
group had their food removed at the beginning of the dark cycle (continuous) or beginning of their first hour of ethanol access (binge-like).
There were significant correlations between amount of ethanol consumed and BACs achieved for the continuous (r = 0.83, P = 0.003) and
binge-like (r = 0.92, P < 0.001) groups. One-hour food restriction resulted in significantly higher BACs achieved without affecting overall eth-
anol intake for both groups (continuous: 1.5 g/kg and 45 mg% versus 1.7 g/kg and 101 mg%; binge-like: 2.1 g/kg and 59 mg% versus 2.3 g/kg
and 129 mg%, for the ad lib and food restricted subgroups, respectively). The lower panel (c) indicates that the animals with three 1-hour eth-
anol access periods per day consumed approximately 6, or more, g/kg/day, whereas the animals with continuous access consumed approx-
imately 10, or more, g/kg/day. The lower panel (d) indicates that the amounts of ethanol consumed per hour by the binge-like access animals
during the third hour on Mondays (averaged across weeks) were higher than that seen for Fridays (averaged across weeks), but this did not
reach statistical significance (P > 0.05; i.e. there was no alcohol deprivation effect over the weekend)

In one study, with single deprivation periods of 2 first day that ethanol access is restored, and this elevated
8 weeks, and 24-hour free-choice access to 10% ethanol, drinking is maintained on the second day of re-exposure
adult female P rats displayed increased ethanol intakes as well (Rodd-Henricks et al. 2001). To place this in the
from 5 g/kg/day (baseline), just prior to deprivation, to context of basic research, with selectively bred high alco-
around 9 g/kg/day on the first day of re-exposure to eth- hol-consuming rats, it should be noted that in the HAD
anol access (Rodd-Henricks et al. 2000b), which reflects replicate lines, the expression of an ADE is dependent
an 80% increase in ethanol drinking. Under these condi- upon repeated deprivations, at least when a single con-
tions, ethanol intake returns toward baseline levels dur- centration of ethanol is made available (Rodd-Henricks
ing the second day. However, if multiple concentrations of et al. 2000a). Also, neither the AA (Hilakivi et al. 1984;
ethanol (10%, 20% and 30%) are concurrently available, Sinclair & Tiihonen 1988; Sinclair & Li 1989) nor the sP
along with water, the intakes also increase by 80% on the rat (Agabio et al. 2000) display an ADE across the first

2006 The Authors. Journal compilation 2006 Society for the Study of Addiction Addiction Biology, 11, 270288
280 Richard L. Bell et al.

24-hour period of ethanol re-exposure following an deprived for just 2 weeks (Rodd-Henricks et al. 2000b),
extended (2-week) deprivation interval. Thus, the P suggesting that neuronal alterations are occurring dur-
rat is unique among the selectively bred high alcohol- ing this deprivation period, which result in an extended
consuming rat lines in the expression of an ADE following bender. In another study (Rodd-Henricks et al. 2001), in
a single extended deprivation period. which adult female P rats had concurrent access to mul-
Research has indicated that the drinking pattern of tiple concentrations (10%, 20% and 30%) of ethanol and
most alcoholics includes periodic intervals of voluntary water, the animals were taken through four cycles of
or imposed abstinence (cf. Finney & Moos 1991; 2 weeks of deprivation from and 2 weeks of re-exposure to
McMillen 1997). Because this is part of the criteria for ethanol access following an initial 6 weeks of 24-hour,
assessing relapse-like drinking in a valid animal model of free-choice drinking. Animals were housed on a reverse
alcoholism (cf. McBride & Li 1998), the effects of repeated dark-light cycle (09:00 hour lights off), and re-exposure
deprivation cycles on relapse-like drinking in the P line of was initiated at dark onset (09:00 hour). Although
rats has been examined. In an initial study with adult baseline alcohol intakes (around 6 g/kg/day) were not
female P rats, animals were housed on a reverse dark- significantly different from groups given only a single
light cycle (09:00 hour lights off), and re-exposure was concentration of ethanol, P rats given concurrent access
initiated at dark onset (09:00 hour). This appears to limit to multiple ethanol concentrations demonstrated a
the effects of food in the stomach on ethanol absorption, marked increase (relative to values after the first
because rats are sleeping, for the most part, during the deprivation) in ethanol intake after a second deprivation
light cycle. The rats had access to a single concentration (1516 g/kg for the first 24 hours of re-exposure), with
(10%) of ethanol for an initial 6 weeks, followed by three elevated intakes extending into the fifth and sixth day by
cycles of 2 weeks of deprivation from and 2 weeks of re- the fourth re-exposure cycle (Rodd-Henricks et al. 2001).
exposure to ethanol access. Multiple cycles of deprivation Consequently, with concurrent access to multiple con-
resulted in an extended ADE (3 days of significantly ele- centrations of ethanol, P rats consumed over 10 g/kg/day
vated ethanol intakes of 810 g/kg/day) compared with a of ethanol for 56 days meeting criteria for a bender
single day of elevated ethanol intake seen after a single (NIAAA National Advisory Council 2004). Moreover,
deprivation (Rodd-Henricks et al. 2000b). Moreover, the during the first 2 hours of the fourth re-exposure (see
group of P rats that were initially deprived for 8 weeks dis- Fig. 4, upper panels), P rats consumed approximately 6 g/
played a longer (up to 5 days) elevation in ethanol intake kg ethanol and attained an average BAC of approximately
after a second deprivation than the group initially 100 mg% by the end of this initial 2-hour re-exposure

Figure 4 Amounts of ethanol consumed and blood alcohol concentrations (BACs) achieved for adult female P rats following repeated dep-
rivations. The deprived animals had continuous, 24-hour, free-choice concurrent access to 10%, 20% and 30% ethanol for an initial 6 weeks,
which was followed by four (upper panels) or five (lower panels) cycles of 2 weeks of deprivation from and 2 weeks of re-exposure to eth-
anol. Regression analyses revealed significant correlations between amount of ethanol consumed and BACs achieved for both non-deprived
and deprived animals (rs > 0.75, Ps < 0.05). The graphs depict data for the first 2 hours of re-exposure to ethanol access (initiated at lights
off), such that the deprived animals readily displayed relapse-like behavior by consuming greater than 5 g/kg during the initial 2 hours of re-
exposure, with the average BAC exceeding 100 mg% upon the fourth re-exposure and approximating 150 mg% upon the fifth re-exposure

2006 The Authors. Journal compilation 2006 Society for the Study of Addiction Addiction Biology, 11, 270288
Excessive ethanol drinking in P rats 281

(Rodd-Henricks et al. 2001). When a fifth re-exposure Overall, these findings indicate that the P rat is highly
cycle was initiated, the BACs exceeded 100 mg%, with vulnerable to relapse-like drinking and is a good model for
many above 150 mg%, by the end of the initial 2 hours of studying mechanisms underlying this phenomenon. In
re-exposure (Fig. 4, lower panels). The robust increase in addition, the results reveal that two or more deprivations,
drinking with repeated deprivations was a result of a shift in conjunction with concurrent access to multiple con-
in preference from the lowest (10%) to the two higher centrations of ethanol, can markedly exacerbate relapse-
(20% and 30%) ethanol concentrations (Rodd-Henricks like behavior, suggesting that additional neuronal alter-
et al. 2001). Overall, this pronounced increase in drink- ations are produced with repeated cycles of deprivation
ing with repeated deprivations suggests that alterations and relapse. This repeated deprivation model has some
in the reinforcing and/or aversive effects of ethanol weaknesses with regard to the human condition, such
occurred after a single extended deprivation period, that the cycles of abstinence and relapse drinking are
which, in turn, were enhanced after repeated regular, as opposed to being irregular in the human
deprivations. alcoholic population. Nevertheless, the above results do
Using a repeated ADE protocol, with selectively bred P illustrate the compounding consequences of repeated epi-
or inbred P (derived from selectively bred P) rats, a num- sodes of alcohol withdrawals. Therefore, this model is
ber of important findings have been published. For exam- highly important for understanding the neurobiological
ple, chronic ethanol drinking (over 6 months) resulted in alterations that occur with repeated deprivations and
significantly increased self-administration of ethanol how these alterations contribute to relapse behavior.
directly into the posterior ventral tegmental area (pVTA), Finally, the choice of gender to use in the binge and
compared with nave controls, and cycles of ethanol relapse drinking experiments was based upon the avail-
access and deprivation (i.e. a repeated ADE protocol) ability of animals, i.e. if male P rats were available then
resulted in significantly greater self-administration of male rats were used, if female P rats were available, then
ethanol into the pVTA, compared with the chronic drink- female rats were used. However, in chronic ethanol drink-
ers (25 versus 30 versus 40 infusions of 75 mg% eth- ing experiments in which stereotaxic surgeries are used,
anol per session for the nave, chronic drinking and it is necessary to use female P rats, because changes in
repeatedly deprived rats, respectively: Rodd et al. the head size and skull thickness are more dramatic in
2005a,b). In another study, chronic ethanol drinking male P rats, making stereotaxic placements much more
(2 months) resulted in a significant increase in basal DA difficult. Results thus far indicate that there are no signif-
neurotransmission in the accumbens, compared with icant differences between male and female P rats in binge
nave controls, which persisted for 2 weeks in the absence and relapse drinking.
of ethanol (Thielen et al. 2004). Additionally, these
An adolescent animal model of ethanol abuse
authors reported that chronic drinking resulted in a sig-
nificant decrease in basal serotonin (5-HT) neurotrans- Alcohol abuse among adolescents is a major health and
mission in the accumbens of the same animals tested developmental problem. The prevalence of ethanol usage
above, compared with nave controls; whereas following is indicated by the findings that 7590% of high school
2 weeks of deprivation, this effect was reversed and students report they have used ethanol (Zucker & Hart-
increased 5-HT release was observed (Thielen et al. ford 1983; Fournet, Estes & Martin 1990; Windle 1990).
2004). In a third study, both inbred P rats that drank eth- Excessive drinking, greater than five or more drinks per
anol chronically (14 weeks) and those that experienced occasion (a modified definition of binge drinking), has
repeated cycles of 2 weeks of deprivation from and re- been reported in approximately 30% of high school
exposure to ethanol access (total of 10 weeks ethanol seniors (Johnston, OMalley & Bachman 1991, 1993;
access) displayed significantly greater D1 receptor bind- Rose et al. 2001), and there is evidence of ethanol use in
ing densities in the anterior accumbens core, compared preadolescent children, as well (Quine & Stephenson
with nave controls, with the repeatedly deprived group 1990; Windle 1990). When assessing the effects of phar-
displaying significant increases in D1 binding densities in macological pretreatment during adolescence on adult
the anterior regions of the lateral and intercalated nuclei behaviors in male and female rats [as is done in our lab-
of the amygdala as well (Sari, Bell & Zhou 2006). Addi- oratory (cf. McBride et al. 2005)], Spear (2000) has sug-
tionally, these authors reported that both chronic drink- gested that the conservative estimate of an adolescent
ing and repeatedly deprived rats displayed significantly window in rats [postnatal day (PND) 2842] could be
greater D2 binding densities in the anterior accumbens extended to PND 60. Our laboratory has adopted this
core and shell, compared with nave controls, with the window of periadolescent exposure, and found that peri-
repeatedly deprived group also displaying significantly adolescent (PND 30 through 60) P rats will consume
increased D2 binding densities in the dorsal striatum, as amounts of ethanol that exceed one of the original crite-
well (Sari et al. 2006). ria [>5 g/kg/day (cf. McBride & Li 1998; Murphy et al.

2006 The Authors. Journal compilation 2006 Society for the Study of Addiction Addiction Biology, 11, 270288
282 Richard L. Bell et al.

periadolescence (PND 30 through PND 60) were exam-


ined for operant self-administration (in two-lever operant
chambers, with one lever for ethanol and one lever for
water) of ethanol during adulthood (>PND 75; Rodd-
Henricks et al. 2002a). Compared with ethanol-nave P
rats, animals that had access to ethanol during periado-
lescence displayed (1) quicker acquisition of operant self-
administration of ethanol; (2) retarded extinction (where
responding decreases to the point that pressing activity
on the ethanol bar does not differ from pressing activity
on the water bar) of this responding; and (3) a robust
operant ADE upon re-exposure to ethanol (Rodd-
Henricks et al. 2002a). In contrast, female P rats that
experienced pre-exposure to ethanol during adulthood
Figure 5 Amounts of ethanol consumed, g/kg/day, by periadoles-
cent [postnatal day (PND) 30 through 60] male and female P rats
did not differ from ethanol-nave rats on these operant
given 24-hour, free-choice access to either a single concentration of parameters (Rodd-Henricks et al. 2002b). Additionally,
ethanol (15%, v/v) or concurrent access to multiple concentrations of using microdialysis techniques, our laboratory has exam-
ethanol (10%, 20% and 30%, v/v). Overall (a) periadolescent P rats ined whether free-choice access to ethanol (15%) during
readily self-administer ethanol during this stage of development (b) periadolescence (PND 3060) would alter the mesolim-
animals with concurrent access to multiple concentrations of ethanol bic DA system during adulthood (>PND 75: Sahr et al.
consumed more ethanol than animal with access to a single concen-
2004). Compared with nave P rats, P rats with adoles-
tration of ethanol, and (c) the amounts of ethanol consumed exceed
cent access to ethanol displayed a greater extraction frac-
one of the original criteria for selective breeding of P rats, such that
greater than 5 g/kg/day of ethanol is self-administered. *, indicates a tion (a putative measure of DA reuptake/clearance from
significant (P < 0.05) main effect of ethanol access condition (multiple the synaptic cleft), but not increased extracellular levels
concentration versus single concentration) across weeks of DA in the accumbens. Additionally, these same animals
displayed a more prolonged increase in accumbal extra-
cellular DA after a 2.5 g/kg (i.p.) challenge of ethanol
2002; Bell et al. 2005)] for selective breeding of P rats (Sahr et al. 2004). Therefore, ethanol drinking during
(McKinzie et al. 1999; Bell et al. 2003, 2006). Figure 5 periadolescence can have long-lasting effects that may
shows the ethanol intakes of male and female P rats increase the probability of initiating ethanol drinking
beginning at 33 days of age (PND 33). Within the first during adulthood and, by extension, interfere with the
week, ethanol intakes have reached approximately 5 g/ ability to maintain abstinence during this period, as well.
kg/day for the group given access to a single concentra- Moreover, adolescent exposure to ethanol alters neuro-
tion of ethanol (15%). Over the following 3 weeks ethanol circuitry (the accumbens as part of the mesolimbic DA
intake increased to approximately 8 g/kg/day. However, if system) mediating ethanol reward, and this may be one
the periadolescent P rats were given concurrent access to mechanism by which an individual develops a predispo-
multiple concentrations of ethanol (10%, 20% and 30%), sition to abuse ethanol.
ethanol intakes in the first week were approximately 8 g/
kg/day, with intakes increasing to around 10 g/kg/day by
CONCLUSION
PND 60. These results indicate that (1) P rats have early
onset high alcohol-consuming behavior; (2) this high The results reviewed above indicate that (1) the P rat
ethanol consumption can be exacerbated with access to meets criteria proposed for a valid animal model of alco-
multiple concentrations of ethanol; and (3) there were holism; (2) the self-administration behavior of the P rat
minimal gender differences in acquisition of ethanol parallels many of the clinical criteria for a diagnosis of
drinking during periadolescence. alcohol abuse or alcoholism; (3) the P rat displays many
Adolescent ethanol drinking may have enduring con- behavioral and neurochemical phenotypes seen in genet-
sequences, as indicated by an association between early ically predisposed individuals and/or alcoholics; and (4)
onset of ethanol use and increased risk for later drug- the P rat displays excessive ethanol (binge-like or bender)
related problems (Chou & Pickering 1992; Anthony & drinking with different environmental manipulations
Petronis 1995), as well as general effects on brain devel- including: concurrent access to multiple concentrations
opment and behavioral consequences (Spear 2000; of ethanol, multiple scheduled access intervals during the
Chambers, Taylor & Potenza 2003). Research with the P dark period, and testing under relapse-like conditions.
rat has revealed similar findings. For example, female P Therefore, the P rat can be effectively used in models
rats given free-choice access to 15% ethanol during assessing alcohol-preference, a genetic predisposition for

2006 The Authors. Journal compilation 2006 Society for the Study of Addiction Addiction Biology, 11, 270288
Excessive ethanol drinking in P rats 283

alcohol abuse and/or alcoholism, the effects of ethanol deprivation in ethanol-experienced alcohol-preferring (P)
drinking during adolescence on adult behaviors, and rats. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 28:(5 Suppl.):98A.
Bell RL, Rodd ZA, Boutwell CL, Murphy JM, Lumeng L, Li T-K,
excessive drinking using protocols of binge-like or
McBride WJ (2004b) Repeated daily scheduled access results
relapse-like drinking. Pertaining to clinical typologies of in binge-like ethanol consumption in adult inbred P rats.
alcoholism, of which there are many (cf. Hesselbrock Alcohol Clin Exp Res 28:(5 Suppl.):143A.
1995), the P rat fits some of the criteria proposed by Bell RL, Rodd ZA, Kuc KA, Lumeng L, Li T-K, Murphy JM,
Cloninger (1987) for the Type 2 alcoholic, in terms of McBride WJ (2003) Effects of concurrent access to a single or
multiple concentrations of ethanol on the intake of ethanol by
early onset of ethanol drinking, a genetic predisposition
male and female periadolescent alcohol-preferring (P) rats.
to abuse ethanol, and the display of impulsive/sensation- Alcohol 29:137148.
seeking behavior. However, the P rat does not necessarily Bell RL, Rodd ZA, Murphy JM, McBride WJ (2005) Use of selec-
fit the behavioral phenotype of aggressive antisocial tively bred alcohol-preferring rats to study alcohol abuse,
behavior. In addition, both male and female P rats have relapse and craving. In: Preedy VR, Watson RR, eds. Compre-
similar characteristics of ethanol drinking and its conse- hensive Handbook of Alcohol Related Pathology, Vol. 3, pp.
15151533. New York: Academic Press, Elsevier Science.
quences. These latter findings are not consistent with the
Bell RL, Rodd ZA, Sable HJK, Schultz JA, Hsu CC, Lumeng L,
Type 2 classification, which, for the most part, is male- Murphy JM, McBride WJ (2006) Daily patterns of ethanol
specific (e.g. Babors classification subtype with charac- drinking in periadolescent and adult alcohol-preferring (P)
teristics similar to Cloningers Type 2 was found among rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 83:3546.
both male and female alcoholics: Babor et al. 1992). It is Bell RL, Rodd-Henricks ZA, Webster AA, Lumeng L, Li T-K,
McBride WJ, Murphy JM (2002) Heart rate and motor-acti-
clear that much has been accomplished in determining
vating effects of orally self-administered ethanol in alcohol-
neurobiological factors associated with alcoholism, but, preferring (P) rats. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 26:11621170.
at the same time, much has yet to be done. The P rat offers Bell RL, Stewart RB, Woods JE II, Lumeng L, Li T-K, Murphy JM,
many possibilities for advancing our understanding of McBride WJ (2001) Responsivity and development of toler-
the neurobiological mechanisms underlying excessive ance to the motor impairing effects of moderate doses of eth-
anol in alcohol-preferring (P) and nonpreferring (NP) rat
ethanol drinking, and ethanol relapse behavior. In addi-
lines. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 25:644650.
tion, the P rat is one preclinical model of ethanol drinking Bloomfield K, Stockwell T, Gerhard G, Reh N (2003) Interna-
for screening potential therapeutic agents to treat alco- tional comparisons of alcohol consumption. Alcohol Res
holism and ethanol abuse. Health 27:95109.
Brienza RS, Stein MD (2002) Alcohol use disorders in primary
Acknowledgements care. J Gen Intern Med 17:387397.
Burish TG, Maisto SA, Cooper AM, Sobell MB (1981) Effects of
The present work was supported in part by NIAAA voluntary short-term abstinence from alcohol on subsequent
Grants AA 10256, AA 07611, AA 07462, AA 11261, drinking patterns of college students. J Stud Alcohol
AA 13522 (an INIA project), and INGEN (supported in 42:10131020.
part by the Lilly Foundation). Caberlotto L, Thorsell A, Rimondini R, Sommer W, Hyytia P,
Heilig M (2001) Differential expression of NPY and its recep-
References tors in alcohol-preferring AA and alcohol-avoiding ANA rats.
Alcohol Clin Exp Res 25:15641569.
Agabio R, Carai MA, Lobina C, Pani M, Reali R, Vacca G, Gessa Casu MA, Pisu C, Lobina C, Pani L (2004) Immunocytochemical
GL, Colombo G (2000) Development of short-lasting alcohol study of the forebrain serotonergic innervation in Sardinian
deprivation effect (ADE) in Sardinian alcohol-preferring rats. alcohol-preferring rats. Psychopharmacology 172:341351.
Alcohol 21:5962. Chambers RA, Taylor JR, Potenza MN (2003) Developmental
American Psychiatric Association (1994) Diagnostic and Statis- neurocircuitry of motivation in adolescence: a critical period
tical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edn. Washington, DC: of addiction vulnerability. Am J Psychiatry 160:10411052.
Author. Chiauzzi EJ (1991) Preventing Relapse in the Addictions: A Biop-
Andrucci GL, Archer RP, Pancoast DL, Gordon RA (1989) The sychosocial Approach. New York: Pergamon Press.
relationship of MMPI and sensation seeking scales to adoles- Childress AR, Hole AV, Ehrman RN, Robbins SJ, McLellan AT,
cent drug use. J Pers Assess 53:253266. OBrien CP (1993) Cue reactivity and cue reactivity interven-
Anthony JC, Petronis KR (1995) Early onset drug use and risk of tions in drug dependence. NIDA Res Monogr 137:7395.
later drug problems. Drug Alcohol Depend 40:915. Chou SP, Pickering RP (1992) Early onset of drinking as a risk
Babor TF, De Hofmann MI, Boca FK, Hesselbrock VM, Meyer RE, factor for lifetime alcohol-related problems. Brit J Addict
Dolinsky ZS, Rounsaville B (1992) Types of alcoholics. II. Evi- 87:11991204.
dence for an empirically-derived typology based on indicators Ciccocioppo R, Angeletti S, Weiss F (2001) Long-lasting resis-
of vulnerability and severity. Arch Gen Psychiatry 49:599 tance to extinction of response reinstatement induced by eth-
608. anol-related stimuli: role of genetic ethanol preference.
Barrick C, Connors GJ (2002) Relapse prevention and maintain- Alcohol Clin Exp Res 25:14141419.
ing abstinence in older adults with alcohol-use disorders. Cicero TJ (1979) A critique of animal analogues of alcoholism.
Drugs Aging 19:583594. In: Majchrowicz E, Noble EP, eds. Biochemistry and Pharma-
Bell RL, Rodd ZA, Boutwell CL, Lumeng L, Li T-K, McBride WJ, cology of Ethanol, Vol. 2, pp. 533560. New York: Plenum
Murphy JM (2004a) A conditioned heart rate response during Press.

2006 The Authors. Journal compilation 2006 Society for the Study of Addiction Addiction Biology, 11, 270288
284 Richard L. Bell et al.

Cloninger CR (1987) Neurogenetic adaptive mechanisms in Fournet GP, Estes RE, Martin GL (1990) Drug and alcohol atti-
alcoholism. Science 236:410416. tudes and usage among elementary and secondary students. J
Cloninger CR (1996) Assessment of the impulsive-compulsive Alcohol Drug Educ 35:8186.
spectrum of behavior by the seven factor model of tempera- Froehlich JC, Harts J, Lumeng L, Li T-K (1988) Differences in
ment and character. In: Oldham JM, Hollander E, Skodol AE, response to the aversive properties of ethanol in rats selectively
eds. Impulsivity and Compulsivity, pp. 5995. Washington, bred for oral ethanol preference. Pharmacol Biochem Behav
DC: American Psychiatric Press. 31:215222.
Conner BT, Noble EP, Berman SM, Ozkaragoz T, Ritchie T, Anto- Gatto GJ, McBride WJ, Murphy JM, Lumeng L, Li T-K (1994) Eth-
lin T, Sheen C (2005) DRD2 genotypes and substance use in anol self-infusion into the ventral tegmental area by alcohol-
adolescent children of alcoholics. Drug Alcohol Depend preferring rats. Alcohol 11:557564.
79:379387. Gatto GJ, Murphy JM, Waller MB, McBride WJ, Lumeng L, Li T-K
Conrod PJ, Peterson JB, Pihl RO, Mankowskie S (1997) Biphasic (1987) Chronic ethanol tolerance through free-choice drink-
effects of alcohol on heart rate are influenced by alcoholic ing in the P line of alcohol-preferring rats. Pharmacol Bio-
family history and rate of alcohol ingestion. Alcohol Clin Exp chem Behav 28:111115.
Res 21:140149. Gianoulakis C, Guillaume P, De Waele J-P, Angelogianni P
Conrod PJ, Pihl RO, Ditto B (1995) Autonomic reactivity and (1995) Effects of stress and alcohol on the Proopiomelanocor-
alcohol-induced dampening in men at risk for alcoholism and tin/-Endorphin System. In: Hunt WA, Zakhari S, eds. Stress,
men at risk for hypertension. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 19:482 Gender, and Alcohol-Seeking Behavior, pp. 145165.
489. Research Monograph no. 29. Bethesda, MD: National Insti-
Conrod PJ, Pihl RO, Vassileva J (1998) Differential sensitivity to tute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism.
alcohol reinforcement in groups of men at risk for distinct Gill JS (2002) Reported levels of alcohol consumption and binge
alcoholism subtypes. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 22:585597. drinking within the UK undergraduate student population
Davis KM, Wu JY (2001) Role of glutamatergic and GABAergic over the last 25 years. Alcohol Alcohol 37:109120.
systems in alcoholism. J Biomed Sci 8:719. Gmel G, Bissery A, Gammeter R, Givel J-C, Calmes J-M, Yersin B,
De Montis MG, Grappi S, Gambarana C, Leggio B, Nanni G, Daeppen J-B (2006) Alcohol-attributable injuries in admis-
Scheggi S, Tagliamonte A (2004) Sardinian alcohol-prefer- sions to a Swiss emergency room an analysis of the link
ring rats show low level 5-HT extraneuronal levels in the between volume of drinking, drinking patterns, and preatten-
mPFC and no habituation in monoaminergic response to dance drinking. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 30:501509.
repeated ethanol consumption in NAcS. Brain Res 1006:18 Gongwer MA, Murphy JM, McBride WJ, Lumeng L, Li T-K
27. (1989) Regional brain contents of serotonin, dopamine and
Deutsch JA, Eisner A (1977) Ethanol self-administration in the their metabolites in the selectively bred high- and low-alcohol
rat induced by forced drinking of ethanol. Behav Biol 20:81 drinking lines of rats. Alcohol 6:317320.
90. Griffin JF (2002) A strategic approach to vaccine development:
Drummond DC, Cooper T, Glautier S (1990) Conditioned learn- animal models, monitoring vaccine efficacy, formulation and
ing in alcohol dependence: implications for cue exposure delivery. Adv Drug Deliv Rev 54:851861.
treatment. Brit J Addict 85:725743. Heilig M, Thorsell A (2002) Brain neuropeptide Y (NPY) in stress
Edenberg HJ, Dick DM, Xuei X, Tian H, Almasy L, Bauer LO, and alcohol dependence. Rev Neurosci 13:8594.
Crowe RR, Goate A, Hesselbrock V, Jones K, Kwon J, Li T-K, Heinz A (2002) Dopaminergic dysfunction in alcoholism and
Nurnberger JI Jr, OConnor SJ, Reich T, Rice J, Schuckit MA, schizophrenia psychopathological and behavioral corre-
Porjesz P, Foroud T, Begleiter H (2004) Variations in lates. Eur Psychiatry 17:916.
GABRA2, encoding the 2 subunit of the GABAA receptor, Herz A (1997) Endogenous opioid systems and alcohol addic-
are associated with alcohol dependence and with brain oscil- tion. Psychopharmacology 129:99111.
lations. Am J Hum Genet 74:705714. Hesselbrock MN (1995) Genetic determinants of alcoholic sub-
Ehlers CL, Chaplin RI, Lumeng L, Li T-K (1991) Electrophysio- types. In: Begleiter H, Kissin B, eds. The Genetics of Alcohol-
logical responses to ethanol in P and NP rats. Alcohol Clin Exp ism, pp. 4069. New York: Oxford University Press.
Res 15:739744. Heyser CJ, Schulteis G, Koob GF (1997) Increased ethanol self-
Ehlers CL, Chaplin RI, Wall TL, Lumeng L, Li T-K, Owens MJ, administration after a period of imposed ethanol deprivation
Nemeroff CB (1992) Corticotropin releasing factor (CRF): in rats trained in a limited access paradigm. Alcohol Clin Exp
studies in alcohol-preferring and -nonpreferring rats. Psy- Res 21:784791.
chopharmacology 106:359364. Hilakivi L, Eriksson CJ, Sarviharju M, Sinclair JD (1984) Revital-
Ehlers CL, Li T-K, Lumeng L, Hwang BH, Somes C, Jimenez P, ization of the AA and ANA rat lines: effects on some line char-
Mathe AA (1998) Neuropeptide Y levels in ethanol-naive acteristics. Alcohol 1:7175.
alcohol-preferring and nonpreferring rats and in Wistar rats Hilbrom ME (1990) Alcohol withdrawal seizures and binge ver-
after ethanol exposure. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 22:17781782. sus chronic drinking. In: Port RJ, Mattson RH, Cramer JA,
Erwin VJ, McClearn GE, Kuse AR (1980) Interrelationships of Diamond I, eds. Alcohol and Seizures: Basic Mechanisms and
alcohol consumption, actions of alcohol and biochemical Clinical Concepts, pp. 206215. Philadelphia, PA: FA Davis.
traits. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 13:297302. Holloway FA, Bird DC, Devenport JA (1984) Periodic availability:
Files FJ, Samson HH, Denning CE, Marvin S (1998) Comparison factors affecting alcohol selection in rats. Alcohol 1:1925.
of alcohol-preferring and -nonpreferring selectively bred rat Holter SM, Engelmann M, Kirschke C, Liebsch G, Landgrad R,
lines. II. Operant self-administration in a continuous-access Spanagel R (1998) Long-term ethanol self-administration
situation. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 22:21472158. with repeated ethanol deprivation episodes changes ethanol
Finney JW, Moos RH (1991) The long-term course of treated drinking pattern and increases anxiety-related behavior
alcoholism. I. Mortality, relapse and remission rates and com- during ethanol deprivation in rats. Behav Pharmacol 9:41
parisons with community controls. J Stud Alcohol 52:4454. 48.

2006 The Authors. Journal compilation 2006 Society for the Study of Addiction Addiction Biology, 11, 270288
Excessive ethanol drinking in P rats 285

Hwang BH, Lumeng L, Wu J-Y, Li T-K (1990) Increased number tive/hypnotic effects of alcohol. Pharmacol Biochem Behav
of GABAergic terminals in the nucleus accumbens is associ- 53:585591.
ated with alcohol preference in rats. Alcohol Clin Exp Res Kweon YS, Lee HK, Lee CT, Lee K-U, Pae CU (2005) Association
14:503507. of the serotonin transporter gene polymorphism with Korean
Hwang BH, Zhang JK, Ehlers CL, Lumeng L, Li T-K (1999) male alcoholics. J Psych Res 39:371376.
Innate differences of neuropeptide Y (NPY) in hypothalamic Lankford MF, Roscoe AK, Pennington SN, Myers RD (1991)
nuclei and central nucleus of the amygdala between selec- Drinking of high concentrations of ethanol versus palatable
tively bred rats with high and low alcohol preference. Alcohol fluids in alcohol-preferring (P) rats: valid animal model of
Clin Exp Res 23:10231030. alcoholism. Alcohol 8:293299.
Jaffe SL (2002) Treatment and relapse prevention for adolescent Lappalainen J, Kranzler HR, Malison R, Price LH, Van Dyck C,
substance abuse. Pediatr Clin North Am 49:345352. Rosenheck RA, Cramer J, Southwick S, Charney D, Krystal J,
Johann M, Putzhammer A, Eichhammer P, Wodarz N (2005) Gelernter J (2002) A functional neuropeptide Y Leu7Pro poly-
Association of the 141C del variant of the dopamine D2 morphism associated with alcohol dependence in a large pop-
receptor (DRD2) with positive family history and suicidality in ulation sample from the United States. Arch Gen Psychiatry
German alcoholics. Am J Med Genet (Neuropsych Genet) 59:825831.
132B:4649. Lappalainen J, Krupitsky E, Remizov M, Pchelina S, Taraskina A,
Johnston LD, OMalley PM, Bachman JG (1991) Drug Use Zvartau E, Somberg LK, Covault J, Kranzler H, Krystal JH,
Among American High School Seniors, College Students and Gelernter J (2005) Association between alcoholism and -
Young Adults, 19751990. Volume 1: High School Seniors amino butyric acid 2 receptor subtype in a Russian popula-
(DHHS Publication No. ADM91-1813). Superintendent of tion. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 29:493498.
Documents. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing L AD, Kiianmaa K (1988) Characteristics of ethanol tolerance
Office. in alcohol drinking (AA) and alcohol avoiding (ANA) rats.
Johnston LD, OMalley PM, Bachman JG (1993) Drug Use Psychopharmacology 94:479483.
Among American High School Seniors, College Students and Lester D, Freed EX (1973) Criteria for an animal model of alco-
Young Adults, 19751990. Volume 1: Secondary Students holism. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1:103107.
(DHHS Publication No. ADM93-3597). Superintendent of Lex BW, Lukas SE, Greenwald NE, Mendelson HH (1988) Alco-
Documents. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing hol-induced changes in body sway in women at risk for alco-
Office. holism: a pilot study. J Stud Alcohol 49:346356.
Kalant H, LeBlanc AE, Gibbins RJ (1971) Tolerance to, and Li T-K, Lumeng L (1977) Alcohol metabolism of inbred strains of
dependence on, some non-opiate psychotropic drugs. Phar- rats with alcohol preference and non-preference. In: Thur-
macol Rev 23:135191. man RG, Williamson JR, Drott H, Chance B, eds. Alcohol and
Kampov-Polevoy AB, Matthews DB, Gause L, Morrow AL, Over- Aldehyde Metabolizing Systems, Vol. 3, pp. 625633. New
street DH (2000) P rats develop physical dependence on alco- York: Academic Press.
hol via voluntary drinking: changes in seizure thresholds, Li T-K, Lumeng L, McBride WJ, Murphy JM (1987a) Alcohol-
anxiety, and patterns of alcohol drinking. Alcohol Clin Exp Res ism: is it a model for the study of disorders of mood and
24:278284. consummatory behavior? Ann N Y Acad Sci 499:239
Katner S, Weiss F (2001) Neurochemical characteristics associ- 249.
ated with ethanol preference in selected alcohol-preferring Li T-K, Lumeng L, McBride WJ, Murphy JM (1987b) Rodent lines
and -nonpreferring rats: a quantitative microdialysis study. selected for factors affecting alcohol consumption. Alcohol
Alcohol Clin Exp Res 25:198205. Alcohol 1:9196.
Keith LD, Roberts A, Wisen KM, Crabbe JC (1995) Lovinger DM (1999) 5-HT3 receptors and the neural actions of
CorticosteroidAlcohol Interactions. In: Hunt WA, Zakhari S, alcohols: an increasingly exciting topic. Neurochem Internat
eds. Stress, Gender, and Alcohol-Seeking Behavior, pp. 181 35:125130.
196. Research Monograph no. 29. Bethesda, MD: National Lumeng L, Hawkins TD, Li T-K (1977) New strains of rats with
Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. alcohol preference and non-preference. In: Thurman RG, Wil-
Kimpel MW, Bell RL, Strother WN, Thomas D, Ringham H, liamson JR, Drott H, Chance B, eds. Alcohol and Aldehyde
Pedrick N, Witzmann FA, McBride WJ (2005) Bouts of exces- Metabolizing Systems, Vol. 3, pp. 537544. New York: Aca-
sive alcohol drinking alter protein expression in limbic regions demic Press.
of inbred alcohol-preferring (iP) rats. Alcohol Clin Exp Res Lumeng L, Li T-K (1986) The development of metabolic toler-
29:(5 Suppl.):15A. ance in the alcohol-preferring P rats: comparison of forced
Khnke MD, Batra A, Kolb W, Khnke AM, Lutz U, Schick S, and free-choice drinking of ethanol. Pharmacol Biochem
Gaertner I (2005) Association of the dopamine transporter Behav 25:10131020.
gene with alcoholism. Alcohol Alcohol 40:339342. Lumeng L, Waller MB, McBride WJ, Li T-K (1982) Different sen-
Konishi T, Calvillo M, Leng A-S, Lin K-M, Wan Y-JY (2004) Poly- sitivities to ethanol in alcohol-preferring and -nonpreferring
morphisms of the dopamine D2 receptor, serotonin trans- rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 16:125130.
porter, and GABAA receptor 3 subunit genes and alcoholism McBride WJ, Bell RL, Rodd ZA, Strother WN, Murphy JM (2005)
in Mexican-Americans. Alcohol 32:4552. Adolescent alcohol drinking and its long-range consequences:
Kornet M, Goosen C, Van Ree JM (1990) The effect of interrupted studies with animal models. Recent Dev Alcohol 17:123
alcohol supply on spontaneous alcohol consumption by 142.
rhesus monkeys. Alcohol Alcohol 4:407412. McBride WJ, Chernet E, Dyr W, Lumeng L, Li T-K (1993) Den-
Kuntsche E, Rehm J, Gmel G (2004) Characteristics of binge sities of dopamine D2 receptors are reduced in CNS regions of
drinkers in Europe. Soc Sci Med 59:113127. alcohol-preferring P rats. Alcohol 10:387390.
Kurtz DL, Stewart RB, Zweifel M, Li T-K, Froehlich JC (1996) McBride WJ, Chernet E, McKinzie DL, Lumeng L, Li T-K (1998)
Genetic differences in tolerance and sensitization to the seda- Quantitative autoradiography of mu-opioid receptors in the

2006 The Authors. Journal compilation 2006 Society for the Study of Addiction Addiction Biology, 11, 270288
286 Richard L. Bell et al.

CNS of alcohol naive alcohol-preferring P and -nonpreferring Murphy JM, Waller MB, Gatto GJ, McBride WJ, Lumeng L, Li T-K
NP rats. Alcohol 16:317323. (1988) Effects of fluoxetine on the intragastric self-
McBride WJ, Li T-K (1998) Animal models of alcoholism: neu- administration of ethanol in the alcohol preferring P line of
robiology of high alcohol-drinking behavior in rodents. Crit rats. Alcohol 5:283286.
Rev Neurobio 12:339369. NIAAA National Advisory Council (2004) NIAAA Council
McKinney WT (2001) Overview of the past contributions of ani- approves definition of binge drinking. NIAAA Newsletter
mal models and their changing place in psychiatry. Semin 3:5.
Clin Neuropsychiatry 6:6878. Nowak KL, Ingraham CM, McKinzie DL, McBride WJ, Lumeng L,
McKinzie DL, McBride WJ, Murphy JM, Lumeng L, Li T-K (1999) Li T-K, Murphy JM (2000) An assessment of novelty-seeking
Rat lines selectively bred for alcohol preference: a potential behavior in alcohol-preferring and -nonpreferring rats. Phar-
animal model of adolescent alcohol drinking. In: Hannigan macol Biochem Behav 66:113121.
JH, Spear LP, Spear NE, Goodlett CR, eds. Alcohol and Alco- OBrien CP, Childress AR, McLellan AT, Ehrman R (1992) A
holism: Effects on Brain and Development, pp. 135160. learning model of addiction. In: OBrien CP, Jaffe JH, eds.
Mahweh, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Addictive Sates, pp. 157177. New York: Raven Press.
McKinzie DL, McBride WJ, Murphy JM, Lumeng L, Li T-K (2000) Presley CA, Meilman PW, Lyerla R (1994) Development of the
Effects of MDL72222, a serotonin3 antagonist, on operant Core Alcohol and Drug Survey: initial findings and future
responding for ethanol by alcohol-preferring P rats. Alcohol directions. J Am Coll Health 42:248255.
Clin Exp Res 24:15001504. Quine S, Stephenson JA (1990) Predicting smoking and
McKinzie DL, Nowak KL, Yorger L, McBride WJ, Murphy JM, drinking intentions and behavior of pre-adolescents: the
Lumeng L, Li T-K (1998) The alcohol deprivation effect in the influence of parents, siblings, and peers. Fam Sys Med 8:191
alcohol-preferring P rat under free-drinking and operant 200.
access conditions. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 22:11701176. Rajan I, NagaVenkatesha Musthy PJ, Ramakrishnan AG, Gan-
McMillen BA (1997) Toward a definition of a valid animal model gadhar BN, Javakiramaiah N (1998) Heart rate variability as
of alcoholism: multiple animal models for multiple diseases. an index of cue reactivity in alcoholics. Biol Psychiatry
Alcohol 14:409419. 43:544546.
Meisch RA (1976) The function of schedule-induced polydipsia Rasmussen DD, Boldt BM, Wilkinson CW, Mitton DR (2002)
in establishing ethanol as a positive reinforcer. Pharmacol Rev Chronic daily ethanol and withdrawal: 3. Forebrain pro-
27:465473. opiomelanocortin gene expression and implications for
Melendez RI, Rodd-Henricks ZA, Engleman EA, Li T-K, McBride dependence, relapse, and deprivation effect. Alcohol Clin Exp
WJ, Murphy JM (2002) Microdialysis of dopamine in the Res 26:534546.
nucleus accumbens of alcohol preferring (P) rats during Rehm J, Room R, Graham K, Monteiro M, Gmel G, Sempos CT
anticipation and operant self-administration of ethanol. Alco- (2003) The relationship of average volume of alcohol con-
hol Clin Exp Res 26:318325. sumption and patterns of drinking to burden of diseasean
Mello NK, Mendelson MD (1972) Drinking patterns during overview. Addiction 98:12091228.
work-contingent and noncontingent alcohol acquisition. Psy- Rhodes JS, Best K, Belknap JK, Finn DA, Crabbe JC (2005) Eval-
chosom Med 34:139164. uation of a simple model of ethanol drinking to intoxication in
Morzorati S, Lamishaw B, Lumeng L, Li T-K, Bemis K, Clemens J C57BL/6J mice. Physiol Behav 84:5363.
(1988) Effect of low dose ethanol on the EEG of alcohol- Roberts AJ, Heyser CJ, Cole M, Griffin P, Koob GF (2000) Exces-
preferring and nonpreferring rats. Brain Res Bull 21:101 sive ethanol drinking following a history of dependence:
104. animal model of allostasis. Neuropsychopharmacology 22:
Mottagui-Tabar S, Prince JA, Wahlestedt C, Zhu G, Goldman D, 581594.
Heilig M (2005) A novel single nucleotide polymorphism of Rodd ZA, Bell RL, Kuc KA, Murphy JM, Lumeng L, Li T-K,
the neuropeptide Y (NPY) gene associated with alcohol depen- McBride WJ (2003) Effects of repeated alcohol deprivations on
dence. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 29:702707. operant ethanol self-administration by alcohol-preferring (P)
Murphy JM, Gatto GJ, McBride WJ, Lumeng L, Li T-K (1989) rats. Neuropsychopharmacology 28:16141621.
Operant responding for oral ethanol in the alcohol-preferring Rodd ZA, Bell RL, McKinzie DL, Webster AA, Murphy JM,
P and alcohol-nonpreferring NP lines of rats. Alcohol 6:127 Lumeng L, Li T-K, McBride WJ (2004a) Low-dose stimulatory
131. effects of ethanol during adolescence in rat lines selectively
Murphy JM, Gatto GJ, Waller MB, McBride WJ, Lumeng L, Li T-K bred for high alcohol intake. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 28:535
(1986) Effects of scheduled access on ethanol intake by the 543.
alcohol-preferring (P) line of rats. Alcohol 3:331336. Rodd ZA, Bell RL, McQueen VK, Davids MR, Hsu CC, Murphy JM,
Murphy JM, McBride WJ, Lumeng L, Li T-K (1982) Regional Li T-K, Lumeng L, McBride WJ (2005a) Chronic ethanol
brain levels of monoamines in alcohol-preferring and drinking by alcohol-preferring rats increases the sensitivity of
-nonpreferring rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 16:145 the posterior ventral tegmental area to the reinforcing effects
149. of ethanol. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 29:358366.
Murphy JM, McBride WJ, Lumeng L, Li T-K (1987) Contents of Rodd ZA, Bell RL, McQueen VK, Davids MR, Hsu CC, Murphy JM,
monoamines in forebrain regions of alcohol-preferring (P) Li T-K, Lumeng L, McBride WJ (2005b) Prolonged increase in
and -nonpreferring (NP) lines of rats. Pharmacol Biochem the sensitivity of the posterior ventral tegmental area to the
Behav 26:389392. reinforcing effects of ethanol following repeated exposure to
Murphy JM, Stewart RB, Bell RL, Badia-Elder NE, Carr LG, cycles of ethanol access and deprivation. J Pharmacol Exp
McBride WJ, Lumeng L, Li T-K (2002) Phenotypic and geno- Ther 315:648657.
typic characterization of the Indiana University rat lines selec- Rodd ZA, Bell RL, Sable HJK, Murphy JM, McBride WJ (2004b)
tively bred for high and low alcohol preference. Behav Genet Recent advances in animal models of alcohol craving and
32:363388. relapse. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 79:439450.

2006 The Authors. Journal compilation 2006 Society for the Study of Addiction Addiction Biology, 11, 270288
Excessive ethanol drinking in P rats 287

Rodd ZA, Bell RL, Zhang Y, Murphy JM, Goldstein A, Zaffaroni A, Schuckit MA (1994) Low level of response to alcohol as a pre-
Li T-K, McBride WJ (2005c) Regional heterogeneity for the dictor of future alcoholism. Am J Psychiatry 151:184189.
intracranial self-administration of ethanol and acetaldehyde Schuckit MA, Gold EO (1988) A simultaneous evaluation of
within the ventral tegmental area of alcohol-preferring (P) multiple markers of ethanol/placebo challenges in sons of
rats: involvement of dopamine and serotonin. Neuropsychop- alcoholics and controls. Arch Gen Psychiatry 45:211216.
harmacology 30:330338. Sinclair JD (1971) The alcohol-deprivation effect in monkeys.
Rodd-Henricks ZA, Bell RL, Kuc KA, Murphy JM, McBride WJ, Psychon Sci 25:122.
Lumeng L, Li T-K (2002a) Effects of ethanol exposure on Sinclair JD, Li T-K (1989) Long and short alcohol deprivation:
subsequent acquisition and extinction of ethanol self- effects on AA and P alcohol-preferring rats. Alcohol 6:505
administration and expression of alcohol-seeking behavior in 509.
adult alcohol-preferring (P) rats. I. Periadolescent exposure. Sinclair JD, Senter RJ (1967) Increased preference for ethanol in
Alcohol Clin Exp Res 26:16321641. rats following deprivation. Psychon Sci 8:1112.
Rodd-Henricks ZA, Bell RL, Kuc KA, Murphy JM, McBride WJ, Sinclair JD, Tiihonen K (1988) Lack of alcohol deprivation effect
Lumeng L, Li T-K (2002b) Effects of ethanol exposure on in AA rats. Alcohol 5:8587.
subsequent acquisition and extinction of ethanol self- Sinclair JD, Walker S, Jordan W (1973) Behavioral and physio-
administration and expression of alcohol-seeking behavior in logical changes associated with various durations of alcohol
adult alcohol-preferring (P) rats. II. Adult exposure. Alcohol deprivation in rats. Q J Stud Alcohol 34:744757.
Clin Exp Res 26:16421652. Slawecki CJ, Walpole T, Somes C, Li T-K, Ehlers CL (1999) Dif-
Rodd-Henricks ZA, Bell RL, Murphy JM, McBride WJ, Lumeng L, ferences in neurophysiological indices of associative learning
Li T-K (2001) Effects of concurrent access to multiple ethanol in alcohol-preferring and nonpreferring rats. Alcohol Clin Exp
concentrations and repeated deprivations on alcohol intake of Res 23:828834.
alcohol-preferring (P) rats. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 24:747753. Soderpalm B, Ericson M, Olausson P, Blomqvist O, Engel JA
Rodd-Henricks ZA, McKinzie DL, Murphy JM, McBride WJ, (2000) Nicotinic mechanisms involved in the dopamine acti-
Lumeng L, Li T-K (2000a) The expression of an alcohol dep- vating and reinforcing properties of ethanol. Behav Brain Res
rivation effect in the high-alcohol-drinking replicate rat lines 113:8596.
is dependent on repeated deprivations. Alcohol Clin Exp Res Soini SL, Honkanen A, Hyytia P, Korpi ER (1999) [3H]ethylke-
24:747753. tocyclazocine binding to brain opioid receptor subtypes in
Rodd-Henricks ZA, McKinzie DL, Shaikh SR, Murphy JM, alcohol-preferring AA and alcohol-avoiding ANA rats. Alco-
McBride WJ, Lumeng L, Li T-K (2000b) The alcohol depri- hol 18:2734.
vation effect is prolonged in the alcohol preferring (P) rat Soini SL, Ovaska T, Honkanen A, Hyytia P, Korpi ER (1998)
following repeated deprivations. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 24:8 Brain opioid receptor binding of [3H]CTOP and [3H]naltrin-
16. dole in alcohol-preferring AA and alcohol-avoiding ANA rats.
Room R, Babor T, Rehm J (2005) Alcohol and public health. Alcohol 15:227232.
Lancet 365:519530. Spear LP (2000) The adolescent brain and age-related behav-
Rose RJ, Dick DM, Viken RJ, Kaprio J (2001) Geneenvironment ioral manifestations. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 24:417463.
interaction in patterns of adolescent drinking: regional resi- Spuhler K, Deitrich RA (1984) Correlative analysis of ethanol-
dency moderates longitudinal influences on alcohol use. Alco- related phenotypes in rat inbred strains. Alcohol Clin Exp Res
hol Clin Exp Res 25:637643. 8:480484.
Sahr AE, Thielen RJ, Lumeng L, Li T-K, McBride WJ (2004) Stefanini E, Frau M, Garau MG, Garau B, Fadda F, Gessa GL
Long-lasting alterations of the mesolimbic dopamine system (1992) Alcohol-preferring rats have fewer dopamine D2
after periadolescent ethanol drinking by alcohol-preferring receptors in the limbic system. Alcohol Alcohol 27:127130.
rats. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 28:702711. Stewart RB, Kurtz DL, Zweifel M, Li T-K, Froehlich JC (1992) Dif-
Salimov R, Salimov NB, Klodt P, Maisky A (1993) Interaction ferences in the hypothermic response to ethanol in rats selec-
between alcohol deprivation and morphine withdrawal in tively bred for oral ethanol preference and nonpreference.
mice. Drug Alcohol Depend 34:5966. Psychopharmacology 106:169174.
Samson HH (1986) Initiation of ethanol reinforcement using a Stewart RB, McBride WJ, Lumeng L, Li T-K, Murphy JM (1991)
sucrose-substitution procedure in food- and water-sated rats. Chronic alcohol consumption in alcohol-preferring P rats
Alcohol Clin Exp Res 10:436442. attenuates subsequent conditioned taste aversion produced
Samson HH, Files FJ, Denning C, Marvin S (1998) Comparison by ethanol injections. Psychopharmacology 105:530534.
of alcohol-preferring and -nonpreferring selectively bred rat Stewart RB, Murphy JM, McBride WJ, Lumeng L, Li T-K (1996)
lines. I. Ethanol initiation and limited access operant self- Place conditioning with alcohol in alcohol-preferring and
administration. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 22:21332146. -nonpreferring rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 53:487491.
Sandi C, Borrell J, Guaza C (1990) Enkephalins interfere with Strother WN, Lumeng L, Li T-K, McBride WJ (2003) Regional
early phases of voluntary ethanol drinking. Peptides 11:697 CNS densities of serotonin 1A and dopamine D2 receptors in
702. periadolescent alcohol-preferring P and alcohol-nonprefer-
Sari Y, Bell RL, Zhou FC (2006) Effects of chronic alcohol and ring NP rat pups. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 74:335342.
repeated deprivations on dopamine D1 and D2 receptor levels Strother WN, Lumeng L, Li T-K, McBride WJ (2005) Dopamine
in the extended amygdala of inbred alcohol-preferring rats. and serotonin content in select brain regions of weanling and
Alcohol Clin Exp Res 30:4656. adult alcohol drinking rat lines. Pharmacol Biochem Behav
Schuckit MA (1985) Ethanol-induced changes in body sway in 80:229237.
men of high alcoholism risk. Arch Gen Psychiatry 42:3375 Tabakoff B, Ritzman RF (1979) Acute tolerance in inbred and
3379. selected lines of mice. Drug Alcohol Depend 4:8790.
Schuckit MA (1986) Genetic aspects of alcoholism. Ann Emerg Thielen RJ, Engleman EA, Rodd ZA, Murphy JM, Lumeng L, Li T-
Med 15:991996. K, McBride WJ (2004) Ethanol drinking and deprivation alter

2006 The Authors. Journal compilation 2006 Society for the Study of Addiction Addiction Biology, 11, 270288
288 Richard L. Bell et al.

dopaminergic and serotonergic function in the nucleus Weiss F, Lorang MT, Bloom FE, Koob GF (1993) Oral alcohol self-
accumbens of alcohol-preferring rats. J Pharmacol Exp Ther administration stimulates dopamine release in the rat nucleus
309:216225. accumbens: genetic and motivational determinants. J Phar-
Waller MB, McBride WJ, Gatto GJ, Lumeng L, Li T-K (1984) macol Exp Ther 267:250258.
Intragastric self-administration of ethanol by ethanol- Windle M (1990) Alcohol use and abuse: some findings from the
preferring and -nonpreferring lines of rats. Science 225:78 National Adolescent Student Health Survey. Alcohol Health
80. Res World 15:510.
Waller MB, McBride WJ, Lumeng L, Li T-K (1982) Induction of Wise RA, Bozarth MA (1987) A psychomotor stimulant theory
dependence on ethanol by free-choice drinking in alcohol- of addiction. Psychol Rev 94:469492.
preferring rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 16:501507. Wolfgramm J, Heyne A (1995) From controlled drug intake to
Waller MB, Murphy JM, McBride WJ, Lumeng L, Li T-K (1986) loss of control: the irreversible development of drug addiction
Effect of low dose ethanol on spontaneous motor activity in in the rat. Behav Brain Res 70:7794.
alcohol-preferring and -nonpreferring lines of rats. Pharma- Zhou FC, Bledsoe S, Lumeng L, Li T-K (1991a) Immunostained
col Biochem Behav 24:617623. serotonergic fibers are decreased in selected brain regions of
Wechsler H, Lee J, Kuo M, Lee H (2000) College binge drinking in alcohol-preferring rats. Alcohol 8:425431.
the 1990s: a continuing problem results of the Harvard Zhou FC, Bledsoe S, Lumeng L, Li T-K (1991b) Serotonergic
School of Public Health 1999 College Alcohol Study. J Am immunostained terminal fibers are lower in selected forebrain
Coll Health 48:199210. regions of alcohol-preferring rats. Alcohol 8:17.
Wechsler H, Nelson TF (2001) Binge drinking and the American Zhou FC, Pu CF, Lumeng L, Li T-K (1991c) Fewer number of
College student: whats five drinks? Psychol Addict Behav immunostained serotonergic neurons in raphe of alcohol-
15:287291. preferring rats. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 15:315.
Weiss F, Ciccocioppo R, Parsons LH, Katner S, Liu X, Zorilla EP, Zhou FC, Zhang JK, Lumeng L, Li T-K (1995) Mesolimbic dopam-
Valdez GR, Ben-Shahar O, Angeletti S, Richter RR (2001) ine system in alcohol-preferring rats. Alcohol 12:403412.
Compulsive drug-seeking behavior and relapse. Neuroadapta- Zucker RA, Hartford TC (1983) National study of the demogra-
tion, stress, and conditioning factors. Ann NY Acad Sci phy of adolescent drinking practices in 1980. J Stud Alcohol
937:126. 44:974985.

2006 The Authors. Journal compilation 2006 Society for the Study of Addiction Addiction Biology, 11, 270288

Вам также может понравиться