Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 90

1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.

com

contents

volume 1 no 3 july 1998

http://neurosci.nature.com

Timm staining of the hippocam-


pus, showing zinc localization. editorial
On page 185, Zheng et al.
provide evidence that Src mod-
Making sense of channel diversity. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 169
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

ulates NMDA receptor function


by reducing its inhibition by
zinc. Photo courtesy of Dr.
Jacqueline McGinty, Dept. of
news and views
Anatomy, East Carolina With color in mind . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 171
University.
Charles Heywood and Alan Cowey SEE ARTICLE, PAGE 235

Zinc, Src and NMDA receptorsa transmembrane connection . . . . . . . . . . . . . 173


Philippe Ascher SEE ARTICLE, PAGE 185

Probing a complex question: when are SNARE


proteins ensnared? . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 175
Timothy A Ryan SEE ARTICLE, PAGE 192

Getting a line on pain: is it mediated by


dedicated pathways? . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 177
Edward R Perl SEE ARTICLE, PAGE 218

Cortical control of the thalamus: top-down


processing and plasticity. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 179
Artists prefer light from Josef P Rauschecker SEE ARTICLE, PAGE 226
the upper left.
Page 183

book review
Brain, Vision and Memory: Tales of the History
of Neuroscience . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 181
by C G Gross
REVIEWED BY D PURVES

Structure/function correlations
scientific correspondence
in neurons mediating pain. Where is the sun? . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 183
Pages 177 and 218 J Sun and P Perona

Nature Neuroscience (ISSN 1097-6256) is published monthly by Nature America Inc., headquartered at 345 Park Avenue South, New York, NY 10010-1707. Editorial Office: 345 Park
Avenue South, New York, NY 10010. Telephone 212 726 9200, Fax (212) 696 9635. North American Advertising: Nature Neuroscience, 345 Park Avenue South, New York, NY 10010-
1707. Telephone (212) 726-9200. Fax (212) 696-9006. European Advertising: Nature Neuroscience, Porters South, Crinan Street, London N1 9SQ. Telephone (0171) 833 4000. Fax
(0171) 843 4596. New subscriptions, renewals, changes of address, back issues, and all customer service questions in North America should be addressed to Nature Neuroscience
Subscription Department, PO Box 5054, Brentwood, TN 37024-5054. Telephone (800) 524-0328, Direct Dial (615) 377 3322, Fax (615) 377 0525. Outside North America: Nature
Neuroscience, Macmillan Magazines Ltd, Brunel Road, Basingstoke, Hants RG212XS, U.K. Annual subscription rates: U.S./Canada: U.S. $595, Canada add 7% for GST (institution-
al/corporate), U.S. $195, Canada add 7% for GST (individual making personal payment BN: 14091 1595 RT); U.K./Europe:395 (institutional/corporate), 175 (individual making
personal payment); Rest of world (excluding Japan): 450 (institutional/corporate), 195 (individual making personal payment); Japan: Contact Japan Publications Trading Co. Ltd.,
2-1 Sarugaku-cho 1 chome, Chiyoda-ku, Tokyo 101, Japan, phone (03) 292-3755. Back issues: U.S./Canada, $45, Canada add 7% for GST; Rest of world: surface U.S. $43, air mail U.S.
$45. Reprints: Nature Neuroscience Reprints Department, 345 Park Avenue South, New York, NY 10010-1707. Subscription information is available at the Nature Neuroscience home-
page at http://neurosci.nature.com. POSTMASTER: Send address changes to Nature Neuroscience Subscription Department, P.O. Box 5054, Brentwood, TN 37024-5054. Executive Offi-
cers of Nature America Inc: Nicholas Byam Shaw, Chairman of the Board; Mary Waltham, President; Edward Valis, Secretary-Treasurer. Printed by Publishers Press, Shepherdsville, KY,
USA. Copyright 1998 Nature America Inc.

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 i


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

contents

***
*** *
articles
*
Tyrosine kinase potentiates NMDA receptor currents
* by reducing tonic zinc inhibition . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 185
V1
V2 VP V4V
V8 F Zheng, M B Gingrich, S F Traynelis and P J Conn SEE NEWS AND VIEWS, PAGE 173

Multiple kinetic components of exocytosis distinguished


by neurotoxin sensitivity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 192
T Xu, T Binz, H Niemann and E Neher SEE NEWS AND VIEWS, PAGE 175
Mapping a new human
color center.
Pages 171 and 235 Presynaptic modulation of CA3 network activity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 201
K J Staley, M Longacher, J S Bains and A Yee

Input synchrony and the irregular firing of cortical neurons . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 210


100 C F Stevens and A M Zador
75
50 Nociceptive and thermoreceptive lamina I neurons
25 are anatomically distinct . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 218
Z-S Han, E-T Zhang and A D Craig SEE NEWS AND VIEWS, PAGE 177
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

0
25
50 Cortically induced thalamic plasticity in the
75 primate somatosensory system. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 226
100 E R Ergenzinger, M M Glasier, J O Hahm and T P Pons SEE NEWS AND VIEWS, PAGE 179
Motion perception in
cortical color blindness. Strengthening of horizontal cortical connections
Page 242 following skill learning . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 230
M-S Rioult-Pedotti, D Friedman, G Hess and J P Donoghue

Retinotopy and color sensitivity in human visual


cortical area V8 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 235
N Hadjikhani, A K Liu, A M Dale, P Cavanagh
and R B H Tootell SEE NEWS AND VIEWS, PAGE 171

Complete sparing of high-contrast color input to


motion perception in cortical color blindness . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 242
P Cavanagh, M-A Hnaff, F Michel, T Landis,
T Troscianko and J Intriligator

The effects of frontal eye field and dorsomedial


frontal cortex lesions on visually guided eye movements. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 248
P H Schiller and I Chou

Top-down influences on stereoscopic depth-perception . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 254


I Blthoff, H Blthoff and P Sinha

Do depth cues contribute


to figure recognition? classified advertising
Page 254
see back pages

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 ii


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

editorial

Making sense of channel diversity


When the eminent British geneticist J.B.S. Haldane was asked what berg; Steve Heinemann, Salk Institute). Robert Reenen (University of
God had revealed about himself through his works, Haldane is said Connecticut) has now found that the Drosophila Na+ channel encod-
to have replied an inordinate fondness for beetles. Were he alive ed by the paralytic gene is also edited at several different sites, and
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

today, Haldane might instead have cited ion channels; although their that like the glutamate receptors, para editing is under tight devel-
diversity may no longer be absorbing the creative energies of the opmental regulation. Why RNA editing has been exploited by the
Almighty, they did at least attract several hundred people to a three- nervous system in this way, and in such diverse species, is still unclear;
day meeting in New York last month. The event was organized by one possibility is that it may allow the expression of two almost iden-
the New York Academy of Sciences, and it provided an excellent tical sequences without the risk of gene conversion.
overview of recent progress in understanding the molecular basis of The greatest source of diversity, however, arises from the fact that
ionic conductances, including ionotropic receptors as well as volt- most channels and receptors are composed of multiple subunits,
age-gated and other channels. Although the advances have been which can be assembled in different combinations. In many cases,
impressive, it was also clear that the field faces a formidable chal- a given channel can show profoundly different behavior depending
lenge in making sense of what has already been discovered. on which modulatory subunits are present. Many examples were
An unprecedentedly comprehensive picture of channel and presented, including K+ and Ca2+ channels as well as all the major
receptor diversity is now emerging from large-scale genome sequenc- classes of ionotropic receptors; to cite just one, Terry Snutch (Van-
ing projects, notably of the nematode worm Caenorhabditis elegans couver) showed how P- and Q-type calcium currents, long thought
(the sequence of which is now around 80% complete). As discussed to be distinct (both are voltage-gated but they differ in their ability to
by Larry Salkoff (St Louis), the worm sequencing project has already undergo spontaneous inactivation), can both arise from the same
led to the identification of large numbers of new channel and recep- pore-encoding 1a subunit. Moreover, this difference can be caused
tor subunits. In particular, at least 80 potassium channel subunits not only by differential association with regulatory subunits, but
have been found, a remarkable number considering that the ner- also by alternative splicing of the 1a subunit itself. The properties
vous system of C. elegans contains only 302 neurons, which have of cloned channels must generally be studied in heterologous expres-
been classified into 118 types. About 50 of these genes belong to a sion systems, but just because a particular subunit combination can
new class, distinguished both by their four transmembrane domains form in vitro does not necessarily mean that it occurs in the brain.
and by the lack of knowledge about their function (the best guess is The process of determining which subunits associate with which
that they are leak channels that regulate cell excitability). By tagging others in vivo is long and laborious, yet essential if the lessons from
the coding sequences with green fluorescent protein, it is possible recombinant channels in vitro are to be extended to real neurons.
to visualize their expression patterns; many of the subunits are The number of channels and receptors that can be encoded by
restricted to single cell types, and at least one is expressed only in a the genome is thus enormous, and although this may be good news
single interneuron. If this can be extrapolated to the mammalian for pharmaceutical companies, it presents a daunting prospect for
brain (which is not yet clear), not only does this imply a very large any attempt to understand the underlying principles of brain orga-
number of channels, but some may be so restricted in expression nization. The challenge, of course, is to determine what this prodi-
that they are unlikely to be discovered except by genomic sequencing. gious molecular diversity might signify in functional terms. The easy
The mammalian sequencing projects are far less advanced, the avail- answer is that the brain is very complex and that it needs a corre-
able evidence suggests that any given channel family will contain spondingly vast number of molecules to perform its diverse func-
many more members in mammals than in worms. For example, C. tions; but although this may be true, it is hardly satisfying.
elegans has a single voltage-gated K+ channel of the Shaker class, It is possible, of course, that not all the observed diversity has any
whereas at least eight have already been identified in humans. adaptive significance. Gould and Lewontin have warned against the
In addition to the large number of genes encoding channel and uncritical acceptance of adaptive explanations in biology, and it is
receptor subunits, there are several other levels at which diversity at least possible that some of the diversity that has arisen among dif-
can arise. One is alternative splicing; some of the new K+ channel ferent families of channels and receptors has no purpose genes
genes discovered in C. elegans, for instance, can give rise to six or may duplicate and diverge in evolution simply because they can, in
seven different isoforms. Another is RNA editing, a remarkable other words because there is no selective disadvantage to doing so
process by which single base changes (and hence changes in the and because the process is not easily reversed once it has occurred. To
encoded protein) can be introduced into an already-transcribed take one simple scenario, imagine that a gene encoding a particular
mRNA. Editing has been described in the mammalian brain for both channel undergoes duplication, and that the two sequences drift
AMPA- and kainate-type glutamate receptors, where it is known to apart and acquire differences in their promoters; although they may
regulate ion selectivity and channel kinetics (Rolf Sprengel, Heidel- at first be mutually redundant, if certain populations of neurons lose

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 169


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

editorial

the ability to express one or the other gene, they both become essen- visual cortex show different patterns of AMPA receptor expression,
tial even if they differ very little in their functional properties. and she hopes to determine how specific patterns of channel and
One way to address the question of adaptive significance is to receptor expression can be related to cortical information processing.
ask whether individual members of gene families show conserva- Ultimately, it seems clear that to understand how channels and
tion in evolution. Broadly, the answer so far seems to be that they receptors determine neuronal behavior, the field will have to go
do. For instance, Salkoff noted that the major classes of potassium beyond the level of molecular description and adopt a more quan-
channels that exist in humans all have recognizable homologs in titative and biophysical approach. This is perhaps the greatest chal-
C. elegans, suggesting that the evolution of more complex nervous lenge for the years ahead. To explain the electrical properties of a
systems has not been accompanied by the appearance of new types neuron, it is not sufficient merely to specify the types of channels it
of channels, but rather by diversification of pre-existing types. expresses; one must also know their densities and distributions, as
Moreover, even the individual family members often show high they relate to the fine structure and cable properties of axons and
conservation; the human Slo1 gene (which encodes a high-con- dendrites. Such an analysis would seem essential for any serious
ductance calcium-gated K+ channel), for example, is much closer attempt at understanding channel function at the cellular level, but
to nematode Slo1 than to human (or nematode) Slo2, and the surprisingly the question hardly came up in the meeting.
same principle holds for other classes of K channels. Perhaps the Consider, for instance, how a neuron might achieve the appro-
most striking demonstration of functional conservation was the priate number and distribution of each of the channels it express-
worm homolog of one of the Long QT-type K+ channels, muta- es. To obtain the desired pattern of excitability, there must
tions of which lead to abnormal heart rhythms in humans. The presumably be some form of feedback from activity to channel
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

worm KQT homolog is expressed in the pharynx, which like the expression. Yet how this might occur is almost entirely mysterious.
heart generates a rhythmic pumping action. Moreover, when the To what extent is channel density regulated by activity, and if so by
human mutation is introduced into the worm coding sequence, what feedback pathways? At what level is control exerted? It could
the mutant animals show a defect analogous to the human condi- be transcription, or at post-transcriptional levels such as protein
tion, a long pharyngeal pump syndrome, as it were. Examples like synthesis, degradation, trafficking or association with modulatory
this offer hope that comparing model systems will reveal some proteins. In muscle fibers, the distribution and turnover times for
general principles of how different patterns of channel expression different types of acetylcholine receptors are regulated with great
determine the properties of different classes of neurons. precision, both during development and in response to changing
The current favorite method for determining the function of a patterns of electrical activity, but whether this is also true for neu-
channel is to knock it out genetically, but it was clear from many of ronal receptors and ion channels is still very unclear.
the presentations that this approach has serious limitations. Although Not only the number but also the precise localization of different
gene knockouts avoid the problems associated with lack of speci- molecules must in some cases be specified. Efforts to understand
ficity in pharmacological blocking agents, they raise interpretation- this were exemplified by the presentation from Ole Ottersen (Oslo),
al problems of their own. Often, mutant phenotypes are either who has used immunogold labeling to study the fine structure of
nonexistent or too subtle to be recognized using the available tech- cerebellar Purkinje cells. He has shown that different molecules are
niques. Even in cases where a mutant phenotype is found, it is often targeted to different sites; the 2 glutamate-like receptor, for instance,
difficult to rule out the possibility that the absence of the gene dur- is present at the postsynaptic sites formed with parallel fibers but
ing development has led to compensatory changes that complicate not with climbing fibers. Another molecule that is precisely local-
the interpretation of the result. The ideal gene-knockout method ized in these cells is the glutamate transporter EAAT4, which is
would be cell-type specific and under tight temporal control. But known to play a role in clearing glutamate and shortening the EPSC.
despite several apparently encouraging reports in the literature, such How it does so is unclear; Ottersen has shown that the main site of
techniques are far from robust, as Peter Seeberg (Heidelberg) empha- EAAT4 expression is at the base of dendritic spines, close to the site
sized. At present, the technology does not exist to inactivate specif- of contact with glial cells and several microns away from the post-
ic genes in specific parts of the mammalian nervous system with synaptic membrane where ionotropic glutamate receptors are con-
high efficiency and specificity, let alone in a rapidly inducible or centrated. How EAAT4 can affect synaptic activation given its
reversible manner that would eliminate concerns about develop- exclusion from the site of transduction remains to be determined,
mental compensation. A reliable method for doing this would be but the results highlight the importance of precise molecular descrip-
invaluable, but it does not yet seem to be close to realization. tions of synaptic structure if the details of synaptic transmission are
The challenge in understanding ion channel function may be to be understood in quantitative terms.
reduced to two broad questions: what do specific channels contribute The molecular basis of this structural specificity is even less clear,
to the behavior of the cells in which they are expressed, and how but some details are starting to emerge. Morgan Sheng (Massachu-
does the behavior of these cells contribute to the working of the sys- setts General Hospital), Mary Kennedy (Caltech) Heinrich Betz
tem as a whole? Although certain mutations have given interpretable (Frankfurt) and Nat Heintz (Rockefeller University) each discussed
and interesting phenotypes, there are major obstacles to be over- molecular components of postsynaptic sites, and have identified
come before this can be achieved on a routine basis. For one thing, various molecules that may govern how receptors and channels
the sheer effort of descriptive analysis will be considerable. It will be become localized. An important goal now is to determine how these
essential to correlate ion channel expression with single-cell prop- various components interact, and how the appropriate density and
erties, and this is far from trivial in vivo. In situ hybridization or anti- distribution of synaptic signaling components is achieved and main-
body staining can provide information about expression patterns, tained. In the longer term, it will also be important to find out
but it is also necessary to correlate this with cellular physiology. One whether similar mechanisms regulate channel distribution elsewhere
powerful approach, discussed by Hannah Monyer (Heidelberg), is on the membrane, and thus whether they play a more general role in
to record from single neurons via a whole-cell patch pipette and then regulating neuronal excitability.
to aspirate the contents of the cell into the pipette, so that mRNA
New York Academy of Sciences conference: Molecular and functional diversity of ion chan-
expression can be analyzed by PCR. Monyer has used this technique nels and receptors. New York, May 14-17, 1998. See http://www.nyas.org/brochion.html for
to show that principal neurons and interneurons in the primary program details.

170 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

news and views

With color in mind


Charles Heywood and Alan Cowey

Which human brain area produces color blindness when damaged? High-resolution
functional neuroimaging suggests that it is area V8, not the favorite candidate V4.

Dorothys whirlwind departure from a proposed chiefly on the a


A
monochrome Kansas into the vividly chro- (not uncontested) view that
matic world of Oz, in the 1939 classic film V4 contains a comparative-
The Wizard of Oz, highlights the substan- ly high proportion of cells
tial contribution that color makes to our that respond selectively to
visual world. Yet equally revealing is the wavelength and color5. The
Calcarine
ease with which we view monochrome film results of a functional imag- sulcus
or television, where the absence of color ing study by Hadjikhani
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

does not compromise our enjoyment and and colleagues, reported in


can pass unnoticed. This is not so in the this issue of Nature Neuro- Striate
clinical condition of cerebral achromatop- science (pp 235241), sug- cortex
Lingual Fusiform Collateral
sia where patients, following characteristi- gest however that the gyrus gyrus sulcus
cally ventral occipitotemporal brain human color center is dis-
damage (see Fig. 1a) inhabit a drab world, tinct from area V4. The
devoid of color, and may be painfully aware newlydefined color area b
B Lunate
sulcus
of their complete loss of chromatic vision1. contains a complete retino-
Attempts at understanding the nature of topic map of the contralat-
cerebral achromatopsia and its neural basis eral visual half field, V4
have spawned controversy ever since Louis responds more robustly to
Verreys description2 of such a case in 1888 color than neighboring
(see ref. 3 for review). The ensuing debate regions and, unlike V4, is
lasted for more than a century, and not sur- activated by the induction
prisingly the protagonists reflected oppos- of color aftereffects. The Superior
temporal
ing views about whether any cognitive or fMRI signal elicited by an sulcus
perceptual function was regionally local- aftereffect thus mimics the
ized. At issue was whether achromatopsia response to a real colored Inferior V4 TEO IT
occipital
results from the deletion of a cortical stimulus, providing sup- sulcus
Bob Crimi
region specialized for the processing of porting evidence that V8 is
color. The demonstration4, using positron implicated in processes Fig. 1. Visual areas in the human and monkey brains. (a)
emission tomography, of increased cere- involved in perceiving color. The medial view of the left hemisphere of the human brain.
bral blood flow in an area of cortex when These properties, the Striate cortex, area V1, is shown in red, partly buried in the
observers view chromatic scenes was cer- authors suggest, make it a calcarine sulcus. The region along the collateral sulcus,
tainly consistent with this notion, because ready candidate for a region whose destruction leads to cerebral achromatopsia, is
the activated region, dubbed the human responsible for our con- shown in blue. Area V8 lies in the middle of the collateral
color center, is invariably damaged in cases scious perception of a col- sulcus, whereas V4 lies slightly more posterior and medial.
The damage indicated by blue would therefore include V4v
of cortical color blindness. By then, how- ored world.
and V8. (b) A lateral schematic view of the right hemisphere
ever, the cluster of 2030 visual areas occu- Hadjikhani and col-
of the macaque monkey, in which the labeled sulci have been
pying almost half of the neocortex of leagues used existing, but opened up. Areas V1 (red) and V4 (purple) extend onto the
monkeys had been identified, and the improved, techniques of ventral and medial surfaces, respectively. Areas in the tem-
debate turned to whether the human color functional neuroimaging poral lobe (TEO, orange and IT, dark green), have been
center was homologous to the fourth visu- to reveal, with increased implicated in color vision of the macaque monkey, and
al area of the monkey, cortical area V4 (see senstivity, brain areas Hadjikhani and colleagues raise the possibility that TEO may
Fig. 1b). This correspondence had been involved in the processing correspond to human V8.
of color. Functional mag-
Charles Heywood is the Sir Derman netic resonance imaging
Christopherson Research Fellow at the (fMRI) relies on endoge-
Department of Psychology, Science Laboratories, nous changes in magnetic susceptibility, the human brain6. These areas are the
South Road, Durham DH1 3LE, UK which result from changes in local cere- presumed homologues of those identi-
(C.A.Heywood@Durham.ac.uk) bral blood flow and oxygenation. Such fied in the monkey brain using a variety
Alan Cowey is at the Department of changes are activity dependent, and their of invasive techniques, such as cellular
Experimental Psychology, South Parks Road, measurement in response to visually pre- recording and experimental neuroanato-
Oxford OX1 3UD, UK sented stimuli have already established my. Delineation of an area relies on the
(alan.cowey@psy.ox.ac.uk) the borders of a number of visual areas in presence of an orderly retinotopic map

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 171


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

news and views

of the visual world, which is a feature of severe deficits in the discrimination of red/green borders without signaling the
many visual areas. The authors have hue that is the hallmark of cerebral nature of the colors of which the border
exploited the fMRI technique to identify achromatopsia 7 . Conversely, impair- is composed, i.e. which is red and which is
such maps, using stimuli consisting of ments in the discrimination of visually green. Form can thus be derived from the
slow-moving patterns of luminance mod- present form and pattern vision that processing of wavelength differences and
ulation. As the light and dark areas pass invariably follow damage to V4 in the yield information about the visual scene
across the visual field, they elicit period- monkey are not an invariable feature of without encoding its chromatic content.
ic excitation at the associated cortical the vision of achromatopsic people. The It should perhaps come as no surprise
location. Moreover, the phase of the location of V8 within the region that achromatopsic patients, lacking area
response specifies the polar angle or destroyed in achromatopsic patients, V8, can show a preserved capacity to use
eccentricity, for rotation or radial move- although the latter is always more exten- wavelength variation to detect motion
ment around the fixation point respec- sive and includes white matter damage, and form12, presumably mediated by ear-
tively, of the visual field region is strong but not conclusive evidence that lier and intact extrastriate areas. Detec-
represented at that location. Thus a V8 is the critical area whose removal can tion of equiluminant chromatic form can
Fourier analysis on the response profile result in the complete loss of the con- even be achieved when it is disguised by
of a single voxel of the image, along with scious representation of color. As Had- accompanying rapid random luminance
a consideration of the sign of the response jikhani and colleagues themselves point variation. The latter implies that patients
to identify mirror- versus non-mirror- out, the question naturally arises as to must retain color-opponent processing
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

image representations, will yield the where area V8 is concealed in the mediated by the well known P-channel of
retinotopy of a visual area, which can dis- macaque monkeys brain. Localization of primate vision, which unlike its partner
played, by cortical flattening, as a two- macaque V8 would lead to confirmato- the M-channel is blind to the introduc-
dimensional map. Using such techniques, ry evidence that its removal results in tion of rapid flicker. Moreover, another
Hadjikhani and colleagues compared the cortical color blindness. They speculate paper in this issue 13 reports that pro-
effect of luminance-defined visual pat- that area TEO8,9 (Fig. 1b), a region lying foundly achromatopsic patients lacking
terns with that of identical patterns that anterior to V4, may be the culprit. evidence for residual color-opponent
were defined by equiluminant color vari- Indeed this region, along with more processes are still able to extract motion
ation, that is, variations in color but not anterior temporal lobe areas (Fig. 1b), from high-contrast color cues.
in luminance. In addition to finding has been implicated in color vision (Van- Achromatopsic patients can therefore
stronger activation to color than to lumi- duffel, W. et al., Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 23, process wavelength differences to extract
nance in cortical areas V1, V2, V3/VP and 334.7, 1997; Katsuyama, N. et al., Soc. information about form and motion,
the ventral subdivision of V4 (V4v), a Neurosci. Abstr. 23, 803.11, 1997). Large but their brain damage nevertheless ren-
region in the middle of the collateral sul- lesions to anterior and inferior portions ders them blind to color differences.
cus was identified that responded prefer- of the temporal lobe do render monkeys Such brain damage is, of course, likely
entially and especially effectively to color. achromatopsic10, and a high proportion to encroach on territory other than area
Its location corresponded to that previ- of color-selective cells, revealed by meta- V8, including the adjacent V4v.
ously (and in the absence of identifica- bolic labeling and electrophysiological Although loss of conscious representa-
tion of retinotopic boundaries, recording, reside in this area11. tion of hue characterises achromatop-
prematurely) described as human V4. By Although the precise role of area V8 sia, another very different explanation
adopting improved techniques, including has yet to be clarified, determining what has been offered3, namely that it is a fail-
a high-field scanner, signal averaging and is spared, as opposed to lost, in cerebral ure of color constancya loss of the
improved visual displays, the authors achromatopsia may provide some sign- invariance of an objects perceived color
established that the true color center lies posts. Color makes a ubiquitous contri- despite wide variation in the wavelength
beyond the anterior border of the previ- bution to vision. Color differences can, composition of the illuminating light.
ously reported area V4v (i.e. outside v4 among other things, provide information Although it has been suggested that the
altogether). Furthermore the retinotopy about form, texture and motion. This is chromatic responses of neurons in area
of the color center differs from its neigh- presumably reflected in the number, and V4 show color constancy14, the effects
bors. Areas V4v, VP and inferior V2 con- wide variation, of activated areas reported of ablating this in the monkey have yet
tain quarter-field representations of the in neuroimaging studies when subjects to convincingly demonstrate a corre-
upper visual field and share a contiguous perform a wide variety of color-related sponding deficit. Might V8 be assigned
representation of the fovea. In contrast, tasks. Cerebral blood flow can be modu- such a role? An explanation of cerebral
the color area contains a map of the lated by the nature and difficulty of the achromatopsia as resulting from the
upper and lower half-fields with a foveal behavioral tasks (whether they entail pas- destruction of V8 with a concomitant
representation located at its anterior bor- sive viewing, active discrimination or deficit in color constancy does not read-
der. It now seems clear that the color cen- directed attention) and the properties of ily explain why two very different equi-
ter is distinct from area V4, and the visual display (containing equilumi- luminant hues are indistinguishable to
accordingly, the authors refer to this new, nant color, with or without form, with or an achromatopsic patient, nor why the
previously unreported region as area V8. without associated brightness differences). world should be described in shades of
Area V8 poses as many new questions The physical basis of color is the wave- gray. A direct test of color constancy in
as its identification sought to resolve. length composition of light. Many corti- one such patient does not lend unequiv-
That it is distinct from area V4 is cer- cal areas prior to V8 contain cells that are ocal support for this view15. When two
tainly consistent with hitherto puzzling sensitive to, but not selective for, wave- patches of different spectral composi-
demonstrations that ablation of V4 in the length differences. For example, cells may tion were presented against two differ-
macaque monkey does not result in the respond vigorously to equiluminant ent backgounds, one to each eye, the

172 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

news and views

patient reported them to be indistin-


guishable only when the ratios of the
retinal cone response of path/back-
Zinc, Src and NMDA
ground were preserved. When the ratios
were different for each eye, the patches receptorsa transmembrane
no longer appeared identical. These
responses are akin to those of the nor-
mal observer and are presumed to be
connection
mediated by retinal mechanisms. How- Philippe Ascher
ever, the achromatopsic patient departed
from normal performance when pre- Zinc and tyrosine kinases produce opposite effects on the
sented with more complex scenes requir- NMDA receptor; new evidence suggests that Src-induced
ing multiple cone contrast comparisons. potentiation is due to the relief of zinc inhibition.
Achromatopsic patients may therefore
retain rudimentary color constancy
mechanisms, and it remains an open Recently, scientists have become increas- but in some recombinant NMDA recep-
question as to whether V8 is essential for ingly aware of the potent actions of zinc tors (those assembled from NR1 and
consolidation of information across and tyrosine kinases on NMDA receptors. NR2A subunits) the IC50 of the voltage-
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

large regions of complex scenes. NMDA receptors are a group of ionotrop- independent inhibition is exceptionally
However, what these patients do lack ic glutamate receptors that are permeable low (10 nM-100nM) 4-6. This is the range
is the conscious representation of color. If to both calcium and sodium, and have of concentration at which zinc is present
it transpires that a single cortical area, been implicated in many forms of synap- as a contaminant in most experimental
area V8, is indispensable for our con- tic transmission, synaptic plasticity and in solutions, but also in the cerebrospinal
scious percept of color, it will indeed be a cell death. Zinc had long been known to fluid. Thus, even without adding any
rare triumph for the view that regional interact with many neurotransmitter additional zinc, a large fraction of the
specialization underlies the cluster of receptors, but its inhibition of NMDA high-affinity, voltage-independent,
visual areas that occupy a substantial receptors has attracted particular interest inhibitory sites on NR1-NR2A receptors
proportion of the neocortex in primates. because of a possible functional role. Zinc are already occupied, so that the addition
On a note of caution, however, Had- is stored in synaptic vesicles in a number of a zinc-chelating agent to the bath is
jikhani and colleagues hint that area V8 of glutamatergic terminals of the fore- usually sufficient to double the amplitude
responds to a wide variety of visual stim- brain, is released during synaptic activity, of the baseline response4.
uli. The challenge will then be to estab- and its concentration in the synaptic cleft The interaction of tyrosine kinases
lish the precise role of this, and other has been suggested to rise to the micro- with NMDA receptors a priori seemed to
areas, in the cortical processing of color. molar range1. Early studies of the interac- have little relation with that of zinc. The
Hadjikhani and colleagues have pointed tion of zinc with NMDA receptors 2,3 early observations of Salter and col-
us in the correct direction. identified two effects, a voltage-dependent leagues 7,8 showed that tyrosine kinase
inhibition which resembles that of mag- inhibitors inhibit some NMDA respons-
1. Cowey, A. & Heywood, C.A. Trends Cog.
Neurosci. 1, 133139 (1997).
nesium, and a voltage-independent inhi- es, which conversely can be potentiated
bition, which occurs at a different site. The by intracellular injection of either a con-
2. Verrey, L. Archs. Ophtalmol. (Paris) 8, 289301
(1888). two effects were initially described for zinc stitutively-active form of the intracellular
3. Zeki, S.A Vision of the Brain. (Blackwell concentrations in the micromolar range, tyrosine kinase Src or peptide fragments
Scientific Publications: Oxford Univ. Press, and this fitted nicely with estimates of zinc that activate Src 8. Similar effects were
1993). concentration in the synaptic cleft fol- observed with another tyrosine kinase,
4. Zeki S. et al. J.Neurosci. 11, 641649 (1991). lowing glutamate release. Recently, after fyn9. These observations were reinforced
5. Zeki, S. Nature 284, 412418 (1980). the cloning of NMDA receptors subunits by reports that tyrosine kinase inhibitors
6. Sereno, M.I. et al. Science 268, 998893 allowed the expression of recombinant interfere with some forms of long-term
(1995). receptors, the two effects of zinc were fur- potentiation (a cellular model for learn-
7. Heywood, C.A., Gadotti, A. & Cowey, A. J. ther analyzed using NMDA receptor sub- ing and memory), and that some tyrosine
Neurosci. 12, 40564065 (1992). types built from specific combinations of kinase deficient mice had perturbed LTP
8. Boussaoud, D., Desimone, R. & Ungerleider, subunits. Using these recombinant recep- and behavioral abnormalities (for refer-
L.G. J. Comp. Neurol. 306, 554575 (1991). tors, additional evidence was obtained to ences see 10). Once again, recombinant
9. Zeki, S. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 263, 15391544 support the hypothesis that the voltage- NMDA receptors were used to reveal a
(1996).
dependent effect of zinc occurs via the subunit specificity: Src only acted on
10. Heywood, C.A., Gaffan, D. & Cowey, A. Eur. J. same site as the voltage-dependent mag- receptors assembled from NR2A subunits
Neurosci. 7, 10641073 (1995).
nesium block within the channel pore, but and a restricted group of splice variants
11. Komatsu, H., Ideura, Y., Kaji, S. & Yamane, S.
J. Neurosci. 12, 408424 (1992). that zinc permeates the channel better4. of NR19.
12. Heywood, C.A., Kentridge, R.W. & Cowey, A.
The binding site involved in the voltage Zheng and colleagues on page 185 of
Exp. Brain Res. (in press). independent block is less well identified, this issue of Nature Neuroscience11 have
13. Cavanagh, P. et al. Nature Neuroscience 1, linked these two sets of apparently inde-
242247 (1998). Philippe Ascher is at the Laboratoire de pendent observations by experiments that
14. Zeki, S. Neuroscience 9, 741765 (1983). Neurobiologie, Ecole Normale Superieure, 46 strongly suggest that the potentiating
15. Hurlbert, A.C., Bramwell, D.I., Heywood, rue dUlm, Paris 75005, France effect of Src on NR1-NR2A receptors
C.A. & Cowey, A. Exp Brain Res. (in press). e-mail: pascher@wotan.ens.fr results from the suppression of the ambi-

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 173


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

news and views

NR1 NR2A NR1 NR2A signaling (see Fig. 1). zinc concentration in experiments in
Previous studies of which glutamate or NMDA are bath
receptor tyrosine applied, and in which the concentration
kinases have popular- of zinc is fixed. None of the available
ized an orthograde chelators, however, will prevent the rise
model, where the of zinc concentration if zinc is released
binding of an extra- in the synaptic cleft in the millisecond
cellular ligand to a range, especially if the local concentra-
receptor transmits a tion in the cleft can reach the micromolar
signal through the range. This is mainly because calcium
Src
membrane to activate also competes with zinc to bind to these
P Src a tyrosine kinase on heavy metal chelators. The problem may
the cytoplasmic side. be resolved by controlling the presynap-
Zinc
The data of Zheng tic zinc content, either with chelators
Gly
Glu
and colleagues sug- having selective access to the synaptic
Magnesium gest that the NMDA vesicles (for example see ref. 13) or from
Bob Crimi
receptor mediates a genetically modified animals in which the
Fig. 1. Left: the NMDA receptor has been activated by the simul- trans-membrane zinc transporters and buffers (and in par-
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

taneous binding of glycine to the NR1 subunit and of glutamate to message in the oppo- ticular the storage of zinc in synaptic vesi-
the NR2A subunit, but the binding of zinc reduces the time spent
site direction; activa- cles14) will have been modified.
in the open state. Right: the interaction of Src with the cytoplas-
tion of a tyrosine Zinc is also strongly suspected to play
mic tail of the NR2 subunit induces a conformational change
which, by preventing the binding of zinc on the extracellular side
kinase on the cyto- a role in various pathologic conditions,
(arrow), potentiates the NMDA response. plasmic side alters the such as neuronal death following global
binding of extracellu- ischemia or massive seizures 15. Zinc is
lar ligands. The ligand known to be cytotoxic, and there is evi-
that is most clearly dence that this toxicity results from its
ent zinc inhibition. Src mimics the effect affected is zinc, which seems to be dis- entry into the cell, even though its intra-
of adding an extracellular zinc chelator, placed by the process (its apparent bind- cellular targets are not rigorously identi-
and the potentiation induced by remov- ing affinity decreases), but the kinetics of fied. The cell death induced by exposure
ing zinc from the extracellular medium glutamate dissociation from the receptor to zinc is increased by glutamate and can
and the potentiation induced by the addi- is also slowed. (Glycine affinity may also be reduced by NMDA antagonists15, sug-
tion of Src occlude each other, suggesting be affected but this has not yet been stud- gesting that a major route of zinc entry
a shared mechanism. Dose response ied.) However, the retrograde signaling into neurons could be through NMDA
curves confirm that the EC50 of the volt- analyzed by Zheng and colleagues does receptors, which appear to be both
age-independent, high-affinity zinc inhi- not exclude a mechanism working in the blocked by and permeable to zinc. The
bition is increased by Src. orthograde direction; if zinc and Src act NR1-NR2A receptors, which contain a
The picture of what exactly is happen- on allosteric sites of the NMDA receptor, high affinity, voltage independent, zinc
ing at the molecular level, however, allosteric theory predicts that if Src alters inhibitory site, could play a particular role
remains fragmentary. From the work of zinc binding, zinc binding can alter Src in the zinc toxicity because they have an
Salters group 9 we know that Src binds to action. additional surprising property, namely
the NMDA receptor through a region that From a physiological point of view, that the zinc inhibition remains incom-
is distinct from the catalytic site, but we the observations suggest that Src and zinc plete even when this site is saturated57.
do not know whether Src directly phos- play opposite roles in synaptic transmis- This means that at zinc concentrations in
phorylates the NMDA receptor or sion. If there are glutamatergic synapses the micromolar range, zinc retains access
whether it acts via a third protein. Zheng in vivo where the postsynaptic NMDA to the channel and therefore to the cell
and colleagues11 have now identified three receptors have the very high zinc sensi- interior. Therefore, the NMDA receptors
specific tyrosines in the NMDA receptor tivity observed in recombinant NR1- most sensitive to zinc may be, paradoxi-
subunit NR2A that appear to be involved NR2A receptors, extracellular ambient cally, the most dangerous route of zinc
in NMDA receptor modulation; it is zinc will set the NMDA response to about entry into neurons. If we consider that Src
tempting to speculate that they are targets half of its maximal value, which would induces a rightward shift of the zinc con-
for Src-mediated phosphorylation, but place the receptors in an optimum posi- centration response curve, we can predict
this has not been directly demonstrated. tion to be regulated bidirectionally. The that Src activation will potentiate the
The explanation of the high zinc senstivi- response could be either increased (by entry of zinc. Thus in pathologic disease
ty of NR1NR2A remains uncertain. The activation of a tyrosine kinase) or inhib- states, Src and zinc may combine their
simple explanation was that either zinc or ited (by zinc released from glutamatergic effects instead of neutralizing each other.
Src would specifically interact with NR2A. terminals). This hypothesis remains Although Zheng and colleagues have
However, two of the tyrosines identified untested, and in particular no study has not directly proven that Src can phos-
by Zheng and colleagues are common to yet investigated the modulation of phorylate NMDA receptors and have not
NR2A and NR2B. Furthermore, recent NMDA synaptic currents by zinc. In such identified the zinc binding site, they have
evidence12 suggests that NR1 is involved a study, one experimental obstacle will be raised a number of intriguing questions
in the zinc modulation. Nevertheless, the difficulty of chelating synaptically about the interaction between zinc bind-
whatever the details of the picture, the released zinc. Zinc chelators like EDTA, ing and tyrosine phosphorylation. More
data strongly suggest a kind of retrograde TPEN or tricine allow one to control the work is needed to determine whether

174 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

news and views

these observations made on recombinant (Lond.) 429, 429449 (1990). 10. Lu, Y.M., Roder, J.C., Davidow, J. & Salter,
M.W. Science 279, 13631368 (1998).
NMDA receptors in vitro will apply to 3. Christine, C.W. & Choi, D.W. J. Neurosci. 10,
real NMDA receptors in vivo (which may 108116 (1990). 11. Zheng. F., Gingrich, M.B., Traynelis, S.F. &
4. Paoletti, P., Ascher, P. & Neyton, J. J.Neurosci. 17, Conn, P.J. Nature Neurosci. 3, 185191
have different combinations of subunits). (1998).
57115725 (1997).
What zinc does in vivo, and why NMDA 12. Traynelis, S.F., Burgess, M.F., Zheng, F.,
receptors should have evolved zinc sensi- 5. Williams, K., Neurosci. Lett. 215, 912 (1996).
Lyuboslavsky, P. & Powers, J.L. J. Neurosci.
tivity at all, still remains a mystery. How- 6. Chen, N., Moshaver, A. & Raymond, L.A. Mol. (in press).
Pharmacol. 51, 10151023 (1997).
ever, Zheng and colleagues have put us 13. Budde, T., Minta, A., White, J.A. & Kay, A.
7. Wang, Y.T. & Salter, M.W. Nature 369, 233235 R. Neuroscience 79, 347358 (1997).
one step closer to understanding the intri- (1994).
cacies of this modulatory mechanism. 14. Wenzel, H.J., Cole, T.B., Born, D.E.,
8. Yu, X.M., Askalan, R., Keil, G.J. & Salter, M.W. Schwartzkroin, P.A. & Palmiter, R.D. Proc.
1. Frederickson, C.J. Intl. Rev. Neurobiol. 131, Science 275, 674678 (1997). Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 4, 1267612681 (1997).
145238 (1989). 9. Khr, G. & Seeburg, P.H. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 492, 15. Choi D.W. & Koh, J.Y Annu. Rev. Neurosci.
2. Legendre, P., & Westbrook, G.L. J. Physiol. 445452 (1996). 21, 347375 (1998).

bly is carried out by the ATPase NSF (N-


Probing a complex question: ethyl-maleimide-sensitive fusion pro-
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

tein) together with SNAPs (soluble

when are SNARE proteins NSF-attachment proteins).


The questions of where, when and
how SNARE assembly occurs within
ensnared? secretory terminals and what specific
state the stable NSF-SNAP-sensitive
Timothy A. Ryan ternary complex corresponds to in the
progression of vesicle traffic are now
A recent study uses elegant microphysiological and central to forming an accurate descrip-
molecular tools to investigate the molecular basis and tion of presynaptic function. Several
kinetics of vesicle exocytosis. recent reports7,8 and a technical tour-de-
force in the current issue of Nature Neu-
roscience 2 have begun probing these
Perhaps one of the most sensitive sub- required to tackle these issues is given questions. The approach of Xu and col-
strates for modulating information flow by Xu and colleagues in this issue of leagues was to apply a combination of
in the brain is the molecular machinery Nature Neuroscience 2 , a study that elegant techniques to precisely stimu-
of the synaptic terminal. A detailed mol- probes the molecular basis of neuro- late and measure catecholamine secre-
ecular understanding of presynaptic transmitter secretion using modern tion from neuroendocrine cells while
function is important because it is the microphysiological and molecular tools. simultaneously interfering with SNARE
target of many therapeutic reagents for Several different approaches to function. To accomplish this, they used
neurological disease, as well as the site understanding membrane transport whole-cell capacitance measurements to
of action of most stimulants and drugs have converged over the last five years, monitor cell-surface area, carbon-fiber
of abuse. The molecular description of revealing a cast of molecular players amperometry to detect catecholamine
postsynaptic events during synaptic that lie at the heart of vesicle trafficking release, and photo-uncaging of chelat-
transmission has advanced rapidly over and presynaptic neurotransmitter ed calcium to deliver step changes in
the last twenty years, thanks to the fruit- release 3,4. Considerable evidence now intracellular calcium, triggering secre-
ful combination of electrophysiology implies that SNARE proteins5, a family tion. The key to these studies is that
and molecular biology 1 . Our under- of compartmentally specific integral synaptobrevin, syntaxin and SNAP-25
standing of molecular events in the membrane proteins with cytoplasmic are all specific substrates for digestion
presynaptic terminal, however, has only tails, provide a core interaction that by various botulinum and tetanus tox-
recently begun to emerge. The charac- determines the specificity of membrane ins, and these proteins are only vulner-
terization of presynaptic processes is dif- pairing. Recently, the assembly of able to these toxins when not assembled
ficult because of the inherent appropriately paired SNAREs has been in the tight ternary complex. Thus a
complexity of the underlying organelle shown to provide the minimal machin- measurement of secretion after intra-
cell biology and because, unlike with ion ery required for the successful mixing cellular toxin application provides an
channels, the study of any single mole- of artificial membrane bilayers 6 . The estimate of the fraction of secretory-
cule reveals little about the system as a best-characterized SNAREs are involved competent vesicles that depend on
whole. An example of the combined in neuronal exocytosis. They include the SNAREs in a toxin-sensitive, or
functional and molecular approach synaptic vesicle associated protein unassembled state.
synaptobrevin, and the plasma mem- Secretory terminals are comprised of
Timothy Ryan is at the Department of brane associated proteins SNAP (synap- vesicles in at least two different states of
Biochemistry, Cornell University Medical tosome-associated protein)-25 and readiness with respect to the speed at
College,1300 York Avenue, syntaxin. In vitro, these three proteins which they can be caused to fuse with
New York, NY 10021, USA spontaneously assemble into a very sta- the plasma membrane. In typical synap-
email: taryan@mail.med.cornell.edu ble ternary complex, whose disassem- tic terminals, there is a distinct subset

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 175


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

news and views

Fig. 1. A model of the role of SNAREs that application of all but one
in mediating membrane fusion in neu-
of the appropriate neurotoxins
ronal exocytosis. Prior to docking with
leads to a complete abolition of
the plasma membrane, SNARE proteins
on the vesicle membrane are subject to
secretion, which is to say that
cleavage by botulinum or tetanus toxin. both the fast and slow phases
SNARE proteins on docked vesicles are of membrane fusion are
coiled tightly in a ternary complex, pro- NSF-ATP?
blocked. Therefore, all of the
NSF-ATP SNAREs relevant for secretion
tecting them from toxin attack.
Following the elevation of intracellular in this assay must have been in
Ca++ a toxin-sensitive state. What
calcium, the two membranes fuse. NSF
acts to dissamble the SNAREs, freeing X X X X about the SNAREs on docked
them and allowing the vesicle to be vesicles? The authors propose
recycled. In this model, the membrane that the docked state be a
binding step is reversible, allowing reversible one, so that during
docked vesicles to cycle continuously Bob Crimi the toxin incubation period, all
through a toxin-sensitive state, without Syntaxin SNAP-25 of the readily releasable vesicles
proceeding to fusion. cycle through a state where the
Synaptobrevin Tetanus or
Botulinum toxin SNAREs are vulnerable to toxin
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

attack (Fig. 1). This preserves


the original idea that release-
of vesicles, representing roughly 10% of intracellular calcium elicit an exocytot- ready vesicles have their SNAREs assem-
the total population, that can be seen in ic burst proceeding very rapidly for bled in a tight complex, but it predicts
very close proximity to the plasma about one second, followed by a pro- that over a five-minute period (the time
membrane, the so-called morphologi- longed slower phase of secretion. In to dialyze in the toxin), this state cycles
cally docked pool. Given the very short both types of secretory systems, it is continuously through an assembly-dis-
time delay between the arrival of a believed that release-ready vesicles are assembly sequence. A second possibili-
presynaptic action potential and the the first to go when a stimulus arrives. ty consistent with these results is
release of neurotransmitter (less than Given the importance of SNAREs in depicted in Fig. 2. Here, the docked
one millisecond), it is believed that vesicleplasma membrane interactions, state would consist of a partial assem-
secretion must draw upon a pool in a what is the arrangement of SNAREs in bly of SNAREs that is primed and ready
release-ready state, probably a subset of the docked or release-ready vesicles? In to proceed promptly to fusion with the
the morphologically docked vesicles. In its original form, the SNARE hypothe- elevation of intracellular calcium. Only
chromaffin cells, brief elevations in sis proposed that vesicles would fuse after fusion would SNAREs be trapped
with target membranes via in a tight ternary complex as was pro-
the following sequence of posed by Hanson and colleagues9. This
events 3: first, SNAREs on model does not require a reversible
the vesicle and target docking process. At present there is lit-
membranes assemble with tle in vitro biochemical evidence that a
each other, forming the partially assembled SNARE complex
tight ternary complex and sensitive to botulinum or tetanus toxin
docking the vesicle to the exists. However, such partial assembly
target membrane; second, could be stabilized by one of a number
cytosolic factors, SNAP of SNARE interacting proteins such as
NSF-ATP
and the ATPase NSF Rab, Sec1, synaptotagmin, complexins
Ca++ sequentially bind. The or the recently discovered tomosyn10.
energy derived from ATP The proposal that the stable ternary
X X hydrolysis then serves to complex represents a postfusion state is
fuse the two membranes also consistent with the ability of appro-
and to disassemble the priately paired unassembled SNAREs to
SNAREs. Thus, according mediate membrane fusion6. The ener-
Bob Crimi to this scenario, one would gy released during the formation of the
Syntaxin SNAP-25 predict that docked vesi- ternary complex could potentially serve
Synaptobrevin Tetanus or cles would be invulnerable to drive the fusion event, leaving a sta-
Botulinum toxin
to toxin attack, and that ble low-energy complex that requires
Fig. 2. Alternate model for SNARE-mediated exocytosis. measurements of secretion disassembly and energy input prior to
As in Fig. 1, the SNARES on vesicles prior to docking are in the presence of toxins vesicle recycling.
sensitive to toxin attack. After vesicle docking, the SNAREs would reveal a small These experiments thus constrain
are partially assembled and still sensitive to toxins. This amount of secretion corre- the debate about the placement of the
state probably requires stabilization by other SNARE-inter- sponding to that derived stable SNARE complex in the secreto-
acting proteins. Vesicle fusion and recycling as in Fig. 1. The only from the docked ry-vesicle life cycle. The use of the cel-
membrane binding step need not be reversible, but a new pool. lular milieu as the modern cuvette for
stable but toxin-sensitive SNARE arrangement that docks The experiments of Xu studying native molecular interactions
vesicles is proposed. and colleagues indicate promises to become increasingly impor-

176 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

news and views

tant in determining the inner workings 1. Coloquhoun, D. & Sakmann, B. Neuron 20, 6. Weber, T. et al. Cell 92, 759772 (1998).
381387 (1998).
of secretory machinery. In particular, it 7. Banerjee, A., Barry, V.A., DasGupta, B.R. &
should now be possible to pinpoint the 2. Xu, T., Binz, T., Niemann, H. & Neher, E. Martin, T.F.J. J. Biol. Chem. 271,
Nature Neurosci. 1, 192200 (1998). 2022320226 (1996).
roles of NSF and SNAP as well as that of 3. Rothman, J.E. Nature 372, 5563 (1994). 8. Schweizer, F.E. et al. Science 279, 12031206
the growing family of SNARE-interact- (1998).
4. Scheller, R.H. Neuron 14, 893897 (1995).
ing proteins in the synaptic-vesicle life 9. Hanson, P.I., Roth, R., Morisaki, H., Jahn, R.
5. Sllner, T., Bennett, M.K., Whiteheart, S.W.,
cycle, using methods like those of Xu Scheller, R.H. & Rothman, J.E. Cell 75, & Heuser, J.E. Cell 90, 523535 (1997).
and colleagues. 409418 (1993). 10. Fujita,Y. et al. Neuron 20, 905915 (1998).

Midline

Getting a line on pain: is it Central


sulcus

Area 3a

mediated by dedicated Area 24c


Cerebral

pathways?
cortex
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

Thalamus Dorsal
Edward R. Perl anterior
insula

MDvc
Are painful stimuli signaled to the thalamus by distinct
VPI
pathways? A new finding of structure/function correlations
VMpo
in spinal neurons suggests that the answer is yes.

Most people accept that the sensations of provides strong evidence favoring
vision, hearing, taste and vibration are at least a partially dedicated system.
subserved by dedicated neural pathways, Pain in normal animals and COLD

in which the physical stimulus is trans- human beings is usually caused by NS

duced by specialized sensory cells and stimuli that are strong enough to HPC
processed by certain neurons and regions threaten the integrity of the tissues
of the central nervous system that are involved. Detection of such events Lamina I
WDR

dominated by their particular sensory is called nociception; sense organs


input. Is this also true of pain? Aristotle with the appropriate characteristics Spinothalamic
held that pain was an emotion rather for nociception have been known Spinal cord tract

than a sensation, and although this view for over thirty years and are highly
had been largely abandoned by the 20th conserved between different mam- Bob Crimi

century 1 , the question of whether the malian species6. However, pain can Fig. 1. Diagram representing the relationships
neural basis of pain resembles that of arise from stimuli or events that are between functional category, cellular morphology
other sensations remains highly contro- not noxious. This has been used to and thalamic projection of spinal dorsal horn lamina I
versial to this day2,3. One concept is that argue that nociception is not neurons proposed by Han, Zheng and Craig
pain is processed by dedicated pain directly associated with pain 3 , (adapted from ref. 2). MDvc, medial dorsal nucleus
receptors that provide input to specific although there is substantial evi- ventral caudal portion; VMpo, ventral medial
central pain pathways. An alternate dence that noxious stimuli are nucleus, posterior part; VPI, ventral posterior infe-
view3,4 is that pain is not signaled by a closely related to indications of rior nucleus; NS, nociceptive specific neuron; COLD,
specific labeled-line system of neurons aversive behavior in animals or of innocuous cooling neuron; HPC, heat, pinch, cooling
but instead by a special form of activa- pain in human beings. neuron; WDR, multireceptive neuron (wide
tion in neurons that are also concerned Han and colleagues have used a dynamic range).
with other somatic sensations. In this combination of electrophysiology
view, painful events would be distin- and histology to characterize the
guished from innocuous stimuli by a neurons of lamina I in the dorsal
combination of functional characteris- horn of the cat spinal cord, one of the ear- thinly myelinated or unmyelinated fibers
tics, including the frequency and pattern liest stages in the nociception pathway. of specific thermoreceptors and various
of firing in neurons whose lesser or dif- The dorsal-horn gray matter is divided nociceptors7, suggesting a role in sensing
ferent activities result in other sensory into zones, or laminae, based on varia- tissue damage and temperature. Consis-
experiences. A new study in this issue tions in the size, shape and density of its tent with this, some neurons of lamina I
(pp. 218225) by Han, Zhang and Craig neurons. Primary sensory fibers from the are selective either for noxious stimuli or
somatic and visceral tissues terminate for innocuous changes in temperature8,9.
Edward Perl is at the Department of Physiology, throughout the dorsal horn, and the ter- Moreover, lamina I neurons project to the
University of North Carolina-Chapel Hill, CB# minals from different functional classes thalamus and several other brain regions
7545, Chapel Hill, North Carolina 27599, USA are segregated in different laminae. Lam- via the contralateral spinothalamic tract,
email: erp@med.unc.edu ina I receives its input largely from the which is important for the perception of

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 177


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

news and views

pain from the opposite side of the body1. ies? In most experiments, Han and col- We now know from brain imaging stud-
An important question is whether the leagues used a more selective method than ies that a given sensory stimulus acti-
neurons of lamina I are specialized for other researchers to choose their neurons, vates multiple cerebral cortical zones.
different types of painful stimuli, and testing for antidromic activation of pro- Somatic sensations, including pain,
whether these different classes send their jecting neurons by finding and stimulat- seem to involve an as yet unknown com-
outputs to particular brain regions. Such ing a restricted thalamic region that bination of ascending afferent informa-
an arrangement would have significant activates lamina I neurons. Lamina I neu- tion with an intrinsic background of
implications for understanding how rons are difficult to record, because they neural activity modified by past history
afferent signals combine to give rise to are flattened at the interface between the or disease.
pain. Previous papers from Craigs labo- outermost part of the gray matter and the It seems likely that there is merit in
ratory and others have described, in cat largely myelinated fibers of the spinal cord the views of each of the most diametri-
and monkey, at least three functional tracts. Moreover, their form is not easily cally opposed camps in the controversy
classes of neurons in lamina I that pro- appreciated unless viewed in horizontal about the nature of pain mechanisms.
ject to the thalamus2,9. One class is noci- histological preparations, which were not Accepting the observations of Han and
ceptive-specific (NS) with inputs from used in previous studies 1315. Another colleagues, there is a system of central
one or more types of nociceptor, anoth- consideration is that most previous stud- neurons, morphologically and func-
er responds to innocuous cooling ies did not concentrate exclusively on lam- tionally distinct beginning in lamina I
(COLD) and a third class is multimodal, ina I neurons, but included observations of the spinal cord, whose usual function
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

responding to heat, pinch and cooling in the subjacent lamina II, possibly blunt- is to inform the organism about tissue
(HPC). The authors have reported pre- ing distinctions. A further difficulty is the damage or the threat of such damage. It
viously that these different functional variation between individual neurons seems to be situated so that activity in
classes project to different thalamic within the categories (e.g., fusiform, some of its constituents at several levels
nuclei, which in turn project to different pyramidal) reported by Han and col- of the central nervous system normally
cortical regions (Fig. 1). The present leagues; although this need not invalidate leads to pain or equivalent reactions.
findings greatly strengthen their pro- their classification, it would have made However, once these messages reach the
posed functional classification, by show- matters difficult for previous workers brain, they are processed in parallel by
ing that the three classes of neurons lacking the advantage of the horizontal more than one set of central neurons.
defined by their physiological selectivity plane in seeking structure/function rela- The eventual sensory experience almost
correspond to particular morphological tionships within this population. surely represents the interplay in a mix-
types, distinguished by the shapes of The degree of specificity implied by ture of serial and parallel processing
their cell bodies and dendritic processes. the new results does not necessarily indi- integrated into the activity of other
This is consistent with a particular cate that the system is genetically hard- neural systems related to elaboration of
defined pathway for nociception and wired. The shape of neurons could consciousness, emotion and memory.
pain and seems difficult to reconcile with equally reflect their environments,
a model in which pain arises from func- including the connections they receive 1. Keele, K.D. Anatomies of Pain (Thomas,
tional interactions between neurons that from other neurons, rather than being Springfield, Illinois, 1957).
are not themselves specified for particu- an inherent property of each neuron 2. Craig, A.D. Pain Forum 7, 114 (1998).
lar sensory experience. itself. But whichever mechanism is 3. Wall, P.D. Pain 62, 389391 (1995).
The new findings leave unresolved the responsible for the observed 4. Melzack, R. & Wall, P.D. Science 150, 971979
relative importance for pain of the NS and structure/function relationship, the exis- (1965).
HPC neurons of lamina I and the so- tence of neurons partially dedicated to 5. Belmonte, C. & Cervero, F. Neurobiology of
called wide-dynamic-range (WDR) neu- conveying information about noxious or Nociceptors (Oxford Univ. Press, New York,
rons described by other researchers. WDR thermal stimuli fails to explain in itself 1996).
neurons are excited only modestly by either the sensation of pain or its aber- 6. Dubner, R., Kenshalo, D.R., Maixner, W.,
innocuous mechanical stimuli and fire rations, just as our present understand- Bushnell, M.C. & Oliveras, J.L. J.
Neurophysiology 62, 450457 (1989).
most strongly in response to noxious ing of the auditory system fails to
7. Light, A.R. The Initial Processing of Pain and
mechanical or heat stimuli. They are explain hearing. Even if the functional Its Descending Control: Spinal and Trigeminal
found mainly in laminae V and VII and and structural specialization of spinal Systems (Karger, Basel, 1993).
project to the thalamus via the spinothal- neurons projecting pain-relevant infor- 8. Christensen, B.N. & Perl, E.R. J. Neurophysiol.
amic tract10. They are relatively rare in mation is reminiscent of other sensory 33, 293307 (1970).
lamina I (especially in cats), and so the systems, this does not indicate a tele- 9. Dostrovsky, J.O. & Craig, A.D. J. Neurophysiol.
authors were not able to characterize them phonic relay arrangement of the sort 76, 36563665 (1996).
in the present study. However, they have implied by the term labeled line. Mam- 10. Willis, W.D. The Pain System (Karger, Basel,
been proposed to convey pain signals to malian sensory systems are generally 1985).
the brain in humans and to be important plastic and reflect their past history. It is 11. Mayer, D.J., Price, D.D. & Becker, D.B. Pain 1,
5158 (1975).
for aversive behavior in animals6,11. WDR not surprising that the same should be
12. Ritz, L.A. & Greenspan, J.D. J. Comp. Neurol.
neurons are multipolar cells12, similar to true of a system associated with pain, 238, 440452 (1985).
HPC neurons, and project to at least one and as Han and colleagues acknowledge,
13. Ferrington, D.G., Sorkin, L.S. & Willis, W.D. J.
thalamic locus2. Is it possible that HPC the specificity they describe may not be Physiol. (Lond.) 388, 681703 (1987).
and WDR neurons are of the same type absolute, so that transmission by the dif- 14. Light, A.R., Trevino, D.L. & Perl, E.R. J. Comp.
and serve similar functions? ferent pathways might be modulated not Neurol. 186, 151171 (1979).
Why has this structure/function cor- only by past experience but also by 15. Woolf, C.J. & Fitzgerald, M. J. Comp. Neurol.
relation not been found in previous stud- ongoing activity from other brain areas. 221, 313328 (1983).

178 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

news and views

over, simultaneous recordings of cortical


Cortical control of the and LGN activity revealed that cortical
firing was associated with an increased
thalamus: top-down probability of firing in LGN neurons
with overlapping receptive fields but not

processing and plasticity more distally, suggesting that corticofu-


gal connections contribute to thalamic
excitation in a very specific manner
Josef P. Rauschecker dependent on their respective locations
on the retinotopic grid.
Blocking cortical NMDA receptors leads to a dramatic expansion The latter result ties in well with
of thalamic receptive fields. The results help to illuminate the another study8 on the visual system, in
role of corticofugal connections in sensory processing. which the authors recorded simultane-
ously from pairs of LGN neurons that
were stimulated by a moving bar and
Sensory information is relayed to the plasticity also is a top-down process and then removed cortical feedback. With
cortex via the thalamus, yet there are that plastic changes can be propagated cortical feedback, the LGN neurons
nearly ten times as many fibers project- in the reverse direction. fired synchronously, whereas without it,
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

ing back from the cortex to the thala- These plastic changes seem likely to this synchrony was lost. In this case, the
mus as there are in the forward be related to the normal role of the cor- corticothalamic loop would be expect-
direction from thalamus to cortex 1,2 . ticofugal system in the transmission of ed to amplify the response to the mov-
The function of this massive feedback sensory information. To
projection has puzzled neuroscientists understand how this
for decades. Initial attempts to address might be, it is useful to Layer IV
this question were largely confined to review some of the
the visual system, but more recently, findings on corticofugal Cortex

studies on the corticofugal modulation connections in other Layer VI

of subcortical responses in the auditory systems. In one early


system 3,4 have provided additional study6, the cat primary
insight. The most recent contribution5, visual cortex was
in this issue of Nature Neuroscience reversibly cooled to
(p 226), extends the story to the inactivate the corticofu-
somatosensory system and adds a new gal feedback to the lat-
twist. From the combined results in dif- eral geniculate nucleus
ferent sensory systems, we can see the of the thalamus. This Thalamus
emergence of a more generalized under- inactivation reduced
standing of the role of the corticothala- both the light-evoked
mic loop in sensory information responses and the level
processing. of spontaneous activity Sensory
Ergenzinger and colleagues 5 deliv- in most thalamic relay periphery

ered an NMDA receptor antagonist neurons. Thus, corti-


(APV) into the primary somatosensory cofugal fibers seem to
cortex of monkeys, at the site of the directly but weakly +
hand representation. After several excite relay cells, as well
months of treatment, the tactile recep- as disinhibit them by
Bob Crimi
tive fields within the somatosensory part reducing the strength of
of the thalamus (the ventroposterior intrinsic inhibitory Fig. 1. Thalamocortical network in which the corticofugal
(VP) nucleus) showed an enormous mechanisms. A later projection provides narrow, highly specific positive feedback
enlargement in the hand region. Simi- study 7 provided more (dark orange) to a thalamic neuron with a classical center-sur-
lar, albeit less dramatic, enlargements specific evidence about round receptive field, as well as negative feedback (via
were also found after acute treatment. the wiring of the corti- inhibitory interneurons, shown in green) to a large number of
The results are intriguing because they cothalamic loop; gluta- surrounding thalamic neurons (pale orange), creating a wide
inhibitory suppression field (ref. 2). During normal sensory
challenge the traditional view of senso- mate, applied
processing, this circuitry helps to sharpen the contrast of the
ry plasticity as a bottom-up process, iontophoretically to a sensory input in time and space (or frequency in case of the
whereby changes on one level are sim- small patch of the pri- auditory system) and suppresses irrelevant information (ego-
ply passed on to the next higher level. mary visual cortex, centric selection, ref. 3, 4, 9). When the cortex is inactivated,
By contrast, the new study shows that excited LGN neurons e.g. by means of NMDA receptor antagonists (ref. 5), this
that shared receptive leads to an unmasking of the suppressed input from recurrent
Josef Rauschecker is at the Georgetown Institute fields with cortical neu- collaterals of other thalamic neurons within the suppression
for Cognitive and Computational Sciences, rons at the iontophore- field, which causes an enlargement of thalamic receptive
Georgetown University, Washington, DC 20007, sis site but inhibited fields. As the cortical activity block continues for several
USA LGN neurons that were months, this effect on thalamic neurons can be surprisingly
email: rauscheckerj@giccs.georgetown.edu out of register. More- large, perhaps magnified by secondary plastic changes.

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 179


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

news and views

ing bar stimulus, because synchroniza- Singers classical paper was based solely tion). Thus, immediately after applying
tion of thalamic relay neurons means on data then available from the visual APV to the cortex, receptive fields in the
that they will elicit temporally overlap- system, the concept can now be extend- thalamus are enlarged, which can be
ping excitatory postsynaptic potentials ed to the auditory (and perhaps most explained as a disinhibition of the sup-
(EPSPs) in any common cortical target other) sensory systems, where corti- pression effects of the corticofugal fibers.
cells, thereby increasing the chance that cofugal feedback may play a role in anal- Applying the APV chronically, as in the
such cortical cells will fire. ogous processes such as auditory stream main finding of the study, renders the
The early work of Tsumoto and col- separation and scene analysis11. thalamic receptive field enlargements
leagues in vision is complemented in an The circuitry required for the mixed enormous, suggesting that the original
intriguing fashion by recent work of excitatory-inhibitory feedback provid- disinhibition effect leads to further plas-
Suga and colleagues on the corticofugal ed by the cortico-thalamic loop is tic changes, perhaps in the thalamus
projections of the bats auditory system. depicted in Fig. 1. The highly repetitive itself. This may also yield a late explana-
When cortical neurons tuned to specif- structure seemingly does not leave any tion of the earlier results by Pons and
ic frequencies are inactivated by local room for selectivity, but as pointed out colleagues of a massive expansion of the
injections of lidocaine 4 , the auditory by Koch 12 , the answer may lie in the cortical hand representation after chron-
responses of neurons in the thalamic gating properties of the thalamic ic deafferentation14.
medial geniculate nucleus (MGN) tuned NMDA receptors. Thalamic relay neu- As Yan and Suga point out in their
to the same frequency are reduced, rons receive excitatory glutamatergic latest paper, published earlier this year
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

whereas responses of neurons tuned to input both from peripheral sensory also in Nature Neuroscience9, more sub-
different frequencies are enhanced. The pathways and from corticofugal con- tle forms of plasticity could also be
same is true, incidentally, for neurons nections. Assuming that these inputs act mediated by the corticofugal loop. It
in another subcortical auditory nucle- partly through NMDA receptors, the could be that thalamic maps are con-
us, the inferior colliculus, suggesting relay neurons would be activated only stantly adjusted by sensory experience,
that the role of the corticofugal projec- if they receive sensory input and cortical as are cortical maps. In conclusion,
tion may be quite general9. Moreover, if feedback at the same time. Given the function and plasticity have often been
the cortical site is (electrically) stimu- results of Sillito and colleagues8, only thought of as highly interwoven aspects
lated rather than blocked, the effect is those inputs that occur within a narrow of cortical structure, neural activity
to increase and sharpen the responses of temporal window would be amplified. being the factor that ties them together.
a particular subclass of neurons impor- Similarly, only stimuli that fall within a The study by Ergenzinger et al.5 makes
tant in the bats echolocation behavior, defined region of the sensory receptor it clear that these dual aspects of high-
the delay-tuned neurons, in both thala- surface (for instance the retina or er perceptual and cognitive systems
mus and colliculus3. Like frequency and cochlea) would be amplified. Anything need to be extended to the thalamus, at
retinotopic location, delay tuning is that is part of the irrelevant din would least insofar as the latter is controlled by
mapped in a topographic fashion corti- be suppressed, because without cortical its cortical input.
cally and subcortically and again, the feedback (and hence NMDA receptor
effect of cortical stimulation is highly activation), the inputs from the periph- 1. Guillery, R. W. J. Comp. Neurol. 130,
197222 (1967).
specific for neurons with the same delay ery are not strong enough to overcome
2. Jones E. G. The Thalamus (Plenum, New
tuning on both levels. the effects of thalamic inhibitory York, 1985).
These findings suggest a common interneurons, which are themselves dri-
3. Yan, J. & Suga, N. Science 273, 11001103
role for the corticothalamic loop in sen- ven by both sensory and corticofugal (1996).
sory information processing in different inputs (see Fig. 1). Thus, although the 4. Zhang, Y. Suga, N. & Yan, J. Nature 387,
systems: the corticofugal projection pro- corticofugal input to the thalamus helps 900903 (1997).
vides positive feedback to the correct to amplify certain inputs (similar to a 5. Ergenzinger, E. R., Glasier, M. M., Hahm, J.
input while at the same time suppress- winner-take-all mechanism), it also O. & Pons, T. P. Nature Neurosci. 1, 226229
ing irrelevant information. It seems that generates a large suppression field 2 , (1998).
the positive feedback is very precisely which exists outside the classical recep- 6. Kalil, R. E. & Chase, R. J. Neurophysiol. 33,
459474 (1970).
matched between cortex and thalamus, tive field.
such that a given thalamic neuron This circuit model for the selective 7. Tsumoto, T., Creutzfeldt, O. D. & Legendy,
C. R. Exp. Brain Res. 32, 345364 (1978).
receives excitation only from a small area modulation of sensory information can
8. Sillito, A. M., Jones, H. E., Gerstein, G. L. &
of cortex that shares the same stimulus also account for the results of Pons and West, D. C. Nature 369, 479482 (1994).
selectivity (for instance, a particular colleagues. Applying an NMDA recep- 9. Yan, J. & Suga, N. Nature Neurosci. 1, 5458
location in visual space or a particular tor antagonist to the cortical neurons (1998).
sound frequency). Based on their work essentially silences this part of the cor- 10. Singer, W. Physiol. Rev. 57, 386420 (1977).
in the auditory system, Suga and col- tex (an effect that complicates the inter- 11. Bregman, A.S. Auditory Scene Analysis (MIT
leagues have termed this principle ego- pretation of the early results on the Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 1990)
centric selection. This process leads to a effects of NMDA receptor antagonists in 12. Koch, C. Neuroscience 23, 399406 (1987).
temporal and spatial contrast sharpen- visual cortex; see ref. 13). The antago- 13. Rauschecker, J.P. Physiol. Rev. 71, 587615
ing of the sensory input and, as suggest- nist has no direct effect on the thalamus (1991).
ed by Singer10, may play a crucial role in (in contrast to cortical lesions, which 14. Pons, T. P. et al. Science 237, 18571860
figure-ground discrimination. Although lead to retrograde thalamic degenera- (1991).

180 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

scientific correspondence

Where is the sun? We addressed these questions by measuring the time it takes
to detect, within a group of distractor bubbles, a single target
bubble that is lit differently (Fig. 1b). Recent studies suggests that
Jennifer Sun1 and Pietro Perona1,2 the light-from-above assumption is used by the visual system for
1
interpreting quickly and in parallel some basic aspects of 3D
California Institute of Technology 136-93, Pasadena, California 91125, USA scenes48; the target pattern may be detected quickly (pop-out)
2 Universita di Padova, Via Ognissanti 72, 35131 Padova, Italy only when the distractors, but not the target, can be interpreted
Correspondence should be addressed to P.P. (perona@vision.caltech.edu) as convex and lit from above59. We simulated different directions
of lighting by varying the shading gradient of the distractor bub-
When we interpret a shaded picture as a three-dimensional (3D) bles. The target bubble was shaded to simulate illumination from
scene, our visual system often needs to guess the position of the light the opposite direction (Fig. 1b).
source in order to resolve a convex-concave ambiguity. For more Data from twelve naive subjects shows that the visual system
than a century, psychologists have known that the visual system does not respond uniformly to all lighting directions that are above
assumes that light comes from above and have argued that this the horizon (Fig. 1c). There is clearly a preferred direction of light-
assumption is ecologically justified because our everyday light source ing where detection requires the shortest display time. Surpris-
(the sun) is overhead. Our experiments reveal that peoples preferred ingly, this preferred direction is not directly overhead (zero
lighting direction is not directly overhead, but rather shifted to the degrees). Subject PG, for instance, performs best with a lighting
left, and this preference is reflected in art spanning two millennia. direction that is between 30 and 60 degrees left of the vertical
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

Furthermore, we find a strong correlation between peoples hand- (Fig. 1c). Furthermore, there is a consistent preference for left light-
edness and their preferred lighting. We suggest that what counts is ing over right lighting. This same marked leftright asymmetry is
not so much where the sun is, but where you like the sun to be. evident in the averaged data of all twelve subjects. As the angle of
The shaded shapes in Fig. 1a are typically perceived as convex illumination increases, the preference for left lighting becomes
bubbles surrounding concave indentations, all lit from above. Note increasingly pronounced (Fig. 1d).
that this image is also consistent with a different physical scene: This asymmetry in our data may explain a qualitative observa-
indentations surrounding bubbles, all lit from below. This second tion made by Gestalt psychologist Metzger, who noted that left-lit
perception, however, is difficult to achieve. Such an asymmetry scenes have a superior perceptual value over right-lit ones10. He
between the perceptual saliency of equally valid 3D interpretations ascribed this asymmetry to the convention of setting up desk lamps
demonstrates that our visual system prefers the assumption that on the left, presumably so that the writing hand does not cast a
light is coming from above13. Does this preference apply uniform- shadow on the page. Over time, he hypothesized, one learns to per-
ly to all lighting directions that are above the horizon? Perhaps there ceive left-lit scenes as being more natural. Metzgers explanation
is instead a preferred direction? If so, one might reason it to be direct- may be somewhat restrictive: our visual environment extends
ly overhead. Is this intuitive guess correct? beyond our writing desk. We tend to position a movable light
Performance
Performance versus
vs Lighting lighting
Direction direction
Subject PG
a b c 500 (subject PG)
Left Lighting Right Lighting
450 Left lighting Right lighting
(ms)

400
duration msec

350
MFV Duration

300
MFV

250

200

150
180 150 120 90 60 30 0 30 60 90 120 150
Lighting direction
Lighting Direction of distratorsdegrees
of Distractors (degrees)
Fig. 1. Shaded displays that may be interpreted as 3D shapes and measurement of preferred
lighting direction. (a) Rotate the page to invert the shapes. (b) Images were generated on a
Silicon Graphics Indigo2. Each `bubble spanned approximately one degree. One target pattern d Perfomance differences
g g
at
various lighting directions
was present at random among 23 distractor patterns in 50% of the trials. The remaining trials 140

contained 24 distractors and no target. The lighting direction is determined by the shading gra-
Durations msec

12 Subjects
Difference in MFV durations

120 pp<<0.001
0.001
dient of the distractors. Target-present test screens are shown for 2 of the 12 lighting direc-
tions used in our experiment. We denote a lighting direction by its deviation from the vertical 100
in degrees. Positive degrees indicate lighting from the left, and negative degrees indicate lighting
from the right. Accordingly, lighting from directly overhead is designated as 0 degrees, and light- 80
(ms)

pp<< 0.007
0.007
Difference in MFV

ing from directly below as 180 degrees. (c) We used a two-alternative forced-choice stimulus
60
onset asynchrony (SOA) design with masking. Data was collected using a staircase method that
converged at 67% accuracy performance. The most frequently visited (MFV) duration within 40 pp << 0.08
0.03
each block was used to estimate 67% accuracy performance. The duration necessary for 67%
accuracy for each lighting direction is shown for subject PG. (d) We computed the mean dif- 20
ference in necessary display duration between pairs of corresponding left-right lighting direc-
0
tions over all 12 naive subjects. The necessary duration for a left-lighting condition is 30, 30 60, 60 90, 90
subtracted from the necessary duration for the corresponding right-lighting condition. Lighting
Lighting Directionscompared
directions Compared (rightleft)
(right-left)

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 183


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

scientific correspondence

Preferred angles of Preferred lighting Fig. 2. The preferred light direction corre-
a 35 lighting direction b 60
direction versus handedness lates with handedness. (a) Subjects were

Direction degrees
lefthander
50 categorized based on which hand they used
p << 0.0001
0.0001 righthander

Preferred lighting direction


30
40 for writing. The mean preferred lighting
Angle left of the vertical

25 direction was compared between the six


30
right-handed and the six left-handed sub-

(degrees)
20
20 jects. Although both groups show a prefer-
(degrees)

Preferred Lighting
15 p << 0.03
0.03 10 ence for left-lighting, right-handers, as a
Left Preference group, prefer a larger angle left of the verti-
10 0 cal (23.3 degrees) than do left-handers (7.9
Right Preference
5 -10 degrees). (b) Preferred lighting direction is
-20 correlated with handedness (r = 0.83,
0 - 10 -5 0 5 10 p<0.01). * lefthander, p righthander.
Lefthanders
Lefthanders Righthanders
Righthanders Handedness
Handedness

source, or position ourselves in relation to a fixed light source, such the effect mirror symmetric, with left-handers having a preference for
that our hand does not cast a shadow upon the object of our manip- right-lighting instead of left? This may be explained because left-
ulation. Right handers would then develop a preference for left- handers live in a right-handers world and are often forced to func-
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

lighting, and, as suggested by van Fieandt11, left-handers may well tion in environments that are designed for right-handers. If lighting
show the opposite lighting preference. preference is not determined by a biological trait correlated with
To investigate this possibility, we derived the preferred light- handedness itself, but rather by handedness-related experience, then
ing direction and handedness for our subjects. We defined each one would expect that right-handers would prefer left-lighting,
subjects preferred lighting direction to be the lighting direction whereas left-handers, because of their mixed experience, would
for which target detection performance was best. This was esti- exhibit a weaker preference for either left or right lighting.
mated by fitting a parabola to the central portion of each subjects Quantitative measurements of the perception of shape from
time versus direction curve (e.g. Fig. 1c). and calculating the direc- shading using static displays have revealed no asymmetry between
tion for which the parabola reached a minimum. When prefer- left- and right-lighting conditions14,15, thereby failing to confirm
ence for lighting direction is considered for left- and right-handers Metzgers observation. One might, therefore, suspect that the pref-
separately, we find that both groups have a preference for left- erence for left lighting we observe with our fast-presentation para-
lighting. However, the right-handers as a group prefer a lighting digm is confined to the earlier stages of visual processing, and that
direction that is significantly more toward the left than the left- this effect may become negligible under ecological viewing condi-
handers preference (Fig. 2a). tions. We have reason to think otherwise. We asked two naive sub-
Handedness, however, is not a strictly binary trait; rather it varies jects, one right-hander and one left-hander, to survey 225 master
in a continuum12. We used a standard ten-item questionnaire that paintings and determine the predominant angle of lighting for each.
evaluated the relative strengths of our subjects handedness13. The The histogram of the measurements (Fig. 3) shows that the artists
resulting score ranges from -10 to 10, with positive values indicat- most often chose a lighting direction that is left of the vertical. This
ing a bias for the right hand, and negative values for the left. When preference for top-left lighting may have resulted from an accidental
each subjects preferred lighting direction is plotted against this hand- artistic convention. However, this is unlikely, as preference left-light-
edness score, a strong correlation is found (Fig. 2b). ing is found across schools and periods: from Roman mosaics,
If lighting preference is indeed related to handedness, why isnt through Renaissance, baroque, and impressionist art. It is therefore
possible that top-left lighting may actually have a higher perceptual
g g
Evaluation of lighting direction in paintings value than top-right lighting in natural viewing conditions, which
70 involve frequent saccades over the entire scene. Perhaps when sub-
Left Right jects are required to make their shape judgments under prolonged
60
scrutiny of a localized portion of the test stimulus14,15, the effect
50 drops below a measurable level.

40 Acknowledgements
The authors are grateful to Dr. Marianne Tauber for providing the references to the
30
Gestalt literature. Support was provided by the NSF Engineering Reasearch Center
20 for Neuromorphic Systems at Caltech.

10 1. Rittenhouse, D. Trans. Am. Phil. Soc. 2, 3742 (1786).


2. Brewster, D. Edinburgh Phil. Trans. 15, 657 (1847).
3. Ramachandran, V.S. Nature 331, 163166 (1988).
0
90 60 30 0 30 60 90 4. Enns, J.T. & Rensink, R.A. Science 247, 721723 (1990).
Lighting direction
Lighting of of
Direction painting (degrees)
Painting degrees 5. Braun, J. Perception 19, A112 (1990).
6. Braun, J. Spatial Vision 7, 311322 (1993).
Fig. 3. Painters tend to light scenes from top-left. 100, 100 and 25 7. Kleffner, D.A. & Ramachandran, V.S. Percept. Psychophys. 52, 1836 (1992).
8. Sun, J.Y. & Perona, P. Vision Res. 36, 25152529 (1996).
paintings were randomly selected from catalogues of the Louvre, 9. Sun, J.Y. & Perona, P. Nature 379, 165168 (1996).
the Prado, and the Norton Simon Museum. Two naive subjects eval- 10. Metzger, W., Gesetze des Sehens. Frankfurt am Main (1936).
uated these paintings for lighting direction. Using a protractor, 11. van Fieandt, K. Psychologischen Institut Monog. (Helsinki University, 1938).
12. Durost, W.N. Genet. Psychol. Monog. 16, 225335 (1934).
77 0.55% of the paintings were classified as being lit from the left 13. Oldfield, R.C. Neuropsychologia 9, 97113 (1971).
(p<0.05). The artists most often selected lighting directions that are 14. Blthoff, H.H. & Mallot, H.A. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 5, 17491758 (1988).
between 30 and 60 degrees to the left of the vertical. 15. Todd, J.T. et al. Invest. Ophthal. Vis. Sci. 37, 4282 (1996).

184 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Tyrosine kinase potentiates NMDA


receptor currents by reducing tonic
zinc inhibition
F. Zheng, M.B. Gingrich, S.F. Traynelis and P.J. Conn

Department of Pharmacology, Emory University School of Medicine, 1510 Clifton Road, Atlanta, Georgia 30322, USA
Correspondence should be addressed to F.Z. (fzheng@emory.edu)

Activation of the tyrosine kinase Src potentiates NMDA-receptor currents, which is thought to be
necessary for induction of hippocampal long-term potentiation. Although the carboxy(C)-terminal
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

domain of the NR2A subunit contains potential tyrosine phosphorylation sites, the mechanism by
which Src modulates synaptic plasticity and NMDA receptor currents is not fully understood. Here
we present evidence from NR1 mutants and splice variants that Src potentiates NMDA-receptor cur-
rents by reducing the tonic inhibition of receptors composed of NR1 and NR2A subunits by extracel-
lular zinc. Using site-directed mutagenesis, we have identified three C-terminal tyrosine residues of
NR2A that are required for Srcs modulation of the zinc sensitivity of NMDA receptors. Our data link
two modulatory sites of NMDA receptors that were previously thought to be independent.

Recent studies suggest that tyrosine kinases are important for other NR2 subunits1821. This sensitivity to zinc is high enough
synaptic plasticity 13. The requirement for Src activation in to allow ambient zinc (either in vivo22 or as a contaminant of
long-term potentiation (LTP) induction has been attributed experimental solutions23) to tonically inhibit NR1/NR2A recep-
to its enhancement of NMDA-receptor currents46 because an tors. Here we show that Src reduces zinc sensitivity of recombi-
NMDA-receptor antagonist blocks this effect3. The non-recep- nant NR1/NR2A and NR1/NR2B receptors, thereby relieving
tor tyrosine kinases Src and Fyn potentiate receptors composed tonic inhibition by zinc and potentiating the NMDA-receptor
of NR1 and NR2A subunits, but have no apparent effect on response. Splice variants and mutants of NR1 subunits that have
receptors composed of NR1 and other NR2 subunits 5. Fur- a low apparent affinity for zinc are, as predicted, potentiated to a
thermore, deletion of the C-terminal domain of NR2A elimi- lesser degree by Src. Conversely, NR1 mutants with higher appar-
nates the potentiation of NR1/NR2A-receptor currents by Src5 ent zinc affinity are potentiated to a greater degree. Using site-
and impairs synaptic plasticity and contextual memory7. Thus, directed mutagenesis, we have identified three C-terminal tyrosine
several lines of evidence suggest that a unique tyrosine phos- residues of NR2A that are required for Srcs modulation of
phorylation site on the C-terminal of NR2A may confer sen- NMDA-receptor zinc sensitivity. By showing that Src potentiates
sitivity to Src and be critical for the induction of LTP. However, NMDA receptors by specifically reducing tonic zinc inhibition,
the mechanisms by which tyrosine kinase potentiates NMDA- our data link two modulatory sites of NMDA receptors that were
receptor function are unknown. previously thought to be independent.
In addition, NR2B subunits are phosphorylated by one of
the Src family of tyrosine kinases8, but no functional effect of Results
this phosphorylation has been identified. This discrepancy SRC REDUCES TONIC INHIBITION OF NR1/NR2A RECEPTORS
between biochemical and physiological data raises the possibil- Our hypothesis was that potentiation of NMDA receptor cur-
ity that either tyrosine phosphorylation of NR2B affects recep- rents by Src may be due to its relief of tonic inhibition by zinc
tor properties other than channel activity, or another property of acting at the voltage-independent site, in a manner analogous
NR2A may be responsible for the apparent selective potentia- to polyamine relief of tonic proton inhibition of NMDA recep-
tion by tyrosine kinase of receptors containing this subunit. tors 13 . To test this hypothesis, we examined the effects of
NMDA receptors are allosterically modulated by a variety of EDTA, a chelator of transition metals such as zinc, on Srcs
endogenous extracellular ions913. Zinc, one of these modulators, potentiation of NR1/NR2A receptors. The NMDA receptor
is accumulated in some nerve terminals in specific brain regions currents were recorded with a rapid perfusion system in
and released during neuronal activity14. Zinc inhibits NMDA HEK293 cells transiently transfected with NR1-1a/NR2A
receptors at two independent sites1517. A low-affinity zinc site is receptors. In control experiments, the amplitudes of evoked
probably located inside the channel pore, and binding of zinc to NMDA-receptor currents did not change significantly over
this site causes a voltage-dependent inhibition of NMDA-receptor time (Fig. 1a and b). When purified recombinant human c-
channels. A high-affinity site is likely located outside the channel Src was added to the internal solution, the amplitude of
pore and causes a voltage-independent inhibition. Receptors con- NMDA-receptor currents was gradually increased over a five-
taining NR2A subunits exhibit far greater affinity for zinc at the minute period (Fig. 1a). On average, the peak of NMDA
extracellular, voltage-independent site than receptors containing receptor currents evoked by 100 M glutamate and 30 M

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 185


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 1. Src potentiates NR1/NR2A receptors by No Src


reducing tonic zinc inhibition. (a) Removal of tonic
a b
zinc inhibition of NR1-1a/NR2A receptors occludes

% of initial current
the potentiation of these receptors by Src. NMDA-
receptor current traces at left (labeled 1) were the Src
first of a series of current traces recorded 6090 sec-
onds after obtaining the whole-cell configuration.
Traces at right (labeled 30) were the last of 30 con-
secutive traces recorded every ten seconds over a
five-minute period. EDTA is added into extracellular Src and EDTA
solutions to chelate zinc in some experiments,
whereas recombinant human c-Src is added into the Time (min)
internal solution of patch pipettes. Under control
conditions, no significant rundown of the NMDA-
c Control Src
d
receptor current was observed. Inclusion of c-Src
(30 units per ml) resulted in a 50% potentiation.

IEDTA/INO EDTA
However, this potentiation was not observed in the
presence of 10 M EDTA. (b) Average changes of
the NMDA-receptor current amplitudes over the
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

same periods. Error bars are standard error for all


panels. Number of cells is indicated in parentheses. EDTA EDTA
P control (5), L Src (10), G Src and EDTA (5). Control Src
(c) The effects of EDTA (10 M) on NMDA currents
evoked by agonists (100 M glutamate, 30 M glycine) from representative HEK 293 cells with or without inclusion of Src in the pipette solu-
tion (Vh = -50 mV). (d) The potentiation of whole-cell NR1-1a/NR2A-receptor currents by EDTA in HEK cells is reduced by inclusion of Src
in the patch pipette (*p<0.01). Circles are control cells; squares are cells perfused with Src.

glycine was potentiated by 53 9 % (n = 10), whereas the peak there are several other candidate ions, such as cadmium or cop-
of currents evoked by 300 M glutamate and 100 M glycine per18. To directly assess the effect of Src on zinc-induced inhi-
was potentiated by 53 10% (n = 4). Our data agree with the bition of NR1/NR2A-receptor currents, we used
40% potentiation of recombinant NR1-3a/NR2A receptor by tricine-buffered zinc to determine the IC50 value of zinc act-
Src reported previously 5. As others5 and we have used satu- ing at its high-affinity, voltage-independent site on NR1/NR2A
rating concentrations of glutamate and glycine, it is unlikely receptors 18 . In Xenopus oocytes expressing NR1-1a/NR2A
that the potentiation of NMDA receptors by Src is due to a receptors, tricine-buffered zinc inhibited NMDA-receptor cur-
change in apparent affinity for glutamate or glycine. rents with an IC50 value of 15 nM for the high-affinity zinc site
If our hypothesis is correct, complete removal of the tonic inhi- (data not shown), in agreement with a previous report18. In
bition should prevent the potentiation of NR1/NR2A receptor cur- HEK293 cells, low concentrations of zinc caused a similar con-
rents by Src. To remove transition-metal contamination, we added centration-dependent inhibition (Fig. 2a). With zinc concen-
a trace amount of EDTA (10 M) to all recording solutions. Assum- trations up to 2.2 M, the NR1-1a/NR2A-receptor current still
ing 100 nM contaminant zinc and 1 mM extracellular Ca2+, the free exhibits a linear IV relationship (Fig. 2b), indicating that up
zinc is less than 0.03 nM, whereas the free Ca2+ remains at 0.99 mM. to this concentration, zinc acts exclusively on the voltage-inde-
In the presence of EDTA, Src failed to potentiate NR1/NR2A recep- pendent, high-affinity site (n = 3). The IC50 for the high-affin-
tors (Fig. 1a and b). In the presence of 10 mM tricine, another metal ity zinc site of NR1-1a/NR2A in HEK cells is 86 nM (Fig. 2c),
chelator, Src also failed to potentiate NR1/NR2A receptors (n = 10, which is fourfold higher than the IC50 obtained in oocytes18.
data not shown). Thus, removal of tonic inhibition of NR1/NR2A We are uncertain about the cause of this fourfold difference.
receptors by transition metals occludes potentiation by Src. Based on the fitted doseresponse curves and I EDTA/INo EDTA
We used the ratio of the peak NMDA-receptor current in the values (2.86 for oocytes, n = 10; 2.13 for HEK cells, n = 6), we
presence of EDTA (IEDTA) to the peak current in the absence of estimate the zinc contamination to be 275 nM for both record-
EDTA (INo EDTA) as a measure of the amount of tonic inhibition ing systems, which is in line with a previously reported value
of NR1/NR2A by transition metals. A smaller ratio indicates less (300 nM)23 and our own measurements (338 nM by VG Plas-
tonic inhibition. Without Src added to the patch pipettes, ma Quad 3 ICP-MS mass spectrometer, 330 nM by Jarrell-
IEDTA/INo EDTA ranged from 1.6 to 2.5 (mean, 2.2 0.1, n = 6, Ash965 ICP plasma emission spectrometer). Addition of Src
Fig. 1c and d). With Src added to the patch pipettes, IEDTA/INo increased the IC50 for NR1-1a/NR2A receptors in HEK cells to
EDTA ranged from 1.2 to 1.7 (mean = 1.4 0.1, n = 7). Thus, Src 392 nM, approximately fivefold (Fig. 2c). Based on this IC50
significantly reduced the tonic inhibition of NR1/NR2A recep- value and the estimated zinc contamination level (275 nM),
tors by transition metals (p<0.005). Inclusion of the phosphatase we predict that the potentiation caused by EDTA in the pres-
inhibitor orthovanadate (500 M) with Src results in an ence of Src should be 42%, which is very close to the actual
IEDTA/INo EDTA value of 1.18 0.03 (n = 3), which is not signif- potentiation observed experimentally (44 9%).
icantly different from the value with Src alone.
SRC POTENTIATION CORRELATES WITH ZINC INHIBITION
SRC SHIFTS IC50 FOR THE HIGH-AFFINITY, ZINC SITE If Src potentiates NMDA receptors by reducing tonic inhibi-
Zinc is probably the transition metal responsible for the EDTA- tion by zinc, NMDA receptors that are less sensitive to zinc
sensitive tonic inhibition of NR1/NR2A receptors, although should also be potentiated to a lesser degree by Src. Recent

186 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

NR1-1a/NR2A, Fig. 2. Src shifts IC50 for the


studies have identified some splice vari- a HEK293 b voltage-independent zinc site
ants and mutant NR1 receptors with
of NR1/NR2A receptors. (a)
reduced zinc sensitivity21. These recom-
Typical current traces showing

I (pA)
binant receptors were tested in HEK cells dose-dependent inhibition of
to determine the amount of tonic inhi- NR1-1a/NR2A receptors by
bition and Src potentiation for each. The zinc in HEK 293 cells (Vh, -50
IC50 value for NR1-1a(C744A,C798A)24, mV). Tricine was used to buffer
co-expressed with NR2A, is 168 nM in zinc. (b) The IV relationship
[Zn2+] (nM)
oocytes, about eightfold higher than the V (mV) of the peak NMDA-receptor
wild-type NR1-1a/NR2A receptor (Fig. currents shows no voltage-
3a). In HEK cells, this mutant is only c dependent inhibition at up to 2
slightly inhibited by ambient zinc, as M buffered zinc. P 2230 nM,
indicated by the IEDTA/INo EDTA ratio of L 223 nM, G 0 nM. (c) Src
1.11 0.03 (Fig. 3b). As predicted, NR1- shifts the IC50 for the high-
I/Imax
1a(C744A,C798A) is not potentiated by affinity, voltage-independent
Src in HEK cells (Fig. 3c). NR1-1b, zinc site of NR1-1a/NR2A
which has a 21-amino-acid insertion in receptors. Tricine was used to
the N-terminal region encoded by alter- buffer zinc. Each point on the
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

natively spliced exon 5, also shows curve is averaged data from


reduced zinc sensitivity, with an IC 50 49 cells. The IC50 is 86 nM
value of 198 nM for heteromeric recep- without Src and 392 nM with
[Zn2+] (nM) Src. P control, L Src.
tors containing NR2A in oocytes (see
also refs. 18 and 21). As expected,
recombinant NR1-1b/NR2A receptors,
with an IEDTA/INo EDTA ratio of 1.08 0.04 in HEK cells, were inhibition as indicated by an IEDTA/INo EDTA ratio of 1.77 0.09
not potentiated by Src (Fig. 3b and c). (Fig. 3b). Again, consistent with our hypothesis, the peak cur-
With the NR1-1b and the double cysteine mutant of NR1- rent of NR1-1bm207-211/NR2A receptors is potentiated by Src
1a, we demonstrated that reduction of tonic inhibition indeed (Fig. 3c). Overall, the amount of tonic inhibition of the NMDA
reduces the Src potentiation of NMDA receptors. By the same receptors (I EDTA /I No EDTA ) is highly correlated (R = 0.997,
reasoning, if a mutation within NR1 exon 5 could restore zinc p<0.005) with the amount of potentiation by Src of NMDA
sensitivity of the heteromeric receptors to levels comparable receptors (Fig. 3d).
to wild-type receptors lacking exon 5, it should also restore the
potentiation by Src. Several point mutations within exon 5 are C-TERMINAL TYROSINES ARE CRITICAL FOR SRCS ACTION
reported to restore zinc sensitivity to various degrees21. NR1- To locate the tyrosine residues critical for modulation of zinc
1bm207-211, a triple point mutant (K207G, R208G, K211G) sensitivity by Src, we made a series of point mutations in the C-
in the exon 5 region13, exhibits the highest level of zinc sensi- terminal region of NR2A (Fig. 4a). The amino-acid sequence of
tivity among all mutant receptors tested, with an IC50 value of the C-terminal region contains a consensus phosphorylation
40 nM in oocytes (Fig. 3a). Recombinant NR1-1bm207- site25 for Src around tyrosine residue 1267 (Fig. 4a). Thus, a
211/NR2A receptors are under a significant amount of tonic point mutation of Y1267 to phenylalanine was constructed.

Fig. 3. Potentiation of NMDA-receptor currents by Src corre- a b


lates with the amount of tonic inhibition of NR1/NR2A recep-
IEDTA/INO EDTA

tors by zinc. (a) Doseresponse curves for the high-affinity zinc


I/Imax

inhibition of heteromeric NR1/NR2A receptors in oocytes


(NR1-1a, n = 14; NR1-1b, n = 12; NR1-1a(C744A,C798A), n =
8; NR1-1bm207-211, n = 19). As previously reported21, a splice
variant with exon 5 (such as NR1-1b) exhibits an IC50 nearly
tenfold higher than a splice variant without exon 5 (such as
NR1-1a). Mutation of two cysteine residues of NR1-
1a(C744A,C798A)21 also shifts the IC50 for zinc by approxi- [Zn2+] (nM)
mately tenfold. A triple mutation in exon 5 (NR1-1bm207-211: d
K207G, R208G, K211G)21 nullifies the effects of exon 5 on zinc c
inhibition. L 1bm209211, g 1b, p 1a, H 1aC744,798A
Src potentiation
% of initial current

(b) The ratio of the current in the presence of 10 M EDTA to


that in the absence of EDTA is a measurement of tonic inhibi-
(%)

tion of NMDA-receptor currents by trace amounts of zinc in


HEK 293 cells. The amount of tonic inhibition by zinc is consis-
tent with the IC50 values obtained in oocytes. (c) Effects of Src
on current amplitudes of NR1/NR2A in HEK293 cells. Note
that NR1-1a and NR1-1bm207-211 are potentiated by Src, Time (min) IEDTA/INO EDTA
whereas NR1-1b and NR1-1aC744,798A are not. p 1a/2A
(10), P 1aC744,798A/2A (5), l 1b/2A (9), L 1bm207211/2A (6). (d) Correlation between the tonic inhibition by zinc and potentiation by Src
in HEK cells measured six minutes after obtaining the whole-cell configuration (correlation factor, R = 0.997, p<0.005).

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 187


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 4. Point mutations of tyrosine


residues in NR2A C-terminal block Srcs
a d
potentiation of NR1/NR2A currents and Y1267F
relief of zinc inhibition. (a) A diagram
showing mutations of C-terminal tyrosine
residues of NR2A subunit. Note that
Consensus sequence for Src
Y1267 is part of a putative consensus
phosphorylation site for Src. Other tyro-
sine residues with high surface probabil- b
ity 37 were also mutated as indicated. Y1105F

% of initial
(b) Summary bar graph showing the

current
effects of nine point mutations of tyrosine
residues to phenylalanine on the Srcs
potentiation of whole-cell NR1/NR2A-
receptor currents measured six minutes
after obtaining the whole-cell configura- c
tion (*p<0.01 when compared to the wild-
type NR2A by ANOVA and Y1387F
% of initial
current

Newman-Keuls post hoc test). For all pan-


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

els, number of cells is shown in parenthe-


ses. (c) Point mutation of Y1105F, Y1267F
or Y1387F blocks Srcs potentiation of
whole-cell NR1/NR2A-receptor currents.
p wt (10), L Y1105F (5), G Y1267F (8), Time (min) [Zn2+](nM)
H Y1387F (7). (d) Zinc sensitivity of het-
eromeric receptors containing three mutant NR2A subunits. Each data point is the average from 47 cells. Solid lines are fitted zinc curves
for wild-type NR1/NR2A receptor without Src; dashed lines are the curves with Src. Note that Y1267F has lower affinity for zinc and that
Src failed to relieve the zinc inhibition further. Although Y1105F and Y1387F have normal zinc affinity, Src failed to relieve zinc inhibition. p
control, l Src.

Additional tyrosine residues with high surface probability in the EDTA AND SRC INCREASE THE CURRENT DECAY TIME CONSTANT
neighboring region were also mutated. Point mutation Y1267F Published evidence conflicts about the mechanism by which
blocked the potentiation by Src (Fig. 4b and c). In addition, Src potentiates NMDA receptors. It has been reported that Src
mutants Y1105F and Y1387F also abolished Src potentiation increases the open probability, open time, burst length, clus-
(Fig. 4b and c). In contrast, mutations of several other tyrosine ter length and supercluster length of NMDA channels 6 .
residues in the same region had no detectable effects on Src- Although the relaxation time constant of macroscopic current
induced potentiation (Fig. 4b). Consistent with our hypothesis, may be influenced by the first latency of NMDA channels26, an
Src failed to change the zinc sensitivity of receptors with any one increase in the duration of individual channel activation by Src
of the three tyrosine mutations that eliminated Src potentiation should increase the relaxation time constant. However, no
of NMDA receptor currents (Fig. 4d). The zinc-inhibition curves change of the relaxation time constant is reported when the
for receptors with two of these mutations (Y1105F and Y1387F) macroscopic current amplitude is potentiated5. In our whole-
were identical to the zinc-inhibition curve for the wild-type cell recording, the decay of NR1/NR2A current could not be
receptor. In these cases, the only effect of the tyrosine mutation fitted with a single exponential component. To circumvent the
was to prevent the Src-induced reduction in zinc sensitivity. The possibility that the slower, second component reflects slow
third mutation (Y1267F) reduced zinc sensitivity of the recep- removal of the agonists due to turbulence around the whole
tor in the absence of Src, which precluded further reduction of cell, we investigated the decay time constant on NMDA-recep-
zinc sensitivity by Src. The IC 50 for zinc of NR1-
1a/NR2A(Y1267F) in oocytes was 92 nM (n = 13, data not
shown), which is approximately fivefold higher than the IC50 Table 1. Inhibition of wild-type versus mutant NR1/NR2A
for the wild-type NR1/NR2A. The amount of maximal inhibi- receptors by tricine-buffered zinc (223 nM) in HEK293
tion was not altered (data not shown). A similar shift of IC50 for cells
zinc was also observed in HEK cells (Fig. 4d). In contrast, the
Internal Solution No Src Src
EC50 values for glutamate (n = 7) and glycine (n = 7) and the
IC 50 value for proton inhibition (n = 8) for NR1- Wild-Type NR2A 0.48 0.04 (5) 0.69 0.05 (6)*
1a/NR2A(Y1267F) were not significantly different from those Y1105F 0.54 0.05 (7) 0.50 0.03 (5)
of the wild-type receptors (data not shown). These data suggest Y1267F 0.74 0.04 (4)* 0.75 0.05 (4)*
that the Y1267F mutation results in a receptor that behaves like Y1387F 0.49 0.04 (5) 0.49 0.05 (6)
one that is fully potentiated by Src. We have also constructed Y1105F,1387F 0.44 0.09 (4) 0.45 0.07 (5)
double tyrosine mutations NR2A(Y1105F,Y1387F) and
Y1267F,1387F 0.79 0.03 (3)* 0.82 0.04 (4)*
NR2A(Y1267F,1387F). There was no detectable difference
between NR2A(Y1105F,1387F) and the corresponding single Data presented in the table are I/Imax. Imax is the current recorded under
tyrosine mutants (Y1105F and Y3187F) (Table 1). nominally zinc-free conditions (in the presence of 10 mM tricine without
added zinc). Number of observations is indicated in parentheses. *significant
NR2A(Y1267F,Y1387F) behaved like Y1267F, with a reduced difference (p<0.05) compared to wild type by ANOVA and Newman-Keuls
apparent zinc affinity and lack of modulation by Src (Table 1). post-hoc test.

188 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 5. EDTA and Src increase the time constant of the


slower component of NMDA currents. (a) Typical cur-
a b Control EDTA

rent responses from an outside-out patch expressing


NR1/NR2A receptors. A brief glutamate-concentration
jump (10 ms, to 100 M) was applied to the patches.
Saturating glycine (1030 M) was present in all solu-
tions. Traces shown are averaged from 25 consecutive
responses for control and 20 responses for EDTA. The
top traces in (a) and (b) show the glutamate application
time. (b) The decay of NMDA-receptor-mediated cur-
rents was fitted with two exponential components
c
(smooth lines). The raw current trace is scaled to the
same size to illustrate the change of relaxation time con-
stant. (c) EDTA and Src increased the time constant of

1 (ms)

2 (ms)
the slower exponential component significantly (p<0.05,
paired t-test for EDTA, unpaired t-test for Src), whereas
the time constant for the faster exponential component
is not changed. p control, l EDTA, g Src.
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

Control EDTA Src Control EDTA Src

tor currents in outside-out patches exposed to brief pulses (10 mechanism for other NMDA-receptor subunits? To address this
ms) of glutamate (with submillisecond solution-exchange rates question, we examined the effects of Src on zinc sensitivity of
at the open tip 27). Under such conditions, two exponential recombinant NR1-1a/NR2B receptors. Zinc inhibits NR1/NR2B
components were still needed to fit the decay of the macro- receptors with an IC50 value of 4.6 M under control conditions
scopic NMDA receptor currents. The two averaged time con- in HEK293 cells (Fig. 6a and b). In the presence of c-Src (30 units
stants were 36.6 5.3 and 79.8 8.9 ms without EDTA and per ml), the IC50 was shifted to 17.6 M. Thus, NMDA receptors
Src (n = 9). Inclusion of EDTA increased the peak of macro- comprised of NR2A or NR2B subunits are modulated by tyrosine
scopic NR1/NR2A currents recorded from outside-out patch- kinase Src in a similar way, through a reduction of zinc sensitivity.
es by 63 30% (n = 9, p<0.05, paired t-test), which is
consistent with our whole-cell data as well as a previous report Discussion
that Src increases open probability6. In addition, EDTA signif- Taken together, these data suggest that Src potentiates recombi-
icantly increased the slower relaxation time constant to 130 nant NR1/NR2A-receptor function by reducing tonic inhibition
19 ms, whereas the faster relaxation time constant remained of these receptors by extracellular zinc. Furthermore, Src also
unchanged with a mean of 38.9 6.0 (Fig. 5). Interestingly, Src reduces zinc sensitivity of NR1/NR2B receptors. Thus, reduction of
induced a similar selective change in the slower relaxation time zinc inhibition by Src could be a widely utilized mechanism for
constant. The averaged time constants in the presence of Src the modulation of NMDA receptor function in the brain. Our data
were 42.0 4.1 and 117.7 9.5 ms (n = 7). Because the are consistent with biochemical observations that both NR2A and
amount of calcium influx during an EPSC is determined by NR2B are phosphorylated by the Src family of tyrosine kinases8,28
the charge integral (Q = iA i i) of the NMDA current, the because they show that receptors comprised of either the NR2A
resulting increase of synaptic calcium influx could be ampli- or NR2B subunit are modulated functionally by Src. The selective
fied by as much as 2.3-fold by Src. These observations indicate potentiation of NR1/NR2A receptors by Src in HEK293 cells is
that Src and EDTA change the NMDA-receptor channel kinet- due to the high zinc sensitivity of NR1/NR2A receptors and the
ics in similar ways, consistent with our hypothesis that Src ambient zinc contamination in the recording solutions that caus-
potentiates NMDA receptors by relieving tonic zinc inhibition. es them to be tonically inhibited. By contrast, receptors comprised
of NR1/NR2B are not potentiated by Src in HEK cells because their
SRC REDUCES ZINC SENSITIVITY OF NR1/NR2B RECEPTORS relatively lower sensitivity to zinc prevents tonic inhibition.
Is the reduction of zinc sensitivity by Src unique for receptors con- Zinc is actively accumulated in certain nerve terminals in a
taining NR2A subunits, or is it a more generalized regulatory region-specific manner and can be released into synaptic clefts at

Fig. 6. Src reduces zinc sensitivity a Control Src b


of NR1/NR2B receptors. (a) The
effects of zinc (3 and 10 M) on
NR1/NR2B-receptor currents
I/Imax

evoked by agonists (100 M gluta-


mate, 30 M glycine) from repre-
[Zn2+](M) [Zn2+](M)
sentative HEK 293 cells with or
without inclusion of Src in the
patch pipette solution (V h, -50 mV). (b) Src shifts the IC 50 for the high-affinity, voltage-
independent zinc site of NR1/NR2B receptors. Each point on the curve is averaged data
from 35 cells. The IC 50 is 4.6 without Src and 17.6 M with Src. P control, L Src. [Zn2+] (M)

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 189


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

concentrations up to 0.1 mM14. The reported ambient zinc con- surface loop, seems to reduce zinc sensitivity in a manner that
centration in the brain varies from 150 nM to more than 2 M22. can be mimicked by extracellular polyamines. In addition, muta-
Assuming ambient zinc levels of 1 M, more than 70% of tions of acidic residues in the NR1 subunit that reduce proton
NR1/NR2A receptors would be inhibited, compared to 25% of sensitivity similarly reduce zinc sensitivity. One possible expla-
NR1/NR2B receptors. The native NMDA receptors in the brain nation for these (and by analogy Srcs) effects is that they may be
most likely include both NR2A and NR2B subunits. If, as seems due to reduced electronegativity of the electron-donating residues
likely, ambient zinc concentrations are sufficient (in excess of 1 that form the zinc coordination site, which would reduce the abil-
M22) to tonically inhibit NMDA receptors containing NR2A, then ity of these residues to coordinate zinc. Such an effect could occur
such receptors might serve as as marginally functional receptors by inductive electron withdrawal through portions of the
whose full function can be restored through activation of tyrosine polypeptide chain secondary to Src binding or phosphorylation.
kinases such as Src29. If ambient zinc concentrations are insuffi- Alternatively, Src could change the channel properties in such a
cient to cause significant tonic inhibition, Src could still alter the way that occupation of the zinc site is less likely to force closure of
responsiveness of NMDA receptors to synaptically released zinc. an open channel, requiring a greater probability of zinc occu-
The C-terminal region of NR2A is critical for modulation by pancy to inhibit the channel, which might appear as a lower IC50.
Src. A C-terminal deletion mutant of NR2A is not potentiated In either case, the effects of Src and tyrosine phosphorylation
by Src in HEK293 cells. Mutant mice expressing NR2A with the need to be transmitted from intracellularly accessible residues to
C-terminal truncation have impaired synaptic plasticity. We have extracellular residues. Clearly additional functional as well as
identified three tyrosine residues in the C-terminal region of structural information will be required to ultimately understand
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

NR2A that are critical for the modulation of NR1/NR2A recep- Srcs effects on zinc modulation.
tors by Src. The Y1105 and Y1387 residues function as predict- Finally, by demonstrating that potentiation of the NMDA-
ed for potential tyrosine phosphorylation sites, which is that their receptor current by Src reduces the apparent zinc affinity of
mutation to phenylalanine causes no apparent changes by itself NMDA receptors, we link two modulatory sites of NMDA
but blocks the reduction of zinc affinity by Src. Because both receptors that have been previously thought to be indepen-
these tyrosine residues are conserved between NR2A and NR2B, dent. Our data provide further support for the convergence
it is tempting to speculate that phosphorylation of the corre- of different NMDA receptor modulatory systems that has
sponding tyrosine residues in NR2B is responsible for reducing become evident recently13,21,24,31.
the apparent zinc affinity of NR1/NR2B receptors. The function
of the third tyrosine residue, Y1267, is peculiar. Mutation of Methods
Y1267 to phenylalanine results in a receptor with reduced zinc SITE-DIRECTED MUTAGENESIS. All of the cDNAs used in this study except a
affinity. This tyrosine residue and the small stretch of peptide mutant of NR1-1b and NR2B (pCDNAI/amp, Invitrogen) were sub-
surrounding it are unique to NR2A. We do not know the mech- cloned into pCIneo vector (Promega). Site-directed mutagenesis was
anism behind this change and can only speculate at this point. done with Pfu DNA polymerase (Stratagene) to linearly replicate the
One possibility is that this residue is phosphorylated constitu- parental strand with desired mismatch incorporated into the primer.
Methylated parental DNA template was then degraded with Dpn I. The
tively and may be involved in interactions with the SH2 domain
nicked double-stranded mutant DNA was transformed into E. coli. The
of Src or other proteins. The C-terminal domains of NMDA nicks in the plasmid were repaired after transformation. Colonies were
receptor subunits are larger in size than those of other receptor selected by screening for a silent mutation that introduces a new restric-
subunits and may be involved in complex interactions with other tion site. The mutations were verified by didioxy sequencing through
proteins in the postsynaptic density and cytoskeleton. One exam- both strands in the region of the mutation.
ple of such an interaction is the reduction of NMDA-receptor TRANSFECTION OF HEK CELLS. HEK293 cells (CRL 1573; American Type
activity by direct binding of calmodulin to NR1 subunits, which Culture Collection, Rockville, Maryland) were maintained at 37C and
inactivates NMDA receptors30. Little is known about the possible 5% CO2 in DMEM supplemented with L-glutamine (200 M), sodium
proteinprotein interactions of the C-terminal regions of NR2 pyruvate (100 M), penicillin/streptomycin (100 units per ml), and 10%
subunits other than their interaction with the PSD95 family of fetal bovine serum. Low-confluence cells were transfected by calcium-
proteins. Although the mechanism by which mutation of Y1267 phosphate precipitation32. Cells were cotransfected with a mixture con-
reduces zinc sensitivity of NR1/NR2A receptors needs to be taining NR1, NR2 and green fluorescent protein33 plasmids (1, 2, and
explored, our data do show that its lack of tonic inhibition due 0.3 g per 12 mm diameter coverslip, respectively). After transfection,
200 M D-AP5 was added to the culture medium.
to reduced zinc sensitivity occludes Src potentiation, supporting
our hypothesis that Src potentiates NMDA-receptor function by EXPRESSION OF NMDA RECEPTORS IN XENOPUS OOCYTES. cRNA was synthe-
reducing tonic zinc inhibition. sized from linearized template cDNA according to manufacturers spec-
From a structural perspective, our results suggest that tyro- ification (Ambion). The quality of synthesized cRNA was assessed by gel
electrophoresis and quantified by spectroscopy and gel electrophoresis.
sine phosphorylation and/or Src binding on the cytosolic C-ter-
Preparation of oocytes and injection of cDNAs coding for wild-type and
minal domain of NR2A alter the apparent affinity of an mutant NMDA receptors were performed as described21.
extracellular zinc binding site. Although structural information
BUFFERED ZINC SOLUTIONS. The tricine-buffered zinc solutions used to
will ultimately be required to understand the interaction between
obtain the zinc doseresponse curves were prepared according to the
Src and the zinc site, several possible explanations deserve men- empirically established binding constant18 105 M, by adding into 10 mM
tion. Conformational constraints imposed by Src binding and/or tricine (pKa, 8.15) the following concentrations of zinc (in M): 0.26,
tyrosine phosphorylation (the two events may occur separately) 0.78, 2.6, 7.8, 26, 77.5, and 254. The corresponding estimated concen-
may specifically reduce the affinity of zinc by altering the geom- trations of free zinc for HEK recording at pH 7.4 were calculated with
etry of the coordination site. Alternatively, Src may change other WINMAXC34 and BAD35, and were (in nM) 2.3, 6.88, 22.3, 68.8, 223,
aspects of the NMDA-channel properties that perturb the bind- 688, 2230, respectively.
ing of zinc. Several modulators or mutations21 that modify zinc VOLTAGE-CLAMP RECORDINGS FROM XENOPUS OOCYTES. Two-electrode volt-
sensitivity have previously been described. For example, expres- age-clamp recordings were made as described21. Briefly, oocytes were
sion of NR1 alternative exon 5, which encodes a highly charged perfused with a solution comprised of (in mM) 90 NaCl, 1 KCl, 10

190 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

HEPES and 0.5 BaCl2 at pH7.4, and held under voltage clamp at -30 to - 12. Peters, S., Koh, J. & Choi, D.W. Zinc selectively blocks the action of N-
40 mV. Electrodes were filled with 300 mM KCl and had resistance of methyl-D-aspartate on cortical neurons. Science 236, 589593 (1987).
13. Traynelis, S.T., Hartley, M. & Heinemann, S.F. Control of proton sensitivity
110 M. Solution exchange was computer controlled through an eight- of the NMDA receptor by RNA splicing and polyamines. Science 268,
valve manifold. 873876 (1995).
WHOLE-CELL AND OUTSIDE-OUT RECORDINGS FROM HEK293 CELLS. Patch- 14. Smart, T.G., Xie, X. & Krishek, B.J. Modulation of inhibitory and excitatory
amino acid receptor ion channels by zinc. Prog. Neurobiol. 42, 393441
clamp recording in the whole-cell configuration and outside-out patch (1994).
recording were done as described36 with Axopatch 200A or 200B ampli- 15. Mayer, M.L., Vyklicky, L. Jr & Westbrook, G.L. Modulation of excitatory
fier. Recording electrodes (512 M) were filled with (in mM) 140 Cs- amino acid receptors by group IIB metal cations in cultured mouse
gluconate, 5 HEPES, 4 NaCl, 2 MgCl2, 0.5 CaCl2, 1 ATP, 0.3 GTP and 5 hippocampal neurones. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 415, 329350 (1989).
16. Christine, C.W. & Choi, D.W. Effect of zinc on NMDA receptor-mediated
BAPTA (pH 7.4). The recording chamber was continually perfused with channel currents in cortical neurons. J. Neurosci. 10, 108116 (1990).
recording solution composed of (in mM) 150 NaCl, 10 HEPES, 1.0 CaCl2, 17. Legendre, P. & Westbrook, G.L. The inhibition of single N-methyl-D-
3 KCl and 20 mM mannitol. Glutamate (20100 M) and glycine (1030 aspartate-activated channels by zinc ions on cultured rat neurones. J. Physiol.
M) were applied using a multibarrel pipette driven by a piezo-electric (Lond.) 429, 429449 (1990).
bimorph (exchange time under 0.5 ms) as described27. In some experi- 18. Paoletti, P., Ascher, P. & Neyton, J. High-affinity zinc inhibition of NMDA
NR1-NR2A receptors. J. Neurosci. 17, 57115725 (1997).
ments, recombinant human c-Src (Upstate Biotech.) was added into 19. Williams, K. Separating dual effects of zinc at recombinant N-methyl-D-
internal solution to a final concentration of 530 units per ml. aspartate receptors. Neurosci. Lett. 215, 912 (1996).
20. Chen, N., Moshaver, A. & Raymond, L.A. Differential sensitivity of
DATA ANALYSIS AND STATISTICS. All pooled data were expressed as mean recombinant N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor subtypes to zinc inhibition.
standard error. Statistical comparisons were done with unpaired Stu- Mol. Pharmacol. 51, 10151023 (1997).
dents t-test unless otherwise stated. To obtain IC50 values, pooled zinc 21. Traynelis, S.F., Burgess, M.F., Zheng, F., Lyuboslavsky, P. & Powers, J.L.
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

inhibition data were fitted with least-squares criterion (SigmaPlot) to Control of voltage-independent zinc inhibition of NMDA receptors by the
equation I/Imax = (1-a)/(1+([Zn2+]/IC50)n)+a where n is the Hill slope NR1 subunit. J. Neurosci. (in press).
22. Bogden, J.D., Troiano, R.A. & Joselow, M.M. Copper, zinc, magnesium, and
and a is the residual response. (n and a were not constrained.) If the fit- calcium in plasma and cerebrospinal fluid of patients with neurological
ting algorithm returned a value less than 0.05 for a (our estimated limit diseases. Clin. Chem. 23, 485489 (1977).
for detection), we fixed a to 0 and refit the zinc inhibition data to equa- 23. Li, C., Peoples, R.W. & Weight, F.F. Proton potentiation of ATP-gated ion
tion I/Imax = 1/(1+([Zn2+]/IC50)n) (as in Fig. 6). channel responses to ATP and Zn2+ in rat nodose ganglion neurons. J.
Neurophysiol. 76, 30483058 (1996).
24. Sullivan, J.M. et al. Identification of two cysteine residues that are required
Acknowledgements for redox modulation of the NMDA subtype of glutamate receptor. Neuron
We thank S.F.Heinemann for NR1 and NR2B cDNAs, N. Nakanishi for NR2A 13, 929936 (1994).
25. Kemp, B.E. & Pearson, R.B. Protein kinase recognition sequence motifs.
cDNA and M. Chalfie, M. Mayer and P. Seeburg for green fluorescent protein Trends Biochem. Sci. 15, 342346 (1990).
plasmid. We also thank P. Ascher, J. Neyton and I. Mody for critically reading 26. Edmonds, B. & Colquhoun, D. Rapid decay of averaged single-channel
the manuscript. NMDA receptor activations recorded at low agonist concentration. Proc. R.
Soc. Lond. B 250, 279286 (1992).
27. Traynelis, S.T. & Wahl, P. Control of rat GluR6 glutamate receptor open
RECEIVED 11 MARCH: ACCEPTED 22 MAY 1998 probability by protein kinase A and calcineurin. J. Physiol. 503, 513531
(1997).
28. Lau, L.-F. & Huganir, R.L. Differential tyrosine phosphorylation of N-
1. ODell, T.J., Kandel, E.R. & Grant, S.G.N. Long-term potentiation in the methyl-D-aspartate receptor subunits. J. Biol. Chem. 270, 2003620041
hippocampus is blocked by tyrosine kinase inhibitors. Nature 353, 558560 (1995).
(1991). 29. Dikic, I., Tokiwa, G., Lev, S., Courtneidge, S.A. & Schlessinger, J. A role for
2. Grant, S.G.N. et al. Impaired long-term potentiation, spatial learning, and Pyk2 and Src in linking G-protein-coupled receptors with MAP kinase
hippocampal development in fyn mutant mice. Science 258, 19031910 (1992). activation. Nature 383, 547550 (1996).
3. Lu, Y.M., Roder, J.C., Davidow, J. & Salter, M.W. Src activation in the induction 30. Ehlers, M.D., Zhang, S., Bernhardt, J.P. & Huganir, R.L. Inactivation of
of long-term potentiation in CA1 hippocampal neurons. Science 279, NMDA receptors by direct interaction of calmodulin with the NR1 subunit.
13631368 (1998). Cell 84,745755 (1996).
4. Wang, Y.T. & Salter, M.W. Regulation of NMDA receptors by tyrosine kinases 31. Chen, L. & Huang, L.-Y.M. Protein kinase C reduces Mg2+ block of NMDA-
and phosphatases. Nature 369, 233235 (1994). receptor channels as a mechanism of modulation. Nature 356, 521523
5. Khr, G. & Seeburg, P.H. Subtype-specific regulation of recombinant NMDA (1992).
receptor-channels by protein tyrosine kinases of the Src family. J. Physiol. 32. Chen, C. & Okayama, H. High-efficiency transformation of mammalian cells
(Lond.) 492, 445452 (1996). by plasmid DNA. Mol. Cell. Biol. 7, 27452752 (1987).
6. Yu, X.M., Askalan, R., Keil, G.J. & Salter, M.W. NMDA channel regulation by 33. Chalfie, M., Tu, Y., Euskirchen, G., Ward, W.W. & Prasher, D.C. Green
channel-associated protein tyrosine kinase Src. Science 275, 674678 (1997). fluorescent protein as a marker for gene expression. Science 263, 802805
7. Sprengel, R. et al. Importance of the intracellular domain of NR2 subunits for (1994).
NMDA receptor function in vivo. Cell 92, 279289 (1998). 34. Bers, D., Patton, C. & Nuccitelli, R. A practical guide to the preparation of Ca
8. Miyakawa, T. et al. Fyn-kinase as a determinant of ethanol sensitivity: buffers. Methods Cell Biol. 40, 329 (1994).
Relation to NMDA-receptor function. Science 278, 698701 (1997). 35. Brooks, S.P.J. & Storey, K.B. Bound and determined: a computer program for
9. Nowak, L., Bregestovski, P., Ascher, P., Herbert, A. & Prochiantz, A. making buffers of defined ion concentrations. Anal. Biochem. 201, 119126
Magnesium gates glutamate-activated channels in mouse central neurones. (1992).
Nature 307, 462465 (1984). 36. Hamill, O., Marty, A., Neher, E., Sakmann, B. & Sigworth, F. Improved patch-
10. Mayer, M.L., Westbrook, G.L. & Guthrie, P.B. Voltage-dependent block by Mg2+ clamp techniques for high-resolution current recording from cells and cell-
of NMDA responses in spinal cord neurones. Nature 309, 261263 (1984). free membrane patches. Pflgers Arch. 391, 85100 (1981).
11. Westbrook, G.L. & Mayer, M.L. Micromolar concentrations of Zn2+ 37. Emini, E.A., Hughes, J.V., Perlow, D.S. & Boger, J. Induction of hepatitis A
antagonize NMDA and GABA responses of hippocampal neurons. Nature virus-neutralizing antibody by a virus-specific synthetic peptide. J. Virol. 55,
328, 640643 (1987). 836839 (1985).

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 191


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Multiple kinetic components of


exocytosis distinguished by
neurotoxin sensitivity
Tao Xu1, Thomas Binz2, Heiner Niemann2 and Erwin Neher1

1 Department of Membrane Biophysics, Max-Planck-Institute for Biophysical Chemistry, Am Fassberg 11, D-37077, Gttingen, Germany
2 Department of Biochemistry, Medizinische Hochschule, D-30625, Hannover, Germany
Correspondence should be addressed to T.X. (txu@gwdg.de)

The secretion of synaptic and other vesicles is a complex process involving multiple steps. Many mol-
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

ecular components of the secretory apparatus have been identified, but how they relate to the
different stages of vesicle release is not clear. We examined this issue in adrenal chromaffin cells,
where capacitance measurements and amperometry allow us to measure vesicle fusion and
hormone release simultaneously. Using flash photolysis of caged intracellular calcium to induce exo-
cytosis, we observed three distinct kinetic components to vesicle fusion, of which only two are
related to catecholamine release. Intracellular dialysis with botulinum neurotoxin E, D or C1 or
tetanus-toxin light chains abolishes the catecholamine-related components, but leaves the third
component untouched. Botulinum neurotoxin A, which removes nine amino acids from the
carboxy(C)-terminal end of SNAP-25, does not eliminate catecholamine release completely, but
slows down both catecholamine-related components. Thus we assign a dual role to SNAP-25 and
suggest that its nine C-terminal amino acids are directly involved in coupling the calcium sensor to
the final step in exocytosis.

Functional studies on secretory cells have supplied ample evi- characterized two kinetic components that were differentially
dence that synaptic and other secretory vesicles can exist in affected by clostridial neurotoxins. We compared the capaci-
distinct functional states14. Typically, only a fraction of all tance signal with simultaneously measured catecholamine
vesicles of a secretory cell can be released in a certain time release, and thereby identified a third very prominent kinet-
window, independent of the strength of a given stimulus. ic component, which apparently was not related to cate-
These vesicles in a specific release-ready state are termed a cholamine release, and which was not affected by toxins.
pool. The question arises whether pools defined in this or Together, these data suggest possible relationships between
other ways can be associated with well defined macromolecu- late steps in the release process and their biochemically defined
lar complexes of synaptic proteins, which have recently been molecular counterparts.
characterized biochemically57. Some of the proteins of these
complexes, such as SNAP-25, synaptobrevin and syntaxin, are Results
the targets of very specific proteolytic neurotoxins, which MULTIPLE COMPONENTS OF EXOCYTOSIS IN CHROMAFFIN CELLS
cleave the proteins at unique sites810. These proteins are high- To study the kinetic components of secretion, we employed
ly susceptible to toxin attack in monomeric form, whereas they fast-resolution capacitance measurements to estimate the secre-
are protected to various degrees in macromolecular complex- tory response after spatially homogeneous elevation of inter-
es11. This property and differences between the toxins actions nal calcium concentration ([Ca2+]i) by photorelease of caged
may allow us to link certain functional pools to some of the calcium, nitrophenyl-EGTA. Membrane capacitance (Cm) is
biochemically defined complexes by their toxin susceptibili- proportional to the surface area of the cell, and it increases
ty. Indeed, different kinetic components are impaired differ- when secretory vesicles fuse with the plasma membrane 16,17.
entially by toxins12,13. Also, botulinum neurotoxin type A may In most of our measurements, we used the following protocol
be special among the clostridial neurotoxins, as its action in (Fig. 1a). After ten minutes of whole-cell dialysis with either
some preparations can be overcome by strong stimuli14,15. toxin-free or toxin-containing pipette solutions, we first gave
For a precise dissociation of functional steps and their cor- low-intensity flashes (usually two) to generate small [Ca2+]i
relation with molecular data, it would be desirable to study jumps to about 20 M. This readily elicited secretory respons-
exocytosis at fast resolution, as the final steps in the neurose- es (Fig. 1b). When there was a clear indication that secretion
cretory pathway are known to be fast, and most likely neuro- was depressed, we then gave strong flashes to elevate [Ca 2+]i
toxins act at later steps. We therefore studied catecholamine to higher values (over 100 M). Intervals between the flashes
secretion from bovine chromaffin cells using capacitance mea- were 120 seconds. In response to the first flash, there was always
surement as a fast assay of exocytosis combined with flash a robust Cm increase with two clearly distinct phases, which
photolysis of caged calcium as a fast and strong stimulus. We we call the exocytic burst 18 and the slow component. Our

192 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 1. Multiple secretory

i(M)
a A 100

[Ca2+]]i(M)
components in response to
2+ 50

2+
different [Ca ]i levels.

Cm(pF) [Ca
0
(a) Overview of the experi-

Gs(nS) Cm(pF)
ment. Sample responses
from one cell are illustrated. 400

Gm(nS) Gs(nS)
The individual traces are 200
intracellular free calcium 0
1.0
concentration ([Ca2+]i) as

Imon(pA) Gm(nS)
0.5
measured by furaptra, mem-
0.0
brane capacitance (Cm),
Imon(pA)
0

series conductance of the -20


-40
equivalent circuit (Gs),
membrane conductance 0 200 400 600 800 1000
Time
Time(s)
(s)
(Gm) and membrane current
(Imon). Arrows indicate b
Cm(pF)

time points, where flashes


are given. They are followed
by gaps in the record, during
[Ca2+]i(M) IAMP(pA)
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

which data sampling


switched to a high-resolu-
tion mode. During ten min-
utes of whole-cell dialysis,
no loading transient was
seen in NP-EGTA-containing
internal solution. The Cm
Time (s) Time (s) Time (s) Time (s)
traces remained flat until the
onset of flashes (arrows).
The flashes were given every 120 s. (b) Fast-resolution recordings of Cm, amperometric current and [Ca 2+]i in response to individual
flashes in (a) are displayed. The flashes were triggered at 200 ms in each trace.

working hypothesis is that the exocytic burst represents those nentials. One had an amplitude of 404 76 fF and a time con-
granules that are in a release-ready state and require only ele- stant of 1.1 0.2 s. The second exponential had a time con-
vation of [Ca2+]i for exocytosis. Subsequently, granules have stant of 6.4 0.9 s and 361 62 fF in amplitude (n = 12). This
to undergo slower steps of recruitment to the release-ready large membrane increase, which constituted roughly 10% of
pool before they can exocytose. Therefore, further capacitance the total cell membrane, was not associated with cate-
increase occurs at a much slower rate (see Discussion for details cholamine release (see Figs. 1b and 6). Also, the time constant
and a model). In 84 experiments, exocytic bursts were evoked did not change with [Ca2+]i (Fig. 2b). It rather seemed that
at [Ca2+]i averaging 29.3 18.9 M (mean SD). The time Cm increases of this kind were elicited whenever [Ca 2+ ] i
course could be fitted by double exponentials with average time exceeded a certain threshold.
constants of 62.6 5.9 ms (n = 44 of 84) and 295.6 23.1 ms
(n = 76 of 84), and average amplitudes of 368 40 fF and 302 ATP DEPENDENCE OF CALCIUM -TRIGGERED EXOCYTOSIS
27 fF, respectively. This is similar to previously reported val- To test the ATP dependence of different exocytic components,
ues19 except for somewhat slower kinetics. The slow compo- we dialyzed the cytosol of chromaffin cells with 80% calcium-
nent showed a time constant in the range of ten seconds, but loaded NP-EGTA internal solution containing
this is probably an underestimate, as capacitance at late times nonhydrolyzable ATP analogs, either adenosine 5-[,-meth-
may be reduced by endocytosis and may be curtailed by a slow ylene]triphosphate (AMP-PCP) or ,-Imidoadenosine 5-
decline in [Ca2+]i. The amperometric current, which remained triphosphate (AMP-PNP), via the patch pipette. While the
on a plateau level for up to ten seconds in some cases, also sug- basal [Ca2+]i was kept at 100300 nM, too low to support exo-
gests that the time constant of this slow component may be cytosis, ATP was washed out of the cell and replaced by the
over ten seconds. The increase in Cm always was accompanied analogs during the first five minutes of perfusion. Subse-
by two phases of catecholamine release as monitored by car- quently no responses could be elicited by flash photolysis, nei-
bon fibers (Fig. 2a). It should be noted that the faster compo- ther when secretion was measured by Cm nor by amperometry
nent of the exocytic burst was also accompanied by (Fig. 3a; n = 28 cells in paired experiments). These data com-
catecholamine release (Fig. 2a, inset). plement the finding that the release capability of permeabi-
Following flashes, [Ca2+]i dropped back to basal values at lized cells can be increased (primed) by preincubation with
times longer than ten seconds. Second flashes in a given exper- ATP and diminished (deprimed) by preincubation in the
iment usually elicited very small Cm increases and only rarely absence of ATP1. Our results differ from a report that MgATP-
were accompanied by amperometric spikes, which might sug- independent exocytosis remained undiminished for even six
gest the depletion of a vesicle pool during first flashes. How- minutes2, although that experiment was done at a lower tem-
ever, when [Ca2+]i was raised above 100 M in third flashes, perature than ours. The authors suggested that this form of
again an intense Cm increase was observed (Fig. 2b). This low- depriming may require magnesium, which was absent in their
calcium-affinity Cm component increased with two expo- experiments. We therefore performed additional experiments

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 193


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 2. Kinetic analysis of multiple


using an internal solution without a Cm components. (a) An example
magnesium and DM-nitrophen to
of secretory kinetics at low
chelate magnesium. The free con-

Cm (pF)
[Ca2+]i (27 M). The Cm trace is
centration of magnesium in this reasonably fitted by two expo-
solution was estimated to be less nentials (superimposed thick
than 10 nM. Again no secretion was dashed line) with time constants
observed in the presence of 2 mM of 90 ms and 6.29 s. The corre-
AMP-PCP (data not shown). Thus sponding amperometric current
we conclude that magnesium does also shows a double exponential

IAMP (pA)
not matter for this effect. decay. The onset of secretion
We also used AMP-PNP to from 0.2 to 1 s (boxed area) is
replace ATP. The effect of AMP-PNP expanded in the inset. (b) Kinetic
was weaker than that of AMP-PCP analysis of Cm response to high
(Fig. 3b). In some cells, AMP-PNP [Ca2+]i. One example of Cm
only partially blocked exocytosis, Time (s) increase at [Ca2+]i of 287 M is
whereas in other cells AMP-PNP shown in the left panel.
showed strong inhibition of exocy- b The Cm trace is reason-
tosis. However, AMP-PNP com- ably fitted by two expo-
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

pletely blocked the secretion elicited nentials (superimposed


Cm (pF)

by a second flash, suggesting that thick dashed line) with

fast (s)
ATP is necessary for the refilling of time constants of 0.82 s
and 4.15 s. The right panel
vesicles (Fig. 3c).
displays a summary of the
Our results confirm the impor-
faster time constants of
tance of ATP in secretion. Although the Cm increases versus
replacement of ATP by the nonhy- the [Ca2+]i levels from 58
drolyzable analogs AMP-PCP and [Ca2+]i (M) experiments. The dashed
Time (s)
AMP-PNP completely blocked both line is the mean value.
the exocytic burst and the slow exocy-
tosis within five minutes, it is unlikely
that ATP hydrolysis is involved in the
exocytic burst for the following reasons. Experiments using the Complexes of synaptobrevin, syntaxin and SNAP-25
caged calcium DM-Nitrophen have shown that exocytosis can (termed SNARE complexes) are found in synaptic vesicle
readily be induced earlier in a whole-cell recording in the com- membrane in vitro 24,25 , and such ternary complexes are
plete absence of free magnesium and in a nominally ATP-free reversibly disassembled by treating synaptic vesicles with ATP-
solution2,4,18. This conclusion is in line with studies on perme- NSF and SNAPs25. It is therefore possible that, in living cells,
abilized cells2021 and with recent investigations showing that most of the SNARE complexes are continuously formed on
the action of N-ethylmaleimide fusion protein (NSF) is required the vesicle and continuously disassembled because of the activ-
before contact of vesicle and target membrane22,23. ity of ATP-NSF, which might be the last step of ATP hydroly-

a b c Fig. 3. ATP dependence of


secretion. (a) The effect of 56-
[Ca2+]i (M)
[Ca2+]i (M)

[Ca2+]i (M)

minute dialysis of AMP-PCP on


secretion. Averaged [Ca2+]i lev-
els, capacitance traces and
amperometric currents in
response to the first flashes
from paired experiments in the
Control Control presence of MgATP as control
Control
(solid lines; n = 11) and in the
Cm

Cm
Cm

AMP-PNP (2 mM)
presence of 2 mM AMP-PCP
substituting for MgATP (dashed
AMP-PCP (2 mM)
AMP-PNP (2 mM) lines; n = 17) are displayed.
Flashes were triggered at 200
ms in the graph. (b) The effect
of 56 minute dialysis of AMP-
PNP on secretion. Similar to (a),
IAmp (pA)

but 2 mM AMP-PNP was used to


IAmp (pA)
IAmp (pA)

substitute for MgATP (n = 12).


Control responses from paired
experiments are from 11 cells.
(c) Similar to (b) but for the
successive second flashes. Cells
Time (s) Time (s) Time (s) are the same as those in (b).

194 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 4. The effect of BoNT/E and BoNT/A on


a b secretion. (a) BoNT/E blocks both components of
secretion at low [Ca2+]i. Averaged [Ca2+]i levels,
[Ca2+]i (M)

[Ca2+]i (M)
capacitance traces and amperometric currents in
response to the first flashes from paired experi-
ments in control (solid lines; n = 18) and in the
presence of 400 nM BoNT/E-LC (dashed lines;
n = 15) are displayed. Flashes were triggered after
Control Control 10 min of whole cell dialysis, corresponding to 200
ms in the graph. The capacitance was normalized
to its pre-flash values. (b) BoNT/A partially blocks
Cm

Cm
BoNT/A
secretion at low [Ca2+]i. Averaged [Ca2+]i levels,
capacitance traces and amperometric currents in
response to the first flashes from paired experi-
BoNT/E ments in control (solid lines; n = 24) and in the
presence of 800 nM BoNT/A-LC (dashed lines;
n = 37) are displayed. The capacitance was normal-
IAmp (pA)

IAmp (pA)

ized to its pre-flash values. Flashes were triggered


after 10 min of whole cell dialysis, corresponding
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

to 200 ms in the graph. The capacitance trace gives


the impression that the slow phase of secretion is
blocked completely, whereas the amperometry
trace reports some continuing release two sec-
Time (s)
onds following the flash. This discrepancy may be
Time (s)
due to slow endocytosis, which conceals a slow-
c release process in the capacitance trace at later
d time. (c) BoNT/A slows down the exocytic burst. The upper
solid trace is the averaged Cm response from 55 cells without
BoNT/A. It can be fitted by two exponentials (superimposed
dashed line) with a fast time constant of 51.6 ms and a slow one
(Cm/t)max (pF/s)

of 195.3 ms. The lower solid trace is the averaged Cm response


from 40 cells poisoned with 800 nM BoNT/A-LC. The onset of
Cm

capacitance increase only shows a slow exponential component


with a time constant of 207.8 ms. Superimposed dashed line is a
single exponential fit. The capacitance was normalized to its pre-
flash values. (d) BoNT/A reduced maximal rate of secretion.
Maximal rates of secretion Cm/t were measured from the
capacitance response. t was set to half the fastest time con-
Control BoNT/A stant analyzed in the exponential fit of the cell.
Time (s)

sis. We have shown that replacement of ATP by AMP-PCP linum neurotoxin (BoNT) serotypes B, D, F and G specifical-
after a period of five minutes completely blocked both com- ly cleave synaptobrevin at unique peptide bonds 810. BoNT
ponents of secretion. The in vitro results mentioned above serotypes A and E cleave SNAP-25 at two different sites locat-
offer an explanation for these findings: when ATP is replaced ed close to the carboxyl terminals29,30, whereas the targets of
by a nonhydrolyzable analogue, the continuous disassembly BoNT serotype C are syntaxin and SNAP-2531,32. To explore
of SNARE complex stops. Therefore the SNAREs on a given the role of SNARE proteins in the secretory pathway, we asked
vesicle tend to form ternary complexes and are no longer avail- whether the kinetic components described above are differ-
able for complexes linking vesicle and plasma membrane. entially influenced by different clostridial neurotoxins.
However, our data do not exclude additional requirements for If we assume there are 22,000 vesicles per chromaffin cell33,
ATP, such as in maintaining PtdIns-4,5P2 levels2628. In fact, and each vesicle contains 100 copies of synaptobrevin, the final
our finding that the exocytic burst is lost within five minutes concentration of synaptobrevin is calculated to be 3 M in a
in the absence of MgATP implies either that one of the last single chromaffin cell. To test the time course of the cleavage
steps before fusion is affected by the prolonged absence of of our recombinant light chains of clostridial neurotoxins, we
MgATP, or else that transmembrane SNARE complexes can first performed in vitro cleavage assays. The results show that,
be disintegrated without the action of NSF. in our internal solution at 32C, 330 nM TeNT-LC are capa-
ble of cleaving more than 95% of 5 M recombinant GST-
CLOSTRIDIAL NEUROTOXINS AND CATECHOLAMINE SECRETION synaptobrevin2 within three minutes, and 40 nM BoNT/E-LC
Clostridial neurotoxins are potent inhibitors of synaptic-vesi- can cleave more than 95% of 5 M recombinant SNAP-25His6
cle exocytosis in nerve terminals. It is now well established (data not shown). For a 10 M access-resistance pipette, a
that the light chains (LC) of clostridial neurotoxins act as zinc- protein with molecular weight of 50 kD, which is the size of
dependent metallopreoteases, which specifically cleave SNARE the light chains of clostridial neurotoxins, can diffuse into the
proteins. In particular, tetanus neurotoxin (TeNT) and botu- cytosol with a time constant of five minutes 34. Thus within

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 195


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 5. The effect of


a b c BoNT/C1, BoNT/D and
TeNT on secretion.
[Ca2+]i (M)

[Ca2+]i (M)

[Ca2+]i (M)
BoNT/C1 (a), BoNT/D
(b) and TeNT (c) block
both components of secre-
tion at low [Ca2+]i. The fig-
ure displays the averaged
Control [Ca2+]i levels, capacitance
Control Control traces and amperometric
currents in response to the
Cm

Cm
Cm

Tetx_LC (4 M)
first flashes from paired
BoNT/D (400 nM) experiments in the control
BoNT/C1 LC(2M)
condition (solid lines) and
in the presence (dashed
lines) of 2 M BoNT/C1-
LC (n = 7; control n = 6),
400 nM BoNT/D (n = 11;
IAMP (pA)

IAMP (pA)
IAMP (pA)

control n = 12) and 4 M


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

TeNT (n = 14; control


n = 17). Flashes were trig-
gered after 10 min of
whole-cell dialysis, corre-
sponding to 200 ms in the
Time (s) Time (s) Time (s) graph. Scaling bars, 200 fF.

five minutes, the cytosol concentration of the light chains of in BoNT/A-poisoned cells, which was that exocytic bursts were
neurotoxins would reach 36% of the pipette concentration. slowed down. The averaged Cm response from 50 control cells
Therefore, to test the role of SNAP-25 in exocytic processes, displayed an exocytic burst having two exponentials with time
we used 400 nM BoNT/E-LC in the patch pipette and waited constants of 51.6 ms and 195.3 ms. However, the response of
for ten minutes before elevation of [Ca2+]i to ensure complete 46 BoNT/A-treated cells could be well fitted by a single expo-
action of the toxin. As the action of clostridial neurotoxins are nential with a time constant of 207.8 ms, which is similar to
also steeply temperature dependent35, the experiments were the second component of the control cells (Fig. 4c). As a
conducted at 32C. result, the initial maximal rate of rise of Cm was reduced 4.7-
Under our experimental protocol, BoNT/E-LC not only fold (see Fig. 4d). When Cm responses from BoNT/A-treat-
blocked the slow phase of Cm increase, but also inhibited the ed cells were analyzed individually, 33 out of 38 flash responses
exocytic burst (Fig. 4a; see also Table 1). Next, we wanted to could be adequately fitted by a single exponential with an
assess the effects of BoNT/A, which cleaves the Q197R198 pep- overall average amplitude of 398 37 fF and a time constant
tide bond very close to the C-terminus of SNAP-25 and there- of 281 27.8 ms. This value is not different from that of the
by removes only nine amino acids, in contrast to BoNT/E, second component of exocytic bursts in control cells. More-
which cleaves the R 180 I 181 peptide bond, 17 residues over, the time constant of this slow component was more or
upstream. In vitro cleavage studies showed that BoNT/A has less constant in the [Ca2+]i range between 15 and 70 M. We
similar kinetics as BoNT/E (data not shown). However, even at therefore suggest that the truncated form of SNAP-25 cannot
800 nM, which is twice the concentration used for BoNT/E, mediate a particularly rapid interaction between calcium-sen-
BoNT/A only partially inhibited secretion (Fig. 4b). BoNT/A sor and release machinery for catecholamine release (see Dis-
reduced both the exocytic burst (Table 1) and the slow phase cussion). Alternatively, this suppression of the rapid phase of
of secretion. The capacitance trace (Fig. 4b) seems to indicate the exocytic burst by BoNT/A may represent the block of a
complete block of the slow phase. However this result cannot
be taken at face value, because endocytosis may compensate Table 1. Amplitudes of the exocytic burst in paired
for ongoing exocytosis. Indeed, the amperometric trace indi- experiments
cates a small slow exocytic component after toxin treatment.
Experiment Control (fF) Treatment (fF)
By analyzing the amperometric trace, we concluded that the
exocytic burst (approximately the integral response over the AMP-PCP/NP-EGTA 242 47 (n = 9) 21 3 (n = 16)
first two seconds) is reduced by a factor of 1.7, whereas the AMP-PCP/DM-nitrophen 340 82 (n = 4) 14 7 (n = 10)
slow component (two to ten seconds) is reduced by a factor AMP-PNP/DM-nitrophen 496 61 (n = 11) 79 27 (n = 12)
of 2.7. This strong reduction of the slow component suggests BoNT/A (800 nM) 511 38 (n = 50) 363 27 (n = 38)
that BoNT/A affects a relatively early step in the secretory BoNT/E (400 nM) 196 27 (n = 13) 37 9 (n = 14)
pathway by reducing the supply rate of release-ready granules.
BoNT/C1 (2 M) 260 37 (n = 6) 29 4 (n = 6)
The complete block by BoNT/E (as compared to the partial
one of BoNT/A) indicates that residues 181197 at the C-ter- BoNT/D (400 nM) 423 86 (n = 11) 43 11 (n = 13)
minus of SNAP-25 are essential for exocytosis, in accordance TeNT (4 M) 226 28 (n = 13) 44 8 (n = 12)
with previous studies13,36. The capacitance values were measured at 800 ms after flashes. Values given
Scrutinizing exocytic bursts revealed an additional change are mean SE.

196 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 6. Secretion at high [Ca2+]i is sensitive to ATP but not to


clostridial neurotoxins. (a) Comparison of secretory
a Control AMP-PCP

responses to high [Ca2+]i after exhaustion of exocytosis at

[Ca2+]i (M)
low [Ca2+]i. Averaged [Ca2+]i levels, capacitance traces and
amperometric currents from paired experiments in the pres-
ence of MgATP as control (n = 7) and in the presence of
2 mM AMP-PCP substituting for MgATP (n = 17) are dis-
played. The flashes were usually the third ones in a given cell.
(b) The effect of TeNT on secretory responses to high
[Ca2+]i after exhaustion of exocytosis at low [Ca2+]i.
Averaged [Ca2+]i levels, capacitance traces and amperometric

Cm
currents from paired experiments in control (n = 7) and in
the presence of 4 M TeNT-LC (n = 6) are displayed. The
flashes were usually the third ones in a given cell.

IAMP (pA)
small, more rapid and more toxin-sensitive compo- Time (s) Time (s)
nent of exocytosis, not necessarily related to cate- b
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

cholamine release. In conclusion, BoNT/A has a dual [Ca2+]i (M) Control Tetx LC
effect, one at a relatively early step and another one
modifying the exocytic burst, which is considered to
be the final step in exocytosis.
BoNT/C1 cleaves both syntaxin and SNAP-25 in
chromaffin cells 31. This toxin cleaves SNAP-25 at a
peptide bond (R 198A 199) adjacent to the BoNT/A
cleavage site (V.V. and H.N., unpublished results). To
test the role of syntaxin in different components of
Cm

secretion in chromaffin cells, we used 2 M


BoNT/C1-LC in the pipette solution. If an intact syn-
taxin were not essential for exocytosis, we would
IAMP (pA)

expect the same incomplete inhibition as seen with


BoNT/A. However, BoNT/C1 completely blocked the
secretion at low [Ca 2+]i, suggesting that syntaxin is
Time (s) Time (s)
important in the final step of exocytosis (Fig. 5a).
Synaptobrevin is the substrate for TeNT and
BoNT/B, D, F and G. In the presence of 400 nM BoNT/D-LC, We further tested this Cm increase for its sensitivity to clostridi-
capacitance and amperometric measurements (Fig. 5b) show al neurotoxins. To do so, we first depleted the Cm responses by
that both components of secretion are blocked at low [Ca2+]i. repetitive stimulations at low [Ca2+]i. Then, we elevated [Ca2+]i
Similar results were also obtained for 4 M TeNT-LC (Fig. 5c), to high levels in order to elicit this component. This way we could
suggesting that synaptobrevin also is critical for the final steps infer that this Cm increase does not represent the same pool as
of exocytosis. Comparing effective toxin concentrations shows that measured at low [Ca2+]i. This protocol also provided the
that BoNT/D is much more potent in blocking secretion than advantage that we could judge whether the neurotoxins were active
TeNT. The inactive mutants of TeNT-LC (E234Q) and in a given experiment. Even when the neurotoxins succeeded in
BoNT/C1-LC (E230A) had no effect on secretion, suggesting blocking secretion at low [Ca2+]i, they failed to block the Cm
that the blockage of clostridial neurotoxins that we observed increases at high [Ca2+]i. Figure 6b shows that TeNT-LC did not
here was due to the toxins metalloprotease activities. inhibit Cm responses at high [Ca2+]i. The same results were
It may be considered surprising that exocytosis is completely obtained with BoNT/E, D, A and C1. These results suggest the
blocked within a time window of about ten minutes by many existence of an additional type of vesicles devoid of catecholamines,
toxins that do not cleave their targets as long as those proteins which are fusion competent at [Ca2+]i over 100 M. Exocytosis of
are part of a SNARE complex, as many recent models envisage these vesicles is not sensitive to clostridial toxins but is dependent
an intact SNARE complex as part of a release-ready fusion on ATP. Above, we concluded that this intermediate component
machinery. If this is the case, our findings imply that the is elicited whenever [Ca2+]i exceeded a certain threshold. This point
respective proteins must cycle through a nonprotected state is strengthened by the toxin results reported here, as no indication
(which is the monomeric form) within a few minutes. Other- of this component was observed after toxin treatment when [Ca2+]i
wise, we would expect the exocytic burst to be toxin resistant. increases were restricted to below 70 M.
The existence of a Cm increase not related to catecholamine
ATP-DEPENDENT, TOXIN-INSENSITIVE CAPACITANCE COMPONENT release that is triggered at [Ca2+]i above 100 M is somewhat
We have shown that there is an additional large Cm increase at puzzling. The presence of vesicles other than chromaffin gran-
high [Ca2+]i that is not related to catecholamine release. This ules may be an explanation. One possibility might be that low
Cm response is dependent on ATP. When we substituted ATP [Ca2+]i mainly triggers the fusion of catecholamine-containing
with AMP-PCP, this component vanished completely (Fig. 6a). chromaffin granules, whereas high [Ca2+]i preferentially triggers
This result gives us some confidence that the signal is not mere- the exocytosis of synaptic-like microvesicles, which contain
ly an artifact of Cm measurement. acetylcholine. This postulate agrees with the general view that

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 197


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

-SNAP

NSF
BoNT/B,D,F,G
BoNT/C
t-SNAREs
1 BoNT/A,E
TeNT
v-SNARE
Morphological

Ca2+-Sensor
Docking

-SNAP
NSF Pro
MgATP Rea ductive
ssem
2 3
al

bly
tin ic

Maturation Exocytosis
ac ort

NSF 10 s 10 - 100 ms
C

?
4 5 Ca2+ 6
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

Fig. 7. Hypothetical model for the kinetic steps leading to exocytosis. Left, Morphological docking may require cortical actin, Munc18, Doc2
and similar proteins, but may not involve the interaction of v- and t-SNAREs. The 7S complex existing on the vesicle membrane prevents the
v-SNAREs from interacting with plasma membrane t-SNAREs. The 20S complex is formed after the 7S complex binds to -SNAP and NSF.
It is disassembled by ATP hydrolysis, which makes v-SNAREs available to interact with plasma membrane t-SNAREs. For simplicity, 7S com-
plexes are displayed as consisting only of one v-SNARE and one t-SNARE. In the productive reassembly of SNARE complexes between mem-
branes, the energy released by complex formation is used to put the membranes under tension and to make the vesicle release ready.
Calcium-sensor protein may interdigitate with the distal tail of the SNARE complexs coiled coil structure. A calcium-dependent conforma-
tional change might put additional torsion on the coiled-coil, forcing the two membrane anchors together and initiating fusion. The numbers
represent functionally discrete pools of vesicles.

the [Ca2+]i threshold for triggering small clear vesicles is higher granules (the readily releasable pool, RRP). This size of this pool
than that for large dense-core vesicles. However, our data are depends on [Ca2+]i preceding the stimulus and on activation of
inconsistent with this idea. Chromaffin cells express acetylcholine PKC4,40. Following the exocytic burst, release proceeds at a much
receptor channels. Therefore one might expect to observe a tran- slower rate, which is believed to represent the combined process
sient conductance following a flash if acetylcholine were released. of recruiting vesicles to the RRP. Flash photolysis of caged calci-
This was not the case, however. Furthermore, the resistance of um together with rapid assays of exocytosis, such as membrane-
this non-catecholamine Cm increase to clostridial neurotoxins capacitance measurement and amperometry, allow a detailed
suggests that the involvement of SNAREs is not necessary for the kinetic analysis of secretory responses. Our goal was to establish
fusion of the underlying vesicles. By exclusion, then, the expla- links between the distinct kinetic components of such [Ca2+]i
nation for this peculiar Cm increase seems to be that of an unspe- jump experiments and molecular processes.
cific membrane fusion triggered by high level of [Ca2+]i, which Five of the findings reported in our study bear on this issue.
the absence of ATP somehow prevents. Alternatively, this fusion First, we eliminated one of the kinetic components as a candidate
event could represent a previously undetected process that for a step in the release of catecholamines. Second, we showed
involves a toxin-resistant set of SNARE proteins, as has been sug- that five minutes of neurotoxin action in the absence of stimula-
gested for the apical route in polarized cells37,38. tion are sufficient to eliminate the exocytic burst. This implies all
of SNAP-25 or syntaxin or synaptobrevin must cycle through a
Discussion toxin-sensitive state within that time period. Thus, any toxin-pro-
In previous work, at least three late steps have been distinguished tected SNARE complex as part of a mature fusion machinery must
in the secretory pathway of neuroendocrine cells: morphological have a lifetime shorter than five minutes. Third, cleavage of SNAP-
docking, priming, and exocytosis. Various properties have been 25 by BoNT/A suppresses exocytosis only partially and manifests
assigned to the individual stages; thus, docking is ATP-dependent28, itself both at a slow (seconds to minutes) and at the fastest step
priming is dependent on both ATP and calcium20,39 and exocytosis by retarding the reactions in which SNAP-25 is involved. Fourth,
shows a steep calcuim dependence18,19. Evidence for these proper- the size of the exocytic burst is only partially affected by BoNT/A.
ties has been obtained from different laboratories by very different Fifth, the time constant of the exocytic burst after BoNT/A action
techniques and by experiments on diverse time scales. It is a ques- is not calcium dependent above 20 M.
tion of intense current research how features defined in such dif- In the following, we explain these findings in the framework
ferent ways correspond to each other, how they relate to molecularly of a specific model of exocytosis control although we are aware
defined states, and which of these states are susceptible to (or else that alternative interpretations are possible. We start with the
protected from) the proteolytic action of clostridial neurotoxins. recent proposal of a productive reassembly of SNARE complexes
Electrophysiological experiments showed that sufficiently between membranes, in which the energy released by complex
strong stimuli elicit a so-called exocytic burst, which is inter- formation is used to put the membranes under tension41. We
preted to represent complete exocytosis of a pool of release-ready incorporate this into a model similar to those previously pro-

198 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

posed2,40, including the recent finding that v-SNARE and t- down the exocytic burst and does so more at high than at low
SNAREs are in parallel orientation in the complex7 and arrive at [Ca2+]i is strong evidence that this portion of the molecule is
Fig. 7. In this model, we allow for morphological docking, for involved in mediating the action of a calcium sensor in rapid cal-
NSF action to separate SNARE complexes42, for SNARE complex cium-regulated exocytosis.
formation (productive reassembly) and for fusion. For rapid
action of calcium, the last step should be steeply dependent on
calcium and fast, such that the readily releasable pool (state 5) Methods
can be depleted rapidly within the exocytic burst. In addition, a CELL PREPARATION AND SOLUTIONS. Chromaffin cells from bovine adrenal
glands were prepared and cultured as described48. Cells were used one
step of SNARE complex maturation is considered in the model
to four days after preparation. The external bathing solutions for exper-
to allow for the finding that this pool recovers from depletion iments contained (in mM) 150 NaCl, 2.8 KCl, 2 CaCl2, 1 MgCl2, 10
(following strong stimulation) on the time scale of ten seconds43 HEPES and 2 mg per ml glucose (pH 7.2, 320 mosm). For preparing
and that its size depends on PKC and [Ca2+]i39,40. This step (from pipette solutions, we generally used 2 concentrated buffers, which con-
state 4 to state 5) may correspond to the recruitment of other tained 250 mM Cs-glutamate, 80 mM HEPES (pH 7.2). We added dif-
synaptic proteins to the complex such as the calcium-dependent ferent concentrations of NP-EGTA (gift from Dr. Ellis-Davies,
activator protein for secretion (CAPS)28 and the calcium sensor Philadelphia), fura-2 (Texas Fluorescence Labs, Austin, TX), furaptra
(possibly synaptotagmin). We also allow morphological undock- (Molecular Probes, Eugene, OR), CaCl2 or ATP for different purposes
ing on the time scale of minutes44. Reversal of the productive as indicated in the text. The resulting mixtures were diluted with dou-
reassembly step may involve NSF action. Such a model fits all of ble-distilled water for the appropriate osmolarity (310 mosm). The NP-
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

EGTA-containing internal solutions for control consisted of (in mM)


the findings summarized above. It explains why SNARE com-
84.5 Cs-glutamate, 10 NP-EGTA, 8 CaCl2, 1 MgCl2, 2 MgATP, 0.3 GTP,
plexes are not protected from clostridial toxin action because 0.5 furaptra, 27 HEPES. For the DM-nitrophen experiment, internal
NSF intermittently separates SNAREs within a time span of five solutions contained (in mM) 110 Cs-glutamate, 5 DM-nitrophen, 4
minutes, which permits proteolytic action of the toxins. Action of CaCl2, 2 MgATP, 0.3 GTP, 0.5 furaptra, 35 HEPES. The basal [Ca2+]i was
most of the toxins, with the exception of BoNT/A, would com- measured to be 100300 nM by fura-2. The pipette solution was adjust-
pletely prevent the productive reassembly and thereby prevent ed to pH 7.2 by either HCl or CsOH. All experiments were performed
the formation of a release-ready pool of vesicles. at 32-33C.
The model also provides a hint about why BoNT/A fails to elim- PHOTOLYSIS OF CAGED CALCIUM AND [CA2+]I MEASUREMENT. Flashes of ultra-
inate secretion completely and seems to act both at an early and at a violet light and fluorescence excitation light were generated as
late step. BoNT/A cleaves off SNAP-25 the nine amino acids farthest described48. To avoid any influence resulting from the loading tran-
from the plasma membrane, at the C-terminus. Truncated SNAP- sient4, we used the more calcium-selective caged compound Nitro-
25 still is able to form SNARE complexes that are disassembled by phenyl-EGTA49. The flash photolysis efficiency was also measured as
NSF45,46, but the C-terminal end of the molecule is likely to be described48 except for the following changes: we used 1 mM fura-2, 2
mM NP-EGTA and 2 mM CaCl2 during flash experiments. Small flash
involved in initial contact during productive reassembly41. Lack of
intensities were applied by adding neutral-density filters. From mea-
these nine amino acids may slow down rather than prevent this step. surements of [Ca2+]i before and immediately after flashes, we calculated
In our experiments, such a change would be reflected in a decrease the photolysis efficiency for NP-EGTA. The photolysis efficiency of a 375
of the rate of secretion at high [Ca2+]i, once the readily releasable V discharge flash for NP-EGTA was determined to be 52%. As the [Ca2+]i
pool is consumed. In other words, it would slow down the late com- should decay significantly during 10 s of Cm measurement after flash-
ponent of secretion, as we observed. The same lack of nine amino es48, we used the fluorescence excitation light to measure [Ca2+]i and to
acids may also explain the slowdown of the exocytic burst itself, if simultaneously photorelease calcium after the flashes in order to keep
the calcium sensor for exocytosis (possibly synaptotagmin) inter- [Ca2+]i more or less constant. [Ca2+]i was calculated from the fluores-
acts with the C-terminal end of the SNARE complex. Such interac- cence ratio R according to ref. 50. The calibration constants for furap-
tra measurements before and after 375 V discharge flashes were measured
tion between the C-terminus of SNAP-25 and synaptotagmin was
as described48. For 10 mM NP-EGTA, 0.5 mM furaptra, the changes of
actually shown to occur in vitro (R.R.L. Gerona & T.F.J. Martin, per- the calibration constants for a ratiometric measurement at 340 and 380
sonal communication). A specific proposal of how this might hap- nm were as follows: Rmin changed from 0.238 to 0.247, Rmax from 6.554
pen is shown in Fig. 7, referring to the finding that the ternary to 4.708, Keff from 2128 M to 2053 M.
complex of SNAREs has a coiled-coil structure41. If the calcium sen-
WHOLE-CELL PATCH CLAMP AND CAPACITANCE MEASUREMENT. Convention-
sor interdigitates with the tail of this structure, a calcium-dependent al whole-cell recordings were done with sylgard-coated 2-3 M pipettes.
conformational change might put additional torsion on the coiled Series resistance ranged from 412 M. An EPC-9 patch-clamp ampli-
coil, forcing the two membrane anchors together and initiating fier was used together with Pulse software (HEKA Electronics, Lam-
fusion. Shortening of the SNAP-25 tail would most likely reduce the brecht, Germany). Capacitance measurements used the Lindau-Neher
efficacy of the calcium sensor in transmitting the calcium signal to technique17 implemented as the sine+dc mode of the software lock-in
the SNARE complex. On the other hand, the affinity of the sensor extension of pulse software, which allowed long duration Cm measure-
itself is not expected to change (apart from possible allosteric cou- ment in single sweeps. A 800 Hz, 50 mV peak-to-peak sinusoid voltage
pling effects). In the model of Heinemann and colleagues19, such a stimulus was applied above a DC holding potential of -70 mV. Currents
change would be most conveniently represented by a reduction of were filtered at 2 kHz and sampled at 12 kHz. The capacitance traces
were imported to IGOR Pro (WaveMetrics, Inc., Lake Oswego, OR). The
the rate constant (the rate constant of exocytosis from a triply cal- analyses were conducted on a Macintosh computer using IGOR Pro.
cium-bound state of the sensor) to a value of about five per second. Unless otherwise stated, the data are given as mean SE.
Given the intrinsic dissociation constant of the calcium sensor of 13
AMPEROMETRY. Carbon-fiber electrodes were prepared from 10 m
M19 and a calcium-binding rate to the sensor of 8106 per mole per
diameter carbon fibers (Amoco performance products, Greenville,
second47, a saturation of the calcium dependence of the release-rate South Carolina) and were canulated through glass capillaries. A con-
constant would be expected for [Ca2+]i in the range 20 to 70 M, as stant voltage of 780 mV versus Ag/AgCl reference was applied to the
observed after BoNT/A treatment. electrode. The tip of the carbon-fiber electrode was gently pressed
Irrespective of the specific mechanism suggested here, the against the cell surface. The amperometric current was filtered at 3
finding that removal of nine amino acids from SNAP-25 slows kHz, sampled at 10 kHz and further digitally filtered at 1 kHz. Arti-

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 199


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

facts of amperometry due to flash irradiation were subtracted using 22. Nichols, B.J., Ungermann, C., Pelham, H.R.B., Wickner, W.T. & Hass, A.
the averaged trace for the same fiber at the end of the experiment Homotypic vacuolar fusion mediated by t- and v-SNAREs. Nature 387,
199202 (1997).
when there was no secretion. 23. Colombo, M.I., Taddese, M., Whiteheart, S.W. & Stahl, P.D. A possible
predocking attachment site for N-ethylmaleimide-sensitive fusion protein.
Insights from in vitro endosome fusion. J. Biol. Chem. 271, 1881018816
Acknowledgments (1996).
We would like to thank Dr. Ellis-Davies for samples of NP-EGTA, Drs. Corey 24. Hhne-Zell, B. & Gratzl, M. Adrenal chromaffin cells contain functionally
Smith, Reinhard Jahn and Tobias Moser for feedback on the manuscript, and different SNAP-25 monomers and SNAP-25/syntaxin heterodimers. FEBS
Lett. 394, 109116 (1996).
Frauke Friedlein and Michael Pilot for cell preparation. This work was 25. Otto, H., Hanson, P.I. & Jahn, R. Assembly and disassembly of a ternary
supported by grants from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (Nr. CHV- complex of synaptobrevin, syntaxin, and SNAP-25 in the membrane of
113/65/0) and from the European Community (Nr. CHRX-CT940500 ) to E.N. synaptic vesicles. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 94, 61976201 (1997).
26. Hay, J.C. & Martin, T.F.J. Phosphatidylinositol transfer protein required for
T.B. and H.N. were supported by the Fonds der chemischen Industrie and by the ATP-dependent priming of Ca2+-activated secretion. Nature 366, 572580
Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (Nr. IIB2-Bi660/1-1). (1993).
27. Hay, J.C. et al. ATP-dependent inositide phosphorylation required for Ca2+-
activated secretion. Nature 374, 173177 (1995).
RECEIVED 29 APRIL: ACCEPTED 23 MAY 1998 28. Martin, T.F.J. Stages of regulated exocytosis. Trends Cell Biol. 7, 271276
(1997).
29. Binz, T. et al. Proteolysis of SNAP-25 by types E and A botulinal neurotoxins.
1. Holz, R.W., Bittner, M.A., Peppers, S.C., Senter, R.A. & Eberhard, D.A. J. Biol. Chem. 269, 16171620 (1994).
MgATP-independent and MgATP-dependent exocytosis. J. Biol. Chem. 264, 30. Blasi, J. et al. Botulinum neurotoxin A selectively cleaves the synaptic protein
54125419 (1989). SNAP-25. Nature 365, 160163 (1993).
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

2. Parsons, T.D., Coorssen, J.R., Horstmann, H. & Almers, W. Docked granules, 31. Foran, P., Lawrence, G., Shone, C.C., Foster, K.A. & Dolly, J.O. Botulinum
the exocytic burst, and the need for ATP hydrolysis in endocrine cells. Neuron neurotoxin C1 cleaves both syntaxin and SNAP-25 in intact and chromaffin
15, 10851096 (1995). cells: Correlation with its blockade of catecholamine. Biochemistry 35,
3. Rosenmund, C. & Stevens, C.F. Definition of the readily releasable pool of 26302636 (1996).
vesicles at hippocampal synapses. Neuron 16, 11971207 (1996). 32. Blasi, J. et al. Botulinum neurotoxin C1 blocks neurotransmitter release by
4. Neher, E. & Zucker, R.S. Multiple calcium-dependent processes related to means of cleaving HPC-1/syntaxin. EMBO J. 12, 48214828 (1993).
secretion in bovine chromaffin cells. Neuron 10, 2130 (1993). 33. Plattner, H., Artalejo, A.R. & Neher, E. Ultrastructural organization of bovine
5. Sllner, T. et al. SNAP receptors implicated in vesicle targeting and fusion. chromaffin cell cortexAnalysis by cryofixation and morphometry of
Nature 362, 318324 (1993). aspects pertinent to exocytosis. J. Cell Biol. 139, 17091717 (1997).
6. Sdhof, T.C. The synaptic vesicle cycle: a cascade of protein-protein 34. Pusch, M. & Neher, E. Rates of diffusional exchange between small cells and a
interactions. Nature 375, 645653 (1995). measuring patch pipette. Pflgers Arch. 411, 204211 (1988).
7. Hanson, P.I. & Jahn, R. Structure and conformational changes in NSF and its 35. Poulain, B. et al. Differences in the multiple step process of inhibition by
membrane receptor complexes visualized by quick-freeze/deep-etch electron tetanus toxin and botulinum neurotoxins type A and B at aplysia synapses.
microscopy. Cell 90, 523535 (1997). Neuroscience 70, 567576 (1996).
8. Foran, P., Lawrence, G. & Dolly, J.O. Blockade by botulinum neurotoxin B of 36. Bittner, M.A. & Holz, R.W. Protein kinase C and clostridial neurotoxins
catecholamine release from adrenochromaffin cells correlates with its affect discrete and related steps in the secretory pathway. Cell. Mol. Neurobiol.
cleavage of synaptobrevin and a homologue present on the granules. 13, 649664 (1993).
Biochemistry 34, 54945503 (1995). 37. Ikonen, E., Tagaya, M., Ullrich, O., Montecucco, C. & Simons, K. Different
9. Niemann, H., Blasi, J. & Jahn, R. Clostridial neurotoxins: new tools for requirements for NSF, SNAP, and rab proteins in apical and basolateral
dissecting exocytosis. Trends Cell Biol. 4, 179185 (1994). transport in MDCK cells. Cell 81, 571580 (1995).
10. Montecucco, C. & Schiavo, G. Mechanism of action of tetanus and botulinum 38. Weimbs, T., Low, S.H., Chapin, S.J. & Mostov, K.E. Apical targeting in
neurotoxins. Mol. Microbiol. 13, 18 (1994). polarized epithelial cells: theres more afloat than rafts. Trends Cell Biol. 7,
11. Hayashi, T., Yamasaki, S., Nauenburg, S., Binz, T. & Niemann, H. Synaptic 393399 (1997).
vesicle membrane fusion complex: action of clostridial neurotoxins on 39. von Rden, L. & Neher, E. A Ca-dependent step in the release of
assembly. EMBO J. 13, 50515061 (1994). catecholamines from adrenal chromaffin cells. Science 262, 10611065
12. McMahon, H.T. et al. Tetanus and botulinum toxins type A and B inhibit (1993).
glutamate, GABA, asparate and metenkephalin release from synaptosomes: 40. Gillis, K.D., Mner, R. & Neher, E. Protein kinase C enhances exocytosis
clues to the locus of action. J. Biol. Chem. 267, 2133821343 (1992). from chromaffin cells by increasing the size of the readily releasable pool of
13. Lawrence, G.W., Foran, P., Mohammed, N., DasGupta, B.R. & Dolly, J.O. secretory granules. Neuron 16, 12091220 (1996).
Importance of two adjacent C-terminal sequences of SNAP-25 in exocytosis 41. Hanson, P.I., Heuser, J.E. & Jahn, R. Neurotransmitter releasefour years of
from intact and permeabilized chromaffin cells revealed by inhibition with SNARE complexes. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 7, 310315 (1997).
Botulinum neurotoxins A and E. Biochemistry 36, 30613067 (1997). 42. Barnard, R.J.O., Morgan, A. & Burgoyne, R.D. Stimulation of NSF ATPase
14. Dreyer, F., Rosenberg, F., Becker, C., Bigalke, H. & Penner, R. Differential activity by alpha-SNAP is required for SNARE complex disassembly and
effects of various secretagogues on quantal transmitter release from mouse exocytosis. J. Cell Biol. 139, 875883 (1997).
motor nerve terminals treated with botulinum A and tetanus toxin. Naunyn- 43. Moser, T. & Neher, E. Rapid exocytosis in single chromaffin cells recorded
Schmiedebergs Arch. Pharmacol. 335, 17 (1987). from mouse adrenal slices. J. Neurosci. 17, 23142323 (1997).
15. Capogna, M., McKinney, R.A., OConnor, V., Ghwiler, B.H. & Thompson, 44. Steyer, J.A., Horstmann, H. & Almers, W. Transport, docking and exocytosis of
S.M. Ca2+ or Sr2+ partially rescues synaptic transmission in hippocampal single secretory granules in live chromaffin cells. Nature 388, 474478 (1997).
cultures treated with botulinum toxin A and C, but not tetanus toxin. J. 45. Otto, H., Hanson, P.I., Chapman, E.R., Blasi, J. & Jahn, R. Poisoning by
Neurosci. 17, 71907202 (1997). botulinum neurotoxin A does not inhibit formation or disassembly of the
16. Neher, E. & Marty, A. Discrete changes of cell membrane capacitance synaptosomal fusion complex. Biochem. Biophys. Res. Comm. 212, 945952
observed under conditions of enhanced secretion in bovine adrenal (1995).
chromaffin cells. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 79, 67126716 (1982). 46. Hayashi, T., Yamasaki, S., Nauenburg, S., Binz, T. & Niemann, H.
17. Gillis, K. D. in Single-Channel Recording 2nd edn. (eds. Sakmann, B. & Neher, Disassembly of the reconstituted synaptic vesicle membrane fusion complex
E.) 155198 (Plenum, NewYork, 1995). in vitro. EMBO J. 14, 23172325 (1995).
18. Thomas, P., Wong, J.G., Lee, A.K. & Almers, W. A low affinity Ca2+ receptor 47. Klingauf, J. & Neher, E. Modeling buffered Ca2+ diffusion near the
controls the final steps in peptide secretion from pituitary melanotrophs. membrane: implications for secretion in neuroendocrine cells. Biophys. J. 72,
Neuron 11, 93104 (1993). 674690 (1997).
19. Heinemann, C., Chow, R.H., Neher, E. & Zucker, R.S. Kinetics of the 48. Xu, T., Naraghi, M., Kang, H. & Neher, E. Kinetic studies of Ca2+ binding and
secretory response in bovine chromaffin cells following flash photolysis of Ca2+ clearance in the cytosol of adrenal chromaffin cells. Biophys. J. 73,
caged Ca2+. Biophys. J. 67, 25462557 (1994). 532545 (1997).
20. Bittner, M.A. & Holz, R.W. Kinetic analysis of secretion from permeabilized 49. Ellis-Davies, G.C. & Kaplan, J.H. Nitrophenyl-EGTA, a photolabile chelator
adrenal chromaffin cells reveals distinct components. J. Biol. Chem. 267, that selectively binds Ca2+ with high affinity and releases it rapidly upon
1621916225 (1992). photolysis. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 91, 187191 (1994).
21. Banerjee, A., Barry, V.A., DasGupta, B.R. & Martin, T.F.J. N-Ethylmaleimide- 50. Grynkiewiez, G., Poenie, M. & Tsien. R.Y. A new generation of Ca2+
sensitive factor acts at a prefusion ATP-dependent step in Ca2+-activated indicators with greatly improved fluorescence properties. J. Biol. Chem. 260,
exocytosis. J. Biol. Chem. 271, 2022320226 (1996). 34403450 (1985).

200 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Presynaptic modulation of CA3


network activity
Kevin J. Staley, Mark Longacher, Jaideep S. Bains and Audrey Yee

Departments of Neurology and Pediatrics, University of Colorado Health Sciences Center, Box B182, 4200 East 9th Avenue, Denver, Colorado 80262, USA
Correspondence should be addressed to K.J.S. (Kevin.Staley@UCHSC.edu)

The simultaneous discharge of hippocampal CA3 pyramidal cells is a widely studied in vitro model of
physiological and pathological network synchronization. This network is rapidly activated because of
extensive positive feedback mediated by recurrent axon collaterals. Here we show that population-
burst duration is limited by depletion of the releasable glutamate pool at these recurrent synapses.
Postsynaptic inhibitory conductances further limit burst duration but are not necessary for burst ter-
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

mination. The interval between bursts in vitro depends on the rate of replenishment of releasable
glutamate vesicles and the probability of release of those vesicles at recurrent synapses. Therefore
presynaptic factors controlling glutamate release at recurrent synapses regulate the probability and
duration of synchronous discharges of the CA3 network.

Recurrent axon collaterals of CA3 pyramidal cells make extensive not necessary for burst termination. Finally, systems characterized
excitatory synapses on the dendrites of neighboring pyramidal by the amount of positive feedback present in CA3 become
cells1, so that when one pyramidal cell fires, its neighbors are pow- locked into the state favored by the feedback7,15. For example,
erfully excited2,3. This facilitates the rapid synchronization of during an action potential, depolarization of the neuronal mem-
action-potential firing in CA3 neurons that underlies the normal brane is accelerated by the positive feedback provided by volt-
electroencephalographic pattern known as hippocampal sharp age-dependent activation of depolarizing sodium conductances.
wave activity4,5 and periodic in vitro burst discharges6. This syn- The positive feedback must be removed by inactivation of the
chrony is important for activity-dependent modification of synap- depolarizing sodium conductances before inhibitory potassium
tic strength4, but the positive feedback from recurrent collaterals conductances can repolarize the membrane16.
that underlies the synchronization should produce continuous Analogous to the action potential, positive feedback in area
discharging of all CA3 neurons7, such as may occur during human CA3 could be reduced during the burst by a use-dependent lim-
temporal lobe seizures8. Instead, in hippocampal slices from nor- itation of synaptic strength at recurrent synapses. Modeling stud-
mal animals, experimental manipulations that decrease inhibi- ies demonstrate that the bursts are not sustained below a critical
tion or increase excitation 6,8 produce only brief population synaptic strength6. Synaptic strength could be decreased by recep-
discharges that resemble sharp waves4 and the pathological elec- tor desensitization or by inhibition of glutamate release via presy-
troencephalographic interictal spike pattern that indicates a naptic metabotropic glutamate receptors17. However, these effects
propensity for temporal lobe seizures8. During these discharges, may not produce the necessary rate of decrease in synaptic
CA3 neurons fire a burst of action potentials during a 50 to 200 strength1719, and use-dependent failure of synapses from CA3
millisecond depolarization that begins and ends within a few mil- to CA1 pyramidal cells is independent of these two processes20.
liseconds of the rest of the population4,6,8, followed in vitro by a Only a few neurotransmitter-containing vesicles are imme-
relatively silent period that lasts until the next burst6. What process diately available for release from hippocampal terminals.
terminates burst activity? Calcium influx during the burst trig- Anatomical studies suggest there are 2 to 36 docked glutamate
gers a potassium current that results in an afterhyperpolarization vesicles per pyramidal cell synapse21,22, and electrophysiological
that can terminate bursts of action potentials initiated by a depo- studies indicate that between 1 and 15 vesicles are available for
larizing current injection9. Thus the afterhyperpolarization, in immediate release20,2325. When this supply is exhausted, it is
conjunction with inhibitory postsynaptic conductances, is a log- replenished from a larger reserve pool23 with a time constant (at
ical mechanism for burst termination6. 36 C) of two to ten seconds20,24,25. Here we demonstrate that
Three lines of evidence, however, raise the possibility that the number of releasable glutamate vesicles and the probability of
inhibitory conductances are not the primary mechanism termi- their release regulates the positive feedback mediated by recur-
nating CA3 bursts. First, the inhibitory conductance necessary rent collateral synapses, and thereby the synchronous activity of
to counteract the excitatory input to a bursting CA3 pyramidal the CA3 network.
cell should produce a dramatic increase in membrane conduc-
tance during the course of the burst10, but studies to date do not Results
provide evidence for such a conductance increase11,12. Second, INHIBITORY CONDUCTANCES DURING CA3 POPULATION BURSTS
CA3 bursts are only modestly prolonged by experimental manip- We performed three experiments to assess the role of inhibitory
ulations that block the afterhyperpolarization and inhibitory conductances in terminating CA3 population bursts. First, we
postsynaptic -aminobutyric acid (GABA) type A13 and B14 reasoned that if CA3 population bursts are terminated by nega-
receptor conductances, indicating that these conductances are tive feedback in the form of inhibitory conductances, then the

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 201


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 1. Membrane currents


in CA3 pyramidal cells dur-
a b
ing population bursts. (a)
Top panel, whole-cell
recording of the membrane
potential of a pyramidal cell
during a CA3 population
burst induced by 6 mM
extracellular potassium
([K+]o) (resting membrane 50 ms
50 ms
potential -55 mV). Middle
25 mV
panel, in the same cell, 1 nA
1 nA
membrane currents 20 nS
recorded when the mem- Voltage clamp
brane was clamped at the
indicated potentials during
a population burst. Inset, in
a different slice, simultane-
ous recording of CA3 EC field potential
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

extracellular field potential


and the membrane current
in a pyramidal cell demon- Clamp current
strates that the initial rip-
ples in intraburst voltage
clamp currents reflect
spikes in the extracellular
field potential. Bottom
panel, estimate of input
conductance during a pop-
ulation bursts, obtained by subtracting the currents shown in the middle panel, and dividing by the difference in test potential. There
is no evidence of an inhibitory current or conductance of sufficient amplitude to terminate the burst10. Recording performed with a
potassium electrode solution. (b) Membrane currents during CA3 population bursts before and after blocking GABA A conductances
with picrotoxin. Currents were recorded using cesium and QX314 in the electrode solution. Currents at all time points during the
burst reverse at similar potentials (symbols, inset), so that there is no evidence for burst termination by a large, late inhibitory cur-
rent. Bottom panel, in the same cell, the burst is prolonged after blocking GABA A receptors, and the reversal potential of the currents
at all time points during the burst is shifted positive by 10 mV. The uniform shift in reversal potential at all time points during the burst
indicates that the GABA A conductance is already maximal at the earliest part of the burst.

input conductance of neurons participating in the burst should burst compared to just prior to a burst. We induced spontaneous
increase as these conductances become large enough to negate CA3 population bursts using 8.5 mM extracellular potassium,
excitatory input from recurrent collaterals and terminate the and recorded the number of action potentials that could be trig-
burst10. We induced CA3 population bursts by increasing the gered in pyramidal cells by a 500-millisecond depolarizing current
extracellular potassium concentration to 8.5 mM, and then esti- of the same magnitude as that recorded during spontaneous
mated the membrane conductance of CA3 pyramidal cells by bursts (1 nA; e.g. Fig. 1b). The number of action potentials elicit-
measuring the currents required to clamp the membrane at var- ed within one second after a population burst (11.53 0.3) was
ious potentials during the bursts (Fig. 1). The membrane con- not significantly different from the number elicited one second
ductance was maximal near the beginning rather than the end prior to a burst (12.44 0.4; Fig. 2a and b; n = 4 cells, 35 cur-
of the burst, and the reversal potential of the membrane currents rent injections). Thus, although the pyramidal cell firing rate
did not change substantially during the burst (n = 3), suggesting drops during prolonged depolarizing currents, the time scale over
that there was no increase in inhibitory conductances near the which this occurs is poorly correlated with burst duration (Fig.
end of the burst. Further, blocking inhibitory GABAA conduc- 2a), and bursts produce only a small decrement in the probabil-
tances with picrotoxin had the same effect on the reversal poten- ity of action potential firing.
tial of membrane currents at all time points during a burst (n = Finally, if CA3 population bursts are limited by negative
3). This indicates that GABAA currents do not increase near the feedback in the form of inhibitory conductances, then phar-
end of a burst, and excludes the possibility that a progressive macological blockade of these conductances should result in
increase in GABAA inhibitory conductance was masked by a pro- continuous activation of the CA3 network. When the after-
portional decrease in excitatory conductances. hyperpolarization was blocked with norepinephrine26, popu-
The second experiment was based on the effect of postsynaptic lation bursts were only moderately prolonged, and there was
inhibitory conductances on the probability of firing an action no significant impact on burst frequency (n = 6; Fig. 2b and c).
potential in response to excitatory postsynaptic current10. If CA3 Burst length and frequency were also modestly affected by
population bursts are limited by inhibitory conductances, then enhancing or blocking inhibitory postsynaptic GABA A con-
the number of action potentials triggered by depolarizing cur- ductances (n = 10; Fig. 2d). Even when the afterhyperpolariza-
rent injection should be substantially reduced at the end of a tion, GABAA, and GABAB conductances were simultaneously

202 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 2. Role of postsynaptic inhibitory


conductances in the control of CA3
a b
burst discharges. (a) Effect of the after-

Number of spikes in burst


hyperpolarization on CA3 pyramidal
cell burst firing. Similar trains of action
potentials were elicited in a CA3 pyra-
midal cell by 500 ms, 1 nA current
injections at various times after bursts
induced by 8.5 mM [K+]o. Top traces
are membrane potential responses to
current injection; bottom traces are
schematics of the timing of current Time after spont. burst (ms)
injections. (b) Number of action
potentials in each train as a function of
c d
the delay between the end of a popula-
tion burst and the start of the current
injection for the cell shown in (a). The

Burst interval (sec) P


average interburst interval was 2.6 s.

Burst length(ms) J
For each delay, points represent means
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

and bars represent standard error; n =


1 to 4 for each delay. (c) Effect of
blocking IAHP (afterhyperpolarization
current) on burst discharge.
Intracellular and extracellular record- 1s
ings are superimposed. Top two traces,
in control solution (8.5 mM [K+]o), the
duration of the afterhyperpolarization
does not predict the timing of the next Minutes
burst. Bottom trace, blocking IAHP with
40 M norepinephrine does not alter e f
burst timing; burst length increased by
23 7% (average standard error), and

Burst length (ms) j


interburst interval decreased by 1
Burst interval (sec) P

12% (n = 6). Scale, 20 mV IC, 2 mV EC.


(d) Effect of GABAA conductances on
burst length (large symbols) and the
interval between CA3 population
bursts (small symbols) was assessed by
bath application of 60 M pentobarbital
(pb) and 100 M picrotoxin (ptx).
Bursting was induced by 8.5 mM [K+]o.
Heavy line is a moving average.
Burst number
Increasing the GABAA conductance
with 60 M pentobarbital decreased
burst length by 12 11% and decreased the interburst interval by 20 14 % (n = 10). Blocking GABAA conductances with picrotoxin
increased burst length by 39 19 % and increased the interburst interval by 50 15 % compared to control (n = 10). (e) Bursts recorded
from two CA3 pyramidal cells in 6 mM [K+]o (control) and after blockade of the afterhyperpolarization, GABAA, GABAA, conductances and
glutamate receptor desensitization. Top panels are current-clamp recordings in a cell recorded with K+ electrode solution; bottom panels are
membrane currents in a cell voltage clamped at 55 mV with cesium and QX314 in the electrode solution. (f) Burst length (l ) and inter-
burst interval (P ) for the current-clamped pyramidal cell shown in (e), during perfusion with 6 mM [K +]o and after addition of NE/PTX/CGP
and cyclothiazide. Lines represent average values for each condition.

blocked by bath application of 40 M norepinephrine, 100 M the effect of the rate of glutamate release from the presynaptic
picrotoxin and 1 M CGP55845A, prolonged CA3 discharges terminal (RGlu ) on synaptic strength. RGlu is the product of the
never occurred (Fig. 2e and f), indicating that inhibitory con- number of releasable vesicles (NR) in the presynaptic terminal
ductances are not necessary for burst termination. multiplied by the probability of release of a releasable vesicle
(PR) (reviewed in ref. 27). PR is considered here to be a contin-
REGULATION OF BURSTS BY SUPPLY OF RELEASABLE GLUTAMATE uous function of time whose value is relatively low between
We then considered the idea that burst termination is due at bursts and high during a burst due to the barrage of action
least in part to a decrease in the positive feedback mediated by potentials reaching the presynaptic terminal27. As glutamate
glutamatergic recurrent collateral synapses. In computer mod- vesicles are released at recurrent synapses during a burst, N R
els, population bursting is a very sharp function of the strength decreases until exhaustion of releasable glutamate results in
of recurrent excitatory synapses6. Synaptic strength is deter- synaptic failure (i.e. NR 0 causes RGlu 0). If this occurs at
mined by several pre- and postsynaptic factors; we considered enough synapses, the strength of recurrent synapses and their

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 203


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

positive feedback would be diminished to a

Normalized burst length


the point that bursts are terminated6.
The rate of increase in the ability of the
CA3 network to support a burst was mea-
sured by triggering a second burst at ran-
dom time intervals after a spontaneous
burst in 8.5 mM extracellular potassium
spont
(Fig. 3a and b). The length of the evoked
burst increased in proportion to the time
elapsed since the end of the last sponta-
neous burst with a time constant of 0.6 to Stimulus delay (ms)
2 seconds (n = 12). Burst length was not
zero at the shortest stimulation times at b
least in part because of evoked glutamate
release at synapses that did not participate

Burst length (ms)


in the burst, such as at terminals on per-
forant path axons, mossy fibers, and cut
recurrent collaterals. Although these slices
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

exhibited a wide range of spontaneous spont


burst lengths (60420 ms; compare Fig. 3a
to Fig. 3b), the fractional increase in burst
length followed similar time courses.
Because the afterhyperpolarization and
postsynaptic inhibitory conductances that Stimulus delay (ms)
modulate burst length (Figs 1 and 2) were c
not blocked, the time constants obtained
in this experiment reflect the decay of

Normalized burst length


those conductances as well as the re-accu-
mulation of releasable glutamate. When
the afterhyperpolarization, GABAA, and
GABAB conductances were blocked, the
time constant for the rate of increase in
spont
burst length increased to 7.1 1.9 seconds
(Fig. 3c; n = 7). The time course for the
increase in burst length is similar to the
time course both for successfully inducing
a second population burst by triggering Stimulus delay (ms)
action potentials in a single CA3 pyrami-
dal cell after a spontaneous burst3, and for Fig. 3. Releasable glutamate +
and burst timing. (a) In a CA3 network bursting spontaneously
the replenishment of releasable glutamate in 8.5 mM extracellular K , the length of pyramidal cell bursts evoked by single electrical
at individual synapses in the hippocampal stimuli in s. moleculare is proportional to the time interval between stimulation and the ter-
mination of the preceding spontaneous burst. Resting membrane potential is -63 mV. Left
slice20. These findings support the idea
panel, examples of bursts evoked at the delays indicated (seconds) compared to a sponta-
that depletion of releasable glutamate is a
neous burst (Spont). Right panel, plot of evoked burst length versus post-burst stimulus delay
determinant of burst termination, and that (bars are standard error, n = 2 to 12 for each delay value). Solid line represents the least
the degree of replenishment modulates the squares fit exponential (time constant 600 ms). (b) Extracellular recording from the same
probability of a subsequent discharge. type of experiment as shown in (a). Although the CA3 burst length in this slice is only half as
Alternatively, the strength of recurrent long as in (a), the rate of increase in burst length shows a similar proportionality to the inter-
synapses could be limited by a postsynaptic val since the last burst (time constant of 900 ms). Average interval between spontaneous
mechanism such as desensitization. How- bursts was 5.4 seconds. (c) Extracellular CA3 recording of the same experiment in a slice in
ever, when glutamate receptor desensitiza- which GABAA and GABAB postsynaptic conductances and IAHP were blocked. Evoked burst
tion was inhibited by 100 M length increased with a time constant of seven seconds following a spontaneous burst.
cyclothiazide28, there was only a modest
increase in the length of CA3 bursts record-
ed during blockade of GABAA, GABAB,
and after-hyperpolarization conductances (17 12%; n = 4; Fig. eight cells; Fig. 4ac). However, as in the experiments utilizing
2e and f). To directly evaluate postsynaptic glutamate receptor electrical stimulation (Fig. 3), the later, polysynaptic response was
function, we measured the current induced by glutamate appli- substantially increased at longer post-burst application intervals,
cation to the dendrites of bursting CA3 pyramidal cells. There was indicating an increase in the strength of recurrent synapses.
no correlation between the amplitude of the initial glutamate- These results suggest that during a burst, the strength of recur-
induced current and the time elapsed since the end of the last rent synapses is limited by a presynaptic mechanism. Evaluation of
burst (basal dendrites, n = 4; distal apical dendrites, n = 22; 10 spontaneous excitatory postsynaptic currents recorded before
cells, 9 slices, 6 animals; bursts induced by 8.5 mM external potas- bursting began compared to events recorded between CA3 bursts
sium in two cells, and by tetanization of pyramidal cell layer in induced by 8.5 mM extracellular potassium revealed a much larg-

204 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 4. Glutamate receptor function is


not altered following a burst. (a)
a glutamate b glutamate

Currents evoked by four consecutive 0.6 s


applications of 100 M glutamate (arrow-
head) to the distal apical dendrites of a 100 ms 50 ms
20 pA
pyramidal cell voltage clamped at -50 mV. 10 ms 100 pA
Bursting induced by 8.5 mM extracellular 2s 20 pA
0.2 s
potassium. Intervals between the end of 1.5 s
the preceding burst and glutamate appli-
2.5 s
cation were randomly generated and are 9.4 s 2.6 s
8.0 s
noted next to the current recordings. The
average interval between bursts was 2.3 cC d
D
seconds; thus the glutamate application
mean (%)
100
2.5 seconds after the previous burst trig-
1.0 -
gers a burst, but the initial rate of rise of
from mean
+
50
the current is the same as for the current control
elicited by glutamate application 0.2 sec-

Probability
from

Probability
onds after the last burst. Glutamate was -0
% difference

applied after every tenth spontaneous


Difference

20 ms
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

burst. (b) The experiment shown in (a) -50


1 pA
was repeated in a slice in which CA3 +
control
bursting was induced by tetanic stimula- 0.0 -
-100
tion of the CA3 pyramidal cell layer. The 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 0 2 4 6 8
average interburst interval in this prepara- seconds after
Seconds after burst
burst Interevent interval
interval, sec
Interevent (sec)
tion was 13 seconds, which permitted
longer delays between the end of a burst
and glutamate application. As in (a), glutamate application at delays closer to the interburst interval triggered a burst more rapidly, but the ini-
tial response to glutamate was unchanged (inset). Glutamate was applied after every fourth burst in (b) and (c). (c) Summary of initial
response to glutamate application in three experiments in which the average CA3 interburst interval was 10 to 15 seconds. Glutamate
response was calculated as the current averaged over the first 30 ms following glutamate application. Points represent the difference at each
delay value from the mean. There is no significant change in glutamate receptor function during or after CA3 bursts. (d) Cumulative proba-
bility plots of the intervals between spontaneous excitatory postsynaptic currents (EPSCs). EPSCs were recorded for two-minute intervals in
a CA3 pyramidal cell voltage clamped at -50 mV under three conditions: normal extracellular media (control); five minutes after switching to
8.5 mM [K+]o, representing the two-minute interval just prior to the onset of burst activity (-); and five minutes after the onset of bursting in
8.5 mM [K+]o (+). Inset shows averaged EPSCs for each condition. EPSCs were recorded using a potassium electrode solution.

er decrease in frequency versus amplitude (Fig. 3c; n = 4; ref. 29). seconds immediately following a burst (n = 18 cells in 15 slices
This result supports the idea that the mechanism for decreasing from 13 animals). The time constants describing the rate of
the strength of recurrent excitatory synapses is presynaptic18, but change in EPSC frequency (1.8 to 5 seconds; Fig. 5c) were simi-
does not indicate the nature of the mechanism. To evaluate the lar to the time constants for the change in burst length illustrat-
possibility that glutamate release is limited by negative feedback via ed in Fig. 3. The EPSC frequency was not expected to be zero
presynaptic metabotropic glutamate receptors17,19, we blocked immediately after a burst because of release of glutamate from
this receptor using the broad-spectrum metabotropic antagonist terminals that did not participate in the burst.
MCPG. Although 250 M MCPG produced a 37 11% increase
in the frequency of CA3 bursts induced by 6 mM extracellular REGULATION OF CA3 BURSTING BY PROBABILITY OF RELEASE
potassium and 100 M picrotoxin, the length of bursts was not These findings support the hypothesis that excitation at CA3
affected (4 2 % increase, n = 8), indicating that metabotropic recurrent collateral synapses can be terminated by depletion of
receptor feedback was not the critical determinant of the strength releasable glutamate. When depletion of releasable glutamate
of recurrent synapses during a burst. during a burst causes recurrent synaptic strength to fall below
To directly assess the supply of releasable glutamate available the value at which bursting is supported6, not only is the burst
at the end of a burst, glutamate release was evoked by applica- terminated, but in addition the next burst cannot occur until the
tion of hyperosmotic extracellular fluid (500 mM mannitol) to supply of releasable glutamate is replenished. If the probability
the slice30,31. Glutamate release by hyperosmotic media is not of initiating the next burst is proportional to the rate of gluta-
dependent on action potentials or intracellular calcium and is mate release between bursts29, then the next burst requires both
not affected by known presynaptic inhibitory feedback mecha- adequate NR so that synapses have the strength to propagate the
nisms32. Bursts were induced by a tetanic stimulation of the CA3 burst, and adequate glutamate release (NRPR) to initiate the
pyramidal cell layer and recorded during blockade of GABAA and burst. Changing the value of PR between bursts would then lead
GABAB postsynaptic conductances and the afterhyperpolariza- to a proportionate change in the degree to which NR needed to
tion. Mannitol substantially increased the frequency of sponta- recover in order for glutamate release to be sufficient to trigger
neous glutamate release as measured by the frequency of another burst; the change in the recovery of NR would be reflect-
spontaneous EPSCs (Fig. 5a and b). Despite blocking all inhibito- ed in the interburst interval.
ry processes, the EPSC frequency during the two seconds pre- Thus agents that decrease PR such as adenosine and baclofen33
ceding a burst was 6.5 0.8 times the frequency during the two should increase the interval between bursts34,35, whereas increas-

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 205


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

ing PR by increasing external potassium23 should decrease the feedback inhibition should be proportional to the intensity of the
interburst interval (Fig. 6ac). The burst length changed in pro- burst, but the opposite relationship is found (Fig. 6e). However, if
portion to the burst interval under these conditions, supporting the duration of a CA3 population burst is limited by the amount
the idea that the amount of glutamate available for release is of releasable glutamate at recurrent synapses, then any factor that
increasing throughout the interburst interval. The effects of these changes the postsynaptic effect of glutamate should affect burst
manipulations are most consistent with a presynaptic locus of duration. Thus, although we hypothesize that inhibitory conduc-
action, as they are the opposite of what would be expected based tances are neither necessary nor sufficient for burst termination,
on the postsynaptic effects of these manipulations (for instance, they will substantially alter the excitatory effect of released gluta-
increasing potassium extracellularly would decrease inhibition mate10. Inhibitory conductances therefore can further shorten a
due to the decrease the driving force for inhibitory currents36, burst that is limited by the supply of releasable glutamate (Fig. 2cf).
and thus would tend to increase burst length as picrotoxin does in The length of CA3 population bursts described in the litera-
Fig. 2d). By measuring the rate of increase in evoked burst length ture varies substantially13,3740. Much of this variation can be
with time elapsed from the last burst as in Fig. 3, the
effects of baclofen were shown to be mediated by a aA
decrease in P R rather than a change in the rate of

control
Burst

Control
burst
increase of NR (Fig. 6d). Over the wide range of burst
lengths and intervals in the baclofen experiments, burst
length did not predict the time interval until the next
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

burst, but the length of the interval preceding a burst


was correlated with burst length (Fig. 6e). This pro-
vides further evidence for the idea that burst termina-
tion is the result of depletion of an excitatory factor
rather than accumulation of an inhibitory factor.
Mannitol
mannitol

Discussion
During synchronous CA3 population bursts in vitro,
the finite supply of releasable neurotransmitter lim-
its the strength of recurrent synapses. Because pop-
ulation bursting is strongly dependent on the strength
of these positive-feedback synapses, the availability
of releasable glutamate effectively controls the dura-
tion of population bursts. The interval between bursts 500 ms
reflects the replenishment of releasable glutamate. 50 pA
This replenishment increases both the strength of
recurrent synapses and the rate of glutamate release,
bB C
1.0 mannitol 5
which contributes to the initiation of the next burst29,
as schematized in Fig. 7.
(Hz)

4
frequency, Hz

Although we have emphasized burst termination by


EPSCfrequency
Probability

exhaustion of releasable glutamate, inhibitory conduc-


probability

control 3
tances also decrease burst duration (Figs 1b, 2df and
0.5
3c). How do these mechanisms interact to terminate
2
bursts? The experiments in which inhibitory conduc-
EPSC

tances were blocked demonstrate that exhaustion of


1
releasable glutamate is sufficient to terminate bursts (Fig.
2cf). Whether exhaustion of releasable glutamate is also
0.0 0
necessary for burst termination can not be proved as 0 200 400 600 800 1000 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
directly, because it is not possible to prevent this exhaus- interevent interval,
Interevent intervalms
(ms) seconds after
Seconds afterend of burst
end of burst
tion. However, three lines of evidence support the
hypothesis that depletion of releasable glutamate is nec-
essary for burst termination, as opposed to providing a Fig. 5. Amount of releasable glutamate available at the end of a burst. (a)
fail-safe mechanism should inhibition fail. First, inhibi- Spontaneous EPSCs recorded in a CA3 pyramidal cell before and after sponta-
neous bursts (bursts are truncated). Bursting was triggered by a tetanic stimula-
tion does not increase at the end of a burst in a manner
tion of the CA3 pyramidal layer; GABAA and GABAB postsynaptic conductances
consistent with a burst termination mechanism10 (Fig.
and IAHP were blocked. Top two records are consecutive bursts that were
1). Second, when extracellular potassium is increased, recorded prior to mannitol application (control); bottom ten records are con-
the driving forces for inhibitory chloride and potassium secutive bursts recorded after application of 500 mM mannitol to the distal api-
currents are decreased36. If inhibition terminated bursts cal dendritic layer of CA3. Following a burst, the EPSC frequency is depressed.
independently of the supply of releasable glutamate, then Average interburst interval was ten seconds. Holding potential was 70 mV,
the decrement in inhibition caused by increased potas- records were low-pass filtered at 1 kHz, and the electrode solution contained
sium should increase burst length, but over a very wide cesium and QX314. (b) Cumulative probability plots of EPSC intervals demon-
range of potassium concentrations, this does not occur strate that EPSCs are increased during application of mannitol. (c) EPSC fre-
(Fig. 6a and b). Finally, if bursts were terminated by feed- quency as a function of time elapsed since the end of the last spontaneous burst.
back inhibition, then the interval following a burst would EPSCs were binned in one-second intervals. Solid line, fit exponential function,
be proportional to burst length, because the amount of time constant, 1.8 seconds.

206 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 6. Presynaptic modulation of CA3 population bursts.


a A
b B (a, b) Effect of [K+]o on CA3 burst frequency and burst
length. Burst interval and length decreased as [K+]o was

(sec)
(sec)

20 3.5 500 25 150


interval

interval, sec
500 ms increased, consistent with an increase in PR and a decrease
interval, sec

ms
length

length

burst length, ms
20

Burst length
interburstinterval
interburst interval

15 2 mV in the degree to which NR had recovered at the start of the

length,
8.5

(ms)

(ms)
15 burst. Points are averaged over all bursts acquired during

Burst
10 30-minute recording periods; bars are standard error.
10

burst

Interburst
Interburst

interval length Experiment in (a) was performed in a transverse brain slice,


5 5
250
and (b) in a coronal brain slice; note the difference in the
0 110 range of burst lengths. (c) Doseresponse relationships for
3 6 9 5 6 7 8 9 10
the decrease in burst frequency and increase in burst length
+
KK+oo (mM)
, mM K+Ko+(mM)
o mM
with baclofen. The length and interval of CA3 population
cC Dd bursts induced by 8.5 mM [K+ ]o increased as baclofen was
(sec)

30 110 120 applied by bath at the indicated concentrations; bursting


interval
stopped in 8 M baclofen. Picrotoxin (100 M) was used in
interval, sec

ms
msms

length
length, ms
interburstinterval

all baclofen and [K+]o experiments to avoid confounding


burst length,
length,

20
effects due to decreased GABA release and altered Cl gra-
burst length,

dients. (d) The decreased burst frequency in baclofen is not


Interburst

burst
burst

10 due to a change in the rate of increase of NR. CA3 popula-


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

tion bursts were induced by 8.5 mM [K+]o, and bursts were


70 50 evoked after every fourth spontaneous burst as described
0 1 2 3 4 0 1 2 3 4 5 6
in Fig. 2b. Under control conditions () and in baclofen
M
M baclofen
baclofen Stimulus
stimulusdelay
delay,(sec)
sec
e 2 M (o), the time constant describing the increase in burst
E f length at increased stimulus delays was essentially the same
1.0 1.0
(solid line). Longer delays could be included in baclofen
interval
burst interval

because the spontaneous burst interval was increased. (e,f)


burstlength
length

The duration of a burst does not predict the time interval


0.5
nextburst

0.5 until the next burst (e) but the interval preceding a burst
Burst

predicts the burst duration (f; compare to d). Using the


Next

data shown in averaged form in (c), normalized time inter-


0.0 0.0
vals and burst lengths are plotted for each concentration of
0.0 0.5 1.0 0.0 0.5 1.0 baclofen (control P , baclofen 1 M p , baclofen 2 M l ,
burst length
Burst length burst
Burst interval
interval baclofen 4 M L ).

explained by differences in PR produced by the experimental pro- vivo is regulated not only by the supply of releasable glutamate
tocols used to induce bursting. The differences in PR will lead to but also by another presynaptic mechanism, synapse-specific vari-
proportional differences in NR at the start of the burst, and burst ation in PR, which may be enhanced by neuromodulators17,33.
duration is limited by NR (Figs 3, 6 and 7). Some of the variation Whether or not the supply of releasable transmitter affects other
in CA3 burst length, for instance the difference noted between modes of network activity, for instance those in which inhibition
septal and temporal slices (compare Fig. 6a and b), is not easily alternates with excitation48,49, will depend on the rate of trans-
explained by differences in PR. This suggests that there may be mitter release, the number of release sites, and the rate of replen-
septo-temporal differences in the number of glutamate release ishment of releasable transmitter during the inhibitory periods.
sites or in the factors that modulate the effect of released gluta- Termination of network activation by exhaustion of releasable
mate, such as glutamate receptor desensitization or re-uptake glutamate rather than postsynaptic inhibition increases the com-
characteristics41. Another potential explanation for the septo-tem- putational flexibility of the network, because network activity is not
poral difference is that the interneuronal connectivity of tempo- constrained by a progressive dampening as inhibitory conductances
ral transverse slices is maximized by the plane of section. This terminate the burst. The causal link between the properties of indi-
connectivity makes possible the prolonged driving of CA3 by other vidual synapses such as PR and NR and the behavior of the network
networks in which epileptiform discharges are induced4244. suggests that the plasticity of synaptic properties should also be
Prolonged CA3 discharges that are not driven by other net- reflected in the behavior of the network. For instance, tetanization of
works35,45,46 consist of a primary burst followed by a series of hippocampal afferents results in an increase in the strength of the
brief bursts at short intervals, which are termed afterdischarges47. activated synapses31 as well as an alteration in the modes of network
Although our data do not address afterdischarges explicitly, the activity43. Regulation of CA3 burst duration by the supply and prob-
relationship between afterdischarge length and the interval since ability of release of neurotransmitter is a novel mechanism for con-
the last discharge seems to follow the relationship shown in Figs trol of a neural network that presents new avenues for understanding
3 and 7, suggesting that afterdischarge duration and interval are the physiology of neurotransmitter release as well as new strategies
also governed by the supply of releasable glutamate. If so, then for the treatment of epilepsy.
series of afterdischarges may represent burst behavior under con-
ditions in which the interburst probability of release remains very Methods
high at the end of the initial burst, for instance as a consequence The duration of reported CA3 bursts varies along the septo-temporal
of prolonged depolarizing conductances47. axis37 and with the method used to induce bursting6,7,1214,3840. In order
In CA3 in vivo, not all pyramidal cells fire during a sharp to ascertain the range of applicability of the present findings, CA3 bursts
wave4,5. This suggests that the activation of the CA3 network in were studied in both septal and ventral CA3, and bursts were induced

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 207


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 7. Schematic of proposed


presynaptic modulation of burst ter-
a P = 0.25
A
bB cC
P RR0= 0.25 PPR = =.32
0.32
max
max 0 R
0 0
mination and timing. (a, b) The pat-
tern of bursting if the rate of 8 120
glutamate release from the presy-

interval, secsec

ms ms
naptic terminal at recurrent

NRR xPPRR
6 length

interburstinterval,

length,
synapses limits synaptic strength at

Burst length,
these synapses. Glutamate release is

N
burst 4
calculated as the product of PR x

burst
threshold

interburst
NR. The value of NR x PR at which a 2 interval
population burst is likely to be trig- min
min
gered29 and propagated6 is desig- 20
0
nated burst threshold. Bursts stop
max
max
when PR x NR falls below this min PR max

threshold; bursts are possible when


PR x NR exceed threshold. For sim-
NRR

6 100
N

sec sec
plicity we assume bursts occur as

ms ms
interval,
soon as they are possible. In (a) and
min

length,
min

interburst interval,
4
(b), NR x PR, NR, and PR are plotted length
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

burst length,
for 2 different interburst values of 1

interburst
PR (interburst PR = 0.25 in a and 2

burst
0.32 in b). Between bursts, NR interval
R
PR

increases at a rate proportional to


P

0 0
the number of empty release sites
min rate
min rate ofofNNR Rincrease
increase max
max
remaining, so that dNR / dT = krefill * 0
(NRmax - NR), and NR = NRmax x (1- 2 4 6 8 2 4 6 8
max
max burst threshold
burst threshold min
min
e (t / ) ), where = 1 / krefill . NR seconds
seconds seconds
seconds
decreases during a burst at a rate
proportional to the number of releasable vesicles25, so that during a burst dNR / dT = - kdecrease *Nr, and NR = NRmax x e (t / ), where = 1
/ kdecrease. PR between bursts is assumed to have been increased by an experimental manipulation such as increased extracellular [K+]o.
During a burst, Pr is assumed to increase further because of bursts of action potentials in the presynaptic cell. (c) Top panel, the relationships
between interburst PR, burst length, and interburst interval. Burst interval decreases as PR increases because for a given rate of increase in
NR after a burst, NR x PR reaches burst threshold more rapidly for larger values of PR. Burst length decreases as PR increases because NR
does not recover to a very high value before PR x NR exceeds burst threshold, and so there are fewer releasable glutamate vesicles at the
start of the next burst. Bottom panel, the rate at which N R increases between bursts determines when NR x PR reaches burst threshold and
therefore sets the interburst interval. Burst length is not affected, because NR at the start of the burst is not changed. Increasing the burst
threshold increases the burst interval, because in order for NR x PR to cross burst threshold, NR must increase to closer to its maximum
value, where the rate of increase in NR is low (a, middle panel). However, increasing the burst threshold does not change the amount by
which NR decreases during the burst: although N R is higher at the start of the burst, the burst also terminates at a higher value of N R because
NR x PR drops below the increased burst threshold sooner.

using three different manipulations: increased extracellular potassium ings were accepted when the access resistance remained under 10 M for
([K+]o), GABAA receptor block, and tetanic stimulation of the CA3 pyra- voltage-clamp experiments, and under 20 M in experiments where no
midal layer43,44. current was injected. Recordings using Axoclamp 2B amplifiers were dig-
itized at 2 KHz using routines written in Axobasic (Axon Instruments, Fos-
S LICE PREPARATION . Recordings from area CA3 of the adult rat hip- ter City, California). Burst length rather than burst area was used to estimate
pocampus were made from 400-m thick hemibrain slices at 35C. The burst intensity to facilitate comparisons between extra and intracellular
brain was cut in the coronal plane except where transverse plane is spec- recordings (Fig. 3). Burst length was calculated as the time during which the
ified. For both planes of section, recordings were made from slices per- absolute value of the burst was above a threshold value, generally three
pendicular to the long axis of the hippocampus. Therefore, slices from times the baseline noise. Similarly, for stimuli applied after a burst, the end
the septal (rostral) end of the hippocampus were used when the brain of a spontaneous burst was defined as 50 ms after the time at which the
was cut in the coronal plane, and from the temporal (caudal) end when burst amplitude was less than a defined value, generally three times the
the brain was sliced transversely. Extracellular solution was saturated baseline noise. Intervals between the end of a burst and the stimulus were
with 95% O 2, 5% CO 2 and contained (in mM) NaCl 126, KCl 2.5, randomly generated, then rounded to the nearest 200 ms. Electrical stim-
NaHCO3 26, CaCl2 2, MgCl2 2, NaH2PO4 1.25, glucose 10. uli were applied after every fourth burst to the pyramidal cell layer, and
the intensity was set to elicit one population spike in control media.
RECORDINGS. Whole-cell recordings were performed using a filling solu-
tion containing (in mM) potassium methylsulfonate 123, MgCl2 2, NaCl 8, BURST INDUCTION. Bursting in CA3 was induced by increasing extracellular
potassium ethylene glycol-bis(b-aminoethyl ether) N,N,N,N-tetraacteic potassium to between 6 and 8.5 mM as noted in the text, by blockade of
acid (EGTA) 1, potassium adenosine 5-triphosphate 4, and sodium guano- GABAA inhibition with 100 M picrotoxin or by a single tetanic stimula-
sine 5-triphosphate 0.3. The whole-cell solution was buffered with 16 mM tion43 of the CA3 pyramidal cell layer. The tetanic stimulus consisted of a
KHCO3 and saturated with 95% O2, 5% CO2. In voltage-clamp experi- 100 Hz, one-second train of stimuli that were of sufficient amplitude to
ments, 2 mM QX314 (Astra Pharmaceuticals) was added to the pipette elicit a population spike when delivered at a lower frequency. If a single
solution to block voltage-dependent sodium conductances, and where tetanus did not induce bursting, it was repeated after a ten-minute inter-
noted cesium replaced potassium to improve the space clamp. Extracellu- val43,44. When inducing bursting by tetanic stimulation, extracellular solu-
lar recordings were performed using saline-filled patch pipettes. Record- tions were modified43 to Ca2+ 1.3 mM, Mg2+ 0.9 mM, and K+ 3.3 mM.

208 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

GLUTAMATE AND MANNITOL APPLICATION. Glutamate and mannitol were 21. Harris, K.M. & Sultan, P. Variation in the number, location, and size of
applied via a whole-cell pipette using a 40 psi pressure pulse lasting 5 to synaptic vesicles provides an anatomical basis for the nonuniform
probability of release at hippocampal CA1 synapses. Neuropharmacology 34,
50 ms. Intervals between the end of a burst and glutamate application 13871395 (1995).
were randomized to a value between 200 ms and the average interburst 22. Schikorski, T. & Stevens, C.F. Quantitative ultrastructural analysis of
interval. Mannitol was applied every 4 to 30 seconds as described31. hippocampal excitatory synapses. J. Neurosci. 17, 58585867 (1997).
23. Liu, G. & Tsien, R.W. Properties of synaptic transmission at single
REAGENTS. Drugs were applied by bath. Appropriate precautions were hippocampal synaptic boutons. Nature 375, 404408 (1995).
taken to avoid oxidation of norepinephrine26. Where noted, afterhyper- 24. Stevens, C.F. & Tsujimoto, T. Estimates for the pool size of releasable quanta
polarization was blocked by addition of 40 M norepinephrine, GABAA at a single central synapse and for the time required to fill the pool. Proc. Natl
conductance by 100 M picrotoxin, GABA B conductance by 1 M Acad. Sci. USA 92, 846849 (1995).
25. Murthy, V.N., Sejnowski, T.J. & Stevens, C.F. Heterogenous release properties
CGP55845A (Ciba Geigy, Basel), and glutamate-receptor desensitization of visualized individual hippocampal synapses. Neuron 18, 599612 (1997).
by 100 M cyclothiazide to the extracellular fluid. Reagents were obtained 26. Madison, D.V. & Nicoll, R.A. Noradrenaline blocks accommodation of
from Sigma (St. Louis, Missouri). Ten micromolar acetazolamide was pyramidal cell discharge in the hippocampus. Nature 299, 636638 (1986).
applied with pentobarbital to diminish GABAA-receptor-mediated den- 27. Stevens, C.F. Quantal release of neurotransmitter and long-term
dritic depolarization50. potentiation. Neuron 10(suppl), 5563 (1996).
28. Patneau D.K., Vyklicky L. Jr & Mayer M.L. Hippocampal neurons exhibit
cyclothiazide-sensitive rapidly desensitizing responses to kainate. J Neurosci.
13, 34963509 (1993).
Acknowledgements 29. Chamberlin, N.L., Traub, R.D. & Dingledine R. Role of EPSPs in initiation of
spontaneous synchronized burst firing in rat hippocampal neurons bathed in
We thank Thomas Dunwiddie, Darrell Lewis, Charles Stevens, and Roger Traub high potassium. J. Neurophysiol. 64, 10001008 (1990).
for comments and discussions. This work was supported by the NIH and the 30. Fatt, P. & Katz, B. Spontaneous subthreshold activity at motor nerve endings.
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

Epilepsy Foundation of America. J. Physiol. 117, 109128 (1952).


31. Manabe T., Renner, P. & Nicoll, R.A. Postsynaptic contribution to long-term
potentiation revealed by the analysis of miniature synaptic currents. Nature
RECEIVED 16 APRIL: ACCEPTED 7 MAY 1998 355, 5055 (1992).
32. Rosenmund, C. & Stevens, C.F. Definition of the readily releasable pool of
vesicles at hippocampal synapses. Neuron 16, 11971207 (1996).
1. Li, X.G., Somogyi, P., Ylinen, A. & Buzsaki, G. The hippocampal CA3 33. Capogna, M., Ghwiler, B.H. & Thompson, S.M. Presynaptic inhibition of
network: an in vivo intracellular labeling study. J. Comp. Neurol. 339, calcium-dependent and independent release elicited with ionmycin,
181208 (1994). gadolinium, and -latrotoxin in the hippocampus. J. Neurophysiol. 75,
2. MacVicar, B.A. & Dudek, F.E. Local circuit interactions in rat hippocampus: 20172028 (1996).
interactions between pyramidal cells. Brain Res. 242, 341344 (1982). 34. Dunwiddie, T.V. Endogenously released adenosine regulates excitability in
3. Miles, R. & Wong, R.K.S. Single neurones can initiate synchronized the in vitro hippocampus. Epilepsia 21, 541548 (1980).
population discharge in the hippocampus. Nature 306, 371373 (1983). 35. Swartzwelder, H.S., Lewis, D.V., Anderson, W.W. & Wilson, W.A. Seizure-like
4. Buzsaki, G. Hippocampal sharp waves: their origin and significance. Brain events in brain slices: suppression by interictal activity. Brain Res. 410,
Res. 398:242252 (1986). 362366 (1987).
5. Chrobak, J.J. & Buzsaki, G. High-frequency oscillations in the output 36. Thompson, S.M. & Ghwiler, B.H. Activity-dependent disinhibition. II.
networks of the hippocampal-entorhinal axis of the freely behaving rat. J. effects of extracellular potassium, furosemide, and membrane potential on
Neurosci. 16, 30563066 (1996). ECl in hippocampal CA3 neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 61, 512523 (1989).
6. Traub, R.D. & Miles R. in Neuronal networks of the hippocampus Ch. 6 37. Anderson, C.M. & Jefferys, J.G.R. Ventral CA3 exhibits a lower threshold to
(Cambridge Univ. Press, 1991). disinhibition than dorsal hippocampus. Epilepsia 38(Supp 8), 16 (1997).
7. Traub, R.D., Knowles, W.D., Miles, R. & Wong, R.K.S. Synchronized 38. Korn, S.J., Giacchino, J.L., Chamberlin, N.L. & Dingledine, R. Epileptiform
afterdischarges in the hippocampus: simulation studies of the cellular burst activity induced by potassium in the hippocampus and its regulation by
mechanism. Neuroscience 12, 11911200 (1984). GABA-mediated inhibition. J. Neurophysiol. 57, 325340 (1987).
8. Jefferys, J.G.R. in The Epilepsies, 4th edn (eds Laidlaw, J., Richens, A. & 39. Scharfman, H.E. EPSPs of dentate gyrus granule cells during epileptiform
Chadwick, D.W.) 241276 (Edinburgh, Churchill Livingstone, 1993). bursts of dentate hilar mossy cells and area CA3 pyramidal cells in
9. Wong, R.K.S. & Prince, D.A. Afterpotential generation in hippocampal disinhibited rat hippocampal slices. J. Neurosci. 14, 60416057 (1994).
pyramidal cells. J. Neurophysiol. 45, 8697 (1981). 40. Whittington, M.A. & Jefferys, J.G.R. Epileptic activity outlasts disinhibition
10. Bush, P.C. & Sejnowski, T.J. Effects of inhibition and dendritic saturation in after intrahippocampal tetanus toxin in the rat. J. Physiol. 481, 593604
simulated neocortical pyramidal cells. J. Neurophysiol. 71, 21832193 (1994). (1994).
11. Miles, R., Wong, R.K.S. & Traub, R.D. Synchronized afterdischarges in the 41. Tanaka, K. et al. Epilepsy and exacerbation of brain injury in mice lacking the
hippocampus: contribution of local synaptic interactions. Neuroscience 12, glutamate transporter GLT-1. Science 276, 16991702 (1997).
11791189 (1984). 42. Barbarosie, M. & Avoli, M. CA3-driven hippocampal-entorhinal loop
12. Rutecki, P.A., Lebeda, F.J. & Johnston, D. Epileptiform activity in the controls rather than sustains in vitro limbic seizures. J. Neurosci. 17,
hippocampus produced by tetraethylammonium. J. Neurophysiol. 64, 93089314 (1997).
10771088 (1990). 43. Stasheff, S.F., Anderson, W.W., Clark, S. & Wilson, W.A. NMDA antagonists
13. Mueller, A.L. & Dunwiddie, T.V. Anticonvulsant and proconvulsant actions differentiate epileptogenesis from seizure expression in an in vitro model.
of alpha- and beta-noradrenergic agonists on epileptiform activity in rat Science 245, 648651 (1989).
hippocampus in vitro. Epilepsia 24, 5764 (1983). 44. Rafiq, A., DeLorenzo, R.J. & Coulter, D.A. Generation and propagation of
14. Chamberlin, N.L. & Dingledine, R. Control of epileptiform burst rate by CA3 epileptiform discharges in a combined entorhinal cortex/hippocampal slice.
hippocampal cell afterhyperpolarizations. Brain Res. 492, 337346 (1989). J. Neurophysiol. 70, 19621974 (1993).
15. Mayr, O. The origins of feedback control (MIT Press, Cambridge, 45. Traub, R.D., Borck, C., Colling, S.B. & Jefferys J.G. On the structure of ictal
Massachusetts, 1970). events in vitro. Epilepsia 37, 879891 (1996).
16. Martina, M. & Jonas, P. Functional differences in Na+ channel gating 46. Taylor, G.W., Merlin, L.R. & Wong, R.K. Synchronized oscillations in
between fast-spiking interneurones and principal neurones of rat hippocampal CA3 neurons induced by metabotropic glutamate receptor
hippocampus. J. Physiol. 505, 593603 (1997). activation. J. Neurosci. 15, 80398052 (1995).
17. Scanziani, M. et al. Use-dependent increases in glutamate concentration activate 47. Traub R.D., Miles, R. & Jefferys, J.G. Synaptic and intrinsic conductances
presynaptic metabotropic glutamate receptors. Nature 385, 630634 (1997). shape picrotoxin-induced synchronized after-discharges in the guinea-pig
18. Otis, T., Zhang, S. & Trussell, L.O. Direct measurement of AMPA receptor hippocampal slice. J Physiol. (Lond.) 461, 525547 (1993).
desensitization induced by glutamatergic synaptic transmission. J. Neurosci. 48. Ylinen, A. et al. Intracellular correlates of hippocampal theta rhythm in
16, 74967504 (1996). identified pyramidal cells, granule cells, and basket cells. Hippocampus 5,
19. von Gersdorff, H., Schneggenburger, R., Weis, S. & Neher, E. Presynaptic 7890 (1995).
depression at a calyx synapse: the small contribution of metabotropic 49. Kim, U., Sanchez-Vives, M.V. & McCormick, D.A. Functional dynamics of
glutamate receptors. J. Neurosci. 17, 81378146 (1997). GABAergic inhibition in the thalamus. Science 278, 130134 (1997).
20. Dobrunz, L.E. & Stevens, C.F. Heterogeneity of release probability, 50. Staley, K.J., Soldo, B. & Proctor W. Ionic mechanisms of neuronal excitation
facilitation, and depletion at central synapses. Neuron 18, 9951008 (1997). by inhibitory GABAA receptors. Science 269, 977981 (1995).

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 209


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Input synchrony and the irregular


firing of cortical neurons
Charles F. Stevens and Anthony M. Zador

Howard Hughes Medical Institute and Sloan Center for Theoretical Neurobiology, Salk Institute for Biological Studies, La Jolla, California 92037, USA
Correspondence should be addressed to A.Z. (zador@salk.edu)

Cortical neurons in the waking brain fire highly irregular, seemingly random, spike trains in response
to constant sensory stimulation, whereas in vitro they fire regularly in response to constant current
injection. To test whether, as has been suggested, this high in vivo variability could be due to the
postsynaptic currents generated by independent synaptic inputs, we injected synthetic synaptic cur-
rent into neocortical neurons in brain slices. We report that independent inputs cannot account for
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

this high variability, but this variability can be explained by a simple alternative model of the
synaptic drive in which inputs arrive synchronously. Our results suggest that synchrony may be
important in the neural code by providing a means for encoding signals with high temporal fidelity
over a population of neurons.

Cortical neurons in the waking brain fire highly irregular spike We therefore set out to determine experimentally whether
trains that have more in common with the ticking of a Geiger the high variability observed in vivo could arise from the
counter than of a clock. What is the source of this irregular fir- superposition of independent excitatory and inhibitory inputs.
ing? Softky and Koch1 noted a theoretical conundrum posed by Because reconciliation of the in vivo variability with the synap-
the irregularity of cortical neurons firing at a constant average tic drive depends critically on the details of the spike genera-
rate in vivo. If each cortical neuron were providing independent tion mechanism, we have used a direct approach rather than
input to the other similar cortical neurons it contacts, then the rely on theoretical models of the spike generator. We have
input to any neuron would just be a shower of statistically inde- injected synthetic synaptic currents through a somatic elec-
pendent excitatory postsynaptic potentials (EPSPs) with a con- trode to drive neurons in neocortical slices to fire. With this
stant mean rate. Yet such an input, when tested on a theoretical method, we can assess the output variability in response to
model of a cortical neuron, gave rise to a firing variability much any input ensemble. We have complemented this approach by
less than that observed in vivo. What, then, is the source of the testing the response to miniature excitatory postsynaptic cur-
unexpectedly large variability in spike timing that characterizes rents (EPSCs) whose rate of release was elevated at many
in vivo firing behavior? Is the high variability due to some sub- synapses independently through the local application of
tle interplay between the noise resulting from randomly timed hypertonic solution.
synaptic inputs and the nonlinear spike-generating mechanism, We find that when neocortical neurons are driven by a pop-
or instead due to unexpectedly large fluctuations in the synaptic ulation of independent inputs, the spike variability is consis-
input itself? Although most earlier workers have argued that the tently lower than that observed in vivo. We thus cannot confirm
large variability arises from the interaction between this noisy the earlier conclusions that unstructured synaptic noise inter-
synaptic input and the spike-generating mechanism, we will con- acting with the spike-generating mechanism can account for
clude that large fluctuations in the synaptic drive, such as might the unexpectedly large variability of neuronal activity in the
arise from the synchronous arrival of inputs from many neu- neocortex. However, when a population of transiently syn-
rons, are necessary to account for the high in vivo variability. chronous inputs is added to the background of independent
The neuronal spike generator converts input current into inputs, the observed firing variability is within the range
output spike trains with high fidelity2,3. Irregular firing must, observed in vivo. The high variability observed in vivo is there-
then, reflect fluctuations in the currents that drive the spike gen- fore inconsistent with the activity of independent excitatory
erator, rather than some intrinsic noise in the spike-generating and inhibitory inputs, but could arise from large rapid fluctu-
mechanism itself4. Most previous attempts to identify the source ations in the synaptic drive, such as would result from the near-
of irregular firing have focused on the details of the spike-gen- ly synchronous firing of subpopulations of afferents.
erating mechanism and on the importance of the inhibitory
synaptic noise57 (Tsodyks et al., Soc. Neurosci. Abstr., 20, 1527, Results
1994). These workers have concluded that unstructured synap- The results are organized as follows. First we show that, in
tic noise processed by a model spike generator can produce the response to a synthetic synaptic input corresponding to an
unexpected variability in spike timing. However, most of these ensemble of purely excitatory inputs firing at a constant rate,
proposals have relied on theoretical models of the neuronal spike the coefficient of variation (CV) of the interspike interval dis-
generator (but see ref. 8), and thus one may question the extent tribution is well below that observed in vivo. Next we show
to which such models can be relied upon to provide a sufficiently that CV increases but remains below the in vivo level when
realistic representation of spike generation. the input consists of a steady mix of excitatory and inhibitory

210 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

a b 0.5 (mean standard devia-


tion) and a skewed distri-
0.4 bution (Fig. 1b). Under
slice recording conditions,

Probability
Probability
0.3 this spontaneous release
rate was too low to cause
0.2 fluctuations of more than
about a millivolt (Fig. 1c),
0.1 and was never sufficient to
cause spontaneous spiking.
0 We therefore generated
0 5 10 15 20 25
Amplitude(pA)(pA) synthetic synaptic currents
Amplitude
to test whether such EPSCs,
arriving at a sufficiently
c high rate, could account for
Fig. 1. Spontaneous synaptic event recorded in a
the variability observed in
layer 2/3 cortical neuron. (a) A short record
vivo. In order to simulate
showing EPSCs recorded at the soma under volt-
this drive in a neocortical
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

age clamp at a holding potential of -60 mV, in the


presence of tetrodotoxin (TTX). Calibration, 40 slice, synthetic synaptic cur-
ms, 30 pA. (b) The distribution of spontaneous rents were generated online
miniature EPSCs in this neuron. Inset, a simulated and injected through a
scaled mEPSC is superimposed on a typical somatic patch electrode.
mEPSC from (a). (c) A short record from the Chemical synaptic inputs
same neuron showing EPSPs. Calibration, 40 ms, were pharmacologically
0.5 mV. silenced, so that all the drive
was supplied by the elec-
trode. The synthetic cur-
rents were constructed to
mimic the current that
would be generated at the
inputs. We then repeat the analysis of the response variability soma by a population of excitatory inputs, each firing indepen-
for a complementary measure, the Fano factor. We then con- dently according to a Poisson process with a constant mean rate.
firm that the spiking variability in response to miniature These currents have the advantage that the synthetic synaptic noise
EPSCs evoked by hypertonic solution is still below the in vivo can be created with known statistical structure. The unitary event
levels. Finally, we demonstrate that input synchrony can yield used to construct the synthetic synaptic inputs, 30 pA, was chosen
in vivo levels of variability. to be at the extreme high end of the observed range. Because the
sum of independent Poisson processes is itself a Poisson process,
PURELY EXCITATORY INPUT the input statistics were determined solely by the net rate at which
Cortical neurons in vivo are driven to fire by a shower of EPSCs. excitatory inputs showered onto the soma. We made no explicit
An estimate of the size of the unitary EPSCs that comprise this assumption about the number of synapses or the rate of neuro-
shower was obtained by recording spontaneous EPSCs from transmitter release from each synapse, so that, for example, 100
pyramidal neurons in rat neocortical slices under voltage-clamp synapses each generating 20 EPSCs per second would be indis-
conditions (Fig. 1a). As previously observed911, spontaneous tinguishable from 200 synapses generating 10 EPSCs per second.
EPSCs varied in size, with a mean amplitude of 6.4 3.4 pA Figure 2a shows the results of a typical experiment in which

25
a b
Fig. 2. Fluctuating currents affect the
20
fine structure of the spike train but
(Hz)

not the mean rate. (a) Sample traces


rate (Hz)

15
show typical responses of a layer 2/3
Firingrate

cortical neuron to a constant current


Firing

(left), and a fluctuating current (right) 10

consisting of the sum of independent


Poisson EPSCs firing at a population 5

rate of 2.4 per ms. Calibration, 200


ms, 10 mV, 0.3 nA. (b) The average 0
0 0.05 0.1 0.15 0.2 0.25
number of spikes in a one second Input current
Input (nA)
current (nA)
trial is plotted as a function of the
mean input for the constant (o) and fluctuating (*) currents. Except at very c
low firing rates, the mean spike rate depends only on the mean of the input
current. (c) An example of a spike train recorded extracellularly from area MT
of an alert macaque monkey in response to a constant-velocity visual stimulus
(for details, see ref. 16). Time (ms)

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 211


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

1
a b
0.8

distribution
Distribution
0.6

of ISI
0.4

of ISI
C.V.C.V.
0.2

0
0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1
Ratio
Ratio of of inhibitory to
inhibitory to excitatory
excitatory drive
drive

1
c
0.8
Fig. 3. Variability in response to mixed excitatory
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

and inhibitory input is less than in vivo. (a) Response


Fano factor
0.6
Fano factor

to mixed excitatory and inhibitory input. Typical


responses of a layer 2/3 cortical neuron to a fluctuat-
0.4
ing current (Ri = 0.5) consisting of the sum of a mix of
independent Poisson EPSCs (4.4 per ms) and IPSCs
(2.2 per ms). Calibration, 200 ms, 10 mV, 0.3 nA. (b) 0.2
Dependence of CV, the coefficient of variation of the
interspike interval distribution, on the ratio Ri of inhi- 0
0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1
bition to excitation. The CV increases with Ri, but
RatioRatio of inhibitoryto
of inhibitory toexcitatory
excitatory drive
drive
remains below the in vivo level even at the largest
value of Ri tested. The dotted line indicates the CV of
a Poisson process. The error bars indicate the standard error. (c) Dependence of the Fano factor F (the
variance divided by the mean of the spike count) on the ratio Ri. The solid line shows the actual Fano factor,
whereas the dashed line shows the Fano factor predicted from F = CV2. Even for high values of Ri, the Fano
factor of the response to synthetic synaptic currents remains far below that observed in vivo. The dotted line
indicates the Fano factor of a Poisson process. The error bars indicate the standard error.

a neuron from layer 2/3 of rat neocortex was driven to fire but the converse is not true: one cannot conclude that a
with synthetic synaptic currents. In this example, the input process is Poisson simply because CV is one.
rate of 2.4 EPSCs per millisecond yielded a firing rate of 21 The CV of spike trains from cortical neurons recorded in
Hz, indicating that about 115 EPSCs were required for each vivo are generally near or above unity1,1315. The CV depends
action potential. For comparison, the response to an injection on a number of factors, including the firing rate and the degree
of constant current is also shown. The striking difference of adaptation during a response. In order to provide a specific
between the responses in Fig. 2a is in the fine temporal struc- benchmark against which to compare the present in vitro
ture of the spike trains. In response to a constant input, spikes results, we computed the CV of responses of neurons in the
arrive at regular intervals (which become longer during the middle temporal (MT) cortex of alert macaque monkeys driven
one-second stimulus as a result of spike adaptation), where- with constant-motion stimuli 16 . An example of one such
as in response to the synthetic synaptic current, the interspike response, emphasizing the markedly irregular activity of neo-
intervals are much more irregular. cortical sensory neurons in vivo, is depicted in Fig. 2c. To min-
Although the fine temporal structure of the spike trains imize the effect of adaptation within a trial, only responses
generated by the synthetic synaptic current was very different after the first 500 ms were considered, and to minimize any
from that generated by the constant stimulus, the mean output effect of slow trial-to-trial drift, the CV from each trial was
firing rate in response to this ensemble depended only on the computed separately and then these CVs were averaged. The
mean input. The fI curves (the firing frequency f, averaged mean CV was 1.1 0.16 (range, 0.91 to 1.4; n = 10) for neu-
over the one second stimulus, as a function of the input cur- rons with a mean firing rate of = 17 6.7 Hz (range, 11 to
rent I) for the two stimulus ensembles are fairly linear, show- 33 Hz). Based on these data, it is reasonable to consider a CV of
ing some slight saturation at high firing frequencies, and are in 0.8 as a lower limit for the in vivo range at the firing rates con-
close agreement over the range of currents tested (Fig. 2b). sidered here.
Is the irregularity of the spike train in Fig. 2a as high as The spike trains from the neuron shown in Fig. 2 had a CV
that seen in vivo? One approach to quantifying spike irregu- of 0.28. Similar results were obtained in all other neurons for
larity is based on the coefficient of variation (CV) of the dis- which similar stimuli were tested (CV = 0.29 0.09, = 21
tribution of interspike intervals (ISIs). The CV is defined as 2.4 spikes per s; n = 9). These values are much lower than the
the standard deviation isi divided by the mean isi of the ISI CV value of 1.1 observed in MT cortical neurons in vivo. In
distribution, CV = isi/isi. For a Poisson process, CV is one 12, other experiments, we examined a range of synthetic synaptic

212 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

obtained in all other neurons for which similar stimuli were


tested (CV = 0.43 0.09, = 20.7 2.4 spikes per s; n = 9).
Thus although this input does increase spike irregularity, the
resultant values are still substantially below the CV value of
1.1 observed in vivo.
The fraction of the total input to a cortical neuron pro-
vided by inhibition has not been measured in vivo. Could a
higher inhibitory-to-excitatory ratio Ri boost the CV into the
in vivo range? The dependence of the CV on R i is shown in
Fig. 3b. As expected, the CV increased with increasing R i, but
remained well below unity for the highest values tested (CV
= 0.72 0.07, = 19.2 1.2 spikes per s; n = 5 for Ri = 0.9).
Thus at the highest levels of Ri tested, the CV approached, but
remained below, the values observed in vivo. Because ratios
of inhibition to excitation are unlikely to exceed 0.9 (see ref.
17), we conclude that these experiments represent an approx-
imate upper limit on the variability that can arise from synap-
tic noise from uncorrelated sources, and that such noise thus
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

Fig. 4. Responses elicited by hypertonic solution evoked increases cannot account for the high CV observed in vivo.
in the rate of miniature EPSC release. (a) A regular train of action
potentials driven by miniature EPSCs elicited by a one-second appli- TRIAL-TO-TRIAL VARIABILITY
cation of hypertonic solution from a puffer pipette positioned about The CV is one of several standard measures of firing variabil-
40 m from the soma. The variability of these responses (CV = 0.26, ity. A high CV may reflect irregularity in the fine structure of
Fano factor F = 0.05) was close to that elicited by comparable syn- the spike train, but it may arise from changes arising on a time
thetic synaptic currents. (b) The synaptic current elicited by hyper- scale longer than a typical interspike interval, such as those
tonic solution in the same neuron. Calibration, 300 ms, 20 mV induced by spike adaptation. Indeed, the response to the con-
(top), 100 pA (bottom). The bath solution contained standard stant stimulus shown in Fig. 2 has a CV of 0.43, which is about
Ringers with 100 M picrotoxin, and 30 M cadmium added to pre- the same as that for the mixed excitatory/inhibitory input with
vent recurrent activity. The hypertonic solution consisted of the R i of 0.5. The response to the purely excitatory synthetic
bath solution plus 0.5 mM sucrose. synaptic input also shows strong adaptation.
We therefore also considered a second measure of vari-
ability, the Fano factor. The Fano factor F is defined as the
variance divided by the mean of the spike count N, F = N2/N
currents and found that, as expected, the CV was smaller for where the spike count N is the number of spikes generated on
currents constructed from smaller synthetic EPSCs. For this a (typically one-second) particular trial. The Fano factor is
reason, we chose to test an EPSC amplitude at the upper limit of insensitive to slow changes such as adaptation that occur with-
the spontaneous responses recorded in slice (30 pA), because in a single trial, as long as they occur consistently from one
any smaller EPSC would have yielded a lower CV even more trial to the next. For any Poisson process (including both
inconsistent with the in vivo range. We also examined higher homogeneous and rate-modulated Poisson processes), the
firing rates and found that, as expected, the CV decreased as Fano factor is exactly one; spike trains from cortical neurons
the firing rate increased. The CV reported here, then, can be in vivo are consistently found to have a Fano factor near or
considered an upper limit on the CV likely to be generated by above one16,19. For example, neurons from the middle tem-
the random superposition of independent inputs. Thus a pure- poral (MT) area of alert monkeys were reported to have a
ly excitatory steady drive from independent inputs does not Fano factor of 1.3 (ref. 16).
account for the observed irregularity of cortical spike trains. In certain limiting cases, the Fano factor and the CV are
related by F = CV 2. The main requirement is that the spike
MIXED IPSPS AND EPSPS train be a stationary renewal process 12, that is, a process in
The synaptic drive to cortical neurons in vivo contains a sub- which each interspike interval (ISI) is statistically independent
stantial inhibitory component mediated by GABA recep- of every other ISI. Although there are many ways in which a
tors17,18. It has been suggested that the irregularity observed spike train could deviate from a renewal process, in the pre-
in vivo might arise in part from added fluctuations in the drive sent context deviations are most likely to occur if successive
caused by inhibitory inputs6,7. Inhibition increases the out- (or neighboring) ISIs are statistically dependent (for example,
put variability by increasing the variance in the input drive neurons show some degree of bursting or short ISIs tended to
associated with a given mean. To test whether the inclusion of be followed by long ISIs), or if the spike train adapts, for
inhibitory inputs could account for the irregularity of spike instance slows down, during the one-second stimulus. Because
trains in vivo, we synthesized synaptic currents consisting of the response both to the constant input and to the synthetic
a mixture of excitatory and inhibitory currents. Figure 3a synaptic drive can show considerable adaptation, at least the
shows the response of a neuron to such an input. In this exam- possibility of adaptation during the stimulus must be consid-
ple, the total inhibitory current Ri was half the excitatory cur- ered. Thus the Fano factor can provide additional and inde-
rent (Ri = 0.5); the excitatory drive (the average rate of EPSC pendent information about how well the responses to the
occurrence) was increased in order to maintain a high firing synthetic currents mimic the variability observed in vivo.
rate. As expected, the addition of this inhibitory component The Fano factor of the response to the constant stimulus was
increased the irregularity, to CV of 0.44. Similar results were only 0.02, compared with the Fano factor (F = 0.432 = 0.18) pre-

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 213


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

a b C.V.
C.V. F.F.
F.F. tic terminals simultane-
ously, it is not a good
model for the asynchro-
1.2 1.2 nous and independent
barrage of synaptic stim-
1 1
ulation under considera-
0.8 0.8
tion here. By contrast,
local perfusion with
0.6 0.6 hypertonic solution ele-
vates the rate of minia-
0.4 0.4 ture EPSC release from
many terminals indepen-
0.2 0.2
dently and thereby gen-
0 0
erates a slowly varying
E E/I sync E E/I sync average current. The
Fig. 5. Input synchrony yields in vivo variability. (a) mechanism of this
Response to an input drive consisting primarily of occa- sucrose-evoked trans-
sional large brief (3050 ms) excitatory events, ran- mitter release is indepen-
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

domly distributed in time, each consisting of the nearly dent of presynaptic


synchronous arrival of on average 100200 EPSCs, action potentials and cal-
yielding transient peak currents as high as 1 nA or cium influx 11,21,22 . The
more. Calibration, 200 ms, 10 mV (top), 0.1 nA (bot- miniature EPSCs re-
tom). (b) Summary of CV and Fano factor. Left, CV for leased under these con-
purely excitatory (E), mixed excitatory/inhibitory (E/I), and synchronous (sync), inputs. Right, same statistics ditions therefore provide
for the Fano factor. For both graphs, the error bars indicate standard errors. The dotted lines indicate the a much closer approxi-
value of unity expected for both CV and Fano factor for a perfect Poisson process and are near the values typ- mation of the drive to a
ically observed from cortical spike trains in vivo. cortical neuron expected
if all inputs were inde-
pendent.
Figure 4 shows the
dicted from the renewal assumption (where F rather than F is used response of a layer 2/3 neuron to a hypertonic solution-evoked
to denote the Fano factor predicted from the CV). The large dis- increase in the miniature EPSC rate. The spike train was very
crepancy indicates that for a constant stimulus the renewal assump- regular, with CV of 0.26 and Fano factor of 0.05. These results
tion is not satisfied: marked adaptation within a single trial leads to are in very close agreement with values obtained for the pure-
a large CV that is not reflected in the trial-to-trial measure F. The ly synthetic current (CV = 0.29 and F = 0.06; see Fig. 2). Thus
Fano factor for the purely excitatory input was only slightly high- the hypertonic solution-evoked responses showed much less
er than for the constant current (F = 0.06 0.03; n = 9), and close variability than observed in vivo.
to that predicted from the renewal assumption (F = 0.292 = 0.08). We were unable to elicit spiking in the absence of blockers
However, for the highest inhibitory/excitatory ratios tested (Ri = of (inhibitory) GABAergic inputs (n = 4), presumably because
0.9), the observed value (F = 0.29 0.12; n = 5) was substantially the hypertonic solution did not activate a sufficiently high
less than the predicted value (F = 0.722 = 0.52) and well below the ratio of excitatory to inhibitory input. This may be due in part
in vivo levels of one or above (Fig. 3c). The finding that the in vitro to the positioning of the puffer pipette near the soma, where
Fano factors, even for responses driven by inputs with a strong the density of GABAergic terminals may be higher. We were
inhibitory component, were consistently lower than those observed therefore unable to use hypertonic solution to validate the
in vivo reinforces the previous conclusion that independent inputs variability measurements for the synthetic inputs that mixed
cannot account for the high variability observed in vivo. excitation and inhibition. Nevertheless, the close agreement
ASYNCHRONOUS EPSC-EVOKED ACTION POTENTIALS between the synthetic and hypertonic solution-evoked vari-
The results presented so far have relied on current injected ability for the purely excitatory input suggests that currents
through a somatic electrode as a model for the synaptic cur- injected at the soma can adequately mimic synaptic inputs.
rents that drive neurons in vivo. Injected current may not,
however, mimic synaptic current in all respects. For example, INPUT SYNCHRONY YIELDS IN VIVO FIRING VARIABILITY
injected current implicitly treats each synapse as a current Because a steady input consisting of independent EPSCs and
source, whereas in fact a synapse is more properly considered IPSCs fails to drive firing that is as irregular as is observed in
as a conductance in series with the driving potential at the vivo, we tested the possibility that some degree of input syn-
subsynaptic membrane. (The dynamic clamp20 can only par- chrony might be required. We considered a simple model in
tially compensate for this error, as most excitatory synapses which the input consisted primarily of occasional large brief
onto cortical neurons are onto dendrites that are electrotoni- (3050 ms) excitatory events, randomly distributed in time
cally remote from the soma and whose subsynaptic voltage (average inter-event interval, 100 ms), each consisting of the
cannot be controlled by a somatic electrode.) We therefore nearly synchronous (3050 ms) arrival of on average about
developed a more direct approach to study the spiking vari- 100 to 200 EPSCs, yielding peak currents of 1 to 2 nA (Meth-
ability in response to synaptic stimulation. ods). Evidence from a variety of sources suggests the impor-
Because conventional extracellular stimulation triggers a tance of correlated firing in the neocortex9,16,2330.
large increase in the rate of transmitter release from all synap- Figure 5a shows a typical experiment in which the syn-

214 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

chronous input was synthesized according to this simple give rise to the observed spiking variability. The conundrum is
model. The irregularity of the resulting spike train was much that the fluctuations in the input drive generated by inde-
higher, with both the CV and the Fano factor in the in vivo pendent excitatory afferents seem too small to account for the
range in all neurons tested (CV = 1.3 0.11; F = 0.94 0.2, high variability of in-vivo cortical firing. The initial report1
= 19.8 1.0 spikes per s; n = 5). The data (Fig. 5b) demon- emphasized the difficulty in accounting for the high variabil-
strate that input correlations could account for the high degree ity at very high firing rates (over 100 Hz). In our experiments,
of variability observed in vivo. we found that even at more moderate firing rates (1525 Hz),
independent EPSCs cannot account for the variability
Discussion observed in vivo; at higher firing rates, independent EPSCs
The observations1 on the paradox posed by the irregularity of induced even lower output variability.
in vivo cortical spike trains have sparked considerable debate Previously proposed resolutions fall into two main classes.
on the underlying mechanism57,3234. Although Softky and First, simulations suggested that inhibitory inputs increase the
Koch1 raised the possibility that the unexpectedly large vari- input fluctuations and thereby the output variability suffi-
ability might arise from input synchrony, later workers have ciently to account for the in vivo responses57. Our experi-
argued that statistically stationary synaptic noise, interacting mental results, however, demonstrate that the output variability
with the spike-encoding mechanism, can account for the large in response even to a drive with a very strong inhibitory com-
variability. All of these previous studies have, however, relied ponent remains well below the in vivo levels, indicating that
on simulations of the spike encoder, and the results are sen- some alternative mechanism must be responsible.
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

sitive to assumptions about the encoder properties. The second class of explanation invokes the details of the
Our results do not rely on any theoretical model of the spike-generating mechanism. In one proposal7, the reset volt-
spike encoder. Rather, we have used the actual spike encoder of age following an action potential was used to match the slope
neurons in vitro to test directly whether the high variability or gain of the firing-intensity curve for the first interspike
observed in vivo could arise from a constant shower of statis- interval in an integrate-and-fire model. In simulations, the
tically independent synaptic inputs. We conclude that it could output variability of responses generated by this model was
not, and propose that the high in-vivo variability arises from near that found in vivo. This model failed, however, to pre-
correlations among the inputs28,29. We illustrate this by match- dict the responses of cortical neurons to the synthetic synap-
ing the in-vivo variability with a simple model of the input tic currents we used (C.S. and A.Z., unpublished results). The
drive, in which occasional large brief synchronous events are initial report1 proposed a more radical modification of the
superimposed on a tonic background. spike generator. Here it was suggested that the coincident
Our conclusions rest on two main assumptions. First, we arrival of just a few EPSPs might activate powerful nonlinear
have assumed that somatic current injection represents a rea- dendritic conductances that would then trigger a somatic
sonable model of synaptic drive. Although the close agree- spike. Although our experiments cannot rule out this mecha-
ment between the somatic current injection and hypertonic nism (and indeed, the large events shown in Fig. 5 could in
solution-evoked miniature EPSC results (Fig. 4) supports this principle arise from the activation of dendritic nonlineari-
notion, it is possible that the agreement would not be as good ties), we favor the notion that the dynamics of cortical net-
when the inhibitory drive was very strong. Second, we have works, rather than the properties of single neurons, underlie
assumed that the in-vitro biophysical properties of neocortical the requisite large fluctuations in the input drive.
neurons can be used to make inferences about their in-vivo The model of synaptic drive that we have used is by no
behavior. This supposes, for example, that the spontaneous means unique in its ability to account for the variability
miniature EPSCs recorded in slices accurately reflect the in- observed in vivo; countless other input ensembles would cer-
vivo synaptic amplitude distribution. In addition, the tainly have done as well. The key requirement is that the input
inputoutput transformation might be different in vivo, current contain very large fluctuations, much larger than
because of neuromodulatory influences35 for example, or the would be expected if all the synapses were releasing quanta
activation of large nonlinear conductances 36. independently and at a constant rate. These fluctuations could
arise from the brief and coordinated increase in the firing rate
MECHANISM UNDERLYING SPIKING VARIABILITY IN VIVO of a large number of excitatory, or perhaps inhibitory 37 ,
Irregular firing in vivo might in principle arise from input synapses. Moreover, if a small number of presynaptic neurons
variability or from noise in the spike-generating mechanism. each made dozens of powerful synaptic contacts onto a sin-
Because the neuronal spike generator is very reliable, input gle postsynaptic target38, then the requisite postsynaptic fluc-
variability is likely to be the primary source2,3. In spinal neu- tuations might arise from correlated activity in just this
rons, synaptic noise (fluctuations in membrane potential smaller subpopulation of neurons. Our experiments do not
arising from a barrage of EPSPs) fully accounts for output address the specific network mechanisms that might give rise
variability 4 . In the neocortex, the in-vivo synaptic drive to this correlated synaptic activity. Nevertheless, the present
inferred from current clamp recordings could, when injected results indicate that, however the input current is generated,
into neocortical neurons in vitro, reproduce their basic firing some kind of correlation among the synaptic inputs is likely
statistics 8 . These latter experiments did not interpret the to play a critical role in generating the high degree of vari-
injected currents in terms of the underlying synaptic proper- ability observed in vivo.
ties and correlational structure of the inputs, nor did they
establish the properties of the drive necessary or sufficient to SYNCHRONY AND THE NEURAL CODE
account for the high variability. The irregularity of cortical spike trains has led to two rather
In the present study, we therefore began with the assump- different models of how cortical circuitry operates. In the con-
tion that output variability reflects fluctuations in the input ventional model, this irregularity represents noise around
current and asked what form the synaptic noise must take to the signal, a perturbation around a mean firing rate that is

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 215


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

obtained by averaging over some period (such as one second) Recordings were obtained using an Axopatch 200A or 200B (Axon
that is much longer than a typical ISI. In this view, the precise Instruments). Most recordings were obtained in the current-clamp
timing of individual spikes conveys little information, because mode, without series-resistance compensation. Traces were filtered
it reflects only the noisy activity of other neurons attempting at 2 KHz and digitized at 4 KHz. Series resistance, monitored prior
to each trace, was typically in the range of 820 M, and never more
to signal their own mean rate. This model follows readily from
than 30 M. To compensate for any slow drift in membrane potential,
the idea that the drive to cortical neurons is composed of the prior to each trace sufficient current was injected to return the mem-
uncorrelated activity of its synaptic inputs; it would be hard brane to a value determined at the start of the experiment (between -
to imagine how the precise timing of spikes could represent 60 and -75 mV); if the actual rest potential decreased by more than
more than noise in this model, because adding or removing 5 mV from this level, the experiment was terminated. The response
just a few EPSPs might perturb the spike timing appreciably. to constant current injection (an FI curve) was also periodically
In the second model, the irregular spikes reflect modula- monitored to assess washout, and if the spike rate at the maximum
tion on a fine time scale of the neurons output rate. It is now intensity decreased by more than 30%, the experiment was termi-
established that under some conditions, the fine temporal nated (usually 3060 minutes).
structure of the spike train (the precise location of each spike) Forty-two regular spiking neurons43 from cortical layers 2/3 were
can carry information16,39,40. For example, when visual stim- analyzed. In addition, results from two layer 5 neurons were consistent
uli have fine temporal structure, the timing of spikes from with the other results and so were included in the averaged data. Cur-
neurons in MT cortex can be tightly (25 ms) locked to the rent stimulation was usually applied for one second every five or ten
stimulus16,41. Recordings of local field potentials suggest that seconds; the long interstimulus rest period helped to reduce the influ-
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

ence of one trial on the next. For the analysis, only neurons whose aver-
this high fidelity of spike timing is achieved by rapid comod- age firing rate was between 15 and 25 Hz were used. At higher sustained
ulation of the rates of the input neurons16; under these con- firing rates, recording stability tended to degrade more quickly.
ditions, the input correlations encode temporal edges within
Data were acquired using a National Instruments AT-MIO-16-F-
the stimulus. However, our results suggest that even when the
5 A/D card on a 120 MHz Pentium-based computer under the Win-
sensory stimulus does not have fine temporal structure, spikes dow NT (Microsoft) operating system. Software written in Labview
in cortical neurons may nevertheless arise from large events (National Instruments) with Dynamic Data Exchange links to Mat-
in the input drive that represent the correlated activity of many lab (Mathworks) allowed convenient online synthesis and injection
neurons. Such large events are presumably more robust to of arbitrary synthetic current waveforms.
noise than the small fluctuations that are posited to drive fir- SYNTHETIC SYNAPTIC CURRENTS. Neurons were driven to fire through
ing in the first model. Thus even when the sensory stimulus currents injected through a somatic patch electrode. Several models of
is devoid of fine temporal structure, spikes may be encoding correlated synaptic inputs were tested. In the independent EPSC model,
something with high temporal fidelity. the synaptic drive was given by I e = P(r e ) * I ampa , where P(r e ) is a
sequence of independent Poisson points arriving at a rate re, Iampa =
We et/e is the waveform of the basic EPSC (We = 30 pA, e = 3 ms),
and * indicates convolution. The firing rate under these conditions is
Methods determined by a single free parameter, the rate of synaptic impulses re.
SLICE PREPARATION. Brain slices were prepared from Long-Evans rats
(postnatal day 1428) that were deeply anesthetized with metofane In the mixed model, an inhibitory drive given by Ii = P(ri) * Igaba
and then decapitated. The skull was rapidly opened and the brain was added to the excitatory drive, where as above P(ri) is a sequence
placed in ice-cold Ringer solution. The cooled brain was glued with of independent Poisson points arriving at a rate ri, Igaba = Wi et/e is
cyanoacrylate to the stage of a vibratome and 400-m slices were cut. the waveform of the basic IPSC (Wi = 30 pA, i = 6 ms), and * indi-
Slices were transferred to a holding chamber and incubated for at least cates convolution. The total input under these conditions was given by
one hour at room temperature in a solution continuously bubbled Ib = Ii + Ie. In this scenario, the firing rate depended both on ri and re.
with a mixture of 95% O2 and 5% CO2, and then placed in a record- The value of ri was chosen to keep the ratio Ri of total inhibitory and
ing chamber. excitatory currents fixed, R i = r i/ r e where = (W e e)/(W m m)
takes into account differences in the synaptic waveforms.
PATCH-CLAMP RECORDING. Whole-cell, patch-clamp recordings were
made from neurons in the sensory neocortex visualized through an Finally, we examined the effect of synchronous excitatory inputs,
upright microscope equipped with infrared light and differential inter- Is = C(t) * Iampa, where C(t) was the stochastic function that describes
ference optics42. Recordings were performed at 3335C. Slices were the occurrence of synchronous events. C(t) consisted of 3050-ms
continuously perfused with a Ringers solution containing (in mM) periods of elevated input activity; these periods occurred in a ran-
NaCl 120, KCl 3.5, CaCl2 2.6, MgCl2 1.3, NaH2PO4 1.25, NaHCO3 26, dom (Poisson) fashion at a rate of 10 Hz. During each 3050-ms peri-
glucose 10, pH 7.35. Unless otherwise indicated, recordings were od of elevated activity, about 200 EPSCs were distributed in 28
obtained in the presence of the AMPA receptor antagonist 6-cyano-7- shorter events. The synchronous input Is was added to Ie and Ii.
nitroquinoxaline-2,3-dione (CNQX; 10 M). Recording pipettes were
filled with an internal solution consisting of (in mM) K gluconate, Acknowledgements
170; HEPES, 10; NaCl, 10; MgCl 2 , 2; EGTA, 1.33; CaCl 2 , 0.133; We thank G. Buracas for the MT data and L. Dobrunz, E. Huang, K. Miller,
MgATP, 3.5; GTP, 1.0, pH 7.2 and 290300 mOsm. Resistance to bath P. Latham, T. Troyer and K. Zhang for comments. This work was supported
was 35 M before seal formation. by the Howard Hughes Medical Institute (C.F.S.), National Institutes of
In experiments in which the rate of miniature EPSCs was elevated Health grant NS 12961 (C.F.S.) and the Sloan Center for Theoretical
by locally perfusing with hypertonic solution, the bath Ringers con- Neurobiology (A.M.Z.).
tained 100 M picrotoxin (to block -aminobutyric acid type A recep-
tor responses), 30 M Cd+2 to block synaptic responses that could RECEIVED 9 APRIL: ACCEPTED 25 MAY 1998
cause recurrent activity and no CNQX. To elicit hypertonic-solution-
evoked miniature EPSCs, visual guidance was used to position a 2 m
puffer pipette (containing bath Ringers to which 0.5 mM sucrose had 1. Softky, W. & Koch, C. The highly irregular firing of cortical cells is
inconsistent with temporal integration of random epsps. J. Neurosci. 13,
been added) close to dendrites about 3050 m from the somatic 334350 (1993).
recording electrode. A picospritzer II was then used to apply 12 sec- 2. Mainen, Z.F. & Sejnowski, T.J. Reliability of spike timing in neocortical
ond pulses (36 psi) of the pipette solution. neurons. Science 268, 15031505 (1995).

216 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

3. Zador, A. The impact of synaptic unreliability on the information 23. Abeles, M. CorticonicsNeural Circuits of the Cerebral Cortex. (Cambridge
transmitted by spiking neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 19, 12301238 (1998). Univ. Press, 1991).
4. Calvin, W.H. & Stevens, C.F. Synaptic noise and other sources of randomness 24. Alonso, J.M., Usrey, W.M. & Reid, R.C. Precisely correlated firing in cells of
in motoneuron interspike intervals. J. Neurophysiol. 31, 574587 (1968). the lateral geniculate nucleus. Nature 383, 815819 (1996).
5. Shadlen, M.N. & Newsome, W.T. Is there a signal in the noise? Curr. Opin. 25. deCharms, R.C. & Merzenich, M.M. Primary cortical representation of
Neurobiol. 5, 248250 (1995). sounds by the coordination of action-potential timing. Nature 381, 610613
6. Shadlen, M.N. & Newsome, W.T. The variable discharge of cortical neurons: (1996).
Implications for connectivity, computation, and information coding. J. 26. Nelson, J.I., Salin, P.A., Munk, M.H., Arzi, M. & Bullier, J. Spatial and
Neurosci. 18, 3870-3896 (1998). temporal coherence in cortico-cortical connections: a cross-correlation study
7. Troyer, T.W. & Miller, K.D. Physiological gain leads to high ISI variability in a in areas 17 and 18 in the cat. Visual Neurosci. 9, 2137 (1992).
simple model of a cortical regular spiking cell. Neural Computation 9, 27. Singer, W. & Gray, C.M. Visual feature integration and the temporal
971983 (1997). correlation hypothesis. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 555586 (1995).
8. Nowak, L.G., Sanchez-Vives, M.V. & McCormick, D.A. Influence of low and 28. Usher, M., Stemmler, M., Koch, C. & Olami, Z. Network amplification of
high frequency inputs on spike timing in visual cortical neurons. Cereb. local fluctuations causes high spike rate variability, fractal firing patterns and
Cortex 7, 487501 (1997). oscillatory local field potentials. Neural Computation 6, 795836 (1994).
9. Berretta, B. & Jones, S.G. A comparison of spontaneous EPSCs in layer II and 29. van Vreeswijk, C. & Sompolinsky, H. Chaos in neuronal networks with
layer IV-V neurons of the rat entorhinal cortex in vitro. J. Neurophysiol. 76, balanced excitatory and inhibitory activity. Science 274, 17241726 (1996).
10891100 (1996). 30. Whittington, M.A., Traub, R.D., Faulkner, H.J., Stanford, I.M. & Jefferys, J.G.
10. Burgard, E.C. & Hablitz, J.J. NMDA receptor-mediated components of Recurrent excitatory postsynaptic potentials induced by synchronized fast
miniature excitatory synaptic currents in developing rat neocortex. J. cortical oscillations. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 94, 1219812203 (1997).
Neurophysiol. 70, 18411852 (1993). 31. Zohary, E., Shadlen, M.N. & Newsome, W.T. Correlated neuronal discharge
11. Bekkers, J.M., Richerson, G.B. & Stevens, C.F. Origin of variability in quantal rate and its implications for psychophysical performance. Nature 370,
size in cultured hippocampal neurons and hippocampal slices. Proc. Natl 140143 (1994).
Acad. Sci. USA 87, 53595362 (1990). 32. Shadlen, M.N. & Newsome, W.T. Noise, neural codes and cortical
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

12. Feller, W. An Introduction to Probability Theory and its Applications, vol. 2, organization. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 4, 569579 (1994).
2nd edn. (Wiley, New York, 1971). 33. Softky, W.R. Simple codes versus efficient codes. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 5,
13. Burns, B.D. & Webb, A.C. The spontaneous activity of neurones in the cats 239247 (1995).
cerebral cortex. Proc. Royal Soc. Lond. B 194, 211223 (1976). 34. Reich, D.S., Victor, J.D., Knight, B.W., Ozaki, T. & Kaplan, E. Response
14. Holt, G.R. Softky, W.R., Koch, C. & Douglas, R.J. Comparison of discharge variability and timing precision of neuronal spike trains in vivo. J.
variability in vitro and in vivo in cat visual cortex neurons. J. Neurophysiol. Neurophysiol. 77, 28362841 (1997).
75, 18061814 (1996). 35. Tang, A.C., Bartels, A.M. & Sejnowski, T.J. Effects of cholinergic modulation
15. Noda, H. & Adey, R. Firing variability in cat association cortex during sleep on responses of neocortical neurons to fluctuating inputs. Cereb. Cortex 7,
and wakefulness. Brain Res. 18, 513526 (1970). 502509 (1997).
16. Buracas, G., Zador, A., Deweese, M. & Albright, T. Efficient discrimination of 36. Softky, W.R. Sub-millisecond coincidence detection in active dendritic trees.
temporal patterns by motion-sensitive neurons in primate visual cortex. Neuroscience 58, 1341 (1994).
Neuron 20, 959969 (1998). 37. Hausser, M. & Clark, B.A. Tonic synaptic inhibition modulates neuronal
17. Berman, N.J., Douglas, R.J., Martin, K.A. & Whitteridge, D. Mechanisms of output pattern and spatiotemporal synaptic integration. Neuron 19, 665678
inhibition in cat visual cortex. J. Physiol. 440, 697722 (1991). (1997).
18. Nelson, S., Toth, L., Sheth, B. & Sur, M. Orientation selectivity of cortical neurons 38. Markram, H. A network of tufted layer 5 pyramidal neurons. Cereb. Cortex 7,
during intracellular blockade of inhibition. Science 265, 774777 (1994). 523533 (1997).
19. Gershon, E.D., Wiener, M.C., Latham, P.E. & Richmond, B.J. Coding 39. Bialek, W., Rieke, F., de Ruyter van Steveninck, R.R. & Warland, D. Reading a
strategies in monkey V1 and inferior temporal cortices. J. Neurophysiol. 79, neural code. Science 252, 18541857 (1991).
11351144 (1998). 40. Rieke, F., Warland, D., de Ruyter van Steveninck, R.R. & Bialek, W. Spikes:
20. Sharp, A.A., ONeil, M.B., Abbott, L.F. & Marder, E. Dynamic clamp: Exploring the Neural Code. (MIT Press, 1997).
computer-generated conductances in real neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 3, 41. Bair, W. & Koch, C. Temporal precision of spike trains in extrastriate cortex
992995 (1993). of the behaving macaque monkey. Neural Computation 8, 11841202 (1996).
21. Hubbard, J.I., Jones, S.F. & Landau, E.M. An examination of the effects of 42. Stuart, G.J., Dodt, H.U. & Sakmann, B. Patch-clamp recordings from the
osmotic pressure changes upon transmitter release from mammalian motor soma and dendrites of neurons in brain slices using infrared video
nerve terminals. J. Physiol. 197, 639657 (1968). microscopy. Pflug. Archiv. Eur. J. Physiol. 423, 511518 (1993).
22. Stevens, C.F. & Tsujimoto, T. Estimates for the pool size of releasable quanta 43. McCormick, D.A., Connors, B.W., Lighthall, J.W. & Prince, D.A.
at a single central synapse and for the time required to refill the pool. Proc. Comparative electrophysiology of pyramidal and sparsely spiny stellate
Natl Acad. Sci. USA 92, 846849 (1995). neurons of the neocortex. J. Neurophysiol. 54, 782806 (1985).

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 217


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Nociceptive and thermoreceptive


lamina I neurons are anatomically
distinct
Z.-S. Han1,2, E.-T. Zhang1 and A. D. Craig1

1 Divisions of Neurobiology and Neurosurgery, Barrow Neurological Institute, 350 West Thomas Rd., Phoenix, Arizona 85013, USA
2 Present address: RS Dow Neurological Sciences Institute, 1120 NW 20th Ave., Portland, Oregon 97209, USA
Correspondence should be addressed to A.D.C. (bcraig@mha.chw.edu)

Pain and temperature stimuli activate neurons of lamina I within the dorsal horn of the spinal cord,
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

and although these neurons can be classified into three basic morphological types and three major
physiological classes, earlier studies did not establish a structure/function correlation between their
morphology and their physiological responses. We recorded and intracellularly labeled 38 cat
lamina I neurons. All 12 fusiform cells were nociceptive-specific, responsive only to pinch and/or
heat. All 11 pyramidal cells were thermoreceptive-specific, responsive only to innocuous cooling. Of
ten multipolar cells, six were polymodal, responsive to heat, pinch and cold, and four were nocicep-
tive-specific. Five unclassified cells had features consistent with this pattern. These results support
the view that central pain and temperature pathways contain anatomically discrete sets of
modality-selective neurons.

Lamina I of the superficial dorsal horn is an integral component ical differences that could be reflected in the morphology of
of the central representation of pain and temperature sensitivi- their cell bodies in lamina I. In horizontal sections, lamina I
ties in mammals13. It receives direct input from A and C pri- neurons in rat, cat and monkey can be anatomically catego-
mary afferent fibers, including specific nociceptors and rized almost comprehensively into three basic, albeit variegat-
thermoreceptors, and it is the main source of output from the ed, cell types: fusiform, pyramidal, and multipolar 2730. The
superficial dorsal horn. In contrast to the deep dorsal horn with fusiform cells appear to have unmyelinated axons, whereas
its modality-ambiguous wide dynamic range (WDR) nocicep- pyramidal and multipolar cells have myelinated axons; this
tive neurons, which have been studied extensively, lamina I con- matches the physiological difference in conduction velocities
tains unique concentrations of modality-selective nociceptive between NS cells and COLD or HPC cells. These physiological
and thermoreceptive neurons. In primates, the axons of these and morphological classifications were not distinguished in
neurons project in the lateral spinothalamic tract (which is crit- prior intracellular labeling studies of lamina I cells for method-
ical for pain and temperature sensation) to a dedicated nocicep- ological reasons. We directly re-examined the characteristics of
tive- and thermoreceptive-specific relay nucleus in posterolateral lamina I neurons, using intracellular recording and labeling,
thalamus4 and to other sites3 that together provide input to the which revealed a clear correspondence between these anatom-
cortical regions that are activated by painful and thermal stim- ical and physiological classifications.
uli in humans5,6.
These characteristics support the concept that pain is sub- Results
served by distinct sets of neurons both peripherally and cen- PHYSIOLOGICAL CHARACTERISTICS
trally. This view is not shared by all investigators in the field of We recorded intracellularly from a total of 126 cells in the super-
pain research7, in part because numerous intracellular labeling ficial dorsal horn of 41 anesthetized cats. Stable intracellular
studies did not uncover distinct types of neurons in the super- recordings were obtained for several minutes to over one hour.
ficial dorsal horn816. This contrasts with the visual and motor Altogether 76 cells were injected with biocytin, 53 of which were
systems, where distinct structure/function correlations have recovered histologically, and 38 of which were lamina I neurons
been identified17,18, and accumulating evidence has suggested that had been characterized physiologically. The remaining recov-
that this issue deserves re-examination. Physiological studies ered cells were uncharacterized, unresponsive and/or were locat-
of lamina I spinothalamic cells have documented three major ed in lamina II or deeper. For some identified cells, there was
classes of modality-selective neurons in cat and monkey: noci- apparent injury during penetration, based on the gradual dete-
ceptive-specific (NS) cells, thermoreceptive-specific (COLD) rioration of the membrane potential and action-potential ampli-
cells, and polymodal nociceptive (HPC) cells (and also WDR tude, yet the spontaneous activity pattern and the response
cells in monkey)1925. These classes have different axonal con- properties to natural stimuli remained stable.
duction velocities and thalamic terminations, as shown with All 38 characterized, labeled lamina I neurons were respon-
antidromic mapping (refs 21, 24, 26; Dostrovsky, J.O. & A.D.C., sive to natural cutaneous stimulation of the ventral hindpaw. The
Soc. Neurosci. Abstr., 646.9, 1993), features indicating anatom- response characteristics of these neurons were consistent with

218 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

NS cell COLD cell HPC cell

Fig. 1. Intracellular responses of NS, COLD and HPC cells. Bars indicate the application of
stimuli. The intensity of the pinch stimuli was increased during application. The NS cell (mul-
tipolar cell 36-739) had a resting membrane potential of -45 mV and a spike amplitude that
varied with firing rate. It developed a high ongoing discharge rate, which may have occluded a
phasic response to the brief cold stimulus. The COLD cell (pyramidal cell 25-670) had a rest-
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

ing membrane potential of -40 mV, a steady spike amplitude of 45 mV, and an afterhyperpolarization of 10 mV. It had irregular ongoing dis-
charge at room temperature (about 23C) prior to any stimulation. The HPC cell (multipolar cell 44-1000) had a resting membrane potential
of -55 mV and a spike amplitude of 60 mV. The small arrow indicates hyperpolarizing current pulses (0.5 nA, 50 ms, 1Hz) applied to stabilize
the recording.

observations in prior studies2023, and so the cells were classified Based on their intracellular responses to natural stimuli, 18 of
as nociceptive-specific (NS) cells responsive only to pinch and/or the 38 identified lamina I neurons were categorized as NS cells,
heat, thermoreceptive-specific (COLD) cells responsive only to 13 as COLD cells and 7 as HPC cells (examples in Fig. 1). None
innocuous cooling and inhibited by warming, and polymodal of the cells was responsive to low-threshold (weak) stimuli. Of the
nociceptive (HPC) cells responsive to heat and/or pinch and also 18 NS cells, 8 responded only to noxious pinch, and 10 respond-
to cold. (WDR lamina I neurons are rare in the cat8,9,11,15,21,26.) ed to both pinch and noxious heat (Fig. 1). The NS cells often
Although the need for rapid characterization prevented us from showed prolonged afterdischarge following a noxious stimulus
systematically delimiting the receptive fields of nociceptive cells, (Fig. 1) and moderate-sized spontaneous excitatory postsynaptic
they were generally small and confined, ranging in size from part potentials (Fig. 2d). The 13 COLD cells were readily excited by
of one toe pad to most of the ventral surface of the paw. Many cooling but were not excited by pinch or heat; rather, their dis-
receptive fields occupied the glabrous pads on one or two digits, charges were usually inhibited by heat or even by the warmth of
sometimes including a region of hairy skin between the toes or the fingers when used for pinching (Fig. 1). One COLD cell (19-
near the central pad. The receptive fields of COLD cells were gen- 558) showed a paradoxical burst response to heat. Most HPC cells
erally larger, including half or more often all of the ventral paw. responded vigorously to noxious heat and in a graded manner to

a c

b d

Fig. 2. Intracellular responses of different classes of lamina I cells to prolonged current injection (0.5 nA, 500600 ms, indicated by the bars).
(a, b) The COLD cell in (a) and the HPC cell in (b) both produced repetitive discharges without significant frequency adaptation. Note the
deep but brief afterhyperpolarization following each COLD cell action potential in (a) and the more shallow but broad and variable afterhy-
perpolarization following each HPC cell action potential in (b). (c, d) In contrast, both of the NS cells responded to current injection with
action potential trains that exhibited pronounced frequency adaptation, and both displayed a prolonged hyperpolarizing potential following
current offset (triangular arrowheads). Both also had spontaneous EPSPs (small downward arrows). The cell in (c) had action potentials rid-
ing on EPSPs and often fired in doublets (upward arrows). The cell in (d) showed variable, brief afterhyperpolarizations.

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 219


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

noxious mechanical stimuli; their responses to cold 17-699 pinch + heat, U 22-820 pinch + heat, F
were less brisk than those of COLD cells and were
often delayed (Fig. 1). As in prior work, HPC cells had
varied thresholds to each submodality, and with the 43-451 pinch + heat, F
22-964 pinch + heat, F
rapid characterization methods we employed, three
HPC cells were detected that apparently had high
thresholds to thermal stimulation. Two of these (27-
551 and 44-1000) showed only a transient response
to the brief (3 to 5 second) cold stimuli, which is char- 16-865 pinch, F 28-1007 pinch + heat, F
acteristic of HPC cells that have high noxious cold
thresholds and produce a tonic response to stimuli of
longer duration (30 to 60 seconds) than was practical
for this study21. One (20-910) that responded clearly 35-1169 pinch + heat, F 25-690 pinch + heat, F
to pinch and to cold had only a weak, poorly repro-
duced response to radiant heat, similar to HPC cells
that have high thresholds to maintained heat upon
quantitative examination with a Peltier stimulator21.
We applied antidromic stimulation to the con- 23-795 pinch, F 17-755 pinch + heat, F
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

tralateral thalamus in 22 of the 41 experiments, in an


attempt to identify spinothalamic lamina I neurons26.
Antidromic responses to trains of stimuli were
observed in two of three identified cells in which
extracellular recordings were obtained prior to pen-
etration and staining (25-690 and 28-1007). Howev- 45-794 pinch, M 43-750 pinch, M
er, in intracellular recordings, only one or two
time-locked action potentials were observed with
antidromic stimulation in four identified lamina I
cells (17-755, 19-558, 21-633, and 25-670). This is
consistent with the reported failure of antidromic
somatic invasion in intracellularly recorded lamina I
neurons following stimulation of the midbrain 36-739 pinch, M
parabrachial region9. Thus, whereas only these 6 of
the 24 neurons identified in 22 cats could be consid-
ered putative lamina I spinothalamic cells, many oth- 100 M
ers may also have been spinothalamic.
The intracellular responses to a step-depolarizing Fig. 3. Camera-lucida drawings of the soma and proximal dendrites of each iden-
current pulse (500 milliseconds) corresponded with tified NS cell. For each cell, the response characteristics and the morphological
the classification of the identified neurons in the small classification (F, fusiform; M, multipolar; U, unclassified or transitional) are indi-
subset that was tested. A single action potential was cated. The shading indicates processes that extended dorsally or ventrally out of
the plane of the horizontal section or that were more weakly labeled. Rostral is
normally evoked at threshold intensities (0.2 to 0.4
left, medial is up. Scale bar, 100 m.
nA), but depolarization at suprathreshold intensities
(over 0.5 nA, 500 milliseconds) produced a train of
non-decrementing action potentials with a steady
interspike interval in COLD cells (Fig. 2a, n = 4) and HPC cells (Methods), examined by light microscopy and documented with
(Fig. 2b, n = 3). In contrast, NS cells (n = 4) displayed signifi- photomicrographs and camera lucida drawings. An additional
cant frequency adaptation (Fig. 2c and d). Further, COLD cells eight cells were stained with FITC-avidin (in double-labeling
showed a prominent afterhyperpolarization following each action experiments performed to test various immunohistochemical
potential (Figs 1 and 2a), whereas variable or no afterhyperpo- markers) and thus were not drawn. Most of the identified neu-
larization was generally present in NS or HPC cells (Figs 1 and rons were located in the dorsal cap at the lateral apex of the dor-
2). However, NS cells displayed a prolonged hyperpolarization sal horn, where lamina I spinothalamic cells are concentrated.
following intracellular current injection (Fig. 2c and d). These The somata and dendrites of all labeled cells were well stained
correlations of membrane properties with functional class resem- and generally could be followed longitudinally within lamina I
ble those reported for dorsal horn cells31 and are clearly sugges- for hundreds of microns (see Figs 6 and 7). The shape and ori-
tive of differences between these physiological categories in entation of the somata and proximal dendrites of most cells could
voltage-gated channels. There was also a tendency for the mean be assessed in one section. For a few cells, labeled elements in
half-amplitude spike widths of COLD cells to be greater, although adjacent serial sections needed to be carefully fitted with the use
it was not statistically significant (COLD, 0.91 0.51, n = 8, ver- of tracing paper or digital overlays. Their axons, if observable,
sus NS, 0.55 0.19, n = 6, p = 0.12; COLD versus HPC, 0.59 were very fine and were not included in the drawings.
0.25, n = 7, p = 0.20). We categorized the identified lamina I neurons anatomically
as fusiform, pyramidal, multipolar and unclassified or transition-
MORPHOLOGICAL CHARACTERISTICS al neurons, based on the morphological criteria of somatoden-
Of the 38 physiologically characterized lamina I cells injected dritic shape and orientation in horizontal sections (see below) that
with biocytin, 30 were stained with the ABC/DAB reaction we have used in prior studies of retrogradely labeled lamina I

220 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

23-734 cold, P 31-672 cold, P Of the 18 NS neurons identified, 10 were typ-


ical spindle-shaped, longitudinally oriented,
bipolar fusiform lamina I neurons of various
sizes (Fig. 3). Cell 35-1169 differed slightly in
that its large soma was located at the border
22-903 cold, P 35-1129 cold, P between lamina I and lamina II, but its dendrites
extended dorsally into lamina I. Two other NS
cells (23-795 and 17-755) belonged to the T-
shaped variety of fusiform cells, using the crite-
45-777 cold, P
ria described previously29,30, because they had a
19-558 cold, P
minor dendrite protruding perpendicularly from
the longitudinal, spindle-shaped soma (see Fig.
3). One NS cell (17-699) is an example of a bipo-
lar cell with an eccentric soma, a shape that was
termed unclassified in our prior studies, but
27-176 cold, P 25-670 cold, P which could simply be another subtype of the
fusiform category. The other unclassified NS cell
found was an unusual bipolar cell (FITC-labeled,
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

not shown) that has a very large, round soma


and widely radiating secondar y dendritic
branches. In addition, four NS cells were excep-
tional in that they were multipolar neurons;
43-627 cold, U 27-333 cold, U three of these (45-794, 43-750 and 47-726 [not
shown]) are of the tube-like variety described
earlier29,30, and one (36-739) is of the radiating
subtype. Cell 36-739 was one of only two iden-
tified neurons that had spiny dendrites.
Of the 13 COLD neurons identified, all
showed a general triangular shape, and 11 were
100 M
classified as pyramidal lamina I neurons with
three major dendritic poles and three or four
Fig. 4. Camera-lucida drawings of the soma and proximal dendrites of each identi- dendrites. (Fig. 4 shows eight of these; three were
fied COLD cell. For each cell, the response characteristics and the morphological labeled with FITC.) As in our prior observations
classification (P, pyramidal; U, unclassified or transitional) are indicated. The shading of lamina I spinothalamic cells, the dendrites of
indicates processes that extended dorsally or ventrally out of the plane of the hor- most of these pyramidal COLD neurons issued
izontal section or that were more weakly labeled. Rostral is left, medial is up. Scale few arbors and extended primarily longitudinal-
bar, 100 m. ly, though a few cells had more widespread ram-
ifications (e.g., 22-903 or 27-176). Cell 25-670
differed in that it had only two major dendrites.
In addition, two exceptional COLD cells (43-627
spinothalamic cells in cat and monkey29,30. Even though there was and 27-333) were categorized morphologically as unclassified
variation in the fine details of the morphology of the neurons in or transitional neurons, because even though they had gen-
this sample, which is similar to the variation observed in prior erally triangular shapes, they had multiple dendritic origins.
work2730, overall there was an obvious correspondence between Cell 43-627 also differed because of a protruding bulge at the
the functional category and the somatodendritic shape of these rostral, apical (left in Fig. 4) end of the soma, and because its
cells, which indicates that there are three major structural/func- basilar dendrites on the caudal (right) end were rotated out
tional types of lamina I neurons. Of the 18 NS cells identified, 12 of the horizontal plane.
were fusiform neurons, whereas 4 were multipolar and 2 were Of the seven HPC cells identified, six were multipolar lam-
unclassified. Of the 13 COLD cells, 11 were pyramidal, whereas 2 ina I neurons that had polygonal somata with multiple den-
were unclassified. Of the 7 HPC cells, 6 were multipolar and 1 was drites that arborized both longitudinally and mediolaterally.
unclassified. The converse description reveals this correspondence Of these, three (21-633, 44-1000, 32-740) were of the radiat-
even more clearly. Of the 12 fusiform neurons, all were NS cells. ing variety of multipolar cells and three (24-514, 27-551 and
Of the 11 pyramidal neurons, all were COLD cells. Of the 10 mul- 20-910) were of the tube-like variety29,30. Cells 21-633 and
tipolar cells, 6 were HPC cells and 4 were NS cells. The interme- 44-1000 were clearly similar to the -shaped subtype of mul-
diate characteristics of the unclassified neurons were consistent tipolar lamina I spinothalamic cells described in the mon-
with this pattern. The drawings in Figs 35 show the physiologi- key30, but infrequently observed in the cat29. Cell 32-740 was
cal characteristics, the anatomical classification and the somato- the only cell that resembled somewhat the quadrilateral sub-
dendritic morphology of each cell from the sample of 30 cells type that was relatively commonly observed in both cat and
stained with diaminobenzidine, organized according to the three monkey. In addition, one HPC cell (26-882) was a large,
functional categories identified (NS, COLD, HPC) . The pho- unclassified neuron located on the laminae I-II border that
tomicrographs in Fig. 6 show examples of each morphological cell had two major and several minor radiating, spiny dendrites
type. Figure 7 shows the full dendritic arborization of one repre- extending into lamina I and the dorsal white matter and also
sentative neuron of each major structural/functional category. into lamina II; such a cell has not been observed before.

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 221


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

21-633 heat + pinch + cold, M 44-1000 heat + pinch + cold, M of a homeomorphic correspondence between these respec-
tive classes. This finding supports the view that there are
three basic types of lamina I neurons with correlated phys-
iological and morphological characteristics: fusiform NS
cells, pyramidal COLD cells, and multipolar HPC cells.
32-740 heat + pinch + cold, M 24-514 heat + pinch + cold, M This evidence provides direct support for the concept
that pain and temperature are represented centrally by dis-
tinct sets of modality-selective neurons13. It indicates that
the functional and anatomical characteristics of these neu-
rons are linked and developmentally specified. We infer for
27-551 heat + pinch + cold, M several reasons that these lamina I cell types are a general
mammalian feature. These morphological types have been
observed in rat, cat and monkey. The three physiological
26-882 heat + pinch + cold, U classes have been demonstrated in cat and in monkey. In the
rat, physiological data from a lamina I projection target
(parabrachial nucleus34) indirectly support this idea. Fur-
thermore, a thermoreceptive-(COLD)-specific subnucleus
20-910 pinch + cold, M
within trigeminal lamina I of the owl monkey has recently
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

been identified, and it contains almost entirely pyramidal


trigeminothalamic neurons, whereas fusiform and multi-
polar neurons are present in the adjacent portions of lamina
I, where NS and HPC cells are recorded (Blomqvist, A., E.-
100 M T.Z. & A.D.C. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 48.15, 1995).
The correspondence of the three morphological types with
Fig. 5. Camera-lucida drawings of the soma and proximal dendrites of the three major physiological classes validates the biological
each identified HPC cell. For each cell, the response characteristics and relevance of these respective categories. Further, this evidence
the morphological classification (M, multipolar; U, unclassified or transi- for discrete categories of lamina I neurons implies the exis-
tional) are indicated. The shading indicates processes that extended dor-
tence of other pharmacological and neurochemical correlates
sally or ventrally out of the plane of the horizontal section or that were
that, when uncovered, will facilitate the analysis of specific
more weakly labeled. Rostral is left, medial is up. Scale bar, 100 m.
pathways activated by painful and thermal stimuli. For exam-
ple, many NS lamina I cells that respond to substance P in the
rat35 are fusiform cells that bear NK1 receptors36,37, which are
Discussion found on very few pyramidal neurons (Yu, X.H., A.D.C. et al.,
Prior physiological and morphological findings indicate that lam- unpublished observations). Similarly, the COLD-specific subnu-
ina I neurons belong to different classes. There are at least three cleus within trigeminal lamina I of the owl monkey is devoid of
different physiological classes, which differ not only in their fibers immunoreactive for substance P or serotonin, in contrast to
response characteristics but also in the conduction velocities of the remainder of lamina I (Blomqvist, A., E.-T.Z. & A.D.C. Soc. Neu-
their ascending axons, the distribution of their terminals in the rosci. Abstr. 48.15, 1995). In addition, these cell types may contain
thalamus, their susceptibility to descending controls, and their different peptides in the rat38. Conversely, physiological inferences
responses to pharmacological modulation20,21,23,32,33. Similarly, can now be made based on anatomical knowledge of the types of
there are three basic morphological types of neurons that differ in lamina I cells that terminate in a region. For example, retrograde
their somatodendritic shape, the myelination of their axons and labeling work indicates that all three types of lamina I cells project to
their longitudinal distribution in the spinal cord2730. In the pre- the autonomic cell column in the ventrolateral medulla in cat; this
sent study, we re-examined the possibility of a correlation suggests that, in addition to nociceptive sensitivity39, neurons in this
between cell morphology and response characteristics with intra- region may respond to innocuous cooling, which was recently con-
cellular labeling in the cat, and we have obtained strong evidence firmed (Krout, K. & A.D.C., unpublished observations).

Fig. 6. Digital photomicrographs of intracellularly stained, identified lamina I neurons. F1, fusiform NS cell 28-1007, with a small arrowhead
indicating a second faintly labeled cell; F2, fusiform NS cell 22-820; P1, pyramidal COLD cell 35-1129; P2, pyramidal COLD cell 23-734; M1,
multipolar HPC cell 21-633; M2, multipolar NS cell 45-794; T1, T-shaped fusiform NS cell 17-755; U1, unclassified or transitional COLD cell
27-333. Scale bar, 100 m.

222 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fusiform NS
was deemed practical during intracellular characteri-
28-1007
zation in the present study.
The correspondence we have identified was not dis-
covered in numerous previous attempts to correlate
function and morphology in lamina I with intracellu-
lar HRP labeling816, probably due to methodological
Pyramidal COLD reasons. First, sagittal or transverse sections were cut in
23-734 all of the previous studies. This precluded recognition
of the morphological types differentiated in the pre-
sent study, which can be consistently observed only in
the horizontal (or tangential) plane in which lamina
I neurons arborize. Second, innocuous and noxious
cold stimuli were not applied in most of the previous
studies. This precluded identification of the COLD
and HPC functional classes. Notably, cold stimuli were
Multipolar HPC used in the earliest study8, and the single lamina I cell
24-514 identified as specifically sensitive to innocuous cool-
ing was described as a pyramidal cell, although in
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

transverse sections. The one cell that was identified as


responsive to heat, pinch and cold (explicitly ascribed
to polymodal C nociceptor input, albeit perhaps sen-
sitized) had several dendrites extending horizontally
into lamina I, but its shape cannot be discerned from
the sagittal reconstruction.
In almost all of the other previous studies, cold
stimuli were not used, and lamina I neurons were char-
acterized only as NS or WDR cells. Significantly, the
100 M
physiological classification of lamina I cells can be con-
Fig. 7. Camera lucida reconstructions of the full dendritic extent of one exam- founded by unintentional thermal stimulation. Both
ple of each of the identified functional/morphological categories of lamina I neu- COLD cells and HPC cells with high cold sensitivity
rons. Nearly all of the dendrites were contained in two or three serial can be misconstrued as WDR cells if the temperatures
horizontal sections for each cell. Rostral is left, medial is up. of the probes used for characterization are uncon-
trolled, because even a room-temperature probe can
evoke a graded low-threshold response in such cells,
whereas a neutral probe (warmed to skin temperature)
It must be noted that, although the morphological charac- does not. The resemblance between the thermal sensitivity of
teristics of each of the three basic categories are generally con- some WDR lamina I spinothalamic cells24 and that of HPC lam-
sistent, there is nonetheless variation within each. This may ina I spinothalamic cells22,23 supports this possibility. In addi-
reflect a degree of ambiguity, or incomplete selectivity, in devel- tion, NS lamina I cells can also be misinterpreted as WDR cells,
opmental specification. This may also imply that subdivisions because they can become sensitized to weak stimulation follow-
of these basic categories of lamina I neurons remain to be iden- ing repeated noxious or C-fiber stimuli, such as commonly used
tified. We consider this latter possibility rather likely, in part for searching26,44. These considerations underscore the recogni-
because lamina I receives inputs from all tissues of the body tion of three major physiological/morphological categories of
(including muscles, joints, viscera, cornea and teeth26,40; for lamina I cells. Nevertheless, the possibility remains that WDR
other references, see ref. 3), whereas we stimulated only skin, lamina I cells, which have been observed more often in monkey
and in part because subtypes of each category are consistently than in cat, might yet be distinguishable, and this should be
recognizable morphologically2730. For example, in the present directly re-examined in light of the present observations.
sample we obtained almost no quadrilateral multipolar cells, Finally, the observation of distinct types of nociceptive and
although these are common in the cat, which indicates that such thermoreceptive lamina I neurons is consistent with the concept
neurons may not receive cutaneous input. that these neurons supply substrates for discrete sensory chan-
Accordingly, the four exceptional NS cells that are multipo- nels for pain and temperature sensibilities. Prior comparative
lar neurons could represent cells that received additional input psychophysical and physiological results obtained using the ther-
from untested tissues or that were sensitive to untested sub- mal grill illusion (in which a sensation of burning, ice-like pain is
modalities that evince polymodal C fiber activity, such as chem- evoked by interlaced cool and warm stimuli) indicated that the
ical or metabolic stimuli 4143 . It is also possible that these activity of COLD lamina I spinothalamic neurons can be asso-
multipolar cells were polymodal nociceptive HPC cells with high ciated directly with cold sensation, whereas the activity of HPC
thermal thresholds that were incompletely characterized, which lamina I spinothalamic neurons can be associated with the burn-
would conform with the remaining observations. The HPC lam- ing pain caused by noxious cold or unmasked by the thermal
ina I spinothalamic cells can have different sensitivities to the grill6,22. Furthermore, other work indicates that these neurons
submodalities of heat, pinch and cold, and some respond to cold can provide the input for modality-selective somato-autonomic
only below 15 C or to heat only above 50 C when tested with a (homeostatic) reflexes to painful and thermal stimuli45,46. These
Peltier stimulator21,22 (unpublished observations). Such noxious findings have led to the proposal that the fundamental role of
thermal stimuli can require a longer stimulus application than lamina I neurons is to distribute modality-selective sensory infor-

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 223


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

mation on the physiological status of the tissues of the entire pinch applied with fingers or a smooth forceps, noxious heat). The cuta-
mammalian body to both sensory and homeostatic substrates. The neous stimuli were applied at various sites on the ventral hindpaw for
present observations provide firm evidence supporting this con- 35 s; the intensity of the radiant heat lamp was adjusted such that, held
cept and argue that modality-selective lamina I projection neu- at a fixed distance, it became painful to the investigators after about 5 s
rons may justifiably be considered to be anatomically specified. (about 45C) and intolerable after about 8 s (about 50C). Responses
were monitored through a speaker and an oscilloscope and were record-
ed on magnetic tape.
Methods Compromise between the competing needs for proper response char-
GENERAL PREPARATION. These procedures were approved by the local acterization and for quick initiation of current injection for intracellular
institutional animal care and use committee. Acute recording experi- staining meant that stimuli were repeated only three or four times to
ments were performed in 41 adult cats of either sex (2.5-4.5 kg) pre- demonstrate reproducibility, that receptive fields were regionally but not
pared as described2022. Briefly, the animals were anesthetized with precisely delimited, and that thermal stimuli were not always maintained
pentobarbital (40 mg per kg i.p.). Cannulae were inserted in the right sufficiently long (cold, 3060 s; heat, 510 s) to generate the noxious
cephalic vein (for infusions) and left carotid artery (for the blood pres- stimulus required by some HPC cells2123. Cells that responded to low-
sure monitor). Dexamethasone (10 mg i.v.) was administered. Sup- threshold cutaneous stimuli, cells with high ongoing discharge, and cells
plemental pentobarbital (approximately 4 mg per kg per h) was given that were silent or unresponsive were also encountered, but these were
as needed to maintain areflexia and pupillary constriction and blood not studied in detail in these experiments, primarily because prior stud-
pressure near 120 mmHg. A tracheostomy was performed, the animal ies indicated that lamina I spinothalamic cells in cat do not include such
was artificially ventilated with 25% O 2 and 75% air, and a bilateral cells. These cells were occasionally stained, however, and they were sub-
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

pneumothorax was induced. End-tidal CO 2 was maintained at sequently found to lie in lamina II or deeper.
3.54.5%. Body temperature was maintained at 37.5C with a heating INTRACELLULAR STAINING. Following physiological characterization of
pad and a feedback-controlled infrared lamp. Pancuronium was given a cell, depolarizing current pulses (0.5 to 5.0 nA, 500-ms duration at
every 6090 min for paralysis; withdrawal reflexes were tested between 1 Hz for 1 to 7 min) were applied to inject biocytin. In most experi-
doses. A dorsal laminectomy exposed the lumbosacral enlargement, ments, several intracellular injections were attempted at sites separat-
and a craniectomy was performed to provide access to the right thal- ed longitudinally by 0.5 to 5 mm that were carefully mapped for later
amus. The animal was mounted in a stereotaxic holder, and the spinal reconstruction. A period of one to eight hours was allowed following
cord was stabilized with vertebral clamps. The dura was reflected, and the injections in order to permit diffusion of the biocytin into cell
the cord was covered with a pool of mineral oil or Tyrodes solution processes. The animals were then given an overdose of barbiturate and
that was maintained at 3738C with a DC heater element. perfused transcardially with a rinse of 0.1 M PBS (pH 7.4, room tem-
ANTIDROMIC STIMULATION. Extracellular recordings were made with perature), a fixative consisting of two liters of 4% paraformaldehyde
tungsten-in-glass microelectrodes to identify the right somatosenso- and 0.1% glutaraldehyde in 0.1 M PB, and then one liter of 10%
ry thalamus in 22 of these cats. Based on these coordinates, an array of sucrose in 0.1 M PB. The lumbosacral spinal cord was removed and
eight concentric bipolar electrodes was implanted in order to placed in 30% sucrose in PB overnight. Serial 50-m frozen sections
antidromically activate lamina I spinothalamic neurons, as in prior cut in the horizontal plane were rinsed, incubated in avidin-biotiny-
studies. Stimulus trains consisting of four pulses (0.12 mA, 2 ms) at lated horseradish peroxidase complex (ABC, 1:100, Vector) in 0.05 M
200 or 250 Hz were delivered to individual electrodes or between pairs Tris-buffered saline (TBS) at 4 C overnight, and reacted with 0.05%
of electrodes. Spinal cells that responded to such trains in a one-for- 3,3-diaminobenzidine (DAB) containing 0.01% hydrogen peroxide
one, time-locked manner at a fixed latency with a distinct threshold in TBS for 510 min. Labeled cells were identified in the light micro-
were recognized as spinothalamic neurons; we considered this suffi- scope as lamina I neurons if they were located in the most superficial
cient, because lamina I cells that fulfill this criterion have in all our aspect of the gray matter, dorsal to the substantia gelatinosa, which is
prior studies demonstrated a strict antidromic collision interval when clearly recognizable with light- and dark-field illumination. Pho-
tested. Extracellular recordings were made from the lumbosacral tographs were made of all cells with Kodak Technical Pan film and
spinal cord at sites in L7S1 exposed by small openings in the pia, in Ektachrome film using 20x and 40x apochromatic objectives. Digital
order to ensure that lamina I spinothalamic cells were antidromical- microscopic images were made directly with a Leaf Microlumina scan-
ly activated by the thalamic array and to localize regions containing ner (at 3380 x 2253 pixels) and a 20x objective, and Adobe Photoshop
lamina I cells with receptive fields on the ventral hindpaw for subse- was used to enhance contrast and apply labels. Camera lucida draw-
quent intracellular recording. ings were made using 40x and 60x (oil) objectives.
INTRACELLULAR RECORDINGS. Recordings were made with glass micro- In most cases, only one biocytin-labeled cell was found at the
electrodes fabricated from 1.0 mm standard or 1.2 mm borosilicate glass appropriate location, but for 9 of the 30 identified cells, one to four
pipettes and an Axoclamp 2-A (Axon Instruments) amplifier in bridge neighboring cells were also labeled. These were usually small, light-
mode. The electrodes were back-filled with 2% biocytin (Sigma) in 2 M ly labeled fusiform neurons (Fig. 6, F1, arrowhead), in contrast to the
potassium methylsulphate; their resistance ranged from 40 to 80 M. darkly labeled cell that we identified as the characterized neuron.
The electrodes were lowered into the superficial dorsal horn near the (Interestingly, this occurred most often, in five of these nine instances,
dorsal root entry zone in 2.0 or 2.5 m steps. Cells were sought at a depth when the identified cell was also a fusiform neuron.) In two instances,
of 200-800 m below the surface. Extracellular recordings were obtained we found two adjacent, well labeled cells that had similar shapes (once
infrequently, using antidromic stimulation as a search method. Standard fusiform neurons, once multipolar neurons), but one was more dark-
methods of current application through the microelectrode were used ly stained and had signs of penetration (a darkly stained, disrupted
to facilitate cell impalement and to clear debris from the electrode tip. section of membrane). In one instance, four cells with differing shapes
Intracellular recording was signaled by a sudden large action potential, were labeled with similar intensity and without obvious signs of pen-
a resting membrane potential of at least -40 mV and action potentials of etration in any particular one, and thus the physiologically charac-
45 to 65 mV. Hyperpolarizing current pulses (0.5 nA, 50 ms) were often terized cell could not be identified and the data were excluded.
applied in an effort to stabilize the recording.
RESPONSE CHARACTERIZATION. Following cell impalement, antidromic
activation was tested and natural stimuli were quickly used for unit char- Acknowledgements
acterization2023. Innocuous stimuli (light touch, tapping, gentle pres- We thank Elizabeth OCampo, Maribeth Tatum, and Jan Carey for assistance.
sure, limb movement) were generally tested first, followed by thermal This work was supported by NIH grants NS 25616 and DA 07402 and the
stimuli (cooling with a wet ice cube or cold beaker, warming with the James R. Atkinson Pain Research Fund administered by the Barrow
hand or a radiant heat lamp) and then noxious stimuli (pressure and Neurological Foundation.

224 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

1. Perl, E.R. in Handbook of Physiology, Section 1, The Nervous System, Volume 25. Price, D.D., Hayes, R.L., Ruda, M. & Dubner, R. Spatial and temporal
III, Sensory Processes (ed. Darian-Smith, I.) 915975 (American Physiological transformations of input to spinothalamic tract neurons and their relation to
Society, Bethesda, 1984). somatic sensations. J. Neurophysiol. 41, 933947 (1978).
2. Willis, W.D. The Pain System. (Karger, Basel, 1985). 26. Craig, A.D. Jr. & Kniffki, K.-D. Spinothalamic lumbosacral lamina I cells
3. Craig, A.D. in Somesthesis and the Neurobiology of the Somatosensory Cortex responsive to skin and muscle stimulation in the cat. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 365,
(eds Franzen, O., Johansson, R. & Terenius, L.) 2739 ( Birkhuser, Basel, 197221 (1985).
1996). 27. Gobel, S. Golgi studies of the neurons in layer I of the dorsal horn of the
4. Craig, A.D., Bushnell, M.C., Zhang, E.-T. & Blomqvist, A. A thalamic nucleus medulla (trigeminal nucleus caudalis). J. Comp. Neurol. 180, 375394 (1978).
specific for pain and temperature sensation. Nature 372, 770773 (1994). 28. Lima, D. & Coimbra, A. A Golgi study of the neuronal population of the
5. Coghill, R.C. et al. Distributed processing of pain and vibration by the human marginal zone (lamina I) of the rat spinal cord. J. Comp. Neurol. 244, 5371
brain. J.Neurosci. 14, 40954108 (1994). (1986).
6. Craig, A.D., Reiman, E.M., Evans, A. & Bushnell, M.C. Functional imaging of 29. Zhang, E.T., Han, Z.S. & Craig, A.D. Morphological classes of spinothalamic
an illusion of pain. Nature 384, 258260 (1996). lamina I neurons in the cat. J. Comp. Neurol. 367, 537549 (1996).
7. Wall, P.D. Pain in the brain and lower parts of the anatomy. Pain 62, 389391 30. Zhang, E.T. & Craig, A.D. Morphology and distribution of spinothalamic
(1995). lamina I neurons in the monkey. J. Neurosci. 17, 32743284 (1997).
8. Light, A.R., Trevino, D.L. & Perl, E.R. Morphological features of functionally 31. Lopez-Garcia, J.A. & King, A.E. Membrane properties of physiologically
defined neurons in the marginal zone and substantia gelatinosa of the spinal classified rat dorsal horn neurons in vitro: Correlation with cutaneous
dorsal horn. J. Comp. Neurol. 186, 151172 (1979). sensory afferent input. Eur. J. Neurosci. 6, 9981007 (1994).
9. Light, A.R., Sedivec, M.J., Casale, E.J. & Jones, S.L. Physiological and 32. Dostrovsky, J.O., Shah, Y. & Gray, B.G. Descending inhibitory influences
morphological characteristics of spinal neurons projecting to the from periaqueductal gray, nucleus raphe magnus, and adjacent reticular
parabrachial region of the cat. Somatosens. Mot. Res. 10, 309325 (1993). formation. II. Effects on medullary dorsal horn nociceptive and
10. Bennett, G.J., Abdelmoumene, M., Hayashi, H., Hoffert, M.J. & Dubner, R. nonnociceptive neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 49, 948960 (1983).
Spinal cord layer I neurons with axon collaterals that generate local arbors. 33. Mokha, S.S., Goldsmith, G.E., Hellon, R.F. & Puri, R. Hypothalamic control
Brain Res. 209, 421426 (1981). of nocireceptive and other neurones in the marginal layer of the dorsal horn
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

11. Molony, V., Steedman, W.M., Cervero, F. & Iggo, A. Intracellular marking of of the medulla (trigeminal nucleus caudalis) in the rat. Exp. Brain Res. 65,
identified neurones in the superficial dorsal horn of the cat spinal cord. Q. J. 427436 (1987).
Exp. Physiol. 66, 211-223 (1981). 34. Menendez, L., Bester, H., Besson, J.M. & Bernard, J.F. Parabrachial area:
12. Hoffert, M.J., Miletic, V., Ruda, M.A. & Dubner, R. Immunocytochemical Electrophysiological evidence for an involvement in cold nociception. J.
identification of serotonin axonal contacts on characterized neurons in Neurophysiol. 75, 20992116 (1996).
laminae I and II of the cat dorsal horn. Brain Res. 267, 361364 (1983). 35. De Koninck, Y. & Henry, J.L. Substance P-mediated slow excitatory
13. Woolf, C.J. & Fitzgerald, M. The properties of neurones recorded in the postsynaptic potential elicited in dorsal horn neurons in vivo by noxious
superficial dorsal horn of the rat spinal cord. J. Comp. Neurol. 221, 313328 stimulation. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 88, 1134411348 (1991).
(1983). 36. Brown, J.L. et al. Morphological characterization of substance P receptor-
14. Miletic, V., Hoffert, M.J., Ruda, M.A., Dubner, R. & Shigenaga, Y. immunoreactive neurons in the rat spinal cord and trigeminal nucleus
Serotoninergic axonal contacts on identified cat spinal dorsal horn neurons caudalis. J. Comp. Neurol. 356, 327344 (1995).
and their correlation with nucleus raphe magnus stimulation. J. Comp. 37. Marshall, G.E., Shehab, S.A.S., Spike, R.C. & Todd, A.J. Neurokinin-1 receptors
Neurol. 228, 129141 (1984). on lumbar spinothalamic neurons in the rat. Neuroscience 72, 255263 (1996).
15. Steedman, W.M., Molony, V. & Iggo, A. Nociceptive neurones in the 38. Lima, D., Avelino, A. & Coimbra, A. Morphological characterization of
superficial dorsal horn of cat lumbar spinal cord and their primary afferent marginal (lamina I) neurons immunoreactive for substance P, enkephalin,
inputs. Exp. Brain Res. 58, 171182 (1985). dynorphin and gamma-aminobutyric acid in the rat spinal cord. J. Chem.
16. Hylden, J.L.K., Hayashi, H., Dubner, R. & Bennett, G.J. Physiology and Neuroanat. 6, 4352 (1993).
morphology of the lamina I spinomesencephalic projection. J. Comp. Neurol. 39. Sun, M.-K. & Spyer, K.M. Nociceptive inputs into rostral ventrolateral
247, 505515 (1986). medulla-spinal vasomotor neurones in rats. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 436, 685700
17. Moschovakis, A.K., Burke, R. & Fyffe, R. The size and dendritic structure of (1991).
HRP-labeled gamma motoneurons in the cat spinal cord. J. Comp. Neurol. 40. Cervero, F. & Tattersall, J.E.H. Somatic and visceral inputs to the thoracic
311, 531545 (1991). spinal cord of the cat: marginal zone (lamina I) of the dorsal horn. J. Physiol.
18. Tamamaki, N., Uhrich, D.J. & Sherman, S.M. Morphology of physiologically (Lond.) 383, 383395 (1987).
identified retinal X and Y axons in the cats thalamus and midbrain as revealed 41. MacIver, M.B. & Tanelian, D.L. Activation of C fibers by metabolic
by intraxonal injection of biocytin. J. Comp. Neurol. 354, 583607 (1995). perturbations associated with tourniquet ischemia. Anesthesiology 76,
19. Christensen, B.N. & Perl, E.R. Spinal neurons specifically excited by noxious 617623 (1992).
or thermal stimuli: marginal zone of the dorsal horn. J. Neurophysiol. 33, 42. Pickar, J.G., Hill, J.M. & Kaufman, M.P. Dynamic exercise stimulates group
293307 (1970). III muscle afferents. J. Neurophysiol. 71, 753760 (1994).
20. Craig, A.D. & Hunsley, S.J. Morphine enhances the activity of 43. Schmelz, M., Schmidt, R., Bickel, A., Handwerker, H.O. & Torebjrk, H.E.
thermoreceptive cold-specific lamina I spinothalamic neurons in the cat. Specific C-receptors for itch in human skin. J. Neurosci. 17, 80038008
Brain Res. 558, 9397 (1991). (1997).
21. Craig, A.D. & Serrano, L.P. Effects of systemic morphine on lamina I 44. Woolf, C.J., Shortland, P. & Sivilotti, L.G. Sensitization of high
spinothalamic tract neurons in the cat. Brain Res. 636, 233244 (1994). mechanothreshold superficial dorsal horn and flexor motor neurones
22. Craig, A.D. & Bushnell, M.C. The thermal grill illusion: unmasking the burn following chemosensitive primary afferent activation. Pain 58, 141155
of cold pain. Science 265, 252255 (1994). (1994).
23. Dostrovsky, J.O. & Craig, A.D. Cooling-specific spinothalamic neurons in the 45. Craig, A.D. Propriospinal input to thoracolumbar sympathetic nuclei from
monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 76, 36563665 (1996). cervical and lumbar lamina I neurons in the cat and the monkey. J. Comp.
24. Ferrington, D.G., Sorkin, L.S. & Willis, W.D. Responses of spinothalamic Neurol. 331, 517530 (1993).
tract cells in the superficial dorsal horn of the primate lumbar spinal cord. J. 46. Craig, A.D. Distribution of brainstem projections from spinal lamina I
Physiol. (Lond.) 388, 681703 (1987). neurons in the cat and the monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 361, 225248 (1995).

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 225


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Cortically induced thalamic plasticity in


the primate somatosensory system
E.R. Ergenzinger1,2, M.M. Glasier1, J.O. Hahm3 and T.P. Pons1

1 Department of Neurosurgery and 2Program in Neuroscience, Wake Forest University School of Medicine, Winston-Salem, North Carolina 27127, USA
3 Department of Neurosurgery, Georgetown University, Washington, D.C. 20057, USA
Correspondence should be addressed to T.P.P. (tpons@wfubmc.edu)

The influence of cortical feedback on receptive field organization in the thalamus was assessed in
the primate somatosensory system. Chronic and acute suppression of neuronal activity in primary
somatosensory cortex resulted in a striking enlargement of receptive fields in the ventroposterior
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

thalamus. This finding demonstrates a dramatic top-down influence of cortex on receptive field
size in the somatosensory thalamus. In addition, this result has important implications for studies of
adult neuronal plasticity because it indicates that changes in higher-order areas of the brain can
trigger extensive changes in the receptive field characteristics of neurons located earlier in the
processing pathway.

Although the descending projection from the primary consistent with those reported in previous studies by us and oth-
somatosensory cortex to the ventroposterior nucleus (VP) of the ers in normal animals1012. In accordance with previous stud-
thalamus is known to be seven to ten times greater than the ies9, our recordings throughout the cortical hand representation
ascending projection from VP to cortex1, the functional role of one to five months after D-APV administration revealed that
this massive corticofugal projection has remained elusive2. Pre- only 10% of cortical recording sites were responsive to somatic
vious studies on the contribution of neural feedback connections stimulation (Table 1), compared to 100% in control animals.
to the processing of sensory information have reported relative- Analysis of the responsive 10% of recording sites in the cortex of
ly minor and inconsistent effects on receptive field size or the experimental animals showed that these neurons exhibited
response properties of thalamic relay neurons following cortical RFs comparable in size to those found in the cortex of the control
perturbations37. Although the connectivity of the system sug- animals. There was no evidence of degeneration in the thalamus
gests a substantial influence from cortex over the processing of or cortex in any of the animals used in the present study.
somatosensory information at the level of the thalamus, there These results on RF size obtained at the cortical level in the
has been no definitive demonstration of a major cortical influ- chronic D-APV treated animals contrasted sharply with those
ence on receptive field size, response properties or somatotopic found at the thalamic level of these same animals, where RFs
organization in VP. were greatly enlarged compared to those seen in controls, fre-
A majority of corticofugal projection cells in primary quently encompassing more than one digit and often half or
somatosensory cortex (area 3b) express NMDA receptors8, and more of the entire glabrous hand (Fig. 1c). In some instances
chronic systemic blockade of NMDA receptors produces large
unresponsive areas in somatosensory cortex9. In this study, we
determined that chronic administration of an NMDA receptor Table 1. Responsivity of recording sites in cortex and
antagonist directly into the area 3b cortical hand representa- thalamus following chronic cortical administration of D-
tion results in a suppression of activity in area 3b and an enor- APV or saline1
mous enlargement of receptive fields (RFs) in the VP hand Control D-APV
representation. In addition, acute cortical suppression also Cortex (Area 3b)
resulted in an enlargement of RFs in VP. These findings force a % Responsive Sites
re-evaluation of traditional bottom-up models of sensory pro- Infusion Zone (Hand) 100%(197) 10%(754)
cessing, which view the thalamus as a simple relay nucleus to Other 100%(47) 99%(82)
the cortex, and they also have important implications for stud-
Fringe Zone (Hand & Other) N.A. 78%(262)
ies of adult neuronal plasticity.
% Large RF Sites 0%(197) 6%(78)
Results Thalamus (VP)
Over a period of one to five months, either saline (two control % Responsive Sites
monkeys, Macaca mulatta) or the NMDA receptor antagonist D- Hand 100%(102) 99%(388)
2-amino-5-phosphonovaleric acid (D-APV; five monkeys, Maca- Other 99%(116) 100%(243)
ca mulatta) was infused directly into the area 3b hand
% Large RF Sites 2%(102) 58%(383)
representation. The percentage of responsive sites and RF size for
the cortex and VP in the saline control group was completely 1see Methods for details on data analysis.

226 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

a b c

Fig. 1. Representative RFs in VP following chronic administration of D-APV to somatosensory cortex. (a) Representative RFs, indicated by
thinner black lines, recorded from sites localized within the hand region of VPL from saline controls. (b) Representative electrode tracks
through a coronal section of the thalamus in a macaque that had received D-APV to the area 3b hand representation (CM, central median; LP,
lateral posterior; VPM, ventroposterior medial; VPI, ventroposterior inferior; VPL, ventroposterior lateral). Tick marks on tracks indicate
recording sites. Red, recording sites with RFs on the face. Blue, recording sites with RFs on or including the hand. Green, recording sites with
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

RFs on the forearm. Colors are matched with RFs in (c). (c) Representative RFs recorded from sites localized within VP from a monkey with
D-APV administered to area 3b. All RFs were contralateral to VP. Arrow denotes rotation of the hand and forearm to allow visualization of
their ventral surfaces.

these RFs included the entire distal forearm and hand (Fig. 1c), a an enormous expansion of RF size after delivery of pharmaco-
type of RF that is never seen in normal animals. Such large RFs logical agents to either the cortex or thalamus has not been
were recorded from 58% of the recording sites within the VP reported previously37.
hand representation of our experimental animals. The RF We next determined if acute administration of D-APV would
enlargement was specific in that these RFs were observed only produce results similar to those from chronic treatment. We first
for recording sites within the VP hand representation and not in recorded from the cortex and thalamus of two additional mon-
body-part representations outside of the D-APV infusion zone keys before administering D-APV. We then delivered injections
in cortex (Fig. 1c). of D-APV directly to the cortex of these same animals via a
These findings indicate that chronic D-APV administration Hamilton syringe and immediately assessed the results. Acute
in the area 3b hand representation blocks much of the stimulus- cortical recordings were similar to those found in the chronical-
driven activity at the cortical level and tremendously expands the ly administered animals, with 93% of 83 sites unresponsive to
size of RFs in the portion of the VP thalamus that normally rep- somatic stimulation. Recordings in the thalamus of the acutely
resents the hand (within the ventroposterior lateral nucleus or administered animals showed an increase in RF size, though not
VPL). This expansion of RF size occurred for the vast majority as great as that observed in the chronically administered animals.
of hand-responsive recording sites throughout VPL (Fig. 2). Such Although a comparable number of sites (45% of 55 sites) showed
RFs that encompassed one or more digits, compared to only 2%
of 51 sites before acute D-APV administration, the magnitude
of the RF enlargement was not as great as that observed in chron-
70 Control ically administered animals, in that no RFs larger than half of the
D-APV
60 hand were observed (Fig. 3).
% recording sites

50
Discussion
40 The present findings demonstrate that, at least under our exper-
imental conditions, top-down projections from cortex can induce
30
large-scale reorganization of RFs at a relatively early processing
20 station in the somatosensory system. This effect seems most like-
ly to be mediated directly by corticothalamic projections or indi-
10
rectly via corticocuneate projections. Additionally, the increase
0 in RF size for VP neurons could occur through a loss of excita-
1 Pad < 1 Digit > 1 Pad 1 Digit tory corticothalamic input on inhibitory interneurons within VP
or on the thalamic reticular nucleus. Of course, unknown changes
in additional somatosensory cortical areas such as SII, the insu-
Fig. 2. Distribution histogram of RF sizes for recording sites local- la and/or posterior parietal cortex could conceivably mediate the
ized within the VPL hand representation between control and D- RF enlargement through complex corticothalamic processing,
APV groups. RFs were categorized as to whether they were 1 pad, though this latter possibility seems less likely. That these results
< 1 digit, > 1 pad, or 1 digit. RFs that were classified as > 1 pad might be explained by retrograde degeneration seems highly
were restricted to the pads and did not encompass any digits of the unlikely, given that lesions of the somatosensory cortex, which
hand. RFs that were classified as 1 digit were those RFs that incor- are known to produce massive retrograde degeneration, do not
porated at least one digit, and included RFs as large as the entire result in expanded thalamic RFs13. Furthermore, the expansion of
hand and forearm (see Fig. 1). RFs immediately after delivery of D-APV to the cortex would

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 227


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

with an initial dose of ketamine


a 80 Pre
Post
b hydrochloride (15 mg per kg) fol-
70
lowed by intubation and admin-
istration of isoflurane (0.5% to
% recording sites

60
3%) to effect. Throughout
50
surgery, which was performed
40 using aseptic precautions, the ani-
30
mals received an intravenous drip
of a solution of 5% dextrose and
20 0.45% sodium chloride, and their
10 heart rate, respiratory rate, and
temperature were monitored and
0 maintained within normal limits.
1 Pad < 1 Digit > 1 Pad 1 Digit Pre Post
For the chronic administration
Fig. 3. RF size in VPL before and after acute cortical D-APV administration. (a) Distribution histogram of of D-APV, we implanted a tran-
RF sizes for recording sites localized within the VPL hand representation before (pre) and after (post) scranial catheter directly into the
acute cortical D-APV administration. RFs were categorized as in Fig. 2. (b) Representative RFs recorded hand representation in area 3b and
from sites localized within VPL before (pre) and after (post) acute cortical D-APV administration. attached the catheter to an osmot-
ic pump containing 50 M D-APV
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

or physiological saline delivered at


a rate of 2.5 l per h. D-APV or
saline was administered from
seem to further eliminate retrograde degeneration as a possible between one and five months, and the osmotic pump was replaced every
mechanism explaining our results. 28 days. Ketamine was not used during osmotic pump replacements because
Although some earlier electrophysiological studies of the nor- of potential interactions with glutamatergic neurotransmission. Instead,
mal thalamus in monkeys have reported extra-lemniscal neu- valium was used to make the animal receptive to isoflurane anesthesia.
rons with large RFs 11,12 , such RFs were localized almost For the acute administration of D-APV, a Hamilton syringe was used
exclusively in the VPLo (oralis) nucleus11, far anterior to the VPLc to inject D-APV directly into the hand representation in area 3b. In order
(caudalis)14 where our recordings were made. In addition, record- to assure that the entire hand representation was affected by the D-APV
as in the chronic study, multiple injections of 23 l of D-APV were made
ings lateral and medial to the affected thalamic zone indicated
approximately 1 mm apart across the mediolateral extent of the hand
neurons had normal RF size, further corroborating that we were representation in cortex (56 injections total). The injections consisted of
recording within VPLc, and not in the extralemniscal pathway. 50 M D-APV.
Finally, histological analysis from our cases confirmed that our
ELECTROPHYSIOLOGICAL RECORDING PROCEDURES. A recording chamber and
recordings were from the hand region in VPLc. Importantly, there
head fixation device were attached to the skull. The chamber was made
was no evidence of any degeneration of neurons in the thalamus from dental acrylic and positioned to provide maximum access for
or cortex. Regardless of the precise mechanism(s) responsible for recording from VP and area 3b. The bone within the chamber was
the expansion, however, the magnitude of the RF expansion is removed to expose the dura, the dura then reflected and a high resolu-
completely unexpected and highlights the contribution of feed- tion picture of brain vasculature was then obtained with a CCD camera
back processing loops on RF properties. using appropriate filters. The brain picture was used to help in the place-
Many previous studies of adult neural plasticity after periph- ment of electrode penetrations during recording, with the RF and respon-
eral perturbations have focused attention on the earliest point in sivity characteristics defined for each recording point.
the ascending pathway where plastic changes could occur (spinal The mapping and recording procedures were similar to those used
cord15,16, brainstem17,18, or thalamus1921), with the interpreta- previously25. In the chronic preparation, electrode penetrations were
tion that plastic changes at early stations are simply relayed to placed at 0.30.5 mm intervals across the mediolateral extent of VP and
cortex. The timing, nature and magnitude of our present find- area 3b and at 0.30.5 mm intervals across its rostrocaudal extent.
ings challenge this view of the system as a simple hierarchical Approximately 2040 electrode penetrations were made across VP in
each hemisphere studied, and approximately 5060 electrode penetra-
pathway22,23, providing a definitive demonstration of a dramat-
tions were made through area 3b in each hemisphere studied. In the acute
ic and substantial role for the cortex on neuronal processing early preparation, 45 penetrations were made across VP, and 56 penetra-
in the somatosensory pathway. This study demonstrates that tions were made across area 3b before and after cortical D-APV injec-
some RF characteristics within somatosensory pathways result tions. Penetrations across VP were placed at 0.30.5 mm intervals
from a series of interconnected dynamic loops, with changes at mediolaterally and rostrocaudally. Penetrations across area 3b were placed
any given level capable of triggering extensive changes in the RFs approximately 1 mm apart mediolaterally. The location of pre- and post-
of neurons at both earlier and later stations in the processing injection penetrations were the same. Microelectrodes were hydraulical-
chain. Such a view of feedback connections is entirely consistent ly advanced through cortex and thalamus until single- or multiple-unit
with a recent hypothesis24 that recognizes a substantial role for responses to mechanical stimulation of the body could be isolated. Small
corticothalamic feedback loops on the modification of receptive marker lesions (10 A for 10 s) were placed in the thalamus and cortex at
strategic points to assist in locating the recordings sites post mortem.
properties but not for the major driving of thalamic neurons by
the cortex. The extent to which such feedback connections mod- H ISTOLOGICAL ANALYSIS . On completion of all electrophysiological
ulate activity and the precise mechanisms responsible for such recording experiments, animals were given a lethal dose of pentobar-
bital and then perfused intracardially with 0.9% saline followed by 4%
modulation should be ripe areas for future research.
paraformaldehyde. Brains were cut in the coronal plane on a freezing
microtome into 50 m sections, and every fifth section was mounted
Methods and stained for Nissl substance to assess the placement of recording
SURGICAL PROCEDURES. All procedures in the present study were approved tracks, marker lesions, and placement of the cannulae in cortex.
by the Wake Forest University Animal Care and Use Committee. All mon- Recording sites were identified by reconstructing electrode tracks and
keys used in this study were Macaca mulatta. Monkeys were anesthetized marker lesions as described25.

228 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

DATA ANALYSIS. Percent responsive sites was determined as the number of 8. Conti, F. & Minelli, A. in Excitatory Amino Acids and the Cerebral Cortex. (eds
responsive sites per total number of sites. Percent large RF sites are given Conti, F. & Hicks, T.P.) 8198 (MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 1996).
9. Garraghty, P.E. & Muja, N. NMDA receptors and plasticity in adult
as number of sites with RFs encompassing one or more digits (includ- primate somatosensory cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 367, 319326 (1996).
ing RFs encompassing the hand and forearm) per number of hand- 10. Pons, T.P., Wall, J.T., Garraghty, P.E., Cusick, C.G. & Kaas, J.H.
responsive sites. In the chronic D-APV animals the infusion zone was Consistent features of the representation of the hand in area 3b of
defined as the region of cortex surrounding the cannula site in which macaque monkeys. Somatosensory Res. 4, 309331 (1987).
responsiveness was largely suppressed (90% of recording sites) and cor- 11. Poggio, G.F. & Mountcastle, V.B. The functional properties of
ventrobasal thalamic neurons studied in unanesthetized monkeys. J.
responded to a 5 mm mediolateral expanse of cortex within the hand Neurophysiol. 26, 775806 (1963).
representation. The fringe zone was defined as the region in which 12. Loe, P.R., Whitsel, B.L., Dreyer, D.A. & Metz, C.B. Body representation in
responsiveness was only partially suppressed (25% of sites) and extend- ventrobasal thalamus of macaque: a single-unit analysis. J. Neurophysiol.
ed approximately 1 mm medial and lateral to the infusion zone. Corti- 40, 13391355 (1977).
13. Bava, A., Fadiga, E. & Manzoni, T. Extralemniscal reactivity and
cal recordings indicated that the face representation was not affected by commisural linkages in the VPL nucleus of cats with chronic cortical
the D-APV administration. lesions. Arch. Ital. Biol. 106, 204226 (1968).
14. Kaas, J.H. & Pons, T.P. in Comparative Primate Biology, Neurosciences.
(eds Steklis, H.P. & Erwin, J.) 421468 (Alan R. Liss, New York, 1988).
Acknowledgements 15. Dostrovsky, J.O., Millar, J. & Wall, P.D. The immediate shift of afferent
drive of dorsal column nucleus cells following deafferentation: A
This research was supported by NIH grants MH11950-01, MH53369-02 and comparison of acute and chronic deafferentation in gracile nucleus and
NS35246-01. spinal cord. Exp. Neurol. 52, 480495 (1976).
16. McMahan, S. B. & Wall, P.D. Plasticity in the nucleus gracilis of the rat.
Exp. Neurol. 80, 195207 (1983).
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

17. Pollin, B. & Albe-Fessard, P. Organization of somatic thalamus in


RECEIVED 20 MAY: ACCEPTED 26 MAY 1998 monkeys with and without section of dorsal spinal track. Brain Res. 173,
431449 (1979).
18. Garraghty, P.E. & Kaas, J.H.. Functional reorganization in adult monkey
1. Liu, X.B., Honda, C.N. & Jones, E.G. Distribution of four types of synapse on thalamus after peripheral nerve injury. Neuroreport 2, 747750 (1991).
physiologically identified relay neurons in the ventral posterior thalamic 19. Merzenich, M.M. et al. Topographic reorganization of somatosensory
nucleus of the cat. J. Comp. Neurol. 352, 6991 (1995). cortical areas 3b and 1 in adult monkeys following restricted
2. Jones, E.G. The Thalamus. (Plenum, New York, 1985). deafferentation. Neuroscience 8, 3355 (1983).
3. Yuan, B., Morrow, T.J. & Casey, K.L. Responsiveness of ventrobasal thalamic 20. Merzenich, M.M. et al. Somatosensory cortical map changes following
neurons after suppression of S1 cortex in the anesthetized rat. J. Neurosci. 5, digit amputation in adult monkeys. J. Comp. Neurol. 224, 591605 (1984).
29712978 (1985). 21. Pons, T.P. et al. Massive cortical reorganization after sensory
4. Ghosh, S., Murray, G.M., Turman, A.B. & Rowe, M.J. Corticothalamic deafferentation in adult macaques. Science 252, 18571860 (1991).
influences on transmission of tactile information in the 22. Pons, T.P., Garraghty, P.E., Friedman, D.P. & Mishkin, M. Physiological
ventroposterolateral thalamus of the cat: effect of reversible inactivation of evidence for serial processing in somatosensory cortex. Science 237,
somatosensory cortical areas I and II. Exp. Brain. Res. 100, 276286 (1994). 417420 (1987).
5. Burchfiel, J.L. & Duffy, F.H. Corticofugal influences upon cat thalamic 23. Pons, T.P. in Somesthesis and the Neurobiology of the somatosensory cortex,
ventrobasal complex. Brain Res. 70, 395411 (1974). (eds Franzn, O., Johansson, R. & Terenius, L.)187195 (Birkhuser,
6. Shin, H.C. & Chapin, J.K. Mapping the effects of SI cortex stimulation on Basel, Switzerland, 1996).
somatosensory relay neurons in the rat thalamus: direct responses and 24. Crick, F. & Koch, C. Constraints on cortical and thalamic projections:
afferent modulation. Somatosens. Motor Res. 7, 421434 (1990). the no-strong-loops hypothesis. Nature 391, 245250 (1998).
7. Tsumoto, T. & Nakamura, S. Inhibitory organization of the thalamic 25. Pons, T.P., Garraghty, P.E., Cusick, C.G. & Kaas, J.H.. The somatotopic
ventrobasal neurons with different peripheral representations. Exp. Brain organization of area 2 in macaque monkeys. J. Comp. Neurol. 241,
Res. 21, 195210 (1974). 445466 (1985).

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 229


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Strengthening of horizontal cortical


connections following skill learning
Mengia-S. Rioult-Pedotti1, Daniel Friedman1, Grzegorz Hess1,2 and John P. Donoghue1

1 Department of Neuroscience, Brown University, Providence, Rhode Island 02912, USA


2 Permanent address: Jagiellonian University, Institute of Zoology, 31-102 Krakow, Poland
Correspondence should be addressed to M.-S. R.-P. (mengia_rioult@brown.edu)

Learning a new motor skill requires an alteration in the spatiotemporal pattern of muscle activation.
Motor areas of cerebral neocortex are thought to be involved in this type of learning, possibly by
functional reorganization of cortical connections. Here we show that skill learning is accompanied
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

by changes in the strength of connections within adult rat primary motor cortex (M1). Rats were
trained for three or five days in a skilled reaching task with one forelimb, after which slices of motor
cortex were examined to determine the effect of training on the strength of horizontal intracortical
connections in layer II/III. The amplitude of field potentials in the forelimb region contralateral to
the trained limb was significantly increased relative to the opposite untrained hemisphere. No
differences were seen in the hindlimb region. Moreover, the amount of long-term potentiation (LTP)
that could be induced in trained M1 was less than in controls, suggesting that the effect of training
was at least partly due to LTP-like mechanisms. These data represent the first direct evidence that
plasticity of intracortical connections is associated with learning a new motor skill.

The primary motor area (M1), a cortical region necessary for rat M1 horizontal connections increase after learning and prac-
skilled voluntary movements, seems also to participate in learn- ticing a skilled reaching task. The amount of LTP that could be
ing motor skills. This conclusion is based largely upon findings induced by electrical stimulation was also reduced after learn-
that adult M1 representations are modifiable14, that dendritic ing, implying that the observed strengthening of horizontal con-
morphology of M1 pyramidal neurons is altered by experience5 nections may involve an LTP-like mechanism. Plasticity of M1
and that connections among M1 neurons are capable of activi- connections may therefore create cortical circuits needed to
ty-dependent, long-term changes in efficacy611. Despite these acquire or perform new motor behaviors.
suggestive findings, there is no direct evidence that learning is
accompanied by functional modifications of M1 circuits. Modi- Results
fications outside of the cortex, which have been repeatedly doc- Rats were trained to reach through a hole in a food box with a
umented, might account for cortical motor or sensory map single forepaw in order to retrieve small food pellets using a
changes that can be produced by experience or nerve lesions12,13. grasping motion. Training and subsequent practice lasted three
Further, there is no evidence that morphological changes, such (n = 1) or five (n = 13) successive days with one training session
as an increase in the number of dendritic branches, actually alter (approximately one hour) per day. Successful performance of
functional interactions in the cortex. Finally, there has been no this skill occurred in the first one or two sessions, and the remain-
definitive evidence that LTP-like mechanisms are engaged with- ing sessions consisted of repeated practice and refinement of the
in cortex during any form of learning. Demonstration of synap- skill. By the final two days of training, all rats achieved a perfor-
tic modification in conjunction with learning is an essential step mance of about 1.5 pellet retrievals per minute, with few errors in
towards understanding how cortical circuits support motor skills the reach, grasp or retrieval actions. A group of comparably han-
or other forms of learning. dled, age- and sex-matched cage mates (termed paired controls,
The intrinsic horizontal pathways are a potential substrate n = 14) and another group of naive rats (unpaired controls,
for experience-dependent reorganization of relationships among n = 12) served as controls.
M1 neurons. Layer II/III pyramidal cells form a broad, intrin- The strength of intrinsic horizontal synaptic connections
sic horizontal projection system in M1, and their intracortical within layers II/III was evaluated ex vivo using coronal brain
pattern correlates with sites that reorganize after nerve slices containing both hemispheres (Fig. 1a); experimenters were
lesions14,15. Pharmacological adjustments of the excitatory- blind to whether the animal was trained, and if so, which fore-
inhibitory balance within M1 restructures motor representa- limb was trained. Slices were taken 2045 hours after the last
tions, apparently by uncovering latent horizontal pathways16. In training session to rule out effects that might persist immedi-
addition, horizontal connections are capable of LTP, providing a ately after practice of the task. Field-potential responses evoked
potential activity-dependent mechanism for synaptic modifica- in the horizontal pathway by electrical stimuli were examined
tion3,7,9,10,17. These findings raise the possibility that changes in simultaneously through stimulating and recording electrodes
horizontal connection strength may accompany motor learn- that were mirror symmetrically positioned in layer II/III of the
ing. Here we show that field potentials evoked by stimulation of left and right M1 (Fig. 1a). Thus, one side in each trained rat

230 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 1. Conse-
quences of motor
a b c Trained
rec Trained n=7
skill learning on rec
field-potential stim stim
responses evoked in
layer II/III horizontal
connections of M1. Control
Control
(a) Mirror-symmet- n=20
ric placement of
stimulating (stim)
and recording (rec)
microelectrodes
bilaterally in layers
II/III of M1 in a coronal slice containing both hemispheres. wm, white matter. (b) Single-case examples of field potentials (averages of five
sweeps), evoked at 60% maximum stimulation intensity from a single trained (top) and a single paired-control (bottom) animal. Dark lines
represent the trained M1 or left M1, hatched lines, the untrained M1 or right M1. (c) Group average responses for trained (top, n = 7) and
control (bottom, n = 20, paired and naive) rats at 60% maximal stimulation intensity, illustrating enhanced field potential in the horizontal
pathway of M1 contralateral to the limb used in the reaching task. Same format as (b).
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

provided a within-animal control because the majority of untrained M1 at every stimulation intensity, whereas there were no
engaged M1 neurons are located in the hemisphere contralater- differences between left and right sides in the control groups
al to the limb they influence18. For trained animals, we term M1 (Fig. 2a and b). To compare the relative change between trained
contralateral to the trained limb the trained M1, and its coun- and untrained M1, the common logarithm of the response-ampli-
terpart on the other side the untrained M1. In all control ani- tude ratio between sides was calculated for each animal at each
mals, the terms left M1 and right M1 are used. stimulation intensity. The average log ratio obtained for trained
Stimulation evoked an initially negative-going field potential animals was significantly different from zero (p < 0.05) over a
of similar shape in all rats as previously described7. However, broad range of stimulus intensities, reflecting larger peak ampli-
for each rat that had learned the skilled-reaching task, field tudes in the trained M1. This ratio was not different in control
potentials evoked in the trained M1 were consistently larger in animals (p > 0.05; Fig. 2c). Figure 2d plots the distribution of the
amplitude than in the untrained M1 (Fig. 1b and c). Ampli- entire data set; the trained animals show a marked skew toward
tudes in the trained M1 were also larger than those observed larger responses on the trained side, whereas untrained controls
for the control animals. Amplitude differences between trained show no difference between the two hemispheres.
and untrained M1 were not a result of stimulus intensity Amplitude differences observed in the trained M1 might arise
because absolute current intensities used on the two sides were by several mechanisms. Larger responses in the horizontal path-
not significantly different (p = 0.23). Indeed, in 71% of the ways could result from newly formed synapses, from postsynap-
cases, the stimulation intensity was slightly larger tic excitability increase or from engaging an LTP-like mechanism
(27.24 3.38%, n = 10) on the untrained side. that increased the strength of existing synapses. A larger initial
The amplitude differences between trained M1 and slope for field potentials evoked at 60% maximal stimulation
untrained M1 were specific to the region of the M1 forelimb intensity in the trained M1 (trained M1, 0.735 0.079 mV per
representation. Layer II/III field-potential measurements from ms; untrained M1, 0.439 0.079 mV per ms; p < 0.025) suggest-
the hindlimb region of the trained M1 and untrained M1 in an ed that modifications occurred by a change in synaptic efficacy,
additional group of trained rats showed no significant side-to- rather than an excitability change. Because layer II/III horizontal
side amplitude differences at any stimulation intensity connections are capable of LTP, we compared the ability to poten-
(p = 0.20.8, n = 9), whereas slices taken from the forelimb tiate this pathway in trained M1 and untrained M1. We postulat-
region of the same animals showed a larger response in the ed that if learning recently engaged an LTP-like process in the
trained than untrained M1. Field potentials in the hindlimb trained pathway, further electrically induced potentiation of this
and forelimb areas were similar in shape. Peak amplitudes for pathway might be occluded. LTP induction was attempted by
the hindlimb at 2.5 and 5 times threshold intensity were theta-burst stimulation simultaneously delivered during a tran-
0.92 0.12 mV and 1.47 0.19 mV in the trained M1 and sient reduction of GABAA-receptor-dependent synaptic inhibi-
0.97 0.13 mV and 1.45 0.19 mV in the untrained M1. tion by focal application of small amounts of bicuculline at each
The relationship between stimulus intensity and response of the two recording sites6,7, an established method to obtain LTP
amplitude was evaluated systematically using two different reliably in neocortex. In accord with previous results7, LTP was
approaches (Fig. 2ac) to rule out the possibility that these effects readily induced in the untrained M1 (24.1 11.7% increase, n = 6)
were a consequence of the particular intensities used. One series of and in both hemispheres of control rats (left M1, 39.7 10.5%;
7 trained and 20 control (8 paired, 12 unpaired) rats was tested right M1, 36.1 12.8%; p = 0.8, n = 7). The difference between
with stimuli that were a constant fraction of the stimulus inten- untrained M1 and left and right M1 of controls was not signifi-
sity evoking a maximum response (absolute intensity less than or cant (p = 0.35). In contrast, the average increase in field potential
equal to 220 A). A second series of seven trained and six paired amplitude in the trained M1 was only 6.5 4.2% (n = 6; Fig. 2e),
control rats was tested with stimuli that were a constant multiple which was significantly less than the untrained M1 (p<0.035,
of the stimulus intensity evoking a minimal response of about 0.1 n = 6). Thus, although identical stimulation and recording pro-
mV (absolute threshold intensity 1230 A). In both series, the cedures were applied to both hemispheres, the amount of LTP
average responses for the trained M1 were larger than the that could be induced in the trained hemisphere was reduced.

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 231


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 2. Response differ-


ences in trained and
a Trained
b Trained d Trained

control rats. (a) Field-


potential amplitudes for
trained (top, n = 7) and
paired- and naive-con-

FP amplitude (mV)
log (trained M1/untrained M1)
trol (bottom, n = 20)
animals. Stimulation Control
intensities are plotted Control Control
as a percentage of the
stimulus evoking the
maximal response.
Filled symbols repre-
sent the trained M1 or
log (left M1/right M1)
left M1, open symbols
the untrained M1 or (% max) (x threshold)
e
right M1. Note the Trained
Stimulation intensity
larger response magni-

FP amplitude (% baseline)
tude in the trained M1 c
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

across intensities.
(b) Field-potential
amplitudes for trained
Log ratio

(top, n = 7) and paired- Control


control (bottom, n = 6)
animals. Stimulation
intensities were varied
as multiples of thresh-
old intensity inducing a (% max) (x threshold)
Time
minimal response Stimulation intensity
(about 0.1 mV).
Symbols as in (a).
(c) Comparison of average ( standard error) log-response ratios for trained/untrained M1 (filled symbols) and left/right M1 (open symbols)
for series in (a) (left) or series in (b) (right). Asterisks indicate statistical differences between control and experimental ratios. (d) Log ratio of
field-potential amplitudes for trained (top) and control (bottom) groups. The histograms show the distributions of all log ratios for all rats at all
stimulation intensities (entire data set). The mean value for trained animals (0.17) is significantly different from zero (p<0.001), demonstrating
a shift towards the trained M1. (e) Effect of training on electrically induced LTP. Each point indicates the relative field-potential amplitude
before and after LTP induction in both hemispheres (arrow) in trained (top, n = 6) and paired control (bottom, n = 7) animals. Averages after
LTP induction are compiled from the last 50 min of recordings because of a variable duration of the transient bicuculline effect (interruption in
the x-axis). Open symbols (+ standard error) for right M1 or untrained M1, filled symbols (- standard error) for left M1 or trained M1.

Discussion behavior that occur when the task is first achieved and the slow-
These results demonstrate that learning and practicing a motor er improvements in skill that occur with subsequent practice.
skill is accompanied by an increased efficacy of horizontal con- Both can be considered as forms of learning. Increased efficacy of
nections in motor cortex. Although there is extensive data show- horizontal connections would not seem to be a consequence of
ing that motor skill learning modifies cortical movement alone. Although the number of movements might be
representations1,2,19,20 and alters dendritic morphology5 and that different for the practiced limb, the actual number is small when
cortical connections are capable of activity-dependent strength considered as a fraction of the total number of movements made
changes, our results provide the first direct evidence for a func- over the one to two days after training ended. The movements
tional change of a cortical connection associated with motor skill made were at a low rate (about 1.6 per minute), required little
learning. Our results compare with changes recently observed in force and were mixed with many overlearned bilateral move-
the amygdala following fear conditioning, a markedly different ments in consummatory actions, yet enhanced horizontal-con-
form of learning21,22 and might provide a basis for correlation- nection strength persisted in M1 in the region of the forelimb.
strength changes that have been observed during auditory con- Changes in horizontal connections are also not widespread in
ditioning23. Plasticity of horizontal connections could contribute M1 because learning did not modify the layer II/III horizontal
to the reorganization of motor cortical representations that pathway in the hindlimb area.
accompanies motor skill learning, because information from one The marked effect of learning upon field-potential amplitude
region of M1 would be spread more effectively to other regions. within the M1 forelimb region suggests that a large number of
This hypothesis is consistent with recent findings demonstrating connections within this area have been modified in conjunction
that only the parts of M1 receiving strong horizontal inputs reor- with skill learning. It is difficult to conceptualize how such a gen-
ganize immediately after nerve lesions in the rat14 and that M1 eralized effect can provide a substrate for implementing the
representations in monkeys and humans enlarge or rearrange detailed pattern of movements acquired. Studies of human
during motor skill learning4,2426. Our data cannot differentiate motor-skill learning using transcranial magnetic stimulation sug-
between changes that result from the early modifications in gest that motor-cortical maps shrink again after explicit knowl-

232 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

edge of the task is gained25 . Thus, the seemingly generalized with a 1.3 cm diameter hole through which the food was retrieved. Small
changes we observe after five days of training may eventually lead food pellets (45 mg; Noyes Precision Food Pellets) were placed on the
to more specific circuits suitable for producing the skill. By con- floor of this food box within reaching distance. Rats learned to reach into
trast, fMRI studies during human motor-skill learning indicate the food box with their preferred paw to retrieve food pellets using a
grasping action. It was impossible for the rats to reach in the food box
that representations continue to enlarge with repeated prac-
with both paws, although initial attempts sometimes involved trying to
tice24,26. We will need to examine additional time points to deter- reach with both paws or attempting to place the snout in the food box
mine what happens to field-potential increases and decrements in and reach with the tongue. All but one animal, which was excluded from
LTP before we can make more definitive statements of the role the analysis, selected the right forelimb to perform the task. Animals
of these modification in acquiring new motor skills. received one training session per day lasting for one hour. The training
Modifications in horizontal connections seem to result from and practice period lasted five days for thirteen rats and three days for
changes in synaptic efficacy, perhaps through LTP-like mecha- one rat. Because there was no readily apparent difference in the reach-
nisms, rather than other means. Learning seems to occlude LTP ing behavior nor in the electrophysiological results after the three- and
induction, suggesting they share a similar mechanism. The five-day training, data were grouped together. The second animal from a
pair served as a paired control, received a comparable amount of han-
increase in the initial slope of field potentials from the trained
dling and was given similar numbers of food pellets. The remaining
M1 is consistent with the hypothesis that learning occurred twelve animals were used as naive controls.
through synaptic modification. Although compelling evidence
is lacking, LTP is a strong candidate mechanism for many forms The experimenter was unaware of the rats training condition until
of learning because it can lead to long-lasting modification of data analysis of the pair was completed. Coronal brain slices containing
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

activated synapses27 at a number of sites, including horizontal the region of the M1 forelimb representation33, 12 mm anterior to the
cortical connections7,17. Other mechanisms are potentially plau- bregma, were prepared as described7 and superfused with artificial cere-
sible but are not fully consistent with our results. For instance, brospinal fluid (ACSF) of the following composition (in mM): 126 NaCl,
3 KCl, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 26 NaHCO3, 1 MgSO4, 2 CaCl2, and 10 glucose,
a different form of learning, involving classical conditioning,
bubbled with a 95% O2, 5% CO2 mixture at 35 0.5C. The humidified
leads to increases in the membrane excitability of M1 neurons28, atmosphere over the slices was saturated with 95% O2, 5% CO2. Coro-
rather than synaptic modification. If increases in horizontal nal slices from the hindlimb region were cut at the level of the anterior
field potentials were due to excitability changes in postsynap- part of the hippocampal formation.
tic neurons, no initial slope changes would be expected and
tetanization would likely produce greater postsynaptic depo- Field potentials were recorded using glass micropipettes placed in layer
larization and hence, a larger amount of LTP29. Growth of new II/III, 200350 m below the pial surface in the region of the M1 fore-
synaptic connections, which has been suggested to occur in limb representation (22.2 mm lateral to the midline). Concentric bipo-
lar stimulating electrodes were displaced horizontally by 500 m from
adult cortex30,31, could also lead to larger responses after learn-
each recording electrode (Fig. 1a). Consistent mirror-symmetrical place-
ing. However, these new synapses would have to be unable to ment of the electrodes at identical locations in both hemispheres was
undergo LTP to be consistent with the finding of less LTP in the achieved with a reticle. Slices were not attached by the corpus callosum,
trained M1. We therefore think it unlikely that these mecha- but remained attached to each other during tissue slicing. For stimula-
nisms underlie the increased field-potential amplitudes seen tion, constant current pulses (0.2 ms) were delivered at 0.033 Hz. We
after skill learning, although additional studies are required to used the amplitude of the field potential evoked in the layer II/III hori-
identify the exact mechanism involved. zontal pathway to measure of the population excitatory synaptic response
We only examined the horizontal pathway after three or five because it reflects a monosynaptic current sink9 and correlates well with
days of practice following skill acquisition. Although synaptic intracellular excitatory postsynaptic potentials evoked in this pathway7.
modification seems to occur during this period, other mecha- Inputoutput curves for a range of stimulation intensities were con-
structed for each stimulationrecording pair. In the first set of experi-
nisms may operate during initial skill acquisition or during later
ments, we averaged three to five sweeps evoked at 20, 40, 60, 80, 100 and
skill improvement. Intracellular studies and measurements of the 120% of the intensity inducing a maximum response. In the second set,
time course of change may help to clarify what leads to such dra- we averaged three responses to stimuli of 2, 2.5, 3, 3.5, 4, 4.5 and 5 times
matic increases in this pathways efficacy. It is likely that this effect the intensity that evoked a 0.1 mV (threshold) response. Stimulation did
is not peculiar to M1. Plasticity of synapses formed by horizon- not influence the contralateral hemisphere because the slices used did
tally oriented axon collaterals may operate throughout many areas not contain the corpus callosum. Field-potential peak amplitudes were
of the cerebral cortex to restructure various representation pat- calculated from averages of three to five waveforms, and the common
terns. For example, within visual cortex, filling-in phenomena logarithm of the left /right ratio was calculated for each animal. Because
and reorganization of visual receptive fields after lesions or other the log of this ratio fit a Gaussian distribution, parametric testing was
used (paired t-test). Similar input-output curves were constructed for
perturbations appear to be mediated via horizontal connec-
field potentials recorded from the hindlimb area of trained animals.
tions32. The common occurrence of these connections in all cor-
tical areas suggests that plasticity of synapses formed by horizontal After establishing a 20 min period of stable response amplitudes using
pathways may be an important contributor to learning-related a stimulation intensity 50-60% of maximum, LTP induction was attempt-
processes throughout the cerebral cortex. ed with an established and reliable protocol for MI7. Prior to tetanic stim-
ulation, the GABAA receptor antagonist bicuculline (3.5 mM) was applied
Methods within 100 m from the recording electrodes using a glass pipette; the
Animals were cared for in accordance with National Institute of Health time of application on each side was separated by less than two minutes,
guidelines for laboratory animal welfare. All experiments were approved and the same pipette was used for each side of a slice. The bicuculline
by the Brown University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. pipette was retracted as soon as field-potential responses to test stimu-
Forty adult female Sprague-Dawley rats (150225 g) housed on a nor- lation increased to about 150200% of baseline (typically within 1030 s).
mal 12:12 light-dark cycle were used for this study. Twenty-eight animals Immediately following bicuculline application, LTP was attempted by
were housed in pairs and were food restricted to maintain their body delivering theta-burst stimulation (5 sequences of 10 bursts 10 seconds
weight at roughly 85% of their free-feeding weight. Water was provided apart; 1 burst is 5 pulses at 100 Hz) at double test intensity simultane-
ad libitum. One rat from each pair was placed in an operant test cage ously through both stimulating electrodes7. The LTP effect was recorded
(22.8 cm cube), which contained a Plexiglas food box (3.2 x 4.5 x 5 cm) for at least 30 min after it reached a stable plateau.

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 233


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Acknowledgements 15. Donoghue, J.P. Limits of reorganization in cortical circuits. Cereb. Cortex 7, 9799
(1997).
We thank Drs. Barry Connors, Mark Bear, and Marc G. Rioult for critical 16. Jacobs, K. & Donoghue, J. Reshaping the cortical map by unmasking latent
comments. This work was supported by NIH grant NS22517. G. H. is an intracortical connections. Science 251, 944947 (1991).
international scholar of the Howard Hughes Medical Institute. 17. Hirsch, J. & Gilbert, C. Long-term changes in synaptic strength along specific
intrinsic pathways in the cats visual cortex. J. Physiol. (Lond) 461, 247262
(1993).
RECEIVED 21 JANUARY: ACCEPTED 26 MAY 1998 18. Price, A.W. & Fowler, S.C. Deficits in contralateral and ipsilateral forepaw
motor control following unilateral motor cortical ablation in rats. Brain Res.
205, 8190 (1981).
1. Cohen, L.G., Brasil, N.J., Pascual-Leone, L.A. & Hallett, M. Plasticity of 19. Garraghty, P.E. & Kaas, J.H. Dynamic features of sensory and motor maps.
cortical motor output organization following deafferentation, cerebral Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2, 522527 (1992).
lesions, and skill acquisition. Adv. Neurol. 63, 187200 (1993). 20. Sanes, J.N. & Donoghue, J.P. Motor areas of the cerebral cortex. J. Clin.
2. Donoghue, J.P. Plasticity of sensorimotor representations. Curr. Opin. Neurophysiol. 11, 382396, (1994).
Neurobiol. 5, 749754 (1995). 21. Rogan, M.T., Staeubli, U.V. & LeDoux, J.E. Fear conditioning induces associative
3. Donoghue, J.P., Hess, G. & Sanes, J.N. in Acquisition of Motor Behavior (eds. long-term potentiation in the amygdala. Nature 390, 604607 (1997).
Bloedel, J., Ebner, T. & Wise, S.P.) 363386 (MIT Press, Cambridge, 1996). 22. McKernan, M.G. & Shinnick-Gallagher, P. Fear conditioning induces a lasting
4. Nudo, R.J., Milliken, G.W., Jenkins, W.M. & Merzenich, M.M. Use- potentiation of synaptic currents in vitro. Nature 390, 607611 (1997).
dependent alterations of movement representations in primary motor cortex 23. Ahissar, E. et al. Dependence of cortical plasticity on correlated activity of
of adult squirrel monkeys. J. Neurosci. 16, 785807 (1996). single neurons and on behavioral context. Science 257, 14121415 (1992).
5. Greenough, W.T., Larson, J.R. & Withers, G.S. Effects of unilateral and 24. Grafton, S.T. et al. Functional anatomy of human procedural learning
bilateral training in a reaching task on dendritic branching of neurons in the determined with regional cerebral blood flow and PET. J. Neurosci. 12,
rat motor-sensory forelimb cortex. Behav. Neural Biol. 44, 301314 (1985). 25422548 (1992).
6. Hess, G. & Donoghue, J.P. Long-term potentiation of horizontal connections 25. Pascual-Leone, A., Grafman, J. & Hallett, M. Modulation of cortical motor
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

provides a mechanism to reorganize cortical motor maps. J. Neurophysiol. 71, output maps during development of implicit and explicit knowledge. Science
25432547 (1994). 263, 12871289 (1994).
7. Hess, G., Aizenman, C.D. & Donoghue, J.P. Conditions for the induction of 26. Karni, A. et al. Functional MRI evidence for adult motor cortex plasticity
long-term potentiation in layer II/III horizontal connecitons of the rat motor during motor skill learning. Nature 377, 155158 (1995).
cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 75, 17651778 (1996). 27. Morris, R.G.M., Davis, S. & Butcher, P. in Long Term Potentiation: A Debate of
8. Hess, G. & Donoghue, J.P. Long-term depression of horizontal connections Current Issues (eds Baudry, M. & Davis, J.L.) 267300 (MIT Press,
in rat motor cortex. Eur. J. Neurosci. 8, 658665 (1996). Cambridge, 1991).
9. Aroniadou, V.A. & Keller, A. Mechanisms of LTP induction in rat motor 28. Woody, C.D., Gruen, E. & Birt, D. Changes in membrane currents during
cortex in vitro. Cereb. Cortex 5, 353362 (1995). Pavlovian conditioning of single cortical neurons. Brain Res. 539, 7684
10. Castro-Alamancos, M.A., Donoghue, J.P. & Connors, B.W. Different forms of (1991).
synaptic plasticity in somatosensory and motor areas of the neocortex. J. 29. Yoshimura, Y. & Tsumoto, T. Dependence of LTP induction on postsynaptic
Neurosci. 15, 53245333 (1995). depolarization: a perforated patch-clamp study in visual cortical slices of
11. Asanuma, H. & Pavlides, C. Neurological basis of motor learning in young rats. J. Neurophysiol. 71, 16381645 (1994).
mammals. Neuroreport 8, ivi (1997). 30. Darian, S.C. & Gilbert, C.D. Axonal sprouting accompanies functional
12. Kaas, J.H. Plasticity of sensory and motor maps in adult mammals. Annu. reorganization in adult cat striate cortex. Nature 368, 737740 (1994).
Rev. Neurosci. 14, 137167 (1991). 31. Kleim, J.A., Lussnig, E., Schwarz, E.R., Comery, T.A. & Greenough, W.T.
13. Merzenich, M.M., Recanzone, G., Jenkins, W.M., Allard, T.T. & Nudo, R.J. in Synaptogenesis and Fos expression in the motor cortex of the adult rat after
Neurobiology of Neocortex (eds Rakic, P. & Singer, W.) 4167 ( Wiley, New motor skill learning. J. Neurosci. 16, 45294535 (1996).
York, 1988). 32. Gilbert, C.D. Rapid dynamic changes in adult cerebral cortex. Curr. Opin.
14. Huntley, G.W. Correlation between patterns of horizontal connectivity and Neurobiol. 3, 100103 (1993).
the extent of short term representational plasticity in rat motor cortex. Cereb. 33. Donoghue, J.P. & Wise, S.P. The motor cortex of the rat: cytoarchitecture and
Cortex 7, 143156 (1997). microstimulation mapping. J. Comp. Neurol. 212, 7688 (1982).

234 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Retinotopy and color sensitivity in


human visual cortical area V8
Nouchine Hadjikhani, Arthur K. Liu, Anders M. Dale, Patrick Cavanagh
and Roger B. H. Tootell

Nuclear Magnetic Resonance Center, Massachusetts General Hospital, 149 13th Street, Charlestown, Massachusetts 02129, USA
Correspondence should be addressed to N.H. (nouchine@nmr.mgh.harvard.edu)

Prior studies suggest the presence of a color-selective area in the inferior occipital-temporal region
of human visual cortex. It has been proposed that this human area is homologous to macaque area
V4, which is arguably color selective, but this has never been tested directly. To test this model, we
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

compared the location of the human color-selective region to the retinotopic area boundaries in the
same subjects, using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), cortical flattening and
retinotopic mapping techniques. The human color-selective region did not match the location of
area V4 (neither its dorsal nor ventral subdivisions), as extrapolated from macaque maps. Instead
this region coincides with a new retinotopic area that we call V8, which includes a distinct
representation of the fovea and both upper and lower visual fields. We also tested the response to
stimuli that produce color afterimages and found that these stimuli, like real colors, caused preferen-
tial activation of V8 but not V4.

In Old World primates such as macaque monkeys and humans, ing studies, a small patch of color-selective activity near the mid-
visual information about color is processed in anatomically dle of the collateral sulcus has been named V4 (ref. 2022).
segregated columns, layers, channels or areas. It is important This choice of name presupposes that (1) an area homologous to
to know to what extent color is processed in separate versus macaque V4 exists in humans, (2) V4 is color-selective, and (3)
convergent visual information pathways, because the added this region in or near the collateral sulcus is the macaque V4
dimension of color is so rich in visual information. For exam- homolog. However, in humans, the location of this color-selec-
ple, we can discriminate about fifteen hundred different levels tive region has not yet been compared with the map of retino-
of luminance1, whereas we can make several million discrimi- topic areas, to see whether color selectivity is really is in a
nations if we also consider variations in color2. It is likely that retinotopically defined human area V4. Furthermore, the degree
this glut of color information is incorporated into the labeled of color selectivity in macaque V4 is itself controversial1719.
lines of the neural architecture in some organized way. This issue is not just of academic interest. In an intriguing
In macaque monkeys, an anatomical segregation between chro- clinical syndrome (achromatopsia), human patients report
matic-opponent versus achromatic-opponent cells has been report- that the visual world becomes colorless following damage to a
ed as early as the lateral geniculate nucleus. Color-specific anatomical cortical region that apparently includes this color-selective
segregation has also been described in primary (V1) and secondary area in the collateral sulcus2325. This suggests that the con-
(V2) visual cortex. In V1, prominent populations of color-selective scious percept of color involves that area, although it is
cells have been reported in specific layers35 and in the cytochrome- known that physical wavelength-dependent differences are
oxidase blobs46, though the latter claim has been disputed7,8. Sim- coded throughout prior levels of the visual system as well. If
ilar (and equally controversial) claims have been made about the we can define better which area this is in humans, we can learn
prominence of color-opponent cells in the thin stripes in area V2, something about where conscious perceptions of color arise.
to which the V1 blobs project (ref. 9,10, but see 11). Accurate localization in humans should also make it possible
However, the most prominent controversy about the to study the homologous area in macaques using more inci-
anatomical segregation of color-selective neurons occurs at a sive (but invasive) classical neurobiological techniques.
higher level, in cortical area V4. According to different reports, We have attempted to clarify these issues in humans using
a high percentage of color-selective cells is either present1215 or functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Technical details
absent16 in the largest and best-studied portion of that area, were similar to those described elsewhere26, except that here we
dorsal V4 (V4d). A high percentage of color-selective cells has manipulated the color content of the visual stimuli. We also used
not been reported in the smaller, ventral subdivision of V4 a high-field MRI scanner and other improvements to substan-
(V4v). More recent evidence suggests that brain mechanisms tially increase the sensitivity of the retinotopic maps (Methods).
critical for color selectivity are located not in macaque V4, but
rather in areas anterior to it (ref. 1719, Vanduffel et al. Soc. Results
Neurosci. Abstr. 23, 845, 1997). COLOR- VERSUS LUMINANCE-VARYING STIMULI
This controversy about color selectivity in V4 has now been First, we compared the activity produced by color variations to
extended to human visual cortex. Based on human neuroimag- that produced by variations in luminance, in the same sub-

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 235


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 1. Topography of
color-selective activity in
a b
human visual cortex.
(a, b) The inferior,
inflated cortex, with
posterior to the left and
anterior to the right, in
two subjects. (c, d) The
posterior portion of c d
cortex in fully flattened
format, another view of
the same data shown in
(a) and (b), respectively.
In all panels, gyri from
the original brain are
shown as light gray and
sulci as dark gray. The
fundus of the collateral
sulcus (cs) is indicated
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

by the dashed black line.


The borders of previ-
ously described retinotopic areas (V1, V2, V3, VP, V3A, and V4v) are indicated in white (horizontal meridians, solid lines, upper vertical
meridians, dotted lines, lower vertical meridians, dashed lines). Typically, color-varying stimuli produced relatively higher activation in the
foveal representation of V1 and often V2 and V3/VP and a distinctive patch of color-selective activation approximately midway in the collat-
eral sulcus. When present, the latter patch was always located just anterior to the horizontal meridian representation marking the anterior
border of area V4v, rather than within V4v.

jects (Fig. 1). Our stimuli consisted of slowly moving sinu- of the retina. A similar foveal color bias is found in routine clin-
soidal radial gratings (pinwheels) of low spatial frequency, ical perimetry and in numerous psychophysical studies.
defined by either color or luminance contrast (Methods). As In 25 of 26 hemispheres (13 subjects) tested, we found an
shown earlier in V1 and V2 (ref. 27), we found that both additional region that responded preferentially to color, locat-
color- and luminance-varying stimuli produced robust acti- ed midway along the length of the collateral sulcus. Based on
vation in many areas of visual cortex, when compared with a the anatomical location and the nature of the functional com-
uniform gray field (data not shown). parison used here, this collateral color-selective patch appears
Here we focused on those locations where the color stimuli equivalent to the previously reported area involved in achro-
produced more activation than the luminance stimuli. In the matopsia20,22, which has been proposed as the human homo-
classically retinotopic visual areas (V1, V2, V3/VP, V3A and logue of macaque area V4 (refs 2022, 2830).
V4v), we found prominent color-selective activation in the rep- However, when we compared the location of that collateral
resentations of the fovea (center of gaze) but not in peripheral color-selective patch to the retinotopic borders in the same sub-
representations (Fig. 1). A foveal color bias has not been report- jects, we found that the color-selective patch was consistently
ed in previous imaging studies, perhaps because it is more obvi- located just beyond the most anterior retinotopic area defined
ous in our flattened maps. However, such a foveal color bias is previously, area V4v. Earlier reports 26,3133 suggested that
consistent with the well known predominance of cone pho- human V4v is a quarter-field representation of the contralater-
toreceptors, and the corresponding absence of rods, in the fovea al upper visual field. The more sensitive retinotopic mapping

Fig. 2. Retinotopic features of area V8 by fMRI a b


mapping. (ac) Retinotopy of polar angle in the infe-
rior row of cortical areas, from three flattened
hemispheres. From left to right, each panel shows
the representations of the contralateral upper quar-
ter field (red through blue or vice versa; see
pseudocolor logo) in inferior V1, then inferior V2,
then VP, then V4v. To the right of (anterior to) V4v
is the distinctive half-field representation comprising
V8 (green through blue through red, from upper to
lower in this figure). (d) Retinotopic representation c d
of eccentricity (the other dimension in polar space),
from the same hemisphere shown in (c). The repre-
sentations of central-through-more-peripheral
eccentricities are coded in red-through-blue-
through-green, respectively (see pseudocolor logo,
bottom right). The representations of the center of gaze are indicated with an asterisk. Area V8 has its own representation of the fovea, quite
distinct (and 3.5 cm) from the foveal representation in adjacent area V4v.

236 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

a e In conventional Talairach coordi-


nates, foveal V4v (as defined retino-
topically in this study) was centered at
32, -87, -16, whereas foveal V8 was
centered at 33, -65, -14. When we
b f averaged the Talairach coordinates of
the color-selective area V4 described
in previous studies ( 26, -67, -9), we
found that it was about twice as close
to the location of our retinotopically
c g defined V8, compared to our retino-
topically defined V4v. This supports
all the other evidence that the color-
selective activity is located in area V8,
rather than in V4.
d h In each hemisphere, V8 comprises
a continuous representation of the
entire contralateral half of the visual
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

field. Although the authors did not


attempt polar-coordinate retinotopy or
flattened mapping, a prior study also
Fig. 3. Detailed retinotopy of the polar angle representation, from the same hemisphere shown
in Fig. 2a. This figure shows the peak fMRI response (noisy white lines) corresponding to polar
concluded that this human color-selec-
angle gradients of approximately 20o, superimposed on the standard pseudocolor rendering of tive region included a representation
areas V1, V2, VP and V4v (drawn in a). To the right of each panel is a logo indicating the specific of upper and lower visual fields 22 .
polar angle (white line) stimulated. The complete contralateral visual field is represented in V8, Together with V8 in the opposing
from the lower visual field (a and b), across the horizontal meridian (ce) to the upper visual hemisphere, the entire visual field is
field (fh). Note in (eh) that the upper visual field representation in V8 can clearly be distin- thus represented. Such a complete rep-
guished from, and is mirror symmetric to, that in adjacent V4v. resentation of the visual field would be
appropriate in an area processing high-
er-order, large-field color information.

methods used here confirmed that V4v represents just that HUMAN VERSUS MACAQUE MAPS
quarter field, with its foveal representation located superiorly Because so much of the historical controversy about corti-
alongside that of adjacent areas V3/VP (Figs 2 and 3). cal color processing arose in studies of macaque monkey, it is
The improved retinotopic methods also revealed additional natural to wonder which area in macaque corresponds to
retinotopic features anterior to V4v. Taken together, these fea- area V8 in humans. To clarify the topographic relationship
tures indicate the presence of an additional retinotopic map, of the human and macaque maps, we first averaged together
comprising a previously undifferentiated cor-
tical area that we call V8. This continues the
naming scheme begun by Zeki and colleagues, a b
who identified areas V1 through V61215,21,22,34.
(We also identified an area V7, which is a rep-
resentation of the contralateral lower visual
field anterior to human V3A.)
Area V8 has a unique polar angle retinotopy
and a distinctive foveal representation. This
contrasts significantly with the three extrastri-
ate representations posterior to V8 (V4v, VP,
and the inferior wing of V2), all of which are
quarter-field representations of the contralat-
eral upper visual field. Although the polar-
angle retinotopy in V8 includes an additional
representation of this quarter field, it also
extends further to include a lower-field repre-
sentation as well (Figs 2 and 3). These three
extrastriate visual areas (V4v, VP and inferior
V2) also share a contiguous representation of Fig. 4. Comparison of the polar angle retinotopy in human visual cortex, relative to
the fovea, at the top (superior) end of this row that reported in macaque monkeys. In both species, visual cortex is shown in flat-
of areas (Fig. 2d). However, the foveal repre- tened format, with visual area boundaries and polar angle continua as in Fig. 2ac.
sentation in V8 is not part of this contiguous Area MT is shown in gray. In macaque, dorsal area V4 is also indicated (V4d). The
foveal band; instead it is located about 3.5 cen- retinotopy of V8 is similar to that reported in area TEO, in that both areas are located
timeters away along the cortical surface, at the immediately adjacent to area V4v. However, the two areas differ in overall shape, and
anterior border of V8 (Fig. 2d). the retinotopy of V8 is rotated approximately 90o relative to that reported in TEO.

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 237


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

a Alternating Constant b
colors colors

Fixation Fixation Fixation

% MIRI signal
% MIRI isgnal

Color Fixation

Time (s) Time (s)


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

Fig. 5. The time course of V8 activity is related to the perception of color afterimages. (a) Time course from all voxels in retinotopically
defined V8 that responded (p<0.00001) to the colored stimuli, relative to the initial presentation of the uniform gray stimulus, averaged
across 16 MR scans, showing the response to both the actual and the illusory color stimuli. Epochs in which subjects viewed a uniform grey
field, or an illusory afterimage on a gray background, are indicated with a white background; epochs when the subjects viewed colored stim-
uli (alternating- or constant-colored) are indicated with a gray background. After the color stimulus, subjects viewed a uniform gray field, or
an illusory afterimage on a gray background. During a color afterimage, the fMRI response was quite prolonged, consistent with the time
course of the percept of the illusory colors. (b) The MR afterimage is shown more directly by subtracting signal during constant color and
subsequent gray period from alternating color and subsequent grey period.

six of the most robust human retinotopic maps, using digital in the retina42,43. However, they also presumably trigger activ-
morphing techniques (Fig. 4b) as described26. These aver- ity at higher levels, as would a real stimulus that was similarly
aged maps could then be compared to the map of macaque stabilized on the retina29,44. To test for the presence of fMRI
retinotopy, as estimated from single-unit mapping 35 responses to these illusory colors, we produced such color after-
(Fig. 4a). This comparison suggests that human area V8 images in the MR scanner, along with control stimuli that were
shows some retinotopic and topographic similarity to very similar but did not produce color afterimages.
macaque area TEO. Furthermore, TEO and/or more anteri- Figure 5 shows the time course of fMRI activity produced
or areas have appeared strongly color selective in recent stud- by these stimuli in area V8. As expected, the colored stimuli
ies of macaque visual cortex (refs 1719, 28, Vanduffel et al. produced robust fMRI activity in V8, whether alternating or
Soc Neurosci. Abstr. 23, 845, 1997). constant. However, only the constant-colored stimuli pro-
However, the retinotopic similarity between V8 and TEO is duced a perceptual color afterimage and a corresponding fMRI
far from exact (Fig. 4). Furthermore, other investigators have aftereffect in V8 during subsequent viewing of the uniform
proposed different area boundaries in this region of macaque gray field (Fig. 5a). The alternating-colored stimuli produced
cortex 3638 . Unfortunately, those alternative models of the neither a perceptual color afterimage nor a prolongation of
macaque maps are even less similar to the empirical human the normal fMRI return to baseline during the subsequent
maps in this region of cortex. Thus it is not clear which macaque viewing period (Fig. 5a). The duration of the isolated fMRI
area is homologous to human area V8. However, the flat maps color aftereffect was prolonged, consistent with the prolonged
in Fig. 4 do make it clear that this human collateral color area is duration of the illusory color percept (Fig. 5b). Overall, this
not topographically similar to macaque V4 (neither dorsal nor strongly suggests that these fMRI responses were related to
ventral subdivisions). Human V8 is also topographically incon- the processing of the illusory colors.
sistent with the location of subdivisions proposed in macaque One unexpected finding was that the stimuli with alter-
dorsal V4, such as V4t (ref. 39) and V4A (e.g. ref. 14). nating colors produced slightly more activity than the stim-
uli in which color remained constant (Fig. 5a). This may
COLOR AFTERIMAGES reflect the fact that the hues in the constant-colored stimuli
Another way to assess functional selectivity is by measuring the become progressively less saturated (less densely colored) with
fMRI responses during visual aftereffects, rather than during time because of chromatic adaptation45. Essentially one begins
the fMRI effects produced by the visual stimuli themselves. In to see the mixture of the color afterimage and the actual color,
other dimensions such as motion40 and orientation41, such indi- Because these two colors are complementary, they produce a
rect aftereffects have ironically proven to be functionally more less saturated, more washed-out color. This decreased fMRI
selective than the effects themselves. Here we tested whether response to the decreasingly saturated colors supports the
illusory color would also activate V8, as did real color stimuli. other evidence that V8 is involved in color perception.
If one stares for a time at a saturated color, then looks away Findings similar to our fMRI afterimages in V8 were
at a uniform gray field, one sees an illusory percept of the com- reported from scattered voxels in single-slice imaging through
plementary color. Unlike motion or orientation aftereffects, nearby posterior fusiform gyrus29, but the location of those
these negative color afterimages are thought to arise primarily voxels was not localized to any specific cortical area. However,

238 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

tial activation in the foveal representation of all retinotopic areas.


Thus, V8 may seem especially biased for color stimuli merely
because its foveal representation sets it topographically apart
from the conjoined foveal color responses of its neighbors (Figs
2 and 3). This is a relatively trivial explanation for the color selec-
tivity reported earlier, but we cannot rule it out completely. This
idea is further supported by the fact that area V8 responds at
reasonable levels to a wider variety of visual stimuli.
However, other evidence argues that V8 is involved in
wavelength-dependent processing and perhaps in the con-
scious perception of color itself. The robust and selective
response to illusory colors (Figs 5 and 6) strongly supports
this idea. Also, the anatomical colocalization of V8 compared
with the previous clinical data makes it likely that area V8 is
damaged in achromatopsic patients2325.
What do these human data tell us about macaque visual
cortex? This question is constrained by several factors. The
human data are based on clinical and neuroimaging data,
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

Fig. 6. The perception of color afterimages produces relatively whereas the macaque data are derived from different tech-
higher activation in cortical area V8, compared with other cortical niques (e.g., single units, lesions and DG imaging), which
areas. The activation shown here represents all regions that could conceivably produce different results. Also, there may
responded significantly more (p0.00001) during viewing of the uni- be significant biological differences between the cortical orga-
form gray stimulus following the constant color stimulus, compared nization of color sensitivity in humans compared with
with viewing of the same gray stimulus following the alternating macaques. As we learn more about human and macaque visu-
color stimulus. al cortex, the number of differences between these species are
increasing correspondingly26,4648.
Despite these caveats, the data suggest that the area of
macaque cortex that is homologous to the human achro-
none of these data address the possibility that similar fMRI matopsia area should be located in or anterior to TEO, rather
afterimages occur nonspecifically throughout much wider than in V4. This is supported by data from macaque 1719 as
areas of visual cortex. This could arise from retinal color pro- well as the present data from human V8.
cessing that is transmitted passively to cortex, or from glob-
ally increased attention when viewing the color afterimages. Methods
To test this, we reanalyzed our data to find the areas in an GENERAL PROCEDURES. Except for modifications described below, the
activity map that respond differentially during the presence methods in this study are similar to those described26. Informed writ-
of the visual afterimage (Fig. 6). At lower levels of significance ten consent was obtained for each subject prior to the scanning ses-
than shown here, a number of additional visual areas (e.g. V1, sion, and all procedures were approved by Massachusetts General
V2) do respond more to color afterimages. However, at the Hospital Human Studies Protocol numbers 907227 and 967464.
more strict significance threshold used in Fig. 6, the activity Normal human subjects, with (or corrected to) emmetropic vision,
in V8 was more prominent than in any other area. In partic- were scanned in General Electric magnetic resonance (MR) scanners
ular, the wider areas of foveal color selective activities includ- retrofitted with ANMR echo-planar imaging. Most scans were
acquired in a high-field (3 T) scanner, but some early scans were
ing areas such as V1, V2, etc., were relatively less prominent acquired in a scanner of conventional (1.5 T) field strength. Based on
in the afterimage test, compared to the direct comparisons of signal-to-noise ratios obtained during otherwise comparable condi-
color versus luminance (see Fig. 1c and Fig. 6, showing the tions, four functional scans at 1.5 T were found to be approximately
same hemisphere). equal to one functional scan at 3 T, so this was the ratio used to equate
data acquired from the two scanners. Head motion was minimized
Discussion by using bite bars with deep, individually molded dental impressions.
The retinotopic maps make it clear that an additional area The subjects task in all experiments was to fixate the center of each
(V8) exists beyond those areas described previously in human type of visual stimulus throughout the period of scan acquisition.
visual cortex. Area V8 is retinotopically distinct from the pre- MR images were acquired using a custom-built quadrature surface
viously described area V4v, based on at least four different cri- coil, shaped to fit the posterior portion of the head. MR slices were
teria. First, V4v and V8 have separate foveal representations, 3-4 mm thick, with an in-plane resolution of 3.1 x 3.1 mm, oriented
approximately 3.5 cm apart along the cortical surface. Second, approximately perpendicular to the calcarine fissure. Each scan took
V4v and V8 each include separate representations of the upper either 4 min 16 s (color-versus-luminance and color afterimage scans)
or 8 min 32 s (retinotopy), using a TR of either two or four seconds,
visual field, separated from each other by a representation of respectively. Each scan included 2,048 images, comprised of 128
the horizontal meridian. Third, V8 differs from V4v in its images per slice in 16 contiguous slices.
global functional properties, including but not limited to color
Improved retinotopic maps were obtained from 32 subjects (79
sensitivity. Fourth, the nature of the retinotopy in V8 is dif-
scans polar angle, 79 scans eccentricity, 323,584 images total). Among
ferent from that in V4v. them, 13 subjects were also tested for color-versus-luminance (112
The direct comparisons between color- and luminance-vary- scans, 229,376 images total). Of these, five subjects were tested exten-
ing stimuli (Fig. 1) indicate that area V8 responds slightly better sively for color afterimages (100 scans; 204,800 images total). In most
than neighboring cortical regions to colored stimuli. However, subjects, additional scans were done to clarify the location of area
we also found that the color-varying stimuli produce preferen- MT and other visual areas.

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 239


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

VISUAL STIMULI. The goal of the first color-related experiment (Fig. 1) initially analyzed by doing a fast Fourier transform on the MR time course
was to map the MR activity produced by color- versus luminance- from each voxel. Statistical significance was calculated by converting the
varying stimuli throughout visual cortex, using conventional psy- Fourier magnitude of the response to an f-statistic. The phase of the sig-
chophysical stimuli. Prior to scanning, the equiluminance values for nal at the stimulus frequency was used to track stimulus location in the
different color combinations (red-cyan, CIE x and y coordinates 0.645, case of retinotopic stimuli, and to distinguish between positive- or negative-
0.345 and 0.185, 0.248 respectively, or green-purple, CIE x and y coor- going MR fluctuations in the case of two-condition stimulus comparisons.
dinates 0.277, 0.684 and 0.351, 0.220, respectively) were measured for
each subject, outside the scanner. Equiluminance was measured using Scans comparing more than two stimuli (e.g., the color afterimage
a motion-null test, with the same stimulus projector (NEC model MT data) were analyzed by selective averaging of two conditions. This was
800), lens and color software used subsequently in the MR experi- followed by statistical comparison using a t-test of the difference of the
ments. In the first experiment, both color- and luminance-varying first seconds following onset of the next epoch (here stimulus offset).
stimuli were produced using slowly moving (0.5 Hz) sinusoidal radi-
al gratings (pinwheels) of low spatial frequency (3 cycles per revo- For topographic clarity, all data were analyzed and displayed in cor-
lution). The gratings varied either in achromatic luminance tical surface format, as described 36,43,44 . This made it possible to
(maximum, 95% luminance contrast) or in equiluminant color (at extract the MR time courses from voxels in specific cortical areas,
maximum available saturations of the display device within con- which were defined in the same subjects. The specific areas sampled
straints of approximately 140 cd per m2 mean luminance and approx- were V1, V2, V3/VP, V3A, V4v, MT+, and V8. Area MT+ was defined
imately white mean chromaticity), in direct alternation, in 16-second on the basis of additional scans comparing moving and stationary
epochs, using 16 epochs per scan. stimuli26,40. All other areas were based on retinotopic criteria.
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

In a second experiment (Figs 5 and 6), color afterimages were pro- For ease of comparison, all hemispheres are shown in right hemi-
duced by showing subjects colored adaptation patterns. Subjects sphere format. Above a minimum threshold, the statistical significance
adapted to two general types of stimuli; one produced a pronounced of the displayed pseudocolor range has been normalized according to
color afterimage when subjects subsequently viewed a uniform gray the overall sensitivity of each subject, as described elsewhere.
field, but a very similar control stimulus did not produce such a color
afterimage. There were five epochs in each scan, presented in the fol- Acknowledgments
lowing order: (1) uniform gray, (2) alternating-color (control adap- This work was supported by grants from the Human Frontiers Science
tation), (3) uniform gray, (4) constant-color (experimental Foundation and NEI EY07980 to R.B.H.T., NEI EY09258 to P.C. and Swiss
adaptation) and (5) uniform gray. All epochs were 48-s long, except Fonds National de la Recherche Scientifique to N.H. We thank Terry
that the final fixation period was prolonged 16 s to reveal the final
Campbell and Mary Foley for scanning and participation in these
traces of the MR afterimage. In the analysis for Fig. 5b, the last 16 s
experiments, Robert Savoy, Ken Kwong, Bruce Fischl and Kevin Hall for
in that final epoch was truncated to match the duration of all other
epochs. Both the constant- and the alternating-colored patterns were advice, Tommy Vaughan for coil design and manufacture and Martin Sereno
comprised of complementary colors (red-cyan or green-purple), spa- for modifying pilot stimuli. Wim Vanduffel, Ekkehardt Kustermann and
tially arranged in opposed quarter-fields (i.e. two-cycle-per-revolu- Irene Tracy also helped in preliminary versions of this experiment.
tion polar square waves), akin to interleaved bow ties. All hues were
presented at equal luminance, based on motion-null tests in each sub- RECEIVED 16 APRIL: ACCEPTED 21 MAY 1998
ject. Following adaptation to the constant colors, subjects initially
experienced a prominent color afterimage against the uniform gray
1. Cornsweet, T.N. Visual Perception (Academic Press, New York, 1970).
background, which faded over tens of seconds. The afterimage was 2. Judd, D.B. & Wyszecki, G. Color in Business, Science, & Industry 3rd edn
retinotopically similar to the adaptation stimulus, but of comple- 388 (Wiley, New York, 1975).
mentary color. As controls, we presented stimuli equivalent to the 3. Dow, B.M. Functional classes of cells and their laminar distribution in
constant colors, except that the colors alternated between color and monkey visual cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 37, 927946 (1974).
complementary color, reversing every 2 s. The latter condition pro- 4. Livingstone, M.S. & Hubel, D.H. Anatomy and physiology of a color
system in the primate visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 4, 309356 (1984).
duced no perceptual color afterimages during the subsequent epoch of 5. Tootell, R.B.H., Silverman, M.S., Hamilton, S.L., De Valois, R.L. &
uniform gray stimulation. Switkes, E. Functional anatomy of macaque striate cortex: III. Color J.
Neurosci. 8, 15691593 (1988).
Stimuli for retinotopic mapping were slowly moving, phase-encod- 6. Tso, D.Y., Frostig, R.D., Lieke, E.E. & Grinvald, A. Functional
ed thin rays or rings comprised of counterphasing black and white organization of primate visual cortex revealed by high resolution optical
imaging. Science 249, 417420 (1990).
checks, scaled according to polar coordinates, similar to those 7. Lennie, P., Krauskopf, J. & Sclar, G. Chromatic machanisms in striate
described26,31,32,50. However, to produce the most informative retino- cortex of macaque. J. Neurosci. 10, 649669 (1990).
topic maps possible, several stimulus modifications and new proce- 8. Leventhal, A.G., Thompson, K.G., Liu, D., Zhou, Y. & Ault, S.J.
dures were implemented. First, all retinotopic measurements were Concommitant sensitivity to orientation, direction and color of cells in
layers 2, 3 and 4 of monkey striate cortex. J. Neurosci. 15, 18081818 (1995).
made in the 3 T scanner. This increased the MR amplitudes by a factor 9. Hubel, D.H. & Livingstone, M.S. Segregation of form, color and stereopsis
near four, and the physiological signal-to-noise ratio by a factor near in primate area 18. J. Neurosci. 7, 33783415 (1987).
two. Second, we signal-averaged the information from 412 scans 10. Tootell, R.B.H. & Hamilton, S.L. Functional anatomy of the second
(8,19224,576 MR images) of polar angle or eccentricity. Data were cortical visual area (V2) in the macaque. J. Neurosci. 9, 26202644 (1989).
also combined from different slice prescriptions on the same cortical 11. Gegenfurtner, K.R., Kiper, D.C. & Fenstemaker, S.B. Processing of color,
form and motion in macaque area V2. Vis. Neurosci. 13, 161172 (1996).
surface, to reduce intervoxel aliasing. Third, the retinotopic stimuli 12. Zeki, S.M. Colour coding in rhesus monkey prestriate cortex. Brain Res.
were increased in extent both foveally and peripherally, to extend from 27, 422427 (1973).
0.2 through 1830. This activated correspondingly more surface in 13. Zeki, S.M. Colour coding in the superior temporal sulcus of rhesus
each cortical area. Fourth, the visual stimuli were presented using a monkey visual cortex. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 197, 195223 (1977).
14. Zeki, S. Uniformity and diversity of structure and function in rhesus
new LCD projector of higher spatial resolution (800 x 600), using bet- monkey prestriate visual cortex. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 277, 273290 (1978).
ter optics than previously (aperture lens, bypassing shielding screen, 15. Zeki, S. The distribution of wavelength and orientation selective cells in
etc.). Fifth, the retinotopic stimuli varied in color as well as luminance, different areas of monkey visual cortex. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 217, 449470
to better activate any color-selective cells in the region. The sum of all (1983).
these manipulations produced very robust retinotopic maps. 16. Schein, S.J., Marrocco, R.T. & de Monasterio, F.M. Is there a high
concentration of color-selective cells in area V4 of monkey visual cortex?
J. Neurophysiol. 47, 193213 (1982).
DATA ANALYSIS. Data from two-condition experiments (e.g., color-versus- 17. Heywood, C.A., Gadotti, A. & Cowey, A. Cortical area V4 and its role in
luminance comparisons) and phase-encoded retinotopic experiments were the perception of color. J. Neurosci. 12, 40564065 (1992).

240 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

18. Heywood, C.A., Gaffan, D. & Cowey, A. Cerebral achromatopsia in 35. Boussaoud, D., Desimone, R. & Ungerleider, L.G. Visual topography of
monkeys. Eur. J. Neurosci. 7, 10641073 (1995). area TEO in the macaque. J. Comp. Neurol. 306, 554575 (1991).
19. Cowey, A. & Heywood, C.A. Theres more to colour than meets the eye. 36. Felleman, D.J. & Van Essen, D.C. Distributed hierarchical processing in
Behav. Brain Res. 71, 89100 (1995). the primate cerebral cortex. Cereb. Cortex 1, 147 (1991).
20. Lueck, C.J. et al. The colour centre in the cerebral cortex of man. Nature 37. Zeki, S. Are areas TEO and PIT of monkey visual cortex wholly distinct
340, 386389 (1989). from the fourth visual complex (V4 complex)? Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 263,
21. Zeki, S. et al. A direct demonstration of functional specialization in 15391544 (1996).
human visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 11, 641649 (1991). 38. Maguire, W.M. & Baizer, J.S. Visuotopic organization of the prelunate
22. McKeefry, D.J. & Zeki, S. The position and topography of the human colour gyrus in rhesus monkey. J. Neurosci. 7, 16901704 (1984).
centre as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Brain 120, 39. Van Essen, D.C., Maunsell, J.H. & Bixby J.L. The middle temporal visual
22292242 (1997). area in the macaque: Myeloarchitecture, connections, functional
23. Pearlman, A.L., Birch, J. & Meadows, J.C. Cerebral color blindness: An properties and topographic organization. J. Comp. Neurol. 199, 293326
acquired defect in hue discrimination. Ann. Neurol. 5, 253261 (1979). (1981).
24. Damasio, A., Yamada, T., Damasio, H., Corbett, J. & McKee, J. Central 40. Tootell, R.B.H. et al. Visual motion aftereffect in human cortical area MT
achromatopsia: Behavioral, anatomic, and physiologic aspects. Neurology 30, revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nature 375, 139141
10641071 (1980). (1995).
25. Zeki, S. A century of cerebral achromatopsia. Brain 113, 17211777 41. Tootell, R.B.H. et al. Functional analysis of primary visual cortex (V1) in
(1990). humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 95, 811817 (1998).
26. Tootell, R.B.H. et al. Functional analsis of V3A and related areas in human 42. Craik, K.J.W. Origin of visual afterimages. Nature 145, 512 (1940).
visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 17, 70767078 (1997). 43. Brindley, G.S. Two new properties of foveal afterimages and a
27. Engel, S., Zhang, X. & Wandell, B. Colour tuning in human visual cortex photochemical hypothesis to explain them. J. Physiol. 164, 168179
measured with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nature 388, 6871 (1962).
(1997). 44. Schiller, P.H. & Dolan, R.P. Visual aftereffects and the consequences of
28. Kennard, C., Lawden, M., Morland, A.B. & Ruddock, K.H. Colour visual system lesions on their perception in the rhesus monkey. Vis.
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

identification and colour constancy are impaired in a patient with Neurosci. 11, 643665 (1994).
incomplete achromatopsia associated with prestriate cortical lesions. 45. Jameson, D., Hurvich, L.M. & Varner, F.D. Receptoral and postreceptoral
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 260, 169175 (1995). processes in recovery from chromatic adaptation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
29. Sakai, K. et al. Functional mapping of the human colour centre with echo- USA 76, 30343038 (1979).
planar magnetic resonance imaging. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 261, 8998 (1995). 46. Tootell, R.B.H. & Taylor, J.B. Anatomical evidence for MT and additional
30. Kleinschmidt, A., Lee, B.B., Requardt, M. & Frahm, J. Functional mapping cortical visual areas in humans. Cereb. Cortex 5, 3955 (1995).
of color processing by magnetic resonance imaging of responses to selective 47. Tootell, R.B.H. et al. Functional analysis of human MT and related visual
P- and M-pathway stimulation. Exp. Brain Res. 110, 279288 (1996). cortical areas using magnetic resonance imaging. J. Neurosci. 15,
31. DeYoe, E.A. et al. Mapping striate and extrastriate visual areas in human 32153230 (1995).
cerebral cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 93, 23822386 (1996). 48. Jacobs, G.H. & Deegan, J.F. Spectral sensitivity of macaque monkeys
32. Sereno, M.I. et al. Borders of multiple visual areas in humans revealed by measured with ERG flicker photometry Vis. Neurosci. 14, 921928 (1997).
functional magnetic resonance imaging. Science 268, 889893 (1995). 49. DeYoe, E.A., Felleman, D.J., Van Essen, D.C. & McClendon, E. Multiple
33. Tootell, R.B.H., Dale, A.M., Sereno, M.I. & Malach, R. New images from processing streams in occipitotemporal visual cortex. Nature 371,
human visual cortex. Trends Neurosci. 19, 481489 (1996). 151154 (1994).
34. Galletti, C. Fattori, P., Battaglini, P.P., Shipp, S. & Zeki, S. Functional 50. Engel, S.A., Glover, G.H. & Wandell, B.A. Retinotopic organization in
demarcation of a border between areas V6 and V6A in the superior human visual cortex and the spatial precision of functional MRI. Cereb.
parietal gyrus of the macaque monkey. Eur. J. Neurosci. 8, 3052 (1996). Cortex 7, 181192 (1997).

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 241


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Complete sparing of high-contrast color


input to motion perception in cortical
color blindness
Patrick Cavanagh1, Marie-Anne Hnaff2, Franois Michel2, Theodor Landis3,
Tom Troscianko4 and James Intriligator5

1 Department of Psychology, Harvard University, 33 Kirkland Street, Cambridge, Massachusetts 02138, USA
2 INSERM U-280, 151 cours A.Thomas, Lyon, 69003, France
3 Department of Neurology, University Hospital Geneva, rue Micheli-du Crest, CH - 1211 Genve 14, Switzerland
4 Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Bristol, 8 Woodland Road, Bristol, BS8 1TN, UK
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

5 Department of Neurology, Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center, 330 Brookline Avenue, Boston, Massachusetts, 02215, USA
Correspondence should be addressed to P.C. (patrick@wjh.harvard.edu)

It is widely held that color and motion are processed by separate parallel pathways in the visual
system, but this view is difficult to reconcile with the fact that motion can be detected in
equiluminant stimuli that are defined by color alone. To examine the relationship between color and
motion, we tested three patients who had lost their color vision following cortical damage (central
achromatopsia). Despite their profound loss in the subjective experience of color and their inability
to detect the motion of faint colors, all three subjects showed surprisingly strong responses to high-
contrast, moving color stimuli equal in all respects to the performance of subjects with normal
color vision. The pathway from opponent-color detectors in the retina to the motion analysis areas
must therefore be independent of the damaged color centers in the occipitotemporal area. It is
probably also independent of the motion analysis area MT/V5, because the contribution of color to
motion detection in these patients is much stronger than the color response of monkey area MT.

In central achromatopsia, patients lose the sensation of color particular spared response could mediate other responses to
as a result of damage to the cortex and often report that the color stimuli. For example, to an observer with normal color
world appears solely in shades of gray, as in a black-and-white vision, a saturated color appears brighter than its luminance
movie 14. Despite this almost total loss of color sensation, would predict17. If an achromat lost the experience of color
there are several reports that central achromats paradoxical- but retained this supplementary brightness response8,11, equi-
ly preserve other contributions of color to visual performance. luminant color bars would have a residual brightness differ-
Some of these preserved functions may indicate that the dam- ence. In this case, the visibility of the bars and their motion
age to the color analysis areas was less than total. For exam- could be reduced but never eliminated. Conversely, if motion
ple, tests of sensitivity to color increments and color contrast responses to color are totally preserved in central achro-
in several central achromats59 and in one patient with par- matopsia, it would argue against a residual, spared mecha-
tial achromatopsia10 have, up to now, revealed performance nism and suggest that colors contribution to motion takes a
quite similar to that of subjects with normal color vision. In route that is anatomically remote from the damaged areas.
contrast, the achromats in our experiments all show profound We now resolve this question with a population of central
losses on these tests, indicating more extensive damage to color achromats who show little or no preserved opponent-color
processing areas. On the other hand, it seems that all central processing in spectral sensitivity, detection or motion tasks at
achromats, including those in our experiments, can see color- threshold. At suprathreshold levels, however, these same
defined shapes due to the preserved visibility of borders achromats surprisingly demonstrate normal levels of motion
between colors they no longer distinguish8,1114. A likely can- responses to moving colored gratings.
didate for this border response is the magnocellular, or non- To evaluate this performance, we included two control
opponent pathway, which responds to color transitions15,16. groups: normal subjects and congenitally red/green-deficient
Both the perception of motion for equiluminous color observers (where the site of loss is in the retina). These indi-
stimuli11 and a color-specific motion aftereffect 5 have been viduals are commonly described as color blind; however, they
demonstrated in one central achromat. However, neither study suffer only a partial loss of color experience limited to the
established the relative strength of the preserved motion red/green dimension, a loss much less extensive than that
response. If motion responses to color stimuli were only weak- experienced by the central achromats. Because these observers
ly preserved, we could attribute this to the partial sparing of have little chromatic response to red/green stimuli, however,
some color-analysis areas adjacent to the cortical damage. Any we can use their performance to evaluate the level of residual

242 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

JPC light JPC dark reduction of vision following a brief loss of consciousness.
(WM is described in more detail in ref. 18.) MRI and com-

Log relative sensitivity


Log relative sensitivity

puter tomography subsequently revealed extensive occipi-


totemporal lesions bilaterally in the territory of both posterior
cerebral arteries. JPN was 41 and had suffered bilateral strokes
of the occipital region, producing a large lesion of the right
occipital pole and a lesion of the left lingual and fusiform gyri.
JPN reported no conscious sensations of color and showed
chance-level color naming in a forced-choice task. JPC noticed
only reds (and some yellows) as being weakly tinted but he
Wavelength (nm) Wavelength (nm) did not name them as red because the tint differed from the
red he remembered. WM reported no conscious sensations of
WM light TT light normal color but when forced to guess, he demonstrated better-than-
Log relative sensitivity

Log relative sensitivity

chance naming of red and yellow.

SPECTRAL SENSITIVITY
The increment threshold spectral sensitivity task is a measure
of the degree of function of the opponent-color pathways19,20.
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

A chromatic stimulus of varying intensity is presented briefly


on a white background, and the observer reports whether any-
thing has been presented. In the normal observer, the sensi-
tivity peaks at three different wavelengths, which are
Wavelength (nm) Wavelength (nm)
characteristic of opponent-cone interactions20. If the tests are
presented on a dark background, the luminance mechanism
Fig. 1. Sensitivity for increment on light and dark fields as a function
is more sensitive than any of the opponent mechanisms, and
of wavelength for two central achromats, JPC and WM, and one
normal, TT. The form of the function on a dark field was similar for
the sensitivity curve has a single peak. Both of these tests were
both achromats and for the normal, and only one plot is shown performed on two of the patients, JPC and WM, and a nor-
(JPC, top right). Only the normal observer shows the typical three- mal observer, TT, one of the authors. The data for WM have
peaked function on the light field. Both achromats show a single- been reported elsewhere13.
peaked function on light and dark fields, indicating that there is little Both patients showed single-peaked functions for thresh-
or no contribution of opponent-color mechanisms to these detec- olds on the light and dark backgrounds. The normal observer
tion thresholds. showed the expected three-peaked function with light back-
grounds and single-peaked function with dark background.
These results (Fig. 1) indicate that the two patients have sig-
nificant losses in the opponent-color pathways, which mediate
luminance signals in our red/green color stimuli. broad ranges of the typical three-peaked threshold function
Our tests reveal that motion responses to high-contrast seen in normal subjects.
color gratings are at normal levels in the three patients but This experiment was very similar to previous experi-
absent in the congenitally red/green-deficient observers. These ments 7,8 on two other central achromats. Both of those
results suggest that the cortical damage that produces achro- patients showed the normal, three-peaked function. One
matopsia spares a particular cortical route for high-contrast explanation of these earlier results8 was that some color-oppo-
color information, allowing it to contribute to motion per- nent response was spared and contributed to a brightness per-
ception without contributing to color sensations. cept for the stimuli but not a color percept. We conclude that
the previously tested patients had some sparing of high-level
Results color centers, whereas WM and JPC have a more profound
CASE HISTORIES loss with no evidence of preserved color-opponent contribu-
The three patients with central achromatopsia were similar in tion to increment thresholds.
that they all had bilateral lesions, made several errors in read-
ing the colored digits of the Ishihara text and, when asked to CONTRAST THRESHOLDS
order several colored disks according to their hue (Farnsworth- These tests evaluated the color contrast that allow observers
Munsell test), they made virtually random orderings. All three to detect a red/green sinusoidal grating or to determine its
also had visual agnosia and prosopagnosia (deficits in object direction of motion. Both smoothly moving gratings and grat-
and face recognition). Two had upper-field loss (WM, JPC) ings moving in 90 steps were used. The 90 steps11,21,22 effec-
and one (JPN) had only one full quadrant of vision (lower tively control for motion cues from the borders, which central
right). Visual acuity and contrast sensitivity for achromatic achromats are known to detect well. With each step, the new
tests in the preserved visual fields showed that at least coarse positions of the red/green transitions fall exactly halfway
spatial resolution was retained for all three patients2,12. between the previous positions, rendering the border cue
At the time of testing, JPC was 43, a former florist, and his ambiguous.
lesions were the result of an assault that caused bilateral hem- All three central achromats showed very high discrimina-
orrhage in the occipital-temporal regions. Magnetic resonance tion (Fig. 2) and detection thresholds. The thresholds for WM
imaging (MRI) revealed lesions principally in both fusiform were exceptionally high. These thresholds suggest that WM,
gyri but sparing the lingual gyri and the left posterior fusiform JPC and JPN have severe losses in their response to color. We
gyrus. WM was 74, a former electrician who suffered a severe did not find any evidence of a preserved brightness response

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 243


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

opposed to a high-level, tracking response21,25. To make the


measurements, color and luminance gratings were superim-
Threshold color contrast (%)

posed and set in motion in opposite directions. At a particu-


lar balance of the contrast of the two gratings, their motions
cancelled and ambiguous motion or flicker was seen. The con-
trast of the luminance grating that just cancelled the motion of
the color grating was taken as the equivalent luminance con-
trast of the color grating21. Moreover, by again using con-
genitally red/green-deficient subjects as a control group, we
were able to estimate the baseline response of the luminance
mechanisms to residual luminance components in the color
stimulus.
Despite catastrophic loss in color sensitivity, when tested
Motion jump ( of phase) with these suprathreshold color gratings, the three achro-
matopsic patients have equivalent luminance contrasts that
Fig. 2. Threshold color contrast for the three central achromats lie near or within the range of values found in normal sub-
for direction discrimination of the moving color gratings. The range
jects (Fig. 4). The responses for the 90 jump conditions can
of values measured for two congenitally red/green-deficient
only be mediated by color-specific motion analysis because
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

observers is shown by a yellow band and that for two normals by a


the red/green borders are producing ambiguous motion sig-
green band. Detection thresholds are not shown but are similar to
the discrimination thresholds for all subjects, except for the nor- nals in this stimulus.
mals, in which detection thresholds are lower than discrimination The congenitally red/green-deficient observers showed
thresholds by a factor of two to three. motion strengths in the 2% to 4% range, indicating that the
sum of their weak color response and luminance artifacts from
monitor and optical sources was less than 4%. The difference
between these values and the strengths measured for the cen-
to color, which was proposed as the source of the preserved tral achromats is therefore a lower bound on the actual
chromatic sensitivity in MS and other previously tested achro- strength of the color-specific input to motion for these
mats11. patients. The high levels of equivalent luminance contrasts
There were no marked differences between the thresholds found for the three patients, as well as their normal equilu-
for smoothly moving or jumping stimuli, indicating that the minance settings, again demonstrate that they have normal
response to the chromatic borders in these patients was not a red- and green-sensitive cone function21.
significant factor in producing these threshold responses. It is nevertheless possible that some residual cues might
Despite the elevated thresholds, the red/green equiluminance differentiate the red and the green of the gratings for the cen-
values for the achromats were in the normal range, indicating tral achromats and allow them to track individual grating bars.
normal function of at least the red- and green-sensitive cone It is unlikely, however, that these strategies (which the patients
classes 21 . Of the two congenitally red/green-deficient did not report) could produce equivalent luminance contrasts
observers, the deutan (with losses in the green-sensitive cone in the normal range seen here. On the other hand, JPC does
class) showed an equiluminance setting in the normal range, report some residual color sensations for red stimuli, and so all
whereas the protan (with losses in the red-sensitive cone class) the tests were repeated with purple/green gratings (differen-
required significantly more luminance for red. tially stimulating principally the blue-sensitive cones) for both
WM and JPC. The patients again showed the same motion
MOTION NULLING strength as normal subjects.
Like a subject (MS) in a previous study11, the three patients How can these patients have normal levels of equivalent
tested in the previous experiment could report the direction luminance contrast but red/green-deficient levels of color-
of a color grating (once it was above threshold). However, contrast threshold? How can the response to color be absent
unlike MS, these three patients have severe losses in chromat- up to a significantly elevated threshold and then jump to
ic sensitivity and require very high contrasts for accurate robust, normal levels once above threshold? We tested one
judgements. A luminance mechanism, which is nominally but patient (JPC) for blindsight 26, to test the possibility that he
never perfectly insensitive to color, may start to respond to might be able to report the direction of motion for stimuli he
these very strong color stimuli. Therefore when the three claims not to see. After instructions to guess, his reports of
patients tested here do respond to color at
high contrasts, it may be due to a small
Fig. 3. A sinusoidal, equiluminous,
response of the luminance pathway to color Superimposed color grating (red/green) and a sinu-
or it may be due to the response of a color- gratings Luminance
soidal luminance grating (light and
specific mechanism. grating
dark yellow) were superimposed,
To resolve this question, we used a stim- moving in opposite directions. (The
ulus (Fig. 3) that dissociates the two com- stimuli are depicted here as square-
ponents, color and luminance, at wave gratings for convenience.) At a
suprathreshold levels21,2325. This gives us particular contrast of the luminance
a measure of the strength of the color con- grating, the two motions null each
tribution to motion for the patients and for other, establishing the equivalent
Color
the normal observers. This test evaluates grating
luminance contrast of the color
the low-level motion response to color as grating.

244 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

patients appears to be the most profound yet tested. The pre-


Equivalent luminance contrast (%)

served motion responses to color were found despite this


extreme loss of color processing.
To gauge the magnitude of colors contribution to motion
in our patients, consider that the maximum red/green color
contrast we used produced 14% and 35% of cone contrast for
the red- and green-sensitive cones, respectively. This is the
same order of magnitude as the luminance contrast required
to null it, 12% to 23% in these patients (and the normal sub-
jects). In other words, the contribution of a color stimulus to
motion, where the cone responses are out of phase, seems to
be quite similar to the contribution of a luminance stimulus,
where the same cone contrasts are in phase.
Motion jump ( of phase) This result seems at first at odds with the many reports of
degraded motion perception for chromatic stimuli. Howev-
Fig. 4. Equivalent luminance contrasts for color gratings moving in er, recent studies have shown that thresholds for motion of
smooth (10.8) and 90 jumps. Data are shown individually for the gratings defined by color are actually lower than those for
three central achromats. The range of values measured for six con- luminance-defined gratings 27,28. The explanation is that the
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

genitally red/green-deficient observers is shown as a yellow band threshold for seeing a color-defined stimulus is lower still, the
and that for normals (five observers) as a green band. lowest of all stimuli29, so that color gratings can be seen before
their motion is noticed. The result is the phenomenon of
stopped motion30,31, which led to the erroneous belief that
color was a poor source of motion information. More recent
motion direction for red/green gratings were random for stim- studies demonstrate a strong21 and independent32 contribu-
uli in the 8% to 15% range of color contrast just below his tion of color to motion, and our results here show a contri-
detection threshold (but still about ten times higher than nor- bution at a level that could not be mediated by any secondary,
mal thresholds). It seems that the loss of performance at low residual effect. The secondary, residual responses probably
color contrast was real and is independent of the performance account for the eventual detection of the color stimuli at the
at high color contrast. very high thresholds found in the patients and the congeni-
We also tested his responses in the motion-nulling test tally red/green-deficient observers. This suggest that the sec-
(described above) at low contrasts. The color grating had no ondary cues have about one-tenth the power of the color
effect once it was below threshold; the motion of the opposing signal.
luminance grating was the only direction the patient reported. One possible site for mediating the response to color is MT,
Above his color threshold, however, the contrast of the lumi- an area specialized in motion analysis, which is located far
nance grating required to null the motion of the color grat- from the areas damaged in achromatopsia. Physiological
ing rose rapidly. Indeed, a normal subject (PC) showed the recordings in monkeys 3335 and fMRI studies in humans 36
same, rapid rise in equivalent luminance contrast above 20% have shown responses to equiluminous chromatic gratings in
color contrast. However, the normal observer also had a weak area MT. Experiments with a 90 stimulus like that used here
residual equivalent luminance contrast (1% to 2%) for lower have shown that this response can be based on color infor-
chromatic contrasts, whereas the achromat showed none. The mation, not just on the color borders 33 . However, this
patient seems to share with the normal subject a motion response is very weak, with an equivalent luminance contrast
response to color that rapidly gains strength at high contrast. of only 2.5%, within the range that congenitally red/green-
The patient has lost a more sensitive process that operates at deficient observers show and therefore attributable to distor-
low contrasts in normal subjects and apparently depends on tions in the monitor, the eye or the non-opponent pathway.
the functioning of higher color centers. This level of response is far too low to explain the performance
of normal subjects and patients whose equivalent luminance
Discussion contrasts were in the range of 12 to 23%.
At high color contrasts, the strength of the color contribution If MT is not the site of the robust, low-level motion
to motion for the three central achromats was equivalent to response to chromatic stimuli that we report here, where could
that for normal subjects, demonstrating that there is a direct it be? The site of cortical damage in humans with achro-
path for color information from the retina to cortical motion matopsia invariably includes the ventral surface of occipi-
detectors that is spared in central achromatopsia. Motion totemporal region, and although this deficit is often claimed to
responses mediated by this pathway were fully preserved, sug- involve the human homologue of monkey V4, recent lesion
gesting that the pathway was completely independent of the studies in monkeys suggest that this may not be the case37.
lesioned color centers of the ventral surface of the occipi- In humans, the most recent fMRI studies38 suggest a more
totemporal region. anterior site, V8, for color analysis in a location consistent
Previously tested central achromats have shown residual with the damage in the achromatopsic patients. The human
opponent-color processing as indicated by a three-peaked homologue to V4 on the ventral surface (V4v) is probably also
spectral sensitivity curve and normal detection thresh- damaged in these patients but it includes only a representa-
olds79,11. In contrast, in our study, both patients who were tion of the upper visual fields39. The matching area that rep-
tested for spectral sensitivity had only a single-peaked func- resents the lower visual field has not been identified with
tion, and all three had significantly elevated color-detection certainty. If there is a dorsal equivalent to V4 with a map of
thresholds. The damage to the color centers in these three the lower visual fields, we speculate that it might offer a pos-

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 245


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

the chromatic contrast for which the subject reported the direction
sible site for the strong contribution of color to low-level
correctly on 75% of the trials.
motion in these patients and in normals. In monkeys, V4 does
have a fair percentage of cells that are directionally selective40. MOTION NULLING. The observers were the three central achromats, six
Moreover, V4 in monkeys is directly involved in maintaining congenitally red/green-deficient observers and five normal observers.
The red/green-deficient subjects (two deutans, four protans) each
a representation of motions in a delayed match-to-sample
failed at least 18 of the 24 Ishihara plates, whereas the normal sub-
task 41 . Although the site of the robust motion response to jects made no more than three errors.
high-contrast color remains to be determined, our results
demonstrate that the path from the retina to this area does The stimulus was a rotating wheel of eight cycles of a red/green
sinewave as before but now (Fig. 4) superimposed additively on a sim-
not pass through the damaged ventral area of these patients. ilar grating with eight cycles of a light/dark yellow sinewave rotating
This suggests that color information does not flow as a single, in the opposite direction. The color contrast was set at the maximum
hierarchically organized stream through the visual system but available between the red and green phosphors with allowance for the
rather projects in parallel to several different centers, only one superimposed luminance grating. The resulting color contrast was
of which leads to the conscious experience of color. between about two (WM) and five (JPC, JPN) times threshold con-
trast. The stimulus was otherwise identical in all respects to that used
Methods in the previous experiment.
SPECTRAL SENSITIVITY. The observers were two central achromats, WM In a forced-choice procedure, while fixating the bulls-eye, the
and JPC, and one normal subject, TT, one of the authors. Observers in observers reported the direction of the global motion seen in the com-
this and following experiments gave informed written consent before bined stimulus. The luminance contrast that produced a motion null
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

the experiments, which were approved by the Ethisches Kommitee (equal frequency of reports favoring luminance and color directions)
der neurologischen Klinik des Universittsspital Zrich, by the Comit was taken as the equivalent luminance contrast of the color grat-
consultatif de protection des personnes dans la recherche biomdi- ing21. We also tested red/green luminance ratios bracketing the equi-
cale du centre Lon Brard, Lyon and by the F.A.S. Human Subjects luminance setting and, in every instance, the minimum equivalent
Committee, Harvard University. luminance contrast occurred at the predetermined setting.
The uniform light surround was 20 cd per m 2 and the dark sur-
round was 0.3 cd per m 2. The test stimuli were presented for 0.5 s
with a gap of 2.5 s between stimuli. The target was a slightly blurred Acknowledgments
rectangle subtending about 2.2 by 1.6. Fixation was not monitored We thank Manfred Fahle, Anne Kurtenbach and Lukas Rttiger for the loan
during the experiment. The chromatic tests were provided by a mono- of the spectral-sensitivity measuring equipment and its calibration and Seth
chromator with a bandwidth of 10 nm. Hamlin for technical assistance. Supported by NEI grant EY09258 to PC,
After adapting to the light or dark background for several minutes, Swiss National Science Foundation Grant NR: 2100-045699.95 to TL, and
testing began at the shortest wavelength. Increment thresholds were UK Defense Research Agency grant D/ER1/9/4/2034/102/RARDE to TT.
determined using a method of limits, and the procedure was repeat-
ed three times, reversing the order of the wavelengths each time. The RECEIVED 9 MARCH: ACCEPTED 27 MAY 1998
mean of the three values was used to calculate the sensitivity for each
wavelength.
1. Damasio, A., Yamada, T., Damasio, H., Corbett, J. & McKee, J. Central
CONTRAST THRESHOLDS . The observers were the three central achro- achromatopsia: Behavioral, anatomic, and physiologic aspects. Neurology
mats, two congenitally red/green-deficient (one deutan and one 30, 10641071 (1980).
2. Grsser, O.-J. & Landis, T. in Vision and Visual Dysfunction, Vol 12: Visual
protan) and two normal subjects. The red/green-deficient subjects Agnosias. (ed. Cronly-Dillon, J.) 394400 (MacMillan, London, 1991).
each failed at least 18 of the 24 Ishihara plates, whereas neither normal 3. Pearlman, A.L., Birch, J. & Meadows, J.C. Cerebral color blindness: An
subject made any errors. acquired defect in hue discrimination. Ann. Neurol. 5, 253261 (1979).
4. Zeki, S.M. A century of cerebral achromatopsia. Brain 113, 17211777
The stimulus was a rotating wheel of eight cycles of a color grat- (1990).
ing, varying sinusoidally between red and green (Fig. 2). The wheel 5. Mollon, J.D., Newcombe, F., Polden, P.G. & Ratcliff, G. in Colour Vision
was set in rotation either smoothly (because of the 66.7 Hz raster Deficiencies (ed. Verriest, G.) 130135 (Hilger, Bristol, 1980).
rate, the actual step was 10.8 on each frame but it appeared to move 6. Wooten, B.R. Partial cerebral achromatopsia with selective hue loss. Doc.
Ophthalmol. Proc. Series 33, 139144 (1982).
smoothly) or in 90 jumps. The rate of rotation was fixed at 2 Hz in all 7. Jaeger, W., Krastel, H. & Braun, S. Cerebral achromatopsia (symptoms,
cases (0.25 revolutions per second). The outer diameter of the wheel course, differential diagnosis and examination strategy). II. [Article in
was eight degrees of visual angle, and its inner radius was three German] Klin. Monatsbl. Augenheilkd. 194, 3236 (1989).
degrees. Its mean luminance was approximately 45 cd per m2, and the 8. Heywood, C.A., Cowey, A. & Newcombe, F. Chromatic discrimination in
surround was dark. The control of luminance was linearized through a cortically colour blind observer. Eur. J. Neurosci. 3, 802812 (1991).
9. Heywood, C.A., Nicholas, J.J. & Cowey, A. Behavioural and
gamma correction look-up tables. electrophysiological chromatic and achromatic contrast sensitivity in an
The chromatic contrast of a grating was defined in terms of the achromatopsic patient. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 60, 638643
(1996).
percentage of the maximum chromatic modulation obtainable with 10. Victor, J.D., Maiese, K., Shapley, R., Sidtis, J. & Gazzaniga, M.S. Acquired
the phosphors involved. 100% color contrast of the monitor produces, central dyschromatopsia: Analysis of a case with preservation of color
at equiluminance, 14% cone contrast for the red-sensitive cones and discrimination. Clin. Vis. Sci. 4, 183196 (1989).
35% cone contrast for the green-sensitive cones. 11. Heywood, C.A., Cowey, A. & Newcombe, F. On the role of parvocellular
(P) and magnocellular (M) pathways in cerebral achromatopsia. Brain
The colors were set to equiluminance42 individually for each observ- 117, 245254 (1994).
er. Thresholds were measured at that value and often two adjacent 12. Henaff, M.A. & Michel, F. A case of central achromatopsia: Possible
values bracketing the equiluminance setting as well. In every instance implicit treatment of hue. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 34, 745 (1993).
13. Troscianko, T. et al. Human colour discrimination based on a non-
tested, the highest thresholds occurred at the predetermined equilu-
parvocellular pathway. Curr. Biol. 6, 200210 (1996).
minance setting. Observers were instructed to fixate the central bulls- 14. Barbur, J.L., Harlow, A.J. & Plant, G.T. Insights into the different exploits
eye, and the tests were presented on the display until a response was of colour in the visual cortex. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 258, 327334 (1994).
given. Observers reported whether a stimulus was absent or present in 15. Lee, B.B., Martin, P.R. & Valberg, A. The physiological basis of
the detection conditions or moving clockwise versus counterclock- heterochromatic flicker photometry demonstrated in the ganglion cells of the
macaque retina. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 404, 323347 (1988).
wise in the direction-discrimination condition. Detection thresholds 16. Schiller, P.H. & Colby, C.L. The responses of single cells in the lateral
were taken as the chromatic contrast for which the subject reported geniculate nucleus of the rhesus monkey to color and luminance contrast.
present on 50% of the trials. Discrimination thresholds were taken as Vision Res. 23, 16311641 (1983).

246 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

17. Wagner, G. & Boynton, R.M. A comparison of four methods of heterochromatic 30. Ramachandran, V.S. & Gregory, R. Does colour provide an input to
photometry. J. Opt. Soc. Am. 62, 15081515 (1972). human motion perception? Nature 275, 5556 (1978).
18. Scheidler, W., Landis, T., Rentschler, I., Regard, M. & Baumgartner, G. A pattern 31. Cavanagh, P., Tyler, C.W. & Favreau, O.E. Perceived velocity of moving
recognition approach to visual agnosia. Clin. Vis. Sci. 7, 175193 (1992). chromatic gratings. J. Opt. Soc. Am. 1, 893899 (1984).
19. Harwerth, R.S. & Sperling, H.G. Effects of intense visible radiation on the 32. Cropper, S.J. & Derrington, A.M. Rapid colour-specific detection of
increment-threshold spectral sensitivity of the rhesus monkey eye. Vision Res. motion in human vision. Nature 379, 7274 (1996).
15, 11931204 (1975). 33. Dobkins, K.R. & Albright, T.D. What happens if it changes color when it
20. King-Smith, P.E. & Carden, D. Luminance and opponent-color contributions to moves?: the nature of chromatic input to macaque visual area MT. J.
visual detection and adaptation and to temporal and spatial integration. J. Opt. Neurosci. 14, 48544870 (1994).
Soc. Am. 66, 709717 (1976). 34. Saito, H., Tanaka, K., Isono, H. Yasuda, M. & Mikami, A. Directionally
21. Cavanagh P. & Anstis S.M. The contribution of color to motion in normal and selective response of cells in the middle temporal area (MT) of the
color-deficient observers. Vision Res. 31, 21092148 (1991). macaque monkey to the movement of equiluminous opponent color
22. Dobkins, K.R. & Albright, T.D. What happens if it changes color when it moves?: stimuli. Exp. Brain Res. 75, 114 (1989).
psychophysical experiments on the nature of chromatic input to motion 35. Gegenfurtner, K.R. et al. Chromatic properties of neurons in macaque MT.
detectors. Vision Res. 33, 10191036 (1993). Vis. Neurosci. 11, 455466 (1994).
23. Chichilnisky, E.J., Heeger, D. & Wandell, B.A. Functional segregation of color 36. Ffytche, D.H., Skidmore, B.D. & Zeki, S. Motion-from-hue activates area
and motion perception examined in motion nulling. Vision Res. 33, 21132125 V5 of human visual cortex. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 260, 353358 (1995).
(1993). 37. Heywood, C.A., Gaffan, G. & Cowey, A. Cerebral achromatopsia in
24. Teller, D.Y. & Palmer, J. Infant color vision: motion nulls for red/green vs monkeys. Eur. J. Neurosci. 7, 10641073 (1995).
luminance-modulated stimuli in infants and adults. Vision Res. 36, 955974 38. Hadjikhani, N.K., Liu, A.K., Cavanagh, P., Dale, A.M. & Tootell, R.B.H.
(1996). Retinotopy and color sensitivity in human visual cortical area V8. Nature
25. Cavanagh, P. Attention-based motion perception. Science 257, 15631565 Neurosci., 1, 235241 (1998).
(1992). 39. Tootell, R.B., Dale, A.M., Sereno, M.I. & Malach, R. New images from
26. Weiskrantz, L. Blindsight revisited. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 6, 215220 (1996). human visual cortex. Trends Neurosci. 19, 481489 (1996).
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

27. Metha, A.B., Vingrys, A.J. & Badcock, D.R. Detection and discrimination of 40. Zeki, S.M. Uniformity and diversity of structure and function in rhesus
moving stimuli: the effects of color, luminance, and eccentricity. J. Opt. Soc. Am. monkey prestriate visual cortex. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 277, 273290 (1978).
11, 16971709 (1994). 41. Ferrera, V.P., Rudolph, K.K. & Maunsell, J.H. Responses of neurons in the
28. Stromeyer, C.F. III, Kronauer, R.E., Ryu, A., Chaparro, A. & Eskew, R.T. Jr parietal and temporal visual pathways during a motion task. J. Neurosci.
Contributions of human long-wave and middle-wave cones to motion 14, 61716186 (1994).
detection. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 485, 221243 (1995). 42. Cavanagh, P., Anstis, S.M. & MacLeod, D.I.A. Equiluminance: Spatial and
29. Chaparro, A., Stromeyer, C.F. III, Huang, E.P., Kronauer, R.E. & Eskew, R.T. Jr temporal factors and the contribution of blue-sensitive cones. J. Opt. Soc.
Colour is what the eye sees best. Nature 361, 348350 (1993). Am. 4, 14281438 (1987).

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 247


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

The effects of frontal eye field and


dorsomedial frontal cortex lesions
on visually guided eye movements
Peter H. Schiller and I-han Chou

Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, Massachusetts 02139, USA
Correspondence should be addressed to P.H.S (schiller@wccf.mit.edu)

In the frontal lobe of primates, two areas play a role in visually guided eye movements: the frontal
eye fields (FEF) and the medial eye fields (MEF) in dorsomedial frontal cortex. Previously, FEF lesions
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

have revealed only mild deficits in saccadic eye movements that recovered rapidly. Deficits in eye
movements after MEF ablation have not been shown. We report the effects of ablating these areas
singly or in combination, using tests in which animals were trained to make saccadic eye movements
to paired or multiple targets presented at various temporal asynchronies. FEF lesions produced large
and long-lasting deficits on both tasks. Sequences of eye movements made to successively presented
targets were also impaired. Much smaller deficits were observed after MEF lesions. Our findings indi-
cate a major, long-lasting loss in temporal ordering and processing speed for visually guided
saccadic eye movement generation after FEF lesions and a significant but smaller and shorter-lasting
loss after MEF lesions.

In primates with a small foveal region of densely packed pho- movement to the target was rewarded with a drop of apple juice.
toreceptors in their retinae that provide high acuity vision, each The second task was saccadic eye movements to paired targets.
eye is moved about by six extraocular muscles, innervated by Paired targets were presented with various temporal onset asyn-
neurons in the brainstem oculomotor centers. Rapid saccadic eye chronies. Monkeys were rewarded for making a saccadic eye
movements are executed two to four times per second to bring movement to either target. The onset asynchrony between paired
visual objects into central view, and slower, smooth-pursuit eye targets was varied to determine the temporal delay required to
movements are made to keep objects in view when either the produce equal probability of eye movements to each target. The
observer or the object is in motion. In the frontal lobe, two areas third task was saccadic eye movements to sequential targets. On
have been shown to be significant in visually guided eye move- each trial, two targets were presented in rapid sequence with var-
ments: the frontal eye fields (FEF) and the medial eye fields ious temporal durations and with various delays between them.
(MEF), both of which make direct projections to brainstem ocu- The task was to repeat the order of the target presentations by
lomotor centers111. The MEF, located in the dorsomedial frontal making successive saccadic eye movements to the target loca-
cortex, are also known as the supplementary eye fields8. Single- tions. The fourth task was saccadic eye movements to targets in
cell recording and microstimulation studies have shown that these arrays. Following fixation, eight stimuli were presented equidis-
two areas perform different coding operations for the generation tant from the fixation spot; one of the stimuli, the target,
of saccadic eye movements3,7,11. Electrical stimulation of the FEF appeared at various times prior to the other seven stimuli. The
elicits saccades that have specific directions and amplitudes; pro- monkey was rewarded only for eye movements directed to the
longed electrical stimulation yields a staircase of identical sac- target stimulus. In all four tasks tested, we observed prominent
cades with intervening fixations. By contrast, electrical deficits following FEF lesions. Considerably smaller deficits that
stimulation of the MEF produces saccades that take the eyes to recovered more rapidly were observed after MEF lesions. The
a particular orbital position; prolonged stimulation keeps the effects of combined FEF and MEF lesions were no greater than
eyes at that position. These findings indicate that the FEF carry a FEF lesions alone.
vector code and the MEF a place code10,11. To shed further light
on the functions of these two areas, we examined the effects of Results
ablating them, either singly or in combination, and studied the Figure 1 shows the performance of two monkeys on the single-
effects on visually guided eye movements. Most studies have target task. Each panel shows the distribution of rightward and
shown only mild, temporary deficits after FEF lesions1216. leftward saccadic movement latencies made to individual visual
Deficits in visually guided saccadic eye movements have not pre- targets presented randomly in one of four locations. Panels (a)
viously been demonstrated after MEF lesions in the monkey. and (b) compare the distributions before and after a left FEF
Here we present results obtained on four tasks. The first task lesion. The insets display eye-movement records that show some-
was saccadic eye movements to single targets. Following fixation what less accurate saccades to the right after the left FEF lesion; a
of a small central spot, a single target appeared in one of several significant change in saccadic peak velocities accompanies this
locations on the monitor. Execution of an accurate saccadic eye for right saccades from a preoperative 534 to a postoperative 459

248 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 1. Distribution of sac-


cadic latencies to single tar-
a a
Pre-op
Pre-op
b b
3 wks post LFEF c
3 wks post LFEF
gets before and after lesions.
Data are shown from two 100
140
monkeys with (a) and (d) rep- 80
resenting pre-operative data. 100
60
(b) shows the distribution of
left and right saccadic eye 60 40

Numberofofsaccades
saccades
movements three weeks after 20
left FEF lesion; (e) shows simi- 20
lar data two weeks after a 60 100 140 180 220 60 100 140 180 220
right MEF lesion in the second
animal. (f) shows data col- d Pre-opd e 2
e
wks post RMEF f 2 wks post MEF and LFEF
f
Number

Pre-op 2 wks post RMEF 2 wks post MEF and LFEF


lected after a left FEF lesion
120
had been made in the same 140 250
animal subsequent to bilateral 100
200
MEF lesions. Eye-movement 100 80 left saccades
records before and after the 150
60 right saccades
left FEF lesion appear in the
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

60 100 40
inset of (a) and (b). Mean left
and right saccadic latencies 50 20
20
were as follows: (a) L, 125; R,
121, (b) L, 112; R, 166, (d) L, 60 100 140 180 220 60 100 140 180 220 60 100 140 180 220
130; R, 130, (e) L, 141; R, 130, Latency in milliseconds
Latency milliseconds
(f) L, 121; R, 175. The 11-ms
difference in mean left and right saccadic latencies two weeks after the right MEF lesion (e) is significant at the .05 level. Differences after FEF
lesions (b and f) are significant beyond the .001 level. (c) Reconstruction of an MEF and FEF lesion from a third monkey. The major sulci are
labeled as are the locations of the FEF and MEF. Ant: anterior portion of the brain. Number of trials per histogram: (a) n = 1200, (b) n = 720,
(d) n = 1120, (e) n = 2160, (f) n = 1092.

degrees per second (p < .001, t-test). In (b) an increase of 45 ms monkeys execute equal numbers of saccadic eye movements to
is evident in the mean latency of saccadic eye movements made to the left and to the right during each session, thereby forestalling
the right as compared with pre-operative performance (p < .001, the emergence of position biases.
t-test); the lesion also produced a broadening in the latency dis- The inset in Fig. 2 shows preoperatively collected eye-move-
tribution for rightward saccades. Saccadic eye movements made ment records for paired targets that appeared either simultane-
to the left after the left FEF ablation yielded significantly shorter ously or with asynchronies favoring either the left or the right
latencies than pre-operatively by 13 ms (p < .05). Panels (d), (e), stimulus in the pair. With simultaneous presentation, the prob-
and (f) show pre- and post-operative latency distributions from ability of making saccadic eye movements to the left and right
another monkey. The right MEF lesion produced a small, but targets was equal, whereas when one target preceded the other
significant increase in latencies for leftward saccadic eye move- by 33 ms, most saccades were made to the stimulus that had
ments (11 ms, p < .05, t-test). Saccadic accuracy and velocity were appeared first. The percent of saccades made to the left target at
unaffected by the MEF lesions. The left FEF lesion made in this different asynchronies are plotted in the center of the graph
animal subsequent to the bilateral MEF lesion (Fig. 1f) produced labeled pre-op. Also plotted is the percent of saccadic eye move-
deficits similar to those obtained with a single left FEF lesion, ments made into the left visual field at various times after a left
yielding a difference of 54 ms in the mean latencies between left FEF and a right MEF lesions in two different animals. Two weeks
and right saccades. Four months after the left FEF lesion in mon- after the left FEF lesion, the curve shifts to the far left. To achieve
key 1, the latency difference between saccades made to the left an equal probability of left and right saccades (50% crossover
and right dropped to 25 ms; four months after the paired FEF point), the target in the affected right hemifield had to be pre-
and MEF lesions in monkey 2, the difference dropped to 24 ms sented 116 ms prior to the target in the left hemifield. In the
(both with p < .001, t-test). Saccadic velocity differences for left- weeks following the lesion, the size of the asynchrony required
ward and rightward saccadic eye movements were also dimin- for equivalent target choice decreased gradually, as shown by the
ished but remained significant. successive curves on the left side of the graph in Fig. 2.
The paired-target task we devised permitted us to quantify the The shift in target choice after a right MEF lesion, as shown
degree to which target selection was biased by the cortical lesions. by the curves on the right in Fig. 2, was much smaller in magni-
We presented paired targets with different onset asynchronies to tude. Two weeks after the lesion, an asynchrony of 31 ms was
determine the interval required to produce equal probability of required for the monkey to make saccades with equal probabili-
target selection appearing in the right and left hemifields. Results ty to either target. Recovery after the MEF lesion was complete
obtained on this task using targets with an angular separation of by the 16th week. At a comparable time after the left FEF lesion,
90 degrees relative to the fixation spot appear in Fig. 2. The paired there was still a strong bias in favor of the ipsilateral target, requir-
targets appeared either above or below the fixation point, with ing a 54 ms target-onset asynchrony for equal probability per-
one stimulus falling in the left and the other in the right visual formance. At comparable post-operative times, the asynchronies
hemifield. Paired targets were interspersed with single targets that required to produce equal probability choice were more than
appeared twice as often; conditions were configured to have the twice as long as the latency differences between left and right-

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 249


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 2. Paired

targets
targets pre- Pre-op eye movements made to paired targets
100 LFEF lesion pre-op
pre-op Pre-op eye movements made to paired targets

targets
sented with var- RMEF lesion L,
L, 33ms
33 ms 00ms
ms R, 33
R, 33ms
ms
ied asynchronies. 80

leftleft
wk 3
wk3
wk
wk22
The percent of wk16
wk 16
to to
60
saccades made saccades
to the left target 40 wk16
wk 16 wk 2
saccades
wk2
as a function of
20
temporal offset
Percent

between the 0
Percent

Right target first Left target first


paired targets is
plotted. Data are 220 200 180 160 140 120 100 80 60 40 20 0 20 40 60 80 100 120 140 160 180 220 220
shown at various Onset
Onset asynchrony in milliseconds
milliseconds
times after
recovery from FEF and MEF lesions as well as pre-operatively (pre-op). Squares and solid lines depict data from the animal with the FEF
lesion; circles and dotted lines depict data from the animal with the MEF lesion. Records of saccadic eye movements made to paired targets
with various temporal offsets in the intact animal appear as an inset. For each of the post-operative weeks plotted (wk 2wk 16), data were
collected for 5 or 6 successive days.
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

ward saccades made to single targets (see Fig. 1). There was no targets at which vector-averaged saccades occur most com-
further improvement on the paired target task when the mon- monly; as a result of the lesion there are now no vector-aver-
key was tested five months after FEF lesions. aged saccades when the targets are simultaneous; instead, they
When two targets have an angular separation of 90 degrees, become most frequent when the target in the hemisphere con-
most of the time monkeys generate accurate saccadic eye move- tralateral to the lesion is presented 67100 ms prior to the tar-
ments to one target or the other, as shown in the inset in Fig. 2. get in the left hemisphere. These findings suggest that FEF
However, when the target separation is decreased, animals lesions retard the rate at which information can be processed
begin to make averaging saccades that land the center of gaze at for the generation of visually guided eye movements.
intermediate positions between the two targets17. Such vector- The dorsomedial frontal area within which the MEF resides
averaged saccades are presumably the product of simultane- has been implicated in the temporal processing of events, par-
ously arriving commands from the cortex to the superior ticularly in the execution of sequential motor acts2022. We there-
colliculus or the brainstem to move the eyes to each target. fore wanted to determine how MEF and FEF lesions alter the
Studies have shown that concurrent electrical stimulation of production of a sequence of eye movements to successively pre-
two sites in the superior colliculus of the FEF produces sac- sented targets. In Fig. 4, data from the sequential task are pre-
cadic eye movements that are a vector average of saccades elicit- sented. Four different sequences with several different durations
ed by stimulating each site alone 18,19 . Figure 3 shows data were presented in randomized order. Pre-operative and post-
collected with targets that had an angular separation of 40 operative data collected at various times after the lesions are
degrees; under such conditions vector- averaged saccades shown. The results demonstrate a significant but mild deficit
become common. In the unlesioned monkey, vector-averaged after MEF lesions, which is consistent with findings in humans20.
saccades occur most frequently when the targets appear simul- However, a much larger deficit was evident after the FEF lesion.
taneously; the frequency of such saccades falls off rapidly as The inset shows eye-movement records collected two months
target-onset asynchrony is increased. The consequence of an after a left FEF lesion using four sets of sequentially presented
FEF lesion is a dramatic shift in the asynchrony between the targets with a sequence duration of 117 ms. Performance on

a bb
averaged

50 Intact monkey LFEF lesion


vectoraveraged

40
Percent

30
Percent

Intact monkey
saccadesvector

20
L, 67 ms 0 ms R, 67 ms R, 100 ms R, 133 ms 10
saccades

0
LFEF lesion
60 40 20 0 20 40 60 80 100 120 140
Left target first Right target first
Onset
Onsetasynchrony
asynchronybetween
between paired targets
targets

Fig. 3. Data collected with paired targets having a 40% angular separation. (a) Eye-movement records obtained at various target asyn-
chronies; text shows which target appeared first (L, left; R, right) and by how many milliseconds (ms). (b) Plot of the frequency of vector-
averaged saccades as a function of target asynchrony. The data were obtained from an intact animal and from an animal with a left FEF lesion.
We counted as vector averaged those saccades that fell within plus or minus nine angular degrees of the midpoint between the two targets.

250 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 4. Pre and post-operative data


for percent correct performance are
a b Post RMEF
RMEF lesion
lesion
Preop Post Eye movements, post LFEF
shown for left and right saccadic eye wk
wk55 Eye movements, post LFEF
100 100
movements made to sequential tar- L
gets before and after a right MEF 80 80
lesion (a, b) and before and after a R
60 60
left FEF lesion (c, d). Two stimuli wk 1
wk1
appeared in succession using four dif- 40 40
ferent sets of locations and several
L
different overall durations. The inset 20 20

shows eye movements made to one


Percent correct
Percent Correct
set of target sequences after a left FEF 125 150 175 200 225 250 125 150 175 200 225 250 275
lesion, when presented for a
sequence duration of 117 ms, which cc Preop d Post LFEF lesion
is completed well before the initiation 100
L 100
L
of the first eye movements. The four
sets of sequential targets, as indicated 80 80
wk11
wk 11
R
in the inset, appeared at positions 60 60 wk
wk33
wk 6
wk6
A2B1, E2D1, A4B5 and E4D5.
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

40 40
Each trial began with a central fixation
R
spot followed by the appearance of 20 20
one sequence. The eye movements wk
wk22
0 0
shown in the inset demonstrate cor-
rect sequences made to the intact left 125 150 175 200 225 250 125 150 175 200 225 250 275 300 325 350 375 400 425 450 475 500
side and mostly incorrect eye move- Sequenceduration
Sequence Duration in
in milliseconds
milliseconds
ments made for sequences presented
to the right.

sequences presented to the right was severely impaired. The most suggest that for the generation of sequences of eye movements,
common error was the execution of a single saccade, instead of the FEF are more important than the MEF.
a sequence of saccades, that landed the center of gaze near either The paired-target and sequential tasks do not directly test the
of the target locations or between them. ability of animals to make a temporal discrimination. To assess
Examination of saccadic latencies made to the first target in deficits in ascribing temporal order to successively appearing
the sequence suggests that target reaction times make only a small events, we devised a task that required the animals to discrimi-
contribution to the deficit. For example, three weeks after the left nate stimuli on the basis of the order in which they appeared.
FEF lesion, the mean latencies to the first target in the sequences Eight identical stimuli were presented equidistant from fixation,
we used were 130 ms to the left and 198 ms to the right (a laten- one of which preceded the others by various times. Only a direct
cy difference of 68 ms). Yet the comparable performance to the saccadic eye movement made to the stimulus that appeared first
left and right occurs with a sequence duration difference of more was rewarded. This task explicitly requires that the animal dis-
than 200 ms (Fig. 4). criminate the relative onset of the stimuli. Data collected four
Full recovery on the sequential task is evident five weeks months after a left FEF and paired MEF and left FEF lesions
after MEF lesions. Following FEF lesions, considerable recov- appear in Fig. 5a and b. A major impairment is evident for eye
ery occurs by week 11. Combined MEF and FEF lesions pro- movements made into the contralateral visual field. This deficit is
duced deficits and recovery times similar to those obtained not due to loss of perception of high-frequency temporal infor-
from the FEF lesion alone. The results from the sequential task mation per se, as the monkeys were unimpaired on flicker sen-
sitivity. This suggests that the
deficit lies not in perceiving tem-
a a lesion
LFEF lesion b b
MEFand
MEF & LFEF
LFEFlesions
lesions poral discontinuity but in a dis-
ability to assess the temporal order
100 100 of presentations for the generation
of saccadic eye movements.
correct

80 80
Percent correct

left hemifield In addition to the four tests


60 60 described here, the performance of
right hemifield
Percent

40 40 monkeys was also assessed for eye


movements made to targets that
20 20
moved at various velocities, on
0 N = 3220 0 N = 2720 sensitivity to stimuli of varied con-
trasts, on their ability to discrimi-
50 100 150 200 250 300 50 100 150 200 250 300
Target onset asynchrony in milliseconds
nate targets that were of a different
Target onset asynchrony in milliseconds
size, shape or color from other,
Fig. 5. Performance on the discrimination of time differences. Percent correct performance is shown simultaneously appearing stimuli,
separately for targets appearing in the left and right hemispheres. (a) Data from a monkey with a left and their ability to discriminate
FEF lesion. (b) Data from a monkey after paired MEF and FEF lesions. (a) n = 3220, (b) n = 2720. flickering stimuli from steadily illu-

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 251


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

cus 6 mm medial and 6 mm lateral from the posterior tip of the princi-
minated ones. Except for an increase in saccadic latencies, per-
pal sulcus as well as the gray matter between the arcuate and the posterior
formance on these tasks was largely unaffected by the lesions. tip of the principalis. The fundus and the posterior bank of the arcuate
involved in smooth pursuit eye movements were spared3133. Anteriorly
Discussion these lesions encroached upon area 46. In the second animal, the left
In foveate animals, with each shift in gaze not only are stimuli in dorsomedial cortex area was destroyed after we recorded and stimulated
central vision analyzed but a decision has to be made as to where the area extensively to establish the exact location of the MEF; this par-
to look next. The findings we report here suggest that the FEF ticular lesion was produced by repeated, closely spaced lidocaine injec-
are central to this process of target selection. Particularly, the FEF tions until neural activity was permanently shut down, resulting in a
are involved in the timing of saccadic eye-movement generation lesion verified histologically and shown in Fig.1c. Subsequently in this
and in optimizing the speed with which eye movements can be animal, the right FEF was removed by aspiration and was also verified
histologically. In the third animal, the same region of dorsomedial frontal
initiated to objects in the visual scene. In intact animals, there is
cortex was removed as in the second animal. The lesions extended 8 mm
cooperative interaction between the two FEF23. Following a uni- laterally from the midline and 6 mm anterior and posterior from a point
lateral FEF lesion, target selection and processing speed are biased that was in the same coronal plane as the posterior tip of the principal
in favor of the intact hemisphere. Even five months after the sulcus. Detailed photographs were taken during surgery before and after
lesion, sizeable deficits remained on our tasks that tapped this each aspiration. Electrophysiological mapping was carried out only in
competition. In contrast, the MEF lesions yielded much smaller the second animal. All protocols were approved by the MIT Animal Care
deficits suggesting that this area is not as centrally involved in Committee and followed NIH guidelines.
target selection and in the execution of sequences of eye move- The animals were tested using a color monitor placed at a distance of
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

ments. This is consistent with single-cell recording studies show- 57 cm. The head was restrained during testing, and eye movements were
ing that neuronal activity in the FEF is more predictive of saccade measured using the scleral search coil method. Monkeys readily per-
generation and timing than is the MEF (ref. 24, 25, Patterson, formed 8003000 trials per day. Background luminance for the single,
W.F. II & Schall, J.D. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr., 185.8, 1997). paired and multiple target tasks was 2.26 cd per m2. The targets were
Our paired-stimulus task is similar to a task used to study small, 0.34 degree of visual angle squares with a luminance of 90.27 cd per
m2. Saccadic latencies were computed from the time of target onset to
patients with brain lesions in which paired visual or tactile stimuli
the time of departure of the eye-movement trace from an electronic win-
are presented in the right and left hemifields or parts of the body. dow set around the fixation spot. The targets most commonly appeared
The tendency to ignore the stimulus that appears in space con- at an eccentricity of 12 degrees from fixation.
tralateral to the lesion has been termed the extinction phenome-
The sequential task was made distinct from the other tasks in four
non.26 Adding precisely defined temporal asynchronies to the ways. First, twenty-five equally spaced small outline squares of low con-
presentation of paired targets, as we did in this study, makes it pos- trast were present on the screen throughout. Second, the targets appeared
sible to obtain exact measurements of the temporal factors involved. inside the selected squares and were bright red in color. Third, the tar-
As a result, it is possible to ascribe an exact value in time for the gets flashed on briefly in rapid succession, with a gap between them.
magnitude of the deficit incurred and its recovery. Our results raise Fourth, the animal was rewarded only when two correct successive sac-
the possibility that the extinction phenomenon seen after frontal cadic eye movements were made to the successively appearing target
and parietal lesions is due not to a loss of attention per se, but to a positions. The target durations and the delays introduced between the
loss in the speed of processing in the impaired hemisphere. first and second target were arrived at experimentally and were chosen
What brain structures might be responsible for bringing about to optimize the ability of monkeys to perform the sequential task suc-
cessfully. Several different sets or paired target locations were studied,
the gradual improvement in performance found on the tasks we
which were run in blocks always using four pairs arranged in a mirror-
had used? It has been proposed that two major cortical streams image fashion to allow us to compare performance in the left and right
contribute to the control of visually guided saccadic eye move- hemispheres. The first target duration ranged between 33 and 300 ms,
ments, the anterior and the posterior 27. Because even after the interval between 50 and 117 ms and the second target between 33
removal of both major areas of the anterior stream, the FEF and and 83 ms using 16.7 ms steps (the frame rate for the monitor). Within
the MEF, there is notable improvement in performance over time, each block the sequence durations as well as the mirror-imaged pair loca-
it is probable that the posterior stream is involved in the recovery. tions appeared in randomized order. (See inset in Fig. 4 showing eye
This stream originates in the occipital and parietal lobes and movements made to one set of four pairs used in a block.)
reaches the brainstem oculomotor areas predominantly through For the temporal-discrimination task, eight stimuli appeared, one of
the superior colliculus2830. which preceded the others by various times ranging from 16 ms to 300
ms. The eight stimuli were identical and the same size and luminance as
the targets presented in the single- and paired-target case. All the stimuli
Methods
were equally spaced around a circle with a radius of 12 degrees centered
Four monkeys were trained on a variety of visually guided eye-movement on the fixation spot.
tasks that allowed us to assess the relative effects of frontal eye field, dor-
somedial frontal cortex and combined lesions on the execution of sac- Acknowledgements
cadic eye movements to single and paired target stimuli. Following We thank J. Colby and W. Slocum for their technical assistance.
training, in the first animal, a unilateral lesion was made of the left FEF. In
the second animal, successive lesions of the left dorsomedial frontal cor- RECEIVED 3 MARCH: ACCEPTED 18 MAY 1998
tex and of the right FEF were made several months apart. In the third ani-
mal, three lesions were made, also several months apart; initially the left
dorsomedial frontal cortical area containing the left MEF was ablated, 1. Bizzi, E. Discharge of frontal eye field neurons during eye movements in
then the right MEF area, and finally the left FEF. The monkeys were test- unanesthetized monkeys. Science 157, 15881590 (1967).
2. Bruce, C.J. & Golberg, M.E. Primate frontal eye fields: I. Single neurons
ed extensively after each lesion for several months before the second and discharging before saccades. J. Neurophysiol. 53, 603635 (1985).
third lesions were made. The fourth monkey served as a control animal. 3. Chen, L.L. & Wise, S.P. Supplementary eye field contrasted with frontal eye
field during acquisition of conditional oculomotor associations. J.
All but one of the lesions were made by aspiration under aseptic con- Neurophysiol. 73, 11221134 (1995).
ditions in anesthetized animals with the aid of a surgical microscope. 4. Mann, S.E., Thau, R. and Schiller, P.H. Conditional task-related responses in
The FEF was removed by aspirating the anterior bank of the arcuate sul- monkey dorsomedial frontal cortex. Exp. Brain Res. 69, 460468 (1988).

252 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

5. Robinson, D.A. & Fuchs, A.F. Eye movements evoked by stimulation of 20. Gaymard, B., Pierrot-Deseilligny, C. & Rivaud, S. Impairment of sequences
frontal eye fields. J. Neurophysiol. 32, 637648 (1969). of memory-guided saccades after supplementary motor area lesions. Annals
6. Russo, G.S. & Bruce, C.J. Neurons in the supplementary eye field of the Neurol. 28, 622626 (1990).
rhesus monkeys code visual targets and saccadic eye movements in an 21. Mushiake, H., Inase, M. & Tanji, J. Neuronal activity in the primate premotor,
oculocentric coordinate system. J. Neurophysiol. 76, 825848 (1996). supplementary and precentral motor cortex during visually guided and
7. Schall, J.D. Neuronal activity related to visually-guided saccades in the frontal internally determined sequential movements. J. Neurophysiol. 66, 705718
eye fields of rhesus monkeys: comparison with supplementary eye fields. J. (1991).
Neurophysiol. 66, 530579 (1991). 22. Tanji, J. & Shima K. Role for supplementary motor area cells in planning
8. Schlag, J. & Schlag-Rey, M. Evidence for a supplementary eye field. J. several movements ahead. Nature 371, 413416 (1994).
Neurophysiol. 57, 179200 (1987). 23. Schlag, J., Dassonville, P. & Schlag-Rey, M. Interaction of the two frontal eye
9. Shook, B.L., Schlag-Rey, M. & Schlag J. Primate supplementary eye field: I. fields before saccade onset. J. Neurophysiol. 79, 6472 (1998).
Comparative aspects of mesencephalic and pontine connections. J. Comp. 24. Hanes, D.P. & Schall, J.D. Neural control of voluntary movement initiation.
Neurol. 301, 618642 (1990). Science 274, 427430 (1996).
10. Tehovnik, E.J. The dorsomedial frontal cortex: eye and forelimb fields. Behav. 25. Hanes, D.P., Patterson, W. F. & Schall, J.D. The role of frontal eye fields in
Brain Res. 67, 147163 (1995). countermanding saccades: visual, movement, and fixation activity. J.
11. Tehovnik, E.J. & Lee, K.M. The dorsomedial frontal cortex of the rhesus Neurophysiol. 79, 817834 (1998).
monkey. Topographic representation of saccades evokes by electrical 26. Bisiach, E. & Vallar, G. in Handbook of Neuropsychology Vol. 1 (eds Boller, E. &
stimulation. Exp. Brain Res. 96, 430442 (1993). Grafman, J.) 195222 (Elsevier, Amsterdam, 1988).
12. Deng, S-Y., Goldberg, M.E., Segraves, M.A., Ungerleider, L.G. & Mishkin, M. 27. Schiller, P.H. in Cognitive Neuroscience of Attention. (ed Richards, J.) 350
in Adaptive Processes in the Visual & Oculomotor Systems (eds Keller, E. & Zee, (Lawrence Erlbaum, London, 1998).
D. S.) 201208 (Pergamon, Oxford, 1986). 28. Keating, E.G., Gooley, S.G., Pratt, S. & Kelsey, J. Removing the superior
13 Dias, E.C., Kiesau, M. & Segraves, M.A. Acute activation and inactivation of colliculus silences eye movements normally evoked from stimulation of the
macaque frontal eye field with GABA-related drugs. J. Neurophysiol. 74, parietal and occipital eye fields. Brain Res. 269, 145148 (1983).
27442748 (1995). 29. Schiller, P.H. The effect of superior colliculus ablation on saccades elicited by
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

14. Latto, R.A. & Cowey, A. Visual field defects after frontal eye field lesions in cortical stimulation. Brain Res. 122, 154156 (1977).
monkeys. Brain Res. 30, 124 (1971). 30. Tehovnik, E.J., Lee, K-M. & Schiller, P.H. Stimulation-evoked saccades
15. Schiller, P.H., Sandell, J.H. & Maunsell, J.H.R. The effect of frontal eye field from the dorsomedial frontal cortex of the rhesus monkey following
and superior colliculus lesions on saccadic latencies in the rhesus monkey. J. lesions of the frontal eye fields and superior colliculus. Exp. Brain Res. 98,
Neurophysiol. 57, 10331049 (1987). 179190 (1994).
16. Sommer, M.A. & Tehovnik, E.J. Reversible inactivation of macaque frontal 31. Gottlieb, J.P., Bruce, C.J. & MacAvoy, M.G. Smooth eye movements elicited
eye field. Exp. Brain Res. 116, 229249 (1997). by microstimulation in the primate frontal eye field. J. Neurophysiol. 69,
17. Ottes, F.P., Van Gisbergen, J.A.M. & Eggermont, J.J. Metrics of saccade responses 786799 (1993).
to visual double stimuli, two different modes. Vision Res. 24, 11691197 (1984). 32. Keating, E.G. Lesions of the frontal eye field impair pursuit eye movements
18. Robinson, D.A. Eye movements evoked by collicular stimulation in the alert but preserve the predictions driving them. Behav. Brain Res. 53, 91104
monkey. Vision Res. 12, 17951808 (1972). (1993).
19. Schiller, P.H., True, S.D. & Conway, J.L. Paired stimulation of the frontal eye 33. Lynch, J.C. Saccade initiation and latency deficits after combined lesions of
fields and the superior colliculus of the rhesus monkey. Brain Res. 179, frontal and posterior eye fields in monkeys. J. Neurophysiol. 68, 19131916
162164 (1979). (1992).

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 253


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Top-down influences on stereoscopic


depth-perception
Isabelle Blthoff1, Heinrich Blthoff1 and Pawan Sinha2

1 Max-Planck-Institut fr biologische Kybernetik, 72076 Tbingen, Germany


2 Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin, Madison, Wisconsin 53706, USA
Correspondence should be addressed to I.B. (isabelle.buelthoff@tuebingen.mpg.de)

The interaction between depth perception and object recognition has important implications for the
nature of mental object representations and models of hierarchical organization of visual
processing. It is often believed that the computation of depth influences subsequent high-level
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

object recognition processes, and that depth processing is an early vision task that is largely immune
to top-down object-specific influences, such as object recognition. Here we present experimental
evidence that challenges both these assumptions in the specific context of stereoscopic depth-
perception. We have found that observers recognition of familiar dynamic three-dimensional (3D)
objects is unaffected even when the objects depth structure is scrambled, as long as their two-
dimensional (2D) projections are unchanged. Furthermore, the observers seem perceptually
unaware of the depth anomalies introduced by scrambling. We attribute the latter result to a top-
down recognition-based influence whereby expectations about a familiar objects 3D structure over-
ride the true stereoscopic information.

Our visual system can often recognize objects not only on the basis Second, they show that top-down recognition-based influences
of their static appearance but also by observing how they move. can strongly alter depth perception, such that expectations about
In some cases, impoverished image sequences can be recognized a familiar objects 3D structure override the true stereoscopic
from their pattern of motion despite the fact that no single frame information. Consistent with this hypothesis, we have found that
has enough figural information to support recognition (Fig. 1). reducing the recognizability of objects reduces the magnitude of
Image sequences such as those devised by Johansson1, which con- the top-down influence on depth perception.
vey vivid impressions of humans engaged in various dynamic activ-
ities, are elegant and powerful demonstrations of this fact. Results
Since the first reports of this work, the issue of how motion As stimuli, we used variants of previously described biologi-
sequences are interpreted by the primate brain has been exten- cal motion sequences1 (see Fig. 1). They were 3D stereo ani-
sively studied. Previous work2 has shown that observers can mations showing twelve points on a male human (three points
identify human individuals from their gait alone. Coding the- positioned at the joints of each limb) as he walked on a tread-
ory principles have also been applied to model gait percep- mill at a normal pace. We compared the normal version of this
tion3. A model for the interpretation of these sequences has stimulus with a depth-scrambled version in which the depth
been developed4 by adapting structure-from-motion ideas5. positions of the joints were randomly altered in the z-axis. This
Similar approaches have also been developed by other rendered their 3D trajectories arbitrary within the volume
researchers 6,7. Neurons that respond selectively to biological defined by the original structure, while leaving their 2D pro-
motion sequences have been described in the visually respon- jections on the retina unchanged (discounting the minor posi-
sive areas of the primate temporal cortex 8 , a region that is tional shifts induced by the need to incorporate depth-disparity
known to be involved in object recognition. information). The degree of arbitrary depth scrambling of the
Most of the studies that have attempted to explain human scrambled walker was a continuously variable parameter. We
perception of biological motion sequences have done so largely in defined the extent of added depth noise as a function of the
terms of bottom-up mechanisms, in which the object geometry depth-extent (say, D) of the original undistorted walker. Thus,
is extracted from low-level features without recourse to higher- a noise level of 0 % corresponded to an undistorted sequence,
level internal representations of objects. In this report, however, whereas a noise level of 50 % implied a sequence wherein the
we examine the possibility that internal object representations individual body points could assume any depth value (with
may also play a top-down role in this process. Our stimuli were uniform probability) within a bound of D/2 about their orig-
stereo views of walking human figures that were defined by a inal depth position. Additionally, we generated random ver-
small number of dots; we have used depth-distorted versions of sions of both the normal and the depth-scrambled walkers by
these figures to study the interactions between depth cues and scrambling the x and y positions of their constituent points.
recognition. Our results provide two pieces of evidence in this Unlike the purely depth-scrambled version, this created dis-
regard. First, they suggest that the anomalous stereo-depth cues tortions of the 2D retinal projection. In all sequences, the
do not significantly influence the recognizability of the stimuli. added offsets were kept constant from frame to frame.

254 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

a would be expected to increase in going from a viewing direction


perpendicular to the distorted depth axis (90) to one parallel
to it (0), as the monocular view comes to resemble more close-
ly a 2D human figure.
The key result (Fig. 3) is that irrespective of the depth struc-
ture of the sequences, viewpoints preserving the normal 2D pro-
jections yielded biological motion percepts (high ratings). Other
viewpoints for the scrambled sequence yielded percepts of ran-
domly moving dots (low ratings). The data strongly indicate that
the recognition process used by the subjects in this task is heav-
ily biased towards 2D traces. Stereo-depth information does not
seem to contribute significantly to the recognition processes.
The most surprising result of this experiment is that depth-
Fig. 1. Example of stimulus. (a) Stereogram scrambled motion sequences that had normal 2D traces were
of a single frame of a motion sequence of a
b
rated as highly as unscrambled sequences. There are at least two
human walker represented by dots only. The possible explanations for this. Either subjects might be percep-
structure of a human walker is only recog- tually aware of the depth scrambling but decide nevertheless to
nizable if the dots begin to move accordingly base their ratings on the similarity of the 2D projection, or they
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

to their biological motion pattern for as


might be perceptually unaware of the depth scrambling, possi-
short as 100 to 200 ms1. (b) We illustrate
bly due to a top-down object-specific influence that actively
the structure of the human figure by adding
imposes the expected structure on the input and thus suppress-
connecting lines to the dots.
es the perception of 3D anomalies.
To distinguish between these possibilities, we designed a sec-
ond experiment (depth-plane experiment) to test for the exis-
tence of any recognition-dependent influences that might serve to
In our first experiment (recognizability experiment), we suppress information about depth-anomalies being provided by
asked whether randomizing the depth structure of the moving low-level stereo processes. To assess observers ability to perceive
figure while preserving its 2D traces would adversely affect its the true depth structure of these sequences, we designed a simple
recognizability as a human. Subjects viewed in stereo the dis- task that required them to report whether three indicated points
torted walker sequences interspersed with random and human in the structure were in the same fronto-parallel depth plane.
(unscrambled) sequences. The viewing position was varied Stereo viewing was used throughout the experiment. Each exper-
between 0 (the figure is seen walking in place facing right; depth imental trial commenced with a presentation of either a depth-
distortion does not affect the positions of the points in the image scrambled walker or a random pattern for the duration of one
plane) and 90 (the walker is walking toward the observer; the walk-cycle, this being sufficient time to allow the moving figure
same distortion now results in displaced points in the new image to be recognized. As the presentation continued into the next
plane, see Fig. 2). The subjects rated all sequences for their struc- cycle, three of the points were highlighted by thin red outlines.
tural goodness as a human on a scale from 1 (completely ran- After two-thirds of the duration of a walk-cycle, the screen then
dom) to 5 (completely human). We expected that if stereo-depth turned blank. Subjects had been instructed to report whether the
information were critical for the recognition processes, subjects three red dots lay in the same fronto-parallel depth plane. In 50 %
would perceive our depth-scrambled sequences as random of the sequences, the three dots were in the same plane and in
objects from all viewpoints because the 3D structure of the 50% they were not. In the depth-distorted figures, we could vary
depth-distorted walker would be completely different from a the depth of the highlighted dots independent of their positions
human figure. The ratings assigned to these sequences would, on the limbs (same versus different limb); this allowed us to ask
therefore, be uniformly low for all viewing positions. If, howev- whether the perceived depth was influenced by the expectation
er, recognition required merely 2D congruence, then the rating that points on the same limb would be at the same depth.

a (a)
Y
b Y

(b)

X X
Z Z
90 90
0 0

Fig. 2. In the recognizability experiment, subjects viewed depth-scrambled and normal sequences in stereo from different viewing positions
indicated by the arrows along the equator at waist level in (a) and (b). (a) Undistorted walker. (b) Depth-scrambling a biological motion
sequence involves adding depth noise to the positions of the joints while leaving their 2D positions (in the xy-plane) largely unchanged. From
other viewing positions (e.g., in the yz projection shown on the right), the original 2D pattern of a human figure is severely distorted. In all fig-
ures, connecting lines serve to enhance recognizability of the human figure and are not shown in the experiments.

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 255


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 3. Results of the recognizability experiment

a human
In Fig. 4a and b, the 5
averaged across 22 subjects. The following stimulus

a human
false-alarm rate (a response
sequences were presented: human walker (no dis-
of in same plane when the
4 z-distortion tortion), depth-distorted walker (a constant z-dis-

as as
z-dist
dots are in fact not in the xz-distortion
xz-dist
tortion was applied from frame to frame, the amount

goodness
no-dist
no distortion
same plane) is shown as a

Structuralgoodness
n=22 of depth noise added was 100%) and random pattern
function of depth-noise 3
(constant xz-distortion). The subjects rated them on
level and depth-disparity a scale from 1 (very random) to 5 (very human).
respectively. The curves 2 Abscissa: viewing position in degrees, at 0 the
Structural
show data for three stimu- walker is seen walking to the right with its depth axis
lus types: (1) human 1 parallel to the viewing axis; at 90 the walker is seen
sequences with dots on the 0 9 18 27 36 45 54 63 72 81 90 walking towards the observer with its depth axis
same limb, (2) human Viewingposition
Viewing position(deg)
(deg) perpendicular to the viewing axis. P z-distortion, R
sequences with dots on dif- xz-distortion, H no distortion. n = 22.
ferent limbs, and (3) ran-
dom sequences. If the
perceived depth is affected
by prior expectations based on object recognition, the dots on experiment suggests that the recognition process is based large-
the same limb would be most likely to be perceived as copla- ly on matching the stimulus to an internal representation of the
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

nar, and those on different limbs would be least likely. The false objects 2D trace-structure rather than its 3D geometry. Consis-
alarm rate is highest for the same limb condition. In Fig. 4c, tent with this idea, the monkey infero-temporal cortex, which is
the hit-rate (correct responses of in same plane) is shown as known to be involved in object recognition, has recently been
a function of depth noise. It is uniformly high for the same reported to contain view-tuned neurons, which respond to 3D
limb condition (average value, 94%, SE 1%), lower for the ran- objects only when they are seen from a certain viewpoint9. An
dom sequences (average value, 72%, SE 1%) and lowest for the alternative possibility, which we must consider, is that recogni-
different limbs condition. Thus, the perceived depth is influ- tion might involve structure-from- motion processes. This idea
enced by prior expectations about the depth structure of the is based on the fact that a vivid perception of 3D structure can
image, which are in turn determined by its recognizability. arise from the 2D projection of a rotating object, in the absence
of stereoscopic depth cues. Structure-from-motion perception
Discussion can occur independent of object recognition, because even unfa-
Our results have important implication for the nature of the men- miliar rotating objects give rise to a 3D percept. Our subjects
tal representations for dynamic objects. The limited influence of might therefore have derived a 3D structure from the moving
depth information on object recognition observed in the first 2D projection of the walking figure and matched this to an inter-
nal 3D representation. We
b believe, however, that this is
a b unlikely, because it has been10
100% 100% demonstrated that recogni-
tion based on 2D cues pro-
rate
rate

80% 80%
ceeds unhindered even when
alarmrate

alarmrate

60% 60% the 3D structure suggested by


False alarm

Falsealarm

n =n=11
11
structure-from-motion
40% 40%
processes is inconsistent with
False

False

nn=11
= 11
20% 20%
the object identity.
We interpret the results of
0% 0% the depth-plane experiment
0% 50% 100% 150% 200% 5 10 15 20 25 30 as pointing to the existence of
Noise level
Noise level Disparity (pixels)
Disparity (pixels)
a top-down influence capable
c of modulating the informa-
Fig. 4. Results of the depth-plane experiment aver- c tion provided by the early
aged across 11 subjects. The false alarm rate (a depth-perception processes
100%
response of in same plane when the dots are physi- based on binocular disparities.
cally not in the same plane) is plotted against the 80% There are other related exam-
maximum random depth-distortion allowed in the
ples, e.g. the hollow mask
rate

sequence in (a) and against the maximum disparity in


Hitrate

60%
nn=11
= 11 effect 11 and the cyclopean
pixels (each pixel subtends 0.015 degrees of visual 12 (in which an
Necker-cube
Hit

40%
angle) between the three highlighted dots in (b). For
comparison the hit rate (a correct response when ambiguous 2D image gives
the three dots are in the same plane) is shown in (c).
20%
rise to two alternating per-
Three conditions are plotted in each graph. 0%
cepts with different depth
__ Human figure with the 3 marked dots on the 0% 50% 100% 150% 200% structures in conflict with the
same limb; ._._._. Human figure with the 3 marked Noise
Noise level
Level disparity given by the stere-
dots on different limbs; .. Random figure. The ogram), which argue in favor
viewing position for all trials was 0 (the walker is seen walking to the right); this viewing position insured good of the influence of high-level
recognizability of the moving figure. The specific noise levels we used for the distorted walker in this experiment cues on depth perception.
were 0, 25, 50, 100, 150, and 200 %, which is around the noise level used in the first experiment. This influence can, in turn, be

256 nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998


1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

modulated by factors that change the recognizability of a stimu- walk cycle. The stimuli were generated by displaying each point as a bright
lus. This hypothesis would explain why the human sequences give dot (0.03 degree of visual angle) on a gray background (mean luminance:
rise to false depth perception so much more frequently than do 20 cd per m2). The experiments were conducted on a Silicon Graphics
the random sequences. Other important factors that need to be Indigo 2 workstation. All sequences were presented in stereo using a pair
of StereoGraphics Crystal-Eyes (TM) LCD shutter glasses synchronized
considered in interpreting these results include grouping induced
with the display. Two views were generated for each frame to allow stereo-
by common motion or proximity. That is, dots may be more like- scopic vision. All subjects were tested to ensure that they had functioning
ly to be perceived as coplanar if they move in synchrony or if they stereoscopic ability, two subjects were rejected and all subjects were naive
are close together. We believe, however, that in our experiments as to the purpose of the experiments.
the effect of such factors would be relatively limited, because the
sequences in the different conditions had very similar mid-level
attributes such as motion and density distributions. Specifically Acknowledgments
the differences in performance between the walker and the ran- We wish to thank N. Logothetis, B. Tjan and D. Kersten for insightful comments
dom conditions suggest the importance of object-specific, recog- on earlier versions of the manuscript and P. Lipson for providing the biological
nition-based influences over general configurational ones. The motion data set.
motion trajectories of the individual dots were the same in both
conditions, and only their 2D offsets were randomized. Thus, two RECEIVED 29 JANUARY: ACCEPTED 22 MAY 1998
dots of the walker that moved in phase continued to do so in the
random stimulus, thereby largely maintaining the mid-level group-
1998 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

1. Johansson, G. Visual perception of biological motion and a model of its


ing cues and the articulation geometry in the two conditions. Yet, analysis. Percept. Psychophys. 14, 201211 (1973).
the suppression of binocular disparity perception occurs only when 2. Cutting, J. E. & Kozlowski, L. T. Recognition of friends by their walk. Bull.
Psychonom. Soc. 9, 353356 (1977).
the walker is recognizable. 3. Cutting, J. E. Coding theory adapted to gait perception. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum.
The idea that top-down influences can affect perception is Percept. Perform. 7, 7187 (1981).
certainly not a new one. Several well known visual illusions, 4. Hoffman D. D. & Flinchbaugh B. E. The interpretation of biological motion.
Biol. Cybern. 42, 195204 (1982).
such as the Dalmatian dog picture 13 or the mother-in- 5. Ullman S. The Interpretation of Visual Motion (MIT Press, Cambridge, 1979).
law/daughter-in-law figure14 demonstrate the significance of 6. Shibata T., Sugihara, K., & Sugie, N. Recovering three-dimensional structure
top-down expectations in interpreting ambiguous stimuli. and motion of jointed objects from orthographically projected optical flow.
Trans. IECE 68-D, 16891696 (1985).
Recent computational models of the neocortex have argued that 7. Webb J. & Aggarwal, J. Structure from motion of rigid and jointed objects.
feedback cortico-cortical projections might allow top-down Artif. Intell. 19, 107131 (1982).
influences to propagate from the higher cortical areas that are 8. Oram, M. W. & Perrett, D. I. Responses of anterior superior temporal
polysensory (STPa) neurons to biological motion stimuli. J. Cog. Neurosci. 6,
involved in object recognition back to the earlier areas that sup- 99116 (1994).
port lower-level processes15,16. Our study now provides evi- 9. Logothetis, N. K., Pauls, J. & Poggio, T. Shape recognition in the inferior
temporal cortex of monkeys. Curr. Biol. 5, 552563 (1995).
dence that even the very low-level process of stereo-depth 10. Sinha, P. & Poggio, T. Role of learning in three-dimensional form perception.
perception, which was previously considered to be a purely bot- Nature 384, 460463 (1996).
tom-up process17,18, is in fact susceptible to top-down influ- 11. Gregory, R. L. in Illusion in Nature and Art (eds Gregory, R. L. & Gombrich, E.
H.) 4996 (Duckworth, London, 1973).
ences. Additionally, our experimental results provide indirect 12. Julesz, B. Foundations of Cyclopean Perception (Univ. of Chicago, Chicago,
evidence that dynamic three-dimensional objects might be rec- 1971).
ognized by the visual system based on their 2D traces rather 13. Goldstein, B. Sensation and Perception (Brooks/Cole, Pacific Grove, 1996).
14. Boring, E. G. A new ambiguous figure. Am. J. Psychol. 42, 444445 (1930).
than on their 3D structural descriptions. 15. Ullman, S. Sequence seeking and counter streams: a computational model for
bidirectional information flow in the visual cortex. Cereb. Cortex 5, 111
(1995).
Methods 16. Mumford, D. On the computational architecture of the neocortex. II. The
The biological motion sequences used in our experiments were based on role of cortico-cortical loops. Biol. Cybern. 66, 241252 (1992).
data collected at the Gait Analysis Laboratory of the Spaulding Rehabil- 17. Julesz, B. Early vision and focal attention. Rev. Mod. Phys. 63, 735772
(1991).
itation Hospital in Boston, Massachusetts. The data comprised the 3D 18. Nakayama, K., Shimojo, S. & Silverman, G. H. Stereoscopic depth: Its relation
positions of twelve points on a male human as he walked in place. The to image segmentation, grouping, and the recognition of occluded objects.
point positions were updated 39 times over the course of one complete Perception 18, 5568 (1989).

nature neuroscience volume 1 no 3 july 1998 257

Вам также может понравиться