Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 94

1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.

com

contents

volume 2 no 2 february 1999

http://neurosci.nature.com

Three-dimensional reconstruction
of a Bergmann glial cell. Grosche
and colleagues show that
editorial
microdomains in these glia wrap Science and terrorism in Europe . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 99
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

around synaptic structures. See


page 139.

letter to the editor


Overuse of impact factors suppresses controversial ideas . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 101

news and views


Speech boundaries, syntax and the brain. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 103
Cyma Van Petten and Paul Bloom
SEE ARTICLE, PAGE 191

Coupling calcium to SNARE-mediated synaptic vesicle fusion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 104


Sabine Hilfiker, Paul Greengard and George J. Augustine
SEE ARTICLE, PAGE 119
Snapin may link calcium
and exocytosis.
Pages 104 and 119.
The smell of adrenaline . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 106
Stuart Firestein and Anna Menini
SEE ARTICLE, PAGE 133

Weighing the evidence: how the brain makes a decision . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 108


Jeffrey D. Schall
SEE ARTICLE, PAGE 176

obituary
Sir Alan Lloyd Hodgkin. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 111
By Denis Baylor and King-Wai Yau

book review
Sir Alan Hodgkin, 19141998. The Biophysics of Computation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 113
Page 111. BY CHRISTOF KOCH
Reviewed by Eve Marder

Nature Neuroscience (ISSN 1097-6256) is published monthly by Nature America Inc., headquartered at 345 Park Avenue South, New York, NY 10010-1707. Editorial Office: 345 Park
Avenue South, New York, NY 10010. Telephone 212 726 9200, Fax (212) 696 9635. North American Advertising: Nature Neuroscience, 345 Park Avenue South, New York, NY 10010-
1707. Telephone (212) 726-9200. Fax (212) 696-9006. European Advertising: Nature Neuroscience, Porters South, Crinan Street, London N1 9SQ. Telephone (0171) 833 4000. Fax
(0171) 843 4596. New subscriptions, renewals, changes of address, back issues, and all customer service questions in North America should be addressed to Nature Neuro-
science Subscription Department, PO Box 5054, Brentwood, TN 37024-5054. Telephone (800) 524-0384, Direct Dial (615) 377 3322, Fax (615) 377 0525. Outside North America: Nature
Neuroscience, Macmillan Magazines Ltd., Houndsmill, Brunel Road, Basingstoke, RG21 6XS, U.K.. Tel: +44-(0)1256-329242. Fax: +44-(0)1256 812358. Email: subscriptions@nature.com.
Annual subscription rates: U.S./Canada: U.S. $595, Canada add 7% for GST (institutional/corporate), U.S. $195, Canada add 7% for GST (individual making personal payment BN:
14091 1595 RT); U.K./Europe:395 (institutional/corporate), 175 (individual making personal payment), 99 (student); Rest of world (excluding Japan): 450 (institutional/corpo-
rate), 195 (individual making personal payment), 110 (student); Japan: Contact Japan Publications Trading Co. Ltd., 2-1 Sarugaku-cho 1 chome, Chiyoda-ku, Tokyo 101, Japan, phone
(03) 292-3755. Back issues: U.S./Canada, $45, Canada add 7% for GST; Rest of world: surface U.S. $43, air mail U.S. $45. Reprints: Nature Neuroscience Reprints Department, 345 Park
Avenue South, New York, NY 10010-1707. Subscription information is available at the Nature Neuroscience homepage at http://neurosci.nature.com. POSTMASTER: Send address changes
to Nature Neuroscience Subscription Department, P.O. Box 5054, Brentwood, TN 37024-5054. Executive Officers of Nature America Inc: Nicholas Byam Shaw, Chairman of the Board;
Mary Waltham, President; Edward Valis, Secretary-Treasurer. Printed by Publishers Press, Shepherdsville, KY, USA. Copyright 1999 Nature America Inc.

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 i


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

contents

scientific correspondence
Neurotrophin-3 is required for proper cerebellar development . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 115
B Bates, M Rios, A Trumpp, C Chen, G Fan, J M Bishop and R Jaenisch

articles
NT-3 and cerebellar development.
Page 115. Snapin: a SNARE-associated protein implicated in synaptic transmission . . . . . . 119
J M Ilardi, S Mochida and Z-H Sheng
SEE NEWS AND VIEWS, PAGE 104

Casein kinase-II regulates NMDA channel function in hippocampal


neurons . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 125
D N Lieberman and I Mody

Adrenaline enhances odorant contrast by modulating signal encoding


in olfactory receptor cells . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 133
F Kawai, T Kurahashi and A Kaneko
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

SEE NEWS AND VIEWS, PAGE 106

Microdomains for neuron-glia interaction: parallel fiber signaling to


Bergmann glial cells . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 139
J Grosche, V Matyash, T Mller, A Verkhratsky, A Reichenbach and H Kettenmann

Voltage-activated sodium channels amplify inhibition in neocortical


pyramidal neurons . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 144
G Stuart

Retention of heroin and morphine-6-glucuronide analgesia in a new


line of mice lacking exon 1 of MOR-1. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 151
A G P Schuller, M A King, J Zhang, E Bolan, Y-X Pan, D J Morgan, A Chang,
M E Czick, E M Unterwald, G W Pasternak and J E Pintar
Modeling the thalamic
reticular nucleus.
Page 168. SOD1 rescues cerebral endothelial dysfunction in mice overexpressing
amyloid precursor protein . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .157
C Iadecola, F Zhang, K Niwa, C Eckman, S K Turner, E Fischer, S Younkin,
D R Borchelt, K K Hsiao and G A Carlson

The construction of movement by the spinal cord . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 162


M C Tresch, P Saltiel and E Bizzi

Self-sustained rhythmic activity in the thalamic reticular nucleus mediated


by depolarizing GABAA receptor potentials . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 168
M Bazhenov, I Timofeev, M Steriade and T J Sejnowski

Neural correlates of a decision in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex


of the macaque . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 176
J-N Kim and M N Shadlen
SEE NEWS AND VIEWS, PAGE 108

Weighted combination of size and disparity: a computational model


for timing a ball catch . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 186
S K Rushton and J P Wann
The timing of ball-catching.
Page 186.
Brain potentials indicate immediate use of prosodic cues in natural
speech processing . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 191
K Steinhauer, K Alter and A D Friederici
SEE NEWS AND VIEWS, PAGE 103

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 ii


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

editorial

Science and terrorism in Europe


The animal rights movement, particularly in its more extreme The ALF announced that if Horne died, ten people would be
manifestations, has been a long-standing thorn in the side of the assassinated in retribution; among the named targets were Blake-
biomedical research community, but the problem has taken on more and Mark Matfield, the president of the Research Defence
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

a new urgency in the last few weeks, with renewed death threats Society, a London-based biomedical advocacy organization. The
being made against two prominent European neuroscientists, crisis was averted when Horne called off his strike after 68 days,
Colin Blakemore of Oxford University and Wolf Singer of the and while it was not clear that he had intended to kill himself (he
Max-Planck Institute in Frankfurt. Such threats are of course had in fact been taking some food), the effect was to trigger a
deplorable in any context, but they are particularly disturbing wave of protests and acts of vandalism in Britain and elsewhere.
because Blakemore and Singer are among the few neuroscien- The German animal rights movement has never been as vio-
tists who have been willing to publicly defend the use of animals lent as its British counterpart, but it is nevertheless active and influ-
in their own research. By targeting them, the animal rights ential, a close second to the British movement, according to
extremists are attempting to silence public debate, at a time when Matfield. Singer, like Blakemore, has received many threats in the
newly elected governments in both Britain and Germany appear past, and has been the target of a persistent campaign of vilifica-
to be more sympathetic than their predecessors to the animal tion and misinformation; for instance, he says that German web
rights movement, making it more important than ever that sci- sites provide translations of his publications, in which experimental
entists should make their voices heard. details are distorted, for instance by omitting any mention of anes-
The animal rights movement, like the anti-abortion move- thesia.
ment in the USA, comprises a broad range of opinions and tac- Singers former student Andreas Kreiter has also been targeted
tics, ranging from legitimate political activity to violent by a similar campaign. Kreiter is now a faculty member at the Uni-
extremism. The extreme end of the spectrum is represented pri- versity of Bremen, where he studies visual cortical physiology in
marily by the Animal Liberation Front (ALF), a loosely organized awake behaving monkeys. Soon after he had accepted the posi-
group that originated in Britain and now extends to many coun- tion in Bremen, a large advertisement appeared on a billboard in
tries in both Europe and North America, helped in part by the the city center, announcing that Kreiter was a monkey-torturer
internet, which allows easy sharing of information about poten- and giving his home address and telephone number. This trig-
tial targets and tactics. The ALF home page (http://www.animal- gered an intense campaign of protest, in which Kreiter received
liberation.net), for example, sets the tone with the statement: hundreds of letters from all over Germany. Some were threatening,
The earth is not dying, it is being killed. And those that are and on at least one occasion there was an attempted attack on his
killing it have names and addresses. laboratory. He is now forced to conceal his new address, and since
The group operates under a variety of names for the summer of 1997 he has been under daily police protection.
instance the Animal Rights Militia and the Justice Depart- Kreiter attributes this campaign in part to the Deutscher Tier-
ment but these are generally thought to be flags of conve- schutzbund (German Association for the Protection of Animals),
nience only. Recently, dramatic insight into the tactics of the a large and influential national organization whose charismatic
ALF came from a British television documentary, in which an leader, Wolfgang Apel, happens to live in Bremen.
infiltrator posing as a potential recruit filmed several of their Although Kreiter says that his fellow neurobiologists have been
leaders with a hidden camera. Among these was Robin Webb, sympathetic, he has been dismayed by the lukewarm support he
press officer for the British ALF and its most prominent has received from the university authorities, who he feels have
spokesman; although Webb has often denied ALF involvement failed to take a clear stand against the protesters. Many of his col-
in acts of violence, the film showed him giving detailed leagues in other departments, he says, are ignorant of animal
instructions on how to construct letter bombs. In the early research issues, and some are openly hostile; over a hundred fac-
1990s, the ALF conducted an extended bombing campaign ulty members at the university recently signed a memorandum
against scientists in Britain, and although nobody was killed demanding that he cease his experiments. The local media have
and most of the bombs were fairly crude, some included plas- also been unsympathetic, and on one occasion quoted without
tic explosives and were undoubtedly life-threatening. As a comment a comparison to Josef Mengele, the infamous doctor
result of these attacks, Blakemore, one of the most prominent of Auschwitz.
targets, is now forced to live under heavy security. It is tempting to see these campaigns as the work of extrem-
The latest crisis in Britain arose when an animal rights activist ists who are isolated from the rest of society, but this would be
named Barry Horne, who is serving an 18-year prison sentence to underestimate the extent of public sympathy for the aims,
for a series of arson attacks, began a hunger strike last October. if not the tactics, of the animal rights movement. Blakemore

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 99


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

editorial

believes that the latest crisis in Britain was due at least in part The issues have to be brought into sharper focus if the biomed-
to the actions of the Labour party, who during the 1997 elec- ical community is to defend itself effectively.
tion campaign promised to adopt a more stringent policy But the unpalatable truth is that terrorism works the cam-
toward animal research. To some extent they have already done paigns of the extremists are sufficiently threatening that few sci-
so, and new rules requiring local as well as central approval for entists are willing to speak out. By targeting those who do, the
animal protocols will take effect from April this year; never- ALF seeks to ensure that the arguments in favor or animal research
theless, the changes did not go far enough to satisfy some sup- are not heard. If more individual scientists were to make their case
porters, and the result was a backlash from extremists whose in public, the threat to any one person would be diluted; Matfield
hopes had been falsely raised. Although the governments posi- points out that the ALF, despite its violent posturing, has never
tion has been hardened to some extent by the need to resist actually killed anyone, and were they to do so, they would face an
terrorism, the animal rights issue remains on the political agen- enormous public backlash. Yet, it is understandable that few peo-
da. Among those voicing their concern is Michael Dexter, the ple want to take the risk of being the first to speak out.
director of the Wellcome Trust, who comments that the gov- The degree of public ignorance, and its reflection in govern-
ernment is still under tremendous pressure from the animal ment policy, testify to this reluctance. Singer, for instance, says
rights movement, and that scientists must stand up and be he is appalled by the lack of information about biomedical
counted. He has recently written to the Home Office minis- research that he encounters routinely even among highly edu-
ter, George Howarth, urging the government to adopt a cau- cated people. The potential consequences of this dismal situa-
tious approach and avoid issuing a statement that could be tion are illustrated by the experience of Switzerland, which last
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

interpreted as a commitment to further reduce and ultimately year narrowly escaped a yes vote in a national referendum that
eliminate animal use for medical research. would have banned all research on transgenic animals, and by
In Germany, the political situation is more threatening; in the current situation in Germany, where the future of animal
late January a bill will be introduced into the parliament that experiments is currently very much in the balance.
would amend the constitution to give protection to animals. Few neuroscientists would deny that the use of animals in
Although the final text has not yet been agreed, the majority research raises legitimate ethical questions; arguably, these are more
of political parties support some form of amendment. The acute in the case of neuroscience than for any other discipline. The
main opposition comes from the conservative Christian brain cannot be studied simply by looking at its parts (for instance
Democrats, but following their recent electoral defeat, it is in tissue culture), and certain questions about the human brain
unclear whether there will be enough votes in parliament to can only be answered studying the brains of closely related species.
block the measure. According to Jan Erik Bohling of the But these are also the species which seem likely to have the great-
Gesellschaft Gesundheit und Forschung (Society for Health est capacity for pain, fear and other forms of suffering, and for
and Research), a Frankfurt-based lobby group, the amendment which the ethical dilemma is therefore most pressing.
will be debated in the next few months and could be approved How then should neuroscientists defend their work? On the
as early as the end of April. Bohling, along with many scien- one hand, there are practical issues; animal experiments are
tists, believes that this will be potentially very damaging to Ger- tightly regulated by law, and researchers are normally very care-
man biomedical research; although freedom of research is ful not to cause unnecessary suffering to their experimental
currently protected under the constitution, the elevation of subjects. Standards of laboratory animal care are undoubted-
animal rights to constitutional level will open the door to a ly better now than they were in the past, and it would be fool-
protracted series of court battles, and long before research is ish to deny that these improvements are in part a response to
banned outright, it will become so difficult that researchers the valid concerns of animal rights advocates. Moreover,
will have little choice but to leave the country. Many researchers researchers have strong financial as well as ethical motives for
are also concerned about what they perceive as a lack of sup- minimizing the number of experimental animals used, and the
port by the government. Reinhard Grunwald, secretary gener- number of animals used in research has been falling steadily
al of the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG; the main for many years. Another important point, not widely appreci-
government funding agency), was recently quoted in Nature ated, is that some of the experiments that the public finds most
as saying, A constitutional change would not be necessary if troubling, those involving awake behaving monkeys, in fact
researchers limited their animal experiments to those that are require that the animals be in good health psychologically as
really important. This has caused consternation in the research well as physically; the monkeys must be calm and willing par-
community because it could be construed as an accusation of ticipants if the experiment is to succeed.
illegal behavior, given that only essential experiments are per- But if biomedical research is to be secure in the long term,
mitted under German law. Grunwald, however, now claims the public must approve not only its methods but also its goals.
that he was misquoted, and says that the DFG plans to make a In cases where there is a strong focus on a specific disease, the
public statement by the end of January opposing any amend- argument is relatively easy to make. Much current research in
ment to the constitution. neuroscience, however, is concerned with more basic questions,
The underlying problem, of course, is that scientists have been whose relevance to human welfare remains to be established,
relatively ineffectual in explaining and defending their work to and whose likely benefits lie in the distant future. The argu-
the public. Opposition to animal experimentation draws sup- ments for this type of research are more difficult to convey in
port from a widespread public distrust of science and technolo- a short soundbite, but Singer puts it well: since we left the Gar-
gy, and animal rights activists tend to be allied to a broad range of den of Eden, he says, humans have been responsible for their
causes, including opposition to animal farming and meat con- own fate, and with that responsibility comes the obligation to
sumption, genetic engineering of crops, hunting and environ- understand, a moral obligation to be curious about ourselves
mental destruction. Many of these causes enjoy wide support and our world. That is the message that neuroscientists must
(including that of many scientists), and as a result science finds get across, and they must not allow its communication to be
itself on the defensive against a broad sector of public opinion. prevented by the fear of terrorism.

100 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

letter to the editor

Overuse of impact factors suppresses


controversial ideas

TO THE EDITORThe welcome criticism crimination, no longer regulated by gen- agree, no matter what the scientific rea-
of the overwhelming weight given to the uine scientific considerations. Increas- sons for the disagreement. In other
impact factor, in the December editor- ing submission rates require larger words, there is no longer space for
ial (Nature Neuroscience 1, 641642) has panels of would-be referees, but most debate, controversial results or diver-
listed some drawbacks and misuses of editors rely on a list of people they gent opinions. I have experienced this
this bibliometric method that raise seri- know, who are established experts, unscientific situation both as author
ous concerns. Another adverse effect not hopefully honest and able to provide and as referee. Unexpected observations
mentioned in this editorial is also worth their reports within reasonable time (a and new ways of thinking vitally need
considering. The intense desire to get very important point from the editors to be submitted to the scientific com-
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

ones papers published in journals with point of view). In practice, papers are munity for experimental confirmation
the highest possible impact factor pro- examined by a rather small number of and discussions. The present concen-
duces an avalanche of papers submitted people, a situation that tends to favor tration of manuscripts in a small num-
to these journals and a progressive dis- conformity and to result in the sup- ber of highly ranked journals is working
affection for the less-quoted journals, a pression of unorthodox results or ideas. against these confrontations, so essen-
self-amplifying process that quickly Moreover, stringent space limitations tial for progress in science.
leads to a diminished reputation and imply high rejection rates, and this gen-
possible financial troubles for the latter. erates questionable criteria for accep- J.M. Gillis
This has the unintended effect of con- tance; for instance, some editorial Department of Physiology,
ferring on the editors of highly ranked offices are instructed to reject a manu- Catholic University of Louvain
journals an enormous power of dis- script if the two referees reports dis- Belgium

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 101


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

news and views

Speech boundaries, syntax


and the brain
Cyma Van Petten and Paul Bloom

Speech comprehension requires rapid decoding of grammatical relationships. Electrical scalp


recordings show that the brain responds immediately to intonational cues signifying phrase
boundaries. Thus, these cues may control initial decisions about sentence structure.

Language comprehension consists alent of a comma. They describe no


of the successful transformation of Peter verspricht Anna zu ARBEITEN less than four physical differences
physical signals into abstract and und das Bro zu putzen between German sentences analo-
meaningful information. From the gous to the example above and
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

Peter verspricht
same set of air-pressure waves, we ANNA zu entlasten those with the more typical verb-
und das Bro zu putzen
might understand a new idea, deter- then-object structure (as in Since
mine the speakers age, emotional Fig. 1. An example of the German sentences used in the experi- Jay always jogs five miles this seems
state and educational level, and add ments of Steinhauer and colleagues, illustrating how the pauses like a short distance to him;
a new word to our vocabulary. between words differ in the two conditions. Fig. 1). These differences in word
Understanding how the human duration, pause duration, pitch
brain accomplishes this feat requires contour and loudness serve to
a careful description of the stages of this tences, and that listeners use this informa- group the words of each sentence into dis-
transformation, beginning with the physi- tion almost immediately. tinct intonational phrases. The critical
cal signal. In speech sounds, changes in fre- To see how this works, consider the fol- question is whether this prosodic infor-
quency and amplitude occur on two time lowing poorly written sentence: Since Jay mation can override the usual syntactic
scales. Rapid changes occurring within always jogs five miles seems like a short dis- preference for assigning a noun to the
20200 ms convey information about the tance to him. In the absence of a comma nearest preceding verb.
individual phonemes that make up words, after jogs, readers find this sentence diffi- Resolution of this question requires an
whereas slower prosodic modulations of fre- cult to interpret. They are led down the gar- immediate measure of comprehension.
quency and amplitude typically extend over den path of assuming that a noun phrase Querying listeners at the end of a sentence
multiple words. Computer-synthesized (five miles) following a verb (jogs) is the can reveal only their final analyses, which
speech demonstrates both the adequacy and object of that verb rather than the subject of may not indicate whether they avoided the
the limitations of phonological information a later verb (seems). Given this potential garden path, or have been there and back.
alone. Although intelligible, synthetic speech ambiguity, why dont readers simply post- The ERP recorded from the scalp provides
is difficult to understand because it lacks pone decisions about the interpretation of a continuous measure of brain electrical
prosody or intonation cues. the noun phrase until they reach the end of activity with millisecond temporal resolu-
Prosody has been granted a limited role the sentence? This strategy avoids the gar- tion, and so it has been widely used to
in most theories of language comprehen- den path, but at the cost of imposing a study language, for which timing is criti-
sion. For instance, rising versus falling pitch memory burden of holding the unassigned cal. Two components of the ERP elicited
toward the end of a sentence distinguishes material in mind while continuing to read. by words reflect comprehension difficul-
questions from statements, and sarcastic Working memory capacity is severely lim- ties. A negative voltage deflection peaking
intonation indicates that a speakers intend- ited1, and it is needed for other aspects of at about 400 ms after stimulus onset
ed meaning is the opposite of the literal one. sentence comprehension, such as linking (N400) is small when a word is easily inte-
In these traditional examples, prosody serves pronouns to their antecedents2. Without grated with prior semantic context, but
only to modify sentence meanings that have contrary evidence (such as a comma), link- shows graded increases in amplitude as the
already been computed. In this issue of ing a noun with the earliest available verb is prior context becomes less and less useful.
Nature Neuroscience (pages 191196), Stein- an efficient processing strategy. Moreover, A word like sugar will elicit no N400 at
hauer and colleagues use event-related the consequences of being led down the gar- the end of a sentence like I take my coffee
potential (ERP) recordings to provide per- den path by a less-than-thoughtful writer with cream and..., a medium-sized N400
suasive evidence that prosody is far more are fairly trivialat worst, one might have at the end of a sentence like At the store,
central to language comprehension. Their to reread the sentence. I bought a pound of... and a large N400
data indicate that speakers use prosody to In contrast, listeners lack the luxury of in I finally asked my boss for a ...3. The
disambiguate potentially problematic sen- instant replay and may be severely misled N400 offers a rapidly changing index of the
by an incorrect initial parse. So why arent ease or difficulty of semantic processing,
The authors are at the Department of we chronically confused in our daily con- beginning well before a spoken word is
Psychology, University of Arizona, Tucson, versations? Steinhauer and colleagues even completely pronounced4. A second
Arizona 85721, USA. email: began by documenting the auditory cues a measure of comprehension difficulty is a
bloom@u.arizona.edu and speaker produces when saying a potential- positive voltage deflection following the
vanpettc@u.arizona.edu ly ambiguous sentencethe verbal equiv- N400, termed the P600; large P600s are

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 103


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

news and views

triggered by words that violate rules of CPS to full intonational phrases remains to 1. Miller, G. A. Psychol. Rev. 63, 8197 (1956).
agreement, word order or other syntactic be discovered. Prosodic contours are also 2. Carpenter, P. A., Miyake, A. & Just, M. A. in
principles. The P600 may serve as a gener- used to mark word boundaries within a Handbook of Psycholinguistics (ed. Gernsbacher,
M. A.) 10751122 (Academic, New York, 1994).
al index of surprise at encountering a phrase the distinction between green
grammatical problem, or the attempt to house and greenhouse. Segmentation of 3. Kutas, M. & Hillyard, S. A. Nature 307, 161163
(1984).
revise ones initial parse of a sentence5. the speech stream into words is a basic prob-
4. Van Petten, C., Coulson, S., Rubin, S., Plante, E.
Steinhauer and colleagues found that lem that must be solved by infants9,10, so & Parks, M. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn.
when sentences with typical and atypical developmental studies of the CPS may shed (in press).
syntactic structure were presented with nor- light on the perceptual strategies and brain 5. Coulson, S., King, J. W. & Kutas, M. Lang. Cogn.
mal intonation, there was no sign of an processes underlying language acquisition. Processes 13, 2158 (1998).
enlarged N400 or P600 during the atypical The precursor to a complete under- 6. Bloom, P. Curr. Directions Psychol. Sci. 5, 9094
sentences. In other words, when subjects standing of human language is a defini- (1996).
heard a sentence like Since Jay always jogs tion of the right units of analysis. Linguists 7. Von der Heydt, R., Peterhans, E. &
five miles seems like a short distance to him, and psychologists have not always agreed Baumgartner, G. Science 224, 12601262
(1984).
there was no sign of trouble at the word on these fundamental issues: whether
8. Bregman, A. S. Auditory Scene Analysis: The
seems. A critical third condition pitted the phonemic features, phonemes or syllables Perceptual Organization of Sound (MIT Press,
syntactic preference against the prosodic are the smallest speech units11, or whether Cambridge, MA, 1990).
information. The authors spliced the first words, phrases or sentences are the prim-
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

9. Jusczyk, P. W. The Discovery of Spoken Language


part of the sentence (Since Jay always jogs itive conceptual unit12,13. Physiological (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1997).
five miles), said with the intonation sug- methods for monitoring brain activity 10. Bloom, P. in Handbook of Psycholinguistics (ed.
gesting a verbal comma between jogs and bring a new and rich dimension of data Gernsbacher, M. A.) 741780 (Academic, New
York, 1994).
five, to the second part of the more com- to bear on these old questions. Other
mon structure (this seems like a short dis- recent studies report brain potentials trig- 11. Remez, R. R. in Handbook of Psycholinguistics
(ed. Gernsbacher, M. A.) 145172 (Academic,
tance to him). This created a hybrid gered at syntactic boundaries (clauses) in New York, 1994).
condition: initial prosodic cues suggested both spoken and written sentences14,15. 12. Van Petten, C. Psychophysiology 32, 511525
the atypical sentence structure, while the The discovery of a brain event marking (1995).
words specified the more typical verb-then- intonational units adds another tool that 13. Bach, E. Informal Lectures on Formal Semantics
object construction. In this condition, a can be used to understand the complex (SUNY, New York, 1989).
large N400 and P600 were elicited by the interplay of phonological, prosodic, syn- 14. Muller, H. M., King, J. W. & Kutas, M. Cogn.
first word that indicated the sentences true tactic and semantic information in deriv- Brain Res. 5, 193203 (1997).
structure, although this structure would be ing meaning. 15. Kutas, M. Psychophysiology 34, 383398 (1997).
entirely expected in written language. These
results show that listeners take prosodic cues
seriously and adjust their syntactic strate-
gies accordingly.
Given the importance of intonational
Coupling calcium to SNARE-
phrases in sentence processing, Steinhauer
and colleagues went on to ask whether mediated synaptic vesicle
recognition of prosodic boundaries was
reflected in scalp-recorded brain activity.
The answer was yes: the authors report a dis-
fusion
tinct ERP component beginning shortly Sabine Hilfiker, Paul Greengard and George J. Augustine
after the conclusion of each intonational
phrase. This is an exciting finding because Calcium triggers vesicle fusion, but the molecular mechanism
discovering boundaries is a fundamental is largely unknown. Now Ilardi and colleagues identify
process in multiple domains of perception Snapin, a protein that may provide the missing link.
and cognition6. The contours defining a
visual object must be perceived before the
object can be recognized, and neurons early Neurotransmitter release involves a series of Among the many molecules implicated in
in the visual cortical pathways respond to interactions between the membranes of neurotransmitter release, most attention has
even occluded contours7. Early auditory synaptic vesicles and the presynaptic termi- focused on two types of proteins, the
processes partition the acoustic input into nal, culminating in the calcium-dependent SNAREs and synaptotagmin. In this issue
distinct streams of sound arising from dif- fusion of the two membranes. However, the of Nature Neuroscience, Ilardi and colleagues
ferent locations; otherwise we might amal- molecular mechanism by which calcium (pages 119124) identify and characterize a
gamate the sound of a dog barking next triggers fusion remains largely unknown. novel presynaptic protein, called Snapin,
door with water running in the kitchen and which promotes the interaction of SNAREs
a radio playing in the living room into one Sabine Hilfiker and Paul Greengard are at and synaptotagmin and may be important
incomprehensible acoustic event8. The short Rockefeller University, 1230 York Avenue, New for synaptic vesicle fusion.
latency of the closure positive shift (CPS), York, New York 10021, USA. George Augustine Synaptotagmin, an integral calcium-
as the authors call this new ERP component, is at the Department of Neurobiology, Duke binding protein of synaptic vesicle mem-
suggests that detection of intonational Medical Center, Box 3209, Durham, North branes, is thought to mediate the action of
boundaries is an equally primary process in Carolina 27710, USA. calcium in neurotransmitter release. Evi-
speech perception. The specificity of the email: georgea@neuro.duke.edu dence for this notion includes the observa-

104 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

news and views

tions that transmitter release evoked by of docked vesicles and inhibit neurotrans- 2) There have been several reports that
presynaptic action potentials is greatly mitter release. Snapin might also prime vesi- calcium binding to synaptotagmin can dra-
decreased by presynaptic injection of synap- cles for fusion, for example by increasing the matically promote association of this pro-
totagmin fragments1 or by mutation or binding of synaptotagmin to the SNARE tein with both syntaxin8,9 and SNAP-2510
2,3
deletion of the synaptotagmin I gene . The complex, before a Snapin-independent reac- (H. Tokumaru, unpublished observations).
synaptic vesicle-associated protein synap- tion causes vesicle fusion. Finally, vesicle Based on these reports, it is widely believed
tobrevin (also called VAMP), a v-SNARE, fusion per se might be mediated by a that this calcium-triggered interaction can
interacts with two membrane-associated Snapin-dependent mechanism. Earlier in- promote the fusion of synaptic vesicles (Fig.
proteins (t-SNAREs), SNAP-25 and syn- vitro work examining the interactions of 1b). However, this model also has limita-
taxin, to form a stable SNARE complex. SNARE proteins and synaptotagmin sug- tions. First, it is not at all clear how binding
These three SNARE proteins are essential gested at least three possible models for cal- of SNAREs to synaptotagmin could account
for neurotransmitter release46, and the cium-triggered vesicle fusion (Fig. 1a-c), and for fusion. Second, the rate of binding
formation of a complex among these pro- the identification of Snapin now provides between these proteins may be too slow (H.
teins is the minimal molecular requirement an additional model for connecting SNAREs Tokumaru, unpublished observations) to
for membrane fusion in vitro7. Although it and synaptotagmin during the fusion reac- account for the finding that neurotransmit-
is clear that both SNAREs and synaptotag- tion (Fig. 1d). Thus, some of the possibili- ter release occurs approximately 100 s after
min have important roles in neurotrans- ties for this reaction now include the calcium binding11,12.
mitter release, the mechanism by which following: 3) There have been other reports that
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

synaptotagmin confers calcium sensitivity 1) The SNARE proteins and synapto- calcium binding decreases, rather than
onto the SNARE-based fusion machinery tagmin could act sequentially without bind- increases, the affinity of synaptotagmin for
is unclear. The discovery of Snapin by Ilar- ing to each other. For example, the the SNARE complex13. Such a calcium-
di and colleagues may provide the long- rod-shaped structure formed by the paired induced displacement of synaptotagmin
awaited answer to this question. parallel helices of synaptobrevin, SNAP-25 could precede the formation of the rod-
Snapin is a neuron-specific protein that and syntaxin6 could bring the vesicle and shaped complex among the SNAREs (Fig.
is found predominantly on synaptic vesi- plasma membranes in close apposition, so 1c). This helix zippering mechanism has
cles. Snapin binds to the SNARE complex, that calcium binding to synaptotagmin been postulated to underlie evoked trans-
via a direct interaction with SNAP-25, and could allow synaptotagmin to bind to mem- mitter release6 and can account for the
thereby seems to promote the interaction brane lipids and thereby cause fusion (Fig. occurrence of spontaneous release in the
of synaptotagmin with the SNAREs. The 1a). A limitation of this model is that it does absence of synaptotagmin. However, at pre-
carboxy-terminal domain of Snapin is pre- not account for observations that sponta- sent it is not clear that the SNAREs can form
dicted to form a coiled-coil structure. Pep- neous transmitter release, and thus mem- complexes rapidly enough to account for
tide fragments corresponding to this brane fusion, can occur even after the speed of synaptic vesicle fusion7,14,15.
domain reversibly inhibit this proteinpro- synaptotagmin has been genetically delet- 4) The new findings suggest that the
tein interaction, and partially block synap- ed2,3. It also neglects a variety of data in vitro coupling between synaptotagmin and the
tic transmission when introduced into indicating a direct interaction between SNARE complex may be controlled by
superior cervical ganglion neurons. Taken synaptotagmin and the SNARE complex, Snapin. Ilardi and colleagues show that this
together, these results demonstrate the phys- which is a central feature of the three other protein can promote the attachment of
iological relevance of the interaction of models we discuss. synaptotagmin to SNARE complexes in vitro
Snapin with SNAP-25 and suggest
the exciting possibility that Snapin
serves as a link between the SNAREs a b c d
and synaptotagmin during calcium-
dependent neurotransmitter release.
The precise role of Snapin in neu-
rotransmitter release awaits further
experimental efforts. It is possible
that Snapin acts as a modulator,
rather than as an essential compo-
nent, of exocytosis. This is consistent
with the authors observation that
injected Snapin peptides only par-
tially abolish neurotransmitter
release. Alternatively, Snapin might
be an essential component in the
molecular mechanism of exocytosis
by participating in vesicle docking,
priming or fusion reactions. For Fig. 1. Four possible models for calcium-triggered vesicle fusion. (a) The SNARE proteins initially bring the
two membranes into close apposition, and subsequent binding of calcium to synaptotagmin induces mem-
example, Snapin might promote the
brane fusion without a direct interaction between synaptotagmin and SNAREs. (b) The association of cal-
docking of synaptic vesicles, by link- cium-bound synaptotagmin with the SNARE complex can trigger membrane fusion. (c) A calcium-induced
ing vesicles to the plasma membrane displacement of synaptotagmin from the SNARE proteins allows formation of a SNARE complex that spans
through an interaction with SNAP- the two membranes, leading to fusion. (d) Snapin binds to the SNARE complex, by binding to SNAP-25.
25. Interfering with this interaction The subsequent formation of a calcium/synaptotagmin/SNARE/Snapin complex results in vesicle fusion.
in vivo would decrease the number Synaptotagmin may bind to the SNAREs either before or after binding calcium.

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 105


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

news and views

and that perturbing these interactions in and structural analyses of the consequences 3. Geppert, M. et al. Cell 79, 717727 (1994).
vivo inhibits transmitter release. Thus, it is of perturbing Snapin function also will help 4. Sdhof, T. C. Nature 375, 645653 (1995).
possible that synaptotagmin triggers fusion to distinguish among the possible functions 5. Augustine, G. J., Burns, M. E., DeBello, W. M.,
by binding to Snapin-associated SNARE of this protein. Of course, the same kinetic Pettit, D. L. & Schweizer, F. E. Annu. Rev.
Pharmacol. Toxicol. 36, 659701 (1996).
complexes, either before or after synapto- issues raised for the other models would be
tagmin binds calcium (Fig. 1d). This new relevant for any Snapin-based fusion 6. Weis, W. I. & Scheller, R. H. Nature 395,
328329 (1998).
model is consistent with what we now know hypothesis; future work will need to define
7. Weber, T. et al. Cell 92, 759772 (1998).
about Snapin. However, it is still possible the speed of binding of synaptotagmin to
8. Li, C. et al. Nature 375, 594599 (1995).
that Snapin has a role in one of the many Snapin-associated SNARE complexes.
other vesicle trafficking steps involved in In summary, the paper by Ilardi and col- 9. Kee, Y. & Scheller, R. H. J. Neurosci. 16,
19751981 (1996).
neurotransmitter release. leagues introduces Snapin, an intriguing
10. Schiavo, G., Stenbeck, G., Rothman, J. E. &
To clarify the precise role of Snapin in protein that seems to be involved in the Sllner, T. H. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 94,
neurotransmitter release, it would be useful interaction between the SNARE complex 9971001 (1997).
to know whether Snapin acts before or after and synaptotagmin. This protein promises 11. Llins, R., Steinberg, I. Z. & Walton, K. Biophys.
calcium entry into the nerve terminal. There to be an important component of the exci- J. 33, 323352 (1981).
are several relevant biochemical questions: tation/secretion coupling mechanism and 12. Augustine, G. J., Adler, E. M. & Charlton, M. P.
Does calcium influence the ability of Snapin may therefore have a central role in the reg- Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 635, 365381 (1991).
to bind to SNAP-25? Is the ability of Snapin ulation of neurotransmitter release. 13. Mehta, P. P., Battenberg, E. & Wilson, M. C.
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 93, 1047110476


to induce binding of synaptotagmin to the (1996).
SNARE complex calcium dependent? Does 1. Bommert, K. et al. Nature 363, 163165 (1993). 14. Calakos, N., Bennett, M. K., Peterson, K. E. &
Snapin influence the binding of calcium to Scheller, R. H. Science 263,11461149 (1994).
2 . Broadie, K., Bellen, H. J., DiAntonio, A.,
synaptotagmin? Does Snapin bind directly Littleton, J. T. & Schwarz, T. L. Proc. Natl. Acad. 15. Ungermann, C., Sato, K. & Wickner, W. Nature
to synaptotagmin? Additional physiological Sci. USA 91, 1072710731 (1994). 396, 543548 (1998).

neuron begins in the cilia (Fig. 1). Odorant


The smell of adrenaline binding to receptors in the cilia activates a
G protein, causing adenylyl cyclase to pro-
Stuart Firestein and Anna Menini duce cAMP, which directly opens ion chan-
nels in the plasma membrane. The inward
Adrenaline modulates signaling by the olfactory epithelium. transduction current is composed of sodi-
A new study suggests that this effect may result from um and calcium ions, with the latter acti-
channel phosphorylation in olfactory receptor neurons. vating a calcium-dependent chloride
current that further depolarizes the cell. The
depolarization spreads passively from the
The olfactory system must make sense of Beyond its purely sensory role, the olfac- cilia via a single dendrite to the soma, trig-
thousands of chemical ligands or odorants. tory system is closely tied to behavioral gering action potentials that are then con-
This is no mean trick because the ligand responses, especially those with an emo- ducted to the olfactory bulb. Until now, this
repertoire is not only enormous but also tional component. For example, territorial- has been considered a fairly complete
diverse (including aromatic and aliphatic ity, sexuality, feeding and aggression are all description of olfactory signal transduction;
structures with many different functional strongly influenced by olfactory stimuli, and it was thought that no further processing of
groups) and complex (including mixtures it has been suggested that behavioral con- the signal occurred until it reached the
and elaborately structured molecules). text might modulate activity in the epitheli- olfactory bulb.
Increasing the difficulty, there are no appar- um via descending central inputs (as occurs In the alternative scheme proposed by
ent primary odors (as for colors) or funda- for instance in the cochlea). Whether such Kawai and colleagues, adrenaline acts
mentals (as for acoustics) or even a clear inputs actually exist, however, remains downstream of the initial transduction
spatial organization (as for visual or unclear. The olfactory epithelium is exten- event, but upstream of the first synapse,
somatosensory maps). This information sively innervated by centrifugal adrenergic to alter the firing pattern of the receptor
must be encoded by the primary sensory fibers3, and -adrenergic receptors are neuron. The authors show that the effect
neurons in the olfactory epithelium; each found in the olfactory epithelium, but of adrenaline is two-fold: an increase in
neuron is thought to express a single recep- adrenaline was thought to affect the epithe- the threshold for spike initiation and an
tor gene from a repertoire of about 1000 lium indirectlymost likely affecting either increase in the firing rate in response to
receptors (in mammals), each of which is the gland cells that produce and secrete the strong stimuli. Early experiments in the
presumably tuned to slightly different subset mucous that covers the epithelium or its olfactory epithelium5,6 showed that the
of odors1,2. dense vasculature4. However, striking new spike frequency is a function of odor con-
results from Kawai and colleagues (pages centration, and a more recent analysis has
Stuart Firestein is at the Department of 133138) in this issue show that olfactory shown that this function is quite steep
Biological Sciences, Columbia University, neurons themselves are modulated by (Reisert, J. & Matthews, H. J. Physiol.
New York, New York, USA. adrenergic agonists, thus suggesting the exis- 506P, 83P, 1998). Now the results of
email: sjf24@columbia.edu tence of a new regulatory pathway and help- Kawai et al. indicate that adrenaline could
Anna Menini is at the Instituto di Cibernetica e ing to explain some previously puzzling produce an even steeper doseresponse
Biofisica, CNR, Genova, Italy. findings. relationship, further amplifying the final
menini@icb.ge.cnr.it Transduction in the olfactory receptor cellular response to odors. How does

106 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

news and views

Fig. 1. The major signaling pathways in


report8 from 1993, which con-
olfactory receptor neurons. In the cilia,
odorants are detected and transduced cluded that animals developed
OLFACTORY
into a generator potential through a well CILIUM sensitivity to an odor to which
defined second-messenger system. they were initially not very
Cyclic AMP produced in the cilia opens a DENDRITE responsive after being exposed
cation-permeable cyclic-nucleotide-
R
-40 mV to it for several weeks. Those
ODORS
gated channel, which depolarizes the G ATP changes appeared to occur at the
membrane from its normal resting level AC GENERATOR
cAMP
Na + POTENTIAL epithelium, rather than more
(60 mV) to as much as 40 mV. An ++ centrally, suggesting perhaps
Ca
unusual calcium-dependent chloride Cl
some local feedback pathway by
channel is also activated, and because the
intracellular concentration of chloride is Ca ++ which odor experience could
high, the outward flow of chloride ions drive sensitivity. The results have
adds to the depolarization. The sensory always been considered some-
generator potential then spreads pas- SOMA
what controversial, partly
sively to the somatic membrane, where because no clear mechanism
it activates calcium, sodium and potas- NUCLEUS ADRENALINE was shown. The investigators
A
sium channels to produce an action ?
G proposed that increased recep-
potential. A spike train of action poten- AC tor expression could account for
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

cAMP
tials propagates down the axon to the ++ the effect, but again there was
Ca
synaptic terminals in the olfactory bulb. It PKA ATP
no direct evidence and no
is the structure of this spike train that Na
+

Kawai and colleagues showed is modu- mechanism for translating odor


lated by adrenaline through the action of
+
K
SPIKE TRAIN exposure into modulation of
the second messenger cAMP. R, odorant
AXON gene expression. Another possi-
receptor; G, G protein; AC, adenylyl ACTION bility, perhaps less likely, is that
cyclase; A, adrenergic receptor; PKA, POTENTIAL
TO OLFACTORY
odorants might affect the for-
BULB
protein kinase A. mation or survival of new sen-
sory neurons, given that they are
known to be generated contin-
adrenaline produce these effects? By com- inputs can indeed modulate the activity of uously throughout life, but again there
paring the properties of somatic voltage- sensory neurons in vivo. It will also be was no evidence for how this might occur.
gated sodium and calcium currents with important to determine under what cir- The work of Kawai and colleagues sug-
and without adrenergic agonists, the cumstances this pathway is active. One pos- gests two plausible mechanisms for these
authors show that adrenaline increases the sibility is that the adrenergic inputs to the earlier results. First, the increased sensitivi-
amplitude of the sodium current and epithelium could be part of a feedback cir- ty might be due to the effects of adrenaline
decreases the amplitude of the T-type cal- cuit, in which signals from the epithelium on the firing rates of selected neurons,
cium current. What are the molecular are carried via the olfactory or trigeminal working via central feedback circuits that
mechanisms underlying these effects? The nerves to interneurons that eventually acti- increase the odor sensitivity of chronically
authors find that introducing cAMP into vate the centrifugal adrenergic pathway. stimulated neurons. Second, the increase in
the olfactory receptor cell mimics the Another possibility is that the adrenergic somatic intracellular cAMP by adrenaline
effects of adrenaline, and that the effects neurons might be under circadian or hor- provides a possible way of controlling gene
of cAMP are mediated by phosphoryla- monal control, which could elevate sensi- expression. Cyclic AMP acting through
tion of sodium channels and T-type cal- tivity in the epithelium in particular CREB9 or other signal transduction path-
cium channels (Fig. 1). Note that the behavioral or environmental contexts. ways could provide a feedback pathway for
cAMP generated by adrenergic activation The adrenergic pathway may not be the gene expression in cells that are chronically
in the soma is unlikely to interact with the only important central input to the epithe- active. Many questions remain to be
cAMP produced by activation of the odor lium. The nervus teminalis, the so-called answered for either of these speculations to
receptors in the cilia, as the cilia and the zeroth cranial nerve and somewhat of an be accepted, of course, but by providing a
soma are almost certainly biochemically enigma, releases gonadotropin-releasing rationale underlying the earlier observed
isolated compartments. hormone onto the olfactory epithelium7. sensitivity increase, these findings suggest
Where does this centrifugal adrenergic There is also extensive histochemical evi- that this issue might be worth revisiting.
input originate? Although the data are dence for cholinergic inputs to the epitheli- With new tools available from gene target-
somewhat skimpy on this issue, most (and um3. These data together suggest that the ing, in which all cells expressing a particular
perhaps all) of the fibers probably come olfactory epithelium is not simply com- receptor are marked by lacZ or green fluo-
from the sympathetic superior cervical gan- posed of isolated receptors signaling from rescent protein expression10, it has also
glia, which innervate other regions of the the periphery, but is intimately tied to the become possible to determine whether the
face and nasopharyngeal cavities and are central nervous system and probably to the number of cells with a particular receptor
associated primarily with the blood supply. endocrine system. From the work of Kawai is increased under conditions of chronic
Once in the epithelium, the fibers may et al., it is clear that these external inputs odor exposure. In any case, it is clear that
branch to secretory cells, and perhaps even may not regulate glandular activity exclu- the olfactory system, one of evolutions old-
neurons. Alternatively, their action on neu- sively, but may have direct consequences for est inventions, still holds many secrets.
rons may be more paracrine, with no well- neuronal signaling.
defined synaptic connections. It will be This new finding may also throw some 1. Buck, L. & Axel, R. Cell 65, 175187 (1991).
important to confirm that adrenergic light on an interesting but often ignored 2. Zhao, H. et al. Science 279, 237242 (1998).

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 107


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

news and views

3. Zielinski, B. S., Getchell, M. L., Wenokur, R. L. 521-540 (1978). Science 260, 9981000 (1993).
& Getchell, T. V. Anat. Record 225, 232245 6. Kauer, J.S. & Shepherd, G.M. Brain Res. 85, 108- 9. Silva, A.J., Kogan, J.H., Frankland, P.W. &
(1989). 113 (1975). Kida, S. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 21, 127-48
4. Getchell, M. L. & Getchell, T. V. J. Comp. 7. Wirsig-Wiechmann, C. & Jennes, L. Neurosci. (1998).
Physiol. A 155, 435443 (1984). Lett. 160, 201204 (1993). 10. Mombaerts, P. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 6,
5. Getchell,T. & Shepherd, G.M. J Physiol. 282, 8. Wang, H.-W., Wysocki, C. J. & Gold, G. H. 481486 (1996).

this task can be accounted for based on the


Weighing the evidence: how activity of a small number (around 100) of
MT neurons5,6. They have also shown that

the brain makes a decision artificial stimulation of MT during the stim-


ulus presentation influences the monkeys
Jeffrey D. Schall judgement in a predictable way, thereby
demonstrating directly that neurons in MT
provide the sensory representation on which
Kim and Shadlen investigate how neurons of the prefrontal
the perceptual decision is based.
cortex interpret slowly accumulating signals from visual How then is this sensory representation
cortex to make a perceptual decision. converted into a behavioral outcome? In
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

other words, how and where are the signals


...in the night, imagining some fear Kim and Shadlen have now begun to inves- from MT read? The final behavioral
How easy is a bush supposd a Bear? tigate the intermediate stage, in which the response to the stimulus is an eye movement
Shakespeare, A Midsummer Nights sensory evidence is evaluated to arrive at a to one of two targets, so an obvious place to
Dream, Act 5 decision. look is the frontal eye field and adjacent pre-
The authors took advantage of a method frontal cortex, which is known to be
As Shakespeare observed, even perception developed in Newsomes laboratory, in involved in producing eye movements, and
may require an active decision about how which monkeys are trained to report the net also receives inputs from area MT. More-
to interpret the raw data. On page 176 of direction of visual motion in an array of over, prefrontal cortex is a region in which
this issue, Kim and Shadlen examine the moving dots. They report their perception visual representations are combined with
neural basis of a simple visual perceptual by making an eye movement to one of two knowledge, goals and desires to generate
decision. Their study, along with other targets that appear on either side of the dis- action7.
recent work (see refs. 13 for reviews), illus- play after the dots disappear.
trates how the subjective process of decid- When all the dots move in
ing may be soon be explained in terms of the same direction, the task
objectively observable brain processes. is easy, and monkeys can
Perceptual decisions involve several steps. respond correctly on every
?
First, a preliminary representation of the trial. If some of the dots

sensory stimulus is converted into higher- move in random directions,
level explicit representations of the features the motion signal is diluted,
time
that will form the basis for the decision. Sec- and as the fraction of ran-
ond, one of the competing sensory repre- domly moving dots is
sentations must be selected; in other words, increased, the task becomes
the ambiguous representation of the rele- harder and the proportion of
vant features must be translated into an correct responses decreases.
explicit representation of one of the possi- Eventually, when all the dots
ble alternatives. Finally, the sensory decision are moving at random, there
must lead to an appropriate behavioral is no net motion signal, and
response. A neural explanation of decision so the monkeys response
making must describe the neural basis of reflects preference in the
each of these steps. absence of evidence.
Important insights into the first step This simple design allows
have come from the work pioneered by precise measurements of
William Newsome and colleagues4, who both the stimulus and the
have examined the sensory representations response. The advantage of
on which a perceptual decision is based. using motion as a cue is that Fig. 1. To earn a reward, monkeys had to decide the net direction of
Meanwhile, other researchers have shown the site of motion represen- motion of a stimulus composed of variable fractions of coherently
how behavioral responses are generated in tation is well characterized; and randomly moving dots. Monkeys reported their decision by
the face of alternatives (reviewed in ref. 3). motion cues are represented making an eye movement to either of two targets. Psychologists
seek to understand choice behavior in terms of computational or
in the extrastriate visual area
cognitive processes, symbolized by the bubble. Neurophysiologists
Jeffrey Schall is at theVanderbilt Vision Research MT, whose neurons respond seek to understand choice behavior in terms of neural activity mon-
Center, Department of Psychology, Wilson Hall, preferentially to moving itored in particular parts of the brain, symbolized by the raster dis-
Vanderbilt University, stimuli. Newsome and col- play. Ultimately, we would like to understand the relationship
Nashville, Tennessee 37240, USA leagues have shown that between the neural activity and the behavior and associated cogni-
email: jeffrey.d.schall@vanderbilt.edu monkeys performance in tive processes, symbolized by the arrow.

108 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

news and views

Kim and Shadlen therefore examined opportunity to report its choice. evokes a strong leftward signal, then the
activity in this area, using a modified ver- In some trials, there was no net motion, total activity from a random sample of left-
sion of Newsomes original experimental so the monkeys had to make choices in the signaling neurons is greater than that of a
design. In the original experiments, the absence of any compelling evidence for sample of right-signaling neurons, so the
monkey was required to report the direc- either alternative. In other trials, monkeys monkey makes few errors. As the stimulus
tion of movement immediately after view- made errors, that is, a choice that contra- becomes less coherent, it evokes a weaker
ing the dots, but Kim and Shadlen dicted the evidence. In both cases, prefrontal leftward motion signal. As a result, there is
introduced a delay between the presentation neural activity during the delay period cor- an increased probability that the activity sig-
of the moving dots and the cue to make an responded to the choice and not to the evi- naling left will be less than the activity sig-
eye movement. After the moving dots dis- dence. This indicates that the prefrontal naling right, and so the monkey makes
appear, neurons in area MT cease to cortex neurons are signaling something more errors. When Kim and Shadlen incor-
respond, indicating that the decision must more than just the sensory evidence during porate values for the magnitude and vari-
be stored in some other area. Many studies the delay period. Based on their location and ability of the signals previously observed in
have shown that neurons in the prefrontal properties, it is likely that most of the neu- MT, they can account for the performance
cortex continue to fire during the delay peri- rons Kim and Shadlen recorded are not of the monkeys and the responses of the
od while the monkey remembers and plans directly involved in producing the eye move- prefrontal neurons.
what he is supposed to do7. By studying ment. Thus, Kim and Shadlen reason, the Through research like that of Kim and
activity in the prefrontal cortex during the neurons are encoding the decision in an Shadlen, neuroscience engages significant
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

delay period, the authors could distinguish abstract sense. Similar observations have philosophical and ethical issues9. For exam-
the representation of the decision having been made in posterior parietal cortex8 and ple, if the neural events leading to a choice
been made from the representation of the the superior colliculus (G.D. Horwitz & become publicly observable, then it should
stimulus on which it was based. W.T. Newsome, Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 24, be possible to predict the choices made by
In the original studies of area MT, the 1498, 1998). an agent if the correct brain processes are
randomly moving dots were placed in the Although prefrontal cortex has been monitored. This is not philosophical fiction.
neurons receptive field (so the neural studied before in conditions requiring an I have had the profound experience of being
response was to the dots themselves). For arbitrary response based on a visual dis- able to predict with high reliability the
the present study of prefrontal neurons, the crimination7, one major advance in Kim choice a monkey would make during a
targets for the eye movements were instead and Shadlens study is the use of a stimulus study of binocular rivalry10, and Shadlen
placed in the neurons receptive field. Thus, with discriminability that could be manip- reports the same experience during this
when a prefrontal cortex neuron fires in ulated in a psychophysical protocol. Anoth- study (personal communication). It seems
response to the moving dots, it does so not er important aspect was the use of the reasonable to assume that the same types of
because the dots themselves are within the random dot motion stimuli, for which the mechanisms may underlie other types of
receptive field (they are not), but because sensory representation was very well char- decisions, in our own brains as well as those
the monkey has been conditioned to inter- acterized. This provided for what may be of monkeys. Ultimately, philosophical argu-
pret a particular pattern of moving dots as the most important and innovative aspect ments have been developed that reconcile a
an instruction to make an eye movement to of the study, a quantitative model of the mechanistic explanation of decision mak-
the receptive field of that neuron. decision process. ing, one of the most sacrosanct of mental
Kim and Shadlen show that during the Presumably the prefrontal activity that acts, with our own sense of free will, respon-
period when the moving dots are visible, the authors observe must be driven by sibility and dignity11.
neural activity grows in prefrontal cortex to responses in MT during the motion view-
1. Schall, J. D. & Bichot, N. P. Curr. Opin.
signal one or the other choice. Important- ing period. The authors did not record from Neurobiol. 8, 211217 (1998).
ly, the rate of growth and magnitude of MT in these experiments, but based on ear- 2. Leon, M. I. & Shadlen, M. N. Neuron 21,
activity during the viewing period was pro- lier modeling of MT responses6, they use 669672 (1998).
portional to the quality of the evidence, that signal-detection theory to suggest a model 3. Schall, J. D. & Thompson, K. G. Annu. Rev.
is, to the strength of the motion signal. The for how the prefrontal cortex reads the evi- Neurosci. 22, 241259 (1999).
authors therefore propose that the gradual dence from MT. In simple terms, their 4. Parker, A. J. & Newsome, W. T. Annu. Rev.
increase in prefrontal activity reflects the model supposes that a neuron in prefrontal Neurosci. 21, 227277 (1998).
accumulation of sensory evidence. Although cortex monitors the activity of two popula- 5. Britten, K. H., Newsome, W. T., Shadlen, M. N.,
this interpretation is plausible, we should tions of MT neurons, each signaling motion Celebrini, S. & Movshon, J. A. Vis. Neurosci. 13,
87100 (1996).
not overlook alternative explanations; for in opposite directions, say left and right.
6. Shadlen, M. N., Britten, K. H., Newsome, W. T.
instance, the activity may reflect the prepa- However, the motion signals in MT are ran- & Movshon, J. A. J. Neurosci. 16, 14861510
ration of the eye movement response, or the domly variable across trials. To decide which (1996).
monkeys growing expectation that it will direction the dots are moving, they propose 7. Schall, J. D. in Extrastriate Cortex of Primates,
earn a reward. Distinguishing these alterna- that the input to the prefrontal cortex neu- Vol. 12 Cerebral Cortex (eds. Rockland, K,
tives will require not only further empirical ron represents a random sample of the Peters, A. & Kaas, J.) 527638 (Plenum, New
York, 1997).
work but also operational clarification of the activity of each population. Suppose the net
8. Shadlen, M. N. & Newsome, W. T. Proc. Natl.
concepts of preparation, expectation and so motion is leftward. If the MT activity sig- Acad. Sci. USA 93, 628633 (1996).
on. What is clear from the data, however, is naling left exceeds the activity signaling
9. Editorial. Nat. Neurosci. 1, 535 (1998).
that prefrontal neurons reach a state that right, then the monkey chooses left. How-
10. Logothetis, N. K. & Schall J. D. Science 245,
predicts the monkeys choice while the mov- ever, if by chance the activity signaling left 761763 (1989).
ing dots are visible and maintain that state happens to be less than the activity signal-
11. Dennett, D. C. Elbow Room: The Varieties of Free
throughout the delay period when no dots ing right, then the monkey will make an Will Worth Wanting (MIT Press, Cambridge,
are present until the monkey is given the error. When a coherent motion stimulus MA, 1984).

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 109


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

obituary

Sir Alan Lloyd Hodgkin 1914 1998


by Denis Baylor and King-Wai Yau

Sir Alan Hodgkin, one of the great physi- have several heroes, but Alan is the one I
ologists of the twentieth century, died at always try to imitate. His unparalleled
Cambridge, England on December 20, analytic powers grew out of formidable
1998. He was 84. mathematical skills, largely self-taught, as
He was born in Banbury, England and well as a remarkable intuition. Whereas
educated at Greshams School, Holt, and some think of him as a theorist because
Trinity College, Cambridge. Early in his of the quantitative flavor of many of his
career, his scientific genius manifested itself papers, he was in reality a consummate
in a now-classic piece of work demon- experimentalist. He relished good mea-
strating that conduction of the nerve surements and enjoyed complete immer-
impulse requires the spread of electrical sion in them, down to the smallest details.
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

current between active and inactive regions When experimental results were in con-
of the nerve fiber. During the Second flict with a theory, he immediately reject-
World War, he made key contributions to ed the theory and started over, no matter

Godfrey Argent
the development of radar for night fight- how elegant the theory might have
ers, and although a biologist, he became seemed. In spite of his hunger to under-
Principal Scientific Officer for radar devel- stand nature, he did not try to force an
opment by 1944. This work, as well as his answer when there wasnt one.
earlier life and subsequent career, is detailed For Hodgkin, science was an intensely
in his book Chance and Design, Reminis- behave has proved to be remarkably pre- personal experience to be enjoyed in all its
cences of Science in Peace and War. scient and robust. aspects. He therefore preferred to work on
In 19371938, he spent a year in the Subsequently, Hodgkin turned his a single project at a time and to have a very
United States at the Rockefeller Institute attention to the sodium pump and small research group. His insistence on tak-
for Medical Research and became showed that its function depends on the ing on a fair share of all aspects of the work
acquainted with K. S. Cole, who intro- consumption of ATP. He discovered that was itself an inspiration. Discussions of the
duced him to the voltage-clamp method. potassium ions move through potassium latest results were serious and focused, and
In the late 1940s, Hodgkin, Andrew Huxley channels in single file and characterized although he was patient with coworkers
and Bernard Katz used this method in a the membrane conductances that under- and receptive to their ideas, he was impa-
brilliant series of experiments that eluci- lie excitability in muscle fibers. He also tient with error and sloppy thought. He
dated the ionic mechanisms underlying the made the first direct measurements of the had an unerring sense of what would stand
nerve impulse. These experiments, along activity of the sodiumcalcium exchanger, the test of time and what would fall by the
with an elegant and original quantitative which is an important pathway for calci- wayside. His negative impression, indicat-
description of the voltage-dependent mem- um extrusion in cells. ed by a characteristic wrinkling of the nose,
brane conductances, remain a landmark in In the early 1960s, Hodgkin collaborat- was a bad omen indeed.
our understanding of how neurons work. ed with Michaelangelo Fuortes in a study Hodgkin received many other honors,
For this work, Hodgkin and Huxley were of the light response of photoreceptor cells including Knight of the British Empire,
awarded the Nobel Prize for Medicine or in the eye of the horseshoe crab Limulus. the Order of Merit and membership in
Physiology in 1963 (shared with John From a kinetic analysis of the light the Royal Society as well as many foreign
Eccles). The ion channels whose existence response, they made the then-heretical sug- academies. While serving as President of
Hodgkin and Huxley inferred have now gestion that light activated ionic conduc- the Royal Society and Master of Trinity
been purified, cloned and sequenced and tances in the membrane via a series of College, Cambridge, he managed to keep
form a standard part of biological doctrine chemical reactions. These types of bio- his research going at full throttle. In spite
taught at the high school level. Hodgkins chemical cascades continue to be studied of his many accomplishments and hon-
and Huxleys picture of how ion channels to the present day. The collaboration with ors, he remained a modest person with
Fuortes sparked his interest in vision, simple tastes. He had a delightful dry
Denis Baylor is at the Department of which he studied for the rest of his career. sense of humor.
Neurobiology, Stanford University School of Between 1970 and the late 1980s, when he His professional success and enjoyment
Medicine, 299 Campus Drive, Room D200, retired, his laboratory pioneered biophys- of life owed much to a wonderfully sup-
Stanford, California 94305-5125, USA. ical investigations of signal transduction in portive family. His devoted wife Marion
email: dbaylor@leland.stanford.edu retinal rods and cones, again setting new (Marni), the daughter of Nobel laureate
King-Wai Yau is at the Department of standards for experimentation and analysis. Peyton Rous, had prior knowledge of the
Neuroscience and the Howard Hughes Medical For many scientists, particularly those demands of the scientific way and backed
Institute, Johns Hopkins University School of who had the privilege of knowing and Hodgkin in all things. He is survived by
Medicine, 725 N. Wolfe Street, Baltimore, working with him, Hodgkin represented Marni and four accomplished children.
Maryland 21205, USA. the epitome of scientific excellence. A Scientists of Hodgkins talent and dis-
email: kwyau@welchlink.welch.jhu.edu renowned physiologist once remarked, I tinction are rare indeed.

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 111


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

scientific correspondence

Neurotrophin-3 is a number of tissues, but the NT-32lox allele was almost fully
recombined in the CNS (Fig. 2b) and in the germ line of mice
required for proper that carried the nestin-cre transgene (data not shown). There-
fore, to generate NT-3 conditional mutants, NT-32lox/+ males
cerebellar development bearing the nestin-cre transgene were crossed to NT-3 2lox/+
nontransgenic females. Conditional mutants generated from
this cross carried the transgene and had the genotype NT-
Brian Bates1, Maribel Rios1,3, Andreas Trumpp2,3, 31lox/2lox at the NT-3 locus. Conditional mutants were produced
Cindy Chen1, Guoping Fan1, J. Michael Bishop2 in the expected numbers and were fertile (data not shown),
and Rudolf Jaenisch1 indicating that the conditional mutation did not lead to peri-
natal lethality, unlike the NT-3-null mutation8. Northern blot-
1 Whitehead Institute for Biomedical Research, 9 Cambridge Center, ting revealed no NT-3 mRNA in the brain and developing
Cambridge, Massachusetts 02142, USA cerebellum of conditional mutants (Fig. 2c and d), demon-
2 G.W. Hooper Foundation, University of California, San Francisco, California strating that the NT-32lox allele was fully recombined and no
94143, USA NT-3 was produced.
3 These authors contributed equally to this work Because NT-3 is highly expressed during postnatal cere-
bellar development2,6 (see also Fig. 2d) and can influence the
Neurotrophin-3 (NT-3) is a member of the neurotrophin fam- survival and differentiation of developing cerebellar neurons2,
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

ily, which includes nerve growth factor (NGF), brain-derived we examined the development of the cerebellum in the NT-3
neurotrophic factor (BDNF) and neurotrophin-4/5 (NT-4/5). conditional mutant. Like BDNF mutants3, NT-3 conditional
These factors are crucial for development of the peripheral mutants had defective cerebellar foliation at P8, distinguished
nervous system1, but not the central nervous system (CNS), by the absence of folium VII and the posterior superior fis-
except that NT-3 and BDNF have been implicated in the post- sure, which separates it from folium VI (Fig. 3a and b). This
natal development of the cerebellum2,3. Here we created a con- defect was not simply due to a delay in folding, as folium VII
ditional NT-3-deficient mutant, which showed abnormal was also absent at P28 and later (data not shown). Formation
cerebellar morphology. of the characteristic layers of the cerebellum and their asso-
Postnatal cerebellar development, which involves active ciated cell types seemed to be unaffected in the conditional
proliferation and migration of neuronal precursors, is com-
plete by postnatal day 21 (P21) in the mouse 4. During this
period, the neurotrophins BDNF and NT-3 are highly a
expressed in the cerebellum5,6. BDNF-deficient mutants gen-
erally die within the first two weeks of life, but cerebellar devel-
opment can be assessed during this time window 7 . NT-3
-deficient mutants, on the other hand, rarely survive past P1
(ref. 8), preventing a meaningful assessment of postnatal cere-
bellar development. To circumvent this problem, we used the
phage P1 cre recombinase-loxP system9 to create an NT-3 con-
ditional mutant that was viable, yet lacked NT-3 expression b
in the CNS.
A targeting vector (Fig. 1a) designed to introduce loxP sites
around the NT-3 coding exon (exon II)10 was transfected into
embryonic stem cells. The selection cassette was removed from
homologously targeted clones by transient transfection of a cre
recombinase expression vector, leaving loxP sites surrounding
exon II (Fig. 1c; data not shown). Embryonic stem cells carry- c
ing this new allele of NT-3, referred to as NT-32lox, were used to
generate a mouse line (data not shown). Mice bearing the NT-
32lox allele were intercrossed, generating viable and fertile NT-
32lox/2lox homozygotes, without behavioral abnormalities (data
not shown). To delete NT-3 in the CNS, we crossed NT-32lox/+
mice to a transgenic strain bearing the cre recombinase gene
expressed under the control of the rat nestin promoter/intron Fig. 1. Targeting strategy and Cre mediated recombination. (a) NT-3
2 enhancer (A.T., unpublished data). Nestin-cre-mediated genomic locus (NT-3+), with the homologous targeting vector
recombination resulted in the deletion of NT-3 exon II, leav- depicted below. Filled triangles indicate loxP sites. Hatched rectangle
ing a single loxP site remaining in the genome (Fig 1c). As exon indicates the cytomegalovirus-promoter-driven hygror/thymidine
II contains the entire coding sequence of the protein, this kinase fusion gene used for positive/negative selection. Probes used
NT-31lox allele is equivalent to the NT-3-null mutation8. South- for detection of integration and recombination indicated above.
(b) Structure of NT-3 genomic locus after homologous integration of
ern blot analysis detected cre-mediated recombination in whole
the targeting vector. This new allele is referred to as NT-33lox. (c) Top,
embryos as early as 9.5 days post coitum (E9.5). Recombina- NT-3 allele resulting from cre-mediated recombination removing the
tion increased during embryonic development such that it was selection cassette, referred to as NT-32lox. Bottom, the result of cre-
nearly complete in brain and spinal cord by E15.5, whereas mediated recombination of NT-32lox, referred to as NT-31lox. For (ac),
other tissues showed a much smaller extent of recombination restriction sites (B, BamHI; S, Spe I; Xb, Xba I; C, Cla I; Xh, Xho I) are
(Fig. 2a). In adults, incomplete recombination was detected in shown, with the resulting fragment sizes given in kilobases (kb).

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 115


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

scientific correspondence

Fig. 2. Cre-mediated recombination and expression of NT-3 in the


a conditional mutant. (a) Southern blot of embryonically derived sam-
ples that carried the nestin-cre transgene and had the genotype
NT-32lox/+ at the NT-3 locus. NT-3+, NT-32lox and NT-31lox bands are
indicated. Note the near-complete conversion of NT-32lox into
NT-31lox by E15.5 in the brain and spinal cord. Whole emb., whole
embryo; sp.cord, spinal cord; carcass refers to whole embryo minus
head, liver, heart and limbs. (b) Southern blot showing recombina-
tion in an NT-32lox/+ adult. Rem. brain is the portion of the brain
remaining after dissection into the other brain regions shown. Sal.
gl., salivary gland; olf. bulb, olfactory bulb; cereb., cerebellum; cor-
b tex, cerebral cortex; sp. cord, spinal cord. (c) Northern blot com-
paring NT-3 mRNA in the whole brain of wild-type (w) and
conditional mutant (m) mice. 28s and 18s indicate the position of the
rRNA markers. Blot stripped and reprobed with GAPDH shown
below. (d) Northern blot comparing NT-3 mRNA in cerebella of
wild-type (w) and conditional mutant (m) mice at various postnatal
time points. Ad, adult. rRNA loading control shown below.

c
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

d
During cerebellar development, excess granule neurons are
produced and subsequently lost via apoptotic cell death. In
the mouse, the peak of apoptosis in the cerebellum occurs
around P8 (ref. 11). At this age, TUNEL staining showed that
conditional mutants had 75% higher density of apoptotic cells
in the granule cell layer (mutant mean standard error,
37.3 11.4 cells/mm 2 ; wild-type, 21.3 6.98 cells/mm 2 ;
p < 0.0001, unpaired students t-test) During this time, both
astrocytes and granule neurons are undergoing apoptosis in
the granule cell layer12. However, granule neurons express the
NT-3 receptor TrkC5, and their survival in vivo is enhanced
mutant (Fig 3c and d). The external germinal layer, site of by NT-3 (ref. 2), suggesting that this increase in apoptosis is
granule cell precursor proliferation, was present at P8. Cells due to granule neuron death. We observed no increase in
in the proliferative zone of this layer incorporated 5-bromod- TUNEL-positive cells in the external germinal layer, suggest-
eoxyuridine at P8 in wild-type mice and in conditional ing that granule neuron precursors did not require NT-3
mutants (Fig. 3e and f). NT-3 has been implicated in differ- for survival (mutant, 4.1 0.31 cells/mm; wild type,
entiation of developing Purkinje neurons2. Purkinje neurons 3.8 0.26 cells/mm; p = 0.38, unpaired students t-test).
were present in the vermis of the conditional mutant, and We have used the cre/loxP system to create a conditional
although not quantified, they did not appear to be reduced in mutation that eliminated NT-3 expression in the CNS but did
number. The elaboration of the Purkinje dendritic tree, as not cause early postnatal death. The absence of NT-3 in the
revealed by staining with an anti-calbindin D28 antibody, also developing CNS caused abnormal cerebellar development,
appeared qualitatively normal (Fig. 3g and h). providing evidence that it is required for the correct forma-

a b e f

g h
c d

Fig. 3. Conditional mutants have aberrant cerebellar foliation but normal layering, external germinal layer proliferation and Purkinje arboriza-
tion. (ad) Paraffin-embedded, cresyl-violet-stained midsagittal sections through the cerebellar vermis of wild-type (a, c) and mutant mice (b,
d) at P8. Triangle in (b) marks the area of the absent folium VII in the mutant. (c, d) Higher magnification showing the characteristic layers of
the cerebellum at P8. EGL, external germinal layer; ML, molecular layer; PCL, Purkinje cell layer; GL, granule cell layer. Scale bars in (a) and (b),
500 m; in (c) and (d), 40 m. (e, f) Granule cell precursor proliferation assessed by injection of 5-bromodeoxyuridine (BrdU). Incorporation
of BrdU is detected by immunohistochemistry using an antibody to BrdU in mutant (f) and wild-type mice (e). Scale bars, 20 m.
(g, h) Purkinje cells detected with an antibody to Calbindin D28. The mutant (h) appears qualitatively similar to wild-type (g) at P8.

116 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

scientific correspondence

tion of at least some components of the CNS. Indeed, this B.B. also thanks Jeanne Reis and Noah Duffey for technical support and
mutation was also associated with behavioral abnormalities, Scharam Akbarian, Larry Schwartz and Kuo Fen Lee for comments on the
such as increased gait width and impaired balance (data not manuscript. This research was supported by a grant from NIH/NCI.
shown), consistent with a role for NT-3 in the development
and maintainence of the cerebellar system. The finding that RECEIVED 29 O CTOBER; ACCEPTED 18 DECEMBER 1998
cell death was increased in the granule cell layer further sug-
gests that NT-3 may act as a survival factor for a subset of
1. Conover, J. C. & Yancopoulos, G. D. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 1327 (1997).
granule neurons at P8. Because granule neurons express both 2. Neveu, I. & Arenas, E. J. Cell Biol. 133, 631646 (1996).
NT-3 and TrkC2,5,6, the factor may be acting in an autocrine or 3. Schwartz, P. M., Borghesani, P. R., Levy, R. L., Pomeroy, S. L. & Segal, R.
paracrine fashion. NT-3 is also expressed in the adult cere- A. Neuron 19, 269281 (1997).
4. Goldwitz, D. & Hamre, K. Trends Neurosci. 21, 375382 (1998).
bellum6(see also Fig. 2d), suggesting that it may be involved 5. Ernfors, P., Merlio, J. P. & Persson, H. Eur. J. Neurosci. 4, 11401158
in adult cerebellar function and long-term neuronal survival. (1992).
Apart from the well characterized activity of NT-3 as a neu- 6. Rocamora, N., Garcia-Ladona, F. J., Palacios, J. M. & Mengod, G. Mol.
Brain Res. 17, 18 (1993).
ronal survival factor, it has other effects on CNS neurons, 7. Ernfors, P., Lee, K. F. & Jaenisch, R. Nature 368, 147150 (1994).
including dendritic arborization2,13 and synaptic plasticity14. 8. Ernfors, P., Lee, K. F., Kucera, J. & Jaenisch, R. Cell 77, 503512 (1994).
This conditional mutant therefore will allow the exploration of 9. Gu, H., Marth, J. D., Orban, P. C., Mossmann, H. & Rajewsky, K. Science
265, 103106 (1994).
other NT-3 functions in the adult CNS, as well as the effica- 10. Leingartner, A. & Lindholm, D. Eur. J. Neurosci. 6, 11491159 (1994).
cy of NT-3 substitution therapy. 11. Wood, K. A., Dipasquale, B. & Youle, R. J. Neuron 11, 621632 (1993).
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

12. Krueger, B. K., Burne, J. F. & Raff, M. C. J. Neurosci. 15, 33663374


(1995).
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS 13. McAllister, A. K., Katz, L. C. & Lo, D. C. Neuron 18, 767778 (1997).
The authors thank Peggy Lee and Steven OGormann for providing reagents. 14. Kang, H. & Schuman, E. M. Science 267, 16581662 (1995).

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 117


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Snapin: a SNARE-associated protein


implicated in synaptic transmission
Jeffrey M. Ilardi1, Sumiko Mochida2 and Zu-Hang Sheng1

1 Synaptic Function Unit, National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, National Institutes of Health, Building 36, Room 5A23,
9000 Rockville Pike, Bethesda, Maryland 20892, USA
2 Department of Physiology, Tokyo Medical University, Tokyo 1608402, Japan
Correspondence should be addressed to Z.-H.S. (zsheng@codon.nih.gov)

Synaptic vesicle docking and fusion are mediated by the assembly of a stable SNARE core complex of
proteins, which include the synaptic vesicle membrane protein VAMP/synaptobrevin and the
plasmalemmal proteins syntaxin and SNAP-25. We have now identified another SNAP-25-binding pro-
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

tein, called Snapin. Snapin was enriched in neurons and exclusively located on synaptic vesicle
membranes. It associated with the SNARE complex through direct interaction with SNAP-25. Binding
of recombinant Snapin-CT to SNAP-25 blocked the association of the SNARE complex with synaptotag-
min. Introduction of Snapin-CT and peptides containing the SNAP-25 binding sequence into presynap-
tic superior cervical ganglion neurons in culture reversibly inhibited synaptic transmission. These
results suggest that Snapin is an important component of the neurotransmitter release process
through its modulation of the sequential interactions between the SNAREs and synaptotagmin.

The molecular mechanisms that underlie calcium-dependent protein with a calculated molecular mass of 15 kDa (Fig. 1a).
exocytosis have been successfully investigated by identifying pro- The start codon of the open reading frame was consistent with
teins that are involved in the vesicle docking/fusion process at Kozak consensus sequences13. EST databank searches identified
presynaptic nerve terminals and analyzing their interactions13. a murine Snapin homologue that shares 98% amino-acid
Synaptic vesicle docking and fusion at release sites require the sequence identity with human Snapin. Analysis of the Snapin
association of proteins on both vesicle and plasma membranes. sequence indicated that it is composed of an amino-terminal
The SNARE core complex is a biochemical intermediate essen- hydrophobic segment characteristic of a transmembrane domain
tial for vesicular transport and/or fusion processes. It consists and a carboxyl-terminal region with the potential (p = 0.62) to
of the synaptic-vesicle-associated protein synaptobrevin/VAMP form a coiled-coil structure, suggesting a role in mediating synap-
and the plasma-membrane-associated proteins syntaxin and tic proteinprotein interactions14 (Fig. 1b).
SNAP-25 (synaptosome-associated protein-25; refs 410). Immunoblot analysis using a polyclonal antibody raised
Although many classes of exocytosis-related proteins have been against the carboxyl terminal peptide sequence from mouse
identified to date, the number and identity of the essential and Snapin showed that a band of mass 15 kDa (as expected from
regulatory proteins involved remains unclear. The identification the open reading frame of Snapin) was readily detected in rat
of neuron-specific regulators or molecular switches involved in brain and synaptosome preparations and was barely present in
the assembly and disassembly of the ternary SNARE complex, other tissues (Fig. 2a). Moreover, by immunoblotting various
and its structural coupling to a calcium sensor during synaptic homogenates, we found Snapin to be widely present in all twelve
vesicle docking and calcium-regulatory fusion processes is crit- anatomically and functionally distinct areas of rat brain includ-
ical to understanding the molecular mechanisms underlying ing cortex, cerebellum, hippocampus and spinal cord (data not
neurotransmitter release and synaptic plasticity. shown). To determine where Snapin is localized within neurons,
rat cerebral synaptosomes were fractionated into cytosolic,
RESULTS synaptic vesicle and synaptic plasma membrane fractions, and
Cloning and subcellular localization of Snapin then analyzed by immunoblotting. Snapin protein was exclu-
To identify regulatory proteins in synaptic vesicle exocytosis that sively associated with the synaptic vesicle fraction and absent
bind to components of the SNARE complex, we used SNAP-25 from the cytosolic and plasma membrane fractions (Fig. 2b),
(ref. 7) as a molecular bait and screened a human brain cDNA consistent with the prediction of an amino-terminal hydropho-
library via yeast two-hybrid selection11. Positive prey clones were bic transmembrane segment from structural analysis. The vesi-
rescued and re-transformed into fresh yeast cells with SNAP-25 cle fraction also contained the majority of immunoreactivity
bait or various control baits. The specificity of the bait and prey corresponding to VAMP-2, synaptophysin and synaptotagmin,
interactions was then confirmed by transactivation assays of - markers of synaptic vesicles. As controls, neither Na+/K+-
galactosidase induction and histidine autotrophy. We isolated ATPase, a marker of the plasma membrane, nor lactate dehydro-
three classes of complementary DNAs, encoding syntaxin 1A5,6, genase (LDH), a marker of the cytosolic fraction, were detected
Hrs-2 (ref. 12) and a new protein called Snapin, which interact- in the synaptic vesicle fraction. Syntaxin-1 and SNAP-25 were
ed selectively with the SNAP-25 bait. The predicted open read- present primarily in the presynaptic plasma membrane frac-
ing frame for human Snapin cDNA encodes a 136-amino acid tions and to a lesser extent in synaptic vesicle fractions.

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 119


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 1. Molecular cloning of Snapin. a


(a) Alignment of the predicted amino-acid
sequences of human and mouse Snapins.
Residues that differ between the two iso-
forms are indicated by bold letters. The
two sequences are 98% identical at the
amino-acid level. The GenBank accession
numbers for human and mouse Snapin are
AF086837 and AF086838, respectively. b
(b) Domain structure of Snapin. A 37-
amino-acid predicted coiled-coil domain
(CC) and an amino-terminal hydrophobic
segment (HS) are indicated.

Biochemical extraction experiments showed that Snapin was Snapin interacts preferentially with SNAP-25 in a saturable and
resistant to extraction from membrane by either high salt con- stoichiometric manner, and suggest that they may interact in vivo.
centration (up to 1 M) or extreme pH (3 and 11; data not Using anti-SNAP-25 affinity-column chromatography, we
shown), whereas it was soluble in HEPES buffer with 1% Triton examined whether Snapin is a SNAP-25- and/or SNARE-com-
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

X-100. These data suggest that Snapin behaves as an integral, plex-associated protein in the rat synaptosome preparation.
vesicle-associated, membrane protein. Snapin, along with other components of the SNARE complex,
was coimmunoprecipitated by the anti-SNAP-25 antibody but
Association of Snapin with SNARE complex not by normal control IgG, as demonstrated with both
Because the yeast two-hybrid system can detect low-affinity inter- Coomassie blue staining and sequential immunoblot analysis
actions that may not occur in vitro and in vivo, we tried to con- on the same membrane using antibodies to synaptotagmin I,
firm the selective and direct interaction between Snapin and syntaxin 1, SNAP-25, VAMP2, and Snapin (Fig. 4a). Under the
SNAP-25 using in-vitro binding assays with recombinant pro- conditions used for this experiment, these proteins were copre-
teins. Whereas His-tagged Snapin bound strongly to GST-SNAP- cipitated at molar ratios of approximately 1:1:0.3:0.3 (syn-
25, no binding was detectable to GST alone or to other synaptic taxin:SNAP-25:VAMP2:Snapin). The relative low
proteins tested including syntaxin-1A, SNAP-23 (ref. 15), stoichiometry of the complex may reflect the presence of abun-
VAMP-2 (ref. 4), and the cytoplasmic domain (amino acids dant dimers of syntaxin-SNAP-25 in vivo that are not bound to
80421, ref 16) of synaptotagmin 1 (Fig. 3a). SNAP-25 binding to the vesicle-membrane-associated proteins VAMP2 and Snapin.
Snapin saturated at 100 nM, a 5-fold molar excess of the soluble Furthermore, all SNARE components, including syntaxin,
His-Snapin over the immobilized GST-SNAP-25 (Fig. 3b). His- SNAP-25, VAMP2 and synaptotagmin, were co-precipitated by
Snapin bound to GST-SNAP-25 at a stoichiometry of 1:1, as the anti-Snapin antibody (data not shown). Anti-VAMP2 and
observed on Coomassie-blue-stained gels (data not shown). Thus, anti-syntaxin antibodies also co-precipitated Snapin (data not
both in-vitro binding and yeast two-hybrid analysis indicate that shown). This indicates that Snapin is a SNAP-25-binding pro-
tein that forms a newly discovered
complex by association with Syntaxin,
a b SNAP-25 and VAMP2.
Yeast two-hybrid selection and in-
vitro binding studies (Fig. 3) showed
that the carboxyl-terminal half of
Snapin (Snapin-CT, amino acids
79136) is sufficient for the interaction
with SNAP-25. To investigate whether
Snapin-CT could compete with the
full-length Snapin (Snapin-FL) for
binding to SNAP-25, we did in-vitro
Fig. 2. The tissue and subcellular distribution of Snapin.
(a) Tissue distribution of Snapin protein. Rat tissue
binding competition studies with
homogenates (50 g protein per lane) and solubilized recombinant proteins. GST-SNAP-25
synaptosome preparation (10 g per lane) were was bound to glutathione-Sepharose
immunoblotted with the Snapin antibody. Bands were visu- beads and incubated with a constant
alized by ECL. (b) Distribution of Snapin in subcellular frac- concentration of Snapin-FL and
tions of rat brain synaptosomes. Equal amounts (8 g) of increasing concentrations of Snapin-
synaptosome fractions enriched in presynaptic cytosol CT. The signal intensity of Snapin-FL
(PC), synaptic vesicles (SV), and plasma membrane (PM) diminished progressively while that of
were analyzed by SDS polyacrylamide gel electraphoresis Snapin-CT increased (Fig. 4b). These
(PAGE) and sequentially immunoblotted with antibodies as
results demonstrate that both Snapin-
indicated after stripping previous antibodies in the same
blot membrane. The subcellular localization of Snapin was FL and -CT indeed compete for the
determined by comparing it to markers for synaptic vesi- same binding region on SNAP-25.
cles (VAMP, synaptophysin and synaptotagmin), plasma To examine the cellular functions of
membrane (K/Na-ATPase, syntaxin, and SNAP-25), and Snapin, we repeated the immunopre-
cytoplasm (lactate dehydrogenase, LDH). cipitation of Snapin with the SNARE

120 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 3. In-vitro interaction of Snapin with


SNAP-25. (a) Binding of Snapin-CT a b

Snapin bound (pixels, 100)


(79136) to SNAP-25 in vitro. GST or GST
fusion proteins (approximately 1 g each)
were immobilized on glutathione-Sepharose
and then incubated with His-tagged Snapin.
Bound protein complexes were eluted from
the matrix and separated by SDS-PAGE, and
immunoblotted with anti-His and anti-GST
antibodies. (b) Recombinant Snapin-CT
(79136) binds SNAP-25 in a saturable man-
ner. Approximately 1 g GST-SNAP-25
immobilized on glutathione-Sepharose was
incubated with increasing amounts of His-
Snapin as indicated. The bound Snapin was His-Snapin (nM)
detected by immunoblotting, and binding
intensities were quantified using NIH Image
based on standard curves.
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

complex and synaptotagmin from brain synaptosomes (Fig. 4a) excess recombinant Snapin-FL might increase formation of the
in the presence of excess exogenous, bacterially expressed SNARE-Snapin complex. Thus, the opposite effects of full-
Snapin-FL or Snapin-CT. We found that both recombinant length and C-terminal recombinant Snapin on the interaction
Snapin-CT and Snapin-FL coprecipitated with the SNARE of SNAREs with synaptotagmin suggest that Snapin is required
complex, suggesting that Snapin, syntaxin and VAMP-2 interact for high-affinity synaptotagminSNARE interaction.
simultaneously with SNAP-25 and do not compete with each
other (Fig. 4c). Interestingly, coincubation of synaptosomes The functional effect of Snapin on synaptic transmission
with excess Snapin-CT (containing the coiled-coil domain and To assess the physiological function of Snapin, we examined its
presumably competing with endogenous Snapin for SNAP-25 role in synaptic transmission at the well characterized choliner-
binding) prevented the association of synaptotagmin with the gic synapses formed among superior cervical ganglion neurons
SNARE complex even in the presence of 200 M free calcium (SCGN) in culture 1719. We injected the carboxyl terminal
(Fig. 4c). Conversely, Snapin-FL significantly enhanced the domain of mouse Snapin (Snapin-CT) into the presynaptic
association of synaptotagmin with the SNARE complex, as SCGNs to determine whether it would disrupt the interaction of
compared with the addition of a 6His-peptide control. We the SNARE core complex with Snapin in vivo. This synapse is an
observed that Snapin was less abundant in synaptosomes than ideal system for these experiments for three reasons. First, pro-
syntaxin and SNAP-25 (data not shown), and the addition of teins or peptides can be introduced into the relatively large

Fig. 4. Association of Snapin with


the SNARE complex in vivo. a c
(a) Coimmunoprecipitation of
Snapin with the SNARE complex.
Immunoprecipitates with anti-
SNAP-25 antibody or control IgG
were analyzed with Coomassie
blue staining (ST, right panel) or
by sequential immunoblotting (IB,
left panel). (b) Binding competi-
tion between Snapin-FL and
Snapin-CT. Immobilized GST-
SNAP-25 (100 nM) was incubated
with equal concentrations of
recombinant Snapin-FL (250 nM)
and increasing concentrations of
Snapin-CT as indicated. Bound
b
proteins were visualized by
sequentially immunoblotting with
both anti-His and anti-GST anti-
bodies. (c) Effects of recombinant
Snapin on the association of
synaptotagmin with SNARE com-
plex. Anti-syntaxin (-stx)
antibody immunoprecipitated
synaptotagmin with SNARE pro-
teins from rat brain synaptosomes in the presence or absence of His-Snapin-CT or His-Snapin-FL as indicated. Proteins bound were analyzed by
sequential immunoblotting. Solubilized synaptosomes (SS) were loaded in the left lane as a detection control for antibodies. Note that synapto-
tagmin association with the SNARE complex is blocked by 5 M His-Snapin-CT, and significantly increased by 5 M His-Snapin-FL.

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 121


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 5. Effects of Snapin-CT on synaptic


transmission of SCGNs in culture. a b

Normalized average EPSP amplitude


(a) Snapin-CT was introduced into the
presynaptic neuron by diffusion from a
suction pipette beginning when the mem- Denatured (n = 5)
brane was disrupted by applying suction at Normal (n = 5)
t = 0. The pipette concentration of Snapin-
CT was 50 M. Postsynaptic potentials
from one representative experiment
recorded 5 min before injection and 20
and 50 min after injection are illustrated.
(b) Normalized average excitatory post-
synaptic potentials (EPSPs) were plotted
from five experiments with Snapin-CT (l)
and heat-denatured Snapin-CT (L) at a
Time after injection (min)
concentration of 50 M.

(3040 m) presynaptic cell bodies by microinjection. Second, cellular fusion protein in the cell body and nerve terminal. In
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

the injected proteins can rapidly diffuse to the nerve terminals contrast, injection of 50 M heat-denatured Snapin-CT produced
forming synapses with adjacent neurons. Third, the effects on no significant decrease in EPSP amplitude during one hour of
stimulated release of acetylcholine can be accurately monitored by recording (0.9 8.6% at 30 min after injection, n = 5; Fig. 5b),
recording the excitatory postsynaptic potentials (EPSPs) evoked indicating that the inhibitory effect of Snapin-CT on synaptic
by action potentials in the presynaptic neurons1719. After a sta- transmission is dependent on the native conformation of Snapin-
ble period of control recording for 2030 min, 50 M recombi- CT. Although the peak of EPSPs was reduced by Snapin-CT, the
nant Snapin-CT was diffused into presynaptic neurons from a time course was not significantly changed (Fig. 5a), indicating
suction pipette (at t = 0) for 23 minutes. EPSP amplitude grad- that the kinetics of exocytosis were not affected by introduction
ually decreased over a period of 20 minutes (Fig. 5a and b). The of Snapin-CT. The simplest interpretation of our results is that
maximum decrease, 29 2.3 % (n = 5, mean standard error), inhibition of synaptic transmission is due to competitive block
was observed 2040 min after commencing the injection, and of the endogenous Snapin binding to SNARE complex by the
the EPSPs recovered to the control amplitude by 6080 min. We excess injected Snapin-CT and resultant decrease of the association
assume that recovery follows proteolytic degradation of the intra- of the SNAREsynaptotagmin complex. This interpretation is con-
sistent with the results from our in-vitro
biochemical studies, which showed that
a Snapin-CT interrupted the interaction
of synaptotagmin with the SNARE
complex (Fig. 4c).
Snapin protein is composed of two
b major domains: an amino-terminal
hydrophobic segment and a carboxyl-
terminal coiled-coil domain that is
d present in different families of vesicu-
H M2 (n = 5) lar fusion proteins, including SNAP-
Normalized average EPSP amplitude

R Sc (n = 6)
g W2 (n = 6) 25, and is the binding domain of
p W4 (2.5 mM, n = 6) SNARE components. To define the
l W4 (5 mM, n = 6)
minimal sequence and critical amino-
acid residues required for Snapins
c binding to SNAP-25 and interfering
with endogenous Snapin function,
four 20-mer peptide fragments (W1,
2, 3 and 4; Fig. 6a) corresponding to
the coiled-coil domain of mouse
Fig. 6. Inhibition of binding to SNAP-25 and Snapin were synthesized. They were
neurotransmitter release by the Snapin pep- tested for their ability to affect in vitro
tides. (a) Amino-acid sequences of four Time after injection (min) binding competition (Fig. 6b and c)
mouse Snapin peptides (W1W4), two and their ability to inhibit synaptic
point-mutated peptides (M1, M2) and one transmission between SCGNs in cul-
scrambled peptide (Sc). (b) Effects of pep- ture (Fig. 6d). W4 but not W1, W2 or
tides (50 M) on the binding of Snapin (0.6 M) to immobilized GST-SNAP-25. The binding was signifi-
W3 peptides markedly competed with
cantly inhibited by W4 and M1 but not by W1, W2, W3, M2 or Sc. (c) The concentration-dependent
inhibition of Snapin-FL (250 nM) binding to SNAP-25 (100 nM) by W4 peptide. (d) Differential effects of
recombinant Snapins ability to bind
W2, W4, Sc and M2 peptides on synaptic transmission of SCGNs in culture. Normalized average EPSPs to SNAP-25 (Fig. 6b and c) and block
were plotted from six experiments with 2.5 mM W2 (g), 2.5 mM W4 (p), 5 mM W4 (l)or 2.5 mM Sc the co-immunoprecipitation of native
(R) and from 5 experiments with 2.5 mM M2 (H) peptides. Peptides were introduced into presynaptic synaptotagmin with the SNARE com-
neurons at t = 0 by diffusion from a suction pipette. plex in synaptosomes (data not

122 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

shown). The same W4 peptide also inhibited neurotransmitter zones contributing to transmitter release, rather than the inherent
release (Fig. 6d). The maximal decrease in EPSP amplitude, inability of these molecules to fully block fast synaptic transmis-
34 6.8% (n = 6), was observed 1525 min after starting injec- sion. Similarly, in our SCGN experiments, the Snapin-CT protein
tion with a pipette containing 2.5 mM W4 peptide (Fig. 6d). and W4 peptide may not reach all the active zones in the presynap-
Inhibition 1.3-fold greater and prolonged was observed when a 2- tic terminals of cultured SCGNs that characteristically form an
fold higher pipette concentration of W4 peptide (5 mM) was used, extensive network of varicosities25. A 2-fold higher concentration
indicating that the effect of the W4 peptide was not near saturation of W4 peptide (5 mM) produced 1.3-fold greater and prolonged
in nerve terminals. The maximum decrease in EPSP amplitude, inhibition (Fig. 6d), indicating that the effect of W4 peptide was
44 4.2% (n = 6), was observed 4050 min after injection. W2, not near saturation in nerve terminals. Alternatively, the partial
used as a representative of the other peptides, reduced synaptic inhibition we observed probably reflects a modulatory role of
transmission to a lesser extent (15 1.8%, n = 6, mean of maxi- Snapin during synaptic transmission rather than its acting as an
mal decrease in EPSP amplitude in six individual experiments; essential factor.
Fig. 6d). This suggests that the sequence of the W2 peptide is not Together, our results suggest that Snapin regulates a step
sufficient to compete with recombinant or native Snapin for bind- between vesicle docking and neurotransmitter release through
ing to SNAP-25 in vitro, but may interfere with endogenous Snapin its ability to potentiate the interaction of synaptotagmin with
for functional interaction with SNARE complex in nerve termi- the SNAREs, which then leads to the final fusion step triggered
nals. Sc, a peptide with an amino-acid composition identical to by calcium influx into nerve terminals through voltage-depen-
that of W4, but with a scrambled sequence, had no effect on the dent calcium channels. Alternatively, Snapin, as a v-SNARE,
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

binding of Snapin to SNAP-25 (Fig. 6b), on the block of synapto- could participate in vesicle docking at the release sites through
tagmin interacting with SNARE complex (data not shown) or on its specific interaction with the plasma-membrane-associated
transmitter release (Fig. 6d). Moreover, replacing three central protein SNAP-25 (t-SNARE). This interaction may explain the
residues of the W4 peptide with the positively charged amino acids finding that synaptic vesicles are capable of docking even when
Lys or Arg (Leu124Lys, Asp125Lys and Val128Arg), designed to another pair of SNAREs, synaptobrevin/VAMP (v-SNARE) and
destabilize the coiled-coil structure and interfere with syntaxin (t-SNARE) are deleted26 or cleaved by specific prote-
proteinprotein contact, yielded a peptide (M2) inactive in either olytic neurotoxins27, 28.
competition of binding or inhibition of synaptic transmission. Regulation of exocytosis is necessary for proper synaptic func-
These results suggest that these three residues of Snapin are critical tion. Our findings add another possible target of protein interac-
to maintaining an active interaction with SNAP-25. The inhibitory tions for modulation of transmitter release as well as neuronal
effects of W4 were apparent 510 min after starting injection and plasticity. Further characterization of the biochemical mecha-
reached a maximum within 1525 min at 2.5 mM or 4050 min at nisms and modulation of the interactions between Snapin and
5 mM pipette concentrations. The W4 peptide may act faster than SNARE proteins should reveal their physiological significance dur-
Snapin-CT not only due to the difference in the injected concen- ing the process of exocytosis and lead to progress in understanding
trations but also to the difference in their diffusion time from cell the molecular basis of learning and memory.
body to active zones. These results indicate that the W4 peptide
acts in a sequence-specific manner, and that its effects reflect an METHODS
action related to Snapin. Identification and cloning of Snapin. The yeast two-hybrid system was used
to clone SNAP-25-binding proteins. Full-length mouse SNAP-25b cDNA
DISCUSSION was inserted in-frame into the pGBT9 bait vector containing the GAL4 DNA-
binding domain (Clontech, Palo Alto, California). Yeast two-hybrid screens
Here we have identified a new synaptic vesicle membrane protein
of a human brain cDNA library in vector pACT1 (Clontech) with the GAL4
called Snapin, which associates with the SNARE complex through activation domain were done and evaluated according to the protocols
its direct interaction with SNAP-25. Binding of recombinant Snapin- described for the MATCHMAKER yeast two-hybrid system (Clontech).
CT to SNAP-25 blocks the association of the SNARE complex with Yeast cells (Y190) were sequentially cotransformed with SNAP-25 bait and
synaptotagmin, one of the putative calcium sensors involved in neu- library prey vectors, and then plated on selection medium lacking Trp, Leu
rotransmission16,20,21. The molecular mechanisms of the interac- and His. Two independent, but overlapping human Snapin partial clones
tion of Snapin with SNAP-25 during the process of vesicle expressing His3 and -galactosidase activity were isolated. EST databank
docking/fusion are still unclear. However, the relatively rapid inhi- searches identified both human and mouse cDNAs with a single open read-
bition of synaptic transmission resulting from injection of recom- ing frame encoding for 136 amino acid residues. The full-length mouse
Snapin cDNA was purchased from ATCC31.
binant Snapin-CT and the SNAP-25-binding site peptide indicates
that the interaction between Snapin and SNAP-25 and/or the SNARE Fusion proteins and in-vitro binding. Full-length syntaxin 1A, SNAP-25b,
complex is involved in a late step of exocytosis. SNAP-23, VAMP-2 and the cytoplasmic domain of synaptotagmin 1
Only partial inhibition of synaptic transmission was observed (80421) were subcloned into GST-fusion vectors pGEX-2 or pGEX-4T
when 50 M Snapin-CT (29 2.3%) and 5 mM W4 peptide (Pharmacia, Piscataway, New Jersey). Both Snapin-FL (1136) and Snapin-
(44 4.2%) were injected. Based on the color density of co- CT (79136) were subcloned into hexahistidine-tagged fusion protein vec-
injected Fast Green FCF dye and correction for the effect of molec- tor (pET28C and A; Novagen, Madison, Wisconsin). Fusion proteins were
ular mass22, the estimated concentration of Snapin-CT in the cell prepared as crude bacterial lysates by mild sonication in PBS (50 mM sodi-
body was 1.5 M (approximately 3% of the concentration in the um phosphate, pH 8.0, 300 mM NaCl, 1% TX-100 plus protease inhibitors).
pipette), exceeding the concentration required for half-maximal His-Snapin proteins were purified by binding to nickel-charged nitrilotri-
acetic acid agarose columns (Qiagen, Valencia, California) and eluted with
binding to SNAP-25 in vitro (100 nM; Fig. 3b). This partial inhibi-
500 mM imidazole in PBS. The eluates were concentrated with Centriprep-
tion has also been reported in other synapse systems after injection 10 filtration units (Amicon, Beverly, Massachusetts and dialyzed in a 10,000
of peptides at much higher concentrations than those required to molecular-weight cutoff dialysis cassette (Pierce, Rockford, Illinois) against
block the action of key synaptic proteins in vitro23,24. It is likely that PBS. Approximately 1 g of GST-fusion proteins were bound to glutathione-
partial inhibition in these synapses may reflect barriers in neurons Sepharose beads (Pharmacia) in TBS buffer (50 mM Tris-HCl, pH 7.5, 140
that prevent diffusion of the injected molecules to all the active mM NaCl, 0.1% TX-100 plus protease inhibitors), incubated at 4C for 1 h

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 123


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

with constant agitation, and washed with TBS to remove unbound proteins. 1. Rothman, J. E. Mechanisms of intracellular protein transport. Nature 372,
Glutathione-Sepharose beads coupled with similar amounts of GST-fusion 5563 (1994).
2. Bajjalieh, S. M. & Scheller, R. H. The biochemistry of neurotransmitter
proteins were added to the His-Snapin protein and incubated with gentle secretion. J. Biol. Chem. 270, 19711974 (1995).
shaking for 3 h at 4C. The beads were washed three times with TBS buffer, 3. Sdhof, T. C. The synaptic vesicle cycle: a cascade of protein-protein
and bound proteins were eluted in 15 mM reduced glutathione in 50 mM interactions. Nature 375, 645653 (1995).
Tris-HCl (pH 8). Eluates were separated from the beads by centrifugation 4. Trimble, W. S., Cowan, D. M. & Scheller, R. H. VAMP-1: a synaptic vesicle-
at 10,000 g for 1 minute and processed for 1020% SDS/tricine gradient gel associated integral membrane protein. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 85,
45384542 (1988).
electrophoresis. Bound His-Snapin was detected by monoclonal anti-T7.Tag 5. Bennett, M. K., Calakos, N. & Scheller, R. H. Syntaxin: a synaptic protein
antibody (Novagen) and GST-fusion proteins were detected by anti-GST implicated in docking of synaptic vesicles at presynaptic active zones. Science
antibody (Pharmacia). HRP-conjugated secondary antibodies and ECL 257, 255259 (1992).
chemiluminescence (Amersham, Arlington Heights, Illinois) were used to 6. Yoshida, A. et al. HPC-1 is associated with synaptotagmin and -conotoxin
receptor. J. Biol. Chem. 267, 2492524928 (1992).
visualize the bands. 7. Oyler, G. A. et al The identification of a novel synaptosomal-associated
protein, SNAP-25, differentially expressed by neuronal subpopulations. J.
Immunoprecipitation. Rat brain synaptosomes were prepared by differ- Cell. Biol. 109, 30393052 (1989).
ential and discontinuous Ficoll gradient centrifugation and solubilized29. 8. Sllner, T. et al. SNAP receptors implicated in vesicle targeting and fusion.
Crude synaptosomes were then separated into fractions enriched in presy- Nature 362, 318324 (1993).
9. Calakos, N., Bennett, M. K., Peterson, K. & Scheller, R. H. Protein-protein
naptic cytosol (PC), synaptic vesicles (SV) and plasma membrane (PM) as interactions contributing to the specificity of intracellular vesicular
described30. Solubilized synaptosome protein (100300 g) was incubat- trafficking. Science 263, 11461149 (1994).
ed with 3 g of anti-SNAP-25, anti-syntaxin (10H5) monoclonal anti- 10. Hayashi, T. et al Synaptic vesicle membrane fusion complex: action of
bodies3 or control mouse IgG (Zymed, South San Francisco, California) in clostridial neurotoxins on assembly. EMBO J. 13, 50515061 (1994).
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

1 ml TBS with 0.1% TX-100, 0.1% BSA and protease inhibitors, and incu- 11. Fields, F. & Song, O. A novel genetic system to detect protein-protein
interactions. Nature 340, 245246 (1989).
bated on a microtube rotator at 4C for 1 h. Protein A-Sepharose CL-4B 12. Bean, A. J., Seifert, R., Chen, Y. A., Sacks, R. & Scheller, R. H. Hrs-2 is an
resin (2.5 mg; Pharmacia) was added to each sample, and the incubation ATPase implicated in calcium-regulated secretion. Nature 385, 826829
continued for an additional three hours, followed by three washes with (1997).
TBS/0.1% TX-100. Subsequent interaction assays of immunoprecipitated 13. Kozak, M. An analysis of 5-noncoding sequences from 699 vertebrate
messenger RNAs. Nucleic Acids Res. 15, 81258131 (1987).
and immobilized protein complexes are described in the section on in- 14. Weimbs, T. et al. A conserved domain is present in different families of
vitro binding assays above. For multiple detection with different anti- vesicular fusion proteins: A new superfamily. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 94,
bodies, blots were first stripped in a solution of 62.5 mM Tris-HCl, pH 30463051 (1997).
7.5, 20 mM DTT and 1% SDS for 30 min at 50C with agitation and 15. Ravichandran, V., Chawla, A. & Roche, P. A. Identification of a novel
washed with TBS/0.1 % Tween-20 for 2 15 min. syntaxin- and synaptobrevin/VAMP-binding protein, SNAP-23, expressed in
non-neuronal tissues. J. Biol. Chem. 271, 1330013303 (1996).
16. Matthew. W. D., Tsavaler, L. & Reichardt, L. F. Identification of a synaptic
Synaptic transmission between SCGNs. SCG cells from seven-day post- vesicle-specific membrane protein with a wide distribution in neuronal and
natal rats were prepared as described1719. After 34 weeks in culture, con- neurosecretory tissue. J. Cell. Biol. 91, 257269 (1981).
ventional intracellular recordings were made from two neighboring 17. Mochida, S. et al. Myosin II is involved in transmitter release at synapses
formed between rat sympathetic neurons in culture. Neuron 13, 11311142
neurons using microelectrodes filled with 1 M potassium acetate (4070 (1994).
M). Postsynaptic responses (EPSPs) were recorded from one of the neu- 18. Mochida, S., Saisu, H., Kobayashi, H. & Abe, T. Impairment of syntaxin by
rons when action potentials were generated in the other neuron by passage botulinum neurotoxin C1 or antibodies inhibits acetylcholine release but not
of current through an intracellular recording electrode. Neurons were Ca2+ channel activity. Neuroscience 65, 905915 (1995).
superfused with modified Krebs solution consisting of 136 mM NaCl, 19. Mochida, S., Sheng, Z.-H., Baker, C., Kobayashi, H. & Catterall, W. A.
Inhibition of neurotransmission by peptides containing the synaptic protein
5.9 mM KCl, 5.1 mM CaCl2, 11 mM glucose and 3 mM Na-HEPES (pH interaction site of N-type Ca2+ channels. Neuron 17, 781788 (1996).
7.4). Recombinant Snapin-CT or peptides were dissolved in the suction 20. Li, C. et al Ca2+-dependent and -independent activities of neural and
pipette solution (150 mM KAc, 5 mM Mg2+-ATP, 10 mM HEPES, pH nonneural synaptotagmins. Nature 375, 594599 (1995).
7.4), and introduced into the presynaptic cell body by diffusion from a 21. Bommert, K. et al. Inhibition of neurotransmitter release by C2-domain
peptides implicates synaptotagmin in exocytosis. Nature 363, 163165 (1993).
suction glass pipette (1720 M tip resistance). Fast Green FCF (5%,
22. Pusch, M. & Neher, E. Rates of diffusional exchange between small cells and a
Sigma, St. Louis, Missouri) was included in the injection peptide solu- measuring patch pipette. Pflgers Arch. 411, 204211 (1988).
tion to confirm their entry into the presynaptic cell body. The injection 23. Cornille, F., Deloye, F., Fourni-Zaluski, M.-C., Roques, B. P. & Poulain, B.
pipette was removed 23 min after starting injection. EPSPs were record- Inhibition of neurotransmitter release by synthetic proline-rich peptides
ed once every 20 s (0.05 Hz). Electrophysiological data was collected and shows that the N-terminal domain of vesicle-associated membrane
protein/synaptobrevin is critical for neuroexocytosis. J. Biol. Chem. 270,
analyzed using software written by Dr. Tauc (CHRS, France). For Figs. 5b 1682616832 (1995).
and 6d, the peak amplitudes of EPSPs were measured and averaged. The 24. Doussau, F., Clabecq, A., Henry, J.-P., Darchen, F. & Poulain, B. Calcium-
resultant values were smoothed by an eight-point moving average algo- dependent regulation of rab 3 in short-term plasticity. J. Neurosci. 18,
rithm and plotted against recording time with t = 0 indicating the begin- 31473157 (1998).
ning of the presynaptic injection. 25. Wakshull, E., Johnson, M. I. & Burton, H., Postnatal rat sympathetic neurons
in culture. I. A comparison with embryonic neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 42,
14101425 (1979).
26. Broadie, K. et al. Syntaxin and synaptobrevin function downstream of vesicle
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS docking in Drosophila. Neuron 15, 663673 (1995).
We thank Q. Lao for help with the yeast two-hybrid system, X. Zheng for help 27. Hunt, J. M. et al. A post-docking role for synaptobrevin in synaptic vesicle
fusion. Neuron 12, 12691279 (1994).
with synaptosome fractionation, M Takahashi for antibodies, P.A. Roche for
28. Sweeney, S. T., Broadie, K., Keane, J., Niemann, H. & OKane, C. J. Targeted
SNAP-23 cDNA and S. C. Landis, H. Gainer, R. D. G. McKay, M. Mayer, H. expression of tetanus toxin light chain in Drosophila specifically eliminates
Arnheiter and J. Rettig for comments on the manuscript. This work was synaptic transmission and causes behavioral defects. Neuron 14, 341351 (1995).
29. Sheng, Z.-H., Rettig, J., Cook, T. & Catterall, W. A. Calcium-dependent
supported by the intramural research program of NINDS, NIH (J.M.I and Z.- interaction of N-type calcium channels with the synaptic core complex.
H.S.), HHMI-NIH Research Scholars Program (J.M.I.), grants from The Nature 379, 451454 (1996).
Japanese Ministry of Education, Science and Culture (S.M.) and the Human 30. Dunkley, P. R. et al. A rapid percoll gradient procedure for isolation of
synaptosomes directly from an S1 fraction: homogeneity and morphology of
Frontier Science Program (S.M.). subcellular fractions. Brain Res. 441, 5971 (1988).
31. Hillier, L. et al., Generation and analysis of 280,000 human expressed
RECEIVED 6 OCTOBER; ACCEPTED 15 DECEMBER 1998 sequence tags. Genome Res. 6, 807828 (1996).

124 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Casein kinase-II regulates NMDA


channel function in hippocampal
neurons
David N. Lieberman1 and Istvan Mody2

1 Neuroscience Graduate Program, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, Californina 94305, USA
2 Departments of Neurology and Physiology, UCLA School of Medicine, Los Angeles, California 900951769, USA
Correspondence should be addressed to D.N.L (liebdude@leland.stanford.edu)

Several second-messenger-regulated protein kinases have been implicated in the regulation of


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) channel function. Yet the role of calcium and cyclic-nucleotide-inde-
pendent kinases, such as casein kinase II (CKII), has remained unexplored. Here we identify CKII as
an endogenous Ser/Thr protein kinase that potently regulates NMDA channel function and
mediates intracellular actions of spermine on the channel. The activity of NMDA channels in cell-
attached and inside-out recordings was enhanced by CKII or spermine and was decreased by selec-
tive inhibition of CKII. In hippocampal slices, inhibitors of CKII reduced synaptic transmission
mediated by NMDA but not AMPA receptors. The dependence of NMDA receptor channel activity
on tonically active CKII thus permits changes in intracellular spermine levels or phosphatase
activities to effectively control channel function.

The modulation of neuronal glutamate receptors by phosphory- stitutively active protein kinase CKII continuously controls the
lation is considered to be critical for synaptic plasticity. In par- basal function of NMDA receptor channels. In contrast to the
ticular, altering NMDA-type glutamate receptor properties by phasic nature of NMDA channel regulation by second-messen-
phosphorylation111 may regulate the unique function of these ger-activated protein kinases, the tonic influence of CKII
receptors in developmental, physiological or pathological neu- establishes a continuous cycle of phosphorylation/dephos-
ronal plasticity. The numerous protein kinases that mediate phorylation from which channel activity can be upregulated by
synaptic plasticity became obvious candidates for the identifica- spermine through stimulation of CKII, or downregulated by
tion of kinases capable of increasing NMDA channel activity. calcineurin following its activation by calcium/calmodulin.
Using exogenous calcium-dependent protein kinases, initial stud- These findings, together with our results demonstrating the
ies on NMDA-receptor-mediated responses demonstrated CKII insensitivity of AMPA-type glutamate receptors, reveal a
enhancing effects of such kinases on NMDA channel function. novel and selective means of regulating the gain of excitatory
Yet many of these studies have shown minimal effects of specific synaptic transmission at hippocampal synapses by a tonically
kinase inhibitors on NMDA-receptor-mediated currents510. active protein kinase.
Whereas these studies demonstrate the capacity of various can-
didate protein kinases to alter channel activity, they leave in ques- RESULTS
tion their ability to regulate basal channel activity in situ. We wanted to determine which kinase mediates augmentation
Furthermore, unrecognized differences in subunit composition of NMDA channel activity following inhibition of calcineurin3
and stoichiometry of the NMDA receptor channel complex in in cell-attached recordings of single NMDA channels from acute-
different tissues and preparations have encouraged controversy ly dissociated adult rat dentate gyrus granule cells. We focused
surrounding the various effects of protein kinases on NMDA on calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein kinaseII (CaMKII)
channel function. for several reasons. First, calcineurin is frequently colocalized
More recently, the control of NMDA channel gating by with CaMKII in principal cells of the hippocampus and cortex12.
Ser/Thr kinases has been inferred from experiments in which Second, exogenous CaMKII seems to regulate both AMPA and
the cycle of phosphorylation/dephosphorylation was disrupted NMDA receptors10, and third, this kinase phosphorylates Ser-
by inhibiting phosphatase activity, particularly that of the 1303 of NR2B and/or a comparable site on NR2A11. The cell-
calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein phosphatase calcin- permeable specific CaMKII inhibitor KN-62 (ref. 13) and its
eurin3,4. Because the protein kinase responsible for modulation inactive analogue KN-04 were used to probe the involvement of
of NMDA channel gating has not been clearly characterized in intrinsic CaMKII in altering NMDA channel function.
physiological studies, we explored the different effects of protein Exposure to KN-62 (10 M) reversibly reduced the fre-
kinases on channel gating in central neurons. Using cell- quency of NMDA channel openings (po = 34 7% of control,
attached recordings of single NMDA channels from acutely dis- p < 0.05, paired t-test, n = 6), but did not significantly change
sociated adult hippocampal dentate granule cells to preserve the open duration, the length of bursts and clusters or the
endogenous second messenger systems, we found that the con- total open time in these complex openings, (Fig. 1ac,

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 125


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 1. Effects of CaMKII and CKII Control Control


inhibitors on NMDA channel function in a
cell-attached patches. (a) Continuous
traces of cell-attached recordings before
(upper traces) and after (lower traces)
bath application of kinase inhibitors.
Scale bars, 2 pA/10 ms. (b) Continuous 10 M KN-62 100 M DRB
measurements of NMDA channel
N po before, during (horizontal bar)
and after addition of KN-62 (left panel)
or DRB (right panel) to the bath.
Openings were binned into 10-s inter-
vals in the KN-62 experiment and 1-
minute intervals in the DRB experiment.
b
The example shows the most extreme
reduction in po by KN-62 (po = 12% of
control). It also demonstrates that large
N popen

decreases in po are not necessarily

Npopen
accompanied by significant changes in
the duration of open times (the mean
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

open time in the presence of KN-62 is


93% of that in control medium).
(c) Cumulative probability distributions
of all open durations before, during and
after exposure to KN-62 (left panel) and Time (min) Time (min)
DRB (right panel) for the experiments
shown in (b). DRB significantly reduced
the duration of all openings (mean open
c
time, 68% of control, p < 106,

Cumulative probability
Cumulative probability

Kolmogorov-Smirnov test), whereas


KN-62 had no significant effect on the
duration of openings.

Open time (ms) Open time (ms)

Table 1). This effect of KN-62 on NMDA channel activity did greatest number of consensus phosphorylation sites are those
not result from its known antagonism of voltage-dependent potentially phosphorylated by CKII15.
calcium channels14, because the inorganic calcium channel To examine the role of CKII in modulating NMDA channel
blocker cadmium (200 M, n = 4, data not shown) failed to function, we tested the effect of the cell-permeable CKII inhibitor
reproduce the effects of KN-62. Furthermore, it is unlikely 5,6-dichloro-1--D-ribofuranosyl benzimidazole (DRB, 50100
that KN-62 antagonized NMDA receptors, because the struc- M)19,20 on NMDA channel activity in cell-attached recordings.
turally related, but CaMKII-inactive, analogue KN-04 had no In biochemical assays using casein as a substrate and GTP as a
effect (n = 3, data not shown). Based on these experiments, donor, 60 M DRB inhibits over 80% of the CKII activity mea-
endogenous CaMKII tonically regulates NMDA channel sured in rat hippocampal slices, and produces a 65% reduction in
opening probability (po) by altering the frequency of open- the activity of 1 pmol (140 ng) of recombinant CKII (P.T.
ings, but not their duration. Accordingly, this kinase cannot Tuazon, J. Roig and J.A. Traugh, personal communication). Not
be responsible for the characteristic prolongation of simple only did DRB significantly reduce the po to 39 9 % of control
and complex channel openings found after phosphatase inhi- (p < 0.05, paired t-test, n = 6), it also significantly shortened the
bition3. Thus, we looked for another kinase that could pro- mean open time, burst length, cluster duration and total open
duce changes in NMDA channel openings qualitatively similar time during these events (Fig. 1ac, Table 1). The changes in
to those found after calcineurin inhibition. NMDA channel gating, notably the reduction in the duration of
We focused our attention on CKII, an ubiquitously dis- openings, presumably caused by inhibition of CKII by DRB,
tributed Ser/Thr protein kinase, whose activation is indepen- appear conspicuously similar to those observed following cal-
dent of intracellular calcium or cyclic nucleotides1520. CKII cineurin activation3. Consistent with the involvement of an
is more abundant in brain than in any other tissue, and is active phosphatase, the reduction of NMDA channel activity fol-
more concentrated in neurons than in glia17,18. Despite its lowing DRB was rapid and reversible, in contrast to the long half-
distinct nuclear, cytosolic and membrane-bound activities in life of dephosphorylation reported for some CKII substrates in
other cell types, little is known about its role in neurons. cell-free preparations15,16. To corroborate our findings with
However, a PROSITE analysis of the C-termini of the NMDA DRB, we then investigated the effects of other compounds, unre-
receptor subunits NR1, NR2A and NR2B revealed that the lated to DRB, that are known to affect CKII activity.

126 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Table 1. Effects of various protein kinase inhibitors and activators on NMDA channel openings in cell-attached and inside-
out recordings.
Mean open Burst Total Cluster Total Supercluster
Cell-attached time length open/burst length open/cluster length n
KN-62 92 7 89 7 85 6 95 26 74 6 107 23 6
DRB 75 4* 60 5* 67 4* 54 6* 57 5* 55 9* 6
FK-506 + DRB 95 9 89 10 90 9 83 12 87 6 78 19 3
Chrysin 77 7* 70 6* 71 7* 52 7* 57 8* 67 13 5
Spermine 153 30* 257 56* 195 51* 238 61* 290 81* 180 49 6
Spermine + DRB 108 13 102 3 101 10 92 10 93 17 61 16 4
Phorbol ester (PDBu) 103 4 106 6 105 6 99 7 104 7 115 22 8
H-7 106 8 104 7 97 9 102 8 93 12 107 13 4
Sp-8-CPT-cAMPS 102 6 105 14 113 10 98 13 112 17 112 28 8
Rp-8-CPT-cAMPS 119 25 117 28 119 25 123 25 131 23 101 26 5

Inside-out
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

Casein kinase II 174 17* 174 23* 183 25* 179 22* 202 32* 183 15* 9
DRB 71 14* 61 8* 62 9* 49 11 47 6* 67 25 3
ATP + Heparin 66 7* 57 4* 58 5* 60 10* 54 7* 59 10 6
BAPTA 157 26* 169 33* 188 45 147 20 229 61* 223 83 4
GTP vs ATP 136 11* 120 15 120 15 130 25 150 36 91 17 10
GTP + Heparin 70 15* 58 14* 58 16* 54 7* 47 7 57 17 3
Spermine 126 6* 147 14* 149 11* 177 39* 176 26* 167 45 4
The values represent the average standard error percent of control responses under the various experimental conditions. The bold typeface and * denote
significant (p < 0.05, two-tailed paired t-tests) differences in pair-wise comparisons of the opening characteristics in each cell. The n represents the number of
recordings with at least 5 minutes of NMDA channel activity (average > 2000 openings) during each control and experimental period. None of the treatments
had any significant effect on single-channel conductance.

Endogenous cellular regulators of CKII activity include the of biochemical experiments, spermine was found to produce a
polyamines spermine and spermidine, which markedly two-fold increase CKII activity in hippocampal slices (P.T.
increase the activity of this protein kinase15,16. These Tuazon, J. Roig and J.A. Traugh, personal communication),
polyamines also regulate NMDA channel function2124. consistent with the enhancement of single NMDA channel
Whereas early studies questioned whether the site of spermine activity produced by spermine in our electrophysiological
modulation of NMDA channel activity was intracellular or experiments.
extracellular, more recent studies have shown that spermine If CKII controls NMDA channel gating through phospho-
enhances NMDA channel activity by interacting with its N- rylation, then inhibiting endogenous CKII should also prevent
terminal extracellular domain2224. It is important to note, blockers of the dephosphorylation process from prolonging
however, that the vast majority of spermine in the brain NMDA channel openings. In adult hippocampal neurons, the
resides intracellularly25, where it may act as a second messen- protein phosphatase most effectively involved in the feedback
ger. To investigate this possibility, we examined the potential regulation of NMDA channel activity is the calcium-calmod-
of intracellular spermine to physiologically activate CKII and ulin-dependent protein phosphatase calcineurin3. We applied
thus enhance NMDA channel activity. the calcineurin inhibitor FK-506 to cells pretreated with DRB
To prevent direct interaction between spermine and the to examine whether CKII mediates the increase in NMDA
extracellular domain of NMDA channels under the patch channel function when calcineurin-dependent dephosphory-
pipettes, we applied spermine extracellularly during cell- lation is eliminated. Indeed, pretreatment of dentate granule
attached recordings (Fig. 2). Thus, spermine would have to cells with 60 M DRB prevented the characteristic prolonga-
enter the cytoplasm via polyamine transporters26,27, other glu- tion of open times3 by the calcineurin inhibitor FK-506
tamate receptors28 or by other means before altering the func- (500 nM, n = 3, Table 1). Based on these findings, we conclude
tion of the NMDA receptor channel complex. Within 510 min that CKII is responsible for the enhancement of NMDA chan-
following addition of spermine (200500 M) to the extracel- nel activity when the dephosphorylation process mediated by
lular solution, NMDA channel open times were significantly calcineurin is inhibited.
prolonged (Fig. 2a and b, n = 6; Table 1). This experiment sug- The CKII inhibitor DRB is considered to be highly specific,
gests that spermine must activate an intracellular pathway that with no effect on PKC, PKA, CaMKII or the src tyrosine
controls NMDA channel activity. kinase15,16. To ensure that the effect of DRB in granule cells was
Next, we pretreated cells with DRB to determine whether not secondary to changing the activities of PKC5,6 or PKA7,8
CKII mediates the actions of spermine on the NMDA channel (shown to alter NMDA channel function in various prepara-
(Fig. 2c). Inhibiting CKII with DRB (50100 M, n = 4, tions), we used selective PKC and PKA activators and inhibitors
Table 1) prevented the prolonging effect of spermine on NMDA and examined their effects on single NMDA channel openings.
channel open times, suggesting that this augmentation In our preparation, neither phorbol 12,13-dibutyrate (PDBu,
occurred through a CKII mediated pathway. In a separate series 110 M, n = 8), an activator of endogenous PKC, nor the

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 127


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 2. Extracellular spermine


increases NMDA channel activity in a Control Spermine

cell-attached patches. (a) Continuous


traces of cell-attached recordings in
the same cell before (left panel, mean
open time, 2.18 ms) and after (right
panel, mean open time, 3.94 ms) bath
application of 300 M spermine.
Scale bars, 2 pA/10 ms. (b) Open
dwell-time histograms for the record-
ings shown in (a). Each histogram
shows the frequency distributions of
log binned (9 bins per decade) shut b
and open times. The values on the
abscissae are plotted on a square- w = 116 ms w = 150 ms
root scale. The individual exponential
distributions of the dwell times are
Frequency (square-root scale)

indicated by thin lines. Each peak of


the distribution corresponds to a
time constant of the compound
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

exponentials. The weighted mean


shut or open times (w, which also
include the dwell-times of missed
events) are indicated in each panel. Shut time (ms) Shut time (ms)
(c) Cumulative probability distribu-
tions of total open time per cluster
w = 1.4 ms w = 2.8 ms
before and after exposure of two
cells to spermine (300 m). The first
cell was recorded in the absence (left
panel) and the second cell in the pres-
ence (right panel) of the CKII
inhibitor DRB (100 m). The increase
in total open time in clusters found
with spermine alone (left panel,
p < 106, Kolmogorov-Smirnov test)
was prevented by DRB (right panel). Open time (ms) Open time (ms)
c
Cumulative probability

Cumulative probability

Total open/cluster (ms) Total open/cluster (ms)

broad spectrum kinase inhibitor H-7 (1020 M, n = 4) had In spite of their kinase specificities, DRB and chrysin may
any effect on NMDA channel openings (Table 1). Furthermore, have altered NMDA channel activity by nonspecific effects on
the selective cell-permeable activator and inhibitor of PKA 8, other CKII-mediated cellular functions, including RNA tran-
Sp-8-CPT-cAMPS (50100 M) and Rp-8-CPT-cAMPS scription and translation19,20. Because CKII is also found in the
(50100 M) respectively, were also without effect on granule nucleus15,16,18, to exclude possible nuclear targets of CKII, we
cell NMDA channel openings (Table 1). Whereas these experi- used excised inside-out patch recordings maintained with ATP
ments do not rule out a general role for PKC or PKA in regulat- on the cytosolic side, where application of DRB proved to be
ing NMDA channel function, it is unlikely that in adult dentate equally effective in reducing NMDA channel openings (n = 3,
gyrus granule cells DRB could have produced its effect on Table 1). This finding is consistent with CKII being closely asso-
NMDA channels by inhibiting any of these protein kinases. To ciated with the plasma membrane and/or the NMDA receptor
corroborate our findings with DRB, we next tested the recently complex, and allowed us to undertake a more rigorous exami-
described specific, but structurally unrelated, CKII inhibitor nation of CKII effects in inside-out recordings.
chrysin29. Chrysin (50 M) reproduced the effects of DRB on To further test the hypothesis that intracellular spermine can
NMDA channel openings (Table 1), further supporting a role enhance NMDA channel activity through CKII, we directly
for CKII in regulating the gating of NMDA channels. applied spermine to the cytosolic side of inside-out membrane

128 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 3. The effects of activation or inhibi-


tion of CKII on NMDA channel activity a

Cumulative probability
recorded in inside-out patches. (a) Left

Mean open time (ms)


panel, mean open time calculated for 10-s
bins is plotted against the recording time.
The patch was excised 80 s after beginning
the recording, was subsequently trans-
ferred into a well containing GTP, and was
washed with cytosolic media before it was
exposed to ATP. Right panel, cumulative
distribution of mean open times from indi- Time (min) Mean open time (ms)
vidual experiments. Addition of 2 mM
GTP (n = 10) or 4 mM ATP (n = 18) to the b
cytosolic side of inside-out patches signifi-
cantly (p < 0.05, Students t -test)
Cumulative probability

Cumulative probability
increased the openings of NMDA chan-
nels compared to those in cell-attached or
freshly excised patches. In the absence of
exogenous ATP or GTP, channel activity
invariably runs down (data not shown).
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

(b) Heparin (1020 g/ml) perfused onto


the cytosolic side reduces the open time
within a cluster of channel openings (mea-
sured 5 minutes after the addition of
heparin). Six minutes after its addition to Total open/cluster (ms) Total cluster length (ms)
the bath, spermine (200 M) applied to
the cytosolic face of the patch increases c
cluster duration. (c) Raw current traces
(left panel) before and after addition of
CKII (200 U/ml) to the cytosolic side of

Mean open time (ms)


the patch. Scale bars, 2 pA/10 ms. CKII
rapidly prolongs single NMDA channel
openings (right panel) when added to an
inside-out patch. Both human recombi-
nant holoenzyme and purified rabbit brain
CKII holoenzyme were used. The addition
artifact at seven minutes is blanked out.

Time (min)

patches containing the NMDA receptor. In the presence of ATP, To better understand the role of CKII in the regulation of
application of exogenous spermine (200 M) to the cytoplasmic NMDA channel openings, we designed experiments to address
side, significantly prolonged NMDA channel openings (n=5, several unique physiological and pharmacological properties of
Fig. 3b and Table 1). Thus, in addition to controlling ion flow CKII. Unlike most Ser/Thr protein kinases, CKII can use high-
through inwardly rectifying potassium channels30 and certain energy phosphate groups provided by either ATP or GTP15,16.
non-NMDA glutamate receptors,31,32 intracellular spermine We tested this characteristic of CKII in inside-out patches,
effectively regulates NMDA receptor function through activa- where GTP (n = 10) was used to substitute for ATP (n = 18) in
tion of CKII. These findings do not contradict the known maintaining NMDA channel openings (Fig. 3a, Table 1). All
actions of spermine on the extracellular domains of NMDA kinetic parameters but the mean open time (Fig. 3a, Table 1)
receptors2224, but reveal an additional intracellular mecha- were comparable when NMDA channel activity was supported
nism33,34 possibly responsible for effects of extracellularly by GTP, rather than by ATP. The 36% longer mean open time in
applied spermine. the presence of GTP versus ATP could be due to a more effective
The potentiating effect of cytosolic spermine on NMDA utilization of GTP by CKII, or could result from the involve-
channel activity was mimicked by direct exposure of the ment of some additional pathway linked to GTP hydrolysis.
patches to purified or recombinant CKII (n = 9, Fig. 3c; If tonic CKII-dependent phosphorylation regulates NMDA
Table 1), but not by perfusion of vehicle or of heat-inactivated channel activity, then the effect of DRB on NMDA channel
enzyme (n = 4, data not shown). The rapid prolongation of openings should be reproduced by heparin, the classic
mean channel open time by addition of the holoenzyme form inhibitor of CKII in biochemical studies15,16. Indeed, in the
of the kinase suggests a direct phosphorylation of the NMDA presence of ATP, heparin (1020 g/ml) perfused onto the
receptor channel complex. Furthermore, this increase in cytosolic side of inside-out patches dramatically reduced
NMDA channel activity produced by exogenous CKII suggests NMDA channel open durations (n = 6, Fig. 3b; Table 1). When
that phosphorylation by endogenous CKII is not fully satu- the experiments were repeated in the presence of GTP instead
rated, allowing for a CKII-dependent dynamic control of of ATP, heparin shortened the channel open duration to the
NMDA channel function. same extent (n = 3, Table 1). The lack of significant difference

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 129


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 4. Inhibition of CKII selectively reduces the NMDA


component of synaptic transmission in the dentate gyrus. a EPSPAMPA EPSPNMDA

(a) Superimposed traces of field EPSPs recorded in the mol-


ecular layer show no effect of 100 M DRB on AMPA-
receptor-mediated EPSPs (EPSPA, recorded in 50 M
D-APV), whereas NMDA-receptor-mediated EPSPs (EPSPN,
recorded in 15 M CNQX) are significantly reduced by
DRB in a fully reversible fashion. (b) The graph shows the
time course of NMDA EPSP inhibition by 50 (n = 5) and 100
M DRB (n = 6) applied for 20 min (during the bar), without
alteration in AMPA EPSPs (50 M DRB, n = 6, and 100 M b
DRB, n = 7). In each experiment, four sweeps were averaged
at each timepoint. The graphs represent percent alterations
DRB
in the normalized mean EPSP area ( standard error) as a
function of time. (c) Chrysin (50 M; applied for 30 min

Percent of baseline
indicated by the bar), a selective CKII inhibitor structurally
unrelated to DRB, shows the same selectivity for gluta-
matergic synaptic transmission as DRB (EPSPN, n = 6, EPSPA, p EPSPN 50 M DRB
n = 5). Percentage changes ( standard error) from normal- l EPSPA 50 M DRB
ized baseline responses recorded during the control period P EPSPN 100 M DRB
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

are plotted as in (b). L EPSPA 100 M DRB

between heparins actions on NMDA channel activ- c


Time (min)

ity in the presence of ATP or GTP excludes the possi-


bility of an effect through inhibition of a 50 M Chrysin
G-protein35. More importantly, heparin is known
Percent of baseline

for its potent antagonism of calcium release from


inositol-tris-phosphate (IP3)-sensitive cellular cal- L EPSPA
cium stores36, and the attachment of such cytoplas- P EPSPN
mic organelles to the inside of membrane patches
cannot be entirely ruled out37. Even if IP3-sensitive
calcium stores were present in our inside-out
patches, a heparin-mediated reduction of calcium
release on the cytosolic side would be expected to
increase NMDA channel open duration. Indeed,
NMDA channel openings were significantly pro- Time (min)
longed by the calcium chelator BAPTA (15 mM),
added to the intracellular side of inside-out patches
(n = 4; Table 1). This finding is expected if one assumes that antagonists exerted their effects by altering RNA transcrip-
activation of calcineurin is prevented following the high-affin- tion19,20,29, we also tested the effect of actinomycin D
ity binding of calcium to BAPTA. (3060 M), a known transcription inhibitor, on NMDA-
Although our data clearly show that dentate granule cell receptor-mediated synaptic responses. Actinomycin D had no
NMDA channel openings are regulated by CKII, it needs to be effect on NMDA-receptor-mediated synaptic transmission
demonstrated that CKII also regulates the function of synaptic (n = 5; data not shown), consistent with a specific reduction in
NMDA receptors. We thus examined NMDA receptor-medi- CKII activity by DRB and chrysin.
ated synaptic transmission in hippocampal slices. In the dentate
gyrus, the CKII inhibitor DRB reversibly inhibited synaptically DISCUSSION
elicited NMDA-receptor-mediated field responses (50 M Casein kinase II (CKII) is a ubiquitously distributed calcium-
DRB, n = 5; 100 M DRB, n = 6). There were no concomitant and cyclic-nucleotide-insensitive Ser/Thr protein kinase, highly
changes in AMPA-receptor-mediated EPSPs (50 M DRB, concentrated in the brain, where its function is virtually
n = 6, 100 M DRB, n = 7; Fig. 4a and b). The unaltered AMPA- unknown. Here we have demonstrated a new action of CKII in
receptor-mediated synaptic events in the presence of these con- neurons: the selective regulation of NMDA channel gating,
centrations of DRB38 indicate a lack of DRB effect on glutamate including the prolongation of channel openings when dephos-
release despite possible presynaptic targets of CKII-mediated phorylation by calcineurin is prevented3. According to our data,
phosphorylation26,39,40. The CKII-dependent modulation of the phosphorylation state of the NMDA receptor channel com-
synaptic NMDA receptors is not restricted to the dentate gyrus plex in adult hippocampal neurons is set by a balance between
because NMDA (n = 4), but not AMPA responses (n = 3), were the tonic activity of the spermine-sensitive CKII and the calci-
significantly reduced by CKII inhibition in the CA1 region of um-mediated negative feedback provided by calcineurin. Known
the hippocampus (data not shown). We further examined the biochemical properties of CKII, including its inhibition by
specificity of CKII inhibition for NMDA responses using heparin, DRB and chrysin, potentiation by spermine and hydrol-
chrysin (n = 5). Like DRB, chrysin (50 M) selectively dimin- ysis of either GTP or ATP, were used to demonstrate the role of
ished NMDA (n = 6) but not AMPA (n = 5) mediated field this kinase in the regulation of NMDA channel gating.
EPSPs (Fig. 4c). To eliminate the possibility that both CKII Previous studies may have failed to find any effect of intracel-

130 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Inside-out patch recordings. Inside-out patches were excised in a bath


lular spermine on NMDA channel function22 because the
of 120 mM NaCl, 5 mM CsCl, 10 mM HEPES, 10 mM D-glucose, 1 mM
absence of intracellular ATP or GTP in those recordings most pyruvic acid and 1 M TTX with no added calcium. Fire-polished and
likely precluded the ability of spermine-activated CKII to phos- Sylgard-coated borosilicate glass pipettes (610 M) were filled with
phorylate the NMDA receptor channel complex. Judged by the the sulfate-containing extracellular solution3,50, including 1.8 mM CaCl2,
robust effect of CKII on NMDA channel function, however, the 200500 nM L-aspartic acid and 8 mM glycine. The patches were moved
regulation of CKII by spermine may have profound functional to a well containing 140 mM Csgluconate, 10 mM HEPES, 1 mM MgCl2,
consequences on NMDA-receptor-mediated events during 150 nM CaCl2 and either 4 mM Mg-ATP or 2 mM GTP and were con-
physiological and pathological alterations in intracellular sper- tinually perfused with this solution.
mine levels. Following ischemic insults to the brain, polyamines
are released into the extracellular space, spermine synthesis is Extracellular recordings. Coronal or horizontal brain slices (400 m
thick) were prepared from rats of similar age to those used in the single-
stimulated, and active cellular uptake of spermine is increased in
channel experiments. Slices were submerged at 35oC in a recording cham-
the recovery period41,42. Thus, increased CKII activity resulting ber and constantly perfused with a ACSF containing 126 mM NaCl, 2.5
from increased intracellular levels of spermine could contribute mM KCl, 1.25 mM NaH2PO4, 2 mM CaCl2, 0.3 mM MgCl2, 26 mM
to the anoxia-induced enhancement of NMDA responses. NaHCO3 and 10 mM glucose in an atmosphere of humidified 95% O2,
In central neurons, CKII may modulate the activity of sev- 5% CO2. Constant current stimuli were delivered to either the perforant
eral key target proteins, including calmodulin, MAP 1B, - path or the Schaffer collaterals every 30 s via a bipolar stimulating elec-
tubulin, tau, the regulatory subunit of type II PKA, protein trode, and extracellular field EPSPs were recorded in stratum
phosphatase inhibitor-2, DARP-32 (refs 1518) and CKII itself moleculare in the dentate and stratum radiatum of CA1. AMPA-receptor-
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

through autophosphorylation of its -subunit1518. Whereas mediated EPSP areas were measured in the presence of 50 M D-APV,
whereas the areas of NMDA-receptor-mediated EPSPs were examined
PKC, PKA, CaMKII and the src tyrosine kinase seem to alter
in the presence of 15 M CNQX. Field EPSP area rather than slope was
both NMDA and AMPA receptors1,2,510, CKII seems to selec- measured to take into account the long time course of NMDA-receptor-
tively control only the function of NMDA receptors. It was mediated synaptic potentials. To prevent possible effects of CKII
beyond the scope of the present study to determine which, if inhibitors on GABA-receptor-mediated inhibition, 50 M picrotoxin
any, of the numerous consensus CKII phosphorylation sites on was included in some experiments.
NMDA receptor subunits are phosphorylated by CKII. As a positive control for the phorbolester used in our cell-attached
However, a direct phosphorylation of NMDA channels by CKII recordings, PDBu was bath applied to hippocampal slices, producing a
need not be postulated. Indirect effects of CKII on NMDA clear augmentation of AMPA-receptor-mediated synaptic transmission
channels may include the phosphorylation of calmodulin43, in extracellular recordings. The ability of Sp-8-CPT-cAMPS to regulate
leading to a change in the direct regulatory effect exerted by the neuronal function was examined in slice recordings, where this com-
pound both enhanced AMPA-receptor-mediated field EPSPs, presum-
calmodulin bound to NMDA channels44.
ably through a presynaptic action, and reduced GABA-receptor-mediated
Spermine may be one of many endogenous regulators of IPSCs (P. Poisbeau and I. Mody, unpublished observations).
CKII activity in neurons. Other means of CKII activation may
bear on short- and long-term alterations of neuronal excitabil-
ity. For example, high-frequency synaptic activity leads to the ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
transient elevation of CKII activity during induction of LTP in We thank Jolinda Traugh for the gift of purified CKII, for the sharing of her
an NMDA-receptor-dependent manner45, and neurotrophins biochemical findings, and for her comments on the manuscript. We thank
may elevate CKII activity through the activation of other pro- Walter Pyerin for reviewing an earlier version of the manuscript, Georg Khr for
tein kinases46. Furthermore, the regulation of NMDA channel his early work with phorbol esters on NMDA channels and Brian Oyama for
function described in our study may be complemented by a technical assistance. This work was supported by a Howard Hughes predoctoral
CKII-dependent regulation of biochemical events in neurons. fellowship to D.N.L and by the NINDS grant NS 27528 and the Coelho
CKII seems to control the nuclear translocation of the tran- Endowment to I.M.
scription factor NF--B via I-B phosphorylation47 and other
proteins through phosphorylation-regulated nuclear localiza- RECEIVED 1 OCTOBER; ACCEPTED 16 DECEMBER 1998
tion sequences48. These latter findings are consistent with a role
for CKII as a second messenger relaying the consequences of 1. Roche, K. W., Tingley, W. G. & Huganir, R. L. Glutamate receptor
glutamate receptor activation to the nucleus49. In summary, phosphorylation and synaptic plasticity. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 4, 383388
our results identify the NMDA receptor channel complex as a (1994).
2. Moss, S. J. & Smart, T. G. Modulation of amino acid-gated ion channels by
constitutively phosphorylated target of CKII, and reveal a selec- protein phosphorylation. Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 39, 152 (1996).
tive means of regulating the gain of excitatory synaptic trans- 3. Lieberman, D. N. & Mody, I. Regulation of NMDA channel function by
endogenous Ca2+- dependent phosphatase. Nature 369, 235239 (1994).
mission in the hippocampus. 4. Tong, G., Shepherd, D. & Jahr, C. E. Synaptic desensitization of NMDA
receptors by calcineurin. Science 267, 15101512 (1995).
METHODS 5. Chen, L. & Huang, L. Y. Protein kinase C reduces Mg2+ block of NMDA-
receptor channels as a mechanism of modulation. Nature 356, 521523
Cell-attached recordings. Dentate granule cells from adult rats were (1992).
acutely dissociated as described50. Cell-attached recordings were done at 6. Durand, G. M., Bennett, M. V. L. & Zukin, R. S. Splice variants of the N-
room temperature in a bath of 110 mM Na2SO4, 5 mM Cs2SO4, 10 mM methyl-D-aspartate receptor NR1 identify domains involved in regulation by
HEPES, 10 mM D-glucose, 1.8 mM CaCl2, 1 mM pyruvic acid and 1 M polyamines and protein kinase C. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 90, 67316735
tetrodotoxin (TTX, final pH 7.25, osmolarity 285305 mOsm). Fire-pol- (1993).
7. Greengard, P., Jen, J., Nairn, A. C. & Stevens, C. F. Enhancement of the
ished, Sylgard-coated borosilicate glass (Garner Glass, Claremont, Cali- glutamate response by cAMP dependent protein kinase in hippocampal
fornia) pipettes (815 M) were filled with 200500 nM L-aspartic acid neurons. Science 253, 11351138 (1991).
and 8 M glycine dissolved in the bath solution. Channel activity was 8. Raman, I. M., Tong, G. & Jahr, C. E. -adrenergic regulation of synaptic
sampled at 20 kHz and filtered at 2 kHz for analysis and display of NMDA receptors by cAMP- dependent protein kinase. Neuron 16, 415421
(1996).
records. Details of the single-channel analysis using the PAT program 9. Lu, Y. M., Roder, J. C., Davidow, J. & Salter, M. W. Src activation in the
(courtesy of J. Dempster) with a 50% threshold crossing algorithm for induction of long-term potentiation in CA1 hippocampal neurons. Science
event detection are given elsewhere50. 279, 13631367 (1998).

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 131


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

10. Kolaj, M., Cerne, R., Cheng, G., Brickey, D. A. & Randic, M. Alpha subunit of cytoplasmic polyamines as the mechanism of intrinsic rectification. Nature
calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase enhances excitatory amino 372, 366369 (1994).
acid and synaptic responses of rat spinal dorsal horn neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 31. Bowie, D. & Mayer, M. L. Inward rectification of both AMPA and kainate
72, 25252531 (1994). subtype glutamate receptors generated by polyamine-mediated ion channel
11. Omkumar, R. V., Kiely, M. J., Rosenstein, A. J., Min, K. T. & Kennedy, M. B. block. Neuron 15, 453462 (1995).
Identification of a phosphorylation site for calcium/calmodulindependent 32. Kamboj, S. K., Swanson, G. T. & Cull-Candy, S. G. Intracellular spermine
protein kinase II in the NR2B subunit of the N-methylDaspartate receptor. confers rectification on rat calcium-permeable AMPA and kainate receptors.
J. Biol. Chem. 271, 3167031678 (1996). J. Physiol. (Lond.) 486, 297303 (1995).
12. Goto, S. et al. Cellular colocalization of calcium/calmodulin-dependent 33. Herman, M. D., Reuveny, E. & Narahashi, T. The effect of polyamines on
protein kinase II and calcineurin in the rat cerebral cortex and hippocampus. voltage-activated calcium channels in mouse neuroblastoma cells. J. Physiol.
Neurosci. Lett. 149, 189192 (1993). (Lond.) 462, 645-660 (1993).
13. Ishikawa, N., Hashiba, Y. & Hidaka, H. Effect of a new Ca2+-calmodulin- 34. Ye, C., Kanazirska, M., Quinn, S., Brown, E. M. & Vassilev, P. M. Modulation
dependent protein kinase II inhibitor on GABA release in cerebrospinal fluid by polycationic Ca(2+)-sensing receptor agonists of nonselective cation
of the rat. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 254, 598602 (1990). channels in rat hippocampal neurons. Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun. 224,
14. Li, G., Hidaka, H. & Wollheim, C. B. Inhibition of voltage-gated Ca2+ 271280 (1996).
channels and insulin secretion in HIT cells by the Ca2+/calmodulin- 35. Willuweit, B. & Aktories, K. Heparin uncouples alpha 2-adrenoceptors from
dependent protein kinase II inhibitor KN-62: comparison with antagonists of the Gi-protein in membranes of human platelets. Biochem. J. 249, 857863
calmodulin and L- type Ca2+ channels. Mol. Pharmacol. 42, 489488 (1992). (1988).
15. Pinna, L. A. Casein kinase 2: an eminence grise in cellular regulation? 36. Ehrlich, B. E., Kaftan, E., Bezprozvannaya, S. & Bezprozvanny, I. The
Biochim. Biophys. Acta 1054, 267284 (1990). pharmacology of intracellular Ca2+-release channels. Trends Pharmacol. Sci.
16. Tuazon, P. T. & Traugh, J. A. Casein kinase I and IImultipotential serine 15, 145149 (1994).
protein kinases: structure, function, and regulation. Adv. Second Messenger 37. Sokabe, M. & Sachs, F. The structure and dynamics of patch-clamped
Phosphoprotein Res. 23, 123164 (1991). membranes: a study using differential interference contrast light microscopy.
17. Girault, J. A., Hemmings, H. C. J., Zorn, S. H., Gustafson, E. L. & Greengard, J. Cell Biol. 111, 599606 (1990).
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

P. Characterization in mammalian brain of a DARPP-32 serine kinase 38. Nguyen, P. V., Abel, T. & Kandel, E. R. Requirement of a critical period of
identical to casein kinase II. J. Neurochem. 55, 17721783 (1990). transcription for induction of a late phase of LTP. Science 265, 11041107
18. Krek, W., Maridor, G. & Nigg, E. A. Casein kinase II is a predominantly (1994).
nuclear enzyme. J. Cell Biol. 116, 4355 (1992). 39. Apel, E. D., Litchfield, D. W., Clark, R. H., Krebs, E. G. & Storm, D. R.
19. Zandomeni, R. & Weinmann, R. Inhibitory effect of 5,6-dichloro-1-beta-D- Phosphorylation of neuromodulin (GAP-43) by casein kinase II.
ribofuranosylbenzimidazole on a protein kinase. J. Biol. Chem. 259, Identification of phosphorylation sites and regulation by calmodulin. (Au: is
1480414811 (1984). this title correct?). J. Biol. Chem. 266, 1054410551 (1991).
20. Zandomeni, R., Zandomeni, M. C., Shugar, D. & Weinmann, R. Casein 40. Bennett, M. K., Miller, K. G. & Scheller, R. H. Casein kinase II phosphorylates
kinase type II is involved in the inhibition by 5,6-dichloro-1-beta-D- the synaptic vesicle protein p65. J. Neurosci. 13, 17011707 (1993).
ribofuranosylbenzimidazole of specific RNA polymerase II transcription. J. 41. Gilad, G. M., Gilad, V. H. & Wyatt, R. J. Accumulation of exogenous
Biol. Chem. 261, 34143419 (1986). polyamines in gerbil brain after ischemia. Mol. Chem. Neuropathol. 18,
21. Williams, K., Romano, C. & Molinoff, P. B. Effects of polyamines on the 197210 (1993).
binding of [3H]MK-801 to the N- methyl-D-aspartate receptor: 42. Johnson, T. D. Polyamines and cerebral ischemia. Prog. Drug Res. 50,
pharmacological evidence for the existence of a polyamine recognition site. 193258 (1998).
Mol. Pharmacol. 36, 575581 (1989). 43. Sacks, D. B. et al. Phosphorylation by casein kinase II alters the biological
22. Lerma, J. Spermine regulates N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor desensitization. activity of calmodulin. Biochem. J. 283, 2124 (1992).
Neuron 8, 343352 (1992). 44. Ehlers, M. D., Zhang, S., Bernhardt, J. P. & Huganir, R. L. Inactivation of
23. Benveniste, M. & Mayer, M. L. Multiple effects of spermine on N-methyl-D- NMDA receptors by direct interaction of calmodulin with the NR1 subunit
aspartic acid receptor responses of rat cultured hippocampal neurones. J. Cell 84, 745755 (1996).
Physiol. (Lond.) 464, 131163 (1993). 45. Charriaut-Marlangue, C., Otani, S., Creuzet, C., Ben-Ari, Y. & Loeb, J. Rapid
24. Traynelis, S. F., Hartley, M. & Heinemann, S. F. Control of proton sensitivity activation of hippocampal casein kinase II during long- term potentiation.
of the NMDA receptor by RNA splicing and polyamines. Science 268, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.USA. 88, 1023210236 (1991).
873876 (1995). 46. Blanquet, P. R. Neurotrophin-induced activation of casein kinase 2 in rat
25. Shaw, G. G. & Pateman, A. J. The regional distribution of the polyamines hippocampal slices. Neuroscience 86, 739749 (1998).
spermidine and spermine in brain. J. Neurochem. 20, 12251230 (1973). 47. Barroga, C. F., Stevenson, J. K., Schwarz, E. M. & Verma, I. M. Constitutive
26. Harman, R. J. & Shaw, G. G. High-affinity uptake of spermine by slices of rat phosphorylation of I kappa B alpha by casein kinase II. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
cerebral cortex. J. Neurochem. 36, 16091615 (1981). USA. 92, 76377641 (1995).
27. Gilad, G. M. & Gilad, V. H. Polyamine uptake, binding and release in rat 48. Hbner, S., Xiao, C. Y. & Jans, D. A. The protein kinase CK2 site (Ser111/112)
brain. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 193, 4146 (1991). enhances recognition of the simian virus 40 large T-antigen nuclear
28. Bhring, R., Bowie, D., Benveniste, M. & Mayer, M. L. Permeation and block localization sequence by importin. J. Biol. Chem. 272, 1719117195 (1997).
of rat GluR6 glutamate receptor channels by internal and external 49. Kaltschmidt, C., Kaltschmidt, B. & Baeuerle, P. A. Stimulation of ionotropic
polyamines. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 502, 575589 (1997). glutamate receptors activates transcription factor NF-kappa B in primary
29. Critchfield, J. W., Coligan, J. E., Folks, T. M. & Butera, S. T. Casein kinase II is neurons. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 92, 96189622 (1995).
a selective target of HIV 1 transcriptional inhibitors. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 50. Khr, G., De Koninck, Y. & Mody, I. Properties of NMDA receptor channels
USA. 94, 61106115 (1997). in neurons acutely isolated from epileptic (kindled) rats. J. Neurosci. 13,
30. Lopatin, A. N., Makhina, E. N. & Nichols, C. G. Potassium channel block by 36123627 (1993).

132 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Adrenaline enhances odorant


contrast by modulating signal
encoding in olfactory receptor cells
Fusao Kawai1,2, Takashi Kurahashi1,3,4 and Akimichi Kaneko1,5

1 Department of Information Physiology, National Institute for Physiological Sciences, Myodaiji, Okazaki 444-8585, Japan
2 Department of Neuroscience, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania 19104-6058, USA
3 Present address: Department of Biology, Osaka University, Toyonaka 560-0043, Japan
4 Present address: Precursory Research for Embryonic Science and Technology (PRESTO), Senri Life Science Center 10F,
Sinsenri, Toyonaka 565-0082, Japan
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

5 Present address: Department of Physiology, Keio University School of Medicine, Shinjuku, Tokyo 160-8582, Japan
Correspondence should be addressed to T.K. (kurahasi@bio.sci.osaka-u.ac.jp)

Olfactory perception is influenced by hormones. Here we report that adrenaline can directly affect
the signal encoding of olfactory receptor cells. Application of adrenaline suppressed action
potentials near threshold and increased their frequency in response to strong stimuli, resulting in a
narrower dynamic range. Under voltage-clamp conditions, adrenaline enhanced sodium current and
reduced T-type calcium current. Because sodium current is the major component of spike generation
and T-type calcium current lowers the threshold in olfactory receptor cells, the effects of adrenaline
on these currents are consistent with the results obtained under current-clamp conditions. Both
effects involved a common cytoplasmic pathway, cAMP-dependent phosphorylation. We suggest
that adrenaline may enhance contrast in olfactory perception by this mechanism.

Olfactory perception, which starts in the olfactory receptor cells sensory contrast. We therefore conclude that adrenaline enhances
(ORCs) of the olfactory epithelium17, is influenced by behav- odorant contrast in olfactory perception.
ioral states, presumably via hormonal regulation8. Cellular olfac-
tory sensitivities are known to be influenced by hormones. The RESULTS
olfactory mucosa is innervated by adrenergic fibers911 and is Adrenaline modulates spike generation
strongly immunoreactive for the -adrenergic receptor12. Sym- We examined the effect of adrenaline on spike generation in iso-
pathetic nerve stimulation, which releases noradrenaline, increas- lated olfactory cells. The olfactory receptor potential was mim-
es electrical activity in the olfactory nerve13. Adrenaline also icked by injecting an extrinsic current from the recording pipette.
enhances the amplitude of the electro-olfactogram induced by The current amplitudes were chosen to correspond to ORC
an odorant14. Because these experiments were done in olfactory responses to a typical range of odor concentrations. There are
tissues containing many types of cells, however, the actual target several advantages in using current injection over the natural
of adrenaline has not yet been identified. odorant stimuli. First, in isolated ORCs, odorants have secondary
Because olfactory cilia, the cellular site of olfactory transduc- effects15 of nonselectively suppressing voltage-gated ionic chan-
tion17, are exposed to the external environment, a widely accept- nels and therefore action potentials16, which do not occur with
ed hypothesis is that the adrenergic fibers do not affect the current injection. Second, one can regulate the amount of depo-
transduction machinery at the receptor cell level8, but instead larizing current precisely, whereas cell stimulation by odorant
regulate the secretion of mucus covering the olfactory epitheli- molecules is difficult to control. Because the main purpose of
um. Thick mucus would obstruct odorants from the ciliary mem- our study was to examine the effect of adrenaline on the spike
brane of ORCs912. Here we investigated whether adrenaline generation at the cell body, we mainly used ORCs that had lost
directly affects olfactory cells, focusing on electrical activity in their cilia.
the cell body and axon, which are exposed to the interstitial solu- Bath-applied adrenaline had complex, reversible effects on
tion. Single newt olfactory receptor cells were dissociated from spike generation. Under weakly stimulated conditions (thresh-
the olfactory epithelium, and their inputoutput relationship old level, Fig. 1a), adrenaline suppressed spike generation. In
was examined by whole-cell patch clamp. Because the site where contrast, for strong stimuli causing repetitive spikes, adrenaline
adrenaline could affect the olfactory cell is assumed to be the den- increased spike frequency (Fig. 1b). In the presence of adrena-
drosomatic membrane, we mainly focused on cell body activity line, the stimulusresponse relationship became steeper, with a
and resulting action potential generation. Application of adren- narrower dynamic range (Fig. 1c). Mean slopes between 6 and
aline narrowed the dynamic range of spike generation, which 9 pA were 0.4 spikes/secondpA in the control and 1.9 spikes/sec-
increased the slope of the inputoutput relationship. In general, ondpA in the presence of adrenaline. Adrenaline thus amplified
changing the slope of the intensityresponse curve changes the the mean slope by approximately fivefold.

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 133


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 1. Adrenaline narrows the


dynamic range of spike fre- a b c
quency in isolated ORCs.
(a) Responses to near-thresh-

Impulses/s
old depolarization induced by

mV
mV
injection of 5-pA current,
recorded in control Ringers
solution (thin line) or 3 minutes
after addition of 10 M adrena-
line (thick line). The recording
pipettes were filled with potas-
sium solution. (b) Responses
to a depolarization induced by injection of 7-pA current, recorded from the same cell as (a). (c) The relationship between current injection and spike
frequency in control solution (filled circles) and 10 M adrenaline (filled triangles). Each symbol represents the mean of five cells standard error.

Effects of adrenaline on INa and ICa,T and a Hill coefficient of 1.1. The activation curves of INa were
Spikes in ORCs are triggered by activation of sodium (INa)1722 fitted by a single Boltzmann function with a half-activation volt-
and T-type calcium (ICa,T) currents21,22. To understand the mech- age of 37 mV in control solution (Fig. 2e), as previously report-
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

anism underlying modulation of spikes by adrenaline, we exam- ed 17 . Adrenaline increased the relative conductance (g Na )
ined its effects on INa and ICa,T under voltage clamp. Adrenaline between 70 mV and 40 mV (half-activation voltage, 41 mV).
(10 M) increased the peak amplitude of I Na by 17 4% In contrast, adrenaline did not change the inactivation curve
(mean standard error, n = 5) within 150200 seconds of appli- significantly (Fig. 2e). These results suggest that the augmenta-
cation in all ORCs recorded and accelerated both activation and tion of INa by adrenaline was caused by a shift in the activation
inactivation (Fig. 2a). The effect was reversible (Fig. 2b) and was curve toward a more negative voltage.
evident between 70 mV and 40 mV (Fig. 2c). Similar aug- Adrenaline also reversibly reduced the peak amplitude of ICa,T
mentation of INa (18 5%, n = 4) was obtained from ORCs with in all recorded ORCs by 33 7% (n = 4), but did not change the
their cilia attached, suggesting that the effect is mainly due to activation and inactivation time courses (Fig. 3a and b). A sim-
adrenergic receptors on the soma rather than on the cilia. ilar reduction (31 6%, n = 4) was observed in ORCs with the
The effects of adrenaline on I Na were dose dependent cilia attached. The reduction occurred over a wide voltage range
(Fig. 2d). The doseresponse curve was fitted by the Hill equa- (Fig. 3c). The effects of adrenaline on ICa,T were dose depen-
tion with a half-augmenting concentration (EC50) of 290 nM dent, with an IC 50 of 150 nM and a Hill coefficient of 1.1

a b c
Relative amplitude
pA

Fig. 2. Adrenaline augments INa in isolated


ORCs. (a) INa induced by depolarization to 40 d e
mV (holding potential, Vh = 100 mV) in control
Relative conductance

solution (thin line) or 10 M adrenaline (thick


Relative amplitude

line) and after washout (dotted line). These


responses were recorded at 0 s (control), 180 s
(adrenaline) and 600 s (wash) of the experiment
shown in (b). The recording pipette was filled
with cesium solution. (b) Normalized ampli-
tudes of INa plotted against time. INa was
recorded every 30 s. (c) IV relationship of the
cell shown in (a). Peak INas in control solution
(filled circles) or in 10 M adrenaline (filled tri- Concentration (M) mV
angles) were plotted against test-pulse voltage.
(d) Relationship between the augmentation of
INa and adrenaline concentration (n = 5). The line represents the Hill equation obtained by least-squares nonlinear fit to the data. (e) Effects of
10 M adrenaline on the activation and inactivation curves of INa (n = 5). Vh for activation curves was 100 mV. Test voltage for inactivation curves
after a 1-s conditioning pulse was 0 mV. Lines represent a single Boltzmann function obtained by least-squares nonlinear fit to the data.

134 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

a b c

Relative amplitude
pA

Fig. 3. Adrenaline inhibits ICa,T in isolated ORCs. d e


(a) ICa,T induced by depolarization to 40 mV (Vh
= 100 mV) in control solution (thin line), in 10 M

Relative conductance
Relative amplitude

adrenaline (thick line) and after washout (dotted


line). These responses were recorded at 0 s (con-
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

trol), 210 s (adrenaline) and 600 s (wash) of the


experiment shown in (b). (b) Time course of nor-
malized amplitudes of ICa,T, recorded every 30 s.
(c) Effects of 10 M adrenaline on the IV relation-
ship of the cell shown in (a). (d) Doseresponse
curve of ICa,T (n = 4). (e) Activation and inactiva-
tion curves of ICa,T (n = 4).
Concentration (M) mV

(Fig. 3d). Adrenaline did not change activation and inactivation the sodium channel or by cAMP-dependent phosphorylation,
curves significantly (Fig. 3e), suggesting that it does not mod- we used the catalytic subunit of the cAMP-dependent protein
ulate the voltage dependence of ICa,T. Thus, inhibition of ICa,T kinase (PKA). After intracellular application of PKA by the
by adrenaline results from a reduction in the total T-type calci- whole-cell pipette, INa increased by 37 8% (n = 5; Fig. 5b). In
um channel conductance. addition, a PKA inhibitor blocked the effect of 8-bromo-cAMP
(n = 4; Fig. 5c), excluding the possibility of a direct action of
Effects of cAMP on INa and ICa,T cAMP on the sodium channel. These results suggest that the
Because the -adrenergic receptor uses cyclic adenosine effect of cAMP on INa is mediated by PKA.
monophosphate (cAMP) as a second messenger in a wide variety Application of 8-bromo-cAMP reduced the amplitude of ICa,T
of tissues2325, we next examined the effect of cytoplasmic cAMP over the entire voltage range examined (Fig. 4d and e). 8-bromo-
on INa and ICa,T. Addition of a membrane-permeable cAMP ana- cAMP did not change the activation and inactivation curves of
logue (1 mM 8-bromo-cAMP; note that the increase in cyto- ICa,T significantly (Fig. 4f), consistent with the effects of adrena-
plasmic cAMP concentration was lower than this value) to the line (Fig. 3e) and reports using other preparations29,30. The
superfusate increased the peak amplitude of I Na by 28 5% doseresponse curve was fitted by the Hill equation with a half-
(n = 6; Fig. 4a). This value was larger than that obtained with blocking concentration (IC50) of 11 M and a Hill coefficient of
10 M adrenaline (17 4%, Fig. 2d). This difference might be 1.8. This reduction of ICa,T is unlikely to be due to run-down,
due to the high concentration of 8-bromo-cAMP (1 mM) used. because the 8-bromo-cAMP-induced reduction in current
The augmentation was observed between 70 mV and 40 mV (39 5%, 1 min after application) was significantly larger than
(Fig. 4b) and was reversible. The effect of 8-bromo-cAMP on INa the reduction due to run-down (6 3%, at 10 min after rupture
was dose dependent; the doseresponse curve was fitted by the of the patch membrane, n = 4).
Hill equation with an EC50 of 75 M and a Hill coefficient of 1.5. When cAMP was introduced into the cell interior from the
Cyclic AMP applied directly to the cytoplasm from the patch patch pipette, ICa,T was also reduced. The amplitude of ICa,T was
pipette also increased INa (121 7%, n = 5). These results are decreased by 39 4% (n = 5). Extracellular forskolin also reduced
consistent with previous reports using other preparations from ICa,T by 37 5% (n = 5; Fig. 5d). Furthermore, application of
several tissues2628. Furthermore, the effects of 8-bromo-cAMP PKA via the patch pipette reduced I Ca,T by 34 5% (n = 4;
on the activation and inactivation curves of INa were consistent Fig. 5e), and the PKA inhibitor blocked the reduction caused by
with the effects of adrenaline. Application of 8-bromo-cAMP 8-bromo-cAMP (n = 4; Fig. 5f). These results suggest that the
strongly enhanced the activation curve between 70 mV and effect of cAMP on ICa,T is also mediated by PKA.
40 mV (Fig. 4c).
A direct activator of adenylyl cyclase (10 M forskolin) added DISCUSSION
to the bath also increased the amplitude of INa, by 34 6% (n = 5; In isolated ORCs under whole-cell, patch-clamp recording, we found
Fig. 5a), equivalent to the increase induced by cAMP. This result that adrenaline can directly affect spike encoding by modulating INa
suggests that cAMP was produced intracellularly by an adenylyl and ICa,T via cAMP-dependent protein phosphorylation. Adrena-
cyclase in the dendrosomatic membrane of ORCs. To test whether line would presumably activate -adrenergic receptors on the soma,
the enhancement of INa is mediated by a direct action of cAMP on which would shift the spike threshold to a higher voltage by sup-

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 135


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

a b c

Relative conductance
pA

d e f

Relative conductance
pA
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

Fig. 4. 8-bromo-cAMP augments INa and inhibits ICa,T. (a) INa induced by depolarization to 40 mV (Vh = 100 mV) in control solution (thin line) and
in 1 mM 8-bromo-cAMP (thick line). The recording pipette was filled with cesium solution. (b) Effects of 8-bromo-cAMP on the IV relationship of
INa. (c) Effects of 8-bromo-cAMP on the activation and inactivation curves of INa (n = 6). (d) Effects of 8-bromo-cAMP on ICa,T. (e) IV relationship
of ICa,T. (f) Effects of 8-bromo-cAMP on activation and inactivation curves of ICa,T (n = 6).

pressing ICa,T and increase the firing frequency in response to a strong aline, as a fivefold increase in the slope of intensityresponse
stimulus by enhancing INa. These two opposing effects on spike gen- curve seems likely to be functionally significant.
eration probably narrow the dynamic range of ORCs, but increase Guanine nucleotides (GTP and GTPS) have been shown
the gain slope of signal encoding within this dynamic range. Con- to increase the amplitude of INa in the frog ORC34, suggesting
sequently, cells would be more sensitive to the difference between that activated G-proteins might affect the sodium channel
the presence of an odor stimulus and its absence, because the ampli- directly. Although our present data initially seem similar
tude of the transduction current rises with increasing odor concen- because G-protein activation causes the cyclase to produce
tration31. Under natural conditions, this is equivalent to improving intracellular cAMP, closer examination reveals an important
the cells ability to identify the presence of odorants. difference between our results and this previous work. Aug-
Several findings in vivo suggest that similar effects may occur mentation of INa in our experiments was due to a shift of the
under physiological conditions. Sympathetic nerve stimulation activation curve to a negative voltage, whereas in the previous
increases electrical activity in the rabbit olfactory nerve13, and report, it was attributed to a shift of the inactivation curve to a
adrenaline and noradrenaline enhance the amplitude of the elec- positive direction34. In addition, the authors did not include
tro-olfactogram (EOG) induced by an odorant14. Although the Mg-ATP in their pipette, and therefore may not have been able
EOG is thought to originate mainly from the receptor potential, to see the phosphorylation process.
some fraction might originate from spike bursting. Here we Odorant signal transduction is widely considered to be medi-
showed that adrenaline increases spike activity by enhancing the ated by cAMP at the cilia. One might therefore expect cross-talk
amplitude of INa when strong current is injected into ORCs. between cAMP produced at the cilia and cAMP produced by
Because these previous studies used high concentrations of odor- adrenaline at the cell body. We estimate, however, that the cilia
ant, the enhancement of EOG amplitude that they demonstrated and the cell body are independent in this regard. It is very unlike-
might be due to an increase in INa amplitude by adrenaline. ly that cAMP produced by an odor stimulus in the cilia increas-
An alternative hypothesis is that adrenaline affects ORC es cAMP concentration in the soma, because the total volume of
responses by regulating vasomotor tone and secretion from Bow- the cilia is only 0.18% of the cell body volume (five 30 m long
mans glands, which modulate odorant access to and clearance cylindrical cilia with 0.25 m diameter versus a spherical cell
from the olfactory epithelium911,32. Adrenergic fibers are found body with 10 m diameter). Furthermore, phosphodiesterase
in human olfactory mucosa by immunohistochemistry9, and - (PDE) activity in the cilia is so high that the cytoplasmic cAMP is
adrenoceptors are expressed in nasal glands as well as ORCs12. hydrolyzed quickly35. Therefore, cAMP induced by odor stimu-
Stimulation by the -adrenergic agonist isoproterenol causes lus in the cilia is unlikely to affect INa and ICa,T in the newt ORC.
release of secretory granules from Bowmans glands in the dog33 This strong PDE activity would also form a boundary sep-
and tiger salamander10. Although our data do not exclude this arating the cilia from the cell body, by breaking down cAMP
possibility, adrenalin-induced modulation of activity at the cell synthesized in the cell body by adrenaline. Indeed, the con-
body of ORCs should be regarded as an important effect of adren- centration of cAMP introduced from the whole-cell recording

136 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

a b c

d e f
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

Fig. 5. Cyclic AMP augments INa and inhibits ICa,T via PKA. (a) INa induced by depolarization to 40 mV (Vh = 100 mV) in control solution (thin line) and
in 10 M forskolin (thick line). (b) INa recorded just after (thin line) and five min after (thick line) rupture of the patch membrane. The recording pipette
was filled with cesium solution containing the catalytic subunit of protein kinase A (PKA, 5 g/ml). (c) INa recorded in control solution (thin line) and in 1
mM 8-bromo-cAMP (thick line). The recording pipette contained PKA inhibitor (PKI, 400 g/ml). The superfusate was changed five min after rupture of
the patch membrane. (df) Effects of forskolin (d), PKA (e) and PKI (f) on ICa,T . Each ICa,T was recorded with the same protocols used in (ac).

pipette to the dendritic tip that is necessary to induce a Solutions and drugs. The recording pipette was filled with cesium solu-
detectable response is higher than 100 M (ref. 36). This value tion (119 mM CsCl, 1 mM CaCl2, 5 mM EGTA, 10 mM HEPES and 1
is much higher than that of cAMP-gated channels measured mM Mg-ATP, pH adjusted to 7.4 with CsOH) to suppress outward cur-
in excised inside-out patch preparations (as low as 1 M; refs rents, or pseudo-intracellular (potassium) solution (119 mM KCl, 1
37, 38). Moreover, olfactory cilia show adaptation to prolonged mM CaCl 2 , 5 mM EGTA, 10 mM HEPES and 1 mM Mg-ATP, pH
adjusted to 7.4 with KOH) to record action potentials. The resistance of
stimulation, which is regulated by calcium feedback39,40. There- the pipette was about 10 M. The control extracellular solution used to
fore, even if a small amount of cAMP produced by adrenaline record INa contained 80 mM NaCl, 3.7 mM KCl, 1 mM CoCl2, 35 mM
gradually diffused into cilia from the cell body, the basal trans- TEA, 2 mM HEPES and 15 mM glucose, and the solution used to
duction current would not be affected, but additional odorant record ICa,T contained 77 mM choline-Cl, 3.7 mM KCl, 3 mM CaCl2,
stimulation would increase adenylyl cyclase activity and cause 0.1 mM CdCl2, 35 mM TEA, 2 mM HEPES and 15 mM glucose.
normal transduction current. Drugs were applied either to the bath or to the cytoplasm from the
Generally in sensory systems, changes in the dynamic range patch pipette. To examine the effect of a drug applied from the patch
are related to sensitivity contrast. This concept is widely used in pipette, we used the record obtained immediately after the establish-
industrial sensors (such as photo-scanning systems). Based on ment of the whole-cell recording condition as a control. Records
obtained several minutes after rupture of the patch membrane were
the present experimental results, therefore, we suggest that the
compared against the control record. Forskolin was initially dissolved
adrenergic system acts to enhance odorant contrast in olfac- in dimethylsulfoxide (DMSO) at a concentration of 10 mM and then
tory perception by modulating signal encoding at the cell body diluted to 1/1000 with bath solution, giving a final concentration of
and/or axon of ORCs. 10 M. 8-bromo-cAMP (1 M1 mM), forskolin and adrenaline (10
METHODS nM100 M) were bath applied from the U-tube system. The catalyt-
Preparation and recording procedures. ORCs were dissociated enzy- ic subunit of PKA (from bovine heart) was initially dissolved in 60 l
matically from the olfactory epithelium of the newt, Cynops of 5 mM dithiothreithol in water and was diluted with the pipette solu-
pyrrhogaster as reported31. Isolated cells were viewed on an Olympus tion. Cyclic AMP (1 mM), protein kinase inhibitor peptide (400 g/ml,
upright microscope with differential interference contrast optics (40 type II) and the catalytic subunit of PKA (5 g/ml) were applied from
water-immersion objective). To study I Na and I Ca,T of the somatic the patch pipette. All chemicals were purchased from Sigma.
membrane, we mainly selected ORCs that had lost their cilia. Mem-
brane voltages and currents were recorded in the whole-cell configu- ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
ration 41 using a patch-clamp amplifier (Axopatch 1-D, Axon We thank Peter Sterling, Robert Smith, Noga Vardi, Michael Freed, Loren
Instruments) linked to a computer. Recording procedures were con- Haarsma and Jonathan Demb for comments and support, Yoshikuni Ito for
trolled by pCLAMP software (Axon Instruments). Data were low-pass technical support and Akiko Kawai for secretarial assistance. This work was in
filtered (4-pole Bessel type) with a cut-off frequency of 5 kHz and then part supported by grants from HFSPO and the Japanese MESSC (to TK).
digitized at 10 kHz by an analog-to-digital interface. All experiments
were done at room temperature (2325C). ACCEPTED 19 NOVEMBER 1998

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 137


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

1. Gold, G. H. & Nakamura, T. Cyclic nucleotide-gated conductances: a new threshold of spike generation in the newt olfactory receptor cell. J. Gen.
class of ionic channels mediates visual and olfactory transduction. Trends Physiol. 108, 525535 (1996).
Pharmacol. 8, 312316 (1987). 22. Kawai, F., Kurahashi, T. & Kaneko, A. Quantitative analysis of Na+ and Ca2+
2. Bakalyar, H. A. & Reed, R. R. The second messenger cascade in olfactory current contributions on spike initiation in the newt olfactory receptor cell.
receptor neurons. Curr. Biol. 1, 204208 (1991). Jpn. J. Physiol. 47, 367376 (1997).
3. Breer, H. & Boekhoff, I. Second messenger signalling in olfaction. Curr. Biol. 23. Lamb, T. D. & Pugh, E. N. Jr. G-protein cascade, gain and kinetics. Trends
2, 439443 (1992). Neurosci. 15, 291298 (1992).
4. Ronnett, G. V. & Snyder, S. H. Molecular messengers of olfaction. Trends 24. Hille, B. in Ionic Channels of Excitable Membranes (ed. Hille, B.) 170201
Neurosci. 15, 508513 (1992). (Sinauer, Sunderland, 1992).
5. Firestein, S. Electrical signals in olfactory transduction. Curr. Biol. 2, 444448 25. Hille, B. Modulation of ion-channel function of G-protein-coupled receptor.
(1992). Trends Neurosci. 17, 531535 (1994).
6. Kurahashi, T. & Yau, K.-W. Tale of an unusual chloride current. Curr. Biol. 4, 26. Seelig, T. L. & Kendig, J. J. Cyclic nucleotide modulation of Na+ and K+
256258 (1994). currents in the isolated node of Ranvier. Brain Res. 245, 144147 (1982).
7. Restrepo, D., Teeter, J. H. & Schild, D. Second messenger signaling in 27. Matsuda, J. J., Lee, H. & Shibata, E. F. Enhancement of rabbit cardiac sodium
olfactory transduction. J. Neurobiol. 30, 3748 (1996). channels by -adrenergic stimulation. Circ. Res. 70, 199207 (1991).
8. Takagi, S. F. in Human Olfaction (ed. Takagi, S. F.) 1481 (Univ. of Tokyo 28. Ono, K., Fozzard, H. A. & Hanck, D. A. Mechanism of cAMP-dependent
Press, Tokyo, 1989). modulation of cardiac sodium channel current kinetics. Circ. Res. 72,
9. Chen, Y., Getchell, T. V, Sparks, D. L. & Getchell, M. L. Patterns of adrenergic 807815 (1992).
and peptidergic innervation in human olfactory mucosa: age-related trends. 29. Narahashi, T., Tsunoo, A. & Yoshii, M. Characterization of two types of
J. Comp. Neurol. 334, 104116 (1993). calcium channels in mouse neuroblastoma cells J. Physiol. (Lond.) 383,
10. Getchell, M. L. & Getchell, T. V. -adrenergic regulation of the secretory 231249 (1987).
granule content of acinar cells in olfactory glands of the salamander. J. Comp. 30. Pfeiffer-Linn, C. & Lasater, E. M. Dopamine modulates in a differential
Neurol. 155, 435443 (1984). fashion T- and L-type calcium currents in bass retinal horizontal cells. J. Gen.
11. Zielinski, B. S., Getchell, M. L., Wenokur, R. L. & Getchell, T. V. Physiol. 102, 277294 (1993).
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

Ultrastructural localization and identification of adrenergic and cholinergic 31. Kurahashi, T. Activation by odorants of cation-selective conductance in the
nerve terminals in the olfactory mucosa. Anat. Rec. 225, 232245 (1989). olfactory receptor cells isolated from the newt. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 419,
12. Woodhead, C. J. & Nimmo, A. J. Beta adrenoceptors in human nasal mucosa. 177192 (1989).
J. Laryngol. Otol. 105, 632634 (1991). 32. Getchell, T. V., Margolis, F. L. & Getchell, M. L. Perireceptor and receptor
13. Beidler, L. M. in Sensory Communication (ed. Rosenblith, W. A.) 143157 events in vertebrate olfaction. Prog. Neurobiol. 23, 317345 (1985).
(Wiley and MIT Press, New York, 1961). 33. Hall, L. J. & Jackson, R. T. Effects of alpha and beta adrenergic agonists on
14. Arechiga, C. & Alcocer, H. Adrenergic effects on electro-olfactogram. Exp. nasal blood flow. Ann. Otol. Rhinol. Laryngol. 77, 11201130 (1968).
Med. Surg. 27, 384394 (1969). 34. Pun, R. Y. K., Kleene, S. J. & Gesteland, R.C. Guanine nucleotides modulate
15. Kurahashi, T., Lowe, G. & Gold, G. H. Suppression of odorant responses by steady-state inactivation of voltage-gated sodium channels in frog olfactory
odorants in olfactory receptor cells. Science 265, 118120 (1994). receptor neurons. J. Membr. Biol. 142, 103111 (1994).
16. Kawai, F., Kurahashi, T. & Kaneko, A. Nonselective suppression of voltage- 35. Lowe, G. & Gold, G. H. Nonlinear amplification by calcium-dependent
gated currents by odorants in the newt olfactory receptor cells. J. Gen. Physiol. chloride channels in olfactory receptor cells. Nature 366, 283286 (1993).
109, 265272 (1997). 36. Kurahashi, T. The response induced by intracellular cyclic AMP in isolated
17. Firestein, S. & Werblin, F. S. Gating currents in isolated olfactory receptor olfactory receptor cells of the newt. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 430, 355371 (1990).
neurons of the larval tiger salamander. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 88, 37. Kurahashi, T. & Kaneko, A. High density cAMP-gated channels at the ciliary
62926296 (1987). membrane in the olfactory receptor cell. Neuroreport 2, 58 (1991).
18. Schild, D. Whole-cell currents in olfactory receptor cells on Xenopus laevis. 38. Kurahashi, T. & Kaneko, A. Gating properties of the cAMP-gated channel in
Brain Res. 78, 223232 (1989). toad olfactory receptor cells. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 466, 287302 (1993).
19. Miyamoto, T., Restrepo, D. & Teeter, J. H. Voltage-dependent and odorant- 39. Kurahashi, T. & Shibuya, T. Ca2+-dependent adaptive properties in the
regulated currents in isolated olfactory receptor neurons of the channel solitary olfactory receptor cells of newt. Brain Res. 515, 261268 (1990).
catfish. J. Gen. Physiol. 99, 505530 (1992). 40. Kurahashi, T. & Menini, A. Mechanism of odorant adaptation in the olfactory
20. Dubin, A. E. & Dionne, V. E. Action potentials and chemosensitive receptor cell. Nature 385, 725729 (1997).
conductances in the dendrites of olfactory neurons suggest new features for 41. Hamill, O. P., Marty, A., Neher, E., Sakmann, B. & Sigworth, F. J. Improved
odor transduction. J. Gen. Physiol. 103, 181201 (1994). patch-clamp techniques for high resolution current recording from cells and
21. Kawai, F., Kurahashi, T. & Kaneko, A. T-type Ca2+ channel lowers the cell-free membrane patches. Pflugers Arch. 391, 85100 (1981).

138 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Microdomains for neuronglia


interaction: parallel fiber signaling
to Bergmann glial cells
Jens Grosche1, Vitali Matyash2, Thomas Mller2, Alexej Verkhratsky2, Andreas Reichenbach1
and Helmut Kettenmann2

1 Paul Flechsig Institute for Brain Research, University of Leipzig, Jahnallee 59, D-04109 Leipzig, Germany
2 Max Delbrck Center for Molecular Medicine, Cellular Neurosciences, Robert-Rssle-Str. 10, D-13122 Berlin, Germany
The first two authors contributed equally to this work.
Correspondence should be addressed to H.K. (hk@nero.glia.mdc-berlin.de)
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

Astrocytes are considered a reticulate network of cells, through which calcium signals can spread easily.
In Bergmann glia, astrocytic cells of the cerebellum, we identified subcellular compartments termed
glial microdomains. These elements have a complex surface consisting of thin membrane sheets,
contain few mitochondria and wrap around synapses. To test for neuronal interaction with these
structures, we electrically stimulated parallel fibers. This stimulation increased intracellular calcium con-
centration ([Ca2+]i) in small compartments within Bergmann glial cell processes similar in size to glial
microdomains. Thus, a Bergmann glial cell may consist of hundreds of independent compartments
capable of autonomous interactions with the particular group of synapses that they ensheath.

Astrocytes are described as a reticulate network of cells. A series that these subcellular compartments appear organized and may
of cell-culture experiments established the concept that propa- spatially restrict neuron-glia interactions.
gating waves of intracellular calcium are a form of glial excitabil-
ity1,2. These waves can be initiated by a local stimulation, spread RESULTS
from one cell to the next via gap junctions, and can travel Appendages show a complex morphology
through tens of astrocytes. To analyze astrocytic calcium sig- We used serial electron microscopy and three-dimensional recon-
naling in situ, in cerebellar slices, we examined the Bergmann struction of processes from individual Bergmann glial cells to
glial cell, a radially aligned form of astrocyte, as a model for search for basic construction principles that might have been
local neuronalglial interactions. In cerebellar development, hidden in previous two-dimensional views. Furthermore, we
the Bergmann glial cell fibers are important in guiding imma- were interested in possible interactions between such glial sub-
ture granule cells away from the external granular layer3. Dur- structures and neurons. Because neither glial fibrillary acidic pro-
ing granule cell migration, the glial cell processes are rather thin tein nor S-100 protein are sufficiently enriched in fine terminal
and smooth, except for occasional bulges due to mitochondria. processes for their unequivocal immunocytochemical visualiza-
This appearance changes dramatically as soon as the granule tion, living Bergmann glial cells were injected with Lucifer Yel-
cells have reached their target zone. Then, the stem fibers thick- low in a murine cerebellar slice preparation (Fig. 1a). After
en and develop a massive outgrowth of lateral branches. These fixation, the dye was photoconverted for transmission electron
lateral appendages determine the characteristic morphological microscopy. Complete series of 40 or more sections were obtained
feature of an adult Bergmann glial cell and can be visualized, from 5 different cells. In one case, we were able to cut a complete
at the lightmicroscopic level, by Golgi impregnation, immuno- series of about 450 consecutive ultrathin sections, correspond-
cytochemical labeling or Lucifer Yellow injection4,5. Electron- ing to a more than 20-m segment of one stem fiber and its
microscopic inspection of glial elements in the molecular layer appendages. From these sections, images of the labelled glial pro-
revealed an elaborate plexus of fine processes, which varied con- files were transferred to a computer for three-dimensional recon-
siderably in diameter (from about one micron to about twice struction (Fig. 1b and c).
the membrane thickness). Unfortunately, the morphological Two classes of extensions from the radial fiber could be dis-
features of individual appendages cannot be reconstructed in tinguished. The first type was a short lamellar or thorn-like off-
conventional electron-microscopic studies because the shoot that densely covered the entire fiber. The second type was
appendages of these glial cells intermingle and cannot be dis- a much longer, frequently branching and highly complex out-
tinguished from one another. There have been a few reports of growth (Fig. 1b). The lateral appendages represented up to 90%
estimated surface-to-volume ratios of Bergmann glial process- of the cells total membrane surface area. Whereas a surface-to-
es, which were as high as about 13 to 15 per m (ref. 6). These volume ratio of 4.15 per m was calculated for the fiber (mean
observations have created the impression that glial appendages diameter about 3 m), values of up to 25 per m were calculat-
are irregular, random-shaped structures. Here we show instead ed for the appendages. The average surface-to-volume ratio of

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 139


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 1. Reconstruction of an appendage based on elec-


tron microscopic data. (a) Fluorescence light micro-
graph of a dye-injected Bergmann glial cell process is
shown; the red square (20 x 20 m) corresponds to the
portion that was reconstructed from consecutive ultra-
thin sections. (b) One of the lateral appendages, arising
directly from fiber (thick arrow), with all the other side
branches omitted for clarity. (c) The same appendage
as shown in (b), but with one of the appendages marked
by blue. This labeled structure is shown in isolation and
at higher magnification in Fig. 2 (left).

a b c

the reconstructed part of the cell was close to 13 per m. Thus The outgrowths from these thin stalks were highly polymorphic.
appendages increase the cells surface-to-volume ratio more than Extremely thin, flat or thread-like profiles were connected to thick
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

five times as compared to the radial process. and bulging parts in an unpredictable manner. This geometry is
in sharp contrast to that of neuronal dendrites where successive
The appendages are composed of microdomains branching occurs at more or less predictable distances, and these
Although they appeared extremely irregular at first sight, sever- branches are predictable in size7.
al general rules of the organization of these complex outgrowths The geometry of the glial appendages was not entirely irregu-
became obvious from the reconstruction. One surprising obser- lar, however. A smallest unit was recognized as consisting of a
vation was that all the many branches arose from only a few thin long, thin stalk connected to a cabbage-like head (Figs. 1c and 2).
stalks that left the fiber in a more or less orthogonal orientation. We term this structure a glial microdomain. The head shown in
Fig. 2 gave rise to a surface area of 316.9 m2 com-
pared to a volume of only 16.1 m3. The stalks had
a diameters of about 100 to 300 nm and could be more
than 7 m long. In the appendage shown in Fig. 2,
the almost cylindrical stalk had a surface area of 5.4
m2 and a volume of 0.37 m3. Thus it was mainly
the head that contributed to the enormous surface-
to-volume ratio of the appendage. Such a
microdomain may occur as a complete short out-
growth of the fiber or as a repetitive element consti-
tuting, in series, longer side branches. Thus, at some
degree of abstraction, Bergmann glial cell appendages
consist of repeating head elements connected to the
fiber, and/or to each other, by thin stalks.
The head had one or several swollen compart-
ment(s) containing mitochondria (Fig. 2); the num-
ber of mitochondria depended on the size of the
individual head. The presence of mitochondria in
each microdomain indicates that the microdomains
are metabolically independent. The metabolic sub-
strate for energy-consuming interactions with the
Fig. 2. Fine structure of appendages and rela-
tion to synapses. (a) A small lateral appendage,
b ensheathed neuronal compartments, essential for var-
arising from the reconstructed part of the glial
ious plasmalemmal transporters8,9, may be provid-
fiber (blue in Fig. 1c), is shown as a slightly ed by the local mitochondria. An alternative energy
turned, isolated three-dimensional reconstruc- source might be ATP supplied from the soma or fiber;
tion (left). Electron micrographs of four sections however, this seems unlikely because the diffusion of
contributing to the reconstruction (designated molecules through the long thin stalks must be sub-
14) are shown on the right; glial compartments ject to intolerable delay and decay.
appear black after conversion of the injected The leaflets of the head contacted individual
dye. The location of these sections in the recon- synapses, or small groups of synapses (sometimes
struction is indicated by the labeled arrows. (1) even apparent in two-dimensional sections such as
Region directly contacting synapses. (2) Glial
Fig. 2a, right). Although the method was optimized
compartments without direct synaptic contacts.
(3) Bulging glial structure containing a mitochon- for a detection of fine glial cell processes rather than
drion. (4) The stalk of the appendage. (b) Another example of a synapse contacted by glial for visualization of the neuronal ultrastructure, in
compartments, appearing as black structures (white arrows); in this case, the postsynaptic several cases the postsynaptic elements of such
element can be traced back along the spine (black arrows) to the Purkinje cell dendrite synapses could be traced back to their origin from
(reddish overlay; the presynaptic terminal is labeled by a green overlay). Purkinje cell dendrites (Fig. 2b). This shows that the

140 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

a b

Fig. 3. Stimulation of parallel fibers triggers local calcium


signals in Bergmann glial cells. (a) Experimental protocol.
Parallel fibers (PF) were stimulated via a pipette con-
nected to a stimulator (STIM) while calcium-dependent
fluorescence responses were recorded in a Bergmann
glial cell (BG); PCL, Purkinje cell layer. (b) Confocal fluorescence intensity image of a patch-clamped Bergmann glial cell dialyzed with the calcium indi-
cator, Oregon green 488 BAPTA-1 (right). Three processes were distinguished (indicated as 13). Calcium signals in response to PF stimulation were
measured independently for each process (left). Time of PF stimulation is marked by arrows. The responding process (1) was then further subdivided
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

into five regions of interest, in which calcium signals were measured separately (right). Time of PF stimulation is marked by an arrow and a dotted line.
The left image was obtained from a sequence of serial sections and shows the cell in depth (turned by 90 as compared to the right image). The lines
indicate the focal plane used for [Ca2+]i recordings. The process is thus within the volume from which recordings were obtained.

microdomains ensheath synapses between axon terminals of par- s. We found that not all processes from a given cell responded to the
allel fibers and dendritic spines of Purkinje cells. These sheaths stimulation, but rather observed that only small areas of the cell
were not complete, but glial leaflets were always found in the close showed an increase in fluorescence (Fig. 3b). To assess this more
vicinity of synaptic clefts (Fig. 2). quantitatively, we separately analyzed small compartments of about
20 to 50 m2 in size, and found that the signal was confined to
Local calcium signals only few of these compartments. In one example (Fig. 3b), we ana-
Next, we asked whether neuronglial signaling could be confined lyzed five compartments (~30 m2) along a Bergmann glial fiber.
to structures the size of a glial microdomain. We qualitatively mea- In two compartments, we found a fluorescence response. In three
sured intracellular calcium concentration ([Ca2+]i) in individual other compartments, no response could be detected. Because the
Bergmann glial cells in a cerebellar slice preparation by dialyzing the (projected) area covered by a microdomain is in the range of less
cell with the calcium-sensitive fluorescent dye Oregon green 488
BAPTA-1 hexapotassium salt via a patch pipette. For imaging, we
used a confocal microscope, with a lateral resolution just below 1 a b
m. Thus, we were not able to resolve the actual substructure of
the glial microdomain, but we could record the integrative response
from areas about the size of the microdomain. Slices were cut par-
allel to the orientation of the parallel fibers, which allowed us to
stimulate them by placing the electrode about 200 m away from
the Bergmann glial cell under study (Fig. 3a). Stimulation of par-
allel fibers by 10 brief pulses (200 s, 50 Hz), an experimental pro-
tocol that routinely induces synaptic responses in Purkinje
neurons10, elicited a rapid and transient increase in calcium indi-
cator fluorescence in glial processes. (We observed fluorescence
responses in 75 cells out of 126.) The signal peaked about 1 to 2 s
after stimulation and returned to the baseline level within 15 to 30
Control Control

Fig. 4. Inhibition of nerve impulse propagation and synaptic transmis-


sion blocks glial calcium responses. (a) Bergmann glial cell process fluo-
rescence intensity image (top). A region of interest is defined by the
white line. The time course of the fluorescence change (corresponding Cd2+
TTX
to intracellular [Ca2+] changes) was measured from the region of inter-
est in response to parallel-fiber stimulation (bottom). The fluorescence
signal recorded in control conditions was not observed in the presence
of tetrodotoxin (1 M TTX). The response recovered after a 10-min
washout. Time of PF stimulation is marked by an arrow. (b) Bergmann
Washout
glial cell process fluorescence intensity image (top). A region of interest
is defined by the white line. As in (a), [Ca2+]i signals in the glial cell
process were recorded in control conditions and after two minutes of
slice superfusion with 100 M cadmium (bottom). Time of PF stimula-
tion is marked by an arrow.

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 141


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

a b DISCUSSION
The emerging picture of independent glial microdomains, capa-
ble of glianeuronal interactions in a subcellular microenviron-
ment, changes the concept of the glial cells as integrated
members of a large interacting meshwork. The common image
of astrocytes describes these cells as members of a large network,
in which calcium waves can travel with little decrement within
the syncytium11,12. Appendages of Bergmann cells are obvious-
ly not designed for the spread of calcium waves within the cell
or between adjacent glial cells. Rather, calcium signals seem to
be confined to modules that each interact with only a few
synapses. These local [Ca2+]i increases could strongly influence
the physiological properties of the glial microdomain. Our pre-
vious studies show that [Ca2+] i elevation blocks the resting
Fig. 5. Spontaneous activity in Bergmann glia microdomains (a) A fluo- potassium conductance in Bergmann glial cells, leading to mem-
rescence intensity image of the Bergmann glial cell with selected regions brane depolarization6, and blocks communication via gap junc-
of interest. (b) The normalized fluorescence intensity (F/F0) traces tions either between cells or within the cell via autocellular
demonstrate spontaneous [Ca2+]i transients in regions 1, 2 and 4; coupling13. This may have consequences for synaptic transmis-
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

regions 3 and 5 were quiescent. Note that the responses of regions 1 sion within the extent of the microdomain. Astrocytes, including
and 2 were asynchronous. Bergmann glia, are thought to efficiently take up transmitters,
such as glutamate14. One may speculate that the glial membrane
depolarization would slow the uptake rate, as shown for retinal
glia15, and a lack of gap junctional communication would con-
than 100 m2, the extent of the fluorescence signal matches the fine both the depolarization and the change in uptake to the
size of the glial microdomain. To ensure that responding and non- microdomain. This may increase transmitter availability and
responding areas were within the same focal plane, we collected synaptic efficacy for the synapses ensheathed by a glial
120 images above and below the analyzed area after the physio- microdomain but not for neighboring synapses. Another pos-
logical experiment (n = 5; with a z-step of 0.36 M). This allowed sible way Bergmann glial cells could influence neuronal activity
us to reconstruct the position of the process in depth. Rotating the is by releasing biologically active substances. Coactivation of
process by 90 relative to analyzed plane (Fig. 3b) demonstrated AMPA/kainate and metabotropic glutamate receptors in hip-
that the fluorescence signal was confined to the analyzed volume, pocampal astrocytes triggers glutamate release by a calcium-
assuming 2.5 m as the depth of the focal plane for our 40 objec- dependent process16. Bergmann glial cells similarly coexpress
tive, suggesting that heterogeneity in calcium signaling along the both ionotropic (AMPA/kainate 5) and metabotropic (mGluR
Bergmann glial process was not due to sampling artifacts. 1 and 5; ref. 17) glutamate receptors. Micromodular organiza-
tion of the Bergmann glial cell would confine the release of glu-
The glial calcium response involves synaptic activation tamate to a defined and limited area. Both mechanisms could
To exclude a stimulation artifact and to verify the synaptic ori- serve to modulate only those synapses that are ensheathed by a
gin of glial calcium signals, we used pharmacological agents given microdomain, independent of the behavior of neighboring
known to interfere with nerve-impulse propagation and [Ca2+]i synapses and their glial partners. Purkinje cells express two func-
entry into synaptic terminals. First, blockade of voltage-gated tionally distinct populations of synaptic spines, and individual
sodium channels (and thus action potential propagation) by spines are capable of independent activation18,19. It may well be
tetrodotoxin (1 M) completely eliminated the local glial [Ca2+]i that the Bergmann glial microdomains are adjusted to meet this
changes (Fig. 4a, n = 7). This blockade was reversible; after a ten- functional diversity of Purkinje cell synapses.
minute washout, stimulation again triggered a calcium signal in
the Bergmann glial microdomain. Similarly, blockade of voltage- METHODS
gated calcium channels with 100 M cadmium completely elim- Tissue slices. Slices of 120 to 200 m were prepared from 2022 day old
inated glial [Ca 2+ ] i elevation in response to parallel-fiber mice as described 5. The slices were either cut parallel (for electron
stimulation (Fig. 4b, n = 4). Because neither we nor others have microscopy) or perpendicular to the orientation of the Purkinje cell den-
dritic tree (for parallel-fiber stimulation). Slices were perfused with phys-
evidence of calcium channels in Bergmann glial cells, we con- iological saline (2224C) containing 154 mM NaCl, 5.6 mM KCl, 2 mM
clude that cadmium blocked neuronal calcium channels and CaCl2, 1 mM MgCl2 1.25 mM NaH2PO4, 26 mM NaHCO3 and 10 mM
hence inhibited the calcium-dependent synaptic release of neu- glucose, pH 7.4 when continuously gassed with 95% CO2 and 5% O2.
rotransmitter. Recovery of the response was observed only in one
cell after the five-minute washout. Calcium imaging. The patch-clamp technique was used in the whole-cell
Further substantiating the concept of functionally indepen- configuration. Pipettes had resistances of 46 M. The intrapipette solu-
dent subcompartments within Bergmann glial cell processes, we tion contained 150 mM KCl, 2 mM MgCl2, 10 mM HEPES, 3 mM Na2ATP
observed spontaneous [Ca2+]i fluctuations within restricted areas and 0.5 mM Oregon green 488 BAPTA-1 (Molecular Probes), pH 7.3. A
along the processes. In about 50% of recordings (15 of 27 cells), confocal laser microscope (Noran, Odyssey XL mounted on Zeiss Axio-
scope, with 40 water immersion objectives, either from Zeiss, numerical
[Ca2+]i increased transiently within areas less than 100 m2. We
aperture 0.75, or from Olympus, numerical aperture 0.8) was used to
observed regionally confined signaling in up to five compart- acquire fluorescence images at 7.5 Hz. Parallel fibers were stimulated from
ments within our visual field. Moreover, in some of these com- an isolated stimulus circuit by a glass micropipette with a resistance of
partments, signaling was clearly not synchronized (Fig. 5). about 1 M. Calcium responses of Bergmann glial cells were recorded
Stimulation of parallel fibers could trigger [Ca2+]i signals in the while parallel fibers were stimulated by 1 and occasionally 10 square puls-
spontaneously active subcompartments. es of 200-s duration, with a frequency of 50 Hz and voltage of 50 volts.

142 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Lucifer Yellow staining. Bergmann glial cells were dialyzed via a patch 3. Hatten, M. E. Riding the glial monorail: a common mechanism for glial-
pipette containing 130 mM KCl, 0.5 mM CaCl2, 5 mM EGTA, 2 mM guided neuronal migration in different regions of the developing
mammalian brain. Trends Neurosci. 13, 179184 (1990).
MgCl2, 10 mM HEPES and 1 mg/ml Lucifer Yellow. After a 10-min dial- 4. Chan-Palay, V. & Palay, S. L. High voltage electron microscopy of rapid
ysis, the patch pipette was pulled off after damaging the seal by a large golgi preparations. Neurons and their processes in the cerebellar cortex of
hyperpolarizing current injection. monkey and rat. Z. Anat. Entwicklungsgesch. 137, 125153 (1972).
5. Mller, T., Mller, T., Berger, T., Schnitzer, J. & Kettenmann, H. Calcium
Tissue preparation for electron microscopy. The tissue was fixed with entry through kainate receptors and resulting potassium-channel
blockade in Bergmann glial cells. Science 256, 15631566 (1992).
2% paraformaldehyde and then incubated in 3,3 diaminobenzidinete- 6. Reichenbach, A. Siegel, D., Senitz, T. & Smith, G. Jr A comparative fractal
trahydrochloride (DAB, Sigma); photoconversion was started by intense analysis of various mammalian astroglial cell types. Neuroimage 1, 6977
blue light illumination. After further fixation in 2% paraformaldehyde (1992).
and 2% glutaraldehyde, the tissue was transferred to 1% osmium tetrox- 7. Rall, W. et al. Matching dendritic neuron models to experimental data.
Physiol. Rev. 72, S159186 (1992).
ide, dehydrated and embedded in EPON, cut at a constant thickness of 8. Sweadner, K. J. in Neuroglia (eds. Kettenmann, H. & Ransom, B. R.)
60 nm and examined with a Zeiss 10 electron microscope at 80 kV. 259272 (Oxford Univ. Press, New York, 1995).
9. Rosenthal, M. & Sick, T. J. Glycolytic and oxidative metabolic
Three-dimensional reconstruction of electron-microscopic images. A series contributions to potassium ion transport in rat cerebral cortex. Can. J.
of electron micrographs from the appendage was taken at a magnification Physiol. Pharmacol. 70, S165169 (1992).
10. Konnerth, A., Llano, I. & Armstrong, C. M. Synaptic currents in
of 5,000. The outlines of all DAB-stained glia profiles were scanned into cerebellar Purkinje cells. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 87, 26622665 (1990).
special computer programs. Each single image was carefully fit to its neigh- 11. Kuffler, S. W. Nicholls, J. G. & Orkand, R. K. Physiological properties of
boring sections, to compensate for slight deformations of the sections that glial cells in the central nervous system of amphibia. J. Neurophysiol. 29,
unavoidably occur due to cutting and heating by the electron beam. Final- 768787 (1966).
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

ly, a three-dimensional reconstruction of the whole appendage was made 12. Verkhratsky, A. & Kettenmann, H. Calcium signalling in glial cells. Trends
Neurosci. 19, 346352 (1996).
(Image Volumes, Minnesota Datametrics Corp., St. Paul, Minnesota, USA). 13. Mller, T., Mller, T., Neuhaus, J. & Kettenmann, H. Electrical coupling
among Bergmann glial cells and its modulation by glutamate receptor
activation, Glia 17, 274284 (1996).
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS 14. Clark, B. A. & Barbour, B. Currents evoked in Bergmann glial cells by
The authors thank S. Lyons, A. Konnerth and B. R. Ransom for comments on the parallel fibre stimulation in rat cerebellar slices. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 502,
manuscript. This work was supported by grants from Deutsche 335350 (1997).
15. Sarantis, M. & Attwell, D. Glutamate uptake in mammalian retinal glia is
Forschungsgemeinschaft (SFB 515; 436UKR), Volkswagen-Stiftung and voltage- and potassium-dependent. Brain Res. 516, 322325 (1990).
Interdisciplinary Center for Clinical Research at the University of Leipzig, 16. Bezzi, P. et al. Prostaglandins stimulate calcium-dependent glutamate
(01KS9504, Project C5). release in astrocytes. Nature 391, 281285 (1998).
17. Kirischuk, S., Kettenmann, H. & Verkhratsky, A. Metabotropic receptors
involved in calcium signalling in mouse Bergmann glial cells. J. Physiol.
RECEIVED 3 SEPTEMBER; ACCEPTED 23 NOVEMBER 1998 (Lond.) 493, 46 (1996).
18. Eilers, J., Augustine, G. J. & Konnerth, A. Subthreshold synaptic Ca 2+
signalling in fine dendrites and spines of cerebellar Purkinje neurons.
1. Finkbeiner, S. Calcium waves in astrocytesfilling in the gaps. Neuron 8, Nature 373, 155158 (1995).
11011108 (1992). 19. Denk, W., Sugimori, M. & Llins, R. Two types of calcium response
2. Verkhratsky, A. Orkand, R. K. & Kettenmann, H. Glial calcium: Homeostasis limited to single spines in cerebellar Purkinje cells. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
and signalling function. Physiol. Rev. 78, 142 (1998). USA 92, 82798282 (1995).

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 143


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Voltage-activated sodium channels


amplify inhibition in neocortical
pyramidal neurons
Greg Stuart

Division of Neuroscience, John Curtin School of Medical Research, Australian National University, Canberra, A.C.T. 0200, Australia
Correspondence should be addressed to G.S. (Greg.Stuart@anu.edu.au)

Inhibitory postsynaptic potentials (IPSPs) in neocortical pyramidal neurons are increased in duration
and amplitude at depolarized membrane potentials. This effect was not due to changes in the time
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

course of the underlying synaptic current. The role of postsynaptic voltage-activated channels was
investigated by mimicking the voltage change that occurs during an IPSP with current injections.
The peak and integral of these simulated IPSPs increased during depolarization of the membrane
potential in a tetrodotoxin-sensitive manner. This amplification presumably occurs as the
hyperpolarization associated with IPSPs turns off sodium channels that are tonically active at
depolarized membrane potentials. IPSP amplification increased the ability of IPSPs to inhibit action
potential firing and promoted IPSP-induced action potential synchronization.

Synaptic integration in neurons involves the temporal and spatial inhibitory postsynaptic potentials (IPSPs) at depolarized mem-
summation of synaptic events, which can lead to action poten- brane potentials. The present results show that non-inactivating
tial initiation if the membrane potential in the axon becomes suf- sodium channels can cause significant amplification of IPSPs at
ficiently depolarized. The effectiveness with which a neuron sums depolarized, subthreshold membrane potentials. The extent of
different synaptic inputs depends on the membrane time con- this IPSP amplification depends on the amplitude and time course
stant, the time interval between synaptic events, their location of the inhibitory synaptic current and significantly increases the
and how they propagate within the neuron1. Even in an electri- ability of IPSPs to inhibit action potential firing. These findings
cally passive neuron, synaptic integration is nonlinear because therefore indicate that voltage-activated sodium channels, which
changes in membrane potential associated with one synaptic are usually associated with excitation in the central nervous sys-
event affect the driving force for ion flow through other synaptic tem, can also amplify inhibition. A preliminary report of this work
conductances1. The ability of neurons to sum synaptic inputs is has appeared (G.S. Soc. Neurosci. Abst. 23, 261.2, 1997).
further complicated by the large range of voltage-activated chan-
nels that they express2. Although many of these voltage-activat- RESULTS
ed conductances are only active during action potential Voltage dependence of evoked IPSPs
generation, others are activated at subthreshold membrane poten- Somatic gramicidin perforated-patch recordings16 were used to
tials. Furthermore, some voltage-activated conductances are locat- gain intracellular access to visually identified neocortical layer-5
ed in the soma and dendrites of neurons, distant from the site of pyramidal neurons without modifying the internal chloride con-
action potential initiation3. centration. Monosynaptic IPSPs evoked by extracellular stimula-
The activation of voltage-activated conductances at mem- tion were recorded in the presence of the glutamate receptor
brane potentials below the threshold for action potential ini- antagonists CNQX and APV at different somatic membrane
tiation influences the ability of neurons to integrate synaptic potentials (Fig. 1a). These IPSPs were completely blocked by bath
inputs. For example, both the peak and duration of excitato- application of the GABAA receptor antagonist bicuculline (20 M;
ry postsynaptic potentials (EPSPs) are increased at depolar- n = 3). Their average reversal potential was 63.1 1.2 mV
ized, subthreshold membrane potentials414. In many cases, (n = 10). After correction for a 3-mV junction potential between
amplification of these EPSPs is mediated by activation of a recording and bath solutions, this gives an estimated internal chlo-
non-inactivating or persistent sodium current 5,1014 . This ride concentration of 11.2 mM, similar to the value reported in
tetrodotoxin (TTX)-sensitive sodium current is activated at cortical neurons from approximately two-week-old animals17.
membrane potentials approximately 10 mV below action IPSPs had longer durations at depolarized membrane poten-
potential threshold, and it is thought to result from the acti- tials than at hyperpolarized membrane potentials (Fig. 1a;
vation of the same voltage-activated sodium channels respon- n = 10). This effect was more obvious when the amplitudes of
sible for action potential generation that have switched into a IPSPs at depolarized and hyperpolarized membrane potentials
non-inactivating mode15. In many neuron types, this sodium were normalized (Fig. 1b). In addition, the time to peak of
current is also responsible for an apparent increase in input IPSPs was delayed at depolarized membrane potentials com-
resistance at depolarized membrane potentials15. pared with hyperpolarized membrane potentials (Fig. 1b). This
A non-inactivating sodium current is likely to also influence increased IPSP duration at depolarized membrane potentials

144 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 1. Effect of membrane potential on evoked IPSPs.


(a) Gramicidin perforated-patch recording of evoked
a c
IPSPs at different membrane potentials between 71 and

Peak (mV)
56 mV. (b) IPSPs recorded at the most hyperpolarized
(71 mV) and depolarized (56 mV) membrane poten-
tials were normalized to the same peak amplitude. Note
that the IPSP at the depolarized membrane potential
peaks later and has a longer duration than the IPSP at the
hyperpolarized membrane potential. (c, d) IPSP peak
amplitude (c) and integral (d) versus membrane potential. Membrane potential (mV)
The data were fitted with a modified version of the GHK
equation between 71 and 62 mV. Note that at depolar-
ized membrane potentials, IPSP peak, and especially IPSP
integral, deviate substantially from the fitted curve. All b d
Normalized
recordings are from the same cell.

Integral (mV s)
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

Membrane potential (mV)

was not due to a GABAB-receptor-mediated IPSP component, as generated close to the soma of layer-5 pyramidal neurons, and
similar results were found in the presence of the specific GABAB decayed with a double-exponential time course (Fig. 2b and
receptor antagonist CGP 55845A (ref. 18). below).
At membrane potentials more negative than the IPSP reversal Unlike IPSPs, the time course of normalized IPSCs did not
potential, the peak amplitude and integral of IPSPs were well fit change at different membrane potentials (Fig. 2b; n = 5). Fur-
by a modified version of the Goldman-Hodgkin-Katz (GHK) thermore, the relationships of IPSC peak and integral to mem-
equation (Fig. 1c and d). At potentials depolarized to the IPSP brane voltage were well described by the GHK equation over the
reversal potential, however, the measured values clearly deviat- entire voltage range examined (Fig. 2c and d; n = 5). These
ed from the predictions of the GHK equation (Fig. 1c and d), results suggest that the increase in peak amplitude and duration
particularly for IPSP integral (Fig. 1d). Similar results were of IPSPs at depolarized membrane potentials cannot be
observed in nine other cells. explained by changes in the time course or amplitude of the
The increased amplitude and duration of evoked IPSPs at underlying synaptic current.
depolarized membrane potentials could occur because of an
increase in the underlying inhibitory postsynaptic current IPSP amplification requires sodium channels
(IPSC), or because the voltage response to this synaptic current The second possibility, that IPSPs are modified by the activation or
is amplified by activation or deactivation of voltage-dependent deactivation of voltage-dependent conductances, was addressed
conductances. The first possibility was addressed by voltage by using somatic current injections to mimic the voltage change
clamping the somata of layer-5 pyramidal neurons
using low-access, whole-cell recording and evok-
ing monosynaptic IPSCs by extracellular synaptic a c
stimulation close to the soma in the presence of
CNQX and APV. IPSCs were recorded over rough-
Peak (pA)

ly the range of membrane potentials used for IPSP


recordings (Fig. 2a). These IPSCs had fast rise
times (average 20-to-80% rise time, 0.48 0.12
ms; n = 5), consistent with the idea that they were

Holding potential (mV)


Fig. 2. Effect of membrane potential on evoked IPSCs.
(a) Whole-cell, voltage-clamp recordings of evoked
IPSCs at different holding potentials between 75 and
45 mV. (b) The IPSCs shown in (a) normalized to the b Normalized d
same peak amplitude. The smooth line shows the triple
Integral (pA s)

exponential (one rising, two decaying) fit to these IPSCs


(taurise = 0.5 ms; taudecay(fast) = 5.0 ms and taudecay(slow) =
37.5 ms). (c, d) Averaged IPSC peak amplitude (c) and
integral (d) versus holding potential in five cells. The
data were fitted with a modified version of the GHK
equation between 75 and 45 mV. Note that both IPSC
peak amplitude and IPSC integral are well fit over the
Holding potential (mV)
entire voltage range.

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 145


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

a b tion at depolarized membrane poten-


tials is mediated by the activation or
deactivation of postsynaptic voltage-
dependent channels.
The role of non-inactivating sodi-
um conductances was investigated by
recording simulated IPSPs at differ-
ent membrane potentials under con-
trol conditions and after bath
application of the sodium channel
blocker TTX (1 M). In TTX, the
Fig. 3. Generation of simulated IPSPs. (a) Infrared differential interference contrast image during double time course of simulated IPSPs at
somatic recording from a neocortical layer-5 pyramidal neuron. The voltage-recording and current-pass- depolarized and hyperpolarized
ing pipettes are indicated. (b) Comparison of the time course of evoked (solid line) and simulated (dot- membrane potentials were similar
ted line) IPSPs recorded at the resting membrane potential and normalized to the same peak amplitude. (Fig. 4c; n = 8); the small difference
remaining in TTX is probably due to
the hyperpolarization-activated
inward current I h (see below). TTX
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

that occurs during evoked IPSPs. In these experiments, hyperpo- completely blocked the amplification of both simulated IPSP
larizing current with the same time course as the evoked IPSC peak and integral at depolarized membrane potentials, reduc-
was injected via a second somatic recording pipette (Fig. 3a). The ing them to values typically observed at the resting membrane
time course of the injected current was based on triple-exponen- potential (Fig. 4d and e; n = 8). Under control conditions,
tial fits to normalized and averaged evoked IPSCs (Fig. 2b). In depolarization increased the integral of simulated IPSPs more
five cells, the average rising time constant was 0.67 0.24 ms and than the peak, and amplification of both was maximal at the
the fast and slow decay time constants were 3.8 1.1 ms and most depolarized membrane potentials tested (Fig. 4d and e).
34.3 17.8 ms, with an average ratio of the amplitudes of the fast On average, the peak amplitude of simulated IPSPs approxi-
and slow decaying time constants of 9:2. Simulated IPSPs gen- mately doubled (202 8%), and IPSP integral increased
erated by somatic current injection with this time course ade- approximately fourfold (443 45%) at the most depolarized,
quately mimicked the voltage change that occurs during evoked just subthreshold, membrane potentials relative to the rest-
IPSPs (Fig. 3b). ing membrane potential. Thus, amplification of simulated
Depolarizing the membrane potential increased both the IPSPs at depolarized membrane potentials is substantial, and
amplitude and the duration of simulated IPSPs (Fig. 4a; n = 8). this amplification is mediated via TTX-sensitive, voltage-acti-
When these IPSPs were normalized, depolarization increased vated sodium channels.
the duration and time to peak of simulated IPSPs, as observed Whether evoked IPSPs are also amplified at depolarized mem-
for evoked IPSPs (Fig. 4b; compare with Fig. 1b). These results brane potentials by voltage-activated sodium channels was inves-
show that somatic current injection with a time course similar to tigated with the internal sodium channel blocker QX-314.
that of evoked IPSCs can mimic the effects of membrane depo- Somatic whole-cell recordings of evoked IPSPs were obtained at
larization on evoked IPSPs, and indicate that IPSP amplifica- different membrane potentials. As with the gramidicin perfo-

a d
Peak (mV)

Fig. 4. Effects of membrane potential and TTX on


simulated IPSPs. (a) Simulated IPSPs recorded at dif-
ferent membrane potentials between 68 and 51 mV.
control (b) Simulated IPSPs recorded at hyperpolarized (68
TTX
b Control
mV) and depolarized (51 mV) membrane potentials
were normalized to the same peak amplitude. Note
that the IPSP at the depolarized membrane potential
Membrane potential (mV) peaks later and has a longer duration than the IPSP at
e the hyperpolarized potential (compare with Fig. 1b).
(c) After bath application of TTX (1 M), simulated
IPSPs recorded at hyperpolarized (67 mV) and depo-
Integral (mV s)

larized (51 mV) membrane potentials (normalized to


the same peak amplitude) have a similar time course.
c TTX (d, e) Simulated IPSP peak amplitude (d) and integral
(e) versus membrane potential without (solid circles)
and with TTX (1 M; open circles). Note that the IPSP
peak and integral show a substantial (2 to 4-fold)
increase at membrane potentials more depolarized
than approximately 60 mV, and that IPSP amplification
is completely blocked by TTX. All recordings are from
Membrane potential (mV) the same cell.

146 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 5. Effect of QX-314 on evoked IPSPs. (a) Whole-cell, current-


a Control b QX-314
clamp recordings of evoked IPSPs at different membrane potentials
between 75 to 55 mV (top). IPSPs recorded at the most hyperpolar-
ized (75 mV) and depolarized (55 mV) membrane potentials were
normalized to the same peak amplitude (bottom). (b) Evoked IPSPs
recorded at different membrane potentials between 75 and 55 mV
after intracellular perfusion with QX-314 (1 mM) via a second somatic
whole-cell recording pipette (top). IPSPs recorded at the most hyperpo-
larized (75 mV) and depolarized (55 mV) membrane potentials were
normalized to the same peak amplitude (bottom). Note that after intra-
cellular perfusion with QX-314, IPSPs at depolarized and hyperpolarized
membrane potentials have a similar time course. All recordings are from
the same cell.
Normalized Normalized
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

rated-patch recordings, depolarization increased the duration Dependence on IPSP amplitude and time course
and time to peak of evoked IPSPs (Fig. 5a). A second somatic The dependence of IPSP amplification on the size and duration
whole-cell recording was then made from the soma of the same of inhibitory events was investigated by generating simulated IPSPs
neuron using a pipette filled with the same intracellular solution of different amplitudes or durations at depolarized membrane
plus 1 mM QX-314, which was allowed to diffuse into the neuron potentials with or without TTX (1 M; Fig. 6). Simulated IPSPs
for approximately five minutes. After intracellular perfusion with were used in these experiments to avoid complications arising
QX-314, the time courses of evoked IPSPs at depolarized and from changes in the driving force for IPSPs of different ampli-
hyperpolarized membrane potentials were similar (Fig. 5b; n = 4). tudes, and to be able to vary IPSP duration over a physiological
These results indicate that evoked IPSPs, like simulated IPSPs, range. When IPSP amplitudes were varied, the kinetics of simu-
are also amplified at depolarized membrane potentials by volt- lated IPSCs were the same as those described above, whereas when
age-activated sodium channels. IPSP durations were varied, IPSC kinetics were varied over a range
from those of fast GABAA receptor IPSCs to slower
GABAB receptor IPSCs (see Fig. 6 legend).
Increasing the amplitude of simulated IPSPs
a Control b Control
decreased amplification of both IPSP peak and inte-
gral (Fig. 6a; n = 4), whereas increasing the duration
of simulated IPSPs increased amplification of IPSP
peak, but reduced amplification of IPSP integral
(Fig. 6b; n = 4). This dependence of IPSP amplifica-
tion on the amplitude and duration of simulated
IPSPs was significantly reduced by ZD7288 (100 M;
TTX TTX n = 3), which blocks Ih (ref. 19). These results indi-

Fig. 6. Effect of IPSP amplitude and duration.


(a) Simulated IPSPs with different amplitudes recorded at
depolarized membrane potentials in control (52 mV; top)
and in 1 M TTX (50 mV; middle). Simulated IPSC ampli-
tudes were 100 pA, 250 pA, 500 pA and 1 nA. The percent
increase in control IPSP peak (open circles) and integral
(solid circles) relative to that in the presence of TTX is
plotted against simulated IPSC amplitude at the bottom
Percent increase

Percent increase

(n = 4). (b) Simulated IPSPs with different durations but


similar amplitude at the resting membrane potential
recorded at depolarized membrane potentials in control
(51 mV; top) and in 1 M TTX (50 mV; middle).
Simulated IPSCs had exponentially rising and decaying time
constants of 0.5 and 4 ms, 2 and 10 ms, 5 and 20 ms, 10
and 50 ms, and 50 and 100 ms, with corresponding ampli-
tudes of 500 pA, 263 pA, 185 pA, 143 and 132 pA. The
0 percent increase in control IPSP peak (p) and integral (P)
relative to that in the presence of TTX is plotted against
IPSC amplitude (pA) IPSC half width (ms) simulated IPSC half-width at the bottom (n = 4).

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 147


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

cate that as inhibition becomes more powerful during synchro- es the ability of IPSPs to inhibit action potential firing. Simulat-
nized activation of inhibitory input, or more prolonged during ed IPSPs, rather than evoked IPSPs, were used to avoid any presy-
a burst of inhibitory input or during activation of inhibitory naptic action of TTX on transmitter release. Low concentrations
events of longer duration (for example, GABAB receptor IPSPs) of TTX were used rather than intracellular QX-314 because pre-
amplification of IPSPs by voltage-activated sodium channels is liminary experiments showed that QX-314 concentrations suf-
reduced on balance because of the increased activation of Ih. ficient to block IPSP amplification at depolarized membrane
potentials significantly reduced action potential amplitude.
IPSP amplification and action potential firing Action potentials were evoked by constant depolarizing cur-
As amplification of IPSPs is greatest at membrane potentials close rent injection and had an average firing frequency of 13.9 0.5
to the action potential threshold, it might be expected to increase Hz in control and 14.8 2.1 Hz in TTX. In control, simulated
the ability of IPSPs to inhibit action potential firing. Therefore, IPSPs (approximately 2.5 mV in amplitude at the resting mem-
the ability of simulated IPSPs to inhibit action potential genera- brane potential) inhibited action potential firing to an average
tion was investigated under control conditions and in the pres- frequency of 0.5 0.5 Hz during a 35-millisecond time window
ence of submaximal concentrations of TTX (5 nM). The rationale after their onset (Fig. 7a; a 96% reduction; n = 4). In low con-
was that these concentrations of TTX would partially block IPSP centrations of TTX, however, the average action potential fre-
amplification without significantly affecting action potentials, quency during the same time window was 7.9 1.4 Hz (Fig. 7b;
and so reveal the extent to which amplification of IPSPs increas- a 43% reduction; n = 4). These low concentrations of TTX
reduced amplification of simulated IPSP peak
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

and integral by on average 63 7% and


a Control b 5 nM TTX 62 10% respectively (Fig. 7; n = 4).
Thus, low concentrations of TTX signifi-
cantly reduced the ability of simulated IPSPs to
inhibit action potentials, which is unlikely to be
due to an effect on the action potential itself, as
these low concentrations of TTX had only a
small effect on action potential amplitude and
rate of rise. On average, action potential ampli-
tude measured from threshold was reduced by
6.4 5%, whereas action potential rate of rise
was decreased by 9.4 7%. These changes were
Simulated IPSPs not statistically significant. Furthermore, if any-
thing, low concentrations of TTX should
increase the effectiveness of IPSPs at inhibiting
action potential firing, as one would expect this
treatment to make the initiation of action
potentials less secure.
These TTX concentrations also reduced the
synchronization of action potentials following
Post-stimulus time histogram simulated IPSPs (Fig. 7). Autocorrelations of
action potential timing during a 150-millisec-
ond time window starting from the simulated
IPSP peak were used to assess IPSP-induced
Counts

action potential synchronization. These auto-


correlations (digitally filtered at 40 Hz) showed
clear peaks spaced approximately 50 ms apart
(Fig. 7a). Bath application of low concentrations
of TTX (5 nM) reduced the amplitude of the
peaks in these autocorrelations (Fig. 7b), indi-
Autocorrelation cating a reduction in IPSP-induced action
potential synchronization.

DISCUSSION
The results presented here show that IPSPs in
neocortical layer-5 pyramidal neurons are
amplified at depolarized membrane potentials.
This IPSP amplification was blocked by TTX
and intracellular QX-314, indicating that it is
Fig. 7. Effect of IPSP amplification on action potential firing. (a, b) Inhibition of action potential
mediated by voltage-activated sodium channels.
firing and subsequent IPSP-induced action potential synchronization in control (a) and in a low
concentration of TTX (5 nM; b). Top, inhibition of action potential firing by simulated IPSPs. The As this amplification is greatest at membrane
time of simulated IPSP onset is indicated by arrows. Upper middle, simulated IPSPs recorded at potentials close to action potential threshold,
resting (63 mV) and depolarized (55 mV) membrane potentials. Lower middle, post-stimulus IPSP amplification is likely to be important in
time histograms of action potential firing. Bottom, autocorrelation of spike timing during a 150- synaptic integration. Consistent with this idea,
ms time window starting after the peak of the IPSP. All recordings are from the same cell. IPSP amplification by voltage-activated sodium

148 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

channels was found to increase the ability of IPSPs to inhibit been reported in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons31, where
action potential firing and to promote IPSP-induced action this effect is thought to be associated with a rebound depolar-
potential synchronization. ization after IPSPs mediated by activation of Ih. The present
results suggest that non-inactivating sodium channels are also
Mechanisms underlying IPSP amplification involved in action potential synchronization by IPSPs. Consis-
Amplification of IPSPs at depolarized membrane potentials pre- tent with this idea, action potential synchronization in other
sumably occurs because the hyperpolarization generated during regions of the cortex involves an interaction between both Ih and
an IPSP is able to turn off a tonically active (non-inactivating) non-inactivating sodium channels32.
inward sodium current, thereby generating a functional out- As mentioned earlier, EPSPs in many neuronal types are also
ward current, which adds to the outward current occurring dur- amplified at depolarized membrane potentials by non-inacti-
ing IPSPs, and so amplifies their hyperpolarization. IPSPs in vating sodium channels15. Although IPSP and EPSP amplifica-
thalamocortical neurons are amplified via a similar mechanism, tion occur through different mechanisms, with EPSP
but in this case by turning off a tonically active calcium current20. amplification resulting from turning on non-inactivated sodi-
The onset of IPSP amplification therefore depends on the rate um channels, the effects on EPSP and IPSP time course and the
of deactivation of non-inactivating sodium channels, whereas voltage-dependence of the amplification are similar12. Hence,
the duration of IPSP amplification depends on their rate of reac- the same conductance amplifies both excitatory and inhibitory
tivation. At depolarized membrane potentials, the increase in the synaptic events at depolarized membrane potentials in a similar
IPSP time to peak indicates that deactivation of non-inactivat- manner. Such an increase in synaptic gain at potentials near
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

ing sodium channels takes several milliseconds near action poten- action potential threshold is presumably important for synaptic
tial threshold, whereas the significantly increased IPSP duration integration in all neurons that have a significant non-inactivating
indicates that reactivation of the same channels takes tens of mil- sodium current.
liseconds. The extent and duration of IPSP amplification also
depends on the interaction of non-inactivating sodium channels METHODS
with Ih, such that larger and/or more prolonged inhibitory events All recordings were made from the somata of visually identified layer-5
are less amplified. A role of Ih in influencing the duration of IPSPs pyramidal neurons in 300-m sagittal brain slices from somatosensory
has been noted previously21. neocortex of 3 to 4 week-old rats33. During recordings, slices were con-
The location of the sodium channels involved in IPSP ampli- tinuously perfused with a solution of the following composition: 125
fication is unknown. Both the soma and dendrites of neocorti- mM NaCl, 25 mM NaHCO 3, 25 mM glucose, 3 mM KCl, 1.25 mM
cal layer-5 pyramidal neurons contain voltage-activated sodium NaH2PO4, 2 mM CaCl2 and 1 mM MgCl2, bubbled with 95% O2/5%
CO2 (pH 7.4) at 35 1C.
channels22. The highest density of voltage-activated sodium chan- Somatic perforated-patch and whole-cell, current-clamp recordings
nels in these neurons, however, is thought to be in the axon23,24. were made using Axoclamp-2 amplifiers (Axon Instruments, Foster City,
Given that the non-inactivating sodium channels that contribute California), whereas low-access, somatic, whole-cell, voltage-clamp
to IPSP amplification are the same sodium channels involved in recordings (uncompensated series resistance < 10 M) were made using
action potential generation, switched into a different mode15, an Axopatch 200A (Axon Instruments) with greater than 80% series
inhibitory inputs onto the axon initial segment are likely to be resistance compensation. Perforated-patch recordings were made using
amplified locally by voltage-activated sodium channels to a gramicidin16. Pipette tips were filled with 150 mM KCl plus 10 mM
greater extent than inhibitory inputs onto the soma or dendrites. HEPES (pH 7.4 with KOH) and then backfilled with the same solution
This makes inhibitory axo-axonic synapses25 ideally placed to containing gramicidin (9 g/ml) plus Lucifer Yellow (1 mg/ml). Perfo-
ration was detected by a slow reduction in series resistance to 3065 M.
inhibit action potential firing, both because of their location near
During these experiments, fluorescence microscopy was used to deter-
the site of action potential initiation26, and because their effect mine if the recorded cell was stained with Lucifer Yellow (indicating rup-
would be significantly amplified at depolarized membrane poten- ture of the patch membrane). If staining was detected, the data were
tials by voltage-activated sodium channels. IPSPs generated by discarded. Whole-cell current- and voltage-clamp recordings were made
inhibitory interneurons that target dendritic regions are also like- with pipettes filled with the following solution: 120 mM K-gluconate, 7
ly to be amplified locally by dendritic sodium channels. Such an mM NaCl, 10 mM HEPES, 10 mM EGTA, 2 mM Na2-ATP and 2 mM
effect presumably increases the ability of dendritic inhibitory MgCl2, pH 7.3 with KOH. This solution has a chloride concentration
inputs to inhibit dendritic calcium electrogenesis27,28 and back- (11 mM) similar to the intracellular chloride concentration determined
propagation of action potentials into the dendritic tree29. from the reversal potential of IPSPs during gramicidin perforated-patch
recordings (see Results).
Monosynaptic IPSPs and IPSCs were evoked by extracellular stimu-
Physiological significance of IPSP amplification lation close to the soma (approximately 100 m lateral from the soma
The main physiological effect of amplifying IPSPs is to increase of the recorded cell) in the presence of CNQX (10 M; Tocris, Bristol,
their ability to block action potential firing. Several mechanisms UK) and D-APV (50 M; Tocris), and in some cases also CGP 55845A
therefore act together to enhance inhibition at membrane poten- (1 M; gift from Novartis Pharma, Basel, Switzerland). Unless otherwise
tials near action potential threshold, as depolarization increases stated, simulated IPSPs (see Results) were generated by current injection
both the driving force for chloride, and therefore the underlying via a second somatic recording pipette with an exponential rise and dou-
IPSC, and also leads to amplification of IPSPs via non-inactivat- ble exponential decay (taurise = 0.67 ms; taudecay(fast) = 3.8 ms; taude-
ing sodium channels. Strong depolarizations would also be cay(slow) = 34.3 ms; ratio of decay tau amplitudes fast:slow of 9:2). During
expected to increase the duration of GABAA-receptor-mediated current clamp recordings, the membrane potential was altered by somat-
ic current pulses (500 ms duration; IPSPs evoked 200 ms after pulse
IPSCs30, although presumably this only has a significant effect
onset) or by constant current injection.
on IPSC duration at membrane potentials substantially more Voltage and current were filtered at 10 and 2 kHz, respectively, and
depolarized than action potential threshold. sampled at 10 to 20 kHz using a Macintosh computer together with an
In addition to increasing the effectiveness of inhibition, ampli- analog-to-digital converter (Instrutech, Great Neck, New York). For
fication of IPSPs also enhanced their ability to synchronize action evoked IPSP/Cs, the stimulus artifact was either blanked or removed by
potential firing. Action potential synchronization by IPSPs has subtraction of the average response recorded at the reversal potential.

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 149


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

IPSP/C peak amplitude was measured from a baseline set before the 13. Lipowsky, R., Gillessen, T. & Alzheimer, C. Dendritic Na+ channels amplify
IPSP/C, and the integral was defined as the area of an IPSP/C above or EPSPs in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal cells. J. Neurophysiol. 76, 21812191
below this baseline. IPSP/C peak and integral versus membrane or hold- (1996).
14. Urban, N. N., Henze, D. A. & Barrionuevo, G. Amplification of perforant-
ing potential plots were fit with a modified version of the GHK equation: path EPSPs in CA3 pyramidal cells by LVA calcium and sodium channels. J.
Peak/integral = A V ([Cl]i - [Cl]o exp(VF/RT) )/(1 - exp(VF/RT)) Neurophysiol. 80, 15581561 (1998).
15. Crill, W. E. Persistent sodium current in mammalian central neurons. Annu.
where A is a constant, V is the membrane or holding potential, [Cl]i is Rev. Physiol. 58, 349362 (1996).
the intracellular chloride concentration, [Cl]o is the extracellular chlo- 16. Kyrozis, A. & Reichling, D. B. Perforated-patch recording with gramicidin
ride concentration, and F, R and T have their usual meanings. All data avoids artifactual changes in intracellular chloride concentration. J. Neurosci.
Methods 57, 2735 (1995).
were analyzed with Axograph (Axon Instruments). Unless otherwise stat- 17. Owens, D. F., Boyce, L. H., Davis, M. B. & Kriegstein, A. R. Excitatory GABA
ed, no correction was made for the junction potential between pipette responses in embryonic and neonatal cortical slices demonstrated by
and extracellular solutions. Averages at different membrane potentials gramicidin perforated-patch recordings and calcium imaging. J. Neurosci. 16,
were compiled from 3 to 20 sweeps. Pooled data are expressed as mean 64146423 (1996).
18. Davies, C. H., Pozza, M. F. & Collingridge, G. L. CGP 55845A: a potent
standard error, and statistical difference was evaluated by paired t-test at antagonist of GABAB receptors in the CA1 region of rat hippocampus.
a significance level of 0.05. Neuropharmacology 32, 10711073 (1993).
19. BoSmith, R. E., Briggs, I. & Sturgess, N. C. Inhibitory actions of ZENECA
ZD7288 on whole-cell hyperpolarisation activated inward current (If) in
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS guinea-pig dissociated sinoatrial node cells. Br. J. Pharmacol. 110, 343349
I thank J. Bekkers and S. Williams for discussions and comments on an earlier (1993).
20. Williams, S. R., Tth, T. I., Turner, J. P., Hughes, S. W. & Crunelli, V. The
version of the manuscript. This work was supported by an Australian Research window component of the low threshold Ca2+ current produces input signal
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

Council QEII Research Fellowship, the Human Frontiers Science Program and amplification and bistability in cat and rat thalamocortical neurones. J.
the Wellcome Trust. Physiol. (Lond.) 505, 689705 (1997).
21. van Brederode, J. F. & Spain, W. J. Differences in inhibitory synaptic input
between layer II-III and layer V neurons of the cat neocortex. J. Neurophysiol.
RECEIVED 31 AUGUST; ACCEPTED 10 DECEMBER 1998 74, 11491166 (1995).
22. Stuart, G. J. & Sakmann, B. Active propagation of somatic action potentials
into neocortical pyramidal cell dendrites. Nature 367, 6972 (1994).
1. Johnston, D. & Wu, S. Foundations of Cellular Neurophysiology (MIT Press, 23. Mainen, Z. F., Joerges, J., Huguenard, J. R. & Sejnowski, T. J. A model of spike
Cambridge, MA, 1995). initiation in neocortical pyramidal neurons. Neuron 15, 14271439 (1995).
2. Hille, B. Ion Channels of Excitable Membranes (Sinauer, Sunderland, MA, 24. Rapp, M., Yarom, M. & Segev, I. Modeling back propagating action potentials
1994). in weakly excitable dendrites of neocortical pyramidal cells. Proc. Natl. Acad.
3. Johnston, D., Magee, J. C., Colbert, C. M. & Christie, R. Active properties of Sci. USA 93, 1198511990 (1996).
neuronal dendrites. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 19, 165186 (1996). 25. Buhl, E. H., Halasy, K. & Somogyi, P. Diverse sources of hippocampal unitary
4. Masukawa, L. M. & Prince, D. A. Synaptic control of excitability in isolated inhibitory postsynaptic potentials and the number of synaptic release sites.
dendrites of hippocampal neurons. J. Neurosci. 4, 217227 (1984). Nature 68, 823882 (1994).
5. Stafstrom, C. E., Schwindt, P. C., Chubb, M. C. & Crill, W. E. Properties of 26. Stuart, G., Spruston, N., Sakmann, B. & Husser, M. Action potential
persistent sodium conductance and calcium conductance of layer V neurons initiation and backpropagation in neurons of the mammalian central
from cat sensorimotor cortex in vitro. J. Neurophysiol. 53, 153170 (1985). nervous system. Trends Neurosci. 20, 125131 (1997).
6. Miles, R. & Wong, R. K. Excitatory synaptic interactions between CA3 27. Kim, H. G., Beierlein, M. & Connors, B. W. Inhibitory control of excitable
neurones in the guinea-pig hippocampus. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 373, 397418 dendrites in neocortex. J. Neurophysiol. 74, 18101814 (1995).
(1986). 28. Miles, R., Tth, K., Gulyas, A. I., Hjos, N. & Freund, T. F. Differences
7. Thomson, A. M., Girdlestone, D. & West, D. C. Voltage-dependent currents between somatic and dendritic inhibition in the hippocampus. Neuron 16,
prolong single-axon postsynaptic potentials in layer III pyramidal neurons in 815823 (1996).
rat neocortical slices. J. Neurophysiol. 60, 18961907 (1988). 29. Tsubokawa, H. & Ross, W. N. IPSPs modulate spike backpropagation and
8. Sutor, B. & Hablitz, J. J. EPSPs in rat neocortical neurons in vitro I. associated [Ca2+]i changes in the dendrites of hippocampal CA1 pyramidal
Electrophysiological evidence for two distinct EPSPs. J. Neurophysiol. 61, neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 76, 28962906 (1996).
607620 (1989). 30. Collingridge, G. L., Gage, P. W. & Robertson, B. Inhibitory post-synaptic currents
9. Sutor, B. & Hablitz, J. J. EPSPs in rat neocortical neurons in vitro II. in rat hippocampal CA1 neurones. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 356, 551564 (1984).
Involvement of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors in the generation of EPSPs. J. 31. Cobb, S. R., Buhl, E. H., Halasy, K., Paulsen, O. & Somogyi, P.
Neurophysiol. 61, 621634 (1989). Synchronisation of neuronal activity in hippocampus by individual
10. Deisz, R. A., Fortin, G. & Zieglgnsberger, W. Voltage-dependence of GABAergic interneurons. Nature 378, 7578 (1995).
excitatory postsynaptic potentials of rat neocortical pyramidal neurons. J. 32. Alonso, A. & Llins, R. R. Subthreshold Na+-dependent theta-like
Neurophysiol. 65, 371382 (1991). rhythmicity in stellate cells of entorhinal cortex layer II. Nature 342, 175177
11. Hirsch, J. & Gilbert, C. D. Synaptic physiology of horizontal connections in (1989).
the cat visual cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 11, 18001809 (1991). 33. Stuart, G. J., Dodt, H.-U. & Sakmann, B. Patch-clamp recordings from the
12. Stuart, G. & Sakmann, B. Amplification of EPSPs by axosomatic sodium soma and dendrites of neurones in brain slices using infrared video
channels in neocortical pyramidal neurons. Neuron 15, 10651077 (1995). microscopy. Pflgers Arch. 423, 511518 (1993).

150 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Retention of heroin and morphine-


6-glucuronide analgesia in a new
line of mice lacking exon 1 of MOR-1
Alwin G.P. Schuller1, Michael A. King2, Jiwen Zhang1, Elizabeth Bolan2, Ying-Xian Pan2,
Daniel J. Morgan1, Albert Chang2, Maureen E. Czick1, Ellen M. Unterwald3,4,5,
Gavril W. Pasternak2 and John E. Pintar1

1 Dept. of Neuroscience and Cell Biology, UMDNJ-Robert Wood Johnson Medical School, CABM, RM 326, 675 Hoes Lane,
Piscataway, New Jersey 08854, USA
2 The Cotzias Laboratory of Neuro-Oncology, Memorial Sloan-Kettering Cancer Center, 125 York Avenue, New York, New York 10021, USA
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

3 Dept. of Psychiatry, NYU Medical Center, 550 First Avenue, New York, New York 10016, USA
4 Present address: Department of Pharmacology, Temple University School of Medicine, 3420 N. Broad Street,
Philadelphia, Pennsylvania 19140, USA
5 Present address: The Rockefeller University, 1230 York Avenue, New York, New York 10021, USA
Correspondence should be addressed to J.E.P. (pintar@mbcl.rutgers.edu)

Morphine produces analgesia by activating mu opioid receptors encoded by the MOR-1 gene.
Although morphine-6-glucuronide (M6G), heroin and 6-acetylmorphine also are considered mu opi-
oids, recent evidence suggests that they act through a distinct receptor mechanism. We examined
this question in knockout mice containing disruptions of either the first or second coding exon of
MOR-1. Mice homozygous for either MOR-1 mutation were insensitive to morphine. Heroin, 6-acetyl-
morphine and M6G still elicited analgesia in the exon-1 MOR-1 mutant, which also showed specific
M6G binding, whereas M6G and 6-acetylmorphine were inactive in the exon-2 MOR-1 mutant. These
results provide genetic evidence for a unique receptor site for M6G and heroin analgesia.

Morphine, one of the oldest and most widely used pain relievers, (MOR-1) has been cloned17,18, and several splice variants have
elicits a variety of pharmacological responses, including analgesia, been identified19,20. Antisense probes against MOR-1 block mor-
respiratory depression and inhibition of gastrointestinal transit. phine analgesia in both rats7,16 and mice9,10, but detailed anti-
These actions are mediated by mu opioid receptors, which have been sense mapping of MOR-1 revealed striking differences between
divided pharmacologically into several subtypes1. Despite the need morphine and M6G9,10,16. Probes targeting exon 1 all blocked
for opiates in the treatment of pain, their abuse potential has limit- analgesia caused by morphine but not by M6G, whereas others
ed both opiate availability and optimal use. The most widely abused targeting exons 2 and 3 blocked M6G analgesia, raising the pos-
opioid is heroin, which is synthesized by acetylating morphine at sibility that these receptors might be splice variants of a common
both the 3- and 6- positions. In vivo, heroin itself is very short-lived, gene911,16. Gene knockouts offer an alternative approach to iden-
being rapidly converted into 6-acetylmorphine, which is thought to tifying the functional significance of specific genes, avoiding many
mediate most of its actions2. It was initially thought that pharma- of the potential problems encountered with antisense. Multiple
cokinetic differences alone, rather than distinct mechanisms of disruptions of the MOR-1 gene show that the MOR-1 gene is
action, could account for the differences in heroin and morphine essential for morphine-induced analgesia as well as development
responses26. However, recent studies indicate that heroin and 6- of morphine dependence2124. However, its role in mediating anal-
acetylmorphine, as well as the major morphine metabolite mor- gesia elicited by other opioids such as heroin, 6-acetylmorphine
phine-6-glucuronide (M6G), may act through an opioid receptor and M6G analgesia has not been systematically investigated.
distinct from that mediating morphine actions711. For example, Therefore, we determined the analgesic potential of these drugs
the CXBK mouse strain, developed in the early 1970s, is insensitive in two strains of MOR-1 knockout mice, in which either the first
to intracerebroventricular morphine injections12 consistent with or second coding exon of MOR-1 was disrupted.
significantly lower mu1 binding levels13. In contrast, M6G, heroin
and its active metabolite 6-acetylmorphine remain potent analgesics RESULTS
in CXBK mice10,14, suggesting that these agents may act on recep- Generation of MOR-1 exon-1 knockout mice
tor systems different from those activated by morphine. This possi- To define the role of MOR-1 in the actions of morphine, heroin and
bility is further supported by antisense studies that distinguish M6G, we compared mice with disruptions in exon 2 of MOR-1,
specific G-protein subunits mediating the effects of morphine described previously21, with a new mouse strain deficient in exon 1
(Gi2) from those involved with M6G (Gi1) in mice15 and rats16. of MOR-1. Exon-1-deficient mice were generated using a targeting
A single gene encoding the mouse mu opioid receptor vector in which exon 1 was replaced by a neomycin resistance cassette

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 151


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

a H BE N EH B B HE H E E X B 1 kb N
b
MOR1 Locus 1
2.8 kb
7.9 kb

c
N EH B E H E E X
MOR1 Targeting Vector NEO HSV tk

MOR1 Targeted Allele H B E EH B E H E E X B


NEO
Int. probe 3' probe
2 kb
7.7 kb
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

Fig.1. Creation of MOR-1 exon-1 mutant mice. (a) Schematic representation of the restriction map of the MOR-1 locus, targeting vector and the
MOR-1-targeted allele. Exon 1 is indicated by a black box. Restriction sites identified by Southern blot that are outside the MOR-1 genomic clone are
indicated in the dotted region of the upper panel. In the targeting vector, 2 kb including exon 1 of the MOR-1 gene has been replaced by a neomycin
resistance cassette. Successful targeting of the MOR-1 locus results in changes in expected BamH1 and EcoR1 fragment lengths (underlined) as
detected by respectively a 3 external and an internal probe (black bars). Positions of the upstream BamH1 (B), EcoR1 (E) and HindIII (H) are also
retained in the targeted allele. N, Not1; X, Xho1. (b) Southern-blot analysis using the 3 external probe on BamH1-digested ES DNA identifies one of
the targeted ES lines (+/). (c) Southern-blot analysis using the internal probe on EcoR1-digested tail DNA from offspring originating from MOR-1
heterozygous mating identifies homozygous (/), heterozygous (+/) and wild-type (+/+) mice.

(Fig. 1a). Introduction of the targeting vector into CCE embryonic receptor knockout mice21,24. In situ hybridization with delta (DOR-
stem cells (ES) produced three ES lines carrying a targeted mu opi- 1), kappa (KOR-1) and orphan (ORL-1) receptor probes showed
oid receptor allele, as determined by hybridizing BamH1-digested no compensatory changes in mRNA levels derived from the cognate
ES cell DNA with an external probe located 3 of the sequences genes in MOR-1-deficient mice. Similarly, proenkephalin and pro-
included in the targeting construct (Fig. 1b). Targeted ES cells from dynorphin mRNA levels did not differ significantly in mutant and
two individual clones were injected into blastocysts and both gave wild-type mice (data not shown). These results confirm and extend
rise to germline-transmitting chimeras. Heterozygous offspring were previous observations21 in a new strain of MOR-1-deficient mice.
mated to produce mice homozygous for the deleted allele (Fig. 1c).
To ensure that the region 5 of MOR-1 exon 1 was not affected by Opioid analgesia in exon-1 MOR-1 knockout mice
the recombination event, tail DNA from MOR-1 wild-type and We next examined the effects of exon-1 disruption on morphine
homozygous mice was digested with BamH1, HindIII and EcoR1 analgesia in wild-type, heterozygous and homozygous knockout
and hybridized with a 3.4-kb Not-1/BamH1 fragment correspond- mice using the tail-flick assay as described710,16,25. Wild-type
ing to the 5 region of the MOR-1 gene. This strategy revealed the 129SvEv/C57BL/6 mice of the F2 and F3 generations given fixed
alterations in EcoR1 and HindIII fragments expected from a homol- morphine doses systemically, spinally or supraspinally (Fig. 2)
ogous recombination event. In addition, all digests resulted in responded similarly to CD-1 mice. In contrast, morphine was vir-
hybridizing fragments derived from restriction sites 5 of the cloned tually inactive in the homozygous mutants, whereas the heterozy-
MOR-1 gene that were identical in wild-type and homozygous gotes had an intermediate response. Doseresponse curves
mutant mice, indicating that this region was not altered by the confirmed these initial observations (Table 1). The ED50 (effective
recombination event (data not shown). Offspring derived from het- dose for 50% of the population) in the CD-1 and the wild-type mice
erozygous matings (n = 367) showed a Mendelian inheritance pat- were indistinguishable. There was a small shift in the curve for het-
tern (24.5% wild-type, 51.8% heterozygous and 23.7% homozygous erozygotes, but morphine was inactive in the homozygous mice,
mice), indicating the absence of embryonal or postnatal lethality of even at the highest dose tested (100 mg/kg, subcutaneous). We also
homozygous mutant mice. Homozygous mutant mice were viable examined several additional mu analgesics. Systemic methadone
and fertile, with no gross morphological abnormalities. and supraspinal DAMGO were inactive in the knockout group at
Receptor-binding assays revealed no detectable binding of the doses that caused greater than 60% analgesia in wild-type mice
mu receptor agonist [3H]DAMGO ([D-Ala2,MePhe4,Gly(ol)5]- (Fig. 2a). The heterozygotes gave intermediate responses. The actions
enkephalin) in the brains of the knockout mice, whereas of the kappa1 agonist U50,488H, the kappa3 analgesic naloxone ben-
[3H]DAMGO labeling in heterozygous mice (mean standard error, zoylhydrazone (NalBzoH) and DPDPE were unaffected by the dis-
Bmax, 46 5.3 fmol/mg protein; Kd, 1.4 0.4, n = 4) was approxi- ruption of exon 1 of the MOR-1 gene. Thus, the elimination of exon
mately half that found in wild-type mice (Bmax, 78.3 3.3 fmol/mg 1 blocked the actions of three different mu analgesics, confirming
protein; Kd, 1.3 0.3, n = 4). Binding of the tritiated delta-selective the importance of MOR-1 in traditional mu analgesia, without inter-
compound [D-Pen2,D-Pen5]enkephalin ([3H]DPDPE) was similar fering with kappa or delta analgesia. Furthermore, the intermediate
in all genotypes (data not shown), consistent with the lack of change responses of the drugs in heterozygotes implies that the response to
in mRNA levels for the delta DOR-1 receptor (see below). These the mu analgesics depended on gene dosage.
binding results are similar to those reported in other strains of mu The exon-1-deficient mice responded quite differently to M6G

152 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

a Systemic b Supraspinal

(percent of mice)

(percent of mice)
Analgesia

Analgesia
*p < 0.001
*p < 0.005 **p < 0.002

Fig. 2. Analgesic action of opioids in wild-type, heterozygous or


homozygous mice. (a) Groups of mice (n = 8) received subcutaneous c Spinal
injections of morphine (13 mg/kg), methadone (2 mg/kg), U50,488H
(5 mg/kg) or NalBzoH (60 mg/kg) and were tested for analgesia 30

(percent of mice)
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

min later. (b) Groups of mice (n = 9) received intracerebroventricular

Analgesia
injections of morphine (0.7 g), DAMGO (6 ng), M6G (12.5 ng) or 6-
acetylmorphine (1.2 g) and were tested for analgesia 15 min later.
(c) Groups of mice (n = 8) received intrathecal injections of mor-
phine (0.8 g), M6G (12.5 ng) or DPDPE (500 ng) and were tested for
analgesia 15 min later. Analgesia is expressed as the percentage of
mice responding. *p < 0.002

and heroin. Given systemically, M6G and heroin both retained their previously established in CD-1 mice. The mu-selective antagonists
analgesic activity in homozygous exon-1-deficient mice, with a slight -funaltrexamine (-FNA) and naloxonazine1 both effectively block
reduction in potency (Table 1). High doses of both drugs were anal- M6G analgesia in CD-1 mice26. Here both -FNA and naloxonazine
gesic in all animals, confirming the lack of a ceiling effect. The anal- significantly antagonized M6G analgesia in all three groups of mice
gesic responses of supraspinal M6G and 6-acetylmorphine, as well as (Fig. 3a). Analgesia induced by M6G was not blocked in any of the
spinal M6G, in homozygous mice did not differ significantly from three groups by the kappa1 antagonist nor-binaltorphimine (NorB-
those in wild-type mice (Fig. 2b and c). The slight decrease in sen- NI) or by the delta antagonist naltrindole. The selective M6G antag-
sitivity of systemic M6G and heroin in the exon-1 knockout animals onist 3-methoxynaltrexone (3-MeONtx; 0.25 mg/kg, subcutaneous),
may reflect a small contribution of the traditional morphine recep- which blocks M6G, heroin and 6-acetylmorphine analgesia at doses
tor to their overall analgesic activity. that are inactive against morphine27, did not affect morphine-
induced analgesia in wild-type mice, but it effectively antagonized
M6G analgesia in exon-1 MOR-1 knockout mice M6G analgesia in all groups (Fig. 3b). Thus, the pharmacological
The pharmacological characteristics of M6G analgesia in both exon- sensitivity of M6G analgesia in the exon-1 MOR-1 mutant mice is
1-deficient and wild-type mice were indistinguishable from those identical to that found in wild-type and CD-1 mice.

a b
(percent of mice)
(percent of mice)

Analgesia
Analgesia

*p < 0.05
*p < 0.02
**p < 0.005

M6G

Fig. 3. Inhibition of M6G analgesia by selective opioid receptor antagonists. (a) Groups of mice (n = 5) received a fixed dose of intracerebroventric-
ular M6G (12.5 ng, i.c.v.) alone or with subcutaneous -FNA (40 mg/kg), naloxonazine (35 mg/kg), naltrindole (20 mg/kg) or NorBNI (10 mg/kg). -
FNA and naloxonazine were given 24 h before the agonist. The other antagonists were given 30 min before the analgesics. Analgesia is expressed as
the percentage of mice responding. -FNA and naloxonazine significantly lowered M6G analgesia, as determined by the Fischer exact test. (b) Groups
of mice received intracerebroventricular M6G (12.5 ng) alone (Control) or 15 min after subcutaneous 3-MeONTX (0.25 mg/kg). Anot her group of
wild-type mice received i.c.v. morphine (0.7 g) alone (Control) or with subcutaneous 3-MeONTX (0.25 mg/kg).

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 153


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

than its ED50 in exon-1-deficient mice (Table 2). These results


demonstrate that exon 2, but not exon 1, of the MOR-1 gene is
important for M6G and 6-acetylmorphine analgesia.
(percent in mice)

MOR-1 transcripts in exon-1-deficient mice


Analgesia

The above results suggested that the MOR-1 exon-2-containing tran-


script is necessary for M6G analgesia. To determine whether mRNA
transcripts containing exons 2 and 3 were present in exon-1-defi-
cient mice, we used reverse transcriptase/polymerase chain reactions
on RNA extracts from their brains (Fig. 6). Primers restricted to
exon 1 failed to reveal a product in the exon-1-deficient mice, but
*p < 0.002
**p < 0.006
not wild-type mice, confirming the deletion of this exon. However,
primers spanning exons 2 and 3 produced a transcript of the pre-
dicted size (~556 bp). The authenticity of this product was con-
Fig. 4. Antisense effects of M6G analgesia. Groups of mice (n = 10)
received saline or the indicated oligodeoxynucleotide (5 g, i.c.v.) on firmed by sequencing in both directions. A second set of reactions on
days 1, 3 and 5. On day 6, all mice were tested with M6G (12.5 ng, i.c.v.), extracts from the periaqueductal gray, again using primers spanning
and analgesia was assessed. The antisense probe targeting exon 2 signif- exons 2 and 3 (position 621642 and 11201140), also produced a
icantly blocked M6G analgesia. band of the expected size with its identity also confirmed by sequenc-
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

ing (data not shown). Thus, expression of MOR-1 transcripts con-


taining exons 2 and 3 is retained in the exon-1-deficient mice.

Antisense has also been used to distinguish the analgesic mech- DISCUSSION
anisms of morphine and M6G analgesia811,16. We have demon- Although heroin and M6G have long been considered traditional
strated that an antisense probe targeting exon 2 of MOR-1 blocked mu analgesics that differed from morphine only in their pharma-
M6G-induced, but not morphine-induced analgesia in CD-1 cokinetics26, recent studies suggest that these drugs may act through
mice8,9,16. In this study, this same antisense probe blocked M6G an opioid receptor mechanism distinct from the one now known to
analgesia in wild-type and exon-1 MOR-1 mutant mice (Fig. 4). The mediate morphine actions711,16. To genetically differentiate the
mismatch antisense probe was inactive, confirming the specificity actions of morphine from those of heroin and M6G, we examined
of the effect. Thus, M6G analgesia is pharmacologically indistin- the analgesic potential of these drugs in exon-1-deficient mice. As
guishable in wild-type and exon-1 MOR-1 mutant mice. expected, morphine was inactive in these mice, consistent with results
The retention of M6G analgesia in exon-1-deficient mice, and from other knockout mice containing mutations in either exon 1
its continued sensitivity to mu receptor antagonists and MOR-1 (refs 22, 23), exon 2 (ref. 21) or exons 2 and 3 of MOR-1 (ref. 24).
antisense, raised the question of whether there may be an alterna- However, M6G and heroin, as well as its active metabolite 6-acetyl-
tive mu receptor in these knockout mice. [3H]M6G labels traditional morphine, retained their analgesic activity in the exon-1-deficient
mu receptors in brain and in CHO cells transfected with MOR-1 mice. Pharmacologically, M6G analgesia in the exon-1-deficient ani-
moderately well. However, the brain contains a high-affinity mals was indistinguishable from that observed in wild-type mice.
[3H]M6G binding site of low abundance not observed in the trans- Mu antagonists still reversed M6G analgesia in exon-1 MOR-1
fected CHO cells, which might correspond to the M6G receptor14. mutant mice, whereas delta and kappa selective antagonists were
The exon-1 MOR-1 mutant mice also had low levels of [3H]M6G ineffective. Thus, M6G, heroin and 6-acetylmorphine analgesia can
binding (Fig. 5). [3H]M6G binding was reliably detected in repeat- be mediated, at least in part, through mu receptors that contain exon
ed experiments, which had to be done on fresh tissue because freez- 2, but not exon 1, of MOR-1 and that are distinct from traditional
ing markedly diminished the binding levels. The binding was high morphine receptors.
affinity, with a Bmax far lower than those typically observed for mu The blockade of M6G analgesia by an exon-2 antisense oligonu-
receptors in wild-type mice. Nonlinear regression analysis of the cleotide in the exon-1 MOR-1 mutant mice implied that the M6G
combined binding data yielded a Bmax value of 12.4 2.5 fmol/mg receptor is generated either from an alternative transcript from the
protein with a KD value of 3.7 1.2 nM. MOR-1 locus or from a distinct gene sharing sequence homology
with exon 2 of the MOR-1 gene. However, this latter possibility seems
M6G and 6-Ac-morphine analgesia in mice lacking exon 2 quite unlikely because disruption of exon 2 of MOR-1 eliminates
The sensitivity of M6G analgesia to an exon-2 antisense probe in M6G and 6-acetylmorphine analgesia (Table 2), as reported for a
both wild-type and exon-1-deficient mice, as well as in CD-1 knockout in which both exons 2 and 3 had been disrupted24. If the
mice8,9,16, strongly suggested that the M6G receptor contains exon 2 M6G receptor were generated from a different locus, M6G and 6-
of MOR-1. To explore this possibility further, we next compared acetylmorphine analgesia should not have been lost in the exon-2-
M6G analgesia in mice with a disrupt-
ed exon 2 (ref. 21) to that in the exon-1-
deficient strain. M6G remained an Table 1. Analgesic activity of opioids in CD-1 and 129SvEv C57BL/6 mice.
effective analgesic in the exon-1 MOR- Drug CD-1 +/+ +/ / ratio (/)/(+/+)
1 mutant mice, but it was inactive in the Morphine 5.4 (3.6, 8.2) 5.0 (2.6, 9.7) 6.3 (4.4, 9.2) > 100 > 19
exon-2 MOR-1 mutant mice, even at a M6G 3.7 (2.4, 5.6) 3.8 (2.4, 6.0) 7.7 (4.5, 13.1) 8.5 (6.6, 10.8) 2.3
dose over sevenfold greater than the
Heroin 1.5 (1.0, 2.0) 1.2 (0.7, 1.8) 2.7 (2.1, 3.5) 4.0 (3.4, 4.7) 2.7
ED50 value in the exon-1-deficient mice
(Table 2). Similarly, 6-acetylmorphine ED50 values (mg/kg) with 95% confidence limits determined from at least three doses of each drug given sub-
was inactive in exon-2 MOR-1 mutant cutaneously in a cumulative doseresponse protocol (see Methods for doses). In the / group, morphine at
100 mg/kg did not induce analgesia in any of the mice.
mice at a dose over threefold greater

154 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

[3H]M6G binding (cpm)

Exon 1 Exon 2 and 3

Fig. 6. RT-PCR of MOR-1 transcripts in exon-1-deficient mice. The pre-


[3H]M6G (nM) dicted sizes of the amplified cDNA fragments were 362 bp for exon
1and 556 bp for exons 2 and 3. Lanes 1 and 5 contain cDNA from exon-
1-deficient mice. Lanes 2 and 6 contain cDNA from wild-type mice.
Fig. 5. [3H]M6G binding in exon-1 MOR-1 mutant mice. Results are
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

Lanes 3 and 7 contain genomic DNA from wild-type mice. Lanes 4 and 8
from three independent replications. contain no added DNA.

deficient mice. Thus, the M6G receptor is probably encoded by an mice that is distinct from that responsible for morphine analgesia.
alternative transcript from the MOR-1 locus. The current data provide direct genetic evidence for a unique mech-
If the M6G receptor were an alternatively spliced variant of MOR- anism for the actions of heroin, 6-acetylmorphine and M6G, which
1, we should be able to detect the persistence of MOR-1 transcripts may prove valuable in the development of new therapeutics target-
in the mutant mice. In mRNA from the brains of exon-1-deficient ing pain and drug abuse.
mice, RT-PCR revealed no evidence of exon 1 . However, we did It is important to note that we did not target the promoter of
detect transcripts containing exons 2 and 3 with RT-PCR, using an MOR-1 in our construct and that this region remains intact, as
upstream primer in exon 2 and a downstream primer in exon 3. shown by Southern analysis. We predict that retention of an intact
Sequencing confirmed their identity. Although exon 1 in these promoter would be essential to generate the spliced RNA tran-
knockout mice was lost, alternative MOR-1 transcripts were still pre- scripts detected here and that disruption of this region could
sent. The low levels of [3H]M6G binding in the knockout mice sug- eliminate all transcripts and thus possibly yield a different anal-
gested that the abundance of these residual transcripts also might gesic phenotype. Subtle differences in targeting vectors could
be low, which is confirmed by our ability to detect them only with thus lead to different results.
RT-PCR and not with in situ hybridization or northern blots. There
is also evidence indicating that MOR-1-derived transcripts can be METHODS
translated in the MOR-1-deficient mice. For example, immunos- Gene targeting. Gene-targeting experiments were done essentially as
taining with antisera targeting a newly cloned alternative exon 4 of described30 . A MOR-1 genomic clone was isolated from a 129/ReJ genom-
MOR-1 (Y.-X.P., Catherine Abbadie, E.B., Amy Zuckerman, Jin Xu, ic library after screening with a digoxigenin-labeled mouse MOR-1 cDNA
Grace C. Rossi and G.W.P., unpublished data) demonstrates its con- probe containing exon 1 and part of exon 2, obtained from C. Evans, and
characterized by restriction mapping. A 3.4-kb Not1/BamH1 fragment, locat-
tinued expression in the exon-1-deficient animals. Although the ed 5 of exon 1 and a 6.5kb HindIII/Xho1 fragment corresponding to part
relationship of this new exon to the receptor responsible for M6G of intron 1 were subcloned into a targeting construct containing the
analgesia remains unknown, it indicates that MOR-1 transcripts are neomycin resistance cassette and the HSV-TK gene. The linearized target-
still present and can be translated in these exon-1-deficient mice. ing vector was introduced into ES cells by electroporation. DNA isolated
Persistent expression of alternative transcripts in knockout mice has from G418 and gancyclovir double-resistant ES clones was digested with
been observed in other systems. For example,
initial characterization of neuronal nitric oxide
synthase (nNOS) knockout mice revealed no Table 2. Analgesic activity of M6G and 6-acetylmorphine in MOR-1-deficient
detectable nNOS mRNA or protein28. How- mice.
ever, subsequent investigations uncovered the Drug Analgesic response
persistent expression of low levels of alterna-
Exon 1 deficient Exon 2 deficient
tively spliced transcripts in these mutant
29 +/+ / +/+ /
mice . Exon 1 encodes the first transmem-
brane spanning region of the MOR-1 recep- M6G 12.5 ng 90% 50% 80% 10% p < 0.003
tor. Because G-protein-coupled receptors all 62.5 ng N.D. 100% N.D. 0%
contain seven transmembrane regions, the 6-Acetylmorphine 1.2 g 80% 70% 75% 0% p < 0.002
M6G receptor presumably contains an alter- 3.6 g N.D. N.D. N.D. 0%
native exon 1 to maintain the structure.
Percentage of mice (n = 10, except for the homozygous exon-2-deficient mice receiving 1.2 g of 6-
Together, these observations indicate that acetylmorphine, where n = 8) that showed analgesia by the tail-flick assay after receiving the indicated
M6G analgesia in the exon-1-deficient ani- drugs by intracerebroventricular injection. The responses of exon-2-deficient mice were significantly dif-
mals was mediated, at least in part, through a ferent between the +/+ and / groups, as indicated by the Fisher exact test. Doses that were not
receptor mechanism present in wild-type examined are designated as N.D. (not determined).

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 155


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

BamH1 and analyzed by genomic Southern blotting using a 0.8-kb 2. Way, E. L., Young, J. & Kemp, J. Metabolism of heroin and its
Xho1/BamH1 fragment located 3 of the sequences included in the target- pharmacological implications. Bull. Narcotics 17, 2533 (1965).
3. Way, E., Kemp, J., Young, J. & Grasetti, D. R. The pharmacologic effects of
ing vector. Two different lines of ES cells carrying a targeted MOR-1 allele heroin in relationship to its rate of biotransformation. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther.
as determined by a 7.9-kb wild-type and a 7.7-kb targeted BamH1 fragment, 129, 144154 (1960).
were injected into C57Bl6 blastocysts to generate chimeras. Offspring of 4. Oldendorf, W. H., Hyman, S., Braun, L. & Oldendorf, S. Z. Blood-brain
germline-transmitting chimeras were genotyped as described above or by barrier: penetration of morphine, codeine, heroin, and methadone after
hybridizing EcoR1-digested tail DNA with a 1.2-kb EcoR1/BamH1 probe carotid injection. Science 178, 984986 (1972).
5. Inturrisi, C. E. et al. The pharmacokinetics of heroin in patients with chronic
revealing a 2.8-kb wild-type and 2-kb targeted EcoR1 fragment. pain. N. Engl. J. Med. 310, 12131217 (1984).
6. Inturrisi, C. E. et al. Evidence from opiate binding studies that heroin acts
Binding assay and RT-PCR. [3H]M6G binding was measured in brain mem- through its metabolites. Life Sci. 33 Suppl 1, 773776 (1983).
brane fractions from MOR-1-deficient mice as described14. For RT-PCR, 7. Rossi, G., Pan, Y.-X., Cheng, J. & Pasternak, G. W. Blockade of morphine
analgesia by an antisense oligodeoxynucleotide against the mu receptor. Life
total RNA from the brains of exon-1-deficient mice were extracted and Sci. 54, PL375379 (1994).
reverse transcribed using random hexamers to generate a first-strand cDNA. 8. Rossi, G. C., Standifer, K. M. & Pasternak, G. W. Differential blockade of
To detect MOR-1 exon-1 expression, two primers were designed from the morphine and morphine-6 beta-glucuronide analgesia by antisense
nucleotide sequence of mouse MOR-1 (GenBank submission #U26915) at oligodeoxynucleotides directed against MOR-1 and G-protein alpha subunits
positions 195213 (sense) and 531556 (antisense). To determine expres- in rats. Neurosci. Lett. 198, 99102 (1995).
9. Rossi, G. C., Pan, Y.-X., Brown, G. P. & Pasternak, G. W. Antisense mapping
sion of exons 2 and 3, we used one primer set based on sequences 624648 the MOR-1 opioid receptor: evidence for alternative splicing and a novel
(sense) and 11601180 (antisense) and another based on sequences 621642 morphine-6 beta-glucuronide receptor. FEBS Lett. 369, 192196 (1995).
and 11201140 (not shown). Samples were heated to 94oC and cycled 35 10. Rossi, G. C., Brown, G. P., Leventhal, L., Yang, K. & Pasternak, G. W. Novel
times through 94oC for 30 s, 63oC for 30 s, 72oC for 90 s. The final incuba- receptor mechanisms for heroin and morphine-6 beta-glucuronide analgesia.
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

tion was at 72oC for 2 min. The mixture was then run on an agarose gel with Neurosci. Lett. 216, 14 (1996).
11. Pasternak, G. W. & Standifer, K. M. Mapping of opioid receptors using
the indicated markers. antisense oligodeoxynucleotides: correlating their molecular biology and
pharmacology. Trends Pharmacol. Sci. 16, 344350 (1995).
Tail-flick assay. Analgesia was assessed in the tail-flick assay as a doubling or 12. Pick, C. G., Nejat, R. J. & Pasternak, G. W. Independent expression of two
greater of the baseline latencies of each mouse, which ranged from 23 s. A pharmacologically distinct supraspinal mu analgesic systems in genetically
different mouse strains. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 265, 166171 (1993).
maximal latency of 10 s was used to minimize tissue damage. Comparisons 13. Moskowitz, A. S. & Goodman, R. R. Autoradiographic distribution of mu1
were made using the Fisher exact test. Drugs were given under halothane and mu2 opioid binding in the mouse central nervous system. Brain Res. 360,
anesthesia by a 2-l intracerebroventricular injection. 117129 (1985).
14. Brown, G. P. et al. 3H-morphine-6beta-glucuronide binding in brain
Cumulative dose-response curves. Morphine doses tested were 3, 6, 9, 10, membranes and an MOR-1-transfected cell line. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 282,
12911297 (1997).
13, 20, 50 or 100 mg/kg. M6G doses were 2.2, 4.4, 6.6, 8, 8.8, 11 or 15. Standifer, K. M., Rossi, G. C. & Pasternak, G. W. Differential blockade of
13.2 mg/kg. Heroin doses were 0.8, 1.6, 2.4, 3.2, 4, 4.8, 5.6 or 6.4 mg/kg. opioid analgesia by antisense oligodeoxynucleotides directed against various
Cumulative doseresponse curves were determined by giving all mice the G protein alpha subunits. Mol. Pharmacol. 50, 293298 (1996).
lowest opioid dose. Mice that showed analgesia were removed from further 16. Rossi, G. C. et al. Antisense mapping of MOR-1 in rats: distinguishing
between morphine and morphine-6beta-glucuronide antinociception. J.
testing, and the remainder were given the next higher dose. Again, mice Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 281, 109114 (1997).
showing analgesia were removed, and additional doses were given to the 17. Reisine, T. & Bell, G. I. Molecular biology of opioid receptors. Trends
remaining mice. ED50 values with 95% confidence limits were determined Neurosci. 16, 506510 (1993).
using the Bliss program, as described710. Values for morphine in CD-1 mice 18. Uhl, G. R., Childers, S. & Pasternak, G. An opiate-receptor gene family
determined by cumulative doseresponse curves do not differ significantly reunion. Trends Neurosci. 17, 8993 (1994).
19. Zimprich, A., Simon, T. & Hollt, V. Cloning and expression of an isoform of
from those obtained with separate groups of +/+ mice from the MOR-1 the rat mu opioid receptor (rMOR1B) which differs in agonist induced
knockout receiving each morphine dose. desensitization from rMOR1. FEBS Lett. 359, 142146 (1995).
20. Bare, L. A., Mansson, E. & Yang, D. Expression of two variants of the human
Antisense experiments. The probe targeting exon 2 was TTGGTG- mu opioid receptor mRNA in SK-N-SH cells and human brain. FEBS Lett.
354, 213216 (1994).
GCAGTCTTCATTTTGG (572-593) and the mismatch was CGCCCC-
21. Matthes, H. W. et al. Loss of morphine-induced analgesia, reward effect and
GACCTCTTCCCTT (195213). Both probes have been used previously in withdrawal symptoms in mice lacking the mu-opioid-receptor gene. Nature
CD-1 mice79. 383, 819823 (1996).
22. Sora, I. et al. Opiate receptor knockout mice define mu receptor roles in
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS endogenous nociceptive responses and morphine-induced analgesia. Proc.
Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 94, 15441549 (1997).
The authors thank Rhuna Shen and Qian Ye for their contributions in mapping the 23. Tian, M. et al. Altered hematopoiesis, behavior, and sexual function in mu
mu opioid receptor gene and constructing the targeting vector. We thank Chris opioid receptor-deficient mice. J. Exp. Med. 185, 15171522 (1997).
24. Loh, H. H. et al. Mu opioid receptor knockout in mice: effects on ligand-
Evans, Jim Douglass and Grahaem Bell for providing probes for DOR-1,
induced analgesia and morphine lethality. Mol. Brain Res. 54, 321326
proenkephalin/prodynorphin and KOR-1, respectively, Z.P. Chen for providing the (1998).
mouse ORL in situ probe, and Liz Robertson for providing CCE ES cells. We also 25. Chen, X. H. et al. An antisense oligodeoxynucleotide to mu-opioid receptors
inhibits mu-opioid receptor agonist-induced analgesia in rats. Eur. J.
thank Brigitte Kieffer and Hans Matthes for providing the exon-2 MOR-1 mutant
Pharmacol. 275, 105108 (1995).
mice. This work was supported by grants from the National Institute on Drug Abuse 26. Paul, D., Standifer, K. M., Inturrisi, C. E. & Pasternak, G. W. Pharmacological
to J.E.P. (DA-09040 and DA-08622), Y.-X.P. (DA-00296) and G.W.P. (DA-07241, characterization of morphine-6 beta-glucuronide, a very potent morphine
metabolite. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 251, 477483 (1989).
DA-02615 and a Research Scientist Award DA-00220) and a core grant from the 27. Brown, G. P. et al. 3-Methoxynaltrexone, a selective heroin/morphine-6beta-
National Cancer Institute to Memorial Sloan-Kettering Cancer Center (CA-08748). glucuronide antagonist. FEBS Lett. 412, 3538 (1997).
D.J.M. was supported by NIH training grant R25-GM-55145. 28. Huang, P. L., Dawson, T. M., Bredt, D. S., Snyder, S. H. & Fishman, M. C.
Targeted disruption of the neuronal nitric oxide synthase gene. Cell 75,
12731286 (1993).
RECEIVED 2 NOVEMBER; ACCEPTED 11 DECEMBER 1998 29. Brenman, J. E. et al. Interaction of nitric oxide synthase with the postsynaptic
density protein PSD-95 and alpha1-syntrophin mediated by PDZ domains.
Cell 84, 757767 (1996).
1. Pasternak, G. W. Pharmacological mechanisms of opioid analgesics. Clin. 30. Joyner, A. L. Gene Targeting: A Practical Approach (Oxford Univ. Press,
Neuropharmacol. 16, 118 (1993). Oxford, 1993).

156 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

SOD1 rescues cerebral endothelial


dysfunction in mice overexpressing
amyloid precursor protein
Costantino Iadecola1, Fangyi Zhang1, Kiyoshi Niwa1, Chris Eckman2, Sherry K. Turner3,
Elizabeth Fischer4, Steven Younkin2, David R. Borchelt5, Karen K. Hsiao1 and George A. Carlson3

1 Department of Neurology, University of Minnesota, 420 Delaware Street S.E., Minneapolis, Minnesota 55455, USA
2 Mayo Clinic Jacksonville, 4500 San Pablo Road, Jacksonville, Florida 32224, USA
3 McLaughlin Research Institute, 1520 23rd Street South, Great Falls, Montana 59405, USA
4 Rocky Mountain Laboratory, National Institute of Allergy and Infectious Diseases, 903 South 4th Street, Hamilton, Montana 59840, USA
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

5 Division of Neuropathology, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, 720 Rutland Avenue, Baltimore, Maryland 21205, USA
Correspondence should be addressed to C.I. (iadec001@tc.umn.edu)

Peptides derived from proteolytic processing of the -amyloid precursor protein (APP), including
the amyloid- peptide, are important for the pathogenesis of Alzheimers dementia. We found that
transgenic mice overexpressing APP have a profound and selective impairment in endothelium-
dependent regulation of the neocortical microcirculation. Such endothelial dysfunction was not
found in transgenic mice expressing both APP and superoxide dismutase-1 (SOD1) or in APP
transgenics in which SOD was topically applied to the cerebral cortex. These cerebrovascular effects
of peptides derived from APP processing may contribute to the alterations in cerebral blood flow
and to neuronal dysfunction in Alzheimers dementia.

There is substantial evidence that peptides derived from proteolyt- flow (CBF) was continuously monitored in the exposed parietal
ic processing of the -amyloid precursor protein, including the amy- cortex by a laser-Doppler probe. Resting CBF did not differ among
loid- peptide (A), are involved in the pathogenesis of Alzheimers the groups of mice studied (p > 0.05; data not shown). Topical
dementia1. However, the mechanisms by which these peptides exert neocortical application of acetylcholine, a neurotransmitter that
their pathogenic effects remain undefined2. Generation of reactive increases CBF by activating endothelial muscarinic receptors and
oxygen species by A has been proposed as a potential link between releasing endothelial nitric oxide 1315, increased CBF in
A and neurotoxicity36. Studies in isolated blood vessels and in APP/SOD1 mice (Fig. 1a). This increase was substantially atten-
endothelial cell cultures have suggested that peptides derived from uated in littermates expressing APP but not SOD1 (p < 0.001).
APP processing produce deleterious vascular effects that may con- The reduction in acetylcholine-induced vasodilation was related
tribute to neurodegeneration in Alzheimers dementia710. Addition to reactive oxygen species, because the altered response was not
of micromolar quantities of synthetic A peptide to rings of rat aorta found in mice overexpressing both APP and SOD1. Similarly, the
causes loss of vasodilation in response to acetylcholine and increased increase in CBF produced by bradykinin, a vasodilator that acts
contractility in response to vasoconstrictors, effects caused by severe through specific endothelial receptors and cyclooxygenase prod-
damage to the endothelial lining of the vascular rings7. These toxic ucts1517, or by the calcium ionophore A23187, a compound that
effects of A are prevented by addition of the superoxide-scavenging produces endothelium-dependent vasodilation through receptor-
enzyme SOD. However, it is not known whether nanomolar eleva- independent mechanisms14,18, was also attenuated in APP+/SOD1
tions in A comparable to those occurring in Alzheimers demen- mice (Fig. 1b and c). Again, this reduction in vasodilation was not
tia result in superoxide-dependent alterations of cerebrovascular found in APP+/SOD1+ mice. In contrast to endothelium-depen-
regulation in vivo. Therefore, we used transgenic mice overexpress- dent cerebrovascular responses, increases in CBF produced by
ing APP to explore whether elevation of the levels of APP and A vasodilators that do not act through the endothelium, such as the
alter cerebrovascular reactivity and, if so, whether the effect is medi- nitric oxide donor S-nitroso-N-acetylpenicillamine (SNAP) or
ated by reactive oxygen species. hypercapnia19, were not reduced in APP+/SOD1 mice (Fig. 2).
These endothelial-cell-independent responses did not differ sig-
RESULTS nificantly among APP+/SOD1+, APP/SOD1+ and APP/SOD1
Crossing mice from a transgenic line Tg1130H expressing high mice. Thus, APP overexpression influences only vasodilator
copy numbers of the human mutant APP11 with an outbred stock responses mediated through the endothelium.
overexpressing human SOD1 (ref. 12) provided littermates express- We then investigated the cerebrovascular effect of the throm-
ing APP alone (APP+/SOD1), SOD1 alone (APP/SOD1+) or both boxane A2 analogue U46619, a potent vasoconstrictor that acts
transgenes (APP+/SOD1+), along with transgene-negative con- directly on vascular smooth muscles20,21. The decrease in CBF elicit-
trols (APP/SOD1). For cerebrovascular studies, cerebral blood ed by U46619 was exacerbated in mice overexpressing APP, but not

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 157


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

a Acetylcholine b Bradykinin To examine the possibility that the nor-


mal response to endothelium-dependent
vasodilators in APP+/SOD1+ mice resulted
CBF (% increase)

CBF (% increase)
from reduced A levels, we measured
human A peptides 140 and 142 in the
brains of APP+/SOD1 and APP+/SOD1+
mice11,23. In APP+/SOD1+ mice, levels of
both forms of A were slightly, but signifi-
cantly, lower than those of APP+/SOD1
mice (Fig. 3). To determine whether the lack
of endothelial dysfunction in APP+/SOD1+
U46619 mice could be attributed to this reduction
A23187
c d in A, we studied a line of wild-type human
CBF (% reduction) APP transgenic mice (Tg6209) that express-
es less transgene-encoded protein than
CBF (% increase)

Tg1130H (ref. 11). Despite their lower A


levels (Fig. 3), Tg6209 mice showed alter-
ations in endothelium-dependent cere-
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

brovascular responses indistinguishable


from those of Tg1130H APP+/SOD1 mice
(percent CBF increases: acetylcholine, APP
27 1, APP + 8 1; bradykinin, APP
44 4, APP+ 18 2; A23187, APP 49 2,
APP + 14 5; n = 56 per group). Thus, the
Fig. 1. Effects of endothelium-dependent vasodilators and vasocontrictors on cerebral blood flow
+
in nontransgenic mice (APP /SOD1 ) and in littermates overexpressing APP (APP /SOD1 ), SOD1 protection exerted by SOD1 overexpression
(APP/SOD1+) or both APP and SOD1 (APP+/SOD1+). (a) Acetylcholine (10 M). (b) Bradykinin in APP mice cannot be attributed to SOD1-
(50 M). (c) A23187 (3 M). (d) The vasoconstrictor U46619 (1 M). Data were collected from related reduction in A concentration.
58 mice per group. *p < 0.0010.003, analysis of variance and Tukeys test. Synthetic A has been reported to pro-
duce endothelial damage in isolated arter-
ies 79. There is no evidence for
cerebrovascular abnormalities in Tg1130H
in APP+/SOD1+ mice (Fig. 1d). SOD1 overexpression alone did not mice at the light-microscope level, with the only consistent pathol-
alter responses to vasodilators and vasoconstrictors (Figs. 1 and 2). ogy being hypertrophic corticolimbic astrocytic gliosis11. We there-
These results indicate that APP overexpression leads to attenuation fore used electron microscopy to determine whether endogenous
of endothelium-dependent cerebrovascular vasodilation, and to A produced cerebral endothelial damage in vivo. The morpholo-
enhancement of vasoconstriction by mechanisms involving reactive gy of endothelial cells was not grossly altered in APP+/SOD1 mice
oxygen species. (Fig. 4). Therefore, in contrast to previous investigations in which
Expression of SOD1 transgenes also provides protection against high concentrations of synthetic A were used in isolated vessels7,
22
APP-induced premature death . Twenty-five of thirty-one we find that the disturbance in endothelium-dependent vasodila-
APP+/SOD1 mice died within 150 days of birth, whereas 19 tion in the transgenic mice results from perturbation of endothelial
APP+/SOD1+ mice survived for at least 1 year, and 1 mouse died at cell function and not from endothelial cell destruction.
167 days. This difference in survival is statistically significant The observation that endothelium-dependent cerebrovascular
(p < 0.001; Fishers exact test). responses are intact in transgenic mice expressing both APP and

SNAP Hypercapnia
a b
CBF (% increase)
CBF (% increase)

n=8

Fig. 2. Effect of endothelium-independent vasodilators S-nitroso-N-acetylpenicillamine (SNAP) or hypercapnia (pCO2 = 5060 mmHg) in nontrans-
genic mice and in littermates overexpressing APP, SOD1 or both APP and SOD1.

158 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

compatible with a reduction in normal processing than with alter-


Tg1130/SOD1- (n = 6)
Tg1130/SOD1+ (n = 9)
ations in peptide catabolism.
The finding that the endothelial dysfunction can be rescued by
Tg6209/SOD1- (n = 8)
exogenous SOD or prevented by SOD1 overexpression suggests that
A (pmol/g)

reactive oxygen species are involved in mediating the effect. Super-


oxide anions could be produced either intracellularly, for example,
by activation of the receptor for advanced glycation end products5, or
extracellularly by the endothelial NADH-dependent oxidoreductase,
which is membrane bound25. The finding that both SOD1, expressed
intracellularly in endothelial cells of SOD1 transgenics26, and exoge-
nous SOD, which does not cross cell membranes24, are protective
suggests that superoxide is not generated within endothelial cells.
A (140) A (142) This hypothesis is supported by two observations in APP transgen-
ics. First, endothelial cells do not show histochemical markers or
Fig. 3. Brain A levels in Tg1130H-derived APP+/SOD1 or ultrastructural evidence of free-radical-mediated damage (G. Perry
APP+/SOD1+, and in transgene-positive FVB/N Tg6209 mice. *p < 0.01
and K.K. H., unpublished observations, ref. 3 and Fig. 4). Second,
from Tg1130/SOD1; #p < 0.001 from both Tg1130/SOD1 and
Tg1130/SOD1+ (analysis of variance and Tukeys test). the endothelial dysfunction is not irreversible because it can be res-
cued by exogenous SOD. However, vascular production of reactive
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

oxygen species remains to be demonstrated in APP transgenics.


There is ample evidence that reactive oxygen species result in
SOD1 implicates reactive oxygen species in the mechanism of the cerebral endothelial dysfunction, and they are thought to be respon-
endothelial dysfunction. To study the possible source of superoxide, sible for loss of endothelium-dependent vasodilation in atheroscle-
we tested whether the cerebrovascular dysfunction could be reversed rosis, diabetes and hypertension2730. However, the mechanisms of
by local treatment of the cerebral cortex with SOD, an enzyme that this effect are not understood31. Considering that a common
does not easily cross cell membranes24. In APP+ mice (Tg6209), denominator in the vasodilator effect of acetylcholine, bradykinin
SOD superfusion completely reversed the attenuation of the CBF and A23187 is an increase in endothelial calcium31, superoxide and
increase evoked by acetylcholine without affecting the response to other radicals could perturb endothelial calcium homeostasis and
hypercapnia (Fig. 5). In APP littermates, SOD superfusion did not impair production of endothelial vasoactive factors.
influence vasodilator responses to acetylcholine or hypercapnia Overexpression of APP in the FVB/N strain of mice leads to pre-
(Fig. 5). SOD superfusion did not affect resting CBF in either group mature death11,22. The observation that SOD1 protects against the
of mice (p > 0.05; data not shown). lethal effects of APP overexpression could provide a link between
reactive oxygen species and early mortality22. Although SOD1
DISCUSSION expression prevents both cerebrovascular dysfunction and premature
We have demonstrated that APP overexpression in living mice leads death, the relationship, if any, between the two phenomena remains
to a profound and selective alteration in endothelial vascular regu- to be determined. Although FVB/N mice could be more sensitive
lation. The cerebral vascular reactivity to acetylcholine, bradykinin to induction of superoxide-dependent cerebrovascular dysfunction
and A23187 are all reduced in APP mice, suggesting that APP over-
expression leads to a global impairment in endothelium-dependent
responses rather than to an alteration of a specific endothelial recep- c e
tor or vasodilator mechanism. The impairment in endothelium-
a
dependent vasodilation would be expected to render cerebral blood
vessels more susceptible to vasoconstriction. Consistent with this
hypothesis, the response to the vasoconstrictor U46619 is enhanced
in mice overexpressing APP.
The fragment of APP that is involved in the mechanisms of
this endothelial dysfunction remains to be identified. The obser-
vation that the abnormality in endothelium-dependent vasodi-
lation is present both in Tg1130H and Tg6209 mice, despite a
twofold difference in A(140) levels, suggests that this APP frag-
ment may not mediate the endothelial dysfunction. However, b d f
A(142) is not substantially reduced in Tg6209, and this APP
fragment could conceivably be involved. Transgenic mice with
deletions and mutations of candidate APP regions will have to
be studied to determine which APP domain is responsible for
vascular dysfunction.
Although insufficient to account for SOD1-mediated protec-
tion against vascular dysfunction and against premature death,
the reduction in A peptide concentration by SOD1 expression
is noteworthy. It is not clear whether SOD1 alters the processing of
APP so that less A is produced or whether A peptide is removed Fig. 4. Electron micrographs of thin sections from parietal cortex of mice.
more efficiently. Both the short (140) and more amyloidogenic The morphology of endothelial cells (arrowheads) in transgene-negative
long (142) forms of A peptide are reduced by SOD1 overex- control mice (a) is comparable to that of APP/SOD1+ (b), APP+/SOD1
pression. The ratio of the two forms is unchanged, a result more (c and d) or APP+/SOD1+ mice (e and f). Scale bar, 1.0 m.

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 159


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Hypercapnia
a
Acetylcholine b
CBF (% increase)

CBF (% increase)
APP+ APP APP+ APP

Fig. 5. Effect of topical application of SOD to the cerebral cortex on CBF responses in Tg6209 and APP+ APP littermates. (a) Response to acetyl-
choline. (b) Response to hypercapnia. Data were collected from 56 mice per group. * p < 0.001, t-test.
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

than other mouse strains, it is also possible that the involvement of SOD1 mice (line Tg(SOD1)76) were constructed with a 12-kb genomic clone
reactive oxygen species is not related to their cerebrovascular effects. containing the entire human gene12. Offspring of Tg(APP) Tg(SOD1)
The findings also raise the possibility that cerebrovascular mice were typed for the presence of the transgenes as described22. All exper-
endothelial dysregulation may contribute to the neuronal dysfunc- iments were done on littermates that shared genetic background except for
tion observed in APP+ mice11. Vascular dysregulation may impair the transgene(s) being overexpressed. This experimental design eliminates
the possibility that the observed effects are a consequence of differences in
brain function by altering the balance between flow-dependent sub- genetic background rather than expression of the transgene.
strate delivery and energy demands imposed by neural activity. In Because SOD1 activity is a reflection of copper concentrations42 and
addition, the endothelial dysfunction may disrupt the blood-brain because APP is a copper-binding protein that reduces Cu2+ to Cu+ (ref.
barrier and impair the delivery to the brain of critical substrates. In 43), we examined the effects of expression of each protein on the other.
support of this possibility, subtoxic concentrations of A reduce the To verify that APP overexpression did not influence SOD1 activity in dou-
transfer of glucose across endothelial cells, an effect likely to be medi- ble transgenics, we measured SOD1 activity in whole blood of
ated by reactive oxygen species10. In addition, the vascular dysreg- APP/SOD1+ and APP+/SOD1+ mice using the Bioxytech SOD1-525 spec-
ulation induced by APP overexpression makes the brain more trophotometric assay44. In SOD1 mice (n = 6), SOD1 activity was 3.6
susceptible to the effects of cerebral ischemia32. Thus, cerebrovas- 0.4 units, significantly (p < 0.05) less than the 9.2 1.5 units in
APP/SOD1+ mice (n = 5) or 10.3 0.6 units in APP+/SOD1+ mice (n =
cular dysfunction resulting from APP overexpression renders the
5). APP/SOD1+ and APP+/SOD1+ were not significantly different from
brain more vulnerable to injury and could amplify the pathogenic one another (p > 0.05, t-test). Therefore, APP overexpression did not
process in Alzheimers dementia. interfere with the elevation in SOD1 activity in SOD1+ mice. Similarly,
It is well established that Alzheimers dementia and aging are western blots did not show any obvious change in APP expression as a
associated with alterations in cerebral hemodynamics and cere- consequence of SOD1 overexpression.
brovascular regulation33,34. Resting CBF is decreased and the vas- Determination of cerebral blood flow. Techniques used for studying the
cular reactivity to selected vasodilator stimuli is reduced in cerebral circulation in mice were similar to those described32. Mice were
Alzheimers dementia33,35,36. There is evidence that endothelium- anesthetized with halothane (maintenance 1%), intubated and artifi-
dependent vascular relaxation is impaired in isolated cerebral ves- cially ventilated with an oxygen-nitrogen mixture. End-tidal CO2, mon-
sels from elderly humans and in cerebral arterioles from aged itored by a CO2 analyzer (Capstar-100, CWI, Ardmore, Pennsylvania),
rats37,38. Although the CBF alterations in Alzheimers dementia could was maintained at 2.62.7% (pCO2 = 3335 mmHg)32. Arterial pO2
be secondary to neuronal degeneration or to morphological alter- was 161 8, and the pH 7.41 0.03. The parietal cortex was exposed,
ations in cerebral blood vessels, these factors are less likely to be and the site was superfused with Ringer solution (37C; pH 7.37.4)32.
involved in the cerebrovascular changes occurring early in the disease CBF was monitored in the exposed cortex with a laser-Doppler probe
positioned stereotaxically. Because CBF measurements by laser-Doppler
when pathological changes have not yet occurred39. Our results sug- can vary depending on the monitoring site45, the probe was placed at
gest that alteration of endothelial regulation of the cerebral circula- the same stereotaxic coordinates in all mice. Acetylcholine (10 M),
tion could be an important factor in the mechanisms of the bradykinin (50 M), A23187 (3 M), U46619 (1 M) or SNAP (100500
cerebrovascular dysfunction in Alzheimers dementia and aging. M) were superfused on the cerebral cortex for 35 min. Agents were
Furthermore, our finding that exogenous SOD rescues the endothe- applied at concentrations that produce 50% of maximal responses (data
lial dysfunction in APP transgenics adds further support to the valid- not shown). Hypercapnia (pCO2 = 5060 mmHg) was produced by
ity of antioxidant treatment in Alzheimers dementia40. introducing CO2 through the circuit of the ventilator. Arterial pressure,
monitored via a femoral arterial catheter, was 85 3 mmHg in
METHODS APP/SOD1 mice, 84 3 in APP+/SOD1 mice, 88 3 in APP+/SOD1+
Transgenic mice. The APP and SOD1 transgenic mice used in these studies mice, 86 3 in APP/SOD1+ mice, 86 3 in Tg6209 APP+ mice and 84
have been described11,12. Tg1130H mice overexpress the human APP695 4 in Tg6209 APP mice. Topical superfusion with vasoactive agents
isoform with the V717I familial Alzheimers dementia mutation, along with or hypercapnia did not affect arterial pressure or blood gases (data not
V721A and M722V changes. Tg6209 express wild-type human APP695. shown). In experiments in which effects of topical SOD were studied,
Both transgenes have a 3-myc epitope tag. A hamster prion protein gene- CBF responses to acetylcholine (10 M) and hypercapnia were tested
derived cosmid vector41 was used to drive expression of APP in these trans- during Ringer superfusion and 30 min after superfusion with bovine
genic lines, both of which were produced on an inbred FVB/N background. liver SOD (1000U /ml; Sigma)46.

160 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Determination of A. Techniques for A measurement have been activate K+ channels. Stroke 28, 22902294 (1997).
described in detail11,23. Brain tissue was homogenized in 1 ml of 70% 17. Mayhan, W. G. Role of activation of bradykinin B2 receptors in disruption of
the blood-brain barrier during acute hypertension. Brain Res. 738, 337341
formic acid and centrifuged at >100,000 g for 1 hour. Samples were dilut- (1996).
ed and analyzed directly using either the BNT77/BA27 or BNT77/BC05 18. Rosenblum, W. I. & Nelson, G. H. Endothelium dependence of dilation of
sandwich ELISA system. A values were obtained by comparing the pial arterioles in mouse brain by calcium ionophore. Stroke 19, 13791382
absorbance obtained from duplicate samples to standard curves of either (1988).
A140 (BNT77/BA27) or A142 (BNT77/BC05) with a standard curve 19. Wang, Q., Pelligrino, D. A., Koenig, H. M. & Albrecht, R. F. The role of
endothelium and nitric oxide in rat arteriolar dilatory responses to CO2 in
(Bachem, Torrance, California). vivo. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 14, 944951 (1994).
20. Faraci, F. M., Williams, J. K., Breese, K. R., Armstrong, M. L. & Heistad, D. D.
Electron microscopy. After transcardial perfusion of anesthetized mice Atherosclerosis potentiates constrictor responses of cerebral and ocular
with 2% paraformaldehyde in phosphate-buffered saline, a sample of pari- blood vessels to thromboxane in monkeys. Stroke 20, 242247 (1989).
21. Mayhan, W. G., Faraci, F. M. & Heistad, D. D. Responses of cerebral arterioles
etal cortex was removed and fixed overnight in 2.5% glutaraldehyde and to adenosine 5-diphosphate, serotonin, and the thromboxane analogue U-
4% paraformaldehyde in 0.1 M sodium cacodylate buffer, 0.05 M sucrose 46619 during chronic hypertension. Hypertension 12, 556561 (1988).
at 4C. Samples were then postfixed, stained en bloc in 1% uranyl acetate, 22. Carlson, G. A. et al. Genetic modification of the phenotypes produced by
dehydrated with ethanol and embedded in Spurrs resin. Thin sections amyloid precursor protein overexpression in transgenic mice. Hum. Mol.
were cut with an RMC MT-7000 ultramicrotome (Research and Manu- Genet. 6, 19511959 (1997).
23. Hsiao, K. et al. Correlative memory deficits, A elevation, and amyloid
facturing Company, Tucson, AZ), stained with 1% uranyl acetate and plaques in transgenic mice. Science 274, 99102 (1996).
Reynolds lead citrate and observed at 80 kV on a Philips CM-10 trans- 24. Stamler, J. S. A radical vascular connection. Nature 380, 108111 (1996).
mission electron microscope. 25. Mohazzab, K. M., Kaminski, P. M. & Wolin, M. S. NADH oxidoreductase is a
major source of superoxide anion in bovine coronary artery endothelium.
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

Data analysis. Data in text and figures are expressed as mean standard Am. J. Physiol. 266, H25682572 (1994).
26. Wang, P. et al. Overexpression of human copper, zinc-superoxide dismutase
error. Two-group comparisons were analyzed by the two-tailed t-test for (SOD1) prevents postischemic injury. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 95,
independent samples. Multiple comparisons were evaluated by the analy- 45564560 (1998).
sis of variance and Tukeys test. Survival data were analyzed by the Fish- 27. Yang, S. T., Mayhan, W. G., Faraci, F. M. & Heistad, D. D. Endothelium-
ers exact test in a 2 2 contingency table. Probability values of less than dependent responses of cerebral blood vessels during chronic hypertension.
Hypertension 17, 612618 (1991).
0.05 were considered statistically significant. 28. Wei, E. P., Kontos, H. A., Christman, C. W., DeWitt, D. S. & Povlishock, J. T.
Superoxide generation and reversal of acetylcholine-induced cerebral
arteriolar dilation after acute hypertension. Circ. Res. 57, 781787 (1985).
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS 29. Mayhan, W. G. Impairment of endothelium-dependent dilatation of basilar
This work was supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health (NS34179, artery during chronic hypertension. Am. J. Physiol. 259, H14551462 (1990).
30. Sobey, C. G., Faraci, F. M., Piegors, D. J. & Heistad, D. D. Effect of short-term
NS37853, NS35806 to C.I.; NS33249 to K.K.H; AG10681 to G.A.C.) and the regression of atherosclerosis on reactivity of carotid and retinal arteries.
Fraternal Order of Eagles (G.A.C.). We thank G. Perry for the oxidative stress data Stroke 27, 927933 (1996).
and Karen MacEwan for editorial assistance. 31. Faraci, F. M. & Heistad, D. D. Regulation of the cerebral circulation: Role of
endothelium and potassium channels. Physiol. Rev. 78, 5397 (1998).
32. Zhang, F., Eckman, C., Younkin, S., Hsiao, K. K. & Iadecola, C. Increased
RECEIVED 24 JUNE; ACCEPTED 23 DECEMBER 1998 susceptibility to ischemic brain damage in transgenic mice overexpressing
the amyloid precursor protein. J. Neurosci. 17, 76557661 (1997).
33. Mentis, M. J. et al. Visual cortical dysfunction in Alzheimers disease
1. Lendon, C. L., Ashall, F. & Goate, A. M. Exploring the etiology of Alzheimer evaluated with a temporally graded stress test during PET. Am. J. Psychiatry
disease using molecular genetics. JAMA 277, 825831 (1997). 153, 3240 (1996).
2. Mattson, M. P. Cellular actions of beta-amyloid precursor protein and its 34. Jagust, W. J., Eberling, J. L., Reed, B. R., Mathis, C. A. & Budinger, T. F.
soluble and fibrillogenic derivatives. Physiol. Rev. 77, 10811132 (1997). Clinical studies of cerebral blood flow in Alzheimers disease. Ann. NY Acad.
3. Smith, M. A. et al. Amyloid-beta deposition in Alzheimer transgenic mice is Sci. 826, 254262 (1997).
associated with oxidative stress. J. Neurochem. 70, 22122215 (1998). 35. Hock, C. et al. Decrease in parietal cerebral hemoglobin oxygenation during
4. Hensley, K. et al. A model for beta-amyloid aggregation and neurotoxicity performance of a verbal fluency task in patients with Alzheimers disease
based on free radical generation by the peptide: relevance to Alzheimer monitored by means of near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS)correlation
disease. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 91, 32703274 (1994). with simultaneous rCBF-PET measurements. Brain Res. 755, 293303
5. Yan, S. D. et al. RAGE and amyloid-beta peptide neurotoxicity in Alzheimers (1997).
disease. Nature 382, 685691 (1996). 36. Warkentin, S. & Passant, U. Functional imaging of the frontal lobes in
6. Behl, C., Davis, J. B., Lesley, R. & Schubert, D. Hydrogen peroxide mediates organic dementia. Regional cerebral blood flow findings in normals, in
amyloid beta protein toxicity. Cell 77, 817827 (1994). patients with frontotemporal dementia and in patients with Alzheimers
7. Thomas, T., Thomas, G., McLendon, C., Sutton, T. & Mullan, M. -amyloid- disease, performing a word fluency test. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 8,
mediated vasoactivity and vascular endothelial damage. Nature 380, 168171 105109 (1997).
(1996). 37. Hatake, K., Kakishita, E., Wakabayashi, I., Sakiyama, N. & Hishida, S. Effect
8. Thomas, T., McLendon, C., Sutton, E. T. & Thomas, G. Cerebrovascular of aging on endothelium-dependent vascular relaxation of isolated human
endothelial dysfunction mediated by beta-amyloid. Neuroreport 8, basilar artery to thrombin and bradykinin. Stroke 21, 10391043 (1990).
13871391 (1997). 38. Mayhan, W. G., Faraci, F. M., Baumbach, G. L. & Heistad, D. D. Effects of
9. Crawford, F., Suo, Z., Fang, C. & Mullan, M. Characteristics of the in vitro aging on responses of cerebral arterioles. Am. J. Physiol. H11381143 (1990).
vasoactivity of beta-amyloid peptides. Exp. Neurol. 150, 159168 (1998). 39. Johnson, K. A. et al. Preclinical prediction of Alzheimers disease using
10. Blanc, E. M., Toborek, M., Mark, R. J., Hennig, B. & Mattson, M. P. Amyloid SPECT. Neurology 50, 15631571 (1998).
beta-peptide induces cell monolayer albumin permeability, impairs glucose 40. Sano, M. et al. A controlled trial of selegiline, alpha-tocopherol, or both as
transport, and induces apoptosis in vascular endothelial cells. J. Neurochem. treatment for Alzheimers disease. The Alzheimers Disease Cooperative
68, 18701881 (1997). Study. N. Engl. J. Med. 336, 12161222 (1997).
11. Hsiao, K. K. et al. Age-related CNS disorder and early death in transgenic 41. Scott, M. R., Kohler, R., Foster, D. & Prusiner, S. B. Chimeric prion protein
FVB/N mice overexpressing Alzheimer amyloid precursor proteins. Neuron expression in cultured cells and transgenic mice. Protein Sci. 1, 986997 (1992).
15, 12031218 (1995). 42. Harris, E. D. Copper as a cofactor and regulator of copper, zinc superoxide
12. Wong, P. C. et al. An adverse property of a familial ALS-linked SOD1 dismutase. J. Nutr. 122, 636640 (1992).
mutation causes motor neuron disease characterized by vacuolar 43. Multhaup, G. et al. The amyloid precursor protein of Alzheimers disease in
degeneration of mitochondria. Neuron 14, 11051116 (1995). the reduction of copper(II) to copper(I). Science 271, 14061409 (1996).
13. Sobey, C. G. & Faraci, F. M. Effects of a novel inhibitor of guanylyl cyclase on 44. Nebot, C. et al. Spectrophotometric assay of superoxide dismutase activity
dilator responses of mouse cerebral arterioles. Stroke 28, 837843 (1997). based on the activated autoxidation of a tetracyclic catechol. Anal. Biochem.
14. Rosenblum, W. I., McDonald, M. & Wormley, B. Calcium ionophore and 214, 442451 (1993).
acetylcholine dilate arterioles on the mouse brain by different mechanisms. 45. Iadecola, C. & Reis, D. J. Continuous monitoring of cerebrocortical blood
Stroke 20, 13911395 (1989). flow during stimulation of the cerebellar fastigial nucleus: A study by laser-
15. Rosenblum, W. I. Endothelial dependent relaxation demonstrated in vivo in doppler flowmetry. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 10, 608617 (1990).
cerebral arterioles. Stroke 17, 494497 (1986). 46. Zhang, F., Slungaard, A., Vercellotti, G. M. & Iadecola, C. Superoxide-
16. Sobey, C. G., Heistad, D. D. & Faraci, F. M. Mechanisms of bradykinin- dependent cerebrovascular effects of homocysteine. Am. J. Physiol. 274,
induced cerebral vasodilatation in rats. Evidence that reactive oxygen species R17041711 (1998).

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 161


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

The construction of movement by


the spinal cord
Matthew C. Tresch1,2, Philippe Saltiel1 and Emilio Bizzi1

1 Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, E25526, Cambridge, Massachusetts 02139, USA
2 Present address: Section of Neurophysiology, Panum Institute, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark
Correspondence should be addressed to E.B. (emilio@ai.mit.edu)

We used a computational analysis to identify the basic elements with which the vertebrate spinal
cord constructs one complex behavior. This analysis extracted a small set of muscle synergies from
the range of muscle activations generated by cutaneous stimulation of the frog hindlimb. The
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

flexible combination of these synergies was able to account for the large number of different motor
patterns produced by different animals. These results therefore demonstrate one strategy used by
the vertebrate nervous system to produce movement in a computationally simple manner.

The ease with which we move in and interact with our environ- evoked responses with a strong activation of vastus externus
ment belies the complexity inherent in even the simplest of these (VE) with a weaker activation of a few other muscles, such as
tasks. Movements we make effortlessly, such as reaching for an biceps femoris (BF), iliopsoas (IP) or adductor magnus (AM).
object or walking over uneven terrain, involve the specification Stimulation of sites on the foot (regions 510) typically evoked
and control of a vast number of variables1, ultimately involving responses with a strong activation of semitendinosus (ST) and
the activation of many thousands of motor units spanning dozens IP and weaker activation of other muscles such as sartorius
of muscles. The question of how the nervous system overcomes (SA), rectus internus (RI), AM, vastus internus (VI) and BF. As
these complexities to produce movement effortlessly and effi- the stimulation site was moved along the front of the calf toward
ciently is central to the study of the neural control of movement. the knee (regions 1114), the contribution of ST gradually
We describe one strategy used by the vertebrate nervous sys- decreased, whereas the contribution of other muscles, partic-
tem to produce a range of behavior in a simple manner. We ularly SA and AM gradually increased (n = 7).
show that the neural circuitry within the frog spinal cord pro- We examined whether these responses were produced from
duces motor responses to hindlimb cutaneous stimulation by the combination of distinct groups of muscles. Such muscle
the combined recruitment of a small number of distinct muscle groups might be observed either in the variations among respons-
groups. Such a muscle group, in which the activation level of a es evoked from different skin regions (as shown in Fig. 1b), or
set of muscles is specified together, has been termed a muscle in the variations among responses evoked from the same skin
synergy previously2,3. Several researchers have proposed that location (as shown in Fig. 2a; each of these five responses were
such spinally organized muscle groupings might underlie the evoked from stimulation of the front of the calf near the knee
production of movement47. The present results provide direct indicated in Fig. 1a). Although the averaged and normalized
support for these proposals, presenting a quantitative assess- responses (Fig. 2b) were generally similar to one another, there
ment of a testable form of this hypothesis. These experiments were clear systematic differences between them. For instance, the
thus provide a novel perspective on this fundamental question fourth and fifth responses were very similar except that there was
of how the vertebrate nervous system produces movement in a a stronger activation of BF and IP in the fifth response. In the
simple and efficient manner. first and second responses, the activation of ST was very similar,
but there was a weaker activation of SA, AM, VI, BF and IP in
RESULTS the second response. These comparisons suggest the existence of
We examined the patterns of muscle activations evoked from a set of at least three groups of muscles that could be controlled
cutaneous stimulation of the hindlimb of spinalized frogs (frogs independently: one with activation of ST, one with activation of
whose spinal cord was transected). We stimulated a number of SA, AM and VI, and one with activation of BF and IP.
different skin locations to evoke a range of motor responses, to We investigated more systematically whether the patterns of
try to understand how this range was produced by the nervous muscle covariations shown in Fig. 2b and the range of muscle
system. An example of the muscle activation patterns (Fig. 1b) activations shown in Fig. 1a could both result from the combi-
evoked from stimulating different regions (Fig. 1a) along the nation of a small number of muscle activation patterns or mus-
rostral and caudal margins of the hindlimb is shown for one cle synergies. An explicit formulation of this hypothesis has been
animal. The activation level of the majority of muscles in each proposed8,9,10, and we evaluated this formulation here. Accord-
frog was significantly dependent on the stimulus location on ing to this hypothesis, any given response should be describable
the leg (p < 0.05, ANOVA). This dependency caused the pro- as the linear combination of a small number of such muscle syn-
duction of a range of different muscle activation patterns. Stim- ergies. Further, both the elements of the synergies and their
ulation of sites on the back of the calf (regions 14) typically weighting within each response should be positive, because we

162 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 1. Muscle activation pat- ST SA RI


terns evoked from cutaneous a b
stimulation of the frog
hindlimb. (a) The divisions of
stimulation sites used to
examine the variation of mus-
cle activation patterns with

Normalized EMG
AM VI SM
stimulus location shown in (b).
Stimulation was applied to
many sites across the skin sur-
face, and the evoked pattern of
muscle activation was mea-
sured for each site. The VE BF IP
hindlimb configuration shown
here corresponds to the con-
figuration used in these exper-
iments. The dot within region
12 indicates the stimulus site
from which the responses
Skin location
shown in Fig. 2a were evoked.
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

(b) The variation of muscle activation with stimulus location for one animal. Stimulation sites on the back and front of the femur did not consis-
tently evoke responses in each frog and so were excluded from analysis. The averaged, normalized activation for each recorded muscle is shown
as a function of stimulus location. For this plot, we also normalized each observed response to be of unit magnitude (taken as the vector norm).
The values here therefore reflect the relative contribution of each muscle to a given response. The numbers on the horizontal axis refer to the
stimulus regions indicated in (a). Error bars represent one standard deviation from the mean activation level at each region. The data shown here
were obtained from a total of 688 responses.

are ultimately considering muscle activations (Galagan, J. et al. each animal. In each of the seven animals examined here, with
Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 23, 512.4, 1997). This specific hypothesis can an average of 392 responses per animal, the algorithm consis-
be formalized in the following model: tently explained a large amount of variance in the patterns of
N muscle activations (mean r2 = .90 .03).
mobs
j =
i=1
cwij i cij, wi 0 (1) The synergies shown in Fig. 2c were generally similar between
animals. The average similarity (calculated as the normalized dot
product) between synergies found in different animals was
where mjobs is the jth observed pattern of muscle activations, cij .74 .20. The similarity of the first three synergies shown in Fig.
is the positive weighting coefficient of the ith muscle synergy for 2c was generally higher than that of the fourth (.91 .09,
the jth response, wi is the ith muscle synergy and N is the number .80 .11, .80 .22, .45 .28, respectively). This similarity, how-
of muscle synergies. We tested how well this positive linear com- ever, was less than the similarity between synergies found on
bination of muscle synergies model explained the patterns of repeated iterations of the algorithm for the same animal (.74 ver-
muscle activations observed experimentally. sus .97), suggesting that there were differences between the syn-
We extracted a set of muscle synergies from the observed mus- ergies found in different animals. Differences in such factors as
cle activations for each animal using a computational analysis. electrode placements, link lengths or possibly the strategies used
This analysis aimed at finding the set of muscle synergies that by different animals could have contributed to these differences.
could best predict the observed responses according to the model We compared the results obtained by this algorithm to the
described above. Briefly, the algorithm began with a set of arbi- results obtained by the k-means algorithm11,12. K-means attempts
trary synergies, wi. The positive weighting coefficients of these to explain each observed response as one of only a small num-
arbitrary synergies, cij, that best predicted each response were then ber of possible responses. In contrast to our algorithm, k-means
found. The muscle synergies were updated by performing a gra- does not allow an observed response to be a combination of more
dient descent on the error between the observed response and the basic patterns; in k-means, each response reflects the recruitment
best-fit, predicted response. This process was then iterated until of a single pattern. To explain 90% of the variance in the observed
the algorithm converged on a particular set of muscle synergies. set of responses, k-means required at least 15 different patterns.
In a typical animal (Fig. 2c), the algorithm extracted both a This demonstrates that cutaneous stimulation of the hindlimb
set of synergies and the weighting coefficients of each synergy did not simply evoke only a few distinct types of muscle patterns.
used to reconstruct the responses evoked from cutaneous stim- Instead, the range of responses evoked from cutaneous stimula-
ulation. The algorithm extracted a synergy with ST, a synergy tion was better explained as the combination of a small number
with SA, AM and VI, a synergy with VE, and a synergy with BF of muscle synergies.
and IP. These synergies corresponded well to the patterns of We also examined how the results of the algorithm explained
covariation observed in Fig. 2b. The derived synergies were very the range of muscle activation patterns evoked from different
robust to differences in initial conditions, with an average nor- regions of the skin, such as those shown for the animal in Fig.
malized dot product of .97 .04 between the synergies found 1b. We show for three animals how the weighting coefficient of
on different iterations of the algorithm. Qualitatively, the each synergy extracted by the algorithm varied with the stimulus
observed (Fig. 2b) and predicted (Fig. 2d) responses were very location on the hindlimb (Fig. 3). These are the weighting coef-
similar to one another. Quantitatively, this similarity between ficients of the synergies used to reconstruct each response
observed and predicted responses was found to be very large for (Fig. 2c). For instance, in the animal shown in Fig. 3a, to recon-

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 163


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

a b Observed responses

Time (ms)
c d
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

Predicted responses

Fig. 2. Example of muscle covariation patterns within evoked responses. (a) Raw EMGs for five responses evoked from stimulation of the same skin
region. The hindlimb was fixed in the configuration shown in Fig. 1a for all responses and the region of the skin indicated in Fig. 1a was stimulated using
a handheld probe (see Methods). (b) Averaged, normalized activation for the muscles recorded in each of the responses shown in (a). Note that each
muscle was normalized to the maximal value observed for that muscle across all responses evoked from any stimulation site in this animal. As a result,
the muscle balances seen in (a) are slightly different than those shown in (b). (c) Responses can be explained as a linear combination of a set of mus-
cle synergies. The synergies obtained from applying the algorithm described in the text to the entire set of responses obtained from this animal are
shown to the left. The weightings of each of these synergies used to reconstruct the responses in (b) are shown to the right. For instance, the first
response was reconstructed as .65 of the first synergy, plus 2.47 of the second synergy, 0 of the third synergy, and 1.09 of the fourth synergy.
(d) Responses resulting from the combination of muscle synergies shown in (c).

struct responses from sites of the back of the leg (regions 14), synergies, with the weighting coefficients of these synergies either
the algorithm used a strong weighting of the synergy with VE being zero or large for most responses. In the animals shown in
(the third synergy in Figs. 2c and 3a). This strong VE activation Fig. 3a and 3b, however, there appeared to be more of a gradual
is consistent with the responses observed from the back of the transition in the recruitment of different synergies, as indicated
leg (Fig. 1b). Similarly, to reconstruct responses from sites on the in the more intermediate weighting coefficients of each synergy.
foot (regions 510), the algorithm used a strong weighting of the This type of gradual transition was seen in most animals, espe-
synergy with ST (the first synergy in Figs. 2c and 3a) along with cially for the transition from the foot to the front of the leg. These
weaker weightings of the synergy with SA, AM and VI (the second observations suggest that each animal has access to a similar set of
synergy in Figs. 2c and 3a). As stimulation sites moved along the muscle synergies when producing responses to cutaneous stim-
front of the leg (regions 1114), the weighting of the synergy with ulation, but that there is a degree of flexibility in the exact pat-
ST gradually decreased, whereas the activation of the SA, AM tern of their use.
and VI synergy and of the VE synergy increased. Activation of Finally, we examined the generality of the synergies derived
the fourth synergy was more variable across regions of the skin from the responses evoked from stimulation of the sites shown
surface. Generally similar patterns of recruitment of synergies in Fig. 1a. We examined how well these synergies were able to
were observed in each animal (compare Fig. 3a, b and c). explain the responses evoked from other regions of the skin sur-
Although Fig. 3 suggests that the use of each type of synergy face; if similar synergies were used to produce responses from
was generally similar in each animal, the exact pattern of recruit- these other regions, we would expect the responses to be
ment of these synergies could differ between animals. In the ani- explained well by these synergies. We evoked responses from
mal shown in Fig. 3c, for instance, there were sharp transitions stimulation of sites on the dorsal surface of the foot, the dorsal
in the weighting of each synergy between different skin regions. surface of the calf, the contralateral ankle, the ipsilateral and con-
These sharp transitions were especially clear for the first three tralateral back and both forelimbs. The synergies obtained from

164 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

a b c method to identify the basic elements


underlying the production of movement
in a systematic and objective manner. The
consistency of the results of this algorithm
with qualitative examination of the data
Weighting coefficient of synergy

helps to validate the approach used here.


This explicit formulation of the hypothe-
sis also allowed it to be falsified: the results
of this algorithm could easily have failed
to consistently explain the responses with
the fidelity observed here. Further, the
particular model we tested, as expressed
in Eq. 1, was relatively simple, assuming
a linear and independent combination of
muscle synergies. This simplicity makes it
all the more surprising that the model was
able to explain these responses so well.
The results and methods of our study
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

Skin location might be used to examine how the spinal


Fig. 3. The contribution of each muscle synergy to responses evoked from different regions of the
cord is used by the rest of the nervous sys-
skin surface for three different animals (a, b, c). The order of the synergies shown here corresponds tem to produce movement. We found that
to the order of the synergies shown in Fig. 2c. The correspondence between synergies was made the set of synergies described here could
using the normalized dot product between them. The numbers on the horizontal axis refer to the explain some, but not all, of the respons-
regions shown in Fig. 1a. The vertical axis shows the average weighting coefficient for each muscle es evoked from other regions of the skin
synergy used to reconstruct the observed responses, such as those shown in Fig. 1b. surface. This result suggested that these
synergies might be utilized by different
pathways in the production of movement.
We might be able to use similar proce-
stimulation of the hindlimb were able to explain over 80% of the dures to examine whether descending systems also use these syn-
variance in the responses evoked from sites on the dorsal surface ergies to produce movement. For instance, vestibular stimulation
of the foot and calf, the ipsilateral back and the contralateral ankle in the frog evokes a low-dimensional set of hindlimb move-
(Fig. 4). Responses from the other skin regions, however, were ments13. It will be interesting to examine the relationship between
not well explained by these same synergies. The observation that the low dimensionality of that set of movements and the com-
the synergies explained some responses poorly also demonstrates bination of a small number of muscle synergies identified here.
that the synergies were not so general as to be able to explain any Further, it will be important to examine whether the model
possible motor response. These results suggest that the synergies described here is capable of explaining the complete time course
used to produce the responses from the hindlimb might also be of motor responses. In many behaviors, there is a great deal of
used in the motor responses evoked through other pathways. temporal precision in muscle activation patterns3, and it will be
interesting to examine whether this precision can be described
DISCUSSION in terms of a small number of synergies. Further experiments
These results demonstrate that the neural circuitry within the
vertebrate spinal cord seems to produce a behavior through the
combination of a small number of muscle synergies. By using a
computational analysis, we show that such a combination of mus-
cle synergies is able to explain the range of muscle activations
evoked by cutaneous stimulation of the frog hindlimb. Such a
strategy of combining a small number of basic motor elements
r2

greatly simplifies the complexity intrinsic to the production of


movement by the nervous system.
These results provide direct support for previous hypotheses
of the production of movement by the spinal cord 46 . For
instance, the nervous system was proposed to produce a range
of movement through the combination of a small number of unit
burst generators organized within the spinal cord5. Each of these
unit bursters was proposed to control the activation of a small
group of synergistic muscles. These units could then be coupled
in many different ways to produce a wide range of behavior. The Stimulation site
hypothesis considered here, that the spinal cord produces move-
Fig. 4. The ability of synergies from sites on the rostral and caudal mar-
ment through the combination of a small number of motor ele- gins of the hindlimb to explain responses evoked from other skin
ments, is similar to this unit burst generator hypothesis. regions. Synergies were derived from the regions shown in Fig. 1a and
The main contribution of this study has been to state this then used to describe the responses evoked from the other skin regions
hypothesis in an explicit form, allowing it to be examined quan- indicated on the horizontal axis. The vertical axis shows the r2 values for
titatively. The computational analysis described here provided a the fits for each of these data sets.

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 165


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

will be required to define the extent and limits of the generality of using these different transformations were very similar. We only pre-
the synergies described here to other behaviors, including those sent the results of the algorithm using the exponential transformation.
produced by descending systems. Given a certain set of synergies, the set of best-fit weighting coeffi-
Sherrington first proposed that the responses to cutaneous cients, cij, was found using the non-negative least-squares algorithm20
stimulation might also be used in other classes of movement, supplied by Matlab. This algorithm finds the set of nonnegative coeffi-
cients that minimizes the error between the predicted and observed
such as locomotion4. Although the movements produced by the
responses, given a particular set of synergies. This procedure is similar
spinal cord in response to cutaneous stimulation are often con- to standard regression techniques except that the weighting coefficients
sidered to be simple and stereotyped, it is well established that are constrained to be positive.
these responses can be precisely tuned depending on the site of Once the weighting coefficients were found, the error between the
stimulation1417. This flexibility might be used in the movements predicted and observed responses was used to update the synergies. The
produced by descending systems18. Our study might provide a error was minimized using gradient descent, in which the synergies are
novel perspective from which to test this hypothesis. incrementally changed to reduce the prediction error. The change in the
synergies was calculated as Eq. 2:
METHODS
Data acquisition. All procedures were approved by the Committee on wi = (mobs pre
j m j ) cijg(wi) (2)
Animal Care at M.I.T. Seven adult frogs (Rana catesbiana) were spinal-
ized at the level of the obex under tricaine anesthesia. Nine hindlimb where g is the first derivative of the transformation used in Eq. 1 and
muscles were implanted with bipolar EMG electrodes (see ref. 9 for determines the step size of the gradient descent. Because the process of min-
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

details): semitendinosus (ST), sartorius (SA), rectus internus (RI), imizing the error using gradient descent is generally very erratic, the step
adductor magnus (AM), vastus internus (VI), semimembranosus size of the descent is usually set to be low; we used a value of = .005. To fur-
(SM), vastus externus (VE), biceps femoris (BF) and iliopsoas (IP). In ther ensure smoothness of the error minimization, we used a momentum
one animal, we recorded from rectus anterior in the place of IP. coefficient, in which the change in synergy at iteration t is equal to the
Crosstalk from electrical pickup between adjacent muscles was elimi- change in the synergy calculated by Eq. 2 plus a fraction (we used .99) of
nated by suturing small pieces of insulation to the fascia in the major- the change in weight at iteration t 1. This momentum coefficient serves as
ity of animals. The effectiveness of this insulation was confirmed by a type of running average of the synergy changes. These procedures are
observing no EMG signals in muscles with their nerve supply cut. The standard in gradient descent techniques11,12. After updating the synergies,
hindlimb was fixed in the horizontal plane at the level of the hip by they were normalized so that the vector norm of each synergy was equal to
means of bone screws attached at the ankle and the metatarsus. This one. The best-fit weighting coefficients were then again found using these
restraint prevented all movement of the hindlimb. The foot was fixed new synergies, the synergies updated, and the process repeated.
at a right angle relative to the calf (see Fig. 1a). The hindlimb was We applied this algorithm to 90% of the data for each animal, chosen
mechanically stimulated by scratching restricted sites on the skin sur- randomly from the responses evoked from the sites in Fig. 1a. After each
face (1 mm2 tip, .01 to .1 N force, 14 Hz, over a range of 13 mm). iteration, we assessed the ability of the synergies found by the algorithm
Stimulation strength was regulated so as to evoke only threshold to predict the remaining 10% of responses. When the amount of vari-
responses. EMG signals were filtered and amplified (25 k) before being ance explained
j
by the derived synergies in this set of test data decreased
digitally sampled (1 kHz) for offline analysis. Response onsets and off- for 20 consecutive iterations, we considered the algorithm to have con-
sets were identified offline using interactive software written in Matlab, verged. This procedure reduces overfitting by the algorithm. We repeat-
and the activation level of each muscle was averaged within each ed the algorithm on 10 different sets of 90% of the data for each animal
response. The activation of each muscle was then normalized to the with different initial synergies for each repetition, to examine effects of
maximal value observed for that muscle in any response. Weak different initial conditions on the results of the algorithm. Qualitative
responses with a magnitude (taken as the vector norm) less than .3 analyses suggested that the algorithm required around 50100 respons-
were excluded from further analysis because EMG noise often obscured es to produce consistent results. The algorithm was applied to the entire
the balance of muscle activations in these weaker responses. data sets from each animal, ranging between 177 and 688 responses.
We assessed the repeatability of the stimulation procedures used here We calculated the similarity between two synergies found by the algo-
by examining the variability in the responses evoked from repeated stim- rithm as the normalized dot product between them. This value ranges
ulation of a site on the skin. The standard deviation in the most activat- between zero and one, with one representing identical synergies. To estab-
ed muscle in a response was .27 across all animals. This variability was lish correspondences between two different sets of synergies, we took all
comparable to the variability in the responses evoked from repeated elec- possible dot products between the two sets of synergies. A synergy from
trical stimulation of the same site in spinal cord (standard deviation, one set was matched to the synergy in the other set with which it had its
0.21, data not shown). Such electrical stimulation would be expected to largest dot product. The process of correspondence was also inspected
activate the same neural substrate on each application of stimulation and visually in case of ambiguities.
should therefore give a lower bound of the variability in the motor We also applied the k-means algorithm to the set of evoked respons-
responses evoked in this preparation. Such variability in the responses es11,12. This algorithm is similar to the algorithm described above except
evoked in this preparation has been observed previously19. that the cij are equal to zero for all except one synergy.
The algorithm described above is generally similar to factor analytic
Algorithm to extract muscle synergies. A set of synergies, wi, consists of models21, with the difference that both the factor loading and factor
coefficients chosen randomly between 0 and 1 for each muscle. We then scores are constrained to be positive. It is also similar to that described22
approximated each observed response as a positive linear combination except that we are not explicitly imposing a particular prior distribution
of these synergies, described by Eq. 1. A slightly different form of Eq. 1 in for the weighting coefficients. We therefore implicitly impose a uniform
the actual fitting procedure was used to impose the restriction of the syn- distribution.
ergies to positive numbers. We describe the results of the algorithm using four synergies to fit the
N data for the following reasons. First, we did a principal components
j m j = cijg(wi)
mobs pre
(1) analysis on this data and found that the smallest number of components
i=1
necessary to explain at least 90% of the variance in each animal was four.
mjobs is the jth observed pattern of muscle activations, mjpre is the jth Similarly, four synergies were needed for the algorithm used here to
predicted pattern of muscle activations and g(x) is a positive valued explain at least 90% of the variance. Adding an additional synergy to the
function of x. We tried several different forms of g(x), including one in algorithm explained only an extra 3% of the variance. Finally, applying
which negative values of x were arbitrarily set to 0, the simple quadratic the algorithm with four synergies corresponded well to a visual inspec-
g(x) = x2 and an exponential g(x) = ex. The results of the algorithm tion of the data and gave consistent results between different animals.

166 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS primitives in vertebrate motor control. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 91,
We thank Sandro Mussa-Ivaldi and Andrea dAvella for reading versions of this 75347538 (1994).
11. Bishop, C. M. Neural Networks for Pattern Recognition (Oxford Univ. Press,
manuscript and Simon Giszter, Peter Dayan, Kuno Wyler and James Galagan Oxford, 1995).
for suggestions. M.C.T. was supported by a HHMI predoctoral fellowship. This 12. Hertz, J., Krogh, A. & Palmer R. G. Introduction to the Theory of Neural
research was supported by NIH NS09343 and ONR N00014-95-I0445 to E.B. Computation (Addison-Wesley, Reading, MA 1991).
13. dAvella, A. & Bizzi, E. Low dimensionality of supraspinally induced force
fields. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 95, 77117714 (1998).
RECEIVED 30 SEPTEMBER; ACCEPTED 30 NOVEMBER 1998 14. Fleshman, J. W., Lev-Tov, A. & Burke, R. E. Peripheral and central control of
flexor digitorum longus and flexor hallucis longus motoneurons: the
synaptic basis of functional diversity. Exp. Brain Res. 54, 133149 (1984).
1. Bernstein, N. The Coordination and Regulation of Movements (Pergamon 15. Pratt, C. A., Chanaud, C. M. & Loeb, G. E. Functionally complex muscles of
Press, New York, 1967). the cat hindlimb. IV. Intramuscular distribution of movement command
2. Lee, W. A. Neuromotor synergies as a basis for coordinated intentional signals and cutaneous reflexes in broad, bifunctional thigh muscles. Exp.
action. J. Motor Behav. 16, 135170 (1984). Brain Res. 85, 281299 (1991).
3. Macpherson, J. M. in Motor Control: Concepts and Issues (eds. Humphrey, D. 16. Schouenborg, J. & Kalliomaki, J. Functional organization of the nociceptive
R. & Freund, H.-J) 3347 (Wiley, Chichester, 1991). withdrawal reflexes I. Activation of hindlimb muscles in the rat. Exp. Brain
4. Sherrington, C. S. Flexion-reflex of the limb, crossed extension-reflex and Res. 83, 6778 (1990).
reflex stepping and standing. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 40, 28121 (1910). 17. Schouenborg, J. & Weng, H.-R. Sensorimotor transformations in a spinal
5. Grillner, S. in Handbook of Physiology, sec. 1, vol. 2 (ed. Brooks, V. B.) motor system. Exp. Brain Res. 100, 170174 (1994).
11791236 (American Physiological Society, Bethesda, MD, 1981). 18. Schouenborg, J., Weng, H.-R. & Holmberg, H. Modular organization of
6. Stein, P. S. G. & Smith J. L. in Neurons, Networks, and Motor Behavior (eds. spinal nociceptive reflexes: a new hypothesis. News Physiol. Sci. 9, 261265
Stein P. S. G., Grillner, S., Selverston A. I. & Stuart D. G.) 6173 (MIT Press, (1994).
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

Cambridge, MA, 1997). 19. Berkinblitt, M. B., Feldman, A. G. & Fukson, O. I. Adaptability of innate
7. Jacobs, R. & Macpherson, J. M. Two functional muscle groupings during motor patterns and motor control mechanisms. Behav. Brain Sci. 9, 585638
postural equilibrium tasks in standing cats. J. Neurophysiol. 76, 24022411 (1986).
(1996). 20. Lawson, C. L. & Hanson, R. J. Solving Least Squares Problems. (Prentice-Hall,
8. Bizzi, E., Mussa-Ivaldi, F A. & Giszter S. F. Computations underlying the Englewood Cliffs, NJ, 1974).
production of movement: a biological perspective. Science 253, 287291 21. Harman, H. H. Modern Factor Analysis (Univ. of Chicago Press, Chicago,
(1991). 1976).
9. Giszter, S. F., Mussa-Ivaldi, F. A. & Bizzi, E. Convergent force fields organized 22. Olshausen, B. A. & Field, D. J. Emergence of simple-cell receptive field
in the frog spinal cord. J. Neurosci. 13, 467491 (1993). properties by learning a sparse code for natural images. Nature 381, 607609
10. Mussa-Ivaldi, F. A., Giszter, S. F., & Bizzi, E. Linear combinations of (1996).

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 167


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Self-sustained rhythmic activity in the


thalamic reticular nucleus mediated by
depolarizing GABAA receptor potentials
M. Bazhenov1, I. Timofeev2, M. Steriade2 and T.J. Sejnowski1,3

1 Howard Hughes Medical Institute, The Salk Institute, Computational Neurobiology Laboratory, 10010 North Torrey Pines Road,
La Jolla, California 92037, USA
2 Laboratory of Neurophysiology, School of Medicine, Laval University, Quebec G1K 7P4, Canada
3 Department of Biology, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, California 92093, USA
Correspondence should be addressed to M.B. (bazhenov@salk.edu)
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

Intracellular recordings from reticular thalamic (RE) neurons in vivo revealed inhibitory postsynaptic
potentials (IPSPs) between RE cells that reversed and became depolarizing at the hyperpolarized
membrane potentials that occur during sleep. These excitatory IPSPs can directly trigger low-thresh-
old spikes (LTSs). The oscillatory mechanisms underlying IPSP-triggered LTSs crowned by spike
bursts were investigated in models of isolated RE networks. In a one-dimensional network model,
external stimulation evoked waves of excitation propagating at a constant velocity of 25150 cells
per second. In a large-scale, two-dimensional model of the reticular nucleus, the network showed
transient or self-sustained oscillations controlled by the maximum conductance of the low-threshold
calcium current and the membrane potential. This model predicts that the isolated reticular nucleus
could initiate sequences of spindle oscillations in thalamocortical networks in vivo.

Sleep spindle oscillations consist of waxing-and-waning field poten- cells were hyperpolarized below the chloride reversal potential,
tials of 7 to 14 Hz, which last 1 to 3 seconds and recur every 5 to 15 GABAA-receptor-mediated synaptic excitation led to a low-thresh-
seconds. These oscillations are generated in the thalamus by inter- old calcium action potential crowned by sodium spikes. In a com-
action between thalamic reticular (RE) and thalamocortical (TC) puter model of a hyperpolarized RE network, this mechanism
cells14. The waxing phase of spindle oscillations is associated with produced propagating patterns of spike-burst activity, which could
recruitment of neurons from dorsal thalamic and RE nuclei5, where- be transformed into self-sustained oscillations.
as the waning phase results from calcium-induced cAMP upregu-
lation of hyperpolarization-activated cation current, Ih, in TC cells68. RESULTS
Extracellular neuron and field-potential recordings from the isolat- Low-threshold spike bursts in RE neurons in vivo
ed RE nucleus in vivo show that the deafferented RE nucleus can Intracellular recordings from RE cells (n = 97) in cats were charac-
itself generate 714 Hz oscillations9, and so the reticular nucleus terized by prolonged spike bursts with accelerando-decelerando pat-
may be involved in initiating sequences of spindle oscillations in the terns, during either spontaneous oscillatory activity or activity
intact RETC network. evoked by dorsal thalamic stimulation (Figs. 13). Such prolonged
The GABAergic neurons of the RE nucleus are usually thought spike bursts, consisting of progressively decreasing followed by
to interact by reciprocal inhibition. Computational models of increasing interspike intervals, are in contrast to the short spike
inhibitory RE networks produce different types of population bursts of thalamocortical neurons15. Some RE neurons were also
activity, including full and partial spatial clustering10 and tempo- morphologically identified by intracellular staining with Neurobiotin
ral synchronization alternating between periods of coherent oscil- (data not shown). The resting membrane potential (Vm) of RE neu-
latory activity and periods of almost chaotic behavior11. The rons was 62.5 2.9 mV (mean standard error), which is rela-
oscillations in these models occur because the synaptic inhibition tively depolarized compared to results reported with patch pipettes13.
causes burst discharges when the reversal potential for inhibito- After Vm was determined, most of the intracellular recordings were
ry synapses is sufficiently negative to remove inactivation of the done with slight (0.20.5 nA) DC hyperpolarization.
low-threshold calcium current. During the state of resting sleep with long-range synchroniza-
For the reciprocal GABAA receptor synapses between RE cells, tion of electrical activity, neocortical and thalamic neurons display
the chloride reversal potential is about 71 mV, which is depolar- three major rhythms: slow oscillations (<1 Hz), delta waves (14
ized compared to the reversal potential in TC cells12. Thus, at a rest- Hz) and spindles (715 Hz). In animals under ketamine-xylazine
ing membrane potential of about 78 mV (ref. 13), the anesthesia, RE neurons are hyperpolarized during the depth-positive
GABAA-receptor-mediated IPSPs in an RE cell reverse and could cortical electroencephalogram (EEG) component of the slow oscil-
trigger a burst of sodium spikes14. Here we used in-vivo recordings lation and depolarized during the depth-negative EEG waves16. This
and computational models of RE cells to investigate cellular dynam- cortically generated spontaneous slow oscillation was used to test
ics at different membrane potentials. We found, in vivo, that when RE the responsiveness of RE neurons at different membrane voltages

168 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 1. Reversed IPSP in an RE neuron in vivo directly triggers a


low-threshold spike (LTS). Intact thalamocortical connections.
Left column, responses of an RE neuron to four stimuli applied to
the thalamic VL nucleus (arrowhead). At relatively depolarized Vm
(61 mV), low-intensity thalamic stimuli evoked an IPSP-rebound
sequence. When the stimulus (same parameters as above)
occurred during the hyperpolarizing phase of the slow oscillation,
the IPSP reversed and (at 78 mV) directly triggered an LTS
crowned by spike burst. The latency of LTS was in the range of
4050 ms. The early part of two responses (at 61 mV and at 78
mV) are enlarged at right. Bottom right, the amplitude of the
postsynaptic response, measured 75 ms after the stimulus, is plot-
ted against Vm. The IPSP depended linearly on Vm from depolar-
ized levels to the reversal potential (68 mV); thereafter, the
depolarizing IPSP directly activated the LTS, which added a depo-
larizing deflection to the linear function (solid and dotted lines).

because it affects both somatic and dendritic compartments


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

of RE neurons, unlike the current pulses that are usually


applied with somatic impalement. This is particularly
important for RE neurons because their dendritic voltage is
poorly controlled from the soma17, so current pulses prob-
ably do not affect remote parts of the dendritic tree.
Electrical stimulation applied to the dorsal thalamus elic-

Amplitude of IPSPs (mV)


its an EPSP crowned by a spike burst in RE neurons, fol-
lowed by an IPSP and a period of disfacilitation: a
long-lasting absence of synaptic activity18. Here we stimu-
lated the thalamic VL nucleus and confirmed these findings.
In 10 out of 32 neurons tested, diminishing the stimulus
intensity resulted in long-latency (32.2 3.5 ms) hyperpo-
larizing IPSPs when stimuli were applied at a relatively depo-
larized Vm (Fig. 1). Because RE neurons are GABAergic and
reciprocally connected19, this primary IPSP probably orig-
inated from neighboring RE neurons that were excited by
axons of thalamocortical and/or corticothalamic cells. How-
ever, during the hyperpolarizing phase of the slow oscilla-
tion, VL stimuli with the same parameters resulted in reversed IPSPs Similar burst patterns were observed in intracellularly recorded
crowned by high-frequency spike bursts (Fig. 1). RE neurons. At rest or under slight DC hyperpolarizing currents,
Stimuli were delivered to VL every five seconds. Some of them RE neurons showed periodic (0.30.6 Hz) burst activity (Fig. 3a).
occurred at relatively depolarized Vm levels, whereas others were Usually, this bursting activity was abolished by release of the neu-
delivered during the hyperpolarizing phase of the slow oscillation, ron from DC hyperpolarizing current or by slight DC depolariza-
associated with different degrees of disfacilitation. Stimuli applied tion. Under these conditions, RE neurons showed hyperpolarizing
during a relatively depolarized Vm elicited an inhibitory IPSP fol- potentials resembling IPSPs. The frequency of the hyperpolarizing
lowed by a rebound spike burst (Fig. 1). As stimuli were delivered potentials was almost identical to the frequency of bursts recorded
at Vm levels closer to the reversal potential for chloride ions, hyper- under DC hyperpolarizing current (Fig. 3). This observation, and
polarizing responses disappeared and were replaced by depolariz- the finding that the IPSPs of RE neurons can only slightly shunt the
ing responses. At more hyperpolarized Vm levels, these reversed low-threshold spikes (LTSs)21, suggest that, at the relatively hyper-
IPSPs triggered a full-blown, high-frequency spike burst. polarized Vm levels that occur during resting sleep with EEG syn-
We studied the activity patterns of RE neurons in ipsilaterally chronization, the reversed IPSPs cause burst firing in RE neurons,
decorticated cats. Decortication removes the inputs generated by which may maintain persistent activity within the RE nucleus and
the cortical slow oscillation, leaving intact the thalamically generat- initiate spindle activity.
ed spindle and delta oscillations20. RE neurons showed two main
forms of spontaneous oscillatory activity. The first pattern was a Computer models of activity patterns in the RE network
spindle oscillation characterized by rhythmically recurring (712 The ability of a reversed IPSP to trigger a low-threshold calcium
Hz) high-frequency spike bursts on a depolarizing envelope19. The spike in RE cells was investigated in computer models of an isolat-
second pattern was the occurrence of periodic spike bursts, mainly ed RE neuron. We first simulated an RE cell with a GABAA recep-
at frequencies around 23 Hz (Fig. 2). In multi-site, multi-unit tor IPSP at different levels of membrane potential (Fig. 4). At the
recordings, these bursts did not occur simultaneously, but followed relatively depolarized membrane potential of 65 mV, the low-
each other, even when spindle oscillations occurred almost simul- threshold calcium current in the RE cell was inactivated. Hyper-
taneously. For example, in a typical recording (Fig. 2), a perispike polarization evoked by the GABA A IPSP led to the partial
histogram triggered by the first action potential in the burst of one deinactivation of the low-threshold calcium current and a weak
cell shows that most spikes in a pool of neurons recorded through rebound LTS crowned by a single sodium spike. At a more nega-
another electrode occurred either before or after these bursts. tive level of membrane potential, the low-threshold calcium cur-

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 169


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 2. Spontaneous activity of RE neurons in vivo is char-


acterized by spindle activity interspersed with low-fre-
quency ( 2 Hz) spike bursts. Ketamine-xylazine
anesthesia. Cortical activity was depressed by application
of potassium acetate. Two traces show simultaneous
recording of multi-neuron activity from RE nucleus (elec-
trodes separated by 1 mm). Spindles occurred virtually
simultaneously. Expanded fragments show spindles (top
left arrowhead) and lower-frequency spike bursts of RE
neurons during interspindle lull (bottom right arrow-
head). The histogram (10-ms bins) triggered by the first
spike in a burst in the top recording depicts the temporal
relationship between the two multi-neuron traces (filled
bars for upper trace and open bars for lower trace). The
burst firing in one neuron preferentially occurs between
n = 10

the bursts of the other neuron (peaks of filled histogram


cut off between 0 and 100 ms).
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

ms

rent in the RE cell was partially deinactivated at rest; however, near second stimulus. This is different from an EPSP-evoked LTS, which
the chloride reversal potential, the IPSP was diminished, and, at can sustain burst discharges at a frequency of 10 Hz during spin-
76 mV, the chloride IPSP in the RE neuron was reversed. Activa- dle oscillations5 or augmenting responses22.
tion of the GABAA receptor synaptic current at the hyperpolarized Dynamical properties of the isolated RE network hyperpolar-
level led to depolarization, followed by an LTS and a burst of sodi- ized below the chloride reversal potential were investigated in a
um spikes in the RE cell. During burst discharge in the RE cell, one-dimensional network model with 100 RE cells (see Methods).
depolarization inactivated the low-threshold calcium current, so An external AMPA receptor stimulus applied to the RE cell locat-
the cell failed to produce a second burst of spikes after a 200-ms ed at the boundary of the network triggered a localized pattern of
delay. However, a third stimulus delivered after a 400-ms delay was spike-burst activity that propagated with constant velocity through
again followed by a burst of sodium spikes. The IPSP-evoked depo- the RE network (Fig. 5a). The mechanism of propagation depend-
larization was relatively weak, and strong deinactivation of low- ed on the level of the membrane potential in RE cells. At mem-
threshold calcium current was required to produce an LTS followed brane potentials of about 75 mV, the low-threshold calcium
by a burst of sodium spikes. Furthermore, immediately after the current in RE cells was deinactivated. Bursts of spikes in presy-
burst of spikes, the cell was hyperpolarized as much as 23 mV, and naptic RE cells led to reversed GABAA receptor IPSPs, followed by
additional depolarization was required to elicit an LTS. This hyper- a low-threshold calcium spike and a burst of sodium spikes in
polarization resulted from inactivation of the low-threshold calci- neighboring RE cells (Fig. 5b). The temporal inactivation of the
um current, which was slightly activated at membrane potentials low-threshold calcium current in an RE cell after a burst discharge
around 76 mV and depolarized the cell about 2 mV before the prevented oscillations from developing in the cell. All RE cells were
burst. Consequently, a relatively long time delay (about 350 ms) grouped into clusters that fired simultaneously, whose size depend-
between shocks was required to obtain a burst of spikes after the ed on the radius of synaptic interconnections (N). Only some of

a b

Fig. 3. Spontaneous activity of RE neurons. Different cells (a and b) are from two decorticated cats. At the resting membrane potential (60 mV), the
RE neurons showed periodic hyperpolarizing potentials (marked by asterisks). Under slight DC hyperpolarization (76 mV in a and 68 mV in b), the
hyperpolarizing potentials were not longer visible, but high-frequency spike bursts occurred with approximately the same period icity. Fragments
expanded at right are marked by large asterisks.

170 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 4. Response of a model RE cell stimu-


network. Only transient waves were observed in networks
lated by a 5-Hz train of GABAA receptor
IPSPs at different levels of membrane smaller than about 25 25 cells.
potential. At a resting membrane potential A similar transformation of transient oscillations into self-
of 65 mV, the low-threshold calcium cur- sustained oscillations in a two-dimensional RE network could
rent was inactivated, and the GABAA be produced by depolarizing the RE cells. In typical time
receptor IPSP was followed by a weak traces of RE cells from a network with 33 33 RE cells at dif-
rebound LTS with a single spike. For Vm = ferent levels of membrane potential produced by DC current
76 mV, the IPSP was reversed and evoked injection (Fig. 8), only transient oscillations in the form of
a burst of sodium spikes. Inactivation of cylindrical waves, similar to those in Fig. 7a, were observed
the low-threshold calcium current during
at the resting membrane potential. A weak depolarization
the first burst discharge reduced the
response to the second shock.
transformed them into an almost periodic self-sustained
oscillation (spiral wave) with a frequency around 2.5 Hz (Fig.
8a). Further depolarization increased the population fre-
quency up to about 10 Hz (Fig. 8b) and reduced the ampli-
tude of the average activity (compare the amplitude scales in
the RE cells from each cluster displayed sodium spikes, whereas Fig. 8a and b). The decrease reflected a reduction from four spikes
others showed reversed IPSPs and partial calcium spikes but no per burst discharge in the RE cells at more hyperpolarized levels of
sodium spikes (see cells 6 and 8 in Fig. 5). membrane potential into weaker and mostly single-spike discharges
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

IPSP-based waves of excitation propagated through the network when the RE cells were only slightly hyperpolarized below the chlo-
with a constant velocity of 25150 cells per second (Fig. 6). The size ride reversal potential. Population oscillations with a frequency
of the clusters of RE cells that were synchronously activated on each around 10 Hz were produced by RE cells oscillating at around 5 Hz,
cycle of oscillation increased with N, which increased the propaga- which occurred by delayed activation of the low-threshold calcium
tion speed. Increasing the GABAA receptor conductances (gGABAA) spikes in RE cells placed just below the chloride reversal potential.
between the RE cells reduced the time delay between IPSP onset and These neurons showed long (13 s) epochs of subthreshold oscilla-
the appearance of sodium spikes, which also raised the speed when tions interrupted by the sequences of spiking-bursting activity.
gGABAA was less than 0.2 S. For gGABAA > 0.2 S, the speed of prop-
agation decreased for N < 7 or displayed slow oscillations around DISCUSSION
an average value, which depended on the radius of interconnections, Our intracellular recordings from RE neurons at resting and hyper-
for N > 7. Stronger GABAA receptor inhibition between RE cells polarized membrane potentials in vivo showed that reversed IPSPs
reduced the length of the burst discharges23, which limited an between RE cells can directly trigger an LTS. Models of thalamic net-
increase in the speed. Propagating patterns did not occur for gGABAA works examined here demonstrate that this reversal may have impor-
< 0.04 S or for gGABAA > gGABA
lim lim
A, where the limiting gGABAA
increased with increasing N (see Fig. 6). Wave activity was
also absent for all networks with N < 2.
In a one-dimensional network with flow boundary a
conditions (see Methods), the localized patterns termi-
RE cell number

nated at the boundaries. To check the effect of network


geometry, we simulated a two-dimensional network of
RE cells hyperpolarized below the chloride reversal poten-
tial. The spatiotemporal patterns in a two-dimensional
network depended on the maximal conductance for the
low-threshold calcium current in RE cells. For gCa below
Time (ms)
some critical value (gCalim
2.3 mS/cm2 for a network of 50
50 cells with N = 4), a single stimulus applied at the
boundary led to a cylindrical wave of spike-burst activity b
that traveled through the network and disappeared at the
boundary (Fig. 7a). The wavefront consisted of almost
synchronously firing RE cells. Increase of the maximal
lim
conductance for IT above gCa led to self-sustained oscil-
lations; the wave of excitation evoked by the initial stim-
ulus transformed into a rotating spiral wave (Fig. 7b). The
location of the spiral center was random and depended
on the initial stimulation as well as the variability in the
parameters of the cells in the network. The individual cells
from the network showed bursts of sodium spikes every
250300 ms corresponding to the fronts of the spiral wave.
Further increase of the maximal conductance for the low-
Fig. 5. Localized pattern of activity propagating through the model of an isolated RE net-
threshold calcium current transformed the single spiral
work. (a) RE cell #1 was stimulated at t = 0, which triggered a localized pattern that
wave into nonregular burst discharges of the RE cells. The traveled with constant velocity through the RE network. (b) Six neighbor RE cells from
critical values of the maximal conductance for IT at tran- the network. A burst of spikes in one RE cell led to a depolarizing GABA IPSP followed
A
sitions between the transient mode (single cylindrical by an LTS and a burst of sodium spikes in the neighboring RE cells. Temporal inactivation
wave), the periodic oscillations (spiral wave) and the non- of the low-threshold calcium current in RE cell after the burst discharge prevented the
regular oscillations decreased with increasing size of the oscillations from developing. Triangles mark t = 0, the time of stimulation.

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 171


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

a b

RE cell #
RE cell #
Velocity (cells/ms)

Time (ms) Time (ms)


Fig. 6. Velocity of the traveling wave as a function of GABAA receptor coupling
strength and radius of RERE connections in a one-dimensional RE network
model. (a) Velocity of propagation as a function of GABAA conductance. N is the
radii of connections. For N > 6 and gGABAA > 0.2 S, the speed of propagation
showed only a weak dependence on GABAA receptor coupling and radius of
RERE connections. The solid curves are the nonlinear fits of the calculated data
GABAA (S)
(indicated by different symbols). (b) Localized activity patterns propagating with
different speed: gGABAA = 0.1 S, N = 2 (left) and gGABAA = 0.4 S, N = 7 (right).

tant consequences for the collective dynamics of large populations of by spiral waves of electrical excitation in heart cells32. However, local
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

RE cells. A single stimulus applied to an isolated one-dimensional electrical coupling is required to maintain spiral waves in this sys-
RE network hyperpolarized below the chloride reversal potential tem, which leads to the same mechanism of propagation as in the
triggered an isolated wave of spike-burst activity that traveled reaction-diffusion systems. In our RE network model, the mecha-
through the RE network and either terminated at the boundaries or nisms promoting pattern formation did not depend on having local
circulated indefinitely when periodic boundary conditions were interconnections between cells. The burst activity in the presynaptic
used. The mechanism of propagation was based on GABAA-recep- RE neurons initiated burst discharges in the target cells through the
tor-mediated depolarization, followed by a calcium spike in the post- GABAA-receptor-mediated depolarization when the neurons were
synaptic RE cells. Similar localized patterns have been described in below the chloride reversal potential. At the same time, these synaps-
network models of cortical excitatory cells24,25 and simplified es had an inhibitory influence among synchronously firing RE neu-
inhibitory cells26; however, in the latter study, wave propagation was rons. Such dual excitatory/inhibitory synaptic interconnections may
caused by asymmetric inhibition between neurons and
rebound bursts, but not by direct triggering of the LTS
by IPSPs. Activity patterns traveling through networks
a
of excitatory and inhibitory cells have been investigat-
ed in computational models of hippocampal slices27,28.
In these models, the propagation from one cell to
another was mediated mostly by AMPA receptors
between pyramidal neurons, which also synchronized
the burst discharges elicited by NMDA-receptor-medi-
ated synaptic currents.
A one-dimensional network of RE cells serves as a
model for an RE slice preparation cut transversely to
the thin, two-dimensional sheet of RE neurons19. We
also tested a two-dimensional RE network model
hyperpolarized below the chloride reversal potential.
In this model, the transient wave patterns were trans- b
formed into persistent oscillations in the form of the
spiral waves if the maximal conductance for the IT cur-
rent in RE cells exceeded a critical value, which depend-
ed on the size of the network and the level of membrane
potential in the RE cells. The spiral pattern that
appeared when gCa for IT reached a bifurcation is sim-
ilar to those in general reaction-diffusion systems,
which may display spiral patterns near the point of
bifurcation from spatially homogeneous solutions29. If
the size of the system is large enough, coexisting spirals
occur in the large-scale, two-dimensional, reaction-dif-
fusion model, and an increase of the control parameter
in this system leads to defect-media turbulence in the
form of spontaneously appearing and annihilating spi- Fig. 7. Localized patterns of activity in a two-dimensional network model of 50 50 RE
cells (x- and y-axes) with flow boundary conditions. (a) The maximal conductance for
ral waves30,31. This suggests that in large-scale, two- low-threshold calcium current in RE cells was g = 2.2 mS/cm2. Initial stimulation of the
T
dimensional networks of RE cells, regimes similar to RE cell #(1,1; left upper corner) at t = 0 led a cylindrical wave traveling through the RE
defect-media turbulence may be observed. One of the network with a constant velocity. (b) gT = 2.45 mS/cm2. Increasing the maximal conduc-
first known examples of spiral-wave activity in a bio- tance for low-threshold calcium current led to self-sustained patterns of activity in the
logical system is cardiac arrythmias, which are initiated form of a rotating one-arm spiral wave.

172 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 8. The influence of the membrane poten-


a

Amplitude (mV)
tial on the frequency of oscillations in a two- 50 mV
dimensional network model with 33 33 RE
cells. Left, the membrane potentials of two RE
cells and the average activity of 112 RE cells
from the center of the network. Right, the
Fourier spectrum of the average activity. (a) A
relatively hyperpolarized RE network showed 5 mV Frequency (Hz)
oscillations at around 2.5 Hz. (b) After depo-
larization (just below the chloride reversal
potential), the frequency of the population
oscillations shifted to around 9 Hz. In the
b

Amplitude (mV)
Fourier spectrum, the smaller peak at about 50 mV
4.5 Hz corresponded to the oscillation fre-
quencies of the individual RE cells.

1 mV Frequency (Hz)
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

explain the formation of localized activity patterns without highly spindle lull deinactivates the low-threshold calcium current, so the
synchronous oscillatory states in a network with nonlocal coupling local RE-evoked IPSPs could trigger a new spindle sequence. In-vivo
between neurons. data presented here indicate that persistent activity in an RE nucle-
The same synaptic mechanisms were responsible for traveling us during interspindle intervals occurs at low frequencies (0.33
patterns in one-dimensional chains and two-dimensional lattices of Hz). This confirms the suggestion that the mechanism underlying
RE cells. In particular, the speed of the two-dimensional waves was this activity is based on the reversal of GABAA receptor IPSPs fol-
close to that of traveling waves in a one-dimensional RE network. lowed by calcium spikes in RE cells. If this hypothesis is correct, then
The same conditions on cellular and network parameters generated persistent low-frequency activity in the RE nucleus will lead to the
propagating activity patterns in all of the networks studied. In its more or less simultaneous (within 1 to 2 cycles of spindle oscilla-
oscillatory mode, the average membrane potentials of RE cells in a tions) activation of new spindle sequences at different thalamic foci,
two-dimensional RE network varied with frequencies from 2 to 10 whereas transient patterns will result in nonsynchronous initiation
Hz depending on the absolute levels of the membrane potentials. of spatially localized spindle sequences. Future experiments will test
The frequency increased with depolarization until it reached around these predictions.
10 Hz just below the chloride reversal potential, which is close to the In a previous model of spindle oscillations, new spindle
frequency of spindle oscillations recorded in the isolated RE nucle- sequences were triggered without persistent activity inside the RE
us in vivo9. At relatively hyperpolarized levels of membrane poten- network by including spontaneously oscillating (initiator) TC cells38.
tial, the network oscillations were maintained through the Our model predicts that even in the absence of such TC cells, the
re-excitation of RE cells by the circulating waves of bursting neu- RE nucleus could initiate sequences of spindle oscillations in thal-
rons. When cells were sufficiently depolarized, the spatiotemporal amocortical networks in vivo.
activity in the RE network no longer resembled rotating spirals but METHODS
became more or less irregular. The remaining synchrony of network In-vivo recordings. In-vivo experiments were done on 46 unilaterally decor-
activity was reflected in occasional 910 Hz population oscillations, ticated cats and 52 cats with intact thalamocortical connections. All animals
lasting 0.51 second and separated by epochs of less regular oscilla- were maintained under ketamine and xylazine (1015 mg/kg and 23 mg/kg,
tions (see Fig. 8b). A similar temporal pattern of alternations was i.m.) anesthesia. In addition, tissues to be incised and pressure points were
observed in a previous network model of RE cells depolarized above infiltrated with lidocaine. The EEG from the intact hemisphere was contin-
the chloride reversal potential, where coherent oscillatory activity uously recorded, and additional doses of anesthetics were administered at
was accompanied by travelling waves11. However, in that model, the the slightest tendency toward an increase in frequency and decrease in ampli-
oscillations were driven by GABAA-receptor-mediated hyperpolar- tude of EEG field potentials. The cats were paralyzed with gallamine tri-
ethiodide and artificially ventilated to the end-tidal CO2 of 3.53.8%. The
ization followed by rebound bursts in RE cells. heartbeat was monitored and kept constant (acceptable range, 90110
The proposed mechanism of reciprocal excitation between beats/min). Body temperature was maintained at 3739oC. Glucose saline
GABAergic cells is not unique to the RE nucleus of the thalamus. (5% glucose, 10 ml i.p.) was given every 34 h during the experiments, which
In-vitro recordings from hippocampal slices have revealed that some lasted for 814 h. The stability of intracellular recordings was ensured by
GABAergic synapses between interneurons and onto pyramidal neu- cisternal drainage, bilateral pneumothorax, hip suspension and filling the
rons could function as excitatory synapses3335. This effect could be hole made in the skull with a solution of agar-agar (4%). All experimental
explained by the depolarizing component of GABAA receptor IPSPs procedures were done according to national guidelines.
whose reversal potential depends on the chloride concentration dif- For microelectrode recordings from thalamic RE neurons, the surface of
ference across the plasma membrane36,37. the cortex that corresponds to the anterior half of the marginal and supra-
sylvian gyri was cauterized with silver nitrate. The cortex and white matter
Spike-bursting activity in RE nuclei may be important in initi-
were removed by suction until the head of the caudate nucleus was exposed.
ating sequences of spindle oscillations in the intact RETC network. Micropipettes were then lowered through the head of the caudate nucleus
Depolarization of TC cells during a sequence of spindle oscillations to reach the rostrolateral sector of the RE nucleus. Intracellular recordings
terminates oscillations in these cells but may also trigger a set of pat- were made with conventional sharp electrodes filled with a 2.5 M solution of
terns propagating through the RE network (M.B., I.T., M.S. and potassium acetate (DC resistance of 3070 M). Stable intracellular record-
T.J.S., unpublished). Slow repolarization of TC cells during an inter- ings had resting membrane potentials more negative than 55 mV and over-

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 173


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

shooting action potentials. Stimulations of the ventrolateral (VL) thalamic 10. Golomb, D., Wang, X. J. & Rinzel, J. Synchronization properties of spindle
nucleus and motor cortical area 4 were delivered with variable durations oscillations in a thalamic reticular nucleus model. J. Neurophysiol. 72,
11091126 (1994).
(0.050.2 ms) and intensities (0.050.3 mA). To suppress cortical activity in 11. Destexhe, A., Contreras, D., Sejnowski, T. J. & Steriade, M. A model of spindle
5 experiments, the 2 M solution of potassium acetate was washed over the rhythmicity in the isolated thalamic reticular nucleus. J. Neurophysiol. 72,
cortical surface in area 4. Multi-neuron activity in the RE nucleus was record- 803818 (1994).
ed with tungsten microelectrodes (resistance 710 M). At the end of an 12. Ulrich, D. & Huguenard, J. R. Nucleus-specific chloride homeostasis in rat
experiment, the animal was given a lethal dose of pentobarbital. thalamus. J. Neurosci. 17, 23482354 (1997).
13. Ulrich, D. & Huguenard, J. R. -Aminobutyric acid type B receptor-dependent
burst-firing in thalamic neurons: A dynamic clamp study. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
Computational model. We examined a single-compartment model of an USA 93, 1324513249 (1996).
RE cell, which included voltage-dependent currents described by Hodgkin- 14. Contreras, D., Curr Dossi, R. & Steriade, M. Electrophysiological properties
Huxley kinetics: of cat reticular thalamic neurones in vivo. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 470, 273294
(1993).
Cm dV = g (V E ) Iint Isyn (1) 15. Domich, L., Oakson, G. & Steriade, M. Thalamic burst patterns in the naturally
dt L L
sleeping cat: a comparison between cortically projecting and reticularis
where Cm = 1 /cm2 is the membrane capacitance, gL = 0.05 mS/cm2 is the neurones. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 379, 429449 (1986).
leakage conductance, EL = 77 mV is the reversal potential, Iint is a sum of 16. Contreras, D. & Steriade, M. Cellular basis of EEG slow rhythms: a study of
active intrinsic currents (Iintj) and Isyn is a sum of synaptic currents (Isynj). dynamic corticothalamic relationships. J. Neurosci. 15, 604622 (1995).
The area of the RE cell was SRE = 1.43 x 104 cm2. The set of intrinsic cur- 17. Destexhe, A., Contreras, D., Steriade, M., Sejnowski, T. J. & Huguenard, J. R. In
rents included a fast sodium current INa, a fast potassium current IK (ref. 39), vivo, in vitro, and computational analysis of dendritic calcium currents in
thalamic reticular neurons. J. Neurosci. 16, 169185, (1996).
a low-threshold calcium current IT (refs. 40, 41) and a potassium leak current 18. Contreras, D., Timofeev, I. & Steriade, M. Mechanisms of long-lasting
IKL = gKL(V EKL). The expressions for the transition rates of all currents hyperpolarizations underlying slow sleep oscillations in cat corticothalamic
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

are given in ref. 23. networks. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 494, 251264 (1996).
GABAA receptor synaptic current was modeled by a first-order activation 19. Steriade, M., Jones, E. G. & McCormick, D. A. Thalamus: Organization and
Function (Elsevier, Oxford, 1997).
scheme42 and was calculated according to the equation: 20. Timofeev, I. & Steriade, M. Low-frequency rhythms in the thalamus of intact-
Isyn = gsyn [O] (VEsyn) (2) cortex and decorticated cats. J. Neurophysiol. 76, 41524168 (1996).
21. Ulrich, D. & Huguenard, J. R. GABAA-receptor-mediated rebound burst firing
where gsyn is the maximal conductivity, [O](t) is the fraction of open channels and burst shunting in thalamus. J. Neurophysiol. 78, 17481751 (1997).
and Esyn = 70 (ref. 12) is the reversal potential. The decay time constant of the 22. Timofeev, I. & Steriade, M. Cellular mechanisms underlying intrathalamic
GABAA synapses between RE cells was varied from 10 ms to 100 ms43 to augmenting responses of reticular and relay neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 79,
27162729 (1998).
ensure the robustness of the results. We found that the main difference 23. Bazhenov, M., Timofeev, I., Steriade, M. & Sejnowski, T. J. Cellular and
between the models with slow and fast synaptic decay kinetics was the dura- network models for intrathalamic augmenting responses during 10 Hz
tion of the burst discharges in RE cells, which was reduced slightly when slow- stimulation. J. Neurophysiol. 79, 27302748 (1998).
er kinetics was used. The rest of the parameters were the same as in ref. 23. 24. Golomb, D. & Amitai, Y. Propagating neuronal discharges in neocortical slices:
We simulated two network models: (1) A one-dimensional chain of Computational and experimental study. J. Neurophysiol. 78, 11991211 (1997).
25. Golomb, D. Models of neuronal transient synchrony during propagation of
M = 100 RE cells and (2) a two-dimensional lattice of M M (33 33 activity through neocortical circuitry. J. Neurophysiol. 79, 112 (1998).
and 50 50) RE cells. In the first model, each RE cell xi (i [1,M]) was 26. Rinzel, J., Termal, D., Wang, X. J. & Ermentrout, B. Propagating activity
connected with N nearest cells (xj, j [i-N,i+N], N = 1 to 9), and in patterns in large-scale inhibitory neuronal networks. Science 279, 13511355
the second model each RE cell (xi,j) was connected with all cells inside a (1998).
27. Traub, R. D., Miles, R. & Jefferys, J. G. R. Synaptic and intrinsic conductances
circuit with radius N = 4 cells (xk,l, (k,l) S, S = {(k,l): k2+l2 N2 }). shape picrotoxin-induced synchronized after-discharges in the guinea-pig
Flow boundary conditions (V j = V j , j = j+1 if j [N+1,0], hippocampal slice. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 461, 525547 (1993).
j = 2Mj+1 if j [M+1,M+N] in one dimension or Vk.l = Vk,l in two 28. Traub, R. D., Miles, R. & Jefferys, J. G. R. Analysis of the propagation of
dimensions with the same rules for calculation k and l) were used to disinhibition-induced after-discharges along the guinea-pig hippocampal slice
reduce the effect of the boundaries. The maximal conductance for IKL in vitro. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 472, 267287 (1993).
29. Kuramoto, Y. Chemical Oscillations, Waves, and Turbulence (Springer-Verlag,
was initialized with some random variability (variance 10%) through Berlin, 1984).
the network to insure the robustness of the results 23. 30. Coullet, P., Gil, L. & Lega, J. Defect-mediated turbulence. Phys. Rev. Lett. 62,
16191622 (1989).
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS 31. Bazhenov, M. & Rabinovich, M. Synchronized disorder in a 2D complex
Ginzburg-Landau equation. Physica D 73, 318334 (1994).
This research was supported by the Howard Hughes Medical Institute, the Sloan 32. Winfree, A. When Time Breaks Down (Springer, New York, 1987).
Center for Theoretical Neurobiology, the Human Frontier Science Program and the 33. Michelson, H. B. & Wong, R. K. S. Excitatory synaptic responses mediated by
Medical Research Council of Canada. GABAA receptors in the hippocampus. Science 253, 14201423 (1991).
34. Michelson, H. B. & Wong, R. K. S. Synchronization of inhibitory neurones in
the guinea-pig hippocampus in vitro. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 477, 3545 (1994).
RECEIVED 14 SEPTEMBER; ACCEPTED 16 NOVEMBER 1998 35. Staley, K. J., Soldo, B. L. & Proctor, W. R. Ionic mechanisms of neuronal
excitation by inhibitory GABAA receptors. Science 269, 977981 (1995).
36. Nicoll, R. A., Malenka, R. C. & Kauer, J. A. Functional comparison of
1. Steriade, M., Deschnes, M., Domich, L. & Mulle, C. Abolition of spindle neurotransmitter receptor subtypes in mammalian central nervous system.
oscillations in thalamic neurons disconnected from nucleus reticularis thalami. Physiol. Rev. 70, 513565 (1990).
J. Neurophysiol. 54, 14731497 (1985). 37. Qian, N. & Sejnowski, T. J. When is an inhibitory synapse effective. Proc. Natl.
2. Steriade, M. & Llins, R. R. The functional states of the thalamus and the Acad. Sci. USA 87, 81458149 (1990).
associated neuronal interplay. Physiol. Rev. 68, 649742 (1988). 38. Destexhe, A., Bal, T., McCormick, D. A. & Sejnowski, T. J. Ionic mechanisms
3. Steriade, M., Jones, E. G. & Llins, R. R. Thalamic Oscillations and Signaling underlying synchronized oscillations and propagating waves in a model of
(Wiley, New York, 1990). ferret thalamic slices. J. Neurophysiol. 76, 20492070 (1996).
4. von Krosigk, M., Bal, T. & McCormick, D. A. Cellular mechanisms of a 39. Traub, R. D. & Miles, R. Neuronal Networks of the Hippocampus (Cambridge,
synchronized oscillation in the thalamus. Science 261, 361364 (1993). Cambridge Univ. Press, 1991).
5. Steriade, M., McCormick, D. A. & Sejnowski, T. J. Thalamocortical oscillations 40. Huguenard, J. R. & Prince, D. A. A novel T-type current underlies prolonged
in the sleeping and aroused brain. Science 262, 679685 (1993). Ca2+-dependent burst firing in GABAergic neurons of rat thalamic reticular
6. Bal, T. & McCormick, D. A. What stops synchronized thalamocortical nucleus. J. Neurosci. 12, 38043817 (1992).
oscillations? Neuron 17, 297308 (1996). 41. Huguenard, J. R. & McCormick, D. A. Simulation of the currents involved in
7. Budde, T., Biella, G., Munsch, T. & Pape, H. C. Lack of regulation by rhythmic oscillations in thalamic relay neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 68, 13731383
intracellular Ca2+ of the hyperpolarization-activated cation current in rat (1992).
thalamic neurones. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 503, 7985 (1997). 42. Destexhe, A., Mainen, Z. F. & Sejnowski, T. J. Synthesis of models for excitable
8. Lthi, A., Bal, B. & McCormick, D. A. Periodicity of thalamic spindle waves is membranes, synaptic transmission and neuromodulation using a common
abolished by ZD7288, a blocker of Ih. J. Neurophysiol. 79, 32843289 (1998). kinetic formalism. J. Comp. Neurosci. 1, 195230 (1994).
9. Steriade, M., Domich, L., Oakson, G. & Deschnes, M. The deafferented 43. Zhang, S. J., Huguenard, J. R. & Prince, D. A. GABAA receptor-mediated Cl
reticular thalamic nucleus generates spindle rhythmicity. J. Neurophysiol. 57, currents in rat thalamus reticular and relay neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 78,
260273 (1987). 22802286 (1997).

174 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Neural correlates of a decision in


the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
of the macaque
Jong-Nam Kim and Michael N. Shadlen

Department of Physiology and Biophysics and Regional Primate Research Center, University of Washington Medical School, Box 357290,
Seattle, Washington 98195-7290, USA
Correspondence should be addressed to M.N.S. (shadlen@u.washington.edu)

To make a visual discrimination, the brain must extract relevant information from the retina,
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

represent appropriate variables in the visual cortex and read out this representation to decide which
of two or more alternatives is more likely. We recorded from neurons in the dorsolateral prefrontal
cortex (areas 8 and 46) of the rhesus monkey while it performed a motion discrimination task. The
monkey indicated its judgment of direction by making appropriate eye movements. As the monkey
viewed the motion stimulus, the neural response predicted the monkeys subsequent gaze shift,
hence its judgment of direction. The response comprised a mixture of high-level oculomotor signals
and weaker visual sensory signals that reflected the strength and direction of motion. This combina-
tion of sensory integration and motor planning could reflect the conversion of visual motion
information into a categorical decision about direction and thus give insight into the neural compu-
tations behind a simple cognitive act.

The brain uses sensory information to form interpretations and arranged the motion-discrimination task so that one of two
decisions that guide behavior. Such interpretations often out- response targets appeared in the neurons RF (Fig. 1a). The mon-
last the fleeting sensory impressions on which they are based, key was trained to make a delayed eye movement to one or the
so that sensory input can motivate subsequent behavior. To study other response target, depending on the direction of random dot
this process, we trained rhesus monkeys to discriminate the motion. We found that the activity of many PFC neurons reveals
direction of motion in a dynamic random dot display1. The dif- the monkeys intention to make a saccade to one or the other tar-
ficulty of the task was controlled by varying the fraction of coher- get. The time course and intensity of neural activity seem to rep-
ently moving dots. At high motion coherences, the animal can resent the formation of a decision about motion direction.
commit to an action the moment the stimulus is seen. In con-
trast, near psychophysical threshold, the monkey must base its RESULTS
direction judgments on weak sensory signals perceived over hun- We studied 88 neurons in the frontal eye field (FEF) and the pos-
dreds of milliseconds1. terior third of the principal sulcus (PS) region (areas 8Ar and 46)
The extrastriate visual cortex (areas MT and MST) contains that responded selectively when the monkey planned a saccade
the neural representation of visual motion that allows the monkey to a region of space (the RF). For example, the PS neuron shown
to perform this demanding task14. The direction-selective neu- in Fig. 2 responded during the delay preceding saccades to tar-
rons in these areas represent the evidence favoring one direction gets that appeared up and to the right, but not down and to the
or another, but this evidence must be interpreted to reach a deci- left of the fixation point (Fig. 2a and b).
sion. This distinction between sensory evidence and decision can To determine this neurons behavior during a perceptual deci-
be appreciated by imposing a delay between motion viewing and sion, we monitored its discharge while the monkey judged the
behavioral response. Direction-selective neurons stop respond- direction of random dot motion. The random dots appeared in
ing when the motion stimulus is absent5, whereas neurons that a five degree aperture outside the neurons RF (see Methods).
encode a decision must maintain their activity when the visual Motion direction was toward or away from the RF, and motion
motion is no longer present, until the animal responds. strength was varied to span psychophysical threshold. After a delay,
The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (PFC) is felt to be critical the monkey indicated its direction judgment by making an eye
for tasks with a delay between instruction and execution6,7. Dur- movement to one of the two targets. If the motion was up-right,
ing the delay, many PFC neurons show sustained discharge, which the monkey was rewarded for choosing the target inside the RF.
is often selective for a particular object or location811. We studied Conversely, if the motion was down-left, the monkey was reward-
neurons with sustained activity through the memory/delay peri- ed for choosing the other target, outside of the RF.
od before an eye movement to a restricted region of the visual This neurons response predicted the monkeys decision. The
field, termed the neural response field (RF). We hypothesized that response was larger on trials in which the monkeys eyes moved
such neurons may be involved in linking the sensory evidence in toward the RF (Fig. 2c, e and g) and attenuated when they moved
visual cortex to a behavioral plan to shift the gaze. To test this, we away from the RF (Fig. 2d, f and h). The spike discharge from this

176 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 1. Behavioral tasks and neu-


ron locations. (a, b) Direction-dis- a
crimination task. The monkey
gazed at the fixation point for 350
ms. Then two targets appeared,
one of which was in the neural
response field (RF, shaded). After
200300 ms, the random dot kine-
matogram appeared between the b motion delay saccade
targets and outside the RF. The
direction of motion was toward
one of the two targets. Motion
strength was varied from trial to
trial by adjusting the percentage of
coherently moving dots. After 1 s, e
m
the random dots were turned off, Ti
leaving only the fixation point and
targets. After 0.51.5 s, the fixa-
tion point was extinguished, signal-
ing the monkey to indicate its
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

choice by shifting its gaze to one of


the targets. The monkey was
rewarded for choosing the target
along the direction of random dot
Probability correct (mean stdev)

motion, or randomly when there c


was no net motion (0% coher- d flash delay saccade
ence). T1 and T2, saccade targets;
FP, fixation point. (c) Average psy-
chometric function for 88 experi-
ments. Error bars are standard
deviations of the proportion of
correct choices. (d) Memory-sac-
cade task used to screen neurons.
A target was flashed (100 ms) at a
random location in the visual field.
The monkey maintained fixation
through a variable delay until the Motion strength (% coherence)
fixation point was extinguished.
The monkey was then required to
shift its gaze to the remembered
location of the flashed target.
(e) Location of the recording cylin- e f
der in a schematic diagram of the
rhesus monkey brain. (f) Magnetic
resonance imaging. Fast spin-echo,
short-T1, inversion-recovery scan
through the electrode grid of mon-
key S. This is one of a series of
images obtained in the coronal
plane (slice thickness 1.5 mm). The
recording grid was filled with ster-
ile saline to reveal the angle and
location of electrode guide tubes in the coronal plane. A second series was obtained in the sagittal plane to determine the position and angle
of guide tubes in the anteriorposterior direction. The section shows the arcuate sulcus and prearcuate gyrus at the caudal end of the princi-
pal sulcus (ps, principal sulcus; as, arcuate sulcus).

neuron began to reveal the monkeys decision as early as 200300 ms Although this pattern of response was common in the FEF
after the onset of random dot motion and remained informative and PS region, we also encountered many neurons that modu-
until the saccade. Moreover, the response was modulated more lated their activity only during the delay after the random dot
strongly when the task was easier: the neuron discharged more motion was turned off, presumably after the monkey had reached
intensely when the monkey viewed coherent motion toward the RF its decision. This activity (Fig. 3a) clearly predicted the monkeys
(up-right) and attenuated more profoundly to coherent motion plan to look to the target in the RF on the memory-saccade task.
away from the RF. Thus, the response reflected not only the mon- However, during the motion-discrimination task, the response
keys impending eye movement, but also the sensory input that did not reveal the monkeys decision until the delay (Fig. 3c
determined it. This mixture of visual-sensory and visuomotor and d). During motion viewing, this neuron failed to signal the
response properties is thought to occur at the nexus of sensory-to- direction of the next eye movement. Such responses may reflect
motor conversion1214 where the decision is computed. motor preparation, but they provide little insight into the deci-

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 177


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 2. Response of a principalis neuron during the


motion-discrimination and memory-saccade tasks.
The response field of the neuron is shown in gray.
The arrow indicates the direction of the monkeys
saccade at the end of the trial. (a, b) Response on
memory saccades to targets in (a) and out (b) of a b
the RF. The time axis is broken to align the
response to two events: target appearance and
saccade initiation. (ch) Response during the
motion-discrimination task. Responses are aligned
to the onset of random dot motion and to the
time of the saccade. The left column (c, e, g)
depicts trials in which the monkey decided the
motion was toward the RF. The right column (d, f,
h) shows trials in which the monkey decided that
the direction was away from the RF, leading to a
saccade to the target outside the RF. Three motion
strengths are shown (sub-, near- and supra-thresh-
old). For the 0% coherence stimulus, there is no
net direction of motion. Only correct discrimina-
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

tion trials are shown for nonzero motion


strengths. For clarity, only 10 trials are shown in
the rasters. The response preceding the onset of c d
motion was associated with fixation and the
appearance of the saccade targets.

sion-making process. Unless the monkey


makes eye movements capriciously a pos- e f
sibility precluded by the well behaved psy-
chometric function (Fig. 1c) the decision
must form during motion viewing.
For each neuron, we computed an index
of predictive activity using the responses
measured during motion viewing and dur-
ing the delay. The index measures the g h
response distribution overlap from trials in
which the monkey judged motion as
toward versus away from the RF and esti-
Spikes/s

mates the probability that an ideal observ-


er could predict the monkeys decision
based on the spike rate (Methods). For
example, an index of 0.5 indicates a chance
association (complete overlap of response
distributions associated with the two choic-
es), whereas an index of 1 indicates perfect correspondence ed to encounter similar patterns of predictive activity in the FEF
between predicted and observed choices (no overlap). and PS region (Fig. 4, p > 0.08, two-dimensional Kolmogorov-
Most neurons (76/88, 86%) predicted the monkeys choice Smirnov test15), which may result from our strategy of sampling
reliably during motion viewing or the delay or both (Fig. 4; pre- neurons of similar type in both areas.
dictive index > 0.5 and p < 0.01 in at least one epoch). Sixty
percent (53/88) predicted the monkeys decision reliably dur- Time course of predictive activity
ing motion viewing (index > 0.5 and p < 0.01) and could reflect To perform above chance on this task, the monkey must decide
the association between motion processing and behavioral about the motion direction based on the evidence acquired
response. However, about 1/4 of the neurons failed to indicate during motion viewing. Those neurons that predicted the mon-
the monkeys choices until the delay (9/26 neurons in the FEF keys subsequent eye movement during motion viewing may
and 10/62 in the posterior PS region), which is too late for them therefore divulge properties of the decision process itself, the
to be involved in the decision process. linkage between sensory processing and saccade planning. A
Some neurons (11%, 1 FEF, 9 PS) failed to predict the mon- neural correlate of the decision process is likely to reflect both
keys response at any time during the discrimination task (values the outcome of the decision and the quality of the evidence
near 0.5 on both axes, p > 0.01). Five neurons (1 FEF, 4 PS) reliably upon which it is founded. Moreover, neurons linking sensory
predicted the monkeys choices during motion viewing but in a evidence to a behavioral response should undergo a temporal
pattern opposite to their response on the memory-saccade task transformation in their activity as the evidence produces a cat-
(predictive index < 0.5, p < 0.01). The activity of these five neurons egorical answer. Early responses might reflect properties of the
diminished when the monkey chose the target in the RF, but none sensory stimulus (the strength of the evidence). Responses later
retained this reversed activity pattern during the delay. We tend- in the decision process should reflect a stereotyped outcome,

178 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Movement/memory Fig. 3. Response of a frontal eye field neu-


response field ron during the motion-discrimination and
memory-saccade tasks. (a, b) Response on
memory-guided saccades to targets flashed
briefly inside or outside the response field.
This neuron had a prominent visual
a b response at the onset of the target and a
sustained response when the monkey
planned an eye movement up-leftward.
(c, d) Response on the motion-discrimina-
Spikes/s

tion task. The poststimulus time histogram


includes all trials in which the monkey
chose the correct direction. The response
was stronger when the monkey judged the
motion to be toward the RF, but the effect
was not apparent until the delay (lower
arrows). Responses are aligned to the onset
of random dot motion and to the saccade
initiation. The transient response at the
time of motion onset was caused by the
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

c d appearance of saccade targets.

regardless of whether the evidence was strong or weak, and correct choices at each of six motion strengths. On average, the
whether it was interpreted correctly or incorrectly. These prop- response began to predict the monkeys decision 100200 ms after
erties are evident in the PFC response. onset of the random dot motion. Later in the trial, the response
Motion strength affected the response of many PFC neurons predicted the monkeys choice with greater fidelity, reaching a max-
(Fig. 5a and b). For this analysis, we selected neurons that pre- imum during the delay, just before the eye movement. Although
dicted the monkeys subsequent choice during motion viewing each point represents the predictive activity from just 250 ms of
(n = 53 neurons with predictive index > 0.5 and a significant spike discharge, the curves look like cumulative functions.
permutation test, p < 0.01). For each neuron, we computed the
average spike rate during motion viewing (from 200800 ms
after the onset of dot motion) and normalized the spike rate to
the mean. We applied this procedure separately for the two
directions of motion and restricted the analysis to correct choic-
Predictive index delay period

es. For motion toward the RF, the degree of response enhance-
ment varied by 12.5% across the range of motion strengths
(~3.2 spikes/s; Fig. 5a). For motion away from the RF, the
degree of suppression varied by 22% across the range of motion
strengths (~4.3 spikes/s; Fig. 5b).
This normalization procedure removes the main determinant
of the response magnitude for these neurons: whether an eye
movement is ultimately made to the RF or away from it. The effect
of motion strength is therefore relatively subtle. The regression
analysis was statistically significant (p = 0.0032 and p < 0.00001
for Fig. 5a and b, respectively) and was unaffected by the incor-
poration of eye movement descriptors such as saccadic latency, Predictive index motion viewing period
amplitude, duration and velocity (see Methods). This last point
implies that subtle variations in the actual saccade produced in Fig. 4. Predictive activity for 88 neurons during motion viewing and the
each trial of the experiment does not explain the variation in neur- delay. The predictive index approximates the accuracy with which one
al response found as a function of task difficulty. could guess monkeys decision based on the spike discharge measured
during motion viewing or the delay (Methods). Values larger than 0.5
We represented the evolution of predictive activity during
imply greater accuracy in predicting the monkeys decision from the
motion discrimination by calculating the predictive index in 250- neural response. The histograms summarize the distribution of these
ms epochs beginning 500 ms before the onset of random dot indices; shading indicates a significant departure from 0.5 (p < 0.01 by
motion and ending just after the saccade (Fig. 5c). We computed permutation test; Methods). Blue circles, FEF; red circles, PS (area 8Ar
the index separately for each of the 53 neurons that predicted the and Walker area 46). The open symbols indicate the neurons shown in
monkeys behavior during motion viewing (as above), using only Figs. 2 (red) and 3 (blue).

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 179


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 5. Effect of motion strength on the magnitude and time


of the prefrontal response. (a, b) Effect of stimulus strength a b
on average response during motion viewing for 53 neurons

Normalized response
with statistically significant predictive indices. (a) For deci-
sions favoring motion toward the RF, the response was larger
to stronger random dot motion. The ordinate represents the
response strength relative to the mean for all decisions
toward the RF. Filled points represent the mean standard
error of the normalized response. (b) For decisions favoring
motion away from the RF, the response was more sup-
pressed to stronger motion stimuli. Conventions are the
same as in (a) except that the response is normalized to the
mean of each neurons response associated with choices out- Motion strength (% coherence)
side the RF. (c) The predictive power of the response was
computed in 250-ms epochs whose midpoint is plotted on
the time axis. Each point represents the probability of cor- c Saccade
rectly predicting the monkeys choice from 250 ms of spike
discharge. Curves represent the averaged probabilities from
53 neurons with predictive activity during motion viewing.
The neurons predicted the monkeys choice sooner and
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

more reliably when the motion was stronger.


Probability (mean)

The emergence of predictive activity on the most


difficult motion condition (no coherent motion) indi-
cates that the neurons primarily encode variables that
pertain to the monkeys behavioral response. On the
other hand, when the motion was stronger (that is, eas-
ier), the response predicted the monkeys choice soon-
er and better than when the motion was weaker. This
effect was subtle for individual neurons but highly reli-
able across the population (p < 0.00001, likelihood ratio
test using modified probit analysis; Eq. 3 in Methods).
It reflects a stronger and more consistent rise in the
response when motion was toward the RF and a more Time (s)
profound attenuation of the discharge when motion
was toward the target outside the neurons RF (Fig. 2).
This response pattern could represent the accumulation of the RF was erroneous as compared to correct (p = 0.001; t-test of
motion information from the extrastriate cortex toward a plateau. mean normalized response), and the response was less attenuated
Indeed, some neurons responded to the direction of random dot on erroneous decisions favoring motion away from the RF
motion during passive viewing, when the monkey was not (p < 0.00001). Thus, the response cannot be explained solely by
engaged in the discrimination task (Fig. 6). The weak direction the behavioral outcome. The observation is consistent with the
bias observed in this control experiment favored motion toward idea that the neural response reflects the accumulation of senso-
the RF, suggesting that instructive visual signals reach the PFC ry evidence toward a decision. As shown in the next section, this
even without a task. is because the strength of neural signals favoring the wrong direc-
tion (for example, the responses of rightward motion sensors
Errors when the direction is actually leftward) are unlikely to exceed by
Could passive visual responses explain our finding of predictive much the neural signals that would favor the correct direction.
responses during motion viewing of the discrimination task? This
seems unlikely because the neurons were predictive when the Theory
motion strength was negligible, as in the 0% coherent motion Our results are consistent with the idea that neurons in the dor-
condition (Figs. 2c and d and 5). The neural response is domi- solateral PFC compare sensory signals from the extrastriate cor-
nated by the direction that the monkey judges, and hence the tex that favor motion toward or away from the response field.
direction of the planned saccade. This point is further supported Accumulated spike counts from pools of neurons in areas MT
by examining error trials, in which the motion direction and the and MST can account for the monkeys sensitivity and trial-to-
saccade choice are opposed. The response was larger when the trial choices on the random dot motion task16. We therefore
monkey chose the target in the RF, whether or not this was based expect that neurons involved in the decision process must accu-
on a proper interpretation of the random dot motion (Fig. 7). mulate spikes from groups of sensory neurons and make the
Thus, the response was dominated by what the monkey planned appropriate comparison, and that the neural computations
to do, rather than by what the monkey saw. underlying a decision probably involve integration in time.
There was, however, a subtle difference between correct and The sensory signals that inform the monkeys choices on a
erroneous decisions. On average, neurons modulated their near-threshold discrimination (Fig. 8) are presumed to be the
response less strongly on the error trials. During motion viewing, accumulated spike counts from pools of noisy and weakly corre-
the response was weaker when the monkeys choice of the target in lated direction sensors in the extrastriate visual cortex16. These

180 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

a b

Direction away from response field


Direction toward response field
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

Fig. 6. Response to random dot motion during passive viewing. (a) Direction-tuning function for one neuron. This neurons RF was near the upper
vertical meridian at an eccentricity of 10 degrees. The random dots were shown in a five-degree aperture centered on the fixation point, outside the
RF of the neuron. The response histograms are displayed to indicate the direction of random dot motion. Responses are aligned to motion onset. The
polar plot shows the mean response calculated during viewing. The response was largest when motion was toward the RF. (b) Summary data from 10
neurons tested during passive fixation, comparing responses to passively viewed motion toward or away from the RF. Filled symbols denote neurons
with significant predictive activity during motion viewing on discrimination trials (done in a separate block). These neurons had a stronger direction
bias (points farther from diagonal; p < 0.001, ROC area comparison and permutation test; see Methods). Error bars represent standard error.

accumulations constitute the evidence for a rightward or left- with weaker evidence. At the near-threshold motion strength in
ward choice. Imagine a PFC neuron whose RF is situated so that this example, approximately 24% of the trials would result in
a rightward decision increases its response. According to our errors. For erroneous rightward choices (Fig. 8f), the difference
scheme, this neuron compares the rightward cumulant to the left- signals on error trials tend to be small (compare Fig. 8d and f).
ward cumulant. The monkey chooses right when this difference is The expected mean is 0.84 units of signal standard deviation.
positive and left when it is negative. The magnitude of the differ- This analysis could explain the subtle difference in the neural
ence represents the strength of the evidence favoring a decision. response that we observed on correct versus error trials (Fig. 7).
Figure 8a shows idealized distributions of pooled responses This same analysis can be extended across the range of stimu-
from leftward and rightward sensors to a near-threshold right- lus strengths (Fig. 8g). At each motion strength, there are four
ward motion stimulus. The size of the responses is expressed in possible outcomes: two directions times two choices. At all motion
units of standard deviation (). Notice that the distribution of strengths, whenever the monkey chooses rightward correct or
pooled rightward signals exceeds that of the leftward signals by not the evidence for rightward motion (Fig. 8g) exceeds the
1 on average (that is, the index of discriminability, d, equals 1), evidence for leftward. However, the strength of the evidence
consistent with a motion strength that would support 76% cor- increases with stronger motion (larger values of d), leading to
rect choices. When the stimulus direc-
tion is leftward, the leftward signals are
larger on average (Fig. 8b).
This analysis permits us to assess
Fig. 7. Comparison of the responses on
the strength of the evidence associated error and correct trials. The responses from
with each of four types of decisions as
Response (mean normalized)

53 neurons with predictive activity during


seen from the point of view of a motion viewing were used to construct these
choose right neuron in the PFC (Fig. curves, using all non-zero motion strengths.
8cf). When motion is rightward and Responses were normalized to the mean of
the correct choice follows (Fig. 8d), the each neurons response over the 600-ms
strength of the evidence favoring right- epoch indicated by the gray bar. The black
ward is positive and large on average. curves depict trials in which the monkey
The expected mean difference between judged the direction as toward the RF, culmi-
nating in a saccade to the target in the RF
rightward and leftward sensory signals
(T1); the gray curves indicate decisions
is 1.6 units of signal standard devia- resulting in a saccadic eye movement to the
tion. When motion is leftward and the target outside the RF (T2). Error trials are
correct choice follows, the evidence indicated by the dashed curves. The promi-
favoring a rightward choice is negative nent response that begins before the onset of
and large (Fig. 8e; expected mean, motion was caused by the presentation of
1.6 ). The error trials are associated Time from motion onset the saccade targets.

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 181


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Distribution of signals from rightward and leftward sensors

Motion is rightward (d = 1) Motion is leftward (d = 1)


a b
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

Distribution of difference signals received by a choose right neuron

c Wrong left choice d Correct right choice e Correct left choice f Wrong right choice

SRightSLeft () SRightSLeft () SRightSLeft () SRightSLeft ()

Fig. 8. Theoretical basis for variation in the strength of evidence associated with direction judg-
g
Evidence for rightward ()
ments. The graphs trace the flow of information from populations of opponent direction sensors to
a neuron in the PFC that would increase its response in association with a rightward decision. The
input to the PFC neuron represents the evidence that motion is rightward, in the form of the differ-
ence between rightward and leftward sensory signals. Four scenarios are depicted: motion is either
leftward or rightward, and the choice is either correct or erroneous. (a) Activity of the leftward and
rightward motion sensors when the true direction is rightward. The surface shows the joint proba-
bility distribution of a pair of signals from rightward and leftward sensors. The two signals should be
interpreted as the population average responses from many neurons. The graph depicts the situation
at psychophysical threshold when the rightward signal is one standard deviation larger than the left-
ward signal, on average (projected bell-shaped curves). The points below the surface are 100 ran-
Stimulus strength (d)
dom samples from the overlying distribution. Each point is labeled according to the resulting
decision (blue, rightward decision; red, leftward decision). The majority of samples fall on the side of
the main diagonal that indicates that the rightward signal exceeds the leftward signal. In this plot, the rightward choices are correct. (b) Activity of the
motion sensors when the true direction is leftward. Same conventions as in (a). The leftward sensors produce the larger signal, on average. In this plot,
the leftward choices (red) are correct. (cf) Frequency histograms of the difference in sensory signals between rightward and leftward sensors. The dif-
ferences are tabulated separately for the two directions of motion and the two decisions. The histograms are shown under the simulated points from
which they were obtained in (a) and (b). The difference, SRight SLeft, is positive when the decision is rightward (blue) and negative when the decision is
leftward (red). Notice that there are more samples corresponding to correct choices (d and e), and the difference signals tend to be larger positive and
negative values. (g) Average difference signal, SRight SLeft, accompanying correct and incorrect choices for a range of motion strengths spanning the
psychometric function. The d = 1 case corresponds to a motion strength supporting 76% correct choices (~10% coherence), as illustrated in (af). The
theoretical means for (cf) are shown. When d = 0, there is no net motion direction (0% coherence), and there is no distinction between correct and
error trials. When d = 2, there are very few errors ( 25% coherence). The difference signals associated with correct choices attain greater positive
and negative values when the motion is stronger. The opposite trend is predicted for error trials.

larger responses when the decision is rightward and greater sup- the evidence for rightward changes by only one unit of signal
pression when the decision is leftward. The increasing separation standard deviation for correct choices that lead to the same
of the solid curves in Fig. 8g helps explain the increase in predic- behavioral response (Fig. 8g). The unit, , can be related to the
tive index as a function of motion strength (see Fig. 5). The analy- responses of MT neurons. At a d value of 1, the response of
sis also predicts that the responses associated with errors should pooled rightward-preferring MT neurons exceeds the response
diminish at higher motion strengths. We cannot evaluate this pre- of pooled leftward-preferring MT neurons by one standard
diction because errors occur rarely for strong motion. deviation of the pooled values. Recordings from area MT show
Finally, this analysis helps to explain the relatively subtle that at a motion strength of 10% (corresponding to d~1),
variation in response that we observed in the PFC as a function rightward- and leftward-preferring neurons differ in their
of motion strength. Across the entire psychometric function, responses by ~5 spikes/s1,17. According to the analysis (Fig. 8g),

182 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

the monkeys judgments at the weakest motion strength (0% described algorithm21, but we have failed to find evidence for
coherence) are based on evidence of 1.13 , or ~5 spikes/s such abrupt transitions. Simultaneous recordings from two
from the average MT neuron. We do not know how to convert or more neurons could, in principle, facilitate detection of
this value to a spike rate in the PFC; in the example we are pur- such abrupt changes in firing pattern.
suing, all rightward choices are associated with a high spike rate. Our observations support an emerging view that the dis-
However, as the motion strength increases to span the mon- tinction between sensory and motor systems may be blurred
keys psychometric function, the strength of the evidence within the association areas of the cerebral cortex18,22,23. In
increases (or decreases) by only another unit of or ~5 spikes/s. many association areas, neural responses are predicted by
This value is comparable to the change in PFC response that stimulus qualities as well as motor preparation. It is tempting
we observed across the range of motion strengths used in our to regard the former as instructing the latter. For example,
experiments (see Fig. 5a, b and legend). FEF neurons represent features of a visual stimuli that instruct
an eye movement in a visual search task24. Neurons in area 46
DISCUSSION respond at the moment an instruction is given to guide a
The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex is important in linking sen- future eye movement11. Even neural responses in the primary
sation to action, especially when the linkage involves a delay6,7. and supplementary motor cortex reflect not only the planned
Our study provides a glimpse of this linkage in the activity of behavior but the sensory cue that instructs that action12,2531.
individual PFC neurons. We used a task in which delayed sac- Any brain region containing signals related to both senso-
cades were instructed by a direction judgment. By requiring ry processing and motor preparation may be involved in the
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

the animal to make difficult judgments near psychophysical conversion of the former to the latter. For most tasks, howev-
threshold, we were able to characterize neural activity during er, there is insufficient time to study the process of conver-
a period in which sensory processing gradually gave way to a sion: the moment the instruction is received, the animal can
categorical choice, what we have termed a decision process. prepare an action. In the threshold discrimination task that
Our results demonstrate a gradual unfolding of neural activ- we used, the monkey cannot process the instruction instant-
ity, which we interpret as a correlate of the monkeys decision. ly. The monkey, like human subjects, benefits from the tem-
To find a neural correlate of the monkeys decision about poral accumulation of information 1,32 (J.D. Roitman &
the direction of motion, we searched for neurons that signaled M.N.S., Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 24, 262, 1998). Our results suggest
the monkeys commitment to a particular behavioral option. that prefrontal neurons may do more than hold information
We therefore studied neurons in the FEF and principalis region in short-term memory. They seem to be involved in the accu-
that showed sustained activity during an oculomotor delayed- mulation (that is, integration) and comparison of sensory
response task. These neurons could be said to lie toward the streams toward a categorical outcome or behavioral plan.
motor side of a sensationaction continuum 14, linking the This does not imply that the neurons we recorded are
visual cortex with the motor system 18. directly responsible for deciding direction. They may reflect
The sustained response of PFC neurons has been inter- the computation made at another site in the brain. In fact, the
preted as a neural correlate of short-term memory for spatial neurons described here respond similarly to neurons in the
location19,20, but we do not know if such neural activity rep- lateral intraparietal area (LIP) and the superior colliculus33,34
resents the instruction (for example, location of a spatial cue) during the same motion discrimination task. This is not sur-
or the behavioral plan to move the eyes to that location. Our prising because area LIP, the FEF and the posterior principalis
results suggest that the response may conflate these represen- region are strongly connected with each other3538. Both LIP
tations. Most neurons modulated their response in associa- and the dorsolateral PFC seem to be activated during similar
tion with all the key ingredients of the discrimination task: delayed eye movement tasks39, and both areas project to the
the appearance of a target within the response field, the direc- superior colliculus36,4042. The relative importance of these
tion and strength of random dot motion and the direction of brain regions will need to be addressed with reversible inacti-
a planned saccade. Our analysis showed that early in the task, vation or simultaneous recording. Meanwhile, the activity in
during formation of the decision, the modulation in neural the prefrontal and inferior parietal cortex reveals something
response varies parametrically with the strength of visual about the computations that may underlie the linkage between
motion (Fig. 5). By the end of this process, many neurons in sensory data, interpretation and behavioral planning. We pro-
the PFC reflect the monkeys choice. These observations sug- pose that such neurons compute the time integral of sensory
gest that the PFC represents not simply the final outcome of evidence toward a plateau state.
sensory processing but the conversion of an analog motion Neurons whose dominant mode of response signals the plan
representation to a binary decision variable. to enact a behavior must be influenced by sensory signals. The
This conclusion must be viewed cautiously because we do existence of such a link comes as no surprise, but the mixture
not know the moment that the monkey decides the direction of signals on single neurons, reflecting motor planning and
of motion, or indeed if there is such a discrete moment. It is sensory strength, constrains a view of the brains logical archi-
possible that on different trials, the monkey reaches its deci- tecture. It seems inconsistent with a central executive function
sions at different times and the neural responses change short- that interprets the sensory data, declares an interpretation and
ly thereafter to reveal the intended action. The average recruits circuitry to enact a response. Instead, it supports a
response from many trials might give the appearance of a view of brain organization that would recruit premotor cir-
gradual evolution of the monkeys plan (Fig. 5c). If the deci- cuitry in the interest of several potential actions while querying
sion were to occur sooner, on average, for easier discrimina- sensory streams for evidence to select the appropriate one.
tions, then this could explain the parametric variation in METHODS
response magnitude that we observed with different motion Electrophysiology. We recorded from 88 neurons in the frontal eye
strengths (Fig. 5a and b). We have looked for discrete changes field (FEF, areas 8Ac and 45a) and posterior principalis region (areas
in the firing patterns of our neurons using a previously 8Ar and Walker area 46) of two rhesus monkeys trained on a random

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 183


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

dot motion discrimination task 1. Monkeys were implanted with an normalized spike rate and C is the rank motion strength (0 to 5). For
eye coil, head-holding device and recording cylinder suitable for mag- each trial, we also extracted five descriptors of the saccade: its laten-
netic resonance imaging (MRI; Crist Instrument Co., Damascus, Mary- cy, amplitude, accuracy, maximal speed and duration. We included
land). The recording cylinder was placed over the arcuate sulcus and these factors along with motion strength in a multivariate regression
the posterior third of the principal sulcus (PS; Fig. 1e). Sterile guide analysis, fitting the model,
tubes were placed through a plastic grid (Crist Instruments) to intro- Y = 0 + 1C + 2LAT + 3AMP + 4ACC + 5VMAX + 6DUR + (1)
duce tungsten/glass microelectrodes to the surface of the dura mater.
The grid array was visualized in situ by MRI and registered with the The fit to equation 1 allowed us to test whether motion strength
anatomy (Fig. 1f). We used the MRI to identify the FEF in the anteri- affects the neural response in a manner that cannot be attributed to
or bank of the arcuate sulcus and to distinguish the area principalis variation in saccadic eye movements. This is a test of the null hypoth-
from more caudal portions of the prearcuate gyrus. Action potentials esis, 1 = 0, which is done by comparing the extra sum of squares
were identified using a dual-voltage, time-window discriminator (Bak with and without this regressor and computing an F ratio47.
Electronics, Germantown, Maryland) and stored on computer with 1-
ms precision43. All training, surgery and experimental procedures com- Analysis of predictive activity. For several analyses, we computed an
plied with the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and index that describes the association between neural response and the
Use of Laboratory Animals and were approved by the University of monkeys decision. This index, the probability that the neural
Washington Animal Care Committee. response associated with one behavioral choice exceeds the neural
response associated with the other behavioral choice, approximates
the ability of the experimenter to predict the monkeys behavior from
Electrical stimulation. To aid in identifying recording sites within the
the neural response. Denoting the responses associated with the two
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

frontal eye field, we attempted to elicit saccades using the electrical


choices by {r1} and {r2}, this is the joint probability of observing r1 = k
microstimulation protocol described 44. We classified sites as low
and r2 < k, over all possible values of k:
threshold if we could elicit a fixed-vector saccade with a stimulating

Pr(r = k)Pr(r <k)dk


current less than 50 A. We classified cells as in or out of the FEF
based on their location in the anterior bank of the arcuate sulcus and Predictive index = 1 2
-
(2)
their proximity to low-threshold stimulation sites. This procedure k
unequivocally classified most neurons. = Pr(r = k) [ Pr(r =)d]dk
1 2
- -
Behavioral tasks. Neurons were selected using a delayed-saccade task
that relies on working memory (Fig. 1d)8,45,46. We studied neurons Equation 2 can be estimated by computing the area under a receiv-
that responded during the delay (memory) period preceding saccades er-operator-characteristic (ROC) curve obtained from the two
to a restricted region of the visual field. We refer to this region as the response distributions1,48.
neural response field (RF) to remain agnostic as to function. Many The distribution of the predictive index under the null hypothesis
neurons also responded transiently at the onset of movement or to the is typically not normal. We used a permutation test to estimate the
onset of saccade targets, but this was not a criterion in their selection. probability that the measured index would be observed under the
Similar selection criteria were used in a related study of area LIP33. null hypothesis of a random association between neural and behav-
In the motion-discrimination task (Fig. 1a and b), two response ioral response (that is, the true index is 0.5). Each neural response
targets appeared. One target was in the RF of the neuron; the random and each behavioral response were randomly associated to construct
dot kinematogram and the second response target appeared outside the two distributions, {r1} and {r2}, which therefore contain the same
the RF. After a brief pause (200300 ms), the random dot motion was number of observations as the original {r1} and {r2}. The predictive
displayed for 1 s. The direction of motion was toward one or the other index was computed using Equation 2, and the distribution of its
target. Both the direction of motion and the fraction of coherently absolute difference from 0.5 was estimated using 1000 permutations
moving dots were randomized. After a delay, the monkey indicated its of the data. The probability of obtaining the measured index under
direction judgment by making an eye movement to one of the two the null hupothesis is the fraction of this distribution exceeding the
response targets. The monkey was trained to associate rightward absolute value of the measured indexs difference from 0.5.
motion with the target to the right of the dots aperture, upward motion The same procedure was used to measure of the time course of pre-
with a target above the aperture, and so forth. The monkey received a dictive activity (Fig. 5c) by computing the predictive index from spike
liquid reward for correct responses (and on half of the trials in which counts obtained from 250-ms epochs at intervals designated with
the 0% coherent motion was shown). In all experiments, the monkeys respect to the onset random dot motion or the monkeys saccadic eye
showed a smooth improvement in performance as a function of movement. The resulting sigmoid functions are well fit by the scaled
motion strength (Fig. 1c). The average threshold motion strength sup- integral of a Gaussian function of time:
porting 81% correct choices was 12.9% (range 1.5 to 34.1%; standard
deviation, 5.2%), which is typical of highly trained rhesus monkeys p(t) = a0 + a1C + (b0 + b1C)(t),
in similar studies1,16. where (3)


t
[(0 + 1C]2
We occasionally held neurons long enough to do a passive-fixation 1
control experiment (Fig. 6). No saccade targets appeared on this block (t) = e 2[0 + 1C]2 d
2 (0 + 1C) -
of trials, and the monkey was rewarded simply for maintaining fixa-
tion. Random dot motion (51.2% coherence) appeared in the same
location as in the discrimination block and moved toward the RF or Notice that (t) is a cumulative Gaussian function of time, parame-
in one to seven directions away from the RF. terized by its mean and standard deviation: the mean determines the
position of the sigmoid from left to right, and the standard deviation
Data analysis. Responses were analyzed off-line using custom soft- determines its slope. We modeled the baseline values, a; the scaling
ware. To combine data from several neurons, we first normalized terms, b; and the sigmoid parameters, and , as linear functions of the
each neurons response using the mean spike rate in an epoch from motion strength, C. We fit equation 3 to the measured predictive indices
200800 ms after the onset of random dot motion. This epoch cor- using a simplex algorithm to find the maximum likelihood solution
responds to motion viewing, when the monkey decides the direc- (assuming binomially distributed error). The null hypothesis that
tion of motion and after the prominent transient response to the motion strength does not affect the shape of these sigmoid functions
onset of saccade targets. was tested by fitting the model with a1 = b1 = 1 = 1 = 0 and com-
The effect of motion strength on the neural response was estimat- paring likelihoods under the full and reduced models (likelihood ratio
ed using a linear regression model, Y = 0 + 1C + where Y is the test, df = 4)49. We refer to this method as a modified probit analysis.

184 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS discrimination from saccade programming in macaque frontal eye field.


We thank Melissa Mihali for animal training and technical support. We also J. Neurophysiol. 77, 10461050 (1997).
thank Joshua Gold, Greg Horwitz, Mark Mazurek, Bill Newsome, Jeff Schall 25. di Pellegrino, G. & Wise, S. Visuospatial versus visuomotor activity in the
premotor and prefrontal cortex of a primate. J. Neurosci. 13, 12271243
and Kirk Thompson for helpful suggestions on the manuscript. This research (1993).
was supported by RR00166, EY11378 and the McKnight Foundation. 26. Salinas, E. & Romo, R. Conversion of sensory signals into motor
commands in primary motor cortex. J. Neurosci. 18, 499512 (1998).
RECEIVED 24 AUGUST: ACCEPTED 23 DECEMBER 1998 27. Romo, R., Ruiz, S., Crespo, P., Zainos, A. & Merchant, H. Representation
of tactile signals in primate supplementary motor area. J. Neurophysiol.
70, 26902694 (1993).
1. Britten, K. H., Shadlen, M. N., Newsome, W. T. & Movshon, J. A. The 28. Shen, L. & Alexander, G. E. Neural correlates of a spatial sensory-to-
analysis of visual motion: a comparison of neuronal and psychophysical motor transformation in primary motor cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 77,
performance. J. Neurosci. 12, 47454765 (1992). 11711194 (1997).
2. Newsome, W. & Pare, E. A selective impairment of motion perception 29. Shen, L. & Alexander, G. E. Preferential representation of instructed
following lesions of the middle temporal visual area (MT). J. Neurosci. 8, target location versus limb trajectory in dorsal premotor area. J.
22012211 (1988). Neurophysiol. 77, 11951212 (1997).
3. Salzman, C. D., Murasugi, C. M., Britten, K. H. & Newsome, W. T. 30. Alexander, G. E. & Crutcher, M. D. Neural representations of the target
Microstimulation in visual area MT: Effects on direction discrimination (goal) of visually guided arm movements in three motor areas of the
performance. J. Neurosci. 12, 23312355 (1992). monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 64, 164178 (1990).
4. Celebrini, S. & Newsome, W. T. Neuronal and psychophysical sensitivity 31. Quintana, J. & Fuster, J. Mnemonic and predictive functions of cortical
to motion signals in extrastriate area MST of the macaque monkey. J. neurons in a memory task. Neuroreport 3, 721724 (1992).
Neurosci. 14, 41094124 (1994). 32. Watson, A. B. Probability summation over time. Vision Res. 19, 515522
5. Seidemann, E., Zohary, E. & Newsome, W. T. Temporal gating of neural (1979).
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

signals during performance of a visual discrimination task. Nature 394, 33. Shadlen, M. N. & Newsome, W. T. Motion perception: seeing and
7275 (1998). deciding. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 93, 628633 (1996).
6. Jacobsen, C. F. Functions of frontal association area in primates. Arch. 34. Newsome, W. T. Deciding about motion: linking perception to action. J.
Neurol. Psychiatry 33, 558569 (1935). Comp. Physiol. A 181, 512 (1997).
7. Fuster, J. The Prefrontal Cortex (Raven, New York, 1989). 35. Stanton, G. B., Bruce, C. J. & Goldberg, M. E. Topography of projections
8. Funahashi, S., Bruce, C. & Goldman-Rakic, P. Mnemonic coding of visual to posterior cortical areas from the macaque frontal eye fields. J. Comp.
space in the monkeys dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 61, Neurol. 353, 291305 (1995).
331349 (1989). 36. Lynch, J. C., Graybiel, A. M. & Lobeck, L. J. The differential projection of
9. Wilson, F. A., Scalaidhe, S. P. & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. Dissociation of two cytoarchitectonic subregions of the inferior parietal lobule of
object and spatial processing domains in primate prefrontal cortex. macaque upon the deep layers of the superior colliculus. J. Comp. Neurol.
Science 260, 19551958 (1993). 235, 241254 (1985).
10. Miller, E. K., Erickson, C. A. & Desimone, R. Neural mechanisms of visual 37. Huerta, M. F., Krubitzer, L. A. & Kaas, J. H. Frontal eye field as defined by
working memory in prefrontal cortex of the macaque. J. Neurosci. 16, intracortical microstimulation in squirrel monkeys, owl monkeys, and
51545167 (1996). macaque monkeys. II. Cortical connections. J. Comp. Neurol. 265,
11. Hasegawa, R., Sawaguchi, T. & Kubota, K. Monkey prefrontal neuronal 332361 (1987).
activity coding the forthcoming saccade in an oculomotor delayed 38. Schall, J. D., Morel, A., King, D. J. & Bullier, J. Topography of visual cortex
matching-to-sample task. J. Neurophysiol. 79, 322334 (1998). connections with frontal eye field in macaque: convergence and
12. Zhang, J., Riehle, A., Requin, J. & Kornblum, S. Dynamics of single segregation of processing streams. J. Neurosci. 15, 44644487 (1995).
neuron activity in monkey primary motor cortex related to sensorimotor 39. Friedman, H. & Goldman-Rakic, P. Coactivation of prefrontal cortex and
transformation. J. Neurosci. 17, 22272246 (1997). inferior parietal cortex in working memory tasks revealed by 2DG
13. Riehle, A., Kornblum, S. & Requin, J. Neuronal coding of stimulus- functional mapping in the rhesus monkey. J. Neurosci. 14, 27752788
response association rules in the motor cortex. Neuroreport 5, 24622464 (1994).
(1994). 40. Huerta, M. F., Krubitzer, L. A. & Kaas, J. H. Frontal eye field as defined by
14. Leon, M. I. & Shadlen, M. N. Exploring the neurophysiology of decisions. intracortical microstimulation in squirrel monkeys, owl monkeys, and
Neuron 21, 669672 (1998). macaque monkeys: I. Subcortical connections. J. Comp. Neurol. 253,
15. Press, W. H., Teukolsky, S. A., Vetterling, W. T. & Flannery, B. P. Numerical 415439 (1986).
Recipes in C. The Art of Scientific Computing (Cambridge Univ. Press, 41. Segraves, M. A. & Goldberg, M. E. Functional properties of corticotectal
Cambridge, 1992). neurons in the monkeys frontal eye field. J. Neurophysiol. 58, 13871419
16. Shadlen, M. N., Britten, K. H., Newsome, W. T. & Movshon, J. A. A (1987).
computational analysis of the relationship between neuronal and 42. Stanton, G. B., Goldberg, M. E. & Bruce, C. J. Frontal eye field efferents in
behavioral responses to visual motion. J. Neurosci. 16, 14861510 (1996). the macaque monkey: I. Subcortical pathways and topography of striatal
17. Britten, K. H., Shadlen, M. N., Newsome, W. T. & Movshon, J. A. and thalamic terminal fields. J. Comp. Neurol. 271, 473492 (1988).
Responses of neurons in macaque MT to stochastic motion signals. Vis. 43. Hays, A. V., Richmond, B. J. & Optican, L. M. A UNIX-based multiple
Neurosci. 10, 11571169 (1993). process system for real-time data acquisition and control. WESCON Conf.
18. Goodale, M. A. Visuomotor control: Where does vision end and action Proc. 2, 110 (1982).
begin? Curr. Biol. 8, R489491 (1998). 44. Bruce, C. J., Goldberg, M. E., Bushnell, M. C. & Stanton, G. B. Primate
19. Goldman-Rakic, P. in Handbook of Physiology, Sec I, The Nervous System frontal eye fields. II. Physiological and anatomical correlates of
(ed. Plum, F.) 373417 (Am. Physiol. Soc., Bethesda, MD, 1987). electrically evoked eye movements. J. Neurophysiol. 54, 714734 (1985).
20. Funahashi, S., Bruce, C. & Goldman-Rakic, P. Dorsolateral prefrontal 45. Gnadt, J. W. & Andersen, R. A. Memory related motor planning activity
lesions and oculomotor delayed-response performance: evidence for in posterior parietal cortex of monkey. Exp. Brain Res. 70, 216220
mnemonic scotomas. J. Neurosci. 13, 14791497 (1993). (1988).
21. Commenges, D., Seal, J. & Pinatel, F. Inference about a change point in 46. Hikosaka, O., Sakamoto, M. & Usui, S. Functional properties of monkey
experimental neurophysiology. Math. Biosci. 80, 81108 (1986). caudate neurons. III. Activities related to expectation of target and
22. Seal, J. & Commenges, D. A quantitative analysis of stimulus- and reward. J. Neurophysiol. 61, 814832 (1989).
movement-related responses in the posterior parietal cortex of the 47. Draper, N. & Smith, H. Applied Regresion Analysis, Second Edition (Wiley,
monkey. Exp. Brain Res. 58, 144153 (1985). New York, 1966).
23. Seal, J. Sensory and motor functions of the superior parietal cortex of the 48. Green, D. M. & Swets, J. A. Signal Detection Theory and Psychophysics
monkey as revealed by single-neuron recordings. Brain Behav. Evol. 33, (Wiley, New York, 1966).
113117 (1989). 49. Mood, A. M., Grabill, F. A. & Boes, D. C. Introduction to the Theory of
24. Thompson, K. G., Bichot, N. P. & Schall, J. D. Dissociation of visual Statistics (McGraw-Hill, New York, 1963).

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 185


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Weighted combination of size and


disparity: a computational model
for timing a ball catch
Simon K. Rushton1 and John P. Wann2

1 Department of Psychology, University of Edinburgh, 7 George Square, Edinburgh EH8 9JZ, UK


2 Department of Psychology, University of Reading, 3 Earley Gate, Reading RG6 6AL, UK
Correspondence should be addressed to J.P.W. (J.P.Wann@reading.ac.uk)

How do we time hand closure to catch a ball? Binocular disparity and optical looming provide two
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

sources of information about an objects motion in depth, but the relative effectiveness of the two
cues depends on ball size. Based on results from a virtual reality ball-catching task, we derive a
simple model that uses both cues. The model is sensitive to the relative effectiveness of size and dis-
parity and implicitly switches its response to the cue that specifies the earliest arrival and away from
a cue that is lost or below threshold. We demonstrate the models robustness by predicting the
response of participants to some very unusual ball trajectories in a virtual reality task.

The time to contact (TTC) of an approaching object can be approx- in the specification of binocular TTC5,8,9. The finding that a sen-
imated by its instantaneous distance and velocity. The simplicity of sation of motion in depth requires a background reference at a
this mathematical solution, however, does not reflect the scale of the fixed optic disparity10 suggests that observers can only perceive
problem involved in a neural or robotic implementation of TTC changing relative disparity (), but there is conflicting evidence
estimation. The use of distance and velocity requires reconstruction that a change in binocular subtense () can influence TTC judg-
of a three-dimensional representation of the environment, which is ments5. Psychophysical motion-in-depth tasks10 require a declar-
nontrivial and demanding. Survival in the forest, on the sports field ative judgment, rather than a motor response to an impending
or in the robotics lab requires that TTC be estimated very quickly collision5, and perceptual judgments may be processed through
and without undue call on cognitive resources. To operate as fast as a different subsystem from perception for action11. We therefore
possible, therefore, a TTC mechanism may rely upon a simple sparse, consider the issue regarding the use of in TTC estimation to be
or even crude, abstraction of the visual environment1. In some set- unresolved. We use when outlining the specification of TTC
tings, specific cues may also be lost or fall below the threshold for and discuss as a potential route to estimating TTC.
detection2. People seem to cope well with a range of environmental Under natural circumstances, the distinction between and
conditions, which argues for a robust mechanism for TTC estimation may be irrelevant, as most environments provide many poten-
that degrades gracefully in cases of cue conflict or cue loss. tial reference features, and with a distant reference . Yet given
Optical size and binocular disparity are both functions of the a reference object at 10 m, then for a ball travelling at 5 m/s, the
distance of a ball from the observer, so either could provide esti- error in using /(d/dt) as a TTC estimate is 45 ms (Appendix).
mates of TTC36. Many studies of interceptive timing have ignored An error of this size may thwart one-handed catching12, which
the role of disparity, addressing only the role of changing retinal demonstrates that cue conflict may be commonplace if the observ-
size (looming). Evidence for the exclusive use of retinal looming in er uses relative disparity along with changing size. A related prob-
natural TTC judgment, however, is equivocal7, and it has been lem in the use of binocular disparity is that a reference point for
proposed that changing disparity may be more salient than chang- the detection of relative disparity may be lost during flight (for
ing size for a fast-moving cricket ball2 . As a ball moves in depth, example, when the ball is on a high parabolic arc). The issues of
the rate of looming (d/dt) relative to changing disparity (d/dt or cue conflict and cue loss have not been tackled in previous
d/dt; Fig. 1) is a function of object size (R), relative to inter- accounts of TTC judgments, and so we examined how human
pupillary distance (I), independent of viewing distance2 : observers perform when there are differences in the TTC infor-
R/I = (d/dt)/(d/dt) (1) mation provided by changing size and disparity and how they
cope when cues drop out. As noted above, there may be problems
The salience of either input therefore depends on the size of the in extrapolating from perceptual judgments in psychophysical
object and on the individuals sensitivity to looming or disparity. tasks, with limited viewing periods and ocular fixation require-
Hence a robust TTC system should take account of both chang- ments, to active control in naturalistic settings13. Thus we stayed
ing size and changing disparity inputs and ideally be sensitive to within the bounds of a naturalistic task, but retained psy-
their relative rates of change. chophysical precision, by presenting observers with the task of
An accurate geometric specification of TTC from disparity catching virtual tennis balls within a computer-generated envi-
requires the detection of the binocular angular subtense (; Fig 1). ronment. By dissociating looming and disparity cues, we demon-
There are precedents for considering as the optical parameter strate that the weighting that participants attach to either cue

186 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 1. Retinotopic and spatiotopic information for TTC judgment. A


ball of diameter R travels at a constant velocity V toward an observer
who has an interpupilary distance I. Time to contact with the eye is
specified spatiotopically by TTC = D/V. For D >> R and D >> I, optic
size R/D and I/D, where is the binocular subtense relative to a
parallel optic axis (dotted). Their derivatives can be approximated by
(d/dt) RV/D2 and (d/dt) IV/D2. Given a reference object at a dis-
tance D + D, relative retinal disparity can be approximated by
ID/D(D + D), the derivative of which is identical to that of :
(d/dt) IV/D2. This yields four possibilities for estimating TTC without
recourse to an estimate of D or V: /(d/dt), /(d/dt), /(d/dt) or
TTC /(d/dt) if DR is large (see Appendix).
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

depends on which specifies the shortest TTC. We present a model the two modular estimates (TTCsize and TTCdisp):
that accounts for this behavior and explains why timing errors in
TTC = 1 TTCsize + 2TTCdisp (2)
catching may be minor even in cases of cue conflict or cue loss.
This weighting system would be robust to cue loss and could
RESULTS account for the results of Experiment 1 if 1 and 2 were dynam-
In Experiment 1, we asked observers to catch virtual tennis balls ically revalued using cross ratios of the modular inputs:
with no constraints on their eye movements. Observers were able
1 = TTCdisp / (TTCsize + TTCdisp)
to catch virtual balls accurately. Their grasp response in the con-
2 = TTCsize / (TTCsize + TTCdisp) (3)
trol condition (Condition A) was initiated 146 ms before the ball
arrived (standard error, 15 ms) and completed 22 ms after the This could be considered a variant of weak fusion cue com-
ball arrived (standard error, 14 ms). This accuracy is equivalent to bination14.
that required in natural catching12. We then introduced subtle The third model does not calculate TTC separately for each
differences in the TTC information provided by changing size or input, but combines instantaneous size and disparity and uses
changing disparity ( 100 ms) to assess the weight that partici- the summed parameter to calculate TTC. There is psychophysi-
pants attached to each cue. When looming indicated a different
TTC from disparity, observers modified their grasp time, but the
degree of modification varied across conditions. The earlier, or
B: TTC looming is 100 ms early
more immediate, estimate of arrival had the greater influence
Response shift relative to control trials (ms)

C: TTC looming is 100 ms late


on grasp timing (Fig. 2). Hence looming was the dominant cue
when it specified a TTC 100 ms earlier than disparity (Condi-
tion B), whereas disparity had the most influence when looming
specified a later TTC (Condition C). The results demonstrate
that both optical size and disparity are used in catching, but the
respective influence or weighting of size and disparity is not fixed.

TTC models
To account for these findings, we needed to derive a model that
uses both size and disparity and biases its response toward the
more immediate cue. We identified three potential models that
combine optic size, disparity and their respective rates of
change to estimate TTC (see supplementary figure at http://neu-
rosci.nature.com/web_specials/supp_info/nn0299_186/). The first
model is a fixed weighting modular summation, in which two
separate estimates of TTC are derived, one from size and its rate of Initiation Completion
change, and the other from disparity and its rate of change. The
two estimates are then combined according to some weighting
factor , where = 1 would reduce the model to one that relied Fig. 2. Changes in the timing of hand grasp when catching virtual balls.
exclusively on size3 and = 0 would use only disparity. Although Looming was scaled to provide a TTC estimate 100 ms earlier (Condition
this model includes both size and disparity, its fixed weighting B) or 100 ms later (Condition C) than the binocular TTC information.
The TTC at which the grasp response was initiated and completed in
precludes it from biasing its TTC estimate toward the most imme-
Conditions B and C were compared with nested control trials (Condition
diate cue, and so it cannot account for the flexible response A), in which both TTC cues were commensurate. The unsigned change in
demonstrated in our data. It also predicts a large error in the case grasp time was significantly greater in Condition B (gain for looming, 0.78)
of sudden cue loss. than in Condition C (gain for looming, 0.41) for both initiation and com-
The second model, a variant of the first, introduces changes in pletion (t5 = 3.51, p < 0.01; t5 = 3.34, p < 0.01, one-tailed test, respec-
the weighting on the fly based on the relative immediacy of tively). Error bars are 95% confidence limits for each mean.

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 187


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Fig. 3. Predictions from the dipole model and a modular


model for the plateau conditions of Experiment 2. Solid
lines indicate the potential inputs from changing size and
changing disparity, in which input_1 is veridical and
input_2 indicates a constant TTC of 750 ms. The modu-

Time to contact estimate (ms)


lar estimate is based on a 50:50 weighting of both inputs,
whereas the dipole ( estimate; filled circles) shows an
increasing bias toward the most immediate input. If the
desired response time was 250 ms TTC, then the dipole
would predict a response delay of 70 ms, whereas the
modular model would predict a response well after the
ball had arrived. The error in using relative disparity (;
open circles), with a reference point at 4 m, is minor if
the planned interception is at a short TTC.

Actual time to contact (ms)


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

cal evidence2 for the early combination of size and disparity The retinotopic quantities ( + ) rise nonlinearly as the object
motion signals (d/dt + d/dt), and neurophysiological evidence approaches (d/dt, d/dt 1/D2). As a result, the cue that specifies
for the combination of optic size and disparity ( + ) at an early the earlier arrival has a disproportionate influence. For a nonde-
stage of visual processing15. A TTC estimate can be based on a formable object, with a distant reference for binocular motion, Equa-
ratio of these combined inputs: tion 4 is equivalent to the following:
TTCdipole = ( + ) / (d/dt + d/dt) (4) TTCdipole = (TTCsize + TTCdisp [I/R])/ (1 + [I/R]) (5)
We adopt the label dipole from theory on texture perception16 . A Hence when the diameter of the object equals the interpupilary
single point viewed by two eyes specifies a binocular dipole, and two distance (I = R), the dipole model bases its estimate on an equal
points (such as two opposite edges of an object) viewed by one eye weighting of both inputs, but for a football, the implicit bias would
specify a monocular dipole. Our model sums dipoles and does not be toward an estimate based on TTCsize and for a table-tennis ball
distinguish their origin. An alternative means of estimating the ratio toward TTCdisp. If one input is lost ( = 0 or = 0), then the esti-
in Equation 4 is to take the change in the summed dipole length mate relies upon the remaining single input (Eq. 4). If one rate of
within a logarithmic coordinate system: TTCdipole = d[ln ( + )]/dt. change is below threshold, the model predicts a relatively small
response error (Appendix; Eq. 6). If both cues are above
threshold but indicate different TTC, then the dipole model
Temporal error with looming TTC plateau is 750 ms biases its estimate toward the more immediate cue, equiva-
Temporal error with looming binocular TTC plateau is 750 ms
lent to the explicit weighting of Equation 3. For the cue
Dipole model prediction
Modular model prediction (50:50 weighting)
manipulation used in Experiment 1, the dipole model pre-
dicts temporal shifts of 70 ms when size specifies an ear-
lier arrival and +34 ms when size specifies a later arrival,
Error in completion of grasp (ms)

which is in good agreement with the results in Fig. 2.

Experiment 2
Because the dipole model was designed to account for the
findings of Experiment 1, we wished to test its robustness
under different input conditions. We have outlined that the
model can cope with complete loss of one cue or a static state
for one input. The stalling of one TTC estimate, however,
would predict a specific response delay. In Experiment 2, we
computationally scaled either the optical size or the dispar-
ity so that they produced a TTC plateau. In one condition,
changing size presented a normal decreasing TTC estimate,
but disparity indicated a constant TTC, whereas the recip-
Participant rocal condition produced decreasing TTC for disparity and
constant TTC for size (Fig. 3). In both cases, the perturba-
Fig. 4. Errors in grasp timing for the ball-catching task in Experiment 2. In the tions did not freeze disparity or size. Optic size and dispar-
plateau trials, optic size was scaled to keep TTC calculated from size constant at ity continued to change, but their temporal derivatives were
750 ms, while TTC calculated from disparity continued to decrease in line with
decreased at a rate that kept TTC constant. How might an
the control, or else TTC calculated from disparity was kept constant at 750 ms,
while TTC calculated from size continued to decrease in line with the control tri- observer cope with the strange situation in which one cue
als (see Fig. 3). Predictions from the dipole model and the fixed weighting model indicates that the ball will arrive soon, and the other cue
were produced by using the response time for the control trials for each partici- indicates that it is moving forward but will never arrive? Par-
pant to estimate when that TTC would be achieved for each model. ticipants made only very small TTC errors (Fig. 4), which

188 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Box 1. Mathematical appendix.

Using the symbols defined in Fig. 1, = optical size, = binocular D = I (d/d)/[(d/dt)] (8)
subtense and = relative disparity for an approaching ball. However, substituting Eq. 8 into Eq. 7 reduces this to /(d/dt). If
the observer tracked the ball, an independent estimate of D might
(1) Behavior of the dipole model for scale changes and cue loss be recovered from ocular vergence (I/), but it would then be more
The geometric specification of TTC in the dipole model is based expedient to use TTC = /(d/dt). Observers can estimate TTC
on Eq. 4: to within 310% (51270 ms) from changing disparity when ocu-
TTCdipole = ( + ) / [(d/dt) + (d/dt)] (4) lar vergence is fixed and size cues to D are removed6. This suggests
that TTC judgments must be based on /(d/dt) or that is recov-
This can be rewritten for the case where (d/dt) > 0 and
ered from the combination of and vergence angle. Because d/dt
(d/dt) > 0 to demonstrate that the model biases its estimate
and d/dt have geometric equivalence (Fig 1), the issue is the use
toward whichever expansion pattern is most salient:
of in place of in estimating TTC. Given ID/D(D+D),
TTCdipole = then an estimate of the actual TTC will be biased:
(d / dt) TTC (d / dt) + TTC /(d/dt) = TTC(D/(D+D) = TTC TTC(D /DR)
TTCsize + TTCdisp size (d / dt)
(d / dt) disp
= The error is proportional to the distance of the reference mark
(d / dt) (d / dt)
1+ 1+ and speed of the ball:
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

(d / dt) (d / dt)
TTCerror = TTC D/ DR = TTC2 V/ DR (9)
TTCsize is the collision time estimate arising from changing size, In the dipole model (Eq. 4), this error is partially offset by the
and TTCdisp is the collision time estimate if only binocular infor- changing size cue:
mation is used. Substituting from Eq. 4 gives an approximation
for the ratio of object size R to interpupilary distance I: TTCdipole_error = [ TTC (I/R) ( D /DR)] /( 1 + [I/R] ) (10)
For our case of a cricket or tennis ball where R I this simplifies to
TTCdipole = (TTCsize + TTCdisp [I/R]) / (1 + [I/R]) (5)
If one input is lost, then Eq. 4 reduces to a single-input estimate TTCdipole_error = 0.5 TTC D/ DR = 0.5 TTC2 V/ DR (11)
of /(d/dt) or /(d/dt). It may be the case, however, that one Hence early in the approach, Eq. 11 specifies an error of early
of the derivatives is below threshold (e.g. for a small object when arrival, then during approach the error is reduced as a function of
d/dt is lost). Then TTC = for that input, and this will intro- TTC2. If the final acquisition of visual information for catching is
duce an error term: 300 ms before contact6,12, then for a ball travelling at 5 m/s with
DR of 10 m, the error would be 22 ms. If more distant reference
TTCdipole = /(d/dt) + /( d/dt)
points were available, the error would be negligible. The pre-
The error is then proportional to the ratio of two angular subtenses: dicted timing error for the conditions of Experiment 1 is approx-
TTCsizeTTCdisp (1 + / ) imately 18 ms early. In summary, relative disparity can provide
TTCdipole = (6)
TTCsize + TTCdisp ( / ) a useful estimate of TTC, provided the final adjustments of the
interceptive action are late in the ball trajectory. The dipole model
Given similar relative thresholds for motion detection, errors also reduces the error in using relative disparity through the
should only occur when << or << ; hence the dipole weighting of changing size.
model predicts a minor error in natural settings. In experimen-
tal conditions, size or disparity of a target may be held fixed, (3) Moving away from the eye
which then presents a TTC input of infinity. The dipole error Derivations of TTC estimates in this and previous papers36 only
would then depend on the ratio of static to moving angular sub- hold for collision with the plane of the eye. To catch the ball some
tense. TTC errors occur when TTCsize and TTCdisp are placed in distance Dhand in front of the head, the catcher must estimate the
conflict at 4 s or 8 s, reciprocally5. The dipole estimate would be offset:
a TTC of 5.3 s, which is similar to the judgments recorded. Sim-
ilar TTC errors occur when is static and only disparity changes6. TTChand = TTCeye TTCeye (Dhand/Dball)
This can be approximated by using the disparities of the hand
(2) Using relative disparity to estimate TTC and ball relative to a reference object, which converges on a veridi-
It has been proposed6 that observers could base their judgments cal estimate of TTChand as the ball approaches:
of TTC on relative disparity:
TTChand = TTCeye (1 ball/hand )
TTC = I / [D(d/dt)] (7) Balls with lateral motion may be caught eccentric to the head
There are very few cues to absolute distance (D) of an object in by combining optical expansion with the optical gap between
flight, however, and the estimation of D is likely to be difficult17. the ball and its future catching location 18. Using the dipole
We note that for small optical angles, the distance D of the object model in place of the optical expansion makes this solution more
could be estimated from retinotopic angles: robust for small objects.

means they must have switched the weighting they placed on loom- parity (Fig 4). Stimulus-dependent switching of this type is a basic
ing or disparity from one trial to the next. Observers seemed to feature of the dipole model, and the predictions of the model are
rely almost exclusively on looming information when there was a remarkably similar to the mean tendency across participants. When
plateau in TTC from disparity. In contrast, if the next ball had a both inputs were scaled to a similar plateau, observers simply saw
TTC plateau on looming, they switched their TTC estimate to dis- a ball slowing down, and there was a long delay in their responses.

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 189


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

DISCUSSION cm. D, V and R were randomly varied by 10% across trials.


Catching a moving ball presents a difficult problem in visual per- In Experiment 1, the HMD was fully immersive, and the ball flew
ception, in which a precise estimate of arrival (TTC) must be down a stereoscopic 5-m corridor toward the face of the observer. Naive
gleaned from binocular and monocular looming information. participants (n = 6) received 20 practice trials. A set of 20 control trials
Binocular information is likely to be most effective for small balls, (Condition A) were then interleaved with 20 trials where looming was
computationally scaled to provide a TTC estimate 100 ms earlier (Con-
and looming most effective for larger objects (Eq. 1). The dipole
dition B) or 100 ms later (Condition C) than that specified by binocu-
TTC model implicitly compensates for changes in the relative effec- lar disparity. A virtual image of the moving hand was not available to
tiveness of looming or binocular motion as a function of object participants to avoid intertrial learning.
size (Eq. 5). Detection of relative disparity () for a ball in flight, In Experiment 2, the HMD was see-through and allowed the image
however, requires an environmental reference feature, which may of the ball to be overlaid onto the natural world, which contained edges
be lost for a rising ball, resulting in cue dropout. Relative dispari- at a range of depths. The screen edges of the display were also fusible at
ty may kick in once again if the ball falls and a background refer- approximately 4 m, for someone with an interpupillary distance of 65
ence becomes visible. The dipole model copes with cue dropout mm. The system was calibrated to individual participants using a static
by implicitly switching to the remaining cue, without any explicit image of the ball at three different distances. Naive participants (n = 5)
held their hands next to their cheeks, occluded from their vision, and
evaluation procedure, and switching back to dual weighting if both
received 20 practice trials as in Experiment 1. A set of 20 control trials
cues become available. Finally, if the binocular reference object is were then interleaved with plateau trials, in which the ball moved iden-
only a few meters from the observer, such as a tennis net, then the tically to the control trials until it was 1.5 m from the participant and
TTC estimate from disparity will differ from the TTC estimate
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

then either optic size was scaled to keep TTCsize constant at 750 ms or
from looming. The dipole model can attenuate this error, reducing disparity was scaled to keep TTCdisp constant at 750 ms while the other
the 45 ms cited for the example in the introduction to a more man- input continued to decrease in line with the control trials.
ageable 22 ms (Appendix, Eq. 1214). Our model copes with cue Note: Schematic diagrams of the models discussed may be found on the
conflicts that may arise from biased estimates by implicitly weight- Nature Neuroscience web site at http://neurosci.nature.com/web-
ing its response toward the most immediate cue, which indicates _specials/supp_ingo/nn0299_186/.
the shortest TTC. Cue switching and immediacy bias are both
attractive features for a robust TTC system where, in an ecologi- ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
cal context, responding early is often preferable to responding late. We thank Anna Plooy and Martin Smyth for their input to the second
We tested the response of human participants to small differ- experiment and also Julie Harris, Mike Harris and Graham Schafer for incisive
ences in the TTC inputs arising from changing disparity and chang- comments. This research was supported by the UK EPSRC GR/L18693 and
ing size and found that they did indeed bias their catching response GR/L16125.
toward the more immediate cue. We then presented the challenging
task of catching balls where one of the TTC inputs stalled, but sur- RECEIVED 17 AUGUST; ACCEPTED 10 DECEMBER 1998
prisingly found that participants did not make the mistakes that
would be predicted from a fixed weighting of changing size and dis- 1. Sun, H. & Frost, B. J. Computation of different optical variables of looming
parity. In line with the dipole model, participants seemed to switch objects in pigeon nucleus rotundus neurons. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 296303 (1998).
weighting from trial to trial toward the most immediate input. The 2. Regan, D. & Beverley, K. I. Binocular and monocular stimuli for motion in depth:
Changing-disparity and changing-size feed the same motion in depth
model is also compatible with previous findings on cue conflict5. stage.Vision Res. 19, 13311342 (1979).
The dipole model allows for the influence of both optic size 3. Lee, D. N. A theory of visual control of braking based on information about time-
and disparity on TTC judgments. Cue switching is an implicit to-collision. Perception 5, 437459 (1976).
4. Regan, D. & Hamstra, S. J. Dissociation of discrimination thresholds for time to
feature of its basic architecture, and as such the model provides a contact and for rate of angular expansion.Vision Res. 33, 447462 (1993).
simple appealing account of adaptive behavior, particularly with 5. Heuer, H. Estimates of time to contact based on changing size and changing
respect to cue loss. This behavior results from the combination target vergence. Perception 22, 549563 (1993).
6. Gray, R. & Regan, D. Accuracy of estimating time to collision using binocular and
of retinal cues before TTC estimation and hence avoids the com- monocular information.Vision Res. 38, 499512 (1997).
putational overhead of deriving modular estimates for each input, 7. Wann, J. P. Anticipating arrival: is the tau-margin a specious theory? J. Exp
comparing them and then weighting them appropriately (for Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 22, 10311048 (1996).
8. Tresilian, J. R. Perceptual and motor processes in interceptive timing Hum. Mov.
example, Eq. 2, 3). It is possible to design more cumbersome Sci. 13, 335373 (1994).
modular architectures that would achieve the same ends, but par- 9. Laurent, M., Montagne, G. & Durey, A. Binocular invariants in interception
tasks: a directed perception approach. Perception 25, 14371450 (1996).
simony favors the simplicity and efficiency of the dipole model. 10. Regan, D., Erkelens C. J. & Collewijn, H. Necessary conditions for the perception
of motion in depth. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vision Sci. 27, 584597 (1986).
11. Milner, A. D. & Goodale, M. A. The Visual Brain in Action (Oxford Univ. Press,
METHODS Oxford, 1996).
Displays. Observers wore a head-mounted display (HMD) that placed a liq- 12. Alderson, G. J. K., Sully, D. J. & Sully, H. G. An operational analysis of a one-
uid crystal screen and magnifying optics in front of each eye to project stereo- handed catching task using high-speed photography. J. Motor Behav. 6, 217226
scopic images onto an image plane. In Experiment 1, horizontal display (1974).
13. Tresilian, J. R. Perceptual and cognitive processes in time-to-contact estimation:
resolution was 220 color pixels over 60 degrees of visual angle per eye, and in Analysis of prediction-motion and relative judgment tasks. Percept. Psychophys.
Experiment 2, horizontal resolution was 263 color pixels over 30 degrees of 57, 231245 (1995).
visual angle per eye. The vertical field of view was 45 and 23 degrees respec- 14. Landy, M. S., Maloney, L. T., Johnston, E. B. & Young, M. J. Measurement and
tively. In both cases, display update was >24 frames per second. modeling of depth cue combination: In defense of weak fusion. Vision Res. 35,
389412 (1995).
15. Dobbins, A. C., Jeo, R. M., Fiser, J. & Allman, J. M. Distance modulation of neural
Procedure. Participants began each trial with their hands open by their activity in visual cortex. Science 281, 552555 (1998).
faces, with a real tennis ball attached to their palms. A virtual stereoscop- 16. Julesz, B. Textons, the elements of texture perception and their interactions.
ic textured tennis ball approached them, and they grasped onto the real Nature 290, 9197 (1981).
tennis balls attached to their hands when they thought that the virtual 17. Regan, D. Visual judgements and misjudgements in cricket, and the art of flight.
Perception 21, 91115 (1992).
ball would hit them. Finger and thumb flexion was recorded at 200 Hz 18. Peper, C. L., Bootsma, R. J., Mestre, D. R. & Bakker, F. C. Catching balls: How to
during the flight of the virtual ball. In both experiments, the mean start- get the hand to the right place at the right time. J. Exp Psychol. Hum. Percept.
ing depth D was 4 m, speed V was 2 m/s and initial ball diameter R was 7 Perform. 20, 591612 (1994).

190 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Brain potentials indicate immediate


use of prosodic cues in natural
speech processing
Karsten Steinhauer, Kai Alter and Angela D. Friederici

Department of Neuropsychology, Max Planck Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, PO Box 500 355, D-04303 Leipzig, Germany
Correspondence should be addressed to K.S. (steinhau@cns.mpg.de)

Spoken language, in contrast to written text, provides prosodic information such as rhythm, pauses,
accents, amplitude and pitch variations. However, little is known about when and how these
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

features are used by the listener to interpret the speech signal. Here we use event-related brain
potentials (ERP) to demonstrate that intonational phrasing guides the initial analysis of sentence
structure. Our finding of a positive shift in the ERP at intonational phrase boundaries suggests a spe-
cific on-line brain response to prosodic processing. Additional ERP components indicate that a false
prosodic boundary is sufficient to mislead the listeners sentence processor. Thus, the application of
ERP measures is a promising approach for revealing the time course and neural basis of prosodic
information processing.

Human everyday communication is spoken rather than written, grammatical relations has been accomplished. Psycholinguists
but the overwhelming majority of psycholinguistic research have long debated whether or not the initial syntactic preferences
underlying models of sentence processing is still based on read- can be immediately overridden by non-syntactic influences such
ing rather than speech-processing data. One reason for this asym- as semantic or pragmatic cues6,7. When non-syntactic influences
metry is that written text, as compared to spoken language, can be have been found, the temporal immediacy of these effects has
much more easily controlled in experimental design. Natural been controversial5. When sentences such as 1a and 1b are pre-
speech is always contaminated with prosodic features, which sented auditorily, the initial preference in favor of 1a is consid-
increase word-length variability and introduce pitch and ampli- erably diminished by a prosodic boundary after the first verb2.
tude variations; these variables can be completely avoided with This finding suggests that prosodic information does influence
written text. Prosodic features, however, may be important for decisions about syntactic structure at very early stages.
communication, and some studies on spoken language suggest However, little is known about the exact relationship between
that prosodic information may influence sentence processing to prosody and sentence processing, and in particular the time
such a degree that preferences observed during reading may not course and neural basis of influences such as the one just
apply equally to speech processing13. For example, eye-tracking described8,9. Event-related brain potentials (ERPs) are a useful
measures in a reading task indicate4 that sentence 1a is much eas- tool for the on-line examination of both normal and impaired
ier to understand than sentence 1b: language processing10,11, which may contribute to the develop-
ment of theoretical accounts of prosodic and syntactic interac-
(1a) Since Jay always jogs a mile and a half this seems like a tion. The ERP correlates of initial syntactic misanalyses for both
short distance to him. written and spoken presentation show that the additional costs
(1b) Since Jay always jogs a mile and a half seems like a very are reflected by a late posterior positivity between 500 and 1200
short distance to him. ms (termed the P600 component or syntactic positive shift)1215.
In contrast, difficulties of lexical and semantic processing (for
Initially, the reader prefers to interpret the noun phrase a mile example, He spread the warm bread with socks) generally elic-
and a half as the grammatical object of the preceding verb jogs it an earlier centroparietal negativity between 300 and 900 ms
rather than as the subject of the subsequent verb seems, as (the N400 component)1619 rather than a P600. These two class-
required in 1b. Thus the initial analysis must be revised in 1b, es of ERP effects indicate specific brain responses to different lin-
which results in a prolonged reading time, a phenomenon called guistic features. Here we used ERP measures for the on-line
the garden-path effect. investigation of prosodic features.
The robustness of such effects in reading studies led to the We examined the influence of prosody on human parsing per-
development of the garden-path model of sentence processing4. formance with both behavioral and neurophysiological (ERP)
According to this model, initial preferences are exclusively attrib- measures. The stimulus material consisted of spoken German
uted to the inherent processing principles of an encapsulated syn- sentence pairs similar to the English examples in 1a and 1b. To
tactic processing system (called syntactic parser), which cannot be establish controlled experimental conditions comparable to those
influenced by non-syntactic information5. Semantic interpreta- in reading studies, we needed exhaustive acoustic analyses of the
tion (or meaning) occurs only once the syntactic parsing of speech signals. These analyses revealed systematic prosodic dif-

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 191


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Experiment 1 Experiment 2
a b

(n = 20)
(n = 20)

Fig. 1. Prosodysyntax mismatch. ERPs of conditions B (blue) and C (red) at nine electrodes from the onset of the infinitive marker zu of the crit-
ical second verb until two seconds later, displayed separately for each experiment. Negative amplitudes are plotted upwards. (a) In Experiment 1
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

(n = 20), the prosodysyntax mismatch condition C is characterized by an N400 component followed by a P600. (b) In Experiment 2 (n = 20), the
same biphasic N400P600 pattern as in Experiment 1 was elicited in condition C.

ferences between the two conditions, suggesting a specific prosod- Our hypothesis was that these early prosodic differences of
ic phrasing and accentuation pattern dependent on the syntac- spoken language could be sufficient to prevent the garden-path
tic structure. When the sentences were presented auditorily, the effect in 2b. If the additional IPh boundary indeed changes the
listeners event-related brain potentials reproducibly showed a initial interpretation of Anna, then it should even be possible
characteristic positive-going waveform at prosodic phrase bound- to reverse the garden-path effect. That is, if the early prosodic
aries, indicating immediate decoding of this information. Fur- cues of 2b were introduced in sentence 2a, we expected an initial
thermore, in a third condition, we introduced the initial prosodic misanalysis and garden-path effect in this normally easy-to-
features of one condition into the other condition, leading to a process structure. The noun phrase Anna would then be erro-
prosodysyntax mismatch. Both behavioral and ERP data strong- neously attached to the second verb, which is intransitive and
ly suggest that the prosodic features determined the initial pars- cannot take a direct object as its argument. Using a cross-splicing
ing decisions. The mismatch between prosody and syntax was technique23, we merged the acoustic signals of the first part of
reliably detected by the listeners and elicited an N400P600 pat- 2b and the second part of 2a between Anna and the infinitive
tern of ERP components reflecting a prosody-induced garden- marker zu (to) of the second verb in each of the 48 sentence
path effect. pairs. This resulted in a third condition (2c) with a mismatch

RESULTS
The German sentence material consisted of 48 sentence pairs
such as 2a and 2b, where the bracketing indicates the respective
intonational phrases (IPhs)20 as described below:

(2a) [Peter verspricht Anna zu arbeiten]IPh1 [und das Bro


zu putzen]IPh2
Peter promises Anna to work and to clean the office

(2b) [Peter verspricht]IPh1 [Anna zu entlasten]IPh2 [und das


Bro zu putzen]IPh3
Peter promises to support Anna and to clean the office

As in example 1a, the noun phrase Anna is the object of the


first verb verspricht (promises) in 2a and therefore belongs to
the first IPh. In contrast to 2a but similar to 1b, in sentence 2b
Anna is the object of the second transitive verb entlasten
(support) and belongs to the second IPh. Because of the Ger-
man word order, the correct interpretation of Anna is syntac- Fig. 2. Closure positive shift. Grand-average ERPs of both experi-
ments (n = 40) at the PZ electrode. The waveforms of conditions A
tically disambiguated by the second verb only (that is, arbeiten
(orange) and B (blue) are superimposed. The word onsets of the sen-
(work) in 2a versus entlasten in 2b). Compatible with the tence examples are aligned to the time axis. Both conditions evoke
predictions of certain theories of syntaxprosody mapping21,22, closure positive shifts at their respective IPh boundaries. Only one
however, the 48 speech signals of conditions 2a and 2b differ con- shift is observable in condition A, following the second verb
siderably even before the point of syntactic disambiguation, via arbeiten, whereas two such shifts occur in condition B, before
different intonational phrasing and accentuation (Methods). Anna and after the second verb entlasten.

192 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Experiment 1 Experiment 2
a b

(n = 20)
(n = 20)
(n = 20)

Fig. 3. Sentence-specific ERPs. ERPs of all three conditions at nine electrode sites from sentence onset until four seconds later, plotted separately for
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

each experiment. (a) In Experiment 1 (n = 20), conditions B (blue) and C (red) share the same pattern of two CPSs as opposed to one CPS in condi-
tion A (orange). By two seconds, that is, after onset of the second verb, the prosodysyntax mismatch condition C diverges from B and elicits the
N400P600 pattern. (b) Experiment 2 (n = 20) generally replicates the findings of Experiment 1.

between prosodic information (Methods) and syntactic con- time windows (from 200 to 1800 ms) confirmed that both effects
straints (that is, the intransitivity of the verb arbeiten): were restricted to centroparietal electrodes. The N400 effect was
significant between 400 and 1000 ms (p < 0.01) and the P600
(2c) * [Peter verspricht]IPh1 [Anna zu arbeiten]IPh2 [und das effect between 1200 and 1800 ms (p < 0.001). For both ERP com-
Bro zu putzen]IPh3 ponents, we observed only main effects of the sentence condition
Peter promises to work Anna and to clean the office (condition B versus C) and no further interactions with the task
factor. Baseline-independent, peak-to-peak measures including
The prosodic inappropriateness of 2c becomes obvious only when condition A ruled out the possibility that the effects were simply
the intransitive verb arbeiten is encountered. At this point, the due to the different verbs (transitive verbs in B versus intransi-
sentence should initially be perceived as Peter promises to work tive verbs in C). Although condition A contained the same intran-
Anna, which is certainly ungrammatical and requires revision. sitive verbs as condition C, the amplitude difference between the
According to linguistic convention, ungrammatical sentences are N400 peak and the P600 peak in condition A was significantly
marked by an asterisk.
Forty-eight sentences of each of the three conditions were pre-
sented to 40 subjects in two ERP experiments varying in their a b
task requirements: comprehension task (Experiment 1) and
prosody judgment plus comprehension task (Experiment 2). The
prosody acceptability judgment revealed that the participants
reliably detected the prosodysyntax mismatch in condition C.
In this condition, only 6% of the trials were rated as acceptable,
as opposed to more than 80% in both conditions 2a and 2b
(p < 0.0001). The error rates in the comprehension task were not
significantly increased in the mismatch condition (C) for either
Experiment 1 or 2.
We focused on two questions: first, whether and how ERPs
reflect the influence of prosodic information on early syntac-
tic processes, and second, whether ERPs are sensitive to the
processing of prosodic features per se. We briefly discuss the
effect indicating the interplay between syntax and prosody and
then turn to a newly identified prosody-related ERP compo- B (n = 16)
nent. As Experiments 1 and 2 had very similar results, they C (n = 15)
are presented jointly.
Fig. 4. ERPs after pause removal. ERPs of conditions B (blue) and C
The syntaxprosody mismatch effect (red) in Experiment 3 (n = 16) at the CZ and PZ electrode sites.
(a) Even after removal of the pause between the first verb and the sec-
In reading studies using ERPs, violations of a verbs argument
ond noun phrase, both conditions still display the CPS at the first IPh
structure elicit an N400 component followed by a P600 compo- boundary. (b) As in the first two experiments, the intransitive verb of
nent14,24. As predicted, we found a similar biphasic N400P600 the prosodysyntax mismatch condition C elicits a biphasic
sequence during the intransitive second verb for condition C as N400P600 pattern. This comparison is more reliable than that in Fig.
compared to the grammatically correct transitive verb in condition 4a, as the average window is aligned to the critical verb and more trials
B (Fig. 1). Mean amplitude analyses across 8 consecutive 200-ms entered the averages.

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 193


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

Word and pause durations Fundamental frequency (FO)


a b

Fig. 5. Prosodic parameters. Prosodic differences between the speech signals of conditions A (orange lines) and B (blue lines). (a) Duration measures
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

of sentence fragments and pauses (#) and respective differences between conditions B and A. Condition B shows both a lengthening of the first sen-
tence fragment Peter verspricht (p < 0.0001) and a subsequent pause insertion (#1; p < 0.0001). (b) Fundamental frequency. Whereas the main
pitch accent in condition A is on the verb arbeiten, it is aligned to the noun phrase Anna in condition B. Because of the lengthening in B, however,
both accents occur at approximately the same time.

smaller than that in C (p < 0.005), and it did not differ from B was still perceived by the listeners (n = 16) and guided their ini-
(F < 1). Thus the increased N400 and P600 amplitudes in condi- tial parsing decisions (prosodic acceptability rates, 73.8 % in B,
tion C were not due to either the verb or the prosodic pattern per 10.9 % in C). We still observed the CPS at the first boundary
se but rather to the mismatch between the two. and also the N400P600 effect in condition C (Fig. 4). This find-
ing supports the idea that the CPS reflects the processing of the
An ERP component reflecting prosodic processing prosodic boundary rather than the perception of a pause inter-
The prosodic processing that induced the reversed garden-path rupting the speech input.
effect was also reflected in the ERPs. In both experiments, we
found a large positive waveform at intonational phrase bound- DISCUSSION
aries. The grand-average ERPs across both experiments (n = 40; The present study tested whether prosodic cues in spoken lan-
Fig. 2) showed that condition A evoked a single shift at its IPh guage are immediately used by the listener to solve syntactic
boundary following the second verb. The ERP for condition B, ambiguities that systematically result in initial misunderstand-
in contrast, contained two such positive shifts, corresponding to ings during reading, and to determine whether these prosodic
its two IPh boundaries, the first one preceding Anna and the influences can be monitored on-line by ERP measures. Not only
second after the second verb. This pattern of one versus two pos- did we demonstrate that the prosodic information was sufficient
itive shifts was confirmed statistically by both amplitude com- to reverse syntactic parsing preferences, but we also identified a
parisons (p < 0.001) and peak localizations (p < 0.0001) and did specific ERP component reflecting the decoding of intonational
not differ between Experiment 1 (Fig. 3a) and Experiment 2 phrasing, the closure positive shift.
(Fig. 3b; F < 1). Moreover, the ERP for the mismatch condition With respect to psycholinguistic modeling, the data provide
C, which contains two IPh boundaries, also had the two corre- strong evidence that the syntactic parser can be directly influ-
sponding shifts (Fig. 3). Additional analyses revealed that the enced by prosodic information. The presence of an early IPh
positive shift at IPh boundaries was not due to the word preced- boundary preceding Anna in conditions B and C stopped fur-
ing each IPh boundary being a verb or to so-called exogenous ther syntactic integration into the current first verb phrase and
ERP components (such as N100 or P200) reflecting the physical instead prepared an initial attachment of Anna to the second
features of the stimulus. As we assume that the shift primarily verb. This reversed parsing preference, triggered exclusively by
reflects the closure of an intonational phrase, we term this ERP prosodic information, successfully induced an initial misanaly-
component the closure positive shift (CPS). sis (a garden-path effect) in the mismatch condition C and elicit-
An important question is whether the CPS actually reflects ed the predicted N400P600 pattern of ERP components on the
prosodic phrasing or whether it is more directly related to the incompatible intransitive verb. The cognitive processes underly-
acoustic properties marking the boundary. Because in written ing both components are still subject to discussion27. Here we
sentences terminal words preceding a pause are also associated follow a recent interpretation28 according to which the N400
with positive ERP components25,26, the most likely candidate for effect presumably reflects a lexical re-access necessary to confirm
such direct correspondence is the pause insertion after the first the outright violation of the intransitive verb argument struc-
verb in conditions B and C, that is, the temporary absence of any ture in condition C29. The P600, on the other hand, seems to
speech input at the boundary. Therefore, we ran a third ERP indicate the subsequent structural revision concerning the attach-
experiment in which we carefully removed the entire pause in ment site of Anna. The rapidity of this on-line revision would
both conditions B and C, preserving other intonational cues. also explain why we did not find any increased error rates in the
Behavioral and ERP results for these new conditions B and C comprehension task. When the question was presented one sec-
confirmed that even without the pause the prosodic boundary ond later, the structure had already been repaired. As the repair

194 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

process is very likely to involve subvocal corrections of the into- differences between conditions A and B (Fig. 5a). The additional IPh
national phrasing, we assume that the P600 component may boundary in condition B was signified prosodically by a pause insertion
reflect the costs of both syntactic and prosodic revisions. before Anna (p < 0.0001), as well as by a significant lengthening of the
We also found that prosodic boundaries are associated with first constituent, Peter verspricht (p < 0.0001). Whereas a major accent
occurred on the verb arbeiten in condition A, accentuation was shift-
a large positive-going waveform, which we labeled the closure
ed to the noun phrase Anna in condition B. These differences in accent
positive shift (CPS). This component did not depend on the pres- positions were confirmed both by a locally rising pitch contour in the
ence or absence of a pause acoustically interrupting the stream fundamental frequency (p < 0.0001; Fig. 5b) and by a corresponding
of speech input. Rather, whenever the boundary was perceived loudness maximum (p < 0.01; not shown).
and used to guide parsing, the CPS was found in the corre- Condition C was derived by cross-splicing the first part of B and the
sponding time interval. The CPS may be associated with process- second part of A in the silent phase of the affricate /ts/ of the infinitival
es that serve to structure the mental representation of the speech marker zu (to). This procedure plus an amplitude normalization
signal and to prepare the further analysis of subsequent input. It protected against detectability of the signal manipulation at the splicing
seems that, at least for the sentences used in the present study, point. Conditions B and C of the third experiment were obtained by
the CPS enables monitoring of prosodically driven parsing deci- removing the pause before the second noun phrase, without affecting
the signals of adjacent words.
sions long before the syntactically disambiguating element (the
second verb) is encountered. Procedure. The 144 experimental sentences were intermixed with 144
Because of the confound between prosodic and syntactic units filler sentences and presented auditorily in a pseudo-randomized order
in natural language, the present data leave open whether the CPS
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

in 8 blocks of 36 trials, distributed over 2 sessions. Block order was coun-


is predominantly related to prosodic structuring per se or to its terbalanced across subjects. EEG was continuously recorded (250 Hz/12
consequences on syntactic processing. Our recent ERP study bit sampling rate; Neuroscan DC amplifier) from 17 cap-mounted tin
using auditorily presented artificial language (unpublished data), electrodes while subjects listened to the sentences in a electromagneti-
however, seems to support the view of prosodic processing under- cally shielded chamber. All electrodes were referenced against left mastoid.
lying the CPS. On reinspection of this data to replicate our CPS Impedances were kept below 5 k. In Experiment 1, the task was to
answer yes or no to comprehension questions such as Does Anna
finding, a CPS was observed even in a group of naive subjects
promise to clean the office? in 20% of the trials. In Experiments 2 and 3,
who had not yet acquired the syntax rules of the artificial lan- participants were asked to judge the prosodic adequacy of each sentence
guage. Syntactic ERP effects, in contrast, were found only in par- immediately after presentation in addition to the comprehension task.
ticipants who were already familiar with the syntax rules
(E. Pfeifer & A.D.R., J. Cogn. Neurosci. Suppl., p. 26, 1998). Data analyses. EEG epochs containing eyeblinks or movement artifacts
Whereas the question of which prosodic cues are necessary or were rejected and did not enter the ERP averages. Averages were com-
sufficient to elicit a CPS requires future research, available data puted both across the whole sentence (Figs. 2, 3 and 4a) and for the crit-
indicate that the component is tightly linked to the cognitive ical second verb (Figs. 1 and 4b) using a 200-ms prestimulus baseline.
process of structuring the incoming speech signal. If this finding The mean amplitudes of 8 subsequent 200-ms time windows were com-
holds, the component may serve as a valuable tool for systematically puted from 200 ms after the onset of verb 2 until 1800 ms thereafter to
measure the N400 and P600 components in conditions B and C. Com-
exploring the relationship between prosodic parsing and syntac-
parisons including condition A required additional baseline-indepen-
tic parsing, which has received little attention until recently3032. dent peak-to-peak measures35 to balance the diverging CPS patterns.
Our data provide strong evidence that many garden-path The closure positive shift was quantified by two different approaches.
effects consistently observed during reading simply may not occur First, we compared mean amplitudes across eight subsequent 500-ms
in spoken language processing. This finding would be compatible time windows covering the whole sentence length. Second, we evaluated
with so-called syntax-first models such as the garden-path model the onset and offset latencies of large positive shifts at midline electrodes
only if prosodic information is taken to directly transmit syn- separately for each subject in each condition using peak-to-peak mea-
tactic information. In any case, it underlines the necessity to take sures. Both behavioral and ERP data were statistically tested by ANOVAs.
prosodic processing into account more explicitly. On-line mea- ERP analyses were generally done separately for midline and lateral elec-
trodes. For the midline, a global three-way ANOVA with factors condi-
sures such as the CPS may help to lay the empirical foundations
tions (3) electrode (3) and the between-subject factor task (2) was used.
for an adequate theory of natural speech processing. The present For the lateral electrodes, regions of interest were defined. The resulting
study is only a preliminary step toward understanding the brain ANOVA design included the factors conditions (3) hemisphere (2)
functions underlying prosodic processing. However, our data position (3) task (2). Single comparisons were only computed if the
strongly suggest that ERP measures used in a controlled experi- global ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of condition with df > 1
mental design are a promising on-line approach to shed new light or an interaction with the factor condition. Where appropriate, Huynh
on the role of prosody with regard to both normal and impaired and Feldt corrections36 and a modified Bonferroni correction proce-
speech processing33. dure37 were applied. For illustrative purposes only, the grand-average
ERPs were smoothed off-line using a 5-Hz lowpass filter.
METHODS
Subjects. Twenty students participated in each of the first two ERP exper- ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
iments, and sixteen in the third experiment. All 56 subjects were right- We appreciate the comments of Lee Osterhout and Gerry Altman on an earlier
handed 34, and without hearing or neurological disorders. All three version of the manuscript. We also thank Katja Khn, Annett Schirmer and
experiments complied with German legal requirements. Franziska Kopp for their assistance in generating the stimulus materials and in
analyzing the signals. This work was supported by the Deutsche
Speech signals. Forty-eight sentence pairs such as 2a and 2b were pro-
Forschungsgemeinschaft (FR 519/17-1).
duced by a female native speaker of standard German and recorded in a
soundproof chamber. The digitized speech signals (44.1 kHz/16 bit sam-
RECEIVED 10 AUGUST; ACCEPTED 16 DECEMBER 1998
pling rate) of each sentence were measured with respect to word and
pause durations, fundamental frequency (pitch contour) and loudness
(amplitude squares), and the differences were statistically analyzed in 1. Marslen-Wilson, W. S. et al. Prosodic effects in minimal attachment, Q. J.
paired t-tests or with ANOVAs. There were highly significant durational Exp. Psychol. 45A, 7387 (1992).

nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999 195


1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

articles

2. Warren, P., Grabe, E. & Nolan, F. Prosody, phonology, and parsing in closure 19. Friederici, A. D., Steinhauer, K. & Frisch, S. Lexical integration: Sequential
ambiguities. Lang. Cogn. Processes 10, 457486 (1995). effects of syntactic and semantic information. Mem. Cogn. (in press).
3. Stirling, L. & Wales, R. Does prosody support or direct sentence processing? 20. Nespor, M. & Vogel, I. Prosodic Phonology (Foris, Dordrecht, 1986).
Lang. Cogn. Processes 11, 193212 (1996). 21. Selkirk, E. O. Phonology and Syntax: The Relation between Sound and
4. Frazier, L. & Rayner, K. Making and correcting errors during sentence Structure (MIT Press, Cambridge, 1984).
comprehension: Eye movements in the analysis of structurally ambiguous 22. Halle, M. & Vergnaud, J. R. An Essay on Stress (MIT Press, Cambridge, 1987).
sentences. Cogn. Psychol. 14, 178210 (1982). 23. Gernsbacher, M. A. & Jescheniak, J. D. Cataphoric devices in spoken
5. Frazier, L. in Attention and Performance XII: The Psychology of Reading (ed. discourse. Cogn. Psychol. 29, 2458 (1995).
Coltheart, M.) 559589 (Erlbaum, London, 1987). 24. Rsler, F., Friederici, A. D., Ptz, P. & Hahne, A. Event-related brain
6. Taraban, R. & McClelland, J. R. Constituent attachment and thematic role potentials while encountering semantic and syntactic constraint violations. J.
assignment in sentence processing: Influence of context based expectations. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 5, 345362 (1993).
Mem. Lang. 27, 597632 (1988). 25. Friedman, D., Simson, R., Ritter, W. & Papin, I. The late positive component
7. Altman, G. T. M., Garnham, A. & Dennis, Y. Avoiding the garden-path: Eye (P300) and information processing in sentences. Electroencephalogr. Clin.
movements in context. J. Mem. Lang. 31, 685712 (1992). Neurophysiol. 38, 255262 (1975).
8. Cutler, A., Dahan, D. & van Donselaar, W. Prosody in the comprehension of 26. Van Petten, C. & Kutas, M. Influences of semantic and syntactic context on
spoken language: A literature review. Lang. Speech 40, 141201 (1997). open- and closed-class words. Mem. Cogn. 19, 95112 (1991).
9. Blumstein, S. E. in The Cognitive Neurosciences (ed. Gazzaniga, M. S.) 27. Osterhout, L. & Hagoort, P. A superficial resemblance doent necessarily
915929 (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1995). mean that youre part of the family: Counterarguments to Coulson, King, and
10. Hagoort, P. & Kutas, M. in Handbook of Neuropsychology Vol.10 (eds. Boller, F. Kutas (1998) in the P600/SPS-P300 debate. Lang. Cogn. Processes (in press).
& Grafman, J.) 105134 (Elsevier, Amsterdam, 1995). 28. Friederici, A. D. The time course of syntactic activation during language
11. Swaab, T. Y. The Functional Locus of Comprehension Deficits in Aphasia: An processing: A model based on neuropsychological and neurophysiological
Electrophysiological Approach (Ponsen & Looijen, Wageningen, The data. Brain Lang. 50, 259281 (1995).
Netherlands, 1996). 29. Hopf, J.-M., Bayer, J., Bader, M. & Meng, M. Event-related brain potentials
12. Osterhout, L. & Holcomb, P. J. Event-related brain potentials and syntactic and case information in syntactic ambiguities. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 10, 264280
1999 Nature America Inc. http://neurosci.nature.com

anomaly: Evidence of anomaly detection during the perception of (1998).


continuous speech. Lang. Cogn. Processes 8, 413437 (1993). 30. Bader, M. in Reanalysis in Sentence Processing (eds. Ferreira, F. & Fodor, J. D.)
13. Osterhout, L., Holcomb, P. J. & Swinney, D. A. Brain potentials elicited by (Kluwer, Dordrecht, in press).
garden-path sentences. Evidence of the application of verb information 31. Beckman, M. E. The parsing of prosody. Lang. Cogn. Processes 11, 1767
during parsing. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 20, 786806 (1994). (1996).
14. Osterhout, L. & Holcomb, P. J. in Electrophysiology of Mind: Event-Related 32. Fodor, J. D. Learning to parse? J. Psycholinguistic Res. 27, 285319 (1998).
Potentials and Cognition (eds. Rugg, M. D. & Coles, M. G. H.) 171215 33. Ross, E. D. in Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychology (eds. Feinberg, T. E.
(Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford, 1995). & Farah, M. J.) 699709 (McGraw-Hill, New York, 1997).
15. Friederici, A. D., Steinhauer, K., Mecklinger, A. & Meyer, M. Working 34. Oldfield, R. C. The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh
memory constraints on syntactic ambiguity resolution as revealed by inventory. Neuropsychology 9, 97113 (1975).
electrical brain responses. Biol. Psychol. 47, 193221 (1998). 35. Regan, D. Human Brain Electrophysiology: Evoked Potentials and Evoked
16. Kutas, M. & Hillyard, S. A. Reading senseless sentences: Brain potentials Magnetic Fields in Science and Medicine (Elsevier, New York, 1989).
reflect semantic incongruency. Science 207, 203205 (1980). 36. Huynh, H. & Feldt, L. A. Conditions under which mean square ratios in
17. Holcomb, P. J. & Neville, H. J. Natural speech processing: An analysis using repeated measurement designs have exact F-distributions. J. Am. Stat. Assoc.
event-related brain potentials. Psychobiology 19, 286300 (1991). 65, 15821589 (1979).
18. Kutas, M. & van Petten, C. in Handbook of Psycholinguistics (ed. Gernsbacher, 37. Keppel, G. Design and Analysis: A Researchers Handbook (Prentice-Hall,
M. A.) 83143 (Academic, San Diego, 1994). Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey, 1991).

196 nature neuroscience volume 2 no 2 february 1999

Вам также может понравиться