Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 2

PUBLISHED: 27 MARCH 2017 | VOLUME: 1 | ARTICLE NUMBER: 0111

comment

The impact of architecture on


collective behaviour
Noa Pinter-Wollman, Stephen M. Fiore and Guy Theraulaz
Despite the obvious influence of space on interactions, constraints imposed by the built environment
are seldom considered when examining collective behaviours of animals and humans. We propose an
interdisciplinary path towards uncovering the impact of architecture on collective outcomes.

B
uilt structures, such as animal nests, preliminary accounts of how structures complex social organizations7. Physicists have
serve two overarching purposes: shelter influence the efficiency of collective shown that architectural design can improve
and providing a space where organisms behaviour in animals and collective cognition the efficiency with which pedestrian crowds
interact. Shelter has dominated much of the in humans. Biologists have found that the move8. Despite conceptual similarities
discussion in the literature1. But, as the study speed at which harvester ants recruit to a among these research communities, there
of collective behaviour expands, it is time to food source increases with the connectivity has been little, if any, cross-disciplinary
elucidate the role of the built environment in of nest chambers4 and that obstructions communication. Although the various
shaping collective outcomes. near the nest exit of ants facilitate rapid organisms that occupy built structures
Collective behaviour in social animals evacuation when in distress5. Social differ physiologically, there are functional
emerges from interactions2 and collective scientists have found that the layout of similarities that motivate our research. These
cognition in humans emerges from buildings affects scientific collaborations and similarities include the need to coordinate
communication and coordination3. innovation6. Palaeontologists speculate that activities and collaborate. Conclusions from
Despite the obvious influence of space on the development of complicated forms of studies of such functions can be extended
interactions, because spatial proximity architecture coincide with the emergence of from one discipline to inform others.
is necessary for an interaction to occur,
spatial constraints are rarely considered
in studies of collective behaviour or Box 1 | Quantifying structures and spatial location of social interactions within them.
collective cognition. An interdisciplinary
exchange between behavioural ecologists, Although methods for quantifying Likewise, an examination of network
evolutionary biologists, cognitive scientists, structures are essential for examining representations of structures holds no
social scientists, architects and engineers how structures affect collective outcomes, information on their volume. Finding ways
may facilitate a productive exchange of we currently lack a diverse quantitative to jointly quantify topology and volume
ideas, methods and theory that could lead toolbox. Network representations have is an interdisciplinary challenge that calls
us to uncover unifying principles as well as proven extremely useful for describing the for cross-disciplinary collaborations to
novel research approaches and questions arrangement of structures in studies of develop tools and formulas capable of
in studies of animal and human collective social insects4,13 and humans6,14. However, testing the utility and generalizability of
behaviour. Research collaborations across further development of algorithms that such approaches.
disciplines allow us to study, for example, translate structures into networks, and Once we quantify architectural features,
the impact of structures on the collective of relevant network measures, are still we require methods for examining the
behaviour of animals in ways that can needed to expand this line of research. movements and interactions of the
inspire architects to design spaces that Furthermore, when chambers or rooms occupants of these spaces. Specifically,
facilitate collective movements, interactions, are difficult to define (for example, in an there is a need for methods to quantify
collaboration and innovation of humans. art gallery), networks may not be suitable. the relationship between movements
Further, with new technologies (for example, One possible solution is skeletonization12, and the resulting social interactions and
tracking tools), and analytic techniques (for which reduces complex 3D structures various spatial constraints. The use of
example, network theory), an increased to emphasize their geometrical and sensor technologies, such as tracking
understanding of the effects of structural topological properties (Fig.1). A glaring devices, provides ample spatial data that
constraints on interactions and behaviour methodological gap is our inability can be analysed in similar ways across
is now possible (Box1). Here we suggest an to combine the quantification of both systems. Advances in materials engineering
integrated set of research questions made topology and volume of structures into to create devices that are capable of
possible through interdisciplinary exchange a single variable. Measuring the volume simultaneously capturing information
coupled with recent technological advances. of structures at different depths15 may transfer and movement patterns will allow
provide information on the amount of for studies on both the form and content of
Collective cognition and efficiency space that can be utilized, but it holds information transfer across different spatial
Work on the impact of architecture on no information on structure topology. scales and in different species.
collective outcomes has already provided

NATURE ECOLOGY & EVOLUTION 1, 0111 (2017) | DOI: 10.1038/s41559-017-0111 | www.nature.com/natecolevol 1



2
0
1
7
M
a
c
m
i
l
l
a
n
P
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
L
i
m
i
t
e
d
,
p
a
r
t
o
f
S
p
r
i
n
g
e
r
N
a
t
u
r
e
.
A
l
l
r
i
g
h
t
s
r
e
s
e
r
v
e
d
.
comment

a b c general principles that describe the impact


of architecture on collective behaviours has
far reaching implications. Most basically,
understanding the effect of structures on
the collective behaviour of social animals
may reveal important fitness consequences.
Furthermore, the scientific understanding
of how building architecture influences
human interactions can be used to scaffold
collaborations that drive innovation. But there
1 cm is a greater opportunity for scientific advances
when considering interdisciplinary research
Figure 1 | An example of the skeletonization of a termite nest (Trinervitermes geminatus). a, A picture of that enables studies across multiple species.
the nest. b, A tomographical slice of the nest. c, The network of tunnels in the nest in which edges are Lessons from biology may help formalize the
coloured according to their betweenness centrality value. The technique used to extract this network is quantification of spaces and thus advance
based on ref. 12. studies of current, historical and pre-historical
human architectural features. Furthermore,
interdisciplinary exchange has the capacity
Studies on the location of spaces that are spatial layouts and physical artefacts can to enhance innovations that result from
used for communication in social insects influence collective information processing. teamwork by learning from biological
(for example, the dancing floor in honeybee By manipulating signals and/or the physical systems that have been selected by millions
hives) may inform the positioning of features, environment to examine their impact on the of years of evolution. By initiating a cross-
such as shared break rooms, which are collective actions of social animals, we can disciplinary conversation we hope to inspire
informal spaces that facilitate the spread of develop new, biologically inspired, means to further research on the relationship between
tacit knowledge, in human buildings. Studies improve information processing in humans. architecture and collective behaviour. 
of animal behaviour are highly amenable
to experimental manipulations that are not Structure and behaviour feedback Noa Pinter-Wollman is in the Department of
feasible with human societies. Thus, testing Structures are not formed in a vacuum, there Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of
and manipulating social structures and group is intricate feedback between the architecture California, Los Angeles, USA. Stephen M.Fiore
interactions in social animals, may provide of a structure, its residents, and the external is in the Philosophy Department and the
insights on the causative relationships environment. Animals constantly renovate Institute for Simulation & Training, University of
between certain architectural features and their structures, for example, to alter gas Central Florida, Orlando, Florida 32826, USA.
collective outcomes that can then be used to exchange dynamics10 and respond to changes GuyTheraulaz is at the Centre de Recherches sur la
improve the efficiency of human societies. in colony size11. Thus, there is constant Cognition Animale, Centre de Biologie Intgrative,
feedback between a structure and the needs UMR-CNRS 5169, Universit Paul Sabatier,
Information processing of the individuals that reside in it. Engineers 31062Toulouse Cedex 9, France.
When group members interact to produce and architects of human structures use the e-mail: nmpinter@ucla.edu; sfiore@ist.ucf.edu;
collective outcomes there is information physical attributes and age of construction guy.theraulaz@univ-tlse3.fr
exchange and processing. Physical structures materials and the amount of people that
can serve an important functional role occupy a building to inform renovation plans. References
1. Hansell, M.H. Built by Animals: The Natural History of Animal
in this information processing. First, We argue that the social activities taking place Architecture (Oxford Univ. Press, 2007).
structures can help manage the volume in these structures may also shape them and 2. Gordon, D.M. Ant Encounters: Interaction Networks and Colony
and diffusion of information. Larger spaces should be considered when designing human Behavior (Princeton Univ. Press, 2010).
3. Fiore, S.M. & Salas, E. in Team Cognition: Understanding the
afford greater amounts of information to structures. Currently, we know very little Factors that Drive Process and Performance (eds Salas, E. &
be transmitted. However, being exposed to about how the functional needs of a group Fiore,S.M.) 235248 (American Psychological Association, 2004).
too much information simultaneously can influence their built structures and changes 4. Pinter-Wollman, N. Biol. Lett. 11, 20150695 (2015).
5. Burd, M., Shiwakoti, N., Sarvi, M. & Rose, G. Ecol. Entomol.
be overwhelming and cause individuals to them over time. Testing the bi-directional 35,464468 (2010).
to shut down and reduce interactions relationship between built structures and 6. Kabo, F.W., Cotton-Nessler, N., Hwang, Y.H., Levenstein, M.C.
with their surroundings9. Thus, structures collective behaviours in social animals can & Owen-Smith, J. Res. Pol. 43, 14691485 (2014).
7. Jaubert, J. etal. Nature 534, 111114 (2016).
that segregate individuals (for example, be as simple as examining the architecture 8. Helbing, D., Buzna, L., Johansson, A. & Werner, T. Transport Sci.
chambers and tunnels), may reduce stimuli of structures animals build when subjected 39, 124 (2005).
and help filter unnecessary information. to different environmental constraints 9. Klingberg, T. The Overflowing Brain: Information Overload and
the Limits of Working Memory (Oxford Univ. Press, 2009).
Second, certain spatial cues, such as the to facilitate the most effective collective 10. Korb, J. Naturwissenschaften 90, 212219 (2003).
chemical trails of social insects and road outcomes. Research on social animals can 11. Gautrais, J., Buhl, J., Valverde, S., Kuntz, P. & Theraulaz, G.
signs in human transportation networks, rapidly explore a wide array of environments PLoSONE 9, e109436 (2014).
12. Perez-Reche, F.J. etal. Phys. Rev. Lett. 109, 098102 (2012).
can aid in the interpretation and use of and outcomes, thus expediting our 13. Perna, A. etal. Naturwissenschaften 95, 877884 (2008).
information. Such cues in the environment improvement of human architecturaldesigns. 14. Hillier, B. & Hanson, J. The Social Logic of Space (Cambridge Univ.
can help avoid information overload by Press, 1984).
externalizing knowledge that can then be Conclusions 15. Tschinkel, W.R. Ecol. Entomol. 24, 222237 (1999).

used as a scaffold for further knowledge Interdisciplinary work on architecture and Acknowledgements
building. Despite the economic implications collective behaviour may uncover new We thank the National Academies Keck Futures Initiative
of such structures (furniture, signs, obstacles biomimicry concepts that will create synergies for funding.
etc.) on work productivity in humans, there among biologists, social scientists, physicists, Competing interests
has been little quantitative research on how engineers, and architects. Uncovering The authors declare no competing financial interests.

2 NATURE ECOLOGY & EVOLUTION 1, 0111 (2017) | DOI: 10.1038/s41559-017-0111 | www.nature.com/natecolevol



2
0
1
7
M
a
c
m
i
l
l
a
n
P
u
b
l
i
s
h
e
r
s
L
i
m
i
t
e
d
,
p
a
r
t
o
f
S
p
r
i
n
g
e
r
N
a
t
u
r
e
.
A
l
l
r
i
g
h
t
s
r
e
s
e
r
v
e
d
.

Вам также может понравиться