Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 13

See

discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/254301572

Motor Creativity and Creative Thinking in


Children: The Diverging Role of Inhibition

Article in Creativity Research Journal July 2011


DOI: 10.1080/10400419.2011.595993

CITATIONS READS

15 321

3 authors, including:

Caterina Pesce
Italian University of Sport and Movement "Foro Italico"
97 PUBLICATIONS 933 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Caterina Pesce on 12 August 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. All in-text references underlined in blue are added to the original document
and are linked to publications on ResearchGate, letting you access and read them immediately.
This article was downloaded by: [Alma Mater Studiorum - Universit di Bologna]
On: 21 November 2011, At: 01:08
Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,
37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Creativity Research Journal


Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hcrj20

Motor Creativity and Creative Thinking in Children: The


Diverging Role of Inhibition
a a b
Patrizia Scibinetti , Nicoletta Tocci & Caterina Pesce
a
Department of Education in Sport and Human Motion, Italian University Sport and
Movement
b
Department of Human Motion and Sport Science, Italian University Sport and Movement

Available online: 05 Aug 2011

To cite this article: Patrizia Scibinetti, Nicoletta Tocci & Caterina Pesce (2011): Motor Creativity and Creative Thinking in
Children: The Diverging Role of Inhibition, Creativity Research Journal, 23:3, 262-272

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2011.595993

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic
reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to
anyone is expressly forbidden.

The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents
will be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae, and drug doses should
be independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss, actions, claims,
proceedings, demand, or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in
connection with or arising out of the use of this material.
CREATIVITY RESEARCH JOURNAL, 23(3), 262272, 2011
Copyright # Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
ISSN: 1040-0419 print=1532-6934 online
DOI: 10.1080/10400419.2011.595993

Motor Creativity and Creative Thinking in Children:


The Diverging Role of Inhibition
Patrizia Scibinetti and Nicoletta Tocci
Downloaded by [Alma Mater Studiorum - Universit di Bologna] at 01:08 21 November 2011

Department of Education in Sport and Human Motion, Italian University Sport and
Movement

Caterina Pesce
Department of Human Motion and Sport Science, Italian University Sport and Movement

This study investigated the commonalities and differences between motor creativity and
creative thinking in children and how executive functions differently inuence them.
Thirty-one children, aged 7 to 8 years, were administered Bertsch Test of Motor Crea-
tivity, Torrance Test of Creative Thinking, the Random Number Generation (RNG)
task tapping executive functions, and the Movement Assessment Battery for Children
(M-ABC). Scores for three dimensions of motor creativity and creative thinking (u-
ency, exibility, and originality), selected indexes of executive function (inhibition and
working memory updating) and motor competence scores were submitted to correla-
tional and regression analyses. Results showed that there is no association between
motor creativity and motor competence, but a signicant association between creative
moving and thinking for all their dimensions except for originality. Moreover, orig-
inality in thinking was predicted by low inhibitory ability, although originality in mov-
ing by high inhibitory ability. Results suggested that there are commonalities in the
processes responsible for the uent and exible production of creative thoughts and
movements, whereas there was a domain-specicity as concerns the role played by
inhibitory functions in the production of original solutions during creative moving.

The on-going debate on whether creativity is a general experience that, in turn, generally result from an exten-
ability applying similarly across content areas or a set sive commitment to the domain and=or tasks of interest.
of abilities tailored to specic disciplines and tasks is The model by Baer and Kaufman (2005), the
continuing in more recent research with considerable Amusement Park Theory (APT), is a hierarchic model
implications for creativity theory, assessment, and edu- that starts from general requirements for all creative
cational practices (Lubart & Guignard, 2004; Plucker achievements and moves thorough progressively more
& Zabelina, 2009; Runco, 1987). Two recent hybrid specic levels: general thematic areas (e.g., hands-on
models have shifted attention to the question of what creativity), domains (e.g., motor or bodily-kinaesthetic
components are general and what components are spe- creativity) and microdomains or specic tasks (e.g.,
cic or when they become specic (Baer & Kaufman, gross-motor or ne-motor tasks).
2005; Plucker & Beghetto, 2004). According to Plucker
and Beghettos (2005) developmental perspective,
domain and task specicity of creativity become more MOTOR AND SPORTS CREATIVITY
pronounced with advancing age and with gains in
Creative behavior plays a key role in the performance
Correspondence should be sent to Patrizia Scibinetti, Italian
of many sports. When considering the diverging psy-
University Sport and MovementIUSM, Piazza L. De Bosis 15, chomotor and cognitive demands that characterize dif-
00135 Rome, Italy. E-mail: patrizia.scibinetti@uniroma4.it ferent sports, however, one can assume that creativity is
CREATIVE MOVING AND THINKING AND INHIBITION 263

highly task-specic within this domain. The ability to well as in the maturation of neural substrates in the
produce creative movements and actions may largely cerebellum and prefrontal cortex involved in creative
differ, for instance, when an individual is faced with thinking (Delis et al., 2007; Diamond, 2000; Vandervert,
the conditions characterizing dance during individual Schimpf & Liu, 2007). Also, the cerebellum seems to be
or contact improvisation (Fink, Graif & Neubauer, important for both motor and cognitive functions and is
2009; Torrents, Castaner, Dinusova, & Anguera, considered a computational system contributing to the
2010), or with the conditions characterizing team efciency and adaptability of movements and thoughts
games, where the environment is constantly changing (Vandervert et al., 2007). Therefore, it may be expected
and unpredictable and there is limited time to make that there are some commonalities between motor crea-
decisions and produce unexpected, original solutions tivity and creative thinking in children. On the other
(Memmert, 2007, 2009; Memmert & Roth, 2007). side, there seems to be little overlap in the processes
Although sports, and particularly team games, offer underlying motor ability and creativity-relevant,
the possibility to analyze creative performances involv- higher-level cognitive functions in children (Piek &
Downloaded by [Alma Mater Studiorum - Universit di Bologna] at 01:08 21 November 2011

ing complex motor behaviors, Memmert (2007, 2009) Dyck, 2004); thus, we also hypothesized that some dis-
and Memmert and Roth (2007) did not address the tinct cognitive processes might underlie creative perfor-
question at what extent motor creativity and creative mances in thinking and moving.
thinking contribute to creative actions such as tactical
behaviors in team sports.
The Development of Creativity and Executive
Apart from sport-related creativity research, studies
Function
examining motor creativity are scarce as compared to
the large amount of creativity research focused on cre- Creativity has been associated to higher-level cognitive
ative thinking (see Kim, 2006 for a review). Further- functionsparticularly to the executiveand tests of
more, confounding is generated by the fact that it creative thinking are assumed to reect such functions
has still not clearly dened whether measures obtained (e.g., Naglieri & Kaufman, 2001). Executive function
with tests of creativity in moving are measures of is an umbrella term for a set of component processes,
domain-specic creativity or movement-based measures such as planning, scheduling, working memory, and
of creative thinking. Motor uency and exibility mea- inhibition, required for the successful regulation of
sures obtained with the divergent movement ability thoughts and goal-directed, intelligent behaviors
(DMA) test, evaluating the ability to perform and mod- especially in nonroutine situations. The development
ify fundamental motor patterns, have been referred to as of executive functions occurs throughout childhood
measures of motor creativity, but also as indicative ele- and continues until young adulthood (Best et al.,
ments of divergent and critical thinking (Cleland, 1994; 2009) with distinct developmental trajectories for the
Konstantinidou, Pollatou, & Zachopoulou, 2005; different executive function component processes
Trevlas, Matsouka, & Zachopoulou, 2003). On the (Huizinga et al., 2006).
other hand, the Torrance Thinking Creatively in Action Neurobiological models of executive function have
and Movement (TCAM) test (Torrance, 1981), that was highlighted the key role played by the frontal and parti-
originally designed to sample childrens creative think- cularly prefrontal cortex (e.g., Banich, 2009) and the
ing abilities by means of measures obtained in close linkage between the development of executive
movement-based creativity tasks, is highly correlated functions and the renement of prefrontal cortical net-
with the DMA performance outcomes (Zachopoulou, works (Diamond, 2002). Also, creativity requires that
Makri, & Pollatou, 2009) and has been used as a mea- kind of executive abilities that are typically ascribed to
sure of specic motor creativity (Konstantinidou et al., the prefrontal cortex (Bekhtereva, Danko, Starchenko,
2005). Moreover, few studies have investigated whether Pakhomov, & Medvedev, 2001). In his neuroscientic
motor creativity in childhood may be also inuenced by framework of creativity, Dietrich (2004) suggested that
age-related stages of motor development (Tocci, the prefrontal cortex contributes to the creative process
Scibinetti, & Zelli, 2004; Zachopoulou & Macri, 2005). in three ways: evaluating the appropriateness of a novel
Thus, the degree of differentiation between creative insight, activating executive processes relevant to the
thinking and moving in children is an issue in need for ongoing task, and implementing the goal-oriented
further research. expression of the insight. In studies on motor creativity,
The general aim of this study was to contribute it has still not been investigated what role is played by
further to the understanding of the general versus spe- executive functions. Thus, the second aim of this study
cic aspects of being creative by jointly analyzing motor was to assess whether inhibition and working memory
creativity and creative thinking in children. There is evi- updating, which are involved in creative thinking
dence of a close parallelism in the development of motor (Dietrich, 2004), are similarly or differently involved in
coordination and higher-level cognitive functions, as motor creativity.
264 SCIBINETTI, TOCCI, AND PESCE

METHOD hoop from one line to the other. You can let it go on
its own or take it with you. Show me anything you
Participants can do that comes to mind.
Thirty-one healthy male (n 19) and female (n 12)
Italian children aged 78 years belonging to a primary Ball. Children were situated in the middle of a square
school in Rome participated in this study. The parents measuring 2.50 m on each side. Their task was to use a
of all children who volunteered provided written ball to hit, one at a time, seven 1  1 m large targets
informed consent for their children to be involved in a on the wall, oor, or ceiling areas outside the square.
study in which they would perform paper-and-pencil The child was free to use the ball in whatever way he
tests and motor performance tests that were enjoyable or she wanted. The verbal instruction was: You see
and appropriate for their age. The study protocol was all the targets around you. Imagine they are glasses.
started after institutional approval. Imagine breaking them with this ball without going
Downloaded by [Alma Mater Studiorum - Universit di Bologna] at 01:08 21 November 2011

out of your home (i.e., the square). Whats important


is not so much to break all the glasses but to try to break
Assessment of Motor Creativity
them every time in a different way.
Childrens motor creativity was assessed by using the
test developed and validated for primary school children
Bench. A bench was located in the middle of a room
by Bertsch (1983). This test provides measurements of
and two hoops were positioned at the two ends of the
motor creativity by means of two separate versions
bench representing the starting and arrival point,
(form A and B) composed of four tasks each to be per-
respectively. The verbal instruction was: You have to
formed on the oor, with a bench, a hoop or a ball,
go from one hoop to the other and back, keeping a part
respectively. Although the four tasks vary from mainly
of your body always in contact with the bench. The
gross motor (oor, bench) to mainly ne motor (hoop,
child was free to perform the task in any way he or
ball) coordination demands, the two forms vary in the
she could think of.
degrees of freedom of the movement tasks, with form
A providing no specic performance modality and form
B partially dening it. For instance, to perform the Floor. Two parallel lines delimited a 2.50
bench task in its form A, children standing in front of squared-meter area on the oor. The verbal instruction
the bench are free to do anything they want for a certain was: Your task is to move from one line to the other.
period of time. In the form B of the bench task, they are You are free to do anything you want between these
instead constrained not to loose body contact with the two lines. Show me all possible ways you know or that
bench while freely moving around it. In the present may come to your mind to do that.
study, we selected form B in line with general recom- The test has an acceptable internal consistency, as
mendations by Torrance (1988) on how to study the indexed by the Cronbachs alpha coefcient across the
creativity process. Each child participated individually four tasks (a .76). Thus, the Bertsch test provides
to the four motor creativity tasks in randomized order reliable measures of individual differences in childrens
during school hours for a total test duration of about motor creativity.
30 min. The tasks lasted 2 min and 30 sec (hoop and
oor) or 3 min (ball and bench). Children who stopped
Data coding and scoring. Only those motor beha-
performing were encouraged to stay on task by prompt-
viors that were clearly recorded by both observers were
ing them again to follow the original instructions,
coded by a previously trained investigator. Data coding
described in the following for each task and ending with
consisted in assigning scores on three separate dimen-
the common sentence: Try to nd ways that are orig-
sions, namely uency, exibility, and originality. Flu-
inal and that other children may not think about. Dur-
ency was scored in terms of the number of different
ing each task, two trained observers independently
behaviors displayed by the child during each motor task.
reported childrens behaviors in a written form on separ-
Flexibility and originality were instead coded and scored
ate observation sheets. The two observers participated
by means of scoring schemes developed by Bertsch
to training sessions to reach adequate interobserver
(1983) on the basis of normative data obtained with
agreement rates.
his initial observation of nearly 7,000 different behaviors
in children. Flexibility was scored referring to movement
Hoop. Two parallel lines were put at a distance of behavior categories identied by Bertsch for each
3.50 m. Children were asked to carry a hoop and move task (16 for the hoop and the ball tasks, 44 for the
from one line to the other in whatever way they wanted. bench task, and 36 for the oor task). Such categories
The verbal instruction was: Your task is to move the summarize motor behaviors as a function of body
CREATIVE MOVING AND THINKING AND INHIBITION 265

position, movement direction and type. The exibility motor creativity. Instead, we did not include the mea-
score is the sum of the categories for which at least sure of elaboration and two further measures of creative
one behavior was observed, categories with two or more potential that were added in the third edition (abstract-
observed behaviors being counted only once. Originality ness of titles and resistance to premature change),
was quantied assigning a score ranging from 0 (low because they were not pertinent to the comparison with
originality) to 3 (high originality) to each behavior in creativity measures in the motor domain that included
each category on the basis of the relative frequency of only uidity, exibility, and originality (Bertsch,
such behavior in Bertschs normative sample. The u- 1983). Fluency was scored by the number of gural
ency, exibility, and originality scores for each motor images produced by the examinees; it reects the ability
task were standardized. To obtain a total score for each to generate a large amount of relevant ideas. Flexibility
creativity dimension, standardized scores obtained from was scored by the variety of categories of relevant
the four tasks were added. responses. Originality was scored by the number of
statistically infrequent responses on the basis of norma-
Downloaded by [Alma Mater Studiorum - Universit di Bologna] at 01:08 21 November 2011

tive data; it reects the ability to produce uncommon


Assessment of creative thinking. The Italian ver- or unique ideas. According to the TTCT manual,
sion of the Torrance (1989) Test of Creative Thinking raw scores were converted into standard scores to
(TTCT), Figural Form A was group administered at have comparable ranges for uidity, exibility, and
school. The TTCT Figural Form A is designed for indi- originality.
viduals in kindergarten through graduate school and
beyond. It consists of three timed pencil-and-paper pic-
ture construction and completion activities lasting Assessment of Executive Function
10 min each, with 1-min breaks between tasks, for a total The Random Number Generation (RNG) task is a test
working time of about 30 min. According to testing tapping executive functions (Towse & Neil, 1998). For
guidelines, the administrator invited the examinees to this study, we used the version rst employed by Towse
enjoy these activities and created a playful and McIachlan (1999), demonstrating the feasibility of
problem-solving atmosphere to minimize threatening the RNG task with children. The children were tested
feelings linked to a performance-oriented climate. individually on a quiet area of school, with the RNG
task being introduced as a game involving numbers.
Activity I: Picture construction task. Children had They were instructed to verbally generate a random
to construct a picture using a darkened curve shape sequence of numbers between 1 and 10 to each beat
(jellybean or teardrop) provided on the page as a of a 70-beat sequence with an interbeat interval of 1.5
stimulus which must be integrated in the picture seconds. To offer children an age-appropriate, easily
construction. understandable example of what randomness is, the
instruction included a hide-and-seek type of game
(Towse & McIachlan, 1999). Prior to data collection,
Activity II: Picture completion. Children had to use participants performed a practice series of 70 numbers.
10 incomplete gures to make a gure or object draw- Then, there was a pause of about 4550 sec, during
ings to the incomplete gures, avoiding usual and obvi- which they were allowed to ask questions concerning
ous completions. the test. Thereafter, they generated a second series of
70 numbers that were recorded on a tape. Both the
Activity III: Parallel lines. Children had to use 30 omission of a number generation in correspondence
pairs of straight lines drawn on three pages to make of one tone and the production of numbers lower than
an original picture out of each pair of lines, overcoming 1 (0) or higher than 10 (11, 12, etc.) were considered
the tendency to perceive the same stimuli in the same errors and discarded. If errors exceeded a predened
way. maximum threshold of ve, the entire block must be
repeated. The randomness of the sequence of numbers
was measured by means of 18 different indexes
Data coding and scoring. Scoring of the TTCT was described by Towse and Neil. Among those, we selected
performed by a trained investigator based on three sub- six indexes reecting two creativity-relevant compo-
scales of norm-referenced measures: uency, exibility, nents of executive function: executive inhibition, which
and originality. Although in the third edition of the seems to have a differentiated impact on convergent
TTCT (Torrance & Ball, 1984), the measure of exibility and divergent thinking (White & Shah, 2006) and
was eliminated because of its too high correlation with working memory updating, which is strictly involved
uency, in the present study we maintained it to perform in creative problem solving (Passolunghi & Pazzaglia,
a comparison with the corresponding dimension of 2004).
266 SCIBINETTI, TOCCI, AND PESCE

Computation of Indexes of Randomness threading lace, and ower trail. In the placing pegs task,
the child must place 12 plastic pegs, one at a time, in all
The three indexes theoretically related to the inhibiting
holes on a board. In the threading lace task, the child
function were the turning point index (TPI), the adjac-
must thread a lace back and forth through the holes in
ency score (A comb), and the runs score (Runs). The
a lacing board. In the ower trail task, the child must
TPI is a measure of the similarity between the real fre-
draw, with the preferred hand, one continuous line fol-
quency of turning points, marking a change between
lowing the ower trail on a record form without crossing
ascending and descending series of numbers (e.g., the
its boundaries. In the formal trials of placing pegs and
response change between the digits 2 and 5 in an hypo-
threading lace, the examiner measured the seconds taken
thetical sequence 9, 7, 2, 5, 6, 8) and their theoretical
to complete each task. In the ower trail task, the exam-
frequency in random responses. The A comb measures
iner recorded the number of errors, i.e., the number of
the relative frequency of pairs of adjacent ascending or
times the drawn lines moves outside a boundary.
descending numbers (e.g., 78 or 43) and reects the
Downloaded by [Alma Mater Studiorum - Universit di Bologna] at 01:08 21 November 2011

habitual tendency to count forward or backward. The


Runs score is an index the variability of the number of Ball skills. The two tasks evaluating ball skills in 7-
digits in successive ascending or descending runs. High to 8-year-old children were one-hand bounce and catch
levels of TPI, but low levels of A comb and Runs and throwing a bean bag into a box. In the rst task,
correspond to a high ability to inhibit, avoiding the the child must bounce a tennis ball on the oor and
production of stereotyped strings and prepotent catch it with the same hand. In the throwing a bean
associates. bag into a box task, the child must throw the bean
The three indexes related to the working memory bag into a target box. In both types of tasks, the exam-
updating function were the redundancy score (R), the iner recorded the number of correctly executed trials
coupon score (Coupon), and the mean repetition gap (successful catches and throws, respectively) out of 10
(Mean RG). The R index reects the unbalance of attempts.
response alternative frequencies that derives from a
more frequent usage of given numbers, as compared to
others in a sequence of generated numbers based on Static and dynamic balance. The task evaluating
the theoretical frequencies of each digit. The Coupon static balance in 7- to 8-year-old children was the stork
score designates the number of digits generated until balance, and the tasks evaluating dynamic balance were
the entire set of digits alternatives (1 to 10) has been the jumping in squares task and heel-to-toe walking. In
used. The Mean RG is the average quantity of digits stork balancing, the child must stand on one foot, place
between successive occurrences of the same number cal- the sole of the other foot against the side of the support-
culated for all digits throughout the whole sequence ing knee, and put the hands on the hips. The examiner
(e.g., in the sequence 2, 8, 4, 6, 2, 9, 7, 8, the digits records the number of seconds, up to 20, that the child
2 and 8 reoccur with a mean gap equal to 5). High levels maintained balance without moving the standing and
of Mean RG, but low levels of R and Coupon corre- the nonstanding foot. In jumping in squares, the child
spond to a high ability to update working memory had to make ve continuous jumps forward from a
and employ equality of responses by using preferentially starting square to further ve squares. The examiner
alternation among numbers. recorded the number of correct and consecutive jumps
completed over ve without performance errors such
as landing on or outside the lines, jumping more than
Assessment of Movement Competence once in a square and landing with the feet far apart.
Childrens movement competence was estimated by In heel-to-toe walking, the child had to walk on a line
means of the Italian version of the movement assess- placing the heel of one foot against the toe of the other
ment battery for children (M-ABC) developed by for 15 steps. The examiner recorded the number of con-
Henderson and Sudgen (1992). This test evaluates move- secutive steps performed by the child without leaving
ment competence providing objective quantitative data space between toe and heel or stepping off the line.
on both gross motor and ne motor coordination of
children aged from 4 to 12 years with eight tasks differ-
Data coding and scoring. The M-ABC data were
entiated in four age-related difculty levels. The tasks
standardized to compute average scores for the three
are grouped under three subheadings: manual dexterity,
subheadings of manual dexterity, ball skills, and static
ball skills, and static and dynamic balance.
and dynamic balance. The data were also transformed
into a total score of impairment of motor function
Manual dexterity. The three tasks evaluating manual according to age-related norms (Henderson & Sudgen,
dexterity in 7- to 8-year-old children were placing pegs, 1992). The score indicates the extent to which a child
CREATIVE MOVING AND THINKING AND INHIBITION 267

TABLE 1
Hierarchical Multiple Regression Analysis Model of Fluency, Flexibility, and Originality in Creative Moving as a Function of Fluency and Originality
in Creative Thinking and of Executive Functioning (InhibitionTPI, A Comb, and Runsand Working Memory UpdatingR, Coupon, Mean RG)

Fluency in Moving Flexibility in Moving Originality in Moving

Beta (Std.) t p Beta (Std.) t p Beta (Std.) t p

Factors in Block 1
Fluency in thinking .69 2.10 .048 .64 1.82 .083 .69 2.10 .048
Originality in thinking n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s.
R2 .37 .45 .36
Factors in Block 2
InhibitionTPI n.s. n.s n.s n.s. n.s. n.s. .59 2.71 .013
InhibitionA comb n.s. n.s n.s n.s. n.s. n.s. .83 3.24 .004
InhibitionRuns n.s. n.s n.s n.s. n.s. n.s. .41 2.05 .050
Downloaded by [Alma Mater Studiorum - Universit di Bologna] at 01:08 21 November 2011

Memory updatingR n.s. n.s n.s n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s.
Memory updatingCoupon n.s. n.s n.s n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s.
Memory updatingMean RG n.s. n.s n.s n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s.
R2 change .17 .13 .27
Total R2 .54 Total R2 .58 Total R2 .63
Adjusted R2 .35 Adjusted R2 .39 Adjusted R2 .47

Note. Flexibility in thinking was excluded because of too high collinearity with uency.

falls below the level of his or her age peers, although it controlling for or partialling out the commonalities
does not differentiate between children who perform between motor creativity and creative thinking.
above this level. The results showed a high collinearity between u-
ency and exibility in creative thinking, conrming that
the two measures do not reect two independent dimen-
RESULTS sions (Kim, 2006). Thus, exibility, showing the highest
collinearity (VIF statistic 11), was eliminated from the
To identify eventual commonalities between motor crea- regression model and the three regressions were rerun.
tivity and creative thinking, we rst estimated the The results are reported in Table 1. The table shows
strength of a linear relationship by computing bivariate the predictors statistically signicant regression coef-
correlations (Pearsons r) between scores for three com- cients and the amount of variance explained (R2 and
mon dimensions shared by the tests of motor creativity R2 change) associated with each regression block. Flu-
and creative thinking: uency, exibility, and orig- ency in creative thinking signicantly predicted uency
inality. Results show a moderate, but signicant positive and originality and, marginally signicantly, exibility
correlation between uencies (r .451, p .005) and of performances in the motor creativity test. As regards
exibilities (r .489, p .003), but an absence of the prediction accrued by indexes of executive functions,
signicant correlation between originalities (r .088, only the regression model performed on originality
p .320). scores yielded a signicant percentage of variance
To analyze the predictive value of creative thinking explained by the three inhibition indices.
and cognitive developmental factors for creative motor To assess further whether creative thinking and
performance, uency, exibility and originality scores motor creativity are supported by some distinct cogni-
in the motor domain were submitted to three separate tive processes relying on executive function, also uency,
2-block hierarchical regression analyses. The predictors exibility, and originality scores in creative thinking
were: Torrance scores for uency, exibility, and orig- were regressed on the six indices of inhibition and mem-
inality (rst block); RNG indexes of executive function- ory updating functions.
ing (inhibitionTPI, A comb, and Runsand working Also in this case, the regression model performed on
memory updatingR, Coupon, and mean RG; second originality scores (Table 2) showed a signicant predic-
block). The logic of this heuristic hierarchical model dis- tion accrued by the three inhibition indexes. However,
tinguishes between the impact of more proximal vari- the opposite signs of standardized b statistics obtained
ables, represented by dimensions of creative thinking when regressing inhibition indexes against originality
paralleling those of motor creativity, and more distal measures in moving (Table 1) and thinking (Table 2)
factors, represented by executive functions. In this indicate diverging associations of inhibitory functions
way, any domain-specic predictive value of executive with originality in motor and nonmotor domains. For
functions for motor creativity, if given, may be identied ease of graphic representation, the three indexes of
268 SCIBINETTI, TOCCI, AND PESCE

TABLE 2
Multiple Regression Analysis Model of Fluency, Flexibility and Originality in Creative Thinking as a Function of Executive Functioning (Inhibition
TPI, A comb, and Runsand Working Memory UpdatingR, Coupon, Mean RG)

Fluency in Thinking Flexibility in Thinking Originality in Thinking

Beta (Std.) t p Beta (Std.) t p Beta (Std.) T p

InhibitionTPI n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. .52 2.57 .040
InhibitionA comb n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. .59 2.40 .024
InhibitionRuns n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. .47 2.41 .024
Memory updatingR n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s.
Memory updatingCoupon n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s.
Memory updatingmean RG n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s.
Total R2 .23 Total R2 .18 R2 .36
Adjusted R2 .03 Adjusted R2 .02 Adjusted R2 .20
Downloaded by [Alma Mater Studiorum - Universit di Bologna] at 01:08 21 November 2011

inhibitory ability (TPI, A comb, and Runs) were stan- according to the normative data by Henderson and
dardized and merged into an average index. Before aver- Sudgen (1992). The average score was 1.3 (1.8, range
aging, since high TPI values reect high inhibitory 07.5), corresponding to the 84th86th percentile. No
ability, although high A comb and Runs values reect child fell below the critical threshold of motor develop-
low inhibitory ability, A comb and Runs values were mental impairment corresponding to the 5th percentile
reversed. In Figures 1 and 2, originality values in motor (score 13.5) or below the threshold of borderline
creativity and creative thinking, respectively, are graphi- motor developmental level represented by the 15th per-
cally represented as a function of the global index of centile (score 10.0).
inhibitory function.
We also performed correlational analyses to control
for the potential inuence of individual differences in DISCUSSION
fundamental gross-motor and ne-motor skills on
motor creativity task performances primarily involving The aim of this study was to assess if there are common-
either gross-motor or ne-motor skills. Specically, we alities and=or divergences between motor creativity and
computed bivariate correlations between gross-motor creative thinking in children and what role is played by
creativity task performances (i.e., bench and oor) and creativity-relevant executive functions in the production
gross-motor M-ABC tasks performances (i.e., balance), of creative movements and thoughts. On the whole, the
as well as between ne-motor creativity task perfor- results give some support to middle-ground models
mances (i.e., ball and hoop) and M-ABC ne motor task positioned between domain generality and specicity
performances (manual dexterity and ball skills). (Amabile, 1996; Baer & Kaufman, 2005; Plucker &
We did not nd any signicant correlations. A total Beghetto, 2004; Sternberg & Lubart, 1995) and suggest
impairment score was computed for each child that there is a differentiated inuence of inhibitory

FIGURE 1 Graphical representation of originality in motor creativity (Bertsch test) as a function of the global index of inhibitory function derived
from TPI, A comb, and Runs indexes (RNG test). (Figure is provided in color online.)
CREATIVE MOVING AND THINKING AND INHIBITION 269
Downloaded by [Alma Mater Studiorum - Universit di Bologna] at 01:08 21 November 2011

FIGURE 2 Graphical representation of originality in creative thinking (Torrance test) as a function of the global index of inhibitory function
derived from TPI, A comb, and Runs indexes (RNG test). (Figure is provided in color online.)

executive functions on creative thinking and motor is predicted by low levels of inhibitory ability. The inte-
creativity performances. grative model of creativity by Baer and Kaufman (2005)
On the one hand, there seem to be some commonal- suggests that the determinants of creativity may vary in
ities between motor creativity and creative thinking, as quantity and quality from more general to more specic
indicated by moderate, but signicant, correlations levels. The present results suggest that the role of inhi-
between uencies and exibilities in performing the bition is domain-specic, since it differently impacts cre-
Torrance and Bertsch tests. This suggests that there is ative thinking and moving, as well as dimension-specic,
some similarity in the processes responsible for the u- since it impacts originality, but not uency or exibility.
ent and exible production of a great amount of creative The nonunivocal linkage between inhibition and orig-
thoughts and movements. On the other hand, the inality nds some support in studies showing that orig-
absence of a signicant correlation between originalities inality behaves differently from other creativity
indicates that there is only a partial overlap between dimensions in the relationship with executive functions
dimensions of creative thinking and moving different (Abraham, Windmann, McKenne, & Gunturkun,
from originality. This conrms that the Bertsch test per- 2007) and that the inhibitory function during develop-
formances are not merely movement-based measures of ment is probably more multifaceted than other executive
creative thinking. However, the absence of evidence for functions (Huizinga et al., 2006).
generality as concerns the originality dimension of crea- Both conscious and unconscious control complemen-
tivity cannot be interpreted as evidence for domain tarily contribute to creative behavior and cognition
specicity. The afrmative evidence for specicity (Vandervert et al., 2007). Dietrich (2004) distinguished
emerged when considering the different role played by between spontaneous insights, deriving from uncon-
inhibitory functions in the generation of original pro- scious and unltered thoughts, and deliberate searches
ducts in motor and nonmotor domains. for insights, consisting in the conscious processing of
The regressions performed on uency, exibility, and selected contents in working memory. Given the late
originality in motor creativity revealed that uency in ontogenical development of the principal brain structure
creative thinking is a predictor of all three dimensions, underlying deliberate creative behavior, the prefrontal
suggesting that the generation of many thoughts may cortex (Best et al., 2009), it may be argued that chil-
be a fundamental prerequisite for being creative in drens creative thinking is mainly spontaneous in proces-
movement and action. The fact that inhibition indexes sing mode. Spontaneous creative insights seem to be
predicted original thinking and moving in opposite fash- favored by defocused attention and by the ability to
ion (Figures 12) further advances the understanding of uninhibit the ow of thoughts, as suggested by studies
the relationship between creativity and executive func- showing the positive side effects on creativity of hav-
tion. In fact, it highlights the diverging role played by ing a low inhibitory ability due to specic syndromes
the inhibitory component of executive function in the (White & Shah, 2006) or, vice versa, related to giftedness
production of original solutions in different creativity in creative achievements (Carson, Peterson, & Higgins,
domains. As Figures 12 shows, a high originality in cre- 2003). In the present study, a low level of inhibitory
ative moving is predicted by high levels of inhibitory ability, permitting the emergence of loosely connected
ability, whereas a high originality in creative thinking associations that are assumed to underlie spontaneous
270 SCIBINETTI, TOCCI, AND PESCE

creative thoughts, seems to be a major determinant of A limitation of the study is represented by the narrow
original thinking performance. age range considered. Future research is needed to
In both cases of spontaneous and deliberate gener- explore if there is a close interrelation of developmental
ation of creative insights, their expression is considered trends of inhibitory functions and motor creativity at
conscious and supported by deliberate sustained atten- different ages and expertise levels. It might be that with
tion processes relying on executive function (Dietrich, increasing age and motor expertise, also the role played
2004). The expression of original solutions in the by inhibitory and other executive functions changes. For
Bertsch test involves the planning of more complex instance, it is possible that working memory efciency is
motor outputs than the Torrance test. This difference less relevant in childhood, as suggested by the nonpre-
may be accountable for our nding that to be original dictive value of memory-updating indexes for creative
in moving, children required the ability to actively thinking and moving in this study, and becomes more
inhibit the strong tendency toward the production of relevant in old adulthood, as suggested by a study in
more common routines. In fact, motor response plan- which age-related declines in creative performance were
Downloaded by [Alma Mater Studiorum - Universit di Bologna] at 01:08 21 November 2011

ning particularly requires the inhibition of prepotent, accounted for by reduced working memory efciency
but inappropriate responses, and only to a lesser extent (Roskos-Ewoldsen, Black, & Mccown, 2008).
the manipulation of working memory strategies (Penne- Also, the correlationalregressional nature of this
quin, Sorel, & Fontaine, 2010). Even if this evidence study limits the relevance of the results for applied pur-
probably concerns convergent motor responses more poses. Future research should be aimed at assessing
than divergent and creative ones, these ndings suggest whether motor creativity training may support the
that, at least in children, this may also extend to creative development of inhibition ability. Creativity in complex
motor behavior. We can speculate that automatisms in motor tasks seems to be sensitive to experience, as well
the motor system, linked to individuals and species sur- as to specic strategies, and teaching styles in physical
vival, are more pervasive than automatisms in thinking. education and sports training and seems to be charac-
This might justify the stronger role played by inhibition terized by very long retention times (Bournelli &
in the planning of creative motor responses as compared Mountakis, 2008; Cleland, 1994; Memmert, 2007;
to creative thinking. Memmert & Roth, 2007). Also, executive and speci-
Motor creativity performances in the Bertsch test cally inhibitory functions can be fruitfully submitted to
seem to be largely independent of individual differences educational interventions in normally developing chil-
in the development of fundamental ne-motor and dren (Rueda, Rothbart, McCandliss, Saccomanno, &
gross-motor skills involved in the four motor creativity Posner, 2005) and, more interestingly, movement-based
tasks. In fact, the task analysis correlating performance interventions in physical education can benet inhibi-
on creativity tasks requiring specic fundamental motor tory functions in children affected by developmental
skills (e.g., ball and hope tasks in Bertsch test) with the coordination disorders (Tsai, 2009). When also con-
individual developmental level of such skills as assessed sidering children affected by attention decit and hyper-
on noncreative tasks (e.g., manipulative and ball skills activity disorders, we suppose that a motor creativity
tasks in the M-ABC test) did not show any signicant treatment might support the development of inhibitory
association. It is to consider that the total score of motor functions that are impaired in this type of disorder
impairment obtained with the M-ABC showed all chil- (Fuggetta, 2006). Thus, research addressing the issue
dren being normally developing and scoring above the of the trainability of executive and particularly inhibi-
threshold of borderline motor performance. Sternberg tory functions thorough motor creativity experiences
and Lubart (1995) proposed that there may be thresh- in childhood should be closely linked to contemporary
olds for some components of creativity below which theoretical perspectives on abnormal development in
creativity is not possible regardless of the individual order to obtain relevant practical knowledge for preven-
level on the other components. Considering the inter- tion and treatment of disorders.
play between domain-relevant skills and creativity-
relevant processes (Amabile, 1996), we hypothesize that
the absence of motor developmental impairment may be REFERENCES
a threshold for domain-relevant motor skills over which
childrens level on other components, such as creativity- Abraham, A., Windmann, S., McKenne, P., & Gunturkun, O. (2007).
relevant processes relying on executive function, may Creative thinking in schizophrenia: The role of executive dys-
inuence creative moving. However, the lack of studies function and symptom severity. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 12,
applying the task analysis to identify the creativity 235258.
Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in context. Boulder, CO: Westview.
requirements limits the understanding of the relative Baer, J., & Kaufman, J. C. (2005). Bridging generality and specicity:
importance of different attributes for creativity (Lubart The assessment park theoretical (APT) model of creativity. Roeper
& Guignard, 2004). Review, 27, 158163.
CREATIVE MOVING AND THINKING AND INHIBITION 271

Banich, M. T. (2009). Executive functionThe search for an inte- Memmert, D. (2007). Can creativity be improved by an
grated account. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, attention-broadening training program? An exploratory study
8994. focusing on team sports. Creativity Research Journal, 19, 23,
Bekhtereva, N. P., Danko, S. G., Starchenko, M. G., Pakhomov, S. 281291.
V., & Medvedev, S. V. (2001). Study of the brain organization of Memmert, D. (2009). Noticing unexpected objects improves the cre-
creativity: III. Positron-emission tomography data. Human ation of creative solutionsInattentional blindness by children
Physiology, 27, 390397. inuences divergent thinking negatively. Creativity Research
Bertsch, J. (1983). Le creativite` motrice. Son evaluation et son optimisa- Journal, 21, 302304.
tion dans la pedagogie des situations motrices a lecoleManuel de Memmert, D., & Roth, K. (2007). The effects of non-specic and
tests. [Motor creativity. Evaluation and optimization in the peda- specic concepts on tactical creativity in team ball sports. Journal
gogy of physical educationTest manual]. Paris: INSEP. of Sports Sciences, 25, 14231432.
Best, J. R., Miller, P. H., & Jones, L. L. (2009). Executive function Naglieri, J. A., & Kaufman, J. C. (2001). Understanding intelligence,
after age 5: Changes and correlates. Developmental Review, 29, giftedness and creativity using PASS theory. Roeper Review, 23,
180200. 151156.
Bournelli, P., & Mountakis, C. (2008). The development of motor Passolunghi, M. C., & Pazzaglia, F. (2004). Individual differences in
Downloaded by [Alma Mater Studiorum - Universit di Bologna] at 01:08 21 November 2011

creativity in elementary school children and its retention. Creativity memory updating in relation to arithmetic problem solving.
Research Journal, 20, 7280. Learning and Individual Differences, 14, 219230.
Carson, S. H., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2003). Decreased Pennequin, V., Sorel, O., & Fontaine, R. (2010). Motor planning
latent inhibition is associated with increased creative achievement between 4 and 7 years of age: Changes linked to executive functions.
in high-functioning individuals. Personality and Individual Differ- Brain and Cognition, 74, 107111.
ences, 85, 499506. Piek, J. P., & Dyck, M. J. (2004). The relationship between motor
Cleland, F. E. (1994). Young childrens divergent movement ability: coordination, executive functioning and attention in school-aged
Study II. Journal of Teaching in Physical Education, 13, 228241. children. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 19, 10631076.
Delis, D. C., Lansing, A., Houston, W. S., Wetter, S., Han, S. D., Plucker, J., & Beghetto, R. (2004). Why creativity is domain general,
Jacobson, M., et al. (2007). Creativity lost: The importance of why it looks domain specic, and why the distinction does not
testing higher-level executive functions in school-age children and matter. In R. J. Sternberg, E. L. Grigorenko & J. L. Singer
adolescents. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 25, 2940. (Eds.), Creativity: From potential to realization (pp. 153167).
Diamond, A. (2000). Close interrelation of motor development and Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
cognitive development and of the cerebellum and prefrontal cortex. Plucker, J., & Zabelina, D. (2009). Creativity and interdisciplinarity:
Child Development, 71, 4456. One creativity or many creativities? ZDMThe International
Diamond, A. (2002). Normal development of prefrontal cortex Journal on Mathematics and Education, 41, 511.
from birth to young adulthood: Cognitive functions, anatomy, Roskos-Ewoldsen, B., Black, S. R., & Mccown, S. M. (2008). Age-
and biochemistry. In D. T. Stuss & R. T. Knight (Eds.), Principles related changes in creative thinking. Journal of Creative Behavior,
of frontal lobe function (pp. 466503). New York: Oxford University 42, 3359.
Press. Rueda, M. R., Rothbart, M. K., McCandliss, B. D., Saccomanno, L.,
Dietrich, A. (2004). The cognitive neuroscience of creativity. Psycho- & Posner, M. I. (2005). Training, maturation, and genetic inuences
nomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 10111026. on the development of executive attention. Proceedings of the
Fink, A., Graif, B., & Neubauer, A. C. (2009). Brain correlates under- national Academy of Sciences of the USA, 102, 1493114936.
lying creative thinking. EEG alpha activity in professional vs. novice Runco, M. A. (1987). The generality of creative performance in gifted
dancers. Neuroimage, 46, 854862. and nongifted children. Gifted Child Quarterly, 31, 1622.
Fuggetta, G. P. (2006). Impairment of executive functions in boys with Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1995). Defying the crowd: Cultivating
attention decit=hyperactivity disorder. Child Neuropsychology, 12, creativity in a culture of conformity. New York: Free Press.
121. Tocci, N., Scibinetti, P., & Zelli, A. (2004). Age and gender differences
Henderson, S. E., & Sudgen, D. A. (1992). Movement assessment bat- in motor creativity among Italian elementary school children.
tery for children. [Italian version: Movement ABCBatteria per la Journal of Human Movement Studies, 46, 89104.
valutazione motoria del bambino, 2000]. London: Psychological Torrance, E. P. (1981). Thinking creatively in action and movement.
Corporation. Bensenville, IL: Scolastic Testing Service.
Huizinga, M., Dolan, C. V., & Van der Molen, M. W. (2006). Torrance, E. P. (1988). Torrance tests of creative thinking. Bensenville,
Age-related change in executive function: Developmental trends IL: Scholastic Testing Service.
and a latent variable analysis. Neuropsychologia, 44, 20172036. Torrance, E. P. (1989). Test di pensiero creativo. Firenze, Italy:
Kaufman, J. C., & Baer, J. (2005). The amusement park theory of crea- Organizzazioni Speciali.
tivity. In J. C. Kaufman & J. Baer (Eds.), Creativity across domains: Torrance, E. P., & Ball, O. E. (1984). The Torrance Tests of Creative
Faces of the muse (pp. 321328). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Thinking Streamlined (revised) manual, Figural A and B.
Associates. Bensenville, IL: Scholastic Testing Service, Inc.
Kim, K. H. (2006). Can we trust creativity tests? A review of the Tor- Torrents, C., Castaner, M., Dinusova, M., & Anguera, M. T. (2010).
rance tests of creative thinking (TTCT). Creativity Research Journal, Discovering new ways of moving: observational analysis of motor
18, 314. creativity while dancing contact improvisation and the inuence of
Konstantinidou, E., Pollatou, E., & Zachopoulou, E. (2005). The the partner. Journal of Creative Behaviour, 44, 4561.
effect of verbal instructions on preschool childrens motor creativity. Towse, J. N., & McIachlan, A. (1999). An exploration of random
Inquiries in Sport and Physical Education, 3, 9097. generation among children. British Journal of Developmental
Lubart, T., & Guignard, J. H. (2004). The generality-specicity of Psychology, 17, 363380.
creativity: A multivariate approach. In R. J. Sternberg, E. L. Towse, J. N., & Neil, D. (1998). Analyzing human number generation
Grigorenko & J. L. Singer (Eds.), Creativity: From potential to behavior: A Review of methods used and a computer program for
realization (pp. 4356). Washington, DC: American Psychological describing performance. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments,
Association. and Computer, 30, 583591.
272 SCIBINETTI, TOCCI, AND PESCE

Trevlas, E., Matsouka, O., & Zachopoulou, E. (2003). Relationship White, H. A., & Shah, P. (2006). Uninhibited imaginations: Creativity
between playfulness and motor creativity in preschool children. in adults with attention-decit=hyperactivity disorder. Personality
Early Child Development and Care, 173, 535543. and Individual Differences, 40, 11211131.
Tsai, C. L. (2009). The effectiveness of exercise intervention on inhibi- Zachopolou, E., & Makri, A. (2005). A developmental perspective of
tory control in children with developmental coordination disorder: divergent movement ability in early young children. Early Child
Using a visuospatial attention paradigm as a model. Research on Development and Care, 175, 8595.
Developmental Disabilities, 30, 10681080. Zachopoulou, E., Makri, A., & Pollatou, E. (2009). Evaluation of chil-
Vandervert, L. R., Schimpf, P. H., & Liu, H. (2007). How working drens creativity: Psychometric properties of Torrances Thinking
memory and the cerebellum collaborate to produce creativity and Creatively in Action and Movement test. Early Child Development
innovation. Creativity Research Journal, 19, 118. and Care, 179, 317328.
Downloaded by [Alma Mater Studiorum - Universit di Bologna] at 01:08 21 November 2011

Вам также может понравиться