Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 11

543973

research-article2014
PPSXXX10.1177/1745691614543973SimontonMad-Genius Paradox

Perspectives on Psychological Science

The Mad-Genius Paradox: Can Creative 2014, Vol. 9(5) 470480


The Author(s) 2014
Reprints and permissions:
People Be More Mentally Healthy But sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/1745691614543973
Highly Creative People More Mentally Ill? pps.sagepub.com

Dean Keith Simonton


University of California, Davis

Abstract
The persistent mad-genius controversy concerns whether creativity and psychopathology are positively or negatively
correlated. Remarkably, the answer can be both! The debate has unfortunately overlooked the fact that the creativity-
psychopathology correlation can be expressed as two independent propositions: (a) Among all creative individuals,
the most creative are at higher risk for mental illness than are the less creative and (b) among all people, creative
individuals exhibit better mental health than do noncreative individuals. In both propositions, creativity is defined by
the production of one or more creative products that contribute to an established domain of achievement. Yet when
the typical cross-sectional distribution of creative productivity is taken into account, these two statements can both
be true. This potential compatibility is here christened the mad-genius paradox. This paradox can follow logically
from the assumption that the distribution of creative productivity is approximated by an inverse power function
called Lotkas law. Even if psychopathology is specified to correlate positively with creative productivity, creators as
a whole can still display appreciably less psychopathology than do people in the general population because the
creative geniuses who are most at risk represent an extremely tiny proportion of those contributing to the domain. The
hypothesized paradox has important scientific ramifications.

Keywords
creativity, genius, psychopathology, productivity, Lotkas law

The mad-genius controversy is certainly one of the oldest On the other side are psychologists who believe that
and most problematic issues in psychology (Becker, some connection exists between creativity and psycho-
1978). Ancient Greeks and Romans speculated on the pathology, although few if any would now claim that
possible connection between exceptional creativity and creative geniuses are necessarily insane (Eysenck, 1995;
psychopathology (e.g., Seneca, trans. 1932), and research James, 1902; cf. Babcock, 1895; Lombroso, 1891). Instead,
on the subject continues to the present day (Kaufman, the highly creative share certain cognitive and disposi-
2014; Silvia & Kaufman, 2010). Moreover, psychologists tional traits with people who have some marked vulner-
often take extreme positions on the topic. ability for mental or emotional instability (Carson, 2014).
On one side are psychologists who maintain that the A shared vulnerability can induce symptoms associated
whole concept of the creative mad genius is pure myth, with psychopathology even when the creator falls short
without an ounce of empirical support (e.g., Sawyer, of a clinical diagnosis. This positive linkage is supported
2012; Schlesinger, 2009). Indeed, humanistic and positive by research using experimental, psychometric, psychiat-
psychologists often argue that outstanding creativity con- ric, and historiometric methods (Simonton, 2010, 2014a).
stitutes a sure sign of superior mental health (Bacon, Although any one method must admit the possibility of
2005; Cassandro & Simonton, 2002). Maslows (1970)
cases of self-actualizers included many highly creative
Corresponding Author:
individuals, such as Benjamin Franklin, John Keats, Pierre Dean Keith Simonton, Department of Psychology, One Shields Ave.,
Renoir, and Franz Joseph Haydn (see also May, 1975; University of California, Davis, Davis, CA 95616-8686
Rogers, 1954). E-mail: dksimonton@ucdavis.edu

Downloaded from pps.sagepub.com at E-nformation SRL on March 8, 2016


Mad-Genius Paradox 471

certain artifacts, the diverse methods do not suffer from logicalthough the logic itself is founded on a huge
the same potential problems, and thus the convergence amount of prior empirical data. These data concern the
among the alternative approaches must provide reason- cross-sectional distribution of creative productivity, the
able endorsement. statistical feature that (a) underlies the variation in life-
Rather than further examine the pros and cons, it may time creative productivity that is an essential part of
prove more instructive to consider seriously a third Proposition 1 and (b) permits the distinction between
option: Both camps might be perfectly correct! It may creative and noncreative persons required for Proposition
depend merely on how researchers frame the question 2. I first discuss this distribution and then show how its
and what that question actually implies. To see how this distinctive nature would resolve the mad-genius para-
might happen, consider the following two empirical dox.1 Many paradoxes depend on some trick that shows
propositions. that the contradiction is only apparent, not real. Classic
examples are Zenos paradoxes, which seem to prove
Proposition 1: Among all creative people, highly cre- logically what cannot possibly be true intuitively: In his
ative persons have higher rates of psychopathology arrow paradox, arrows seem to be unable to reach their
than do less creative persons. In particular, the prob- targets, but of course they can. As will be seen, the pro-
ability that an individual displays one or more psycho- posed mad-genius paradox also hinges on a kind of con-
pathological symptoms is a positive linear function of ceptual trick, but a highly important one that has
that persons lifetime creative productivity within a implications for understanding the nature of the creativ-
given domain (cf. Damian & Simonton, 2014; Ludwig, ity-psychopathology relationship.
1992; Simonton, 2014b).
Proposition 2: Among all people, creative persons Productivity
have lower rates of psychopathology than do noncre-
ative persons. A creative individual is here defined as Psychologists are most likely to posit that individual dif-
one who creates at least one product that satisfies the ferences on any cognitive, personality, or behavioral trait
requirements for creativity in a given domain as deter- are adequately described by the symmetrical Gaussian
mined by expert appraisals (Simonton, 2013; cf. distribution, or normal distribution (Simonton, 2008).
Kaufman & Beghetto, 2009). Indeed, the most commonly used statistical techniques
postulate normality from the onset. In stark contrast,
Are these propositions contradictory? At first glance, it when it comes to creative productivity, this assumption
might seem so. The first appears to support the mad- could not be more wrong (OBoyle & Aguinas, 2012;
genius hypothesis, whereas the second seems to reject Walberg, Strykowski, Rovai, & Hung, 1984). It is not sim-
that very same hypothesis. Yet the argument here is that ply that the distribution is highly skewed, for no conven-
both of these propositions can be not only true but also tional data transformation can possibly render the
mutually supportive. As a group, creative people can be distribution normal (such as square roots or logarithms).
more mentally healthy than noncreative people. Yet For the creative individuals that define Proposition 1 (i.e.,
among all creative people, those who ascend to the sta- those who have created one or more products), the peak
tus of creative genius can exhibit more proclivities toward of the distribution is invariably located at the very start, at
mental illness than creative colleagues who do not attain the lowest available score (i.e., a single work) rather than
that high status. Specifically, creative productivity, which falling somewhere between the lowest and highest scores
is considered a defining characteristic of genius (Albert, (OBoyle & Aguinas, 2012). In short, the modal level of
1975), correlates positively with tendencies toward exhib- lifetime creative output among creators is a single nota-
iting certain symptoms or traits associated with psycho- ble product. Thereafter, as the number of total high-
pathology. This conjectured compatibility between impact products increases, the expected number of
Propositions 1 and 2 is here termed the mad-genius creators producing that amount declines in a negative
paradox. monotonic but decelerating function that somewhat
Treatment of this paradox begins with a formal demon- resembles the well-known Ebbinghaus forgetting curve.
stration. Because this demonstration is necessarily abstract, To better illuminate the nature of this distribution, I
it is followed by a discussion that fleshes out the mathe- first present a representative example and then provide a
matical argument, both conceptually and empirically. mathematical specification that will later provide the
basis for the paradox.

Demonstration Concrete illustration.According to the original offi-


The argument in favor of the consistency of Propositions cial guide to the New York Metropolitan Opera, as of the
1 and 2 is based not on empirical data but on pure early 1980s, just 72 composers had created the

Downloaded from pps.sagepub.com at E-nformation SRL on March 8, 2016


472 Simonton

Table 1. Creative Productivity Distribution According to Lotkas Law and the Psychopathology Distribution When the Rate Has a
Positive Linear Relation With Creative Productivity

Total creative Number of persons creating Proportion R(n) showing Number of persons of 155
productivity (n) exactly n total products psychopathology creators showing symptoms
1 100 (100.00) 0.0 0 (0.00)
2 25 (25.00) 0.1 3 (2.50)
3 11 (11.11) 0.2 2 (2.22)
4 6 (6.25) 0.3 2 (1.88)
5 4 (4.00) 0.4 2 (1.60)
6 3 (2.78) 0.5 1 (1.39)
7 2 (2.04) 0.6 1 (1.22)
8 2 (1.56) 0.7 1 (1.09)
9 1 (1.23) 0.8 1 (0.99)
10 1 (1.00) 0.9 1 (0.90)
Sum 155 (154.98) 14 (13.792)

Note: Here creative productivity, or n, is the total number of lifetime contributions, and the corresponding number of creators is given by Lotkas
law f(n) = c/n2, where f(n) is the number of creators with exactly n creative products assuming the constant c = 100 and where 1 n 10. In
line with Proposition 1, the rate of psychopathology for a given level of creative productivity is defined by R(n) = (n 1)/10. Then the number
of creative individuals exhibiting some identifiable psychopathology is then given by f(n) R(n), that is, the product of the second and third
columns. The values in parentheses are the exact values for f(n) and f(n) R(n) used in the calculations; the values preceding the parentheses
have been rounded to the nearest integer so that they can represent whole persons. Hence, according to the two summations, there are
155 creators, only 14 of whom show symptoms of psychopathology.

core repertoire of 150 great operas produced by this output is predicted by f(n) = c/n2, where c is a constant
world-famous company (Freeman, 1984). Of these 72, that can vary according to the domain of creativity. The
only 2 composers contributed 10 or more operas (10 by most significant point is that f(n) represents an inverse
Richard Wagner and 15 by Giuseppe Verdi), whereas power function that first declines very rapidly before lev-
fully 49 were credited with only 1 each (e.g., Luigi Cheru- eling off to an asymptote indicating the point at which
bini, Lo Delibes, Friedrich von Flotow, Alberto Ginas- nobody in the field has published more than some maxi-
tera, Mikhail Glinka, Scott Joplin, Franz Lehr, and Thea mum amount. The upper tail of the distribution is also
Musgrave). The remaining 21 opera composers were col- extremely long, much longer than that for the normal
lectively responsible for the final 76 contributions, or an curve (Simon, 1955; cf. Dennis, 1954; Nicholls, 1972).
average of only 3.62 works each, although the vast major- The first two columns of Table 1 give the number of
ity of these composers contributed just 2 or 3 works. products, n, and the corresponding number of creators,
Only a small minority contributed between 4 and 9 f(n), where c = 100 and n = 1, 2, 3, . . . 10 (i.e., 1 n
operas, and thus they demarcate the onset of the upper 10, explicitly excluding values less than 1 or more than
tail of the distribution. 10). Thus, f(5) = 4 signifies that there are 4 individuals
So-called one-hit wonders clearly dominate the oper- who created exactly 5 products. Although the parentheti-
atic productivity distribution (cf. Kozbelt, 2008). More cal values of f(n) have been maintained to the second
than two thirds (68%) of the composers satisfy only the decimal place for purposes of later calculations, each
minimum criterion for being considered a creator in this value given before the parentheses was rounded off to
highly demanding domain (i.e., they wrote only one the nearest integer to provide a more realistic portrayal of
opera). Hence, to anticipate the upcoming argument a the distribution. (Even creative individuals cannot man-
tiny bit, whatever personal attributes these 49 creators age to exist in fractions.) Whether rounded or not, the
might collectively possess might easily overwhelm the nature of the distribution is manifest. The modal output
characteristics shared by the 2 most prolific contributors. is a single creative product; fully 100 of the creators fall
into this category. This singular act is what qualifies the
Mathematical specification. Although cross-sectional individual as being a creator. That said, because the total
distributions such as the one just presented have been number of creators, according to the parameters and
described different ways, a decent approximation is the assumptions, is ~155 (154.98, as shown in the last row of
so-called Lotkas law (Egghe, 2005; Lotka, 1926; Price, the table), only 55 creators are left to cover the remaining
1986). This law can be formally defined as follows: If n is productivity levels from 2 to 10. Almost half of these, or
the total lifetime creative output for a given creator, then 25, can each be credited with just 2 lifetime contributions
the number of creators producing that particular level of (which is very close to the mean output of 1.89), and yet

Downloaded from pps.sagepub.com at E-nformation SRL on March 8, 2016


Mad-Genius Paradox 473

another ~11 (11.11) account for 3 each. All of this amounts stymied mathematician John Nash, as portrayed in the
to a precipitous drop given that about 88% of the creators 2001 film A Beautiful Mind; Grazer & Howard, 2001) or
are now included in the accumulation. From these condi- permanently (e.g., recurrent depressions that eventually
tions arises the long subsequent tail, very much like the lead to suicide, as in the case of Sylvia Plath; Kaufman,
scree in the successive eigenvalues of a factor analysis. 2001). In any case, although the actual function may
The most productive creator in the sample, with 10 cre- adopt a variety of forms, it will be assumed just for the
ative products, stands all alone, although rounding to the moment that the productivity-psychopathology relation
nearest integer renders the creator with 9 creative prod- is defined by a positive linear function (cf. Ko & Kim,
ucts equally solitary. From the perspective of the normal 2008; Simonton, 2014b).
distribution, these two suggest outliers who stand heads
and shoulders above the restthe bona fide creative Positive linear function. For reasons that will become
geniuses. more clear shortly, let us simply define the rate of such
symptoms appearing according to the linear equation
Specification justification. I must emphasize that the R(n) = (n 1)/10, where R(n) is the expected rate for
constant c = 100 was chosen simply to keep later analy- individuals with exactly n creative products and where n
ses sufficiently succinct to make the necessary points must lie between 1 and 10, inclusively. This specification
about the paradox. The resulting Lotka distribution would is mathematically equivalent to the linear regression line
describe a domain of creativity in which major products R(n) = 0.1 + 0.1 n. The application of this formula is
are few and far betweensuch as creating successful shown in the third column of Table 1.
novels, histories, or operas. In such domains, producing By design, the numbers in this psychopathology rate
even a dozen high-impact works in a career would mark column have four key features. First, the figures are all
a truly exceptional achievement. More importantly, proportions indicating the probabilities that an individual
although I will assume that c = 100, the resolution of the with n number of creative products might display, even
mad-genius paradox is not by any means contingent on transiently, disabling symptoms of psychopathology.
the particular value chosen for c. It could easily be Second, these probabilities all increase with n: The more
increased to accommodate the productivity seen in creative productivity, the higher the likelihood of psycho-
domains that feature smaller least publishable units, pathology. Third, for creators at the bottom of the distri-
such as journal articles in science or patent applications bution, who produced only one creative work, the rate is
in technology (Price, 1986). Just as significantly, the solu- set at zero (i.e., 0.1 + 0.1 1 = 0). In other words, almost
tion to the paradox is not even dependent on the specific two thirds of the creators would manifest no disabling
application of Lotkas law. Any inverse power law of the symptoms! Fourth and last, at the top end of the distribu-
form f(n) = c/na, where a > 1 (e.g., the Pareto distribu- tion, where the creative genius has the maximum output,
tion, in which a = 1.5), would yield comparable conclu- the probability of the person experiencing at least one
sions regarding the paradox. Propositions 1 and 2 can mental or emotional breakdown increases to 90% (i.e.,
still prove consistent. Lotkas law was chosen simply 0.1 + 0.1 10 = 0.9). That means that the odds highly
because of its empirical robustness as an approximation favor some degree of psychopathology in the most pro-
and the simplicity of its formulation (Egghe, 2005).2 lific creatorsthose who might produce the largest
number of high-impact works or masterpieces.
The hypothesized increased risk associated with the
Psychopathology more productive creators is represented graphically in
Given the distribution of creative productivity shown in Figure 1, where the count of creators per level of output
Table 1, the next step in the demonstration is to specify has been rescaled to a proportion of all creators to facili-
how proclivities toward psychopathology might vary as a tate direct comparison. On the one hand, the highly
function of creative productivity, as denoted by n in skewed nature of distribution is quite apparent, with the
Lotkas law. Although psychopathology is probably best inevitable mode at n = 1 and a long tail leading to the
viewed as a dimensional or quantitative variable that rep- maximum set at n = 10 masterworks. On the other hand,
resents a range of clinical and subclinical symptoms (e.g., the rapidly increasing risk is also apparent: The creative
Post, 1994; Simonton, 2014b; see also Caspi etal., 2014), geniuses exhibit the highest psychopathology rates.
I will simplify the analysis by treating it as a discrete or
qualitative variable (e.g., Damian & Simonton, 2014; Paradoxical implication. The third column in Table 1
Ludwig, 1992). For example, it might be defined as the represents a deliberate imposition of Proposition 1 on
appearance of symptoms that are sufficiently severe as to the hypothetical data. Has the truth of Proposition 2 been
incapacitate the individuals creative work either tempo- rendered impossible by this maneuver? At first, one might
rarily (e.g., the paranoid episodes that sometimes think so, for the mean rate of psychopathological

Downloaded from pps.sagepub.com at E-nformation SRL on March 8, 2016


474 Simonton

1.0 13.792/154.977), the result is 0.09 (or 0.089)a measly


9%. The latter rate implies that creators as a whole dis-
0.9 play only a tenth of the risk seen in the creative genius
at the top of the distribution.
0.8
This rate of psychopathological symptoms for the
0.7 entire sample of 155 creators is very likely to be smaller
than that seen in the general population, just as suggested
0.6 in Proposition 2. For example, using a nationally repre-
Proportion

sentative sample, Kessler, Chiu, Demler, Merikangas, and


0.5 Walters (2005) estimated the lifetime prevalence of any
disorder to be 46.4%, which means that we can set R(0) =
0.4
0.46 as an approximation. Given this value, people cre-
0.3 ative enough to make at least one distinguished contribu-
tion to a domain remain more mentally healthy than those
0.2 who never do. If researchers had obtained a control group
of noncreative individuals with a sample size also equal to
0.1 155, they would predict that 71 (rather than just 14) would
exhibit psychopathology. Proposition 2 is thus confirmed
0.0 despite the assumed truth of Proposition 1.
0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Creative Productivity (n) In contrast, if creative productivity were normally dis-
tributed, with the tails ending at n = 1 and n = 10 and the
Fig. 1.Proportion of people at each level of creative productivity center placed midway between, then the rate of mental
(number of works; black bars) and proportion of people at risk of psy-
illness would be identical with the 0.45 figure mentioned
chopathology at each level of productivity (gray bars).
earlier, which is coincidentally very close to the Kessler
et al. (2005) estimate. After all, about two thirds of the
symptoms for creative people would seem to equal 0.45. creators would now fall right in the middle part of the
Almost half of all creators would experience disruptive hypothesized psychopathology dimension (i.e., 1 SD),
mental problemssuggesting that creators might be and thus would flatly contradict Proposition 2. The highly
more prone to psychopathology than noncreators. But skewed inverse power distribution really makes a dra-
importantly, that figure results only if the average is com- matic difference. The general risk rate would increase
puted down the column without paying attention to the fivefold if the productivity distribution were described by
crucial fact that each value of n corresponds to an often the normal distribution rather than Lotkas law (i.e.,
radically different count of the number of creators with 0.45/0.089 = 5.06), yielding a risk rate comparable with
that degree of productivity. Accordingly, a weighted that of the general population. Obviously, this contrast in
mean must be calculated that yields the same mean that expected psychopathology is far from trivial.
would have emerged if the data were first disaggregated Finally, it cannot be overemphasized that the potential
into the 155 separate creators, 100 of whom display no consistency of Propositions 1 and 2 is not dependent in
risk whatsoever for psychopathology. any manner on the absolute number of noncreative per-
To carry out the last task, I generated the fourth col- sons relative to the absolute number of creative persons.
umn in Table 1, which gives the number of creators of The former number may be much smaller than, exactly
the 155 who showed symptoms of psychopathology. equal to, or much larger than the latter, and the mad-
The resulting value for each entry is given by the prod- genius paradox would still hold. It matters not one iota
uct of the second and third columns, f(n) R(n), which whether f(0) equals a dozen, a hundred, a thousand, or a
is the number of individuals creating exactly n products million. The crux of the comparison is not the absolute
times the rate that those individuals might succumb to numbers but the relative rates: the mean rates of those
some degree of a mental or emotional disorder. Again, persons for whom n 1 and those for whom n = 0.
because we cannot have partial persons, these numbers
are rounded off to the nearest integers, once more put-
ting the exact numbers in parentheses. Although not
Discussion
much seems to be happening here, the variation looking Judging from the hypothetical but plausible statistics gen-
minimal, these values address only Proposition 2 when erated in Table 1 and illustrated in Figure 1, both propo-
they are summed up and then divided by the total num- sitions posed earlier can readily be true, thus confirming
ber of creators (see the second summation in the last the paradox. In line with Proposition 2, less than 10% of
row of the table). If the sum of 14 is divided by 155 (or all individuals making at least one creative contribution

Downloaded from pps.sagepub.com at E-nformation SRL on March 8, 2016


Mad-Genius Paradox 475

to a domain can be expected to suffer from symptoms linear function of creative productivity. Moreover, this
sufficiently severe to interfere with their work. Better yet, function was specified by a particular regression line:
nearly two thirds of the total sample of creators (i.e., R(n) = 0.1 + 0.1 n. This particular equation was
those who made one and only one creative contribution) selected because it connects two illustrative endpoints:
have no chance whatsoever of experiencing such a psy- (a) the 100 1-product creators with zero risk and (b) the
chological setback. Thus, for the bulk of creators, the rate single 10-product creator with a 90% risk. Nonetheless,
of psychopathology is very low. Nevertheless, consistent alternative specifications that could yield varying out-
with Proposition 1, the former results were predicated on comes can be easily hypothesized. For example, the
the assumption that tendencies toward psychopathology equation specifying Proposition 1 can be changed so that
increase with creative productivity, where each additional the risk at n = 1 is higher, the risk at n = 10 is lower, or
masterwork adds a 0.1 increment to the potential rate. both at once, which would weaken the positive relation
That surprising outcome took full advantage of the well- between productivity and psychopathology, even to the
established fact that the cross-sectional distribution must degree that the paradox might entirely vanish. In fact,
be described by an inverse power function. This distribu- nothing prevents the regression slope from becoming
tion makes the highly prolific creative geniuses such a either zero (i.e., there is no relationship between produc-
tiny percentage of the wholefar less than 1%that the tivity and psychopathology) or negative (i.e., creative
overall rate of pathology is effectively determined by geniuses are the most mentally healthy). Some published
their potentially healthier and more numerous one- and data actually suggest the latter possibility as a special
two-hit colleagues. case. For instance, Ko and Kim (2008) found that for cre-
As noted at the beginning, paradoxes that embody ative scientists whose work preserved the current para-
only apparent contradictions usually depend on some digmsincrementally developing rather than challenging
trick. By now, this trick should be manifest: Whatever the and overthrowing received theories, questions, and
appearances on first look, Propositions 1 and 2 actually methodseminence was negatively correlated with psy-
propose extremely different comparisons. Simply put, the chopathology. Only for scientists who rejected the old
first proposition compares the more creative people with paradigms and introduced new paradigms did the rela-
the less creative people within the group of creative indi- tion become positive. Given that the strongest correlate
viduals, whereas the second proposition compares all of eminence is lifetime creative productivity (Simonton,
creative people with all noncreative people. Only the lat- 1997), this crossover interaction effect would probably
ter comparison requires the introduction of R(0), the rate hold for productivity as well, so that the mad-genius par-
of psychopathology for those who have not produced a adox might be valid only for scientists leading paradig-
single creative product, whereas the former comparison matic revolutions. Revolutionary scientists such as Isaac
restricts the analysis to creative individuals with rates Newton and Charles Darwin exhibited conspicuous lev-
R(1) to R(n). That is why R(0) is omitted from both Table els of subclinical psychopathology (Ko & Kim, 2008; Post,
1 and Figure 1, which present the explicit argument for 1994).
Proposition 1. In terms of data analyses, Proposition 1 Matters can be taken even further by dispensing alto-
requires the estimate of a regression line that best fits a gether with the linear creativity-psychopathology func-
bivariate scatterplot between quantitative variables, tions and replacing them with curvilinear relations (cf.
whereas Proposition 2 demands a statistical test of a Richards, Kinney, Lunde, Benet, & Merzel, 1988). For
mean difference that might be graphically represented by instance, a recent empirical investigation concerned the
a histogram. Consequently, the two statements can be relation between eminence and psychopathology across
decoupled so that either can be true or false without ref- the five creative domains represented by scientists, think-
erence to the truth or falsity of the other. The specific ers, writers, artists, and composers (Simonton, 2014b).
decoupling inducing the mad-genius paradox is founded Although the associations for literary and artistic creators
on the fact that the distribution of creative productivity is were effectively positive linear associations, those for sci-
better described by Lotkas law than by the normal entists, thinkers, and composers were best described by
distribution.3 curvilinear, single-peaked functions. Furthermore, the
To explain this paradox more fully, I must now briefly peak for scientists came at much lower levels of psycho-
discuss three issues: alternative specifications, substan- pathology than the peak for composers, whereas the
tive explanations, and investigative implications. peak for the thinkers was in the most severe range
almost but not quite positive monotonic. Still, only the
scientists exhibited a function in which the most severe
Alternative specifications
psychopathology was associated with less eminence than
The previous demonstration was based on the assump- having no psychopathology at all. Combining this
tion that the risk of psychopathology was a positive inverted-J curve with the skewed distribution would then

Downloaded from pps.sagepub.com at E-nformation SRL on March 8, 2016


476 Simonton

invalidate the mad-genius paradox. In contrast, because disinhibition that are likely to be necessary for excep-
the linear component of the trend for the composers and tional creative productivity, the ability of intelligence to
thinkers is clearly positive (yielding inverted backward-J exert its moderating effect might become more precari-
curves), the paradox could apply to those creators almost ous, which increases the possibility of one or more
as much as to the literary and artistic creators. psychopathological episodes. After all, the requisite

Again, these inferences assume a strong correlation metacognitive control would become more rare when
between eminence and creative productivity so that the the general intelligence required would itself have to
former can serve as a proxy variable for the latter. It is come from the upper tail of its own cross-sectional dis-
worth pointing out, however, that distributions of emi- tribution (even if, in this case, it was approximately nor-
nence are far more skewed than those for creative pro- mal rather than skewed).5 This delicate balance would
ductivity (e.g., Zusne, 1985). Accordingly, the suggested too often mean that the vulnerable person could teeter-
paradox could actually become more exaggerated when- totter between creativity and psychopathologyan
ever creativity is assessed by impact rather than output. instability seen in the lives of many creative geniuses.
In English literature, for example, William Shakespeares Second, the paradox might prove to be an effect of the
eminence is well out of proportion to the number of personal consequences of creativity. That is, the very pro-
masterworks credited to his pen (Martindale, 1995). cess of generating creative products can induce stresses
In sum, it is possible to anticipate when the mad- and other ramifications that can threaten mental health.
genius paradox is most prone to emerge in a given cre- More specifically, at the commencement of most creative
ative domain. The specified association must be analyzed careers, the situation might be described by Proposition
only in the context of the skewed distribution, which 2, but as careers accumulate creative products, Proposition
accentuates whatever happens at the beginning of the 1 may tend to kick in. For example, consider the Social
curve where lifetime creative productivity approaches Adjustment Rating Scale that assesses events that disrupt
one and only one major work. Because these one-hit everyday life (whether for good or ill) and thereby make
creators make up around two thirds of all creators in the the individual more susceptible to certain psychological
domain, they dominate any statistics computed for all disorders (Holmes & Holmes, 1970). Among the items on
contributors to the domain.4 this scale is any outstanding personal achievement. A
single one of these events earns the person 28 disrup-
tion points, or about the same as having troubles with
Substantive explanations the boss, beginning or ending formal schooling, having a
So far, no rationale has been provided for why the mad- son or daughter leave home, or gaining a new family
genius paradox might actually be expected. The conjec- member. Undoubtedly, the creation of a major product,
tured harmony of Propositions 1 and 2 was merely such as a novel or opera, must obtain at least 28 points if
derived from the highly skewed cross-sectional distribu- not far more, and the greater the number of products cre-
tion of creative productivity. However, it would be useful ated the more points accumulated, with potential adverse
to identify any psychological processes that might pro- consequences down the line. Tellingly, roughly positive
vide for the phenomenon. These processes can most linear creativity-psychopathology functions have been
likely be grouped into two broad categories. found for writers, artists, thinkers, and revolutionary sci-
First, the paradox might ensue from the personal entists (Ko & Kim, 2008; Simonton, 2014b). These cre-
antecedents of creativity. For instance, Carson (2014) has ators contribute to low-consensus domains in which new
systematically studied the role of cognitive disinhibition products may be more subject to rejection, criticism, or
in the creative process. To a certain degree, creativity neglect (Simonton, 2009). A career in these fields is often
requires that the person not filter out putatively extrane- an unrelenting uphill battle, each new work providing
ous ideas and stimuli, thereby allowing the individual to more aggravation than vindication.
think outside the boxthe box that defines the con- Another explanation can be derived from the increased
straints governing routine thinking. Yet cognitive disinhi- eminence that often comes with increased creative pro-
bition is also associated with increased vulnerability for ductivity. Schaller (1997) has provided evidence that
psychopathology. In fact, this mental proclivity is posi- becoming famous can lead to enhanced self-consciousness,
tively associated with elevated scores on psychoticism, a an unpleasant mental state that the creator often sedates
measure of subclinical personality traits (Eysenck, 1995; by substance abuse, especially alcohol. It may be no acci-
see also Stavridou & Furnham, 1996). Fortunately, the dent that alcoholism constitutes one of the most common
risk of mental illness can be ameliorated by high general psychopathologies among highly creative people, par-
intelligence, which provides more metacognitive con- ticularly for literary creators who are more likely to
trol, converting a potential liability to a decided asset achieve popular renown than, say, scientists or philoso-
(Carson, 2014). Even so, at the higher levels of cognitive phers (Ludwig, 1990; Post, 1996). The novelist Ernest

Downloaded from pps.sagepub.com at E-nformation SRL on March 8, 2016


Mad-Genius Paradox 477

Hemingway provides an archetypal case. Ultimately, the essentially zero unless considerable effort is devoted to
substantive explanation for any mad-genius paradox is oversampling at the upper tail.
an empirical question, which brings us to the last issue. One last implication concerns the baseline rate for
psychopathology among noncreative people, or what
has been formally represented as R(0). Although this
Investigative implications
value is not required for assessing the truth of Proposition
Although the emphasis of this article has been on the 1, it is mandatory for testing Proposition 2, which then
logical compatibility of the two propositions underlying makes the rate essential for detecting the mad-genius
the mad-genius paradox, it should be obvious that the paradox. In the earlier demonstration, this rate was
argument has fundamental repercussions regarding defined by the lifetime prevalence of any disorder in a
empirical research. One such implication concerns the representative sample of the population (Kessler et al.,
specific nature of the sample drawn to assess the cre- 2005). Yet it is not self-evident that the general popula-
ativity-psychopathology relationship. Ideally, research- tion provides the best base rate for comparison. For
ers should sample participants that represent the full instance, individuals who create in domains that require
distribution, from the vast majority of low-output cre- higher education, such as the sciences, will have a higher
ators to the supreme elite of prolific creators (plus some likelihood of being drawn from a more select (even elite)
comparable nonproducers as control subjects). Yet that population that would probably display lower rates of
comprehensive sampling never happens in actual psychopathology (Simonton, 2009). This problem helps
research. On the contrary, the usual composition of the explain why many historiometric studies introduce
sample depends largely on the research method comparison groups that cannot represent the general

adopted. On the one hand, investigators who apply population (Simonton, 2014a). I already mentioned how
historiometric methods will study creative geniuses at Ko and Kim (2008) contrasted scientists who made para-
the upper end of the distribution (Damian & Simonton, digm-preserving contributions with scientists who made
2014; Kaufman, 20002001, 2001, 2005; Ko & Kim, 2008; paradigm-rejecting contributions (i.e., the scientific revo-
Ludwig, 1992; Post, 1994; Simonton, 2014b). Including lutionaries). Alternatively, creators in one domain may be
such prototypical examples of mad genius as Vincent compared with creators in one or more different domains
Van Gogh and Virginia Woolf, these historiometric stud- (Raskin, 1936; Simonton, 2014b) or even with eminent
ies most often support Proposition 1 (Simonton, 2014a). individuals in supposedly noncreative domains, such as
On the other hand, researchers who use psychometric political, military, or entrepreneurial leadership (Damian
or interview methods will investigate creators at the & Simonton, 2014; Ludwig, 1992; Post, 1994; Simonton &
lower end of the distribution (Feist, 2014; Nakamura & Song, 2009). This superfluity of options pinpoints a cen-
Fajans, 2014). In doing so, they will more likely support tral difficulty in conducting empirical research on the
Proposition 2 (see, e.g., Csikszentmihlyi, 1997; Roe, paradox: Although the central terms in Proposition 1 are
1953). precisely defined, the corresponding terms in Proposition
To be sure, on occasion a psychometric sample may 2 are not. Creative individuals contributing notable prod-
include enough of the distribution to be able to track a ucts to the same domain can be directly compared with-
segment of the positive monotonic function expressed in out reservations, but the choice of noncreative individuals
Proposition 1 (e.g., Barron, 1963; Gtz & Gtz, 1979; against whom to compare creative individuals may
Rushton, 1990). Such studies have found that the most require some arbitrary choices that will strongly deter-
creative members of the sample exhibit higher rates of mine the inferences drawn.
subclinical symptoms or traits than do the less creative
members of the same sample. Yet these investigations
Conclusion
rarely if ever include any people who might possibly
qualify for the designation genius. Even if the samples Despite the difficulties mentioned in the previous sec-
did manage to do so, we would still have to wonder tion, I am not arguing that the whole research topic is
whether the recruited geniuses were representative of hopeless. Quite the opposite is the case: I maintain that
geniuses as a whole. After all, it is conceivable that those more empirical research is needed, not less. Furthermore,
with severe inclinations toward psychopathology might future research on the creativity-psychopathology rela-
be less likely to volunteer to serve as participants in psy- tion should continue to use the full range of experimen-
chological assessments. Luminaries such as the creative tal, psychometric, psychiatric, and historiometric methods.
writer J. D. Salinger might rather stay home. Worse still, Each method captures different parts of the distribution,
because the creators at the upper end are so terribly rare, and each in its own way (Simonton, 2010). I argue that
the odds of obtaining even one person among the sam- empirical researchers should adopt a more subtle per-
ple size typical of most research of this type can become spective on this question. An investigation confirming

Downloaded from pps.sagepub.com at E-nformation SRL on March 8, 2016


478 Simonton

Proposition 1 does not necessarily disconfirm Proposition plotted on log-log axes, the outcome is a line with a negative
2, and vice versa. The realistic set of circumstances dem- slope (see, e.g., Price, 1986, p. 39, Fig. 2.1). The term log c just
onstrated in this article (e.g., the scenario shown in gives the intercept.
Table1 and Figure 1) justifies both propositions, which 3. This distributional postulate is what makes the conjectured
paradox utterly distinct from superficially similar paradoxes,
validates the existence of the mad-genius paradox.
such as the well-known Simpsons paradox (for a recent review,
Whether or not both propositions are true in a given
see Kievit, Frankenhuis, Waldorp, & Borsboom, 2013). In the
domain ultimately requires scrutiny of the entire distribu- absence of Lotkas law, the paradox would most likely vanish:
tion of creative productivity, including those persons Propositions 1 and 2 then could not both be true.
who contributed nothing at all to any creative domain. 4. In more general terms, whenever R(0) > 0 but R(1) < R(0), so
Finally, the foregoing complications could very well that the risk rate is lower among the one-hit creative individu-
apply to many other research questions in psychological als relative to the comparison group, then the paradox would
science. Although the focus of this article is on the most likely still emerge whenever R(1) < R(2) < R(3) < . . .
creativity-psychopathology relationship, it is clear that
R(n 1) < R(n). Hence, a positive monotonic function can be
parallel issues would arise in any behavioral domain in easily substituted for the positive linear function in Proposition 1.
which the cross-sectional distribution is best described by 5. Carsons (2014) research uses the self-report Creative
Achievement Questionnaire (CAQ; Carson, Peterson, & Higgins,
a power function rather than the normal curve (for numer-
2005). Scores on the CAQ correlate positively not only with
ous examples, see OBoyle & Aguinas, 2012; Walberg
cognitive disinhibition (as assessed by reduced latent inhibi-
etal., 1984). For instance, the factors that distinguish ath- tion) but also with divergent thinking and openness to experi-
letes from nonathletes do not have to be equivalent to ence, two variables associated with creativity (Carson, Peterson,
those that distinguish the rare competitors who won mul- & Higgins, 2003, 2005). Significantly, the CAQ assesses creativity
tiple gold medals from those in the same Olympic events largely in terms of creative products or their evaluations, and
who earned only a single bronze medal. Some of those ranges from no such products (the noncreative individuals in
factors might even be antithetical. The same cautionary Proposition 2) to many such products, including award-winning
tale may also apply to antisocial behaviors, such as homi- works (the creative persons in Proposition 1). In addition, con-
cidal aggression. Variables that distinguish murderers sistent with the argument in this article, the distribution of
from the general population may not be identical with scores on this measure is neither normal nor log normal; rather,
it corresponds reasonably well to an inverse power function,
those that distinguish the more rare serial killers from the
low scores being the most common and high scores the least
much larger pool of murderers. In general, before under-
common, a concave curve connecting the two extremes.
taking any empirical inquiry into extreme human actions,
whether good or bad, the investigator must first ponder
References
whether the data and the hypotheses prove functionally
analogous to the mad-genius paradox. Acar, S., & Runco, M. A. (2012). Psychoticism and creativity: A
meta-analytic review. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity,
and the Arts, 6, 341350.
Acknowledgments
Acar, S., & Sen, S. (2013). A multilevel meta-analysis of the rela-
I thank the four referees, the associate editor, and the editor for tionship between creativity and schizotypy. Psychology of
helping me substantially improve the original submitted Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 7, 214228.
manuscript. Albert, R. S. (1975). Toward a behavioral definition of genius.
American Psychologist, 30, 140151.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests Babcock, W. L. (1895). On the morbid heredity and predisposi-
tion to insanity of the man of genius. Journal of Nervous
The author declared no conflicts of interest with respect to the
and Mental Disease, 20, 749769.
authorship or the publication of this article.
Bacon, S. F. (2005). Positive psychologys two cultures. Review
of General Psychology, 9, 181192.
Notes Barron, F. X. (1963). Creativity and psychological health:
1. Feists (1998) meta-analysis of research on the personality- Origins of personal vitality and creative freedom. Princeton,
creativity relation showed that the traits that distinguished NJ: Van Nostrand.
scientists from nonscientists were not identical to those that dis- Becker, G. (1978). The mad genius controversy: A study in the
tinguished highly creative scientists from less creative scientists. sociology of deviance. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Unfortunately, these comparisons did not include psychometric Carson, S. (2014). Cognitive disinhibition, creativity, and psy-
assessments of subclinical psychopathology (e.g., psychoticism chopathology. In D. K. Simonton (Ed.), The Wiley handbook
or schizotypy; cf. Acar & Runco, 2012; Acar & Sen, 2013). of genius (pp. 198221). Oxford, England: Wiley-Blackwell.
2. An example illustrating its simplicity: if both sides of this Carson, S., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2003). Decreased
function are log-transformed, the result is log f(n) = log c a latent inhibition is associated with increased creative
log n. This represents a straight line with a slope a. Hence, achievement in high-functioning individuals. Journal of
for Lotkas law, the slope is 2. In any event, if n and f(n) are Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 499506.

Downloaded from pps.sagepub.com at E-nformation SRL on March 8, 2016


Mad-Genius Paradox 479

Carson, S., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2005). Reliability, of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity
validity, and factor structure of the Creative Achievement Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62,
Questionnaire. Creativity Research Journal, 17, 3750. 617627.
Caspi, A., Houts, R. M., Belsky, D. W., Goldman-Mellor, S. J., Kievit, R. A., Frankenhuis, W. E., Waldorp, L. J., & Borsboom,
Harrington, H., Israel, S., . . . Moffitt, T. E. (2014). The p D. (2013, August 13). Simpsons paradox in psychologi-
factor: One general psychopathology factor in the structure cal science: A practical guide. Frontiers in Psychology, 4,
of psychiatric disorders? Clinical Psychological Science, 2, Article 513. Retrieved from http://journal.frontiersin.org/
119137. Journal/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00513/full
Cassandro, V. J., & Simonton, D. K. (2002). Creativity and Ko, Y., & Kim, J. (2008). Scientific geniuses psychopathology
genius. In C. L. M. Keyes & J. Haidt (Eds.), Flourishing: as a moderator in the relation between creative contribu-
Positive psychology and the life well-lived (pp. 163183). tion types and eminence. Creativity Research Journal, 20,
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. 251261.
Csikszentmihlyi, M. (1997). Creativity: Flow and the psychology Kozbelt, A. (2008). One-hit wonders in classical music:
of discovery and invention. New York, NY: HarperCollins. Evidence and (partial) explanations for an early career
Damian, R. I., & Simonton, D. K. (2014). Psychopathology, peak. Creativity Research Journal, 20, 179195.
adversity, and creativity: Diversifying experiences in the Lombroso, C. (1891). The man of genius. London, England:
development of eminent African-Americans. Journal of Scott.
Personality and Social Psychology. Advance online publica- Lotka, A. J. (1926). The frequency distribution of scientific pro-
tion. Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000011 ductivity. Journal of the Washington Academy of Sciences,
Dennis, W. (1954). Productivity among American psychologists. 16, 317323.
American Psychologist, 9, 191194. Ludwig, A. M. (1990). Alcohol input and creative output. British
Egghe, L. (2005). Power laws in the information production Journal of Addiction, 85, 953963.
process: Lotkaian informetrics. Oxford, England: Elsevier. Ludwig, A. M. (1992). Creative achievement and psychopathol-
Eysenck, H. J. (1995). Genius: The natural history of creativity. ogy: Comparison among professions. American Journal of
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Psychotherapy, 46, 330356.
Feist, G. J. (1998). A meta-analysis of personality in scientific Martindale, C. (1995). Fame more fickle than fortune: On the
and artistic creativity. Personality and Social Psychology distribution of literary eminence. Poetics, 23, 219234.
Review, 2, 290309. Maslow, A. H. (1970). Motivation and personality (2nd ed.).
Feist, G. J. (2014). Psychometric studies of scientific talent and New York: Harper & Row.
eminence. In D. K. Simonton (Ed.), The Wiley handbook May, R. (1975). The courage to create. New York, NY: W. W.
of genius (pp. 6286). Oxford, England: Wiley-Blackwell. Norton.
Freeman, J. W. (1984). The Metropolitan Opera: Stories of the Nakamura, J., & Fajans, J. (2014). Interviewing highly eminent
great operas. New York, NY: W. W. Norton. creators. In D. K. Simonton (Ed.), The Wiley handbook of
Gtz, K. O., & Gtz, K. (1979). Personality characteristics of genius (pp. 3361). Oxford, England: Wiley-Blackwell.
successful artists. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 49, 919924. Nicholls, J. G. (1972). Creativity in the person who will never
Grazer, B. (Producer), & Howard, R. (Director). (2001). A produce anything original and useful: The concept of cre-
beautiful mind [Motion picture]. United States: Universal ativity as a normally distributed trait. American Psychologist,
Pictures. 27, 717727.
Holmes, T. S., & Holmes, T. H. (1970). Short-term intrusions into OBoyle, E., Jr., & Aguinas, H. (2012). The best and the rest:
the life style routine. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, Revisiting the norm of normality of individual performance.
14, 121132. Personnel Psychology, 65, 79119.
James, W. (1902). The varieties of religious experience: A study Post, F. (1994). Creativity and psychopathology: A study of
in human nature. London, England: Longmans, Green. 291 world-famous men. British Journal of Psychiatry, 165,
Kaufman, J. C. (20002001). Genius, lunatics and poets: Mental 2234.
illness in prize-winning authors. Imagination, Cognition Post, F. (1996). Verbal creativity, depression and alcoholism: An
and Personality, 20, 305314. investigation of one hundred American and British writers.
Kaufman, J. C. (2001). The Sylvia Plath effect: Mental illness in British Journal of Psychiatry, 168, 545555.
eminent creative writers. Journal of Creative Behavior, 35, Price, D. (1986). Little science, big science . . . and beyond. New
3750. York, NY: Columbia University Press.
Kaufman, J. C. (2005). The door that leads into madness: Eastern Raskin, E. A. (1936). Comparison of scientific and literary abil-
European poets and mental illness. Creativity Research ity: A biographical study of eminent scientists and men of
Journal, 17, 99103. letters of the nineteenth century. Journal of Abnormal and
Kaufman, J. C. (Ed.). (2014). Creativity and mental illness. New Social Psychology, 31, 2035.
York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Richards, R., Kinney, D. K., Lunde, I., Benet, M., & Merzel, A.
Kaufman, J. C., & Beghetto, R. A. (2009). Beyond big and P. C. (1988). Creativity in manic-depressives, cyclothymes,
little: The four c model of creativity. Review of General their normal relatives, and control subjects. Journal of
Psychology, 13, 113. Abnormal Psychology, 97, 281288.
Kessler, R. C., Chiu, W. T., Demler, O., Merikangas, K. R., & Roe, A. (1953). The making of a scientist. New York, NY: Dodd,
Walters, E. E. (2005). Prevalence, severity, and comorbidity Mead.

Downloaded from pps.sagepub.com at E-nformation SRL on March 8, 2016


480 Simonton

Rogers, C. R. (1954). Toward a theory of creativity. ETC: A Simonton, D. K. (2010). So you want to become a creative
Review of General Semantics, 11, 249260. genius? You must be crazy! In D. Cropley, J. Kaufman,
Rushton, J. P. (1990). Creativity, intelligence, and psychoticism. A.Cropley, & M. Runco (Eds.), The dark side of creativity
Personality and Individual Differences, 11, 12911298. (pp. 218234). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Sawyer, R. K. (2012). Explaining creativity: The science of Simonton, D. K. (2013). What is a creative idea? Little-c versus
human innovation (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Oxford Big-C creativity. In J. Chan & K. Thomas (Eds.), Handbook
University Press. of research on creativity (pp. 6983). Cheltenham Glos,
Schaller, M. (1997). The psychological consequences of fame: England: Edward Elgar.
Three tests of the self-consciousness hypothesis. Journal of Simonton, D. K. (2014a). The mad (creative) genius: What do
Personality, 65, 291309. we know after a century of historiometric research? In J. C.
Schlesinger, J. (2009). Creative mythconceptions: A closer look Kaufman (Ed.), Creativity and mental illness (pp. 2541).
at the evidence for the mad genius hypothesis. Psychology New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 3, 6272. Simonton, D. K. (2014b). More method in the mad-genius
Silvia, P. J., & Kaufman, J. C. (2010). Creativity and mental ill- controversy: A historiometric study of 204 historic cre-
ness. In J. C. Kaufman & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), Cambridge ators. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts,
handbook of creativity (pp. 381394). New York, NY: 8, 5361.
Cambridge University Press. Simonton, D. K., & Song, A. V. (2009). Eminence, IQ, physi-
Simon, H. A. (1955). On a class of skew distribution functions. cal and mental health, and achievement domain: Coxs 282
Biometrika, 42, 425440. geniuses revisited. Psychological Science, 20, 429434.
Simonton, D. K. (1997). Creative productivity: A predictive and Stavridou, A., & Furnham, A. (1996). The relationship between
explanatory model of career trajectories and landmarks. psychoticism, trait-creativity and the attentional mecha-
Psychological Review, 104, 6689. nism of cognitive inhibition. Personality and Individual
Simonton, D. K. (2008). Distribution, normal. In W. A. Darity Jr. Differences, 21, 143153.
(Ed.), International encyclopedia of the social sciences (2nd Walberg, H. J., Strykowski, B. F., Rovai, E., & Hung, S. S. (1984).
ed., Vol. 2, pp. 415417). Detroit, MI: Macmillan Reference Exceptional performance. Review of Educational Research,
USA. 54, 87112.
Simonton, D. K. (2009). Varieties of (scientific) creativity: A hier- Zusne, L. (1985). Contributions to the history of psychol-
archical model of disposition, development, and achieve- ogy: XXXVIII. The hyperbolic structure of eminence.
ment. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4, 441452. Psychological Reports, 57, 12131214.

Downloaded from pps.sagepub.com at E-nformation SRL on March 8, 2016

Вам также может понравиться