Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 15

The Journal of

a r t icle

The Contrasting Role of Higher Order Awareness in


Hypnosis and Meditation
Rebecca Semmens-Wheeler, MSc*, ; Zoltan Dienes, PhD*

Meditation and hypnosis might be regarded as involving similar processes and skills. For example,
both meditation and hypnosis are associated with high levels of absorption and imaginative capac-
ity, and both can be used for self-regulation. Research has also shown that meditation improves
attentional functioning, and that hypnotic response can involve attentional strategies. However,
we argue that hypnosis and mindfulness meditation are essentially different. Crucially, hypnotic
experience results from a lack of awareness of mental states (specifically, of intentions); by contrast,
mindfulness meditation aims to develop accurate meta-awareness. Hypnosis is a form of self de-
ception; meditation a way of getting to know your mind. We discuss the empirical relation of both
meditation and hypnosis to higher order awareness of mental states, and suggest further research.

Introduction people can create according to the requirements


of the situation (e.g. Dienes, 2012). In the first
Comparisons are frequently made between sense, hypnosis is a state, a way of being; in the

mbr. s y ner g ie s pr a i r ie s .c a
hypnosis and meditation (e.g. Delmonte, 1984; second, it is a way of doing (responding to sug-
Holroyd, 2003; Van Nuys, 1973; Yapko, 2011). Both gestions). Hypnosis as a state could be just a par-
typically involve some physical relaxation, for ticular pattern of phenomenology (attention ab-
example; however deep relaxation is not a neces- sorbed inwards or outwards, time going faster or
sary nor even a necessarily helpful component of slower, self talk increased or decreased, and so on;
either (e.g. Banyai and Hilgard, 1976; Hanh and see e.g. Pekala and Kumar, 2007), or, in addition,
Nquyen, 2006). So care is needed to disentangle according to some theories, a global change in
contingent similarities from core ones, and we how information is processed that causally affects
will attempt in this review to do so. Both hyp- response to suggestions (e.g. impairment of the
nosis and meditation are involved in self regu- executive system; Jamieson and Woody, 2007).
lationbut self regulation can be performed in Responding hypnotically involves a specific mo-
different, even opposing, ways. In this review, we tor or cognitive action accompanied by an altered
highlight similarities and differences between sense of volition or reality. For example, a person
meditation and hypnosis, arguing that they are es- can hold their hands out, imagine they are mag-
sentially different. First we indicate what is meant nets and feel their hands move together seem-
by hypnosis and meditation. Then we consider ingly by themselves. The act of the hands moving
the relation between the two implied by differ- together is mundane; what makes it hypnotic is
ent theories of hypnosis, in terms of the role of the sense that it happens by itself. Or a person
executive systems (attention and metacognition) can, on request, change the color of an object
in hypnosis according to those theories. Next, we from say red to green, with the hallucinated color
consider the empirical evidence for the role of at- seeming external and real. Imagining an object in
tention and metacognition in each of meditation a counterfactual color is mundane; what makes
and hypnosis. Finally, we consider evidence more the cognitive act hypnotic is the sense of reality
directly relating meditation and hypnosis. We will that accompanies the act of imagination. These
argue that at their core, meditation and hypnosis acts constitute hypnotic responding whether or
are opposites. not the person is in a hypnotic state. It is im-
* Sackler Centre for portant to bear the distinction between acts and
Consciousness Science,
University of Sussex,
What is hypnosis? states in mind in comparing hypnosis to medi-
tation: Putative hypnotic and meditative states
Brighton, BN19RH, can be compared; or else hypnotic and meditative
The word hypnosis can either refer to a state that
United Kingdom actions can be compared. Nonetheless, the two
follows a hypnotic induction (cf. Barnier and
e-mail: rebeccajsw@ Nash 2008); or else to the suggested distortions of uses are related. For example, on state theories
gmail.com perception or sense of involuntariness that some of hypnosis, if an induced state did not increase

43 || MBR || Volume: 2 || Issue: 1


a r t icle The Journal of MindBody Regulation

response to hypnotic suggestion even slightly, the parietal regions (associated with recruitment of
state would not be a hypnotic state. Conversely, attentional resources) in the draining condition.
on non-state theories of hypnosis, the state is it- However, the enhancement seen following hyp-
self just a response to a suggestion (to experience notic induction was slight, and highs were able to
that state, however it is conceived by the subject). effectively perform the suggestion even without
Researchers have not settled on a consistent use any hypnotic induction or feeling that they were
of the word hypnosis (Kirsch et al, 2011) and the in any way in a hypnotic state. In sum, there is a
word meditation can also be used to encom- hypnotic way of acting (acting cognitively or be-
pass a wide range of practices (Lutz, Dunne and haviorally such that the sense of reality or volition
Davidson, 2007). is distorted according to task requirements) that
Typically, hypnotic suggestibility is measured can occur either in or out of a hypnotic state.
by giving subjects a hypnotic induction then giv-
ing a series of suggestions. The more suggestions What is meditation?
a person passes, the more hypnotizable they are.
Several predictors of hypnotizability have been Meditation can be described as a complex fam-
found, including response expectancy (Kirsch ily of training practices in attention, emotional
and Braffman, 1999; Raz, 2006), absorption regulation (Lutz, Slagter, Dunne and Davidson,
(Wilson and Barber, 1983; Tellegen and Atkinson, 2008) and metacognitive awareness (Thompson,
1974), fantasy proneness (Wilson and Barber, 2006), which (aim to) contribute to the develop-
1982) and imaginative involvement (Spanos and ment of a more veridical experience of the world.
Barber, 1974). While significant correlations have Meditation practice in the shorter term, in partic-
been found, they are often only moderate, with ular mindfulness-based training, is also used (as
reliable correlations between hypnosis and ab- hypnosis is) for the treatment of stress (Fletcher
sorption typically around 0.20.3 (Kihlstrom, et al, 1995), depression (Ramel, Goldin, Carmona
2003), for example, and are often smaller when and McQuaid 2004; Teasdale et al, 2000), addic-
hypnotizabilty and putative correlates are tested tion (Bowen et al, 2006) and pain management

mbr. s y ner g ie s pr a i r ie s .c a
in a different context (e.g. Council, Kirsch and (Kabat-Zinn, Davidson and Houshmand, 1985).
Hafner, 1986, but see Nadon, Hoyt, Register and Just as hypnotic responding can be distinguished
Kihlstrom, 1991). The most reliable and replicable from a hypnotic state, meditative (mindful) ac-
correlate of suggestibility after an induction is tivity might be viewed as distinct from a medita-
responding to suggestions without being given a tive state. Such mindful activities can be every-
hypnotic induction (around 0.7, e.g. Hilgard and day occurrences, so long as one is fully present
Tart, 1966; Braffman, and Kirsch, 1999). and aware of the sensations of the action. For
Thus, hypnotic responding can be distin- example walking, making tea, eating a raisin or
guished from a special altered state of hypnosis cleaning ones teeth can be performed mindfully.
(see Raz, 2011, for discussion). Although some (Mindfully eating a raisin in an exercise in mind-
researchers do claim that hypnosis is a state (e.g. fulness based stress reduction courses; Kabat-
Crawford, 1994), there is no established theory Zinn, Segal, Williams, and Teasdale 2002.)
of a hypnotic state or states. For example, while It is important to distinguish between the
hypnotic induction has the potential to slightly various styles of meditation, particularly as some
enhance hypnotic suggestibility and produce a of the terms used in modern psychology and
stronger neural response than without an induc- neuroscience are the same as those translated
tion (Derbyshire, Whalley and Oakley, 2009)*, an from Buddhist texts, but refer to different con-
induction is not necessary for highs to success- structs (Lutz et al, 2007). There is no single clear
fully respond to hypnotic suggestions (Kirsch and simple definition of meditation as there are
and Braffman, 2001). Raz et al (2006) found many types from different traditions (including
that although highs were able to reduce Stroop those from different religions, as well as within
interference following hypnotic suggestion for Buddhism). However, Lutz et al (2007) have
printed words to become meaningless, a hypnotic drawn out some fundamental assumptions that
induction made no real difference to the effect. can be made about meditation as a whole. First
McGeown et al (2012) found that highs were able of all, meditation practices must be learned. They
* Although note recent
to successfully drain or add color from a colored are designed to inhibit undesirable traits (e.g.
as yet unpublished data or grayscale stimulus with and without hypnotic negative cognitive or emotional patterns) and
from the same lab found a induction, whereas lows were unable to perform enhance or cultivate desirable ones (e.g. non-
stronger neural response the suggestion in either condition. Subjective rat- reactivity). Many meditation practices achieve
for suggestion without an
induction rather than with
ings of hypnotic depth correlated with activation these by focusing on an aspect of ones experi-
it, Derbyshire, personal in the color processing region (i.e. left fusiform) ence, commonly the breath. Other practices in-
communication, 6 June 2012 in the color adding condition and in frontal and volve developing an open and non-judgmental

44 || MBR || Volume: 2 || Issue: 1


a r t icle The Journal of MindBody Regulation

awareness of ones emotions and cognitions insular cortex was correlated with cumulative
and/or cultivating particular thoughts of feel- meditation experience. Practice effects can be
ings, such as those of compassion. Following also seen in the aforementioned improved at-
on from this, it can be predicted that meditation tentional performance of experienced medita-
will produce specific states. Indeed, evidence tors. Brefczynski-Lewis, Lutz, Schaefer, Levinson
has shown that compassion meditation leads and Davidson (2007) reported greater activation
to improvements in affective regulation (Lutz, in the insula and in frontal parietal regions dur-
Brefczynski-Lewis, Johnstone and Davidson, ing concentration meditation. These areas are
2008) and other studies have demonstrated supe- involved in sustained attention and monitoring
rior attentional performance in meditators than and making attentional adjustments (Vincent,
controls and novice meditators (e.g. Moore and Kahn, Snyder, Raichle and Buckne, 2006; Eckert,
Malinowski, 2009; Wenk-Sormaz, 2005; Slagter, Menon, Walczak, Ahlstrom, Denslow, Horwitz
Lutz, Geichar, Francis, Nieuwenhuis, Davis and and Dubno, 2009). In meditation such monitor-
Davidson, 2007). Another feature of medita- ing is used to detect and signal mental drowsi-
tion practice is that improvements can be seen ness or over-excitability, which lead to a loss of
over time and are reflected in changes in the concentration.
brain. For example, structural differences have For the purposes of investigation and com-
been seen in experienced meditators, who had parison of meditation types, three main catego-
greater cortical thickness in the right anterior ries have been described by Lutz et al, drawing on
insula (associated with interoception; Critchley, practices within the Buddhist traditions (2007;
Wiens, Rotshtein, hman and Dolan, 2004, in- 2008; see Box 1). One prevalent meditation tech-
tegration of cognition and emotion and adaptive nique, which is used across Buddhist and other
decision-making, Damasio, 1996) than controls religious or spiritual traditions is focusing ones
(Lazar, Kerr, Wasserman, Gray, Greve, Treadway, attention on the breath. This practice initially in-
et al 2005). Crucially, cortical thickness in the volves focusing on the breath to develop amatha
(concentration/sustained attention and resil-

mbr. s y ner g ie s pr a i r ie s .c a
Box 1. Meditation styles ience to distracting thoughts and emotions) and
The different meditation styles can Visualization practices are also vipayan, which refers to the clarity of perceiv-
be categorized as: focused attention used to cultivate desired qualities ing the nature of that which is being attended.
(FA); open monitoring (OM) and and fall partially into the category of amatha may be practiced with a range of at-
non referential compassion medi- FA meditation, yet they go beyond tentional objects other than the breath (shapes,
tation. These different meditation concentration on a simple object.
styles have overlapping effects, but do This is commonly done through use
colours, body parts, etc) in order to develop a
recruit different regions of the brain of sdhana, which is meditation on calm sustained attention. Similarly, vipayan in-
(Lutz et al, 2007). a text describing various features volves a range of practices to see how phenomena
(shape, insignia, color and other at- (including mental states) come and go, and how
FA meditation tends to practiced in tributes) of a Buddha (enlightened certain ones tend to lead to certain others. The
stages beginning with breath count- being) or Bodhisattva (a person who
ing and progressing to focus on the is on the brink of enlightenment but
practitioner, through continually checking where
point of entry of the breath into the who holds back out of compassion the mind is focused (self-monitoring), begins to
body, for example. OM meditation, for all beings). The aim of sdhana develop samprajanya, which can be translated
also known as open presence or just practice is to create single-pointed, as meta-awareness (Lutz, Dunne and Davidson,
sitting emphasizes the development undistracted focus of the mind on 2007) or clear knowing (Anlayo, 2010). Meta-
of meta-awareness, where there is the qualities of this being in order to
no such thing as distraction but all cultivate them in oneself (Sangharak-
awareness, and specifically mindfulness, is a
experience is part of the meditation shita, 2001). common component across all different medi-
and the goal is to neither avert nor Other practices involve using what tation styles. Mindfulness can be defined as the
attach ones mind to any experience are known as supports to concentra- practice of bringing awareness to the present mo-
in particular but to become more fa- tion (kammathanas), which include ment with non-judgmental acceptance (Brown
miliar with ones mental tendencies the four brahma-viharas, meditation
and habits. The third style is referred on the decomposition of a corpse and
and Ryan, 2003). In sum, meditation fundamen-
to by Lutz et al as non referential com- the Ten Impurities (various disgust- tally involves the development of attentional and
passion. However, this is a narrow ing aspects of bodily experience). metacognitive processes. Intriguingly, such pro-
term for what is a set of meditations These practices are designed not only cesses have also been fundamental to the main
called the Brahma viharas (Sanskrit to develop one-pointed concentra- theories of hypnosis.
for divine abodes), which essentially tion, but also to relinquish attachment
aim to develop greater awareness of to the body by realizing its transitory,
emotions and to cultivate an attitude impermanent nature (Sangharakshi- Theoretical relation of hyp-
of mett (loving-kindness); karuna ta, 2001) and by breaking down the nosis to meditation
(compassion); mudita (sympathetic illusion of separate selfhood, with the
joy) and upekha (equanimity). aim of promoting greater compassion Theories of hypnosis can be classified according
and non-attachment.
to the role of executive system in hypnosis, i.e.
with that system responsible for metacognition

45 || MBR || Volume: 2 || Issue: 1


a r t icle The Journal of MindBody Regulation

responses, and the other which is unaware of this


Box 2. Metacognition control. In dissociated control theory (Woody and
Metacognition is most broadly con- One of the aims of meditation is to Bowers, 1994) hypnosis is described as a weaken-
strued as cognition about cognition increase metacognitive insight so ing of frontal lobe function so that the supervi-
and encompasses monitoring and that one begins more and more to sory attentional system (i.e. executive system,
control processes (see Beran et al, experience thoughts as just thoughts
Norman and Shallice, 1986) is rendered unable
2012, for an overview of current de- rather than thinking about them as
bates). Nelson and Narens (1990) facts that accurately reflect reality. to control behavior, which is thus controlled by
described a conceptual framework This should, in theory, allow one to the contention scheduling or habit system (hence
that has been influential for thinking have a more veridical experience of the feeling of involuntariness). Hence, behav-
about metacognition. Cognitive pro- the world through overcoming the ior is directly triggered by hypnotic suggestion.
cesses can be divided into object-level delusion of thoughts as facts. For ex-
Gruzeliers (1998) neurophysiological theory also
and meta-level. Object-level process- ample, a thought that one is a failure
es are basic cognitive processes such and worthless could have an associ- explains hypnosis in terms of diminished atten-
as perception, encoding or retrieval. ated metacognitive insight that this tional abilities. The purported greater attentional
Meta-processes monitor object-level belief is not factual. This is precisely abilities of highs leads to a highly concentrated
processes and control them. For ex- how mindfulness meditation is used state, which causes exhaustion of the frontal lobes
ample, when given a memory task, to help individuals overcome depres-
and thus leads to inhibition of executive frontal
a feeling of knowing is a type of sion.
metacognitive monitoring that may functioning that contributes toward hypnotic
lead to initiating search processes In the context of hypnosis, many response and experience. These theories imply
(metacognitive control). Metacogni- theories of hypnosis can be construed hypnosis is not conducive to mindfulness; re-
tive monitoring is constituted both as metacognitive, that is, theories sponding hypnotically essentially involves a lack
by non-conceptual affective signals, about the way object-level processes
of mindfulness.
such as feelings of knowing (consider can be monitored and controlled by
tip of the tongue states), as well as by meta-level processes. For example Socio-cognitive theories (e.g. Sarbin and Coe,
conceptual theories concerning how in Hilgards (1977) neo-dissociation 1972; Spanos, 1986, 1991) do not postulate any
ones mind works (cf Koriat, 2007). theory, both control and monitor- deficit in attention regulation; in fact, hypnotic
ing processes were important in a responses are explained in terms of attentional
Meditation can be seen as a process uniquely hypnotic way, with either
and other strategies (for example, hypnotic pain

mbr. s y ner g ie s pr a i r ie s .c a
of cultivating both metacognitive control or monitoring fractionated
monitoring and control (e.g. sustain- (see also Woody and Sadler, 2008). relief may be produced partly by distraction). By
ing attention, eliminating certain Similarly, Spanos (1986) sociocog- the same token, above-average attentional abili-
sorts of thoughts). Teasdale (1999) nitive theory construed hypnosis as ties are not used to explain hypnotic response
distinguished between metacogni- an error in monitoring (attributing either; hypnotic behavior is seen as being fun-
tive knowledge and metacognitive the causes of our actions to hypnosis
damentally similar to other more mundane
insight. For example, we may know rather than our intentions) (cf Lynn
that we are one day going to die and et al 2008). Similarly, cold control forms of social behavior, with anyone capable of
we will cease to have a point of view theory pinpoints hypnotic response producing hypnotic responses if they have the
(metacognitive knowledge) yet we as caused by a specific metacognitive right expectations, beliefs, purposes, and attri-
probably do not have a felt sense process: Thinking one does not have butions. Although hypnosis may involve neither
of this fact (metacognitive insight). an intention when one does.
enhanced nor diminished attentional abilities on
this account, it does involve an attributional er-
and attentional regulation. Metacognition refers ror, a failure of metacognition, in attributing ones
to processes that monitor or control thoughts and actions not to oneself but to the hypnotist or a
attention (Fernandez-Duque, Baird, and Posner, special hypnotic state. Once again, these theories
2000): Cognition about cognition (see Box 2). postulate an inherent contradiction between re-
Some theories of hypnosis postulate a disturbance sponding hypnotically and being mindful.
in executive systems, others an enhancement. In contrast to the above accounts, Crawford
Given that meditation involves special attentional (1994) suggests that highs are able to respond
and metacognitive abilities, theories of hypnosis hypnotically due to their superior sustained at-
that postulate impaired attentional or metacogni- tentional abilities. Relatedly, James Braid, the
tive processing suggest hypnosis is the opposite person who coined the term hypnosis, claimed
from meditation. Theories that postulate that that successful hypnotic response occurs because
hypnosis involves no special attentional or meta- highs maintain a persistent uncontradicted im-
cogntiive abilities suggest hypnosis is orthogonal age of the required result (a theory revived by
to meditation. Finally, overlap is suggested by the- Baars, 1988). According to this theory, hypnotic
ories that postulate hypnosis also involves special response involves especially good attentional and
attentional abilities. inhibitory abilities. Actions happen automatically
Several theories have explained hypnosis in by sustained thoughts about the actions, and there
terms of a deficit in frontal lobe executive func- is no inherent contradiction with mindfulness.
tion. According to Hilgards neo-dissociation the- In sum, theoretically, the role of attentional
ory (1977) the executive ego is split in two con- ability in producing hypnotic response is contro-
scious streams, one which controls the hypnotic versial. We will consider the actual evidence for a

46 || MBR || Volume: 2 || Issue: 1


a r t icle The Journal of MindBody Regulation

Box 3. Higher order thoughts


According to HOT theory, a mental state a ball in front of me is a first order state: mental states; with third order thoughts
is a conscious mental state when we are Its content is just about the world. It is we become consciously aware of mental
conscious of being in that mental state only by virtue of a second order thought states, i.e. we introspect (Rosenthal, 2002).
(Rosenthal, 1986, 2002). To be conscious such as thinking I see that there is a ball Higher order thoughts are metacognitive
of anything being present, for example, a in front of me that we are conscious of in that they monitor other mental states.
ball in front of you, you need to be in a the first order mental state of seeing and In fact higher order thought theory pro-
mental state representing that there is seeing then becomes a conscious mental vides one way of defining what meta and
a ball in front of me. To be conscious of state. object level mean. Object-level process-
seeing a ball in front of you, therefore you A second order thought becomes con- es are cognitive processes whose content
need to be in a mental state representing I scious by virtue of a third order thought; is just about the world. Meta-level pro-
see a ball in front of me. That is, you need for example, it is by thinking I am aware cesses must have content which is at least
to be in a mental state about a mental state: that I am seeing that there is a ball in front partly about mental states. Higher order
A higher order state. The thought assert- of me that one becomes consciously thoughts are particular metacognitions,
ing that I am seeing a ball in front of me aware of seeing the ball. With second namely, thoughts asserting that one is in a
is a higher order thought. Seeing there is order thoughts we become conscious of contemporaneous mental state.

role of attention in hypnotic response below. But the state. Thus, hypnosis involves having uncon-
* It is not merely the absence
of accurate HOTs that make
a theme common to some of the theories is that scious intentions. The subject exerts intentional
an intentional action hyp- hypnosis involves a failure of metacognition. In control without having accurate HOTs about
notic, but the presence of Hilgards (1977) dissociation theory and also in those intentions; thus hypnosis is constituted by
the inaccurate HOT that one Spanos (1986) socio-cognitive theories, subjects cold control. Hypnosis is a purely meta-cog-
is not intending the action
(Dienes, 2012). If it were
intend to perform various actions without being nitive phenomenon in which inaccurate higher
just the absence of HOTs of aware of the intention. That is, hypnosis is essen- order thought is produced (Dienes, 2012), a stra-
intending, then every absent tially a form of self deception. This idea was taken tegic failure of metacognition. In sum, in terms
minded performance of an up by Dienes and Perner (2007) as a suggestion of metacognition, theories have either postulated
intentional action would
count as hypnotic, and a
for the simplest theory that might explain hypno- hypnosis involves an impairment of metacogni-
sis: Hypnotic response consists of nothing more tion (e.g. Dienes and Perner, 2007; Hilagrd, 1977;

mbr. s y ner g ie s pr a i r ie s .c a
creature, perhaps a dog, not
capable of HOTs of inten- nor less than intending to perform some motor or Spanos, 1986), or postulate no special relation
tion would be permanently cognitive act while thinking one is not intending of hypnotic responding to metacognition (e.g.
responding hypnotically!
If inaccurate HOTs are
the action* (see Box 4). The Dienes and Perner Baars, 1988). We now consider the actual evi-
required, it is only an animal account draws on Rosenthals (2004) higher or- dence for the relation between each of meditation
which possesses mental state der thought (HOT) theory of conscious aware- and hypnosis and attention and metacognition.
concepts of intention that ness (see Box 3). HOT theory claims that a men-
could in principle respond
hypnotically (by believing
tal state is a conscious mental state when we are Attention in hypnosis and
they did not intend the conscious of being in that mental state, i.e. when meditation
action). we have a higher order thought about being in
Both meditation and hypnosis have been claimed
Box 4. Cold control theory to involve enhanced attentional skill, particu-
Cold control theory (Dienes and Per- While cold control theory sees hyp- larly in sustained attention (e.g. s, 1963; Buttle,
ner, 2007) explains hypnotic respond- nosis as intrinsically involving self- 2011; Davidson and Goleman, 1977; Gruzelier,
ing as executive control (e.g. intend- deception, such self deception can be 1998; Karlin, 1979; Lutz, Slagter and Dunne, 2008;
ing to perform a motor or cognitive benign or even useful. Hypnosis in- Raz, 2005; Slagter et al, 2007, 2009; Tang, Ma,
action, e.g. lifting an arm, imagining volves creating illusions of reality or
something present) while having in- automaticity according to situational Wang, Fan, Feng, Yu, et al, 2007; Valentine and
accurate higher order thoughts of requirements; that is, hypnotic re- Sweet, 1999). Recent research has shown that
intending (I am not intending to lift sponding is goal directed striving attentional skills can be trained by meditation
my arm/imagine a pink elephant). (White, 1942), where the hypnotic practice (see Lutz et al, 2008 and Austin, 2006,
On this account, a person has no response is consistent with the over- for reviews.) The relation of attentional skill to
first order abilities in responding to a all goals and intentions of the subject.
hypnotic suggestion that they did not Thus, hypnosis is like a metacognitive hypnosis is more controversial (e.g. Dienes et al
have already. The difference between game: A meta-cognitive strategy of 2009; Jamieson and Sheehan, 2002). We consider
a hypnotic and non-hypnotic action relinquishing metacognition (specifi- meditation and hypnosis in turn.
is that performing the action hypnoti- cally accurate concerning a specific Meditation can be narrowly viewed as a
cally makes it feel like it is happening intention) in order to have the expe- form of attention training (Bishop et al, 2006).
by itself. For example, an intention to riences a situation calls for (e.g. pain
lift ones arm is usually accompanied going away by itself as one imagines Lutz et al (2009) found that intensive medita-
by a higher order thought, e.g. I am a dial being turned; being possessed tion enhanced attentional stability. Meditators
intending to lift my arm, but this is by a great spirit) (Dienes, 2012). Our were compared before and after a three month
not the case in hypnosis, according to contrast of hypnosis with meditation intensive meditation retreat, during which they
cold control theory. (It is cold control is not to deny the potential usefulness practiced open monitoring, focused attention
because there is no accurate HOT.) of hypnosis.
and loving kindness meditation for 1012 hours

47 || MBR || Volume: 2 || Issue: 1


a r t icle The Journal of MindBody Regulation

per day. Participants in the control condition re- They found that while some attentional improve-
ceived a one hour meditation training session be- ments (sustained, visual and temporal attention)
fore each experimental session and meditated for can be accounted for by an increase in attentional
20 minutes per day for one week before each test- effort, only the group who received MBSR train-
ing session. Both groups were asked to perform ing showed improvements on sustained selective
two versions of a dichotic listening task: the open attention, suggesting that MBSR training had an
monitoring version, in which they were asked to effect above and beyond motivation and non-spe-
identify deviant tones in both ears, and the fo- cific stress reduction (Jensen, Vangkilde, Frokjaer
cused attention version, in which they were asked and Hasselbalch, 2012).
to identify a deviant tone in the one attended ear, The above studies compared experts and
whilst ignoring tones in the other ear. Using EEG, novices in meditation; the comparable studies in
they found that meditators showed increased the- hypnosis are those comparing high and low hyp-
ta-band phase consistency compared to pre-train- notizables. Evidence of superior attentional abili-
ing and to controls, consistent with sustained at- ties in highs rather than lows is mixed and the
tention and on-line cognitive control. Meditators issue remains unresolved (contrast e.g. Crawford
also showed reduced variability in neuronal pro- et al, 1993 with Dienes et al, 2009). Studies us-
cessing regardless of whether or not the tone was ing the Stroop test have produced conflicting
deviant, in line with claims that focused attention findings, with either no significant difference
meditation develops the monitoring faculty, and between highs and lows or with differences in
so one remains vigilant to distractions while re- either direction. Without hypnotic induction or
taining a stable focus, as well as enhanced ability suggestions being used, most studies have found
to disengage from distraction. Slagter et al (2007) no significant difference between highs and lows
found that participants in one study using an at- on Stroop interference (Aikens and Ray 2001;
tentional blink paradigm demonstrated similar Egner et al 2005; Kaiser et al 1997; Kallio et al
detection of T1 (the first target) and increased 2001). Dixon et al (1990) and Dixon and Laurence
detection of T2 (the second target), compared to (1992) found significantly more Stroop interfer-

mbr. s y ner g ie s pr a i r ie s .c a
baseline and controls, following three months of ence in highs than lows; however, Rubichi et al
intensive meditation training, indicating medita- (2005) found significantly less Stroop interfer-
tion practice produced a more optimal distribu- ence in highs rather than lows. On a related task
tion of attention. On the Stroop task, expert med- Iani et al (2006) found that highs and lows with-
itators versus controls showed decreased Stroop out an induction were not detectably different in
interference (Moore and Malinowski, 2009; terms of the effect of irrelevant flanking items on
Wenk-Sormaz, 2005). Further, meditators re- the classification of a central letter. While proce-
ported higher level of mindfulness (on the KIMS) dural differences (e.g. responses given as button
and mindfulness was linked to fewer errors on the presses vs. vocalization) may account for the dif-
Stroop task (Moore and Malinowski, 2009) ferent results, the pattern allows only nuanced
The studies to date consistently point in claims about attention and hypnotizability. A
the direction of good attentional skills in expert component of attention is the ability to inhibit
rather than novice meditators, though a general irrelevant information. On a negative priming
problem in the field is accounting for motiva- task, in which participants are instructed to at-
tional differences between experts and controls. tend to some stimuli and ignore others, Dienes et
When experts know they are selected for the al (2009) found with 180 participants the correla-
study based on being experts, they may work tions between hypnotizability and negative prim-
harder to meet the demands of the situation, or ing or between hypnotizability and latent inhibi-
produce expected responses just because they tion were close to zero, with upper limits of about
are expected (response expectancy; Kirsch, 1985; 0.20. Similarly, Varga et al (2011) with 116 subjects
1997). A frequent control in the hypnosis field is found no significant correlations between hypno-
to select high and low hypnotizables for further tizability and reaction time measures of sustained,
testing without subjects knowing the basis on selective, divided or executive attention. In sum,
which they are being selected (Council, Kirsch, the consistent findings that expert meditators are
and Hafner, 1986) but many studies on the ef- superior to novices in attention are not in general
fects of meditation or mindfulness training have matched by evidence for highly hypnotizables be-
not taken this into account. A recent study, how- ing better at attention tasks than lows.
ever, tried to disentangle motivational effects by While highs do not seem especially good at
comparing a control group with other groups that attentional tasks when asked to simply perform
received a financial incentive, mindfulness based them, when they are given a relevant strategy,
stress reduction training (MBSR) and non-mind- they can outperform lows whether or not they
fulness based stress reduction training (NMSR). had been given a hypnotic induction (Raz et al,

48 || MBR || Volume: 2 || Issue: 1


a r t icle The Journal of MindBody Regulation

2006). Specifically, when highs are given the sug- people in versus out of a meditative state). It
gestion that words will appear to them as mean- may be that the act of producing feelings of an
ingless, the Stroop effect can be substantially re- altered state takes up capacity, and thus leaves less
duced (e.g. Parris, Dienes, and Hodgson, in press; capacity for other tasks.
Raz et al 2002; Raz et al 2003; see also Iani et al One interesting marker of attention to task
2006). Similarly, Iani, Ricci Baroni and Rubichi is activity in the default mode network (DMN).
(2009) found that hypnotic suggestion reduced The DMN is associated with mind-wandering
interference from irrelevant spatial stimuli in and self-referential thought and reduced acti-
highs, but not lows in a Simon-like (spatial in- vation is usually seen when focusing or paying
terference) task. The suggestion that reduces the attention during goal-directed and externally
Stroop effect has been shown to be just as effec- oriented tasks (Christoff, Gordon, Smallwood,
tive whether or not a hypnotic induction is given Smith and Schooler 2009; Uddin, Kelly, Biswal,
(Raz et al 2006; contrast Iani et al 2006), so ap- Xavier Castellanos, and Milham, 2009). Further,
pears not to depend on being in a special state, activity tends to be lower during high rather
but on having a certain ability. The effect appears than low cognitive load (Uddin et al, 2009).
non-existent to weak in lows (Parris and Dienes, Consistent with the claim that an induction en-
submitted; Raz and Campbell 2011). Thus, para- courages highs to pay attention to the hypnotist
doxically, asking highs to be less mindful (i.e. to and/or specific strategies, McGeown et al (2009)
act hypnotically) enables them to perform better found activity in the anterior DMN (ventrome-
on the same attention tasks (Stroop) that medita- dial prefrontal cortex and DLPFC) was reduced
tors appear to improve upon by meditation train- following hypnotic induction during rest periods
ing. We do not have a resolution to the paradox between suggestions in highs. Lows, on the other
that both being more mindful (meditation) and hand, showed reduced activity in areas related to
less mindful (hypnosis) improves Stroop, but it is alertness, probably in response to the relaxation
a problem on which we are currently working. induction used. Deeley et al (2012) conducted a
In terms of what happens to attentional abil- similar study, scanning participants during pas-

mbr. s y ner g ie s pr a i r ie s .c a
ity after a hypnotic induction, Gruzelier and sive viewing condition, however no suggestions
Warren (1993) found that highs performed worse were provided. Reduced activity was seen in the
on letter fluency (particularly associated with DMN and anterior cingulate cortex and increased
activation in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; activity in prefrontal attentional systems after
DLPFC) and finger tapping dexterity but im- hypnotic induction, compared to pre-induction.
proved on design fluency (a measure of planning Furthermore, participants reported greater lev-
and cognitive flexibility) following a hypnotic els of self-reported attentional absorption, which
induction. The reduction in performance on the was inversely related to activity in the DMN. The
letter fluency task was replicated by Kallio et al results of McGeown et al and Deeley et al support
(2001) and Wagstaff, Cole and Brunas-Wagstaff the notion that an induction informs subjects to
(2007). However, Wagstaff et al also found that pay attention to whatever strategies are needed to
subjects who reported greater hypnotic depth maintain a feeling of being hypnotized.
(strongly correlated with hypnotic suggestibility) A number of studies have also shown de-
demonstrated better performance on a phonemic creased activation in the DMN in meditators dur-
fluency test during hypnosis than during the non- ing concentration (FA), mett bhavana (loving
hypnotic condition. Egner et al (2005) found evi- kindness) and choiceless awareness (OM) medi-
dence using neuroimaging techniques that there tation, compared to non-meditators (Brewer et al,
is a dissociation of frontal executive and conflict 2011). This may reflect high cognitive load in these
monitoring systems. They used EEG to examine styles of meditation. In support of this conjecture,
functional connectivity and event-related fMRI another study that investigated activity in the
to image highs and lows while they performed DMN in meditators gained the opposite results:
on a task measuring attentional conflict resolu- greater activation in mid frontal brain regions
tion (the Stroop task) following a hypnotic in- overlapping the DMN during meditation practice
duction. There was an increase in gamma band (Travis et al, 2009). Participants were perform-
coherence (associated with concerted attentional ing transcendental meditation (TM), which is re-
focus) in lows and a decrease in highs, along with ported to be a simple, easy and non-demanding
an increase in ACC activation, consistent with task. Thus, work on DMN activity has shown that
poorer efficiency of conflict monitoring in highs hypnosis and meditation generally involve acts of
(although no difference in Stroop performance paying attention, but this is consistent with medi-
was observed). The (albeit checkered) evidence tation, which is explicitly associated with mental
for a general reduction in attentional functioning training, involving especially good attentional
after hypnotic induction contrasts to claims about abilities and hypnotic responsiveness requiring
meditation (though little research has tested only average attentional abilities.

49 || MBR || Volume: 2 || Issue: 1


a r t icle The Journal of MindBody Regulation

In sum, both hypnosis and meditation involve subjects did not differ in their expectancy that
paying attention, but whereas experienced medi- they would respond in the two conditions, so the
tators have better attentional skills than novices, rTMS had an effect on hypnotic experience above
hypnotizability appears unrelated to attentional and beyond expectancies. A further study con-
skill, and the induction of a hypnotic state may ceptually replicated the effect, but this time using
even be associated with impoverished attentional alcohol, which has been shown to particularly af-
skills (though contrast Iani et al, 2006). fect the DLPFC (Wendt and Risberg, 2001). We
recently explored the effect of alcohol on hyp-
Meta-awareness and the dor- notic response with Theodora Duka at Sussex
solateral prefrontal cortex University. Medium hypnotizables were assigned
to either an alcohol or placebo alcohol condition
A fundamental difference between hypnotic sug- and were then tested on nine suggestions and
gestion and meditation is that hypnosis often various frontal tasks. Alcohol indeed disrupted
involves an altered or distorted experience of frontal function and crucially, alcohol increased
reality. The purpose of meditation for long-term hypnotic response by one scale unit compared
practitioners, on the other hand, is to stimulate to placebo, on the same scale as used in the TMS
change and development towards seeing reality study. Although both the TMS and alcohol would
more clearly (Kamalashila, 1992), partly by devel- have affected regions of the brain outside the
oping meta-awareness, or accurate higher order DLPFC, the evidence is consistent with cold con-
thoughts (HOTs). trol, hypnosis as self deception. The evidence is
Accurate HOTs, i.e. awareness of mental also consistent with other theories that postulate
states, has been linked to the dorsolateral prefron- hypnosis involves diminished executive control
tal cortex (DLPFC) (Lau and Passingham, 2006). (Woody and Bowers, 1994) or metacognitive con-
Lau and Passingham found two masking condi- trol (Jamieson and Woody, 2007). Either way, the
tions in which people could discriminate one of increase of hypnotizability following disruption
two shapes to an equal degree but differed in the of the dlPFC supports the distinction between

mbr. s y ner g ie s pr a i r ie s .c a
extent to which they were aware of seeing the hypnosis and meditation, during which increased
shapes rather than just guessing about them. That activation is seen in the DLPFC.
is, the DLPFC was not linked to the first order In contrast to findings in hypnosis literature,
mental state of seeing, but to awareness of seeing. several studies have shown increased activation
Further, when Rounis et al (2010) disrupted the in the DLPFC, among other areas, in meditation
area with theta burst TMS, subjects self-reported practitioners during and after meditation prac-
awareness of seeing was disrupted even when first tice (Brewer et al, 2011; Farb et al, 2007; Kosaza et
order perception was titrated to be the same with al, 2008; Lazar, 2009). For example, Brefczynski-
and without TMS. That is, the disruption Rounis Lewis et al (2007) used fMRI to compare experi-
et al found was purely related to HOTs. We might enced and novice meditators and found greater
expect to see differences between highly hypno- activation in the DLPFC. Concordantly, it has
tizables and meditators in the dorsolateral pre- been claimed that meditators are better at giving
frontal cortex (DLPFC). Specifically, we might self-reports than non-meditators (Kabat-Zinn,
expect less activation in highs and greater activa- 2011). In sum, the differential activation seen
* Cold control theory tion in meditators. in the DLPFC during meditation and hypnosis
makes a more precise set of If the DLPFC is responsible for accurate seems to support the suggestion that hypnosis
predictions. Namely, if the higher order thoughts in general, disrupting the and meditation differ in metacognitive capa-
frontal lobes are impaired
region with rTMS or alcohol should make it hard- bilities subserved by the DLPFC (Dienes, 2012;
sufficiently that the relevant
cognitive or motor action er to be aware of intending to perform an action. Woody and Sadler, 2008).
cannot be performed with Given that people who are highly hypnotizable
conscious intentions then seem to have less accurate HOTs, disrupting the Meditation is hotter than
the same action cannot be
performed as a hypnotic
function of the DLPFC should make it easier to hypnosis
suggestion (Dienes and subjectively respond to a hypnotic suggestion (ac-
Perner, 2007). However, cording to cold control theory)*. One apparent similarity between hypnosis and
if the impairment allows Dienes and Hutton (submitted) tested this meditation is that both seem to involve absorp-
the relevant actions to be
with TMS. Subjects gave ratings on a 05 scale tion. Like high versus low hypnotizables, expert
performed, but is targeted
to impair HOT accuracy, of the extent to which they experienced the re- versus novice meditators score more highly on
then hypnotic performance sponse, for four suggestions (magnetic hands, absorption as measured by the Tellegen absorp-
will be facilitated. Thus far arm levitation, rigid arm and taste hallucination). tion scale (TAS) (Davidson, 1976). We have
at least, in making these pre-
Overall, rTMS to the DLPFC rather than vertex since replicated this finding in our lab at Sussex
dictions, cold control theory
has remained one step ahead increased degree of hypnotic response by about University (in an as yet unpublished study), and
of the data. a third of a rating point on average. Further, found that absorption also correlated positively

50 || MBR || Volume: 2 || Issue: 1


a r t icle The Journal of MindBody Regulation

with self-reported mindfulness as measured by at images while trying to either a) remain at all
the Kentucky Inventory of Mindfulness Skills times aware of seeing the image (meditation
(KIMS; Baer, Smith and Allen, 2004). Although task; cf. Van Nuys, 1973) or b) not consciously see
the phenomenological states of hypnosis and the image for 15 minutes (ignore task; compare
meditative absorptions (dhyanas in Sanskrit) Wegners, 1994, white bear ironic control task,
appear to be similar in some respects (Holroyd, where people are asked to not think of a white
2008), the feeling of absorption involved in both bear). Subjects were asked at random intervals
hypnotic and meditative states may reflect differ- (roughly once a minute) whether they were just
ent processes, as we now discuss. that instant before aware of seeing the image.
Although absorption and hypnosis appear Because people remained physically looking at
to be related, we need to take care in our under- the images there was a persistent first order visual
standing of what precisely absorption is. The sub- representation of each image; but to what extent
jective sense of being absorbed could represent did people have accurate higher order thoughts
four distinct modes of mental processing. Firstly, about seeing it? The difference between a) and b)
one could be mind wandering without being in reports of seeing the image was taken as mea-
aware that one was doing so, thus one could be- suring control in having accurate HOTs, and the
lieve one was or had been absorbed in the main total number of reports of seeing the image in
task (c.f. Schooler, 2006). Particularly when en- both a) and b) as measuring coupling of HOTs to
gaging in open-ended imaginative activity, such first order states, i.e. the tendency to have an ap-
mind wandering may not show in any obvious propriate HOT given that a first order state exists.
way, and indeed may blend with the imaginative We found that highs had poorer HOT coupling
activity itself. Secondly, there could be thoughts than lows, i.e. less accurate higher order thoughts
distracting the mind, but there is meta-awareness across both tasks (Dienes, 2012, and Semmens-
of this distraction, allowing disengagement from Wheeler and Dienes, 2011). This greater propensi-
the distraction to take place. Thirdly, irrelevant ty to mind-wander meant highs were marginally
thoughts may be present, but attention is neither better at ironic control than lows (and non-signif-

mbr. s y ner g ie s pr a i r ie s .c a
attached to nor averted from them; the mind is icantly worse at meditation). This apparent weak
not distracted. Finally, one could actually be sin- coupling may allow highs to decide in appropri-
gle-mindedly or one-pointedly thinking about ate contexts to forgo higher order thoughts of in-
the object of thought (see Taylor, 2002). The first tending in order to respond hypnotically to sug-
state of absorption is only apparent absorption; it gestions. In contrast, we found that meditators
appears to be absorption because of a lack of ac- were poor at ironic control compared to highs, a
curate HOTs (call it cold absorption) In medi- finding which could be explained by the fact that
tation, one aims to progress through these states, they had significantly more HOTs than both lows
becoming aware of the mental chatter that usu- and highs. (It is intriguing to find meditators bad
ally pervades our minds, letting go of thoughts at a mental control task!) Thus it seems unlikely
and entering the dhyanas (absorptions, in which that meditators would experience hypnotic sug-
one becomes aware of more subtle thoughts and gestion through a lack of HOT coupling and we
sensations; see Austin, 2006 and Holroyd, 2003 might even expect that they would not be very
for more in-depth description). Meditation and hypnotizable.
mindfulness practice involve training in the de- Consistent with the evidence that highs are
velopment of second-order awareness (Teasdale, not absorbed in an undistracted way, hypnotic
1999; Wallace 1999). This could be described as a responding itself does not require attending
state of absorption that involves meta-awareness one-pointedly to one idea. Zamansky and Clark
(Lutz, Dunne and Davidson, 2007) and is a goal (1986) asked subjects to engage in imagery incon-
state of meditation. We could call meditative sistent with the hypnotic suggestions given (e.g.
absorption HOT absorption (absorption with for a rigid arm suggestion, to imagine a differ-
HOTs), reflecting a state of absorbed attention in ent world in which their arm is bending). Highs
which one remains meta-aware of the contents were just as responsive to suggestions (e.g. that
of ones consciousness as opposed, possibly, to the arm is unbendable) when engaged in imagery
hypnotic absorption: cold absorption (absorp- inconsistent with the suggestion as when having
tion without HOTs), in which one has less meta- consistent imagery, even as they concurrently
awareness of the contents of ones consciousness reported the imagery. That is, their arm remains
and thus may more easily create inaccurate HOTs unbent, even as the subjects described an image
about their experience. of the arm bending. Thus, the theory that highs
We have tested the hypothesis that highs have attend to one idea in order to achieve hypnotic
fewer accurate HOTs than meditators and con- response is false. Hypnotic response will tolerate
trols at Sussex by asking subjects to keep looking not only mind wandering but also contradictory

51 || MBR || Volume: 2 || Issue: 1


a r t icle The Journal of MindBody Regulation

ideas. By contrast, the absorption in meditation (Note that participants were not probed, but were
can be specifically directed at making the mind asked to report whenever they noticed thoughts,
hold to one idea without distraction. i.e. when they had a HOT.) Based on our dis-
Shaw (2006 p 98), based on the descriptions cussion of hot and cold absorption (i.e. absorbed
of meditation in the Pali canon, describes how attention with and without meta-awareness, re-
the meditator gradually acquires the feeling of spectively), there is an obvious methodological
control of entering, sustaining and leaving ab- problem with the Van Nuys task. The task may
sorbed statesin contrast to the lack of control not have really been measuring the number of
a hypnotized subject may feel (indeed, needs to intrusive thoughts, but the awareness of such
feel in some way for a response to be hypnotic). thoughts arising. Thus, another way of interpret-
In a related way, Gombrich (1996 p 163) empha- ing the results could be to say that it was the lack
sizes the self control and self awareness cultivated of awareness of intrusive thoughts i.e. a lack of
in Buddhism as an antithesis to spirit possession. meta-awareness that predicted hypnotizability
Dienes and Perner (2007) identified spirit posses- (see Smallwood and Schooler, 2006 for descrip-
sion as the same natural kind as hypnosis (i.e. as a tion of meta-awareness and mind wandering). On
case of cold control). this account highs may even have more intrusive
Mindfulness versus self deception are general thoughts than lows (as found by Bryant and Idey,
principles defining the nature of meditation and 2001, on a self-report questionnaire). When we
hypnosis in broad brush stroke; of course specific determined on-line number of intrusive thoughts
hypnotic and meditative experiences may draw by probes, as described above (Dienes, 2012, and
on the other in detail. For example, vajrayana Semmens-Wheeler and Dienes, 2011), allowing
meditation (Gyatso, 2005) involves coming to see the measure of intrusive thoughts to be separated
imagination as reality, but this does not take away from meta-awareness (as per the methodology of
from the fact that meditation must involve cul- Smallwood and Schooler, 2006), hypnotizability
tivation of mindfulness generally to be Buddhist was not associated with concentration on the task
meditation at all. (Even such cultivation will al- in our data (hypnotizability was non-significantly

mbr. s y ner g ie s pr a i r ie s .c a
ways leave scope for inaccurate higher order associated with more intrusions, r=.21, 95% CI
thoughts; cf.Shaw, 2006, p 66, points 2, 4, 5, 6, and [-.09, .51], the confidence interval ruling out the
7 for mistaken beliefs the Buddha apparently had size of effect Van Nuys obtained, i.e. r about -.3)
about his mental states, e.g. recalling past lives.*) Without training, hypnotic suggestibil-
Just as an episode in a hypnotic context may in- ity is relatively stable, with test-retest correla-
volve some cognitive activities being consciously tions ranging from 0.64 to 0.82 (Barnier and
controlled in a mindful way (Dienes, 2012; Yapko, McConkey, 2004). So far a number of attempts
2011) so meditation may involve inaccurate high- have been made to enhance participants hypnot-
er order thoughts. Nonetheless, if a person were ic suggestibility, with some success (e.g. Cooper,
aware and mindful of all intentions they would Banford, Schubot, and Tart, 1967; Diamond, 1972;
have failed to respond hypnotically; and a medi- Engstrom, Perry and Hart, 1970). Apparently suc-
tator misrepresenting a mental state would have cessful attempts have involved changing subjects
failed to be mindful. Where each succeeds in its beliefs, expectations and attitudes to hypnosis in
* Accurate higher order
goals, meditation is hotter than hypnosis. a single session (e.g. the Carleton Skills Training
thoughts depend as much Package, see Gorassini, 2004; see also Wickless
on having a good theory of Directly comparing medita- and Kirsch, 1989, and contrast Benham, Bowers,
mental states as on cultivat-
ing the process of notic-
tion and hypnosis? Nash, and Muenchen, 1998). These training
ing mental states - just as
schemes contrast dramatically with the extensive
accurate observation of the So far we have compared meditation and hypno- attentional training required to make progress
world depends crucially sis indirectly. We will now consider directly the in meditation. Brief meditation training has not
on good theory (consider relationship between meditation and hypnosis. yet been found to increase hypnotic ability (e.g.
telescopes) (cf Hurlburt
and Schitzgebel, 2007, and
First we consider the correlation between success Heide, Wadlington and Lundy, 1980).
Petitmengin, 2009, for a at meditating and responding to hypnosis, then What is the effect of extensive meditation
discussion of the capabilities whether training can improve meditation and training on hypnotic response? We compared
and limits of introspection). hypnosis, and finally the hypnotizability of people scores of twelve expert meditators on the Waterloo
In Bayesian terms, accurate
beliefs about ones mental
who meditate extensively. Stanford Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility
states are improved by hav- Van Nuys (1974; also Spanos, Rivers, and (WGSC; Bowers, 1998) with scores of over 500
ing good priors. Because of Gottlieb, 1978) found that performance on a screened participants in the University of Sussex
this, empirical results from meditation task significantly predicted hypnotiz- database; our preliminary findings show that
experimental psychology
may inform good medita-
ability. Participants carried out a meditation task, meditators, passing on average 3/12 suggestions,
tion practice (and vice which involved participants pressing a button were less susceptible than the average of all
versa). whenever they experienced an intruding thought. subjects in the database combined (average 5.5

52 || MBR || Volume: 2 || Issue: 1


a r t icle The Journal of MindBody Regulation

suggestions). Furthermore, in an as yet unpub- both theoretically and methodologically, as it


lished study at Sussex we found that highly hyp- can explain some effects in both meditation and
notically suggestible individuals (highs) scored hypnosis. For example, if one expects that either
significantly lower on measures of mindfulness, meditation or hypnosis will impair or enhance
which is associated with meditation experience performance on a particular task, then one is
(Semmens-Wheeler and Erskine, 2009). The cor- likely to conform the this belief (Kirsch, 1997).
relation between hypnotizability and mindfulness In clinical practice, techniques called hypnotic or
was -.38. The tendency for meditators for be less mindful may overlap (Lynn, Das, Hallquist, and
hypnotizable than non-meditators may reflect Williams, 2006), but we need to be careful which
poor attitudes and low expectations on the part of specific activities we call hypnotic. Just calling re-
meditators about hypnosis; we are directly testing laxation or the use of imagination or a suggestion
this possibility now. We predict that meditators for improvement hypnotic does not make it so.
will score low on hypnotizability even after con- As we said in the introduction, to turn cognitive
trolling for beliefs and expectations, precisely be- activities into hypnotic responses, those activities
cause they have cultivated having accurate higher must involve distortion in the sense of voluntari-
order thoughts. ness or reality. Further, similar clinical outcomes
may be produced by opposite strategies, for ex-
Conclusion ample, analgesia may be produced by seeing pain
for exactly what the experience is (Salomons and
There are certainly some areas of overlap between Kucyi, 2011), or by using cognitive strategies of
meditation and hypnosis, yet this relationship may distraction and reinterpretation (strategies one
turn out to be orthogonal or opposed, particularly intended without knowing, Dienes, 2012). As
with regard to meta-awareness. We have argued urged by Lynn et al and Yapko (2011), research
that the essential nature of hypnotic response, exploring whether hypnosis and mindfulness are
that which makes it hypnotic at all, is a strategic useful as complimentary integrated clinical strat-
self deception with respect to ones intentions egies is important. Future studies could explore

mbr. s y ner g ie s pr a i r ie s .c a
(Dienes, 2012; Hilgard, 1977; Spanos, 1986; Spanos metacognition and cognitive flexibility (e.g. cog-
and Gorassini, 1999); by contrast, an essential nitive set-shifting ability) in meditators and high
component of meditative practice is mindfulness, hypnotizables and also provide more in-depth
seeing plainly what is there. However, it is im- analysis of phenomenological experience (Pekala,
portant to take into account response expectancy 1982, 1991).

References
Anlayo (2010). Satipatthana: The Direct Barber, T. X., Wilson, S. C. (1978). The Bar- Barnier, A., & Nash, M. R. (2008) Intro-
Path to Realization. Cambridge, England: ber Suggestibility Scale and the Creative duction: A roadmap for explanation, a
Windhorse Publications. Imagination Scale: Experimental and working definition. . In M. R. Nash & A.
clinical applications. American Journal of Barnier (Eds.). The Oxford handbook of
s, A. (1963).Hypnotizability as a func-
Clinical Hypnosis, 21, 84108. hypnosis: Theory, research and practice
tionof nonhypnotic experiences. Journal
(pp. 118). Oxford, England: Oxford
of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 66, Barnier, A. J. Dienes, Z. & Mitchell, C. J.
University Press.
142150. (2008). How hypnosis happens: New cog-
nitive theories of hypnotic responding. In Beran, M., Brandl, J., Perner, J., & Proust,
Baars, B. J.(1998). A Cognitive Theory of
M. Nash & A. Barnier (Eds.) The Oxford J. (Eds.). (2012). The Foundations of
Consciousness. Cambridge, England:
Handbook of Hypnosis: Theory, Research, Metacognition. Oxford, England: Oxford
Cambridge University Press.
and Practice (pp. 141178). Oxford, Eng- University Press.
Baer, R. A., Smith, G.T. & Allen, K. B. land: Oxford University Press.
Bishop. S,, Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L.,
(2004). Assessment of mindfulness by
Barnier, A., & McConkey, K. (2004). Defin- Bishop,S. Anderson, N. D., ... Devins,G.
self-report: The Kentucky inventory of
ing and identifying the highly hypnotiz- (2006). Mindfulness: A proposed opera-
mindfulness skills. Assessment, 11(3),
able person. In M. Heap, R. J. Brown & D. tion definition. Clinical Psychology:Sci-
191206.
A. Oakley (Eds.). The Highly Hypnotizable enceandPractice,11, 230241.
Banyai, E. I., Hilgard, E. R. (1976). A com- Person (pp. 3060). London, England:
Bowen, S., Witkiewitz, K., and Dillworth, T.,
parison of active-alert hypnotic induction Routledge.
Chawla, N., Simpson, T., Ostafin, B., ...
with traditional relaxation induction.
Marlatt, G. (2006). Mindfulness medita-
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 85(2),
tion and substance use in an incarcer-
218224.
ated population. Psychology of Addictive
Behaviors, 20(3), 343347.

53 || MBR || Volume: 2 || Issue: 1


a r t icle The Journal of MindBody Regulation

Bowers, K. S. (1998). Waterloo-Stanford Crawford, H., Brown, A., & Moon, C. (1993). Dixon, M., & Laurence, J.-R. (1992). Hyp-
group scale of hypnotic susceptibility, Sustained attentional and disattentional notic susceptibility and verbal automatic-
form C: Manual and response booklet. abilities: Differences between low and ity: Automatic and strategic processing
International Journal of Clinical and Ex- highly hypnotizable persons. Journal of differences in the Stroop-colour naming
perimental Hypnosis, 46, 250268. Abnormal Psychology, 102(4), 534543. task. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101,
344347.
Braffman, W., & Kirsch, I. (1999). Imagina- Critchley, H. D., Wiens, S., Rotshtein, P.,
tive suggestibility and hypnotizability: An hman, A., & Dolan, R. J. (2004). Neural Eckert, M. A., Menon, V., Walczak, A.,
empirical analysis. Journal of Personality systems supporting interoceptive aware- Ahlstrom, J., Denslow, S., Horwitz. A.,
and Social Psychology, 77, 578587. ness. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 189195. & Dubno, J. (2009). At the heart of the
ventral attention system: The right ante-
Braffman, W., & Kirsch, I. (2001). Reaction Davidson, R., & Goleman, D. (1977). The
rior insula. Human Brain Mapping, 30(8),
time as a predictor of imaginative suggest- role of attention in meditation and hyp-
25302541.
ibility and hypnotizability. Contemporary nosis: A psychobiological perspective on
Hypnosis, 18(3), 107119. transformations of consciousness. Inter- Engstrom, D., Perry, P., & Hart, J. (1970).
national Journal of Clinical and Experi- Hypnotic Susceptibility increased by EEG
Brefczynski-Lewis, J. A., Lutz, A., Schaefer,
mental Hypnosis, 25(4), 291308. Alpha Training. Nature, 227, 12611262.
H. S., Levinson, D. B., & Davidson, R. J.
(2007). Neural correlates of attentional Deeley, Q., Oakley, D. A., Toone, B., Giampi- Farb, N., Segal, Z. V., Mayberg, H., Bean, J.,
expertise in long-term meditation practi- etro, V., Brammer, M. J., Williams, S. C., McKeon, D., Fatima, Z., & Anderson, A.
tioners. Proceedings of the National Acad- & Halligan, P. (2012). Modulating the (2007). Attending to the present: Mind-
emy of Sciences, 104(27) 1148311488. default mode network using hypnosis. fulness meditation reveals distinct neural
The International journal of clinical and modes of self-reference. Social Cognitive
Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The
experimental hypnosis, 60, 206228. and Affective Neuroscience, 2, 313322.
benefits of being present: Mindfulness
and its role in psychological well-being. Delmonte, M. M. (1984). Meditation: Fernandez-Duque, D., Baird, J. A., & Posner,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- Similarities with hypnoidal states and M. I. (2000). Executive attention and
ogy, 84(4), 822848. hypnosis. International Journal of Psycho- metacognitive regulation. Consciousness
somatics, 31, 2434. and Cognition, 9, 288307.
Bryant, R. A., & Idey, A. (2001). Intrusive
thoughts and hypnotisability. Contempo- Derbyshire, S. W., Whalley, M. G., & Oakley, Gfeller, J. D., Lynn, S. J. Pribbe WE (1987).
rary Hypnosis, 18, 1420. D. A. (2009). Fibromyalgia pain and its Enhancing hypnotic susceptibility: Inter-
modulation by hypnotic and non-hypnot- personal and Rapport Factors. Journal
Buttle, H. (2011). Attention and Working

mbr. s y ner g ie s pr a i r ie s .c a
ic suggestion: An fMRI analysis. European of Personality and Social Psychology, 52,
Memory in Mindfulness-Meditation
Journal of Pain, 13, 542550. 586595.
Practices. Journal of Mind and Behavior,
32(2), 123134. Diamond, M. J. (1972). The use of observa- Gombrich, R. F. (1996). How Buddhism
tionally presented information to modify began: The conditioned genesis of the early
Carruthers, P. (2000). Phenomenal conscious-
hypnotic susceptibility. Journal of Abnor- teachings. New Delhi, India: Muchiram
ness naturally. Cambridge, England:
mal Psychology, 79(2), 174180. Manoharlal Publishers.
Cambridge University Press.
Dienes, Z. (2012). Is hypnotic responding Gorassini, D. (2004), Enhancing Hyp-
Chan, D., & Woolacott, M. (2007). Effects of
the strategic relinquishment of metacog- notzability. In M. Heap, R. J. Brown & D.
Level of Meditation Experience on Atten-
nition? In M. Beran, J. Brandl, J. Perner A. Oakley (Eds.). The Highly Hypnotiz-
tional Focus: Is the Efficiency of Executive
& J. Proust (Eds.). The Foundations of able Person. New York, NY: Brunner-
or Orientation Networks Improved? The
Metacognition. Oxford, England: Oxford Routledge.
Journal of Alternative and Complementary
University Press.
Medicine. 13(6), 651658. Gruzelier, J. (1998). A working model of the
Dienes, Z., Brown, E., Hutton, S., Kirsch, neurophysiology of hypnosis: A review of
Christoff, K., Gordon, A. M., Smallwood,
I., Mazzoni, G., & Wright, D. B.(2009). evidence. Contemporary Hypnosis, 15(1),
J., Smith, R., & Schooler, J. W. (2009).
Hypnotic suggestibility, cognitive inhibi- 321.
Experience sampling during fMRI
tion, and dissociation.Consciousness and
reveals default network and executive Gyatso, G. K. (2005). Mahamudra Tantra:
Cognition, 18 837847.
system contributions to mind wandering. The supreme heart jewel nectar, a reduc-
Proceedings of the National Academy if Dienes, Z., & Perner, J. (2007). The cold tion to meditation on tantra. Cumbria,
Science. U. S. A., 106, 87198724. control theory of hypnosis. In G. Jamieson UK: Tharpa Publications.
(Ed.). Hypnosis and conscious states: The
Clayton, P. (2006). The Oxford Handbook of Hanh, T. N., & Nquyen, A. (2006). Walking
cognitive neuroscience perspective (pp
Science and Religion. Oxford, England: meditation. Louisville, CO: Sounds True
293314). Oxford, England: Oxford Uni-
Oxford University Press. Inc.
versity Press.
Council, J. R., Kirsch, I., & Hafner, L. P. Heap, M., Brown, R. J., & Oakley, D. A.
Dienes, Z., & Hutton, S. (submitted). Under-
(1986). Expectancy versus absorption in (Eds.) (2004) The Highly Hypnotizable
standing hypnosis metacognitively: rTMS
the prediction of hypnotic responding. Person. London, England: Routledge.
applied to DLPFC increases hypnotic
Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
suggestibility. Heide, F. J., Wadlington, W. L., & Lundy,
ogy, 50, 182189.
R. M. (1980). Hypnotic responsivity as
Dixon, M., Brunet, A., & Laurence J.-R.
Cooper, L., Banford, S., Schubot, E., & Tart, a predictor of outcome in meditation.
(1990). Hypnotizability and automaticity:
C. (1967). International Journal of Clini- International Journal of Clinical and Ex-
toward a parallel distributed processing
cal and Experimental Hypnosis, 15(3), perimental Hypnosis, 28, 358385.
model of hypnotic responding. Journal of
118124.
Abnormal Psychology, 99, 336343. Hilgard, E. R. (1977). Divided conscious-
Crawford, H. (1994). Brain dynamics and ness: Multiple controls in human thought
hypnosis: Attentional and disattentional and action. New York, NY: Wiley-Inter-
processes. Journal of Clinical and Experi- science.
mental Hypnosis, 42(3), 204232.

54 || MBR || Volume: 2 || Issue: 1


a r t icle The Journal of MindBody Regulation

Hilgard, E. R., & Tart, C. T. (1966). Respon- Kabat-Zinn, J., Massion, A. O., Kristeller, J., Kubose, S. (1976). An experimental inves-
siveness to suggestion following waking Peterson, L. G., Fletcher, K. E., Pbert, L., tigation into the psychological aspects
and imagination instructions and fol- ... Santorelli, S. F. (1992). Effectiveness of meditation. Cited in D. Shapiro, & R.
lowing induction of hypnosis. Journal of of a meditation-based stress reduction Walsh (Eds.). The science of meditation:
Abnormal Psychology, 71, 196208. program in the treatment of anxiety Research, theory and practice. New York,
disorders, American Journal of Psychiatry, NY: Van Nostrand Reinhold.
Holroyd, J. C. (2003). The science of medita-
149(7), 936943.
tion and the state of hypnosis. Ameri- Lau, H. C., & Passingham, R. E. (2006). Rel-
can Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 46(2), Kabat-Zinn, J., Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. W. ative blindsight in normal observers and
109128. G., & Teasdale, J. D. (2002). Mindfulness- the neural correlate of visual conscious-
based cognitive therapy for depression. ness. Proceedings of the National Academy
Hlzel, B. K., Ott, U., Hempel, H., Hackl, A.,
New York, NY: The Guildford Press. of Science, 103, 1876318768.
Wolf, K., Stark, R., & Vaitl, D. (2007). Dif-
ferential engagement of anterior cingulate Kaiser, J., Barker, R., Haenschel, C., Bal- Lazar, S. W., Kerr, C., Wasserman, R. H.,
and adjacent medial frontal cortex in ad- deweg, T., & Gruzelier, J. H. (1997). Hyp- Gray, J. R., Greve, D., Treadway, M. T.,
ept meditators and non-meditators.Neu- nosis and event-related potential corre- ... Fischl, B. (2005). Meditation experi-
roscience Letters, 421, 1621. lates of error-processing in a Stroop type ence is associated with increased cortical
paradigm: A test of the frontal hypothesis. thickness. NeuroReport, 16, 18931897.
Hurlburt, R. T., & Schwitzgebel, E. (2007).
International Journal of Psychophysiology,
Describing inner experience: Proponent Lichtenberg, P., Bachner-Melman, R.,
27, 215222.
meets skeptic. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Gritsenko, I., & Ebstein, R. P. (2000).
Kallio, S., Revonsuo, A., Hmlinen, H., Exploratory association study between
Iani, C., Ricci, F., Gherri, E. & Rubichi, S.
Markela, J., & Gruzelier, J. (2001). Ante- catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT)
(2006). Hypnotic suggestion modulates
rior brain functions and hypnosis: A test high/low enzyme activity polymorphism
cognitive conflict. Psychological Science,
of the frontal hypothesis. International and hypnotizability. American Journal of
17, 721727.
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hyp- Medical Genetics, 96, 771774.
Jamieson, G. A. & Woody, E. Z. (2007). nosis, 49, 95108.
Lutz, A., Brefczynski-Lewis, J., Johnstone, T.,
Dissociated control as a paradigm for
Kamalashila. (1992). Meditation: The Bud- Davidson, R. J. (2008) Regulation of the
cognitive neuroscience research and
dhist way of tranquility and insight. Cam- Neural Circuitry of Emotion by Compas-
theorizing in hypnosis. In G. Jamieson
bridge, England: Windhorse Publications. sion Meditation: Effects of Meditative
(Ed.). Hypnosis and conscious states:
Expertise. PLoS ONE, 3(3), e1897.
The cognitive neuroscience perspective Karlin, R. J. (1979). Hypnotizability and

mbr. s y ner g ie s pr a i r ie s .c a
(pp 111129). Oxford, England: Oxford attention. Journal of Abnonnal Psychology, Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Dunne, J., & David-
University Press. 88(1), 9295. son, R. J. (2008). Attention regulation
and monitoring in meditation. Trends in
Jamieson. G. A., & Sheehan, P. W. (2002). Kihlstrom, J. (2003). The fox, the hedgehog,
Cognitive Sciences, 12(4), 163169.
A critical evaluation of the relationship and hypnosis. International Journal of
between sustained attentional abilities Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 51, Lutz, A., Slagter, H., Rawlings, N. B., Fran-
and hypnotic susceptibility. Contemporary 166189. cis, A. D., Greischar, L. L., & Davidson,
Hypnosis, 119, 6274. R. J. (2009). Mental training enhances
Kirsch, I. (1985). Response expectancy as a
attentional stability: Neural and behav-
Jensen, C. G., Vangkilde, S., Frokjaer, V., determinant of experience and behavior.
ioral evidence. Journal of Neuroscience,
& Hasselbalch, S. G. (2012). Mindful- American Psychologist, 40, 11891202.
29, 1341813427.
ness training affects attentionor is it
Kirsch, I. (1997). Response expectancy
attentional effort? Journal of Experimental Lutz, A., Dunne, J., & Davidson, R. (2007).
theory and application: A decennial
Psychology: General, 141(1), 106123. Meditation and the neuroscience of con-
review. Applied and Preventive Psychology,
sciousness.: An introduction In P. Zelazo,
Jha, A., Krompinger, J., & Baime, M. J. 6, 6979.
M. Moscovitch, E. Thompson (Eds.).
(2007) Mindfulness training modifies
Kirsch, I., & Braffman, W. (1999). Correlates Cambridge Handbook of Consciousness
subsystems of attention. Cognitive, Affec-
of hypnotizability: The first empiri- (pp.499554). Cambridge, England:
tive, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 7(2),
cal study. Contemporary Hypnosis, 16, Cambridge University Press.
109119.
224230.
Lynn, S. J., Das, L. S., Hallquist, M. N., &
Kabat-Zinn, J. (2011). Some clinical applica-
Kirsch, I., Cardea, E., Derbyshire, S., Di- Williams, J. C. (2006). Mindfulness, ac-
tions of mindfulness meditation in medi-
enes, Z., Heap, M., Kallio, S., ... Whalley, ceptance, and hypnosis: Cognitive and
cine and psychiatry: The case for mindful-
M. (2011). Definitions of hypnosis and clinical perspectives. International Journal
ness-based stress reduction (MBSR). In
hypnotizability and their relation to sug- of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis,
J. Kabat-Zinn, R. Davidson & Z. Housh-
gestion and suggesitibility: A consensus 54(2), 143166.
mand (Eds.) The minds own physician: A
statement. Contemporary Hypnosis and
scientific dialogue with the Dalai Lama on Lynn, S. J., Kirsch, I., & Hallquist, M. (2008).
Integrative Therapy, 28(2), 107111.
the healing power of meditation. Oakland, Social cognitive theories of hypnosis.
CA: New Harbinger Publications. Koriat, A. (2007) Metacognition and In M. R. Nash, & A. Barnier (Eds.). The
consciousness. In P. D. Zelazo, M. Oxford handbook of hypnosis: Theory,
Kabat-Zinn, J., Davidson, R. & Houshmand,
Moscovitch, & E. Thompson (Eds.), The research and practice (pp. 111140). Ox-
Z. (Eds.) (1985). The minds own physi-
Cambridge handbook of consciousness (pp. ford, England: Oxford University Press.
cian: A scientific dialogue with the Dalai
289325). Cambridge, England: Cam-
Lama on the healing power of meditation.
bridge University Press.
Oakland, CA: New Harbinger Publica-
tions.

55 || MBR || Volume: 2 || Issue: 1


a r t icle The Journal of MindBody Regulation

Lyons, L. & Crawford, H. (1997). Sustained Ramel, W., Goldin, P., Carmona, P., & Mc- Salomons, T. V., & Kucyi, A. (2011). Does
attentional and disattentional abilities and Quaid, J. (2004). The effects of mindful- meditation reduce pain through a unique
arousability: Factor analysis and relation- ness meditation on cognitive processes neural mechanism? The Journal of Neuro-
ships to hypnotic susceptibility. Person- and affect in patients with past depres- science, 31(26), 1270512707.
ality and Individual Differences, 23(6), sion. Cognitive therapy and research,
Sangharakshita. (2001). A Survey of Bud-
10711084. 26(4), 433455.
dhism. Cambridge, England: Windhorse
Lynn, S. J. & Rhue, J. W. (Eds.) (1991). Disso- Raz, A. (2011). Does neuroimaging of Publications.
ciation: Clinical and Theoretical Perspec- suggestion elucidate hypnotic trance?
Sarbin, T. R., & Coe, W. C. (1972). Hypnosis:
tives. New York, NY: Guilford Press. International Journal of Clinical and Ex-
A social psychological analysis of influ-
perimental Hypnosis, 59(3), 363377.
McGeown, W. Mazzoni, G., Venneri, A. & ence communication. New York, NY: Holt
Kirsch, I. (2009). Hypnotic induction Raz, A., & Buhle, J. (2006). Typologies of Rinehart and Winston.
decreases anterior default mode activity. Attentional Networks. Nature Reviews
Schjoedt, U., Stdkilde-Jrgensen, J., Geertz,
Consciousness and Cognition, 18, 848855. Neuroscience, 7(5), 367379.
A., Lund, T., & Ropestorff, A. (2011). The
Nadon, R., Hoyt, I., Register, P., & Kihlstrom, Raz, A., & Campbell, N. K. J. (2011). Can power of charismaperceived charisma
J. (1991). Absorption and hypnotizability: Suggestion Obviate Reading? Supple- inhibits the frontal executive network of
Context effects reexamined. Journal of menting Primary Stroop Evidence with believers in intercessory prayer. Social
Personality and Social Psychology, 60(1), Exploratory Negative Priming Analyses. Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 6(1),
144153. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(2), 119127.
312320.
Nash, M. & Barnier, A. (Eds.), (2008).The Semmens-Wheeler, R., & Erskine, J. (2009,
Oxford Handbook of Hypnosis: Theory, Raz, A., Fan, J., & Posner, M. I. (2005). Hyp- April). Mindfulness and the myth of con-
Research, and Practice. Oxford, England: notic suggestion reduces conflict in the trol. Talk presented at British Psychologi-
Oxford University Press human brain. Proceedings of the National cal Society Annual Conference, Brighton.
Academy of Sciences of the United States of
Nelson, T. O., & Narens, I. (1990). Semmens-Wheeler, R., & Dienes, Z. (2011,
America, 102(28), 99789983.
Metamemory: A theoretical framework December) Similarities and Differences in
and new findings. In G. Bower, (Ed.), The Raz, A., Kirsch, I., Pollard, J., & Nitkin- Meditation and Hypnosis. Talk presented
psychology of learning and motivation: Ad- Kaner, Y. (2006). Suggestion reduces the at the David Waxman Memorial Lecture,
vances in research and theory (pp. 12 5-17 stroop effect. Psychological Science, 17(2), Royal Society of Medicine, London.
3). New York, NY: Academic Press. 9195.
Shapiro, D., & Walsh, R. (Eds.) (1980). The

mbr. s y ner g ie s pr a i r ie s .c a
Norman, D., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention Raz, A., Shapiro, T., Fan, J., & Posner, M. science of meditation: Research, theory and
to action: Willed and automatic control I. (2002). Hypnotic suggestion and the practice. New York, NY: Van Nostrand
of behavior. In R. Davidson, G. Schwartz, modulation of Stroop interference. Reinhold.
& D. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and Archives of General Psychiatry, 59(12),
Shaw, S. (2006). Buddhist meditation: An
self regulation: Advances in research and 11551161.
anthology of texts from the Pali cannon.
theory, Vol. 4 (pp. 118). New York, NY:
Raz, A., Landzberg, K. S., Schweizer, H. R., New York, NY: Routledge.
Plenum.
Zephrani, Z. R., Shapiro, T., Fan, J., & Pos-
Shor, R. E., Orne, M. T., & OConnell, D. N.
Pekala, R. J. (1982). Phenomenology of Con- ner, M. I. (2003). Posthypnotic suggestion
(1962). Validation and cross-validation of
sciousness Inventory (PCI). (Unpublished and the modulation of Stroop interference
a scale of self-reported personal experi-
psychological test). under cycloplegia. Consciousness and
ences which predicts hypnotizability.
Cognition, 12(3), 332346.
Pekala, R. J. (1991). Quantifying conscious- Journal of Psychology, 53, 5575.
ness: An empirical approach. Emotions, Rosenthal, D. M. (1986). Two concepts of
Slagter, H. A., Lutz, A., Greichar, L. L.,
personality, and psychotherapy. New York, consciousness. Philosophical Studies, 49,
Francis, A. D., Nieuwenhuis, S., Davis, J.,
NY: Plenum Press. 329359.
& Davidson, R. J. (2007). Mental training
Pekala, R. J., & Kumar, V. K. (2007). An Rosenthal, D. M. (2002). Consciousness affects distribution of limited attentional
empirical-phenomenological approach and higher-order thought. In L. Nadel. resources. PLoS Biology, 5(6), 138.
to quanitifying consciousness: With Macmillan encyclopedia of cognitive sci-
Spanos, N. P., & Barber, T. X. (1974). Toward
particular reference to understanding the ence (pp. 717726). Basingstoke, England:
a convergence in hypnosis research.
nature of hypnosis in G. Jamieson (Ed.). Macmillan Publishers Ltd.
American Psychologist, 29(7), 500511.
Hypnosis and conscious states: The cogni-
Rosenthal, D. (2005). Consciousness and
tive neuroscience perspective (pp 167194). Spanos, N. (1986). Hypnotic behavior: A
mind. Oxford, England: Oxford University
Oxford, England: Oxford University socialpsychological interpretation of
Press.
Press. amnesia, analgesia, and trance logic. Be-
Rounis, E., Maniscalco, B., Rothwell, J., Pass- havioral and Brain Sciences, 9, 449502.
Petitmengin, C. (Ed.). (2009). Ten years
ingham, R. E., & Lau, H. (2010). Theta-
of viewing from within: The legacy of Spanos, N., & Gorassini, D. (1999). The Car-
burst transcranial magnetic stimulation to
Francisco Varela. Exeter, England: Imprint leton Skill Training Program for modifying
the prefrontal cortex impairs metacogni-
Academic. hypnotic suggestibility: Original version
tive visual awareness. Cognitive Neurosci-
and variations. In I. Kirsch, A. Capafons,
Proust, J. (2012). Philosophy of Metacogni- ence, 1, 165175.
E. Cardea-Buelna & Amig, S. (Eds.).
tion: Mental agency and self-awareness.
Rubichi, S., Ricci, F., Padovani, R., & Sca- Clinical hypnosis and self-regulation:
Oxford, England: Oxford University
glietti, L. (2005). Hypnotic susceptibility, Cognitive-behavioral perspectives. Dis-
Press.
baseline attentional functioning, and the sociation, trauma, memory, and hypnosis
Stroop task. Consciousness and Cognition, book series (pp. 141177). Washington,
14, 296303. DC: American Psychological Association.

56 || MBR || Volume: 2 || Issue: 1


a r t icle The Journal of MindBody Regulation

Spanos, N. P., Rivers, S. M., & Gottlieb, J. Van Nuys, D. (1973). Meditation, attention Woody, E. Z., & Sadler, P. (2008). Disso-
(1978). Hypnotic responsivity, meditation, and hypnotic susceptibility: A correlation- ciation theories of hypnosis. In M. R.
and laterality of eye movements. Journal al study. International Journal of Clinical Nash & A. J. Barnier (Eds.). The Oxford
of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 566569. and Experimental Hypnosis, 21, 5969. Handbook of Hypnosis: Theory, Research
and Practice. Oxford, England: Oxford
Tang, Y.-Y., Ma, Y., Wang, J., Fan, Y., Feng, S., Varga, K., Nmeth, Z., & Szekely, A. (2011).
University Press.
Yu, Q., ... Posner, M. (2007). Short-term Lack of correlation between hypnotic
meditation training improves attention susceptibility and various components of Woody, E. Z., & Bowers, K. (1994). A frontal
and self-regulation. Proceedings of the attention. Consciousness and Cognition, assault on dissociated control. In S. J.
National Academy of Sciences of the United 20, 18721881. Lynn & J. W. Rhue (Eds.). Dissociation:
States of America, 104(43), 1715217156. Clinical and Theoretical Perspectives (pp.
Vincent, J. L., Kahn, I., Snyder, A. Z., Raichle,
5279). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Teasdale, J. (1999). Metacognition, mind- M. E., Buckner, R. L. (2008). Evidence for
fulness and the modification of mood a Frontoparietal Control System Revealed Yapko, M. D. (2011). Mindfulness and hyp-
disorders, Clinical Psychology and Psycho- by Intrinsic Functional Connectivity. Jour- nosis: The power of suggestion to transform
therapy, 6, 146155. nal of Neurophysiology, 100, 33283342. experience. New York, NY: Norton.
Teasdale, J. D., Moore, R. G., Hayhurst, Wagstaff, G. F., Cole, J. C., & Brunas-Wag- Zamansky, H. S., & Clark, L. E. (1986).
H., Pope, M., Williams, S., & Segal, Z. staff, J. (2007). Effects of hypnotic induc- Cognitive competition and hypnotic
V. (2002). Metacognitive awareness and tion and hypnotic depth on phonemic behaviour: Whither absorption? Interna-
prevention of relapse in depression: Em- fluency: A test of the frontal inhibition tional Journal of Clinical and Experimen-
pirical evidence. Journal of Consulting and account of hypnosis. Revista Internacio- tal Hypnosis, 34, 205214.
Clinical Psychology, 70, 278287. nal de Psicologia y Terapia Psicologica/
International Journal of Psychology and
Thompson, E. (2006). Neurophenomenology
Psychological Therapy, 7, 2740.
and contemplative experience. In P. Clay-
ton (Ed.) (2006). The Oxford Handbook Wallace, B. A. (1999). The Buddhist tradi-
of Science and Religion (pp. 226235). tion of Samatha: Methods for refining
Oxford, England: Oxford University Press and examining consciousness. Journal of
Consciousness Studies, 6, 175187.
Travis, F., Haaga, D. A., Hagelin, J., Tan-
ner, M., Nidich, S., Gaylord-King, C., Walsh, R. (1979). Meditation research:
... Schneider, R. H. (2009). Effects of An introduction and review. Journal of

mbr. s y ner g ie s pr a i r ie s .c a
Transcendental Meditation practice on Transpersonal Psychology, 11, 161174.
brain functioning and stress reactivity in
Wendt,P. E.,& Risberg,J. (2001). Ethanol re-
college students. International Journal of
duces rCBF activation of left dorsolateral
Psychophysiology, 71(2), 170176
prefrontal cortex during a verbal fluency
Uddin, L., Kelly, A., Biswal, B., Xavier Castel- task. Brain and Language, 77(2), 197215.
lanos, F., & Milham, M. (2009). Function-
White, R. W. (1942). A preface to a theory
al connectivity of default mode network
of hypnotism. Journal of Abnormal and
components: Correlation, anticorrelation,
Social Psychology, 36, 477505.
and causality. Human Brain Mapping, 30,
625637 Wilson, S. C., & Barber, T. X. (1982). The
fantasy-prone personality: Implications
Valentine, E. R., Sweet, P. L. G. (1999). Medi-
for understanding imagery, hypnosis,
tation and attention: A comparison of the
and parapsychological phenomena.. In
effects of concentrative and mindfulness
S. J. Lynn & J. W. Rhue (Eds.). Theories of
meditation on sustained attention. Mental
Hypnosis: Current Models and Perspective,
Health, Religion and Culture, 2, 5970.
London, England: The Guildford Press.

57 || MBR || Volume: 2 || Issue: 1

Вам также может понравиться