Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 11

Exp Brain Res (2004) 157: 286295

DOI 10.1007/s00221-004-1842-0

RESEARCH ARTICLES

Maria Pia Bucci . Zo Kapoula . Qing Yang .


Dominique Brmond-Gignac . Sylvette Wiener-Vacher

Speed-accuracy of saccades, vergence and combined eye


movements in children with vertigo

Received: 7 July 2003 / Accepted: 22 December 2003 / Published online: 13 March 2004
# Springer-Verlag 2004

Abstract Vergence abnormalities could lead to inap- tween the saccade and the vergence is present only for
propriate vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR), causing vertigo saccades combined with divergence; for saccades com-
and imbalance (Brandt 1999). Indeed, a recent study by bined with convergence such interaction is abnormal: the
Anoh-Tanon et al. (2000) reported the existence of a saccade is slowed down by the convergence but the
population of children with symptoms of vertigo in the convergence is not accelerated by the saccade. Orthoptic
absence of vestibular dysfunction but with abnormal training improves significantly the accuracy of all eye
vergence findings in orthoptic tests. The purpose of this movements; such improvement was significant for all
study was to examine in such children the accuracy, types of eye movements except for divergence (pure and
duration and mean velocity of vergence and saccades; combined). Furthermore, convergence remains abnormal
additionally, for a few subjects, the effect of orthoptic and the lack of acceleration by the saccade persists. These
vergence training on these parameters was also investi- specific convergence deficits could be of both subcortical
gated. LEDs were used to stimulate saccades, pure and cortical origin. Orthoptic training improves the
vergence along the median plane and combined saccade- accuracy presumably via visual attentional mechanisms,
vergence movements. Movements from both eyes were but cannot completely override deficits related to subcor-
recorded with a photoelectric device (Bouis). The results tical deficiencies.
show that children with vertigo perform saccades as
normal subjects of comparable age. In contrast, vergence, Keywords Accuracy . Duration . Speed . Saccade .
particularly convergence, shows abnormalities: poor ac- Vergence . Combined movements . Vertigo
curacy, long duration and low speed. During combined
movements, the well known reciprocal interaction be-
Introduction
M. P. Bucci . Z. Kapoula . Q. Yang
Laboratoire de Physiologie de la Perception et de 1lAction, For the visual exploration of the natural environment,
LPPA, CNRS-College de France, humans make at least three types of eye movements:
11 place M. Berthelot, saccades (rapid eye movements to change fixation
75005 Paris, France
horizontally or vertically), vergence eye movements (to
D. Brmond-Gignac adjust the angle of visual axes according to the distance of
Hpital Robert Debr, service dOphtalmologie, the object of interest), and combined saccade-vergence
48 Bld. Srurier, movements; the latter are the most common movements
75019 Paris, France used for exploring the natural world. During natural
S. Wiener-Vacher activity humans move the eyes and the head together, and
Hpital Robert Debr, service dORL, the vestibulo-ocular system stabilizes the image of interest
48 Bld. Srurier, on the fovea compensating the head movements. The
75019 Paris, France vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) should be adjusted accu-
M. P. Bucci (*) rately to the distance between the eyes and the target, and
Laboratoire de Physiologie de la Perception et de 1lAction, vergence is one of the signals that the brain can use for
UMR 9950, CNRS-College de France, distance adjustment (Leigh and Zee 1999). If the vergence
11 place Marcelin Berthelot, command is not accurate the VOR will be inappropriate;
75005 Paris, France
e-mail: maria-pia.bucci@college-de-france.fr consequently the image on the fovea will not be well
Tel.: +33-1-44271636 stabilized; such visual instability can cause vertigo and
Fax: +33-1-44271382 headache (Brandt 1999). Anoh-Tanon et al. (2000) showed
287

that about 5% of children consulting the ENT service for The second goal of the study is to explore eventual
vertigo and headaches symptoms have normal vestibular effects of orthoptic training on the spatiotemporal
functions but orthoptic vergence abnormalities; however, characteristics of eye movements.
objective studies on vergence eye movement character-
istics in children with or without vertigo are rather scarce.
Several studies examined latency of saccades in a large Methods
population of normal subjects (Fischer et al. 1997; Munoz
et al. 1998; Fukushima et al. 2000; Klein and Foerster Subject characteristics
2001) and they showed that its latency in children is longer
than in adults and that it shortens with age. Moreover, Eight young subjects participated in the study. The investigation
adhered to the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki and was
Yang et al. (2002) showed that latency of eye movements approved by the Human Experimentation Committee of our
in natural space (saccades and vergence), particularly institute. Subjects age ranged between 8 and 16 years (11.75
those of vergence, is long in young children, reaching 2.91). Subjects were admitted to the ENT and ophthalmology
adult values at the age of 1012 years. All these results service of the childrens hospital because they complained of vertigo
and headache. With vertigo we indicate subjective symptoms
suggest that the central/cortical circuitry involved in the experienced by all our children; more precisely, children felt the
initiation of eye movements is not completely developed environment moving around them or being in imbalance. Such
until 1012 years. symptoms were usually brief, lasting less than 1 min; however, they
In contrast, a recent study by our group (Yang and occurred several times during the day. Other than these symptoms,
no children had neurologic problems, and they did not receive any
Kapoula 2004) explored children from the age of 4.5 years medication. All children underwent a complete vestibular and
and showed that saccades, vergence and combined orthoptic examination (see below).
movements are similarly accurate (close to 90% of the
amplitude of the movement) in children from the age of
4.5 years and in adults; divergence is the slowest Vestibular examination
movement to execute for both children and adults.
Moreover, these authors did not found a significant The vestibular testing included a clinical vestibular examination and
difference in the peak velocity of all types of eye vestibulo-ocular response recording (see Wiener-Vacher et al. 1996,
for details). Briefly, the vestibular clinical examination included a
movements between children and adults. Their observa- caloric test (irrigation of each ear with 30 and 44C warm water).
tions indicate early maturation of all distinct brainstem Hearing examinations were also performed to assess the function of
structures controlling these movements and their interac- the inner ear. The electro-oculography (EOG) was used for
tion; this contrasts with the prior study on latencies (Yang recording the VOR responses to horizontal semi-circular canal and
otolith stimulations. More precisely, the canal VOR was elicited by
et al. 2002), which indicates slow progressive develop- rotatory impulsions about a vertical axis (acceleration and deceler-
ment of the cortical structures controlling the initiation of ation were at 40/s, separated by a rotation at 60/s). The otolith
eye movements. VOR was induced by inclining the chair by 9 with respect to
A recent study by Bucci et al. (2004) reported that in gravity. The results of all these tests were normal for all children
participating in this study.
children with vertigo in the absence of measurable
vestibular dysfunction, latency of eye movements is
abnormally long, particularly that of convergence; this Orthoptic examination before and after orthoptic training
could be due to central/cortical impairment of the
structures controlling the triggering of eye movements. All children had normal binocular vision (60 s of arc or better),
The purpose of the present study is to examine whether which was evaluated with the TNO random dot test for stereoscopic
children with vertigo with no abnormal findings of depth discrimination. No child wore spectacles. Orthoptic evaluation
vestibular function present abnormal trajectories of eye of vergence (made by using prisms, and the Maddox rod and
synoptophore technique) revealed for all subjects vergence abnorm-
movements, particularly of vergence. Indeed, given the alities: the distant near point of convergence was on average 9
complaints and the orthoptic findings for reduced vergence 2 cm, exophoria (i.e. latent deviation of one eye when the other eye
amplitudes (as measured clinically by the bar prism) one is covered) at near viewing was on average 82 pD; and there was a
could expect deficiency (inaccuracy and/or slowness) limited range of fusional vergence (at near: 207 pD and 104 pD
respectively for convergence and divergence).
particularly for convergence movements. Orthoptic training (12 sessions, 3 times per week) was prescribed
Finally, a reciprocal interaction between saccades and for all children. It consisted of several eye movement exercises (e.g.
vergence for combined movements in direction and depth converging and diverging the eyes to follow a small pen-light or a
is well known for adults (Zee et al. 1992; Collewijn et al. letter moving in depth) in order to improve the range of vergence
fusional amplitude (see for details www. childrensvision.com).
1995): the saccade is slowed down by the vergence while After this training a second orthoptic clinical examination was
the vergence is accelerated by the saccade; it seems also done for all children. Clinical signs of vergence disorders
that vergence acceleration is higher for convergence than disappeared: the near point of convergence became normal (5
for divergence (Collewijn et al. 1995). A similar dynamic 1 cm) and the exophoria at near vision decreased, reaching normal
interaction has also been observed by Yang and Kapoula values (32 pD). Finally, the range of vergence at near viewing also
increased after training (375 pD and 156 pD for convergence and
(2004) for normal children. To assess eventual vergence divergence respectively). Most importantly, for all children, subjec-
abnormalities in children with vertigo this study examines tive symptoms of vertigo and headache disappeared.
the speed-accuracy characteristics of isolated saccades,
vergence and combined eye movements.
288
Eye movements recording one at the center, two at 10 and two at 20; fixation to any of
these LEDs required a vergence angle of 2.3. In a dark room the
A PC directed the stimulation of LEDs; data collection was directed subject was seated in an adapted chair with the head stabilized by a
by REX, software developed for real-time experiments and visual forehead and chin support facing the visual display. Subjects viewed
display run on the PC. Horizontal eye movements from both eyes the LEDs binocularly and were asked to move their eyes to the LED
were recorded simultaneously with a photoelectric device (Ocul- as soon and accurately as possible.
ometer, Bouis). This system has an optimal resolution of 2 of arc
and a linear range at 20 (Bach et al. 1983). Eye-position signals
were low-pass filtered with a cutoff frequency of 200 Hz and Oculomotor testing
digitized with a 12-bit analogue-to-digital converter and each
channel was sampled at 500 Hz. Calibration task
Eye movements were recorded before the orthoptic vergence
training in all eight subjects, and after training in four of them (S3,
S5, S6 and S7). The calibration task was done at the beginning and at the end of
each block of the main experiment (see below): a standard saccade
paradigm was used to elicit visually guided saccades to the LED
presented at 10 and 20 at the far isovergence surface. These
Visual display saccades were used to extract the calibration factor for each eye.

Target-LEDs were placed at eye level on two isovergence circles at


different distances from the subject: one at 20 cm and the other at Saccades, vergence and combined movements
150 cm from the subject. On the circle close to the subject three
LEDs were placed: one at the center and the other at 20. The
required mean vergence angle for fixating any of these three LEDs Each trial started by lighting a fixation LED at the center of one of
was 17. On the circle far from the subject, five LEDs were placed: the two circles (far or close). After a 2.5 s fixational period the

Fig. 1AF Typical recordings


of the different types of eye
movements. The saccade trace is
the average of the position of the
two eyes (LE+RE/2); the ver-
gence trace is the difference
between the two eyes (LERE).
Pure saccade to the right (A) and
to the left (B); convergence (C)
and divergence (D) movements
along the median plane. Com-
bined movements: (E) saccade
to the left combined with con-
vergence; (F) saccades to the
right combined with divergence
component. The arrows indicate
the beginning and the end of the
movement. Dashed lines indi-
cate target position. The target-
LED appears at time zero
289
central LED was turned off and a target-LED appeared for 2 s. contaminated by blinks were rejected. The mean percentage of the
When the target-LED was on the center of the other circle it called rejected eye movements was about 6%.
for a pure vergence eye movement, along the median plane. When it For all types of eye movements we measured the gain, that is the
was at the same circle it called for a pure saccade of 20 (left or ratio of the amplitude of the total movement over the target
right), and, when it was lateral and on the other circle, the required excursion amplitude, the duration and the mean velocity (amplitude
eye movement was a combined saccade and vergence eye move- of the movement/duration). Analysis of variance (ANOVA) was
ment. The required saccade amplitude was 20, and the required performed with subjects as random factor and as a fixed factor the
vergence change along the median plane or along lateral axes (for individual means of the gain (amplitude of the movement/amplitude
combined movements) was 15. In each block, the three types of of the target excursion), of the duration and of mean velocity for
trials were interleaved randomly. Each block contained four different types of eye movements. Such analysis was performed for
saccades at far, four pure convergence and four pure divergence the before and after training comparison.
movements, four saccades combined with divergence, and four To evaluate eventual abnormalities of various parameters, data
saccades combined with convergence. Four blocks were run; each of were compared to values of children without vertigo of similar age,
them was separated by a rest of a few minutes. studied by our group under the same conditions (see Yang and
Kapoula 2004). A statistical comparison with normal children was
done using Students t-test.
Data analysis

Calibration and analysis methods are similar to those used in prior Results
studies (Kapoula et al. 2001; Yang et al. 2002). Briefly, a linear
function was used to calibrate the individual eye position signals. Qualitative observations
From these two signals, we calculated the conjugate signal [(left eye
+right eye)/2] and the vergence signal (left eyeright eye). Markers
were placed at different points on eye position signals automatically Figure 1 shows examples of trajectories of three different
by the computer, and were verified afterwards by an investigator. types of eye movements recorded (saccades, vergence and
The onset of the conjugate saccadic component was defined as the combined movements). Saccades to the right (Fig. 1A) as
time when the eye velocity reached 5% of the saccadic peak
velocity; the offset of these signal was defined as the time when the well as to the left (Fig. 1B) show mild undershoot of the
eye velocity dropped below 10/s. The onset and the offset of the target and a small corrective saccade is made for reducing
vergence signals (for pure vergence movements and for the vergence the error. In contrast, convergence and divergence move-
component of the combined movements) were defined as the time ments (Fig. 1C, D) are highly hypometric (their amplitude
point when the eye velocity exceeded or dropped 5/s. These criteria
are standard and similar to those used by other authors (e.g. Takagi is only 10, while the target required 15); note also the
et al. 1995). Eye movements that were in the wrong direction or slowness of these movements. For the combined move-

Fig. 2 Mean gain (amplitude of the movement/target excursion), group means corresponds to the mean value found in normal
duration and mean velocity (amplitude of the movement/duration) of subjects of comparable age (Yang and Kapoula 2004). Asterisks
pure movements: saccades (A), convergence (B) and divergence (C) indicate that the group mean value is significantly different from
for each subject tested. Vertical lines indicate the standard deviation. normal value
Group means are based on eight subjects. Horizontal line on the
290

ments (Fig. 1E, F) the saccade component shows also In contrast, the accuracy of pure vergence movements is
hypometria relative to the targets lateral eccentricity, poor and more variable between the subjects; the gain
particularly when these movements are combined with ranges from 0.51 to 0.90 for convergence and from 0.40 to
convergence (Fig. 1E). The convergence component is 0.90 for divergence movements. The mean group value is
hypometric, only 9 of amplitude instead of the 15 0.730.15 and 0.670.21 for convergence and divergence
required by the target in depth. In contrast, the divergence respectively and these values are significantly smaller than
component during combined movements is performed the gain of pure saccades (F(1,7)=5.89, p<0.02, and F
accurately. Note the remarkable acceleration of divergence (1,7)=5.76, p<0.03 respectively). Importantly, these gain
when combined with the saccade (see Fig. 1F), and the values are significantly lower (t-test, p<0.01) than those
absence of a similar phenomenon for combined conver- observed in normal children (0.920.09 and 0.870.10, for
gence movements (Fig. 1E). Next we will present convergence and divergence, respectively) by Yang and
quantitative data of the accuracy, of the duration and of Kapoula (2004).
the mean velocity of all these types of eye movements. The mean duration of vergence movements is very long:
57396 ms for convergence (range 427700 ms) and 617
82 ms for divergence (range 472720 ms) and they are
Quantitative data not statistically different (F(1,7)=1.00, p<0.33); both
durations are significantly longer than the duration of
Pure movements pure saccades (F(1,14)=200, p<0.00001 and F(1,14)=304,
p<0.00001 for convergence and divergence, respectively).
Figure 2 shows the gain (amplitude of the movement/ Duration of both convergence and divergence movements
target-LED excursion), the duration and the mean velocity is longer than those observed in normal children (304
for pure saccades (A), pure convergence (B) and pure 61 ms and 47093 ms, respectively); moreover, only the
divergence (C). duration of convergence is significantly longer in our
All subjects perform accurate saccades, the gain ranging subjects than in normals (t-test, p<0.01).
between 0.85 and 1 and with a group mean value of 0.91 Consistently with the duration data, the speed of
0.09. The duration of saccades is short, 100 ms; the convergence and divergence is variable over the subjects
mean group value is 899 ms. The speed of saccades (1428 deg/s for convergence and 1222 deg/s for
measured by the mean velocity (amplitude of the move- divergence movements respectively). The mean value is
ment/duration) is quite similar over all subjects; it ranges 209 deg/s for convergence and 176 deg/s for divergence
between 175 and 245 deg/s with a mean value of 209 and they are not significantly different (F(1,7)=165,
29 deg/s. All these values are very similar to the mean p=0.21), while, in normals convergence is significantly
values observed in normal children of comparable age faster than divergence. With respect to the speed of
reported by Yang and Kapoula (2004) and indicated in the normals (467 deg/s and 265 deg/s for convergence and
figure by horizontal lines on the group mean (gain: 0.91 divergence, respectively) the speed of convergence is
0.09; duration: 8011 ms, mean velocity: 22011 deg/s).

Fig. 3 Mean gain (amplitude of the movement/target excursion), duration and mean velocity (amplitude of the movement/duration) of the
saccade (A) and of the convergence component (B) of combined movements. Other notations as in Fig. 2
291

significantly slower (t-test, p<0.01), and that of divergence In contrast, the performance of the convergence com-
tends toward significance (t-test, p<0.06). ponent of combined movements is highly different from
In conclusion, subjects with vertigo perform normal that in normals: indeed, the value of gain, duration and
saccades. In contrast, vergence movements, particularly mean velocity in subjects with vertigo is significantly
convergence, are abnormal, with poor accuracy, long lower (t-test, p<0.01) than that of normal subjects (that is,
duration and low speed. gain: 0.920.09, duration: 272100 ms; mean velocity: 50
8 deg/s). The gain of the convergence component ranges
between 0.40 and 0.84; the group mean value is only 0.57
Combined movements 0.17 and it is significantly smaller than that recorded for
pure convergence movements (F(1,7)=5.43, p<0.05). The
Figure 3 shows the gain, the duration and the mean mean duration of convergence components of combined
velocity for both saccade (A) and convergence (B) movements is 496100 ms (range 298642 ms), and even
components of combined movements. The gain of the if it is shorter with respect to the duration of the
saccade component is low (it ranges between 0.44 and corresponding pure convergence movements it is not
0.80) and the mean group value is 0.640.14; this gain is statistically different (F(1,7)=2.47, p=0.13). The speed of
significantly lower than that of pure saccades the convergence component of combined movements is 19
(F(1,7)=18.91, p<0.0007). This gain is slightly lower than 10 deg/s (the range is 929 deg/s). This value is not
that reported in normal children (0.800.12) by Yang and significantly different to the speed of pure convergence
Kapoula (2004) and not statistically different. movements (F(1,7)=0.95, p=0.34). The absence of a
The mean duration of the saccade component of significant difference in the speed with respect to pure
movements combined with convergence is 9722 ms movements suggests that during combined movements in
(range 66131 ms) and the mean velocity is 14033 deg/s subjects with vertigo, differently to normals, the vergence
(range 98200 deg/s); moreover, the speed of the saccade is not accelerated by the saccade.
component of combined movements is significantly lower Figure 4 shows the gain, the duration and the mean
than that observed for pure saccades (F(1,7)=14.67, velocity for both saccade (A) and divergence (B)
p<0.0018). Both the duration and the mean velocity of components of combined movements. Differently to
the saccade component of movements combined with movements combined with convergence, the performance
convergence even more slowly, and are not significantly of both the saccade and the divergence components of
different to the values reported for normals by Yang and combined movements is very similar to normals. The
Kapoula (duration: 8511 ms, and mean velocity: 164 mean group gain is 0.790.05 (it ranges between 0.70 and
15 deg/s). At least for this component, the dynamic 0.89) and, as for normal subjects, the gain, when measured
properties observed in normals during combined move- at the offset of the primary saccade, is significantly lower
ments seem to exist (that is, the saccade is slower when than that of pure saccades (F(1,7)=8.65, p<0.01). The
combined with convergence than when done alone). duration of these movements is 12514 ms (range 110
148 ms), very similar to that observed in normals (115

Fig. 4 Mean gain (amplitude of the movement/target excursion), duration and mean velocity (amplitude of the movement/duration) of the
saccade (A) and of the divergence component (B) of combined movements. Other notations as in Fig. 2
292

59 ms); and, as normals, it is significantly longer than the duration of both saccade and divergence component of
that of the corresponding pure saccades (F(1,7)=22.81, combined movements is normal; in contrast the conver-
p<0.0003). The speed of the saccadic components is 127 gence component is abnormally inaccurate and of low
13 deg/s (range 101147 deg/s), and it is very similar to speed.
that observed in normals (12211 deg/s). Moreover, the
mean velocity of the saccadic component of combined
movements is significantly slower than that of pure Additional observations: effect of training
saccades (F(1,7)=44.79, p<0.00001), suggesting that the
presence of the saccade-divergence interaction is similar to Figure 5 shows the mean value of the gain, of the duration
that reported for normals (Collewijn et al. 1995) and for and of the mean velocity for the four subjects before and
adults and children (Yang and Kapoula 2004). after training for pure movementssaccades, convergence
The gain of divergence during combined movements is and divergence(A), for saccades combined with con-
0.930.12, ranging between 0.71 and 1. Note that such a vergence (B) and for saccades combined with divergence
gain value is significantly higher than that observed for (C).
pure divergence (F(1,7)=6.69, p<0.02). The mean duration After training, an improvement of the gain can be
of divergence components of combined movements is 573 observed for all types of eye movements. The gain is
107 ms (range 382720 ms), and this is shorter than the significantly higher than that measured before training for
duration of pure divergence, but not statistically different pure saccades and pure convergence (F(1,3)=6.95, p<0.05
to it (F(1,7)=1.09, p=0.31). The mean velocity of the and F(1,3)=6.85, p<0.05, respectively). The gain increased
divergence component of combined movements is 26 also for pure divergence; however, the change is not
8 deg/s (the range is between 21 and 34 deg/s), and this statistically different (F(1,3)=2.87, p=0.14) from that
value is significantly faster than that of pure divergence observed before training, maybe due to the subject
movements (F(1,7)=12.70, p<0.003), suggesting once more variability. Indeed, remember that subject S5 already had
that the saccade-divergence dynamic interaction during a normal gain before training (see Fig. 2B). In contrast,
combined movements is reciprocal and normal. after training for both components of saccades combined
In conclusion, combined movements for subjects with with convergence the gain increased significantly
vertigo show a normal reciprocal interaction between (F(1,3)=20.88, p<0.0038 and F(1,3)=22.53, p<0.0032,
saccade and divergence but unilateral interaction for respectively).
saccades combined with convergence, i.e., the saccade is Finally, the gain of the saccade component of move-
slowed by the convergence and convergence is not ments combined with divergence increases significantly
accelerated by the saccade. The accuracy, the speed and with respect to that recorded before training (F(1,3)=6.57,

Fig. 5 Group mean (on four


subjects) of the gain, duration
and mean velocity before and
after orthoptic vergence training
for pure movements (A: sac-
cades, convergence and diver-
gence), for saccades combined
with convergence (B) and for
saccades combined with diver-
gence (C). A significant before-
after training change is shown
by an asterisk. The horizontal
lines indicate the values re-
corded in normal subjects of
matched age (Yang and Kapoula
2004); crosses indicate a signif-
icant difference from normal
value
293

p<0.0427). In contrast, the mean value of the gain of the Saccades


divergence component increases only a small amount;
indeed, two of the four subjects examined performed quite The first finding of this study is that children with vertigo
well with such types of movements already before and vergence abnormalities perform saccade eye move-
training. Furthermore, it is important to note that after ments as normal subjects (Collewijn et al. 1988; Kapoula
training the accuracy of all types of eye movement reaches and Robinson 1986; Yang and Kapoula 2004): the gain
normal values (horizontal lines on the after training group measure approaches 1 for all subjects examined; as
mean). normals, saccades undershoot the target and they are
After training, the duration decreases for pure saccades followed by small corrective saccades. The duration and
by a small amount (10 ms); vergence movements show a the mean velocity of saccades are also similar to that
larger decrease of the duration: 70 ms and 55 ms for observed from studies examining a large population of
convergence and divergence, respectively. The duration of normal adults as well as in children of comparable age
the saccade components of movements combined with (Fioravanti et al. 1995; Munoz et al. 1998; Yang and
convergence and divergence decreases very little (10 Kapoula 2004). Indeed, Fioravanti et al. (1995) pointed
20 ms), while the decrease of duration of vergence out that the accuracy and the duration of saccades in
components is larger: about 100 ms for the convergence children older than 8 years are similar to that of adults.
component and 37 ms for the divergence component. Moreover, Munoz et al. (1998) examined the metric and
None of these changes, however, is significantly different. the dynamic of saccades in a large population of normal
Moreover, the duration of convergence pure as well as subjects whose age ranged between 5 and 79 years. They
combined remains significantly longer with respect to found that saccades are accurate except for the youngest
normals (t-test, p<0.01). children (58 years), who make hypometric saccades; the
The mean velocity after training increases for all types duration is significantly longer only in older subjects and
of movements studied; however, such an increase is the peak velocity did not change significantly across age.
statistically different only for the saccade component of Similarly, the study by Yang and Kapoula (2004) showed
movements combined with convergence (F(1,3)=5.73, that normal children (>8 years old) performed saccades as
p<0.05). Finally, it is important to observe that while the normal adults. Our observations in children with vertigo
mean velocity of the saccades and divergence (pure and older than 8 years are in agreement with these studies.
combined) recorded after training is not significantly
different from normal values, the velocity of the conver-
gence (pure or combined) in subjects with vertigo remains Vergence
abnormally low (t-test, p<0.01).
In conclusion, the major effect of training is a signif- In contrast to saccades, vergence movements along the
icant improvement of the accuracy of saccades and median plane are severely impaired in our children. First,
convergence (pure or combined). In terms of speed, we found that the accuracy of both convergence and
however, the changes are less remarkable and convergence divergence is poor in these subjects relative to that
remains abnormal. observed in normal adults (Zee et al. 1992; Collewijn et al.
1995), as well as in normal children of similar age and
adults using the same experimental condition in the study
Discussion of Yang and Kapoula (2004) beyond 0.90 for both
convergence and divergence. Moreover, the duration of
The main findings of this study are the following: (1) vergence movements, particularly convergence, is abnor-
children with vertigo perform normal saccades; in contrast, mally long. Indeed, in our subjects, the duration of
vergence movements, particularly convergence, are ab- convergence is significantly longer than that reported in
normal in terms of accuracy, duration and speed; (2) the normals by Yang and Kapoula (2004), where a similar
reciprocal interaction between the saccades and the amplitude of convergence was elicited. The mean velocity
vergence system normally present during combined of convergence is abnormally low, significantly lower than
movements exists also in children with vertigo but only that observed in normals by Yang and Kapoula (2004). We
for saccades combined with divergence; in contrast suggest that such poor vergence accuracy could be caused
saccades combined with convergence show deceleration by a reduced ability of our subjects to localize the target in
of the saccade by the convergence, but not acceleration of depth correctly. This hypothesis is in agreement with the
the convergence by the saccade; (3) orthoptic vergence clinical observations of Riddell et al. (1990). Indeed, these
training improves the accuracy of all types of eye authors observed that the performances in clinical tests
movements; the duration and the speed of such eye assessing spatial discrimination (a synoptophore based
movements does not change; furthermore, the duration and vergence test, the Dunlop test) were significantly worse in
the speed of convergence (pure and combined) remains children with poor vergence control than in normals. We
abnormal. Next we will discuss in more detail each of believe that such impairment in the localization of the
these findings. target could cause difficulty in the preparation and in the
execution of vergence movements, leading to both abnor-
294

mal longer latency (as shown in our previous study, Bucci ing both saccade and vergence command. According to
et al. 2004) and inaccurate vergence movements. these authors, slow vergence is not controlled by these
On the other hand, in children with vertigo vergence burst neurons. In line with this hypothesis, Chaturvedi and
was abnormally slow with low velocity. This finding is van Gisbergen (2000) showed that the superior colliculus
compatible with the hypothesis of dysfunction of the (SC) via the OPN controls the saccade and the vergence
cortical and/or subcortical circuitry that controls the response during combined movements. Indeed, when the
execution of such movements. Indeed, Mays (1984) and target called for combined movement, electrical stimula-
Mays et al. (1996) found in the mesencephalic reticular tion of the rostral region of the superior colliculus of
formation of monkeys, distinct convergence and diver- monkeys inactivated the saccadic and the vergence system
gence burst neurons discharging in relation to vergence in a different way: the saccade was completely stopped
velocity. More recently, Gamlin and Yoon (2000) and while a residual slow vergence response persisted.
Gamlin (2002) provided evidence that the activity of Chaturvedi and van Gisbergen (2000) suggested that the
neurons in the prearcuate area, close to the frontal eye field neural circuit controlling the slow vergence command is
of monkeys, was related to vergence dynamic. On the distinct from that of the fast vergence command activated
basis of these studies, we suggest that the slowness of during combined movements. According to all these
vergence, particularly convergence observed in children studies, we suggest that the lack of acceleration of
with vertigo, could have both a subcortical and a cortical convergence during combined movements in children
origin. with vertigo could be due to impairment in the distinct
neural circuitry responsible for producing such fast
command. In other words, the behavioral deficit could
Combined saccade-vergence movements indicate the presence of a subtle specific deficiency in the
subcortical circuitry.
Our subjects performed differently combined movements
according to the type of vergence, convergence or
divergence associated with saccades. The performance of Effect of orthoptic vergence training
saccade components of movements combined with con-
vergence or divergence is similar to that observed in Orthoptic vergence training is widely used by clinicians
normal children and adults; indeed, the accuracy, the for improving vergence capabilities (von Noorden 2002);
duration and the mean velocity of the saccade components however, only the study of van Leeuwen et al. (1999),
are in the normal range (Collewijn et al. 1995; Yang and limited to one subject, reported objective measures on the
Kapoula 2004). Moreover, the finding of a significant accuracy of vergence movements after orthoptic training.
slowness of the saccade components with respect to the The present study extends the study of van Leeuwen et
corresponding pure saccades suggests that the known al. (1999), showing that not only vergence but also
interaction between the saccade and the vergence system saccades can improve after orthoptic training. Moreover,
during combined movements reported by Collewijn et al. our results show that after training only the accuracy of
(1995) is also present in children with vertigo. different types of eye movements improves, reaching
The performance of the convergence and divergence normal values. Such a finding is compatible with the
components is different; indeed, the divergence compo- hypothesis that training could reinforce the visual percep-
nent has normal accuracy, normal duration and normal tion of the target in the natural space as showed by Fischer
mean velocity; most importantly its mean velocity is and Hartnegg (2000). Indeed, these authors reported that a
significantly higher than that of pure divergence. This daily training of visual tasks (saccades as well as fixation)
means that, in children with vertigo, the reciprocal in dyslexic children improved both the ability in the
interaction between the saccade and the divergence system detection of the orientation of the target and the
during combined movements exists as in normals. In performance of saccades (latency as well as the percentage
contrast, the convergence components of combined move- of errors decreased significantly). Furthermore, in our
ments are abnormal in terms of accuracy, duration and previous study (Bucci et al. 2004) we observed after
mean velocity. Furthermore, the convergence of combined training a decrease of the latency of eye movements, and
movements is not accelerated by the saccade as occurs in we suggested that visual attention mechanisms could
normals; indeed, the mean velocity of the convergence facilitate, after training, the initiation of eye movements. In
component in children with vertigo does not change with a similar way we suggest that attentional mechanisms
respect to that of pure convergence movements. could also be responsible for a better target perception and
Relatively little is known about the neuronal circuitry its localization in the natural space, leading to the
controlling combined movements. Zee et al. (1992) execution of more accurate eye movements.
proposed a model in which combined eye movements Our results show also that the training does not improve
could be produced by a coactivation of the two distinct either the duration of eye movements or the mean velocity
oculomotor systems, that of the saccade and that of the of vergence, suggesting that there is no change in the
vergence. The omnipause neurons (OPN) could gate the subcortical structures controlling the dynamics of these
activity of a specific pool of neurons, the so-called movements. The significant slowness of convergence pure
saccade-related-vergence bursters responsible for generat- and combined with saccades that persists even after
295

training suggests that children with vertigo have a specific Collewijn H, Erkelens CJ, Steinman RM (1988) Binocular co-
problem in the subcortical circuitry responsible for the ordination of human horizontal saccadic eye movements. J
Physiol 404:157182
execution of such movements. Collewijn H, Erkelens CJ, Steinman RM (1995) Voluntary binocular
gaze-shifts in the plane of regard: dynamics of version and
vergence. Vision Res 35:33353358
Consequences of vergence abnormalities on the VOR Fioravanti F, Inchingolo P, Pensiero S, Spanio M (1995) Saccadic
eye movement conjugation in children. Vision Res 35:3217
adjustment 3228
Fischer B, Hartnegg K (2000) Effects of visual training on saccade
An important characteristic of the visual system is that control in dyslexia. Perception 29:531542
single vision is possible when the target is stable on the Fischer B, Biscaldi M, Gezeck S (1997) On the development of
voluntary and reflexive components in human saccade gener-
retina. During rapid head movements, the VOR induces ation. Brain Res 754:285297
compensatory eye movements allowing the stabilization of Fukushima J, Hatta T, Fukushima K (2000) Development of
the target of the retina. Several studies (see Leigh and Zee voluntary control of saccadic eye movements I. Age-related
1999 for review) have shown that the gain of the VOR is changes in normal children. Brain Dev 22:173180
modulated by the viewing distance and the vergence angle. Gamlin PD (2002) Neural mechanisms for the control of vergence
eye movements. Ann N Y Acad Sci 956:264272
Snyder et al. (1992) reported that during vergence eye Gamlin PD, Yoon K (2000) An area for vergence eye movement in
movements the change in the gain of the VOR anticipates primate frontal cortex. Nature 407:10031007
the vergence, suggesting the possibility for predictive Kapoula Z, Robinson DA (1986) Saccadic undershoot is not
control of vergence combined with the control of the VOR inevitable: saccades can be accurate. Vision Res 26:735743
Kapoula Z, Isotalo E, Muri RM, Bucci MP, Rivaud-Pechoux S
system. Furthermore, Coenen (1998) suggested that the (2001) Effects of transcranial magnetic stimulation of the
cerebellum could prepare by learning a predictive ver- posterior parietal cortex on saccades and vergence. Neuroreport
gence signal for the VOR gain. On the basis of these 12:40414046
studies, we could assume that abnormalities of vergence Klein C, Foerster F (2001) Development of prosaccade and
antisaccade task performance in participants aged 6 to
can lead to inappropriate VOR adjustment and thereby 26 years. Psychophysiology 38:179189
poor gaze stabilization and thereby vertigo (Brandt 1999). Leigh RJ, Zee DS (1999) The neurology of eye movement. Oxford
Our after training findings show a complete disappear- University Press, New York
ance of vertigo symptoms for all subjects; however, only Mays LE (1984) Neural control of vergence eye movements:
vergence accuracy improved significantly while its speed convergence and divergence neurons in midbrain. J Neurophy-
siol 51:10911108
did not change after training. According to these results we Mays LE, Porter JD, Gamlin PD, Tello CA (1996) Neural control of
can propose the hypothesis that the vergence signal for a vergence eye movements: neurons encoding vergence velocity.
correct VOR adjustment is the final vergence angle rather J Neurophysiol 56:10071021
than the speed of vergence itself. However, further studies Munoz DP, Broughton JR, Goldring JE, Armstrong IT (1998) Age-
related performance of human subjects on saccadic eye
combining VOR and vergence are needed. movement tasks. Exp Brain Res 121:391400
Riddell PM, Fowler MS, Stein JF (1990) Spatial discrimination in
Acknowledgements. This study was supported by INSERM children with poor vergence control. Percept Mot Skills
(contract no. 4M105E) and by Disfonctionnement Cognitif (MRT 70:707718
Cognitique). Dr. M.P. Bucci was supported by EDF and FRM; Dr. Snyder LH, Lawrence DM, King WM (1992) Changes in the
Q. Yang was supported by CNRS-K.C. WONG. vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) anticipate changes in vergence
angle in monkey. Vision Res 32:569575
Takagi M, Frohman EM, Zee DS (1995) Gap-overlap effects on
latencies of saccades, vergence and combined vergence-
References saccades in humans. Vision Res 35:33733388
van Leeuwen AF, Westen MJ, van der Steen J, de Faber JT,
Anoh-Tanon MJ, Brmond-Gignac D, Wiener-Vacher S (2000) Collewijn H (1999) Gaze-shift dynamics in subjects with and
Vertigo is an underestimated symptom of ocular disorders: without symptoms of convergence insufficiency: influence of
dizzy children do not always need MRI. Pediatr Neurol 23:49 monocular preference and the effect of training. Vision Res
53 39:30953107
Bach M, Bouis D, Fischer B (1983) An accurate and linear infrared von Noorden GK (2002) Binocular vision and ocular motility.
oculometer. J Neurosci Methods 9:914 Theory and management of strabismus. Mosby, St. Louis
Brandt T (1999) Vertigo. Its multisensory syndromes. Springer, Wiener-Vacher S, Toupet F, Narcy P (1996) Canal and otolith
Berlin Heidelberg New York vestibulo-ocular reflexes to vertical and off vertical rotations in
Bucci MP, Kapoula Z, Yang Q, Wiener-Vacher S, Brmond-Gignac children learning to walk. Acta Otolaryngol Stockh 116:657
D (2004) Abnormality of vergence latency in children with 665
vertigo. J Neurol 251:204213 Yang Q, Kapoula Z (2004) Saccade-vergence dynamics and
Chaturvedi V, van Gisbergen JAM (2000) Stimulation in the rostral interaction in children and in adults. Exp Brain Res (in press)
pole of monkey superior colliculus: effects on vergence eye Yang Q, Bucci MP, Kapoula Z (2002) The latency of saccades,
movements. Exp Brain Res 132:7278 vergence, and combined eye movements in children and in
Coenen O (1998) Modeling the vestibulo-ocular reflex and the adults. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 43:29392949
cerebellum: analytical and computational approaches. In: PhD Zee DS, Fitzgibbon EJ, Optican LM (1992) Saccade-vergence
dissertation. University of California, San Diego, CA interactions in humans. J Neurophysiol 68:16241641
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Вам также может понравиться