Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 12

13 066644 PANAGOPOULOU (bc-t) 15/6/06 3:26 pm Page 809

Social Sharing of
Emotion in
Anticipation of Journal of Health Psychology
Copyright 2006 SAGE Publications
Cardiac Surgery London, Thousand Oaks and New Delhi,
www.sagepublications.com
Vol 11(5) 809820
Effects on Preoperative DOI: 10.1177/1359105306066644

Distress

EFHARIS PANAGOPOULOU
Aristotle University, Thessaloniki, Greece Abstract
This study examined associations
STAN MAES between social sharing of
Leiden University, The Netherlands
emotions, social support and
preoperative psychological distress.
BERNARD RIM
Social sharing was operationalized
University of Louvain La Neuve, Belgium
in terms of quantity and quality.
Quantity of social sharing was
ANTHONY MONTGOMERY
Royal College of Surgeons in IrelandMedical University measured daily using a diary
of Bahrain, Kingdom of Bahrain method. Quality of social sharing
was measured with the Quality of
Sharing Inventory. Participants
were 157 hospitalized Greek
patients. Hierarchical regression
analysis showed that controlling
for distress on admission, quality of
social sharing was negatively
related to distress one day before
surgery (p < .01). No effects were
found for quantity of social
sharing. Results emphasize the
importance of studying qualitative
aspects of emotional disclosure.

COMPETING INTERESTS: None declared.


Keywords
ADDRESS. Correspondence should be directed to:
E F H A R I S PA NAG O P O U L O U , Medical School, Aristotle University of
cardiac surgery
Thessaloniki, A. Svolou, 2, 54622, Thessaloniki, Greece. emotional disclosure
[email: EFHARIS@the.forthnet.gr] social sharing
809
13 066644 PANAGOPOULOU (bc-t) 15/6/06 3:26 pm Page 810

JOURNAL OF HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY 11(5)

P S Y C H O L O G I C A L research over the past way they share them. Yet quality of emotional
decades has attempted to establish a link disclosure might be equally or more important
between verbal emotional disclosure and than the extent of emotional disclosure
health. Studies based on Pennebakers (1989) (Kosmicki & Glickauf-Hughes, 1997). Quali-
writing paradigm have consistently shown the tative issues have been partially addressed only
beneficial effects of writing about emotional in relation to the content of disclosure. For
upheavals in a systematic way (Smyth, 1998). example, Pennebaker and Beall (1986) have
Writing about personal experiences in three or examined the differential effects of writing
four consecutive lab sessions resulted in more about factual aspects of the emotional episode
changes in autonomic and muscular activity as compared to writing about feeling aspects.
(Francis & Pennebaker, 1992; Pennebaker, Greenberg and Stone (1992) have explored the
Hughes, & OHeeron, 1987), enhanced immune impact of disclosing traumas of different sever-
functioning (Pennebaker, Kiecolt-Glaser, & ity. However, no systematic attempt has been
Glaser, 1988; Petrie, Booth, Pennebaker, made to theoretically address and empirically
Davison, & Thomas, 1995), fewer visits to assess quality of emotional disclosure as a deter-
medical centers (Pennebaker & Hoover, 1986) minant of physical health and well-being.
and fewer self-reports of physical symptoms Yet, psychological well-being is inextricably
(Greenberg & Stone, 1992), as compared to linked to the quality of social interactions
writing about trivial topics or not writing at all. (Nwesom, Nishishiba, Morgan, & Rook, 2003).
However, studies exploring the impact of While a wide range of studies has demonstrated
disclosure developed spontaneously after an the benefits of positive social interactions,
emotional experience, and not induced in a lab several studies have also examined a series of
setting, have shown a different pattern of results: negative social interactions that have detri-
in a series of recall and diary studies, Rim and mental effects on psychological adjustment
colleagues (Rim, Finkenauer, Luminet, Zech, (Berkman, Glass, Brissette, & Seeman, 2000;
& Philippot, 1998; Rim, Philippot, Boca, & Kiekolt-Glaser, Glaser, Cacioppo, & Malarkey,
Mesquita, 1992) have examined whether social 1998; Rook, 1992). Indeed, studies show that
sharing of an emotional experience leads to negative interactions have a greater impact on
emotional recovery. Social sharing was defined as well-being than positive interactions (Rook,
the telling of an emotional event in a socially 1990). In these studies positive interactions
shared language, while emotional recovery was referred to acceptance, understanding, sympa-
defined as the difference between the initial and thy or caring, while negative interactions
the residual intensity of the emotion elicited by referred to criticism, rejection, violation of
the event (Rim, Mesquita, Philippot, & Boca, privacy, failure to provide help or lack of reci-
1991). No significant associations were found procity (Krause & Jay, 1991; Krause & Rook,
between social sharing of emotion and indices of 2003; Nwesom et al., 2003; Okun & Keith, 1998;
emotional recovery (Rim et al., 1992, 1998). Smith & Ingram, 2004). Based on the differen-
Such a discrepancy between studies conducted tial impact of quality of social exchanges on
in the lab and studies conducted in the field psychological adjustment, it is important to
highlights the need to examine more closely the explore the extent to which quality of emotional
assumptions underlying the study of emotional disclosure influences psychological adjustment.
disclosure and health. Indeed the majority of The second limitation concerns the fact that
studies exploring spontaneous or induced studies have not sufficiently explored the associ-
emotional disclosure have been characterized ation of emotional disclosure to availability of
by three limitations. social support. Social support has been favorably
The first limitation concerns the fact that associated with various health outcomes (Cohen
studies have overemphasized the quantitative & Wills, 1985; Hobfoll & Vaux, 1993; Sarason,
aspects of emotional disclosure, and have not Sarason, & Pierce, 1988). However studies have
sufficiently addressed issues concerning quality not yet examined whether the impact of
(Kennedy-Moore & Watson, 1999; Rim, 1995). emotional disclosure on health and well-being is
Empirical evidence so far is based on how much dependent on the social support available to the
people share their feelings, rather than on the individual. Studies on social interactions show
810
13 066644 PANAGOPOULOU (bc-t) 15/6/06 3:26 pm Page 811

PANAGOPOULOU ET AL.: SOCIAL SHARING AND PREOPERATIVE DISTRESS

that the impact of negative social interactions on emotion, involving the telling of an emotional
well-being does not depend on the level of social experience to some addressee in a socially
support (Rook, 1992; Siegel, Raveis, & Karus, shared language (Rim et al., 1991). For the
1994). In other words, negative social inter- purposes of the study, we focused on the social
actions may have a detrimental effect on psycho- sharing of emotions related to the forthcoming
logical adjustment independent of the positive operation, and distinguished between quantity
effects of social support (Ingram, Jones, Fass, and quality of social sharing. Quantity of social
Neidig, & Song, 1999; Mindes, Ingram, Kliewer, sharing was defined as the extent to which
& James, 2003). It is therefore important to patients shared their feelings in regard to the
explore the extent to which the influence of forthcoming operation. Quality of sharing
quality of emotional disclosure on well-being assessed the extent to which patients reported
depends on availability of social support. intrapersonal (ventilation, relief), and interper-
The third limitation concerns the fact that sonal (receiving understanding, support, being
studies have mostly focused on healthy indi- listened to) benefits to the sharing situation
viduals. Indeed, studies focusing on patients, (Zech & Rim, 2003). Thus high-quality sharing
using emotional disclosure as an independent involved the disclosure of emotional material in
predictor of health improvement, have been a private, pleasant and understanding context,
rare. In a recent meta-analysis exploring the evoking supportive reactions and reassuring
independent effects of emotional disclosure, or feedback. Low-quality sharing involved
emotional inhibition on health, only 15 out of disclosure of more superficial topics in a more
171 studies published between 1980 and 1999, judgmental, less private and tenser context,
were conducted in patient populations initiating reactions that were perceived to be
(Panagopoulou, Kersbergen, & Maes, 2002). The inappropriate, negative or repetitive.
meta-analysis highlighted the need to explore Three hypotheses were examined in this
the implication of emotional disclosure in study: the first hypothesis was based on studies
improving the course of a chronic disease, or showing no associations between quantity of
facilitating the outcome of a medical treatment. social sharing and emotional recovery (Rim
Based on the limitations reviewed with regard et al., 1998) and predicted that quantity of social
to the study of emotional disclosure and health, sharing would have no influence on preopera-
the present study had two goals: the first goal tive distress. Based on the differential impact of
was to investigate the effects of quantity and negative and positive social exchanges on
quality of emotional disclosure on psychological psychological adjustment (Krause & Rook,
distress before cardiac surgery, and addressed 2003), the second hypothesis predicted that
the first limitation. Previous studies have quality of social sharing would be negatively
systematically linked preoperative distress to associated to preoperative distress. Finally,
postoperative recovery, adaptation and quality based on evidence suggesting that the extent to
of life (Andrew, Baker, Kneebone, & Knight, which negative social interactions predict
2000; Bruin, Schaefer, Krohne, & Dreyer, 2001; psychological adjustment does not depend on
Kain, Sevarino, Alexander, Pincus, & Mayes, availability of social support (Ingram et al.,
2000; van der Zee, Huet, Gallandat, & Evers, 1999), the third hypothesis predicted that the
2002). Identifying factors influencing pre- effects of quality of sharing on preoperative
operative distress could help us design inter- distress would be independent to availability of
ventions enhancing patient adjustment before social support. Emotional expressiveness as a
and after surgery. The second goal of the study personality trait, and clinical severity were also
was to explore associations of quality of included in the study as control variables.
emotional disclosure to availability of social
support and addressed the second limitation.
Method
Both research goals concerned psychosocial
adjustment of cardiac patients and addressed Participants
the third limitation. All consecutive patients admitted for non-
In the present study, emotional disclosure was urgent coronary artery bypass grafting (CABG)
operationalized as the social sharing of an in the cardio thoracic surgery unit of a city
811
13 066644 PANAGOPOULOU (bc-t) 15/6/06 3:26 pm Page 812

JOURNAL OF HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY 11(5)

hospital in Greece, between January 1998 and Table 1. Demographic and medical characteristics of
February 1999 were considered potentially the sample of the study (%) N = 157
eligible for the study. Inclusion criteria were (a)
fluency in Greek, (b) physical mobility, (c) Characteristics %
ability to complete psychological question-
Gender
naires, and respond to an interview situation and Male 91.7
(d) minimum scheduled preoperative stay of Female 8.3
two days. Patients using psychoactive medi-
cation were excluded. During the period of data Age
<30 2.0
collection 256 patients were admitted for non-
3140 2.0
urgent CABG in the clinic. Ten patients refused 4150 11.2
to participate; 16 were excluded due to inability 5160 24.5
to understand Greek, or complete the question- 6170 47.7
naires; 27 were scheduled for operation the day >71 12.6
after admission; 12 left the hospital without
Occupational status
undergoing the operation; 12 could not complete Working 40.1
the first interview due to physical pain, tiredness Non-working 7.7
or severe stress symptoms; 4 patients were Retired 52.2
excluded due to hearing problems and 1 due to
Marital status
medication for psychotic episodes. Finally due
Married 87.8
to hospital administration procedures, 17 Single 2.6
patients who were admitted for operation were Divorced 3.8
not contactable. Widowed 5.8
The final sample was composed of 157 patients
New York Heart Association
representing a completion rate of 64 percent.
classification (NYHA)
The participants were predominantly male (n = I 17.5
144), and married (n = 137). Sixty percent of the II 26.5
participants (n = 94) were 60 years old or older. III 35.0
Table 1 shows a detailed description of the IV 21.0
sample.
Grafts
1 10.9
Materials 2 24.6
Quantity of sharing was measured daily, with 3 43.5
one item from the Social Sharing Diary 4 21.10
(Panagopoulou & Maes, 2003). The Social
Previous MI
Sharing Diary (SSD) was constructed based on Yes 46.8
the diary method employed by Rim and No 53.2
colleagues (1998). It comprises of one page for
each preoperative day. Patients rated on a
5-point Likert scale (1 = not at all, 5 = very much associated with one-time retrospective state-
so) the extent to which they had shared their ments (for further information on the psycho-
feelings about the surgery on that particular metric properties of the SSD see Panagopoulou
day. The final score for each patient was & Maes, 2003). The internal consistency co-
obtained by dividing the sum of the daily scores efficient for the sample of this study was satis-
by the number of preoperative days (range: factory ( = 0.93).
15). Ethical and practical considerations Quality of sharing was measured on the day
arising from the severity of the situation faced before surgery with the Quality of Sharing
by the patients prohibited the assessment of Inventory (QSI), which was constructed for the
quantity of sharing using more than one item. purposes of the study. Items for the QSI were
However, the use of multiple, daily assessments, generated by patients, through open-ended
was expected to increase the reliability of the interviews with 10 hospitalized heart-patients
instrument, by avoiding memory biases often who were asked the following question: During
812
13 066644 PANAGOPOULOU (bc-t) 15/6/06 3:26 pm Page 813

PANAGOPOULOU ET AL.: SOCIAL SHARING AND PREOPERATIVE DISTRESS

your stay in the hospital, under what circum- difficulties. The final score for each patient
stances did talking with other people make you consisted of the sum of all answers (range:
feel better/worse? An additional sample of 10 016).
healthy people were asked to indicate under Preoperative distress was measured twice, on
what circumstances did talking with other admission day ( = 0.78), and on the day before
people in general, made them feel better/worse. surgery ( = 0.80), with the negative affect
Based on the distinction made by Zech and subscale of the Positive Affect Negative Affect
Rim (2003) between intrapersonal and inter- Scale (PANAS; Watson & Clark, 1994). The NA
personal benefits of social sharing, responses scale includes 10 items measuring negative
were coded into two categories: the QSI- emotions (e.g. sad; nervous). Patients rated
Interpersonal category included responses on a 5-point Likert scale, the intensity they had
referring to the appropriateness, understanding experienced each emotion on the day of the
and supportiveness of the sharing listener (The interview. The total score on negative affect for
persons I share my thoughts and feelings with, each patient consisted of the sum of all items
understand exactly what I mean), as well as to (range: 1050).
perceptions of privacy, and comfort of the Clinical severity of the cardiac condition was
sharing context, i.e. (During the conversation assessed independently by a cardiologist, and
I feel comfortable). The QSI-Intrapersonal the interviewer, using the New York Heart
category included responses referring to how Association classification (NYHA). A second
personal, and emotional the content of sharing cardiologist was consulted in case of disagree-
was (During the conversation, I reveal lots of ment. Patients were classified in four categories
things about myself), and the extent to which (I, II, III, IV), based on the severity and
social sharing interfered with participants frequency of symptoms and restrictions, experi-
thoughts, feelings and behavior (The discussions enced as a result of anginal pain. The least
I am having influence the way I feel for the severely ill patients were classified in category I,
better). One item was then constructed for each whereas the most severely ill patients were clas-
response in each coding category. An equal sified in category IV.
number of positively and negatively worded Emotional expressiveness as an individual
items were constructed. Items were reviewed difference was assessed on admission day, using
for their relevance, clarity and conciseness by the Emotional Expressiveness Questionnaire
four experts, and four heart-patients. The final (EEQ; King & Emmons, 1990). The EEQ
scale comprised of 23 items: 15 items referred measures the disposition to be generally
to interpersonal characteristics and 8 items emotionally expressive (verbally and non-
referred to intrapersonal characteristics of verbally), and not the disclosure of specific
quality of sharing. Respondents rated on a emotions. It comprised of 16 items (e.g. I often
5-point Likert scale the extent to which they tell people I care for them; When I really like
agreed with each statement (1 = totally disagree, someone, I show it). The ) level for the EEQ
5 = totally agree). scale was less than satisfactory ( = 0.60).
Social support was assessed on admission day Removal of items did not influence the  level,
with the social support subscale of the Leiden so it was decided to use the 16-item scale while
Screening Questionnaire for Heart patients accepting that the internal consistency was less
(van Elderen, Chatrou, Weeda, & Maes, 1997). than good (range: 1680).
The scale measures perceived availability of
emotional, instrumental, informational and Procedure
esteem support. For the purposes of the study Interviews were conducted in a private room by
only the subscale on perceived emotional two trained postgraduate clinical psychology
support was used ( = 0.70). The respondents students. Patients were approached in their
received a list of 8 possible support providers wards by one of the interviewers between 18:00
and rated on a 3-point Likert scale (0 = not at and 20:00 on admission day. They were asked to
all, 1 = partially, 2 = completely) the extent to participate in a research project investigating
which they could rely on each of them every the way patients deal with the experience of
time they faced personal problems or emotional cardiac surgery. After informed consent was
813
13 066644 PANAGOPOULOU (bc-t) 15/6/06 3:26 pm Page 814

JOURNAL OF HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY 11(5)

obtained from them, patients were administered One item (8) loaded on both factors and was
the psychological distress and emotional also omitted from the scale. The analysis was re-
support scales under the form of a structured run after the exclusion of the four items. The
interview. At the end of this interview, patients loadings of the final 19 items are displayed in
were also given the social sharing diary with the Table 2.
appropriate instructions. To make sure patients Consistent with our prediction, all items refer-
understood the questions, they were asked to ring to interpersonal attributes of sharing,
complete the first (admission) day with the loaded on factor QSI-interpersonal (QSI-inter),
interviewer. A second interview took place on accounting for 16.52 percent of the explained
the day before surgery. Due to hospital adminis- variance, while all items referring to intra-
tration procedures, the length of preoperative personal attributes of sharing loaded on the
waiting was different for each patient. Conse- second factor, QSI-intrapersonal (QSI-intra). In
quently, the second interview took place between addition to the factor analysis, confirmatory
the third and the tenth day of preoperative factor analysis (CFA) was conducted using
hospitalization. Patients completed the last structural equation modeling (SEM) analysis,
preoperative day of the diary with the inter- AMOS (Arbuckle, 1997). The maximum likeli-
viewer during the second interview, and they hood method was used to examine the covari-
were also administered the Quality of Sharing ance matrices of the items. In addition to
Inventory, and the psychological distress scale. exploratory factor analysis, confirmatory factor
analysis (CFA) was conducted to test the factor
structure of the QSI. This involved comparing
Results
the two-factor solution with the one-factor solu-
Out of 157 participants interviewed on admis- tion, as per the methodology recommended by
sion day, 9 had to be urgently re-scheduled for Byrne (2001). Results indicated that the two-
an operation on the day following admission, factor model indicated a much better fit than the
and 6 refused to continue with the study on the one-factor solution, (2(1) = 108.95, p < .001).
second interview. In total 142 patients partici- Therefore, the results indicate that the two-
pated in the second interview. Data on the factor solution represents the best fit to the data.
Social Sharing diary were available from 152 Two subscales for the Quality of Sharing
participants. Results showed that 91 percent of Inventory were then computed based on the two
patients shared their feelings to some extent, factors: the subscale of interpersonal quality of
while 15 percent did so to a moderate or large sharing consisted of 9 items ( = .70), and the
extent. No significant differences in quantity of subscale of intrapersonal quality of sharing
social sharing were found between the first and consisted of 10 items ( =.76). Similar internal
last preoperative day. consistency coefficients were found, when the
sample was split in two subsamples of different
Psychometric properties of the clinical severity. The correlation of the two
QSI subscales for the total sample was r = .30, p < .01.
Using principal component analysis, a 2-factor Table 3 reveals further information on the
solution was extracted (rotated using Varimax) psychometric properties of the two subscales.
and accounted for 30.7 percent of the explained No association was found between quantity of
variance. Three items (2, 20, 17) did not load on sharing and interpersonal quality of sharing,
any of the two factors, and were omitted from while a small positive association was found
the scale. Based on our operationalization of between quantity of sharing and intrapersonal
quality of sharing, it was expected that items quality of sharing (r = .23, p < .01). Interpersonal
referring to interpersonal attributes of sharing quality of sharing was not related to emotional
would load onto one factor (QSI-Interper- support, or emotional expressiveness. A small
sonal), while items referring to intrapersonal positive association was observed between
attributes of sharing would load onto the second intrapersonal quality of sharing and emotional
factor (QSI-intrapersonal). support (r = .31, p < .01), and between intra-
Using the criterion of factor loadings >.35, 20 personal quality of sharing and emotional
items loaded significantly onto the two factors. expressiveness (r = .19, p < .05). None of the
814
13 066644 PANAGOPOULOU (bc-t) 15/6/06 3:26 pm Page 815

PANAGOPOULOU ET AL.: SOCIAL SHARING AND PREOPERATIVE DISTRESS

Table 2. Factor loadings of QSI items after orthogonal rotation with two factors

Item QSI-intrapersonal QSI-interpersonal

18. In the conversations Im having I share interesting things .72 .17


about myself
4. During the conversation, I reveal lots of things about myself .67 .29
3. After talking to somebody, I feel relieved .61 .15
7. I feel that peoples reactions every time I talk about my .61 .22
thoughts and feelings help me a lot
13. My conversations are rather superficial .54 .01
24. The conversations I am having influence the way I feel for .52 .29
the better
9. I feel that people think highly of me every time I share my .51 .01
thoughts and feelings
21. The conversations I am having are very personal .47 .09
1. Every time I feel the need to talk to somebody, there is .40 .08
always the right person around
11. Most of the times I am the one who initiates the conversations .39 .08
5. I feel that people misunderstand me when I share my thoughts .01 .75
and feelings
12. The persons I share my thoughts and feelings with, understand .23 .66
exactly what I mean
10. During the conversations I feel comfortable .11 .56
23. When I talk with other people I feel as if there is a distance .27 .54
between them
16. During the conversation there are lots of arguments .28 .53
14. People always say the same things every time I talk about my .21 .51
thoughts and feelings
19. I feel that people get the wrong idea about me every time I .04 .44
talk about my thoughts and feelings
15. During the conversations I find myself in an awkward .03 .41
position, as if I dont know what to say
6. I would have preferred to share my thoughts and feelings with .08 .36
other persons, than the ones I share them with now

Note: Items 2, 5, 6, 13, 14, 15, 16, 19, 23 are to be reversed scored

correlations were high enough to suggest multi- was different for each patient, number of preop-
collinearity (Tabachnik & Fidel, 1996). erative days was not related to any other vari-
able included in the study. Therefore, it was
Associations with preoperative excluded from further analyses.
distress As shown in Table 3, distress one day before
The mean score for distress one day before surgery was positively related to quantity of
surgery was 14.66 (SD = 5.05). Paired samples sharing (r = .23, p < .01), and negatively related
t-test showed a small significant decrease of to interpersonal quality of sharing (r = .47,
distress from admission day to the day before p < .01). No associations were found between
surgery: t(140) = 2.994, p < .01. emotional support and preoperative distress.
Results showed no association between In order to test the three hypotheses, hier-
preoperative distress and emotional expressive- archical regression analysis was performed.
ness, severity of heart disease or intrapersonal Step one tested for the independent effect of
quality of sharing, and therefore these variables distress on admission. In step two, interpersonal
were not included in further analyses. Addition- quality of sharing, quantity of sharing and
ally, although the number of preoperative days emotional support were included. In step three
815
13 066644 PANAGOPOULOU (bc-t) 15/6/06 3:26 pm Page 816

JOURNAL OF HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY 11(5)

Table 3. Means, standard deviations and correlations (Pearsons r) with preoperative distress (N = 142)

Mean
(SD) 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8

1. Quality of sharing-intrapersonal 32.33 .30** .23** .31** .19* NS NS


(7.91)
2. Quality of sharing-interpersonal 34.95 NS NS NS .37** .47**
(6.45)
3. Quantity of social sharing 2.39 .20* .17* .25** .23**
(.82)
4. Emotional support 4.82 .19* NS NS
(2.29)
5. Emotional expressiveness 55.72 NS NS NS
(8.82)
6. Distress on admission 15.85 .64** NS
(5.59)
7. Distress on day before surgery 14.66
(5.05) NS

Note: *p < .05; **p < .01; NS = non significant

the interaction term of interpersonal quality of Results showed no significant F change for
sharing and emotional support (emotional the third step, indicating no interaction between
support x quality) was added in order to test for emotional support and interpersonal quality of
interaction effects (Aiken & West, 1991). An sharing.
interaction effect was considered present only if
the F change of the step including the inter-
Discussion
action term was significant (Holmbeck, 1997).
As shown in Table 4, controlling for the effects Overall, the study indicated support for the
of distress on admission day, the only predictor three hypotheses postulated. Concerning our
of distress one day before surgery was interper- first hypothesis, results confirmed the prediction
sonal quality of sharing: the more patients that the extent to which patients share their
perceived that the interpersonal quality of their emotions in regard to the forthcoming opera-
sharing was high, the less distressed they felt one tion does not influence how distressed they feel
day before surgery. one day before surgery. This finding is in agree-
ment with previous studies showing no signifi-
cant associations between spontaneous social
Table 4. Hierarchical regression analysis for distress
sharing and emotional recovery (Rim et al.,
one day before surgery
1992). Results also confirmed our second
Standardized hypothesis, the prediction that high-quality
coefficients sharing of emotions is associated with reduced
(Beta) of step 2 distress on the day before surgery. Overall, the
study suggests that when it comes to feeling
Distress on admission .506** distressed, the extent to which people share
Interpersonal quality of sharing .275** their feelings is not as influential as the way they
Quantity of sharing .037 do it. Along the same lines Rim (1995) has
Emotional support .047
suggested that the critical factor in the study of
Note: Step 1: Distress on admission (R2 = .396). sharing of emotions lies less in quantitative
Step 2: Distress on admission, total quality of sharing, aspects of sharing and more in qualitative. For
quantity of sharing (R2 = .453, R2 change = .065, example, in a study by Zech and Rim (2003) it
F change (2,132) = 8.025, p = < .01) was shown that perceived intrapersonal and
*p < .05; **p < .01 interpersonal benefits of social sharing were
816
13 066644 PANAGOPOULOU (bc-t) 15/6/06 3:26 pm Page 817

PANAGOPOULOU ET AL.: SOCIAL SHARING AND PREOPERATIVE DISTRESS

able to discriminate between sharing and were not related to psychological distress one
control conditions, while emotional recovery day before surgery. However, previous studies
was not. In addition it has been shown that have shown that the impact of emotional
expressing skills, social feedback and empathy disclosure on health and well-being depends
shown by the listeners, might be more important also on how emotional, personal or stressful the
than the extent to which someone expresses content of the disclosure is (Greenberg & Stone,
his/her feelings (Kosmicki & Glickauf-Hughes, 1992; Pennebaker & Beall, 1986). However in
1997). In regard to the third hypothesis results these studies the content of disclosure was
supported the prediction that the association either controlled, or qualitatively assessed by
between quality of social sharing and preopera- the researcher, while in the present study it was
tive distress was not dependent on perceived participants who evaluated how personal or
availability of emotional support. Results revealing the content of their sharing was. It is
confirm previous findings showing that the possible that the influence of the content of
extent to which negative social interactions emotional disclosure depends on the method
predict psychological well-being is not depen- used to assess it.
dent on the individuals level of social support The observed difference between quantity
(Ingram et al., 1999; Mindes et al., 2003). and quality of social sharing in this study could
This study presents an attempt to theoreti- explain some of the discrepancies reported in
cally address and empirically assess quality of previous studies, between spontaneous and
sharing of emotions as a determinant of psycho- induced emotional disclosure (Rim, 1995;
logical adjustment. In order to assess quality of Rim et al., 1998). Writing about emotional
social sharing, the Quality of Sharing Inventory experiences in a lab setting, controls for the
was developed. Results showed that quality and influence of social and contextual character-
quantity are independent and unrelated aspects istics. However, social and contextual character-
of social sharing. They also suggest that quality istics can influence the impact of emotional
of social sharing is considerably different from disclosure occurring spontaneously in real
external and internal resources (emotional settings.
support, and emotional expressiveness). Psycho- In contrast to previous studies showing the
metric analysis of the new instrument revealed benefits of social support, results of this study
two underlying factors, confirming the assump- showed no associations between emotional
tion that quality of social sharing refers to inter- support and preoperative distress. This could be
personal as well as intrapersonal characteristics attributed to the fact that emotional support was
(Zech & Rim, 2003). assessed in terms of a general context, and not
With regard to the interpersonal attributes of in terms of a hospital-specific setting. Previous
quality of sharing the study showed that social- studies have shown the beneficial effects of
contextual characteristics of sharing predicted social support when assessed in terms of hospi-
psychological distress one day before surgery: tal visits or support received by other patients
sharing emotions in a pleasant, understanding (Kulik & Mahler, 1989; Kulik, Mahler, & Moore,
context, evoking supportive reactions and reas- 1996). An alternative explanation for the lack of
suring feedback was associated with decreased direct effects of emotional support could be that
distress one day before surgery. On the contrary the scale used in the study assesses quantitative
sharing emotions in a tense, unsupportive aspects of social support rather than qualitative
context evoking repetitive or unhelpful reactions aspects. Previous studies have shown that it is
was associated with increased distress one day not the availability (actual, or perceived) of
before surgery. Similarly, studies on negative social resources that influences peoples psycho-
social interactions have shown the detrimental logical state, but whether and how people make
impact of unsupportive social responses on use of them, when they share their feelings
psychological well-being (Smith & Ingram, (Costanza, Derlega, & Winstead, 1988; Derlega,
2004). Metts, Petronio, & Margulis, 1993).
Alternatively, with regard to intrapersonal The difference between quantitative and
attributes of quality of sharing the study shows qualitative aspects of social sharing as well as
that characteristics of the content of sharing the lack of influence of emotional support in this
817
13 066644 PANAGOPOULOU (bc-t) 15/6/06 3:26 pm Page 818

JOURNAL OF HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY 11(5)

study could be attributed to the context in which This study was the first attempt to systemati-
they were measured. When in hospital people cally explore the link between quality of social
can count on high levels of social support and sharing of emotions and psychological well-
opportunities to share. It is possible that quan- being. Further analysis is needed to confirm the
tity or emotional support might emerge as underlying structure of the new concept as a
relevant factors in other settings (e.g. when combination of social-contextual and individ-
dealing when a personal loss, or a disease diag- ual-personal characteristics, and explore their
nosis at home). differential impact on psychological adjustment.
Given the role of preoperative distress in
influencing intra- and postoperative adaptation, Note
findings of this study can be used for the design
of effective interventions aiming at reducing 1. Due to the small number of female participants, no
preoperative distress (Salmon, 1992). In-hospital separate analyses were conducted for male and
female participants. When the analyses were re-
interventions, could focus on promoting success-
run including only male participants no differences
ful processing of surgery-related emotions, by
were found in the structure and strength of the
creating the conditions for patients to disclose emerged relationships.
their feelings in a safe, understanding environ-
ment, or by educating support systems about
appropriate, stress-reducing responses, and References
supportive feedback. Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression:
The results of the present study should be Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park,
treated with caution due to several limitations. CA: Sage.
Quality of social sharing was measured with a Andrew, M. J., Baker, R. A., Kneebone, A. C., &
newly constructed instrument (QSI). Although Knight, J. L. (2000). Mood state as a predictor of
the initial psychometric properties of the instru- neuropsychological deficits following cardiac
surgery. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 48,
ment were satisfactory, more research is needed
537546.
in order to test its convergent and discriminant
Arbuckle, J. L. (1997). Amos users guide version 3.6.
validity, and the underlying factor structure Chicago, IL: Smallwaters Corporation.
identified in this study. In addition interpret- Berkman, L. F., Glass, T., Brissette, I., & Seeman, T. E.
ation of findings could be potentially under- (2000). From social interaction to health: Durkheim
mined by the fact that quantity of sharing was in the new millennium. Social Science and Medicine,
measured on a daily basis, while quality of 51, 843857.
sharing were measured only once. Consequently Bruin, J. T., Schaefer, M. K., Krohne, H. W., & Dreyer,
quality of sharing was not measured for the A. (2001). Preoperative anxiety, coping, and intra-
specific interactions the sharing diary referred operative adjustment: Are there mediating effects
of stress-induced analgesia? Psychology & Health,
to. In addition, although the study tested for the
16, 253271.
effects of distress on admission day, a longitudi-
Byrne, B. M. (2001). Structural equation modeling
nal design can further examine the identified with AMOS, EQS, and LISREL: Comparative
pattern of associations. approaches to testing for the factorial validity of a
Due to the nature of heart disease and the measuring instrument. International Journal of
consecutive sampling procedure, women were Testing, 1, 5586.
underrepresented in the sample of this study.1 Cohen, S., & Wills, T. A. (1985). Stress, social support,
Therefore, results of the study can be general- and the buffering hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin,
ized mainly to male populations. Future 98, 310357.
research should also try to explore the effects of Costanza, R. S., Derlega, V. J., & Winstead, B. A.
(1988). Positive and negative forms of social
other types of sharing, such as sharing about
support: Effects of conversational topics on coping
topics irrelevant to the surgery, as well as the
with stress among same-sex friends. Journal of
differential effects of sharing with different Experimental Social Psychology, 24, 182193.
people. Finally, the relationships identified in Derlega, V. J., Metts, S., Petronio, S., & Margulis, S. T.
the study should be tested for positive preoper- (1993). Coping with stress and social support. In Self-
ative emotions and extended for postoperative disclosure (pp. 89109). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
physical and psychological outcomes. Francis, M. E., & Pennebaker, J. W. (1992). Putting

818
13 066644 PANAGOPOULOU (bc-t) 15/6/06 3:26 pm Page 819

PANAGOPOULOU ET AL.: SOCIAL SHARING AND PREOPERATIVE DISTRESS

stress into words: The impact of writing on physio- Kulik, J. A., Mahler, H. I. M., & Moore, P. J. (1996).
logical, absentee, and self-reported emotional Social comparison and affiliation under threat:
well-being measures. American Journal of Health Effects from recovery from major surgery. Journal
Promotion, 6, 280287. of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 967979.
Greenberg, M. A., & Stone, A. A. (1992). Emotional Mindes, E. J., Ingram, K. M., Kliewer, W., & James,
disclosure about traumas and its relation to health: C. A. (2003). Longitudinal analyses of the relation-
Effects of previous disclosure and trauma severity. ship between unsupportive social interactions and
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, psychological adjustment among women with
7584. fertility problems. Social Science & Medicine, 56,
Hobfoll, S. E., & Vaux,A. (1993). Social support: Social 21652180.
resources and social context. In L. Goldbergen & Nwesom, J. T., Nishishiba, M., Morgan, D. L., & Rook,
S. Breznitz (Eds.), Handbook of stress: Theoretical K. (2003). The relative importance of three domains
and clinical aspects (pp. 685705). New York: The of positive and negative social exchanges: A longi-
Free Press. tudinal model with comparable measures. Psychol-
Holmbeck, G. N. (1997). Toward terminological, ogy and Aging, 18, 746754.
conceptual, and statistical clarity in the study of Okun, M. A., & Keith, V. M. (1998). Effects of positive
mediators and moderators: Examples from the and negative social exchanges with various sources
child-clinical and paediatric psychology literatures. of depressive symptoms in younger and older adults.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences,
599610. 53B, P4P20.
Ingram, K. M., Jones, D. A., Fass, R. J., Neidig, J. L., & Panagopoulou, E., Kersbergen, B., & Maes, S. (2002).
Song, Y. S. (1999). Social support and unsupportive Sick people, healthy patients: The effects of
social interactions: Their association and with emotional (non-) expression in (chronic) disease. A
depression among people living with HIV. AIDS meta-analytic review. Psychology and Health, 17,
Care, 11, 313329. 528545.
Kain, Z. N., Sevarino, F., Alexander, G. M., Pincus, S., Panagopoulou, E., & Maes, S. (2003). Social sharing in
& Mayes, L. C. (2000). Preoperative anxiety and anticipation of cardiac surgery: Who talks more and
postoperative pain in women undergoing hyster- why. Anxiety Stress, and Coping: An international
ectomy: A repeated-measure design. Journal of Journal, 16, 417430.
Psychosomatic Research, 49, 417422. Pennebaker, J. W. (1989). Confession, inhibition, and
Kennedy-Moore, E., & Watson, J. C. (1999). Express- disease. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experi-
ing emotions: Myths, realities, and therapeutic strat- mental social psychology (vol. 22, pp. 211244). New
egies. New York: Guilford Press. York: Academic Press.
Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., Glaser, R., Cacioppo, J. T., & Pennebaker, J. W., & Beall, S. (1986). Confronting a
Malarkey, W. B. (1998). Marital stress: Immuno- traumatic event: Toward an understanding of
logic, neuroendocrine, and autonomic correlates. In inhibition and disease. Journal of Abnormal Psychol-
S. M. McCann & J. M. Lipton (Eds.), Annals of the ogy, 95, 274281.
New York Academy of Sciences: Vol. 840. Neuro- Pennebaker, J. W., & Hoover, C. W. (1986). Inhibition
immunomodulation: Molecular aspects, integrated and cognition: Towards an understanding of trauma
systems, and clinical advances (pp. 656663). New and disease. In R. J. Davison, G. E. Schwartz, &
York: New York Academy of Sciences. D. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and self-regulation
King, L. A., & Emmons, R. A. (1990). Conflict over (vol. 4, pp. 107136). New York: Plenum Press.
emotional expression: Psychological and physical Pennebaker, J. W., Hughes, C. F., & OHeeron, R. C.
correlates. Journal of Personality and Social (1987). The psychophysiology of confession:
Psychology, 58, 864877. Linking inhibitory and psychosomatic processes.
Kosmicki, F. X., & Glickauf-Hughes, C. (1997). Cathar- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52,
sis in psychotherapy. Psychotherapy, 341, 54159. 781793.
Krause, N., & Jay, G. (1991). Stress, social support, and Pennebaker, J. W., Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., & Glaser, R.
negative interaction in later life. Research on Aging, (1988). Disclosure of traumas and immune function:
13, 333363. Health implications for psychotherapy. Journal of
Krause, N., & Rook, K. S. (2003). Negative interaction Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 239245.
in late life: Issues in the stability and generalizability Petrie, K. J., Booth, R. J., Pennebaker, J. W., Davison,
of conflict across relationships. Journal of Gerontol- K. P., & Thomas, M. G. (1995). Disclosure of trauma
ogy: Psychological Science, 58B, P88P99. and immune response to a hepatitis B vaccination
Kulik, J. A., & Mahler, H. I. M. (1989). Social support program. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
and recovery from surgery. Health Psychology, 8, Psychology, 63, 787792.
221238. Rim, B. (1995). Mental rumination, social sharing,

819
13 066644 PANAGOPOULOU (bc-t) 15/6/06 3:26 pm Page 820

JOURNAL OF HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY 11(5)

and the recovery from emotional exposure. In logical well-being of gay men with AIDS: Contri-
J. W. Pennebaker (Ed.), Emotion, disclosure, & bution of positive and negative illness-related
health (pp. 2546). Washington, DC: American network interactions to depressive mood. Social
Psychological Association. Science & Medicine, 39, 15551563.
Rim, B., Finkenauer, C., Luminet, O., Zech, E., & Smith, N. G., & Ingram, K. M. (2004). Workplace
Philippot, P. (1998). Social sharing of emotion: New heterosexism and adjustment among lesbian, gay,
evidence and new questions. In W. Stroebe & and bisexual individuals: The role of unsupportive
M. Hewstone (Eds.), European review of social social interactions. Journal of Counselling Psychol-
psychology (vol. 9., pp. 145189). Chichester: Wiley. ogy, 51, 5767.
Rim, B., Mesquita, B., Philippot, P., & Boca, S. (1991). Smyth, J. M. (1998). Written emotional expression:
Beyond the emotional event: Six studies on the Effect sizes, outcome types, and moderating vari-
social sharing of emotion. Cognition and Emotion, ables. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychol-
5, 435465. ogy, 66, 174184.
Rim, B., Philippot, P., Boca, S., & Mesquita, B. (1992). Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidel, A. S. (1996). Using multi-
Long-lasting cognitive and social consequences of variate statistics. New York: HarperCollins College
emotion: Social sharing and rumination. European Publishers.
Review of Social Psychology, 3, 225258. Van der Zee, K. I., Huet, R. C., Gallandat, C. C., &
Rook, K. S. (1990). Parallels in the study of social Evers, K. (2002). The influence of the premedication
support and social strain. Journal of Social and consult and preparatory information about anes-
Clinical Psychology, 9, 118132. thesia on anxiety among patients undergoing
Rook, K. S. (1992). Detrimental aspects of social cardiac surgery. Anxiety, Stress & Coping: An Inter-
relationships: Taking stock of an emerging litera- national Journal, 15, 123133.
ture. In H. O. F. Veiel & U. Baumann (Eds.), The Van Elderen, T., Chatrou, M., Weeda, H., & Maes, S.
meaning and measurement of social support (1997). Leidse screening vragenlijst voor hartpa-
(pp. 157169). Washington, DC: Hemisphere. tientenII. Leiden University, unpublished manu-
Salmon, P. (1992). Psychological factors in surgical script.
stress: Implications for management. Clinical Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1994). Manual for the
Psychology Review, 12, 681704. positive and negative affect scheduleexpanded
Sarason, I. G., Sarason, B. R., & Pierce, G. R. (1988). form. University of Iowa, unpublished manuscript.
Social support, personality and health. In S. Maes & Zech, E. & Rim, B. (2003). Is talking about an
C. D. Spielberger (Eds.), Topics in health psychology emotional experience helpful? Effects on emotional
(pp. 245256). Chichester: John Wiley & Sons. recovery and perceived benefits. Manuscript
Siegel, K., Raveis, V. H., & Karus, D. (1994). Psycho- submitted for publication.

Author biographies
E F H A R I S PA NAG O P O U L O U is a health is a social psychologist
B E R NA R D R I M
psychologist. She is currently the director the specializing in emotions. He is currently
communication skills training program of the professor of Social Psychology in the Catholic
Medical School of the Aristotle University of University of Louvain La Neuve in Belgium.
Thessaloniki.
A N T H O N Y M O N T G O M E RY is an
S TA N M A E Sis a health psychologist organizational health psychologist. He is
specializing in health behavior change. He is currently a senior lecturer in behavioral
currently a professor of Clinical and Health sciences in The Royal College of Surgeons in
Psychology in Leiden University in The IrelandMedical University of Bahrain.
Netherlands.

820

Вам также может понравиться