Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 9

Tourism Management 59 (2017) 376e384

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Tourism Management
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/tourman

A critique of Response Bias in the tourism, travel and hospitality


research*
Atila Yksel
Tourism Faculty, Adnan Menderes University, Turkey

h i g h l i g h t s

 We examined response bias in leading tourism, travel and hospitality journals.


 We identied that response bias was not properly acknowledged.
 We identied citer myopia with serious repercussions on scientic progress.
 We found that the issue of bias was not taken as seriously as it should be by the editors.

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Measurement accuracy of respondents' opinions, perceptions, attitudes and behaviors is vital for better
Received 10 May 2016 managerial suggestions, theoretical conclusions and advancement of science. There is a growing concern,
Received in revised form voiced frequently in other disciplines that a high rate of response bias exists and this casts serious doubts
2 August 2016
on generalizability of conclusions. Based on limited research on the topic, this paper examines the extent
Accepted 5 August 2016
to which response bias is acknowledged as a limitation in tourism, travel and hospitality research.
Additional analyses were carried out to explore i) researchers' attentiveness to cautions when citing a
biased study and ii) editorial policies of journals against bias. The results revealed that i) acknowl-
Keywords:
Research limitation
edgement of response bias was low, ii) that researchers negligence to reported response bias limitations
Response behavior was high, and iii) that editorial policies of the examined journals were far from encouraging authors to
Response bias discuss and include response bias related limitations. Implications were discussed.
Citer myopia 2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction 2008; Peer & Gamliel, 2011). Response bias related limitations are
important to understand for placing research ndings in context,
There is an increasing concern that in modern research, false interpreting the validity of the scientic work, and ascribing a credibility
ndings may be the majority (Brutus, Gill, & Duniewicz, 2013; level to the conclusions of published research (Ioannidis, 2007, p. 324).
Ioannidis, 2005). Rossiter (2011, p. 79) alleged that that all the Contrary to its signicance, only a number of researchers (e.g.,
ndings in the social sciences based on Likert items and Semantic Dif- Dodd-Mccue & Tartaglia, 2010; Ioannidis, 2005, 2007; Lavrakas,
ferential items are suspectdand this means the majority of ndings! . 2004; Mortel, 2008; Villar, 2008) warn that response bias is prop-
Likewise Kollat, Blackwell and Engel point that It would not be erly acknowledged as a limitation. A review of research articles
surprising if 90% of the ndings and lack of ndings proved to be wrong published in the Journal of Pastoral Care and Counseling
(1972, p. 577). Response biases (i.e., halo effects, social desirability, (1998e2008) demonstrated that only two articles acknowledged
acquiescence, leniency effects etc.) could be the main sources of the potential impact of self-report response bias. Another recent
measurement error (Bagozzi & Yi, 1991). And they may seriously research on the leading scientic publications (e.g., Science, Nature,
mislead results and management policies (Arana & Leon, 2013; Proceedings of the National Academy of Science) showed that only
Campell & Fiske, 1959). Biases spuriously increase internal consis- 17% of articles in these journals included limitations including
tency, decrease validity and produce defective frequency distribu- response bias (Ioannidis, 2007). Similarly, examining 14,275
tions (Dolnicar, 2007; Dolnicar & Grn, 2009; Dolnicar, Grn & Lee, studies, Mortel (2008) found that only 0.002% (N: 31) used a social
desirability scale to detect social desirability bias.
*
This paper was selected as the best paper at the International Conference on Due to its negative implications on research ndings, reported in
Tourism Dynamics and Trends, 2016, Antalya, Turkey
other disciplines, a similar study with a specic focus on travel,
E-mail address: atilayuksel@gmail.com.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tourman.2016.08.003
0261-5177/ 2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
A. Yksel / Tourism Management 59 (2017) 376e384 377

Table 1
Types of response biases.

Response bias A general term that refers to conditions or factors that take place during the process of responding to surveys, affecting the way responses are
provided. Such circumstances lead to a nonrandom deviation of the answers from their true value. Because this deviation takes on average the
same direction among respondents, it creates a systematic error of the measure, or bias. The effect is analogous to that of collecting height data
with a ruler that consistently adds (or subtracts) an inch to the observed units. The nal outcome is an overestimation (or underestimation) of
the true population parameter. Unequivocally identifying whether a survey result is affected by response bias is not as straightforward as
researchers would wish. (Villar, 2008; http://srmo.sagepub.com/)
Acquiescence bias The tendency for survey respondents to agree with statements regardless of their content. Acquiescence response bias could inuence any
question in which the response options involve conrming a statement, but it may be particularly problematic with agree-disagree questions
(Holbrook, 2008a; http://srmo.sagepub.com/)
Extreme responding That drives respondents to only select the most extreme options or answers available.
Social desirability bias That drives an individual to answer in a way that makes them look more favorable to the researcher.
Non-response bias Nonresponse bias occurs when sampled elements from which data are gathered are different on the measured variables in non-negligible ways
from those that are sampled but from which data are not gathered. Essentially, all surveys are likely to have some degree of nonresponse bias
(Lavrakas, 2004; srmo.sagepub.com/)
Common method bias Variance that is attributable to the measurement method rather than to the constructs the measures are assumed to represent.
Recall bias There are two types of recall bias. One, arising from better recall of recent events than those occurring long time ago. Also, serious episodes are
easy to recall than the mild episodes (Indrayan, 2008, p.208).
Recency effect One type of response order effect, whereby the order in which response options are offered to respondents affects the distribution of responses.
Recency effects occur when response options are more likely to be chosen when presented at the end of a list of response options than when
presented at the beginning (Holbrook, 2008b; srmo.sagepub.com/)
Leniency bias The tendency for raters to rate those whom they know well, or whom they are ego involved, higher than they should.

Table 2
Research Articles mentioning response bias, common-method bias, non-response bias and social-desirability bias.

# Articles published Response bias Common method bias Non-response Social desirability bias

Tourism Management (TM) 2521 73 31 20 5


Annals of Tourism Research (ATR) 2137 27 2 11 3
Int. Journal of Hospitality Management (IJHM) 1625 75 71 31 10
Journal of Travel Research (JTR) 803 44 7 22 6
Journal of Sustainable Tourism (JST) 610 10 5 1 3
Journal of Travel and Tourism Marketing (JTTM) 474 16 7 8 2
Cornell Hotel and Restaurant Administration Quarterly (CHRAQ) 968 1 2 1 e
Int. Journal of Contemporary Hospitality Management (IJCHM) 455 16 18 11 2
Journal of Hospitality and Tourism Research (JHTR) 439 30 10 11 2
Total 10,032 292 153 116 33

Source: Scopus database (accessed on-30 December 2014e08 January 2015).

tourism and hospitality journals would be useful to understand the concern about response bias. Overall, the results showed that only
extent to which response bias is acknowledged as a research limita- 0.003 percent of the published articles (N: 10,032), touched on the
tion. In order to realize this objective, accessible research articles subject of response bias. Only 0.012 percent of the articles seemed
published in nine leading journals (e.g., Tourism Management, Journal to have tackled with non-response bias, 0.003 percent with social
of Travel Research, Annals of Tourism Research, Journal of Travel and desirability bias and 0.016 percent with common method bias. A
Tourism Marketing, Journal of Hospitality and Tourism Research, Cornell relatively high rate of citer myopia was traced. The majority of citers
Hotel and Restaurant Administration Quarterly, Journal of Sustainable were found to turn blind eye to bias cautions highlighted in the
Tourism, International Journal of Contemporary Hospitality Manage- quoted articles. Editorial policies of the examined journals were
ment and International Journal of Hospitality Management), archived in found to be far from encouraging authors to discuss and include
general databases (e.g., Scopus) and publishers online content plat- response bias limitations in their submissions.
forms (e.g., Science Direct, Sage, Emerald and Taylor and Francis On-
line) were searched. Such terms as non-response bias, social
desirability bias, common-method bias, recall bias, memory 2. Literature review
bias, extreme response bias, acquiescence bias, recency bias,
mid-point bias and leniency bias were sought. Then, each section Ioannidis (2005, p. 697) denes bias as the combination of
(i.e., literature, methodology, sampling and data collection, various subject related, as well as, design, data, analysis, and pre-
results and discussion, and conclusion, limitations and sugges- sentation factors that tend to produce research ndings when they
tions) of the most recently published articles (N: 179) in which should not be produced. Paulhus describes it as a systematic ten-
response bias was specically referred to were further scrutinized to dency to respond to a range of questionnaire items on some basis other
identify the exact location in which the bias was mentioned. than the specic item content (i.e., what the items were designed to
Considering the potential skip reading behavior common in re- measure) (Paulhus, 1991, p. 18). Several types of biases have been
searchers, the identication of the location where bias is mentioned is reported (Table 1). Response bias might be more common than
important. Hence, an additional content analysis was conducted with thought. Existence of response bias means that even some perfectly
a sample of articles, citing a publication with a response bias limita- developed research would produce inconclusive results when re-
tion. This was done in order to identify the extent to which citers spondents are unmotivated bored or fatigued perceptually blinded
attend to the already raised bias cautions and recommendations. and when noise is added to the data as a result of meaningless
Finally, editorial policies (i.e., guidelines for authors and manuscript responses (Levordashka, 2006; Krosnick, 1991, 1999; Krosnick et al.,
evaluation forms) of nine journals were examined for traces of 2002). Skilled respondents with high motivation may optimize by
carefully following each step of the response process (Levordashka,
378 A. Yksel / Tourism Management 59 (2017) 376e384

2006). Other respondents with less motivation may satisce. And had not given the particular issue any thought prior to the interview
they may take different shortcuts to complete the questionnaire (Erikson, Luttberg, & Tedin, 1988, p. 44). Under social pressures
(Galesic, Tourangeau, Couper, & Conrad, 2008).1 In surveys, re- respondents are likely to give meaningless answers (Converse,
spondents could adopt any of the ve visual interpretive heuristics 1964, 1970)i. To explore how meaningful survey answers are,
and the violation of these heuristics could shape the answering Converse (1964) carried a series of experiments. He asked ques-
process (Tourangeau, Couper & Conrad, 2004). The ve heuristics tions on ctitious issues and found that about approximately half of
include: 1) middle means typical; 2) left and top means rst; 3) near the respondents (30e50%) provided an answer to issues that were
means related; 4) up means good; and 5) items that are close to each fabricated by the researcher (Converse, 1964). Similarly in Hartley's
other are similar (Tourangeau et al., 2004, p. 370). Heaping, or digit (1946) research, the majority of subjects were found to express an
preference, is another prevalent form of survey satiscing. opinion about ctitious nationalities (e.g., Wallonians). In another
Response heaping (also referred to as rounding or digit preference) study respondents were willing to give directions to places that in
occurs when respondents show a preference for rounded numbers fact did not exist (Collett & O'Shea, 1976). Almost identically, sub-
(often those divisible by ve or 10) (Holbrook et al., 2014, p., 591). jects were found to state views about a ctitious political gure (i.e.
Respondents are likely to attempt to make the task of reporting Thomas Walker) (Kolson & Green, 1970). Last but not least, more
their answer as easy as they can. They will use ranges or round values than two-thirds of subjects surveyed expressed an opinion about a
to report numeric quantities To the extent that they are tired, un- ctitious Metallic Metals Act, which in fact did not exist (Gill, 1947).
interested, or generally unable to cope with the demands of the items, Hence, respondents' willingness to state an opinion on researcher-
respondents will be more prone to use strategies that reduce the fabricated matters should cast some doubt on the credibility of
burden that the questions impose (Tourangeau, Rips, & Rasinski, their accounts in survey forms.
2000, p. 254). The fear of appearing uninformed may also Presence of response bias is likely to affect statistical analysis,
tempt many respondents to conjure up opinions even when they and hence research results. Particularly, it would lead to biased
results in any econometric analyses based on correlations (Arana &
Leon, 2013). As satiscing becomes more pronounced, the re-
1
Survey instruments (e.g., questionnaires), generally based on previously liabilities of scales increase, as do the correlations between scales
developed scales are increasingly being used for evidence-based decision-making, (Barge & Gelhbach, 2012). When standard deviations grow and
academic scholarship, and informing public opinion (Dolnicar, 2013; Mortel, 2008;
correlations fall, this will affect particularly factor analysis or
Royal, Ellis, Ensslen, & Homan, 2010). Asking closed-format questions with
response categories is the major approach for collecting data about individuals'
regression analysis for they use correlation analysis as their foun-
beliefs, values, attitudes and behaviors (Lenzner, Kaczmirek, & Galesic, 2011). dation (Chun, Cambell & Yoo, 1974; Hui & Triandis, 1989; Heide &
Despite the regularity, the ability of questionnaires in producing reliable and valid Gronhaug, 1992). Furthermore, fatigue and boredom can increase
ndings has been received criticisms for two reasons: 1) questionnaires are sus- study abandonment (dropout) and thus lead to sampling biases
ceptible to varied kinds of response bias and to inevitable data contamination
(Cook, Heath, & Thompson, 2000). This would pose serious prob-
problems (Campell & Fiske, 1959), and 2) inferences about statistical relationships
may be exaggerated by the problem of common method variance (i.e., variance that lems for studies that rely on representative sampling (e.g., public
is caused by the measurement method) (Donaldson & Grant-Vallone, 2002). The opinion surveys) or random assignment (e.g., experiments), espe-
quality of the data acquired through questionnaires is highly contingent upon the cially if attrition is non-random (Levordashka, 2006). Cote and
respondents' cognitive capacity (Tourangeau et al., 1988; 2004), as well as, their Buckley (1987) reported that almost a quarter of the variance in a
willingness to provide accurate, sincere, reliable and valid answers (Levordhashka,
2006; Schwarz & Clore, 1983). The majority of the predetermined responses in
typical research could be an artifact of common method bias. There
questionnaires are perceptual, meaning that they require respondents to make either is also a substantial evidence that common method bias can have a
a subjective judgment or an estimation (Favero & Bullock, 2014, p. 286). Hence, the considerable inuence on observed relationships between mea-
accuracy, reliability and validity of a response become highly questionable if the sures of different constructs (Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Podsakoff &
respondent's memory is false and there are biased-judgments, inaccurate percep-
Lee, 2003): on average, the amount of variance accounted for
tions, and under or over estimations as a result of respondents' psychology (e.g.,
boredom, fatigue, perceptional blindness, and lack of motivation, knowledge and when common method variance was present was approximately
interest). Questionnaire respondents often answer questions in ways that were 35% versus approximately 11% when it was not present (Podsakoff,
unexpected by the researcher (Stern, Dillman, & Smyth, 2007). Asking people, for MacKenzie, Lee, & Podsakoff, 2003, p. 880). Similarly, Doty and
example, what brands they like may not always be an accurate way of nding out Glick's (1998) results indicated that common method bias was
what they will buy (Prote.in, 2014). Pepsi conducted blind-taste tests to nd out
how people perceived its brand. The results showed that consumers preferred the
pervasive and problematic: only 46% of the variation in measures
taste of Pepsi to that of arch-rival Coca Cola. And yet, as far as the sales numbers was attributable to the constructs, that 32% of the observed variation
were concerned, Coca Cola continued to be the avor of choice (Prote.in, 2014). In in measures was attributable to common methods variance, and that
Chabris and Simon's (2010) research, 65% of the surveyed people strongly agreed common methods variance resulted in a 26% bias in the observed re-
that they could feel if someone behind them was looking at the back of their heads.
lationships among constructs (p. 401).
This is a questionable account of participants since one can't tell when someone
else is looking at him/her, at least not without rst looking back at them. This
statement was debunked in a series of laboratory experiments, conducted with 3. The study
persons who declared themselves peculiarly susceptible to the stare. The experi-
ments have invariably given a negative result (Titchener, 1898; cf.; Gross, 1999). This review paper aims to identify the extent to which response
Graphical and verbal manipulations in visual design of questions in self-
administered questionnaires affect respondents' behavior (Stern et al., 2007). Evi-
bias is recognized as a research limitation in the selected tourism,
dence suggests that responses gathered through a questionnaire may be format- travel and hospitality journals. Accessible research articles pub-
dependent (e.g., fully labeled vs end-point labeled scales, 5-point vs 7-point lished in nine leading journals, namely Tourism Management,
scales etc) instead of question (content)-dependent (Schwarz et al. 1991a,b; Tuten, Journal of Travel Research, Annals of Tourism Research, Journal of
Galesic, & Bosnjak, 2004). Weijters, Cabooter, and Schillewaert (2010) and Hui and
Travel and Tourism Marketing, Journal of Hospitality and Tourism
Triandis (1989) conclude that irrespective of the content, respondents are likely to
show a greater tendency to agree rather than tendency to disagree with questions Research, Cornell Hotel and Restaurant Administration Quarterly,
and different formats could yield different response distributions. Respondents Journal of Sustainable Tourism, International Journal of Contemporary
were observed to spend more time looking at the response options in the rst half Hospitality Management and International Journal of Hospitality
of the vertical response list than at those in the second (Galesic et al., 2008). There Management and archived in general databases of peer-reviewed
is further evidence suggesting that research ndings can be seriously confounded
by a seemingly simple misalignment of response categories (Smith, 1995, p. 1048).
literature (e.g., Scopus) and online content platforms of pub-
Similarly, Toepoel, Das and Soest (2009) concluded that the verbal alignment of a lishers' databases (e.g., Science Direct, Sage, Emerald and Taylor
scale (decremental versus incremental) could alter responses. Francis Online) were subjected to a content analysis in. Similar to
A. Yksel / Tourism Management 59 (2017) 376e384 379

Mortels (2008) approach, the author searched the published arti- Online), subscribed by the author's institution, publishing articles
cles by selecting the specic journal from the source title menu of in English were covered in the study. The search was conducted
the general database rst. The ltering menu in Scopus was used to with accessible full-text articles between the 30th December 2014
choose the document type (articles, conference papers, book re- and 8th of January 2015.
views etc.). Only research articles (N: 10,032) were included. Then,
a keyword search (e.g., research limitation, response bias, non-
4. Findings and discussion
response bias, common method bias, social desirability bias,
etc.) was carried out. A similar analysis, with an extended keyword
The results of an examination of 10,032 research articles pub-
search (e.g., response bias, non-response bias, common
lished in nine leading journals, reveals that only a few published
method bias, social desirability bias, leniency bias, mid-point
study have acknowledged response-bias as a limitation (Table 2).
bias, recall bias, acquiescence bias etc.) was carried out in
According to the search of archived articles in Scopus, only 0.0029
publishers' databases. We referred to Science Direct for the content
percent of the examined articles (N: 10,032) touched on the issue of
analysis of Tourism Management, Annals of Tourism Research and
response bias anywhere in an article. More specically, only
International Journal of Hospitality Management, to Sage for the
0.0011 percent of the articles tackled with non-response bias,
analysis of Journal of Travel Research, Cornell Hotel and Restaurant
0.003 percent with social desirability bias and 0.0015 percent
Administration Quarterly and JHTR, to Emerald for the analysis of
with common method bias. Only 27 articles out of 2137 published
International Journal of Contemporary Hospitality Management and
in Annals of Tourism Research, only 70 articles out of 2521 in Tourism
to Taylor Francis Online for the analysis of Journal of Sustainable
Management, only 10 out of 610 published in the Journal of Sus-
Tourism and Journal of Travel and Tourism Marketing. A total of
tainable Tourism, only 1 out of 968 in the Cornell Hotel and
26,259 articles were found to be published. Only accessible ones
Restaurant Administration Quarterly, only 75 out of 1625 research
were included in the study.
articles published in the International Journal of Hospitality Man-
Next, a sample of most recently published articles, reporting
agement, only 44 out of 803 research articles published in Journal of
response bias, (N: 179) were downloaded and subjected to a further
Travel Research, only 16 out of 474 in the Journal of Travel and
content analysis in order locate the section in which the response
Tourism Marketing, and only 30 out of 439 research articles pub-
bias was referred to. Super Text Search program was used. Simple
lished in the Journal of Hospitality and Tourism Research specically
search for words and phrases (e.g., response bias, non-response
referred to response bias in the main body of the text. This low
bias, common method bias, social desirability bias, leniency
rate of response bias acknowledgement is consistent with similar
bias, mid-point bias, recall bias, acquiescence bias etc.) were
previous studies (Ioannidis, 2007; Mortel, 208).
carried out separately by two coders. The results were then
In order to cross-validate these ndings, additional word checks
compared. Since the second objective of this research was to
through Boolean system was undertaken separately for available
explore researchers' attendance to response bias cautions, an
articles archived in the online content platforms of Science Direct,
additional analysis was conducted. Thus, ve articles out of 29
Sage Publication, Emerald and Taylor and Francis Online. In this
mentioning response bias in the limitations section were
case, articles were checked for an extended range of bias terms (e.g.,
randomly selected. They were then entered into the Google search
response bias, non ( ) response bias, social desirability bias,
to identify the number of citations they had received. This has
common ( ) method bias, recall bias, memory bias, recency bias
produced a total of 102 citing articles. Super Text Search program
(effect), acquiescence bias, leniency bias and mid ( ) point bias)
was then used to examine these articles. This analysis was done to
(Table 3). Out of 26,259 articles, a total of 1206 articles were found
identify the extent to which citers of an article, already acknowl-
to mention about any kind of biases (0.004 percent) (Table 3).
edging a response bias limitation, attend to the raised response bias
Among the acknowledging articles (N: 1206), the majority seems to
cautions and recommendations. As in the previous case, two re-
have concerned with response bias (0.38 percent) and non-
searchers carried out simple search for words and phrases (e.g.,
response bias (0.34 percent), followed by common method bias,
response bias, non-response bias, common method bias,
social desirability bias, recall bias, and memory bias. Interestingly,
social desirability bias, leniency bias, mid-point bias, recall
none of these articles referred to extreme response bias and only
bias, acquiescence bias etc.). The results were then compared. A
one referred to leniency bias.
nal analysis was conducted to identify whether editorial policies
Following this analysis, a sample of the most recently published
of these nine journals encourage authors to discuss and include
articles (N: 179), reporting response bias were subjected to a
response bias limitations. It should be noted that only ve data-
content analysis (Tourism Management (N: 52), Annals of Tourism
bases (Scopus, Science Direct, Sage, Emerald and Taylor and Francis
Research (N: 31), IJHM (N: 62), Journal of Travel Research (N: 34)).

Table 3
Research articles reporting any kind of response bias.

Response Non-response Extreme Social Common Recall Memory Recency Acquiescence Mid-point Leniency
bias bias response desirability method bias bias bias bias Bias bias
bias bias bias

TM* (3983)t 76 51 e 13 32 9 4 e 5 e 1
ATR* (4742)t 40 54 e 8 2 8 2 e e 8 e
IJHM* (2263)t 89 74 e 17 77 4 7 e 1 e e
JST**** (789)t 15 8 e 9 1 e e e e e e
JTTM**** (923)t 37 37 e 1 7 2 4 2 1 e e
IJCHM *** (1181)t 34 24 e 5 26 e 1 e e e e
JHTR** (1193)t 32 41 e 2 7 e 1 e e e e
JTR** (6645)t 113 104 e 8 5 28 2 e 1 e e
CHRAQ** (4540) 27 19 e 4 13 2 e 1 e e e
Total mention of bias:1206 463 412 e 67 170 53 21 3 8 8 1

Source: *Science Direct, **Sage publication, ***Emerald, **** Taylor and Francis Online (accessed between 30.12.2014/08.01.2015).
t
Total number of publications, including, articles, conference reviews, editorial letters, and book reviews is 26,259.
380 A. Yksel / Tourism Management 59 (2017) 376e384

Table 4
Location where response bias was referred to in articles.

# Surveyed Literature review Research methodology, Discussion Conclusion, limitations


articlesa sampling and data collection and suggestions

International Journal of Hospitality Management 62 3 44 6 13


Tourism Management 52 3 38 4 10
Journal of Travel Research 34 11 15 5 4
Annals of Tourism Research 31 1 27 1 2
Total 179 18 124 16 29

The list of these articles can be obtained from the author upon request.
a
Since few articles were found to refer to response bias in more than one location (e.g., methodology and limitation), the number of articles may not match with the line
totals.

This was done to identify the section in which response bias was objective, we have randomly sampled articles which have
referred to. Hence, locations of references to bias related words in mentioned response bias in their limitation sections (Table 5).
articles were checked through Super Text Search program. Re- Google search were then used to check-out the number of citations
searchers appear to prefer different locations of an article for that these studies have received. There is strong evidence sug-
mentioning bias. The majority of researchers (0.69 percent) gesting the existence of citer myopia in the sampled articles. The
preferred the Research methodology, Sampling and Data Collec- ndings suggest that there may be a common negligence in citing
tion section (Table 4). For example, 38 articles in the Journal of behaviors in which the citers utilize the limited study as it is,
Tourism Management discussed response bias within the although the study suffers from a pronounced response bias. Sev-
Research Methodology, Sampling and Data Collection section, enty eight percent of citers were found to be unconcerned with the
whereas only 10 articles preferred to mention about bias in their response bias limitation pronounced in the study they quoted.
Conclusion, Limitation and Suggestions sections. Interestingly, More specically, we have randomly selected ve research ar-
among the 179 examined articles, only 29 (0.16 percent) were ticles from Table 5, which have acknowledged response bias as a
found to caution fellow researchers that their ndings and con- limitation. We then searched for subsequent published articles
clusions might not be generalizable due to a response bias limita- citing these studies in order to examine whether citers attend to
tion (Table 4). response bias cautions and recommendations. An examination of
The non-response bias appears to be the main concern among 102 citing articles showed that while quoted studies warn that
tourism researchers publishing in TM (36 of 52 studies). This was their study has a certain limitation and their ndings should be
followed by only three articles referring to the complications treated cautiously, the majority of citers were found to turn a blind
arising from social desirability. A similar analysis was conducted eye to these cautions (Table 6). Researchers were found not only
with 62 articles published in the International Journal of Hospitality largely myopic to response bias limitations but also to recom-
Management. 44 articles referred to response bias in the Research mendations about how to avoid research bias problems in future
Methodology, Sampling and Data collection section, whereas 13 studies. For example, Lee and Ok (2014, p. 185) caution that The
placed in within the Conclusion, Limitations and Suggestions, six second limitation concerns the cross-sectional design of the study. As
within the results and three within the literature review sec- data for this study were collected from individual respondents at a
tion. Consistent with researchers publishing in Tourism Manage- single point in time, inferences about the causal nature of the rela-
ment, authors in the IJHM appear to concern more with non- tionship examined are prone to biases (Bobko and Stone-Romero,
response bias than social desirability bias, common method bias 1998). In future research they recommend, using longitudinal
(37, 5 and 5 articles respectively). Likewise, an analysis of 31 articles designs and other methods to reduce such biases to further validate the
published in the Annals of Tourism Research suggests that the ma- relationships found in this study; such longitudinal studies would
jority of the researchers mentioned about response bias in the allow a stronger causal inference to be established (p. 185). Ac-
section of Research Methodology, sampling and data collection cording to the search on Google, seven studies were found to cite
(27). Only 2 acknowledged its existence in the limitations section, Lee and Ok's study on emotional labor. Surprisingly, none of these
one covered it in the literature and another one discussed it in the citing studies have attended to Lee and Ok's overt caution about the
Results section. Concern with non-response bias was the main issue common method bias. Instead of undertaking a longitudinal study,
for researchers in the ATM29 articles out of 31). Only four cautioned these seven studies were found to continue using single data
following researchers in the Conclusion, limitations and sugges- collection method, identical to the already criticized method used
tion section, whereas 15 mentioned in the Research Methodol- by Lee and Ok (2014). Likewise, Xie and Heung (2012) warn that in
ogy, sample and data collection section, 11 in Literature and ve order to avoid response bias found in their study future research is
in Findings and Discussion sections. Considering the skip and/or also encouraged to employ longitudinal measures in a real-life setting
partial reading, common among researchers, this nding in relation (p. 742). None of the following ten studies, which cited Xie and
to varying places of bias mentioned in articles needs attention. Heung's concerning article, seems to have taken their caution
Researchers may miss out the bias warnings depending on the seriously. They did not employ longitudinal measures to avoid this
section they prefer to read most. chronic response bias problem, although it has been repeatedly
suggested to eliminate this problem (see http://www.sciencedirect.
com/science/article/pii/S0278431914001650Podsakoff et al., 2003).
4.1. Citing behaviors
Similarly, Nunkoo and Ramkissoon (2012) warned that There
may be nonresponse bias in the data and this may limit the extent to
Once a research limitation is reported, the following researchers
which the ndings can be generalized. Therefore, researchers should
are expected to take necessary measures either to eliminate the
evaluate the results of the study cautiously (p. 1016). According to
limitation or treat ndings of the quoted study cautiously. Hence an
Google search, 38 studies have referred to Nunkoo and Ramkissoon
additional analysis was undertaken to scrutinize whether citers of a
study (2012). We were able to examine only 18 of the citing articles
previously published study pay attention to the raised concerns
due to access limitations to the full text articles and language
about response bias limitations. In order to realize this research
A. Yksel / Tourism Management 59 (2017) 376e384 381

Table 5
Samples of response bias acknowledgments in limitation sections.

Statement of authors about bias Cited

The role of affective factors on perceived cruise vacation value (Teoman Due to our sample constraints, we could not measure non-response bias, a 244
Duman & Anna S. Mattila) potential threat to the validity of our results.
Price fairness and its asymmetric effects on overall price, quality and value Readers are also cautioned against several design related limitations of this 153
judgments: the case of an upscale hotel (Haemoon Oh) study. A within-subject derivation of asymmetric effects may be more desirable,
if related response bias and design complexity can be overcome.
Gender and community Tourism dependence level (M. Jeannie Harvey, John No effort was made to check for non-response bias. In a similar project, 62
Hunt, & Charles C. Harris, Jr.) conducted non-response surveys and found little to no non-response bias.
However, since non-response bias cannot be determined for this project, the
results reported in this paper should be considered tentative and not
generalized to represent the characteristics, perceptions, or attitudes of the total
population.
Residents' support for tourism An identity perspective (Robin Nunkoo & Issues relating to potential non-response bias should also be considered given 54
Dogan Gursoy) the small sample size of the study. Non-response bias means that results of this
study may not be fully representative of everyone in the population of the study.
These sampling issues may limit the generalizability of the ndings. Therefore,
ndings of this study may not be generalized beyond the sample examined.
Future studies with larger samples in different locations are needed to validate
the ndings of this study.
Power, trust, social exchange And community support (Robin Nunkoo & There may be nonresponse bias in the data and this may limit the extent to 38
Haywantee Ramkissoon) which the ndings can be generalized. Therefore, researchers should evaluate
the results of the study cautiously.
Luxury marketing: The inuences of psychological and demographic A self-administered survey by paper-and-pencil may create bias problems. 26
characteristics on attitudes toward luxury restaurants (Jee Hye Leea &
Johye Hwang)
The Place Identity e Performance relationship among tourism Moreover, although we examined the differences of respondents on two key 26
entrepreneurs: A structural equation modeling analysis (Hallak, Brown variables, business type and location, and found no apparent evidence of non-
and Lindsay) response bias, we could not test for non-response bias on the survey items
themselves (Lambert & Harrington, 1990).
Consumer value and self-image congruency at different stages of timeshare It is also possible that the responses given by our participants were inuenced 11
ownership (Beverley Sparks, Graham Bradley & Gayle Jennings) by social desirability, leniency, and other response biases
The effects of brand relationship quality on responses to service failure of The advantages of the scenario method are that it eliminates practical 11
hotel consumers (Danhong Xie & Vincent C.S. Heung) difculties through observation or enactment of service failure incidents in the
eld and avoids participants' response bias (Mattila, 2004; Smith, Jarman, &
Osborn, 1999). Future research is also encouraged to employ longitudinal
measures in a real-life setting.
Resident perceptions of a contentious tourism event (David B. Weaver & Of concern is the very low response rate and concomitant response bias which 7
Laura J. Lawton) may implicate the overrepresentation of females and older residents.
Understanding hotel employees' service sabotage: Emotional labor The second limitation concerns the cross-sectional design of the study. As data 7
perspective based on conservation of resources theory (JungHoon (Jay) for this study were collected from individual respondents at a single point in
Lee & Chihyung Michael Ok) time, inferences about the causal nature of the relationship examined are prone
to biases (Bobko and Stone-Romero, 1998). Therefore, in future research, we
recommend using longitudinal designs and other methods to reduce such biases
to further validate the relationships found in this study; such longitudinal
studies would allow a stronger causal inference to be established.
Guest-to-guest interaction on board cruise ships: Exploring social dynamics Apart from limitations in the chosen research design (i.e. number of remarks as 6
and the role of situational factors (Alexis Papathanassis) measurement of importance, potential response bias), this also demonstrates
the risks involved in conducting and interpreting survey research in the cruise
context; or any social context for that matter. Opinions, preferences and beliefs
are implicit and personal in nature and hence their quantication (e.g. Likert
scales, remark classication and counting) is subjected to the subjective
interpretation of both the researchers and respondents.
Psychographic insights from a South Carolina protected area (David B. Study limitations that qualify the theoretical and practical implications include 5
Weaver) possible non-response bias associated with the 20% participation rate, and the
restriction of the study to walkins, who account for only about one-half of all
visitors.

Table 6
Rate of citers attentiveness to response-bias warning.

# Studies Concerned with bias Unconcerned with bias

Duman & Mattila 29 10 19


Jeannie Harvey, John Hunt, Charles C. Harris, Jr. 25 5 20
Nunkoo & Ramkissoon 18 5 13
Jee Hye Lee &, Johye Hwang 13 4 9
Xie & Heung 10 e 10
Lee & Ok 7 e 7
Total 102 24 78

Note: Due to access limitations to full text and language barriers of publications other than English, only 102 articles were examined. The list of these articles can be obtained
from the author upon request.
382 A. Yksel / Tourism Management 59 (2017) 376e384

Table 7
Editorial policies regarding research limitations.

Impact factor Limitation section

Author guidelines Reviewer score sheet

ATR 2.79 None None


JST 2.392 None None
TM 2.377 None None
JTR 1.884 None None
a
IJHM 1.837 None
IJCHM 1.623 Research limitations/implications (in abstract) Research Limitations
a
CHRAQ 1.165 None
JHTR 1.125 None None
JTTM 0.695 None None
a
Due to access problems, review criteria sheet sent to the reviewers of these Journals was not assessed.

barrier of publications other than English. Remarkably, 13 of the 18 acknowledgement of response bias was very low, ii) the majority of
studies did not attend to non-response bias caution in their new the sampled citers were found to remain negligent to response bias
studies. Only ve of these articles concerned with different forms warnings in the quoted articles, and iii) editorial policies of exam-
response biases. Identically, Duman and Mattila (2005, p. 979) ined journals were far from encouraging authors to discuss and
concluded that Due to our sample constraints, we could not mea- include response bias limitations in their manuscripts.
sure non-response bias, a potential threat to the validity of our re- Several procedural remedies (e.g., careful construction of survey
sults. Due to time and resource constraints, we limited instrumentii, changing question and response formats, labeling
examination to articles cited Duman and Mattila's work in 2014 every point on a response scale, careful wording, balancing posi-
only. An analysis of 27 articles revealed that the majority over- tively and negatively worded items, collecting data for each
looked this caution: only 3 out of 27 tackled with non-response construct through a different method etc.) (Dolnicar, 2013; Dunn
bias, 2 with common method bias. Two studies further acknowl- and Wickham, 2012; Indrayan, 2008; Kalton & Schuman, 1982),
edged the impact of conformation bias and memory bias. Inter- as well as, statistical remedies (e.g., unmeasured latent method
estingly, instead of tackling with the raised problem of non- factor technique, correlation-based marker variable technique,
response bias, one of the citing research used a statement almost regression-based marker variable technique, etc.) can be suggested
identically to that of Duman and Mattila: A possibility of bias of as a way to mitigate the effects of response bias (Podsakof ,
non-respondents existed, and thus, the results cannot be used to MacKenzie, & Podsakoff, 2012). However, effectiveness of these
generalize for the population of the study (2005, p.979). In their remedies on respondents' memory, perceptional blindness, ability
research, Harvey, Hunt, and Harris (1995) warned that No effort and motivational factors that may cause biased responses is still
was made to check for non-response bias However, since non- debatable. There is growing evidence suggesting that respondents
response bias cannot be determined for this project, the results re- may frequently process information with limited cognitive pro-
ported in this paper should be considered tentative and not general- cessing (Chartrand, 2005; Zaltman, 2003). They may use various
ized to represent the characteristics, perceptions, or attitudes of the shortcuts and give ctitious answers (Schwarz, 1999), and self-
total population (p.354). Despite the overt caution, we identied reports may not correctly depict people's voluntary responses
that in 25 studies that cited Harvey et al.s initial study for hy- (Kardes, Posavac, & Cronley, 2004; Micu & Plummer, 2010). This
pothesis building, none had attended to this caution. Only ve means that a considerable portion of identied relationships be-
mentioned about other types of response biases. Similarly, nine of tween studied constructs in tourism, travel and hospitality research
the 13 citers of Lee and Hwang (2012) study did not attend to the could be false positives (Type1 error) and false negatives (Type2
response bias caution raised by Lee and Hwang. error). In other words, due to the negligence of response bias,
generalizability claims in the majority of tourism, travel and
4.2. Editorial policies hospitality research appears highly disputable.
There exists a high risk of subjectivity and data contamination
A nal examination was carried out to identify editorial policies due to psychological, contextual and/or graphical factors, inu-
of these leading journals regarding discussion/inclusion of research encing response behaviors. Despite the meticulous research efforts,
limitations in general and response bias in particular. Information the analyses, based on contaminated data by response bias, may
given in guidelines for authors and evaluation criteria sheets send mislead decision. And misinformed decisions can mean higher
to the reviewers was examined (Table 7). Intriguingly, among the operational inefciencies, and increased costs and risks. In short,
above nine top-cited journals, none of the editorial policies, except current overreliance on respondents probably debatable memory,
the IJCHM, encourages discussion and inclusion of research limi- judgments and/or perceptions in tourism studies brings in more
tations in general and response-bias related limitations in partic- questions than answers. Some of these questions include:
ular. Similar to Ioannidis ndings (2007), importance, novelty,
originality, signicance, relevance, and even payment procedures  How condent are we that respondents participating in ques-
appear frequently in journal guidelines for authors. tionnaires use a process of interpreting and reaching conclu-
sions about phenomenon under question without resorting to
subconscious thought? What happens if scores, statements,
5. Conclusion, limitations and suggestions
reports, etc. are driven by respondents' false beliefs, supersti-
tions, intuitions which have been rooted in popular sub-
Based on concerns raised in other disciplines, this review paper
consciousness?
focused on response bias, a potential source of validity problem in
 Could respondents give ctitious answers? If yes, what happens
tourism research. Existence of bias casts doubts on generalizability
then?
of conclusions. A content analysis of research articles published in
leading tourism, travel and hospitality journals showed that i)
A. Yksel / Tourism Management 59 (2017) 376e384 383

 Do respondents follow an (un)conscious pattern when studies of similar quality that show unfavorable or no-difference
answering? Does human brain likes thinking in a linear and results.
patterned fashion? What is wrong with these patterns? Can the
instinctive rush by the respondent to apply a pattern to a phe-
nomenon during a survey narrow his/her thinking? References
 Could the language (words) used in questionnaires alter mem- Aran~ a, J., & Leo
n, C. (2013). Correcting for scale perception bias in tourist satisfaction
ory, perceptions and judgments and that language distort re- surveys. Journal of Travel Research, 52(6), 772e788.
spondents' interpretations? Bagozzi, R. P., & Yi, Y. (1991). Multitraitemultimethod matrices in consumer
research. Journal of Consumer Research, 17, 426e439.
 Would respondents just see the words that are there on a
Barge, S., & Gehlbach, H. (2012). Using the theory of satiscing to evaluate the
questionnaire or they see words that are not there? If they see quality of survey data. Research in Higher Education, 53(2), 182e200.
words that there are not there and answer accordingly, what Bobko, P., & Stone-Romero, E. (1998). Meta-analysis is another useful research tool
but it is not a panacea. In G. Ferris (Ed.), Research in personnel and human re-
happens then?
sources management (Vol. 16, pp. 359e397). , Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
 How accurate respondents are in remembering and reporting Brutus, S., Gill, H., & Duniewicz, K. (2013). State-of-Science in industrial and orga-
their cognitive and emotional states which happened in the past nizational psychology: A review of self-reported limitations. Journal of Man-
or which will be happening in the future? Could people agement, 39(1), 48e75.
Campell, D. T., & Fiske, D. W. (1959). Convergent and discriminant validation by the
remember and report events, behaviors differently from the way multitrait-multimethod matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 56(2), 81e105.
they have happened? Could they even remember and report Chabris, C., & Simon, D. (2010). The invisible gorilla and other ways our intuitions
events, behaviors never happened at all? deceive us. New York: Crown.
Chartrand, T. L. (2005). The role of conscious awareness in consumer behavior.
 Does the chosen number/category reect the truthful opinion/ Journal of Consumer Psychology, 15(3), 203e210.
perception/attitude of the respondent or is it just a digit of Chun, K., Campbell, J. B., & Yoo, J. H. (1974). Extreme response style in cross-cultural
preference? research. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 5(4), 465e480.
Collet, P., & O'Shea, G. (1976). Pointing the way to a ctional place: A study of di-
 Unless intruded by a question, would respondents always think rection giving in Iran and england. European Journal of Social Psychology, 6(4),
of such issues as how loyal, satised they are, or what kind of 447e458.
effect their mood has on their emotions during an ordinary Converse, P. E. (1964). The nature of belief systems in mass politics. In D. Apter (Ed.),
Ideology and discontent (pp. 238e245). New York: Free Press of Glencoe.
course of, say, a dining experience? Is question and/or ques-
Converse, P. E. (1970). Attitudes and non-attitudes: Continuation of a dialogue. In
tioning by an outsider an inseparable component of ordinary E. R. Tufte (Ed.), The quantitative analysis of social problems (pp. 188e189).
consumption and consumer behavior? Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Cook, C., Heath, F., & Thompson, B. (2000). A meta-analysis of response rates in
web- or internet-based surveys. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 60,
While this research sheds some light on the presence of bias 821.
negligence, we should note that this study is not free of limitations. Cote, J., & Buckley, M. R. (1987). Estimating trait, method, and error Variance:
A simple numeric presentation of what percentage of the sampled Generalizing across 70 construct Validation Studies. Journal of Marketing
Research, 24(3), 315e318.
articles actually reported response bias-related limitations and Dodd-McCue, D., & Tartaglia, D. (2010). Self-report response Bias: Learning how to
where the reported response bias was presented may represent a live with its diagnosis in chaplaincy research. Chaplaincy Today, 26(1), 2e8.
limitation. Finding out a very similar tendency which has already Dolnicar, S. (2007). Accepted Standards Undermining the Validity of Tourism
Research. In A. Woodside (Ed.), Advances in culture, tourism and hospitality
been reported in other disciplines may sound as a very little original research (1) (pp. 131e182). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
contribution. Nevertheless this paper draws attention to the Dolnicar, S. (2013). Asking good survey questions. Journal of Travel Research, 1e24.
concept of response bias and promotes discussions on its potential XX(X).
Dolnicar, S., & Grun, B. (2009). Response style contamination of student evaluation
impacts on research ndings. Accessibility of articles was another data. Journal of Marketing Education, 31(2), 160e172.
limitation. We were unable to scan full-text articles published in Dolnicar, S., Grn, B., & Le, H. (2008). Cross-cultural comparisons of tourist Satis-
English particularly before the mid-1990s due to our institution's faction: Assessing analytical robustness. In A. Yuksel (Ed.), Tourist satisfaction
and complaining Behaviour: Measurement & management issues in the tourism
license agreement. This may have implications on ndings
and hospitality Industry (pp. 137e150). New York: Nova Publishing Sciences.
regarding acknowledgement ratio and locations in which response Donaldson, S., & Grant-Vallone, S. (2002). Understanding self-report bias in orga-
bias was referred to. Limited mention of bias in research limitation nizational behavior research. Journal of Business and Psychology, 17(2), 2.
Doty, D. H., & Glick, W. H. (1998). Common methods bias: Does common methods
sections does not necessarily mean that it is regarded unimportant
variance really bias results? Organizational Research Methods, 1, 374e406.
by researchers at any stages of the research. We have limited our Duman, T., & Mattila, A. (2005). The role of affective factors on perceived cruise
search terms containing the word of bias and hence other words vacation value. Tourism Management, 26(3), 311e323.
conveying bias were excluded. Dunn, A., & Wickham, M. (2012). The mix of research methods in the leading
tourism journals: 2000-2009. Problems and Perspectives in Management, 10(1),
Response behavior and citation behavior could be interesting 8e16.
areas for further research. The psychological, contextual, and/or Emerson, G. B., Winston, M. D., Warme, W. J., Wolf, F. M., Heckman, J. D., Brand, R. A.,
graphical factors, which may inuence subjects decisions to et al. (2010). Testing for the presence of positive-outcome bias in peer review.
Archives of Internal Medicine, 170, 1934e1939.
respond, deserve much scholar attention than current efforts being Erikson, R. S., Luttberg, N. R., & Tedin, K. T. (1988). American public opinion (3rd ed.).
put in achieving high response rate (Yuksel & Yuksel, 2011). New York: Macmillan.
Furthermore, the editorial polices may benet from shifting focus Favero, N., & Bullock, B. J. (2014). An evaluation of scholarly responses to common
source bias. Journal of Public Administration Research, 25, 285e308.
from solely on complex analysis to the root of genuine research, Galesic, M., Tourangeau, R., Couper, M., & Conrad, F. (2008). Eye-tracking data new
with sound approaches to detect and if possible eliminate bias insights on response order effects and other cognitive shortcuts in survey
problems, emanating from both the respondents and the re- responding. Public Opinion Quarterly, 72(5), 892e913.
Gill, S. N. (1947). How do you stand on sin? tide 72 (March 14).
searchers. Effects of response bias on conclusions about hypothesis
Gross, G. H. (1999). History of neuroscience: The Fire That Comes from the Eye. The
tests should be carefully analyzed. This is because biased responses Neuroscientist, 5(1), 58e64.
may give rise to inaccurate estimates of effects and associations Hartley, E. L. (1946). Problems in prejudice. New York: Octagon Press.
between variables. In addition to response bias, evidence from Harvey, M., Hunt, J., & Harris, C. C. (1995). Gender and community tourism
dependence level. Annals of Tourism Research, 22(3), 349e366.
other disciplines (e.g., education, psychology, medicine, biology Heide, M., & Gronhaug, K. (1992). The impact of response styles in surveys: A
etc.) suggests that the travel, tourism and hospitality research may simulation study. Journal of the Market Research Society, 34(3), 215e223.
also suffer from publication bias (Emerson et al., 2010). Thus further Holbrook, A. (2008a). Acquiescence bias. In P. J. Lavrakas (Ed.), Encyclopedia of
survey research methods. http://srmo.sagepub.com/view/encyclopedia-of-
research is needed to examine whether studies with a favorable or survey-research-methods/n3.xml.
statistically signicant outcome are published more than are Holbrook, A. (2008b). Recency effect. In P. J. Lavrakas (Ed.), Encyclopedia of survey
384 A. Yksel / Tourism Management 59 (2017) 376e384

research methods. http://www.srmo.sagepub.com/view/encyclopedia-of- (30.th 12. 2014).


survey-research-methods/n449.xml. Rossiter, J. R. (2011). Measurement for the Social Sciences: The C-OAR-SE Method and
vez, N., et al. (2014).
Holbrook, A. L., Anand, S., Johnson, T. P., Cho, Y. I., Shavitt, S., Cha Why It Must Replace Psychometrics. New York: Springer.
Response heaping in interviewer-administered surveys: Is it really a form of Royal, K. D., Ellis, A., Ensslen, A., & Homan, A. (2010). Rating scale optimization in
satiscing? Public Opinion Quarterly, 78(3), 591e633. survey research: An application of the rasch rating scale model. Journal of
Hui, H. C., & Triandis, H. C. (1989). Effects of culture and response format on extreme Applied Quantitative Methods, 5(4), 607e617.
response style. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 20(3), 296e309. Schwarz, N. (1999). Self-reports: How the questions shape the answers. American
Indrayan, A. (2008). Basic methods of medical research (2nd edition: 2008). Indian Psychologist, 54, 93e105.
Journal of Community Medicine, 33(3), 208. Schwarz, N., Bless, H., Strack, F., Klumpp, G., Rittenauer-Schatke, H., & Simons, A.
Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2005). Why most published research ndings are false. August, (1991a). Ease of retrieval as information: Another look at the availability heu-
2(8), 124. ristic. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 195e202.
Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2007). Limitations are not properly acknowledged in the scientic Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-
literature. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 60, 324e329. being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Per-
Kalton, G., & Schuman, H. (1982). The effect of the question on survey response rates: sonality and Social Psychology, 45, 513e523.
A review. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series A, 145(part 1), 44e45. Schwarz, R., Knauper, B., Hippler, H., Neumann, E., & Clark, L. (1991b). Rating Scales:
Kardes, F. R., Posavac, S. S., & Cronley, M. L. (2004). Consumer inference: A review of Numeric values may change the meaning of scale labels. The Public Opinion
processes, bases, and judgment contexts. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14(3), Quarterly, 55(4), 570e582.
230e256. Smith, T. (1995). little things matter: A sampler of how differences in questionnaire
Kollat, D. T., Blackwell, R. D., & Engel, J. E. (1972). The current status of consumer format can affect survey responses. Chicago: National Opinion Research Center.
Behaviour Research: Development during the 1968-1972 period. In GSS Methodological Report. No. 78.
M. Venkatesan (Ed.), Proceedings of the 3rd annual conference of the association Smith, J. A., Jarman, M., & Osborn, M. (1999). Doing interpretative phenomeno-
of consumer research (pp. 576e584). logical analysis. In M. Murray, & K. Chamberlain (Eds.), Qualitative health psy-
Kolson, K. L., & Green, J. J. (1970). Response style and political socialization research. chology: Theories and methods (pp. 218e240). London: Sage Publications.
Social Science Quarterly, 51, 527e538. Stern, M., Dillman, D., & Smyth, J. (2007). Visual design, order effects, and respon-
Krosnick, J. A. (1991). Response strategies for coping with the cognitive demands of dent. Survey Research Methods, 1(3), 121e138.
attitude measures in surveys. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 5, 213e236. Titchener, E. B. (1898). The feeling of being stared at. Science, 8, 895e897.
Krosnick, J. A. (1999). Survey research. Annual review of psychology, 50, 537e567. Toepoel, V., Das, M., & Van Soest, A. (2009). Design of Web Questionnaires: The
Krosnick, J. A., Holbrook, A. L., Berent, M. K., Carson, R. T., Hanemann, M., Kopp, R. J., Effects of the Number of Items per Screen. Field Methods, 21, 200e213.
et al. (2002). The impact of No opinion response options on data quality: Non- Tourangeau, R., Couper, M. P., & Conrad, F. (2004). Spacing, position, and Order:
attitude reduction or an invitation to satisce? Public Opinion Quarterly, 66, Interpretive heuristics for visual features of survey questions. Public Opinion
371e403. Quarterly, 68(3), 368e393.
Lambert, D. M., & Harrington, T. C. (1990). Measuring nonresponse bias in customer Tourangeau, R., Rasinski, K. A., Abelson, R., Bradburn, N., Campbell, S., & An, R. D.
service mail surveys. Journal of Business Logistics, 11(2), 5e25. (1988). Cognitive processes underlying context effects in attitude measurement.
Lavrakas, P. (2004). Non-response bias. In M. S. Lewis-Beck, A. Bryman, & T. F. Liao Psychological Bulletin, 103(3), 299e314.
(Eds.), The SAGE encyclopedia of social science research methods. http://srmo. Tourangeau, R., Rips, L. J., & Rasinski, K. (2000). The psychology of survey response.
sagepub.com/view/the-sage-encyclopedia-of-social-science-research-methods/ Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
n635.xml. Tuten, T. L., Galesic, M., & Bosnjak, M. (2004). Effects of immediate versus delayed
Lee, & Ok. (2014). Understanding hotel employees' service sabotage: Emotional notication of prize draw results on response behavior in web surveys e an
labor perspective based on conservation of resources theory. International experiment. Social Science Computer Review, 22, 377e384.
Journal of Hospitality Management, 36, 176e187. Villar, A. (2008). Response bias. In P. J. Lavrakas (Ed.), Encyclopedia of survey research
Lee, J., & Hwang, J. (2012). Luxury marketing: The inuences of psychological and methods. http://srmo.sagepub.com/view/encyclopedia-of-survey-research-
demographic characteristics on attitudes toward luxury restaurants. Interna- methods/n486.xml.
tional Journal of Hospitality Management, 30, 658e669. Weijters, B., Cabooter, E., & Schillewaert, N. (2010). The effect of rating scale format
Lenzner, T., Kaczmirek, L., & Galesic, M. (2011). Seeing through the eyes of the on response styles: The number of response categories and response category
Respondent: An eye-tracking study on survey question comprehension. Inter- labels. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 27, 236e247.
national Journal of Public Opinion Research, 23(3). Xie, D., & Heung, V. C. S. (2012). The effects of brand relationship quality on re-
Levordashka, A. (2006). Effects of questionnaire design on respondent experience and sponses to service failure of hotel consumers. International Journal of Hospitality
data quality (Unpublished Master Thesis). Management, 31, 735e744.
Mattila, A. (2004). The impact of service failures on customer loyalty: The moder- Yksel, A., & Yksel, F. (2011). Response psychology: Questioning and responding in
ating role of affective commitment,. International Journal of Service Industry social sciences. Yantlama psikolojisi: Arastrmada soru sormak ve yant almak
Management, 15(2), 134e149. zerine. Seyahat ve Otel Is_ letmecilig
i Dergisi, 6(1). www.soiddergi.com.
Micu, A. C., & Plummer, J. T. (2010). Measurable emotions: How television ads really Zaltman, G. (2003). How Customers Think: Essential Insights into the Mind of the
work : Patterns of reactions to commercials can demonstrate advertising Market. Boston, Mass: Harvard Business School Press.
effectiveness. Journal of Advertising Research, 50(2), 137e153.
Mortel, F. (2008). Faking it: Social desirability response bias in self report research.
Australian journal of advanced. Nursing, 25(4), 40e48. Atila Yksel is Professor of Marketing at the University of
Nunkoo, R., & Ramkissoon, H. (2012). Power, trust, social exchange and community Adnan Menderes, Turkey. He is currently the Dean of
support. Annals of Tourism Research, 39(2), 997e1023. Faculty of Communications. Dr. Yksel has published in
Paulhus, D. L. (1991). Measurement and control of response bias. In J. P. Robinson, the Annals of Tourism Research, Journal of Tourism Man-
P. R. Shaver, & L. S. Wrightsman (Eds.), Measures of personality and social psy- agement, Journal of Hospitality and Tourism Research, Jour-
chological attitudes (pp. 17e59). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. nal of Sustainable Tourism, Journal of Travel and Tourism
Peer, E., & Gamliel, E. (2011). Too reliable to be true? Response bias as a potential Marketing, Cornell Quarterly, Journal of Quality Assurance in
source of ination in paper-and-pencil questionnaire reliability. Practical Tourism and Hospitality, and Journal of Vacation Marketing.
Assessment, Research & Evaluation, 16(9), 1e8. Dr Yksel is the editor of the book Tourist Satisfaction and
Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. M., Lee, J., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method Complaining Behavior: Measurement and Management
variance in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recom- Issues in the Tourism and Hospitality Industry (Nova
mended remedies. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 879e903. Publications) and he has co-authored books on tourism
Podsakof, P., MacKenzie, S., & Podsakoff, N. (2012). Sources of method bias in social management and research methodology. He acts as an
science research and recommendations on how to control it. Annual Review of editorial board member in several tourism and hospitality
Psychology, 63, 539e569. journals and he is currently the editor of the Journal of
Prote.in. (2014). Retrieved from https://www.prote.in/en/briengs/neuromarketing Travel and Tourism Research.

Вам также может понравиться