Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 1

editorial

Should scientists tell stories?


A narrative can effectively communicate scientific information. But when telling a perfect
story becomes an end in itself, the scientific process can be easily compromised.

Everyone loves a good story, and writers of many kinds Finally, a scientific paper is not a glorified laboratory
use narrative techniques to get their message across. A notebook, that is, simply a record of what was done.
recent Points of View article (Krzywinski and Cairo, Rather, it must place the research into a larger scientific
Nat. Methods 10, 687, 2013) described how techniques context in addition to communicating the results and
of storytelling, such as a structured story arc, can effec- explaining its authors conclusions to other researchers
2013 Nature America, Inc. All rights reserved.

tively guide the presentation of scientific data in fig- so that they can assess and build on the findings.
ures. But as pointed out in a Correspondence by Katz Watson and Cricks Nature paper was a single page
(p. 1045, this issue), the notion of communicating sci- with one figure, no data and no methodseffectively a
entific information by storytelling can be taken too far. story, but a powerful one. Most research papers are far
The hypothetical scientist such as the one described more extensive, but authors often shape the narrative
by Katz, who allows a desired storyline to improperly to convey their arguments, presenting experiments in
influence experiments and who embellishes and an order different from that in which they were con-
obfuscates results, is clearly operating in a highly ducted and leaving out aborted lines of inquiry and
undesirable, even unethical, manner. But it is worth failed experiments. Some such liberties will be neces-
distinguishing between the use of rhetorical techniques sary to prevent readers from becoming hopelessly lost:
as a tool for conveying information versus treating the research is a road on which one may take many wrong
telling of a scientific story as an end in itself. turns before a productive direction is found, and rarely
Even so, are rhetorical techniques more likely to must the reader also follow that process.
impede, rather than enable, a proper understand- But there will be cases in which failed experiments
ing of data? Should storytellingsetting the stage bring a necessary nuance to the data, suggesting weak-
for the importance of an experiment, presenting the nesses in the argument or settings where the conclu-
reader with an unknown or an unsolved problem, sions are questionable. Omission of such information
and interweaving interpretation with the reporting may be unjustifiable. What is more, authors can eas-
of resultshave a role in communicating scientific ily segue into frank cherry-picking of data to support
npg

results at all? a desired conclusion, a practice that goes against the


One may argue that in an idealized scholarly world, deepest goals of scientific research.
scientists should avoid storytelling. They should The rise of supplementary information has served
instead describe their original hypothesis, detail their an important function in providing a place for failed
experiments in the order conducted and present the experiments and negative or unexplained results.
data in the rawest form reasonable for interpretation. Efforts by publishers to integrate supplementary infor-
At the end they should state their conclusions. mation into the online version of the manuscript can
But there are many problems with this scenario. crucially expose these data to readers without com-
Scientists are not automata and, in todays world, promising a manuscripts narrative. But hard limits
operate under substantial time pressures. Even if the on supplementary information or efforts to eliminate
scientists colleagues in this idealized setting had the it altogether could complicate a papers narrative or,
patience and time to navigate through a long, unin- alternatively, whittle it down to a tightly told story with
terpreted, purely factual exposition and to sufficiently little room for more than one interpretation.
grasp what was done and its significance, it would still Determining how to handle data that do not fit a
be a cripplingly inefficient process. Furthermore, to particular narrative and deciding how to describe
borrow from the title of science historian Steven ones results takes judgment; notably, this is judgment
Shapins recent book of essays, science is conducted of a scientific, not a rhetorical, nature. When such a
by people situated in time, space, culture and soci- judgment is well made, exploiting the full range of ones
ety, and struggling for credibility and authority. An abilities for exposition may illuminate, rather than
argument for papers written purely as a factual blow- hide, nuance in the results. After all, in science and out
by-blow account of experiments does not sufficiently of it, the most interesting stories are often the complex,
take into account this reality. multilayered ones.

nature methods | VOL.10 NO.11 | NOVEMBER 2013 | 1037

Вам также может понравиться