Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 344

The Ecology and Behavior of Chickadees and Titmice

This page intentionally left blank


The Ecology and
Behavior of
Chickadees and
Titmice

An integrated approach
EDITED BY

Ken A. Otter

1
3
Great Clarendon Street, Oxford OX2 6DP
Oxford University Press is a department of the University of Oxford.
It furthers the Universitys objective of excellence in research, scholarship,
and education by publishing worldwide in
Oxford New York
Auckland Cape Town Dar es Salaam Hong Kong Karachi
Kuala Lumpur Madrid Melbourne Mexico City Nairobi
New Delhi Shanghai Taipei Toronto
With offices in
Argentina Austria Brazil Chile Czech Republic France Greece
Guatemala Hungary Italy Japan Poland Portugal Singapore
South Korea Switzerland Thailand Turkey Ukraine Vietnam
Oxford is a registered trade mark of Oxford University Press
in the UK and in certain other countries
Published in the United States
by Oxford University Press Inc., New York
Oxford University Press, 2007
The moral rights of the author have been asserted
Database right Oxford University Press (maker)
First published 2007
All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced,
stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means,
without the prior permission in writing of Oxford University Press,
or as expressly permitted by law, or under terms agreed with the appropriate
reprographics rights organization. Enquiries concerning reproduction
outside the scope of the above should be sent to the Rights Department,
Oxford University Press, at the address above
You must not circulate this book in any other binding or cover
and you must impose the same condition on any acquirer
British Library Cataloguing in Publication Data
Data available
Library of Congress Cataloging in Publication Data
Data available
Typeset by Newgen Imaging Systems (P) Ltd., Chennai, India
Printed in Great Britain
on acid-free paper by
Antony Rowe Ltd., Chippenham, Wiltshire

ISBN 9780198569992

10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1
Preface

other species. If one stands still while working on


Who cares about chickadees?
chickadees, it isnt uncommon for birds to
This is a fairly common saying of one of my approach closely enough so as to allow reading the
colleagues, mainly as a means of antagonizing my color-combination of their leg bands with the naked
students, who have often been talking about their eye. Such disregard allows researchers to easily
research during previous moments (to be fair, this monitor the behavior of their subjects. The result is
colleague often says the same thing about his greater insight into the subtle aspects of communi-
own study species). It is, however, a very good cation, survival strategies, and social organization
questionwho does care about chickadees? Having that have resulted in a proliferation of research in
worked on species in the Paridae family for nearly animal behavior, physiology, and experimental
15 years, I have always been amazed at the number psychology. Further, many species of Eurasian
of peopleresearchers and non-researchers alike Parids take readily to nestboxes, and the ease
who do. associated with working on such species has
I feel there are two primary reasons for the gen- resulted in some of the longest-term ecological
eral affinity toward this family. First and foremost, studies on any bird species (see Chapter 18).
these birds will closely approach humans. The Finally, Parids appear to take readily to aviaries,
birds in the Paridae family (Parids) are primarily allowing for studies and observations that would
non-migratory, and commonly use bird feeders and be difficult to conduct in field settings.
nest in suburban environments, which often bring My own start on studying Parids was
them into contact with people. Few people in North accidentalI had originally started a Masters on
America, regardless of their knowledge of birds, northern saw-whet owls (Aegolius acadicus), but a
cant count a chickadee or titmouse among the fortuitous crash in the owl population forced me to
wildlife that they recognize. This familiarity brings switch to my supervisors study species, the black-
with it a natural curiosity on the part of the average capped chickadee. Once I began research on these
person about these birds. Second, they are cute. A birds, I discovered the advantages of working with
testament to this fact is the number of paintings of a species that allowed the detailed behavioral
chickadees one sees on various ornamentsan research I was interested in, coupled with extensive
artist who lived less than a km from where I grew knowledge on the species upon which to build (my
up made a career of painting stylized watercolors work on chickadees began the year that Susan
of fledgling chickadees, which I have seen adorning Smith published The Black-Capped Chickadee [Smith
coffee mugs all across North America. This factor 1991]). Chickadees (and titmice) appeared to lend
shouldnt be underestimated in why people appear themselves to a myriad of research avenues, which
to have a stronger affinity to chickadees than to I soon found were already being pursued by many
other common suburban birds. labs around the continent.
But why are these birds so popular with It was the fact that so many labs working in so
researchers? The answer is most likely associated many disparate fields in evolution, psychology,
with the first reason indicated above. Parids, neuroethology, ecology, and animal behavior used
especially the best studied species, tend to tolerate chickadees and titmice as study subjects that
very close approach by researchers compared to became the impetus for this book. Parids are

v
vi P R E FA C E

probably one of the most studied groups of birds animal communication, and Section III is devoted
globally, yet some features of the North American to this aspect of their biology. Finally, recent work
Parids differed markedly from the more exten- on Parids in North America has begun to focus on
sively studied Eurasian birds. The goal was to landscape ecology issues relating to dispersal,
assemble people studying various aspects of adaptation and breeding, which comprise the three
ecology and behavior who used North American chapters in Section IV of the book. Susan Smith
Parids as the primary focal species in their introduces the North American Parids and the con-
research. The chapters of this book address particu- tributions to this volume (Chapter 1), highlighting
lar topics, rather than address the life histories of the advances in our knowledge of this group since
individual species (for excellent life history the publication of her original book on the black-
accounts, the American Ornithologist Union capped chickadee (Smith 1991). Andr Dhondt
Species Accounts are an invaluable supplement (Chapter 18) concludes the book by comparing the
see Table 1). Our goal was to compare research on focus of research efforts between Eurasian and
North American Parids with their Eurasian North American Parid researchers, and surmises
counterparts, and to show how both groups have that the different focuses might reflect differences
contributed to greater understanding of many in the life history patterns of favorite study species
disparate fields in biology. on the two continents.
In August 2005, contributors gathered for the
first North American Parid Workshop in Snowbird,
Contributions, workshop and synopses
Utah (Fig. 1). During 2 days, we spent half of our
With the number of studies conducted on North time individually presenting chapters, and the
American chickadees and titmice, it was a difficult other half engaged in discussions about the syner-
task determining who should be asked to con- gies that could be gained by integrating advances
tribute to this work. I assembled prospective con- made by different groups working on parallel (or
tributors by both scanning the literature for groups sometimes even diverse) topics. The result of this
that had repeatedly published work on North workshop was the synopses chapters for each of the
American chickadees and titmice in the preceding four sections of the book. These synopses identify
5 years, and by contacting those groups that I knew how the individual chapter contributions comple-
to be working on various aspects of ecology or ment to further our understanding of complex eco-
behavior in the family. No such effort can be per- logical or behavioral processes, but as importantly,
fectly inclusive, and mine has definitely missed a also identify where unanswered questions remain
couple of people that have added significantly to that will provide the impetus for research avenues
the literature on this group over the past year or so. over the next decade or more.
Nevertheless, this project has succeeded in assem-
bling a very strong cohort of researchers from
Global distributions of the Paridae and
Canada and the United States.
major taxonomic distinctions
Contributors submitted first drafts of chapters in
July 2005. These chapters grouped into the four sec- Harrap and Quinn (1995) published a thorough
tions of the book. Section I focuses on extrinsic and account of the Paridae in their Chickadees, Tits,
intrinsic mechanisms controlling various behav- Nuthatches and Treecreepers, in which true tits within
iors, such as scatter-hoarding and recovery of seeds the Paridae accounted for 55 species world-wide
in wintering Parids and the control of circannual subdivided into 10 subgenera (Table 1). These
and finer-scale aspects of behavior. Section II species occur in largely discrete distributions on
groups chapters relating to interspecific and three continental land masses: North America,
intraspecific influences on evolution, breeding Eurasia, and sub-Saharan Africa (Fig. 2). North
and/or social behavior in the North American American species include members of the Poecile
Parids. The complexity of vocalizations in this black- and brown-capped chickadees and tits
group has made chickadees a well-studied group in and the Baeolophus titmice. Inclusive areas occupied
P R E FA C E vii

Figure 1 Participants of the first North American Parid Workshop, standing under an appropriately named ski chair-lift at Snowbird, Utah.

by the various Parid species covers almost the the titmice in the Baeolophus groups. Chickadees
entire continent from central Mexico to the occupy a broader expanse of the continent, the var-
Canadian/ Alaskan tundra (see maps in Harrap ious species occupying a range from the northern
and Quinn 1995, or in the species accounts listed boreal forests of Canada and Alaska to central
in Table 1). Mexico, whereas the titmice have a more southerly
Eurasia contains groups that span both Europe distribution from the northeastern US and Canada
and Asia (e.g. the Parus, Cyanistes, Periparus, Poecile, to central Mexico.
and Lophophanes groups) and some groups
restricted to Asia (Sittiparus, Parduliparus, and
Nomenclatural debates and the
Machlophus). The combined range of these groups
potential for resolution
covers almost the entire Eurasian continent with
the exception of desert regions in the Middle East One fly in the Parid ointment has been a disagree-
and Mongolia. Several Eurasian species occupy the ment over systematic nomenclature between North
Northern coastal areas of Africa bordering the America and Eurasian regulating bodies, which
Mediterranean, but a large group of endemic thankfully appears to be near resolution. In 1998, the
African species (all in the Melaniparus, a name likely American Ornithologists Union Check-list of North
influenced by the African black-tits) jointly occupy American Birds (7th Ed) elevated the subgeneric
most of the continent south of the Sahara. names to generic level in renaming its Parid species.
This book focuses predominantly on the North This decision was based on work done by Slikas et al.
American Parids, the chickadees in the Poecile and (1996) on DNA-DNA hybridization that indicated
viii
P R E FA C E
Table 1 Groupings, distribution, and scientific names of species within the true titsthe family Paridae.

Family Paridae
Subgenera/Genera Common name Genus species Distribution AOU account
Groupings (English)

Poecile Marsh tit Poecile palustris Eurasia


black-capped tits Sombre tit Poecile lugubris Eurasia
Willow tit Poecile montana* Eurasia
White-browed tit Poecile superciliosa Eurasia
Pre Davids tit Poecile davidi Eurasia
Black-capped chickadee Poecile atricapillus North America Smith (1993)
Carolina chickadee Poecile carolinensis North America Mostrom et al. (2002)
Mountain chickadee Poecile gambeli North America McCallum et al. (1999)
Mexican chickadee Poecile sclateri North America Ficken and Nocedal (1992)
brown-capped tits Siberian tit/grey-headed Poecile cincta Eurasia/ Hailman and Haftorn (1995)
chickadee North America
Boreal chickadee Poecile hudsonica North America Ficken et al. (1996)
Chestnut-backed chickadee Poecile rufescens North America Dahlsten et al. (2002)
Varied tit Poecile varia Eurasia
Periparus Coal tit Periparus ater Eurasia
coal tit and allies Spot-winged tit Periparus melanolophus Eurasia
Rufous-vented tit Periparus rubidiventris Eurasia
Rufous-naped tit Periparus rufonuchalis Eurasia
Pardaliparus Elegant tit Periparus elegans Eurasia
Asian tits Palawan tit Periparus amabilis Eurasia
Yellow-bellied tit Periparus venustulus Eurasia
Lophophanes Crested tit Lophophanes cristatus Eurasia
Eurasian crested tits Grey-crested tit Lophophanes dichrous Eurasia
Melaniparus Dusky tit Parus funereus Africa
African tits Carps black tit Parus carpi Africa
Southern black tit Parus niger Africa
White-winged black tit Parus leucomelas Africa
White-shouldered black tit Parus guineensis Africa
White-bellied black tit Parus albiventris Africa
White-backed black tit Parus leuconotus Africa
Rufous-bellied tit Parus rufiventris Africa
Cinnamon-breasted tit Parus pallidiventris Africa
Red-throated tit Parus fringillinus Africa
Stripe-breasted tit Parus fasciiventer Africa
Acacia tit Parus thruppi Africa
Miombo tit Parus griseiventris Africa
Ashy tit Parus cinerascens Africa
Southern grey tit Parus afer Africa
Parus Great tit Parus major Eurasia
great tit and allies Black-lored tit Parus xanthogenys Eurasia
Turkestan tit Parus bokharensis Eurasia
White-naped tit Parus nuchalis Eurasia
Green-backed tit Parus monticolus Eurasia
Yellow-cheeked tit Parus spilonotus Eurasia
Groundpecker Parus humilis Eurasia
Machlolophus Yellow tit Parus holsti Taiwan
Sittiparus White-fronted tit Parus semilarvatus Eurasia
Cyanistes Blue tit Cyanistes caeruleus Eurasia
blue tit and allies Azure tit Cyanistes cyanus Eurasia
Baeolophus Tufted titmouse Baeolophus bicolor North America Grubb and Pravasudov (1994)
North American titmice Black-crested titmouse Baeolophus atricristatus North America Grubb and Pravasudov (1994)
Oak titmouse Baeolophus inornatus North America Cicero 2000
Juniper titmouse Baeolophus ridgwayi North America Cicero 2000
Bridled titmouse Baeolophus wollweberi North America Nocedal and Ficken (1998)
Related species Sultan tit Melanochlora sultana Eurasia
Yellow-browed tit Sylviparus modestus Eurasia

Nomenclatural conventions are based on the proposed nomenclature from Gill et al. (2005), which has been adopted formally by the American Ornithologists Union (1998,
2000; Banks et al. 2002, 2003) and the British Onithologists Union (Sangster et al. 2005) and advocated by the International Ornithological Congress (Gill and Wright 2006).
Groupings in the first column are divisions (generic/subgeneric) based on Harrap and Quinn (1995)
*Although the masculine species name for the Willow titmontanuswas recommended for adoption by the BOU (Sangster et al. 2005), Poecile is a feminine noun and
the adjectival species names should also be feminized (e.g. montana rather than montanus) as appears in Gill and Wright (2006). The same convention should apply to
P. cincta, P. hudsonica, P. superciliosa. The exception to this rule is P. atricapillus (Banks et al. 2003): this is based on capillus being a noun cap in Latin, and the adjective

P R E FA C E
atri (black) being applied to the noun makes atricapillus a noun string. As such, it does not have to follow the same convention as species names that are adjectives, and the
ending should not be changed to follow gender of the generic name (F. Gill and D. Donsker, personal communication).

ix
x P R E FA C E

Figure 2 Inclusive ranges of the various members of the family Paridae, in global distribution. Members of this family occupy much of the area
of North America, Eurasia, and sub-Saharan Africa. For distributions of individual species, see Harrap and Quinn 1995.

the divisions between subgenera in this family were did not adopt the suggested nomenclatural changes.
as genetically divergent as the distinction between The most egregious division was in the one species
genera in other families. The result was North that occurs on both continentsthe Siberian tit (tra-
American chickadees being categorized by the ditionally Parus cinctus). The race of this species that
genus Poecile and North American titmice being cat- occurs in Alaska and the Yukon was officially
egorized by the genus Baeolophus. Further species renamed the grey-headed chickadee, Poecile cincta,
divisions were created in North America with the adopting the newer generic name, and a feminized
splitting of the plain titmouse (Parus inornatus) into version of the species name, despite these not being
two distinct species, the oak and the juniper tit- recognized as two distinct species.
mouse (Baeolophus inornatus and B. ridgwayi, respec- This resulted in a fair amount of confusion (see
tivelyCicero 2000), as well as the split of the also Chapter 2). The black-capped chickadee has
black-crested titmouse (Baeolophus atricristatus) from officially had its names changed from Parus atri-
the tufted titmouse (Baeolophus bicolor) into which it capillus to Poecile atricapillus (AOU 1998), then to
had formerly been placed (Banks et al. 2002). Poecile atricapilla (AOU 2000); Poecile was a feminine
This nomenclatural elevation of subgeneric noun and atricapillus was a masculine adjective. At
names, however, also assumed that the Eurasian the same time, other masculinized species names
species would correspondingly be categorized were also changede.g. Poecile hudsonica and Poecile
under the genera, Poecile (black-capped and brown- cincta. However, capillus is a Latin noun, and adding
capped clades), Cyanistes for the blue tit, Periparus the adjectival suffix atri makes this species name a
for the coal tit and its allies, Lophophanes for the noun string, which do not have to follow the same
Eurasian crested tits, and the retention of Parus for gender matching consideration as purely adjectival
the great tit, allies and African tits. A problem arose, species names (D. Donsker, IOC Taxonomic Editor,
however, as the various governing bodies in Eurasia personal communication). Thus, a later ruling
P R E FA C E xi

changed the black-capped chickadee back to Poecile and Wright 2006). We also urge the immediate
atricapillus (Banks et al. 2003), as an exception to the adoption of the feminized versions of P. superciliosa
rule. The failure of new generic name adoption for the white-browed tit. By the widespread adop-
worldwide, however, created additional problems, tion of these naming conventions in all remaining
as it was not uncommon when publishing in jurisdictions, we may be able to finally put an end
European journals for the names to be changed back to this period in the history of the Paridae.
to Parus from either Poecile or Baeolophus. For the remainder of the book, the common
The genetic relationship between the various names of the species in the Paridae will be used
groups is now well established (Gill et al. 2005 without restating the scientific name. Further, refer-
the most recent cladogram is reprinted in ence to the birds in the family Paridae, will be
Figure 2.1), and has been relatively consistent across referred to as Parids.
several genetic techniques (Gill et al. 1993; Slikas
et al. 1996). North American titmice consistently fall
Problem of under-represented groups
out as a separate taxonomic unit, while North
American chickadees are closely allied with the Upon reading this book, most readers will realize
Eurasian species of Poecile. Further, the remaining that we should have titled the book: The Ecology and
Eurasian Parids fall into four additional mono- Behavior of CHICKADEES and titmice. As with the
phyletic groupsParus, which includes birds in Eurasian species, work on North American Parids
Harrap and Quinns (1995) Parus, Melaniparus and has largely focused on a small number of species
Machlolophus groups; Cyanistes for the blue tits (Chapter 18), most notably the black-capped
and allies; Lophophanes for the Eurasian crested tits; chickadee. In addition to documenting what is
and Periparus for the coal tit and allies (Gill et al. known about chickadee and titmice, chapters focus
2005). The American Ornithologists Union (1998, on identifying knowledge gaps that arise from both
2000) and the British Ornithologists Union understudied aspects of North American Parids
(Sangster et al. 2005) have officially adopted these compared to Eurasian Parids, and whether studies
generic groupings as suggested by Gill et al. (2005). on the black-capped or Carolina chickadee transfer
Further, these listings have been supported by the to other chickadees or the titmice species. The
International Ornithological Congress (IOC) in Gill reader will find that such reflections are a recurrent
and Wrights (2006) Birds of the World:Recommended focus of chapters throughout this book, which we
English Names. We have adopted the new nomencla- make unapologetically as a means of identifying
ture in the listing of species in Table 1, and all the goals to drive Parid research in the future.
contributors of this book urge the international use On a global scale, the work presented in this
of a single naming classification for species world- book highlights where North American Parids dif-
wide, so as to maintain the integrity of the concept fer or concur with work conducted on Eurasian
of single scientific names. A check of the Inter- species. One of the largest knowledge gaps in our
national Commission on Zoological Nomenclatures understanding of Parid ecology and behavior is the
(ICZN) official website in Oct 2006, however, still obvious lack of research on the sub-Saharan African
lists all Eurasian species under Parus, yet strangely and endemic south-east Asian species. It is our
also lists all North American species at least twice, hope that by identifying these various knowledge
once under their former Parus designation and also gaps, we ourselves, or a new generation of
under their Poecile/Baeolophus designations. researchers, will take up the call and begin parallel
Certain discrepancies remain, primarily around research on less-represented species and groups.
the gender endings for Eurasian species in the
Poecile group. The BOU officially adopted the name
of the Willow tit as Poecile montanus, although this References
species should follow the feminized naming con- American Ornithologists Union (1998). Check-list of North
vention (P. montana) used for P. cincta and P. hud- American Birds, 7th edn. American Ornithologists
sonica to be technically accurate (as adopted by Gill Union, Washington, DC.
xii P R E FA C E

American Ornithologists Union. (2000). Forty-second Gill, F. and Wright, M. (2006) Birds of the World:
supplement to the American Ornithologists Union Recommended English Names. Princeton University Press,
Check-list of North American Birds. Auk, 117, 847858. New Jersey.
Banks, R. C., Cicero, C., Dunn, J. L., Kratter, A. W., Hailman, J. P. and Haftorn, S. (1995). Siberian Tit (Parus
Rasmussen, P. C., Remsen, J. V., Rising, J. D., and Stotz, cinctus). In: Pool, A. and Gill, F. eds. The Birds of North
D. F. (2002). Forty-third supplement to the American America, no. 196. The Academy of Natural Sciences,
Ornithologists Union Check-list of North American Washington, DC; The American Ornithologists Union,
Birds. Auk, 119, 897906. Philadelphia.
Banks, R. C., Cicero, C., Dunn, J. L., Kratter, A. W., Harrap, S. and Quinn, D. (1995). Chickadees, Tits,
Rasmussen, P. C., Remsen, J. V., Rising, J. D., and Stotz, Nuthatches and Treecreepers. Princeton University Press,
D. F. (2003). Forty-fourth supplement to the American Princeton, New Jersey, 464 pp.
Ornithologists Union Check-list of North American McCallum, D. A., Grundel, R., and Dahlsten, D. L. (1999).
Birds. Auk, 120, 923931. Mountain chickadee (Poecile gambeli). In: Pool, A. and
Cicero, C. (2000). Oak titmouse (Baeolophus inornatus) and Gill, F. eds. The Birds of North America, no. 453. The Birds
Juniper titmouse (Baeolophus ridgwayi). In: Pool, A. and of North America, Inc., Philadelphia.
Gill, F. eds. The Birds of North America, no. 485. The Birds Mostrom, A. M., Curry, R. L., and Lohr, B. (2002). Carolina
of North America, Inc., Philadelphia. chickadee (Poecile carolinensis). In: Pool, A. and Gill, F.,
Dahlsten, D. L., Brennan, L. A., McCallum, D. A., and eds. The Birds of North America, no. 636. The Birds of
Gaunt, S. L. L. (2002). Chestnut-backed chickadee North America, Inc., Philadelphia.
(Poecile rufescens). In: Pool, A. and Gill, F., eds. The Birds Nocedal, J. and Ficken, M. S. (1998). Bridled titmouse
of North America, no. 689. The Birds of North America, (Baeolophus wollweberi). In: Pool, A. and Gill, F., eds. The
Inc., Philadelphia. Birds of North America, no. 375. The Birds of North
Ficken, M. S., McLaren, M. A., and Hailman, J. P. (1996). America, Inc., Philadelphia.
Boreal chickadee (Parus hudsonicus). In: Pool, A. and Sangster, G. Collinson, J. M., Helbig, A. J., Knox, A. G., and
Gill, F., eds. The Birds of North America, no. 254. The Parkin, D. T. (2005) Taxonomic recommendations for
Academy of Natural Sciences, Washington, D.C.; British birds: third report. Ibis 147, 821826.
The American Ornithologists Union, Philadelphia. Slikas, B., Sheldon, F. H., and Gill, F. B. (1996). Phylogeny
Ficken, M. S. and Nocedal, J. (1992) Mexican chickadee of titmice (Paridae): I. Estimate of relationships among
(Parus sclateri). In: Poole, A., Stettenheim, P., and Gill, F., subgenera based on DNA-DNA hybridization. Journal
eds. The Birds of North America, no. 8. The Academy of of Avian Biology, 27, 7082.
Natural Sciences, Washington, DC; The American Smith, S. M. (1991). The Black-Capped Chickadee: Behavioral
Ornithologists Union, Philadelphia Ecology and Natural History. Cornell University Press,
Gill, F. B., Mostrom, A. M., and Mack, A. L. (1993). Ithaca, New York.
Speciation in North American chickadees: I. Patterns of Smith, S. M. (1993). Black-capped chickadee (Parus atri-
mtDNA genetic divergence. Evolution, 47, 195212. capillus). In: Poole, A., Stettenheim, P. and Gill, F., eds.
Gill, F. B., Slikas, B., and Sheldon, F. H. (2005). Phylogeny The Birds of North America, no. 39 The Academy of
of titmice (Paridae): II. Species relationships based on Natural Sciences, Washington, DC; The American
sequences of the mitochondrial cytochrome-B gene. Ornithologists Union, Philadelphia.
Auk, 122, 121143.
Grubb, T. C. Jr and Pravasudov, V. V. (1994). Tufted Ken A. Otter
titmouse (Parus bicolor). In: Pool, A., and Gill, F., eds. Ecosystem Science and Management Program
The Birds of North America, no. 86. The Academy of University of Northern British Columbia
Natural Sciences, Washington, DC; The American Canada
Ornithologists Union, Philadelphia.
Acknowledgements

This book would not have been possible without provision of books on reptiles and putting up with
the enthusiastic contribution of the various authors. me bringing home garter snakes whenever possible
Throughout the process of assembling the work- (our lab. mascots are still a pair of Anole lizards). I
shop, acquiring, and cross-reviewing chapters, and came to appreciate birds in University through the
preparing collaborative synopses, everyone mentoring of Dick Cannings and Jamie Smith at
involved has been positive and tolerant of my con- UBC, and to both I am indebted for pointing and
stant prodding. For this, I thank everyone. encouraging me on what would become a career
Hosting of the first North American Parid path. During my early work on chickadees, I bene-
Workshop at Snowbird, Utah was funded by a fited from interactions with numerous well-estab-
publication grant provided by the University of lished Parid researchers whose enthusiasm,
Northern British Columbia. For his support on this, encouragement-and willingness to share these with
I would like to thank our Vice President of an unheard-of grad student-made a lasting impres-
Research, Max Blouw, for his unfailing efforts to sion; most notable of these were Laurene Ratcliffe,
promote and enhance the research productivity of Susan Smith, Jack Hailman, Andr Dhondt, Peter
people at our institution. Emily Jones, the small McGregor, and Torben Dabelsteen. I had the for-
meetings manager, and the staff at Snowbird Resort tune to work on Parids in Europe with the latter
were instrumental in organizing the details that two, and I am thrilled to list the first four as con-
made the workshop a success. tributors to this book.
The editorial staff at Oxford University Press, Ian The dedication of this book, however, goes to my
Sherman, Abbie Headon, Christine Rode and wife, Barbara, and my son, Devon. Barbara had to
Stefanie Gehrig, have all been very encouraging put up with my being away from home up to
during the effort to put this book together. They 6 months of the year during graduate studies and
replied quickly and decisively to all queries I put to postdoctoral work, and has followed me around the
them, and have been extremely helpful throughout globe during my studies on this family of birds. This
this process. is even more remarkable considering that Barbara is
This is the typical place where the author not a biologist, and exhibits very little enthusiasm
explains their life-long love of birds has been the for the grubbiness and bugs typically associated
impetus for taking on the task of editing a book. In with field work. Devon (3 years old at the writing of
actuality, I was much more fascinated by snakes this) has had to put up with Daddy being gone for
and lizards as a youth, and I thank my parents, large stretches during the winter and spring, but I
Alan and Millie Otter, for allowing me to indulge in am starting him earlyhe can already identify
this obsession with the purchase of pet lizards, black-capped chickadees by both song and call.

xiii
This page intentionally left blank
Contents

Preface v
Acknowledgements xiii
List of contributors xxiii

1 Introduction to the North American Paridae 1


Susan M. Smith

Section I Proximate mechanisms in behavior and evolution 7

2 Neurobiology of spatial behavior 9


David F. Sherry and Jennifer S. Hoshooley

2.1 Introduction 9
2.2 Food storing in the Paridae 9
2.2.1 Food storing in the wild 9
2.2.2 Memory for cache sites 11
2.2.3 Memory for caches versus other spatial locations 11
2.2.4 Spatial memory in food-storing versus non-storing birds 12
2.3 The hippocampus 14
2.3.1 Comparative studies of the hippocampus 16
2.3.2 Neural processing of spatial information 16
2.3.3 Neurogenesis in the hippocampus of the adult black-capped chickadee 18
2.4 Summary and conclusions 20

3 The relationship between environment, corticosterone, food caching, spatial memory, and
the hippocampus in chickadees 25
Vladimir V. Pravosudov

3.1 Introduction 25
3.2 Inter- and intraspecific differences in caching, spatial memory, and the hippocampus 25
3.2.1 Interspecific differences 25
3.2.2 Intraspecific differences 26
3.3 Unpredictable food, corticosterone, spatial memory, and the hippocampus 29
3.3.1 Seasonality in food caching and spatial memory 29
3.3.2 Seasonal variation in hippocampus volume, neuron number, and neurogenesis rates 29
3.3.3 Photoperiod 30
3.3.4 Unpredictable food supply 31
3.4 Corticosterone, food caching, spatial memory, and the hippocampus 32
3.4.1 Corticosterone in Alaskan and Colorado chickadees 34

xv
xvi CONTENTS

3.5 Dominance, corticosterone, spatial memory, and the hippocampus 35


3.6 Conclusions 38

4 Photoperiodism and the annual cycle of black-capped chickadees 43


Leslie S. Phillmore and Scott A. MacDougall-Shackleton

4.1 Introduction 43
4.1.1 Seasonality 43
4.1.2 Photoperiodism and seasonal reproduction 44
4.1.3 Seasonal neural plasticity 44
4.2 Annual cycle of black-capped chickadees 45
4.2.1 Overview 45
4.3 Photoperiodism and reproduction in black-capped chickadees 46
4.3.1 Comparison with other species 46
4.3.2 Photoperiodism in black-capped chickadees 47
4.4 Seasonal changes in song and the song-control system 48
4.5 Seasonal changes in food storing and the hippocampus 49
4.5.1 Food storing 49
4.5.2 Hippocampus 50
4.6 Future directions 51

5 Fine-scale variation in the timing of reproduction in titmice and chickadees 55


Scott M. Ramsay and Ken A. Otter

5.1 Introduction 55
5.2 The history of research on reproductive timing: Lack 1966 56
5.3 Causes of fine-scale variation in timing among individuals 57
5.3.1 Intrinsic factors 57
5.3.1.1 Female age/genetics 57
5.3.2 Extrinsic factors 57
5.3.2.1 Photoperiod 57
5.3.2.2 Temperature 58
5.3.2.3 Food/energy limitation 58
5.3.3 Population adaptations to local habitat 59
5.4 Effects of fine-scale variation in timing 60
5.4.1 Offspring production and recruitment 60
5.4.1.1 Clutch size 60
5.4.1.2 Synchronization with offspring food supply 60
5.4.1.3 Hatching/fledging success 61
5.4.1.4 Recruitment 61
5.4.2 Future effects on adult fitness components 62
5.4.2.1 Survivorship 62
5.4.2.2 Future breeding costs 62
5.5 Consequences of long-term climatic variation on reproductive timing 63
5.5.1 Cyclical climatic variation 63
5.5.2 Directional climatic variation 64
5.6 Reproductive timing in males 65
5.6.1 Questions/predictions 65
5.7 Conclusions 66
CONTENTS xvii

Synopsis I Proximate mechanisms in behavior and evolution 71


David F. Sherry, Vladimir V. Pravosudov, Scott A. MacDougall-Shackleton,
Jennifer S. Hoshooley, and Leslie S. Phillmore

Section II Reproductive ecology, evolution, and behavior 75

6 Phylogeography of chestnut-backed chickadees in western North America 77


Theresa M. Burg

6.1 Introduction 77
6.2 Chestnut-backed chickadees 78
6.2.1 Origin 78
6.2.2 Biogeography 78
6.3 Patterns of contemporary population structure 79
6.3.1 Chestnut-backed chickadee 79
6.3.1.1 Interior and coastal populations 81
6.3.1.2 Central, coastal Alaska 82
6.3.1.3 Queen Charlotte Islands 82
6.3.1.4 Private alleles 83
6.3.1.5 Recolonization patterns 85
6.3.2 Comparison to other Parids 86
6.3.2.1 North American chickadees 86
6.3.2.2 Eurasian Parids 86
6.4 Factors influencing population genetics of Parids 88
6.4.1 Postglacial colonization and glacial refugia 89
6.4.2 Disjunct populations 90
6.4.2.1 Mainland populations 90
6.4.2.2 Island populations 90
6.4.3 Limited dispersal within continuous habitat and peripheral populations 90
6.5 Conclusions and perspectives 90

7 Behavioral aspects of chickadee hybridization 95


Robert L. Curry, Lindsay M. Rossano, and Matthew W. Reudink

7.1 Introduction 95
7.2 Background and methods 96
7.2.1 Study sites 96
7.2.2 Field methods 96
7.2.3 Genetic methods 97
7.2.4 Hybrid index scores 97
7.2.5 Song and call recordings 98
7.2.6 Sound analysis 98
7.3 Results 98
7.3.1 Genetic composition of study populations 98
7.3.2 Song patterns beyond and within hybrid zone 99
7.3.3 Chick-a-dee calls beyond and within hybrid zone 101
7.3.4 Lack of congruence between songs and calls 102
7.3.5 Relationships between behavior and genetics 102
7.4 Discussion 103
7.4.1 Hybridization in south-eastern Pennsylvania 103
7.4.2 Song patterns in the Pennsylvania contact zone 103
xviii CONTENTS

7.4.3 Chick-a-dee calls across the hybrid zone 104


7.4.4 Potentially confounding variables in analysis of vocal patterns 104
7.4.5 Completing the behavioral picture of chickadee hybridization 105
7.5 Hybridization throughout the Paridae 105
7.5.1 Other examples in North America 105
7.5.2 Hybridization among Eurasian Parids 106
7.6 Future directions 107

8 Life in the small-bodied cavity-nester guild: Demography of sympatric mountain and


black-capped chickadees within nest web communities under changing habitat conditions 111
Kathy Martin and Andrea R. Norris

8.1 Introduction 111


8.1.1 Parids as members of the nest web 111
8.1.1.1 Mountain chickadees 112
8.1.1.2 Black-capped chickadees 112
8.1.1.3 Possible competitors of chickadees 112
8.1.2 Facilitators of small-bodied cavity-nesters 115
8.1.3 Predators of small-bodied cavity-nesters 115
8.1.4 Interspecific interactions among small-bodied cavity-nesters 115
8.2 Study area and habitat change 116
8.3 Data analysis 117
8.3.1 Population growth models 118
8.3.2 Excavation history, nest tree, and cavity characteristics 118
8.3.3 Site level changes in mountain chickadee populations using population
growth models 120
8.3.3.1 Population size 120
8.3.3.2 Nest density 121

9 Social dominance and fitness in black-capped chickadees 131


Laurene Ratcliffe, Daniel J. Mennill, and Kristin A. Schubert

9.1 Introduction 131


9.2 General methods 132
9.2.1 Winter field methods 132
9.2.2 Spring field methods 133
9.3 Correlates of social rank 133
9.3.1 Age 134
9.3.2 Sex 135
9.3.3 Seniority 135
9.3.4 Size and condition 135
9.3.5 Plumage 136
9.3.6 Vocalizations 137
9.4 Hierarchy formation and maintenance 137
9.4.1 Consistency in dominance relationships between sites 139
9.5 Variation in rank acquisition 139
9.6 Rank, survival, and lifetime reproductive success 140
9.6.1 Survival 140
9.6.2 Reproductive success 141
9.7 Future research 143
CONTENTS xix

Synopsis II Parid reproductive behavior 147


Daniel J. Mennill, Theresa M. Burg, Robert L. Curry, Kathy Martin, Andrea R. Norris,
Laurene Ratcliffe, Matthew W. Reudink, Lindsay M. Rossano, and Kristin A. Schubert

Section III Vocal communication 151

10 Chickadee vocal production and perception: An integrative approach to understanding


acoustic communication 153
Christopher B. Sturdy, Laurie L. Bloomfield, Isabelle Charrier, and Tiffany T.-Y. Lee

10.1 Introduction 153


10.2 Song production and perception 153
10.2.1 Song production 153
10.2.2 Song perception 155
10.2.2.1 Playback experiments of song perception 156
10.2.2.2 Operant conditioning experiments of song perception 157
10.3 Call production and perception 159
10.3.1 Call production 159
10.3.2 Call perception 161
10.3.2.1 Playback studies of call perception 161
10.3.2.2 Operant conditioning studies of call perception 162
10.4 Summary and future directions 164

11 The gargle call of black-capped chickadees: ontogeny, acoustic structure, population


patterns, function, and processes leading to sharing of call characteristics 167
Myron C. Baker and David E. Gammon

11.1 Introduction 167


11.2 The gargle 168
11.2.1 Brief description of the call 168
11.2.2 Ontogeny of the call 168
11.3 Features of the call and population patterns 171
11.3.1 Persistence and change in gargle calls over time 174
11.3.2 Year-to-year changes in repertoires of individuals 174
11.4 Contexts of use of the call 174
11.5 Interactive playback experiment I 175
11.6 Aviary experiments on gargle changes 175
11.7 Interactive playback experiment II 177
11.8 Combined analysis of interactive playback tests 177
11.9 Synthesis of theory and empirical results 178

12 How postdispersal social environment may influence acoustic variation in birdsong 183
David E. Gammon

12.1 Introduction 183


12.2 Description of model and hypothesis 184
12.3 Description of study system 185
12.4 Acoustic variation before and after the bottleneck 187
12.4.1 Sampling procedure 187
xx CONTENTS

12.4.2 Analysis of data 188


12.4.3 Effect of the bottleneck 189
12.5 Comparison of acoustic variation in Fort Collins 2004 and Islands North 190
12.6 Acoustic variation within juveniles 191
12.7 Ages of singing adults in 2004 191
12.8 Synthesis of empirical results 191
12.8.1 Potential explanations 192
12.8.2 How postdispersal social environment may affect song development 192
12.8.3 Future studies needed 193
12.9 On the origin and maintenance of acoustic variation in birdsong 194
12.9.1 Acoustic variation across space and time in Parids 194
12.9.2 Development of song in Parids 194
12.9.3 Does the possession of novel song material affect reproductive fitness? 194
12.9.4 Implications for the origin and maintenance of dialects and repertoires 195

13 Information and the chick-a-dee call: Communicating with a complex vocal system 199
Jeffrey R. Lucas and Todd M. Freeberg

13.1 Introduction 199


13.2 Information and the chick-a-dee call 200
13.2.1 Basic note types of chick-a-dee calls 200
13.2.2 Variability in note types 200
13.2.3 Signal redundancy 204
13.2.4 Identity 204
13.2.5 Syntactical information 205
13.2.5.1 Syntax 205
13.2.5.2 Context 207
13.2.5.3 Playback studies 209
13.3 Conclusions, concessions, and a call for comparative work 209

14 Status signaling and communication networks in chickadees: Complex communication


with a simple song 215
Daniel J. Mennill and Ken A. Otter

14.1 Introduction 215


14.2 The fee-bee song: Variation on a two-note theme 215
14.3 Context of singing in chickadees 217
14.3.1 Singing context 1: Diurnal countersinging interactions 217
14.3.1.1 Variation in song frequency during countersinging interactions 218
14.3.1.2 Variation in song timing during countersinging interactions 219
14.3.2 Singing context 2: The dawn chorus 220
14.4 Communication networks 222
14.4.1 Male eavesdropping in black-capped chickadees 222
14.4.2 Female eavesdropping in black-capped chickadees 224
14.4.3 Nest cavities and male singing behavior 227
14.5 Singing behavior in other North American Parids 229
14.6 Summary and future studies 230
CONTENTS xxi

Synopsis III Complexities in vocal communication 235


Todd M. Freeberg, Myron C. Baker, Laurie L. Bloomfield, Isabelle Charrier,
David E. Gammon, Jack P. Hailman, Tiffany T.-Y. Lee, Jeffrey R. Lucas, Daniel J. Mennill,
and Christopher B. Sturdy

Overview 235
Vocal complexity of chickadees and titmice 235
The song versus call distinction and social complexity 236
Methodological advances 237
What is to be done? 237

Section IV Landscape ecology, behavior, and conservation issues 241

15 Edge, patch, and landscape effects on Parid distribution and movements 243
Andr Desrochers and Marc Blisle

15.1 Introduction 243


15.2 Response to edges by Parids 243
15.2.1 Food abundance 244
15.2.2 Microclimate 244
15.2.3 Predator avoidance 245
15.2.4 Movement corridors 245
15.3 Responses to patches 247
15.4 Beyond the patch: response to landscapes 248
15.5 Parids as leaders of other forest birds 257
15.6 Conclusions 258

16 Winter adaptations in chickadees and titmice and the added effect of habitat
fragmentation 263
Jennifer R. Olson and Thomas C. Grubb, Jr

16.1 Introduction 263


16.2 Physiological adaptations to winter conditions 263
16.2.1 Nocturnal hypothermia and seasonal metabolic adjustments 263
16.2.2 Thermogenesis by shivering 265
16.3 Behavioral modifications and ecological adaptations to winter conditions 266
16.3.1 Over-wintering in heterospecific flocks 266
16.3.2 Foraging behavior and food caching 268
16.3.3 Cavity roosting 268
16.4 Responses to habitat fragmentation 269
16.4.1 Species richness and density 270
16.4.2 Woodlot edge effect 270
16.5 Effects of fragmentation on wintering permanent resident birds 270
16.5.1 Survivorship 270
16.5.2 Effects of wind and temperature on isolated woodlots 271
16.5.3 Woodlot connectivity and interpatch movement 273
xxii CONTENTS

17 Habitat quality and reproductive behavior in chickadees and tits: Potential for habitat
matrix use in forest generalists 277
Ken A. Otter, Harry van Oort, and Kevin T. Fort

17.1 Introduction 277


17.2 Fragmentation, the matrix, and halo effects 278
17.3 Habitat variation and reproductive success in the Paridae 280
17.3.1 Matrix of similar age or structure but dissimilar species composition 280
17.3.1.1 Blue tits in evergreen versus deciduous oak forests 280
17.3.1.2 Comparison of blue tits to greats tits and other study sites 282
17.3.2 Matrix: Habitat of similar species but dissimilar age 282
17.3.2.1 Chickadees breeding in mature versus young forestssettlement 283
17.3.2.2 Habitat quality and reproductive success in chickadees 284
17.3.2.3 Habitat quality and condition-dependent behaviors in chickadees 285
17.4 Parids and insight into the use of matrix in conservation planning 288

Synopsis IV Landscape ecology, behavior, and conservation issues 293


Andr Desrochers, Ken A. Otter, Marc Blisle, and Jennifer R. Olson

Introduction 293
Focus on the Parids 293
Global perspective 293
Dispersal 294
Habitat selection at the individual level 295
Parids as a model group 296

18 What drives differences between North American and Eurasian tit studies? 299
Andr A. Dhondt

18.1 Introduction 299


18.2 Natural history traits determine the ease of study and the questions asked 299
18.3 The value of long-term studies: Providing answers to questions not yet asked 302
18.3.1 Spring phenology 302
18.3.2 Dispersal and local adaptation 303
18.3.3 Effects of habitat fragmentation 303
18.4 Winter group territoriality: A derived trait that leads to winter limitation 305
18.5 Studies of hybrid zones 307
18.6 Conclusions 307

Index 311
Contributors

Myron C. Baker, Professor Emeritus, Department Cornell University Ithaca, NY 14850, USA.
of Biology, Colorado State University, Fort aad4@cornell.edu
Collins, Colorado 80523, USA. Kevin T. Fort, Species at Risk Biologist, Canadian
mcbaker@lamar.colostate.edu Wildlife Service, Pacific Wildlife Research
Marc Blisle, Assistant Professor, Dpartement de Centre, 5421 Robertson Rd. RR#1, Delta, BC V4K
Biologie, Universit de Sherbrooke, 2500 boul. de 3N2, Canada.
lUniversit Sherbrooke, Qubec J1K 2R1, Canada. Kevin.Fort@ec.gc.ca
marc.m.belisle@usherbrooke.ca Todd M. Freeberg, Assistant Professor,
Laurie L. Bloomfield, Doctoral Student, Department of Psychology, Austin Peay Building
Department of Psychology and Centre for 303A, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN
Neuroscience, P-217 Biological Sciences 37996, USA. tfreeber@utk.edu
Building, University of Alberta, Edmonton, David E. Gammon, Assistant Professor, Campus Box
Alberta T6G 2E9, Canada. 2625, Elon University Elon, NC 27244-2010, USA.
laurie.bloomfield@ualberta.ca dgammon@elon.edu
Theresa M. Burg, Assistant Professor, Thomas C. Grubb Jr, Professor of Evolution,
Department of Biological Sciences, Ecology, and Organismal Biology, Ohio State
University of Lethbridge, 4401 University Drive, University, 318 West 12th, Columbus, Ohio
Lethbridge, Alberta, T1K 3M4, Canada. 43210-1293, USA.
theresa.burg@uleth.ca grubb.1@osu.edu
Isabelle Charrier, Postdoctoral Fellow, Department Jack P. Hailman, Professor Emeritus, Zoology,
of Psychology and Centre for Neuroscience, P- University of Wisconsin, Madison and Research
217 Biological Sciences Building, University of Associate, Archbold Biological Station, Old SR 8
Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta T6G 2E9, Canada. (County 17), Venus, FL 33960, USA.
isabelle.charrier@ibaic.u-psud.fr JHailman@Wisc.edu
Robert L. Curry, Associate Professor, Department Jennifer S. Hoshooley, Doctoral Student,
of Biology, Villanova University, 800 Lancaster Department of Psychology and Program in
Ave., Villanova, Pennsylvania 19085, USA. Neuroscience, University of Western Ontario,
robert.curry@villanova.edu London, Ontario N6A 5C2, Canada.
Andr Desrochers, Director, Centre dtude de la jmckay4@uwo.ca
fort, Facult de foresterie and gomatique, Tiffany T-Y. Lee, Doctoral Student, Department of
Universit Laval, Qubec City, Qubec G1K 7P4, Psychology and Centre for Neuroscience, P-217
Canada. Biological Sciences Building, University of
andre.desrochers@sbf.ulaval.ca Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta T6G 2E9, Canada.
Andr A. Dhondt, Edwin H. Morgens Professor of ttylee@gmail.com
Ornithology and Director of Bird Population Jeffrey R. Lucas, Professor, Dept of Biological
Studies, Department of Ecology and Sciences, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN
Evolutionary Biology, Cornell Laboratory of 47907, USA.
Ornithology, 159 Sapsucker Woods Road, jlucas@bilbo.bio.purdue.edu

xxiii
xxiv C O N T R I B U TO R S

Scott A. MacDougall-Shackleton, Associate Waterloo, Ontario N2L 3C5, Canada.


Professor, Department of Psychology, University sramsay@wlu.ca
of Western Ontario, London, Ontario N6A 5C2, Laurene M. Ratcliffe, Professor and Associate
Canada. Dean of Arts and Sciences, Department of
smacdou2@uwo.ca Biology, Queens University, Kingston,
Kathy J. Martin, Professor, Centre for Applied Ontario K7L 3N6, Canada.
Conservation Research, Forest Sciences ratcliff@biology.queensu.ca
Department, University of British Columbia, Matthew W. Reudink, Doctoral Student,
Vancouver, BC V6T 1Z4, Canada. Department of Biology, Queens University,
kmartin@interchange.ubc.ca Kingston, Ontario K7L 3N6, Canada.
Daniel J. Mennill, Assistant Professor, Department reudinkm@biology.queensu.ca
of Biological Sciences, University of Windsor, Lindsay M. Rossano, Biologist, Mid-Atlantic
Windsor, Ontario N9B 3P4, Canada. Associates, Inc., Colmar, PA 18951, USA.
dmennill@uwindsor.ca lindsay.rossano@villanova.edu
Andrea R. Norris, Masters Student, Centre for Kristin A. Schubert, Doctoral Student, Behavioral
Applied Conservation Research, Forest Sciences Biology Research Group, University of
Department, University of British Columbia, Groningen, P.O. Box 14, 9750 NN Haren, The
Vancouver, BC V6T 1Z4, Canada. Netherlands.
arnorris@interchange.ubc.ca k.a.schubert@rug.nl
Jennifer R. Olson, Doctoral Student, Department David F. Sherry, Professor, Department of
of Evolution, Ecology, and Organismal Biology, Psychology and Program in Neuroscience,
Ohio State University, 356 Aronoff Laboratory University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario
318 West 12th, Columbus, Ohio 43210-1293, USA. N6A 5C2, Canada.
olson.133@osu.edu sherry@uwo.ca
Ken A. Otter, Associate Professor (Biology) Susan M. Smith, Norma Wait Harris and
Ecosystem Science and Management Program, Emma Gale Harris Foundation Professor of
University of Northern British Columbia, 3333 Biological Sciences, Department of
University Way, Prince George, British Columbia Biological Sciences, Mount Holyoke College,
V2N 4Z9, Canada. 50 College Street, South Hadley, MA 01075,
otterk@unbc.ca USA.
Leslie S. Phillmore, Assistant Professor ssmith@mtholyoke.edu
Department of Psychology, Dalhousie Christopher B. Sturdy, Assistant Professor and
University, Life Sciences Centre, 1355 Oxford Alberta Ingenuity Researcher, Department of
Street Halifax, Nova Scotia B3H 4J1, Canada Psychology, University of Alberta, P217
leslie.phillmore@dal.ca Biological Sciences Building, Edmonton, Alberta
Vladimir V. Pravosudov, Assistant Professor, T6G ZE9, Canada.
Department of Biology, University of Nevada, csturdy@ualberta.ca
Reno M/S 314 Reno, NV 89557, USA. Harry van Oort, Biologist, Kingbird Biological
vpravosu@unr.edu Consulting P.O. Box 8617, Revelstoke, B.C. VOE
Scott M. Ramsay, Assistant Professor, 2S2, Canada.
Dept of Biology, Wilfrid Laurier University, hvanoort@gmail.com
CHAPTER 1

Introduction to the North


American Paridae
Susan M. Smith

An amazing amount of new information has been American crested titmice are in the same lineage,
discovered since I published my first book on which is closest to, but distinct from, that of the
chickadees (Smith 1991). Among the most funda- Eurasian crested tits. By contrast, while all of the
mental of these advances is the work of Frank Gill North American chickadees are in the same basic
and his colleagues on the genetic relationships group, they have not diverged sufficiently from cer-
within the Family Paridae. Working with mito- tain Eurasian relatives (e.g. willow and marsh tits,
chondrial DNA, in particular the mitochondrial among others) to be considered a separate lineage
cytochrome-B gene, Gill et al. (2005) have arrived at from those species. The recent adoption of the new
an overview of the degree of relatedness among the generic names by the British Ornithologists Union
members of this avian family. Evidently there have and International Ornithological Congress for all
been two independent invasions of North America tits world-wide appears to be finally resolving this
by Parids. The first of these is estimated to have issue (see Preface).
occurred approximately 4 million years ago (mya), North American chickadees can be further
and led to the present species of crested titmice in divided into two phenotypically distinct groups:
this continent. The second invasion, about 3.5 mya, those with black caps and whistled songs (the black-
led to the chickadees (Gill et al. 2005). capped, Carolina, mountain and Mexican chicka-
It is unfortunate that there has been a general lack dees); and those with brown or grey caps, which
of agreement as to whether the distinct lineages typically lack whistled songs (the chestnut-backed,
among Parids world-wide, as described by Gill and boreal chickadees, and Siberian tits). In general, a lot
his colleagues, should be considered as distinct, more work has been done on the first four species; it
separate genera (as adopted by the American is therefore particularly welcome to have a chapter
Ornithologist Union in North America in the 1990s), in this book exploring the phylogeography of chest-
or whether these should be merely subgenera (the nut-backed chickadees (Chapter 6). Although three
prevalent view until recently on the other side of the subspecies of chestnut-backed chickadees are recog-
Atlantic see Preface). This disagreement between nized, microsatellite analysis indicates that there are
North American and European editors led to the actually four genetically distinct populations now
rather confusing situation in which the species I alive: two in British Columbia (Queen Charlotte
work with, the black-capped chickadee, remains Islands, and south-eastern B.C.); one confined
Parus atricapillus in European journals, while having to Alaska (central Alaska); and a large coastal
undergone the unfortunate and convoluted journey group. Burg set out to discover how this particular
through Poecile atricapilla to Poecile atricapillus in distribution has arisen. In particular, she was
North American journals. Be that as it may, the exploring the effect of Pleistocene glaciations on the
important thing is that this work has clarified which evolution of these four lineages. Based on careful
species belong in which lineages, and how closely analysis of the genetic differences and similarities
each of these lineages is to the others. All North between and among these four groups, Burg has

1
2 I N T R O D U C T I O N TO T H E N O RT H A M E R I C A N PA R I D A E

concluded that the current distribution and lineages is clear that a lot of mistakes are made, as the hybrid
of chestnut-backed chickadees is the result of zone is broad and pushing rapidly northward.
colonization (after glaciation) from multiple refugia, One of the rather startling findings of Gill and his
rather than having either a single northern or single colleagues is that, despite this broad and dynamic
southern source. hybrid zone, it is the mountain chickadee, rather
Another chapter using molecular biological tech- than the Carolina, that is the species most closely
niques is that of Curry et al. (Chapter 7), working related to black-capped chickadees. Martin and
with black-capped and Carolina chickadees. These Norris (Chapter 8) have explored some ecological
two species, while evidently not each others closest aspects of an area in the interior of British Columbia
relatives (Gill et al. 1993, 2005), are nevertheless the where these two species are sympatric. One
two chickadee species that interbreed most regularly, important difference between the two is that
along a quite long and broad hybrid zone. Curry and black-capped chickadees typically excavate their
his colleagues work on this hybrid zone reveals a own nest cavities, while mountain chickadees must
dynamic situation in which the Carolina genes depend on cavities created by other birds.
appear to be pushing northward into what was for- Martin and Norris work in an area with fluctuat-
merly purely black-capped chickadee populations. ing food supply for nesting chickadees. One of the
This is rather horrifying information to people like really interesting findings they have is that in years
me who count on knowing what species they are with a high insect food supply, mountain chicka-
working on! In the broad zone of interbreeding, dees were able to increase their nest density far
Curry documents that an individual birds morpho- more strongly than could the rarer, but dominant,
logical characteristics might suggest it is purely one black-capped chickadees. It would be really inter-
species, while its vocalizations might be very typic- esting to follow up their suggestion and attempt to
ally the other speciesand genetically it might be measure the actual cost of nest site excavation.
anywhere along a continuum from either pure Another factor to investigate is whether the
species. As Curry and his colleagues point out, mor- black-capped chickadees might respond to increas-
phological features such as feather edgings or bib size ing food levels not by varying their breeding
might be genetically based, but at least some aspects density but rather by increasing their clutch size.
of vocalizations, in particular whistled songs, must Ratcliffe, Mennill, and Schubert (Chapter 9)
be learned; hence even a purely Carolina chickadee examine factors affecting winter social rank and
might sing a black-capped chickadee song, and vice fitness in black-capped chickadees. Their work has
versa. And now Curry and his colleagues are finding documented a variety of factors that can influence a
a trend for increasing numbers of bilingual birds in given birds position in its winter flock (I might
their study areas. This applies not only to whistled remind the reader that the relative rank between
songs, but also to some true calls (especially the dee members of a breeding pair typically reverses in the
calls of the chick-a-dee call complex). And just to make breeding season, with females becoming dominant
it even more confusing, a given birds calls similarity over their mates, Smith 1980).
to one of the pure species has no particular correl- The effects of age and of sex are both well known.
ation with the same individuals songs similarity: Ratcliffe et al. have gone well beyond these (while
that is, an individual might give a black-capped further clarifying and quantifying the effects of
chickadee whistled song, but have the shorter dee both). Some of the correlates with dominance
notes of a Carolina chickadee in its chick-a-dee call that they have found include body condition:
notes. And now Curry et al. have shown that birds high-ranked chickadees of each sex are leaner than
giving more Carolina-like calls had more black- lower-ranking birds (thus making them more
capped-like hybrid index scores. I pity the poor maneuverable in predator escape situations).
female attempting to make a suitable mate choice in Ratcliffes group has also shown that plumage
a situation like this! Actually, Curry et al. conclude characteristics, like the darkness of the cap and bib,
that vocal patterns may be less important than other and brighter white, is correlated with rank, at least
aspects of the birds mating systems. Nevertheless, it in males. They have also shown that higher-ranked
I N T R O D U C T I O N TO T H E N O RT H A M E R I C A N PA R I D A E 3

unpaired males are selected faster by females (Otter several factors that can affect the size of this structure.
and Ratcliffe 1996). One of these is geographic location of the population.
How is initial rank achieved and maintained? For example, Pravosudov (Chapter 3) has shown that
Ratcliffe et al. have found, at least for males, that the black-capped chickadees in Alaska have significantly
suppression hypothesis, which states that domin- larger hippocampi than do Colorado chickadees. The
ant individuals condition their subordinates to lose rather more startling continental difference, with
through ongoing attacks, best fits the observed European birds across many avian families having
behavior in the field. Finally, they found, again just significantly larger hippocampi than their North
for males, that males that lived longer (and thus American counterparts, has yet to be fully explained,
achieved higher rank) did indeed have greater fit- although, as Sherry and Hoshooley point out, the
ness, as measured by lifetime reproductive success; difference seems not explicable as merely an artifact
nevertheless this seemed to be a reflection simply of of differing laboratory procedures, but actually a
whether or not the males reproduced at all, rather genuine phenomenon. Clearly this puzzling differ-
than showing any finer-scale difference between ence in hippocampus size is both a necessary and a
individuals. promising area for future research on both sides of
Another area in which an enormous amount of the Atlantic.
work has been done since 1991 is physiology, includ- Another facet of hippocampal research is in adult
ing (especially) neurophysiology. One of the major neurogenesis. A great deal of controversy exists
areas of advance concerns neurophysiological here, and the data are not easily interpreted. Sherry
aspects of food storage, or caching behavior. Black- and Hoshooley suggest that neurogenesis in many
capped chickadees, and other members of the Poecile Parids may, in fact, be relatively constant through-
group, are scatter-hoarders: they can store hundreds out the year, but the attrition rate of old cells may
and often thousands of seeds, each in a different vary with the season.
place. There is now abundant evidence, especially for Actually a number of factors affect the size of the
black-capped chickadees, that the birds can remem- hippocampus, and also the ability of a bird to per-
ber precisely where they stored particular items, and form spatial tasks. One of these is stress, especially as
then can go back and retrieve the stored food. measured by levels of corticosterone. Pravosudov
Exploration of this behavior has proceeded along (Chapter 3) has shown that moderate levels enhance
several lines. One is purely behavioral. In a spatial memory tasks in mountain chickadees,
particularly elegant experiment (Brodbeck 1994) although exactly how this affects the hippocampus is
showed that black-capped chickadees used a not yet known. Dominance rank is also important,
hierarchy of cues to return to the location of a with dominant birds performing memory tasks
particular food item. Rather amazingly, overall significantly better than subordinates. Pravosudov
location within the aviary was most important, found no rank difference in either hippocampus size
relative position within an array of boxes in the or number of neurons in the hippocampus, but the
aviary was second, and color of the box was third difference may be related to cell turnover: his findings
most important for the chickadees. Clearly their suggest that cell proliferation rates in subordinate
spatial capabilities are exceptional, and we need to birds were significantly lower than in dominants.
explore the neural basis for these abilities. One factor that seemingly may affect the hippo-
And so we come to the hippocampus. Although campus, at least directly, is photoperiod (Phillmore
the hippocampus in birds is homologous to that in and MacDougall-Shackleton, Chapter 4), although,
mammals, the avian structure is more accessible for frustratingly, data on free-living birds do not seem
investigation, being not as buried within the brain to agree with data taken on birds in captivity. Any
tissue (Sherry and Hoshooley, Chapter 2). As in photoperiod effect may be mediated by a number of
mammals, this structure is strongly associated with factors present in field conditions but not available
spatial ability. In the last few years, our knowledge in the artificial simplicity of the lab.
about the avian hippocampus, and especially that of Clearly photoperiod has an enormous effect on
Parids, has increased enormously. We now know reproductive physiology. Black-capped chickadees,
4 I N T R O D U C T I O N TO T H E N O RT H A M E R I C A N PA R I D A E

like most songbirds, are absolutely photorefractory Finally, one of the areas where the greatest recent
(Chapter 4). But, as Ramsay and Otter (Chapter 5) progress has been made is the field of vocal commu-
point out, photoperiod does not explain year-to-year nication. The three most complex vocalizations: the
variations in reproductive timing. For that, one whistled fee-bee song (Chapters 10 and 14), the gargle
needs to look at look at population and individual call, which some have argued might be considered
differences. For example, variations in sensitivity to to share some of the functions of a song (Chapters 11
photoperiodic changes can explain population dif- and 12), and the chick-a-dee call complex, with its syn-
ferences in onset of breeding in blue tits (Lambrechts tax and incredible complexities (Chapters 10 and 13),
et al. 1997). Individual females also are remarkably are all addressed in detail in this volume.
consistent from year to year as to when they begin Nevertheless, while an enormous amount is now
laying eggs (Chapter 5). Other factors, such as food known about Parid (and in particular chickadee)
levels, temperature, and parasite loads, may also communication, enormous gaps in our knowledge
affect female timing; now we are beginning to look remain. It is unfortunate that the work on both
at the effects of more global factors, such as the whistled songs and gargles has been done almost
North Atlantic Oscillation, and global warming, on exclusively on males (with females considered,
clutch initiation dates. when at all, primarily as recipients of male-
The last three chapters in this book deal with produced signals). Yet females produce both of
conservation and habitat management. This is a these vocalizations. The function of these notes,
welcome addition to the subjects I attempted to and how recipients of either sex respond to them, is
cover in my first book. Parids are relatively seden- as yet largely uninvestigated.
tary, and not particularly good long-distance flyers. The other, truly enormous hole in our under-
They will go long distances around an open area, standing of communication in Parids is in the area
rather than attempt to cross the open space of visual signals. One approach to the study of
(Desrochers and Blisle, Chapter 15). Habitat frag- visual signals is to investigate variation and func-
mentation can have a big impact on their ability to tion of plumage patches (e.g. Otter and Ratcliffe
move from place to place. Even for relatively large 1999; Mennill et al. 2003; Doucet et al. 2004;
Parids, such as tufted titmice, movements from Woodcock et al. 2005). Black-capped chickadees,
patch to patch are greatly enhanced if those patches which appear superficially to human eyes as mono-
are connected by wooded corridors (Olson and chromatic, are actually sexually dichromatic
Grubb, Chapter 16). Certainly in these days of (Chapter 9). Males have brighter whites and greys
increasing human population, habitat fragmenta- and blacker blacks than females; patch sizes vary
tion will only increase, and it is important for us to consistently too. Remarkably, these subtle plumage
understand the impact of factors, such as patch size variations reflect not only sex but also rank
of blocks of forest, and the importance of connect- (Mennill et al. 2003; Doucet et al. 2004), and are
ing links, on the local bird populations. Otter et al. correlated with female mate choice (Doucet et al.
(Chapter 17) touch on a particularly important area. 2004; Woodcock et al. 2005). Similar functions of
In forest management, matrix (intervening habitat plumage patch variation have been found in other
that connects patches of relatively undisturbed Parids as well (e.g. Ferns and Hinsley 2004).
habitat), has been claimed to provide alternative But where we still know surprisingly little is in the
breeding habitat for a number of species. While this area of postural signals. Susan T. Smith (1972) gave
management technique does provide cover and brief descriptions of the postural signal repertoire of
enhances movements between patches of relatively Carolina chickadees. Black-capped chickadees give
undisturbed habitats, Otter et al. has shown that the many similar signals; I attempted to illustrate some
matrix often is largely unsuitable for successful of these in my more recent book (Smith 1997). A few
breeding among the birds it appears to retain. displays, such as body-ruffling (Piaskowski et al.
Careful studies of this nature and quality are 1991) and single wing flick (Smith 1996) have been
absolutely essential to assess popular management described, but nobody has attempted a comprehen-
techniques. sive overview of visual displays that might balance
I N T R O D U C T I O N TO T H E N O RT H A M E R I C A N PA R I D A E 5

the vocal repertoire papers of Ficken et al. (1978) for Ficken, M. S. (1990b). Vocal repertoire of the Mexican
black-capped chickadees, and Ficken (1990a, 1990b) chickadee. II. Song and song-like vocalizations. Journal
for Mexican chickadees. Indeed, surprisingly little is of Field Ornithology, 61, 388395.
known about visual signals in most other North Ficken, M. S., Ficken, R. W., and Witkin, S. R. (1978). Vocal
repertoire of the black-capped Chickadee. Auk, 95,
American Parids. One can argue that the software
3448.
technology exists for the study of vocal signals, but
Gill, F. B., Mostrom, A. M., Mack, A. L. (1993). Speciation
not for visual signals; I suspect that had the pressure in North American chickadees: I. Patterns of mtDNA
been stronger to develop visual signal software, that genetic divergence. Evolution, 47, 195212.
might have been produced first. One can also argue Gill, F. B., Slikas, B., and Sheldon, F. H. (2005). Phylogeny
that vocal signals are a lot easier to study, and I of titmice (Paridae):II. Species relationships based on
agree. I am not sure I agree with Smith (1972) when sequences of the mitochondrial cytochrome-B gene.
she suggested that visual signals merely back up Auk, 122, 121143.
vocalizations. Certainly we need to obtain data to Grubb, T. C. (1998). Wild Bird Guides: Tufted Titmouse.
test this hypothesis. With the state of video technol- Stackpole Books, Mechanicsburg.
ogy today, both descriptive and manipulative Lambrechts, M. M., Blondel, J., Maistre, M., and Perret, P.
(1997). A single response mechanism is responsible for
experiments are possible, even with fast-moving
evolutionary adaptive variation in a birds laying date.
species such as chickadees (e.g. Baker et al. 1996).
Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA, 94,
Titmice, being somewhat slower, might make even 51535155.
better subjects for a thorough examination of the role Mennill, D. J., Doucet, S. M., Montgomerie, R., and
of visual signals. Their crests make them particularly Ratcliffe, L. M. (2003). Achromatic color variation in
good subjects for such a study. Grubb (1998) refers to black-capped chickadees, Poecile atricapilla: black and
the crest of the tufted titmouse as a semaphore white signals of sex and rank. Behavioral Ecology and
signal (Grubb 1998:16), where raised crests signal Sociobiology, 53, 350357.
excitement or aggression, with flattened crests sig- Otter, K. and Ratcliffe, L. (1996). Female initiated divorce
naling the opposite (passivity and subordination). in a monogamous songbird: abandoning mates for
Quantitative data on such a role should be relatively males of higher quality. Proceedings of the Royal Society of
London Series B, Biology, 263, 351354.
easily obtained. Rigorous study on Parid visual com-
Otter, K. and Ratcliffe, L. M. (1999). Relationship of bib
munication is sorely needed, especially here in
size to age and sex in the black-capped chickadee.
North America. This surely is a wide open area for Journal of Field Ornithology, 70, 567577.
future research. Piaskowski, V. D., Weise, C. M., and Ficken, M. S. (1991).
The body-ruffling display of the black-capped chicka-
dee. Wilson Bulletin, 103, 426434
References
Smith, S. M. (1980). Henpecked males: the general pattern
Baker, M. C., Tracy, T. T., and Miyasato, L. E. (1996). Gargle in monogamy? Journal of Field Ornithology, 51, 5564.
vocalizations of black-capped chickadees: test of reper- Smith, S. M. (1991). The Black-Capped Chickadee: Behavioral
toire and video stimuli. Animal Behaviour, 52, 11711175. Ecology and Natural History. Cornell University Press,
Brodbeck, D. R. (1994). Memory for spatial and local cues: Ithaca, New York.
a comparison of a storing and a non-storing species. Smith, S. M. (1996). The single wing-flick display of the
Animal Learning and Behavior, 22, 119133. black-capped chickadee (Parus atricapillus). Condor, 98,
Doucet, S. M., Mennill, D. J., Montgomerie, R., Boag, P., 885887.
and Ratcliffe, L. M. (2004). Achromatic plumage Smith, S. M. (1997). Wild Bird Guides: Black-capped
reflectance predicts reproductive success in male black- Chickadee. Stackpole Books, Mechanicsburg, PA.
capped chickadees. Behavioral Ecology, 16, 218222. Smith, S. T. (1972). Communication and other social
Ferns, P. N. and Hinsley, S. A. (2004). Immaculate tits: behavior in Parus carolinensis. Publications of the Nuttall
head plumage pattern as an indicator of quality in Ornithological Club, 11, 1125.
birds. Animal Behaviour, 67, 261272. Woodcock, E. A., Rathburn, M. K., and Ratcliffe, L. M.
Ficken, M. S. (1990a). Vocal repertoire of the Mexican (2005). Achromatic plumage reflectance, social domin-
chickadee. I. Calls. Journal of Field Ornithology, 61, ance and female mate preference in black-capped
380387. chickadees (Poecile atricapillus). Ethology, 111, 891900.
This page intentionally left blank
SECTION I

Proximate mechanisms in behavior


and evolution

Extrinsic and intrinsic factors influence the behav- contexts; they then relate the ability to form spatial
ior and ecology of all animals, and are inextricably memories to the structure, gene expression, and
linked with the content presented in every chapter neurogenesis within the hippocampus. Pravosudov
of this book. In most of the chapters, there is at least (Chapter 3) extends this topic by comparing differ-
some emphasis on the factors that regulate the ences in the spatial abilities across disparate popu-
behaviors being discussed. But while some research lations within the same species. In particular, this
emphasizes the ultimate outcome that exhibiting research compares populations in circumstances
certain behavior patterns has on an individuals that vary in food limitation, and how this influ-
reproductive success, others focus upon under- ences the propensity for food caching and may
standing the underlying proximate mechanisms result from physiological differences in the hippo-
the external abiotic/biotic conditions or internal campus and hormone regulation between these
physiologythat regulate the behaviors we see. It same populations.
is this focus, which has formed a large component While enhanced spatial memory may be selected
of Parid research, that forms the relationship for in over-wintering Parids and this ability might be
chapters in this section of the book. more specialized within some avian lineages, other
Parids exhibit some specialized traits that make behaviors and their regulating factors are more
them prime subjects for the neuroethological and general in nature. The factors that control the cir-
physiological studies of controlling mechanisms of cannual rhythms of the life-history traits, such as
behavior. For example, the remarkable ability of the controlling the timing of seasonal patterns
many speciesand notably the two Parid groups in reproduction and wintering behavior, occur in
occupying North Americato both cache and migratory and non-migratory species alike. In
recover enormous numbers of food items over the Chapter 4, Phillmore and MacDougall-Shackleton
course of a winter. In Chapter 2, Sherry and assess the influence of photoperiod as a mechanism
Hoshooley investigate the structural aspects of the of regulating annual cycles. In particular, Phillmore
avian brain, and how this can influence spatial and MacDougall-Shackleton review the interactive
memory. Using cross-species comparisons within influence of changing light levels on regulating the
the Paridae and comparing Parids to other species, annual hormone cycles that influence the develop-
Sherry and Hoshooley review general spatial ment of gonads (breeding regulation) and neural
abilities of chickadees in food-storing and other structures associated with both food caching and

7
8 B E H AV I O R A N D E V O L U T I O N

song control. Ramsay and Otter (Chapter 5) focus this section (for example, Chapter 10). Chapters
on the fine-scale regulation of one of these within this section (Chapter 5) might also have
behaviorsthe timing of clutch initiation and found a home in the next section of the book on
fertility in Parids. This topic, which is the subject of reproductive ecology and behavior of Parids, and
long-term research programs on Eurasian tits and, its position as the last chapter in the section pro-
recently, North American chickadees, considers the vides the bridge to Section II. Within the present
interaction between annual variation in weather section, the chapters represent clear situations
patterns and food availability in regulating where the primary focus of the research is to under-
seasonal and individual variation in nest timing. stand the underlying proximate mechanisms that
As mentioned, all aspects of a species behavior control behavioral variation seen in Parids, and to
and ecology are influenced by external abiotic and relate this to how Parids have been used as model
internal physiological variables, and several other organisms to elicit a greater understanding of
chapters in this book could be easily included in behavior and ecology in general.
CHAPTER 2

Neurobiology of spatial
behavior
David F. Sherry and Jennifer S. Hoshooley

2.1 Introduction 2.2 Food storing in the Paridae


Chickadees and tits seem unlikely subjects for All of the North American chickadees and titmice
research on the neurobiology of spatial behavior. store food, as do most of the well-known Eurasian
Most species are sedentary and there appears to be species (Hampton and Sherry 1992; Sherry 1989).
little about the Parid brain that would recommend Figure 2.1 shows the phylogenetic distribution of
it as a model system for neurobiology. Chickadees food-storing in the Paridae. The behavior is practic-
and tits, along with Corvids, have, however, been at ally universal in the Poecile group and the two
the centre of recent research on spatial ability and a crested tit groups Lophophanes and Baeolophus. The
brain region crucially involved in spatial ability, the only exceptions are three little-known, high-altitude
hippocampus. Two basic discoveries focused atten- species endemic to China and the Himalayas for
tion on chickadees and tits. The first was that chicka- which it remains unknown whether or not they
dees and tits that store food remember the spatial store food. Food storing is known not to occur in the
locations of their caches. These birds are scatter great tit, and although it has been reported for the
hoarders and place very large numbers of food blue tit (Hinde 1952; Southern 1946) it is probably
items in sites dispersed through their winter home rare, if it occurs at all (Haftorn 1956; Healy et al.
range. They can store hundreds of items in a single 1994; Richards 1958). For the many species of the
day and put each cache in a different place, never Periparus and Parus groups found in China, Nepal,
reusing the same site. Remembering the locations the Philippines, and Africa there is not enough
of these caches is a formidable problem and there is information to be certain whether they store food or
now a great deal of experimental evidence showing not. Food storing has not been observed in the wild
that the birds successfully solve it. The second dis- or in captivity in the groundpecker (Parus humilis;
covery was that despite appearances, the Parid Gebauer, personal communication) a very unusual
brain is unusual. Food-storing chickadees and tits, bird (formerly Humes ground jay) recently
along with food-storing jays and nuthatches, have a assigned to the Paridae on the basis of molecular
larger hippocampus than is found in species that do and biochemical characteristics (Gebauer et al. 2004;
not store food. The hippocampus is involved in a James et al. 2003) and placed near the root of the
variety of cognitive processes but one that has been Parid phylogeny proposed by Gill et al. (2005).
consistently identified in birds and mammals
(including humans) is spatial ability. In this chapter
2.2.1 Food storing in the wild
we will describe what has been learned from
chickadees and tits about the neurobiology of Food storing has a strong seasonal component in
spatial ability. Review of a parallel research Parids, beginning in autumn and continuing
program on food-storing Corvids can be found in through the winter into early spring (Brodin 1994;
Balda et al. (1997). Haftorn 1956; Ludescher 1980; Nakamura and

9
10 B E H AV I O R A N D E V O L U T I O N

100 atricapillus
100 gambeli gambeli
82
gambeli baileyi
85 sclateri
rufescens
87
100 hudsonicus
100
cinctus
100 carolinensis extimus Poecile
90 carolinensis carolinensis
100 palustris brevirostris
palustris palustris
93 montanus borealis
davidi
99 superciliosus
varius
lugubris
100 cristatus
Lophophanes
97 dichrous
100 wollweberi
79 100 inornatus Baeolophus
bicolor
ater aemodius
100
melanolophus
97
ater ater
87 Periparus
rubidiventris
100
rufonuchalis
elegans elegans
100 amabilis
elegans mindanensis
100 major
monticolus
92
holsti
100
100 spilonotus
xanthogenys
71 afer Parus
89 albiventris
100
100 niger niger
86
niger niger
73 funereus
95 rufiventris
fasciiventer
Parus humilis
100 Parus caeruleus Cyanistes
Parus cyanus
100 Melanochlora s. sultanea
Melanochlora s. gayeti
Sylviparus modestus

Figure 2.1 The distribution of food-storing in the Paridae, shown on a phylogeny proposed by Gill et al. (2005). Dark shaded boxes show
species known to store, light shaded boxes show species that do not store, or in the case of Parus caeruleus, rarely do so. One species not shown
in this phylogeny, Parus venustulus, is thought to be closely related to Parus ater and has been reported to store food (Harrap and Quinn 1996).
Although there appears to be a phylogenetic pattern to the occurrence of food-storing, there is no information on the presence or absence of the
behavior for most species in the Periparus and Parus groups. Arrows show hypothesized invasions of North America by the Poecile and
Baeolophus groups in the late Tertiary, 3.5 and 4.0 mya, respectively. Branch numbers in this maximum likelihood phylogram show posterior
probabilities greater than 70% (reprinted with permission from Gill et al. 2005).
N E U R O B I O L O G Y O F S PAT I A L B E H AV I O R 11

Wako 1988; Pravosudov 1985). Chickadees and tits of preferred site types, use of a preferred route, or
cache all of the foods they normally eat, including marking of cache sites. These experiments deter-
seeds, nuts, fat from animal carcasses, spiders, and mined the rate of successful cache recovery by cap-
insects, which they sometimes prepare by remov- tive birds and found that chickadees and tits
ing the head and viscera. Any small crevice or perform much better than expected by either
cavity will do as a storage site, and food is also chance or site preference alone (Sherry 1984; Sherry
stored in moss, lichen, dry leaves, or pushed into et al. 1981; Shettleworth and Krebs 1986; Shettleworth
soft ground. Field studies of European marsh tits and Krebs 1982). Memory for cache sites can persist
(Cowie et al. 1981; Stevens and Krebs 1986) and wil- for up to 4 weeks in black-capped chickadees in
low tits (Brodin 1992), in which researchers located captivity and this is probably an underestimate of
cache sites in the wild, found that most food is how long food-storing Parids can remember the
retrieved very soon after it is stored, often by the locations of their caches (Hitchcock and Sherry 1990).
end of the same day and generally within a few But knowing that chickadees remember where they
days at most. Studies in which ingestion of radio- placed their cachesremarkable as that feat of
labeled stored seeds produced a radio-labeled memory may beonly opens up new questions
growth bar on growing rectrices of willow tits about spatial cognition in these little birds. What is
found some food was also retrieved and eaten at remembered about a cache site and what makes up
much longer intervals ranging from six to 40 days a spatial location for a chickadee?
(Brodin and Ekman 1994). The radio labeling
method cannot detect cache recovery after shorter
2.2.3 Memory for caches versus other
intervals because of the time required to initiate
spatial locations
growth of a replacement rectrix, or after longer
intervals because replacement of the feather is com- Experiments with black-capped chickadees, marsh
plete by about 6 weeks. This result shows, never- tits, and coal tits in captivity show that they are
theless, that caches are sometimes left in place for equally good at remembering the locations of food
long periods of time before retrieval, or that stored they have stored and food they have encountered
food is sometimes retrieved and recached multiple but not collected in the course of normal foraging
times (Brodin 1994). On the Japanese island of (Shettleworth and Krebs 1986; Shettleworth et al.
Miyake, Higuchi (1977) observed nuts of the 1990). Even after an interval of 26 h, the birds return
Japanese chinquapin Castanopsis cuspidata are with equal probability to both kinds of site.
among the food brought by varied tits to their Analyses of interference in memory between sites
young. Because these nuts had not been available where food was stored and sites where food was
on trees since the previous winter, Higuchi con- encountered give no indication that the two types of
cluded the birds were likely feeding their young spatial locations are remembered in different ways
from caches made at least several months earlier. (Shettleworth et al. 1990), although the duration of
memory for cache sites may be longer than for sites
where food was encountered (Brodbeck et al. 1992).
2.2.2 Memory for cache sites
Black-capped chickadees memory for spatial
Early field studies of cache retrieval by marsh tits, locations where they have found food shows some
willow tits, and Siberian tits (also known as the of the same properties found in other research on
gray-headed chickadee and the only Parid to occur animal memory using more conventional tasks.
in both the Palearctic and the Nearctic) confirmed Locations and features that are unique, for
by a variety of methods, including direct observa- example, are remembered better than locations and
tion, that birds that cache food later return to collect features that are repeated within successive experi-
it (Cowie et al. 1981; Pravosudov 1986; Stevens and mental trials (Brodbeck et al. 1992). This may
Krebs 1986). Laboratory studies showed that mem- make cache sites, which are unique in the wild,
ory for the spatial locations of caches is the means easier to remember than stimuli that are repeated
of cache retrieval, rather than chance encounter, use on successive trials in laboratory experiments.
12 B E H AV I O R A N D E V O L U T I O N

Black-capped chickadees memory for sites where occupied the location on the wall formerly occu-
they have found food also shows a serial position pied by the rewarded feeder (Fig. 2.2). There was
effectbetter memory for items at the beginning and also no food in any of the blocks. By dissociating
the end of a sequence compared to the middle very cues in this way and placing them in conflict it was
similar to serial position effects found in animal and possible to determine whether the bird chose the
human memory for lists and series of events correct position on the wall, the correct position in
(Crystal and Shettleworth 1994). the array, or the correct color and pattern. Brodbeck
(1994) found that black-capped chickadees
returned preferentially to the block that was in the
2.2.4 Spatial memory in food-storing versus
correct position on the wall. This result suggested
non-storing birds
that the place where the bird had found food was
To compare spatial ability in storing and non-stor- remembered as a location with respect to the larger
ing birds it is obviously necessary to observe features of the aviary, not the arrangement of blocks
behavior other than food storing. Research on in the array or the distinctive colors and patterns on
memory in chickadees and tits, described above, the block. When the birds failed to find food at this
provides a number of non-storing tasks in which block, however, their second choice was most often
chickadees and tits perform about as well as they to the feeder in the correct position in the array, and
do when retrieving caches, and these tasks can be when they failed to find food there, their third
used to compare performance in storing and non-
storing species.
Krebs et al. (1990) compared memory in food-stor-
ing coal tits and non-storing great tits for sites where (a)
food had previously been encountered. In an experi-
ment that required remembering which of seven
previously encountered sites contained food, coal
tits relocated the site after fewer attempts than great
tits, but in an experiment that required remember-
ing which seven sites out of a possible 60 contained
food, both species did equally well. In the latter
experiment, great tits tended to return to all of the
sites they had previously visited whether they con- (b)
tained food or not, while coal tits tended to return
more selectively to sites that had contained food.
Brodbeck (1994) developed a very interesting
task that makes it possible to determine what
aspects of a spatial location are remembered by
food-storing birds. Birds found food in one of four
distinctively colored and patterned blocks of wood
placed in an array on the wall of an aviary. Once a
Figure 2.2 Chickadees searched until they discovered which of
chickadee had found which block contained food
four blocks arranged in an array on the wall of an aviary contained
the bird was removed from the aviary. When it food, indicated in (a) by the white triangle (Brodbeck 1994). The bird
returned the arrangement of the wood blocks had then left the aviary and the array of blocks was modified, as shown in
been changed. The array was displaced on the wall (b). The array has been shifted to the right in this illustration and the
of the aviary while preserving the relative position feeder formerly baited has been exchanged with another. The bird
then returned to the aviary to search for food. This manipulation
of the blocks to one another. In addition, the
dissociates choice of the matching position on the wall, choice for
rewarded block had been switched with another the matching position in the array, and choice for the matching
block in the array. The displacement was done in colors and patterns of the blocks. Redrawn with permission from
such a way that a different feeder in the array now Brodbeck (1994).
N E U R O B I O L O G Y O F S PAT I A L B E H AV I O R 13

choice was to the feeder with the colors and pattern interference is the disruptive effect on memory of
that matched the block that was originally baited. material presented before something that is to be
This result shows that chickadees treat spatial loca- remembered. A phone number in the middle of a list,
tion with respect to surrounding landmarks as pri- for example, is more difficult to remember than a
mary, but also remember smaller-scale spatial number early in the list because of proactive inter-
relations and the local features of sites where they ference by earlier numbers. Proactive interference
have found food. contributes to the serial position effects described
Dark-eyed juncos (Junco hyemalis) in Brodbecks earlier. In Hampton et al.s (1998) experiment,
(1994) experiment also remembered which feeder chickadees and juncos were required to remember
had been baited but on test trials with cues placed the last in a series of one, two, or three stimuli
in conflict showed no preference among cues. Their presented on a touch screen. Hampton et al. (1998)
first, second, and third choices were equally likely reasoned that food-storing chickadees might be
to be to the correct position on the wall, the correct more resistant to proactive interference because of
position in the array, or the correct colors and the large number of cache sites they remember in the
pattern. wild. Greater resistance to proactive interference had
Brodbeck and Shettleworth (1995) modified this been previously reported for food-storing birds by
procedure for the presentation of stimuli on a com- Clayton and Krebs (1994). Chickadee performance
puter touch screen. Birds pecked stimuli on the declined, however, in a linear fashion as the number
screen that corresponded to the wood blocks in of stimuli in the series increaseda clear effect of
Brodbecks original experiment in order to obtain a proactive interference. The performance of juncos
food reward. In the touch screen version of the task showed no effect of proactive interference; their
the absolute position of the array on the screen was behavior was unaffected by the number of stimuli in
not manipulated. Comparison of food-storing the series. Interestingly, in a second experiment by
black-capped chickadees and non-storing dark- Hampton et al. (1998) a seemingly minor change in
eyed juncos showed that while chickadees chose procedure resulted in proactive interference in both
first the stimulus in the correct relative position in chickadees and juncos, with superior performance
the array and rarely the stimulus of the correct color, by chickadees on series of all three lengths. The
juncos did not differ in their choice of position or procedural change was to refrain from providing a
color. Chickadees had a strong tendency to match food reward for pecking the final stimulus in the
spatial location of the rewarded stimulus, not its series. Hampton et al. (1998) suggest that the differ-
color, while juncos matched location and color ence between their two experiments occurs because
equally often. A subsequent experiment showed juncos are more affected than chickadees by the
that when chickadees were required to choose on recent reward history of stimuli. When the last item
the basis of location alone or color alone, their in a series is associated with a food reward, juncos
responses were over 80% correct for location but no consistently choose that item independent of the
better than chance for color. Juncos, in contrast, per- number of items that preceded it, while chickadees
formed equally well and above chance for both loca- do not. When juncos are not provided with food
tion and color stimuli (Brodbeck and Shettleworth reward associated with the final item, both species
1995). These results show that food-storing chicka- show proactive interference and the performance of
dees either fail to attend to or fail to remember the chickadees is consistently better than that of juncos.
color of stimuli in these experiments, in contrast to Whether this explanation is correct or not, this study
the spatial location of stimuli, while non-storing shows that the relative performance of food-storing
juncos have no such bias for location over color. and non-storing birds during laboratory memory
Food-storing chickadees do not necessarily per- tests is not necessarily what one would predict from
form better than non-storing birds on memory tasks. food-storing birds reliance upon memory to recover
Hampton et al. (1998) compared the effects of caches of food. Furthermore, seemingly minor
proactive interference on memory in black-capped changes in experimental procedure can be of major
chickadees and dark-eyed juncos. Proactive significance to the birds.
14 B E H AV I O R A N D E V O L U T I O N

Shettleworth and Westwood (2002) subsequently A multidimensional scaling analysis of responses


conducted similar experiments comparing the per- showed, for example, that chickadees did not make
formance of chickadees and juncos on touch screen finer or more orderly spatial discriminations than
tasks using location and color stimuli. They found, juncos did for stimuli on a touch screen
however, that both chickadees and juncos per- (Shettleworth and Westwood 2002). This pattern of
formed better on location stimuli than on color results neatly characterizes many of the apparently
stimuli, and this was the case whether location and contradictory observations made in the course of
color items were presented independently or as research comparing memory in food-storing and
parts of a compound stimulus. Shettleworth and non-storing birds. Food-storing birds may, in fact,
Westwood (2002) also carefully varied the retention not excel at spatial tasks compared to other birds,
interval over which chickadees and juncos were but spatial tasks are what food-storing birds do
required to remember location and color stimuli. best, at least in experiments that involve pecking
Food-storing chickadees consistently did much bet- colored shapes on a touch screen.
ter with spatial information than they did with
color information, while juncos performed only
2.3 The hippocampus
slightly better, or no better, with location stimuli
compared to color stimuli. Both species performed Having discovered the capacity of food-storing
better than chance with both kinds of stimuli and chickadees and tits to relocate cache sites and the
food-storing birds did not necessarily perform bet- cognitive means by which they do this, researchers
ter than non-storing birds with spatial stimuli. turned to the neural mechanisms behind the birds

Monkey Rat Pigeon

CA3 APH
CA1
DG HP
DG
CA3
CA1

Figure 2.3 The hippocampus of the cynomolgus monkey (Macaca fascicularis), rat, and pigeon. Two interfolded structures, the dentate gyrus
(DG) and Ammons horn, or cornu ammonis (CA), make up the mammalian hippocampus. Scale bars equal 2 mm. APH area parahippocampalis;
CA1, CA3 cells fields of the cornu ammonis; DG dentate gyrus; HP hippocampus. Modified from Colombo and Broadbent (2000).
N E U R O B I O L O G Y O F S PAT I A L B E H AV I O R 15

spatial ability. The structure that has received the structure (Fig. 2.4). Neurons in this region are
most attention has been the hippocampus. In heterogeneous in size and packed closely together.
mammals the importance of the hippocampus for The area parahippocampalis is adjacent to the
spatial behavior has been well-documented. The hippocampus proper and extends laterally to a
neuroanatomical region designated as hippo- boundary marked by a distinct change in cell
campus in the avian brain is found in a very differ- density (Krebs et al. 1989). The area parahippo-
ent location from that of the hippocampus of the campalis is characterized by decreased cell density
mammalian brain (Fig. 2.3). In birds, the hippo- (in comparison to the hippocampus proper) and is
campus lies on the dorsal surface of the brain just comprised of uniformly large neurons. Based on the
above the ventricles (Karten and Hodos 1967; Stokes results of connectivity and electrophysiology
et al. 1974) whereas in mammals the hippocampus is studies, it has been suggested that the V-shaped
embedded in overlying neural tissue. The internal hippocampus proper may be homologous to
structure of the hippocampus also appears quite Ammons horn and that the dorsomedial region of
different in birds and mammals. In mammals, dis- the hippocampus may be homologous to the
crete divisions are apparent in histologically stained dentate gyrus (Siegel et al. 2002; Szekely 1999).
tissue. The interfolded CA1 and CA3 cell fields of Despite these differences in anatomy, many lines
Ammons horn and the dentate gyrus give the of evidence indicate homology between the avian
hippocampus its distinctive appearance. Although and mammalian hippocampus. Developmentally,
the avian hippocampus lacks such clear cellular the hippocampus arises from the same neural pre-
organization, it has structural features that resemble cursor tissue in birds and mammals (Klln 1962;
those found in the mammalian brain. The avian Kuhlenbeck 1938). Connectivity studies have also
hippocampus is composed of two regionsthe shown homology between birds and mammals,
hippocampus proper and the area parahippo- both for connections within the hippocampus
campalis. The hippocampus proper is a V-shaped and for connections between the hippocampus and
band of darkly stained tissue running along the other brain regions (Casini et al. 1986; Krayniak and
most medial and the most ventral edges of the Siegel 1978). Neurochemical comparisons have
shown similar distributions of neurotransmitters
and neuropeptides in the mammalian and avian
hippocampus (Erichsen et al. 1991; Krebs et al.
1991). Two phenomena characteristic of the mam-
malian hippocampus, long-term potentiation (LTP)
and place cells, have been described in the avian
APH hippocampus (Margrie et al. 1998; Siegel et al. 2005).
LTP is a phenomenon in which a few seconds of
electrical stimulation can cause neurons to become
more sensitive to stimulation for days or weeks
afterward. Long-term potentiation may mimic
HP processes involved in learning and the formation of
memory. Place cells are neurons that are most
electrically active whenever the animal enters a
particular place in its environment. Lesion studies
Figure 2.4 The black-capped chickadee hippocampus, made up of of pigeons and zebra finches have also shown the
the hippocampus proper (HP) and the area parahippocampalis (APH). importance of the hippocampus for processing
Neurons immunoreactive for the neuronal nuclei specific protein spatial information (Colombo and Broadbent 2000;
(NeuN) are shown labeled with diaminobenzidine (DAB) at Patel et al. 1997; Watanabe and Bischof 2004). These
5 objective magnification. Arrows indicate the change in cell
density marking the lateral boundary of the hippocampus. V-shaped
converging lines of evidence strongly suggest
line shows the cell density-defined boundary between the that the avian hippocampus and the mammalian
hippocampus proper and the area parahippocampalis. hippocampus are indeed homologous structures,
16 B E H AV I O R A N D E V O L U T I O N

derived with modification from a structure or struc- overall brain size, in the food-storing marsh tit, for
tures in the dorso-medial forebrain of their most example, than in the non-storing great tit (Krebs
recent common ancestor. et al. 1989). Black-capped chickadees, which store
Early work exploring the neural basis of food- food more intensively than either Mexican chicka-
storing spatial behavior took a lesion approach. The dees or bridled titmice, have a larger hippocampus
first study to examine the effects of hippocampal relative to overall brain size than the other two
ablation showed that Eurasian nutcrackers species (Hampton et al. 1995).
(Nucifraga caryocatactes) could not find their Brodin and Lundborg (2003) carried out a meta-
cachesthough they continued to make caches and analysis of available data on hippocampal size in
search for themfollowing removal of the hippo- food-storing Parids and Corvids and concluded
campus (Krushinskaya 1966). Similarly, an intact that, in general, there is no relation between relative
hippocampus is necessary for black-capped chicka- hippocampal size and the degree of caching behav-
dees to accurately retrieve stored food (Sherry and ior in either of these groups of food-storing birds.
Vaccarino 1989). Chickadees with hippocampal This data set has, in turn, been reanalyzed by Lucas
lesions continued to store food (in tree branches et al. (2004) who found that the results of Brodin
arranged in an indoor aviary) but the accuracy of and Lundborgs earlier analysis were affected by an
retrieval was reduced to a level no better than unexpected confound: the relative size of the
expected by chance encounter. Using a different hippocampus is greater in European birds than in
approach Hampton and Shettleworth (1996a, North American birds. After correcting statistically
1996b) compared the effects of hippocampal lesions for this difference between Eurasian and North
in chickadees and non-storing dark-eyed juncos by American birds, Lucas et al. (2004) showed that
assessing performance on some of the cognitive birds that store food to a greater degree do indeed
tasks described earlier. The results showed that the have a relatively larger hippocampus. The contin-
hippocampus is necessary in both chickadees and ent effect they found has subsequently been shown
juncos for tasks that require memory for spatial to occur in both food-storing and non-storing birds
information. In their first study the hippocampus (Garamszegi and Lucas 2005). The cause of this
was found to be necessary for memory of the difference in hippocampal size between European
spatial location of a stimulus but was not necessary and North American birds is not entirely
for memory of the color of the stimulus (Hampton clear. Garamszegi and Lucas (2005) suggest it is not
and Shettleworth 1996a). In their second study, likely to be a result of differences in the latitudinal
hippocampal lesions were found to impair distribution of European and North American
performance by both species on an operant spatial species and favor instead a continental difference
non-matching-to-sample task. These results show in avian ecology and life historyyet to be
that the chickadee hippocampus plays an essential identifiedas the evolutionary explanation for the
role in memory for spatial locations and is involved differences in hippocampal size they found.
in memory for cache sites.

2.3.2 Neural processing of spatial information


2.3.1 Comparative studies of the hippocampus
The discovery of differences in hippocampal size
The hippocampus of food-storing birds such as between food-storing and non-storing birds has
chickadees, nuthatches, and jays is consistently influenced research on the neural mechanisms of
larger than the hippocampus of non-storing birds spatial information processing. This work has
such as thrushes, blackbirds, sparrows, and warb- prompted researchers studying spatial learning in
lers, after correcting for overall differences in brain mammalian systems to look for neural specializa-
size (Garamszegi and Eens 2004; Krebs et al. 1989; tions that might correlate with specializations of
Sherry et al. 1989). This general pattern also holds spatial cognition (Galea and McEwen 1999; Jacobs
within food-storing families of birds such as the et al. 1990; Jacobs and Schenk 2003; Pawluski and
Paridae. The hippocampus is larger, relative to Galea 2006). This vein of research has added an
N E U R O B I O L O G Y O F S PAT I A L B E H AV I O R 17

ecological and evolutionary element to research on can activate the NMDA receptor as well. Activation
the neurobiology of the hippocampus (Sherry 2006; of NMDA receptors is one step in the LTP of
Sherry et al. 1992). neurons, described earlier. Another approach has
This research has also cast chickadees in a new been to examine the expression of immediate early
light, as a model species for examining the genes (IEGs) in the hippocampus during various
operation of the hippocampus and the neural components of food-storing behavior. IEGs are
mechanisms of memory formation. Because their activated in many neurons in response to frequent
food storing behavior requires the processing of a firing of action potentials and their expression in
good deal of spatial information, chickadees may neural tissue therefore indicates areas where strong
provide useful insights into the neurobiology of activation has occurred during the performance of
spatial behavior, much as London taxi drivers have a particular behavior or cognitive process (Mello
provided unexpected insights into the relation et al. 2004). Smulders and DeVoogd (2000) found
between the human hippocampus and spatial that the IEG expression in the chickadee hippo-
ability (Maguire et al. 2003). campus during food-storing was related to the
One line of research has extended comparative accuracy of cache retrieval, suggesting that the
analysis of the hippocampus by examining the intensity of neuronal activity in the hippocampus
ontogeny of hippocampal enlargement in food- has later effects on the strength of memory for
storers. In juvenile mountain chickadees and marsh cache locations
tits food-storing activity and spatial experience are A series of recent studies by Shiflett, DeVoogd,
necessary for hippocampal growth and the and their colleagues examined the effects of
maintenance of increased hippocampal volume disrupting hippocampal neurotransmission and
(Clayton 2001; Patel et al. 1997). Other work has neural processing on the spatial behavior of
compared levels of neurochemicals in the brains of chickadees. In one study the hippocampus was
food-storers and non-storers. One such study reversibly inactivated by the administration of
compared the distribution of two neuropeptides, lidocaine (Shiflett et al. 2003) while in another the
substance P and neuropeptide Y, in black-capped effects of antagonizing NMDA and cannabinoid
chickadees, non-storing blue and great tits, and receptors were examined (Shiflett et al. 2004). In
non-storing dark-eyed juncos (Gould et al. 2001). both studies the same general methods were
Substance P and neuropeptide Y are known to used. Chickadees were tested on spatial memory
modulate memory formation in other species. encoding and retrieval in a task in which food was
While no significant differences in neuropeptide Y hidden in blocks of wood on the walls of a large
expression were found among the four species, indoor aviary. Birds were trained to learn the
chickadees showed significant enlargement of a location of a food reward hidden in one blockthe
region of intense substance P immunoreactivity in target blocklocated in an array of blocks that
the area parahippocampalis (APH). This result were empty. The accuracy of returning to the target
suggests that substance P may play a role in spatial block following various delay intervals was tested.
information processing in APH. A study which In both studies birds were implanted with bilateral
examined the distribution of two different cannulas, the tips of which were in contact with
receptors for the excitatory neurotransmitter the dorsal surface of the hippocampus. Lidocaine,
glutamatethe NMDA and AMPA receptors the NMDA receptor antagonist AP5, and the
found significantly fewer NMDA receptors in food- cannabinoid receptor antagonist SR141716A were
storing marsh tits than in non-storing blue tits administered directly to the hippocampus through
(Stewart et al. 1999). In mammals, the AMPA the cannulas.
receptor is involved in normal glutamate neuro- Reversible inactivation of the hippocampus with
transmission within the hippocampus. The NMDA lidocaine showed that the hippocampus was neces-
receptor is normally inactivated, unless the neuron sary for encoding the spatial location of the target
is experiencing frequent stimulation. When this and for short-term memory of the target location
occurs and the neuron is depolarized, glutamate tested 15 min following the initial encoding. Once
18 B E H AV I O R A N D E V O L U T I O N

the target location was encoded, however, retrieval the migratory patterns of new neurons from their
of its location from memory after 3 hours no longer place of origin, and the morphological and electro-
required activation of the hippocampus. These physiological properties of new neurons. The song
results support the idea that the hippocampus is control nucleus HVC, which is involved in the inte-
more important for the initial encoding of spatial gration of auditory input and motor output related
memories rather than for their retrieval, which is to song learning, exhibits striking seasonal variation
consistent with findings for mammals. The results in the recruitment of new neurons, with maximum
obtained for the NMDA receptor antagonist levels occurring in the fall (Nottebohm 2004;
AP5 showed that in order for information to be Tramontin and Brenowitz 2000). Because fall is the
accessible after long delays, that is, transferred to time of year when many songbirds modify their
long-term memory, NMDA receptors have to be learned songs, it has been proposed that new
functional during initial encoding of the target loca- neurons must be added to neuronal circuits for new
tion. NMDA receptor inactivation during the learning to occur.
15-min period following encoding had no effect on To test the hypothesis that neurogenesis is correl-
transfer to long-term memory. Similarly, NMDA ated with seasonal change in neural processing
receptor activation during encoding was not neces- Barnea and Nottebohm (1994) examined neurogen-
sary for short-term memory of the target location esis in another system, the hippocampus of the
assessed 15 min after encoding. This latter result black-capped chickadee. They proposed that if new
suggests that other receptors are involved in the neurons are necessary for new memories to be
processing of spatial information that results in created then more neurons should be recruited into
short-term memory. the chickadee hippocampus in the fall when
Cannabinoid receptor inactivation produced thousands of caches are being created. Chickadees
intriguing results. If cannabinoid receptors were were caught in the wild throughout the year, injected
functional during the encoding of a first target with the cell birth marker tritiated thymidine, and
location, then memory for a second target location, then released back into the wild to be recaptured
learned on a subsequent day, was better on a probe at various times from 6 weeks to 1 year later.
1 day following the learning of the second location. Examination of the hippocampus of birds caught
That is, cannabinoid antagonism during the 6 weeks postinjection showed greatest levels of
encoding session for the first target location caused neuronal recruitment in birds administered tritiated
errors in recalling the second target location. This thymidine in October. Heightened neuronal recruit-
result suggests that cannabinoid receptor activation ment at the time of year when chickadees were cre-
may be important for learning of multiple spatial ating most cache sites provided further support for
locationsa core component of food-storing the idea that new neurons are incorporated into
behavior. existing neuronal circuitry at times of year when
there are elevated demands on spatial memory.
To further understand the role of neurogenesis in
2.3.3 Neurogenesis in the hippocampus of
spatial memory in the chickadees hippocampus, we
the adult black-capped chickadee
conducted a study of seasonal variation in the pro-
A number of studies have addressed the role of duction of new hippocampal neurons (Hoshooley
adult neurogenesis in the processing of spatial infor- and Sherry 2004). It could be that more neurons are
mation in the hippocampus. The first work to exam- recruited into the chickadee hippocampus in
ine neurogenesis in the black-capped chickadee was October because a greater number of neurons are
that of Barnea and Nottebohm (1994) Extensive generated at that time of year, anticipating height-
research had previously described adult neurogen- ened spatial processing demands. It could also be
esis in the oscine song control system, much of this that neurons are generated at a constant rate
work conducted by Nottebohm and his colleagues throughout the year and usually perish soon after
(Nottebohm 2002), leading to a detailed picture of production, but in October more are incorporated
the location and nature of neural progenitor cells, into hippocampal circuits and survive. To test this
N E U R O B I O L O G Y O F S PAT I A L B E H AV I O R 19

first hypothesis we examined hippocampal neuron the doses examined by Pravosudov and Omanska
production in chickadees in fall, winter, and spring (2005; see also Chapter 3).
by administering the cell birth marker bromod- Because hippocampal neurogenesis varies
eoxyuridine (BrdU) and sacrificing birds 1 to seasonally it is possible that annual changes in
2 weeks later. This interval between birth marker photoperiod influence its timing and magnitude.
administration and sacrifice was chosen to allow We examined hippocampal neuron recruitment in
time for new neurons to express a mature neuronal three groups of birds held in various photoperiodic
phenotype, assessed morphologically, while at the conditions in an attempt to determine whether
same time minimizing the time for possible attri- decreasing daylength in the fall may serve as a cue
tion of new neurons. No significant fluctuations in enhancing the recruitment of fall-born neurons
neuron production were detected over fall, winter, (Hoshooley et al. 2005). The groups were held on
and spring. This suggests that the enhanced recruit- either long-days (15L: 9D) in a photostimulated
ment described by Barnea and Nottebohm (1994) state, on long-days (15L:9D) in a photorefractory
comes about through greater recruitment of state (assessed by gonadal regression and onset of
October-born hippocampal neurons rather than an molting), or on short days (8L:16D) in a photo-
overall increase in the production of such cells. refractory state. No significant differences in
New neurons may be necessary to encode new hippocampal neuron recruitment were detected
spatial memories in the hippocampus because old among the groups, suggesting that photoperiod is
neurons undergo irreversible structural modifica- not a cue that acts directly on the survival of adult-
tion during memory formation that is incompatible generated neurons in the chickadee hippocampus.
with the encoding of new spatial memories (Barnea It is plausible, though not proven, that hippocam-
and Nottebohm 1996; Nottebohm 2002; Nottebohm pal neurogenesis plays a role in spatial memory in
2002). New structurally unmodified neurons may food-storing birds. There are indications that the
be necessary for new information to be encoded. To seasonal pattern found in black-capped chickadees
make room for new neurons in hippocampal cir- does not occur in non-food-storing species such as
cuitry, neurons encoding expendable information house sparrows (Hoshooley and Sherry in press).
may be excised in a process of neuronal replace- Future research determining the properties of new
ment. Some support for this hypothesis comes neurons may tell us more about their function.
from the studies described above (Barnea and The receptor profile of these new cellswhether
Nottebohm 1994; Hoshooley and Sherry 2004) they possess NMDA or cannabinoid receptors, for
which both found that the total number of hippo- examplemay provide important clues about their
campal neurons did not change seasonally, sug- function in the hippocampus. Determining whether
gesting increased attrition must at some point equal adult-generated neurons become interneurons with
increased fall recruitment. The idea of seasonal projections entirely within the hippocampus, or
neuronal replacement is also supported by findings projection neurons exchanging information with
with songbirds showing that neuronal recruitment other brain areas, or both, would likewise provide
to HVC increases following targeted cell death important clues about their function.
(Scharff et al. 2000). The discovery of neurogenesis in the adult brain
Other work examining neurogenesis in chicka- overturned long held ideas about brain structure
dees has revealed that a birds social status plays a and neural plasticity. Much of the initial evidence
modulatory role on cell production in the ventricular for the occurrence of adult neurogenesis came from
zone directly beneath the hippocampus. Dominant research on the song control nuclei of songbirds.
birds show greater numbers of newly divided cells Neurogenesis in the hippocampus of food-storing
(Pravosudov and Omanska 2005). A factor which birds may lead, similarly, to a greater understand-
has been found not to have an effect on hippocampal ing of how spatial cognition, and perhaps other
neurogenesis is corticosterone. Administration of cognitive processes, are implemented in the con-
corticosterone had no significant effect on neural stantly changing network of neurons that make up
proliferation in the hippocampal ventricular zone at the hippocampus.
20 B E H AV I O R A N D E V O L U T I O N

2.4 Summary and conclusions black-capped chickadees. Proceedings of the National


Academy of Sciences USA, 91, 1121711221.
Chickadees and tits have made an unexpected con- Barnea, A. and Nottebohm, F. (1996). Recruitment and
tribution to our understanding of spatial ability and replacement of hippocampal neurons in young and
the hippocampus because of their specialized food- adult chickadees: an addition to the theory of hip-
storing behavior. The jury is still out on the ques- pocampal learning. Proceedings of the National Academy
tion of whether spatial ability or spatial cognition is of Sciences USA, 93, 714718.
specialized in chickadees and tits. Some properties Brodbeck, D. R. (1994). Memory for spatial and local cues:
a comparison of a storing and a nonstoring species.
of spatial ability and spatial memory in food-stor-
Animal Learning and Behavior, 22, 119133.
ing birds are very similar to those of other species.
Brodbeck, D. R., Burack, O. R., and Shettleworth, S. J.
Some quantitative differences in spatial ability have
(1992). One-trial associative memory in black-capped
been found in laboratory studies, however, and no chickadees. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal
laboratory studies have successfully simulated the Behavior Processes, 18, 1221.
storage and recovery of thousands of cached food Brodbeck, D. R. and Shettleworth, S. J. (1995). Matching
items as it occurs in the wild over a period of many location and color of a compound stimulus: comparison
months. One pattern that does emerge is that food- of a food-storing and nonstoring bird species. Journal of
storing birds may tend to solve foraging tasks spa- Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 21,
tially to a degree not found in other birds. 6477.
In contrast to the complex pattern of results in Brodin, A. (1992). Cache dispersion affects retrieval time
in hoarding willow tits. Ornis Scandinavica, 23, 712.
comparative studies of behavior, comparative
Brodin, A. (1994). The role of naturally stored food sup-
neuroanatomical studies of the hippocampus of
plies in the winter diet of the boreal willow tit Parus
food-storing chickadees and tits are somewhat
montanus. Ornis Svevica, 4, 3140.
clearer. Food-storing birds have a larger hippo- Brodin, A. and Ekman, J. (1994). Benefits of food hoarding.
campus than do non-storing birds, whether the Nature, 372, 510.
comparison is made within the Parids or between Brodin, A. and Lundborg, K. (2003). Is hippocampus vol-
Parids and non-storing families. The fine-scale ume affected by specialization for food hoarding in
neuroanatomical causes of this difference in size, birds? Proceedings of the Royal Society London B, 270,
its functional implications, and the role of adult 15551563.
neurogenesis remain the topics of current research. Casini, G., Bingman, V. P., and Bagnoli, P. (1986).
Connections of the pigeon dorsomedial forebrain stud-
ied with WGA-HRP and 3H-proline. Journal of
Acknowledgements Comparative Neurology, 245, 454470.
Clayton, N. S. (2001). Hippocampal growth and mainten-
Preparation of this manuscript was supported by a ance depend on food-caching experience in juvenile
Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council mountain chickadees (Poecile gambeli). Behavioural
Discovery Grant to DFS and by a Canadian Institutes Neuroscience, 115, 614625.
of Health Research Doctoral Research Award to JSH. Colombo, M. and Broadbent, N. (2000). Is the avian
We thank Erin Skinner for all of her help with prep- hippocampus a functional homologue of the mam-
aration of the manuscript and Scott MacDougall- malian hippocampus? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral
Reviews, 24, 465484.
Shackleton, Vladimir Pravosudov, and Leslie
Cowie, R. J., Krebs, J. R., and Sherry, D. F. (1981). Food
Phillmore for their comments and suggestions.
storing by marsh tits. Animal Behaviour, 29, 12521259.
Crystal, J. D. and Shettleworth, S. J. (1994). Spatial list
learning in black-capped chickadees. Animal Learning
References
and Behavior, 22, 7783.
Balda, R. P., Kamil, A. C., and Bednekoff, P. A. (1997). Erichsen, J. T., Bingman, V. P., and Krebs, J. R. (1991). The
Predicting cognitive capacities from natural history. distribution of neuropeptides in the dorsomedial telen-
Current Ornithology, 13, 333366. cephalon of the pigeon (Columba livia): a basis for
Barnea, A. and Nottebohm, F. (1994). Seasonal recruitment regional subdivisions. Journal of Comparative Neurology,
of hippocampal neurons in adult free-ranging 314, 478492.
N E U R O B I O L O G Y O F S PAT I A L B E H AV I O R 21

Galea, L. A. M. and McEwen, B. S. (1999). Sex and Healy, S. D., Clayton, N. S., and Krebs, J. R. (1994).
seasonal differences in the rate of cell proliferation in Development of hippocampal specialisation in two
the dentate gyrus of adult wild meadow voles. species of tit (Parus spp.). Behavioural Brain Research, 61,
Neuroscience, 89, 955964. 2328.
Garamszegi, L. Z. and Eens, M. (2004). The evolution of Higuchi, H. (1977). Stored nuts Castanopsis cuspidata as a
hippocampus volume and brain size in relation to food food resource of nestling varied tits Parus varius. Tori,
hoarding in birds. Ecology Letters, 7, 12161224. 26, 912.
Garamszegi, L. Z. and Lucas, J. R. (2005). Continental Hinde, R. A. (1952). The behaviour of the great tit (Parus
variation in relative hippocampal volume in birds: the major) and some other related species. Behaviour,
phylogenetic extent of the effect and the potential role Supplement 2, 1201.
of winter temperatures. Biology Letters, 1, 330333. Hitchcock, C. L. and Sherry, D. F. (1990). Long-term mem-
Gebauer, A., Jacob, J., Kaiser, M., and Eck, S. (2004). ory for cache sites in the black-capped chickadee.
Chemistry of the uropygial gland secretin of Animal Behaviour, 40, 701712.
Humes ground jay Pseudopodoces humilis and its taxo- Hoshooley, J. S., Phillmore, L. S., and MacDougall-
nomic implications. Journal fr Ornithologie, 145, Shackleton, S. A. (2005). An examination of avian hip-
352355. pocampal neurogenesis in relationship to photoperiod.
Gill, F. B., Slikas, B., and Sheldon, F. H. (2005). Phylogeny Neuroreport, 16, 987991.
of titmice (Paridae): II. Species relationships based on Hoshooley, J. S. and Sherry, D. F. (2004). Neuron produc-
sequences of the mitochondrial cytochrome-B gene. tion, neuron number, and structure size are seasonally
Auk, 122, 121143. stable in the hippocampus of the food-storing black-
Gould, K. L., Newman, S. W., Tricomi, E. M., and capped chickadee (Poecile atricapillus). Behavioral
DeVoogd, T. J. (2001). The distribution of substance P Neuroscience, 118, 345355.
and neuropeptide Y in four songbird species: A com- Hoshooley, J. S. and Sherry, D. F. (in press). Greater
parison of food-storing and non-storing birds. Brain hippocampal neuronal recruitment in food-storing than
Research, 918, 8095. in non-food-storing birds. Journal of Neurobiology.
Haftorn, S. (1956). Contribution to the food biology of tits Jacobs, L. F., Gaulin, S. J. C., Sherry, D. F., and Hoffman,
especially about storing of surplus food. Part IV. A com- G. E. (1990). Evolution of spatial cognition: sex-specific
parative analysis of Parus atricapillus L., P. cristatus L. patterns of spatial behavior predict hippocampal size.
and P. ater L. Det Kgl Norske Videnskabers Selskabs Skrifter, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 87,
154. 63496352.
Hampton, R. R. and Sherry, D. F. (1992). Food storing by Jacobs, L. F. and Schenk, F. (2003). Unpacking the
Mexican chickadees and bridled titmice. Auk, 109, cognitive map: The parallel map theory of hippocampal
665666. function. Psychological Review, 110, 285315.
Hampton, R. R., Sherry, D. F., Shettleworth, S. J., Khurgel, James, H. F., Ericson, P. G. P., Slikas, B., Lei, F.-M., Gill,
M., and Ivy, G. (1995). Hippocampal volume and food- F. B., and Olson, S. L. (2003). Pseudopodoces humilis, a
storing behavior are related in parids. Brain Behavior and misclassified terrestrial tit (Paridae) of the Tibetan
Evolution, 45, 5461. Plateau: evolutionary consequences of shifting adaptive
Hampton, R. R. and Shettleworth, S. J. (1996a). zones. Ibis, 145, 185202.
Hippocampal lesions impair memory for location but Klln, B. (1962). II. Embryogenesis of brain nuclei in the
not color in passerine birds. Behavioral Neuroscience, 110, chick telencephalon. Ergebnisse der Anatomie und
831835. Entwicklungsgeschichte, 36, 6282.
Hampton, R. R. and Shettleworth, S. J. (1996b). Karten, H. J. and Hodos, W. (1967). A Stereotaxic Atlas
Hippocampus and memory in a food-storing and in a of the Brain of the Pigeon. Johns Hopkins Press,
nonstoring bird species. Behavioral Neuroscience, 110, Baltimore, MD.
946964. Krayniak, P. F. and Siegel, A. (1978). Efferent connections
Hampton, R. R., Shettleworth, S. J., and Westwood, R. P. of the hippocampus and adjacent regions in the pigeon.
(1998). Proactive interference, recency, and associative Brain Behavior and Evolution, 15, 372388.
strength: comparisons of black-capped chickadees and Krebs, J. R., Erichsen, J. T., and Bingman, V. P. (1991). The
dark-eyed juncos. Animal Learning and Behavior, 26, distribution of neurotransmitters and neurotransmitter-
475485. related enzymes in the dorsomedial telencephalon of
Harrap, S. and Quinn, D. (1996). Tits, Nuthatches and the pigeon (Columba livia). Journal of Comparative
Treecreepers. Helm , London. Neurology, 314, 467477.
22 B E H AV I O R A N D E V O L U T I O N

Krebs, J. R., Healy, S. D., and Shettleworth, S. J. (1990). Pawluski, J. L. and Galea, L. A. M. (2006). Hippocampal
Spatial memory of Paridae: Comparison of a storing morphology is differentially affected by reproductive
and a non-storing species, the coal tit, Parus ater, and experience in the mother. Journal of Neurobiology, 66,
the great tit, P. major. Animal Behaviour, 39, 11271137. 7181.
Krebs, J. R., Sherry, D. F., Healy, S. D., Perry, V. H., and Pravosudov, V. V. (1985). Search for and storage of food by
Vaccarino, A. L. (1989). Hippocampal specialization of Parus cinctus Lapponicus and P. montanus borealis
food-storing birds. Proceedings of the National Academy of (Paridae). Zoologichesky Zhurnal, 64, 10361043.
Sciences USA, 86, 13881392. Pravosudov, V. V. (1986). Individual differences in for-
Krushinskaya, N. L. (1966). Some complex forms of feed- aging and storing behaviour in Siberian tit Parus cinctus
ing behaviour of nut-cracker Nucifraga caryocatactes, Bodd. and Willow tit Parus montanus Bald. Soviet Journal
after removal of old cortex. Zhurnal Evoluzionni of Ecology, 4, 6064.
Biochimii y Fisiloggia, II, 563568. Pravosudov, V. V. and Omanska, A. (2005). Dominance-
Kuhlenbeck, H. (1938). The ontogenetic development and related changes in spatial memory are associated
phylogenetic significance of the cortex telencephali in with changes in hippocampal cell proliferation rates
the chick. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 69, 273301. in mountain chickadees. Journal of Neurobiology, 62,
Lucas, J. R., Brodin, A. de Kort, S. R., and Clayton, N. S. 3141.
(2004). Does hippocampal size correlate with the degree Pravosudov, V. V. and Omanska, A. (2005). Prolonged
of caching specialization? Proceedings of the Royal Society moderate elevation of corticosterone does not affect
London B, 271, 24232429. hippocampal anatomy or cell proliferation rates in
Ludescher, F.-B. (1980). Fressen und Verstecken von mountain chickadees (Poecile gambeli). Journal of
Smereien be der Weidenmeise Parus montanus im Neurobiology, 62, 8291.
Jahresverlauf unter konstanten Ernhrungsbedingungen. Richards, T. J. (1958). The concealment and recovery of
kologie der Vgel, 2, 135144. food by birds, with some relevant observations on
Maguire, E. A., Spiers, H. J., Good, C. D., Hartley, T., squirrels. British Birds, 51, 497508.
Frackowiak, R. S. J., and Burgess, N. (2003). Navigation Scharff, C., Kirn, J. R., Grossman, M., Mackis, J. D., and
expertise and the human hippocampus: A structural Nottebohm, F. (2000). Targeted neuronal death affects
brain imaging analysis. Hippocampus, 13, 250259. neuronal replacement and vocal behavior in adult
Margrie, T. W., Rostas, J. A. P., and Sah, P. (1998). Long- songbirds. Neuron, 25, 481492.
term potentiation of synaptic transmission in the avian Sherry, D. F. (1984). Food storage by black-capped
hippocampus. Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 12071216. chickadees: memory for the location and contents of
Mello, C. V., Velho, T. A., and Pinaud, R. (2004). Song- caches. Animal Behaviour, 32, 451464.
induced gene expression: a window on song auditory Sherry, D. F. (1989). Food storing in the paridae. Wilson
processing and perception. Annals of the New York Bulletin, 101, 289304.
Academy of Sciences, 1016, 263281. Sherry, D. F. (2006). Neuroecology. Annual Review of
Nakamura, H. and Wako, Y. (1988). Food storing behav- Psychology, 57, 167197.
iour of willow tit Parus montanus. Journal of the Sherry, D. F., Jacobs, L. F., and Gaulin, S. J. C. (1992).
Yamashina Institute of Ornithology, 20, 120. Spatial memory and adaptive specialization of the
Nottebohm, F. (2002). Neuronal replacement in adult hippocampus. Trends in Neurosciences, 15, 298303.
brain. Brain Research Bulletin, 57, 737749. Sherry, D. F., Krebs, J. R., and Cowie, R. J. (1981). Memory
Nottebohm, F. (2002). Why are some neurons replaced in for the location of stored food in marsh tits. Animal
adult brain? Journal of Neuroscience, 22, 624628. Behaviour, 29, 12601266.
Nottebohm, F. (2004). The road we travelled. Discovery, Sherry, D. F. and Vaccarino, A. L. (1989). Hippocampus
choreography, and significance of brain replaceable and memory for food caches in black-capped chicka-
neurons. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, dees. Behavioral Neuroscience, 103, 308318.
1016, 628658. Sherry, D. F., Vaccarino, A. L., Buckenham, K., and Herz,
Patel, S. N., Clayton, N. S., and Krebs, J. R. (1997). R. S. (1989). The hippocampal complex of food-storing
Hippocampal tissue transplants reverse lesion-induced birds. Brain Behavior and Evolution, 34, 308317.
spatial memory deficits in zebra finches (Taeniopygia Shettleworth, S. D. and Krebs, J. R. (1986). Stored and
guttata). Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 38613869. encountered seeds: A comparison of two spatial
Patel, S. N., Clayton, N. S., and Krebs, J. R. (1997). Spatial memory tasks in marsh tits and chickadees. Journal of
learning induces neurogenesis in the avian brain. Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 12,
Behavioural Brain Research, 89, 115128. 248257.
N E U R O B I O L O G Y O F S PAT I A L B E H AV I O R 23

Shettleworth, S. J. and Krebs, J. R. (1982). How marsh tits of freely moving homing pigeons. Hippocampus, 15,
find their hoards: the roles of site preference and spatial 2640.
memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Smulders, T. V. and DeVoogd, T. J. (2000). Expression of
Behavior Processes, 8, 354375. immediate early genes in the hippocampal formation of
Shettleworth, S. J., Krebs, J. R., Healy, S. D., and Thomas, the black-capped chickadee (Poecile atricapillus) during
C. M. (1990). Spatial memory of food-storing tits (Parus a food-hoarding task. Behavioral Brain Research, 114,
ater and P. atricapillus): Comparison of storing and non- 3949.
storing tasks. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 104, Southern, J. B. (1946). Unusual feeding behaviour of tits.
7181. British Birds, 39, 214.
Shettleworth, S. J. and Westwood, R. P. (2002). Divided Stevens, T. A. and Krebs, J. R. (1986). Retrieval of stored
attention, memory, and spatial discrimination in food- seeds by marsh tits Parus palustris in the field. Ibis, 128,
storing and nonstoring birds, black-capped chickadees 513525.
(Poecile atricapilla) and dark-eyed juncos (Junco hye- Stewart, M. G., Cristol, D., Philips, R., Steele, R. J.,
malis). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Stamatakis, A., Harrison, E., and Clayton, N. (1999). A
Behavior Processes, 28, 227241. quantitative autoradiographic comparison of binding
Shiflett, M. W., Smulders, T. V., Benedict, L., and to glutamate receptor sub-types in hippocampus and
DeVoogd, T. J. (2003). Reversible inactivation of the hip- forebrain regions of a food-storing and non-food-stor-
pocampal formation in food-storing black-capped ing bird. Behavioural Brain Research, 98, 8994.
chickadees (Poecile atricapillus). Hippocampus, 13, Stokes, T. M., Leonard, C. M., and Nottebohm, F. (1974).
437444. The telencephalon, diencephalon, and mesencephalon
Shiflett, M. W., Tomaszycki, M. L., Rankin, A. Z., and of the canary Serinus canarius in stereotaxic coordinates.
DeVoogd, T. J. (2004). Long-term memory for spatial Journal of Comparative Neurology, 156, 337374.
locations in a food-storing bird (Poecile atricapilla) Szekely, A. D. (1999). The avian hippocampal formation:
requires activation of NMDA receptors in the hip- subdivisions and connectivity. Behavioural Brain
pocampal formation during learning. Behavioral Research, 98, 219225.
Neuroscience, 118, 121130. Tramontin, A. D. and Brenowitz, E. A. (2000). Seasonal
Siegel, J. J., Nitz, D., and Bingman, V. P. (2002). plasticity in the adult brain. Trends in Neurosciences, 23,
Electophysiological profile of avian hippocampal unit 251258.
activity: A basis for regional subdivisions. Journal of Watanabe, S. and Bischof, H. (2004). Effects of hippocam-
Comparative Neurology, 445, 256268. pal lesions on acquisition and retention of spatial learn-
Siegel, J. J., Nitz, D., and Bingman, V. P. (2005). Spatial- ing in zebra finches. Behavioural Brain Research, 155,
specificity of single-units in the hippocampal formation 147152.
This page intentionally left blank
CHAPTER 3

The relationship between


environment, corticosterone, food
caching, spatial memory, and the
hippocampus in chickadees
Vladimir V. Pravosudov

3.1 Introduction 1995; Chapter 2). Glucocorticoid hormones are also


known to affect spatial memory (Sapolsky 1996;
Many species in the family Paridae are well known
McEwen and Sapolsky 1995; McEwen 2000) and
for their food-caching behavior (Haftorn 1956;
thus the local environment might influence spatial
Pravosudov 1985; Brodin and Lundborg 2003; Lucas
memory via changes in glucocorticoid hormones
et al. 2004). All North American Parids have been
levels (Pravosudov 2003). Here, I review studies
reported to cache food whereas in Eurasia there are
dealing with the relationship between environment,
a few species that do not cache (Brodin and
food caching, spatial memory, corticosterone, and
Lundborg 2003a; Lucas et al. 2004). Natural food
the hippocampus in North American chickadees
caching in free-ranging Parids has been studied most
while providing comparisons with similar studies
intensively in Eurasian specieswillow, coal,
done using Eurasian Parids.
crested, marsh, and Siberian tits (Haftorn 1956;
Pravosudov 1985; Brodin 1994)whereas data on
natural food caching in North American Parids are 3.2 Inter- and intraspecific differences in
extremely scant (Odum 1942; Brodin 2005). Some caching, spatial memory, and the
well-studied Eurasian Parids (willow and Siberian hippocampus
tits) have been reported to make more than a 100 000
3.2.1 Interspecific differences
individual food caches each autumn (Haftorn 1956;
Pravosudov 1985; Brodin 1994) to be used through- Almost all food-caching birds live in environments
out the winter. Food caching thus appears to be an with pronounced differences between seasons in
important adaptation, promoting the birds survival accessibility of food. As a result, the benefits of
during the winter (e.g. Pravosudov and Lucas 2001), strong spatial memory and food caching might
and it is important to understand how these birds typically be higher during the winter when envir-
retrieve their caches. It is known that food-caching onmental conditions are most energetically
birds rely, at least in part, on spatial memory to find demanding. Also, some species experience more
their caches (review in Shettleworth 1995; Chapter 2) severe environmental conditions than others.
and research on spatial memory and the hippocam- Consequently, the advantages of food caching
pus, a part of the brain involved in spatial memory should be much higher for species living in more
processing (Sherry and Vaccarino 1989; Hampton energetically demanding environments, resulting
and Shettleworth 1996a, b), has focused on both in higher selection pressure for more caching and
Eurasian and North American Parids (Shettleworth more efficient cache recovery (Krebs et al. 1989,

25
26 B E H AV I O R A N D E V O L U T I O N

1996). It has been hypothesized that the increased North America all comparative studies focused on
demands for the spatial memory needed to recover food-caching black-capped chickadees and non-
thousands of previously made food caches has caching dark-eyed juncos (Junco hyemalis;
resulted in enhanced spatial memory and enlarged Shettleworth 1995). The results of these compari-
hippocampus in food-caching birdscoined the sons were not consistent; in the majority of tests,
adaptive specialization hypothesis (Krebs et al. food-caching birds outperformed non-caching
1989, 1996; Sherry et al. 1989; Chapter 2). This species (Biegler et al. 2001; Shettleworth 1995), but
hypothesis has been mainly tested using species some comparisons yielded non-significant results
from two avian familiesthe Paridae and (see Shettleworth 1995 for review) and in one study
Corvidaeas both of these families contain food- non-caching dark-eyed juncos outperformed food-
caching and non-caching species (Sherry et al. 1989; caching black-capped chickadees on one spatial
Krebs et al. 1989, 1996; Chapter 2). Here I will briefly memory task (Hampton and Shettleworth 1996b).
review the comparisons using Parids. Hampton et al. (1995) compared three North
Healy and Krebs (1996) reported that food- American Paridsblack-capped chickadees,
caching Eurasian Parids have relatively larger Mexican chickadees, and bridled titmicewhich
hippocampal volume compared to non-caching appear to differ in their food-caching behavior. Of
species. Brodin and Lundborg (2003a) analyzed a these three species, black-capped chickadees have a
large multispecies data set, including both Eurasian more northerly distribution and are well known
and North American Parids (the latter of whom all food-cachers (Brodin 2005). Mexican chickadees and
cache) as well as many other caching and non- bridled titmice, on the other hand, occupy a more
caching species, and found little evidence that the southerly range and have not been observed to cache
degree of caching specialization is related to hip- food in natural conditions (Hampton et al. 1995). In
pocampal volume. Lucas et al. (2004) reanalyzed captivity, however, both Mexican chickadees and
Brodin and Lundborgs (2003a) data set, introducing bridled titmice cached food, indicating that caching
a new variablethe continent on which the birds behavior is present in these two species (Hampton
occurredand their analysis supported the adaptive and Sherry 1992; Hampton et al. 1995). Hampton
specialization hypothesis. The different conclusions et al. (1995) hypothesized that the more southerly
reflected an apparent difference in hippocampal and Mexican chickadees and bridled titmice do not have
brain volumes between Eurasian and North a strong dependence on cached food for their
American Parids; once these differences were statis- survival because of their relatively mild, southern
tically controlled, the relative hippocampal volume environmental conditions and, as a result, they
appeared to be larger in food-caching Parids (Lucas should cache less food and have smaller hippocampi
et al. 2004, see also Chapter 2). Lucas et al. (2004) than black-capped chickadees. Indeed, Hampton
suggested that compared to North American et al. (1995) found that black-capped chickadees have
species, relative hippocampus and overall brain size relatively larger hippocampi than both Mexican
is larger in Eurasian birds, possibly due to different chickadees and bridled titmice but the data on food
environmental conditions (Garamszegi and Lucas caching were ambiguous. In two captive experiments
2005; Chapter 2) but it remains possible, however, there were no significant differences in caching rates
that these continental differences could be due to between black-capped and Mexican chickadees. In
differences between laboratories in processing and one of the two experiments, there were also no
analyzing the brains (Pravosudov and de Kort 2006). statistical differences between all three species in the
While differences in hippocampal volume may amount of food caching (Hampton et al. 1995).
indicate differences in the potential for spatial
memory performance, behavioral tests to confirm
3.2.2 Intraspecific differences
that these differences occur have focused on only a
handful of species. In Europe, most comparisons In all interspecific comparisons employed to
concerned food-caching marsh and coal tits and test the adaptive specialization hypothesis, the
non-caching blue tits (Shettleworth 1995) while in main underlying assumption was that the biggest
THE HIPPOCAMPUS IN CHICKADEES 27

difference between the species lies in their food- (e.g. Pravosudov and Lucas 2001) and, in December,
caching behavior (Krebs et al. 1989; Sherry et al. Alaskan chickadees have only about 5 hours of
1989; Brodin and Lundborg 2003a; Lucas et al. 2004; daylight to obtain enough energy reserves to survive
Chapter 2). However, it is possible that there are about 19 hours of night. In Colorado, on the other
other differences between species, unrelated to hand, these birds have almost twice as much daylight
caching behavior, that could have resulted in differ- hours to complete the same foraging task while prob-
ential selection pressure on both spatial memory ably requiring less energy due to generally warmer
and the hippocampus. Comparing different popu- night-time temperatures (Pravosudov and Clayton
lations within the same species might provide an 2002). In both environments, caches should provide a
opportunity to control for such differences. more predictable food source than naturally available
Many food-caching Parids occupy extremely wide food. Having sufficient food caches, and the capacity
ranges, extending from south to north (Harrap and to successfully find them when needed should thus
Quinn 1995) with greatly varying environmental have much larger survival consequences for chicka-
conditions, especially during the winter. The range of dees in energetically demanding Alaskan conditions.
black-capped chickadees, for example, extends from Such differences in the significance of cached food
New Mexico in the south to Alaska in the north between southern and northern populations are
(Harrap and Quinn 1995). Compared to more likely to provide higher selection pressure in north-
southerly populations, Alaskan black-capped chicka- ern populations for more intense food caching, better
dees regularly experience longer winters with lower spatial memory, and larger hippocampi.
ambient temperatures, deeper and longer lasting When black-capped chickadees from Alaska and
snow cover, and, most importantly, much shorter Colorado were acclimatized for 45 days and then
day-length available for foraging during winter tested in identical laboratory conditions, birds
(Pravosudov and Clayton 2002). Obtaining enough from Alaska cached significantly more food and
food to build energy reserves sufficient to survive performed significantly better on a spatial version of
the night is crucial for survival in these small birds a one-trial associative learning task (Fig. 3.1;

(a) (b)

Alaska Alaska
8
Mean number of sites inspected

Colorado
Colorado 6

6
Number of seeds

4
4

2
2

0 0
Consumed Cached 70 sites 15 sites Color

Figure 3.1 Mean number of seeds consumed (fully or partially) and cached during 20-min food-caching trials (a) and memory performance in a
one-trial associative learning task (b) by black-capped chickadees from Alaska (black bars) and Colorado (hatched bars) (redrawn from Pravosudov
and Clayton 2002). Memory performance was measured as the number of sites inspected ( number of looks) in order to find the site previously
containing food among either 70 or 15 available sites. Fewer looks indicate better memory performance. A color version of a one-trial associative
learning task was used to investigate possible motivational differences between the groups. If one group were less motivated to search for food, it
would show similar poor performance on both spatial and color versions of the task. If there were differences in spatial memory between the
groups, there would be differences in performance on spatial but not on color version of a one-trial associative learning task.
28 B E H AV I O R A N D E V O L U T I O N

Pravosudov and Clayton 2002). Interestingly, experience in the laboratory. A separate analysis of a
Alaskan chickadees were significantly smaller, with control group of birds, which did not participate in
smaller brains, than chickadees from Colorado the experiment while being maintained in the
(Fig. 3.2; Pravosudov and Clayton 2002). Compared laboratory for the same duration of time, revealed no
to Colorado chickadees, however, individuals from differences in the hippocampal volume or neuron
Alaska had significantly larger hippocampi with numbers between the individuals that cached food
more neurons in both absolute terms and relative to and the ones that did not cache food (Pravosudov
telencephalon volume (Fig. 3.2; Pravosudov and and Clayton 2002). At the same time, the differences
Clayton 2002). These results support the adaptive between Alaska and Colorado remained even in
specialization hypothesis; because Alaskan individuals that did not cache food during the
chickadees exist in a more energetically demanding experiment, thus ruling out a possibility that larger
environment, they cache more food, and have hippocampus with more neurons in Alaskan chicka-
better spatial memorywhich may be associated dees resulted from more caching during the experi-
with their larger hippocampi containing more ment. It remains possible, however, that differences
neuronscompared to conspecifics from Colorado between Alaska and Colorado chickadees in food
(Pravosudov and Clayton 2002). caching and cache retrieval experience prior to cap-
Based on the above data, however, it was not ture produced long-term changes in memory and
possible to unambiguously conclude whether the the hippocampus and thus further testing is neces-
differences in caching, spatial memory, and hip- sary to differentiate between a genetic versus envi-
pocampal volume were genetic or experience based. ronmental basis of population differences.
Birds from both Alaska and Colorado were main- Irrespective of the basis of these differences
tained and tested in identical laboratory environ- between Alaskan and Colorado black-capped
ments, suggesting that the differences found chickadees, more food caching, better spatial
between these groups were not related to the imme- memory, and an enlarged hippocampus with more
diate environment. Alaskan chickadees cached more neurons in Alaskan individuals appear to be
food and thus they had more food-caching highly adaptive to life in extremely energetically

20
Volume of the hippocampal formation (mm3)

18

16

14

12

10

8
180 200 220 240 260 280 300 320 340 360 380 400
Volume of the remainder of telencephalon (mm3)

Figure 3.2 The relationship between the hippocampal volume and the volume of the rest of the telencephalon in black-capped chickadees from
Alaska (open circles) and Colorado (filled circles) (redrawn from Pravosudov and Clayton 2002).
THE HIPPOCAMPUS IN CHICKADEES 29

challenging environments. More research is needed underlying spatial memory function. Because birds
to ascertain whether these differences have a that cache more food appear to have larger hippo-
genetic basis. Similar intraspecific studies may also campi, it is possible to predict that if there is
be possible with tufted titmice, Carolina chicka- seasonal variation in spatial memory it may be
dees, Siberian and willow tits, or high-elevation accompanied by variation in hippocampal volume
populations of mountain chickadees, which would and/or neuron numbers.
test the generality of environmental selection on Despite numerous suggestions that spatial mem-
caching behavior across species. ory might vary seasonally, there have been no stud-
ies documenting such variation. Whereas it may be
difficult to measure seasonal variation in spatial
3.3 Unpredictable food, corticosterone, memory, it is possible to test whether any of the
spatial memory, and the hippocampus parameters associated with seasonal changes in the
3.3.1 Seasonality in food caching and environment, such as photoperiod or food supply,
spatial memory have an effect on spatial memory.

Most food-caching Parids live in highly seasonal envi-


3.3.2 Seasonal variation in hippocampus
ronments; food is abundant during summer and early
volume, neuron number, and neurogenesis
autumn but during the winter it is often limited and
rates
unpredictable (Pravosudov and Grubb 1997a). Food-
caching patterns mostly follow a seasonal cycle; birds Smulders et al. (1995, 2000) reported that hippocam-
cache most when food is plentiful outside the breed- pal volume and neuron numbers change seasonally,
ing seasonlate summer and early autumn and, with the largest hippocampal volume and most
sometimes, in spring when there is abundance of neurons occurring during October. Smulders et al.
conifer seeds (Haftorn 1956; Pravosudov 1985; Brodin (1995, 2000) hypothesized that hippocampal volume
1994). During these periods, birds can cache thou- should track seasonally changing demands for spa-
sands of food items (Haftorn 1956; Pravosudov 1985; tial memory and that the maximum hippocampal
Brodin 1994). These food caches appear to be crucial volume should be observed in the autumn when
for winter survival and thus the ability to retrieve food caching is most intense (Haftorn 1956;
them should be of paramount importance. Parids Pravosudov 1985; Brodin 2005). Other food-caching
continue to cache throughout the winter although Parids, however, do not rely on food caches during
they cache a fairly small amount of food during that the period when caching rates are at maximum
time (Pravosudov 1985). It is not clear whether winter because natural food is relatively abundant during
caching involves storing newly found food or this period (Haftorn 1956; Pravosudov 1985). This
recaching previously hidden food stores (Pravosudov appears to be also true for North American Parids
and Grubb 1997a), but winter short-term caching (Brodin 2005). Successful retrieval of food caches
might be important for the birds survival on a daily may be more crucial later during the winter when
basis (Pravosudov and Lucas 2001). failure to find caches might result in increased mor-
In seasonal environments, demands for spatial tality (Pravosudov and Lucas 2001).
memory do not appear to be equal throughout the Smulders and Dhondt (1997) hypothesized that
year. If a bird does not remember where it stored food-caching Parids might specifically need extra
food during summer and early autumn when nat- memory during food caching in order to memorize
ural food is abundant, it is unlikely to die of starva- cache locations and to create optimal cache distri-
tion. During the winter, however, failure to recover bution that minimize cache pilferage. Based on
food caches is more likely to result in mortality and these assumptions, they suggest hippocampal
thus demands for spatial memory should be higher volume might be largest in October, when food
during the winter (Pravosudov and Clayton 2001). caching was assumed to be most intense. Shiflett
Thus there might be seasonal variation in the birds et al. (2003) argued that the hippocampus is
spatial memory abilities and also in mechanisms involved specifically in memory encoding, which
30 B E H AV I O R A N D E V O L U T I O N

occurs during food caching and that the hippocam- from October injections, rather than the number of
pus might not be involved in memory recall during neurons actually produced in October. Such a result
cache retrieval. Clayton (2001), on the other hand, can be achieved through increased neuron produc-
showed that young mountain chickadees that were tion, increased survival of new neurons, or both
allowed to cache, but not to retrieve, seeds had (Prickaerts et al. 2004; Chapter 2). Hoosholey and
smaller relative hippocampal volume than chicka- Sherry (2004) found no significant seasonal variation
dees that both cached and retrieved seeds. This in neuron production rates in the hippocampus of
result seems to suggest that retrieving caches might black-capped chickadees, suggesting that Barnea
provide a stronger impact on hippocampal struc- and Nottebohms (1994) results might be explained
ture than food caching alone. by higher survival rates of neurons that were born in
Another important detail is that, so far, there is October. Higher survival rates of new neurons
little good data on seasonal variation in food- coupled with stable total neuron numbers suggest
caching rates in North American Parids that neuron turn over rates could potentially be
(Pravosudov 2006). A single study showed that lower during October. Neurons born in October may
black-capped chickadees cached most food at the not be immediately functional, as it takes some time
end of September but no observations were avail- for the neurons to mature and to incorporate into the
able for August or early September (Brodin 2005). existing neuronal circuits in the hippocampus. Thus,
Much more extensive data on several Eurasian these neurons may become functional only by the
food-caching Parids show strong variation in food- end of OctoberNovember and, because they appear
caching rates; often these Parids cached most in to survive longer, these new neurons could remain
September and cached as much food in August as in functional in December and later. This might shift
October (Pravosudov 2006). In addition, some the significance of these neurons from October, one
Eurasian Parids might also cache food intensely of the months when food caching is often assumed
during spring (Pravosudov 2006). Although North to be most intense, to NovemberDecember when
American Parids might have different seasonal food caching might be reduced (Haftorn 1956;
food-caching patterns it remains difficult to inter- Pravosudov 1985) and cache retrieval becomes cru-
pret data on seasonal changes in hippocampal struc- cial for survival. Neurons born in December were
ture without knowing the precise patterns of reported to have lower survival (Barnea and
food-caching activity. Nottebohm 1994), but these neurons would most
Studies on seasonal variation in hippocampal vol- likely become functional only in JanuaryFebruary.
ume (Hoshooley and Sherry 2004) or total number of Further research is necessary to understand
neurons (Barnea and Nottebohm 1994; Hoshooley whether hippocampal structure and neurogenesis
and Sherry 2004), however, have also not found sup- consistently undergo seasonal changes specifically
port for seasonal differences (see also Chapter 2). associated with changes in food-caching activity. In
Barnea and Nottebohm (1994) did report seasonal particular, it would be important to investigate
variation in hippocampal neuron recruitment rates whether hippocampal structure and neurogenesis
in black-capped chickadees; more new neurons were vary in individual birds in relation to their specific
observed in the hippocampus of birds injected with food-caching behavior. More data are also neces-
a cell division marker during October than at any sary to document variation in seasonal changes in
other time of year. Barnea and Nottebohm (1994) food-caching behavior.
concluded that neurogenesis supports memory
function because the highest neuron recruitment
3.3.3 Photoperiod
rates coincided with the predicted highest food-
caching rates during October, but they did not pres- Seasonality is associated with predictable changes
ent data on the actual variation in caching rates of in photoperiod and thus it is possible that photo-
birds during these time frames. Further, their results period could trigger seasonal changes, if any, in food
do contain some ambiguity, as they measured the caching, spatial memory, and the hippocampus
number of neurons which survived at least 6 weeks (Chapter 4). Food caching is often most intensive
THE HIPPOCAMPUS IN CHICKADEES 31

during autumn when day-length becomes shorter; 1997a). Food is usually abundant in summer and
however, intensive caching has also been reported autumn but becomes much less abundant during the
during spring when day-length actually becomes winter. Winter is also the most energetically
longer (Haftorn 1956; Pravosudov 1985, 2006). Most demanding season because of short days and low air
laboratory manipulations of photoperiod have temperature, and successful retrieval of food caches
demonstrated that black-capped chickadees and becomes critically important during the winter
marsh tits cache more food on short days (Krebs (Pravosudov and Lucas 2001). Thus food availability
et al. 1995; Shettleworth et al. 1995; Clayton and and predictability might directly affect food caching
Cristol 1996), suggesting that decreasing day-length and cache retrieval (Pravosudov and Clayton 2001).
may be responsible for the autumnal peak in food When food supply was made limited and unpre-
caching (but Karpouzos et al. (2005) reported no dictable in a laboratory for 60 days, mountain
effect of photoperiod on food caching in black- chickadees demonstrated more efficient cache
capped chickadees). However, these results do not recovery (by searching significantly fewer available
explain spring bursts of food caching and thus it cache sites) and showed better spatial memory
remains possible that seasonal peaks in long-term performance compared to individuals maintained
caching are not necessarily under photoperiodic on ad libitum food (Fig. 3.3; Pravosudov and
control (Pravosudov 2006). Clayton 2001). Thus, it appears that chickadees
It is also not clear whether spatial memory is might have better spatial memory, resulting in
affected by photoperiod. Clayton and Cristol (1996) more efficient cache retrieval, during the winter
reported that marsh tits switched from long to short when available food is rather scarce. To determine
days showed better spatial memory performance whether better performance on memory tests by
than their conspecifics maintained on long days. But food-deprived chickadees was associated with bet-
when birds maintained on short, winter-like days ter spatial memory and not with higher levels of
were changed to long days and the birds maintained motivation to search for food, Pravosudov and
on long days were switched to short days, there were Clayton (2001) used an associative learning task in
no differences between the groups in spatial memory which a site containing food was associated with a
performance, even though the birds on short days specific color (local) cues. If non-deprived chicka-
were caching more food. If photoperiod was dees were less motivated to search for food, they
involved in spatial memory regulation, birds on short would show similar performance on both spatial
days should have shown better memory perform- and color-associative learning tasks. Pravosudov
ance, which did not happen during the latter test. and Clayton (2001) found that differences in mem-
Experimental manipulation of photoperiod ory performance between deprived and non-
also failed to induce significant changes in the deprived individuals were present only in the
hippocampal volume in black-capped chickadees spatial test whereas in the non-spatial, color test the
(Krebs et al. 1995; MacDougall-Shackleton et al. 2003) groups showed similar performance (Fig. 3.3). Such
and baseline corticosterone levels were statistically results suggested that the differences between
indistinguishable between mountain chickadees food-deprived and non-deprived chickadees were
maintained on short and long days (Pravosudov most likely due to differences in spatial memory
et al. 2002a). Thus whereas photoperiod appears to and not in motivation to search for food.
affect food-caching rates in captive birds, there Hippocampal volume and the total number of
appears to be insufficient evidence that it has an neurons, however, were statistically indistinguish-
effect on spatial memory and the hippocampus. able between mountain chickadees maintained on
limited and unpredictable food supply and those
on ad libitum food supply (Pravosudov et al. 2002),
3.3.4 Unpredictable food supply
which suggests that changes in spatial memory
Most food-caching Parids live in the temperate zone triggered by changes in food supply were not sup-
and experience seasonal variation in food availabil- ported by structural changes in the hippocampus.
ity and predictability (Pravosudov and Grubb Interestingly, baseline corticosterone levels were
32 B E H AV I O R A N D E V O L U T I O N

(a) (b)
Unpredictable food

Mean number of sites inspected


Ad libitum food
6
Corticosterone (ng/ml)

2 4

1 2

0 0
Unpredictable Ad libitum 144 sites 12 sites color
food food

Figure 3.3 Baseline corticosterone levels (a) and memory performance in a one-trial associative learning task (b) in mountain chickadees
maintained on limited and unpredictable food schedule (black bars) and on ad libitum food (hatched bars). ((a) is reprinted from General and
Comparative Endocrinology, 123, Pravosudov et al. Long-term unpredictable foraging conditions and physiological stress response in mountain
chickadees (Poecile gambeli), pp. 324331, Copyright (2001), with permission from Elsevier; b is redrawn from Pravosudov and Clayton 2001).
Memory performance was measured as the number of sites inspected in order to find a single site previously containing food among either 144
or 12 available sites. Fewer sites inspected indicate better memory performance. A color version of a one-trial associative learning task was used
to investigate possible motivational differences between the groups (see also legend Fig. 3.1).

significantly elevated in birds maintained on corticosterone has an effect on memory in birds


limited food (Fig. 3.3; Pravosudov et al. 2001). Thus (Loscertales et al. 1997; Sandi et al. 1995; Sandi and
relatively long-term (2 months) limited and unpre- Rose 1997). Saldanha et al. (2000) reported that
dictable food supply resulted in moderately but short-term, strong corticosterone elevation resulted
significantly elevated baseline corticosterone and in in improved spatial memory performance in moun-
enhanced spatial memory. Such results suggested tain chickadees. Chickadees in the Saldanha et al.
that elevated corticosterone might have been (2000) study received extremely high doses of
directly responsible for spatial memory improve- corticosterone which likely resulted in unnaturally
ments observed in mountain chickadees in the high corticosterone plasma levels (Pravosudov
absence of any changes in hippocampal volume or 2005); it thus remains unclear whether naturally
neuron numbers. occurring corticosterone elevations would provide
similar effects on memory.
Unlike short-term (hours or maybe days),
3.4 Corticosterone, food caching, spatial
chronic (weeks or months) corticosterone eleva-
memory, and the hippocampus
tions have been reported to result in memory
It is well known that corticosterone has an effect on impairments (Sapolsky 1996; McEwen and
memory function (Sapolsky 1996; McEwen and Sapolsky 1995; McEwen 2000). It appears, however,
Sapolsky 1995; McEwen 2000). It has generally been that baseline corticosterone levels may remain
assumed that short-term (hours, days) elevations elevated in wintering Parids over several months
in corticosterone enhance spatial memory whereas (Silverin 1998willow tits), which would fall
prolonged, chronic (weeks) elevations might have under the long-term category for these rather short-
extremely deleterious effects. Most of the research lived birds. Experimental manipulations of food
addressing the relationship between corticosterone supply designed to decrease food predictability
elevation and memory has been focused on and/or availability also triggered a prolonged
mammals (McEwen and Sapolsky 1995; McEwen elevation of baseline corticosterone in mountain
2000) but there are also studies suggesting that chickadees (Pravosudov et al. 2001) and in red
THE HIPPOCAMPUS IN CHICKADEES 33

knots (Calidris canutus; Reneerkens et al. 2002). It is increase birds probability of survival when envi-
important to note that such elevations were only ronmental conditions are energetically demanding,
moderate and they exceeded the baseline levels such as during the winter. These results suggest
only two or three times compared to stress-induced that prolonged but moderately elevated cortico-
levels which might exceed baseline concentration sterone levels may be highly adaptive by enhancing
by more than 10 times (Silverin 1998; Wingfield memory rather than impairing it, as it has been
et al. 1997, 1998). widely suggested (Sapolsky 1996; McEwen and
Thus it is not clear whether long-term but mod- Sapolsky 1995; McEwen 2000).
erate corticosterone elevations are necessarily asso- Elevated corticosterone might have a positive
ciated with the same negative effects on memory effect on memory through several possible mechan-
and the brain as long-term, stress-induced-like high isms including elevated glucose levels, enhanced
elevations. It appears that prolonged, moderate protein synthesis in the brain, and increased
corticosterone elevations might occur when birds hippocampal neuron firing rates (see review in
face both the highest demands for feeding effi- Pravosudov 2005). Interestingly, hippocampal
ciency in order to survive and when memory for neuron firing rates seem to have an inverted-U
the location of food caches could also be most crit- relationship with corticosterone levels (Diamond
ical for survival (Pravosudov and Clayton 2001; et al. 1992), which is in agreement with our results
Pravosudov 2005). Foraging conditions that caused suggesting that moderate corticosterone elevations
long-term (2 months), moderate corticosterone might enhance spatial memory.
elevation in mountain chickadees also resulted in The fact that birds do not always maintain mod-
enhanced spatial memory and more efficient cache erately elevated corticosterone levels despite its
retrieval (Pravosudov and Clayton 2001), a result apparent benefits suggests that even moderate cor-
potentially contradicting a conventional assump- ticosterone elevations carry costs (e.g. weakening
tion that any chronic elevation in glucocorticoids immune function, Wingfield et al. 1997, 1998). These
might be deleterious for memory function costs, however, do not seem to concern memory
(Sapolsky 1996; McEwen and Sapolsky 1995; function.
McEwen 2000) and suggesting that elevated It has been suggested that chronically elevated
corticosterone might have actually caused the corticosterone might impair memory through
observed memory improvements. increased neuronal death but most experimental
When given corticosterone implants designed to studies used high doses of corticosterone resulting
maintain moderate corticosterone elevations (about in unnaturally high elevations (e.g. 10 baseline)
1.5 times higher than baseline; resembling those (Sapolsky 1996; McEwen and Sapolsky 1995;
observed in natural conditions during winter McEwen 2000). Such elevations might not necessar-
(Silverin 1998) and in birds maintained on unpre- ily occur under naturally stressful conditions and it
dictable food supply (Pravosudov et al. 2001)) for is important to understand the consequences of
several weeks, mountain chickadees demonstrated naturally occurring corticosterone elevations.
better spatial memory performance compared Prolonged, moderately elevated corticosterone
to placebo-implanted individuals (Fig. 3.4b, levels associated with unpredictable food had no
Pravosudov 2003), supporting a link between detectable effects on hippocampal structure
elevated corticosterone and spatial memory. (Pravosudov et al. 2002b). Analogously, there were
Furthermore, compared to placebo-implanted no significant differences between corticosterone-
birds, corticosterone-implanted chickadees fed and placebo-implanted mountain chickadees in
more intensively and cached significantly more either hippocampal volume, total number of hip-
food (Fig. 3.4a, Pravosudov 2003). Thus, long-term, pocampal neurons, or cell proliferation rates in the
moderately elevated corticosterone might facilitate ventricular zone adjacent to the hippocampus and
more foraging and food caching while enhancing to the mesopallium (Pravosudov and Omanska
the spatial memory required for successful cache 2005a). Thus relatively long-term (49 days with cor-
retrieval. This entire suite of behaviors is likely to ticosterone implants and 94 days on limited and
34 B E H AV I O R A N D E V O L U T I O N

(a) (b)

Corticosterone Corticosterone
15 Placebo
Placebo

Mean number of sites inspected


12
Number of nuts

10
8

5 4

0 0
Cached Consumed Location Color Location
(precolor) (postcolor)

Figure 3.4 Mean number of consumed (fully or partially) and cached pine nuts during food-caching trials (a) and memory performance in a
one-trial associative learning task (b) in mountain chickadees implanted with corticosterone (black bars) or placebo (hatched bars) (redrawn from
Pravosudov 2003). Memory performance was measured as the number of sites inspected in order to find the site previously containing food.
Fewer sites inspected indicate better memory performance. A color version of a one-trial associative learning task was used to investigate
possible motivational differences between corticosterone- and placebo-implanted chickadees.

unpredictable food supply) moderate corticosterone


3.4.1 Corticosterone in Alaskan and Colorado
elevation, similar to the elevation observed in
chickadees
chickadees maintained on unpredictable food
schedule, had no detectable effect on hippocampal Black-capped chickadees in Alaska regularly experi-
structure and cell proliferation rates. It has also been ence more energetically challenging environmen-
reported that prolonged stress and associated tal conditions than chickadees in Colorado
prolonged corticosterone elevation result in reduced (Pravosudov and Clayton 2002). Such conditions
neurogenesis rates (Gould and Tanapat 1999). are likely to effect availability and predictability of
Neurogenesis consists of cell proliferation and food supply during the winter, and limited food
neuron survival (Prickaerts et al. 2004; Chapter 2) supply is known to affect corticosterone levels
and thus it remains possible that even moderately in mountain chickadees (Pravosudov 2003).
elevated corticosterone levels might negatively Moderately elevated corticosterone appears to pro-
impact neuron survival rates. vide benefits to wintering birds by mediating more
Experimental results with mountain chickadees intense foraging and food caching and by enhan-
thus suggest that relatively long-term, moderate cing spatial memory, which plays an important
corticosterone elevations in response to challenging role in cache retrieval (Pravosudov 2003). Whereas
environmental conditions might be adaptive, at it appears that even moderately elevated cortico-
least in wintering food-caching birds. When for- sterone levels might carry some costs (Wingfield
aging conditions deteriorate, corticosterone levels et al. 1997, 1998), for birds experiencing energet-
might rise, facilitating more active foraging behav- ically demanding environments every year the
ior and more food caching while enhancing spatial benefits of elevated corticosterone might outweigh
memory which is important for successful cache possible costs and thus selection may favor
retrieval. When environmental conditions improve, elevated corticosterone.
corticosterone levels return to normal suggesting There were, however, no significant differences
that corticosterone elevation has some costs, which in either baseline or stress-induced corticosterone
might be outweighed by the benefits during ener- levels between Alaskan and Colorado chickadees,
getically demanding periods. which indicates that differences in memory found
THE HIPPOCAMPUS IN CHICKADEES 35

between these two populations were not related to higher levels of fat reserves than dominants
changes in corticosterone levels (Pravosudov et al. (Ekman and Lilliendahl 1993willow tits; Chapter
2004). The fact that Alaskan and Colorado chicka- 9black-capped chickadees), but other studies
dees did not differ in their corticosterone levels in found either a reverse pattern (Koivula et al. 1995
identical laboratory conditions also suggests that willow tits; Verhulst and Hogstad 1996willow
different environmental conditions did not cause tits; Pravosudov and Lucas 2000Carolina chick-
permanently elevated corticosterone levels in adees) or no differences between dominants and
Alaskan birds. Instead, it appears that baseline cor- subordinates (Pravosudov and Lucas 2000
ticosterone levels might only respond to immediate Carolina chickadees).
environmental conditions in both populations. In addition to storing energy reserves as body fat,
Compared to birds in Colorado, corticosterone food-caching birds can also accumulate and main-
levels in Alaskan environments may be elevated in tain energy reserves as food caches. Birds usually
black-capped chickadees as a result of differences increase their caching rates when food becomes less
in immediate conditions between Alaska and predictable (Pravosudov and Grubb 1997), thus the
Colorado during the winter. Such elevation (if it idea that subordinates should maintain more
exists) may aid spatial memory benefits already energy reserves to buffer unpredictable conditions
achieved through enlarged hippocampus could be extended to food caching. Brodin et al.
(Pravosudov and Clayton 2002). Whereas the vol- (2001) used a dynamic modeling approach to inves-
ume of the hippocampus and its number of neu- tigate the effect of social status on food-caching rates
rons appear to be permanently larger in Alaskan and their model confirmed this prediction and sug-
chickadees, providing them with permanent gested that subordinates should cache more food
advantages when it comes to spatial memory per- than dominants. The model, however, relied heavily
formance, temporary corticosterone elevations dur- on the untested assumption that dominants can
ing the most difficult times may provide additional freely pilfer caches made by subordinates whereas
memory benefits (Pravosudov 2004). subordinates could never pilfer dominants caches,
and it is not clear how the models predictions
would have changed if this assumption were
3.5 Dominance, corticosterone, spatial
changed. Empirical results were also not consistent;
memory, and the hippocampus
some studies of willow tits found that subordinates
Most Parids live in hierarchically structured social indeed cache more food than dominants (Lahti and
groups in which dominants enjoy priority of access Rytkonen 1996; Brodin and Lundborg 2003b),
to all available resources (Ekman 1989; Ekman and whereas other studies found either the reverse to be
Lilliendahl 1993; Chapter 9). It has been suggested true or no differences between low and high social
that subordinates experience less predictable condi- ranks (Pravosudov 1985Siberian and willow tits;
tions because of their low social status; even when Hitchcock and Sherry 1995black-capped chicka-
they find food they can be supplanted by domin- dees; Lahti et al. 1998willow tits; Pravosudov and
ants at any time (Ekman and Lilliendahl 1993). Lucas 2000Carolina chickadees; Lundborg and
Because social classes differ in the predictability of Brodin 2003willow tits).
their environment they might invoke different tac- Most of the studies investigating the effect of
tics for managing their available energy resources. dominance hierarchy on food-caching behavior
In particular, it has been suggested that birds in less used behavioral and ecological approaches while
predictable environments should accumulate and ignoring possible mechanisms of food-caching
maintain more energy reserves as a buffer against regulation. For example, unpredictable food trig-
the higher probability of starvation in case an ade- gers more intense food caching (Hurly 1992
quate amount of food cannot be obtained at a cri- marsh tits; Pravosudov and Grubb 1997btufted
tical time (Ekman and Lilliendahl 1993; Brodin et al. titmice) and at the same time it also triggers
2001; Chapter 17). Indeed, there is some experi- corticosterone elevation (Pravosudov et al. 2001
mental evidence that subordinate birds maintain mountain chickadees). An implant study with
36 B E H AV I O R A N D E V O L U T I O N

mountain chickadees (Pravosudov 2004) showed levels measured after almost 2 months following
that elevated corticosterone facilitates more eating, formation of dominance relationships were
more intense food caching, and enhanced spatial statistically indistinguishable between dominants
memory. Thus, corticosterone might be a mechanis- and subordinates but maximum corticosterone
tic link between unpredictable foraging conditions levels achieved during a standardized stress
and energy management tactics. If socially subor- response were actually higher in dominants
dinate individuals experience an unpredictable (Pravosudov et al. 2004). These results suggest that
environment their corticosterone levels might be whereas corticosterone might be involved in
elevated, which would support the idea that subor- mediating food-caching behavior and spatial
dinates should cache more food and even have bet- memory, changes in caching rates and memory can
ter memory for food caches. Available data on the occur without changes in baseline corticosterone
relationship between dominance status and cortico- levels. The inferior spatial memory found in
sterone levels from observational unmanipulated subordinate chickadees was not related to any
studies, however, do not show a consistent pattern changes in hippocampal volume or neuron
(see Pravosudov et al. 2004 for review). In some numbers (Pravosudov and Omanska 2005b).
studies corticosterone levels were higher in subor- Barnea and Nottebohm (1994) suggested that
dinate individuals whereas in other studies there spatial memory might be affected by changes in hip-
were no differences in baseline corticosterone levels pocampal neuron recruitment rates without changes
between dominants and subordinates. in the total number of neurons. Interestingly, subor-
To test the hypothesis that dominance hierarchy dinate mountain chickadees which showed inferior
has an effect on food caching and spatial memory, spatial memory performance compared to their
Pravosudov et al. (2004) compared experimentally dominant group mates also had significantly lower
created pairs of dominant and subordinate moun- cell proliferation rates in the ventricular zone
tain chickadees. Previous laboratory studies investi- adjacent to both the hippocampus and to the
gating the effect of social dominance on food mesopallium (Fig. 3.6; Pravosudov and Omanska
caching failed to find significant differences 2005b). Cell proliferation is a first step during the
between dominants and subordinates (Pravosudov process of neurogenesis; after new cells are born,
and Lucas 2000; Lundborg and Brodin 2003). some of them will survive to become functional
However, in all of these studies, dominants and sub- neurons and it is likely that the number of functional
ordinates were tested together in a fairly small room neurons is proportional to the number of new cells
in which subordinates could not escape the dom- (Prickaerts et al. 2004). Pravosudov and Omanska
inants view. Thus it is likely that the immediate (2005b) showed that differences in spatial memory
presence of dominants might have influenced the performance were associated with differences in hip-
behavior of subordinates in these studies. In natural pocampal cell proliferation rates, thus suggesting
conditions, subordinates can easily move out of the that cell proliferation and neurogenesis might be
dominants sight, and, in fact, most birds avoid involved in spatial memory function (Fig. 3.6b).
caching when in a presence of a group mate These data, however, are correlational and they do
(Woodrey 1991; Lahti et al. 1998). Pravosudov et al. not establish a causal effect; it remains possible that
(2004) attempted to simulate a more natural envir- social dominance simultaneously affected both
onment by testing dominants and subordinates memory and cell proliferation. In another study, hip-
separately in an adjacent room. In this experimental pocampal cell proliferation rates did not correlate
design, subordinates were not affected by the pres- with spatial memory performance (Pravosudov and
ence of the dominants during the short duration of Omanska 2005a), suggesting that elevated cortico-
the food-caching trials. Results of this study were sterone might affect memory function without
directly opposite to all predictions; subordinates changes in hippocampal cell proliferation rates.
cached significantly less food than dominants and Irrespective of whether hippocampal cell prolif-
they also performed significantly worse in a cache eration is directly involved in spatial memory
recovery task (Fig. 3.5). Baseline corticosterone function, the results of the mountain chickadees
THE HIPPOCAMPUS IN CHICKADEES 37

(a) (b)
Dominants
12 Dominants Subordinates
8
Subordinates

Mean number of sites inspected


10
6
Number of nuts

6 4

4
2
2

0 0
Cache Eat All caches All caches First cache First two
Unlimited Limited caches
caching caching

Figure 3.5 Mean number of consumed (fully or partially) and cached pine nuts during food-caching trials (a) and memory performance (b) in a
cache-recovery task in dominant (black bars) and subordinate (hatched bars) mountain chickadees. (Reprinted from Hormones and Behavior,
44, Pravosudov et al. The relationship between dominance, corticosterone, memory, and food caching in mountain chickadees (Poecile gambeli),
pp. 93102, Copyright (2003), with permission from Elsevier.)

(a) (b)
14

12
Number of BrdU-labeled cells

*
Number of sites inspected

600
10

8
400
6
*
4
200
2

0 0
HP 0 100 200 300 400 500 600 700 800
M
Number of BrdU-labeled cells

Figure 3.6 Mean number of BrdU-labeled cells in the ventricular zone adjacent to either hippocampus or mesopallium (a) and the relationship
between the number of BrdU-labeled cells in the ventricular zone adjacent to the hippocampus rates and spatial memory performance (b) in dominant
(black bars and circles) and subordinate (hatched bars and open circles) mountain chickadees (redrawn from Pravosudov and Omanska 2005b).

study suggest additional costs of subordination dominants and subordinates did not differ signifi-
subordinate chickadees might cache less food and cantly in the total number of hippocampal neurons
might show inferior spatial memory performance (Pravosudov and Omanska 2005b). It is possible
as a result of their low social status. Stress of social that neuron turnover rates are higher in dominants
subordination is also a likely factor suppressing and that is why they do not have more neurons,
hippocampal cell proliferation rates, although despite the fact that more new cells are being born
38 B E H AV I O R A N D E V O L U T I O N

in the ventricular zone. Lower caching rates and References


less efficient cache retrieval as a result of inferior
Barnea, A. and Nottebohm, F. (1994). Seasonal recruitment
spatial memory could potentially contribute to
of hippocampal neurons in adult free-ranging black-
the higher mortality of subordinates commonly
capped chickadees. Proceedings of the National Academy
reported in literature. of Sciences, 91, 1121711221.
Biegler, R., McGregor, A., Krebs, J. R., and Healy, S. D.
3.6 Conclusions (2001). A larger hippocampus is associated with longer-
lasting spatial memory. Proceedings of the National
Research on spatial memory in Parids has shown Academy of Sciences, 98, 69416944.
that environmental conditions might affect mem- Breuner, C. W., Greenberg, A. L., and Wingfield, J. C.
ory performance through several different ways: (1998). Noninvasive corticosterone treatment rapidly
increases activity in Gambels white-crowned sparrows
1. Temporary, season-like changes in spatial mem-
(Zonotrichia leucophrys gambelii). General and Comparative
ory associated with variation in food availability and Endocrinology, 111, 386394.
predictability were related to changes in baseline Brodin, A. (1994). The role of naturally stored food sup-
corticosterone levels whereas hippocampal volume plies in the winter diet of the boreal willow tit Parus
and neuron numbers appeared to remain stable. montanus. Ornis Svecica, 4, 3140.
2. In birds permanently living in higher latitudes Brodin, A. (2005). Hippocampal volume does not correlate
with harsher conditions, on the other hand, with food-hoarding rates in the black-capped chickadee
enhanced memory was related to larger hippocam- (Poecile atricapillus) and willow tits (Parus montanus).
pal volume and more neurons in the absence of any Auk, 122, 819828.
Brodin, A. and Lundborg, K. (2003a). Is hippocampal vol-
variation in corticosterone levels.
ume affected by specialization for food hoarding in
3. Changes in spatial memory associated with
birds? Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 270, 15551563.
social dominance were unrelated to either cortico-
Brodin, A. and Lundborg, K. (2003b). Rank-dependent
sterone levels or to the hippocampal volume or hoarding effort in willow tits (Parus montanus): a test of
neuron numbers, but they were associated with theoretical predictions. Behavioral Ecology and
variance in hippocampal cell proliferation rates. Sociobiology, 54, 587592.
Brodin, A., Lundborg, K., and Clark, C. W. (2001). The
It is likely that all three of these patterns could be
effect of dominance on food hoarding: a game theoret-
involved the in regulation of spatial memory, sep-
ical model. American Naturalist, 157, 6675.
arately or jointly. Elevated corticosterone levels Clayton, N. S. (2001). Hippocampal growth and mainten-
could provide yet another enhancement to spatial ance depend on food-caching experience in juvenile
memory in chickadees occupying northern parts of mountain chickadees (Poecile gambeli). Behavioral
their range, with less predictable conditions, in Neuroscience, 115, 614625.
addition to enhancements achieved via an enlarged Clayton, N. S. and Cristol, D. A. (1996). Effects of photo-
hippocampus with more neurons. Changes in hip- period on memory and food storing in captive marsh
pocampal structure (volume and neuron numbers), tits, Parus palustris. Animal Behaviour, 52, 715726.
however, are not necessary to induce changes in Diamond, D. M., Bennett, M. C., Fleshner, M. and
Rose, G. M. (1992). Inverted-U relationship between the
spatial memory in food-caching birds.
level of peripheral corticosterone and the magnitude of
hippocampal primed burst potentiation. Hippocampus, 2,
Acknowledgements 421430.
Ekman, J. (1989). Ecology of non-breeding social systems
I would like to thank Nicky Clayton, Sasha
of Parus. Wilson Bulletin, 101, 263288.
Kitaysky, Alicja Omanska, and Pierre Lavenex who
Ekman, J. B. and Lilliendahl, K. (1993). Using priority to
collaborated with me on my studies of memory, food access: fattening strategies in dominance-structured
hormones, and the hippocampus in chickadees. My willow tit (Parus montanus) flocks. Behavioral Ecology, 4,
research has been supported by NIH. Comments 232238.
from Ken Otter, David Sherry, and Laurene Garamszegi, L. Z. and Lucas, J. R. (2005). Continental
Ratcliffe greatly improved the manuscript. variation in relative hippocampal volume in birds: the
THE HIPPOCAMPUS IN CHICKADEES 39

phylogenetic extent of the effect and the potential storing and the hippocampus in birds. Neuro Report, 6,
role of winter temperatures. Biology Letters, 1, 330333. 17011704.
Gould, E. and Tanapat, P. (1999). Stress and hippocampal Krebs, J. R., Clayton, N. S., Healy, S. D., Cristol, D. A.,
neurogenesis. Biological Psychiatry, 46, 14721479. Patel, S. N., and Jolliffe, A. R. (1996). The ecology of the
Haftorn, S. (1956). Contribution to the food biology of tits, avian brain: food-storing memory and the hippocam-
especially about storing of surplus food: Part IV. A pus. Ibis, 138, 3446.
comparative analysis of Parus atricapillus L., P. cristatus, Krebs, J. R., Sherry, D. F., Healy, S. D., Perry, V. H., and
L., and P. ater L. Det Kgl Norske Videnskabers Selskabs Vaccarino, A. L. (1989). Hippocampal specialization of
Skrifter, 4, 154. food-storing birds. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Hampton, R. R. and Sherry, D. F. (1992). Food storing by Sciences, 86, 13881392.
Mexican chickadees and bridled titmice. Auk, 109, Lahti, K. and Rytkonen, S. (1996). Presence of conspecifics,
665666. time of day age affect willow tits hoarding. Animal
Hampton, R. P., Sherry, D. F., Shettleworth, S. J., Khurgel, Behaviour, 52, 631636.
M., and Ivy, G. (1995). Hippocampal volume and food- Lahti, K, Koivula, K., Rytkonen, S., Mustonen, T., Welling,
storing behavior are related in Parids. Brain Behavior and P., Pravosudov, V. V., and Orell, M. (1998). Social influ-
Evolution, 4, 5461. ences on food caching in willow tits: a field experiment.
Hampton, R. R. and Shettleworth, S. J. (1996a). Behavioral Ecology, 9, 122129.
Hippocampal lesions impair memory for location but Loscertales, M., Rose, S. P. R., and Sandi, C. (1997). The
not color in passerine birds. Behavioral Neuroscience, 110, corticosteroid synthesis inhibitors metyrapone and
831835. aminoglutethimide impair long-term memory for a
Hampton, R. R. and Shettleworth, S. J. (1996b). passive avoidance task in day-old chicks. Brain Research,
Hippocampus and memory in a food-storing and in a 769, 357361.
nonstoring bird species. Behavioral Neuroscience, 110, Lucas, J. R., Brodin, A., de Kort, S. R., and Clayton, N. S.
946964. (2004). Does hippocampal size correlate with the degree
Harrap, S. and Quinn, D. (1995). Chickadees, Tits, of caching specialization? Proceedings of the Royal Society
Nuthatches and Treecreepers. Princeton University Press, B, 271, 24232429.
Princeton, NJ. Lundborg, K. and Brodin, A. (2003). The effect of dom-
Healy, S. D. and Krebs, J. R. (1996). Food storing and the inance rank on fat deposition and food hoarding in the
hippocampus in Paridae. Brain Behavior and Evolution, willow tit Parus montanusan experimental test. Ibis,
47, 195199. 145, 7882.
Hitchcock, C. L. and Sherry, D. F. (1995). Cache pilfering MacDougall-Shackleton, S. A., Sherry, D. F., Clark, A. P.,
and its prevention in pairs of black-capped chickadees. Pinkus, R., and Hernandez, A. M. (2003). Photoperiodic
Journal of Avian Biology, 26, 187192. regulation of food storing and the hippocampus vol-
Hoshooley, J. S. and Sherry, D. F. (2004). Neuron produc- ume in black-capped chickadees, Poecile atricapillus.
tion, neuron number, and structure size are seasonally Animal Behaviour, 65, 805812.
stable in the hippocampus of the food-storing black- McEwen, B. S. (2000). The neurobiology of stress: from
capped chickadee (Poecile atricapillus). Behavioral serendipity to clinical relevance. Brain Research, 886,
Neuroscience 118, 345355. 172189.
Hurly, T. A. (1992). Energetic reserves of marsh tits (Parus McEwen, B. S. and Sapolsky, R. M. (1995). Stress and cogni-
palustris): food and fat storage in response to variable tive function. Current Opinions in Neurobiology, 5, 205216.
food supply. Behavioral Ecology, 3, 181188. Odum, E. P. (1942). Annual cycle of the black-capped
Karpouzos, H., Hernandez, A. M., MacDougall- chickadee 3. Auk, 59, 499531.
Shackleton, E. A., and MacDougall-Shackleton, S. A. Pravosudov, V. V. (1985). Search for and storage of food by
(2005). Effects of day-length and food availability on Parus cinctus lapponicus and P. montanus borealis.
food caching, mass and fat reserves in black-capped Zoologicheskiy Zhurnal, 64, 10361043 [in Russian].
chickadees (Poecile atricapillus). Physiology and Behavior, Pravosudov, V. V. (2003). Long-term moderate elevation in
84, 465469. corticosterone facilitates avian food caching behavior
Koivula, K., Orell, M., Rytkonen, S., and Lahti, K. (1995). and enhances spatial memory. Proceedings of the Royal
Fatness, sex and dominance: seasonal and daily body Society B, 270, 25992604.
mass changes in willow tits. Journal of Avian Biology, 26, Pravosudov, V. V. (2005). Corticosterone and memory in
209216. birds. In: Dawson, A. and Sharp, P. J. eds. Functional
Krebs, J. R., Clayton, N. S., Hampton, R. R., and Avian Endocrinology, pp. 257268. Narosa Publishing
Shettleworth S. J. (1995). Effects of photoperiod on food House, New Delhi, India.
40 B E H AV I O R A N D E V O L U T I O N

Pravosudov, V. V. (2006). On seasonality in food caching Pravosudov, V. V., Kitaysky, A. S., Wingfield, J. C., and
behavior in parids: do we know the whole story? Clayton, N. S. (2001). Long-term unpredictable foraging
Animal Behaviour, 71, 14551460. conditions and physiological stress response in moun-
Pravosudov, V. V. and Clayton, N. S. (2001). Effects of tain chickadees (Poecile gambeli). General and Comparative
demanding foraging conditions on cache retrieval Endocrinology, 123, 324331.
accuracy in food caching mountain chickadees (Poecile Pravosudov, V. V., Lavenex, P., and Clayton, N. S. (2002b).
gambeli). Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 268, 363368. Changes in spatial memory mediated by experimental
Pravosudov, V. V. and Clayton, N. S. (2002). A test of the variation in food supply. Journal of Neurobiology, 51,
adaptive specialization hypothesis: population differ- 142148.
ences in caching, memory and the hippocampus in Pravosudov, V. V., Mendoza, S. P., and Clayton, N. S.
black-capped chickadees (Poecile atricapilla). Behavioral (2003). The relationship between dominance, cortico-
Neuroscience, 116, 515522. sterone, memory and food caching in mountain chicka-
Pravosudov, V. V. and de Kort, S. R. (2006). Is the western dees (Poecile gambeli). Hormones and Behavior, 44, 93102.
scrub-jay (Aphelocoma californica) really an underdog Prickaerts, J., Koopmans, G., Blockland, A., and Scheepens,
among food-caching corvids when it comes to hip- A. (2004). Learning and adult neurogenesis: survival
pocampal volume and food caching propensity? Brain, with or without proliferation? Neurobiology of Learning
Behavior and Evolution, 67, 19. and Memory, 81, 111.
Pravosudov, V. V. and Grubb, T. C., Jr (1997a). Energy Reneerkens, J., Piersma, T., and Ramenofsky, M. (2002).
management in passerine birds during the non-breed- An experimental test of the relationship between tem-
ing season: a review. Current Ornithology, 14, 189234. poral variability of feeding opportunities and baseline
Pravosudov, V. V. and Grubb, T. C., Jr (1997b). levels of corticosterone in a shorebird. Journal of
Management of fat reserves and food caches in tufted Experimental Zoology, 293, 8188.
titmice (Parus bicolor) in relation to unpredictable food Saldanha, C. J., Schlinger, B. A., and Clayton, N. C. (2000).
supply. Behavioral Ecology, 8, 332339. Rapid effects of corticosterone on cache recovery in
Pravosudov, V. V. and Lucas, J. R. (2000). The effect of mountain chickadees (Parus gambeli). Hormones and
social dominance on fattening and food-caching behav- Behavior, 37, 109115.
ior in Carolina chickadees, Poecile carolinensis. Animal Sandi, C., and Rose, S. P. R. (1997). Training-dependent
Behaviour, 60, 483493. biphasic effects of corticosterone in memory formation for
Pravosudov, V. V. and Lucas, J. R. (2001). A dynamic a passive avoidance task in chicks. Psychopharmacology,
model of short-term energy management in small food- 133, 152160.
caching and non-caching birds. Behavioral Ecology, 123, Sandi, C., Rose, S. P. R., Mileusnic, R., and Lancashire, C.
324331. (1995). Corticosterone facilitates long-term memory
Pravosudov, V. V. and Omanska, A. (2005a). Prolonged formation via enhanced glycoprotein synthesis.
moderate elevation of corticosterone does not affect Neuroscience, 4, 10871093.
hippocampal anatomy or cell proliferation rates in Sapolsky, R. M. (1996). Why stress is bad for your brain.
mountain chickadees (Poecile gambeli). Journal of Science, 273, 749750.
Neurobiology, 62, 8291. Sherry, D. F. and Vaccarino, A. L. (1989). Hippocampus
Pravosudov, V. V. and Omanska, A. (2005b). Dominance- and memory for food caches in black-capped chicka-
related changes in spatial memory are associated dees. Behavioral Neuroscience, 103, 308318.
with changes in hippocampal cell proliferation rates Sherry, D. F., Vaccarino, A. L., Buckenham, K., and Herz,
in mountain chickadees. Journal of Neurobiology, 62, R. S. (1989). The hippocampal complex of food-storing
3141. birds. Brain Behavior and Evolution, 34, 308317.
Pravosudov, V. V., Kitaysky, A. S., Saldanha, C., Wingfield, Shettleworth, S. J. (1995). Memory in food-storing birds:
J. C., and Clayton, N. C. (2002a). The effect of photo- from the field to the Skinner box. In: Alleva, E., Fasolo,
period on adrenocortical stress response in mountain A., Lipp, H. P., Nadel, L., and Ricceri, L., eds. Behavioral
chickadees (Poecile gambeli). General and Comparative Brain Research in Naturalistic and Semi-naturalistic
Endocrinology, 126, 242248. Settings, pp. 158179. Kluwer, the Hague, the
Pravosudov, V. V., Kitaysky, A. S., Wingfield, J. C., and Netherlands.
Clayton, N. S. (2004). No latitudinal differences in Shettleworth, S. J., Hampton, R. R., and Westwood, R. P.
adrenocortical stress response in wintering black- (1995). Effects of season and photoperiod on food-
capped chickadees (Poecile atricapilla). Comparative storing by black-capped chickadees, Parus atricapillus.
Biochemistry and Physiology A, 137, 95103. Animal Behaviour, 49, 989998.
THE HIPPOCAMPUS IN CHICKADEES 41

Shiflett, M. W., Smulders, T. V., Benedict, L., and DeVoogd, numbers in the hippocampal formation of a food-
T. J. (2003). Reversible inactivation of the hippocampal hoarding bird: the black-capped chickadee. Journal of
formation in food-storing black-capped chickadees Neurobiology, 44, 414422.
(Poecile atricapillus). Hippocampus, 13, 437444. Verhulst, S. and Hogstad, O. (1996). Social dominance and
Shiflett, M. W., Tomaszycki, M. L., Rankin, A. Z., and energy reserves in flocks of willow tits. Journal of Avian
DeVoogd, T. J. (2004). Long-term memory for spatial loca- Biology, 27, 203208.
tions in a food-storing bird (Poecile atricapilla) requires Wingfield, J. C., Breuner, C., and Jacobs, J. (1997).
activation of NMDA receptors in the hippocampal forma- Corticosterone and behavioral responses to unpre-
tion during learning. Behavioral Neuroscience, 118, 121130. dictable events. In: Harvey, S. and Etches, R. J., eds.
Silverin, B. (1998). Stress responses in birds. Poultry and Perspectives in Avian Endocrinology, pp. 267278. Journal
Avian Biology Review, 9, 153168. of Endocrinology Ltd., Bristol, UK.
Smulders, T. V. and Dhondt, A. (1997). How much memory Wingfield, J. C., Maney, D. L., Breuner, C. W., Jacobs, J. D.,
do tits need? Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 12, 417418. Lynn, S., Ramenofsky, M., and Richardson, R. D. (1998).
Smulders, T. V., Sasson, A. D., and DeVoogd, T. J. (1995). Ecological bases of hormone-behavior interactions: the
Seasonal variation in hippocampal volume in a food- emergency life history stage. American Zoologist, 38,
storing bird, the black-capped chickadee. Journal of 191206.
Neurobiology, 27, 1525. Woodrey, M. S. (1991). Caching behavior in free-ranging
Smulders, T. V., Shiflett, M. W., Sperling, A. J., and white-breasted nuthatches: the effect of social dom-
DeVoogd, T. J. (2000). Seasonal changes in neuron inance. Ornis Scandinavica, 22, 160166.
This page intentionally left blank
CHAPTER 4

Photoperiodism and the annual


cycle of black-capped chickadees
Leslie S. Phillmore and Scott A. MacDougall-Shackleton

4.1 Introduction many other aspects of behavior and physiology also


change seasonally. These include geographic move-
4.1.1 Seasonality
ments such as the vernal and autumnal migrations,
Virtually all parts of the planet exhibit seasonal and dispersal from natal territories. In addition,
changes in climate. Some times of the year are more some birds change habitat-use seasonally and may
favorable to survival of young than others, and also alternate between exclusive and non-exclusive
organisms have thus been selected to produce young use of territories. Many aspects of energy balance
during those seasons when the offspring are most change seasonally including diet, food-storing, and
likely to survive. Baker (1938) noted that the envir- fat deposition. Feather molt is timed to occur when
onmental factors that actually influence offspring the costs to reproduction and flight are minimized.
survival may differ from the environmental factors Social behavior, such as aggression and vocaliza-
that animals actually use to time reproduction. He tions, may vary over the annual cycle. In many cases
identified ultimate factors as those that influence we know much less about the proximate environ-
offspring survival; for seasonally breeding birds this mental factors that drive seasonal changes in behav-
is typically food supply. In contrast, proximate envir- ior other than reproduction.
onmental cues are what animals actually respond to Black-capped chickadees (along with many other
in order to time seasonal changes in reproductive Parids) represent ideal species for the study of
physiology and behavior. In some cases, such as seasonal changes in physiology, neurobiology, and
food availability, ultimate and proximate factors behavior. This species is resident year-round in an
may be one and the same. However, animals often extremely seasonal environment. Despite this, we
time reproduction in response to proximate cues that know relatively little about which proximate envir-
are very different from the ultimate factors that actu- onmental cues black-capped chickadees use to
ally determine reproductive success. In seasonally time neural and physiological changes, and the
breeding birds the primary proximate cue typically subsequent changes in behavior. More information
used to time breeding and migration is the annual is available through detailed and extensive studies
change in photoperiod (Dawson et al. 2001). of photoperiodism in European species of titmice,
Although a variety of environmental cues may be such as great tits (Silverin 1994). However, for
used to fine tune the timing of reproductive effort the Paridae as a whole we know relatively little
(Chapter 5), photoperiod seems to be the primary, about photoperiodismespecially in comparison
initial predictive cue used by seasonally breeding to species such as white-crowned sparrows
birds. Other cues tend to modify the main physio- (Zonotrichia leucophrys) and European starlings
logical response driven by photoperiod. (Sturnus vulgaris; see Dawson et al. 2001 for review).
Reproduction has been the most extensively stud- In this chapter we will review photoperiodism and
ied seasonally changing behavior in birds; however, seasonality in Parids focusing on recent studies of

43
44 B E H AV I O R A N D E V O L U T I O N

black-capped chickadees. These studies include the conditions of photosensitivity, photostimulation,


effects of photoperiod on reproduction, on singing and photorefractoriness.
and the neural system controlling song, and on Variation between species in reproductive sched-
food-storing and the hippocampus. We first begin ule is due, in part, to variation between species in
with a general review of photoperiodism. photoperiodic responses. For example, the length
of day required to induce photostimulation and
the duration of long-day exposure required to
4.1.2 Photoperiodism and seasonal
induce photorefractoriness are two variables that
reproduction
could give rise to a variety of breeding schedules
Since the pioneering studies by Rowan (1925), we (Hahn et al. 1997). Another form of interspecies
have known that the seasonal change in day length variation is in the nature of photorefractoriness.
has profound effects on avian physiology and Photorefractoriness in many songbirds is absolute;
behavior. Extensive study of species such as Japanese quail (coturnix japonica), however, instead
European starlings and white-crowned sparrows become relatively photorefractory (Robinson and
have yielded a detailed model of how the annual Follett 1982). Normally, these quail do not undergo
photocycle drives seasonal changes in reproduction gonadal regression until day length declines, and
of seasonally breeding songbirds in particular they can be restimulated by returning them to very
(Nicholls et al. 1988; Dawson et al. 2001; Dawson long days at any time; no prolonged exposure to
2002), that we briefly review here. short days is required to dissipate relative refrac-
In winter and early spring when day length toriness (Nicholls et al. 1988).
is short, birds are in a photosensitive physiological In addition to the distinction between absolute
state: gonad size is small, reproductive hormone and relative refractoriness, recent studies of
levels are low, and birds are not in breeding Cardueline finches suggest that bird species may
condition. However, birds are ready to respond vary in whether gonads spontaneously regress
to increases in day length that occur during spring. when held on constant long days and whether
Once length of day exceeds a critical threshold, birds are completely insensitive to very long days
birds become photostimulated: gonadotropic (24 h light) when photorefractory (MacDougall-
hormones (luteinizing hormone and follicle stimu- Shackleton et al. 2005). Crossbills (Loxia species), for
lating hormone) are released and stimulate example, may never become absolutely photo-
recrudescence of the gonads and subsequent refractory. These opportunistically breeding song-
release of sex steroid hormones and reproductive birds can breed at almost any time of year, but
behavior ensues (Dawson et al. 2001). Prolonged exhibit an autumnal decrease in breeding that may
exposure to long days induces photorefractoriness be due to a form of relative photorefractoriness
such that birds are no longer stimulated by the (Hahn 1995). Crossbills do not appear to regress
previously stimulatory long days: gonads regress gonads when held on long days for many months
and the feathers molt (Nicholls et al. 1988). In some (Hahn 1995) and are responsive to very long
species, photorefractoriness occurs quite early days even when the gonads are regressed in
in the summer and may even precede the summer autumn (MacDougall-Shackleton et al. 2005). There
solstice. In songbirds, this state of photorefractori- is likely greater variation among species in photo-
ness is typically absolute and at this time exposure periodism than previously supposed. Thus, for any
to very long days (even 24 h light) is ineffective given species, experimental studies are required
in inducing reproductive competence (Hamner before seasonal variation in behavior can be attrib-
1968; Nicholls et al. 1988). Usually, exposure to uted to the standard photoperiodism model.
short-days is required to break photorefractoriness
and reinstate photosensitivity (Nicholls et al. 1988).
4.1.3 Seasonal neural plasticity
Thus, the annual cycle of reproduction in songbirds
is driven by a changing sensitivity to long-days In addition to seasonal changes in peripheral physi-
as birds alternate through the physiological ology and behavior, birds also exhibit remarkable
P H OTO P E R I O D I S M A N D T H E A N N UA L C Y C L E 45

seasonal brain plasticity (Tramontin and Brenowitz hippocampus, including changes in volume
2000). There is good evidence that the entire brain (Smulders et al. 1995; Clayton et al. 1997) and in
may exhibit seasonal morphological changes neuron survival and incorporation of new neurons
(Smulders 2002). Most research on birds, however, (Barnea and Nottebohm 1994). Most of the work on
has focused on seasonal plasticity in three neural seasonal plasticity of the hippocampus has been
systems: the gonadotropin-releasing hormone conducted on black-capped chickadees, and we
(GnRH) system, the song-control system, and the review this field further below.
hippocampus.
GnRH is a hypothalamic neuropeptide that stimu- 4.2 Annual cycle of black-capped
lates release of gonadotropins from the pituitary chickadees
and thereby regulates the entire reproductive axis.
4.2.1 Overview
In a large number of seasonally breeding songbirds,
GnRH is dramatically down-regulated when birds Across most of their range, black-capped chicka-
are photorefractory (Ball and Hahn 1997). Although dees are year-round residents; that is, once adults,
this down-regulation in and of itself does not cause individuals live in the same small geographic area
photorefractoriness (Dawson et al. 2001), it may throughout the year (Smith 1991). Because of this,
preclude physiological responses to stimulatory much is known about this species annual cycle of
environmental cues. In species that do not become behavior (Odum 1941a, 1941b, 1942; Smith 1991)
absolutely photorefractory GnRH plasticity is including alternation between winter flocking and
absent or much reduced (Ball and Hahn 1997). breeding territoriality, changing prevalence of
Research on GnRH in Parids is minimal (see Silverin different vocalization types (Ficken et al. 1978), and
1994) and whether or not chickadees exhibit changes in diet and food storing (Odum 1942;
seasonal fluctuations in GnRH remains unknown. Shettleworth et al. 1995). Based on field observa-
Songbirds possess a circuit of specialized brain tions in southern Ontario (populations near
regions that control the learning and production of Kingston, Toronto, and London, Ontario, Canada)
song, called the song-control system. Regions we have compiled a diagram illustrating the modal
within this network, such as HVC, exhibit the most annual cycle of reproduction, singing, molt, and
extreme seasonal plasticity documented among food storing of black-capped chickadees (Fig. 4.1).
vertebrates (Tramontin and Brenowitz 2000). In Note that parameters associated with breeding
HVC, this extreme plasticity involves the incorp- are asymmetric with respect to the annual
oration of tens of thousands of new neurons on an photocyclea hallmark of species that become
annual basis and the volume of this region may absolutely photorefractory (Dawson et al. 2001).
almost double in many species (Tramontin and In fall and winter, chickadees live in flocks of
Brenowitz 2000). Seasonal changes in HVC typic- about six to eight birds, but flocks can range from
ally track changes in reproduction; HVC tends to two to 12 birds (Smith 1991). Flocks, usually, are
be largest in the spring when birds are singing made up of approximately equal numbers of males
most. However, the vernal increase in HVC size is and females. A flock defends a large territory (aver-
rapid and may precede reproduction (Caro et al. ages about 816 hectares, from several years, popu-
2005). Seasonal plasticity of the song-control system lations, and sources; see Smith 1991) over which it
appears primarily to be regulated by seasonal ranges. There is a dominance hierarchy within the
changes in gonadal steroid hormones (Brenowitz flock, with a top, or alpha male; all other males rank
2004); however, gonad-independent effects have in order under this male (see Chapter 9). This dom-
been reported as well (Ball et al. 2004). Other inance is established usually through interactions
hormones or singing behavior itself may also at feeding sites. Females also form a dominance
contribute to seasonal plasticity of the song system hierarchy under an alpha female. In early spring
(reviewed in Ball et al. 2004). flocks break up and males establish and defend ter-
Though less studied than the song system, sea- ritories (e.g. flock break-up by mid-April, Fort and
sonal changes have also been reported for the avian Otter 2004). Breeding territories are formed within
46 B E H AV I O R A N D E V O L U T I O N

Food storing ? ?

Feather molt

Gonad size
?

Eggs in nest

Dawn singing

16
14
Hous daylight 12
10
8
Month
J F M A M J J A S O N D

Figure 4.1 Annual cycle of black-capped chickadees. Hours of daylight are indicated for southern Ontario. Dark portions of horizontal
bars indicate the annual changes in dawn singing, egg-laying, gonad size, feather molt, and food storing. Data for eggs in nest,
gonad size, and feather molt are based on unpublished data collected by the authors in southern Ontario (near the cities of London,
Toronto, and Kingston) over several years. Data for dawn singing rates indicate population levels of dawn song, not individual behavior;
data on this are presented in Phillmore et al. (2006). The bar for food storing is hatched because it is based on more anecdotal reports and
observations. There is often large variation between individuals in the timing of any single variable (see text for more details). Question
marks indicate a lack of data.

the flocks home range, although these individual Although the annual cycle described above is
territories are much smaller than the flock range clearly seasonal in nature, the proximate environ-
(averages about 1.5 to 5 hectares, from several mental cues, such as photoperiod, that drive this
years, populations, and sources; see Smith 1991). cycle remain relatively unstudied. Below, we
Pairs are usually established by rank in the flock, review the evidence for the role of photoperiod in
with the alpha male and female forming a pair, and driving changes in: (i) reproduction, (ii) singing
the rest of the flock pairing off in order of rank. Pair and the song-control centers of the brain, and (iii)
formation occurs in fall or winter with flock forma- food-storing and the hippocampus.
tion and then again in spring (Smith 1991).
Once pairs are established nest building begins, 4.3 Photoperiodism and reproduction
and eggs are laid (early May), incubated, and in black-capped chickadees
hatched. Once young are fledged (about 16 days
4.3.1 Comparison with other species
after hatch, early to mid-June, Smith 1991) they
remain with parents in a family flock until they are There are few data regarding reproductive physi-
old enough to disperse (about 2 to 4 weeks after ology and photoperiodism in black-capped chicka-
fledging, late June, early July; Fort and Otter 2004; dees; however, more extensive research has been
Smith 1991). Young rarely stay in the same immedi- conducted on European species of titmice. This
ate geographical area in which they are born. By the research, using both natural changes in photoperiod
end of summer and early fall, flocks are once again with wild-caught birds and manipulated
beginning to form and larger flock territories and photoperiods with captive birds, shows there are
dominance hierarchies re-established. indeed seasonal and photoperiod-driven fluctuations
P H OTO P E R I O D I S M A N D T H E A N N UA L C Y C L E 47

in gonad size, circulating gonadotropic hormones, changes in brain and behavior of this species. First,
and reproductive behaviors. For example, great tits chickadees captured in spring then transferred to
have seasonal fluctuations in gonad size (Silverin long days eventually initiate feather molt, whereas
1978), circulating levels of luteinizing hormone birds captured at the same time and kept on short
(Silverin 1991), and follicle stimulating hormone days long term do not (Shettleworth et al. 1995).
(Silverin et al. 1997). Chickadees captured in winter then transferred to
Although the onset of photostimulation varies long days also eventually initiate molt (Shettleworth
among and within different Parus species depend- et al. 1995). These results suggest that in late winter
ing on latitude (Silverin et al. 1993), extended and spring black-capped chickadees are photosensi-
exposure to long days in captivity does eventually tive and transfer to long days at this time results in
lead to gonadal regression (Silverin et al. 1993). photostimulation and eventual photorefractoriness
Also, extensive study of great tits (Silverin 1994) and feather molt. More recent data indicate that
shows that these birds likely exhibit absolute exposure of chickadees captured in winter to
photorefractoriness in late summer. At this time constant long days leads to rapid growth of the
great tits spontaneously regress their gonads and gonads, and continued exposure to constant long
are unresponsive to very long days (although days leads to eventual complete gonadal regression
exposure to 24 h light has not been tested). On the (MacDougall-Shackleton et al. 2003b). As well,
whole, the data support the idea that great tits chickadees held long term on constant short days do
exhibit absolute photorefractoriness similar to that exhibit some modest gonadal development, at least
observed in other species. However, the nature of in females (MacDougall-Shackleton et al. 2003b).
photorefractoriness (i.e. absolute versus relative) in These studies suggest that black-capped chickadees
other species of titmice remains unclear and may exhibit photoperiodism similar to other seasonally
vary among species. For example, captive marsh breeding songbirds: with long days inducing both
tits held on long days do not appear to initiate initial photostimulation and eventual photorefrac-
feather molt until they are transferred to short days toriness and molt, and long-term exposure to short
(Clayton and Cristol 1996). This suggests that days leading to some recrudescence of gonads
marsh tits may not become photorefractory while (Dawson et al. 2001).
held on constant long days, although molt and In a recent laboratory study, Phillmore et al.
gonads were not measured systematically. On the (2005) tested directly whether black-capped chicka-
whole, work remains to be done to determine the dees were absolutely or relatively photorefractory.
nature and extent of interspecific variation in Photosensitive chickadees on a short-day photo-
photoperiodic responses and photorefractoriness period (8 h light per day) were exposed to a long-
among species of titmice and chickadees. day photoperiod (15 h light per day). They were
held on this long-day photoperiod until they
became photorefractory (as indicated by regression
4.3.2 Photoperiodism in black-capped
of gonads and onset of moult). Birds were then
chickadees
either switched to constant light (24 h light) or
While the annual cycle of black-capped chickadee maintained on the original long-day photoperiod.
behaviors, including singing, food storing, and flock If chickadees responded to the constant-light
or territory formation, are well known, the informa- challenge by regrowing their gonads and becoming
tion about photoperiodism, or physiological photostimulated, it would indicate that chickadees
response to changing day length, is limited. were relatively photorefractory and did not require
Although some assumptions could be made from exposure to short day photoperiods in order to
studies of European species of tits (see previous sec- return to breeding condition (Hamner 1968).
tion), few direct studies of photoperiodism in black- However, in this experiment chickadees exposed to
capped chickadees have been undertaken. Some constant light did not have larger gonads than birds
data regarding photoperiodism of black-capped maintained on the same long-day photoperiod
chickadees can be gleaned from studies of seasonal (15 h light). These birds also continued to molt
48 B E H AV I O R A N D E V O L U T I O N

feathers. Therefore, evidence from Phillmore et al. size of song-control regions compared to birds kept
(2005) and other studies support the suggestion on short days (MacDougall-Shackleton et al. 2003a).
that black-capped chickadees are, in fact, absolutely Chickadees held in captivity on long days for many
photorefractory, similar to other Parids and season- weeks become photorefractory and fail to show
ally breeding songbirds such as European starlings. gonadal responses to changes in photoperiod
(reviewed above). These birds also fail to show any
changes in the size of song-control regions includ-
4.4 Seasonal changes in song and
ing HVC (Phillmore et al. 2005). These data suggest
the song-control system
the size of song-control regions is influenced by
Chickadees produce a wide range of vocalizations photoperiod-driven changes in gonadal hormones.
that are used in different contexts (Ficken et al. In contrast to laboratory studies, studies of wild-
1978). The chickadee song, called the fee-bee, is a caught black-capped chickadees do not show
two-tone whistled vocalization. It is a learned seasonal changes in HVC size. Smulders and
vocalization; young birds must hear adults sing fee- colleagues measured the size of song-control
bees during development in order to accurately regions in brains of black-capped chickadees
sing it as adults (Shackleton and Ratcliffe 1993; collected from the wild at different times of year.
Chapter 12). The fee-bee is sung primarily by males These birds had been the subject of previous stud-
(but females may sometimes also fee-bee, Smith ies of hippocampal seasonal plasticity (Smulders
1991) and is produced most frequently during et al. 1995, see below) but failed to exhibit seasonal
either the dawn chorus (Horn et al. 1992; Gammon changes in the size of song-control brain regions
2004) or during aggressive territorial interactions (Smulders et al. 2006). More recently, we have
(Shackleton and Ratcliffe 1994; Chapter 14). similarly examined the brains of black-capped
The fee-bee song is most frequently heard in chickadees collected from the wild at four times of
spring, when birds are in breeding condition year: July, October, January, and April. Though the
(Ficken et al. 1978; Phillmore et al. 2006). Dawn largest HVC sizes were found in April and July,
singing may begin in January or February, and there was no statistically significant effect of time of
continue to July, but is most pronounced in the year on HVC volume (Phillmore et al. 2006).
weeks prior to hatch of the young. Other seasonally How can we reconcile the laboratory versus field
produced vocalizations include those associated studies? One explanation is that black-capped
with breeding and reproductive behavior, such as chickadees do not exhibit large seasonal changes in
the faint fee-bee (males and females) and variable see HVC size because they produce learned vocaliza-
(females), both heard when birds were in close tions (e.g. the chick-a-dee and gargle calls) all year
proximity to the nest (Ficken et al. 1978). By con- round, and thus need to maintain HVC size
trast, the chick-a-dee call, also a learned vocalization throughout breeding and non-breeding seasons.
(Hughes et al. 1998), is probably not seasonal as it is However, this would not explain the effect of
heard year round. However, it is more prevalent in photostimulation on HVC in captive birds. A second
the non-breeding season (Ficken et al. 1978). This explanation is that a number of factors not present
call is used in many contexts, for example as a mild in the laboratory and beyond photoperiod influence
alarm call and to maintain flock or pair contact HVC size when birds are in the wild. There is large
(Ficken et al.1978; Chapter 13). interindividual variation in the timing of gonadal
Brain regions such as HVC that are involved in recrudescence and regression in the field (Fig. 4.2),
song learning and song production undergo large and this may reflect large individual variation in
seasonal changes in many species (Tramontin and hormone profiles as well. Supporting this second
Brenowitz 2000). The evidence that this occurs in explanation, in the study by Phillmore et al. (2006),
black-capped chickadees is mixed. Chickadees held HVC was larger in breeding birds than in non-
in captivity on short days for many weeks, then breeding birds as assessed by gonad size. Thus
photostimulated with long-day photoperiods exhib- interindividual variation in the timing of breeding
ited an increase in gonad size and an increase in the may mask seasonal changes in HVC.
P H OTO P E R I O D I S M A N D T H E A N N UA L C Y C L E 49

8
2002
7 2003

Testis length (mm) 5

0
Jan Mar May Jul Sep Nov
Date

Figure 4.2 Annual change in testis size in free-living black-capped chickadees. Data indicate the length of the left testis (measured via
laparotomy) of chickadees captured near London, Ontario, Canada in 2002 and 2003. Note the large variation in gonadal recrudescence in April
and May and in gonadal regression in July.

Other factors such as dominance rank, age, and many species of Parids also exhibit seasonal
amount of singing could also influence circulating changes in food-storing (Odum 1942; Haftorn 1956;
hormones and HVC size. European starlings have Ludescher 1980; Pravosudov 1985; Chapters 2
repeatedly been shown to exhibit seasonally plastic and 3). In fact, food storing is likely an adaptive
song-control regions in laboratory studies, but response to seasonal changes in food supply. In
seasonal changes were not apparent in wild-caught spring and summer, chickadees are primarily
birds (Ball et al. 2004). A similar dichotomy between insectivores when that food source is abundant;
field and laboratory studies has also been demon- birds are not observed to store food at this time. In
strated in wild canaries (Leitner et al. 2001). These autumn and winter birds store more often (Haftorn
results support the explanation that the lack of 1956; Nakamura and Wako 1988; Brodin 1994).
seasonal changes in HVC of free-living birds may Black-capped chickadees are also frequently
result from individual variation in the timing and observed caching in autumn and winter (Odum
magnitude of these seasonal changes, rather than 1942). A recent study of free-living black-capped
the absence of seasonal plasticity per se. Because chickadees reports high storing rates throughout
many factors such as age, dominance and singing the autumn, with highest rates in September and
can be quantified in black-capped chickadees they declining thereafter (Brodin 2005).
represent an excellent species in which to address Because photoperiod is the principal cue regulat-
this hypothesis. ing seasonal changes in reproductive behavior, it is
a tenable hypothesis that it may influence seasonal
changes in food storing as well. For example, the
4.5 Seasonal changes in food storing autumnal increase in food storing could be driven
and the hippocampus by short-day photoperiods. Several studies have
addressed this hypothesis in black-capped chicka-
4.5.1 Food storing
dees. Shettleworth et al. (1995) found that birds
The most extensively studied seasonal behaviors captured in November stored more in captivity
are those associated with reproduction. However, than birds captured in March. Birds at both times of
50 B E H AV I O R A N D E V O L U T I O N

year were then held under different photoperiods, photoperiod and food availability (Karpouzos et al.
and birds tended to store more when on short days 2005). Thus, energy balance seems to be mediated
than on long days. In particular, birds captured by duration of food availability rather than photo-
in March then switched to long days, then switched period per se. Future work is needed to determine
back to short days, greatly increased storing as whether this is also true for food-storing behavior.
day-length decreased (Shettleworth et al. 1995).
MacDougall-Shackleton et al. (2003b) conducted a
4.5.2 Hippocampus
study to determine whether the autumnal increase
in food storing resulted from decreasing day-length The hippocampus is a region of the brain that is
or the onset of photorefractoriness that occurs in important for spatial memory in most vertebrates
late summer. They held birds in one of three condi- and is required for accurate retrieval of food from
tions: long-term long days (photorefractory), long- caches in black-capped chickadees (Sherry and
term short days (photosensitive), or switched from Vaccarino 1989). Indeed, there is much evidence
long-term short days to long days (photostimu- indicating that the hippocampus is adaptively
lated). Birds on short days stored more than those specialized in response to spatial memory demands
on long days, and the photostimulated birds in food-storing birds such as black-capped chicka-
reduced storing when they were switched from dees (Chapter 2). Because food storing is seasonal,
short to long days (MacDougall-Shackleton et al. several researchers have examined the question of
2003b). Thus, regardless of whether birds were whether the chickadee hippocampus shows
photorefractory or photostimulated, long days led seasonal plasticity similar to that observed in the
to a reduction in food storing. song-control regions of many songbirds. Here we
Both captive studies and field observations sug- briefly review the evidence that the hippocampus is
gest that food storing is facilitated by experiencing seasonally plastic and the potential role of photo-
short-day photoperiods. The effects of photoperiod period in such plasticity.
on reproduction are direct; that is, the effects of The evidence for seasonal changes in the size of
long days are not an effect of birds having more the hippocampus is mixed. Smulders et al. (1995,
time to forage and greater energy intake (Dawson 2000) found an effect of season on hippocampus
1986). The stimulatory effects of short days on food size and the number of cells in the hippocampus in
storing, however, could be mediated either directly black-capped chickadees caught from the wild at
or by the fact that birds on short days have less time different times of the year. However, no other study
to forage and are thus food restricted. Food restric- has replicated this finding. Three further studies
tion and food unpredictability do increase food that captured free-living chickadees at different
storing in Parids (Hurly 1992; Pravosudov and times of the year did not find an overall change in
Grubb 1997). A recent study attempted to deter- hippocampus size (Barnea and Nottebohm 1994;
mine whether short days per se or shortened time Hoshooley and Sherry 2004; Hoshooley et al. 2007).
available to forage was responsible for increased Each of these studies was carried out on different
food storing in fall and winter in black-capped birds and a meta-analysis has yet to be conducted.
chickadees. Karpouzos et al. (2005) held birds on Laboratory studies also do not show an effect of
long days, short days, or long days with only short photoperiod on hippocampus size. Photoperiod
duration of food availability. For this latter group, manipulations that successfully affect food-storing
food was removed from the cages of birds held on behavior do not affect hippocampus size (Krebs
a long-day photoperiod at the same time as the et al. 1995; MacDougall-Shackleton et al. 2003b;
lights went out for the short-day group. There was Hoshooley et al. 2005). If hippocampus size does
no significant variation among groups in food stor- change seasonally this effect is not as readily
ing; all groups stored relatively little. However, fat observable as changes in the song-control system.
and mass profiles were identical for the two groups Several studies have now examined seasonal plas-
of birds with short duration of food availability and ticity of the black-capped chickadee hippocampus
both differed from that of birds with long day using cell-birth markers to examine the incorporation
P H OTO P E R I O D I S M A N D T H E A N N UA L C Y C L E 51

of new neurons. In free-living chickadees the recruit- studies of photoperiodism and seasonality. Recent
ment of new neurons to hippocampus appears to be data indicate that dominance status influences
maximal during autumn, based on counts of new neural plasticity in addition to influencing food
neurons that had survived several weeks (Barnea storing (Chapter 3; Pravosudov and Omanska
and Nottebohm 1994). More recently, a study has 2005) and singing (Christie et al. 2004; Chapter 14).
found that incorporation of young cells (6 days old) Another factor that may influence seasonal changes
into hippocampus was greatest in January in brain and behavior is age, but this is not typically
(Hoshooley et al. 2007), a trend that was observed in controlled for in studies of seasonality beyond
a prior study (but was not statistically significant, distinguishing hatch-year from older birds.
Hoshooley and Sherry 2004, Chapter 2). Even in the Because black-capped chickadees are year-round
absence of seasonal changes in overall size, then, the residents of the same small geographic location,
hippocampus may exhibit seasonal plasticity. typically for their entire postdispersal life, they
An open question in regards to seasonal plasti- provide a tremendous opportunity to examine how
city of the hippocampus is what causes the factors such as age and dominance rank interact
observed changes. Photoperiod might play a role, with environmental cues, such as photoperiod, to
by modulating circulating hormones that could drive seasonal changes in brain and behavior.
affect hippocampal tissue. Indeed, the songbird Given the current mixed results in regards to neural
hippocampus is rich in receptors for sex steroids plasticity in this species (possibly due to individual
such as testosterone and estradiol (e.g. Saldanha variation), at the very least dominance rank of indi-
et al. 1998) and the photoperiodically modulated viduals needs to be considered more fully in future
hormone melatonin (Bentley et al. 1999). In addition, studies. In addition, much of the work on seasonal
food storing itself may modulate plasticity of the changes in reproductive physiology in chickadees
hippocampus. Developmentally, the amount of is based on gonadal data. Further analyses of
food storing affects the size of the hippocampus hormones, as has been carried out in Eurasian
(Clayton and Krebs 1994) and this may occur Parids (e.g. Silverin 1991, 1994), are required.
seasonally as well. Finally, dominance status of Another tremendous opportunity provided by
birds may influence hormone levels, food storing, black-capped chickadees is their extremely large
and plasticity in the hippocampus (Pravosudov geographic range. Comparisons among popula-
and Omanska 2005; Chapter 3). Thus, the tions living at different latitudes have already
conditions under which seasonal plasticity of the yielded important results regarding the adaptive
hippocampus is observed, and the factors that specialization of spatial memory and the
cause it, remain largely to be explored. hippocampus (Pravosudov and Clayton 2002;
Chapter 3). This approach could be expanded to
compare populations that experience more or less
4.6 Future directions
annual variance in the duration of daylight, and
Recent studies have revealed that black-capped how they respond to this seasonal change in
chickadees exhibit photoperiodic responses and photoperiod. This comparative approach should
photorefractoriness similar to many other songbird also be expanded to include other Parids that
species. However, seasonal changes in the brain experience the same or different photoperiods as
have produced mixed results, especially when black-capped chickadees. Black-capped chickadees
comparing birds caught from the wild with birds will likely continue to be an important species in
held in captivity. Reconciling the differences among the study of photoperiod and seasonality from an
these studies will likely require more detailed con- integrative ecological approach.
sideration of variation among populations and
variation among individuals.
Acknowledgments
Dominance status is a pervasive factor affecting a
chickadees behavior and physiology (Chapter 9). Our research has been funded by NSERC Canada
However, dominance status is rarely considered in and a Premiers Research Excellence Award from
52 B E H AV I O R A N D E V O L U T I O N

the Province of Ontario. David Sherry and Jennifer Clayton, N. S., Reboreda, J. C., and Kacelnik, A. (1997).
Hoshooley have provided invaluable input to our Seasonal changes of hippocampus volume in parasitic
research and our preparation of this chapter. cowbirds. Behavioural Processes, 41, 237243.
Dawson, A. (1986). The effect of restricting the daily
period of food availability on testicular growth of star-
References lings Sturnus vulgaris. Ibis, 128, 572575.
Dawson, A. (2002). Photoperiodic control of the annual
Baker, J. (1938). The Evolution of Breeding Seasons. cycle in birds and comparison with mammals. Ardea,
Clarendon Press, Oxford, United Kingdom. 90, 355367.
Ball, G. F. and Hahn T. P. (1997). GnRH neuronal systems Dawson, A., King, V. M., Bentley, G. E., and Ball, G. F.
in birds and their relation to the control of seasonal (2001). Photoperiodic control of seasonality in birds.
reproduction. In: Parhar, I. S., Sakuma, Y., eds. GnRH Journal of Biological Rhythms, 16, 365380.
Neurons: Gene to Behavior, pp. 325342. Brain Shuppan, Ficken, M. S., Ficken, S. R., and Witken, S. R. (1978). Vocal
Tokyo. repertoire of the black-capped chickadee. Auk, 95,
Ball, G. F., Auger, C. J., Bernard, D. J., Charlier, T. D., 3448.
Sartor, J. J., Riters, L. V., and Balthazart, J. (2004). Fort, K. A. and Otter, K. A. (2004). Effects of habitat
Seasonal plasticity in the song-control system: multiple disturbance on reproduction in black-capped chicka-
brain sites of steroid hormone action and the impor- dees (Poecile atricapillus) in northern British Colombia.
tance of variation in song behaviour. Annals of the New Auk, 121, 10701080.
York Academy of Sciences, 1016, 586610. Gammon, D. E. (2004). Black-capped chickadee dawn
Barnea, A. and Nottebohm, F. (1994). Seasonal recruitment chorus and subsequent sexual activity. Wilson Bulletin,
of hippocampal neurons in adult free-ranging black- 116, 252256.
capped chicakdees. Proceedings of the National Academy Haftorn, S. (1956). Contribution to the food biology of tits
of Sciences USA, 91, 1121711221. especially about storing of surplus food. Part IV: a
Bentley, G. E., Vant Hof, T. J., and Ball, G. F. (1999). comparative analysis of Parus atricapillus L., P. cristatus
Seasonal plasticity in the songbird telencephalon: a role and P. ater. Det kgl Norske Videnskabrs selskabs Skrifter,
for melatonin. Proceedings of the National Academy of 4, 154.
Sciences USA, 96, 46744679. Hahn, T. P. (1995). Integration of photoperiodic and food
Brenowitz, E. A. (2004). Plasticity of the adult avian song cues to time changes in reproductive physiology by an
control system. Annals of the New York Academy of opportunistic breeder, the red crossbill, Loxia curvirostra
Sciences, 1016, 560585. (Aves, Carduelinae). Journal of Experimental Zoology, 272,
Brodin, A. (1994). The role of naturally stored food 213226.
supplies in the winter diet of the boreal willow tit Parus Hahn, T. P., Boswell, T., Wingfield, J. C., and Ball, G. F.
montanus. Ornis Svecica, 4, 3140. (1997). Temporal flexibility in avian reproduction:
Brodin, A. (2005). Hippocampal volume does not correlate patterns and mechanisms. Current Ornithology, 14, 3980.
with food-hoarding rates in the black-capped chickadee Hamner, W. M. (1968). The photorefractory period of the
(Poecile atricapillus) and willow tit (Parus montanus). house finch. Ecology, 49, 211227.
Auk, 122, 819828. Horn, A. G., Leonard, M. L., Ratcliffe, L., Shackleton, S. A.,
Caro, S. P., Lambrechts, M. M., and Balthazart, J. (2005). and Weisman, R. G. (1992). Frequency variation in
Early seasonal development of brain song control songs of black-capped chickadees (Parus atricapillus).
nuclei in male blue tits. Neuroscience Letters, 386, Auk, 109, 847852.
139144. Hoshooley, J. S. and Sherry, D. F. (2004). Neuron produc-
Christie P. J., Mennill, D. J., and Ratcliffe, L. M. (2004). tion, neuron number, and structure size are seasonally
Pitch shifts and song structure indicate male quality in stable in the hippocampus of the food-storing black-
the dawn chorus of black-capped chickadees. Behavioral capped chickadee (Poecile atricapillus). Behavioral
Ecology and Sociobiology, 55, 341348. Neuroscience, 118, 345355.
Clayton, N. S. and Cristol, D. A. (1996). Effects of Hoshooley, J. S., Phillmore, L. S., and MacDougall-
photoperiod on memory and food storing in captive Shackleton, S. A. (2005). An examination of avian
marsh tits, Parus palustris. Animal Behaviour, 52, 715726. hippocampal neurogenesis in relation to photoperiod.
Clayton, N. S. and Krebs, J. R. (1994). Hippocampal Neuroreport, 16, 987991.
growth and attrition in birds affected by experience. Hoshooley, J. S., Phillmore, L. S., Sherry, D. F., and
Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA, 91, MacDougall-Shackleton, S. A. (2007). Annual cycle of
74107414. the black-capped chickadee: seasonality of food-storing
P H OTO P E R I O D I S M A N D T H E A N N UA L C Y C L E 53

and the hippocampus. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, Odum, E. P. (1942). Annual cycle of the black-capped
in press. chickadee3. Auk, 59, 499531.
Hughes, M., Nowicki, S., and Lohr, B. (1998). Call learning Phillmore, L. S., Hoshooley, J. S., Hahn, T. P., and
in black-capped chickadees (Parus atricapillus): the role MacDougall-Shackleton, S. A. (2005). A test of absolute
of experience in the development of chick-a-dee calls. photorefractoriness and photo-induced neural plasti-
Ethology, 104, 232249. city of song-control regions in black-capped chickadees
Hurly, T. A. (1992). Energetic reserves of marsh tits (Parus (Poecile atricapillus). Canadian Journal of Zoology, 83,
palustris): food and fat storage in response to variable 747753.
food supply. Behavioral Ecology, 3, 181188. Phillmore, L. S., Hoshooley, J. S., Sherry, D. F., and
Karpouzos, H., Hernandez, A. M., MacDougall- MacDougall-Shackleton, S. A. (2006). Annual cycle of
Shackleton, E. A., and MacDougall-Shackleton, S. A. the black-capped chickadee: seasonality of singing rates
(2005). Effects of day-length and food availability on and vocal-control brain regions. Journal of Neurobiology,
food caching, mass and fat reserves in black-capped 66, 10021010.
chickadees (Poecile atricapillus). Physiology and Behavior, Pravosudov, V. V. (1985). Search for and storage of food by
84, 465469. Parus cinctus lapponicus and P. montanus borealis
Krebs, J. R., Clayton, N. S., Hampton, R. R., and (Paridae). Zoologichesky Zhurnal, 64, 10361043.
Shettleworth, S. J. (1995). Effects of photoperiod on food Pravosudov, V. V., and Clayton, N. S. (2002). A test of the
storing and hippocampus in birds. NeuroReport, 6, adaptive specialization hypothesis: population differ-
17011704. ences in caching, memory, and the hippocampus in
Leitner, S., Voigt, C., Garcia-Segura, L. M., Vant Hof, T., black-capped chickadees (Poecile atricapilla). Behavioral
and Gahr, M. (2001). Seasonal activation and inactiva- Neuroscience, 116, 515522.
tion of song motor memories in wild canaries is not Pravosudov, V. V., and Grubb Jr, T. C. (1997). Management
reflected in neuroanatomical changes of forebrain song of fat reserves and food caches in tufted titmice (Parus
areas. Hormones and Behavior, 40, 160168. bicolor) in relation to unpredictable food supply.
Ludescher, F.-B. (1980). Fressen und Verstecken Behavioral Ecology, 8, 332339.
von Smereieien bei der Weidenmeise Parus Pravosudov, V. V., and Omanska, A. (2005). Dominance-
montanus im Jahresverlauf unter koknstanten related changes in spatial memory are associated with
Ernhrungsbedingungen. Okol Vogel, 2, 135144. changes in hippocampal cell proliferation rates in
MacDougall-Shackleton, S. A., Hernandez, A. M., Valyear, mountain chickadees. Journal of Neurobiology, 62, 3141.
K. F., and Clark, A. P. (2003a). Photostimulation induces Robinson, J. E. and Follett, B. K. (1982). Photoperiodism in
rapid growth of song-control brain regions in male and Japanese quail: the termination of seasonal breeding by
female chickadees (Poecile atricapilla). Neuroscience photorefractoriness. Proceedings of the Royal Society of
Letters, 340, 165168. London B, 215, 95116.
MacDougall-Shackleton, S. A., Pereyra, M. E., and Hahn, Rowan, W. (1925). Relation of light to bird migration and
T. P. (2005). GnRH, photorefractoriness and breeding developmental changes. Nature, 115, 494495.
schedules of cardueline finches. In: Dawson, A., and Saldanha, C. J., Popper, P., Micevych, P. E., and Schlinger,
Sharp, P. J., eds. Functional Avian Endocrinology, pp. B. A. (1998). The passerine hippocampus is a site of high
97110. Narosa Publishing House, New Delhi, India. aromatase: inter- and intra-species comparisons.
MacDougall-Shackleton, S. A., Sherry, D. F., Clark, A. P., Hormones and Behavior, 34, 8597.
Pinkus, R., and Hernandez, A. M. (2003b). Shackleton, S. A. and Ratcliffe, L. M. (1993). Development
Photoperiodic regulation of food-storing and hip- of song in hand-reared black-capped chickadees. Wilson
pocampus volume in black-capped chickadees (Poecilie Bulletin, 105, 637644.
atricapilla). Animal Behaviour, 65, 805812. Shackleton, S. A. and Ratcliffe, L. M. (1994). Matched
Nakamura, H. and Wako, Y. (1988). Food storing behavior counter-singing signals escalation of aggression in
of willow tit Parus montanus. Journal of Yamashina black-capped chickadees (Parus atricapillus). Ethology,
Institute of Ornithology, 20, 2136. 97, 310316.
Nicholls, T. H., Goldsmith, A. R., and Dawson, A. (1988). Sherry, D. F. and Vaccarino, A. L. (1989). Hippocampus
Photorefractoriness in birds and comparison with and memory for food caches in black-capped chicka-
mammals. Physiological Review, 68, 133176. dees. Behavioral Neuroscience, 103, 308318.
Odum, E. P. (1941a). Annual cycle of the black-capped Shettleworth S. J., Hampton, R. R., and Westwood, R. P.
chickadee1. Auk, 58, 314333. (1995). Effects of season and photoperiod on food
Odum, E. P. (1941b). Annual cycle of the black-capped storing by black-capped chickadees, Parus atricapillus.
chickadee2. Auk, 58, 518534. Animal Behaviour, 49, 989998.
54 B E H AV I O R A N D E V O L U T I O N

Silverin, B. (1978). Circannual rhythms in gonads and Smulders, T. V. (2002). Natural breeding conditions and
endocrine organs of the great tit Parus major in south- artificial increases in testosterone have opposite effects
west Sweden. Ornis Scandinavia, 9, 207213. on the brains of adult male songbirds. Hormones and
Silverin, B. (1991). Annual changes in plasma levels of LH, Behavior, 41, 156169.
and prolactin in free-living female great tits (Parus Smulders, T. V., Lisi, M. D., Tricomi, E., Otter, K. A.,
major). General and Comparative Endocrinology, 83, Chruszcz, B., Ratcliffe, L. M., and DeVoogd, T. J. (2006).
425431. Failure to detect seasonal changes in the song system
Silverin, B. (1994). Photoperiodism in male great tits nuclei of the black-capped chickadee (Poecile atricapil-
(Parus major). Ethology, Ecology, and Evolution, 6, lus). Journal of Neurobiology (66, 9911001).
131157. Smulders, T. V., Sasson, A. D., and DeVoogd, T. J. (1995).
Silverin, B., Kikuchi, M., and Ishii, S. (1997). Seasonal Seasonal variation in hippocampal volume in a food
changes in follicle-stimulating hormone in free-living storing bird, the black-capped chickadee. Journal of
great tits. General and Comparative Endocrinology, 108, Neurobiology, 27, 1525.
366373. Smulders, T. V., Shifflet, A. J., Sperling, A. J., and
Silverin, B., Massa, R., and Stokkan, K. A. (1993). DeVoogd, T. J. (2000). Seasonal changes in the
Photoperiodic adaptation to breeding at different lati- hippocampal formation of a food hoarding bird: the
tudes in great tits. General and Comparative black-capped chickadee. Journal of Neurobiology, 44,
Endocrinology, 90, 1422. 414422.
Smith, S. (1991). The Black-capped Chickadee: Behavioral Tramontin, A. D. and Brenowitz, E. A. (2000). Seasonal
Ecology and Natural History. Cornell University Press, plasticity in the adult brain. Trends in Neuroscience, 23,
Ithaca. 251258.
CHAPTER 5

Fine-scale variation in the timing


of reproduction in titmice and
chickadees
Scott M. Ramsay and Ken A. Otter

phase, the focus shifted to determining the mech-


5.1 Introduction
anisms responsible for this variation; much of this
Variation among female songbirds in the timing of work continues today (Section 5.3). The third phase
clutch initiation has been studied, nearly continu- has shifted back to effects of variation in reproduc-
ously, for at least the last 60 years. Rather than a tive timing, particularly in the face of current and
focus on control of broad timing patterns across sea- anticipated changes in climate (Sections 5.4 and
sons (Chapter 4), this research has concerned itself 5.5). In this most recent phase, researchers have
with fine-scale adjustment of reproductive timing attempted to link female capacity for adjusting
for strategic breeding purposes. It has benefited clutch initiation to regular climatic cycles, such as
from long-term data sets that have accumulated in a the North Atlantic Oscillation, and anthropogenic
number of studies including, but not limited to, the changes in climate, as well as the changing selec-
great and blue tits breeding at Wytham in Oxford, tion regimes imposed by these climatic effects. In
UK (Lack 1966, and review in McCleery and Perrins all of this research, little effort has been directed
1988), great tits at a number of sites in the toward studying the onset of fertility in males;
Netherlands (Kluijver 1951, and reviewed in van however, the same questions posed about female
Noordwijk and van Balen 1988), and more recent timing apply to males, and males face the same
studies of blue tits nesting in the Mediterranean problems as females with regard to climate vari-
forests of southern France and Corsica. Studies of ation (Section 5.6).
reproductive timing in North American Parids are Parids offer ideal subjects for the study of repro-
limited, but in recent years we have begun studying ductive timing. First and foremost, clutch sizes are
reproductive timing in black-capped chickadees. larger and more variable than in most other species
Our goal is to summarize the knowledge gained on of songbirds; one of the key effects of variation in
reproductive timing in Eurasian species, compare it timing is a seasonal decline in clutch size, thus, the
to the fledgling research on this topic in North effects of variable timing can be more finely dis-
America, and, finally, suggest both directions and sected. Second, most populations of tits are resi-
species where comparable work could be initiated dent, with only occasional irruptive movements.
on this continent. This behavior permits researchers to study the rela-
Historically, there have been three distinct phases tionship between winter ecology and the timing of
of research on reproductive timing, each with a breeding, both as a cause of variability leading up
slightly different focus. In the earliest phase, to the breeding season and as an outcome of early
research focused on describing the degree of vari- or late breeding. Third, most tits will readily visit
ation in timing among females and some of the feeding stations throughout the year, permitting
effects of this variation (Section 5.2). In the second researchers to experimentally manipulate food

55
56 B E H AV I O R A N D E V O L U T I O N

availability. Finally, many of the well-studied popu- inconsistencies did occur (in both species) could
lations nest in boxes, giving researchers a greater not be attributed to temperature.
ability to monitor, and potentially manipulate, An obvious correlate of temperature is food
nesting phenology and clutch sizes than would be availability. Great tits provision their nestlings with
possible in populations nesting in natural cavities the larvae of insects that feed on oak leaves. Insect
(see also Chapter 18). eggs hatch around the time of bud-burst, which
This final point may explain the greater volume occurs later in cold springs, and the date by which
of research on reproductive timing in Eurasian ver- half of all winter moths pupated was tightly
sus North American species. While great and blue matched to the mean hatch date of tits. Lack noted
tits readily take to nest boxes, the most commonly that clutch initiation dates within a population can
studied North American Parid, the black-capped vary by as much as 2 or 3 weeks in any given year.
chickadee, prefers natural cavities (Smith 1991). As a result, some individuals will necessarily be
This can make studies of chronology more difficult early or late in relation to caterpillar peaks. This
despite the availability of long-term data sets. imperfect correspondence led Lack to wonder why
Research on reproductive timing in North America natural selection had not eliminated late laying,
has instead focused on other box-using species, since early-laying birds produce more surviving
such as tree swallows Tachycineta bicolor and eastern young. Lack assumed that some other factor must
bluebirds (Sialia sialis; reviewed in Dunn 2004). affect timing, and surmised this may be the time
Several North American Parids are more apt than required for a female to gather sufficient food to
black-capped chickadees to use nest boxes, such as produce a clutch.
Carolina and mountain chickadees. Increased focus Typical great tit females lay nearly their body
on these species may provide a better understand- weight in eggs over 9 to 10 days. Lack used high
ing of reproductive timing; however, work similar courtship feeding rates by males and age-based dif-
to that conducted on Eurasian Parids is possible ferences between females in clutch initiation as evi-
with black-capped chickadees, as our own studies dence that, both, females must be energy limited
are beginning to reveal. during egg-laying and yearlings are more heavily
affected by food limitation than older females. Blue
tits lay a few days earlier than great tits in the same
5.2 The history of research on
wood. Since nestlings of both species have similar
reproductive timing: Lack 1966
food requirements, Lack concluded that the differ-
Work by David Lack and his colleagues revealed ence in clutch initiation must be due to the food
that blue and great tits can vary in nest initiation by requirements of the adult females. In support of
as much as 5 weeks between years (Lack 1966), this, Lack noted that great tits breeding in gardens
with the two species varying in parallel. This work with winter feeders laid earlier than those breeding
is summarized in Lacks (1966) Populations Studies in woods. Although offspring production was
of Birds, to which all references in this section refer. timed to coincide with caterpillar production, Lack
In both Dutch great and blue tits, the annual mean concluded that selection would constrain females
onset of laying varies with daily temperatures to laying only when their condition reached the
through March and April: warmer springs lead level necessary for egg productionit was a coinci-
to earlier clutch initiation. Lack observed that dence that these factors (food for eggs and food for
temperature could control annual variation in nestlings) have been temporally correlation in
clutch initiation directly or indirectly through some the past.
third factor correlated with temperature. Sudden Lack was aware that female great and blue tits
cold snaps delayed the onset of clutch initiation that laid later produced smaller clutches. He
among females that had not already begun laying; suggested that although the laying date might be
however, Lack also noted that laying dates were constrained by the availability of food for egg
often consistent among a number of woods in production, the decline of clutch size might very
both England and the Netherlands, and what well be related to the amount of food available for
TIMING OF REPRODUCTION 57

nestlings when they reach their peak demand 10 Motherdaughter comparisons in a Dutch popu-
days after hatching. Lack reasoned that this was the lation of great tits revealed that the heritability of
only possible resolution to the apparent paradox laying date is nearly the same as the repeatability
that by delaying laying females would have greater for individual females, suggesting that the repeat-
amounts of food available to them for egg produc- ability itself is genetic (van Noordwijk et al. 1981).
tion. Research in the intervening 40 years since has This finding differs from an English population, in
focused on elucidating the causes and costs of vari- which heritability was non-zero for both females
ation in reproductive timing. In the sections that and males. Further, this heritability varied spatially,
follow we examine the work that has been con- suggesting male characteristics and nest sites, in
ducted on reproductive timing over the last four addition to maternal characteristics, can affect
decades. laying date in some populations (van der Jeugd and
McCleery 2002).

5.3 Causes of fine-scale variation in


timing among individuals 5.3.2 Extrinsic factors
Numerous factors have been identified as affecting 5.3.2.1 Photoperiod
the onset of laying among individual females as Increasing day-length in spring is well established
well as between populations. Among these are as the primary cue that stimulates the production of
intrinsic characteristics of the female, such as her hormones triggering the seasonal development of
age, condition, and genotype, and extrinsic factors the gonads in both males and females (reviewed in
such as photoperiod, weather, and food or energy Wingfield et al. 1992; Chapter 4). As noted above,
limitation. At the population level there may be however, the exact timing of layingalthough con-
adaptations to local habitat characteristics. In the sistent within femalescan vary considerably
sections below we review the evidence relating to within and between populations and between
each of these factors. years. It is best to think of females responding to a
hierarchy of cues that finely tune the onset of repro-
duction. Increasing day-length is the first of these
5.3.1 Intrinsic factors
cues and prevents individuals from nesting during
5.3.1.1 Female age/genetics inappropriate seasons. Photoperiod changes on a
Across species, younger females typically lay later consistent annual cycle, and is the same for all
than older females, and this pattern has been seen females breeding at the same latitude. Therefore, if
in great tits (McCleery and Perrins 1988; Nager and we are interested in understanding variation
van Noordwijk 1995) as well as other tit species between females, both within and between years,
(reviewed in Smith 1991). In spite of this effect of we must consider other cues lower in the hierarchy
age, numerous studies have also revealed effects of that vary spatially and temporally.
genetics and female quality on clutch initiation In spite of the limitations of photoperiod to
dates. explain fine-scale variation within and between
Both great tits (van Noordwijk et al. 1981; van der females at a single location, evidence from island
Jeugd and McCleery 2002) and marsh tits (Smith and mainland populations of French blue tits
1993) show high individual repeatability of laying reveals that females in different populations may
date between years; females that lay relatively early have genetically different sensitivities to changing
or late do so consistently across years. In addition, photoperiod (Lambrechts et al. 1997). Under experi-
the repeatability of laying dates among females was mental breeding conditions females began laying
independent of whether they were with the same or when the photoperiod was similar to their home
different partners between years, whereas males environment, and not the local environment; how-
showed no repeatability with different partners (van ever, females from both populations could be
Noordwijk et al. 1981; Smith 1993); this suggests that induced to lay early by stimulating them with long
consistency between years is a female trait. photoperiods in December (Lambrechts and Perret
58 B E H AV I O R A N D E V O L U T I O N

2000). The evidence from these experiments 4-day delay corresponding to the time required for
suggests that variation in photoresponsiveness egg formation (Smith 1991). Laying interruptions
between birds in evergreen vs. deciduous oak that persist for the duration of cold weather have
forests at the same latitude results from a balance of been observed in great tits (Lessells et al. 2002).
selection and migration (Lambrechts et al. 1997; see They have also been observed in blue tits following
also below). cold winters (Dhondt et al. 1983). Similar effects
have been observed in our northern British
5.3.2.2 Temperature Columbia study site with black-capped chickadees,
As Lack (1966) suggested, temperature may affect where episodic snowfalls and subzero tempera-
clutch initiation either as a direct cue to begin lay- tures can persist through the regular egg laying
ing, or as an indirect cue through its effects on both period of early to mid-May (K. A. Otter, K. T. Fort
food availability and the energy requirements of and H. van Oort, unpublished data).
the female; we discuss food and energy limitation
in the next section. 5.3.2.3 Food/energy limitation
Exceptionally cold weather prior to clutch initi- A model of reproductive output suggested that
ation delayed breeding in a Finnish population of females must attain some threshold of condition
Siberian tits (Eeva et al. 2000). Mean laying date of before they can begin laying (Rowe et al. 1994); the
great tits at the same site, likewise, correlated with later they achieve this threshold the smaller their
mean monthly temperatures in April. Variation in optimal clutch size. There is ample empirical
timing was attributed to the direct effects of tem- evidence from a number of studies to suggest that
perature in both species. Similarly, temperatures females are energy limited prior to clutch initiation.
from mid-March to mid-April, but not the later For example, laying interruptions in great tits were
caterpillar peak, were correlated with the onset of more likely to coincide with the steepest clutch
laying in a Swiss population of great tits, suggest- size/initiation relationships, reflecting years when
ing that females are responsive to temperature females were in poor condition (Dhondt et al. 1983).
independent of its effects on later food availability Most small passerines are income breedersthey
(Nager and van Noordwijk 1995). In the same
study, temperature during egg formation (~4 days
prior to laying) had a secondary influence on clutch
initiation over and above the effects of temperature
in the month before. Black-capped chickadees 35
began laying as much as 2 weeks earlier than usual
30
when conditions in April were warmer and drier
Date (1 May = 1)

than normal (Odum 1941). Clutch completion dates


25
varied by nearly 2 weeks among years in our
population of black-capped chickadees in British
20
Columbia (Fig. 5.1) and were significantly earlier in
the years with warmer temperatures in April. 15
Similar patterns have also been seen in Ontario
populations (S. M. Ramsay and K. A. Otter, 10
unpublished data).
Once laying has begun, females of most species 2002 2003 2004
typically continue until their clutches are complete; Year
this pattern has been observed in great tits (Nager
and van Noordwijk 1995) and black-capped chicka- Figure 5.1 Variation in clutch completion dates over 3 years in a
population of black-capped chickadees from British Columbia,
dees (Kluyver 1961). In these populations, females
Canada. Sample sizes were as follows: 2002, n 34; 2003, n 36;
that have not begun laying will delay clutch initi- 2004, n 14. The mean and variance of clutch completion dates
ation until the cold weather passes, usually with a varied significantly among years (p 0.001).
TIMING OF REPRODUCTION 59

rely on constant energy intake in order to form In support of the energy limitation hypothesis, the
eggsas opposed to capital breeders that rely on resting metabolic rate of great tits was highest in
stored reserves for egg formation. Parids are typical the egg-laying phase of reproduction and correl-
in this respect and evidence from both natural and ated significantly with egg mass (Nilsson and
experimental studies reveals the link between food Rberg 2001). Thus it appears that females are
and the timing of laying. energy limited, but only weakly in ways related to
In great tits, natural food abundance affected lay- thermoregulation.
ing date and clutch size both within and between Infection with blood parasites can have an inter-
years (Nager and van Noordwijk 1995; Seki and mediate effect on timing of egg laying; unpara-
Takano 1998). In one case, food supplementation sitized female great tits laid earlier than parasitized
advanced laying date by approximately 5.5 days females (Allander and Bennett 1995). This appeared
(Nager et al. 1997). In blue tits, the effects of food to be the result of: (i) nutrient depletion to females
supplementation varied between individuals, with through competition with the parasites; and (ii) a
low-quality females experiencing the greatest loss of tissue oxygenation capacity through red
benefit (up to 9 days advance in laying date) while blood cells being occupied by parasites. The
high-quality females showed little or no change connection between blood parasites and laying
(Svensson and Nilsson 1995). In none of these dates deserves further investigation in light of
studies did food supplementation affect clutch size. evidence that a major cost of egg production in
This change in laying date with no co-ordinated zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata) and Eurasian
change in clutch size supports the idea that females starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) is prolonged anemia
must reach some condition threshold before laying, (Williams 2005).
but contradicts the idea that there is an optimal
clutch size associated with specific laying dates
5.3.3 Population adaptations to local habitat
(Rowe et al. 1994).
Two different studies of food supplementation Evidence of adaptive variation of laying dates in
suggest that it is energy and not specific nutrients response to local forest phenology comes from a
that limit egg formation. Female great tits advanced long-term study of tits breeding in deciduous and
their laying dates independent of the protein con- evergreen oak forests of the southern mainland in
tent of the food supplement (Nager et al. 1997). France and on the island of Corsica. This work is
Similarly, female blue tits did not differ in the onset reviewed in detail in Chapter 17, but we highlight
of laying when receiving a high protein versus a the main habitat difference here. Breeding is earlier
high fat supplement (Ramsay and Houston 1997). in the mainland compared to the island popula-
Thus, females seem to be capable of synthesizing tions of both blue and coal tits (Blondel 1985). In
the various nutrients they need for egg formation, both species this delay in breeding on the island is
as long as they have an ample supply of food. accompanied by reduced clutch size and fledging
Energy limitation and parasite loads, independ- success, and a lack of second broods (Blondel 1985).
ent of food supplementation, may also affect female The differences of laying date between mainland
timing. Great and blue tits began laying earlier in and island blue tits was related to phenology of
boxes that were naturally warmer; however, the deciduous (mainland) vs. evergreen (island) oak
effects of box temperatures are not as strong as forests (Blondel et al. 1993). The birds in mainland
would be predicted based on the energy limitation evergreen forests breed too early in relation to local
hypothesis (Dhondt and Eyckerman 1979). Female bud burst, but are well-timed for the neighboring
great tits nesting in experimentally cooled boxes deciduous forest. A captive-rearing experiment
had reduced egg volumes, but temperature manip- revealed that there is a genetic basis for the local
ulation had no effect on the onset of laying (Nager laying dates (Blondel et al. 1990).
and van Noordwijk 1992). Female blue tits in One hypothesis explaining locally adapted lay-
heated boxes experienced fewer interruptions of ing dates is that females respond to cues in the buds
laying than controls (Yomtov and Wright 1993). on local trees. However, three independent captive
60 B E H AV I O R A N D E V O L U T I O N

experiments and one natural experiment in blue 5.4.1 Offspring production and recruitment
and great tits failed to show any effect of bud phen-
5.4.1.1 Clutch size
ology on laying dates (Visser et al. 2002). Thus,
Numerous studies have repeated Lacks observa-
aside from photoresponsiveness, the cues that time
tion of a decline of clutch size with laying date:
reproduction in local habitats remain elusive. One
black-capped chickadees (Kluyver 1961; S. M.
possibility is that females are responsive to other
Ramsay unpublished data); marsh tits (Smith 1993);
aspects of tree phenology. Black-capped chickadees
blue tits (Nilsson and Svensson 1993). The box-
breeding in eastern Canada have been observed
nesting habits of Eurasian tits have allowed
feeding on the catkins of birch and aspen trees lead-
researchers to manipulate nesting conditions, such
ing up to laying (S. M. Ramsay personal observa-
as inducing females to delay laying; these experi-
tion). It would be productive to investigate the
ments have resulted in reduced clutch sizes in
possibility that nutrients or other compounds asso-
manipulated females (great tits, Verhulst and
ciated with the catkins trigger reproduction.
Tinbergen 1991, Barba et al. 1995; blue tits, Sanz
Oak and juniper titmice in the US southwest may
1999). The effects of delayed laying were popula-
provide useful insights into the effects of vegetation
tion-specific in one study of great tits, with the
differences, comparable to the blue tits of France.
quality of individual territories and vegetation-
Formerly classified as a single species (the plain
related food availability between the populations
titmouse), the oak titmouse inhabits warm, dry,
likely causing the difference (Verhulst and
evergreen oak forests in California while the
Tinbergen 1991).
juniper titmouse inhabits juniper or pion-juniper
One potential criticism of experimental delays
forests in the Sierra Nevada and Rocky Mountains.
lies in the typical protocol for these studies; delays
Differences in breeding chronology of these sister
are usually induced by removing first clutches once
species may result from differences in prey abun-
laying is complete. As a result, the experimentally
dance across the divergent habitats that they
delayed females have laid many more eggs than
occupy (see Harrap and Quinn 1995). Although
their unmanipulated counterparts. In the light of
they are reported to readily use nest boxes, little
evidence that females given supplemental food will
work has been done on these species; however, one
advance their laying dates without changing their
study of oak titmice revealed that box-nesting
clutch sizes, it would be reasonable to assume that
females laid earlier, had larger clutches, and had
the declines of clutch size seen in manipulated
higher fledging success than females in natural
females may have more to do with female condi-
cavities (Purcell et al. 1997). Clearly, these species
tion rather than timing per se. One solution to this
offer ample opportunity for further study.
problem is to manipulate food supply in one year
and examine the effects of altered timing in the sub-
5.4 Effects of fine-scale variation sequent year (Gienapp and Visser 2006; see below).
in timing
As Lack (1966) was aware, varying nest initiation 5.4.1.2 Synchronization with offspring food supply
dates can affect a number of individual fitness Lack (1966) suggested that the synchronization of
components. At the most basic level, clutch size offspring demand with food availability is a key
declines with laying date. The effects extend to component of selection on laying date. One bit of
hatching and fledging success, the condition of evidence used in Lacks argument was the observa-
offspringespecially as a function of the synchron- tion that clutch sizes decline with laying date
ization of offspring need with the abundance of despite the fact that food abundance for females
foodrecruitment of offspring into the breeding increases as the laying season progresses.
population, the survivorship of adults, and their English great tits varied clutch size and the onset
ability to invest in future bouts of reproduction. In of incubation in response to temperature (Creswell
this section we examine the evidence for each of and McCleery 2003); this maintained synchrony
these effects. between laying date and food availability for
TIMING OF REPRODUCTION 61

offspring by increasing the time between clutch ini-


tiation and hatch dates without altering the
difference between hatch date and the date of half-
fall of winter moth larvae. Likewise, laying dates in
Dutch great tits have correlated with winter moth
half-fall dates for nearly 40 years (van Noordwijk
et al. 1995). Swiss great tits were most successful
when their broods were synchronized with the
local caterpillar peak (Nager and van Noordwijk
1995). Selection should favor early laying, as these
females could strategically delay hatching if neces-
sary by adjusting both clutch size or the onset of
incubation. Late-laying females, by comparison, Figure 5.2 An example of asynchronous hatching in a brood of
would have little capacity for adjusting hatching black-capped chickadees. Photo credit K. A. Otter.
date (van Noordwijk et al. 1995).
Timing of laying to synchronize offspring
demand with caterpillar production does not
appear to be a constraint in high-latitude popula- Experiments to alter the timing of egg-laying in
tions. Great and Siberian tits breeding in northern blue tits have yielded some surprising results. In
Finland experienced peaks of arthropod abundance addition to the expected reductions of fledging suc-
after young had fledged (Eeva et al. 2000). Although cess in experimentally delayed broods (Sanz 1999),
food may be limited during the critical period of females induced to lay early with supplemental
nestling demand, the delayed food production may food also had reduced fledging success (Nilsson
still be useful for parents with dependent fledglings 1994). Experimentally delayed broods did not suf-
(Eeva et al. 2000). This delay in caterpillar produc- fer a reduction in fledging success as a proportion
tion may also affect the over-winter survival of of eggs laid; however, the fledglings were lighter
adults who begin molting during the nestling than controls, but not naturally late broods (Sanz
phase (see below). 1999). These results suggest that clutch initiation,
incubation, and hatch timing are all under stabiliz-
5.4.1.3 Hatching/fledging success ing selection.
In addition to the effects of clutch initiation date on
clutch size, female timing has also been shown to 5.4.1.4 Recruitment
have effects that carry on into the nestling and fledg- The effects of variation in female timing on
ling stages. Great tits with experimentally delayed offspring extend beyond the period of parental
broods showed greater hatching asynchrony, lighter effort, through to over-winter survival and recruit-
nestlings 15 days posthatch, and lower fledging ment of offspring. Blue and great tits produced in
success (Barba et al. 1995; Verhulst and Tinbergen broods with early or intermediate hatch dates
1991). Hatching asynchrony in late nesting attempts experienced greater survival to winter (Norris 1993;
was also seen in black-capped chickadees pairs Barba et al. 1995; Svensson 1997; Verboven and
renesting after their first nests were depredated Visser 1998). This pattern persisted in cross-
(Smith 1991). This hatch asynchrony appears to be fostering experiments suggesting that the effects on
due to the females initiating incubation prior to survival were due to the female that laid the clutch
completing the clutch (Fig. 5.2; K. A. Otter, personal rather than the parents that raised the brood
observation). Such clutches are often smaller than (Norris 1993). This study also showed that early-
the population average, and the nestlings can hatch hatching broods were more synchronized with
over a period of 3 to 4 days. Pressure to hasten hatch available food.
date in relation to seasonal food availability may be Naturally low recruitment into natal populations
the cause of this premature incubation. in black-capped chickadees makes them poor
62 B E H AV I O R A N D E V O L U T I O N

models to test whether future survival is affected Populations nesting at high latitudes may face
by hatch date. Other North American Parids which a different set of tradeoffs between molt and
exhibit higher philopatry may be more suited to provisioning dependent offspring. In northern
such studies. Regardless, there is cause to suggest populations of great and Siberian tits, molt over-
that early nesting may enhance survival in black- laps with the nestling phase (Dhondt 1981; Eeva
capped chickadees indirectly, through the influence et al. 2000), possibly in response to time limitation
of early settlement on dominance rank. Survival is imposed by the short breeding season. Overall,
correlated with rank in black-capped chickadees parental effort may be lower in these populations;
(Smith 1991), and there is evidence that early settle- adult Siberian tits began caching food items for
ment into flocks in the fall may increase the likeli- autumn and winter while they were still feeding
hood of individuals dominating other same-aged fledglings (Eeva et al. 2000).
juveniles. In aviary trials, relative dominance
between two individuals was influenced by the 5.4.2.2 Future breeding costs
order they were placed in the aviary; first birds The effects of varying clutch initiation can extend to
were more likely to dominate later individuals in future reproductive bouts, both in the current sea-
both females (Ramsay and Ratcliffe 2004) and son and in future years. In blue tits (Nilsson 2000),
males (K. A. Otter, unpublished data). If this pat- great tits (Verboven and Verhulst 1996), and black-
tern occurs in wild flocks, there may be an advan- capped chickadees (Odum 1941) early breeding
tage to early dispersal, which would favor earlier dates are associated with an increased ability of
hatching and fledging dates. Early dispersal in females to raise replacement clutches following
crested tits facilitates settlement in preferred habitat brood failure, or second clutches following the
(Lens and Dhondt 1994). successful first broods. Similarly, female great tits
with experimentally delayed broods were less
likely to renest following nest failure (Barba et al.
5.4.2 Future effects on adult fitness
1995). The ability to attempt second broods may
components
have more to do with time constraints than with a
5.4.2.1 Survivorship females ability to invest. A multipopulation study
Variation in clutch initiation dates has been linked across a latitudinal gradient would be necessary to
to over-winter survival of adults in a number of test this idea: if multiple brooding is time-limited
studies. Female great tits with later laying dates within a season, females breeding at higher
suffered higher mortality leading up to the subse- latitudes should be less likely to produce second
quent breeding season (McCleery and Perrins 1988; clutches than more southerly females of similar
Verhulst and Tinbergen 1991). Reduced survival condition.
was also evident in adult blue tits that raised Between years, females may also be affected by
either experimentally advanced (Nilsson 1994) or the costs of delayed breeding. As noted above, lay-
experimentally delayed broods (Nilsson and ing dates are repeatable within females; however,
Svensson 1996). The delayed blue tits expended great tit females compensated for mistimed breed-
more energy, deposited more fat during cold ing in one year by delaying or advancing laying in
weather, lost more mass overnight when tempera- the next year (Nager and van Noordwijk 1995;
tures were low (Nilsson and Svensson 1996) and Gienapp and Visser 2006), but the effects of
delayed females showed indications of lower experimental mismatches were population specific
condition following fledging of their nests (Sanz (Gienapp and Visser 2006). Experimentally manipu-
1999). Another study of blue tits revealed that lated female blue tits laid later in the following
females with higher provisioning effort had lower season both when their nests were delayed (Nilsson
survival during the following winter (Thomas et al. and Svensson 1996), and when supplemental food
2001). Several of these studies have suggested that was used to reduce the asynchrony between
delayed molts associated with late nesting may natural food abundance and nestling demand
influence later survival. (Grieco et al. 2002). One interpretation of this result
TIMING OF REPRODUCTION 63

is that females may learn local food phenology (i.e. great distances. The NAO is related to sea surface
if a territory is consistently early or late in relation temperatures between the Azores and Iceland.
to other nearby territories) and the manipulated Depending on the phase of the oscillation, which
females may thus have used a strategy that all varies on an approximately 8-year cycle, the NAO
females employ once they have settled on a lifetime affects the temperature and precipitation experi-
breeding territory (Grieco et al. 2002). Females may enced in North America and Europe, especially in
also use social strategies to compensate for delayed winter (Stenseth et al. 2003). Not surprisingly, the
nesting as evidenced by higher rates of divorce and NAO has been predicted to have significant effects
subsequent improvement among late-breeding on the ecology of north temperate species (Stenseth
blue tits (Dhondt and Adriaensen 1994). et al. 2003). Female great and blue tits from a
number of populations across Europe time their
clutch-initiation in relation to winter NAO index
5.5 Consequences of long-term climatic
(Sanz 2002). Although the slope of the relationship
variation on reproductive timing
differs between populations, females typically start
A number of recent studies have focused on the laying earlier following warmer, moister winters.
effects of climatic variation on clutch initiation. The NAO does not appear to affect clutch size,
These studies have focused on two different aspects which suggests that this climate pattern causes
of climatic variation, natural cyclic variation and changes in food phenology in concert with its
directional change due to anthropogenic effects. effects on egg production.
The first of these effects is not exclusive of the The relationship of breeding to NAO values for
second (Fig. 5.3) and, indeed, the coincidence of North American species is starting to emerge
the two necessitates long-term studies to elucidate (reviewed in Dunn 1994). Preliminary analysis of
the changes associated with each. black-capped chickadees nesting in eastern
Ontario, Canada, reveals a somewhat similar pat-
tern to the European data: clutch initiation dates
5.5.1 Cyclical climatic variation
over 7 years, from 1994 to 2001, showed a strong
The North Atlantic Oscillation (NAO), like the El relationship with May NAO values (Fig. 5.4;
Nio Southern Oscillation, is an example of a tele- r2 0.8431, p 0.0035). Unlike the European
connection pattern, a situation where temperature results, clutch size was related to winter NAO
and air pressure gradients affect climate over values (negative contributions of October,
Average spring temperature

Time (years)

Figure 5.3 Hypothetical change of average spring temperatures over several decades. A steady, long-term increase in average temperatures
occurs over a background of cyclical variation. This type of pattern is what we expect to see when we combine the effects of decadal cycles such
as the North Atlantic Oscillation with persistent global warming.
64 B E H AV I O R A N D E V O L U T I O N

18

15
Mean first egg (1 May = 1)

12

Figure 5.4 The relationship between May North Atlantic


0 Oscillation (NAO) index values and average clutch initiation
2 1 0 1 2 dates from 1994 to 2001 for a population of black-capped
May NAO Index chickadees breeding in eastern Ontario, Canada.

December, and March NAO values, and positive warmer spring temperatures, but the phenology of
contributions of January and February NAO values caterpillars has advanced leading to mistiming
over nine breeding seasons; stepwise multiple between offspring demand and food availability
regression, whole model r2 0.9957, p 0.0010). (Visser et al. 1998). These results reveal the conflicting
We treat these results with some caution as our data selection pressures that females face when they begin
span just a single NAO cycle; however, we consider nesting. On the one hand, food and energy require-
them ample evidence to justify further study. ments restrict when females may begin laying, yet the
availability of caterpillars will determine the success
of rearing offspring (Visser et al. 2004). This difference
5.5.2 Directional climatic variation
is predicted to lead to effects for both individuals and
Over the last three decades, English great tits have whole populations. For individuals, selection may
advanced their laying dates in association with favor advanced clutch initiation, reduced clutch sizes,
warming spring temperatures (McCleery and advanced onset of incubation, or some combination
Perrins 1998); the laying date in this population cor- of the three (Visser et al. 2004). At the population level,
relates with the sum of daily maximum tempera- the enhanced success of early broods should lead to
tures over March and April which has increased the evolution of earlier laying dates as long as there is
over the same period. Two decades of breeding sufficient heritable variation among females to
data from 24 populations of great and blue respond (Visser et al. 2004). The evidence from French
tits revealed complex responses to climate blue and coal tits suggests that, at least in the past,
change (Visser et al. 2003). Overall, both species populations have had the capacity to evolve repro-
have shown earlier laying, although there are ductive responses to local forest phenology (Blondel
some exceptions where no change or delays have 1985; see above). Finally, selection may favor reduced
occurred. The strength of the breeding advance phenotypic plasticity in females in response to the
varies among populations regardless of their prox- mistiming (Visser et al. 2004). Continued long-term
imity to one another. The warmer temperatures and studies will be required to reveal which of these
earlier laying have also led to an increase in the responses occurs.
frequency of second broods. Alternately, female may simply be limited in
In one Dutch population of great tits, clutch initi- their ability to adjust clutch initiation, resulting
ation has not changed over a 23-year period despite from constraints imposed by the mechanisms
TIMING OF REPRODUCTION 65

that control timing. The control of timing is Parids. An early study of great and blue tits
hierarchicalcontrolled at a coarse level by revealed that the timing of seasonal gonad devel-
photoperiod, and fine-tuned by temperature and opment is related to annual variation of spring
food availability as noted above. However, if temperatures (Marshall 1949). This result was
temperature cues are advanced, their effect may be further supported by an experiment that showed
restricted by stabilizing selection acting on temperature moderating the rate of testis
photosensitivity, the primary cue stimulating development in willow tits and circulating levels of
reproductionalthough French blue tits offer luteinizing hormone in great tits over and above
evidence of the ability of nearby populations to the effects of photoperiod manipulation (Silverin
evolve different responses to photoperiod and Viebke 1994). The timing of birch bud burst
(Lambrechts et al. 1997). Adjustment on a fine scale had a significant effect on circulating levels of
through the effects of temperature on prey luteinizing hormone but not testosterone or
development may allow females only limited gonadal development in captive Swedish great tits
opportunities to better time clutches. Black-capped (Visser et al. 2002). In a study of male Corsican
chickadees in our northern BC population blue tits, singing activity, circulating levels of
faced a spring where high April temperatures testosterone, and testis development showed the
advanced tree phenology by 3 weeks in 2004. same patterns of local habitat adaptation to
Chickadees advanced laying by nearly a week deciduous-oak versus evergreen-oak phenology as
compared to prior years, but this still may have egg-laying in females from the same populations
been insufficient to compensate for the unusually (Caro et al. 2005); the population breeding in the
early springmore than 51% of monitored pairs deciduous habitat was advanced by approximately
abandoned nesting attempts in this year (see 4 weeks compared to the evergreen population due
Chapter 17). to the rate, but not the onset, of testis development.
A pilot study on black-capped chickadees in
eastern Canada revealed a rapid increase of cloacal
5.6 Reproductive timing in males
sperm count in males over the week leading up to
Research in a variety of species has focused on the the laying of the first eggs in the population
relationship between female reproductive syn- (Fig. 5.5). More studies are needed to fully
chrony and extrapair mating. Because of its poten- understand the timing of sperm release in natural
tial to enhance the reproductive success of males populations.
beyond the number of eggs laid by their social
partners, variation in the timing of female fertility
5.6.1 Questions/predictions
may be an important determinant of the timing of
male fertility; however, the evidence for any rela- What factors beyond photoperiod and weather
tionship between reproductive synchrony and control the onset of male fertility? Evidence from
extrapair mating in tits is mixed. There was no rela- tropical antbirds revealed that the presence of food
tionship between synchrony index and extrapair is an important fine-tuning stimulus for gonad
paternity in blue tits (Kempenaers 1997; Krokene development (Hau et al. 2000); perhaps the most
and Lifjeld 2000; Charmantier and Blondel 2003) or interesting finding from this study was that the
black-capped chickadees (Mennill et al. 2004). By visual presentation of food was sufficient to trigger
contrast, in great tits the difference in laying dates testis development.
between females was positively related to the How is the timing of male fertility in male Parids
number of EPY in a nest (i.e. females that laid early related to the timing of egg-laying? Selection
compared to the mates of their extrapair partners should favor males initiating sperm production
had fewer EPY than later-laying females; Strohbach well in advance of female fertility; males that are
et al. 1998). late in relation to females will face a number of
A few studies have looked at the effects of potential costs including the production of infertile
ecological factors on the timing of male fertility in eggs and the loss of paternity to neighboring males.
66 B E H AV I O R A N D E V O L U T I O N

15

Ln (Sperm count + 1)
10

21/04/2001 23/04/2001 25/04/2001 27/04/2001 29/04/2001


Sample date

Figure 5.5 Sperm counts in male black-capped chickadees over a 1-week period leading up to clutch initiation by females. Filled symbols
represent the first or only samples from individuals, while open symbols represent second samples. Squares represent males that were
subsequently resampled.

For males to successfully predict the onset of


5.7 Conclusions
female fertility, the cues used to time sperm pro-
duction should be correlated with those that time Evidence from a variety of populations of tits
egg production. With climate change, males may reveals that clutch initiation is determined by a
potentially become mistimed, although the magni- hierarchy of cues. Photoperiod regulates timing at
tude of any decoupling of the cues for the onset of the coarsest level, while temperature and food
fertility between males and females is likely to be availability fine tune the onset of egg-laying within
less than the mistiming of egg-laying and offspring a year. There is ample evidence that females face
demand; the difference between clutch initiation costs in a number of fitness components if their
and peak offspring demand for food is approxi- reproduction is not optimally timed. In spite of all
mately 40 days, while the difference between sperm this evidence, some questions remain concerning
mobilization and egg laying may be as little as the inter-relationships of timing, temperature, and
1 week. female quality. In addition, a question that puzzled
Is sperm count related to synchrony among Lack (1966)why selection has not weeded out
females? If females are synchronous, then males females that breed lateremains unanswered
may be able to delay sperm release till just before today. The current emphasis on the relationship of
the onset of female fertility. Conversely, if females female timing to climate change may reveal the
are asynchronous then males should begin sperm scope of populations to respond to selection for
production very early, both to capitalize on possible earlier laying and changing sensitivity to the
EPP benefits, and to avoid lost paternity if their relevant cues. Depending on the degree of climate
own partners are early. The ability of males to change over the coming decade, we may be witness
respond to female synchrony will depend on the to a natural experiment that has the potential to
effects of both genetics and condition on the onset lead to widespread population declines (Visser et al.
of male fertility. 2003). There is also a shortage of field studies on the
TIMING OF REPRODUCTION 67

timing of male fertility, both in terms of the degree Blondel, J., Dias, P. C., Maistre, M., and Perret, P. (1993).
of variation among males within populations and Habitat heterogeneity and life-history variation of
the cost of any variation for individuals. Mediterranean blue tits (Parus caeruleus). Auk, 110,
What has been a productive field of research for 511520.
Blondel, J., Perret, P., and Maistre, M. (1990). On the
most of the last century promises to continue to be
genetical basis of the laying-date in an island popula-
so, but the contribution of North American species
tion of blue tits. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 3,
to our understanding of these issues is still in its 469475.
infancy compared to Eurasian tits. Even here, most Caro, S. P., Balthazart, J., Thomas, D. W., Lacroix, A.,
of our understanding of factors affecting timing Chastel, O., and Lambrechts, M. M. (2005). Endocrine
and breeding comes from blue and great tits, pri- correlates of the breeding asynchrony between two
marily due to the long-term studies of box-nesting Corsican populations of blue tits (Parus caeruleus).
populations. Although the best-studied North General and Comparative Endocrinology, 140, 5260.
American Parid is the black-capped chickadee, this Charmantier, A. and Blondel, J. (2003). A contrast in
species is among the least likely to utilize nest extra-pair paternity levels on mainland and island
boxes. While natural-cavity nesting does not populations of Mediterranean blue tits. Ethology, 109,
351363.
preclude studies on timing, it makes them
Creswell, W. and McCleery, R. (2003). How great tits
more challenging. Capitalizing on Parids that are
maintain synchronization of their hatch date with food
predominantly secondary cavity nesters, such as supply in response to long-term variability in tempera-
the mountain chickadee (see Chapter 8), may be ture. Journal of Animal Ecology, 72, 356366.
more productive in yielding the long-term data Dhondt, A. A. (1981). Postnuptial moult of the Great tit in
needed to address these issues. southern Sweden. Ornis Scandinavica, 12, 127133.
Dhondt, A. A. and Adriaensen, F. (1994). Causes and
effects of divorce in blue tit Parus caeruleus. Journal of
Acknowledgments
Animal Ecology, 63, 979987.
SMR has benefited from many conversations over Dhondt, A. A. and Eyckerman, R. (1979). Temperature and
the last several years at conferences and invited date of laying by tits Parus spp. Ibis 121, 329331.
lectures that have helped to inspire his thinking on Dhondt, A. A., Eyckerman, R., and Hubl, J. (1983).
this topic. H. van Oort, K. Fort, and L. M. Ratcliffe Laying interruptions in tits Parus spp. Ibis, 125, 370376.
Dunn, P. (2004). Breeding dates and reproductive per-
shared unpublished data from collaborations on
formance. Advances in Ecological Research, 35, 6987.
our two study sites. N. Barker helped with
Eeva, T., Veistola, S., and Lehikoinen, E. (2000). Timing of
collecting and organizing the reference material breeding in subarctic passerines in relation to food
for this chapter. Both SMR and KAO receive availability. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 78, 6778.
their primary research support from the Natural Gienapp, P. and Visser, M. E. (2006). Possible fitness con-
Sciences and Engineering Research Council of sequences of experimentally advanced laying dates in
Canada. Great Tits: differences between populations in different
habitats. Functional Ecology, 20, 180185.
Grieco, F., van Noordwijk, A. J., and Visser, M. E. (2002).
References Evidence for the effect of learning in timing of repro-
Allander, K. and Bennett, G. F. (1995). Retardation of duction in blue tits. Science, 296, 136138.
breeding onset in great tits (Parus major) by blood para- Harrap, S. and Quinn, D. (1995). Chickadees, Tits,
sites. Functional Ecology, 9, 677682. Nuthatches and Treecreepers. Princeton University Press,
Barba, E., Gil-Delgado, J. A., and Monros, J. S. (1995). The Princeton, New Jersey, 464 pp.
costs of being late: consequences of delaying great tit Hau, M., Wikelski, M., and Wingfield, J. C. (2000). Visual
Parus major first clutches. Journal of Animal Ecology, 64, and nutritional food cues fine-tune timing of reproduc-
642651. tion in a neotropical rainforest bird. Journal of
Blondel, J. (1985). Breeding strategies of the blue tit and Experimental Zoology, 286, 494504.
coal tit (Parus) in mainland and island Mediterranean Kempenaers, B. (1997). Does reproductive synchrony limit
habitats: a comparison. Journal of Animal Ecology, 54, male opportunities or enhance female choice for extra-
531556. pair paternity? Behaviour, 134, 551562.
68 B E H AV I O R A N D E V O L U T I O N

Kluijver, H. N. (1951). The population ecology of the great Nilsson, J.- . (1994). Energetic bottle-necks during breed-
tit, Parus m. major. Ardea, 39, 1135. ing and the reproductive cost of being too early. Journal
Kluyver, H. N. (1961). Food consumption in relation to of Animal Ecology, 63, 200208.
habitat in breeding chickadees. Auk, 78, 532550. Nilsson, J.- . (2000). Time-dependent reproductive deci-
Krokene, C. and Lifjeld, J. T. (2000). Variation in the fre- sions in the blue tit. Oikos, 88, 351361.
quency of extra-pair paternity in birds: a comparison of Nilsson, J.- . and Rberg, L. (2001). The resting metabolic
an island and a mainland population of blue tits. cost of egg laying and nestling feeding in great tits.
Behaviour, 137, 13171330. Oecologia, 128, 187192.
Lack, D. (1966). Population Studies of Birds. Oxford Nilsson, J.- . and Svensson, E. (1993). Energy constraints
University Press, Oxford, 341 pp. and ultimate decisions during egg-laying in the blue tit.
Lambrechts, M. M. and Perret, P. (2000). A long photo- Ecology, 74, 244251.
period overrides non-photoperiodic factors in blue tits Nilsson, J.- . and Svensson, E. (1996). The cost of repro-
timing of reproduction. Proceedings of the Royal Society of duction: a new link between current reproductive effort
London B, 267, 585588. and future reproductive success. Proceedings of the Royal
Lambrechts, M. M., Blondel, J., Maistre, M., and Perret, P. Society of London B, 263, 711714.
(1997). A single response mechanism is responsible for Norris, K. (1993). Seasonal variation in the reproductive
evolutionary adaptive variation in a birds laying date. success of blue tits: an experimental study. Journal of
Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA, 94, Animal Ecology, 62, 287294.
51535155. Odum, E. P. (1941). A comparison of two chickadee sea-
Lens, L. and Dhondt, A. A. (1994). Effects of habitat frag- sons. Auk, 13, 154159.
mentation on the timing of Crested Tit Parus cristatus Purcell, K. L., Verner, J., and Oring L. W. (1997). A com-
natal dispersal. Ibis, 136, 147152. parison of the breeding ecology of birds nesting in
Lessells, C. M., Dingemanse, N. J., and Both, C. (2002). boxes and tree cavities. Auk, 114, 646656.
Weights, egg component weights, and laying gaps in Ramsay, S. L. and Houston, D. C. (1997). Nutritional con-
great tits in relation to ambient temperature. Auk, 119, straints on egg production in the blue tit: a supplemen-
10911103. tary feeding study. Journal of Animal Ecology, 66,
Marshall, A. J. (1949). Weather factors and spermatogen- 649657.
esis in birds. Proceedings of the Zoological Society of Ramsay, S. M. and Ratcliffe, L. M. (2003). Determinants of
London, 119, 711716. social rank in female black-capped chickadees (Poecile
McCleery, R. H. and Perrins, C. M. (1988). Lifetime repro- atricapilla). Canadian Journal of Zoology, 81, 117121.
ductive success of the great tit, Parus major. In: Clutton- Rowe, L., Ludwig, D., and Schluter D. (1994). Time, con-
Brock, T. H., ed. Reproductive Success: Studies of dition, and the seasonal decline of avian clutch size.
Individual Variation in Contrasting Breeding Systems, pp. American Naturalist, 143, 698722.
136153. University of Chicago Press, Chicago. Sanz, J. J. (1999). Seasonal variation in reproductive suc-
McCleery, R. H. and Perrins, C. M. (1998). . . . temperature cess and post-nuptial moult of blue tits in southern
and egg-laying trends. Nature, 391, 3031. Europe: an experimental study. Oecologia, 121, 377382.
Mennill, D. J., Ramsay, S. M., Boag, P. T., and Ratcliffe, Sanz, J. J. (2002). Climate change and breeding parameters
L. M. (2004). Patterns of extrapair mating in relation to of great and blue tits throughout the western Palearctic.
male dominance status and female nest placement in Global Change Biology, 8, 409422.
black-capped chickadees. Behavioral Ecology, 15, Seki, S.-I. and Takano, H. (1998). Caterpillar abundance in
757765. the territory affects the breeding performance of great
Nager, R. G. and van Noordwijk, A. J. (1992). Energetic tit Parus major major. Oecologia, 114, 514521.
limitation in the egg-laying period of great tits. Silverin, B. and Viebke, P. A. (1994). Low temperatures
Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 249, 259263. affect the photoperiodically induced LH and testicular
Nager, R. G. and van Noordwijk, A. J. (1995). Proximate cycles differently in closely related species of tits (Parus
and ultimate aspects of phenotypic plasticity in timing spp.). Hormones and Behavior, 28, 199206.
of great tit breeding in a heterogeneous habitat. Smith, H. G. (1993). Seasonal decline in clutch size of the
American Naturalist, 146, 454474. marsh tit (Parus palustris) in relation to date specific sur-
Nager, R. G., Ruegger, C., and van Noordwijk, A. J. (1997). vival of offspring. Auk, 110, 889899.
Nutrient or energy limitation on egg formation: a feed- Smith, S. M. (1991). The Black-Capped Chickadee: Behavioral
ing experiment in great tits. Journal of Animal Ecology, Ecology and Natural History. Cornell University Press,
66, 495507. Ithaca, NY.
TIMING OF REPRODUCTION 69

Stenseth, N. C., Ottersen, G., Hurrell, J. W., Mysterud, better than the quality hypothesis. Journal of Animal
A., Lima, M., Chan, K.-S., Yoccoz, N. G., and Ecology, 65, 264273.
dlandsvik, B. (2003). Studying climate effects on Verboven, N. and Visser, M. E. (1998). Seasonal variation
ecology through the use of climate indices: the North in local recruitment of great tits: the importance of
Atlantic Oscillation, El Nio Southern Oscillation and being early. Oikos, 81, 511524.
beyond. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 270, Verhulst, S. and Tinbergen, J. M. (1991). Experimental evi-
20872096. dence for a causal relationship between timing and suc-
Strohbach, S., Curio, E., Bathen, A., Epplen, J. T., and cess of reproduction in the great tit Parus m. major.
Lubjuhn, T. (1998). Extrapair paternity in the great tit Journal of Animal Ecology, 60, 269282.
(Parus major): a test of the good genes hypothesis. Verhulst, S., van Balen, J. H., and Tinbergen, J. M. (1995).
Behavioral Ecology, 9, 388396. Seasonal decline in reproductive success of the great tit:
Svensson, E. (1997). Natural selection on avian breeding variation in time or quality? Ecology, 76, 23922403.
time: causality, fecundity-dependent, and fecundity- Visser, M. E., Adriaensen, F., van Balen, J. H., Blondel, J.,
independent selection. Evolution, 51, 12761283. Dhondt, A. A., van Dongen, S., du Feu, C., Ivankina, E.
Svensson, E. and Nilsson, J.-. (1995). Food supply, terri- V., Kerimov, A. B., de Laet, J., Matthysen, E., McCleery,
tory quality, and reproductive timing in the blue tit R., Orell. M., and Thomson, D. L. (2003). Variable
(Parus caeruleus). Ecology, 76, 18041812. responses to large-scale climate change in European
Thomas, D. W., Blondel, J., Perret, P., Lambrechts, M. M., Parus populations. Proceedings of the Royal Society of
and Speakman, J. R. (2001). Energetic and fitness costs London B, 270, 367372.
of mismatching resource supply and demand in sea- Visser, M. E., Both, C., and Lambrechts, M. M. (2004).
sonally breeding birds. Science, 291, 25982600. Global climate change leads to mistimed avian repro-
van der Jeugd, H. P. and McCleery R. (2002). Effects of duction. Advances in Ecological Research, 35, 89110.
spatial autocorrelation, natal philopatry and pheno- Visser, M. E., Silverin, B., Lambrechts, M. M., and
typic plasticity on the heritability of laying date. Journal Tinbergen, J. M. (2002). No evidence for tree phenology
of Evolutionary Biology, 15, 380387. as a cue for the timing of reproduction in tits Parus spp.
van Noordwijk, A. J. and van Balen, J. H. (1988). The great Avian Science, 2, 7786.
tit, Parus major. In: Clutton-Brock, T. H., ed. Reproductive Visser, M. E., van Noordwijk, A. J., Tinbergen, J. M., and
Success: Studies of Individual Variation in Contrasting Lessells, C. M. (1998). Warmer springs lead to mis-timed
Breeding Systems, pp. 119135. University of Chicago reproduction in great tits (Parus major). Proceedings of the
Press, Chicago. Royal Society of London B, 265, 18671870.
van Noordwijk, A. J., McCleery, R. H., and Perrins, C. M. Williams, T. D. (2005). Mechanisms underlying the costs
(1995). Selection for the timing of great tit breeding in of egg production. BioScience, 55, 3948.
relation to caterpillar growth and temperature. Journal Wingfield, J. C., Hahn, T. P., Levin, R., and Honey, P.
of Animal Ecology, 64, 451458. (1992). Environmental predictability and control of
van Noordwijk, A. J., van Balen, J. H., and Scharloo, W. gonadal cycles in birds. Journal of Experimental Zoology,
(1981). Genetic variation in the timing of reproduction 261, 214231.
in the great tit. Oecologia 49, 158166. Yomtov, Y. and Wright, J. (1993). Effect of heating nest
Verboven, N. and Verhulst, S. (1996). Seasonal variation in boxes on egg-laying in the blue tit (Parus caeruleus). Auk,
the incidence of double broods: the date hypothesis fits 110, 9599.
This page intentionally left blank
SYNOPSIS I

Proximate mechanisms in behavior


and evolution
David F. Sherry, Vladimir V. Pravosudov,
Scott A. MacDougall-Shackleton, Jennifer S. Hoshooley,
and Leslie S. Phillmore

Chickadees and tits have long been known as (Stresemann 1947). The ease of holding birds in
highly tractable subjects for research in behavior captivity has made possible a great deal of research
and ecology. Studies of Eurasian tits and North on behavior, cognition, and neurobiology in species
American chickadees have made substantial for which there is extensive information on behav-
contributions to our understanding of breeding ior in the wild (Chapters 2, 3, and 4). The integra-
phenology (Chapter 4; Chapter 5), food-storing tion of field and laboratory work has, for example,
(Chapter 2; Chapter 3), social behavior (Chapter 9), led to advances in research on control of the annual
communication (Section III), foraging, and other cycle. Although temperature, food supply, and
topics. More recently, research on the Paridae has other factors exert important effects on the timing
begun to address questions about the proximate of reproduction, photoperiod is the stimulus that
neural and neuroendocrine causes of behavior. initiates the sequence of events leading to gonadal
There are many reasons for the intense focus on recrudescence, mating, egg laying, and parental
these small, familiar birds. Most Parids are rela- care. These behavioral and physiological changes
tively tame, year-round residents that live in small are not, strictly, controlled by day-length but
home ranges and are easily attracted to feeders. instead, as Phillmore and MacDougall-Shackleton
Some, such as great and blue tits, readily breed in describe, by an annual cycle in sensitivity to day-
artificial nest boxes (though the most intensely length. Successive stages of photorefractoriness
studied North American Parid, the black-capped and photosensitivity prepare birds to respond to
chickadee tends not to). These traits make it increasing day-length in spring and initiate
possible, at least in principle, to obtain complete breeding. These responses include change in size of
observations on development, social organization, the song control nuclei HVC and RA, and the
communication, and reproduction in individually- recruitment of new neurons into these structures.
marked birds over their entire lifetime. In both Some recent research also shows an annual pattern
North America and Eurasia there are enough in the recruitment of new neurons into the
species to permit comparative analyses, and some hippocampus and change in the overall size of this
species, such as the black-capped chickadee, have structure, but data from different studies have not
distributions large enough to permit comparisons been consistent. There are different opinions about
between populations living in very different the function of such patterns (Chapter 3) and
environmental conditions (Chapters 3 and 5). seasonal change in the hippocampus continues to
Most chickadees and tits thrive in captivity. Early be a topic of active investigation.
in the 1700s Baron von Pernau kept marsh tits in Remarkably, for both the song control nuclei
his room to observe their food-storing behavior and the hippocampus, laboratory and field studies

71
72 B E H AV I O R A N D E V O L U T I O N

present rather different pictures. Change in the (Chapter 5), on fertility in males, who are exposed
size of song control nuclei can be produced in the to the same environmental cues as females but face
laboratory by manipulating photoperiod but is not the problem not of timing hatch to the peak in
found in samples of birds taken from the wild at insect food for the young but timing sperm produc-
different times of year. In contrast, change in the tion to the peak in female fertility.
size of the hippocampus has been found in samples As a buffer against environmental variation in
of birds collected in the field but does not occur food availability, many chickadees and tits, though
when photoperiod is manipulated in the laboratory. not all, have available to them the tactic of food
There are a number of possible explanations for storing. A food-storing chickadee can create many
these seemingly contradictory outcomes, discussed thousands of food caches over the fall and winter
by Phillmore and MacDougall-Shackleton in and these caches make an important contribution to
Chapter 4. These results illustrate, however, one of over-winter survival. Caches are widely scattered
the significant challenges for research on chicka- over the birds home range and chickadees and tits
dees and tits, and other passerines, as well. In have been shown to retrieve stored food by remem-
many field studies, samples are collected from bering where they put it. Remembering large num-
wild populations at different times in order to bers of scattered spatial locations for up to several
understand changes occurring over time within weeks is a striking cognitive achievement and a
individuals. The conclusion that change in a great deal of recent research, described by Sherry
temporal series of samples represents change and Hoshooley (Chapter 2), has investigated
within a typical individual is, however, an infer- exactly how chickadees and tits manage this feat.
ence. When annual change is observed in samples Controlled studies tend to show that chickadees
collected in the wild, as is the case for hippocampal and tits are sometimes not much better than other
size, the observed change may occur not because of birds at laboratory spatial memory tasks. What
annual change within individuals but because of chickadees and tits do possess is a predisposition to
demographic changes in the population from solve problems spatially, rather than by using non-
which the samples are drawn (Chapter 3). In the spatial cues such as color or form. Food-storing
laboratory, control over diet, housing, temperature, birds, including Parids, also have a hippocampus
and the timing of experimental manipulations may that is dramatically larger relative to the size of the
make it possible to detect annual change within rest of the brain than the hippocampus of non-
individuals, for example in the song control nuclei, storing birds such as thrushes, sparrows, and wood
that is masked in samples collected from the wild warblers. The hippocampus is a forebrain structure
because individuals undergo annual change out of found in both birds and mammals and is known to
phase with each other or exhibit other individual play a role in a variety of cognitive functions,
differences (Chapter 4). including spatial memory (Chapter 2). The
The many influences on behavior and physiology enlarged hippocampus of food-storing birds may
in the wild may lead to a more complex determin- thus provide new information on the evolution of
ation of events such as the timing of laying than the brain and the neural basis of spatial cognition.
might be suspected from laboratory studies. As Despite a great deal having been learned about
Ramsay and Otter (Chapter 5) describe, there is the behavior and brain of food-storing Parids,
considerable variation among females in the timing much basic information remains tantalizingly out
of laying, which probably reflects variation in the of view. As Pravosudov (Chapter 3) points out,
ability to obtain the resources for egg production, there is little solid evidence on the exact annual
heritable and perhaps adaptive variation in pattern of food storing in chickadees. There is
response to environmental cues, and exposure to conflicting evidence about how long chickadees
varying environmental conditions. Although a leave their caches in place before retrieving them,
great deal is known about factors that influence and, as noted above, about seasonal changes in the
the timing of laying in females, there has been brain of food-storing chickadees. There is current
much less work, as Ramsay and Otter describe debate over whether differences in the size of the
TIMING OF REPRODUCTION 73

hippocampus that occur between food-storing and behavior. Along with the control and repeatability
non-storing families of birds (Krebs et al. 1989; of laboratory research, however, comes the risk of
Sherry et al. 1989) occur within families like the losing touch with the natural history of behavior
Paridae that contain both storing and non-storing that both inspires new questions about proximate
species (Brodin and Lundborg 2003; Garamszegi mechanisms (Sherry 2005) and grounds studies of
and Eens 2004; Lucas et al. 2004; Garamszegi and causation with reliable information about how
Lucas 2005). There is also controversy over whether chickadees behave in the wild. Field studies remain
recently reported differences between North the indispensable source of new questions about
American and European Parids in the relative size the Paridae and the raison dtre for bringing to
of the brain and hippocampus (Lucas et al. 2004; bear the powerful techniques of laboratory
Garamszegi and Lucas 2005) are simply the result research.
of procedural differences between laboratories
(Pravosudov and de Kort 2006) or the consequence
References
of differences in life history traits (Garamszegi and
Lucas 2005). Research on food storing would bene- Brodin, A. and Lundborg, K. (2003). Is hippocampus
fit enormously from an omniscient glimpse of just volume affected by specialization for food hoarding in
one annual cycle of the caching activity of the mem- birds? Proceedings of the Royal Society London B, 270,
bers of a single, winter flock of chickadees and the 15551563.
Garamszegi, L. Z. and Eens, M. (2004). The evolution of
fate of the food they stored. Better make that ten
hippocampus volume and brain size in relation to food
annual cycles, to deal with year-to-year variation in
hoarding in birds. Ecology Letters, 7, 12161224.
seed and insect abundance, flock demographics, Garamszegi, L. Z. and Lucas, J. R. (2005). Continental
weather, habitat change, and other factors that variation in relative hippocampal volume in birds: the
influence food storing behavior. Short of such phylogenetic extent of the effect and the potential role
omniscience, however, a clearer partial picture of of winter temperatures. Biology Letters, 1, 330333.
food caching in the wild would still be invaluable. Krebs, J. R., Sherry, D. F., Healy, S. D., Perry, V. H., and
As with the discovery of large capacity long- Vaccarino, A. L. (1989). Hippocampal specialization of
lasting spatial memory in food-storers, research on food-storing birds. Proceedings of the National Academy of
the Parids continues to uncover new and unex- Sciences USA, 86, 13881392.
pected properties of proximate mechanisms of Lucas, J. R., Brodin, A. de Kort, S. R., and Clayton, N. S.
(2004). Does hippocampal size correlate with the degree
behavior. Research described by Pravosudov
of caching specialization? Proceedings of the Royal Society
(Chapter 3) on the influence on memory of
London B, 271, 24232429.
corticosteronean adrenal steroid that plays an Pravosudov, V. V. and de Kort, S. R. (2006). Is the western
important role in energy metabolism, stress and scrub-jay (Aphelocoma californica) really an underdog
immune functionis a good example. Unlike among food-caching corvids when it comes to
previous results, which tend to show a positive hippocampal volume and food caching propensity?
effect on memory of acute corticosterone elevation Brain Behavior and Evolution, 67, 19.
and a negative effect of chronic elevation, mountain Sherry, D. F. (2005). Do ideas about function help in the
chickadees show a positive effect on spatial study of causation? Animal Biology, 55, 441456.
memory of prolonged, moderate corticosterone Sherry, D. F., Vaccarino, A. L., Buckenham, K., and Herz,
elevation. R. S. (1989). The hippocampal complex of food-storing
birds. Brain Behavior and Evolution, 34, 308317.
The future of research on proximate mechanisms
Stresemann, E. (1947). Baron von Pernau, pioneer student
of behavior will surely lie in controlled laboratory
of bird behavior. Auk, 64, 3552.
studies of the neural and neuroendocrine causes of
This page intentionally left blank
SECTION II

Reproductive ecology, evolution,


and behavior

Reproductive studies on Eurasian Parids have 10-year data set of black-capped chickadees to
traditionally focused on topics that arise from the determine the influence of relative position in the
ability to induce large numbers of individuals to dominance hierarchy within wintering flocks on
nest in easily accessible boxes, such as optimum the lifetime reproductive potential of individual
clutch sizes, reproductive success, and reproductive birds. Where sympatric populations are found,
timing in relation to abiotic factors (see Chapter 5). interspecific interactions and hybridization poten-
Fewer long-term research programs have been tial add to the complexity of studying Parid repro-
initiated on chickadees and titmice, possibly due to ductive ecology (Chapter 7). Curry, Rossano, and
the greater difficulty in establishing box-nesting Reudink assess hybridization in the black-capped/
populations with the same high-density breeding Carolina chickadee hybrid zone using genetic
potential exhibited by great and blue tits. While markers and behavioral isolating mechanisms,
long-term studies on natural-cavity-nesting Parids often finding little congruence between these two
are increasing, research on North American Parids means of characterizing species.
has tended to focus on the topics of intra and inter- In contrast to interspecific overlap, isolation of
specific interactions and their influence on repro- populations and speciation is an exciting avenue of
ductive ecology, evolution, and behavior. Whereas research on North American species, and has
the previous section of this book focused on the potential among the groups of sister species that
extrinsic and intrinsic factors that affect behaviors, have recently undergone taxonomic splits (see
including reproduction, it is the influence of social Preface). Burg (Chapter 6) assesses the biogeog-
interactions between and within species that form raphy of chestnut-backed chickadees using
the basis for grouping chapters in this section. microsatellite analysis, providing alternative scen-
Typically, North American Parids have more arios for the glacial refugia and dispersal patterns
parapatric distributions than Eurasian Parids. that could explain the present distribution of dis-
In these isolated circumstances, intraspecific tinct, genetic populations. This chapter also consid-
interactions may influence reproduction to a great ers the potential for hybridization within the
extent, as is evident by the work presented brown-capped chickadees to contribute to differen-
by Ratcliffe, Mennill, and Schubert on social tiation among disjunct populations within the
dominance and reproductive potential in black- chestnut-backed chickadees of northwestern
capped chickadees (Chapter 9). Ratcliffe et al. use a Canada and Alaska.

75
76 R E P R O D U C T I V E E C O L O G Y, E V O L U T I O N, A N D B E H AV I O R

Even where hybridization may be limited, inter- chickadees in central British Columbia, especially in
specific interactions of chickadees extends beyond light of an unprecedented epidemic of mountain
the Parid family, as these cavity-nesting species are pine beetle in the region. Being primarily a second-
part of a larger guild of birds and mammals that are ary-cavity user rather than an excavating species,
primary or secondary cavity users (Nest Webs). the interaction between prey cycles and population
Martin and Norris (Chapter 8) examine the levels of other facilitating species, such as wood-
influence of prey availability and interspecific inter- peckers, in these Nest Webs creates an intricate pic-
actions on the population dynamics of mountain ture of interdependence in these communities.
CHAPTER 6

Phylogeography of chestnut-backed
chickadees in western North America
Theresa M. Burg

6.1 Introduction
2005phylogeny reprinted earlier in this book, see
North American chickadees and titmice are Fig. 2.1). The mtDNA restriction data placed the
believed to be descendents of Eurasian lineages Mexican chickadee within the brown-capped group
that crossed the Bering land bridge in the late while phylogenies using mtDNA sequence data
Pliocene 3.5 to 4 million years ago (Gill et al. 2005; showed the three brown-capped species as a mono-
see also Chapters 1 and 2). The North American phyletic clade nested within the black-capped
chickadees are divided into two groups based on clade. Based on standard mtDNA divergence rates
phenotypic differences (references in Gill et al. of 2%/MY (million years), it appears that speci-
1989). The black-capped group contains four ation in the North American chickadees occurred
species: black-capped, Carolina, Mexican, and during the late Pliocene or early Pleistocene
mountain chickadees. As their name suggests, these (Gill et al. 1993, 2005). While any such dates are
have a black-cap and morphologically they are approximate, and debated, the timing corresponds
most similar to the Eurasian species in the Poecile with Pleistocene divergence of other avian taxa,
clade, particularly the willow tit. The brown- especially boreal species (e.g. Johnson and Cicero
capped group contains three species: boreal, chest- 2004; Weir and Schluter 2004; Lovette 2005).
nut-backed chickadees, and the Siberian tit. In The Pleistocene glaciations were influential in
North America, Siberian tits are only found in the shaping the population structure of many species,
northwestern Arctic and are considered by some to particularly those now inhabiting temperate and
be a distinct subspecies (gray-headed chickadee, polar regions. Most of Canada and parts of the
Sibley 2000). Birds of the brown-capped group have northern United States were covered under thick
brownish head feathers and a rusty colored back. sheets of ice (Pielou 1991). Three main ice sheets
Several studies attempted to determine the phylo- were present during the Pleistocene: Cordilleran
genetic relationship of Parids (Gill et al. 1989, 1993, (west of Rocky Mountains), Innuitian (Arctic) and
2005). Gill et al. (1989) using allozymes tentatively Laurentide (east of Rocky Mountains: Pielou 1991;
concluded that the brown-capped and black- Dyke et al. 2002). The glaciers expanded and
capped groups were more similar to each other contracted during the alternating cold glacial and
than to any of the Eurasian lineages, and chestnut- warm interglacial periods creating a dynamic
backed and boreal chickadees were closely related environment for species living at the edges of the
as were Mexican and mountain chickadees. A glaciers. Populations became fragmented and
chromosomal rearrangement found in both Mexican subsequently accrued genetic differences. As the
and mountain chickadees adds support to the glaciers retreated, some of these allopatric popula-
allozyme data (unpublished data cited in Gill et al. tions merged, while others remained separated.
1989). The two subsequent phylogenies employed This chapter focuses on factors influencing the
mitochondrial DNA (mtDNA) (Gill et al. 1993, contemporary patterns of population structure in

77
78 R E P R O D U C T I V E E C O L O G Y, E V O L U T I O N, A N D B E H AV I O R

chestnut-backed chickadees including historical


range expansion, geographic distribution and dis-
cuss potential barriers to dispersal. I then compare
the patterns found in this western North American
species to other North American and Eurasian
Parids.

cAK
6.2 Chestnut-backed chickadees
6.2.1 Origin AA
Grinnell (1904) hypothesized that the chestnut-
backed chickadee diverged from the boreal chicka- QCI NBC
dee in the arid interior of Alaska from a
pre-hudsonicus ancestor. Chestnut-backed chicka-
dees colonized the more humid, coastal regions of
SEBC
Alaska and British Columbia expanding south VI
along the coast to California. Boreal chickadees are
found throughout most of Canada, but are rarely
found along the Pacific Coast. The coastal moun- MtR
tain ranges in British Columbia and Alaska form OR
the western and eastern range limits of the boreal
and coastal chestnut-backed chickadees, respect-
ively. In south-eastern British Columbia, the range
of the interior chestnut-backed chickadee (Fig. 6.1)
overlaps with that of the boreal chickadee. Genetic
studies by Gill et al. (1993, 2005) and Weir and
Schluter (2004) support the hypothesis that chest-
nut-backed and boreal chickadees are sister species
having diverged in the early Pleistocene
(~1.8 MYA). Given the contemporary distribution
of these two species, it is possible that chestnut-
backed chickadees originated in the north and
spread southward (Grinnell 1904). Figure 6.1 Distribution of chestnut-backed chickadees in western
North America. Species range is indicated by dashed lines. Sampling
sites include coastal, mainland Alaska (cAK, n 44), Alexander
6.2.2 Biogeography Archipelago (AA, n 9), Queen Charlotte Islands (QCI, n 43),
northern British Columbia (NBC, n 38), Vancouver Island (VI,
The chestnut-backed chickadee is a year-round resi- n 40), North Cascades Park (NCP, n 21), Mount Rainier (MtR,
dent of mature coniferous forests on the west coast n 16), Oregon (OR, n 8) and south-eastern British Columbia
(SEBC, n 30). The star represents 1904 Coeur dAlene, ID
of North America. Chestnut-backed chickadees are population (Grinnell 1904). Solid lines separate genetically distinct
the only Parid found on the larger coastal islands populations as determined by differences in allele frequency
off the British Columbia coast. On the mainland, distributions (see Table 6.1).
their distribution overlaps with black-capped and
mountain chickadees; however, it appears that they
do not directly compete for resources due to niche black-capped chickadee forages in deciduous trees
partitioning (Smith 1967; Sturman 1968; Brennan (Campbell et al. 1997). In California, where the
et al. 1999, 2000). The chestnut-backed chickadee chestnut-backed chickadee recently expanded its
forages in the canopy of conifers while the range into that of the mountain chickadee, the
PHYLOGEOGRAPHY 79

chestnut-backed chickadees nest in denser forests range expansion may have occurred in the interior,
and forage on Douglas fir (Pseudotsuga menziesii) mainly in Idaho and south-eastern BC. Descriptions
and black oak (Quercus kelloggii) while mountain of the interior populations are limited; Grinnell
chickadees nest in more open sites and forage on (1904) found two sites near Coeur dAlene, ID
Ponderosa (Pinus ponderosa) and sugar pine (P. lam- (Fig. 6.1), however, the current distribution is
bertiana) and white fir (Abies concolor) (Brennan et al. much larger (approx. 200 km2: Grinnell 1904;
1999, 2000). Chestnut-backed chickadees are found Dahlsten et al. 2002). Other surveys in the early
in cedarhemlock forests in the Pacific Northwest 1900s reported an abundance of chestnut-backed
and nest in cavities of old trees, with breeding pairs chickadees west of the Cascades, but very few
defending relatively large territories (Carey et al. sightings east of the Cascades (Bowles 1909; Brooks
1991). However, during the winter months chest- and Swarth 1925).
nut-backed chickadees can be seen in mixed species The limited amount of information available
flocks with red-breasted nuthatches (Sitta canaden- regarding individual movements of chestnut-
sis), bushtits (Psaltriparus minimus), kinglets backed chickadees suggests that short distance
(Regulus spp.), brown creepers (Certhia americana), dispersal does occur. Banding data show that of the
and other species of chickadee, such as boreal, 97 recoveries made from 1929 to 2003, all but five
black-capped, or mountain, depending on the birds were recovered at the location where they
region (Campbell et al. 1997). were banded (Bird Banding Lab). The five birds
Three subspecies of chestnut-backed chickadee that were recovered away from their original
occur in western North America: P. r. rufescens banding site had moved 12 to 56 km. All of these
(Alaska to central California), P. r. neglectus (Point movements took place between the breeding and
Arena to San Francisco Bay, California) and P. r. bar- non-breeding seasons or during the fall, when
lowi (San Francisco to Monterey, California) limited movement is known to occur, however no
(Dahlsten et al. 2002). The distribution of this evidence of dispersal during the breeding season
species follows closely that of the mesic temperate has been found (Campbell et al. 1997).
forests (cedarhemlock), inhabiting a narrow band,
less than 200 km wide, along the Pacific Coast from
California north to Alaska, with a disjunct popula- 6.3 Patterns of contemporary
tion in the western Rocky Mountains of the Pacific population structure
Northwest (Fig. 6.1). Both coastal and interior
6.3.1 Chestnut-backed chickadee
populations appear to have experienced range
expansions in the last 100 years (Grinnell 1904; Microsatellite analyses revealed the presence of
Dahlsten et al. 2002). In the last 55 years, the coastal four genetically distinct populations of chestnut-
population has undergone a rapid range expansion backed chickadees in western North America:
into the Sierra Nevada Mountains of central Queen Charlotte Islands, south-eastern British
California (Brennan and Morrison 1991). A narrow, Columbia, mainland Alaska, and a large coastal
inland strip of forest, more than 500 km, was group (Box 6.1; Fig. 6.1). The levels of genetic
colonized in less than 50 years, demonstrating the differentiation among the four groups are low, but
ability of chestnut-backed chickadees to rapidly significant (Table 6.1). Individuals from each of
colonize new habitat (Fig. 6.1). It is hypothesized the four groups showed higher affinity to other
that the expansion corresponded to forest regener- individuals in the same group than to individuals
ation following widespread logging. The forests from other groups (Fig. 6.2). The Queen Charlotte
now contain a large amount of Douglas fir, a key Island and central Alaska populations are distinct,
component of the chestnut-backed chickadee and they appear as each others closest genetic
habitat, and the current distribution of the neighbors in factorial correspondence analysis
chestnut-backed chickadee in the Sierra Nevada (FCA, Fig. 6.2) and analyses of allele frequency
Mountains corresponds almost exactly to that of the distribution (Burg et al. 2006). The Oregon popula-
Douglas fir (Brennan and Morrison 1991). A second tion shows significant differences from Alaska,
80 R E P R O D U C T I V E E C O L O G Y, E V O L U T I O N, A N D B E H AV I O R

Box 6.1 Samples and genotyping

The sampling area covers the majority of the 20 batches, and 20 000 permutations/batch,
contemporary range of chestnut-backed Miller 1997). TFPGA uses a Markov chain Monte
chickadee P. rufescens, including the disjunct Carlo (McMC) approximation of Fishers exact
population in the western Rocky Mountains. A test (Raymond and Rousset 1995a) and
total of 249 samples were collected from nine significance values are combined across all loci
populations in Alaska, British Columbia, (Fisher 1954).
Washington, and Oregon (Fig. 6.1). The Relative measures of differentiation can be
populations sampled in Washington and difficult to compare directly (Hedrick 1999). The
Oregon are from areas believed to have been genotype likelihood ratio distance (DLR, Paetkau
unglaciated during the Pleistocene. Blood et al. 1997) is well suited to studying fine-scale
samples were collected from 223 individuals population structure and has lower variance than
during the summer of 2002 and 2003. Birds were other distance measures (Paetkau et al. 1997). DLR
caught using mist nets, and blood was taken from is the likelihood of a genotype from one
the brachial vein, dried on filter paper and stored population being identical to a genotype in
in individual plastic bags. Twenty six samples another population. When DLR 1, the genotypes
(Alaska, n 6; Alexander Archipelago, n 9; of individuals from the two populations being
Oregon, n 1; and Queen Charlotte Islands, compared are one order of magnitude more likely
n 10) were obtained from the University of to occur in the individuals own population than
Alaska Museum. in the other population (Paetkau et al. 1997). DLR
DNA was extracted using standard proteinase was calculated in Doh (http://biodb.biology.
K/phenol chloroform extraction followed by ualberta.ca/jbrzusto).
ethanol precipitation (Sambrook et al. 1989). A factorial correspondence analysis (FCA) was
Seven microsatellite primer pairs isolated from performed to aid in visualization of the patterns
other passerine species were used for genotyping of genetic structure using GENETIX 4.02
(for details on primers and PCR amplification see (Belkhir et al. 2000). FCA uses individual
Burg et al. 2006). Alleles were scored using genotypic data to quantify the amount of
GeneImagIR (Scanalytics) and sizing was inertia among populations. The global
confirmed by visual inspection. inertia is proportionally weighted relative to
sample size and the total number of alleles
Statistical analyses present in each sample. The centre of gravity
Tests for departures from HardyWeinberg or combined distribution of all individuals in each
equilibrium (HWE) and for linkage disequilibrium population was plotted in three-dimensional
were examined using exact tests (Guo and space.
Thompson 1992) as implemented in GENEPOP Tests for isolation by distance allow us to
v3.3 (Raymond and Rousset 1995b) and evaluate the relative historical roles of gene flow
sequential Bonferroni corrections for multiple tests and drift on population structure by comparing
were applied (Rice 1989). As estimates of allelic expected pairwise genetic and geographic
diversity can be biased due to unequal samples distances with those expected under a stepping-
sizes, allelic richness was estimated using FSTAT stone model of population structure (Hutchison
v2.9.3.2 (Goudet et al. 2001). and Templeton 1999). They also allow us to
Of the standard statistical methods for testing determine if gene flow is affected by geographic
population differentiation, allelic goodness of fit distance. IBD tests were performed in TFPGA v1.3
tests are the most powerful for detecting (Miller 1997) and significance was determined
population structure when sample sizes are using 999 permutations. Geographic distances to
unequal (Goudet, 1996). TFPGA v1.3 was used to the south-eastern British Columbia population
test for differences in allele frequencies among were calculated as the shortest distance through
populations (1000 dememorization steps, mesic forest habitat.
PHYLOGEOGRAPHY 81

Table 6.1 Exact tests for population differentiation (below diagonal) and DLR (above diagonal) for nine chestnut-backed chickadee
populations; significant pairwise tests are indicated in bold; refer to Fig. 6.1 for abbreviations of sampling sites

cAK AA NBC QCI VI NCP MtR OR SEBC

cAK 1.64 0.74 0.82 2.01 2.69 0.70 1.21 1.21


AA 0.002 0.49 1.63 1.41 1.30 0.90 0.97 1.32
NBC 0.003 0.266 1.47 0.75 0.76 0.05 0.90 0.54
QCI 0.001 0.003 0.001 2.22 2.67 1.58 1.46 1.48
VI 0.001 0.055 0.008 0.001 0.55 0.99 1.16 1.40
NCP 0.001 0.013 0.014 0.001 0.155 0.38 2.69 1.94
MtR 0.001 0.157 0.470 0.001 0.014 0.405 0.96 0.62
OR 0.002 0.025 0.192 0.003 0.004 0.006 0.059 1.11
SEBC 0.001 0.003 0.001 0.001 0.001 0.001 0.001 0.025

OR

QCI
AA VI
Axis 2

SEBC NCP

MtR

AK

is 3
NBC

Ax
Axis 1

Figure 6.2 Factorial correspondence analysis (FCA) of chestnut-backed chickadee populations in western North America. Each point represents
the centre of the distribution of genotypes from all the individuals in the population. Refer to Fig. 6.1 for abbreviations.

Queen Charlotte Islands, and North Cascades chestnut-backed chickadee populations east of the
Park populations, but not the population in south- Cascades (Grinnell 1904; Bowles 1909; Brooks and
eastern British Columbia. The allelic variation is Swarth 1925). Chestnut-backed chickadees are
similar throughout the range, but the number easily detected during auditory surveys; therefore
of private alleles (i.e. alleles found only in one it seems unlikely that all three surveys would have
population) is not (Fig. 6.3). The proportion of failed to find chickadees if they were indeed pre-
private alleles is not significantly heterogeneous sent. The 300 km of arid land presently separating
(Gcorr 13.2, P 0.11); however, the proportion the coastal and interior populations appears to be
of private alleles is significantly correlated with limiting dispersal between these two groups.
latitude (P 0.05). Similar patterns (i.e. coastal and interior popula-
tions) are found in other mesic temperate forest
6.3.1.1 Interior and coastal populations species with similar ecological requirements and
The interior population in south-eastern British distributions as the chestnut-backed chickadee,
Columbia and Idaho is most likely of relatively including plants, salamanders, frogs, and voles
recent, postglacial origin as surveys in the (reviewed in Brunsfeld et al. 2001). Genetic data
early 20th century did not detect extensive show that some of these populations are the result
82 R E P R O D U C T I V E E C O L O G Y, E V O L U T I O N, A N D B E H AV I O R

including additional samples from Oregon and


cAK* samples from California, are required to determine
the colonization route and the source of the interior
population.

AA 6.3.1.2 Central, coastal Alaska


The pattern of genetic differentiation at the north-
QCI NBC* ern end of the range is interesting. Chestnut-backed
chickadees are found as far north as the Kenai
Peninsula (Fig. 6.1); however, the population in
central, coastal Alaska is genetically isolated. A
SEBC closer examination of the forests surrounding
VI
Haines, AK (near cAK, Fig. 6.1) shows that there is
a 100-km break in the coastal forests near the
Alaska/British Columbia/Yukon border. The gap is
NCP* caused by the encroaching glaciers between the
OR Pacific Ocean and the St Elias and Chugach
MtR Mountain ranges. The glaciers create a disruption
in the distributions of western hemlock (Tsuga het-
Figure 6.3 Geographic distribution of private alleles in erophylla) and Alaska/yellow cedar (Chamaecyparis
chestnut-backed chickadees. The percentage of private (black) and nootkatensis), the only two components of the mesic
shared (white) alleles are shown for each population. Populations temperate forest found north of Haines, AK (Little
with higher than average (cAK, NBC) and lower than average (NCP) 1971). Other coniferous species associated with
number of private alleles are indicated (*). Refer to Fig. 6.1 for
mesic forests, such as western red cedar (Thuja pli-
abbreviations.
cata), extend into south-eastern Alaska reaching as
far north as Haines; however, Douglas fir and
mountain hemlock (Tsuga mertensiana) are absent
of ancient vicariance events (large genetic diver- north of the Queen Charlotte Islands (Little 1971).
gence between interior and coastal lineages), while The peripheral location of the Alaskan population
others result from postglacial, inland dispersal may partially explain why it is genetically isolated
from the coast (little or no genetic differences from other populations of chestnut-backed chicka-
between lineages). Genetic data and historical dees. A second possibility is prolonged isolation in
records suggest that the interior chestnut-backed a glacial refugium. This is supported by the large
chickadee population is the result of inland disper- number of private alleles present in the central
sal from the coast (Burg et al. 2006). If the interior Alaska population. Limited postglacial dispersal
population was due to ancient vicariance, a large between central Alaska and other coastal popula-
number of unique alleles should have arisen tions would have restricted the distribution of these
through mutation and the FCA (Fig. 6.2) would alleles and resulted in genetic differentiation along
not position the interior group amongst the coastal the coast (Figs 6.1 and 6.3).
populations. The south-eastern British Columbia
population shows some affinity to the Oregon 6.3.1.3 Queen Charlotte Islands
population (Fig. 6.2). This grouping may be an Glaciation on the Queen Charlotte Islands, a large
artifact of the small number of samples from Oregon archipelago in northern British Columbia (Fig. 6.1),
or it could reflect genetic similarity between these ended earlier than on the adjacent mainland (Pielou
two populations. If it is the latter, then the interior 1991; Mann and Hamilton 1995). Paleoecological
population may have been founded by birds from evidence suggests that areas of Hecate Strait, which
Oregon via dispersal around the southern edge of separates these islands from the mainland, were
the Columbia Basin. Further genetic analysis, exposed during parts of the Pleistocene (Lacourse
PHYLOGEOGRAPHY 83

et al. 2003). In addition, at the end of the Wisconsin central Alaska sites is unlikely to be the result of a
glaciation ~10 kya, while most of British Columbia sampling artifact. Three alternative possibilities
was under thick sheets of ice, the Queen Charlotte exist to explain the pattern of private alleles,
Islands were ice free and available for colonization. particularly in the northern part of the range:
During this time, parts of coastal mainland British multiple refugia, unsampled source population,
Columbia were also ice free and sea levels were and introgression (introduction of genes from one
relatively low. Patterns of genetic structure in other population into a second).
Parid species indicate limited dispersal across large Populations that are isolated for prolonged
expanses of water (Gill et al. 1993; Kvist et al. 2005). periods of time may contain different alleles due to
The reduced sea levels would have effectively mutation and genetic drift. Such isolation could
decreased the width of Hecate Strait, thereby have occurred in populations originating from mul-
reducing the distance required for chickadees to tiple refugia (Fig. 6.4a, b, and e). If the populations
traverse over water from the mainland. As the sea containing a large number of private alleles (i.e.
level rose, it effectively isolated chestnut-backed northern British Columbia and Alaska) did indeed
chickadees on the Queen Charlotte Islands. arise from distinct, northern refugial populations,
Several other resident passerine species on the either directly or indirectly, then these populations
Queen Charlotte Islands also show evidence of may also show higher levels of genetic divergence
genetic isolation including Stellers jay (Cyanocitta from other populations. There is some speculation
stelleri), pine grosbeak (Pinicola enucleator), and regarding the existence of coastal and/or northern
hairy woodpecker (Picoides villosus) (Burg et al. refugia near these locations (e.g. Byun et al. 1997;
2005; K. Winker, personal communication). These Soltis et al. 1997; Ritland et al. 2000; Ritland et al.
species appear to have diverged from mainland 2001). Ritland et al. (2001) found a divergent lineage
populations during the Pleistocene glaciations of yellow cedar, an important component of the
and limited postglacial dispersal has occurred. cedarhemlock ecosystem, in northern British
Although it is possible that chestnut-backed chicka- Columbia and postulated that it may have origi-
dees survived in the Hecate refugium on the east nated in a northern refugium. It is plausible that if
coast of the Queen Charlotte Islands, it is also pos- such a refugium was present, that chestnut-backed
sible that they colonized this archipelago after the chickadees may have been present. Environmental
glaciers melted on the islands. conditions in such a refugium would be similar to
those present in the northern part of the chickadees
6.3.1.4 Private alleles range.
Private alleles are present in each of the nine popu- The large number of private alleles may be the
lations and the proportion of private alleles result of insufficient samples, not in terms of the
decreases from north to south (r2 45.61, P 0.05). number of individuals sampled but the number of
Private alleles are often present in populations populations sampled. No populations in southern
though the ability to detect and accurately identify Oregon or California were sampled. Given the
them is dependent on sample sizes. When high lev- genetic homogeneity of the southern coastal popu-
els of genetic variation are present and only a small lations, samples from Washington and northern
number of samples are analyzed, some alleles may Oregon are likely representative of populations fur-
be incorrectly classified as private alleles. With ther south. However, the tendency of peripheral
the exception of two populations (Alexander isolation in the chestnut-backed chickadee suggests
Archipelago and Oregon), sample sizes of chestnut- that the California P. r. rufescens populations might
backed chickadees were adequate to obtain realistic also have private alleles.
allele profiles (plotting the number of alleles Lastly, the large number of northern private
detected against number of samples showed that alleles could be the result of hybridization between
the number of alleles present reached an asymp- chestnut-backed and boreal chickadees (Fig. 6.4d).
tote). Therefore, the relatively large number of pri- Hybridization is known to occur in other
vate alleles in the northern British Columbia and passerines in the same area as the chestnut-backed
84 R E P R O D U C T I V E E C O L O G Y, E V O L U T I O N, A N D B E H AV I O R

(a) (b) (c)


R1 R1

R2 R2
R1

(d) Boreal (e)


chickadee
R3

R2

R1
R1

Figure 6.4 Alternate scenarios for the origin of private alleles and pattern of colonization in chestnut-backed chickadees. Hypothetical
distributions (white ovals) following dispersal (arrows) from glacial refugium (grey areas) are shown. (a) Colonization from multiple refugia with
admixture; (b) colonization from multiple refugia with no admixture; (c) multiple colonizations from a single refugium; (d) single colonization from
a refugium and hybridization with boreal chickadees; (e) multiple glacial refugia with colonization from only one refugium.

chickadee, namely hermit and Townsends warblers the Townsends warbler population expanded
(Rohwer et al. 2001). Many of the hybrids of these northwards and came into contact with hermit
two warbler species are phenotypically indistin- warblers near the Skeena River Valley along the
guishable from pure Townsends warblers. In northern British Columbia coast (Rohwer et al.
fact, most of the Townsends warbler populations 2001). Like chestnut-backed chickadees, both of
in British Columbia and Alaska, where no hermit these warbler species depend on Douglas fir. In
warblers are found, have hermit warbler mtDNA. addition, it is believed that like the boreal and
Rohwer et al. (2001) hypothesized that the distribu- chestnut-backed chickadees, the Townsends
tion of Townsends and hermit warblers 8000 years and hermit warblers were isolated in interior and
ago was quite different from today. Almost imme- coastal refugia, respectively (Grinnell 1904; Rohwer
diately following the retreat of the ice sheets, the et al. 2001). Therefore like the two warbler species,
hermit warblers expanded northward along the it is possible that boreal chickadees were present in
coast and to the west of the Coast and Cascade the northern part of the contemporary chestnut-
Mountains, a pattern identical to the coastal distri- backed chickadee range and as chestnut-backed
bution of chestnut-backed chickadees described by chickadees expanded northward, they hybridized
Grinnell (1904). Townsends warblers were present with boreal chickadees and eventually displaced
in the Rocky Mountains from Idaho to Colorado. the boreal chickadee from the coast. The two
As the boreal forests invaded the deglaciated chestnut-backed chickadee populations with the
regions of central British Columbia (Pielou 1991), highest number of private alleles are in areas where
PHYLOGEOGRAPHY 85

the ranges of chestnut-backed and boreal chicka- leap-frog, or long distance dispersal. A recent study
dees come into contact. While the hypothesized by Pruett and Winker (2005) provides an excellent
hybridization could be confirmed by genetic example of linear recolonization from a southern
analysis with a non-recombining genetic marker, refugium. The Pacific coast of northern British
such as mtDNA, hybridization in Paridae is not Columbia and Alaska were sequentially colonized
uncommon (black-capped and Carolina chicka- by song sparrows (Melospiza melodia) with popula-
dees, Chapter 7; gray-headed chickadee and willow tions experiencing gradual loss of genetic diversity
tit, Gill et al. 2005; Curry 2005). as colonization progressed northwards.
The contemporary population structure in the
6.3.1.5 Recolonization patterns chestnut-backed chickadee (Figs 6.1 and 6.2) sug-
Sixteen thousand years ago most of the chestnut- gests that recolonization was likely complex. When
backed chickadee current range was heavily inferring colonization patterns, relative genetic dis-
glaciated. One of the largest refugia was found on tances, distribution of alleles, and contemporary
the now submerged continental shelf in Hecate population structure are important. Small genetic
Strait off the east coast of the Queen Charlotte distances result from individual dispersal or popu-
Islands. Paleoecological data show that vegetation lation expansion. Similarly, the absence of private
was present in the Hecate refugium (Lacourse et al. alleles generally indicates dispersal while a large
2003). At the end of the Wisconsin glaciation, ice number of private alleles can indicate prolonged
sheets retreated asynchronously along the Pacific isolation. Dispersal, even as few as one or two
Coast. The Queen Charlotte Islands were ice free migrants per generation, results in genetically
16 kya, while the mainland coast was not ice free homogeneous populations (Wang 2004). The pres-
until 13 kya (Mann and Hamilton 1995). In south- ence of four genetically distinct groups of chestnut-
ern British Columbia, glaciers were present only at backed chickadees suggests little or no dispersal
higher elevations 10 kya (Mann and Hamilton 1995; among these groups and that recolonization was
Clague and James 2002). not simply a linear progression along the coast as
Trees colonized the deglaciated landscape in seen in song sparrows (Pruett and Winker 2005).
stages. The main tree species found in chestnut- Several recolonization scenarios are possible: colon-
backed chickadee habitat are late successional ization from a single southern refugium, coloniza-
species and were one of the last to colonize the north. tion from a single northern refugium, and/or
Based on paleoecological data, fir, hemlock, and colonization from multiple refugia (Fig. 6.4).
cedar advanced northwards arriving at different The extensive glaciation in the north and large
times along that coast. Douglas fir arrived first; ice-free areas in the south would make postglacial
favoring the warmer, drier conditions present dur- colonization from a single southern refugium the
ing the hypsithermal (106 kya in south-western most plausible scenario. Many of the alleles in the
British Columbia and either 52 kya or 96 kya in southern populations are present in the north, as
Alaska; Mann and Hamilton 1995). Hemlock colo- would be predicted based on a northern expansion
nized deglaciated areas during the cooler and wetter from Washington or Oregon. The main problem
conditions that followed the hypsithermal. Western with this scenario is the large number of private
cedar was not present until 6 to 4 kya (Mann and alleles in the two northern populations (Fig. 6.3). It
Hamilton 1995). Long distance dispersal increased is unlikely that two of the populations furthest
the rate of reforestation. The presence of coastal or from a putative southern refugium would contain
northern refugia would have further increased the alleles that are absent in the founding populations
colonization rate of these arboreal species. The limited unless colonization followed a leap-frog pattern
genetic data available for these species support the (Fig. 6.4c) and source populations for both Alaska
presence of multiple refugia along the coast of and northern British Columbia populations were
British Columbia and Alaska (Ritland et al. 2001). not sampled (e.g. southern Oregon or California).
Once suitable habitat was available, recoloniza- Colonization from a single northern refugium is
tion could occur through gradual range expansion, also possible. This would explain the significant
86 R E P R O D U C T I V E E C O L O G Y, E V O L U T I O N, A N D B E H AV I O R

decrease in the number of private alleles with species (chestnut-backed, Mexican, and mountain
decreasing latitude (P 0.05); however, this scen- chickadees) only two populations were sampled.
ario requires that the northern refugium supported With the exception of the chestnut-backed chicka-
large populations as the high levels of genetic dee, all of the species studied by Gill et al. (1993)
variation would require a relatively large showed phylogeographic structure (Fig. 6.5). Black-
population size. Given the high population differ- capped chickadee populations in Newfoundland
entiation present in the north and absence of popu- contained a different set of haplotypes from those
lation structure in southern coastal populations, it in mainland North America. Similarly, boreal
seems unlikely that all of the populations could chickadees in Nova Scotia and Newfoundland
have descended from individuals originating in the were genetically distinct from those in other parts
north. of Canada and the northern United States. The only
The final colonization scenario, colonization other continuous population to show phylogeo-
from multiple refugia in the north and south graphic structure is the Carolina chickadees, which
(Fig. 6.4e), is the most complex of the three scen- exhibit an eastwest division with secondary
arios. In this scenario the population in the southern contact near the Tombigbee River (Gill et al. 1999).
refugium (e.g. Washington) expanded northwards In the south-western United States and Mexico,
following the advancing forests while dispersal disjunct populations of the Mexican chickadee in
from one or more northern refugia was limited. Arizona and Michoacan are genetically distinct
This scenario would explain both the large number (Gill et al. 1993).
of private alleles and the pattern of population
genetic structure. Limited emigration from north- 6.3.2.2 Eurasian Parids
ern refugia means that any alleles present in these Extensive population genetic studies of five
areas would remain there, thus explaining the Palaerctic Parids (Siberian tits, willow tits, great
higher number of private alleles in the north. The tits, blue tits, and azure tits) have been conducted
absence of genetic differentiation in the southern with no fewer than 16 published studies in the last
coastal populations is the result of dispersal from a 15 years. Different phylogeographic patterns are
southern refugium. According to the pioneer model found in each of these species (Fig. 6.6). The
of colonization, populations established ahead of Siberian tit showed no evidence of genetic structure
the leading front, either by long distance dispersal (Uimaniemi et al. 2003). In contrast, four divergent
or existing refugia, create high density barriers lineages were found in the Eurasian willow tit. Six
restricting migration into previously colonized of the nine subspecies from the large, continuous
areas. The resulting genetic pattern is a mosaic of northern portion of the range (Salzburger et al.
isolated populations embedded within a larger 2002a) formed a single clade while three remaining
group of homogeneous populations (Hewitt 1996; subspecies (P. m. weigoldicus, P. m. affinis, P. m.
Ibrahim et al. 1996). songarus) each formed a distinct clade. A similar
pattern was found using the more variable mtDNA
control region, though samples of P. m. weigoldicus
6.3.2 Comparison to other Parids
were not included (Kvist et al. 2001). Four great tit
6.3.2.1 North American chickadees lineages were found across the Eurasian continent:
Limited genetic studies have been conducted on bokharensis, cinereus, major and minor, with the major
North American chickadees (Gill et al. 1993, 2005; and minor groups containing multiple subspecies
Burg et al. 2006). The most extensive study by Gill (Kvist et al. 2003). The fourth Eurasian species
et al. (1993) examined not only the relationship studied, the blue tit, exhibited intermediate levels
between the different Parid species, but also levels of population structure with multiple, highly
of genetic differentiation within populations using divergent clades: northern Europe, southern
mtDNA restriction fragment length polymorphism Europe (Spain, France and Italy), Africa, and the
(RFLP). Most of the North American chickadee Canary Islands (Salzburger et al. 2002b; Kvist et al.
species were included, though for several of the 2004). Additional population structure exists within
PHYLOGEOGRAPHY 87

Boreal
Black-capped
Carolina
Chestnut-backed
Mexican
Mountain

Figure 6.5 Map showing geographic distribution of genetically distinct chickadee populations in North America (Gill et al. 1993; Burg et al.
2006). Each symbol represents a sampling location and genetically distinct populations are indicated by shading. The Carolina chickadee
population near the Tombigbee River (white and black stripes) contains haplotypes from eastern and western lineages.

the Canary Islands with many islands containing complex. The simplest pattern is found in the
closely related, but distinct, lineages (Kvist et al. Siberian tit. It is hypothesized that this species
2005). The phylogeographic study of the fifth survived in a single eastern refugium (Uimaniemi
Eurasian species, the azure tit, was limited with one et al. 2003). For the blue tit, a minimum of four
to three samples from six sites (Salzburger et al. refugia were present: Balkans, Iberian Peninsula,
2002b). Two haplotypes were present, one exclu- Africa, and Canary Islands (Salzburger et al. 2002b;
sively in central and eastern Asia and the second at Kvist et al. 2004). As populations expanded, birds
two sites near the extreme eastern and western colonized the deglaciated landscape. A wide area of
ends of the range. While no clear phylogeographic secondary contact between birds from the two
structure was detected, the small number of sam- Eurasian refugia is present in southern Europe,
ples precludes any definite conclusions. The study though it would appear that most of Europe was
by Salzburger et al. (2002b) shows azure and blue founded by individuals from the Balkan refugium.
tits are paraphyletic with Eurasian and African blue This is apparent not only in the geographic
tits forming two different clades. distribution of haplotypes from the Balkan clade,
Many of the Eurasian species survived the last but also in the resulting phylogeny. The star-burst
glaciation in one or more refugia. The proposed pattern of the mtDNA haplotypes indicates
pattern of colonization ranges from simple to that a population expansion accompanied the
88 R E P R O D U C T I V E E C O L O G Y, E V O L U T I O N, A N D B E H AV I O R

Azure
Blue
Great
Siberian
Willow

Figure 6.6 Genetic structure of Eurasian tit species (see text for references). Each genetic lineage is indicated (white, light grey, dark grey or
black). Blue tits on the Canary Islands (light grey circle) contain at least six closely related lineages.

colonization from the eastern refugium (Kvist et al. refugium in south-east Asia (Kvist et al. 2001;
2004). The Balkan clade contained individuals from Salzburger et al. 2002a). Genetic diversity in willow
all of the sampled Eurasian populations. No evi- tits decreases from east to west, suggesting that
dence of a recent expansion was found in the postglacial colonization proceeded in a westward
Iberian or Canary clades. Instead the pattern of direction (Kvist et al. 2001).
a large number of divergent haplotypes with no Population structure of Eurasian species is similar
one predominant haplotype, suggests that these in many ways to that of chestnut-backed chickadees.
populations are much older. The Iberian clade con- There is evidence of limited dispersal within contin-
tains individuals mostly from Spain and France (i.e. uous populations (great tit, blue tit, and willow tit),
Corsica) and some from Italy, suggesting limited genetic isolation of island populations (blue tit and
postglacial dispersal. Similarly, fixed genetic differ- willow tit), and restricted gene flow between central
ences are found between different islands in the and peripheral populations (blue tit). It would
Canary Island population. Phylogeographic recon- appear that many of the factors influencing disper-
structions for both the willow and great tits suggest sal, and therefore population structure, in North
the presence of four Pleistocene refugia. For both of American and Eurasian Parids are similar.
these species, it is hypothesized that three of
the groups survived in refugium at or near their
6.4 Factors influencing population
present day location. The same Balkan refugium
genetics of Parids
suggested for the blue tits could have been used by
the major group of great tits (Kvist et al. 2003, In the following section, I discuss several recurring
2004). However, the fourth group of willow tits, themes that are present in the patterns of popula-
containing six subspecies, may have persisted in a tion structure for both North American and
PHYLOGEOGRAPHY 89

Eurasian Parids (Table 6.2), namely the role of refu- Peninsula, Balkans, Africa, Canary Islands, and at
gia, disjunct populations, and peripheral isolation least three in Asia. Suture zones or areas of second-
within continuous habitat. ary contact are present in several Eurasian popula-
tions which were previously isolated in different
refugia. In North America, it is reasonable, given
6.4.1 Postglacial colonization and glacial
the mtDNA data, that multiple refugia were also
refugia
present; however, the location of most of these
The Pleistocene glaciations played a critical role in refugia is unknown. Gill et al. (1993) suggested that
shaping contemporary populations in both the for boreal and black-capped chickadees one such
Nearctic and Palaearctic. Genetic studies have refugium may have existed in Newfoundland. It is
shown that the late Plioceneearly Pleistocene was also probable that the distinct lineages of Carolina,
important for Parid speciation and the late mountain, and Mexican chickadees are the result of
Pleistocene was responsible for creating many of isolation in different refugia during the Pleistocene.
the contemporary population genetic patterns. For the chestnut-backed chickadee, microsatellite
Even in unglaciated areas, environmental changes data do not conclusively support or refute the exist-
that accompanied the glaciations altered the distri- ence of multiple Pleistocene refugia. Until the
bution of many species. For many of the Parid source of the private alleles is determined, it is rea-
species, changes in forest distribution would have sonable to speculate that multiple refugia were
altered their distribution. In Eurasia, as many as present; one large refugium in the south and at least
seven refugia have been proposed: the Iberian one in the north.

Table 6.2 Summary of patterns of colonization and population genetic structure in contemporary Parid populations

Multiple refugia Pattern of genetic isolation

Disjunct Islands Within continuous habitat

Overall Peripheral

North America chickadees


Black-capped Y n/a Y (NF) Y Y
Boreal Y n/a Y (NF1) Y Y
Carolina Y n/a n/a Y N
Chestnut-backed2 ? Y Y (QCI) Y Y
Mexican Y Y n/a n/a n/a
Mountain Y n/a n/a Y Y3
Eurasian tits
Blue Y n/a Y (Canary Is., British Isles) Y Y
Great Y N N Y N
Siberian N n/a n/a N N
Willow Y Y Y (Japan4) Y N

For each of the categories the presence (Y) or absence (N) of population genetic structure is given. The three main categories of population structure
are isolation due to: disjunct populations, island populations, and peripheral populations within continuous habitat. The presence of genetic structure
(overall) and isolation of peripheral populations (peripheral) within continuous habitat are listed separately. For some species, the category did not
exist or no data was available (n/a). Azure tits are not included due to insufficient data.
1
Newfoundland and Nova Scotia share haplotypes.
2
Microsatellite data only.
3
Only two sampling sites, both peripheral.
4
One common haplotype found in all Japanese sites, haplotype restricted to Japan.
90 R E P R O D U C T I V E E C O L O G Y, E V O L U T I O N, A N D B E H AV I O R

6.4.2 Disjunct populations the Queen Charlotte Islands) and the adjacent
mainland, thereby facilitating dispersal. Strong
Genetic isolation between physically separated
trade winds are thought to have aided in the
populations can arise through limited dispersal
colonization of the Canary Islands by blue tits
across large areas of unsuitable habitat. In the case
(Kvist et al. 2001). It is not clear what is limiting
of Parids, unsuitable habitat may consist of changes
dispersal as it is evident by the presence of Parids
in forest composition, gaps in forest, large geo-
on these islands that they are capable of dispersing
graphic distances, or water barriers.
across water barriers. Yet on some islands, species
found on the nearby mainland are absent. For
6.4.2.1 Mainland populations example, on Vancouver Island and the nearby
Studies of both chickadees and tits have shown that San Juan Islands, no black-capped or mountain
disjunct mainland populations are genetically iso- chickadees are present despite suitable available
lated. Both the coastal and interior populations of habitat (Sturman 1968). Both of these species
chestnut-backed chickadees and southern US and are present on the British Columbia mainland
Mexican populations of Mexican chickadees are (Campbell et al. 1997).
separated by relatively arid habitat. In Eurasia,
each of the four willow tit lineages occupies a dif-
ferent ecological niche (Salzburger et al. 2002a). The 6.4.3 Limited dispersal within continuous
result is that dispersal between physically isolated habitat and peripheral populations
populations is limited among each of these three Several Parid species exhibit genetic structure
species. In the willow tit and chestnut-backed within a physically continuous habitat; however,
chickadees, population subdivisions are present the isolation of peripheral populations is more
within continuous habitat, indicating limited dis- prevalent in North American species, particularly
persal (Chapter 15). The habitat for the Mexican those with northern distributions (e.g. black-
chickadee is highly fragmented and insufficient capped, boreal, and chestnut-backed chickadees).
data are available to know if dispersal is limited Populations at the edge of a species distribution
within continuous forest fragments. may be genetically distinct due to restricted disper-
sal from the core to the periphery of a species range
6.4.2.2 Island populations (Wisely et al. 2004). This asymmetrical dispersal is
Gill et al. (1993) suggested limited dispersal across due to peripheral populations containing subopti-
water in both black-capped and boreal chickadee mal habitat that further restricts dispersal into the
populations. While the chestnut-backed chickadee area, and limits both population size and density. In
population on Vancouver Island (~30 km from the the case of the chestnut-backed chickadee, dispersal
mainland with numerous intervening islands) is in general may be more restricted due to its linear
not genetically differentiated from the mainland distribution (Slatkin 1993). Populations with linear
population, the Queen Charlotte Island population distributions are more likely to have restricted dis-
(~80 km from the mainland with few islands that persal than widespread ones for some of the same
could connect the mainland to the archipelago) reasons that dispersal is more limited at the edge of
is genetically distinct. The pattern is repeated the species range (i.e. a limited number of direc-
in black-capped chickadees (Newfoundland is tions in which dispersal can occur).
genetically distinct from the mainland) and Canary
Island blue tits (~110 km from African coast and
6.5 Conclusions and perspectives
substantial interisland distances) (Gill et al. 1993;
Kvist et al. 2005). For all three North American The population structure of chestnut-backed
species (black-capped, boreal, and chestnut-backed chickadees, and that of other Parids, appears to be
chickadees) the lower sea levels during glacial complex and influenced by a variety of factors,
periods would have effectively decreased the most notably postglacial colonization and
distance between islands (i.e. Newfoundland and distribution. Many of the factors limiting dispersal
PHYLOGEOGRAPHY 91

in chestnut-backed chickadees seem to be Brooks, A. and Swarth, H. S. (1925). A distributional list of


common in other Parids. These include isolation of the birds of British Columbia. Pacific Coast Avifauna, 17,
peripheral populations, and limited dispersal over 1158.
large water barriers or other areas of unsuitable Brunsfeld, S. J., Sullivan, J., Soltis, D. E., and Soltis, P. S.
(2001). Comparative phylogeography of northwestern
habitat. Within the northern portion of the
North America: a synthesis. In: Silverton, J. and
chestnut-backed chickadee range, a large number
Antonovics, J., eds. Integrating Ecological and
of private alleles are present in two of the popula- Evolutionary Processes in a Spatial Context, pp. 319339.
tions. Further genetic analyses are required to Blackwell Science, Oxford.
determine the origin of these alleles. The simplest Burg, T. M., Gaston, A. J., Winker, K., and Friesen, V. L.
and most probable explanation is that they resulted (2005). Rapid divergence and post-glacial colonization
from prolonged isolation in multiple refugia and in western North American Stellers jays (Cyanocitta
they are not present in other populations due to stelleri). Molecular Ecology, 14, 37453755.
limited emigration. Burg, T. M., Gaston, A. J., Winker, K., and Friesen, V. L.
(2006). Effects of Pleistocene glaciations on population
structure of North American chestnut-backed chick-
Acknowledgements adees. Molecular Ecology. 15, 24092419.
Byun, S. A., Koop, B. F., and Reimchen, T. E. (1997). North
Tim Boucher, Roger Bull, Robert Dickerman, Chris
American black bear mtDNA phylogeography: implica-
Gibb, Sandeep Girn, Andrew Johnson, James Maley, tions for morphology and the Haida Gwaii glacial
Alison Ronson, and members of the Laskeek Bay refugium controversy. Evolution, 51, 16471653.
Conservation Society assisted with sample collec- Campbell, W., Dawe, N. K., McTaggart-Cowan, I., et al.
tion and the University of Alaska Museum provided (1997). Birds of British Columbia, Vol. 3, Passerines-
additional samples. Parks Canada and the US Flycatchers through Vireos. UBC Press, Vancouver, BC.
National Parks permitted sample collection within Carey, A. B., Mae Hardt, M., Horton, S. P., and Biswell,
the parks. I would like to thank Vicki Friesen, Tony B. L. (1991). Spring bird communities in the Oregon
Gaston, Kevin Winker, and Maia Bailey for helpful Coast Range. In: Ruggiero, L. F., Aubry, K. B., Carey,
discussions. Funding was provided by Canadian A. B., and Huff, M. H., eds. Wildlife and Vegetation of
Unmanaged Douglas-fir Forests. General Technical Report
Wildlife Service and Natural Sciences and
PNW-GTR-285, pp. 123142. US Department of
Engineering Research Council of Canada.
Agriculture, Forest Service, Portland, OR.
Clague, J. J. and James, T. S. (2002). History and isostatic
effects of the last ice sheet in southern British Columbia.
References
Quaternary Science Reviews, 21, 7187.
Belkhir, K., Borsa, P., Chikhi, L., Raufaste, N., Bonhomme, Curry, R. L. (2005). Hybridization in chickadees: much to
F. (2000). GENETIX 4.02, logiciel sous WindowsTM pour la learn from familiar birds. Auk, 122, 747758.
gntique des populations. Laboratoire Gnome et Dahlsten, D. L., Brennan, L. A., McCallum, D. A., and
Populations, CNRS UPR 9060, Universit Montpellier Gaunt, S. L (2002). Chestnut-backed chickadee (Poecile
II, Montpellier. rufescens). In: Poole, A. and Gill, F., eds. The Birds of
Bowles, J. H. (1909). Notes on Parus rufescens in western North America. The Birds of North America # 689, Inc.,
Washington. Condor, 11, 5557. Philadelphia, PA.
Brennan, L. A. and Morrison, M. L. (1991). Long-term Dyke, A. S., Andrews, J. T., Clark, P. U., et al. (2002). The
trends of chickadee populations in western North Laurentide and Innuitian ice sheets during the last
America. Condor, 93, 130137. glacial maximum. Quaternary Science Reviews, 21, 931.
Brennan, L. A., Morrison, M. L., and Dahlsten, D. L. Fisher, R. (1954). Statistical Methods for Research Workers.
(1999). Influence of vegetation on occupation of nest Oliver and Boyd, Edinburgh.
boxes by chestnut-backed and mountain chickadees. Gill, F. B., Funk, D. H., and Silverin, B. (1989). Protein rela-
Northwestern Naturalist, 80, 9098. tionships among titmice (Parus). Wilson Bulletin, 101,
Brennan, L. A., Morrison, M. L., and Dahlsten, D. L. 182197.
(2000). Comparative foraging dynamics of chestnut- Gill, F. B., Mostrom, A. M., and Mack, A. L. (1993).
backed and mountain chickadees in the western Sierra Speciation in North American chickadees: I. Patterns of
Nevada. Northwestern Naturalist, 81, 129147. mtDNA genetic divergence. Evolution, 47, 195212.
92 R E P R O D U C T I V E E C O L O G Y, E V O L U T I O N, A N D B E H AV I O R

Gill, F. B., Slikas, B., and Agro, D. (1999). Speciation in the blue tit Parus caeruleus. Journal of Avian Biology, 35,
North American chickadees: II. Geography of mtDNA 352359.
haplotypes in Poecile carolinensis. Auk, 116, 274277. Lacourse, T., Mathewes, R. W., and Fedje, D. W. (2003).
Gill, F. B., Slikas, B., and Sheldon, F. H. (2005). Phylogeny Paleoecology of late-glacial terrestrial deposits
of titmice (Paridae): II. Species relationships based on with in situ conifers from the submerged continental
sequences of the mitochondrial cytochrome b gene. shelf of western Canada. Quaternary Research, 60,
Auk, 122, 121143. 180188.
Goudet, J. (2001). FSTAT, a program to estimate and test Little, E. L. (1971). Atlas of United States Trees. United States
gene diversities and fixation indices (version 2.9.3). Department of Agriculture Misc. Publication 1146.
Available from http://www.unil.ch/izea/softwares/ Lovette, I. J. (2005). Glacial cycles and the tempo of avian
fstat.html. Updated from Goudet (1995). speciation. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 20, 5759.
Goudet, J., Raymond, M., de Meeus, T., and Rousset, F. Mann, D. H. and Hamilton, T. D. (1995). Late Pleistocene
(1996). Testing differentiation in diploid populations. and Holocene paleoenvironments of the North Pacific
Genetics, 144, 19331940. coast. Quaternary Science Review, 14, 449471.
Grinnell, J. (1904). The origin and distribution of the chest- Miller, M. (1997). Tools for Population Genetic Analysis
nut-backed chickadee. Auk, 21, 364382. (TFPGA), version 1.3. Northern Arizona University,
Guo, S. W. and Thompson, E. A. (1992). Performing the Flagstaff, AZ.
exact test of HardyWeinberg proportion for multiple Paetkau, D., Waits, L., Clarkson, P., Craighead, L., and
alleles. Biometrics, 48, 361372. Strobeck, C. (1997). An empirical evaluation of genetic
Hedrick, P. (1999). Perspective: Highly variable loci and distance statistics using microsatellite data from bear
their interpretation in evolution and conservation. (Ursidae) populations. Genetics, 147, 19431957.
Evolution, 53, 313318. Pielou, E. C. (1991). After the Ice Age: the Return of Life to
Hewitt, G. M. (1996). Some genetic consequences of ice Glaciated North America. University of Chicago Press,
ages, and their role in divergence and speciation. Chicago.
Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 58, 247276. Pruett, C. L. and Winker, K. (2005). Northwestern song
Hutchison, D. and Templeton, A. (1999). Correlation of sparrow populations show genetic effects of sequential
pairwise genetic and geographic distance measures: colonization. Molecular Ecology, 14, 14211434.
inferring the relative influences of gene flow and drift Raymond, M. and Rousset, F. (1995a). An exact test for
on the distribution of genetic variability. Evolution, 53, population differentiation. Evolution, 49, 12801283.
18981914. Raymond, M. and Rousset, F. (1995b). GENEPOP (Version
Ibrahim, K. M., Nichols, R. A., and Hewitt, G. M. (1996). 1.2): Population genetics software for exact tests and
Spatial patterns of genetic variation generated by dif- ecumenicism. Journal of Heredity, 86, 248249.
ferent forms of dispersal during range expansion. Rice, W. R. (1989). Analyzing tables of statistical tests.
Heredity, 77, 282291. Evolution, 43, 223225.
Johnson, N. K. and Cicero, C. (2004). New mitochondrial Ritland, C., Pape, T., and Ritland, K. (2001). Genetic struc-
DNA data affirm the importance of Pleistocene speci- ture of yellow cedar (Chamaecyparis nootkatensis).
ation in North American birds. Evolution, 58, 11221130. Canadian Journal of Botany, 79, 822828.
Kvist, L., Broggi, J., Illera, J. C., and Koivula, K. (2005). Ritland, K., Dupuis, L. A., Bunnel, F. L., Hung, W. L. Y.,
Colonisation and diversification of the blue tits (Parus and Carlson, J. E. (2000). Phylogeography of the tailed
caeruleus teneriffae-group) in the Canary Islands. frog (Ascaphus truei) in British Columbia. Canadian
Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution, 34, 501511. Journal of Zoology, 78, 17491758.
Kvist, L., Martens, J., Ahola, A., and Orell, M. (2001). Rohwer, S., Bermingham, E., and Wood, C. (2001).
Phylogeography of a Paelaearctic sedentary passerine, Plumage and mitochondrial DNA haplotype variation
the willow tit (Parus montanus). Journal of Evolutionary across a moving hybrid zone. Evolution, 55, 405422.
Biology, 14, 930941. Salzburger, W., Martens, J., Nazarenko, A. A., Sun, Y.-H.,
Kvist, L., Martens, J., Higuchi, H., Nazarenko, A. A., Dallinger, R., and Sturmbauer, C. (2002a).
Valchuk, O. P., and Orell, M. (2003). Evolution Phylogeography of the Eurasian willow tit (Parus
and genetic structure of the great tit (Parus major) montanus) based on DNA sequences of the mitochon-
complex. Proceedings of the Royal Society, Series B, 270, drial cytochrome b gene. Molecular Phylogenetics and
14471454. Evolution, 24, 2634.
Kvist, L., Viiri, K., Dias, P. C., Rytkonen, S., and Orell, M. Salzburger, W., Martens, J., and Sturmbauer, C. (2002b).
(2004). Glacial history and colonization of Europe by Paraphyly of the blue tit (Parus caeruleus) suggested
PHYLOGEOGRAPHY 93

from cytochrome b sequences. Molecular Phylogenetics Sturman, W. A. (1968). Description and analysis of breed-
and Evolution, 24, 1925. ing habitats of the chickadees, Parus atricapillus and P.
Sambrook, J., Fritsch, E. F., and Maniatis, T. (1989). rufescens. Ecology, 49, 418431.
Molecular CloningA Laboratory Manual. Cold Spring Uimaniemi, L., Orell, M., Kvist, L., Jokimki, J., and
Harbor Laboratory Press. Lumme, J. (2003). Genetic variation of the Siberian tit
Sibley, D. A. (2000). The Sibley Guide to Birds. Alfred A. Parus cinctus populations at the regional level: a mito-
Knopf, Inc., New York. chondrial sequence analysis. Ecography, 26, 98106.
Slatkin, M. (1993). Isolation by distance in equilibrium Wang, J. (2004). Application of the one-migrant-per-gen-
and non-equilibrium populations. Evolution, 47, eration rule to conservation and management.
264279. Conservation Biology, 18, 332343.
Smith, S. (1967). An ecological study of winter flocks of Weir, J. T. and Schluter, D. (2004). Ice sheets promote spe-
black-capped and chestnut-backed chickadees. Wilson ciation in boreal birds. Proceedings of the Royal Society,
Bulletin, 79, 200207. Biological Sciences, 271, 18811887.
Soltis, D. E., Gitzendanner, M. A., Strenge, D. D., and Wisely, S. M., Buskirk, S. W., Russell, G. A., Aubry, K. B.,
Soltis, P. S. (1997). Chloroplast DNA intraspecific phylo- and Zielinski, W. J. (2004). Genetic diversity and struc-
geography of plants from the Pacific Northwest of ture of the fisher (Martes pennanti) in a peninsular and
North America. Plant Systematics and Evolution, 206, peripheral metapopulations. Journal of Mammalogy, 85,
353373. 640648.
This page intentionally left blank
CHAPTER 7

Behavioral aspects of chickadee


hybridization
Robert L. Curry, Lindsay M. Rossano, and
Matthew W. Reudink

of contact may cause abnormal development of


7.1 Introduction
courtship displays such that individuals conse-
Within the Paridae, hybridization is known to occur quently produce mixed signals, or only those char-
regularly in a few pairs of sibling species and acteristic of another species. Even if normal mate
sporadically between other taxa of varying degrees preferences determine mating, hybridization can
of evolutionary proximity (Harrap and Quinn 1995; result if individuals produce displays that incor-
Curry 2005). Interbreeding of black-capped chicka- rectly signal their own identity.
dees and Carolina chickadees represents a unique In many songbirds, females base species recogni-
situation in the family in that although these tion and mate choice in large part on songs and calls
congeners are not each others closest relatives (Gill (Nowicki and Searcy 2005). Unlike morphology,
et al. 2005), they nonetheless hybridize regularly: vocal traits may be vulnerable to misimprinting
observations consistent with interbreeding have because development of songs and calls frequently
been reported from numerous locations along their involves learning, including exposure to sounds
predominantly eastwest line of parapatric contact during an early critical period. To understand mech-
stretching from central New Jersey to south-eastern anisms of hybridization in any songbird system, it is
Kansas, with a dip southward in the Appalachian therefore worthwhile to examine the extent to
Mountains to western North Carolina (reviewed by which males within contact zones produce vocaliza-
Bronson et al. 2005; Curry 2005). Hybridization tions that reflect their own genetic identity, and the
between black-capped and Carolina chickadees has ways in which females respond to these signals.
been investigated most recently in Ohio, where In this chapter, we summarize information from
both observational and experimental evidence have our studies of the hybrid zone between black-
documented reduced reproductive success associ- capped and Carolina chickadees in south-eastern
ated with interbreeding (Bronson et al. 2003a, 2005), Pennsylvania. Our focus is on patterns of vocal vari-
and in Pennsylvania (Reudink et al. 2006). ation in relation to among- and within-population
Production of hybrids can result from either genetic variation. Primary questions we address
reproduction by mixed pairs or extrapair copula- include: (1) What song and call types are produced
tion involving heterospecific individuals. On a within this hybrid zone? (2) Are the vocalization
proximate level, heterospecific mating is likely to types congruent with one another within individu-
be facilitated by sexual imprinting: if individuals als? (3) What insights might we derive from these
are exposed during development to adults or nest- patterns regarding mechanisms of hybridization in
mates of another species, they may develop prefer- chickadees? We then review available information,
ences for mates of that species (Grant and Grant and opportunities for further research, concerning
1997; Irwin and Price 1999; ten Cate and Vos 1999). patterns and mechanisms of hybridization among
Additionally, exposure to heterospecifics in a zone Parids of both North America and Eurasia.

95
96 R E P R O D U C T I V E E C O L O G Y, E V O L U T I O N, A N D B E H AV I O R

7.2 Background and methods in pitch and duration of individual notes, even
among songs that can be grossly categorized based
Black-capped and Carolina chickadees are morph- on number of notes as black-capped-like or
ologically and ecologically similar. Black-capped Carolina-like (Robbins et al. 1986). In some parts of
chickadees, which inhabit much of Canada and the the contact zone, an association between individual
northern tier of the USA, have been studied exten- genetic identity and song parameters may exist
sively in Massachusetts, Ontario, and elsewhere (Sawaya 1990; Sattler 1996). No previous study has
(Smith 1993; see also this volume, Chapters 9, 12, examined quantitative aspects of calls in detail for
and 14). Carolina chickadees, which are resident any local segment of the chickadee contact zone, nor
throughout the south-eastern USA, have been stud- have genotypes, songs, and calls of individuals been
ied less overall (Mostrom et al. 2002) but have been investigated concurrently. Here, we focus our atten-
the subjects of recent research, particularly on tion on songs and on the D note of the chick-a-dee call,
vocalizations (e.g. Chapter 13). which appears to function in flock discrimination
A prominent feature of chickadee behavior is the and species identity (Mammen and Nowicki 1981;
production of whistled song by males, mainly just Charrier and Sturdy 2005; Chapters 10 and 13).
prior to and during the breeding season (Hailman
and Ficken 1996; Chapter 14). Development of the
whistled fee-bee of the black-capped chickadee, 7.2.1 Study sites
despite a high degree of stereotypy across its We have studied chickadees principally at three
extensive geographic range (Kroodsma et al. 1999), sites in south-eastern Pennsylvania (Fig. 7.1). The
appears to involve learning (Kroodsma et al. 1995; southernmost, Great Marsh (GM), covers approxi-
Baker et al. 2003). Pitch characteristics appear rele- mately 200 ha; research here, initiated in 1998,
vant for species identification (Weisman and involves resident Carolina chickadees. Nolde
Ratcliffe 1989; Chapter 10). Song repertoires in Forest (NF), about 24 km north-west, is a state park
Carolina chickadees are broader, with most males of 269 ha where research, initiated in 1998, focuses
producing two general song types: a four-note on a population comprising predominantly hybrid
song involving alternating high and low notes (see- individuals (Reudink et al. 2006; see below). The
bee-see-bay) and an song incorporating one or more third site (HM), about 35 km north of NF, includes
high notes followed by variable numbers of lower ~120 ha within Hawk Mountain Sanctuary, where
notes (e.g. see-see-bay-bay-bay; Smith 1972; see also we began studying chickadees in 2001, as well as 10
Fig. 4C in Ward and Ward 1974). Geographic nearby woodlots (123 ha, totaling ~80 ha) from
variation in Carolina chickadee song is more pro- which we obtained data in a companion study. As
nounced (Ward 1966; Mostrom et al. 2002). recently as 1988, all individuals studied at HM were
Both species also produce a wide variety of calls black-capped chickadees, but beginning in 2002 we
(Hailman and Ficken 1996). The chick-a-dee call, used detected evidence of hybrid individuals in this
throughout the year in pair and flock interactions population (Reudink et al. 2007). We also used some
and during predator mobbing, has been studied in additional recordings from areas in Pennsylvania
depth (Chapter 13). Another complex vocalization, south and east of GM (Carolina chickadees) and
the gargle call, has been examined in black-capped from Nova Scotia, Vermont, and Massachusetts
chickadees (Chapter 11) and has been described in (black-capped chickadees).
Carolina chickadees (Mostrom et al. 2002), but
detailed interspecific comparisons of the call, which
is used in aggressive interactions, are lacking. 7.2.2 Field methods
Unusual vocal patterns, including bilingual We studied mainly resident chickadees using artifi-
singing, have been documented frequently among cial snags (based on Grubb and Bronson 1995) for
chickadees within or close to the presumed contact nesting. We monitored up to 53 snags at GM, 152 at
zone (e.g. Johnston 1971; Robbins et al. 1986). In NF, and 212 at HM. Total samples of nests reaching
Missouri populations shown to include hybrid indi- at least the stage of egg-laying were 151 at GM
viduals based on genetic analysis (Braun and (930 per season, 19982005); 270 at NF (655, in
Robbins 1986; Sawaya 1990), songs are intermediate 19982005); and 54 at HM (120, in 20002005).
B E H AV I O R A L A S P E C T S O F H Y B R I D I Z AT I O N 97

Carbon Monroe
Schuylkill

Hawk Mountain Northampton


(HM)
Lehigh New
Jersey
Berks
Nolde Forest Bucks
Lebanon
(NF)
Montgomery

Lancaster Great Marsh


(GM) Phila.
Delaware
Chester
York N
Delaware 0 20 40 Kilometers
Maryland

Figure 7.1 Locations of primary study sites (names of Pennsylvania counties shown). Great Marsh (GM) is inhabited by Carolina chickadees.
Nolde Forest (NF) is inhabited by hybrid chickadees. The Hawk Mountain (HM) site, inhabited mainly by black-capped chickadees, comprises
property at Hawk Mountain Sanctuary and nearby woodlots. The contact zone is believed to run approximately from south-west (northern York
Co.) to north-east (southern Northampton Co.).

We used mist nets to capture adults at winter gated genetic identity of individuals, as well as
feeding stations or at nests. Each received a Fish parentage patterns, using six microsatellite loci
and Wildlife Service (FWS) aluminum band and developed in prior studies of black-capped chicka-
two or three plastic colored bands. We banded dee and blue tit; see Reudink et al. (2006) for details.
nestlings with FWS bands at 9 to 12 days after
hatching; the few that remained to breed locally 7.2.4 Hybrid index scores
later received colored bands.
Our analyses relied on the assumption that the two
reference populations (HM and GM) included pre-
7.2.3 Genetic methods
dominantly black-capped and Carolina chickadees
We collected blood samples (1040 l from the respectively, and that genetic information from
brachial vein) from nestlings and adults at each nest, these populations could be used to characterize
from which we extracted DNA using commercially individuals at NF. This assumption has received
available kits. Characterization of mitochondrial support from analyses using the program STRUC-
DNA (mtDNA) haplotypes involved polymerase TURE (v. 2.1; Pritchard et al. 2000), which employs
chain reactionrestriction fragment length poly- Bayesian methods to estimate the number of distinct
morphism (PCRRFLP) analysis: amplified cyto- populations and microsatellite allele frequencies
chrome b mtDNA was cut by the enzymes EcoR V among residents in each population (Reudink et al.
(black-capped haplotype) or Xmn I (Carolina 2007). However, this method did not allow us to
haplotype), based on Kvist et al. (1996). We investi- characterize the genetic identities of individuals; to
98 R E P R O D U C T I V E E C O L O G Y, E V O L U T I O N, A N D B E H AV I O R

produce a rule based on microsatellite genotypes at NF, based on repeated observations during the
that we could use to characterize individual breed- laying and incubation periods at active nests.
ers, we performed an iterative maximum-likelihood
assignment test based on genotypes from the two 7.2.6 Sound analysis
reference populations, after omitting a few HM
birds carrying Carolina mtDNA. The analysis We performed acoustic analyses using Canary 1.2
yielded assignment probabilities, where px and py (Cornell Laboratory of Ornithology). We noted the
are the likelihoods of an individual belonging to total number of notes (treating the bee of typical
population x or y respectively, and resulting hybrid black-capped chickadee song as one note) but
index scores (IH) for breeders at all three sites, on a quantified only the first two notes to permit com-
scale from 0 (Carolina) to 1 (black-capped) as parisons among all songs (Table 7.1). For chick-a-dee
follows (after Hansen et al. 2000): calls, we focused on the middle D note in calls with
an odd number of D notes, or the first D of the mid-
IH1ln(px)/[ln(px)ln(py)] dle two notes in calls with an even number of D
For additional details, see Reudink et al. (2006). notes. We used a spectrum of a 2.0 msec section at
the midpoint of the note (44.1 kHz sample rate;
1024 FFT size) to measure frequency range for
7.2.5 Song and call recordings sound components within 30 dB of the loudest part
During the breeding seasons of 2000 and 2001, LMR of the note (based on Nowicki 1989).
recorded songs from 28 breeding males at NF
(Rossano 2003). For comparison, we used songs of 7.3 Results
four individuals from predominantly or exclusively
7.3.1 Genetic composition of study
black-capped populations (HM, Nova Scotia, and
populations
Vermont) and from five birds from Carolina popu-
lations (GM and Montgomery Co., PA). Analysis of mtDNA supported the assumption that
LMR recorded calls between October 2000 and the three primary sites spanned the contact zone. At
October 2001 at NF from the same 28 males whose GM, all breeders in all years had Carolina mtDNA
songs were sampled either near winter feeders or (n 84 individuals, including 40 females and 44
within each males breeding season territory but males). At NF, the majority of breeders exhibited
away from the nest. For comparison, we used Carolina haplotypes: of 83 male breeders analyzed
recorded chick-a-dee calls from eight individuals from 19982005, only three (3.6%, associated with
from reference populations of black-capped and four nests in 19982000) had black-capped mtDNA
Carolina chickadees. and among 67 females sampled, only two (3.0%,
Observations of vocalizations at the three both breeding in 2000) had black-capped mtDNA.
primary sites continued in 20022005. In each year, At HM, all chickadees breeding before 2002 (three
we categorized the song type of each breeding male females, three males) had black-capped haplotypes.

Table 7.1 Song parameters in hybrid zone (NF study site) in relation to corresponding songs from sites outside of the hybrid zone;
repeated-measures least-square means SEM shown for each parameter

Population and song type 1st note frequency 1st note duration Interval between 2nd note 2nd note
(kHz) (msec) 1st and 2nd notes frequency (kHz) duration (msec)
(msec)

Black-capped (n 69) 3.8 0.1 367.2 23.3 124.7 16.5a 3.3 0.1 386.4 37.1
NF, 2-note song (n 99) 3.9 0.1 330.6 6.4 101.4 4.5a 3.3 0.1 364.6 10.8
NF, 4-note song (n 99) 6.8 0.1 213.7 12.9b 117.8 15.5c 4.2 0.1 256.3 18.2
Carolina (n 64) 6.7 0.1 263.1 15.4b 145.2 18.6c 4.3 0.1 253.2 21.8
a,b,c
Means sharing letter different at P 0.05, repeated-measures ANOVA and Tukeys HSD post-hoc contrast.
B E H AV I O R A L A S P E C T S O F H Y B R I D I Z AT I O N 99

Of the HM males breeding in 20022005, 16 (94%) songs), except we recorded one bird in southern
had black-capped mtDNA and 1 (6%) had Carolina Montgomery Co., Pennsylvania, that gave only
mtDNA, whereas among females, 16 (72%) had three-note songs (see-bay-see; n 13) representing
black-capped mtDNA and six (27%) had Carolina the Song Variant category of Smith (1972). All songs
mtDNA (2 3.3, df 1, p 0.068). in 20002001 (n 69) from areas occupied predom-
Analysis of microsatellite genotypes from birds inantly or exclusively by black-capped chickadees,
breeding in 19982003 further showed that the three including HM, represented the characteristic black-
sites differed with respect to the genetic identity of capped fee-bee. Despite genetic evidence of intro-
their resident chickadeesand that the NF popula- gression at HM, this pattern had not changed as of
tion included predominantly hybrids. Hybrid index 2005: we have yet to hear any HM male include
scores ranged from 0.36 to 0.46 (x 0.41 0.03 SE, Carolina song, or even variant black-capped song
n 45) at GM (more Carolina-like) and from 0.53 to (see below), in his repertoire.
0.69 (x 0.60 0.04 SE, n 24) at HM (more black- At the NF hybrid zone site, 80% of the songs
capped-like; Fig. 7.2). Resident chickadees at NF (n 813) produced by the 28 resident males in
had scores ranging from 0.40 to 0.58 (x 0.48 0.03 20002001 matched the typical black-capped fee-bee.
SE, n 145), with 63.9% having intermediate scores Other songs observed, in declining order of occur-
falling between the upper and lower ranges of the rence, included: four-note typical Carolina song
GM and HM populations respectively (Fig. 7.2). (see-bee-see-bay; 9%); variant black-capped type
Approximately 27.8% of NF individuals fell within (e.g. fee-bee-bee; 5%); variant Carolina type (e.g. see-
the GM score range whereas 8.3% fell within the bay-see-bay-see; 3%); and mixed song, combining
HM score range. typical elements of both songs (2%) (Fig. 7.3 and
Rossano 2003).
Although songs at NF sounded superficially like
7.3.2 Song patterns beyond and within
the typical songs of one of the parental species,
hybrid zone
values of several variables differed between the
Song patterns were consistent with the assumption songs from NF relative to the corresponding
that sites other than NF were outside of, or at the parental areas (Table 7.1). In particular, two-note
extreme edge of, the hybrid zone. Among songs songs at NF differed from typical black-capped
sampled in 20002001 from GM and other Carolina fee-bee songs in having a shorter pause between the
localities, all songs analyzed matched the typical notes. Four-note songs at NF differed from those of
see-bee-see-bay Carolina chickadee song type (n 51 typical Carolina chickadees in having shorter first

0.20
Proportion of each population

0.15

0.10

0.05

0.00
0.35 0.40 0.45 0.50 0.55 0.60 0.65 0.70
Hybrid Index scores

Figure 7.2 Hybrid index scores (IH ), based on microsatellite genotypes, of resident chickadees from three primary study sites in south-eastern
Pennsylvania. Values range from low for Carolina chickadees to high for black-capped chickadees. Vertical axis shows relative proportions among
individuals from each site; gray bars, GM (n 45); white bars, NF (n 145); black bars, HM (n 24).
100 R E P R O D U C T I V E E C O L O G Y, E V O L U T I O N, A N D B E H AV I O R

(a) 8

kHz 0

(b) 8

kHz 0

(c) 8

kHz 0
0 0.5 1.0 1.5
Seconds

Figure 7.3 Variant and mixed songs from the hybrid zone study site at Nolde Forest. (a) Example with quantitative characteristics of typical
black-capped fee-bee-ee but with repetition of bee-ee note. (b) Example with quantitative characteristics of typical Carolina chickadee see-bee
see-bay but with extra terminal see note. (c) Example of mixed song, with quantitative characteristics of typical Carolina chickadee see-bee-see
bay followed by bee-ee note of typical black-capped chickadee song.

notes and a shorter interval between the first and singing over time. When our observations began at
second notes. NF in 19971998, we detected no males that
Of the 28 different NF males recorded in produced Carolina-type songs. Birds including
20002001, 19 (68%) produced only typical black- aberrant or Carolina song types in their repertoires
capped fee-bee songs and one gave normal black- appeared in 1999. Of the males examined in
capped song and a black-capped variant. Six (21%) 20002001, 71% gave only black-capped songs
of the males were bilingual, producing both two- while 29% were bilingual (see above). Among
note black-capped song and four-note Carolina males breeding at NF in 2005 (n 45), the propor-
song types. Two other bilingual birds (7%) tion of males singing only two-note black-capped
produced typical songs of both species as well as song had declined to 47% (2 4.40, df 1,
mixed songs that included notes characteristic of p 0.036), while 53% of males gave both song
both species (Fig. 7.3). types (n 23 males) or only songs involving a mix
Based on the number of notes sung (pending of note types from both species (n 1).
detailed acoustic analysis), we detected a trend Song patterns at NF contrast with vocal behavior
toward increasing representation of bilingual in nearby Carolina chickadee populations in two
B E H AV I O R A L A S P E C T S O F H Y B R I D I Z AT I O N 101

ways. First, we have yet to detect any NF males that between the parental species in all parameters
give only Carolina songs; vocally, the population measured (Table 7.2; repeated measures ANOVA
included black-capped males and bilingual or and Tukeys HSD post-hoc contrasts, P 0.05)
mixed-song males, but no males with a pure except minimum frequency. Discriminant function
Carolina repertoire. Second, those males that are analysis (DFA) indicated that the variables most
bilingual produce only the four-note see-bee-see-bay informative for distinguishing between the two sets
Carolina song type; we have not heard any male at of calls were duration, peak amplitude frequency,
NF give any version of Song Variant (e.g. see-see-bay and maximum frequency of the dee note. DFA cat-
bay-bay) that Carolina chickadees in south-eastern egorized 46 (92%) of 50 calls recorded at black-
Pennsylvania routinely include in their repertoire capped sites as black-capped (i.e. having a
(Smith 1972; R. Curry, unpublished data), whereas probability of being black-capped 0.8). The four
all breeding males at GM have done so in every calls categorized as Carolina were produced by a
year (19982005). single male at HM; this bird had black-capped
mtDNA but could have been a hybrid. Of the calls
recorded from Carolina chickadee localities
7.3.3 Chick-a-dee calls beyond and (n 81), 80 (99%) were categorized as Carolina.
within hybrid zone The one other call was intermediate (probability of
Analysis of dee notes of black-capped and Carolina being Carolina 0.74), produced by a bird that also
chickadees (Fig. 7.4) revealed clear differentiation gave 14 other calls classified as Carolina.

(a) 8

kHz 0

(b) 8

kHz 0

(c) 8

2 Figure 7.4 Chick-a-dee calls from hybrid


zone study site (NF) categorized by
kHz 0 discriminant function analysis as: (a) black-
0 0.5 1.0 1.5 capped; (b) Carolina-like (0.5 probability of
Seconds being Carolina 0.8); and (c) Carolina.
102 R E P R O D U C T I V E E C O L O G Y, E V O L U T I O N, A N D B E H AV I O R

Table 7.2 Quantitative characteristics of D notes of chick-a-dee calls at localities beyond and within the chickadee contact zone

Population dee duration (msec) Peak amplitude Maximum frequency (kHz) Interval between
frequency (kHz) dee notes (msec)

Black-cappeda (n 50 calls) 152 21 3.9 0.5 5.0 0.8 62 16


Nolde Forest (n 453) 114 13 4.2 0.3 5.8 0.6 49 12
Carolinab (n 81) 105 12 4.1 0.4 5.8 0.6 48 15
a
Includes samples from Hawk Mountain supplemented with recordings from Nova Scotia, Vermont, Massachusetts, and New York.
b
Includes samples from Great Marsh supplemented with recordings from Montgomery and Delaware Cos., Pennsylvania; Maryland; and North
Carolina.

We used the same DFA formula to categorize without also singing Carolina-type songs in the
calls (n 453), based on dee notes, from the 28 case of bilingual malesbut with all males giving
males at NF whose song were also recorded (see at least some chick-a-dee calls that were Carolina-
above). Most calls (87%) at NF were Carolina (prob- like, and with some doing so exclusively.
ability 0.8); 8% were Carolina-like (0.5 probabil-
ity 0.8); 3% were black-capped-like (0.5 7.3.5 Relationships between behavior
probability 0.8); and 2% were black-capped. and genetics
All males at NF gave individual calls that varied
in their quantitative characteristics and thus fell Vocal patterns in the contact zone at NF were not
into different categories. Of the 28 males, 17 (61%) correlated with genetic variation among individ-
produced exclusively calls categorized as Carolina uals. Among 23 males whose songs we categorized
or Carolina-like. Three other males gave calls that and whose microsatellite genotypes we evaluated
were mostly Carolina/Carolina-like (70% of indi- (all of which had Carolina mtDNA), those giving
viduals calls) with the remaining calls being black- only black-capped songs had average hybrid index
capped-like. The six other birds (21%) gave calls scores (x 0.47 0.036 SD, n 16) that were simi-
spanning all types of calls, including at least a few lar to those of all other bilingual and mixed-song
calls categorized as black-capped. males (n 7; x 0.48 0.037 SD; t 0.50,
df 20, P 0.63). Birds categorized as giving
Carolina-like calls also had hybrid index scores that
7.3.4 Lack of congruence between
were no different (x 0.48 0.031 SD, n 17) from
songs and calls
scores for males categorized as producing inter-
Using the quantitative categorization of chick-a-dee mediate calls (x 0.46 0.031 SD, n 7; t 1.72,
calls from DFA, there was no association between df 22, P 0.10).
an individuals song and call categories among 27 While vocal patterns at NF were not associated
males at NF. Of the 20 males that produced only with genetic variation, aspects of the mating
black-capped chickadee song, 14 (70%) gave system varied in relation to male genotypes
Carolina-type calls and six (30%) gave variable (Reudink et al. 2006). Females at NF that engaged in
calls; among the eight males that sang both song extrapair mating were socially paired with males
types or mixed songs, seven (88%) gave Carolina- having higher hybrid index scores (i.e. more black-
type calls and one (12%) gave variable calls capped-like) than the social mates of females
(2 4.40, df 1, P 0.31). that did not produce any extrapair offspring.
Similarly, we detected no significant differences Furthermore, extrapair sires had hybrid index
in dee note components between NF males that fell scores that were lower (more Carolina-like) than
into the two categories of singer. Therefore, there those of corresponding social males. Because there
was an overall pattern at NF of males producing was no relationship between hybrid index scores
relatively normal black-capped songswith or and vocalizations among NF males, it is unlikely
B E H AV I O R A L A S P E C T S O F H Y B R I D I Z AT I O N 103

that this pattern of extrapair mating was influenced whether genetic and behavioral variability at NF is
by male vocal behavior, but we have not tested similar to that in other areas will require further
these relationships directly. molecular work in Pennsylvania and additional
field study at the local level elsewhere.

7.4 Discussion
7.4.2 Song patterns in the Pennsylvania
7.4.1 Hybridization in south-eastern contact zone
Pennsylvania
A striking finding in our study was the retention of
Our results indicate that extensive hybridization black-capped chickadee songs at NF, where essen-
occurs within the region sampled. The genetic data tially all resident adults are now hybrids (and
reveal an overall difference between the GM and where some may be genetically pure Carolina
HM populations, and intermediacy of chickadees at chickadees). Even those males with the ability to
NF; the results also provide evidence that the sing at least one Carolina song type continue to
contact zone is shifting northward (see also produce black-capped songs as well.
Reudink et al. 2007). At NF, black-capped genes That existence of bilingual chickadees may
appear to be decreasing in the population, while at indicate interbreeding has been long assumed, with
HM, birds with Carolina-like genotypes are increas- some even suggesting that bilingual males in zones
ing. Because all individuals at GM already of overlap might gain a functional advantage by
had Carolina genotypes at the start of our study, being able to deter rivals of both parental species
changes were not apparent there during our study, (Robbins et al. 1986). However, retention of black-
but the absence as of 1998 of black-capped songs, capped song long after hybridization has occurred
reported in the vicinity of GM in the 1960s (Ward at a given site has not been appreciated previously.
and Ward 1974), suggests that the southern edge of This finding has implications for the accuracy of
the hybrid zone has shifted north as well. The distributional surveys, such as the breeding bird
persistence at NF of some black-capped chickadee atlas program, where judgments about residency
genetic influence contemporaneous with appear- are often based primarily on songs heard. The
ance of birds at HM carrying Carolina genes tendency for hybrid males to produce relatively
indicates that the hybrid contact in south-eastern normal black-capped song underscores the likely
Pennsylvania is at least 50 km wide. existence of developmental mechanisms that result
The patterns in Pennsylvania are broadly consist- in song stereotypy across most of the black-capped
ent with evidence from other portions of the chicka- chickadees range, but the nature of those mech-
dee contact zone with respect to hybrid zone anisms remains unknown (Kroodsma et al. 1999).
structure and northward movement. Genetic data A distinct but related aspect of vocal patterns in
suggest a somewhat narrower zone (30 km wide) the hybrid zone concerns production of aberrant
in Missouri and in the mountains of the Virginias song types. Song variants are particularly rare in
(with some evidence of introgression over longer most black-capped chickadee populations; excep-
distances in these regions) and in north-central tions may be restricted to isolated populations at the
Ohio (Sawaya 1990; Sattler and Braun 2000; geographic range limits (see Chapter 14). Defining
Bronson et al. 2005). The hybrid zone in Ohio is aberrant song in Carolina chickadees is more chal-
believed to have moved ~100 km northward over lenging, because of greater inherent variability
approximately 70 years (Bronson et al. 2005). among and within populations in this species.
Our finding that a large percentage of resident Regardless, the tendency for chickadees in contact
chickadees within the contact zone may be hybrids zone populations to produce songs not characteristic
also is consistent with other studies. In Missouri and of parental populations has been noted frequently.
in the Virginias, the proportion of hybrids among Aberrant songs fall into two main categories: (1)
samples of 100 or fewer specimens was 44 to 58% songs with the number of notes and pattern typical
(Sawaya 1990; Sattler and Braun 2000). Determining of parental forms, but with different quantitative
104 R E P R O D U C T I V E E C O L O G Y, E V O L U T I O N, A N D B E H AV I O R

characteristics; and (2) songs that combine notes of song. Second, we observed mismatches at the level
the typical songs of both parental species. We of individual males that produced quantitatively
observed both of these song categories at NF, variable or intermediate calls, independent of the
although many malesincluding some bilingual song types that the same birds sang.
malesnever produced markedly aberrant songs. Several explanations for conflicting patterns are
However, our finding that some seemingly nor- possible. One is that songs are learned whereas call
mal songs had some intermediate quantitative development is subject to stronger genetic influence,
characteristics matches results from Missouri such that birds with hybrid genotypes produce
(Robbins et al. 1986) more so than those from the acoustically intermediate call notes. Alternatively,
Virginias, where the pattern was bimodal (Sattler development of both vocalizations may involve
1996). Our study adds detail to understanding of learning (Chapters 10, 11, and 13), but with different
song patterns the hybrid zone by showing that influences such as the age at which learning occurs
males producing relatively normal black-capped or the social context. Because song is primarily pro-
songswith or without Carolina song as wellcan duced by males, chickadees could be learning the
have Carolina mtDNA, along with microsatellite song of their father or those of neighbor males. In
genotypes consistent with hybrid status. contrast, calls are produced by both sexes and
The vocal and genetic patterns we detected develop early in life; accordingly, chickadees could
suggest that many chickadees in the contact zone, learn their calls not only from their father but from
but perhaps not all, are capable of learning their mother, as might happen when she is brooding.
heterospecific songs. This finding is consistent with
previous findings that individuals of these species
7.4.4 Potentially confounding variables in
can, when reared together, learn notes of the other
analysis of vocal patterns
(Kroodsma et al. 1995). However, the specific mech-
anisms that cause some chickadees to become Our understanding of the vocal behavior of
bilingual while others continue to sing only black- hybridizing chickadees is limited in some respects,
capped song remain to be determined. because in our observational study we cannot
control for several potentially confounding vari-
ables. With respect to songs, an important factor is
7.4.3 Chick-a-dee calls across the hybrid zone
our ability to thoroughly characterize the full reper-
That the D notes of the black-capped chickadees toire of a large sample of breeding male chickadees
chick-a-dee call are longer in duration and lower in at each site. In general, male chickadees display
pitch than those of the corresponding Carolina chicka- their full repertoire most reliably during dawn
dee call has been known for a long time (see Chapter chorus song bouts that they deliver close to active
13). It is therefore surprising that this vocalization has nests, during the phase of the breeding season
not previously received particular attention in relation when females have begun laying eggs or are within
to the hybrid zone between the species, apart from an a few days of beginning to do so (see Chapter 14).
unpublished thesis (Crock 1975). As Crock observed However, male chickadees at NF do not seem to
using a broad geographic approach, we found that participate consistently in the dawn chorus, even
parameters of D notes from the hybrid zone were during the critical period of the breeding season;
quantitatively intermediate on average. Our observa- characterizing song repertoires therefore has had to
tion that individual chickadees in the hybrid zone depend additionally on observations throughout
produce highly variable D notes is novel. the day, when each male sings intermittently
Two aspects of our results suggest that genetic and throughout its territory. While we continue to
environmental influences on development of calls investigate this impression, we can be confident
operate differently than those affecting song learning. about characterizing males as bilingual once we
First, a disparity exists at NF between the average D observe production of both black-capped and
notes characteristics (mainly Carolina-like) at the Carolina song; we can, however, be less certain
population level and the persistence of black-capped that males that we classify as producing only
B E H AV I O R A L A S P E C T S O F H Y B R I D I Z AT I O N 105

black-capped song are not in fact bilingual. documented patterns of extrapair paternity
Similarly, our ability to determine precisely which (Reudink et al. 2006), suggests that chickadees in
males produce aberrant or mixed songs is likely the hybrid zone probably do not base mating deci-
dependent on the sample size of songs of each bird. sions on song categories. Pairing in chickadees
The number of songs recorded at high sound qual- generally occurs within winter flocks, long before
ity also influences our ability to characterize fully males sing actively at the start of the nesting season
the relatively subtle variation in quantitative song (see Chapter 9). It is therefore unlikely that a
parameters, above and beyond the major categories females choice of a social mate would be based on
based on note number, duration, and pitch. the males song repertoire.
However, because we can easily identify nearly all However, extrapair mating decisions could be
notes as either black-capped-like or Carolina-like, influenced by the singing behavior of males in the
we believe that our conclusions about general hybrid zone, as in allopatric populations of black-
patterns of vocal variationincluding especially capped chickadees (Chapter 14). We are currently
the persistence of black-capped song types, and the investigating whether differences exist in the
absence to date of the Song Variant version of the tendency to engage in extrapair mating between
Carolina song, in the hybrid zoneare robust. females socially paired with bilingual males versus
Assessing vocal variation with respect to other males that sing only black-capped song. Regardless
chickadee calls is even more challenging because of of whether such a pattern exists, it is unlikely to be
the inherent complexity of chickadee vocal reper- correlated with the previously observed tendency
toires. Even when focusing only on the chick-a-dee (Reudink et al. 2006) for females paired with black-
call, many sources of variation come into play. Our capped-like males to engage in extrapair mating,
population-level averages for the D notes, especially and to do so with Carolina-like extrapair mates,
given relatively small sample sizes, could incorp- because there does not appear to be an association
orate biases caused by inadvertently sampling between genetic identity and song types in the NF
different proportions of males and females, birds of population. Still, it will be interesting to assess
different flocks, calls produced in different contexts, whether females could use song as a honest signal
or notes taken from calls that vary in overall syn- of male quality within the hybrid zone, as might be
taxall factors that may influence note structure true if the vocal performance of hybrid males were
(see Chapter 13). The same considerations apply to impaired by developmental stress (Nowicki and
our characterization of calls at the level of individual Searcy 2005).
birds, along with an even greater challenge regard-
ing adequate sampling. Nevertheless, we are confi-
dent of the overall conclusion that mismatches exist 7.5 Hybridization throughout the
at NF between black-capped/bilingual singing and Paridae
Carolina-like calls. Recent observations offer further
7.5.1 Other examples in North America
support: in 200506, many large chickadees with
black-capped plumage wintered at NF as part of a While the extended contact zone between black-
major regional irruption. We easily distinguished capped and Carolina chickadees has been studied
these individuals, based on calls, from the local most intensively, three other examples of inter-
(hybrid) residents; mtDNA data have invariably breeding among North American Paridsall
confirmed that the irruption birds had black-capped involving sibling species pairsalso have been
chickadee haplotypes. examined in some detail. Mountain and black-
capped chickadees hybridize at least occasionally
in western North America, where their geographic
7.4.5 Completing the behavioral picture
ranges overlap broadly (McCallum 1998).
of chickadee hybridization
However, these species remain ecologically sep-
The finding that black-capped chickadee song arated by differences in habitat use (Hill and Lein
persists in a hybridizing population, coupled with 1989 Chapter 8); the circumstances leading to
106 R E P R O D U C T I V E E C O L O G Y, E V O L U T I O N, A N D B E H AV I O R

hybridization remain to be worked out. Tufted and bridled titmouse with Mexican chickadee; oak or
black-crested titmice hybridize in a narrow zone in juniper titmice with black-capped chickadees; or
Texas and were formerly considered conspecific, tufted titmouse with Carolina or black-capped
but the combination of geographic stability of the chickadees. Hybridization appears to be exceed-
contact zone, genetic distinctiveness, and vocal dif- ingly rare in these cases, despite large areas of geo-
ferences have supported their treatment as separate graphic overlap; the only known example involved
species (Banks et al. 2002). Similarly, oak and a black-capped chickadee tufted titmouse cross
juniper titmice in the western United States were (Cockrum 1952). The rarity of such matings is
formerly considered conspecific (plain titmouse) unsurprising given that titmice typically are at least
until recent work revealed a low incidence of 50% larger than sympatric chickadees, except in the
hybridization in a small area of secondary contact case of bridled titmouse and Mexican chickadee,
in north-eastern California (Cicero 1996, 2004). which differ little in size but greatly in appearance.
Whether songs and calls, which differ between
these titmice (Cicero 2000), serve as barriers to
7.5.2 Hybridization among Eurasian
hybridization is a subject of current study.
Parids
Opportunities exist for research on potential
hybridization (or the avoidance of interbreeding) in Observations of mixed breeding pairs, or of pheno-
several other pairs of New World Parids that are at typically intermediate individuals, have provided
least partly sympatric. No studies have explored in evidence for hybridization among numerous
detail breeding relationships between any species European and Asian Parids. Rare hybridization,
pairs within the clade of brown-capped chickadees implied by anecdotal reports of mixed pairs or
(see Gill et al. 2005). Hybridization in the past intermediate specimens, has involved willow
between chestnut-backed and boreal chickadees marsh tit; willow coal tit; willow crested tit;
represents one possible explanation for phylogeo- coal crested tit; blue great tit; marsh great tit;
graphic patterns (Chapter 6), but barriers to inter- great coal tit; and willow varied tit (reviewed
breeding between these sibling species have not by Harrap and Quinn 1995; Curry 2005 McCarthy
been investigated. The lack a whistled song in the 2006). Interbreeding occurs more frequently between
brown-capped species suggests that song recogni- Siberian and willow tits in Finnish Lapland where
tion cannot serve as a primary isolating mechanism. the former species is rare and the latter is more
Boreal chickadees also are not known to hybridize common (Jrvinen 1997), an example illustrating a
with Siberian tits where these species co-occur in general principle that large differences in relative
Alaska (Ficken et al. 1996), but knowledge of the abundance facilitate hybridization in passerines
latter species in North America is limited (Hailman (Randler 2002). Blue, azure, and yellow-bellied tits
and Haftorn 1995). Similarly, no study has investi- exhibit a complex pattern: hybridization between
gated reasons why black-capped chickadees appar- the former two species has occurred following
ently never interbreed with boreal chickadees, irruptive movements or range expansion of azure
though the species have broadly overlapping tit into the blue tits range, whereas azure and yel-
geographic ranges and habitat preferences that are low-bellied tits, which some consider conspecific,
shared at least in part. A parallel situation exists appear to maintain a more stable contact zone; the
between mountain and chestnut-backed chickadees; case is complicated by evidence suggesting that
again, no hybridization has been observed azure tits are evolutionarily nested within the blue
(McCallum 1998) but further study is merited. tit clade (Salzburger et al. 2002). In a different case
Sympatric overlap occurs between juniper and involving sibling species, a stable contact zone
bridled titmice in Arizona, at the southern extreme appears to exist between coal tits and black-crested
of the formers range, but no hybridization has been tits in Nepal (Lhrl 1994; but see Harrap and Quinn
reported (Nocedal and Ficken 1998). 1995:302). Until recently, the great tit was con-
Other combinations of sympatric Parids in North sidered a ring species exhibiting terminal contact in
America involve titmouse/chickadee pairs, such as Siberia, in addition to hybridization with the
B E H AV I O R A L A S P E C T S O F H Y B R I D I Z AT I O N 107

Turkestan tit in Central Asia. However, comprehen- Cross fostering tends to reduce dominance status
sive analysis of genetic, morphological, and vocal (Hansen and Slagsvold 2004), which could indir-
patterns support separation into three species ectly affect the tendency to be chosen as a mate;
great tit (including the Turkestan tit), Japanese tit, females that pair with cross-fostered males do not,
and cinereus titwith five different contact zones however, engage in elevated rates of extrapair
(Pckert et al. 2005). paternity (Johannessen et al. 2005).
Analysis of hybridization has progressed to Though blue and great tits seldom hybridize in
analysis of potential isolating mechanisms in stud- nature, the cross-fostering work is relevant to the
ies of several Eurasian Parids, with a major focus study of Parid hybridization in two ways. First, the
on songs (reviewed by Martens 1996). In south- experiments explore one possible mechanism for
western Germany, blue tits do not respond territor- the initial stage of interbreeding, that is interspe-
ially to normal songs of azure tits, but they cific egg-dumping or brood takeover, followed by
sometimes react to artificially constructed combin- sexual misimprinting and heterospecific mate
ations of azure tit notes; natural similarity in song choice (Slagsvold and Hansen 2001). Second,
appears sufficient to permit heterospecific pairing, results demonstrate that exposure to both adults
but no introgression, where both species occur at and nestmates of another species during an indi-
low density (Martens and Schottler 1991). viduals development can have diverse and long-
Breakdown of species recognition based on song lasting effects on its subsequent behavior and
also has been suggested to account for hybridiza- success. The work suggests that far-reaching
tion between great and Japanese tits in Siberia imprinting effectson vocalizations, dominance,
(Pckert et al. 2005). Intraspecific vocal variation mate preferences, and moreshould be expected
within willow tits is particularly complex (Martens as well during naturally occurring hybridization.
1996). Two song types, alpine and lowland,
have been identified based on note structure; in
7.6 Future directions
Central Europe, populations whose males sing only
one version are parapatrically separated. However, The largest gap in understanding of the behavioral
throughout much of the northern and eastern range ecology of hybridization in chickadees concerns
of the species, males of the Siberian song dominance interactions and their relationships with
category are bilingual with respect to the two both social pairing and extrapair mating. To date, no
song types, as they are in some local European thorough field study of dominance relationships
populations where interbreeding occurs between has been completed within any part of the contact
song types. Full understanding of this system zone. Bronson et al. (2003b) found a tendency for
awaits research focused on the responses of Carolina-like males in aviaries to dominate black-
both males (as rivals) and females (as mates) to capped males, and for females of both species to
individual males as a function of their song type(s). associate with the dominant Carolina males.
In contrast, there is less differentiation in song than However, proximate mechanisms involved in nei-
in morphology in coal and black-crested tits ther establishment of dominance nor mate choice by
(Martens 1996). females were examined as part of the study.
Mechanisms that facilitate heterospecific pairing Furthermore, because the work was carried out
can be explored by cross-fostering, an approach using captive birds, its relevance for understanding
that has been pursued recently using blue and great patterns and mechanisms in the field is not yet
tits. Cross-fostered blue and great tit males both known (Curry 2005).
react territorially to heterospecific intruders The preceding discussion underscores a paucity
(Hansen and Slagsvold 2003). Blue tits reared by of available information about relationships
great tits have normal pairing success but occasion- between vocal patterns, dominance relationships,
ally pair with great tits; in contrast, great tits raised and mating systems in Parid hybrid zones. Because
by blue tits suffer low pairing success, and the few chickadees give calls at all times of the year,
that pair do so with blue tits (Slagsvold et al. 2002). those vocalizations potentially play a greater
108 R E P R O D U C T I V E E C O L O G Y, E V O L U T I O N, A N D B E H AV I O R

role in establishing or maintaining dominance References


relationships than do the songs that chickadees
Baker, M. C., Baker, M. S. A., and Gammon, D. E. (2003).
give only in the spring, after pairs have already
Vocal ontogeny of nestling and fledgling black-capped
become established. However, several other
chickadees Poecile atricapilla in natural populations.
factors besides vocal behavior could be important Bioacoustics, 13, 265296.
determinants of dominance, and thus of any Banks, R. C., Cicero, C., Dunn, J. L., Kratter, A. W.,
mating patterns dependent on dominance; these Rasmussen, P. C., Remsen, J. V., Jr, Rising, J. D., and
include, but are not limited to, age and plumage Stotz, D. F. (2002). Forty-third supplement to the
brightness (Chapter 9). American Ornithologists Union Check-list of North
An important question is whether song invari- American Birds. Auk, 119, 897906.
ably trumps other cues as the predominant etho- Braun, M. J. and Robbins, M. B. (1986). Extensive protein
logical isolating mechanism in the Paridae, as has similarity of the hybridizing chickadees Parus atricapil-
lus and P. carolinensis. Auk, 103, 667675.
been accepted by European investigators (Martens
Bronson, C. L., Grubb, T. C., Jr, and Braun, M. J. (2003a). A
1996). That two distantly related North American
test of the endogenous and exogenous selection
chickadees with different songs can continue to
hypothesis for the maintenance of a narrow avian
hybridize challenges this viewunless the ability hybrid zone. Evolution, 57, 630637.
to learn the others song, perhaps because both Bronson, C. L., Grubb, T. C., Jr, Sattler, G. D., and Braun, M. J.
retain ancestral song characteristics, facilitates (2003b). Mate preference: a possible causal mechanism for
interbreeding through sexual misimprinting. The a moving hybrid zone. Animal Behaviour, 65, 489500.
importance of vocalizations relative to other influ- Bronson, C. L., Grubb, T. C., Jr, Sattler, G. D., and Braun,
ences, including calls and social dominance, on M. J. (2005). Reproductive success across the black-
mating decisions in hybridizing Parids can only be capped (Poecile atricapillus) and Carolina chickadee
assessed through further research that addresses (P. carolinensis) hybrid zone in Ohio: implications for
characterization of hybrid zones based on genetic
these relationships directly, as we are attempting to
indices. Auk, 122, 759772.
do in our ongoing studies of chickadees.
Charrier, I. and Sturdy, C. B. (2005). Call-based species
recognition in black-capped chickadees. Behavioral
Acknowledgments Processes, 70, 271281.
Cicero, C. (1996). Sibling species of titmice in the Parus
We thank The Nature Conservancy, Pennsylvania inornatus complex. University of California Publications in
Bureau of State Parks, and Hawk Mountain Zoology, 128, 1217.
Sanctuary for permission to work on their proper- Cicero, C. (2000). Oak titmouse (Baeolophus inornatus) and
ties. We are indebted to S. L. Guers for field work juniper titmouse (Baeolophus ridgwayi). In: Poole, A. and
that yielded many HM blood samples and to Gill, F., eds. The Birds of North America, No. 485, pp.
S. G. Mech for critical help with molecular data 128. The Birds of North America, Inc., Philadelphia.
Cicero, C. (2004). Barriers to sympatry between avian
analyses; S. P. Mullen and K. L. Cornell also
sibling species (Paridae: Baeolophus) in local secondary
contributed significantly through their M.S. thesis
contact. Evolution, 58, 15731587.
projects. For additional assistance, we thank:
Cockrum, E. L. (1952). A check-list and bibliography of
N. A. Lucchi, A. Nicodemus, L. Massarelli, hybrid birds in North America north of Mexico. Wilson
A. Ruscica, S. Slattery, S. Van Pelt, C. Yuan, M. N. Bulletin, 64, 140159.
Weber, K. Whittemore, C. Zecchine, and R. Zitnay. Crock, M. A. (1975). Competition and Chickadee Vocalizations:
Villanova University provided primary funding; Displacement and Convergence. M.S. thesis, University of
MWR also received student research grants from the Pittsburgh.
American Ornithologists Union, the Frank Curry, R. L. (2005). Hybridization in chickadees: much to
M. Chapman Memorial Fund, Sigma Xi, and the learn from familiar birds. Auk, 122, 747758.
Animal Behavior Society. We thank Theresa Burg, Ficken, M. S., McLaren, M. A., and Hailman, J. P. (1996).
Boreal Chickadee. In: Poole, A. and Gill, F. B., eds. The
Tom Grubb, and Ken Otter for comments on the
Birds of North America, No. 254, pp. 123. The Academy
manuscript. Hawk Mountain Sanctuary contribu-
of Natural Sciences, Philadelphia.
tion to conservation science number 137.
B E H AV I O R A L A S P E C T S O F H Y B R I D I Z AT I O N 109

Gill, F. B., Slikas, B., and Sheldon, F. H. (2005). Phylogeny Kroodsma, D. E., Byers, B. E., Halkin, S. L., Hill, C.,
of titmice (Paridae): II. Species relationships based on Minis, D., Bolsinger, J. R., Dawson, J.-A., Donelan, E.,
sequences of the mitochondrial cytochrome b gene. Auk, Farrington, J., Gill, F. B., Houlihan, P., Innes, D., Keller,
122, 121143. G., Macaulay, L., Marantz, C. A., Ortiz, J., Stoddard, P.
Grant, P. R. and Grant, B. R. (1997). Hybridization, sexual K., and Wilda, K. (1999). Geographic variation in black-
imprinting, and mate choice. American Naturalist, 149, capped chickadee songs and singing behavior. Auk, 116,
128. 387402.
Grubb, T. C., Jr, and Bronson, C. L. (1995). Artificial snags Kvist, L., Ruokonen, M., Orell, M., and Lumme, J. (1996).
as nesting sites for chickadees. Condor, 97, 10671070. Evolutionary patterns and phylogeny of tits and chicka-
Hailman, J. P. and Ficken, M. S. (1996). Comparative dees (genus Parus) based on the sequence of the
analysis of vocal repertoires, with reference to chicka- mitochondrial cytochrome b gene. Ornis Fennica, 73,
dees. In: Kroodsma, D. E. and Miller, E. H., eds. Ecology 145156.
and Evolution of Acoustic Communication in Birds, pp. Lhrl, H. (1994). Contribution to the still unsettled ques-
136159. Comstock/Cornell University Press, Ithaca, tion complex of hybridization between coal tit and
New York, USA. black-crested tit (Parus ater, P. melanolophus) (Aves:
Hailman, J. P. and Haftorn, S. (1995). Siberian tit. In: Poole, Passeriformes: Paridae). Zoologische Abhandlungen
A. and Gill, F., eds. The Birds of North America, No. 196, (Dresden), 48, 149157.
pp. 124. The Academy of Natural Sciences, Mammen, D. L. and Nowicki, S. (1981). Individual differ-
Philadelphia. ences and within-flock convergence in chickadee calls.
Hansen, B. T. and Slagsvold, T. (2003). Rival imprinting: Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 9, 179186.
interspecifically cross-fostered tits defend their territor- Martens, J. (1996). Vocalizations and speciation of palearc-
ies against heterospecific intruders. Animal Behaviour, tic birds. In: Kroodsma, D. E. and Miller, E. H., eds.
65, 11171123. Ecology and Evolution of Acoustic Communication in Birds,
Hansen, B. T. and Slagsvold, T. (2004). Early learning pp. 221240. Comstock/Cornell University Press,
affects social dominance: interspecifically cross-fostered Ithaca, New York, USA.
tits become subdominant. Behavioral Ecology, 15, Martens, J. and Schottler, B. (1991). Acoustic barriers
262268. between azure tit Parus cyanus and blue tit Parus
Hansen, M. M., Ruzzante, D. E., Nielsen, E. E., and caeruleus? Journal fr Ornithologie, 132, 6180.
Mensberg, K.-L. E. (2000). Microsatellite and mitochon- McCarthy, E. M. (2006). Handbook of avian hybrids of the
drial DNA polymorphism reveals life-history depend- world. Oxford University Press, Oxford.
ent interbreeding between hatchery and wild brown McCallum (1998). Mountain chickadee. In: Poole, A. and
trout (Salmo trutta L.). Molecular Ecology, 9, 583594. Gill, F. B., eds. Birds of North America, No. 254, pp. 128.
Harrap, S. and Quinn, D. (1995). Chickadees, Tits, Academy of Natural Sciences, Philadelphia.
Nuthatches and Treecreepers. Princeton University Press, Mostrom, A. M., Curry, R. L., and Lohr, B. (2002). Carolina
Princeton, New Jersey. chickadee. In: Poole, A. and Gill, F., eds. The Birds of
Hill, B. G. and Lein, M. R. (1989). Territory overlap and North America, No. 636, pp. 128. The Birds of North
habitat use of sympatric chickadees. Auk, 106, 259268. America, Inc., Philadelphia.
Irwin, D. E. and Price, T. (1999). Sexual imprinting, learn- Nocedal, J. and Ficken, M. (1998). Bridled titmouse. In: Poole,
ing and speciation. Heredity, 82, 347352. A. and Gill, F., eds. The Birds of North America, No. 375, pp.
Jrvinen, A. (1997). Interspecific hybridization between the 120. The Birds of North America, Inc., Philadelphia.
Siberian tit Parus cinctus and the willow tit Parus montanus Nowicki, S. (1989). Vocal plasticity in captive black-
produces fertile offspring. Ornis Fennica, 74, 149152. capped chickadees: the acoustic basis and rate of call
Johannessen, L. E., Slagsvold, T., Hansen, B. T., and convergence. Animal Behaviour, 37, 6473.
Lifjeld, J. T. (2005). Manipulation of male quality in wild Nowicki, S. and Searcy, W. A. (2005). Song and mate
tits: effects on paternity loss. Behavioral Ecology, 16, choice in birds: how the development of behavior helps
747754. us understand function. Auk, 122, 114.
Johnston, D. W. (1971). Ecological aspects of hybridizing Pckert, M., Martens, J., Eck, S., Nazarenko, A. A.,
chickadees (Parus) in Virginia. American Midland Valchuk, O. P., Petri, B., and Veith, M. (2005). The great
Naturalist, 85, 124134. tit (Parus major)a misclassified ring species. Biological
Kroodsma, D. E., Albano, D. J., Houlihan, P. W., and Wells, Journal of the Linnean Society, 86, 153174.
J. A. (1995). Song development by black-capped Pritchard, J. K., Stephens, M., and Donnelly, P. (2000).
chickadees (Parus atricapillus) and Carolina chickadees Inference of population structure using multilocus
(P. carolinensis). Auk, 112, 2943. genotype data. Genetics, 155, 945959.
110 R E P R O D U C T I V E E C O L O G Y, E V O L U T I O N, A N D B E H AV I O R

Randler, C. (2002). Avian hybridization, mixed pairing Sawaya, P. L. (1990). A Detailed Analysis of the Genetic
and female choice. Animal Behaviour, 63, 103119. Interaction at a Hybrid Zone Between the Chickadees Parus
Reudink, M. W., Mech, S. G., and Curry, R. L. (2006). atricapillus and P. carolinensis as Revealed by Nuclear and
Extra-pair paternity and mate choice in a chickadee Mitochondrial DNA Restriction Fragment Length Variation.
hybrid zone. Behavioral Ecology, 17, 5662. Ph.D. dissertation, Department of Biological Sciences,
Reudink, M. W., Mech, S. G., Mullen, S. P. and Curry, R. L. University of Cincinnati.
(2007). Structure and dynamics of the hybrid zone Slagsvold, T. and Hansen, B. T. (2001). Sexual imprinting
between Black-capped and Carolina chickadees in and the origin of obligate brood parasitism in birds.
southeastern Pennsylvania. Auk, 124, in press. American Naturalist, 158, 534367.
Robbins, M. B., Braun, M. J., and Tobey, E. A. (1986). Slagsvold, T., Hansen, B. T., Johannessen, L. E., and
Morphological and vocal variation across a contact Lifjeld, J. T. (2002). Mate choice and imprinting in birds
zone between the chickadees Parus atricapillus and P. studied by cross-fostering in the wild. Proceedings of the
carolinensis. Auk, 103, 655666. Royal Society B, 269, 14491455.
Rossano, L. M. (2003). Vocal Patterns of Black-Capped and Smith, S. M. (1993). Black-capped chickadee. In: Poole, A.,
Carolina Chickadees Hybridization in Southeastern Stettenheim, P., and Gill, F., eds. The Birds of North
Pennsylvania. M.S. thesis, Department of Biology, America, No. 39, pp. 118. The Academy of Natural
Villanova University. Sciences, Philadelphia.
Salzburger, W., Martens, J., and Sturmbauer, C. (2002). Smith, S. T. (1972). Communication and other social
Paraphyly of the blue tit (Parus caeruleus) suggested behavior in Parus carolinensis. Publications of the Nuttall
from cytochrome b sequences. Molecular Phylogenetics Ornithological Club, 11, 1125.
and Evolution, 24, 1925. ten Cate, C. and Vos, D. R. (1999). Sexual imprinting and
Sattler, E. D. (1996). The Dynamics of Vocal, Morphological evolutionary processes in birds: a reassessment.
and Molecular Interaction Between Hybridizing Black- Advances in the Study of Behavior, 28, 131.
Capped and Carolina Chickadees (Parus atricapillus, Parus Ward, R. (1966). Regional variation in the song of the
carolinensis). Ph.D. dissertation, Department of Zoology, Carolina chickadee. Living Bird, 5, 127150.
University of Maryland. Ward, R. and Ward, D. A. (1974). Songs in contiguous
Sattler, G. D. and Braun, M. J. (2000). Morphometric populations of black-capped and Carolina chickadees
variation as an indicator of genetic interactions in Pennsylvania. Wilson Bulletin, 86, 344356.
between black-capped and Carolina chickadees at a Weisman, R. and Ratcliffe, L. (1989). Absolute and relative
contact zone in the Appalachian Mountains. Auk, 117, pitch processing in black-capped chickadees, Parus atri-
427444. capillus. Animal Behaviour, 38, 685692.
CHAPTER 8

Life in the small-bodied cavity-nester


guild: Demography of sympatric
mountain and black-capped
chickadees within nest web
communities under changing
habitat conditions
Kathy Martin and Andrea R. Norris

8.1 Introduction In a cluster analysis of nest-tree size and cavity


characteristics for a cavity-nester community of
8.1.1 Parids as members of the nest web
over 40 species of vertebrates in interior British
Parids live in cavity-nesting vertebrate communi- Columbia, mountain chickadees, black-capped
ties that are structured by hierarchical guilds called chickadees, downy woodpeckers (Picoides pubes-
nest webs, whereby some species depend on others cens), and red-breasted nuthatches (Sitta canadensis)
to produce cavities (a critical resource) for breeding formed one of five clusters, namely the smallest-
or roosting, and interspecific and intraspecific bodied cavity-nesters (Martin et al. 2004). This
interactions are centered around nest-site availabil- group used the smallest diameter of trees, with the
ity (Martin and Eadie 1999). Eurasian and North most advanced decay and the smallest cavity
American Parids contain both excavating and non- entrance holes for nesting (Martin et al. 2004). With
excavating species, with some exhibiting consider- overlap in both foraging habits and nest-site
able intraspecific variation in mode of cavity selection among the small-bodied cavity-nesters,
acquisition (Monkkonen and Orell 1997). There are there is considerable scope for facilitation, competi-
reproductive costs and benefits associated with tion, and consequent niche partitioning patterns of
nesting in newly excavated cavities: an energy resources. These strong inter- and intraspecific
expenditure such as excavation at the beginning of ecological dependencies may vary with habitat
the breeding season may limit nest initiation date features such as forest type, stage, and condition,
and clutch size, however high competition for and temporal variation in resource availability.
previously excavated cavities may be avoided by The purpose of this chapter is to discuss the role
excavation, and parasite loads are typically lower and ecology of two chickadee species as members
in newly excavated cavities (Moller 1993; of the small-bodied cavity-nester guild and their
Monkkonen and Orell 1997; Wiebe et al. 2006). general relationships in the nest web community.
These trade-offs may be weighted differently in We examine the ecological and environmental
closely-related, competing species that occur factors that might explain demographic changes by
sympatrically, leading to niche partitioning of asking the general question of how changes in
excavators and non-excavators. community dynamics and habitat characteristics

111
112 R E P R O D U C T I V E E C O L O G Y, E V O L U T I O N, A N D B E H AV I O R

influence population density and nest-site selection cavity-nesters to obtain suitable holes in dead or
of the Parids. Within the nest web community, decaying aspen trees (Aitken et al. 2002).
positive interspecific interactions may result from
diverse species assemblages, where cavity excava- 8.1.1.2 Black-capped chickadees
tors may enhance or facilitate the breeding condi- Black-capped chickadees, about the same size and
tions for secondary cavity-nesters. In interior mass as mountain chickadees (~1011 g mass), are
British Columbia, population densities of chicka- excavating Parids that inhabit a range of deciduous
dees and other small cavity-nesters (competitors, and mixed forest habitats across North America
facilitators) and predators varied annually across a (Smith 1993). In central BC, black-capped chickadee
range of habitat stands, but most showed substan- detections and nesting densities also increased
tial increases in breeding density over the past across the study but they always remained at about
decade. Since these population increases were one-third or lower abundance than mountain
correlated with changes in habitat condition, we chickadees (Figure 8.1). We found between four
address how habitat use and nest-site selection and ten nests annually (Figure 8.1b). Black-capped
change with density. We approached this problem chickadees completely excavated about 77% of the
at the level of the individual (nest-site selection) 26 cavities located and another 15% of nests were in
and at the stand level (chickadee population natural holes (branch limb holes) with occasional
growth rates) in relation to nesting densities of use of a cavity excavated by a downy woodpecker
other members in the cavity-nester community and or sapsucker (Table 8.1). Here, as in other regions,
changing habitat characteristics. black-capped chickadees did not reuse any existing
chickadee-excavated cavities (Smith 1993; Aitken
8.1.1.1 Mountain chickadees et al. 2002). Since black-capped chickadee densities
Mountain chickadees are small-bodied, secondary- are low in the area and their cavities tend not to be
cavity-nesting birds, common in montane forests in reused, their excavation activities are not expected
western North America (McCallum et al. 1999). to influence mountain chickadee populations.
Typically, they use coniferous trees for foraging and However, as black-capped chickadees tend to be
aspen trees for nesting (Hill and Lein 1989; Aitken dominant when the two species coexist, we
et al. 2002). In a long-term study on mountain expected their presence would alter settlement
chickadee populations in Northern California, nest patterns in mountain chickadees (Minock 1972; Hill
box occupancy was highest in open pine stands and Lein 1988, 1989).
(Dahlsten et al. 1992). In the Cariboo-Chilcotin
region of central British Columbia, mountain 8.1.1.3 Possible competitors of chickadees
chickadees are abundant, year-round residents The red-breasted nuthatch, with similar size and
that typically occupy stands of mixed coniferous- weight to the chickadees (~1011 g mass), is a good
deciduous forest, especially where Douglas-fir excavator, similar to black-capped chickadees, and
(Pseudotsuga menziesii), lodgepole pine (Pinus has preferences for coniferous habitat similar to
contortus), and quaking aspen (Populus tremuloides) mountain chickadees (Ghalambor and Martin
are present (K. Martin, unpublished data). They are 1999). Unlike black-capped chickadees, however,
the most abundant breeding Parid, with detections red-breasted nuthatches use old cavities approxi-
ranging from 0.05 to 0.24 birds/ha over a 10-year mately 51% of the time (9 years; n 173 nests
period and a doubling in detection rates in the where cavity freshness was known; K. Martin,
past 3 years (Figure 8.1a). The increased rate of unpublished data), directly competing with moun-
detections was accompanied by a tripling in the tain chickadees for cavity resources. The red-
number of nests located since 2001 (Figure 8.1b). breasted nuthatch distribution extends across the
Since mountain chickadees are abundant, have boreal forest of Canada and south to the south-west
strongly increased in density, yet rarely excavate, and Appalachian Mountains, with a preference for
they are strong consumers of small cavities fir and spruce forests (Ghalambor and Martin
and must compete with other small-bodied 1999). In our study, nuthatch detection rates
S M A L L - B O D I E D C AV I T Y- N E S T E R G U I L D S 113

Parids
Mountain chickadee
(a) Black-capped chickadee
0.45 Competitors/Predators
Red-breasted nuthatch
0.4 Red squirrel
Average individuals per ha

0.35 Facilitators
Downy woodpecker
0.3 Red-naped sapsucker
0.25

0.2
0.15
0.1
0.05
0
1995 1996 1997 1998 1999 2000 2001 2002 2003 2004

(b)
60
Black-capped chickadee
Downy woodpecker
50
Red-naped sapsucker
Red-breasted nuthatch
40 Mountain chickadee
Number of nests

30

20

10

0
1995 1996 1997 1998 1999 2000 2001 2002 2003 2004

Figure 8.1 (a) Relative densities of chickadees, other small cavity-nesters, and red squirrels in Interior British Columbia across 26 forested sites
as measured by point count detections. (b) Numbers of nests of small cavity-nesting birds found on the same sites. Field data collected from May
through July. Fixed 50 m radius point counts completed from 05000930 hours for 6 min using observation and playback methods to measure
detections. We located nests by systematic nest searches across sites and recorded nest and tree characteristics, and, when possible, determined
which species made the cavity. Because cavity-nesters reused cavities and nest-trees in multiple years (Aitken et al. 2002), we checked existing
cavities and searched for new cavities (trees marked with numbered tags to facilitate relocation). Occupied cavities (contained at least one
offspring) were located by visual inspection of cavities or by looking or listening for excavation, behavior of adult birds, nestling begging, or
other activities.

doubled, similar to mountain chickadees, and (Figure 8.1). Nuthatches and both chickadee species
nesting densities increased from fewer than 10 coexisted on all study sitesdetected via point-
nests annually early in the study to 40 in 2004 count observationsand all three species nested on
114
R E P R O D U C T I V E E C O L O G Y, E V O L U T I O N, A N D B E H AV I O R
Table 8.1 The small cavity-nester sub-nest web: nest flow for small cavity-nesters and the species that excavated their cavity

Nesting species Excavator species Knot-hole Total nests

Mountain Black- Red- Downy Red- Hairy American Northern Pileated


chickadee capped breasted woodpecker naped woodpecker three-toed flicker woodpecker
chickadee nuthatch sapsucker woodpecker

Mountain chickadee 0.01 0.01 0.23 0.17 0.40 0.04 0.02 0.02 0 0.11 163
Black-capped chickadee 0 0.77 0 0.04 0.04 0 0 0 0 0.15 26
Red-breasted nuthatch 0 0 0.77 0.08 0.05 0 0.04 0.01 0 0.05 167
Downy woodpecker 0 0 0 1.00 0 0 0 0 0 0 39
Red-naped sapsucker 0 0 0 0 0.99 0 0 0 0 0 270
Red squirrel 0 0 0.03 0 0.40 0.20 0.02 0.20 0.03 0.12 60
Total cavities 2 22 169 82 376 21 13 289 6 52 562
S M A L L - B O D I E D C AV I T Y- N E S T E R G U I L D S 115

88% of our sites (although not always in the are also consumers of small and medium-sized
same year; K. Martin, unpublished data). Since cavities. Density and activity of red squirrels, in
nuthatches are common in the area and excavate particular, may strongly depress Parid demog-
new cavities regularly, we expected nuthatch raphy because they are abundant and can alter
population increases to facilitate nesting densities resource availability (use suitable cavities) and
and population growth rates of mountain chicka- quality (enlarge small cavities), and prey on Parid
dees in years following nuthatch increases, but to nests (Mahon and Martin 2006). Red squirrel detec-
negatively impact nesting densities where they tions varied strongly over the study from about
overlap spatially and temporally, due to competi- 0.32 detections/ha early in the study, to half that
tion for foraging and cavity resources. level from 1999 to 2001, followed by increases to
comparable detections of mountain chickadees and
nuthatches from 2002 to 2004 (Figure 8.1a).
8.1.2 Facilitators of small-bodied
However, red squirrels are generalist predators and
cavity-nesters
able to switch from bird eggs and adults to conifer
Several other species are important cavity excava- cones and fungi, thus an increase in squirrel
tors that facilitate breeding in mountain chickadees. abundance does not always influence Parid
Downy woodpeckers, a member of the small- demography (Mahon and Martin 2006).
bodied cavity-nesting guild, are exclusively
excavators (Jackson and Ouellet 2002). Downy
8.1.4 Interspecific interactions among
woodpeckers were always at low density with
small-bodied cavity-nesters
abundance varying from being basically absent at
the beginning of our study and increasing to about Competition between mountain and black-capped
0.04 detections/ha in the last 2 years (Figure 8.1a). chickadees may occur because of genetic, behav-
We found only three nests in the first 4 years, but in ioral, and habitat similarities. Phylogenetic analysis
the latter years we monitored three to nine nests of North American chickadees using mitochondrial
annually (Figure 8.1b). Red-naped sapsuckers, DNA placed mountain chickadees closest to black-
(Sphyrapicus ruber) a member of the medium-sized capped chickadees in divergence distance indicat-
cavity-nester guild, are important cavity producers ing that among the Parids, these two species are the
for mountain chickadees (Hill and Lein 1988; most genetically similar (Gill et al. 1993, 2005; see
Martin et al. 2004). Sapsucker numbers varied phylogeny in Chapter 2, Figure 2.1). Occasional
dramatically over the study, and their numbers hybridization between the two species has been
may relate negatively to densities of European star- reported in sympatric populations in Colorado and
lings (Sturnus vulgaris) that also varied annually New Mexico (Howe 1985; Martin and Martin 1996).
(Koenig 2003; Martin et al. 2003). If cavities last for Both species have complex social organizations
multiple seasons, an increase in sapsuckers or within populations, with established dominants
downy woodpeckers may increase nest site avail- and subordinates, usually males over females and
ability for mountain chickadees for several subse- older over younger individuals (Minock 1972;
quent years. Thus, we expect that the densities of Desrochers 1989; Smith 1993; McCallum et al. 1999).
downy woodpeckers and sapsuckers in the previ- We suggest that similarities between these two
ous year will influence population growth rates Parids contribute to a significant niche overlap
and nesting densities of mountain chickadees. where ranges are sympatric.
Black-capped chickadees are the only Parid to
broadly overlap in range with the mountain
8.1.3 Predators of small-bodied cavity-nesters
chickadee in western North America. In sympatric
Cavity-nesting mammals such as red squirrel populations in Alberta, there was segregation in
(Tamiasciurus hudsonicus), northern flying squirrel both nesting and foraging habitats, with mountain
(Glaucomys sabrinus), and bushy-tailed woodrat chickadees foraging less often on branches and
(Neotoma cinerea) are predators of Parid nests and more on trunks and twigs than black-capped
116 R E P R O D U C T I V E E C O L O G Y, E V O L U T I O N, A N D B E H AV I O R

chickadees (Hill and Lein 1988). Mountain chicka- habitat specialist black-capped chickadee may
dees occupied only one of 14 nest cavities drive mountain chickadees into habitats containing
excavated by black-capped chickadees in Alberta a higher coniferous component. In addition to
(Hill and Lein 1988) and we found segregation of providing rich food sources, including bark beetles
nest use in our study (Table 8.1). Segregation of and budworm, coniferous habitats may contain
sympatric mountain and black-capped chickadees more nest sites for the mountain chickadee because
may occur as a result of interspecific behavioral these habitats support a rich assemblage of
dominance. In a behavioral study of sympatric excavating species, including woodpeckers and
populations in Utah, black-capped chickadees were nuthatches. Although the red-breasted nuthatch
frequently dominant over mountain chickadees may be a significant competitor to the mountain
(Minock 1972). During playback experiments in our chickadee in these habitats, they are less ecologic-
study, black-capped chickadees exhibited higher ally similar than the black-capped chickadee (e.g.
aggression towards simulations of mountain nuthatches are typically bark gleaners whereas
chickadee intruders in defending nesting cavities chickadees are typically foliage gleaners) so
than mountain chickadees exhibited towards coexistence in mixed coniferous forest areas might
simulated black-capped chickadee intruders (A.R. be easier. In terms of foraging habitat and nest-site
Norris, unpublished data). Thus, while mountain selection, we expect the most extensive overlap
chickadees are the most abundant Parid, black- between red-breasted nuthatch and mountain
capped chickadees are the more dominant species. chickadee populations; we examine here whether
The mountain/black-capped chickadee relation- this relationship is primarily one of facilitation
ship is analogous to two European Parids: the blue (through creation of cavities) or competition. When
tit and the great tit. The blue tit is smaller than the food availability is high and nest-site competition is
great tit, and therefore can utilize a wider range of low, we expect nuthatches to have a facilitating
cavity sizes, however the great tit is generally dom- effect on mountain chickadees, but when food is
inant to the blue tit and excludes it from nesting limited we expect nuthatches to exclude mountain
habitat (Dhondt 1977). Where ranges overlapped, chickadees. We expect downy woodpeckers and
great tits excluded blue tits from optimal territories red-naped sapsuckers to be primarily facilitators in
during the winter months by competitive dom- their relationship with mountain chickadees,
inance but, during the breeding season, great tit depending on the extent of simultaneous overlap of
clutch size was negatively correlated with blue resource use.
tit density, indicating that blue tits could limit great
tit reproductive success if they could secure a terri-
8.2 Study area and habitat change
tory. A possible explanation for why blue tits were
able to establish territories in the presence of great Our study area, in the Cariboo-Chilcotin region of
tits was that a greater number of cavities were central interior British Columbia, Canada (51 52N,
available to blue tits due to their smaller body size; 122 21W), is comprised of mixed coniferous
these smaller cavities were not suitable for great tits and deciduous forest embedded in a matrix of
(Dhondt 1977; Dhondt and Eyckerman 1980). grassland and shallow ponds within the warm and
Similarly, mountain chickadees, like blue tits, may dry Interior Douglas-fir Biogeoclimatic Zone
be subordinate to black-capped chickadees, and (Meidinger and Pojar 1991). Predominant tree
excluded from black-capped chickadee habitat; species are trembling aspen, Douglas fir
however, mountain chickadees appear better able (Pseudotsuga menziesii), lodgepole pine (Pinus con-
to capitalize on available resources because they torta), and white and hybrid spruce (Picea glauca x
use a broad range of cavities excavated by other engelmannii). Our sampling sites (7 to 32 ha in size)
species, whereas black-capped chickadees rarely varied in character from continuous forest to two
use cavities excavated by other species (Hill and sites that were a series of forest islands (0.2 to
Lein 1988; Aitken et al. 2002; Table 8.1). In terms of 5 ha) within the grassland matrix. Most sites were
foraging habitat, competition with the broadleaf mature forest (80200 years old), nine of which
S M A L L - B O D I E D C AV I T Y- N E S T E R G U I L D S 117

were cut selectively for pine and/or spruce in (Otvos 1979). Small cavity-nester species may
19972002. Additional study area details are given be able to capitalize on enhanced food supply
in Aitken et al. (2002) and Martin et al. (2003). differently.
Recently, the interior forests of British Columbia
have undergone significant changes in condition
8.3 Data analysis
due to outbreaks of mountain pine beetle
(Dendroctonus ponderosae). The Ministry of Forests The same sites were surveyed for cavity-nester
estimated over a 42-fold increase in area affected by detections and nests to obtain accurate estimates of
mountain pine beetle in BC from 1999 to 2004 breeding and nesting densities per site. We counted
(164 567 ha to 7 million ha; BC MoF 2004). Our every individual bird or squirrel detected during a
vegetation surveys also revealed significant point count period as an independent data point.
increases in bark beetle attack on conifers, espe- Data from all point count stations surveyed were
cially lodgepole pine (Figure 8.2). The main factors averaged across each site and year. We also counted
contributing to the outbreak are assumed to be individual nesting attempts as independent data
increased availability of mature pine and drought and calculated the mean number of nests per
conditions causing increased susceptibility of trees hectare surveyed. We compared mean nest
to attack, and a series of mild winters resulting in cavity measurements pre- (19952000) and post-
low over-winter larval mortality (Carroll et al. 2004; (20012004) beetle outbreak years using a series of
Taylor and Carroll 2004). Bark beetle larvae and independent samples t-tests. Homoscedasticity
other forest insects associated with bark beetles was tested using Levenes test for homogeneity
presumably provide substantial increases in food of variance and where tests failed to meet the
availability for insectivorous, small cavity-nesters assumptions, we assumed unequal variances. We

1.00

0.90 1995
1996
Proportion ofconifer trees surveyed

0.80 1997 0.80


Proportion of lodgepole pine
trees with boring insects

1998 0.70
0.70 0.60
1999
0.60 2000 0.50
2001 0.40
0.50 2002 0.30
2003 0.20
0.40 2004 0.10

0.30 0.00
1995 1996 1997 1998 1999 2000 2001 2002 2003 2004

0.20

0.10

0.00
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8

Decay class

Figure 8.2 Changes in the health of conifers on our sites across time as measured by decay class, and the proportion of lodgepole pine
recorded as having at least one beetle attack during the study. We used a tree decay classification system that ranged from 18, with
1 indicating a live and healthy tree, 2 indicating a live tree with visible signs of decay, and 38 indicating dead trees with advancing stages
of decay as per icons (Thomas et al. 1979).
118 R E P R O D U C T I V E E C O L O G Y, E V O L U T I O N, A N D B E H AV I O R

performed all analyses using SAS (SAS Institute (23%), downy woodpecker (17%), and natural holes
Inc. 1998) and SPSS for Windows standard version (11%; Table 8.1). Despite the presence of both
11.5.0 (SPSS Inc. 2002). All statistical tests were chickadee species nesting on 21 of the 26 sites
two-tailed and significance levels of 0.05 between 1995 and 2004, only two of 163 nesting
were used. Continuous variables were tested for attempts by mountain chickadees were in holes
normality using a one-sample Kolmogorov excavated by black-capped chickadees, a lower or
Smirnov test at the 95% confidence level, and equivalent frequency to their use of much larger
residuals of all response variables were plotted holes excavated by hairy woodpecker (Picoides
and visually examined for all regression analyses villosus), three-toed woodpecker (Picoides dorsalis),
(Zar 1999). and northern flicker (Colaptes auratus). Thus despite
similar size, ecology, and coexistence on the same
sites, mountain and black-capped chickadees
8.3.1 Population growth models
exhibited strong segregation in nesting habitats in
We calculated mountain chickadee population these mixed forests. Mountain chickadees may be
growth rates (r) by taking the natural log of the unwilling to use cavities used by black-capped
average number of individuals per study site at chickadees because black-capped chickadees
time (year), t1 divided by the average number of excavate cavities in very decayed trees that are not
detections per site at time t (adding a correction useable (tree down, cavity collapsed, or entrance
factor of 0.5 to each population count because grown in) in a subsequent year, especially if they
census data typically contain zeroes, and the have access to small holes in more sturdy trees. In
addition of a small constant to every estimate of 2004, 84.7% of 137 red-naped sapsucker cavities
population density is needed to avoid division and 64.1% of 103 nuthatch cavities were useable
by zeroes in calculating growth rates-Framstad compared to 56.7% of 30 black-capped chickadee
et al. 1997). We used the natural log of the growth cavities (K. Martin, unpublished data). Thus
rate estimates to account for any spatial variance the non-use of black-capped chickadee cavities by
and spatio-temporal covariances in the density mountain chickadees may be due to a combination
estimates, which reduces the risk of overesti- of low density and lower reusability of black-
mating growth rates (spatial variance) and the capped chickadee cavities compared to the abun-
masking of positive spatio-temporal covariance dant sapsucker and nuthatch cavities.
in population distributions (Steen and Haydon Both black-capped chickadees and red-breasted
2000). nuthatches excavated the majority of their cavities
(both 77%) and each used 4 to 5% of sapsucker-
[rt ln (Nt1 0.5 / Nt 0.5)]
excavated cavities (Table 8.1). Fifteen percent of
Estimates of population growth with respect to black-capped chickadee nests, but only 5% of
mountain chickadee abundance, allow us to exam- nuthatch cavities, were in natural holes (Table 8.1).
ine the effects of the other explanatory variables on The small-bodied cavity-nesting guild followed
density dependence. We used Akaikes Information the same pattern as the overall cavity-nester
Criterion (AIC) as the basis for model selection community in terms of selecting unhealthy aspen
techniques (Burnham and Anderson 2002). for nesting trees (Martin et al. 2004). About 96% of
nest trees chosen by small cavity-nesters were aspen
(Table 8.2). With strong increases in mountain
8.3.2 Excavation history, nest tree, and cavity
chickadees and nuthatches, we expected to find
characteristics
changes in nest tree and cavity characteristics later
In terms of the flow of nest-cavity resources among in the study. In the last 4 years of the study, moun-
the small cavity-nesters, mountain chickadees tain chickadees used nesting trees with significantly
depended primarily on cavities excavated by red- larger diameter at breast height (DBH), and cavities
naped sapsucker (40% of 163 cavities where cavity over 1 m higher in the tree, with shallower horizon-
origin was determined), red-breasted nuthatch tal depth and smaller entrance areas than in the first
Table 8.2 Species and decay class of nest trees used by small-bodied cavity-nesters in interior British Columbia, (a) before beetle (19952000) and (b) after beetle (20012004)

Cavity-nesting species Trembling aspen Douglas-fir Lodgepole pine Hybrid spruce All tree species

DBHa DECAYb nc DBH DECAY n DBH DECAY n DBH DECAY n DBH DECAY n
meanSD median meanSD median meanSD median meanSD median meanSD median
(range) (range) (range) (range) (range)

(a)
Mountain chickadee 23.5 5.8 3 (16) 100 44.8 4.5 (45) 2 19.9 1 1 23.7 6.1 103
Black-capped chickadee 21.3 8.5 3 (17) 24 21.3 8.5 24
Red-breasted nuthatchd 22.9 6.5 3 (110) 76 89.2 40.9 4 (35) 3 19.3 2.7 2 27.2 2.5 2 25.4 15.4 83
Downy woodpecker 23.8 4.4 3 (25) 17 23.8 4.4 17
(b)
Mountain chickadee 26.1 8.0 3 (110) 231 51.3 17.6 4 (34) 4 26.5 8.8 3 (110) 235
Black-capped chickadee 18.7 6.8 3 (25) 29 18.7 6.8 3 (25) 29

S M A L L - B O D I E D C AV I T Y- N E S T E R G U I L D S
Red-breasted nuthatchd 24.8 19.9 3 (15) 157 55.3 9.6 4 (35) 4 30.0 9.9 3 (34) 7 35.3 9.0 4 (34) 3 26.3 20.0 173
Downy woodpecker 25.8 6.1 3 (24) 25 25.8 6.1 3 (24) 25

Updated and reorganized from Martin et al. 2004.


a
DBH: Diameter at breast height (cm).
b
Decay classa total of two nests were in fallen aspen trees (decay class 10)
c
n: Number of occupied nests.
d
Two red-breasted nuthatch nests were in Cottonwood trees.

119
120 R E P R O D U C T I V E E C O L O G Y, E V O L U T I O N, A N D B E H AV I O R

6 years of the study (Tables 8.2 and 8.3). Cavity 8.3.3 Site level changes in mountain
height on the tree also increased by about 1 m after chickadee populations using population
the beetle outbreak for both red-breasted nuthatch growth models
and downy woodpecker (P 0.05, Table 8.3). In con-
8.3.3.1 Population size
trast to all other small-bodied cavity-nesters, black-
Over our decade-long study, we observed strong
capped chickadees used cavities significantly lower
increases in mountain chickadee numbers, with
on the trees (Table 8.3). Thus, mountain chickadees
similar increases in their small-bodied competitors
showed the largest change in nest-site characteris-
and red squirrels (potential predators) and also a
tics, correlated with increases in population density
strong reduction in conifer health, especially lodge-
and bark beetle prevalence. The significance of the
pole pine (Figure 8.2). Population growth models
large increase in cavity height is not immediately
were used to determine the critical drivers in chicka-
apparent. None of the changes in nest-cavity use
dee demography; because the relative abundance
were related to tree growth because most nest trees
of chickadees, competing, and facilitating species,
used by mountain chickadees were mature and
and red squirrels occur at similar spatial scales,
dead or dying with no significant tree growth dur-
changes in relative abundance for all species can be
ing the study (K. Martin, unpublished data).
detected from standardized point counts. Growth
Mountain chickadees did not abandon lower nest
rate was used instead of population size so that
sites, but rather there was an increase in the range of
we could examine effects of other explanatory
nest site heights and other characteristics compared
variables on density dependence. Using model
to earlier in the study.

Table 8.3 Cavity nest-site characteristics for small-bodied cavity nesting birds in interior British Columbia, (a) before beetle (19952000) and
(b) after beetle (20012004)

Cavity-nesting species Height above Vertical Horizontal Entrance Number of


ground (m) depth (cm) depth (cm) area (cm2) cavities in tree

mean SE n mean SE n mean SE n mean SE n mean SE n

(a)
Mountain chickadee 4.4 0.2 103 14.2 1.2 57 9.9 0.5 57 13.5 0.9 57 2.3 0.2 101
Black-capped chickadee 4.6 0.6 24 15.6 1.7 7 6.6 0.8 9 14.6 2.1 9 1.3 0.2 24
Red-breasted nuthatch 4.9 0.3 83 10.8 0.8 22 7.4 0.5 30 9.3 0.8 30 1.9 0.2 82
Downy woodpecker 5.2 0.5 17 17.1 1.9 7 9.1 0.5 7 9.4 1.0 7 2.5 0.5 17
(b)
Mountain chickadee 5.5 0.2 235 12.7 0.4 112 8.7 0.3 118 11.3 0.6 124 2.2 0.1 235
Black-capped chickadee 3.2 0.4 29 14.1 0.9 20 7.1 0.4 20 12.0 1.0 20 1.2 0.1 29
Red-breasted nuthatch 5.8 0.3 174 11.6 0.4 70 7.5 0.3 76 9.9 0.7 78 1.8 0.1 173
Downy woodpecker 7.0 0.6 25 14.9 1.6 7 10.1 1.8 8 9.1 0.9 8 2.7 0.4 25

Updated and reorganized from Martin et al. 2004.


We established vegetation plots and measured trees and cavities in 11.2 m radius plots centered at nest-tree and point count stations 100 m apart
along transect lines throughout each site. Since most sites covered an area that included one or several small cavity-nester territories, habitat
characteristics averaged over all plots on a site represent availability of nesting resources (sampling effort ~1 plot/ha). After nest cavities were
vacated, we recorded for all trees 12.5 cm DBH (British Columbia Ministry of Forests inventory standard) tree species, size (DBH), decay class, and
the number of cavities (used or unused) present. Tree condition was assessed using British Columbia Ministry of Forests guidelines (Finck et al. 1989)
and included the presence of fungal conks, bark beetle sign, and broken top. Vertical cavity depth was measured from the bottom of the cavity
entrance to the bottom of the cavity. Internal cavity diameter was measured from the inner edge of the lower lip of the entrance to the back wall
of the cavity. Entrance hole area (cm2) was calculated using hole height and width and the formula for the area of an ellipse. Cavity volume (cm3)
was calculated using vertical and horizontal depth and the formula for the volume of a cylinder.
S M A L L - B O D I E D C AV I T Y- N E S T E R G U I L D S 121

Table 8.4 General linear models predicting mountain chickadee population growth rate in central British Columbia from 1995 to 2004

Model e2 K n AICC AICC AIC wt

M Bi Bc Rb D Rn Sq St E M*Sq 109.2 12 182 192.6 0.0 0.52


M Bi Bc Rb D Rn Sq St E M*Bi 108.9 12 182 192.0 0.6 0.39
M Bi Bc Rb D Rn Sq St E M*Bc 105.8 12 182 185.7 6.9 0.02
M Bi Bc Rb D Rn Sq St E 104.6 11 182 185.6 7.0 0.02
M Bi Bc Rb D Rn Sq St E M*Rb 105.7 12 182 185.6 7.0 0.02
M Bi Bc Rb D Rn Sq St E M*Bi M*Bc
M*Rb M*D M*Rn M*Sq M*St M*E 114.0 19 182 185.3 7.3 0.01
M Bi Bc Rb D Rn Sq St E M*Rn 105.4 12 182 184.9 7.7 0.01
M Bi Bc Rb D Rn Sq St E M*St 105.0 12 182 184.1 8.5 0.01
M Bi Bc Rb D Rn Sq St E M*D 104.8 12 182 183.8 8.8 0.01
M Bi Bc Rb D Rn Sq St E M*E 104.7 12 182 183.6 9.0 0.01

Variables examined were mountain chickadee (M), mountain pine beetle (Bi), black-capped chickadee (Bc), red-breasted nuthatch (Rb),
downy woodpecker (D), red-naped sapsucker (Rn), red squirrel (Sq) abundance, stem density (St), and percentage edge (E). For each model,
the log-likelihood ratio (e2), the number of parameters (K), the number of observations (n), the second-order Akaikes Information Criterion
(AICC), the relative quasi-likelihood Akaike Information Criterion (AICC), and its weight (AIC wt) are given.

selection techniques, the top two models had about population growth rate, as predicted (Figure 8.3b).
the same level of support, and all models predicting Thus, mountain chickadee population growth rates
site-level mountain chickadee population growth were enhanced by an abundance of forest insects
rate included nine variables (detections of and reduced by high squirrel abundancea high
mountain chickadees, black-capped chickadees, abundance of squirrels, however, does not
red-breasted nuthatches, downy woodpeckers, necessarily mean high depredation of chickadees,
red-naped sapsuckers, red squirrels, amount of as squirrels are generalist foragers and can switch
beetle attack, stem density, and edge) and two to plant foods when these are abundant (Mahon
included interaction terms, the strongest being an and Martin 2006).
interaction term of mountain chickadees with
squirrel abundance (Table 8.4; Figure 8.3). 8.3.3.2 Nest density
Averaging across all models in Table 8.4, parameter Overall nesting densities of mountain chickadees
estimates indicated the most important factors were positively correlated with: relative abundance
predicting mountain chickadee growth rates were of mountain chickadees (point count detections
mountain chickadee, red squirrel, and mountain for that site and year); proportion of bark-
pine beetle abundance and stem density in the beetle-attacked lodgepole pine; red-breasted
previous year (Table 8.5). The negative density nuthatch nest density (both same year and 1-year
dependent effects on mountain chickadee lag); and relative abundance of downy woodpeck-
population growth were ameliorated with high ers (1-year lag). Detections of red-naped sapsuckers
beetle abundance (Figure 8.3a). There was also a (1-year lag), red-breasted nuthatches, and stem
negative density dependent response to squirrel density were negatively correlated with mountain
abundance, but the strength of these effects varied chickadee nesting density (Figure 8.4, Table 8.6).
with chickadee abundance. When abundance of The positive effect of red-breasted nuthatch nest
mountain chickadees was low (below 0.15 individ- density and the negative effect of nuthatch detec-
uals/ha), the model predicted higher chickadee tions is puzzling since nuthatch nest density and
growth rates with high squirrel abundance, abundance were positively correlated. We suspect
whereas at higher mountain chickadee densities, that both nuthatch nest density and abundance
high squirrel abundance more strongly limited significantly interacted with food abundance, but
122 R E P R O D U C T I V E E C O L O G Y, E V O L U T I O N, A N D B E H AV I O R

further analyses are needed to determine the was greater when beetle-attacked pine was higher
strength of these interactions. Nonetheless, the (Figure 8.5a) and the negative effect of red-breasted
positive effect of red-breasted nuthatch nest density nuthatch abundance (see Figure 8.4f) was alleviated

(a) 0.4
Low Beetle
High Beetle
Predicted mountain chickadee
population growth rate 0.2

0.2

0.4

0.6
0 0.2 0.4 0.6
Mountain chickadee population density (Nt)

(b) 0.4
Low Squirrel
High Squirrel
Predicted mountain chickadee

0.2
population growth rate

0.2

0.4

0.6
0 0.2 0.4 0.6
Mountain chickadee population density (Nt)

Figure 8.3 Population growth rate of mountain chickadees at time, t1, with mountain chickadee abundance at time t, predicted from the
model, y 0.30 (1.12)*M (0.21)*Bi (0.14)*Bc (0.12)*Rb (0.06)*Rn (0.26)*Sq (0.0003)*St(0.11)*E(2.44)*M*Bi
(2.98)*M*Bc (0.84)*M*Rb (0.35)*M*D (0.73)*M*Rn (1.58)*M*Sq (0.0004)*M*St (0.19)*M*E. Significant
interactions were between mountain chickadee abundance and (a) beetle abundance, represented from the 25% quartile (low beetle) and 75%
quartile (high beetle); (b) squirrel abundance, represented from the 25% quartile (low squirrel) and 75% quartile (high squirrel). Variables
included in the candidate models as main effects were average number of mountain chickadee (M), black-capped chickadee (Bc), red-breasted
nuthatch (Rb), downy woodpecker (D), red-naped sapsucker (Rn), and red squirrel detections (Sq), at time (t), proportion of lodgepole pine trees
with mountain pine beetle attack (Bi), stem density (St, the average number of trees/ha), and percent edge (E, the percent of vegetation plots
within 50 m of an edge). Models chosen for analysis included all main effects and all two-way interactions of mountain chickadee abundance
with the remaining eight predictor variables. We chose only interaction terms containing mountain chickadee abundance for analyses because we
were interested in the effects of other variables on density dependence. We excluded population growth estimates from the year immediately
after a site was harvested (stem density) to reduce possible influence of disturbance from cutting activities during late winter and early spring
when mountain chickadees establish territories.
S M A L L - B O D I E D C AV I T Y- N E S T E R G U I L D S 123

Table 8.5 Model average parameter estimates for the explanatory variables of mountain chickadee population
growth rate

Parameter Parameter estimate Standard error Upper 95% Lower 95% t-value

St 0.0003 0.0001 0.0002 0.0005 4.5


Intercept 0.28 0.09 0.45 0.10 3.1
M*Bi 2.36 0.84 4.00 0.72 2.8
M 1.08 0.40 0.29 1.87 2.7
M*Sq 1.47 0.57 0.36 2.58 2.6
E 0.10 0.05 0.01 0.19 2.2
Sq 0.24 0.16 0.54 0.07 1.5
M*Bc 3.84 2.86 1.76 9.45 1.3
Rb 0.15 0.12 0.40 0.09 1.2
M*E 0.39 0.35 0.30 1.08 1.1
M*St 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 1.1
M*Rb 1.07 1.01 0.91 3.05 1.1
M*Rn 1.00 0.97 0.90 2.90 1.0
D 0.20 0.22 0.63 0.22 0.9
M*C 0.36 0.42 0.47 1.18 0.8
Bi 0.21 0.25 0.70 0.28 0.8
C 0.02 0.03 0.07 0.04 0.6
Bc 0.16 0.27 0.68 0.36 0.6
Rn 0.03 0.12 0.20 0.27 0.3
M*D 0.44 1.61 3.59 2.71 0.3

Mountain chickadee (M), mountain pine beetle (Bi), black-capped chickadee (Bc), red-breasted nuthatch (Rb),
downy woodpecker (D), red-naped sapsucker (Rn), red squirrel (Sq), abundance and stem density (St), and
percentage edge (E) and all two-way interaction terms. Parameters have a significant effect (P 0.05) on
mountain chickadee growth rates where t-values than absolute value of 1.98 (indicated in bold).

under high beetle attack, and slightly reversed because of a trade-off in resource allocation
under low beetle attack (Figure 8.5b). between excavation and egg formation
Nest density was significantly greater in years of (Monkkonen and Orell 1997; Wiebe et al. 2006). In
high beetle and budworm abundance (mean our system, this pattern appeared to hold for
nests/ha 0.11 0.01 SE, n 170 nests) than in Parids as mountain chickadees, dependent on
years of low beetle and budworm abundance other excavators, had larger clutches than the
(mean 0.03 0.01 SE, n 73; t 4.3; p 0.01). excavating black-capped chickadees. This pattern
In years of high forest insects, mountain chickadee did not, however, hold for other small-bodied
fecundity (mean 5.5 1.5 SE eggs or chicks, cavity nesters, as we observed larger increases
n 117 clutches) showed a non-significant in clutch size in the excavating nuthatches than in
tendency to increase compared to low insect years mountain chickadees. A study on crested tits
(mean 4.9 1.6 SE, n 16; t 1.53, p 0.13). in Belgium found that excavating males fledged
Red-breasted nuthatch fecundity (mean 5.7 1.4 nestlings earlier than non-excavating males and
SE eggs or chicks, n 79 nests) increased the probability of excavating increased with
significantly compared to low insect years male condition and nest initiation date (Lens et al.
(mean 4.3 1.4 SE eggs or chicks, n 18; t 3.7, 1994). Black-capped chickadees may not have
p 0.01). Excavators may be more constrained responded to the recent resource pulse because
than non-excavators in increasing their clutch size their tendency to excavate energetically precludes
124 R E P R O D U C T I V E E C O L O G Y, E V O L U T I O N, A N D B E H AV I O R

(a)
0.6

Mountain chickadee nest density/ha


0.5

0.4

0.3

0.2

0.1

0
0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6
Mountain chickadee individuals/ha

(b)
0.6
Mountain chickadee nest density/ha

0.5

0.4

0.3

0.2

0.1

0
0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7
Lodgepole pine with mountain pine beetle (stems/ha)

(c)
0.6
Mountain chickadee nest density/ha

0.5

0.4

0.3

0.2

0.1

0
0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5
Red-naped sapsucker detections from previous year (individuals/ha)

Figure 8.4 Continues


S M A L L - B O D I E D C AV I T Y- N E S T E R G U I L D S 125

(d)
0.6

Mountain chickadee nest density/ha


0.5

0.4

0.3

0.2

0.1

0
0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6
Red-breasted nuthatch nest density (previous year)

(e)
0.6
Mountain chickadee nest density/ha

0.5

0.4

0.3

0.2

0.1

0
0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6
Red-breasted nuthatch nest density (same year)

(f)
0.6
Mountain chickadee nest density/ha

0.5

0.4

0.3

0.2

0.1

0
0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6
Red-breasted nuthatch individuals/ha

Figure 8.4 Continues


126 R E P R O D U C T I V E E C O L O G Y, E V O L U T I O N, A N D B E H AV I O R

(g)
0.6

Mountain chickadee nest density/ha


0.5

0.4

0.3

0.2

0.1

0
0.00 0.10 0.20 0.30 0.40 0.50 0.60 0.70 0.80 0.90
Stem density (number of trees/ha)

(h)
0.6
Mountain chickadee nest density/ha

0.5

0.4

0.3

0.2

0.1

0
0 0.05 0.1 0.15 0.2 0.25 0.3 0.35
Downy woodpecker density from previous year (individuals / ha)

Figure 8.4 Parameters at the site level with the strongest influences on mountain chickadee nesting densities: (a) relative abundance of
mountain chickadee detections in point counts, (b) density of beetle-infected pine on the stands, (c) relative abundance of red-naped sapsuckers
from the previous year, (d) red-breasted nuthatch nesting densities in the previous year, (e) red-breasted nuthatch nesting densities in the same
year, (f) relative abundance of red-breasted nuthatches (same year), (g) stem density, and (h) relative abundance of downy woodpeckers in the
previous year. Four candidate general linear models were used to examine factors influencing mountain chickadee nest density: (1) Competitor
and predator model which included black-capped chickadee, red-breasted nuthatch and downy woodpecker abundance and nest densities and
red squirrel abundance at time, t; (2) Facilitator model which included red-breasted nuthatch, downy woodpecker, and red-naped sapsucker
abundance and nest densities from the previous year; (3) Habitat model that included mountain pine beetle abundance, stem density, and
proportion of edge habitat, and; (4) The main effects model which included all of the above explanatory variables. Since the strongest model
predicting mountain chickadee nest density was the main effects model (AIC wt 0.999; K 18; n 168), parameter estimation was
calculated from the main effects model and model averaging was not necessary.

them from increasing their clutch size. Presumably, the cost of excavation for any cavity-nesting species
the benefits of excavation are still higher for (Wiebe et al. 2006).
them than those accrued from larger clutches. Changes in forest habitat condition affect the
However, there are essentially no empirical data on reproductive behavior of many forest songbirds
S M A L L - B O D I E D C AV I T Y- N E S T E R G U I L D S 127

Table 8.6 Parameter estimates from the full mixed-effects model predicting mountain chickadee nest density per ha among
26 forest sites from 1995 to 2003

Parameter Parameter estimate Standard error Upper 95% Lower 95% t-value

M 0.27 0.06 0.38 0.15 4.43


Bi 0.16 0.06 0.27 0.04 2.63
Rn Lag 0.16 0.06 0.03 0.28 2.50
Rb Nest Lag 0.26 0.10 0.46 0.05 2.48
Rb Nest 0.22 0.09 0.40 0.04 2.35
Rb 0.15 0.07 0.01 0.29 2.17
St 0.08 0.04 0.01 0.16 2.14
D Lag 0.28 0.14 0.56 0.00 1.98
Rb Lag 0.12 0.07 0.02 0.27 1.68
Sq 0.07 0.04 0.15 0.01 1.61
Rn Nest Lag 0.10 0.10 0.29 0.09 1.05
E 0.03 0.03 0.08 0.03 0.95
D Nest 0.24 0.26 0.74 0.26 0.95
Intercept 0.03 0.04 0.10 0.05 0.66
D 0.05 0.12 0.28 0.18 0.41
Bc Nest 0.08 0.22 0.35 0.51 0.37
Bc 0.03 0.14 0.31 0.24 0.25
D Nest Lag 0.04 0.30 0.54 0.62 0.13

Fixed explanatory variables include: detection of mountain chickadee (M), red-breasted nuthatch (Rb), black-capped chickadee
(Bc), downy woodpecker (D), red squirrel (Sq), and mountain pine beetle (Bi) abundance and percent of edge (E) and stem
density (St) for each site. Time lags of 1 year were examined for red-naped sapsucker (Rn), downy woodpecker (D), and red-
breasted nuthatch (Rb) relative abundance (Species Lag: detections of individuals from the previous year) and nests (Nest Lag:
nesting densities from the previous year). Site was included as a random variable. Parameters have a significant effect (P 0.05)
on mountain chickadee growth rates where t-values than absolute value of 1.98 (indicated in bold).

when the changes affect resource availability (Fort puzzling that they did not capitalize on the resource
and Otter 2004). In our study, the number of nest pulse arising from the enhanced habitat conditions
trees and cavity density did not vary with habitat for insectivores.
change, but rather extrinsic factors related to Although we examined changes in local habitat
food supply, predation pressure and inter- and characteristics and conditions to explain our
intraspecific competition among three species observed changes in demography and population
occupying similar niches. During the breeding growth of mountain chickadees, these are simple
season, successful reproduction is determined by correlations that may not be causally linked. In
food abundance and nest-site availability. Some 2005, S. Coe (Univ. of California, personal commu-
small-bodied cavity-nesting birds may be able to nication) observed dramatic changes in annual
assess food availability prior to insect emergence fecundity of mountain chickadees in Sierra Nevada
(Crawford et al. 1990). Mountain chickadees showed similar to those observed in our study (A. R. Norris
the strongest ability to adapt to local differences in and K. Martin, unpublished data). Thus it is
habitat conditions, and nuthatches also showed possible that some of the temporal changes we
strong positive responses. Since black-capped observed in chickadee demography are driven by
chickadees are behaviorally dominant to mountain larger scale-environmental or ecological influences
chickadees and able to excavate cavities, it is (see Chapter 5).
128 R E P R O D U C T I V E E C O L O G Y, E V O L U T I O N, A N D B E H AV I O R

(a)
0.6
low bi (<15%)
high bi (>15%)

Mountain chickadee nest density/ha


0.5 Linear (low bi (<15%))
Linear (high bi (>15%))

0.4

0.3

0.2

0.1

0
0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6
Red-breasted nuthatch nest density
(b)
0.6
Mountain chickadee nest density/ha

0.5

0.4

0.3

0.2

0.1

0
0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6
Red-breasted nuthatch detections/ha

Figure 8.5 Correlations of mountain chickadee nest densities at low (15%) and high (15%) levels of boring insect abundance in pine trees
at the site level with (a) red-breasted nuthatch nest density, and (b) relative abundance of red-breasted nuthatch detections in point counts.
Straight lines were fit to raw data for each correlation at the two levels of boring insect abundance to demonstrate potential interacting effects
between boring insect abundance and nuthatches on mountain chickadee nest density.

Burnham, K. P. and Anderson, D. R. (1998). Model Selection


References
and Inference: a Practical Information-Theoretic Approach.
Aitken, K. E. H. and Martin, K. (2004). Nest site availabil- Springer-Verlag, New York, USA.
ity and selection in aspen-conifer groves in a grassland Carroll, A. J., Taylor, S. W., and Regniere, J. (2004). Effects
landscape. Canadian Journal of Forest Research, 34, of climate change on range expansion by the mountain
20992109. pine beetle in British Columbia. In: Shore, T. L., Brooks,
Aitken, K. E. H., Wiebe, K. L., and Martin, K. (2002). Nest- J. E., and Stone, J. E., eds. Mountain Pine Beetle
site reuse patterns for a cavity-nesting bird community Symposium: Challenges and Solutions, Kelowna, BC, Oct.
in interior British Columbia. Auk, 119, 391402. 3031, 2003, pp. 223232. Natural Resources Canada,
S M A L L - B O D I E D C AV I T Y- N E S T E R G U I L D S 129

Canadian Forest Service, Pacific Forestry Centre, Middle Rio Grande Valley of New Mexico. M.Sc. thesis,
Victoria, British Columbia, Information Report Univ. of New Mexico, Albuquerque.
BC-X-399. Jackson, J. A. and Ouellet, H. R. (2002). Downy wood-
Crawford, H. S., Jennings, D. T., and Stone, T. L. (1990). pecker (Picoides pubescens). In: Poole, A. and Gill, F., eds.
Red-breasted nuthatches detect early increases in The Birds of North America, no. 613. Academy of Natural
spruce budworm populations. Northern Journal of Sciences, Philadelphia, and American Ornithologists
Applied Forestry, 7, 8183. Union, Washington, D. C.
Dahlsten, D. L., Copper, W. A., Rowney, D. L., and Koenig, W. D. (2003). European starlings and their effect
Kleintjes, P. K. (1992). Population dynamics of the on native cavity-nesting birds. Conservation Biology, 17,
mountain chickadee in northern California. In: 11341140.
McCullough, D. R. and Barrett, R. H., eds. Wildlife 2001: Lens, L., Wauters, L. A., and Dhondt, A. A. (1994). Nest-
Populations, pp. 502510. Elsevier, London. building by crested tit (Parus cristatus) males: an ana-
Desrochers, A. (1989). Sex, dominance and microhabitat lysis of costs and benefits. Behavioural Ecology and
use in wintering black-capped chickadees: a field Sociobiology, 35, 431436.
experiment. Ecology, 70, 636645. Mahon, C. L. and Martin, K. (2006). Nest survival of
Dhondt, A. A. (1977). Interspecific competition between chickadees in managed forests: habitat, predator, and
great and blue tit. Nature, 268, 521523. year effects. Journal of Wildlife Management. 70, in press.
Dhondt, A. A. and Eyckerman, R. (1980). Competition Martin, K. and Eadie, J. 1999. Nest webs: a community-
between the great tit and the blue tit outside the breed- wide approach to the management and conservation of
ing season in field experiments. Ecology, 61, 12911296. cavity-nesting forest birds. Forest Ecology and
Finck, D. M., Humphreys, P., and Hawkins, G. V. (1989). Management, 115, 243257.
Field Guide to Pests of Managed Forests in British Columbia, Martin, K., Aitken, K., and Wiebe, K. L. (2004). Nest sites
Joint Publication No. 16. Canadian Forest Service/British and nest webs for cavity-nesting communities in inter-
Columbia Ministry of Forests, Victoria, BC, Canada. ior British Columbia, Canada: Nest characteristics and
Fort, K. and Otter, K. A. (2004). Effects of habitat disturb- niche partitioning. Condor, 106, 519.
ance on reproduction in black-capped Chickadees Martin, K., Mossop, M. D., and Aitken, K. E. H. (2003). Nest
(Poecile atricapillus) in Northern British Columbia. Auk, Webs: The Structure and Function of Cavity-Nesting and
12, 10701080. Songbird Communities in Interior British Columbia Forests,
Framstad, E., Stenseth, N. C., Bjornstad, O. N., and Falck, 111 pp. Environment Canada/Canadian Wildlife Service
W. (1997). Limit cycles in Norwegian lemmings: tension Technical Report No. 403, CW695/403E.
between phase-dependent and density dependence. Martin, S. G. and Martin, K. A. (1996). Hybridization
Proceedings of the Royal Society London Series B, 246, between a mountain chickadee and black-capped
3138. chickadee in Colorado. Colo. Field Ornithol. J., 30,
Ghalambor, C. K. and Martin, T. E. (1999). Red-breasted 6065.
nuthatch (Sitta canadensis). In: Poole, A. and Gill, F., eds. McCallum, D. A., Grundel, R., and Dahlsten, D. L. (1999).
The Birds of North America, no. 459. Academy of Natural Mountain Chickadee (Poecile gambeli). In: Poole, A. and
Sciences, Philadelphia, and American Ornithologists Gill, F., eds. The Birds of North America, no. 453.
Union, Washington, D.C. Academy of Natural Sciences, Philadelphia, and
Gill, F. B., Mostrom, A., and Mack, A. L. (1993). Speciation American Ornithologists Union, Washington, D.C.
in North American chickadees: Patterns of mtDNA Meidenger, D. and Pojar, J. (1991). Ecosystems of British
genetic divergence. Evolution, 47, 195212. Columbia. British Columbia Ministry of Forests Special
Gill, F. B., Slikas, B., and Sheldon, F. H. (2005). Phylogeny Report Series, no. 6. Victoria, British Columbia.
of titmice (Paridae): II. Species relationships based on Minock, M. E. (1972). Interspecific aggression between
sequences of the mitochondrial cytochrome-B gene. black-capped and mountain chickadees at winter feed-
Auk, 122, 121143. ing stations. Condor, 74, 454461.
Hill, B. G. and Lein, M. R. (1988). Ecological relations of Monkkonen, M. and Orell, M. (1997). Clutch size and cav-
sympatric black-capped and mountain chickadees in ity excavation in Parids (Paridae): The limited breeding
southwestern Alberta. Condor, 90, 875884. opportunities hypothesis tested. American Naturalist,
Hill, B. G. and Lein, M. R. (1989). Territory overlap and 149, 11641174.
habitat use of sympatric chickadees. Auk, 106, 259268. Otvos, I. S. (1979). The effects of insectivorous bird activ-
Howe, W. H. (1985). Hybridization in Black-capped (Parus ities in forest ecosystems: An evaluation. In: Dickson,
atricapillus) and Mountain (P. gambeli) Chickadees in the J. G., Connor, R. N., Fleet, R. R., Kroll, J. C., and Jackson,
130 R E P R O D U C T I V E E C O L O G Y, E V O L U T I O N, A N D B E H AV I O R

J. A., eds. The Role of Insectivorous Birds in Forest E., and Stone, J. E., eds. Mountain Pine Beetle Symposium:
Ecosystems. Academic Press, New York. Challenges and Solutions, October 3031, 2003, Kelowna,
SAS Statistical Institute (1998). SAS/STAT Users Guide, BC, pp. 4151. Natural Resources Canada, Canadian
version 8. SAS Institute, Cary, NC. Forest Service, Pacific Forestry Centre, Victoria, British
Smith, S. M. (1993). Black-capped chickadee (Parus atri- Columbia, Information Report BC-X-399.
capillus). In: Poole, A. and Gill, F., eds. The Birds of North Thomas, J. W., Anderson, R. G., Maser, C., and Bull, E. L.
America, no. 39. Academy of Natural Sciences, (1979). Snags. In: Thomas, J. W., ed. Wildlife Habitats in
Philadelphia, and American Ornithologists Union, Managed ForestsThe Blue Mountains of Oregon and
Washington, D. C. Washington. U S Department of Agriculture, Forest
SPSS Inc. (2002). SPSS, version 11.5.0. SPSS Inc., Chicago. Service Agriculture Handbook, no. 553.
Steen, H. and Haydon, D. (2000). Can population growth Wiebe, K. L., Koenig, W. D., and Martin, K. (2006).
rates vary with the spatial scale at which they are meas- Evolution of clutch size in cavity-excavating birds: the
ured? Journal of Animal Ecology, 69, 659671. nest site limitation hypothesis revisited. American
Taylor, S. W. and Carroll, A. L. (2004). Disturbance, forest Naturalist, 167, 343353.
age, and mountain pine beetle outbreak dynamics in Zar, J. H. (1999). Biostatistical Analysis. 4th edn. Prentice-
BC: A historical perspective. In: Shore, T. L., Brooks, J. Hall, Inc., Upper Saddle River, NJ.
CHAPTER 9

Social dominance and fitness in


black-capped chickadees
Laurene Ratcliffe, Daniel J. Mennill, and
Kristin A. Schubert

and Brodin 2003). Flock size, range, sex ratio,


9.1 Introduction
coherence, and stability of hierarchies vary within
Dominance hierarchies mediate social interactions and between species (Hogstad 1989; Matthysen
in many group-living animals (Piper 1997; 1990; Smith 1991; Verbeek et al. 1999; Hay et al.
Dugatkin and Earley 2004). Such hierarchies often 2004). Despite this variation there is abundant
emerge rapidly as the result of dyadic interactions evidence that social rank within flocks influences
between individuals over access to resources individual success, as a result of differential access
(Drews 1993; Beacham 2003). An individuals rela- to food (Glase 1973; Hogstad 1987b; Desrochers
tive social rank may be influenced by characteris- 1989; Ficken et al. 1990; Smith 1991) and breeding
tics such as personality (Dall et al. 2004), intrinsic partners (Smith 1988; Otter and Ratcliffe 1996;
resource-holding potential and experience (Hansen Mennill et al. 2004), as well as rank-related variation
and Slagsvold 2004), as well as extrinsic, context- in predator avoidance (Zanette and Ratcliffe 1994;
dependent features of competition such as group Koivula et al. 1996).
make-up (Wiley et al. 1999) and the value of Black-capped chickadees are undoubtedly the
resources (Dale and Slagsvold 1995). Although best-studied of the North American Parids, yet data
social rank, by definition, must always be a relative on long-term reproductive output are scarce (Smith
trait (Drews 1993), dominance hierarchies predom- 1991). Only one study (Schubert et al. in press) has
inate in animal groups with stable membership; evaluated the relationship between social rank and
thus rank relationships are typically long-lasting lifetime fitness. Chickadees are an ideal species for
(Wiley et al. 1999; Forkman and Haskell 2004). If such a study (apart, perhaps, from the disinclin-
acquiring and maintaining high status is costly ation of black-capped chickadees to use nest boxes).
(Hogstad 1987a; Hay et al. 2004), dominant birds They are broadly distributed across a range of
should derive sufficient benefits from improved temperate North American habitats. Like most
foraging, mating, or protection from predators to other Parids, including the willow tit (Lahti et al.
offset these costs (Ellis 1995). 1996), black-capped chickadees winter in dom-
Studies of the family Paridae (tits, titmice, and inance-structured flocks of three to 12 birds, in
chickadees) have contributed important informa- which males dominate females and mature birds
tion to our understanding of passerine life histories dominate yearlings (Glase 1973). Flocks typically
(McCleery and Perrins 1988; Dhondt 1989; Blondel consist of two or more adult birds and one or more
et al. 1990; Smith 1991). Many Parids spend the non- yearlings recruited from outside of the local popu-
breeding portion of their annual cycle in flocks with lation. Birds often breed with a member of their
linear dominance hierarchies and depend on food winter flock, and therefore chickadee flocks are
storage for winter survival (Ekman 1989; Lundborg sometimes described as a hierarchy of pairs

131
132 R E P R O D U C T I V E E C O L O G Y, E V O L U T I O N, A N D B E H AV I O R

(Smith 1991). Once recruited, chickadees are highly Smiths (1991) reported average of eight birds
philopatric, so the effects of winter social rank on in western Massachusetts. Flock sex ratios were
breeding success and status in succeeding winter- only slightly male-biased, averaging 1.2 0.1
ing flocks can be measured. males per female. Birds bred overwhelmingly
In this chapter we examine naturally- and sexu- in natural cavities (80% of nests; Mennill and
ally-selected consequences of social rank in a popu- Ratcliffe 2004a) rather than nestboxes. Field proto-
lation of black-capped chickadees studied for over a cols as described below were similar across
decade in eastern Ontario, Canada. We begin by years (see Schubert et al. in press for full details of
reviewing what is known about the characteristics methods and data analysis).
that distinguish individuals attaining high social
rank from subordinates. We describe aspects of
9.2.1 Winter field methods
status signaling, such as song and plumage features,
as well as rank-related differences in survival and From January to March, birds were captured in
annual reproductive success. Using our extensive baited Potter traps (average 146.8 12.5
observations of interactions among color-banded birds/winter) and individually marked with
individuals, we describe the process by which dom- aluminum Canadian Wildlife Service bands and
inance hierarchies are maintained. We then review one to three colored plastic bands. Standard
the effects of individual variation in rank acquisition morphological measures (mass, tarsus, wing,
on lifetime reproductive success (LRS). To date most rectrix length, fat score) and 10 to 80 l of blood
studies of avian life histories, including the excellent were collected from each bird for sexing and pater-
work on great tits (McCleery and Perrins 1988) and nity analysis. Birds were aged as second year (SY)
blue tits (Dhondt 1989), have investigated pheno- or after-second year (ASY) using standard proto-
typic correlates of fitness, but few have examined cols and banding records (Mennill et al. 2004). Flock
how the dynamics of social rank acquisition influ- memberships were obtained by tracking flock
ence LRS. Analysis of LRS permits us to compare movements across the site and scoring arrival times
how the overall genetic contribution of individuals at feeders. Dominance relations among flock mem-
is affected by both lifespan and reproductive strat- bers were already well-established by the start of
egies. Breeding lifespan has been identified as the our observations each January.
most important predictor of LRS in mammals and We assessed dominance hierarchies within flocks
birds (e.g. Clutton-Brock 1988; Newton 1989); never- by tabulating over 20 000 pairwise interactions
theless the interaction between social rank and LRS (average of 2439 712 per year) between banded
may also be significant and warrants investigation birds at feeding platforms which accommodated
(Verhulst and Salomons 2004). only one feeding individual at a time (Fig. 9.1). We
inferred that an individual was dominant over
an opponent when they: (i) supplanted or chased
9.2 General methods an opponent, (ii) resisted a supplanting attack by an
We have monitored black-capped chickadees at opponent, (iii) elicited a submissive posture in an
Queens University Biological Station (QUBS), opponent, or (iv) fed while an opponent waited to
Chaffeys Lock, Ontario, Canada (44 34N, 76 approach the feeder (Ficken et al. 1990; Otter et al.
19W) since 1987. The life history data reported 1998). These four types of interaction did not occur
here were collected from 1992 (initiation of breed- in equal proportion; the last two types of interac-
ing studies) through 2002. Data on winter dom- tion occurred very frequently, supplants and chases
inance and flock memberships (but not flock occurred less often, and resisting a supplanting
formation) were collected from 1994 though 2002. attack occurred very rarely. Hierarchies within
During this period the 2-km2 study site supported flocks were ordered using MatMan software
an average of 17.2 2.1 winter flocks and 60 8.2 (Noldus Information Technologysee review by
breeding pairs per year. Mean flock size was deVries 1998). Dominant (high-ranking) males were
5.5 0.1 birds (N 155 flocks), smaller than categorized as the male that held the top position in
SOCIAL DOMINANCE AND FITNESS 133

flocks with two or three males, and the top two


(a)
positions in flocks with four or five males; subordin-
ate (low-ranking) males held the bottom position or
bottom two positions, respectively. In flocks with
three or five birds, the middle bird was classified as
mid-ranking. Most comparisons used these
nominal categories, so sample sizes were based on
numbers of flocks. In some cases, to allow compari-
sons across flocks, a continuous rank score for each
bird (number of interactions won divided by total
interactions) was also calculated (Mennill et al.
2004). Because dominance interactions occur at a
lower rate and intensity between females than
males, and their functional significance is still
(b)
incompletely understood (Ramsay and Ratcliffe
2003; L. Ratcliffe, unpublished data), we have lim-
ited most analyses presented here to male rankings.

9.2.2 Spring field methods


Pairs were monitored from flock break-up (typically
late March to early April) to determine territory
boundaries and locate nest cavities. Breeding data
were collected from a total of 536 pairings involving
294 males and 282 females. At approximately day 6
post-hatch, we collected 10 to 50 l blood from the
tarsal vein of nestlings for paternity analysis
(c)
(Mennill et al. 2004). Fledglings typically dispersed
outside the boundaries of our study area, precluding
accurate measures of recruitment. Thus we meas-
ured male realized reproductive success (RRS) as the
total number of within- and extrapair nestlings sam-
pled at day 6; female success equaled the number of
offspring in the nest, since egg dumping was
extremely rare (Otter et al. 1998; Mennill et al. 2004).

9.3 Correlates of social rank


Table 9.1 summarizes our current knowledge about
Figure 9.1 Photographs of dyadic dominance interactions in a the morphological, behavioral, and reproductive
wintering flock of black-capped chickadees. (a) A dominant characteristics associated with social rank in back-
individual (left) flies in to supplant a subordinate individual. capped chickadees. Here we review some of these
(b) A dominant individual feeds while a subordinate
features originally described by Smith (1991), and
individual waits to approach the feeder. (c) A dominant
individual (left) elicits a submissive posture in his opponent describe new ones which have emerged from our
(turning his head away from the dominant). Photographs by recent work. These findings provide good evidence
D. J. Mennill. of important variation in features that signal male
quality and individuality. Undoubtedly future
work will reveal additional examples.
134 R E P R O D U C T I V E E C O L O G Y, E V O L U T I O N, A N D B E H AV I O R

Table 9.1 Correlates of social rank in black-capped chickadees

Feature Pattern Reference

Age Adults dominate juveniles Smith 1991, Otter et al. 1999, Schubert 2003
No age effect in females Ramsay and Ratcliffe 2003
Sex Males dominate females Smith 1991
Seniority Residents dominate intruders:
Males Smith 1991
Females Otter and Ratcliffe 1996, Ramsay and Ratcliffe
2003, S Ramsay and L Ratcliffe, unpublished
Early dispersing fledglings dominate later dispersers Smith 1991
No effect of fledgling seniority Leonard et al. 1991
Body condition Dominant males and females leaner Schubert et al. 2006, S Ramsay and L Ratcliffe,
unpublished
Foraging Dominants access more profitable, safer feeding sites Desrochers 1989, Ficken et al. 1990
Predator responses Dominants show less risky behavior (calls and movement) Zanette and Ratcliffe 1994
Plumage Dominant males have darker melanin (cap, mantle, bib), brighter Mennill et al. 2003a, Doucet et al. 2005,
white, and faster tail feather growth Woodcock et al. 2005
Song structure Dominants better at transposing songs across species frequency Christie et al. 2004
range
Singing behavior Dawn chorus of dominants begins earlier in morning, lasts Otter et al. 1997
longer, higher song rate
Response to interactive Subordinates respond more agitatedly to playback, closer Mennill and Ratcliffe 2004b
intruder approach to speaker than dominants
Response to simulated Dominants more likely to approach simulated winner Mennill and Ratcliffe 2004c
vocal duels
Territory Dominants out-compete subordinates for access Desrochers et al. 1988
Dominants have larger territories Smith 1991, Mennill et al. 2004
Mate attraction Dominant widowers attract new female sooner Otter and Ratcliffe 1996, Smith 1991
Dominant males more attractive to nave females Bronson et al. 2003, Woodcock et al. 2005
Divorce Females desert mates for higher ranked males Ramsay et al. 2000
Extrapair paternity Extrapair males higher-ranked than social males Smith 1988, Otter et al. 1994, 1998; Mennill
et al. 2003b, 2004
Clutch size Mates of dominant males more likely to lay eggs, have larger Otter et al. 1999, Doucet et al. 2005
clutches and higher % hatch
Nest success Mates of dominant males incubate longer than mates of Otter et al. 1999
subordinates, feed nestlings less often, have higher nest
survival and fledging success

flock-mates (1.6 0.1 years) (P 0.0001) (Schubert


9.3.1 Age
et al. 2006). Similarly, Otter et al. (1999) found that
Smith (1991) observed that male social rank correl- among 27 pairs of chickadees where intrasexual
ates with age class (ASY birds typically dominate SY ranks and ages of both members were known, male
birds), whereas a females rank may better reflect that age and rank were strongly correlated, but female
of her mate than her own age. Like Smith (1991), we rank was more strongly correlated with her mates
have found that young (SY) males can, in some age and rank than her own age. In aviary-staged
circumstances, dominate older birds (ASY; see dominance contests between females, age by itself
below), but overall high-ranked males were signifi- was also a poor predictor of winning (Ramsay and
cantly older (3.4 0.1 years) than their subordinate Ratcliffe 2003).
SOCIAL DOMINANCE AND FITNESS 135

9.3.2 Sex fledglings. However she also suggested that earli-


est-dispersing juveniles were probably the most
Males are dominant over females (Smith 1991).
successful at joining flocks by pairing with
Although Smith (1991) reported that females may
widowed adults, with a correlated rise in status.
sometimes be dominant over males, we found this
Glase (1973) showed that juveniles with greater
rarely occurred. Looking at a subset of our data,
skull pneumatization (a reliable age indicator) in
including 22 flocks from 2000 and 2001 where there
early fall were more likely to dominate later-arriv-
was a clear hierarchy among both the males and
ing birds. In marsh tits, larger and more aggressive
females, we found that in 21 of 22 flocks the lowest-
fledglings dispersed earlier than smaller fledglings
ranking male was dominant to the highest-ranking
regardless of sex (Nilsson and Smith 1985, 1988). In
female. Our data therefore match those of Glase
contrast, our studies of fledgling aggression and
(1973) who found that males are quite consistently
dispersal in eight chickadee family groups over the
dominant over females. Interestingly, most cases
22-day fledgling period showed aggressors were no
where we observed females dominating males
larger than their victims, nor did they disperse
occurred between birds who ended up as breeding
earlier (although sample sizes were smaller than
partners, where the male waited for his partner to
those in the marsh tit study; Leonard et al. 1991). We
finish feeding before approaching the feeder (inter-
have not documented flock formation in our popu-
action type iv, above).
lation, and thus the ontogeny of first-year rank
remains an important area for future research.
9.3.3 Seniority
9.3.4 Size and condition
Prior residency is an important correlate of social
dominance in tits during interactions at territory Because dominance is associated with preferential
boundaries and during flock formation. However access to food and decreased risk of predation, it
detailed studies of residency effects, especially in seems reasonable to predict that high-ranking birds
females, are rare. In captive great tits, residents carry fewer reserves than subordinates in winter
dominate intruders, but adultspresumably more when foraging conditions are challenging
experienceddominate juveniles when circum- (Cresswell 2003; see also Chapter 3). Modeling the
stances are unfamiliar to both (Sandall and Smith relation between dominance and mass (or condi-
1991). Smith (1991) found that chickadees removed tion) has turned out to be more complex than ori-
from winter flocks for up to 4 days generally ginally thought because of the need to consider
regained their former status. Our work shows that variation in predation risk and environmental
at the onset of the breeding season, females conditions (Lundborg and Brodin 2003; Ekman
removed for up to 96 hours from their high-ranked 2004), including flock stability (Lange and Leimar
mates easily evicted replacement mates (Otter and 2004). Neither Glase (1973) nor Smith (1991) found
Ratcliffe 1996; S. Ramsay and L. Ratcliffe, unpub- any compelling evidence that body size relates to
lished data). In aviary contests, female owners dominance effects in chickadees (but see Hogstad
out-ranked intruders (Ramsay and Ratcliffe 1987b); condition was not measured. However,
2003), as found in willow tits (Koivula et al. 1993). in paired comparisons of the highest- and
Thus although rank differences are more challeng- lowest-ranking males in flocks, Schubert et al.
ing to measure among females than males, females (2006) found dominant males had longer wings and
exhibit clear rank-related aggression outside the tarsi, lower fat scores, and were in leaner condition
context of the pair bond. than subordinates. Importantly, when age-matched
Work with captive tits shows early experience flock-mates were compared, the differences in wing
may affect offspring dominance status (Hansen and and tarsus disappeared but low-ranked birds were
Slagsvold 2004). Smith (1991) reported very low still heavier and had greater fat scores. Thus high-
levels of aggression within chickadee families, with ranked males are leaner but not larger when age is
no clear pattern of dominance hierarchies among taken into account. Similarly, in 15 field trials where
136 R E P R O D U C T I V E E C O L O G Y, E V O L U T I O N, A N D B E H AV I O R

females mated to dominant males were removed (a)


50
for 86 to 94 hours prior to onset of egg-laying, the WHITE
12 females that were able to oust their replacements 40
within a short time of being released and were, on

Reflectance
average, leaner than replacements ( S. Ramsay and 30
L. Ratcliffe, unpublished data, but note sample size
20 GREY
was insufficient to control for age effects). Overall,
our results agree with most other studies of Parids 10
showing dominance is not related to body size per BLACK
se (Hay et al. 2004; but see Braillet et al. 2002 for 0
300 400 500 600 700
population differences), whereas dominants tend to
Wavelength
carry lesser weight reserves than subordinates
(b) 0.6
(Pravosudov and Grubb 1997).

0.4
9.3.5 Plumage

Canonical variate
Visual signaling in chickadees has attracted little 0.2
attention compared to the better-characterized
great and blue tits (Lemel and Wallin 1993; Lange 0
and Leimar 2003; Alonso-Alvarez et al. 2004). The
achromatic (black, white, grey) plumage of black- 0.2
capped chickadees looks monomorphic to the
human eye, as is described for many Parids. Smith 0.4
(1991) surmised from her observations of domin-
ance interactions that chickadees must be able to 0.6
High-ranking Low-ranking
recognize the sex and relative social rank of others
males males
both quickly and accurately from considerable
distances. We confirmed her prescient suggestion Figure 9.2 (a) Average plumage reflectance spectra of the white
that there are substantial sex and rank differences cheek patch, the gray mantle, and black cap in black-capped
chickadees (based on spectrophotometric measurements of 40 males)
in chickadee plumage features across the visual
(from Doucet et al. 2005). (b) Association between male rank and
spectrum which are not detected by the human eye achromatic plumage color. High-ranking males had high canonical
(Fig. 9.2a). Reflectance spectrometry of six body variate scores, indicating darker black caps, grey mantles with
regions of chickadees captured in late winter flocks proportionately greater reflectance at intermediate wavelengths, and
has revealed that not only are males brighter white black bibs with proportionately less reflectance at intermediate
wavelengths (from Mennill et al. 2003a).
than females, they also have larger black bibs and
greater plumage contrast between adjacent white
and black body regions. In addition, plumage
variation is related to differences in social rank. results may reflect effects of manipulating feather
High-ranked males have darker black caps, and regrowth in the latter study. A subsequent study on
differences in the color of their black bibs and grey our QUBS population has shown that the
mantles relative to their subordinates, independent UV-chroma of these melanin-based features
of age (Fig. 9.2b). Measurements of growth bars in successfully predicts male realized reproductive
naturally-molted feather show dominant males success (within and extrapair offspring) (Table 9.2;
also grow their tail feathers significantly faster than Doucet et al. 2005). These data suggest that achro-
subordinates (Mennill et al. 2003a). This last finding matic plumage provides reliable cues about sex and
contrasts with that of Hay et al. (2004) who found quality. Female chickadees exposed to unfamiliar
that dominant coal tits grew induced tail feathers males in aviary choice tests can identify dominant
more slowly than subordinates. The different individuals rapidly without observing them
SOCIAL DOMINANCE AND FITNESS 137

Table 9.2 Significant plumage predictors of male reproductive success from multiple regression models constructed
using a backward stepwise procedure

R2 a df P

Apparent reproductive success


No significant predictors
Proportion of within-pair young
Whole model 0.65 2, 9 0.008
White brightness 0.38 1.01 1, 10 0.002
Melanin UV chroma 0.27 0.65 1, 10 0.03
Overall realized reproductive success
Melanin UV chroma 0.20 1, 38 0.01
a
Standardized partial regression coefficients.
Each model was constructed using four color variables (brightness of white patches, UV chroma of white patches,
brightness of melanin-colored black patches, ultraviolet chroma of black patches), dominance rank, and body condition
as potential predictor variables. Dependent variables (measures of male reproductive success) are shown in italic. Full
details of analysis are presented in Doucet et al. 2005. Only plumage color variables were significant predictors of two
measures of reproductive success.

interact, consistent with this hypothesis (Woodcock the territory edge), dominant territory holders are
et al. 2005). Additional studies are needed to deter- more likely to approach the simulated winner
mine how individual and rank-based differences in (more threatening rival) than are subordinate terri-
plumage mediate competitive interactions in flock tory holders (Mennill and Ratcliffe 2004c). When
hierarchies. Achromatic plumage color may well territory holders are themselves challenged with
provide badges of quality in aggressive, intrasex- aggressive interactions via playback, dominants do
ual encounters in chickadees, as does UV color in not engage with the intruder as intensely as
blue tits (Alonso-Alvarez et al. 2004). subordinates (Mennill and Ratcliffe 2004b).
Engaging in intense song contests may be particu-
larly costly for dominants, since they are unlikely to
9.3.6 Vocalizations
be cuckolded unless they lose in countersinging
The two-note fee-bee song of chickadees functions in interactions with territorial intruders (Mennill et al.
long-distance territorial communication, providing 2002). In a similar vein, dominants in winter flocks
both male and female audiences with a rich array of are risk averse, that is when exposed to predator
cues correlated with the quality of the singer (see models they take longer to call and break the freeze
Chapter 14). Males can frequency-match rivals by than subordinates (Zanette and Ratcliffe 1994).
transposing their single song type up and down a
continuous frequency range. Such pitch shifts
9.4 Hierarchy formation and
introduce fine structural changes in relative note
maintenance
frequency, amplitude, and duration. While the
tendency to shift songs is not related to social rank, Although the correlates of social rank have been
dominant males are superior singers, that is they well established through ongoing research, the
maintain relative note stereotypy in shifted songs process of hierarchy formation and the mechanics
significantly better than low-ranking males of hierarchy maintenance have received little atten-
(Christie et al. 2004). Dominants also begin the tion. Several hypotheses have been proposed to
dawn chorus earlier, sing for longer, and sing at explain how dominance hierarchies are formed and
higher rates (Otter et al. 1997). In mock counter- maintained (reviewed in Forkman and Haskell
singing duels (playback from two loudspeakers at 2004). The first fight hypothesis maintains that
138 R E P R O D U C T I V E E C O L O G Y, E V O L U T I O N, A N D B E H AV I O R

dominance relationships arise from an initial aggressive interactions appeared to be a temporary


encounter between two individuals. The continu- delay in access to food resources; in most observed
ous assessment hypothesis maintains that multiple, cases the supplanted individual returned to the
ongoing interactions and challenges are responsible feeder shortly after the aggressive supplant. This
for developing and maintaining dominance low cost of aggression stands in contrast to the
hierarchies. As an extension of the continuous outcome of fights we have observed at other times
assessment hypothesis, the suppression hypothesis of the year. In mid and late April, when flock
maintains that dominant individuals condition hierarchies have dissolved and birds have begun
their subordinates to lose through ongoing attacks. defending territories, we have observed many
Under the first fight hypothesis, the cost of an indi- aggressive encounters which have escalated to
vidual fight is expected to be high, and aggression extended physical contact, sometimes resulting in
is not necessarily expected to be greater between lost feathers and, in a few cases, prolonged head
pairs of animals who hold adjacent rank positions injuries (noticeable as persistent swollen skin
versus pairs with non-adjacent rank positions. around the eyes). During the breeding season, we
Under the continuous assessment hypothesis, the have observed such high-cost aggressive encoun-
cost of an individual fight is expected to be low and ters occur between two males and, less frequently,
aggression between pairs of animals should occur between two females. From the onset of our studies
more frequently when they hold adjacent rank at QUBS (K. A. Otter, personal communication) we
positions (i.e. when they have similar fighting have also observed a frequent form of aggression
abilities). Under the suppression hypothesis, the during the early breeding season in the form of
cost of an individual fight is expected to be low, and triangle fights, which occur as a low-ranking
dominant animals should instigate aggressive male persistently supplants and chases a high-
encounters with their subordinates whereas ranking female in the presence of her high-ranking
the reverse should occur rarely (Forkman and partner, who only occasionally steps in to harshly
Haskell 2004). supplant the low-ranking male. These triangle
To better understand chickadee dominance hier- fights rarely involve extreme aggression, but never-
archy formation and maintenance, we tested the theless appear to come at a high cost to the female,
predictions of these three hypotheses using a who can spend up to an hour practicing evasive
dataset of 6234 dominance interactions between 278 maneuvers. Relative to these forms of aggression
birds from 48 flocks, collected in January to March during the breeding season, aggression between
of 2000 and 2001 in our eastern Ontario study flock-mates during the non-breeding period
population. Dominance interactions may be appears to come at a low cost.
divided into interflock and intraflock interactions, Does aggression between pairs of individuals
and we focus on 3114 intraflock interactions to occur more frequently between individuals occu-
investigate the three hypotheses for the formation pying adjacent rank positions? To answer this
and maintenance of dominance hierarchies. Of question, we focused on aggressive malemale
these interactions, we observed 129 interactions interactions that occurred in flocks with three, four,
that involved especially aggressive supplanting or five males (in flocks with two males, all
attacks. These highly aggressive interactions are malemale interactions necessarily occur between
particularly helpful for evaluating the predictions birds with adjacent rank position). The highly
for these three hypotheses. aggressive interactions we observed did not occur
Is the cost of intraflock chickadee aggression high between males with adjacent rank positions more
or low? Our observations suggest that the cost of often than expected by chance. In flocks with three
aggressive encounters between flock-mates during males, 69% of highly aggressive interactions
the non-breeding season is low. For the 129 aggres- occurred between males with adjacent rank pos-
sive supplants that we observed, none appeared to itions (binomial test against expected proportion of
lead to an injury in the dominant or subordinate 66%: P 0.68, N 13); in flocks with four males,
bird. The primary cost associated with these 46% of highly aggressive interactions occurred
SOCIAL DOMINANCE AND FITNESS 139

between males with adjacent rank positions (bino- in both great tits and blue tits, dominance relation-
mial test against expected proportion of 50%: ships depend on the proximity to the previous
P 0.42, N 26); and in flocks with five males, years breeding territories (Colquhoun 1942; De
60% of highly aggressive interactions occurred Laet 1984). In contrast, winter dominance relation-
between males with adjacent rank positions (bino- ships within black-capped chickadee flocks are not
mial test against expected proportion of 40%: site-specific. This has been reported qualitatively
P 0.91, N 5). Therefore, our results do not by Hartzler (1970) for a Minnesota population,
support the second prediction of the continuous Smith (1976) for a Massachusetts population, and
assessment hypothesis. Desrochers and Hannon (1989) for an Alberta
Do dominant birds instigate aggressive encoun- population. Using dominance data from our east-
ters? Of the 129 highly aggressive supplanting ern Ontario population, we attempted to quantify
attacks that we observed in 2000 and 2001, we the consistency of chickadee dominance inter-
found that the overwhelming majority involved a actions across sites. Of 2257 dyadic, malemale
dominant individual attacking a subordinate indi- dominance interactions we observed in 2000 and
vidual. For 58 aggressive supplants between males, 2001, we found 27 dyads where the same two males
57 involved the dominant bird supplanting his interacted at two different feeder sites. The
subordinate flock-mate; for 11 aggressive supplants minimum distance between neighboring feeder
between females, 10 involved the dominant female sites at our Ontario study site is 175 m, and in all 27
supplanting her subordinate flock-mate; for 60 cases, the two winter feeders were in positions that
aggressive supplants between a male and a female, fell within two different birds breeding territories
58 involved the dominant bird supplanting their in both the preceding and subsequent breeding
subordinate flock-mate. (Of the latter 58 supplants, seasons. Of the 27 dyads, there were 22 where the
19 were cases where a male supplanted his own dominance relationship was consistent across the
breeding partner.) two sites (binomial test: P 0.0005). Our results
Taken together, these data provide support for therefore agree with previous qualitative assess-
the suppression hypothesis: the cost of an individ- ments in other populationsblack-capped chicka-
ual dominance fight appears to be low, and domin- dee dominance relationships are not site-specific.
ant individuals instigate aggressive encounters
with their subordinates. In contrast to the predic-
9.5 Variation in rank acquisition
tion of the continuous assessment hypothesis,
aggression does not occur between individuals Analysis of rank histories of known males from our
holding adjacent rank positions more often than long-term data set provides a revealing picture of
would be expected by chance. Further research is the factors associated with change in status
required to evaluate the cost of fights during initial (Schubert et al. 2006; K. A. Schubert, C. Kraus, D. J.
encounters when birds first form flocks in the fall, Mennill et al. unpublished data). As in willow tits
so that we may better understand the importance of (Lahti et al. 1996), rank is a dynamic trait. The
first fights in establishing position in chickadee majority of mid- and low-status birds increased in
dominance hierarchies. rank over consecutive years; birds reaching high
rank seldom regressed (Table 9.3; Schubert et al. in
press). Most birds advancing from low to high rank
9.4.1 Consistency in dominance
immediately dominated yearlings (22/29 cases),
relationships between sites
and some younger or older adult flock-mates
Some bird species exhibit dominance relationships (7 cases) (Schubert et al. 2006). Not surprisingly,
which are site-specific or context-specific, including males reaching high rank lived longer, on average
Carolina chickadees (Smith 1991) and mountain (3.4 0.1 years, N 98) than males who never
chickadees (Dixon 1965; interactions between non- moved beyond subordinate status (1.6 0.1 years,
flock-mates were site-specific). Many European N 99, P 0.0001) (Schubert et al. in press).
Parids show site-specific dominance relationships; Similarly, low-ranked adult males were more
140 R E P R O D U C T I V E E C O L O G Y, E V O L U T I O N, A N D B E H AV I O R

Table 9.3 Changes in male rank between two consecutive years rising from low to high rank (39%) (Schubert et al.
2006). These results show male chickadees advance
Original year Subsequent year
in rank predominantly by changing social group
Low Middle High rather than out-living their superior flock-mates.
Thus, changes in local population density probably
Low (n 58) 20 (34.5%) 8 (13.8%) 30 (51.7%)
have a major influence on whether low-ranking
Middle (n 17) 2 (11.8%) 3 (17.6%) 12 (70.6%)
birds can occupy a new home range and attract
High (n 76) 5 (6.6%) 4 (5.3%) 67 (88.2%)
juveniles as flock-mates. For example, several cases
The majority of low- and middle-ranked males increased in rank of low-ranking males rising in status by forming
between years. Few high-ranked males decreased in rank (from new flocks occurred as density declined sharply
Schubert et al. in press).
from 1997 through 1998 and then increased (K. A.
Schubert, C. Kraus, D. J. Mennill et al. unpublished
likely to increase in rank than low-ranked juveniles data; S. Ramsay, personal communication).
(K. A. Schubert, C. Kraus, D. J. Mennill et al.,
unpublished data). Rank advancement was not
9.6 Rank, survival, and lifetime
simply a function of survival, however. Of 65
reproductive success
males that eventually attained high rank, 30%
achieved dominance in their first year, 35% by the Previous shorter-term studies from our group and
age of 2, and 35% required 3 or more years others (Table 9.1) have documented fitness benefits
(Schubert et al. 2006). How first-year males achieve accrued by high-ranking chickadees in the form of
dominance is a fascinating question, and may be greater over-winter survival (Desrochers et al. 1988;
related to reduced population density in late sum- Smith 1991), better or larger territories (Desrochers
mer or early autumn (see below). Again, because et al. 1988; Mennill et al. 2004), enhanced success in
we have not studied the process of flock formation, social and extrapair mate choice (Smith 1988; Otter
our winter rank data do not provide many clues. et al. 1994, 1998; Otter and Ratcliffe 1996; Ramsay
Smith (1991) reported that first-year birds infre- et al. 2000; Bronson et al. 2003; Mennill et al. 2003b,
quently achieved high ranks by replacing dead 2004; Woodcock et al. 2005), and greater clutch size,
birds. This may explain how some juvenile mem- hatching, and fledging success (Otter et al. 1999). A
bers in our population attained high rank, since primary goal of our long-term analyses was to
flocks form in October but we assessed dominance partition the effects of rank from those of age, and
in February. to determine how year effects may influence this
While rank attainment in first-year birds remains relationship.
a mystery, our data show convincingly that changes
in status of adult birds are associated with changes
9.6.1 Survival
in group composition. Rank increases were often a
function of shifting flock membership while former Schubert (2003) found that adult chickadees at
superiors were still alive (16 out of 29 cases) rather QUBS survived about two winters on average, with
than replacement of dead superiors (6 cases) or 25% of males and 20% of females surviving to a
dominance reversal over a former superior (2 third winter, in general agreement with Smiths
cases); the other 5/29 birds stayed in the same flock (1991) value for average lifespan of 2.5 years. In
but became the second high-ranked male by dom- these analyses, where missing adults were assumed
inating new juveniles (Schubert et al. in press). to have died if they were not resighted on the study
Changes in flock membership were rare, in that site, males survived better than females (Cox
surviving flock-matesboth male and female regression: Wald 2 4.17, N 436 females, 427
frequently reassociated between years (Schubert males, P 0.04). This finding is in agreement with
et al. 2006). However, males that remained at low other studies of Parid survival (Desrochers et al.
rank tended to reflock with a greater proportion 1988; Lens and Dhondt 1993). Interestingly, there
of surviving former flock-mates (64%) than males was no difference in mean life expectancy of males
SOCIAL DOMINANCE AND FITNESS 141

that attained high rank in their first year compared contained an age effect ( K. A. Schubert, C. Kraus,
to those starting at low rank, nor was there a signifi- D. J. Mennill et al. unpublished data). Taken
cant difference in survival to second year for males together, these analyses reveal that rank, independ-
that achieved high versus low rank as yearlings. ent of age, has an influence on annual survival in
The latter result contrasts with Koivula et al. (1996) male chickadees, even in the face of environmental
who found social rank predicted survival of and other variation captured by modeling year
juvenile but not adult willow tits. effects.
To examine the effects of age and sex on male and
female annual survivorship, Schubert (2003) first
9.6.2 Reproductive success
used nested, generalized linear models (GLZ) to
analyze January recapture data from 1994 to 2002. High-ranking males are more attractive social part-
Analyses were performed in MlWiN using a ners to female flock-mates (Schubert 2003), consis-
binomial error structure (Goldstein 1995), again tent with results from our short-term studies and
assuming that birds that disappeared had died. Our experiments on femalefemale competition
models revealed no significant effects of age (as (Table 9.1). Analysis of 328 pairings from 1994 to
measured in years), but significant effects of age 2002 showed that over 70% of high-ranked males
class (ASY versus SY), and sex, as well as high but fewer than 40% of low-ranked males paired
annual variability. Models of male survivorship with adult females. An overwhelming majority
which added variables of rank class (e.g. high, mid, (91%) of high-ranked males paired with female
low) or rank score (interactions won divided by all flock-mates, whereas only 72% of low-ranked
interactions) showed rank to be a marginally better males did (P 0.01); male age was not associated
predictor of male survival than age (P 0.08). To with likelihood of pairing within flock (P 0.6;
better untangle the effects of age and rank (here Schubert 2003). Among pairs of known rank where
categorized as high or low only), which are tightly both survived to breed again, high-ranking males
correlated, a more targeted analysis applied retained social mates somewhat more often (50/58,
multistrata, capturemarkrecapture models to or 86%) than mid- (3/6, or 50%) or low-ranking
resighting data from 1997 to 2002 (K. A. Schubert, males (7/11, or 64%) (K. A. Schubert, unpublished
C. Kraus, D. J. Mennill et al. unpublished data). data). Thus high-ranked males benefit from
Population annual survival varied considerably enhanced mate fidelity during the breeding season
over the 6 years (3673%), with high-ranked males (Otter et al. 1998; Mennill et al. 2004).
surviving somewhat better than low-ranked males Figure 9.3 shows annual RRS of males and
(annual survival probabilities 0.56 0.09 for high- females in the QUBS population. Average clutch
ranked males vs. 0.50 0.08 for low-ranked males). size was 6.8 0.1 eggs (N 185 clutches), with
Moreover, none of the well-supported models 92% overall hatching success. Of 240 nests with

0.35
Proportion of observations

0.30 Males
Females
0.25
0.20
0.15
0.10
0.05 Figure 9.3 Annual realized reproductive success (RRS)
of male and female chickadees. Genetic paternity was
0.00 determined at ca. 6 days after hatching (N 126
0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 observations for males and 123 for females; from
Number of genetic offspring Schubert 2003).
142
R E P R O D U C T I V E E C O L O G Y, E V O L U T I O N, A N D B E H AV I O R
Table 9.4 Summary of nested models of the influence of male age, female age, and male rank on annual reproductive success

Nesting Clutch size Hatching RRS Fledging


(i 264, j 159) (i 128, j 93) (i 154, j 106) (i 80, j 64) (i 170, j 108)

Coefficient Wald stat Coefficient Wald stat Coefficient Wald stat Coefficient Wald stat Coefficient Wald stat
(SE) (P-value) (SE) (P-value) (SE) (P-value) (SE) (P-value) (SE) (P-value)

Intercept 0.59 0.48 0.40 0.61 1.24


(0.24) (0.18) (0.17) (0.23) (1.16)
Male age 1.16 6.62 0.29 1.96 0.42 4.62 0.35 1.84 0.46 0.96
(0.45) (0.01) (0.21) (0.16) (0.20) (0.03) (0.26) (0.17) (0.47) (0.33)
Female age NA NA 0.27 2.20 0.05 0.07 0.17 0.57 0.02 0.00.
(0.18) (0.14) (0.17) (0.79) (0.23) (0.45) (0.42) (0.97)
Male rank (H) 2.12 9.94 0.18 0.76 0.13 0.40 0.47 3.17 0.06 0.02
(0.67) (0.01) (0.21) (0.38) (0.20) (0.53) (0.27) (0.08) (0.46) (0.90)
Male rank (M) 0.63 1.34 0.04 0.02 0.21 0.60 0.12 0.14 1.07 3.07
(0.54) (0.25) (0.28) (0.89) (0.27) (0.44) (0.33) (0.71) (0.61) (0.08)

Model coefficients are given with standard errors. Wald statistics are accompanied by significance values at 1 d.f. Sample sizes i and j denote the number of observations and number of individual
male birds, respectively. Clutch size, hatching, and RRS (number of genetic offspring at day six post-hatch) are standardized within year as (x xmean)/SD; models of nesting and fledging success
use binomial response data. The full fledging model included additional year effects not presented here. Male age and female age represent the effects of adult vs. juvenile age category on breeding
success. Male rank variables represent contrasts of high- and mid-ranked males against low-ranked males (modified from Schubert 2003).
SOCIAL DOMINANCE AND FITNESS 143

known outcomes, 56% fledged at least one by lifespan (Ps 0.0001) (Fig. 9.4). To test the effect
offspring. Successful pairs fledged an average of of male rank over time on LRS, we calculated the
6.3 0.1 young; 29.8 % of broods contained extra- sum of all ranks held by a male, where 1 low,
pair young (N 131), with an average of 11% 2 mid, and 3 high rank in each year. Average
extrapair young per brood (N 73), remarkably lifetime rank (the sum of ranks held divided by
consistent with our initial estimates of EPY based lifespan) significantly predicted whether a male
on a much smaller sample of nests (Otter et al. attempted to reproduce (P 0.01). However rank
1994). Annual reproductive success was lower in sum/lifespan did not predict LRS among the subset
males than females: only 87% of males, but 93% of of males that succeeded in reproducing (P 0.2). In
females, paired and attempted to breed (P 0.03). other words, males that lived longer and reached
The standardized variance in annual RRS was higher rank sooner had greater fitness, but this was
larger in males (0.16) than females (0.07), consistent a function of whether birds reproduced at all, rather
with moderate levels of extrapair paternity. than fine-scale differences between individuals.
Table 9.4 summarizes the results of nested GLZ
analyses of the effects of male and female age and
9.7 Future research
male rank on annual reproductive success
(Schubert 2003). Both male age and rank influenced Male black-capped chickadees at QUBS vary in
whether a male paired with a female and attempted their ability to dominate competitors, and high
any nesting (P 0.02). However, compared to age, social rank enhances both annual survival and
male rank may be a stronger predictor of male RRS reproduction, leading in turn to increased LRS.
(P 0.08). Surprisingly rank was not associated Although our study was not initiated in 1992 with
with clutch size, hatching, or fledging success in a long-term design, and the data set is admittedly
this model, in contrast to the results of Otter et al. limited, the results suggest several profitable
(1999), which was based on 2 years breeding data, avenues for further research. One obvious question
including a year of severe nest predation. Thus the concerns the source(s) of individual variability in
benefits of rank probably vary from year to year signaling behavior associated with rank, including
(see Chapter 17 for evidence of annual variation in the role of experience in modulating individuals
effects of habitat quality). responses to competitors and potential mates.
Lifetime reproductive success of both males and Careful measurement of ontogenetic changes in
females was highly variable and heavily influenced plumage, vocalizations, and dominance behavior,

30

25
Number of genetic offspring

20

15

10

Figure 9.4 The relationship between lifespan


5 and lifetime reproductive success (LRS). LRS
Females
estimates are based on the total number of
Males
0 genetic offspring attributed to 51 male and 56
1 2 3 4 5 6 female chickadees and do not include birds
Years that never bred (from Schubert 2003).
144 R E P R O D U C T I V E E C O L O G Y, E V O L U T I O N, A N D B E H AV I O R

coupled with their associated effects on male and Integrated Approach. NATO ASI Series, vol. G-24.
female receivers, are required. Springer-Verlag, Heidelberg.
A second, related question concerns the factors Braillet, C., Charmantier, A., Archaux, F., Dos Santos, A.,
that contribute to individual variation in first-year Perret, P., and Lambrechts, M. M. (2002). Two blue tit
Parus caeruleus populations from Corsica differ in social
rank. Our analyses point to male rank history as a
dominance. Journal of Avian Biology, 33, 446450.
key predictor of fitness, thus it would be useful to
Bronson, C. L., Grubb, T. C. Jr, Sattler, G. D., and Braun,
identify the individual attributes and ecological cir- M. J. (2003). Mate preference: a possible causal mech-
cumstances that combine to permit some males to anism for a moving hybrid zone. Animal Behaviour, 65,
achieve high rank in their first year. For example, 489500.
short-term aviary experiments using juveniles Christie, P. J., Mennill, D. J., and Ratcliffe, L. M. (2004).
captured in late summer could measure behavioral Pitch shifts and song structure indicate male quality in
predictors of dominance (e.g. Verbeek et al. 1999). the dawn chorus of black-capped chickadees. Behavioral
Temporary removal of dominant adults from natural Ecology and Sociobiology, 55, 341348.
flocks might also provide useful opportunities to Clutton-Brock, T. H. ed. (1988). Reproductive Success.
observe flock fission and reassembly, as suggested University of Chicago Press, Chicago.
Colquhoun, M. K. (1942). Notes on the social behaviour of
by Smith (1991). Ideally, such experiments should
blue tits. British Birds, 35, 234240.
be combined with detailed information on both
Cresswell, W. (2003). Testing the mass-dependent preda-
temporal and geographic variation in population tion hypothesis: in European blackbirds poor foragers
density, to determine whether our findings extend to have higher overwinter body reserves. Animal
other parts of the species range. Ironically, this Behaviour, 65, 10351044.
critically important flock formation period remains Dale, S. and Slagsvold, T. (1995). Female contests for nest
the least-studied part of the chickadee annual cycle. sites and mates in the pied flycatcher Ficedula hypoleuca.
Ethology, 99, 209222.
Dall, S. R. X., Houston, A. I., and McNamara, J. M. (2004).
Acknowledgements The behavioural ecology of personality: consistent indi-
We thank Queens University Biology Station and vidual differences from an adaptive perspective.
adjoining landowners for access to study sites and Ecology Letters, 7, 734739.
logistical help and the many field assistants who have De Laet, J. (1984). Site-related dominance in the Great Tit
Parus major. Ornis Scandinavica, 15, 7378.
helped us over the years. Additional, crucial long-term
Desrochers, A. (1989). Sex, dominance and microhabitat
data were kindly provided by K. Otter and S. Ramsay.
use in wintering black-capped chickadees: a field
Funding was provided by grants from NSERC, OGS, experiment. Ecology, 70, 636645.
Queens University, the American Museum of Natural Desrochers, A. and Hannon, S. J. (1989). Site-related
History, American Ornithologists Union, Animal dominance and spacing among winter flocks of black-
Behavior Society, Association of Field Ornithologists, capped chickadees. Condor, 91, 317323.
John K. Cooper Foundation, Sigma Xi, Society of Desrochers, A., Hannon, S. J., and Nordin, K. E. (1988).
Canadian Ornithologists, and by donations from E. Winter survival and territory acquisition in a northern
and I. Ramsay and PPG Canada. population of black-capped chickadees. Auk, 105,
727736.
deVries, H. (1998). Finding a dominance order most con-
References sistent with a linear hierarchy: a new procedure and
Alonso-Alvarez, C., Doutrelant, C., and Sorci, G. (2004). review. Animal Behaviour, 55, 827843.
Ultraviolet reflectance affects malemale interactions in Dhondt, A. A. (1989). Blue tit. In: Newton, I., ed. Lifetime
the blue tit (Parus caeruleus ultramarines). Behavioral Reproduction in Birds. Academic Press, London.
Ecology, 15, 805809. Dixon, K. L. (1965). Dominance-subordination relation-
Beacham, J. L. (2003). Models of dominance hierarchy for- ships in mountain chickadees. Condor, 67, 291299.
mation: effects of prior experience and intrinsic traits. Doucet, S. M., Mennill, D. J., Montgomerie, R., Boag, P. T.,
Behaviour, 140, 12751303. and Ratcliffe, L. M. (2005). Achromatic plumage
Blondel, J., Gosler, A., Lebreton, J.-D., and McCleery, R., reflectance predicts reproductive success in male black-
eds. (1990). Population Biology of Passerine Birds, an capped chickadees. Behavioral Ecology, 16, 218222.
SOCIAL DOMINANCE AND FITNESS 145

Drews, C. (1993). The concept and definition of domi- Lange, H. and Leimar, O. (2003). The function of threat
nance in animal behaviour. Behaviour, 125, 283313. display in wintering great tits. Animal Behaviour, 65,
Dugatkin, L. A. and Earley, R. L. (2004). Individual recog- 573584.
nition, dominance hierarchies and winner and loser Lange, H. and Leimar, O. (2004). Social stability and daily
effects. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 271, body mass gain in great tits. Behavioral Ecology, 15,
15371540. 549554.
Ekman, J. (1989). Ecology of non-breeding social systems Lemel, J. and Wallin, K. (1993). Status signaling, motiv-
among the Paridae. Wilson Bulletin, 101, 263288. ational condition and dominance: an experimental
Ekman, J. (2004). Mass-dependence in the predation risk of study in the great tit, Parus major. Animal Behaviour, 45,
unequal competitors; some models. Oikos, 105, 109116. 549558.
Ellis, L. (1995). Dominance and reproductive success Lens, L. and Dhondt, A. A. (1993). Individual variation in
among non-human animals: a cross-species compari- mate care by alpha males in crested tit winter flocks.
son. Ethology and Sociobiology, 16, 257333 . Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 33, 7985.
Ficken, M. S., Weise, C. M., and Popp, J. W. (1990). Leonard, M. L., Horn, A. G., and Ratcliffe, L. M. (1991).
Dominance rank and resource access in winter flocks of Parental aggression in black-capped chickadees.
black-capped chickadees. Wilson Bulletin, 102, 623633. Behavioral Ecology, 2, 228233.
Forkman, B. and Haskell, M. J. (2004). The maintenance of Lundborg, K. and Brodin, A. (2003). The effect of dom-
stable dominance hierarchies and the pattern of aggres- inance rank on fat deposition and food hoarding in the
sion: support for the suppression hypothesis. Ethology, willow tit Parus montanusan experimental test. Ibis,
110, 737744. 145, 7882.
Glase, J. C. (1973). Ecology of social organization in the Matthysen, E. (1990). Nonbreeding social organization in
black-capped chickadee. Living Bird, 12, 235267. Parus. In: Power, D. M., ed. Current Ornithology, vol. 7,
Goldstein, H. (1995). Multilevel Statistical Models, 2nd edn. pp. 209249. Plenum Press, New York.
Arnold, London. McCleery, R. H. and Perrins, C. M. (1988). Lifetime repro-
Hansen, B. T. and Slagsvold, T. (2004). Early learning affects ductive success of the great tit. Parus major. In: Clutton-
social dominance: interspecifically cross-fostered tits Brock, T. H., ed. Reproductive Success. University of
become subdominant. Behavioral Ecology, 15, 262268. Chicago Press, Chicago.
Hartzler, J. E. (1970). Winter dominance relationships Mennill, D. J. and Ratcliffe, L. M. (2004a). Nest cavity
in black-capped chickadees. Wilson Bulletin, 82, orientation in black-capped chickadees Poecile atricapil-
427434. lus: do the acoustic properties of cavities influence
Hay, J. M., Evans, P. R., Ward, R. M., and Hamer, K. C. sound reception in the nest and extra-pair matings?
(2004). Poor nutritional condition as a consequence of Journal of Avian Biology, 35, 477482.
high dominance status in the coal tit Parus ater. Ibis, 146, Mennill, D. J. and Ratcliffe, L. M. (2004b). Overlapping
103107. and matching in the song contests of black-capped
Hogstad, O. (1987a). It is expensive to be dominant. Auk, chickadees. Animal Behaviour, 67, 441450.
104, 333336. Mennill, D. J. and Ratcliffe, L. M. (2004c). Do male black-
Hogstad, O. (1987b). Social rank in winter flocks of willow capped chickadees eavesdrop on song contests? A
tits Parus montanus. Ibis, 129, 19. multi-speaker playback experiment. Behaviour, 141,
Hogstad, O. (1989). Social organization and dominance 125139.
behavior in some Parus species. Wilson Bulletin, 101, Mennill, D. J., Boag, P. T., and Ratcliffe, L. M. (2003b).
254262. The reproductive choices of eavesdropping female
Koivula, K., Kimmo, L., Orell, M., and Rytkonen, S. (1993). black-capped chickadees, Poecile atricapillus.
Prior residency as a key determinant of social dom- Naturwissenschaften, 90, 577582.
inance in the willow tit (Parus montanus). Behavioral Mennill, D. J., Doucet, S. M., Montgomerie, R., and
Ecology and Sociobiology, 33, 283287. Ratcliffe, L. M. (2003a). Achromatic color variation in
Koivula, K., Orell, M., and Rytkonen, S. (1996). Winter black-capped chickadees, Poecile atricapilla: black and
survival and breeding success of dominant and subor- white signals of sex and rank. Behavioral Ecology and
dinate willow tits Parus montanus. Ibis, 138, 624629. Sociobiology, 53, 350357.
Lahti, K., Koivula, K., Orell, M., and Rytkonen, S. (1996). Mennill, D. J., Ramsay, S. M., Boag, P. T., and Ratcliffe,
Social dominance in free-living willow tits Parus L. M. (2004). Patterns of extra-pair mating in relation to
montanus: determinants and some implications of hier- male dominance status and female nest placement in
archy. Ibis, 138, 539544. black-capped chickadees. Behavioral Ecology, 15, 757765.
146 R E P R O D U C T I V E E C O L O G Y, E V O L U T I O N, A N D B E H AV I O R

Mennill, D. J., Ratcliffe, L. M., and Boag, P. T. (2002). female black-capped chickadees (Parus atricapillus):
Female eavesdropping on male song contests in song- separate strategies with a common target. Behavioral
birds. Science, 296, 873. Ecology and Sociobiology, 49, 1823.
Newton, I. ed. (1989). Lifetime Reproduction in Birds. Sandall, M. and Smith, H. G. (1991). Dominance, prior
Academic Press, London. occupancy, and winter residency in the great tit
Nilsson, J.-A. and Smith, H. G. (1985). Early fledgling (Parus major). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 29,
mortality and the timing of juvenile dispersal in the 147152.
marsh tit Parus palustris. Ornis Scandinavica, 16, 293298. Schubert, K. A. (2003). Lifespan, Social Rank and Lifetime
Nilsson, J.-A. and Smith, H. G. (1988). Effects of dispersal Reproductive Success in Black-Capped Chickadees. MSc
date on winter flock establishment and social dom- thesis, Queens University, Kingston, Canada.
inance in marsh tits Parus palustris. Journal of Animal Schubert, K. A., Mennill, D. J., Ramsay, S. M., Otter, K. A.,
Ecology, 57, 917928. Boag, P. T., and Ratcliffe. L. M. (2006). Variation in social
Otter, K. and Ratcliffe, L. (1996). Female-initiated divorce rank acquisition influences lifetime reproductive suc-
in a monogamous songbird: abandoning mates for cess in black-capped chickadees. Biological Journal of the
males of higher quality. Proceedings of the Royal Society Linnaean Society, 89, in press.
London B, 263, 351354. Smith, S. M. (1976). Ecological aspects of dominance
Otter, K., Chruszcz, B., and Ratcliffe, L. (1997). Honest hierarchies in black-capped chickadees. The Auk, 93,
advertisement and song output during the dawn 95107.
chorus of black-capped chickadees. Behavioral Ecology, Smith, S. M. (1988). Extra-pair copulations in black-
8, 167173. capped chickadees: the role of the female. Behaviour,
Otter, K., Ramsay, S. M., and Ratcliffe, L. (1999). Enhanced 107, 1523.
reproductive success of female black-capped chicka- Smith, S. M. (1991). The Black-capped Chickadee. Behavioral
dees mated to high-ranking males. Auk, 116, 345354. Ecology and Natural History. Cornell University Press,
Otter, K., Ratcliffe, L., and Boag, P. T. (1994). Extra-pair Ithaca, NY.
paternity in the black-capped chickadee. Condor, 96, Verbeek, M. E. M., De Goede, P., Drent, P. J., and
218222. Wiepkema, P. R. (1999). Individual behavioural charac-
Otter, K., Ratcliffe, L., Michaud, D., and Boag, P. T. (1998). teristics and dominance in aviary groups of great tits.
Do female black-capped chickadees prefer high-rank- Behaviour, 136, 2348.
ing males as extra-pair partners? Behavioral Ecology and Verhulst, S. and Salomons, H. M. (2004). Why fight?
Sociobiology, 43, 2536. Socially dominant jackdaws, Corvus monedula, have low
Piper, W. H. (1997). Social dominance in birds: early find- fitness. Animal Behaviour, 68, 777783.
ings and new horizons. In: Nolan, V. Jr, Ketterson, E., Wiley, R. H., Steadman, L., Chadwick, L., and Wollerman,
and Thompson, C. F., eds. Current Ornithology, Vol. 14, L. (1999). Social inertia in white-throated sparrows
pp. 125187. Plenum, New York. results from recognition of opponents. Animal
Pravosudov, V. V. and Grubb, T. C. (1997). Energy man- Behaviour, 57, 453463.
agement in passerine birds during the non-breeding Woodcock, E. A., Rathburn, M. K., and Ratcliffe L. M.
season. In: Nolan, V. Jr, Ketterson, E., and Thompson, (2005). Achromatic plumage reflectance, social
C. F., eds. Current Ornithology. Vol. 14, pp. 189234. dominance and female mate preference in black-
Plenum, New York. capped chickadees (Poecile atricapillus). Ethology, 111,
Ramsay, S. M. and Ratcliffe, L. M. (2003). Determinants 891900.
of social rank in female black-capped chickadees (Poecile Zanette, L. and Ratcliffe, L. M. (1994). Social rank
atricapilla). Canadian Journal of Zoology, 81, 117121. influences conspicuous behaviour of black-capped
Ramsay, S. M., Otter, K. A., Mennill, D. J., Ratcliffe, L. M., chickadees (Parus atricapillus). Animal Behaviour, 48,
and Boag, P. T. (2000). Divorce and extrapair mating in 119127
SYNOPSIS II

Parid reproductive behavior


Daniel J. Mennill, Theresa M. Burg, Robert L. Curry, Kathy
Martin, Andrea R. Norris, Laurene Ratcliffe, Matthew W.
Reudink, Lindsay M. Rossano, and Kristin A. Schubert

North American Parids are excellent organisms for scenarios may explain the current distribution
research on the evolution of avian reproductive patterns found in chestnut-backed chickadees:
behavior, and how these are influenced by intra- multiple colonizations from a single refugium,
and interspecific interactions. As cavity-nesting colonization from multiple refugia, or a single
birds, they exhibit high reproductive success and colonization from a single refugium and possible
populations of most Parid species exist in high introgression. Northern populations possess a large
numbers throughout their range. As social birds, number of alleles that are absent in other popula-
their reproductive behavior is shaped by group tions, indicative of prolonged isolation. Whether
dynamics, including flock hierarchy and territorial this isolation is the result of historical isolation in
behavior. Being non-migratory, their behavior can different glacial refugia or different founding popu-
be studied throughout the year, and their breeding lations is not known. The second pattern that
activities can be easily related to their non-breeding emerged from studying the phylogenetic structure
activities. This section of the book covers a diversity of chestnut-backed chickadees is that disjunct and
of topics within the general field of Parid reproduc- peripheral populations are genetically isolated
tive behavior, using ongoing research on several from the central core population. A similar pattern
species of North American chickadees. The various is found in other Eurasian and North American
chapters discuss: phylogenetic structure within Parid species. Dispersal across large expanses of
populations, hybridization between closely related unsuitable habitat and between the periphery and
species, nesting ecology, and the influence of social core portions of the species range appear to be
dominance on reproductive biology. limited, resulting in population substructuring.
Chapter 6 examines the phylogenetic structure of Future phylogenetic research may determine the
chestnut-backed chickadee populations in western origin of the private alleles in the northern popula-
North America and compares the resulting patterns tions of chestnut-backed chickadees. Is the large
to those found in other North American and number of unique alleles the result of hybridization
Eurasian Parids. Chestnut-backed chickadees are with another species, such as the boreal chickadee?
associated with cedarhemlock ecosystems, and Hybridization between two brown-capped chicka-
their movement into an area is dependent on the dee species has not been reported; however,
availability of suitable habitat. Much of the chest- hybridization between several other Parid species
nut-backed chickadees current range was glaciated is common (Curry 2005; Chapter 7). Do other
during the Pleistocene and was recolonized follow- populations of chestnut-backed chickadees in the
ing the retreat of the Cordilleran ice sheet. Four interior Pacific North-west show the same pattern
genetically distinct groups are recognized: Queen of differentiation as the south-eastern British
Charlotte Islands; southeastern British Columbia; Columbia population? Similarly, are we able to
mainland Alaska; and a large coastal group. Several determine the source population of the recent

147
148 R E P R O D U C T I V E E C O L O G Y, E V O L U T I O N, A N D B E H AV I O R

expansion into central California? On a larger scale, assumed or concluded that vocalizations play key
further studies need to be conducted examining roles in species recognition within the Parids.
population structure in other chickadee species, However, it seems clear that song development,
expanding on the previous work by Gill et al. (1993, and possibly song recognition, in most (if not all)
2005). Are the patterns of peripheral isolation and Parids involves strong learning components (see
isolation of disjunct or island populations found in also Section III). Accordingly, the vocal behavior of
chestnut-backed chickadeesas well as prelimi- an individual male may inaccurately reflect its
nary studies of other North American chickadees genotype, but whether hybridization results from
present when more extensive sampling is female mate choice contingent on song features
conducted? By addressing these questions, we can remains incompletely tested. A second, and pos-
further increase our understanding of movements sibly independent, aspect of Parid behavior that
and speciation in Parids. could exert a strong influence on pairing patterns is
Chapter 7 continues on this theme by examining social dominance. Extensive results from black-
the behavioral aspects of hybridization along the capped chickadees have revealed that dominance
extended contact zone between black-capped rank strongly influences both pairing success and
chickadees and Carolina chickadees in south-east- mating fidelity (Ramsay et al. 2000; Mennill et al.
ern Pennsylvania. Intensive field research in this 2004; Chapter 9). Research on captive birds from
contact zone since 1998 has focused on three large the hybrid zone has further suggested that mate
study sites, with a specific focus on variation in choice may depend mainly on relative dominance,
vocalizations among color-banded individuals, more so than vocal or physical phenotype (Bronson
while concurrently investigating genetic variation et al. 2003). Nevertheless, work on black-capped
among and within the populations. The most strik- chickadees has also shown that both singing behav-
ing finding concerns discrepancies between the ior (e.g. Otter et al. 1997; Mennill et al. 2002 ) and
songs, calls, and genotypes of chickadees at a geo- plumage coloration (Doucet et al. 2005) relate to
graphically intermediate site. Within this popula- patterns of extrapair paternity and reproductive
tion, which mainly comprises hybrid individuals, success. Studies using both observational and
up to half of all males are bilingual, singing the experimental approaches to thoroughly evaluate
typical songs of both black-capped and Carolina the relative strengths of social dominance, song,
chickadees. The rest of the males sing only black- and plumage features within Parid hybrid zones
capped songs, although the proportion of the popu- are needed to produce clearer resolution of the
lation falling into this category has gradually behavioral mechanisms underlying hybridization
declined over time. This pattern of song variation in the family. Further research on vocal develop-
probably represents cultural persistence of learned ment, through experimental studies of captive
behavior. Virtually all males in the population have birds subjected to various acoustic and social stim-
Carolina mitochondrial DNA, and no overall dif- uli, represents an additional area with great prom-
ference in nuclear genotype, assessed using ise for advancing our understanding of the role of
microsatellite DNA, is apparent between the two vocal behavior in Parid hybridization.
types of males based on songs. On the other hand, Interspecific interactions can also influence
characteristics of the dee note of the chick-a-dee call reproductive ecology in manners other than
of the same birds are intermediate between those of hybridization, and these are not limited to include
black-capped and Carolina chickadees; call charac- only other Parids. Chapter 8 examines the nesting
teristics show no apparent correlation with either biology of black-capped and mountain chickadees
song category or genetic background. and their ecological relationships within cavity-
Numerous unresolved questions about behav- nesting vertebrate communities. Parids live in com-
ioral mechanisms associated with Parid hybridiza- munities structured by hierarchical guilds termed
tion remain, both in the black-capped/Carolina nest webs whereby some species depend on others
chickadee contact zone and for other members of to produce cavities for breeding or roosting, and
the family more broadly. Most investigators have interspecific and intraspecific interactions are
SOCIAL DOMINANCE AND FITNESS 149

centered around nest site availability (Martin and temporal changes observed in chickadee abun-
Eadie 1999). Despite similar size, ecology, and dance were driven by larger-scale environmental or
coexistence on the same sites, mountain and black- ecological influences, as the dramatic changes in
capped chickadees exhibit strong segregation in annual fecundity of mountain chickadees they
nesting habitats in the mixed forests of central observed in 2005 were similar to those observed in
British Columbia. From 1995 to 2004, mountain the Sierra Nevada. Future research exploring
chickadee numbers showed a strong increase, with regional- or continental-scale patterns of Parid
similar increases in their small bodied competitors breeding biology will help elucidate these critical
(red-breasted nuthatches) and potential predators influences on demography.
(red squirrels), and also a strong reduction in the Within-species, reproductive potential may be
health of conifer trees, especially lodgepole pine. influenced by social interactions, especially by the
Overall nesting densities of mountain chickadees dominance hierarchies established in wintering
were positively correlated with the proportion of chickadee flocks. The naturally- and sexually-
lodgepole pine trees that had been attacked by bark selected fitness consequences of social dominance,
beetles, nesting densities of red-breasted nuthatch the mechanics of dominance interactions, and the
and downy woodpeckers, and negatively corre- relationship between dominance and lifetime
lated with density of red-naped sapsuckers, red- reproductive success in black-capped chickadees
breasted nuthatches, and tree density. Nest density are explored in Chapter 9. Data from over 10 years
of mountain chickadees was significantly greater in of research on the dominance relationships and
years of high beetle and western spruce budworm breeding behavior of black-capped chickadees at
abundance. Population growth rates demonstrated Queens University Biological Station in eastern
strong negative density-dependent responses to Ontario show that social dominance rank plays an
chickadee density the previous year, but were important role in structuring several aspects of an
enhanced by an abundance of forest insects and individuals life history, from winter flock behavior
density of trees, and reduced by high abundance of to territorial behavior and reproductive activities.
red squirrels. Thus mountain chickadees show Dominant and subordinate birds show distinct
strong functional and numerical responses to phenotypic characteristics (e.g. enhanced plumage
pulses in resource availability despite strong contrast, higher song output for high-ranking
increases in their competitors and predators. males) and life history characteristics (e.g. higher
While mountain chickadees showed the nesting success, larger clutch sizes for high-ranking
strongest ability to adapt to local differences in males). Longitudinal and cross-sectional analyses
habitat conditions, black-capped chickadees of survival and reproduction show that social rank
declined in density in 2003 and 2004. Since black- is a better predictor than age of individual survival
capped chickadees are behaviorally dominant to from one winter to the next. In common with other
mountain chickadees and able to excavate their avian life history studies, lifespan was the best
own cavities, it is puzzling why they were not able predictor of average lifetime reproductive success.
to capitalize on the resource pulse in the enhanced However, male rank history also influenced life-
habitat conditions. It is interesting that black- time realized reproductive success: individuals that
capped chickadees, reasonably strong excavators, attained higher rank earlier had more offspring.
could not respond to the increase in forest insects to Dominance relationships mediate social inter-
the same extent as mountain chickadees; perhaps actions in many Parids, and future studies should
an ecologically-equivalent situation to subordinate explore how widespread are these patterns among
blue tits depressing reproductive success of the the Paridae. Future research exploring the early
dominant great blue tits in sympatric populations. ontogeny of social rank, as well as attributes associ-
Martin and Norris assumed that the functional and ated with high and low rank such as plumage color
numerical responses observed were driven by local and singing behavior, is needed. The process of
pulses in resource availability or mild winter flock formation in the autumn requires more
conditions. However, it is possible that some of the careful behavioral study, as this period of the
150 R E P R O D U C T I V E E C O L O G Y, E V O L U T I O N, A N D B E H AV I O R

annual cycle of black-capped chickadees has been Martin, K. and Eadie, J. (1999). Nest webs: a community-
neglected in the eastern Ontario studies. wide approach to the management and conservation of
cavity-nesting forest birds. Forest Ecology and
Management, 115, 243257.
References Mennill, D. J., Ramsay, S. M., Boag, P. T., and Ratcliffe,
L. M. (2004). Patterns of extrapair mating in relation to
Bronson, C. L., Grubb, T. C., Jr, Sattler, G. D., and Braun,
male dominance status and female nest placement in
M. J. (2003). Mate preference: a possible causal mech-
black-capped chickadees. Behavioral Ecology, 15,
anism for a moving hybrid zone. Animal Behaviour, 65,
757765.
489500.
Mennill D. J., Ratcliffe, L. M., and Boag, P. T. (2002).
Curry, R. L. (2005). Hybridization in chickadees: much to
Female eavesdropping on male song contests in song-
learn from familiar birds. Auk, 122, 747758.
birds. Science, 296, 873883.
Doucet, S. M., Mennill, D. J., Montgomerie, R., Boag, P. T.,
Otter, K., Chruszcz, B., and Ratcliffe, L. (1997). Honest
and Ratcliffe, L. M. (2005). Achromatic plumage
advertisement and singing during the dawn chorus of
reflectance predicts reproductive success in male black-
black-capped chickadees, Parus atricapillus. Behavioral
capped chickadees. Behavioral Ecology, 16, 218222.
Ecology, 8, 167173.
Gill, F. B., Mostrom, A. M., and Mack, A. L. (1993).
Ramsay, S. M., Otter, K., Mennill, D. J., Ratcliffe, L. M., and
Speciation in North American chickadees: I. Patterns of
Boag, P. T. (2000). Divorce and extra-pair mating in
mtDNA genetic divergence. Evolution, 47, 195212.
female black-capped chickadees (Parus atricapillus): sep-
Gill, F. B., Slikas, B., and Sheldon, F. H. (2005). Phylogeny
arate strategies with a common target. Behavioral
of titmice (Paridae): II. Species relationships based on
Ecology and Sociobiology, 49, 1823.
sequences of the mitochondrial cytochrome b gene.
Auk, 122, 121143.
SECTION III

Vocal communication

Parids are among the best-studied avian family, defense) typically associated with song. The intrica-
globally, with respect to vocal communication. cies of these three vocalizations justify individual
Although not necessarily elaborate songsters, chapters on each.
Parids have extremely diversified vocalizations The section begins with a look at the production
which have formed rich fodder for entire research and perception of fee-bees and chick-a-dees in both
careers. North American chickadees and titmice field and laboratory studies of chickadees (Chapter
(and their Eurasian Poecile counterparts) also have 10). Sturdy, Bloomfield, Charrier, and Lee review
vocal attributes that make this family unusual, as studies that use operant techniques to tease apart
they do not follow the commonly held definition the mechanisms by which note structure and
that distinguishes songs (structurally complex, sequence are classified. Lucas and Freeberg
learned vocalizations given primarily by males (Chapter 13) build on this by addressing the
during the breeding season for mate attraction and potential for information encoding in the chick-a-dee
territory defense) from calls (structurally simple, complex in Carolina and other species, and the
innate vocalizations given by both sexes year- potential of this call to convey complex and
round, in contexts other than mate attraction and multifacetted aspects of information transfer. The
territory defense). Poecile chickadeesand emphasis in this chapter is on the use of field
Baeolophus titmice, although less well studiedblur experimentation to determine the context and
these distinctions in the production of three classes message of calls, which complements the work
of vocalizationswhistled songs (often colloqui- on classification and perception outlined by
ally referred to as fee-bees after the song of the well- Sturdy et al.
studied black-capped chickadee), gargles, and Baker and Gammon (Chapter 11) take a relatively
chick-a-dees. unprecedented look at dialectal variation, signal
There is evidence for learning and regional vari- convergence with populations, and signal match-
ation in all three of these vocalizations. Further, the ing in a vocalization technically classified as a
whistled songs of chickadees are typically less com- call, the gargle complex of black-capped chicka-
plex than the structural components of the chick-a- dees. While the complexity of this vocalization is
dee and gargle complexes. Chick-a-dees have a one of the primary features blurring the distinction
syntactical structure that has resulted in direct com- between calls and songs, the matching and inter-
parisons to human language. Yet despite this, whis- active playback studies may indicate subtle differ-
tled songs follow the behavioral patterns (restricted ences between how birds communicate with gargles
use, primarily by males, in context of territorial and with whistled songs.

151
152 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

In Chapter 12, Gammon assesses the potential for the fee-bee of the black-capped chickadeeto con-
postdispersal environments to influence song struc- vey complex information. Through the use of fre-
ture in the fee-bee of black-capped chickadees. This quency shifting, chickadees appear to perform
call is remarkably stereotyped throughout much of similar aspects of signal matching and interactive
the extensive distribution of this species across communication with their single song type typ-
North America, yet extreme cases of song variation ically associated with use of repertoires in other
in isolated populations may give insight into the species. Further, this chapter addresses the ability
nature of dialectal variation that arises through to signal dominance status and condition, as well as
social isolation. the ability of receivers to gather this information
Finally, Mennill and Otter (Chapter 14) investi- about potential mates and rivals, in the context of
gate the potential for an apparently simple song communication networks.
CHAPTER 10

Chickadee vocal production and


perception: An integrative approach
to understanding acoustic
communication
Christopher B. Sturdy, Laurie L. Bloomfield,
Isabelle Charrier, and Tiffany T.-Y. Lee

10.1 Introduction song is produced mainly by males and the peak of


production occurs in the spring and early summer.
The three key elements in the scientific study of ani-
The song of black-capped chickadees has been the
mal communication are the sender and the receiver,
subject of intensive field studies over the past
and the interaction between these two elements.
decade (see Chapters 13 and 14), focusing primarily
Senders produce a signal that is perceived by the
on the functional aspects of singing behavior. Our
receiver and, operationally, communication occurs
focus here will be on vocal production and
when the signal leads to an observable change in
structure.
the behavior of the receiver (Maynard Smith and
Black-capped chickadees sing their two-note
Harper 2003). Just as production and perception are
song at several distinct pitches (Fig. 10.1a). Initially
integral to communication in general, so too must
quantified in detail by Ratcliffe and Weisman (1985)
both sides of the equation be considered when
and Weisman et al. (1990), work over the last decade
studying acoustic communication in any particular
has shown that there is a frequency range over
species or in any animal group. Here we take this
which the species songs are produced, spanning
integrative approach and equally consider both
approximately 1 kHz from lowest to highest songs,
production and perception, in an attempt to under-
and individual males can sing at a variety of
stand acoustic communication in one of the most
discrete pitches within this range (see Chapter 14
well-recognized and well-studied groups of North
for further details on possible functions for pitch
American songbirds, the chickadees.
shifting). However, at all of these various pitches,
the song remains a relatively high-frequency
10.2 Song production and perception vocalization that approximates a pure tone.
Average songs have a starting frequency of about
10.2.1 Song production
4000 Hz. The first note ( fee) then falls by about 200
In the prototypic species of the genus Poecile, the Hz from the start to the end. There is another fre-
black-capped chickadee, the species song is termed quency drop between the end of the fee and the start
fee-bee (Fig. 10.1a; see Box 10.1 for notes on the of the second note (bee) equal to about a 400 Hz
discovery of the black-capped chickadees song). drop. These regularities in pitch structure led
Since song in this species has been investigated Weisman and colleagues (1990) to conduct a bio-
most heavily, it will be used as a model here. Fee-bee acoustic analysis, of a small sample and then an

153
154 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

(a)
6
Frequency (kHz)

0
0 0.5 1.0 1.5 2.0 2.5
Time (sec)
(b)
6

Glissando Relative pitch ratio


~ 1.12
Frequency (kHz)

4 Fee
start Fee
end
Bee Bee
start end

2
Figure 10.1 (a) Two representative fee-
bee songs. Spectrogram settings: Hanning
window, 1024 points. (b) A fee-bee song
indicating the location of specified
0 measurements. Time (sec) is on the x axis
0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 1.2 and frequency (kHz) is on the y axis in both
Time (sec) spectrograms.

exhaustive sample of songs, to determine first the approximately 1.13 for all songs among all males.
acoustic properties of the song and second, which if Even more remarkable was the fact that this relative
any of these properties could be used in species and pitch ratio was maintained when birds produced
individual identification. shifted song. That is, when birds change the overall
One of the key findings from the initial bioa- starting frequency of the song, the frequency ratio
coustic analysis of black-capped chickadee fee-bee between the two notes remained constant. In other
song was males produced their songs with remark- words, male chickadees adjust the relative pitch
able regularity from one rendition to the next, between notes when they frequency shift, so as to
regardless of the initial starting frequency. Another maintain a constant tune. This implies that birds
remarkable finding from these early studies of were maintaining precise control over their vocal
chickadee song is that it maintains a constant and production as well as monitoring their vocal
equivalent relative pitch ratio between the two production in order to maintain this relative
notes. This relative pitch ratio was calculated by pitch ratio.
dividing the end frequency of the fee note by the In the follow-up, Weisman et al. (1990) recorded
starting frequency of the bee note: this ratio was songs from 156 males singing on their territories,
VOCAL PRODUCTION AND PERCEPTION 155

Box 10.1 What is the song of the black-capped chickadee?

Chickadee song, or more specifically what is produced mainly during the breeding season, and
considered chickadee song, has been a matter of are acquired through imitative learning. Calls, by
debate (Ficken 1981). Although chickadees contrast, are thought to be less complex,
produced several vocalizations, the particular produced by both sexes throughout the year in
vocalization that functioned as song per se, contexts outside mate attraction and territory
was contested. Two main contenders were defense, and are largely innate (Catchpole and
identified: the fee-bee and the acoustically Slater 1995). These facts made the identification
complex gargle call (Chapter 11). In fact, early of the chickadee song somewhat contentious. An
reports termed what is now considered to be additional factor, that likely enhanced the
chickadee song (fee-bee) their call (Dwight 1897). confusion around the issue, was not all chickadee
This is in large part due to the fact that species produce a whistled, tonal vocalization. In
chickadees do not conform to commonly held the end, the whistled song was accepted as the
truism for songbird vocal production, this being species-typical vocalization that served the typical
that songs are thought to be a complex functions of songmate attraction and territorial
vocalization, produced mainly by males, are defense.

and measured a greater number of acoustic features the start of the bee note. Importantly, and strikingly,
than simply the constant frequency of the fee and these relationships could be perfectly accounted for
bee, along with temporal measures. Several impor- with a linear regression, so that, for instance, as the
tant findings regarding song production arose from pitch of fee start increased, so did the pitch of fee end.
Weisman et al.s more extensive study. Absolute A similar pattern also emerged for the ratio of fee
pitch was found to be relatively consistent within end to bee, and these relative pitch findings were
birds but variable among birds. Thus, as a species consistent regardless of the starting pitch of the
marker, absolute pitch would provide a coarse song. Moreover, variation in the relative pitch cues
assessment of species identity, but it could be used observed within birds was tiny compared to those
for individual recognition. It was when attention among birds and even smaller than the within-bird
was turned to relative pitch cues, however, that variation seen for absolute pitch cues. Put another
things got particularly interesting. way, birds could potentially use relative pitch cues,
Relative pitch production can be further subdiv- present in either normal or shifted songs, for species
ided into at least two other categories, relative and individual identification.
pitch contour and relative pitch ratio, both of which As intriguing as these findings were, they were
were examined by Weisman et al. (1990). Relative only one side of the coin, that of song production.
pitch contour refers to the ordinal pitch relation- Whether the birds used any or all of the features for
ships between adjacent notes, or features of notes. In species or individual recognition was unclear.
black-capped chickadee song, these relationships Perceptual experiments were required to systemat-
were constant in almost every song sampled, with ically determine the role of each of these features
the starting pitch of the fee greater than the end of identified in the production analyses. We will there-
the fee, and the end of the fee greater than the pitch fore turn to song perception momentarily.
of the bee note. A constant pitch ratio, on the other
hand, requires there to not only be the ordinal rela-
10.2.2 Song perception
tionships described for pitch contour, but it requires
the additional feature of maintaining the same pitch Once potentially relevant, or at least invariant, song
ratio between acoustic features, both between the fee features were identified, the next critical step was to
start and fee end, and between the end of the fee and determine whether and how any or all of these
156 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

features were related to song perception in black- laboratory playback experiments were devised; one
capped chickadees. This required perceptual each to test birds reactions to songs altered either
experiments in which natural and manipulated in absolute or relative pitch. To alter the absolute
versions of the songs, or, in some cases, synthetic pitch, normal base songs were linearly shifted in
representations of the songs, were presented to pitch so that they ranged from below to above the
birds and their responses compared. To this end, absolute range of what is typical for black-capped
two main classes of experiments were conducted, song production. In order to test for only absolute
playback experiments and laboratory-based operant pitch perception, the relative pitch ratio between
conditioning experiments, with each approach the end of the fee and start of the bee note was
having its own advantages and limitations. maintained. To alter the relative pitch, base songs
were manipulated to produce several renditions
10.2.2.1 Playback experiments of song perception that started with the original fee start frequency, but
Laboratory playback procedures were carried out that contained various pitch ratios between the fee
by Ratcliffe and Weisman (Ratcliffe and Weisman end and bee start. Thus, in the second set of songs,
1986, 1988; Weisman and Ratcliffe 1989) as first the absolute starting pitch remained consistent
steps towards determining which, if any, features while the relative pitch ratio between the fee and bee
identified in the initial bioacoustic analyses were notes varied.
used for species recognition in chickadees. Initially, When chickadees were presented with songs out-
Ratcliffe and Weisman (1986, 1988) focused on side of the species-typical range of absolute pitch,
whether chickadees attend to the pitch contour and either higher or lower, birds in general responded
number of notes present in their songs. This was less to these than to normal songs. In particular,
accomplished in a similar manner in each experi- birds were less likely to respond with social
ment; individual male chickadees were isolated vocalizations when song was played outside the
from other birds for a period of time, followed by typical absolute frequency range.
presentations of various versions of altered song Relative pitch-altered songs also lead to a differ-
(e.g. only the fee, bee-fee, bee-bee, fee-bee-bee, etc.). ential response when presented to male chickadees,
These experiments returned several interesting but not in the same manner as those altered in
results. Ratcliffe and Weisman (1986) found that absolute pitch. When the relative pitch cues were
birds discriminated between fee and bee notes and altered outside the species-typical range, birds
also responded less to songs that were either artifi- again responded less to altered songs than to
cially lengthened or shortened. Ratcliffe and normal songs, but whereas birds responded with
Weisman (1988) extended these results by showing less social vocalizations when presented with
that birds responded more slowly to songs that con- absolute pitch-altered songs, birds responded less
tained an extra note at the beginning, such as fee-fee- aggressively when presented with relative pitch-
bee, but not when an extra note was added to the altered songs. Therefore, not only were black-
end, such as fee-bee-bee. These latter results implied capped chickadees processing both absolute and
that birds were in fact sensitive not only to the relative pitch information in their songs, but they
duration of their songs, but also to the temporal were doing so in a way that led to different classes
locations of the two different notes in their songs. of behavioral responses depending on the type and
Taken together, it became clear that chickadees magnitude of frequency deviations.
perceived rather strict rules in their seemingly Based on the results of previous bioacoustic
simple songs, with pitch contour playing an impor- analyses and playback studies on pitch ratio
tant role. perception (discussed above), Shackleton et al.
While these experiments dealt with issues of note (1992) investigated a more subtle pitch change in
type and number discrimination, along with pitch the chickadee song, the small but reliable pitch
contour perception, they did not deal specifically change of about 200 Hz over their approximately
with the role of absolute or relative pitch cues 400 ms duration of the fee note, the glissando
in black-capped chickadee song. Two separate (Fig. 10.1). Testing whether the glissando was either
VOCAL PRODUCTION AND PERCEPTION 157

perceptible or meaningful to the birds in species whether the initial portion of the fee note would be
recognition poses a slight methodological chal- differentially degraded compared with the rest of
lenge. This must be done by creating or finding a fee the fee note. If this were the case, the behavioral
note with little or no pitch change over the course responses observed by Shackleton et al. could be
of the note. This problem was overcome through accounted for by distance estimation. If not, the
the use of synthetic songs of three varieties. Normal conclusion was likely that birds used the pitch ratio
songs had a typical pitch change from the start to in the fee note as a species marker. Bioacoustic
the end of the fee (i.e. a glissando) and from the end analyses, combined with playback results indicat-
of the fee to the start of the bee. The alternative ing that chickadees did not respond differentially to
songs were of two types: one started and ended degraded and non-degraded songs when equalized
on the same pitch as normal fee notes started, with for playback amplitude, were unequivocal; the
no pitch change across the note (i.e. no glissando) glissando in the fee note appears to serve, as does
while the other type started on the pitch of the end the pitch ratio between the fee end and bee start, as
of normal fee notes, also with no pitch change a species marker in black-capped chickadee songs.
across the note. The result was not only a compari- In addition to the pitch change in the fee note and
son of the effect of pitch changes in the fee note, but the pitch ratio between fee and bee notes being
was also a test of the consequence of the manipula- species markers, more recent research has indicated
tions. The pitch ratio between the two notes was a number of other key findings with respect to
also tested, with the first alternative song having other aspects of signaling. First, not only are the
a larger than normal ratio and the second having a fine acoustic features in song relatively unaffected
normal ratio. by distance (Christie et al. 2004a), as was seen for
Altering the pitch ratio of the fee note had a the glissando, but there are individually identifi-
significant effect on the birds behavior. Birds were able features within the song that remain even after
significantly less likely to approach the speaker or degradation over distance. Moreover, the ability of
to emit vocalizations upon hearing either of the two a particular male to produce a reliable pitch ratio is
altered versions of song. Therefore, even subtle related to male quality, and can be used by females
deviations in the relative pitch structure of chicka- to choose a high quality mate (Christie et al. 2004b).
dee song had serious implications for behavioral
responses. The question that then arose from this 10.2.2.2 Operant conditioning experiments of
work was what role in particular did this small but song perception
reliable pitch drop in the fee note play in species While field studies are essential to understanding
recognition? One possibility was that this was used the puzzle that is chickadee communication, they
as a cue for distance estimation, as the higher can not ask or answer all research questions.
frequencies at the start of the fee note would be Furthermore, it is not always feasible or even
expected to degrade more rapidly over distance possible to test some hypotheses through the use of
than the lower pitched remainder of the note. Thus, field experimentation. For example examining
the pitch change would conceivably become muted details of the fundamental perceptual grouping
at longer distances, an idea that was tested directly mechanisms underlying song perception
by Fotheringham and Ratcliffe (1995). and species-recognition, or the comparative percep-
Fotheringham and Ratcliffe (1995) conducted a tion of signals by birds raised under different
two-part field study that included both a transmis- developmental conditions, are best conducted via
sion/recording component and a playback compo- laboratory-based, operant conditioning techniques.
nent. Black-capped chickadee songs were broadcast Results of playback studies indicated that
in both open and forested environments and then chickadees were attentive to both absolute and
re-recorded at a number of distances. Various relative pitch cues in their songs, but the types of
acoustic measures were then taken to estimate the responses to these cues varied. To assess song
effect that propagation over distance had on song perception on a more detailed and controlled level,
features. Specifically, they were investigating Weary and Weisman (1991) conducted an operant
158 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

discrimination experiment to determine the role went further stillif birds were actually using the
of relative pitch cues in the discrimination and pitch ratio as a mechanism of determining whether
generalization of chickadee song. a song is produced by a black-capped chickadee or
In the first of two experiments, stimulus songs not, then it stands to reason that this task should be
were constructed from a sample of natural songs. accomplished quickly. One way that this could be
Positive, or food-rewarded songs, were those that done is if birds treated pitch relations between
had a near-normal pitch ratio of 1.12. Negative, or notes as perceptual categories. To determine
non-food-rewarded songs, were those with a larger whether this was indeed the case, Njegovan and
or smaller than normal pitch ratio. Once training Weisman used a pseudo category discrimination
was complete and male chickadees showed signifi- where rewarded stimuli were selected at random
cant learning and steady-state discrimination and had no common pitch relationships (i.e. the
performance, they were tested with novel songs. rewarded stimuli were not united by a common
These songs were composed in the same manner as 1.12 frequency ratio). In this way they could
training songs, with some having normal and determine whether birds were treating pitch ratios
others having abnormal intervals. Surprisingly, as perceptual categories. If they were, the true
birds did not transfer their training neatly to those category discrimination would be performed much
novel songs with normal pitch ratios. Instead, there more rapidly. If not, there would be no difference
appeared to be a strong absolute pitch component between the two discriminations.
to the discrimination, with birds allocating more Njegovan and Weisman also tested black-capped
responses to test songs that had similar starting chickadees that had been hand-reared in the
pitches to rewarded training songs. The fact that absence of song stimuli on this discrimination.
birds attended to absolute pitch in their songs was Since at least the 1950s and the pioneering work of
not entirely surprising, as previous studies had Thorpe and Marler (e.g. Thorpe 1958; Marler, 1970),
shown that it did indeed have a role in song it had been clear that young songbirds needed to
perception. What was surprising was that birds hear an adult conspecific if they were to develop
attended to this cue even when they were explicitly normal song. This has been shown in a variety of
trained to attend to relative pitch. A follow-up species, including black-capped chickadees
experiment was carried out that, instead of using (Shackleton and Ratcliffe 1993). One of the main
natural songs as discriminative stimuli, used failures in hand-reared black-capped chickadee
wholly synthetic songs. The results of this study song is the lack of normal relative pitch cues seen
were more in line with expectationsbirds did in reliably in the songs of normal, wild-caught birds.
fact attend to relative pitch cues, but there was still The logic, therefore, for including hand-reared
a strong influence of absolute pitch in their results. birds in the frequency ratio discrimination was sim-
Because of the influence of absolute pitch cues in ple: birds that had not learned to produce or been
the perception of relative pitch cues seen in Weary exposed to normal relative pitch cues in song may
and Weismans work with natural or synthetic be at a perceptual disadvantage when asked to per-
songs, Njegovan and Weisman (1997) adopted a form a discrimination that is based on this feature.
different approach to further understanding the The results of Njegovan and Weisman were clear
mechanisms of song perception. For their design, cut. Normal birds learned the true category dis-
Njegovan and Weisman used as their stimuli-of- crimination, that is the discrimination where all
choice pure sine wave pairs that approximated the normal tone pairs were rewarded, much more
acoustic parameters of normal songs. In common quickly than the pseudo-category discrimination,
with Weary and Weisman, Njegovan and Weisman where rewarded tone pairs were selected at ran-
trained birds to discriminate between rewarded dom. The implication is that birds can use percep-
tone pairs with a frequency ratio equal to that seen tual grouping mechanisms to quickly ascertain
in the wild (a ratio of 1.12) and non-rewarded tone whether a particular tone pair matches a particular
pairs with one of two ratios not seen in nature relative frequency ratio and then act accordingly. A
(ratios of 1.00 and 1.24). Njegovan and Weisman further implication, of course, is that they could use
VOCAL PRODUCTION AND PERCEPTION 159

just such a mechanism in the wild to quickly and acoustic discrimination tasks, that songbirds need
efficiently determine whether a song is produced exposure to adults during development not only to
by a black-capped chickadee or not. learn to produce species-typical song, but also to
The second main finding of Njegovan and perceive song accurately.
Weisman was also very clear. Birds that were hand-
reared in the laboratory, without exposure to nor-
mal song, did not perform nearly as well as 10.3 Call production and perception
wild-caught birds on the true category discrimin-
10.3.1 Call production
ation. In fact, even when given significantly more
training than normal birds, hand-reared chickadees Of all chickadee vocalizations, probably their most
could not even approximate performance by nor- well known is the species chick-a-dee call (Fig. 10.2).
mal birds. This finding raised the possibility, which This call, especially in black-capped chickadees, has
has been confirmed in other species with other been exceedingly well studied in a formal sense

(a)
12

A A A A A B D D D D
Frequency (kHz)

0
0 1.0 2.0
Time (sec)

(b)
12
B B C C D D D D D D
Frequency (kHz)

0
0 1.0 2.0
Time (sec)

Figure 10.2 Two representative chick-a-dee calls with note types AAAAABDDDD (a) and BBCCDDDDDD (b). Spectrogram settings: Hanning
window, 512 points. Time (sec) is on the x axis and frequency (kHz) is on the y axis in both spectrograms.
160 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

since about the early to mid 1970s. A description of within about 1 week. Taken together, these results
the call and its usage by Ficken et al. (1978) still gave insight into the mechanisms of call production
stands as a benchmark study. As they and others and a possible acoustic basis of flock recognition.
have noted (e.g. Odum 1942; Saunders 1951), the Several studies of chick-a-dee call production con-
call is produced all year by both sexes and is com- ducted by Jack Hailman and colleagues (e.g. 1985,
posed of four note types; A, B, C, and D. It is used 1986, 1987) have lead to an amazing understanding
as a social signal to co-ordinate flock movements of other aspects of this ubiquitous call, that is the
and to indicate mild alarm. Recently it has been fur- call variety, call syntax, and call note-type compos-
ther demonstrated that the note type composition, ition. One early finding was that the note types of
specifically the number of D notes, of this call which the call is composed seem to form a graded
varies depending on the degree of perceived threat continuum, from A B C D, with As gradually
(Templeton et al. 2005). becoming Bs and so on. Hailman and others also
During the winter months, chickadees form demonstrated that the call follows strict syntactical
flocks of between four and 12 birds that defend a rules, with A notes always preceding B notes,
group foraging territory. One question that arose always preceding C notes, always preceding
from early observational work was by what mech- D notes. This set of syntactical rules, combined with
anism did birds recognize the calls of their flock- the fact that the variety of call types produced is
mates? Classic work by Mammen and Nowicki seemingly endless, makes the chickadee call system
(1981) investigated this issue by conducting an one of the most complicated non-human animal
acoustic analysis of the chick-a-dee call from free- signaling systems ever described. In fact, Hailman
ranging flocks, and comparing these measures and others also proposed that the calls meaning
among flocks. The results of this initial analysis could vary with the note-type composition and the
demonstrated that one of the mechanisms whereby context in which it is used.
birds were able to recognize flock-mates was Other more recent bioacoustic and playback
through flock-specific convergence of their chick-a- studies, many of which are discussed in Chapter 13,
dee calls, specifically via a convergence of D note tested these assumptions derived from research on
acoustics. Mammen and Nowicki then captured black-capped chickadees directly in Carolina
these wild flocks and formed ad hoc artificial flocks chickadees and have shown that Carolina chicka-
in aviaries. After 1 month, call convergence was dees do respond differentially depending on
observed on the same acoustic features posited to note-type composition of calls. These findings,
control flock recognition from the field analyses. combined with those of Nowicki and Mammen dis-
These initial observational and experimental cussed above, along with the more recent findings
studies of call convergence were completed by a of Templeton et al. (2005), clearly point to the fact
final study from Nowicki (1989), who examined in that this is a sophisticated call system that we are
detail both the nature and speed with which these still trying to fully understand. That said, in order
acoustic changes occurred in wholly artificial for any of the research on call note composition etc.
flocks. This final study allowed for analysis of the to be reliable, one must be able to confidently
precise acoustic featuresnamely the first and sec- assign the notes produced in this call into types or
ond frequency components in D notes, and, to a classes. This problem of defining natural categories
lesser degree, the total frequency bandwidth of has been around for as long as acoustic signals have
D notesthat control flock identity. Moreover, been studied scientifically, and has been the topic of
Nowicki showed that no one bird controlled the much debate (see Marler, 1982 and Hauser, 1996 for
convergence and that the pattern of convergence extensive discussions of this problem). It is to this
observed was not determined by dominance rela- area, with specific reference to the chick-a-dee call
tionships (i.e. the flock did not converge on the that we now turn.
acoustic structure of the dominant bird). Rather, One of the first steps in analyzing any vocaliza-
birds modify the acoustic properties of their tion is reducing the chaos that naturally occurs in
D notes to converge on a common mean and did so biological signals into workable units. This
VOCAL PRODUCTION AND PERCEPTION 161

reductionism requires sorting large numbers of some clear winners emerged. Specifically, starting
events into a few manageable types. For the black- frequency and the ascending frequency modulation
capped chickadee call this was relatively straight of notes was found to be the most distinctive
forward. Ficken et al. (1978) labeled four obvious among all note types, and therefore likely to play an
notes types, based on their distinct acoustic important role in note-type discrimination and
structures, A through D. classification. Just how much of a role would be
Nowicki and Nelson (1990) revisited the note- determined in an operant discrimination task, as
type classification in the chickadee call by contrast- described in the following section.
ing several classification methods in an attempt
to determine the reliability, and to some degree
10.3.2 Call perception
the validity, of this classification scheme.
Multidimensional scaling, k-means cluster analysis As well as a significant body of work on call
and visual classification techniques lead to reliable production, there is a growing understanding of
and converging classifications. Recent work from call perception, both from playback studies
our laboratory (Dawson et al. 2006) has found simi- and operant conditioning experiments. Playback
lar results using both artificial neural networks and experiments have been aimed at a variety of issues,
linear discriminant analyses. An open question that including identification of species and flock
numerous researchers, including Hailman and through the chickadee call. Operant conditioning
Nowicki and Nelson, have pointed out is that such experiments have been aimed at related topics of
human-based classifications need to be verified by inquiry including species classification and dis-
the animals that produce the vocalizations being crimination of different species chick-a-dee calls,
classified. Just such studies are described in the note-type categorization, and the mechanisms of
following section on call perception. note-type discrimination. We will deal with
Charrier et al. (2004) set out to conduct a similar research from field playback designs first, followed
analysis of the chick-a-dee call of the black-capped by research from operant conditioning designs.
chickadee with three main goals. The first goal was
to provide a detailed acoustical account of the call 10.3.2.1 Playback studies of call perception
and its note types. The second goal was to deter- Acoustic analyses of field and laboratory recordings
mine which features, in which note types, had (Mammen and Nowicki 1981; Nowicki 1989, dis-
potential individual markers. The third goal was to cussed above) discovered acoustic features that
determine the acoustic features that were respon- could potentially identify flocks on the basis of their
sible for distinguishing among the various note chick-a-dee calls. In an experimental test of flock
types. identification, Nowicki (1983) examined whether
When Charrier et al. (2004) analyzed the call birds discriminate their own flocks calls from those
notes for individualized features, several of the of others, and also whether they respond aggres-
acoustic features had the potential to identify indi- sively to non-flock-mates calls. To investigate these
vidual birds. That is, the between-bird variability questions, Nowicki broadcast resident and foreign
on several features was larger than the within-bird flocks calls to resident chickadee flocks while they
variability, making it theoretically possible for birds were foraging. The results were unambiguous;
to identify individuals on the basis of these acoustic when chickadees heard playbacks of their own
features. Of the four note types, C notes had the flocks chick-a-dee calls, they continued to forage and
greatest potential for individual identity; support did not engage in any calling behavior above base-
for the special status of C notes was observed by line levels. Alternatively, playbacks of foreign calls
Freeberg and Lucas (2002) who demonstrated led to significant decreases in foraging behavior and
clearly that Carolina chickadees responded differ- significant increases in chickadee calling. This study
entially to calls with or without C notes. provides the functional basis for call convergence
Interestingly, when the features that could be and clearly demonstrates that chickadees are not only
used for note-type identification were examined, attending to the macroacoustical characteristics
162 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

in their calls, but also the microacoustical to parallel those documented for species recognition
characteristics. via song, with some important caveats that will be
While the studies of Nowicki and Mammen the subject of future research.
described above clearly showed how chickadees
react to a foreign flocks calls, an unresolved ques- 10.3.2.2 Operant conditioning studies of call
tion remained: what is a chick-a-dee call? Put perception
another way, what particular acoustic features are While research on call perception in the field has
chickadees attending to that allow them to identify dealt mainly with large-scale issues of flock and
the call as species specific? As obvious as this ques- species recognition, research in the laboratory has
tion seemingly is, it had not been tackled until the ability and control to study finer-scale issues of
recently (Charrier and Sturdy 2005). In contrast to the mechanisms of call perception (but see Clucas
the fee-bee song that was more easily paramatized et al. 2004 and Chapter 13 for a recent change in this
and the putative relevant features identified and trend). In particular, one of the outstanding issues
tested, the chick-a-dee call is highly complex with in the area of chickadee call perception is the
multiple parameters that may function in or aid classification of call notes into types. Typically, the
species recognition. Based on a detailed bioacoustic classification is done by humans and is based
analysis already described (Charrier et al. 2004), either on visual sorting of sound spectrograms or
Charrier and Sturdy set out to test the limits of analyses of note features taken from sound
species recognition via chick-a-dee calls using a field spectrogram measurements. However, whether the
playback design. birds that produce these calls perceive the same
To accomplish this, several stimuli were con- note types as humans and statistical techniques
structed, including broadband noise signals, pitch needs to be verified by testing the birds that
shifted calls, and calls in which the tempo had been produce the vocalizations that are classified.
increased or decreased. Several signals were To begin to answer this question, Sturdy et al.
responded to significantly less compared to normal (2000) trained black-capped chickadees to discrimin-
chickadee calls, including the negative control gray- ate among their call-note categories in an operant
crowned rosy-finch (Leucosticte tephrocotis) calls, syn- discrimination task. The goals of this task were
thetic stimuli, and manipulated natural calls, white twofold. The first goal was to determine whether
noise calls, calls that were delivered at a slower the birds perceived the note-type categories of
tempo than normal, calls that were time reversed, as bioacousticians as such. The second related goal
well as those in which the typical syntax was was to determine the mechanism behind call-note
reversed. Moreover, in common with research con- perception. Earlier work (Sturdy et al. 1999a), also
ducted on song, birds also responded less vigorously using operant conditioning techniques, found that
to chick-a-dee calls that were shifted up and down in zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata) perception of
frequency. Interestingly, the responsiveness to fre- their song notes agreed with human-based classifi-
quency-shifted calls was not symmetrical, with those cation methods of these notes (Sturdy et al. 1999b).
shifted up by either 500 Hz or 1000 Hz or shifted It also showed, through the use of transfer tests,
down by 500 Hz not treated differently than normal that zebra finches perceived their song notes as
calls, whereas or those shifted down by 1000 Hz natural, open-ended categories. This suggested that
were treated as different from normal calls. Another songbirds use this fundamental perceptual mecha-
unexpected finding was that calls in which the nism of treating similar but discriminable auditory
amplitude modulation was removed elicited less objects as classes, to perceive the building blocks of
response than normal calls, suggesting that in con- their vocalizations.
trast to many other species tested, amplitude modu- Black-capped chickadees agree with human
lation may play a role in species recognition in classification and use open-ended categorization to
black-capped chickadees. Taken together, the perceive their call-note types. Birds learned to
acoustic properties that appear to control species discriminate among the four call-note types, and
recognition in chickadee calls appear in many ways along the way, provided clues to support the
VOCAL PRODUCTION AND PERCEPTION 163

open-ended categorization hypothesis. First, birds results of the test sessions were striking. Birds
were slower to learn the within-category discrimin- altered their response based on the pitch of the note;
ation than the between-category discrimination. when A notes were reduced in pitch and therefore
This provides clear evidence that note types within made more B-note like, birds responded to them as
a class (defined by humans) were more similar than if they were B notes, and vice versa for B notes
those among classes. Second, birds showed strong shifted up in pitch to be made more A-note like. The
transfer of inhibition (i.e. generalization) from same was true for B versus C notes. Furthermore,
unrewarded, between-category note types used in birds could generally maintain their discrimination
discrimination training to novel notes from these using only the ascending portion of the note
same note categories during reversal training; this whereas the descending portion of the notes was
in spite of the fact that these note types were now not sufficient to maintain discrimination perform-
rewarded (i.e. the contingencies were reversed ance. Whether, and to what extent, other acoustic
relative to during discrimination training). Finally, features in call notes control discrimination and
when birds were exposed to previously unre- categorization remains to be determined. The
warded notes following a period of retraining with results from Charrier et al. (2005), however, showed
rewarded notes from those same classes, they based a role for frequency and initial frequency modu-
their responding on the contingencies most recently lation in note perception and provide another
in effect. This last evidence provided the strongest example of how a solid understanding of a signal
test of transfer of training and, taken together, these can drive research into the perception of the same.
results suggested that birds did agree with human While laboratory studies are ideal for studying
sorters and used open-ended categorization as a fine-grained problems of vocal perception, more
perceptual mechanism to perceive their note types. holistic questions can also be dealt with quite effect-
Interestingly, an analysis of errors lent support ively. In a series of two operant conditioning
for an earlier observation of Hailman (Hailman experiments, Bloomfield et al. (2003) and L. L.
et al. 1985); he suggested that chickadee call-note Bloomfield and C. B. Sturdy (unpublished data),
types formed a continuum from A D notes, with extended the initial findings of Sturdy et al. (2000)
each note type grading into the next. By inspect- for open-ended classification of call-note types to
ing how birds responded to non-rewarded notes, species classification of chick-a-dee calls. Specifically,
Sturdy et al. (2000) were able to show that in fact Bloomfield and colleagues trained chickadees
birds were more likely to confuse adjacent note (black-capped only, 2003; black-capped and
types (e.g. A with B) than they were non-adjacent mountain chickadees, unpublished data) to dis-
note types (e.g. A with C). But how was this criminate and classify both their own and other
accomplished? What features were birds using to species chick-a-dee calls to determine: first, to what
discriminate among their call-note types? degree birds could perform the task with their own
Bioacoustic analyses by Charrier et al. (2004) iden- and other species calls; and second, whether birds
tified some acoustic features, namely that start fre- treated these higher-level species categories in an
quency and the initial ascending portion of notes, open-ended way as they did for lower-level
that are more variable between note types than note-type categories. Results of these studies
within note types and are therefore likely to con- paralleled those of Sturdy et al. (2000), with clear
tribute to note-type discrimination and classifica- evidence through transfer and propagation tests
tion. To determine the extent to which these features that chickadees classified species based on their
were in play, Charrier et al. (2005) trained birds to chick-a-dee call. Taken together, the results of both
discriminate adjacent note types (e.g. A+/B-, A-/B+; the low-level note-type classification and the high-
see Fig. 10.2 for example note types) and then tested level species classification tasks point to a general
them with novel, altered notes. Test stimuli perceptual mechanism driving discrimination and
consisted of notes that were linearly shifted either classification, much in the same way as relative
up or down in pitch by 0.5 SD steps, or cut notes, pitch perception drove song perception as
with only the first or the second half presented. The described earlier in this chapter.
164 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

individual male into which novel songs can be


10.4 Summary and future directions assigned, highly similar to that observed by
Our understanding of vocal communication in the Phillmore et al. (2002) for black-capped chickadees.
North American chickadees has continued to grow Although the studies just described provide won-
over the last 35 plus years, with more research derful complementary evidence to those conducted
being added each year. This has greatly increased on North American chickadees, there are many tit
our knowledge not only of how chickadees use species that remain to be explored and tested if we
acoustic communication, but has also provided are to come to a more extensive comparative under-
more general insights into communication in song- standing of Parid communication in general. This is
birds and in other animal group as well. One of the not only true of tits, but it is also relevant in North
reasons, we believe, for the success that this field America, where the majority of studies have
has seen is due in large part to the reliance on com- focused on chickadee species and largely ignored
plimentary and integrative studies of vocal produc- titmice, leaving a further void in our understand-
tion and perception using a variety of techniques in ing. Further research into vocal production and
the field and in the laboratory. Much has been perception for chickadees, tits and titmice is sure to
learned about chickadee production and percep- add even more to our understanding of Parid
tion, and, importantly, the growth of this know- communication and perception.
ledge has occurred in tandem. That is, production
research has been followed with research aimed at
Acknowledgements
understanding the perception of features eluci-
dated in the studies on production. CBS was supported by an NSERC Discovery Grant,
This general approach has also been profitable in an Alberta Ingenuity New Faculty Grant, a Canada
the understanding of one of the chickadees Foundation for Innovation New Opportunities
European relatives, the great tit. Weary and Grant, and the University of Alberta through start-
McGregor and colleagues (Falls et al. 1982; up funding and CFI partner funding. LLB was sup-
McGregor and Avery 1986; Weary et al. 1990; Weary ported by an NSERC PGS-B Scholarship, an Alberta
and Krebs 1992; Weary 1989, 1990, 1991), in a series Ingenuity Fund Studentship, and a Walter Johns
of studies of song production and perception, have Scholarship from the University of Alberta. IC was
produced similar insights as those described above supported by a Killam Postdoctoral Fellowship and
for the vocalizations of the black-capped chickadee. an Alberta Ingenuity Fund Fellowship. TTYL was
For example, in common with the results of supported by a Province of Alberta Graduate
Charrier et al. (2005) for note-type perception in Scholarship.
black-capped chickadees, Weary (1990), also using The authors dedicate this chapter to Ron
an operant conditioning task, found that great tits Weisman: mentor, collaborator, and friend.
relied mainly on note frequency, and far less on
other acoustic parameters, when classifying their
References
note types. Also in common with findings for the
individualization of black-capped chickadee fee-bee Bloomfield, L. L., Sturdy, C. B., Phillmore, L. S., and
song and chick-a-dee calls (Christie et al. 2004a; Weisman, R. G. (2003). Open-ended categorization of
Weisman et al. 1990; Charrier et al. 2004), great tits chick-a-dee calls by black-capped chickadees (Poecile atr-
icapilla). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 117, 290301.
produce individualized songs, accomplished in
Catchpole, C. K. and Slater, P. J. B. (1995). Bird Song:
part via the production of individual-specific song
Biological Themes and Variations. Cambridge University
types (Weary et al. 1990). In addition, great tit songs Press, Cambridge, UK.
contain cues to individual recognition, and great Charrier, I. and Sturdy, C. B. (2005). Call-based species-
tits can use the individual voice characteristics recognition in Black-capped chickadees. Behavioral
contained in their songs to recognize individuals Processes, 70, 271281.
(Weary and Krebs 1992). This suggests that great Charrier, I., Bloomfield, L. L., and Sturdy, C. B. (2004).
tits can form an open-ended category for each Note types and coding in parid vocalizations. I: The
VOCAL PRODUCTION AND PERCEPTION 165

chick-a-dee call of the black-capped chickadee (Poecile Hauser, M. D. (1996). The Evolution of Communication. MIT
atricapillus). Canadian Journal of Zoology, 82, 769779. Press, MA.
Charrier, I., Lee, T.-Y., Bloomfield, L. L., and Sturdy, C. B. Mammen, D. L. and Nowicki, S. (1981). Individual differ-
(2005). Acoustic mechanisms of note-type perception in ences and within-flock convergence in chickadee calls.
Black-capped chickadees. Journal of Comparative Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 9, 179186.
Psychology, 119, 371380. Marler, P. R. (1970). A comparative approach to vocal
Christie, P. J., Mennill, D. J., and Ratcliffe, L. M. (2004a). learning: song development in white-crowned spar-
Chickadee song structure is individually distinctive rows. Journal of Comparative Physiological Psychology
over long broadcast distances. Behavior, 141, 101124. Monographs, 71, 125.
Christie, P. J., Mennill, D. J., and Ratcliffe, L. M. (2004b). Marler, P. R. (1982). Avian and primate communication:
Pitch shifts and song structure indicate male quality in The problem of natural categories. Neuroscience and
the dawn chorus of black-capped chickadees. Behavioral Biobehavioral Reviews, 6, 8794.
Ecology and Sociobiology, 55, 341348. Maynard-Smith, J. and Harper, D. (2003). Animal Signals.
Clucas, B. A., Freeberg, T. M., and Lucas, J. R. (2004). Oxford University Press, NY.
Chick-a-dee call syntax, social context, and season affect McGregor, P. K. and Avery, M. I. (1986). The unsung songs
vocal responses of Carolina chickadees (Poecile of great tits (Parus major): Learning neighbours songs
carolinensis). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 57, for discrimination. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology,
187196. 18, 311316.
Dawson, M. R. W., Charrier, I., and Sturdy, C. B. (2006). Njegovan, M. and Weisman, R. G. (1997). Pitch discrim-
Using an artificial neural network to classify note types ination in field- and isolation-reared black-capped
in the chick-a-dee call of the black-capped chickadee chickadees (Parus atricapillus). Journal of Comparative
(Poecile atricapillus). Journal of the Acoustical Society of Psychology, 111, 294301.
America, 119, 31613172. Nowicki, S. (1983). Flock-specific recognition of chickadee
Dwight, J. (1897). The whistled call of Parus atricapillus calls. Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology, 12, 317320.
common to both sexes. Auk, 14, 99. Nowicki, S. (1989). Vocal plasticity in captive black-
Falls, J. B., Krebs, J. R., and McGregor, P. K. (1982). Song capped chickadees: the acoustic basis and rate of call
matching in the great tit (Parus major): the effect of sim- convergence. Animal Behaviour, 37, 6473.
ilarity and familiarity. Animal Behaviour, 30, 9971009. Nowicki, S. and Nelson, D. A. (1990). Defining natural cat-
Ficken, M. S. (1981). What is the song of the black-capped egories in acoustic signals: comparison of three meth-
chickadee? Condor, 83, 384386. ods applied to chick-a-dee call notes. Ethology, 86,
Ficken, M. S., Ficken, R. W., and Witkin, S. R. (1978). Vocal 89101.
repertoire of the black-capped chickadee. Auk, 95, Odum, E. P. (1942). Annual cycle of the black-capped
3448. chickadee-3. Auk, 59, 499531.
Fotheringham, J. R. and Ratcliffe, L. (1995). Song degrad- Phillmore, L. S., Sturdy, C. B., Turyk, M. R., and Weisman,
ation and estimation of acoustic distance in black- R. G. (2002). Discrimination of individual vocalizations
capped chickadees (Parus atricapillus). Canadian Journal by black-capped chickadees (Poecile atricapilla). Animal
of Zoology, 73, 858868. Learning and Behaviour, 30, 4352.
Freeberg, T. M. and Lucas, J. R. (2002). Receivers respond Ratcliffe, L. and Weisman, R. G. (1985). Frequency shift in
differently to chick-a-dee calls varying in note compos- the fee-bee song of the black-capped chickadee. Condor,
ition in Carolina chickadees (Poecile carolinensis). Animal 87, 555556.
Behaviour, 63, 837845. Ratcliffe, L. and Weisman, R.G. (1986). Song sequence
Hailman, J. P. and Ficken, M. S. (1986). Combinatorial ani- discrimination in the black-capped chickadee (Parus
mal communication with computable syntax: chick-a-dee atricapillus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 100,
calling qualifies as language by structural linguists. 361367.
Animal Behaviour, 34, 18991901. Ratcliffe, L. and Weisman, R. G. (1988). Representation of
Hailman, J. P., Ficken, M. S., and Ficken, R. W. (1985). The conspecific song by chickadees: comparisons among
chick-a-dee calls of Parus atricapillus: A recombinant sys- embedded fee-bees. Behavioural Processes, 17, 199203.
tem of animal communication compared with written Saunders, A. A. (1951). A Guide to Bird Songs. Country Life
English. Semiotica, 56, 191224. Press, NY.
Hailman, J. P., Ficken, M. S., and Ficken, R. W. (1987). Shackleton, S. A. and Ratcliffe, L. (1993). Development of
Constraints on the structure of combinatorial chick-a- song in hand-reared black-capped chickadees. Wilson
dee calls. Ethology, 75, 6280. Bulletin, 105, 637644.
166 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

Shackleton, S. A., Ratcliffe, L., and Weary, D. M. (1992). variation in their song? Journal of Comparative
Relative frequency parameters and song recognition in Psychology, 103, 320325.
black-capped chickadees. Condor, 94, 782785. Weary, D. M. (1990). Categorization of song notes in great
Sturdy, C. B, Phillmore, L. S., Price, J. L., and Weisman, tits: which acoustic features are used and why? Animal
R. G. (1999a). Song-note discrimination in zebra finches Behaviour, 39, 450457.
(Taeniopygia guttata): Categories and pseudocategories. Weary, D. M. (1991). How great tits use song-note and
Journal of Comparative Psychology, 113, 204212. whole-song features to categorize their songs. Auk, 108,
Sturdy, C. B., Phillmore, L. S., and Weisman, R. G. (1999b). 187190.
Note types, harmonic structure, and note order in the Weary, D. M. and Krebs, J. R. (1992). Great tits classify
songs of zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata). Journal of songs by individual voice characteristics. Animal
Comparative Psychology, 113, 194203. Behaviour 43, 283287.
Sturdy, C. B., Phillmore, L. S., and Weisman, R. G. (2000). Weary, D. M. and Weisman, R. G. (1991). Operant dis-
Call-note discriminations in black-capped chickadees crimination of frequency and frequency ratio in the
(Poecile atricapillus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, black-capped chickadee (Parus atricapillus). Journal of
114, 357364. Comparative Psychology, 108, 253259.
Templeton, C. N., Green, E., and Davis, K. (2005). Weary, D. M., Norris, K. J., and Falls, J. B. (1990). Song fea-
Allometry of alarm calls: black-capped chickadees tures birds use to identify individuals. Auk, 107, 623625.
encode information about predator size. Science, 308, Weisman, R., Ratcliffe, L., Johnsrude, I., and Hurly, T. A.
19341937. (1990). Absolute and relative pitch production in the
Thorpe, W. H. (1958). The learning of song patterns by song of the black-capped chickadee. Condor, 92,
birds, with special reference to the song of the Chaffinch 118124.
Fringilla coelebs. Ibis, 100, 535570. Weisman, R. G. and Ratcliffe, L. (1989). Absolute and
Weary, D. M. (1989). Categorical perception of bird relative pitch processing in black-capped chickadees,
song: how do great tits (Parus major) perceive temporal Parus atricapillus. Animal Behaviour, 38, 685692.
CHAPTER 11

The gargle call of black-capped


chickadees: ontogeny, acoustic
structure, population patterns,
function, and processes leading
to sharing of call characteristics
Myron C. Baker and David E. Gammon

vocal characters in a local population benefits indi-


11.1 Introduction
viduals (i.e. is adaptive) in their social behavior.
In this chapter, we summarize investigations of a Although potential explanations for the advantages
common vocalization in the repertoire of the black- of sharing vocal signals within populations have
capped chickadee. This vocalization, known as the been treated for territorial songs in a general way
gargle call (Ficken et al. 1978; called the dominance (Catchpole and Slater 1995), and considered in
note by Odum 1942) has an ontogeny that is depth for songs of particular species (e.g. Searcy
dependent upon learning events occurring from and Nowicki 1999), few studies have examined non-
early life through adulthood. As is the case for the song calls in this regard (but see Nowicki 1983).
territorial songs of a great many species of song- The gargle call together with the chick-a-dee call
birds (Marler 1997), social interactions most likely and the fee-bee song constitute the great majority of
play a significant role in gargle call ontogeny. Thus, loud chickadee vocal signals one normally encoun-
these calls evidently result from socially biased ters in the field, and they are referred to as the
individual learning (Galef 1996), which we will major vocalizations of the species (Hailman 1989).
simply term social learning in this chapter. These and other vocalizations in the repertoires of
Although vocal plasticity tends to decrease during species in the family Paridae, including Poecile, have
ontogeny, the continuing effect of social learning is previously been examined from a comparative
that members of a local population often come to standpoint (Hailman 1989; Hailman and Ficken
share, in considerable detail, the acoustic character- 1996), and similarities and homologies across
istics of their gargle signals. Such convergence of sig- species have been well described. Hailman (1989),
nal properties in populations results in geographic in particular, provides a comprehensive treatment
or microgeographic variation, or dialects of the form and structures of the vocal signal rep-
(Mundinger 1982). Whereas such differences in ertoires of a number of members of the Paridae.
vocal patterns from one geographic locale to In examining the gargle call, we give a brief
another are relatively easy to describe, uncovering description of the call, provide information on the
the social and developmental processes that gener- ontogeny of the call as it occurs in natural popula-
ate and maintain local convergence and geographic tions, describe the fundamental features of the call,
differences is more difficult. An even more tren- the nature and extent of sharing of calls by birds in
chant problem is to determine how the sharing of a local area, and the similarities and changes in calls

167
168 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

across years. Additionally, we report observational representative set of juvenile sub-gargles at the age
and experimental evidence on the roles of the calls of juvenile dispersal. In this follow-up study, the
in the social lives of the birds, and draw inferences juveniles were not color-banded. As studies of
about the ways social behavior selects for vocal banded birds have shown, family break-up and
convergence among birds in a local area, and the dispersal occur suddenly in chickadees, literally
possible functional significance of convergence. from one day to the next, with the abrupt dis-
appearance of young from a family group and a
corresponding influx of many immigrant juveniles
11.2 The gargle (Smith 1991). That an immigrant we recorded was a
11.2.1 Brief description of the call juvenile and not an adult was evident by the nature
of the vocalization delivery. Juveniles gargle with
The gargle call (Fig. 11.1a) was named in early low amplitude, recombinant syntax, and lack
descriptions of the vocal repertoire of the black- frequency control during production of notes, all of
capped chickadee because of its noisy complexity which are rare in adult gargle episodes (Gammon
(Ficken et al. 1978). Given primarily by males, a call et al. 2005). For our recordings, a given site was
lasts about half a second and consists of several visited daily or every second day, allowing deter-
distinct syllables. An individual may have a reper- mination within one day of the date of natal
toire of three to 18 different gargle call types dispersal from a family group, as well as allowing
(mean 78; Baker et al. 2000; Box 11.1). us to recognize the occurrence of juveniles not
found previously at that location. Our recordings
were therefore taken from juveniles in family
11.2.2 Ontogeny of the call
groups in the 2 days before juvenile dispersal (fam-
Studies of color-banded birds revealed that gargle- ily group dissolution), and from new juveniles the
like syllables begin to emerge in fledglings around first day they were observed at the site.
30 to 35 days of age when the young are still in fam- Examination of sub-gargles from a representative
ily groups together with parents and siblings fledgling still in the family group indicates that
(Baker et al. 2003). The precursor gargle calls are these vocalizations are produced in a variety of
produced in strings of somewhat amorphous forms but lack the sharply articulated structure of
sounds of low amplitude, with the duration of such adult gargle syllables (Fig. 11.1b). Sub-gargles
strings lasting up to a minute or more. The common produced at this predispersal stage of ontogeny not
context for these utterances is while the individual only are poorly formed in comparison to adult
is perched in one place, and apparently not inter- gargles, but they also lack stereotypy when repeti-
acting with another bird. The acoustic structures tion is attempted. Sub-gargle syllables are produced
produced in these rambling strings of elements are in nearly continuous strings with little evidence
comparable to vocalizations often referred to as of partitioning into sets of syllables of typical gargle
sub-song in the song learning literature (Marler and call duration (Fig. 11.1b). In some recordings,
Peters 1982a). We referred previously to these however, we found sub-gargles delivered in tem-
vocalizations as sub-song (Baker et al. 2003), and poral patterns typical of gargle calling in adults
here we denote the gargle-like components of sub- (Fig. 11.1c) even though the constituent sounds
song as sub-gargles to emphasize their uncrystal- remain amorphous compared to adult gargle
lized condition. In our first attempt to identify syllables. During sub-song, elements of the vocal
sub-gargles as matching adult gargle syllables repertoire other than sub-gargles often are included,
characteristic of the local breeding area (Baker et al. elements such as the A-notes of the chick-a-dee
2003), we were able to find only one or two juvenile call (Hailman et al. 1985), entire chick-a-dee calls, high
sub-gargle syllables that were reasonably close zee notes (Ficken and Witkin 1977), and fee-bee songs
approximations to adult syllables. (Ficken et al. 1978; Fig. 11.2ac).
Since that first attempt to monitor gargle devel- For one set of juvenile sub-gargles, recorded from
opment in young chickadees, we have recorded a four to six individuals while their family groups
THE GARGLE CALL 169

(a)
10

(b)
10

6
Frequency (kHz)

10

(c)
10

10

1s

Figure 11.1 Gargle calls of black-capped chickadees. (a) Three gargle types from the repertoire of an adult male. We use spectral
derivative displays (upper three panels) to illustrate chickadee sounds described in this report. Spectral derivatives are changes in power across
time and frequency axes of a sound. Visually, these displays are similar to traditional sonograms (lower three panels are sonograms of the
same calls) but often show more detail. More importantly, spectral derivatives are the analysis framework from which are derived, by
automated techniques, a number of acoustic features that we use for quantitative characterization and similarity measurements of sounds
(Tchernichovski et al. 2000). (b) Representative sub-gargle vocalizations recorded from a fledgling while still in the family group prior to
dispersal. (c) Representative sub-gargles recorded in early July from a hatching year individual showing tendency toward temporal
partitioning into clusters of sub-gargle elements.
170 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

Box 11.1 Repertoire sizes

Cumulative (saturation) plots of the number of etc.) samples in the second time period may
types of gargle calls versus the number of samples reveal new call types, or may show that call
recorded were used to establish both individual types formerly present are not now found.
repertoire sizes and population repertoires. New call types could be ones that have been
Individual repertoire size was estimated from developed during the interim or ones that
the plateau of the cumulative plot as in this were previously in the repertoire but either
illustration. For estimates of individual repertoires, unexpressed or used so rarely that they did not
we used birds for which at least 500 calls were show up in the sample. Similarly, call types that
recorded. A population repertoire refers to all appear to have been dropped from a repertoire
the kinds of gargle calls found in a sample of may have simply been suppressed to a very
birds in a local area. Because of the large extent low level of use. These caveats apply to any
of sharing of call types among birds in a local study of repertoire size.
area, we found that recordings of five to six birds
were usually sufficient to capture all the call types
of the population, as revealed by saturation plots 10
(examples in Baker et al. 2000). Across three Cumulative no. gargle types
populations and eight seasons the number of 8
birds recorded each season in each population
averaged 9.1. One cannot be certain to have 6
uncovered the entire repertoire of call types
(whether of an individual or a population) despite 4
a substantial plateau in the cumulative plot.
Apparent saturation in such plots can provide 2
reasonable confidence in the estimate, but it
remains an estimate nevertheless. 0
In comparing the repertoire of an individual 0 200 400 600 800 1000
across two time periods (e.g. year 1 vs. year 2, Number of calls recorded

were still together, we also obtained a complete set it is also apparent that such juveniles have not yet
of recordings of all adult gargle calls of the same produced adult-quality gargles.
area. This recording site was one of three popula- Our evidence from juvenile sub-gargles,
tions that we studied for eight successive seasons, produced in the few days immediately pre- and
and we had obtained extensive samples of calls postdispersal, suggests juveniles have developed a
there (see Box 11.1). Some fledgling syllables were large variety of gargle-like sounds, can produce
similar to those of adult birds in the area, but there them in a temporal pattern approximating that of
were no apparent matches of whole gargle calls. In adult gargle calling, and express the primitive
the days following family break up, gargle compo- gargle calls as a repertoire of call types.
nents of immigrant young begin to emerge in a Unfortunately, we lack recordings of known-age
more recognizable form. In one especially well- juveniles from late July to early September, an
recorded individual (Fig. 11.3a), we identified important period in development. During the
approximately five different gargle types being north temperate autumn, the juveniles become
expressed as a repertoire, although the lack of established in a local area to which they may
stereotypy makes the estimate uncertain. This have emigrated during dispersal. This is the time
tendency toward the emergence of recognizable when winter flocks are formed, and juveniles
gargle types suggests a later stage of ontogeny, but become members of such flocks (Smith 1991). It is
THE GARGLE CALL 171

(a) 10
Sub-gargle notes

6
High zees

1s

Chick-a-dee call

D-notes
Frequency (kHz)

Introductory notes

(b) 10
Sub-gargle notes B-notes

6 Fee-bee

1s

(c) 10
Sub-gargle & B-notes
6 Fee-bee

1s

Figure 11.2 Sub-song of juvenile black-capped chickadees including: (a) sounds representing sub-gargles, followed by high zee predator
alarm notes grading into a chick-a-dee call with poorly-formed introductory A-, B-, C-notes but well-developed D-notes; (b) sub-gargles followed
by renditions of B-notes and ending with fee-bee song; and (c) sub-gargle notes, B-notes, and fee-bee song.

instructive to compare the quality of juvenile


11.3 Features of the call and population
gargles recorded in mid-September with those
patterns
sub-gargles of mid-July (Fig. 11.3b). In those two
months the sub-gargle calls have developed into We recorded gargle calls in our three study popula-
well-articulated gargle calls with a high degree tions for eight successive years. All captured individ-
of stereotypy. Measurements of the quality uals were banded. Our sample consists of 215
(Wiener entropy: Tchernichovski et al. 2000) of chickadees (some of these were recaptures, usually in
these signals show a marked improvement over the season following first capture) from which we
those calls recorded at dispersal age (M. C. Baker recorded 123 707 gargle calls (average 575 per bird).
and D. E. Gammon, unpublished data). Once the Capture, recording methods, spectrographic ana-
over-wintering flocks have been formed, the gargle lyses, and syllable and gargle classification
call repertoires in a local population can be procedures are detailed elsewhere (Baker et al. 2000).
examined. Briefly, each captive chickadee was stimulated to
172 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

(a) 10
1 2 3 first

10
4 5 3 second

2
Frequency (kHz)

0.5s

(b) 10
1 2 3 first

10
4 5 3 second

Figure 11.3 Juvenile gargle calls. (a) Repertoire of five different gargle calls uttered by an individual soon after natal dispersal (July) and
exhibiting lack of stereotypy (compare first and second attempts of the same gargle type; 26 other calls were uttered before the recurrence of
this similar call). (b) Repertoire of a different individual juvenile 2 months later (September), well after dispersal and establishment in a winter flock
(compare stereotypy of first and second utterances of the same gargle type; 48 other calls were uttered before the recurrence of this similar call).

vocalize its repertoire of gargle calls by viewing its size of the syllable pool for our three main study
mirror image (Censky and Ficken 1982). This evokes populations is approximately 50 to 60 (Baker et al.
an aggressive response including postures and gargle 2000). Many of the syllables are rapid frequency
calls similar to the visual and vocal displays observed sweeps. Some syllables are buzzy, broadband
when two birds, strangers to one another, interact in sounds, and these tend to be at the ends of calls.
close proximity. These recordings show that many of Calls often decline in mean frequency over their
the gargle types in the repertoires of two or more birds brief half-second duration. The rapid frequency
from the same area are shared (Fig. 11.4a, see also modulation of gargle syllables makes them propa-
Baker et al. 2000). The similarity of shared gargle types gate poorly through the environment, and in their
contrasts with the differences in gargle types within a use they are mostly near-distance signals. The
given birds repertoire (Fig. 11.4b). structural properties of gargle calls were described
Gargle calls constituting the repertoires of indi- in detail in previous studies (Ficken and Weise
viduals in a local population are apparently 1984; Hailman et al. 1987; Hailman and Griswold
assembled from a common pool of syllables. The 1996; Baker et al. 2000).
THE GARGLE CALL 173

(a) 10

2
10

(b) 10

6
Frequency (kHz)

10

(c) 10

2
10

0.5s

Figure 11.4 Gargle calls of adult black-capped chickadees showing: (a) the same gargle type produced by six different birds in the same
population; (b) a repertoire of six gargle types produced by one bird from the same population as individuals in (a) (this comparison indicates
that differences between gargle types within a birds repertoire are much larger that differences between birds within a gargle type); and (c)
three gargle calls vocalized by three different birds (upper row) in the 1996/97 season and the form of the same calls uttered by three other birds
from the same population in the 2003/04, the eighth successive year these calls were present in the population.

Because there is considerable sharing of gargle 2000). In one analysis covering two years, we
syllables among gargle types within and between described three adjacent populations with a total of
birds in a local area, and whole gargle types often 56 different kinds of gargle syllables from which the
are shared among birds, it is possible to examine birds constructed an average of 7.6 gargle types as
the individual or population from the standpoint of their repertoires (Baker et al. 2000). Gargle call shar-
the entire syllable repertoire as well as from the ing and syllable sharing are greater within a local
standpoint of the gargle call repertoire (Baker et al. population than between adjacent populations,
174 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

supporting the view that these birds exhibit local significantly larger repertoires than old chickadees
dialects in this call (Ficken and Weise 1984; (young: n 32, mean 9.22, SE 0.55; old:
Miyasato and Baker 1999a; Baker et al. 2000). n 65, mean 7.85, SE 0.37; P 0.03,
MannWhitney U-test; Siegel 1956). These findings
raise questions about the possible social influences
11.3.1 Persistence and change in gargle
that may cause the observed patterns. We address
calls over time
these questions by providing background on the
In one of our populations, 63% of the gargle types ways gargle calls are used and describe several
found in the first year were present 8 years later. In the experiments and observations that provide insight.
intervening years, a low level of turnover in call types
occurred each season. Examples of gargle types first
11.4 Contexts of use of the call
recorded in 1996/97 and their representative form
in 2003/04 shows little change (Fig. 11.4c). From The gargle call was referred to in earlier literature as
trapping efforts in each population each season, we the fighting call (Dixon and Stefanski 1970) or the
know that no individual birds persisted for this time supplanting call (Dixon et al. 1970). It tends to be
period. An earlier analysis of gargle persistence also a close-range signal, and it is observed commonly
showed long-term stability in the forms of this call during interactions at food sources in natural
(Ficken and Popp 1995). Additional to the evidence of circumstances (Ficken et al. 1987). A similar call is
stable calls across time, some gargle types are lost from produced in similar situations by several other
a population, and new gargles discovered in new species of chickadees and titmice (Smith 1972;
recruits. For the population mentioned above, for McLaren 1976; Hailman 1989; Haftorn 1993;
example, we found five new gargle types in 2003/04 Hailman and Haftorn 1995; McCallum et al. 1999).
not present in 1996/97. A detailed accounting of the The call can be elicited by placing two males, unfa-
time course of gargle call persistence and year-to-year miliar with each other, together in a cage or aviary
changes in population repertoires over eight seasons or in closely adjacent cages. In these circumstances
is in preparation (Baker and Gammon, in press). the birds produce gargle calls as they interact in the
establishment of dominance, with the emerging
dominant bird giving the greatest number of gargles
11.3.2 Year-to-year changes in
(Baker et al. 1991). In the field, with color-banded
repertoires of individuals
birds of known dominance relationships, higher-
In our study populations, during the eight seasons ranking individuals give more gargles than lower
of sampling, 32 birds with sufficient samples of ranking ones (Popp et al. 1990).
gargles (500 or more) were recorded in two succes- When two birds interact at a food source in a
sive seasons. Their average repertoire in the first field setting, the individual that gives gargle calls
season was 8.7 gargle types, and a year later it was wins the contest (Ficken et al. 1987; Popp et al. 1990;
7.6. To accomplish this net loss, an average bird personal observations). A single gargle by a bird
added 1.2 gargle types to its repertoire and dropped arriving at a food supply often is sufficient to drive
2.4 gargle types. Given the changes in repertoires away a bird already at the food, suggesting the call
over successive years, and our observations of signals status. Experimentation developed from
juvenile gargle development, we examined a larger these observations showed that two males,
data set to find out if young birds and older birds unfamiliar with each other, when placed together in
differed in repertoire size. Our field season for a neutral cage quickly established a dominance
capture and recording chickadees was from relationship (Baker et al. 1991). One individual
September to February. Birds hatched the previous usually produced a large number of gargle calls
breeding season (May) we refer to as young birds while attacking and supplanting the other bird, and
(HY/SY; Pyle et al. 1987), whereas birds hatched in the dominant individual was the one to attack first
some earlier season we refer to as old birds and gargle. In later meetings of the two birds, the
(AHY/ASY; Pyle et al. 1987). Young chickadees had mere utterance of gargles by the dominant was able
THE GARGLE CALL 175

to cause the other bird to retreat. Subsequent 11.5 Interactive playback


encounters staged days or weeks later indicated a experiment I
reduction in the frequency of gargle calling during
the encounters. Measurements of wing length, an To see if repertoire alteration could be induced by an
index of size, showed that dominants tended to be aggressive encounter in the short term, we
slightly larger than their subordinate opponents. conducted five trials of interactive playback using
These experiments confirmed that gargling was SingIt software (Bradbury and Vehrencamp 1994).
an aggressive signal employed in fighting and in We captured five birds from a single population and
the establishment of dominance relationships. recorded their repertoires by mirror-image stimula-
Smith (1972), in studies of the Carolina chickadee tion. We selected a gargle type from another bird that
rasp call (equivalent to the gargle call), pointed out differed distinctly from any of the gargle types in the
that confrontations between strangers often subjects repertoire. For a playback trial (2030 min),
occurred in the context of individuals from one we placed a small speaker near the mirror in the
flock coming into contact with intruders of another recording chamber. We let the subject give five gar-
flock. In that context, two antagonists may not be gles and then played three of the stimulus gargles in
familiar with one another or at least have not had succession of a temporal pattern typical of normal
recent fights. In field observations of black-capped gargle delivery. We continued this fivethree bout
chickadees, we have witnessed adjacent territory structure about 24 times for four of the five subjects.
holders in gargling contests at territorial borders The fifth bird grew silent after the ninth bout.
during the breeding season (see also Ficken et al. During these tests, subjects switched call types fre-
1987). In these situations, although obviously quently, averaging four to five calls then switching
agonistic, several meters usually separates the birds, to a differing type in their repertoire. Compared to
and gargling tends to be more prolonged than in our experiences recording birds with the mirror
winter contests at food sources. Longer duels reveal alone when obtaining samples for repertoire estima-
more of the gargle call repertoire of an individual. tion, the playback routine appeared to cause more
Experimentation revealed that in an aviary a frequent switching among call types.
repertoire of three gargle types was more aversive During these playback tests, however, subjects
than a single gargle type when both treatments were did not produce a gargle that matched or even
paired with a video display of a chickadee (Baker approximated the stimulus gargle. The only gargles
et al. 1996). Thus, the degree of threat or the intention produced by a subject were the same as those found
to escalate an encounter could be signaled by display by the mirror-only stimulation during initial
of a larger repertoire. In this experiment, the gargle recording. This suggests that, in a single simulated
stimuli were recorded from the same population as agonistic encounter, stimulation by a gargle type not
that of the subjects. In a different laboratory experi- shared in an opponents repertoire did not reveal
ment (Miyasato and Baker 1999b), gargle calls repre- hidden or unexpressed gargle types that were not
sentative of subjects own local population (familiar, previously recorded, nor is the subject able to
shared) were more aversive than calls representative assemble a new and matching gargle type in
of a distant population (unfamiliar, not shared) and response to the different gargle used as the stimulus.
unfamiliar calls were approached more readily. Evidently, short-term gargling encounters may not
These results suggest that it is adaptive to learn the provide the social circumstances that cause a bird to
gargle calls of the local population. change its repertoire. Pursuing this issue further,
Our further experiments and theoretical consid- we conducted a laboratory aviary study.
erations address the issues of how social inter-
actions might induce the repertoire changes we
11.6 Aviary experiments on
documented in our comparisons of young and old
gargle changes
birds, and how the repertoire is used in ongoing
agonistic interactions. We used several experimen- We designed an experiment to determine if chicka-
tal approaches to these questions. dees alter their gargle call repertoire when in the
176 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

presence of birds from a different population for frequent but decreased as days passed, as expected
several weeks. We simulated the immigration of from our previous study (Baker et al. 1991). In
young males, hatched in the recent summer, into a the first year, the subjects were together in
different population in autumn. In its new location, their aviaries from 28 September through 20
a hatching year bird in the field would join a local October (23 days); in the second year, from 12
resident flock and encounter adult males with October through 26 November (46 days); and in the
gargle call repertoires differing from those of the third year, from 18 October through 29 November
juveniles natal population. Our ontogeny studies (43 days).
suggested that by this time of the autumn season Average values of sharing increased from the
the hatching year juveniles would have dispersed beginning to the end of each aviary trial. The
and be faced with social learning circumstances increased sharing was only marginal in one aviary
that may cause them to conform to the gargle calls in 2001 (from 0.38 initially to 0.39 at the end) and
of their current site. one aviary in 2002 (from 0.58 to 0.61), but in
Two aviary trials were conducted each autumn the remaining four trials it was more substantial
for 3 years (20012003). Two experimental aviaries (0.15 0.41; 0.16 0.25; 0.19 0.38; 0.23 0.44).
were in separate rooms removed from auditory Including all six trials, the change in sharing was
contact with each other. Four of the six aviary significant (T 0, P 0.028, Wilcoxon matched-
groups were established with a young male from pairs signed-test; Siegel 1956). We also examined
one population and two old males from another changes in the amount of repertoire sharing for
population. Two aviary groups were established individuals and found no relationship with age
with a young male from one population and one (U 36, P 0.76, MannWhitney U-test) or dom-
old and one young male from another population. inance rank (Xr 2.48, P 0.38, Friedman
We aged the birds according to the condition ANOVA; Siegel 1956).
(pneumatization) of the skull and characteristics of Increased sharing occurred mostly because
the tail feathers (Pyle et al. 1987). Prior to placement individuals dropped unshared gargle types. Over
into the aviaries, each bird was tape-recorded to all aviary trials, 42 gargle types were dropped
obtain a full account of its gargle call repertoire. from repertoires of the 18 subjects and only 18
During the time the birds were together in an gargle types added. Dropping gargles was more
aviary, we made frequent observations to deter- common in young birds: 26 gargle types were
mine dominance relationships. Following an aviary dropped by eight young birds (3.25/bird) whereas
trial, we tape-recorded the birds using the mirror- only 16 gargle types were dropped by 10 old birds
image procedure to obtain the postexperimental (1.6/bird).
repertoire of gargle calls. We conclude that birds in a prolonged social
To evaluate the extent to which gargle types were group alter their gargle call repertoire in a way that
shared among the birds in an aviary, we calculated leads to increased sharing within the group. We
for each bird an index of sharing, defined as the now ask how such a change in repertoire may
proportion of its gargle types shared (range 01) benefit the individuals involved. Within a popula-
with any other bird in its aviary (Hughes et al. 1998; tion, where individuals have repertoires with
Baker et al. 2000). We calculated the index for each substantial sharing of gargle types between them,
bird before the start of the aviary trial and after an individual possesses the plasticity of expression
completion of the trial, for all 18 birds over the six to match the calls of an opponent during aggressive
aviary trials. Further, we examined the average interactions. In a number of songbird species,
sharing index for the group of three birds before matching songs or matching repertoires is a signal
and after an aviary trial. We also determined if age of an increased level of aggression (Krebs et al. 1981;
or dominance status was correlated with changes in Burt et al. 2001; Vehrencamp 2001; Otter et al. 2002;
the sharing index of the subjects. Within the first Mennill and Ratcliffe 2004). To examine the possible
2 days following the introduction of the birds into benefits in sharing gargle calls with an opponent,
the aviary, we observed a clear linear dominance we conducted an additional interactive playback
hierarchy. In these early encounters gargling was experiment.
THE GARGLE CALL 177

11.7 Interactive playback experiment II stimuli should be less than that given to non-match-
ing stimuli. In the latter case, we assumed that if
We tape-recorded subjects by the mirror-only matching indicates escalation, then matching more
stimulation so that their complete repertoires were quickly would reinforce the signal valence. In gen-
known. We tested six birds in a setup like that used eral, our assumption in all these predictions was that
in Interactive Playback Experiment I. The micro- the subject, in its familiar home cage, will perceive
phone led to a sound spectrograph operating in real the stimulus as a challenge/intrusion and will
time, which allowed experimenters to monitor the respond by escalation of the encounter.
vocal output of the subject. Another experimenter We examined the first 50 occasions when we
operated a computer, with icons on the screen responded to a subjects gargle with a non-matching
representing the gargle types in the repertoire of the gargle. We had complete samples for five birds, and
subject (Bradbury and Vehrencamp 1994). The a sixth bird received only eight instances of non-
gargle calls stored in computer memory, and that matching gargles before it quit vocalizing. The
were triggered as stimuli when an icon was clicked, subjects showed a tendency not to switch, but
were recorded from a different bird in the popula- instead to use the same call they used immediately
tion but were classified as the same gargle types as prior to playback stimulation. Of 258 non-matching
those of the subject. Thus, the experimenter could stimuli delivered to the six subjects, there were only
play either a matching or a non-matching gargle in 38 (15%) instances of switching to another gargle
response to the gargle of the subject, but the gargle type. Of these switches, 31 (82%) were to a non-
would always be within the subjects repertoire. matching gargle type. Thus, our experiment did not
During a trial (30 min) we played matching or obtain evidence that subjects followed a matching
non-matching gargles in response to the subjects strategy, perhaps as an attempt to dominate an
gargles. We alternated throughout a trial by match- opponent or signal an escalation of a contest.
ing the subject five to ten times then used non- We also examined the first 50 occasions of match-
matching replies five to ten times, then matching ing presentations and found little evidence for
again for five to ten times, and so on. Sometimes a switching in five of the six subjects. These five
subject gargled a second time before we could iden- received 250 matching stimuli and there were only
tify the gargle type and respond with a stimulus 11 (4%) cases of switching. The subject that received
presentation. This occurred in 25 to 50% (range) of nine matching stimuli, before it stopped respond-
the gargles delivered by subjects. ing, switched in six of the stimulus presentations.
With this simulation, we addressed three ques- We examined the latency of response to the
tions: (i) When a subject hears a non-matching gargle next gargle given by a subject when stimulated
from the presumed opponent, does it switch to with matching versus non-matching gargles. For
match the stimulus, switch to a non-matching gargle, each of five subjects, we compared 50 values of
or produce the same gargle type it gave prior to the latency following matching gargles to 50 values of
stimulus? (ii) When a subject hears a matching gargle latency following non-matching gargles. One bird
from the opponent, does it respond with the same had only nine values for matching and eight for
gargle it gave prior to stimulation, or does it switch to non-matching gargles. Using the median response
a different gargle type? (iii) Is there a difference in times, following matching versus non-matching
response latency by a subject when it hears a match- gargle stimuli for each bird, we found no significant
ing versus non-matching gargle from the simulated difference for the two conditions (T 5, P 0.249,
opponent? Our hypothesis was that if matching Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed-ranks test).
indicates an escalation of fighting then playing non-
matching stimuli to the subject should cause it to
switch gargles and match the stimulus. Second, when
we played matching gargles to the subject it should
11.8 Combined analysis of interactive
continue to match us and not switch gargle types.
playback tests
Third, we predicted that if matching indicates esca- We examined our data from the 11 interactive
lation, then the latency of response to our matching playback trials to address the question of how inter-
178 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

1.0 nine during the trials, but we include their data


1.96*Std.Error because they were qualitatively similar to the others.
0.9
1.00*Std.Error In every subject, H/Hmax was greater during
Mean interactive playback than during mirror-only
Diversity (H/Hmax)

stimulation (Fig. 11.5), although for some birds the


0.8
differences were not large. The overall result, how-
ever, was statistically significant (T 0, P 0.003,
0.7 Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed-ranks test). The
results suggest that a subject uses more of its
0.6 repertoire, switching among gargle types frequently,
when its opponent is responding with gargle calls.
This pattern of use contrasts with the mirror-only
0.5
stimulation in which the subject tends to repeat a
Playback Mirror only
plus mirror gargle in longer strings of the same type before
switching to another type. High rates of switching
Figure 11.5 Diversity of gargle calls uttered by black-capped among territorial song types in other species of
chickadees stimulated by mirror image and gargle calls during birds, has been shown to signal a more aggressive
interactive playback, compared to stimulation by mirror image only interaction (Kramer and Lemon 1983; Kramer et al.
(no playback). Diversity was greater during interactive playback
(P 0.003).
1985).
A caveat is that our analysis of gargle switching
fails to consider a possible order effect. Mirror-only
recordings were performed first and were followed
active playback influenced a subject, regardless of some days later by the interactive playback tests. To
whether the opponents gargles were present or not achieve the main purpose of the interactive tests,
in the subjects own repertoire. In a natural popula- we had to determine the repertoire of the subjects
tion, a bird is likely to encounter opponents who prior to interactive testing and that precluded a
share some of its gargle calls, and other opponents design in which some birds were first recorded then
that do not. We compared the way that a subject tested and others were tested then recorded. We
distributed its gargle calls when stimulated by the believe, however, that the diversity result is prob-
mirror-only treatment to the way it used its reper- ably not an effect of treatment order. In our aviary
toire when stimulated by its mirror image and experiments, for example, we recorded the subjects
gargle playback. We interpret this as a simulation of by mirror-only before and after the time they were
a bird interacting with an opponent that does not together in the aviary. We randomly selected one
gargle, such as a subordinate male or female, versus bird from each of the six experimental aviaries and
one that interacts vocally during the encounter. calculated its repertoire diversity from the before
We used the ShannonWiener species diversity and after recordings. In five of the six birds, the
index to examine the relative abundance of gargle diversity decreased from the first to the second
calls found in each gargle type category. The diver- recording, and in one bird diversity increased.
sity index is: These data therefore suggest that the increased
diversity expressed in the subjects in the playback
H P logP
i i i
experiments did not result from an order effect.

in which Pi refers to the proportion of gargles of


11.9 Synthesis of theory and
the ith gargle type. To take account of birds with
empirical results
differing repertoire sizes we used H/Hmax for our
comparisons. Hmax is the maximum diversity pos- The pattern of use of gargle calls in year-round
sible for a subject with a given repertoire size. Two of agonistic encounters points to the probable function
the subjects delivered fewer gargles than the other of this vocal signal as aiding in the acquisition and
THE GARGLE CALL 179

maintenance of resources (food, female mate, terri- fight using a diverse array of call types (interactive
tory). From the accumulated knowledge of the playback results), serving as a more aversive signal
behavioral ecology of black-capped chickadees (Baker et al. 1996), while the stranger retreats. The
(Smith 1991) we know that postdispersal hatching residents status and access to resources (food,
year males in the autumn of the northern temperate territory, mate) is thus successfully maintained.
zone are joining, or attempting to join, flocks whose Such interactions place a pressure on the immigrant
members will be associated through the winter. individual to reduce the asymmetry in contests
Gargle calling is a pronounced feature of young with local dominants by altering its gargle signals to
males in the early autumn. Furthermore, the major conform to local patterns, which is accomplished
peak of pair formation is in this autumn period, and by the immigrant by adding or dropping gargle
this time of the annual cycle also sees decreasing types so that its repertoire more closely matches
food supplies and increased competition. We those of residents (aviary experiments).
hypothesize that vocal convergence by young males, It is more likely that gargle types are dropped than
still in the condition of vocal plasticity, on gargle call added (aviary experiments), and young birds may
features of older and dominant residents may achieve matching more easily than older birds
provide benefits to the young birds in: (i) gaining because of the larger repertoire size of young birds
access to flock membership; (ii) competing more (population data) and because of their inherent vocal
effectively for food; and (iii) increasing their mating plasticity (ontogeny data). However, in considering
potential in the context of female choice. Attaining a the potential for alterations of the repertoire in older
high ranking in the winter flock evidently is of birds by adding new gargle types, we note that new
advantage to obtaining a breeding territory in the elements may be added to the territorial song
home range of the winter flock, whereas lower- repertoires of older birds, as described in canaries
ranked pairs may be excluded and frequently are (Serinus canaria; Nottebohm et al. 1986), European
unable to obtain breeding territories (Smith 1991). starlings (Sturnus vulgaris; Hausberger 1997), great
From a consideration of the social context in tits (McGregor and Krebs 1989), and northern
which the gargle call is used, together with the mockingbirds (Mimus polyglottos; Derrickson 1987).
results of the population data, aviary experiments, Therefore, while we think it possible that entirely
and the interactive playback trials, we suggest new calls may be developed and added to the
a theoretical explanation of local population repertoires of older birds, we cannot rule out that a
conformity in gargle call structures and the new call, apparently added to a repertoire, may be
evolution of gargle call repertoires. We imagine an the reinstatement of a call long unused. This general
immigrant chickadee arriving in a new population. model we have outlined here (see also Ficken et al.
Upon gargle calling in aggressive encounters with 1987) to explain local conformity of acoustic signals
residents, its calls announce its status. If the immi- has been applied to territorial songs (Payne et al.
grant is a young chickadee, our data on gargle 1988; Craig and Jenkins 1982) and also in the context
ontogeny suggest its calls will still be in a plastic of mate choice (Rothstein and Fleischer 1987),
stage, and such calls will signal juvenile age and although without the addition or deletion of reper-
subordinate status. Although immigration by toire components as the means by which conformity
second-year or older birds is probably rare, if the is achieved.
immigrant is an older bird from another area and Within a local population of chickadees, there is
with well-formed gargle calls, its different call struc- substantial sharing of gargle calls and syllables
tures (Ficken and Weise 1984; Miyasato and Baker among birds (Baker et al. 2000). In the context of the
1999a; Baker et al. 2000) will advertise its status of a local population, however, individuals vary in
stranger. The resident therefore has information repertoire size (Baker et al. 2000) as well as in
about the asymmetrical nature of the agonistic dominance status. Asymmetries in dominance
interaction. As observed in neighborstranger status will be tested frequently during contests over
interactions in territorial singing contests, noted in resources. Under such conditions we can imagine a
many bird species, the resident will escalate the subordinate bird adding a gargle call, common in
180 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

the area, to its repertoire, which sometimes leads to We are only beginning to appreciate the social
its successful bluffing in contests. As modeled by learning circumstances that shape chickadee vocal
Andersson (1980), this would place a social evolu- signals. It seems clear that plasticity in vocal signals
tionary pressure on dominants to develop new calls is greatest early in the lives of black-capped chicka-
to counteract the bluffing tactic. The process, analo- dees when the major classes of signals are being
gous to an arms race, could explain the evolution of established (Baker et al. 2003). Evidently gargle call
gargle call repertoires. development extends into the immediate postdis-
Our interactive playback tests did not reveal persal phase of life, and probably beyond. We point
clear and immediate adjustments by subjects either to our aviary studies in this regard, and the field
through matching or avoiding matching during the evidence that suggests older birds may alter their
ongoing flow of the interaction. The negative repertoires. Although they are preliminary, the
results could derive from an experimental problem, results are reminiscent of patterns seen in territorial
such as not simulating closely a natural encounter. song of a number of other songbird species. In these
In our tests, for example, the visual display seen in cases, the social influences of resident males on
the mirror by a subject would always be that of an younger birds during the time of territory
equal-ranked and equally aggressive bird, preclud- establishment causes the young male to drop
ing a dominantsubordinate visual interaction. certain components of it song patterns and attain
Also, our results could be attributed to the short better matching to residents. The process of gargle
duration of a trial in comparison to repeated call repertoire alteration is therefore not unlike
contests over resources in the field. However, we the selective attrition or action-based learning
did observe a consistent pattern of an increase in observed in territorial song repertoires of a
the diversity of gargle calls that subjects gave number of songbirds (Marler and Peters 1982a;
during playback, and this may be an important Nelson 1992a, 1992b; Marler and Nelson 1993;
finding in these tests. Hailman (1989:336) antici- Marler 1997).
pated this in his review of vocalizations in
the ParidaeIt is possible that acoustical variety
Acknowledgments
represents information at a different level: instead
of each variant representing a different message, We thank the students, undergraduates and
it is the ensembles diversity per se that is graduates, who in various ways assisted this
informative. We believe that additional playback research: Adriana Acosta, Merrill Baker,
experiments with the gargle call would be of benefit Lindsay Barnhill, April Becker, Brad Bisbee, Alice
in addressing this issue. Campbell, Owen Davis, Rachel Dill, Nicole Ernst,
From comparative reviews of the literature that Trina Howard, Stephanie Kane, Mehmet Kpeli,
describes the different kinds of vocalizations of the Rita Lerner, David Logue, Chandra Malpede, Lori
members of the Paridae (Hailman 1989; Hailman Miyasato, Katie Payne, Rebecca Promessi,
and Ficken 1996), it appears to us that the informa- Samantha Sasman, Mala Sawhney, Paul Sweet,
tion being obtained by studies of black-capped Todd Tracy, Wallace Turner, and Meagan Tovado.
chickadees will be broadly applicable to many We also acknowledge gratefully the many research
other species of Paridae. This is especially true for contributions of Susan M. Smith and Millicent S.
members of Poecile. We suggest that as comparable Ficken, which have informed and stimulated our
studies are carried out in other species it will be investigations of the behavioral ecology of chick-
found that vocalizations with functional equiva- adees in general and the gargle call in particular.
lence to the gargle call will show similar patterns of During our investigations, DEG was supported by
local similarity and between-population differ- a predoctoral fellowship from the National Science
ences, and that the local convergence in the acoustic Foundation. The research was supported by the
features of the calls will be influenced strongly by National Science Foundation (grants BNS-8706526,
social learning throughout ontogeny. IBN-0090400 to MCB).
THE GARGLE CALL 181

References Ficken, M. S. and Weise, C. M. (1984). A complex call of


the black-capped chickadee (Parus atricapillus). I
Andersson, M. (1980). Why are there so many threat Microgeographic variation. Auk, 101, 349360.
displays? Journal of Theoretical Biology, 86, 773781. Ficken, M. S. and Witkin, S. R. (1977). Responses of black-
Baker, M. C., Baker, M. S. A., and Gammon, D. E. (2003). capped chickadee flocks to predators. Auk, 94, 156157.
Vocal ontogeny of nestling and fledgling black-capped Ficken, M. S., Ficken, R. W., and Witkin, S. R. (1978).
chickadees Poecile atricapilla in natural populations. Vocal repertoire of the black-capped chickadee. Auk,
Bioacoustics, 13, 265296. 95, 3448.
Baker, M. C., Boylan, J. T., and Goulart, C. A. (1991). Effect Ficken, M. S., Weise, C. M., and Reinartz, J. A. (1987). A
of gargle vocalizations on behavior of black-capped complex vocalization of the black-capped chickadee. II
chickadees. Condor, 93, 6270. Repertoire, dominance and dialects. Condor, 89, 500509.
Baker, M. C. and Gammon, D. E. (in press). Persistence Galef, B. G., Jr. (1996). The adaptive value of social learn-
and change of vocal signals in natural populations of ing: a reply to Laland. Animal Behaviour, 52, 641644.
chickadees: annual sampling of gargle calls over eight Gammon, D. E., Baker, M. C., and Tipton, J. R. (2005).
seasons. Behaviour. Cultural divergence within novel song of the black-
Baker, M. C., Howard, T. M., and Sweet, P. W. (2000). capped chickadee (Poecile atricapillus). Auk, 122, 853871.
Microgeographic variation and sharing of the gargle Haftorn, S. (1993). Ontogeny of the vocal repertoire in the
vocalization and its component syllables in black- willow tit Parus montanus. Ornis Scandinavica, 24,
capped chickadees (Aves, Paridae, Poecile atricapillus) 267289.
populations. Ethology, 106, 819838. Hailman, J. P. (1989). The organization of major vocaliza-
Baker, M. C., Tracy, T. T., and Miyasato, L. E. (1996). tions in the Paridae. Wilson Bulletin, 101, 305343.
Gargle vocalizations of black-capped chickadees: test Hailman, J. P. and Ficken, M. S. (1996). Comparative
of repertoire and video stimuli. Animal Behaviour, 52, analysis of vocal repertoires with reference to chicka-
11711175. dees. In: D. E. Kroodsma and E. H. Miller, eds. Ecology
Bradbury, J. W. and Vehrencamp, S. L. (1994). SingIt!: a and Evolution of Acoustic Comminication in Birds, pp.
program for interactive playback on the Macintosh. 97117. Cornell University Press, Ithaca, NY.
Bioacoustics, 5, 308310. Hailman, J. P. and Griswold, C. K. (1996). Syntax of black-
Burt, J. M., Campbell, S. E., and Beecher, M. D. (2001). capped chickadee (Parus atricapillus) gargles sorts many
Song type matching as threat: a test using interactive types into few groups: implications for geographic
playback. Animal Behaviour, 62, 11631170. variation, dialect drift, and vocal learning. Bird Behavior,
Catchpole, C. K. and Slater, P. J. B. (1995). Bird Song: 11, 3957.
Biological Themes and Variations. Cambridge University Hailman, J. P. and Haftorn, S. (1995). Siberian tit. In: Poole,
Press, Cambridge, UK. A. and Gill, F., eds. The Birds of North America, No. 196,
Censky, E. J. and Ficken, M. S. (1982). Responses of black- pp. 123. Academy of Natural Sciences, Philadelphia,
capped chickadees to mirrors. Wilson Bulletin, 94, and American Ornithologists Union, Washington, DC.
590594. Hailman, J. P., Ficken, M. S., and Ficken, R. W. (1985). The
Craig, J. L. and Jenkins, P. F. (1982). The evolution of com- chick-a-dee calls of Parus atricapillus: A recombinant
plexity in broadcast song of passerines. Journal of system of animal communication compared with
Theoretical Biology, 95, 415422. written English. Semiotica, 56, 191224.
Derrickson, K. C. (1987). Yearly and situational changes in Hailman, J. P., Ficken, M. S., and Ficken, R. W. (1987).
the estimate of repertoire size in northern mockingbird Constraints on the structure of combinatorial chick-
(Mimus polyglottos). Auk, 104, 198207. a-dee calls. Ethology, 75, 6280.
Dixon, K. L. and Stefanski, R. A. (1970). An appraisal of Hausberger, M. (1997). Social influences on song acquisi-
the song of the black-capped chickadee. Wilson Bulletin, tion and sharing in the European starling (Sturnus
82, 5362. vulgaris). In: Snowdon, C. T. and Hausberger, M., eds.
Dixon, K. L., Stefanski, R. A., and Folks, F. N. (1970). Social Influences on Vocal Development, pp. 128156.
Acoustic signals in the mating of mountain and black- Cambridge University Press, New York.
capped chickadees. Auk, 87, 322328. Hughes, M., Nowicki, S., Searcy, W. A., and Peters, S.
Ficken, M. S. and Popp, J. W. (1995). Long-term persist- (1998). Song-type sharing in song sparrows: implica-
ence of a culturally transmitted vocalization of the tions for repertoire function and song learning.
black-capped chickadee. Animal Behaviour, 50, 683693. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 42, 437446.
182 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

Kramer, H. G. and Lemon, R. E. (1983). Dynamics of terri- Nelson, D. A. (1992b). Song overproduction, song
torial singing between neighbouring song sparrows matching and selective attrition during development.
(Melospiza melodia). Behaviour, 85, 198223. In: McGregor, P. K., ed. Playback and Studies of
Kramer, H. G., Lemon, R. E., and Morris, M. J. (1985). Song Animal Communication, pp. 121133. Plenum Press,
switching and agonistic stimulation in the song New York.
sparrow (Melospiza melodia): Five tests. Animal Nelson, D. A. and Marler, P. (1994). Selection-based learn-
Behaviour, 33, 135149. ing in bird song development. Proceedings of the National
Krebs, J. R., Ashcroft, R., and Van Orsdal, K. (1981). Song Academy of Science USA, 91, 1049810501.
matching in the great tit Parus major L. Animal Behaviour, Nottebohm, F., Nottebohm, T. M., and Crane, L. (1986).
29, 918923. Developmental and seasonal changes in canary song
Marler, P. (1997). Three models of song learning: evidence and their relation to changes in the anatomy of song
from behavior. Journal of Neurobiology, 33, 501516. control nuclei. Behavioral and Neural Biology, 46,
Marler, P. and Nelson, D. A. (1993). Action-based learning: 445471.
a new form of developmental plasticity in bird song. Nowicki, S. (1983). Flock-specific recognition of chickadee
Netherlands Journal of Zoology, 43, 91103. calls. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 12, 317320.
Marler, P. and Peters, S. (1982a). Subsong and plastic Odum, E. P. (1942). Annual cycle of the black-capped
song: their role in the vocal learning process. In. chickadee3. Auk, 59, 499531.
Kroodsma, D. E. and Miller, E. H., eds. Acoustic Otter, K., Njegovan, M., and Fotheringham, J. (2002).
communication in birds, vol. 2, pp. 2550. Academic Press, Importance of frequency and temporal song matching
New York. in black-capped chickadees: evidence from interactive
Marler, P. and Peters, S. (1982b). Developmental overpro- playback. Ethology, 108, 181191.
duction and selective attrition: new processes in the Payne, R. B., Payne, L. L. and Doehlert, S. M. (1988).
epigenesis of birdsong. Developmental Psychobiology, 15, Biological and cultural success of song memes in indigo
369378. buntings. Ecology, 69, 104117.
McCallum, D. A., Grundel, R., and Dahlsten, D. L. (1999). Popp, J. W., Ficken, M. S., and Weise, C. M. (1990). How
Mountain chickadee. In: Poole, A. and Gill, F., eds. The are agonistic encounters among black-capped chicka-
Birds of North America, No. 453, pp. 127. Academy of dees resolved? Animal Behaviour, 39, 980986.
Natural Sciences, Philadelphia, and American Pyle, P., Howell, S. N. G., Yunick, R. P., and DeSante, D. F.
Ornithologists Union, Washington, DC. (1987). Identification Guide to North American Passerines.
McGregor, P. K. and Krebs, J. R. (1989). Song learning in Slate Creek Press, Bolinas, CA.
adult great tits (Parus major): effects of neighbours. Rothstein, S. I. and Fleischer, R. C. (1987). Vocal dialects
Behaviour, 108, 139159. and their possible relation to honest status signalling in
McLaren, M. A. (1976). Vocalizations of the Boreal chicka- the brown-headed cowbird. Condor, 89, 123.
dee. Auk, 93, 451463. Searcy, W. A. and Nowicki, S. (1999). Functions of song
Mennill, D. J. and Ratcliffe, L. M. (2004). Overlapping and variation in song sparrows. In: Hauser, M. D. and
matching in the song contests of black-capped chicka- Konishi, M., eds. The Design of Animal Communication,
dees. Animal Behaviour, 67, 441450. pp. 577595. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.
Miyasato, L. E. and Baker, M. C. (1999a). Black-capped Siegel, S. (1956). Nonparametric Statistics. McGraw-Hill,
chickadee call dialects along a continuous habitat corri- New York.
dor. Animal Behaviour, 57, 13111318. Smith, S. M. (1991). The Black-Capped Chickadee: Behavioral
Miyasato, L. E. and Baker, M. C. (1999b). Discrimination Ecology and Natural History. Cornell University Press,
of gargle calls by black-capped chickadees (Poecile atri- Ithaca, NY.
capillus). Bird Behavior, 13, 914. Smith, S. T. (1972). Communication and other social
Mundinger, P. C. (1982). Microgeographic and macrogeo- behavior in Parus carolinensis. Publication of the Nuttall
graphic variation in acquired vocalizations of birds. In: Ornithological Club, 11, Cambridge, MA.
Kroodsma, D. E. and Miller, E. H., eds. Acoustic Tchernichovski, O., Nottebohm, F., Ho, E., Pesaran, B., and
Communication in Birds, vol. 2, pp. 147208. Academic Mitra, P. P. (2000). A procedure for an automated meas-
Press, New York. urement of song similarity. Animal Behaviour, 59,
Nelson, D. A. (1992a). Song overproduction and selective 11671176.
attrition lead to song sharing in the field sparrow Vehrencamp, S. L. (2001). Is song-type matching a con-
(Spizella pusilla). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 30, ventional signal of aggressive intentions? Proceedings of
415424. the Royal Society of London, B, 268, 16371642.
CHAPTER 12

How postdispersal social


environment may influence
acoustic variation in birdsong
David E. Gammon

within which song is acceptable as a learning


12.1 Introduction
model (Brenowitz and Beecher 2005). At one
Learned vocalizations vary across space and time extreme, Puget Sound white-crowned sparrows
in nearly all songbird species examined (Kroodsma (Zonotrichia leucophrys pugetensis) learn only one or
and Baylis 1982) including North American and two song types early in life by precisely imitating
Eurasian Parids (Hailman 1989). To understand the adult conspecific males in their natal area, and they
process behind this nearly universal pattern, we never learn new syllables, phrases, or song types
must understand how social and ecological factors following natal dispersal (Nelson 2000). At the
affect song learning. Specifically we must under- other extreme, European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris)
stand the mechanisms by which cultural innova- continue to learn syllables, phrases, and song types
tions arise in birdsong, because these innovations throughout life by imitating both conspecifics and
must play a role in the generation of acoustic the sounds of other species (Mountjoy and Lemon
variation across space and time. 1995). Despite this diversity of song-learning
The fee-bee song of the black-capped chickadee is programs, there are a few general patterns among
often cited as an exception to the general pattern of songbirds. For example the same neural circuits
acoustic variation across space and time (Kroodsma control song in every songbird species examined
et al. 1999; Chapter 14). Individuals across most of (Brenowitz 1997). Furthermore, the vocal output of
this species North American geographic range a developing bird for a given song type nearly
possess a single song type that does not vary in its always varies early in the learning process, but
acoustic structure. For some chickadee populations, becomes more stereotyped over developmental
however, such as those in Fort Collins, Colorado, time (e.g. Marler and Peters 1982; Tchernichovski
cultural innovations in the fee-bee song have et al. 2001). Novel song material produced early in
become established (Gammon and Baker 2004). By development could represent inaccurate imitation,
examining the social and ecological circumstances improvisation on an existing song type, an inven-
within which such cultural innovations become tion, or a developmental mutation. Some of this
established, we may gain insight into the evolution novel material may persist into adulthood as a
of within and between-individual diversity in cultural innovation and be responsible for acoustic
birdsong. variation across space and time in songbirds.
There is an enormous diversity of song-learning The amount of acoustic variation in the songs of
programs among songbirds. Species vary in the a developing bird is affected by its environment
timing and accuracy of learning, in the number and before and after natal dispersal. For example if
identity of tutors, in the number of song types fledglings do not receive sufficient exposure to
learned, and in the species-specific constraints appropriate conspecific song (e.g. Marler and

183
184 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

Tamura 1964) or if they are malnourished (e.g. juveniles retain novel acoustic material as adults?
Nowicki et al. 2002), they may learn song inaccur- I first present a visual model of song learning
ately. This could result in novel song types or (Fig. 12.1). I then hypothesize how the environment
novel song type variants. In many songbird species following natal dispersal affects variation in the
the environment continues to exert an effect on the vocal output of a juvenile songbird, and I test this
vocal output of juveniles following natal dispersal. hypothesis using data from the fee-bee song of the
Immediately after dispersal in many songbird black-capped chickadee. I end by taking a broad
species, juvenile song is still plastic with regard to look at cultural evolution and the origin and
both the number of song types and the acoustic maintenance of song variation.
structure of each song type (e.g. Kroodsma 1974;
Nelson 2000; Baker et al. 2003). Dispersing juveniles
12.2 Description of model and
might possess song types not shared with their
hypothesis
adult neighbors, or they may possess a song
type shared with neighbors but containing a unique The model asserts that for each song type learned,
acoustic structure such as a novel syllable. Because the vocal output of juveniles in early developmen-
sharing song with neighbors is often adaptive tal stages is variable but becomes more stereotyped
(Beecher et al. 1997; McGregor et al. 1992; Molles over developmental time. This assumption has
and Vehrencamp 2001), juveniles of many species empirical support (Haftorn 1993; Kroodsma et al.
selectively retain only the song material that best 1995; lveczky et al. 2005), and it is represented in
matches their neighbors. As such, novel acoustic Fig. 12.1 as a funnel that begins wide and then
variation seldom becomes established into a popu- narrows over time. When young birds precisely
lation (McGregor and Krebs 1989; Nelson and and accurately model their song(s) after the song(s)
Marler 1994; Nordby et al. 2001). of neighbors following natal dispersalas has been
In this chapter I pose the question, In what shown for numerous songbirds in the field and
social and ecological circumstances should laboratory (e.g. Nelson and Marler 1994; Nordby
et al. 2001)acoustic conformity throughout the
population will result in little acoustic variation
within or between birds for each song type
Natal (Fig. 12.2a). Acoustic variation for a given song type
dispersal Bird A could originate between birds (individuals use
Bird B different variants for that song type, Fig. 12.2b),
within birds (a single individual uses multiple
within a song type
Acoustic variation

VA variants for that song type, Fig. 12.2c), or both


of a bird

between and within birds (Fig. 12.2d). As


Vpop
mentioned in the introduction to this chapter, song
VB sharing between adult neighbors is adaptive
for many songbird species. Therefore, failure of a
juvenile to conform the acoustic structure of its
Developmental time song types to the song types of its neighbors could
result in decreased fitness for the developing bird.
Figure 12.1 In the visual model of song learning for a given song
type, acoustic variation always exists in the vocal output of juveniles I hypothesize that the tendency of an individual
during early developmental stages, but this variation decreases over to retain song material that conforms in its acoustic
time. Further, individual birds may vary slightly in the final production structure to the local pattern varies as a function
of an individual song type (referred to as the range of variation of both the size and degree of isolation of that
within each individualVA or VB). This will result in a range of
individuals adult population. In one scenario a
variation among individuals in the population for this song type (VP).
For birds singing multiple song types (i.e. most songbirds, Krebs and juvenile settles into a large, contiguous population
Kroodsma 1980), one funnel would be needed to represent each (Fig. 12.2a); in the other a juvenile settles into
song type of an individual. a small, isolated population (Fig. 12.2bd).
A C O U S T I C VA R I AT I O N I N B I R D S O N G 185

(a) No variation within or between birds (c) Variation within but not between birds
Bird A Bird A
Bird B Bird B V
A
VA
Vpop Vpop
Acoustic variation within a

VB
song type of a bird

VB

(b) Variation between but not within birds (d) Variation within and between birds
Bird A Bird A
Bird B Bird B VA
VA
Vpop Vpop
VB
VB

Developmental time

Figure 12.2 Juveniles that disperse into large contiguous populations encounter many singing neighbors. With this large number of tutors,
these juveniles develop song that conforms acoustically to the local pattern (a). This leads to small V values for each individual (narrow at the
right end of the individuals funnel), and low acoustic variation between individuals using that song type (VA, VB and VP very similar). Alternately,
juveniles that disperse into small, isolated populations may encounter fewer tutors. This can lead to high regional variation in song types among
individuals (large VP), but this regional variation may arise by three different scenarios in individual learning (bd). In the first scenario (b), each
individual becomes highly stereotyped for the version of the song type it crystallizes upon (low within-individual variation), but individuals
crystallize upon different variants for that song type (high between-individual variation, little overlap between VA and VB). In the second scenario
(c) individuals continue to produce multiple song type variants (high within-individual variation, large values for VA and/or VB), but all individuals
use the same multiple song type variants (low between-individual variation). In the third scenario (d) variation both within and between
individuals is large.

Individuals in large, contiguous populations seldom used by its neighbors (leads to variation
should be more likely to learn precisely and accur- between, but not necessarily within individuals,
ately than individuals in small isolated popula- Fig. 12.2b); second, juveniles might continue to sing
tions because they encounter numerous singing multiple song type variants as adults (variation
neighbors and thus have greater social pressure to within, but not necessarily between individuals,
conform, although this assumption may be true Fig. 12.2c); finally, both variation within and
only in species with highly stereotyped vocaliza- between individuals might develop (Fig. 12.2d).
tions such as the black-capped chickadee. Based on Later in this chapter, I test each of these three
my hypothesis, I predict that in large, contiguous possibilities by separately quantifying variation
populations, an adult will sing with little acoustic within and variation between individuals for
variation in successive renditions for a given song chickadees in small, isolated populations.
type (i.e. low variation within birds) and that
shared song types will possess a similar acoustic
12.3 Description of study system
structure for each adult (i.e. low variation between
birds, Fig. 12.2a). Conversely, in small isolated I first explain what is known about song learning in
populations, developing birds will encounter few black-capped chickadees and then describe my
adult tutors and will be more likely to develop study system. Laboratory audio-tutoring experi-
variation for a given song type. Three possibilities ments demonstrate that chickadees exposed to
exist for how this variation might develop: first, appropriate conspecific song are more likely to
juveniles might introduce novel song type variants develop whistled song than chickadees without
or settle on extreme variants of that song type such exposure (Shackleton and Ratcliffe 1993;
186 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

Kroodsma et al. 1995; K. A. Otter unpublished appear similar to the introductory notes of the
data). Chickadees in all previous audio-tutoring chick-a-dee call, although this similarity may not
experiments failed to imitate song precisely and represent homology. The chick-a-fee-bee also
accurately, suggesting that social and ecological normally contains an amplitude break in the fee
influences missing in the laboratory likely play an syllable not found in the typical fee-bee. The low fee-
important role in normal song development. In the bee (called fee-bee-3 in Gammon and Baker 2004)
field, juvenile chickadees first attempt to sing 2 to differs from the typical fee-bee in that it consistently
3 weeks prior to natal dispersal, and their first uses low frequencies for the fee and bee notes rather
attempts at song are imprecise and inaccurate than transposing them across a large range of
(Baker et al. 2003). At the time of natal dispersal, frequencies.
their vocal output for a given song type is still vari- The three song types of the Fort Collins area vary
able (Gammon et al. 2005), but by the following within and between study sites separated by just a
spring song becomes especially stereotyped for few kilometers, and there is greater acoustic diver-
each song type both within and between individ- gence between birds in the songs of adult birds
uals (Kroodsma et al. 1999). It is unknown whether located in discontinuous, low-abundance habitat
chickadees can learn novel fee-bee song types later islands north of Fort Collins (hereafter referred to
in life, although older birds have been shown to as Islands North) versus continuous high-abun-
acquire new gargle types (Chapter 11). dance habitat located along the Poudre River that
I used song recordings from black-capped runs through Fort Collins (Gammon et al. 2005).
chickadees in and around Fort Collins, Colorado to This divergence usually consists of variation in the
test the model and hypothesis presented in this number or type of syllables and in the
chapter. These chickadees sing a repertoire of three presence/absence of an amplitude break in the fee
discrete song types (Fig. 12.3) that differ acoustic- syllable. These acoustic differences are similar to
ally from the typical fee-bee vocalization that char- the acoustic differences distinguishing the song
acterizes song of most populations of black-capped type variants of a juvenile at the time of its natal
chickadees in their North American range dispersal (Gammon et al. 2005). For example an
(Kroodsma et al. 1999; Gammon and Baker 2004). adult at one study site sang fa-fee-bee whereas
Two of the local song types, chick-a-fee-bee and fa-fee- an adult at a different study site sang fa-fa-fee-bee,
bee, contain novel introductory syllables, whereas and this difference parallels the vocal output of an
the third song type, low fee-bee, does not. The individual juvenile at a third study site, which sang
introductory notes of chick-a-fee-bee superficially both fa-fee-bee and fa-fa-fee-bee, even though all of its

6kHz
Chick-a-fee-bee Fa-fee-bee Low fee-bee

2kHz

1s 1s 1s

Figure 12.3 Spectrograms and oscillograms of the three black-capped chickadee song types found in Fort Collins, Colorado: chick-a-fee-bee,
fa-fee-bee, and low fee-bee. Chick-a-fee-bee and fa-fee-bee possess novel introductory syllables, and chick-a-fee-bee also possesses a novel
amplitude break in the fee syllable.
A C O U S T I C VA R I AT I O N I N B I R D S O N G 187

adult neighbors sang nothing but fa-fee-bee 2004 numerous gaps in distribution occurred along
(Gammon et al. 2005). This and similar findings the same riparian corridor. During a typical dawn
demonstrate that acoustic variation present at chorus in the breeding season of 2003, each singing
the time of natal dispersal could account for male had an average of seven audible singing
geographic variation in adult song. neighbors; during the breeding season of 2004 most
Previous work using this study system individual chickadees in this same habitat had few
(Gammon and Baker 2004; Gammon et al. 2005) or even no singing neighbors (unpublished data).
sampled only a single representative of each song These changes in population size and distribution
type per bird, thus ignoring variation within the offered a chance to test the above hypothesis to see
song types of individuals. This type of variation if there was greater acoustic variation within and
could contribute to population patterns and be between 2004 birds compared to 2003 birds. Such a
functionally important (Stoddard et al. 1988; test may provide insights into the mechanisms of
Nowicki et al. 1999), but it is rarely examined. cultural evolution and specifically into the origin
Furthermore, previous work sampled only one and maintenance of acoustic variation in birdsong.
point in time, thus ignoring how acoustic variation
might change in response to variation in popula-
tion size and/or isolation over time. 12.4 Acoustic variation before and
A bottleneck in the Fort Collins chickadee popu- after the bottleneck
lations in late 2003 provided an opportunity to
12.4.1 Sampling procedure
address both of these issues. Data from the
Christmas Bird Count in Fort Collins show that its I recorded the songs of unbanded adult male
overall population decreased by approximately chickadees along a 6-km stretch of the Poudre River
75% between the 2003 and 2004 breeding seasons in Fort Collins during the 2003 and 2004 breeding
(Fig. 12.4, National Audubon Society 2002). The seasons (see Box 12.1 for details of sampling
cause of this bottleneck is unknown, but it resulted methodology). My goal was to record at least ten
in a more patchy distribution of chickadees high-quality renditions, of each of the three song
throughout Fort Collins. During the breeding sea- types depicted in Fig. 12.3, from each male, so that
son of 2003, populations were distributed continu- I could examine acoustic variation both within and
ously along the Poudre River in Fort Collins, but in between males.
I made spectrograms of all song recordings using
Syrinx software (John Burt; www.syrinxpc.com/
index.html), sampling at 22.05 kHz with 16-bit
2.0
accuracy and 1024-point transform size. This
allowed classification of all songs by song types
Population size

based upon which syllables appeared on the spec-


trogram. Recordings were used only if the quality
1.0
was sufficiently high to allow measurement of the
following variables for a given song: (i) number of
each syllable type ( fee, bee, and the introductory
0.0 syllables chick-a or fa), (ii) presence or absence of an
Dec. Dec. Dec. Dec. amplitude break in the whistled syllables fee and
1994 1997 2000 2003
bee, and (iii) any distinguishing order of syllables.
Figure 12.4 Graph showing the relative size of the populations of After these measurements were made, I randomly
black-capped chickadees over a 10-year period in Fort Collins, chose ten representative renditions of each song type
Colorado. Data were taken from the Christmas Bird Count, and the y for each bird recorded. If there were not ten rendi-
axis represents the average number of chickadees observed per hour
by each group involved in the Fort Collins count. As shown, there was
tions available, I excluded that song type for that
a 75% reduction in population size between the 2003 and 2004 bird. This meant that some birds contributed all
breeding seasons. three song types to my sample (i.e. 30 high quality
188 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

Box 12.1 Sampling method

Most birds were recorded during the dawn different days were always made at least 300 m
chorus, but several were recorded later in the day from each other. The latter precaution makes it
or in response to playback of synthetic songs highly unlikely that the same bird was recorded
modeled after the three Fort Collins song types. twice during the same season, given that territory
There were no differences between 2003 and boundaries are stable throughout the breeding
2004 in the relative frequencies with which each season (Smith 1991). Birds were recorded using a
sampling method was used. Recordings were MiniDisc recorder (either Sony MZ-N1 or Sony
made on multiple days and by multiple recordists, MZ-R700) attached to a Sennheiser microphone
but recordings of a given male were always made (ME62) mounted in a parabola (either 60 or
on the same day, and recordings made on 45 cm).

songs, 10 from each song type) whereas others con- that contributed multiple song types to my sample,
tributed only one or two song types. This problem I computed one diversity value for each of its song
was unavoidable, however, given that ~20% of birds types and then averaged these values across song
lack one of the song types (Gammon and Baker 2004) type to get a single within-bird diversity value
and given the difficulties of obtaining large samples for each bird (range {110}).
of high-quality recordings. To obtain between-bird diversity values, I
examined the amount of acoustic overlap between
12.4.2 Analysis of data birds for a given song type in a given year. As an
example of how this was done, suppose that a
Acoustic variation for a given song type can exist population contained three individuals that con-
within birds (an individual uses multiple variants tributed fa-fee-bees. Bird 1 contributed three typical
for that song type) and between birds (different fa-fee-bee and seven fa-fa-fee-bee, bird 2 contributed
individuals use different variants for that song seven typical fa-fee-bee, one fa-fa-fee-bee and two
type). For example a bird might sing both fa-fee-bee fa-fee, and bird 3 contributed 10 typical fa-fee-bee. I
and fa-fa-fee-bee (variation within that bird), and a would compare each birds set of 10 songs to
second bird might consistently sing fa-fee without another birds set to yield overlap values. In com-
the bee (no variation within the second bird, but paring birds 1 and 2, these overlap with three fa-fee-
variation between the two birds). To quantify bees and a single fa-fa-fee-bee, so the overlap value
this variation, I computed a within-bird and between birds 1 and 2 would be (3 1)/10 40%.
between-bird diversity measure for each bird Birds 1 and 3 overlapped with three fa-fee-bees, due
using information gathered from the acoustic to bird 1 having numerous other variants of this
measurements made on its 10, 20, or 30 representa- song type; therefore, their overlap would be
tive songs. 3/10 30%. Using this same principal, the overlap
For the within-bird measure, I adapted the between birds 2 and 3 is seven fa-fee-bees, so
ShannonWiener species diversity index for each 7/10 70%. I would then average values to get
song type of a given bird. I considered each song a single overlap value per bird for that song
type variant as a different species and computed type (35% for bird 1, 55% for bird 2 and 50% for
a single diversity value for each group of ten songs bird 3). If a bird contributed multiple song types, I
using the formula: would compute an overlap value for each of its
S exp( pi*ln(pi)) song types and then average across that birds song
types to get a single overlap value for that bird.
in which pi refers to the proportion of the ten songs Finally I took the inverse of this overlap value to
that existed as the ith song type variant. For a bird yield a single between-bird diversity value
A C O U S T I C VA R I AT I O N I N B I R D S O N G 189

(range {1}) for each bird. Birds with high song type. For the 2004 data, five birds contributed
acoustic overlap would receive low diversity three song types, five birds contributed two song
values, whereas birds that frequently sang novel types, and two birds contributed one song type.
song type variants not shared with other birds that These numbers do not represent repertoire sizes,
year, would receive high diversity values. only the number of song types for a given bird for
After computing a within-bird and between- which I was able to obtain at least 10 high-quality
bird diversity value for each bird included in my songs of that song type.
sample, I compared the diversity values for 2003 to In 2003, I found very little acoustic variation
those of 2004 birds using a MannWhitney U-test within birds. Ten of the 12 birds sampled in 2003
adjusted for ties. Because I sampled birds in had a within-bird diversity of 1.00, indicating
roughly the same locations each year and because that the ten representative songs for each of their
all birds were unbanded, it is possible that I song types were the same with regard to the
sampled some of the same birds in both years. acoustic measures I took (number and type of
However, given an estimated 2003 population of syllables, presence/absence of amplitude breaks in
70 to 110 male chickadees in my study site (unpub- the whistled syllables, and syllable recombination).
lished data), and given that after the bottleneck Likewise, there was very little acoustic variation
about 40% of the birds in my study population between birds in 2003. The median measure of
were SY birds (M. C. Baker, unpublished data), it is between-bird diversity in 2003 was 1.02, which
unlikely that I sampled many birds twice. If this indicates high overlap between birds in the song
problem existed, however, it would have decreased type variants used. Both of these results contrast
the likelihood of detecting differences when greatly with 2004 data in which only five of the
comparing 2003 versus 2004. 12 birds sampled had a within-bird diversity
measure of 1.00, and the median measure was 1.20
(significantly different than 2003, U 116,
12.4.3 Effect of the bottleneck
P 0.026, MannWhitney U-test, Fig. 12.5a).
Sample sizes were N 12 birds for 2003 and Furthermore, the median measure of between-
N 12 birds for 2004. For the 2003 data, three birds bird diversity for 2004 was 1.45 (significantly
contributed three song types, five birds contributed different than 2003, U 78, P 0.001,
two song types, and four birds contributed one MannWhitney U-test, Fig. 12.5b). Spectrograms

(a) (b)
2.5 2.5
Variation between birds
Variation within birds

2.0 2.0

1.5 1.5 1.45


1.38
1.20
1.0 1.00 1.0 1.02

2003 2004 Islands North 2003 2004


Fort Collins Fort Collins

Figure 12.5 Boxplots comparing the amount of acoustic variation found within (a) and between (b) adult Fort Collins chickadees in 2003
(N 12 birds) vs. 2004 (N 12 birds), and also acoustic variation within adult chickadees located in isolated habitat patches north of Fort
Collins (N 21 birds). Medians (horizontal line within the box, labeled), quartiles (top and bottom of box), 0.05 and 0.95 quantiles (tips of
vertical whiskers) and extreme data points (asterisks) are shown for each treatment. Acoustic variation was always higher in the songs of birds
that came from small isolated populations. See text for details on how the measurements were made.
190 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

illustrating these comparisons are in Fig. 12.6ad. To test this I recorded unbanded Islands North
To conclude this section, acoustic variation in Fort males during the 2003 and 2004 breeding seasons
Collins songs increased both within and between using recording and sampling methods identical to
birds following the population bottleneck. those described above for Fort Collins males. When
a habitat patch was sampled in both 2003 and 2004,
I included songs from only one seasonwhichever
12.5 Comparison of acoustic variation in
season yielded a larger sample. This ensured that
Fort Collins 2004 and Islands North
each bird was represented only once. For each song
Recall that suitable chickadee habitat immediately included in the sample, I made the same acoustic
north of Fort Collins is found only in isolated habi- measurements as described above. I then computed
tat patches. Even when regional populations are a within-bird diversity measure for each bird as
large, Islands North populations contain a median described above. I did not compute a between-
of only two adult males during the breeding season bird diversity measure because the Islands North
and a maximum of four (Gammon et al. 2005). area is many times larger than the Fort Collins area
Because black-capped chickadees are a resident so the comparison would not be valid. I compared
species, I can assume that an adult male in an the within-bird diversity values between Fort
Islands North population must have experienced a Collins 2004 birds and Islands North birds using a
limited social environment during the later stages MannWhitney U-test adjusted for ties.
of his song development with few or no adult male I sampled a total of 21 Islands North birds. For
tutors. If the greater amount of acoustic variation these data, 12 birds contributed three song types,
within 2004 birds resulted from the fact that many five birds contributed two song types, and four
of these birds completed their song development birds contributed one song type. Seventeen birds
with few or no adult song tutors, then similar were sampled in 2003 from 10 habitat patches,
amounts of acoustic variation should exist within and the other four were sampled in 2004 from three
Islands North birds. different habitat patches. I found within-bird

(a) FtC'03

(b) FtC'03

(c) FtC'04

(d) FtC'04

(e) IsN

(f) Juv

1s 1s 1s

Figure 12.6 Spectrograms illustrating the pattern shown in Fig. 12.5. The ticks on the y axis represent 2, 4, and 6 kHz as in Fig. 12.3. In 2003,
adult Fort Collins chickadees sang with little acoustic variation (e.g. Birds a and b), but after the population bottleneck resulted in smaller
discontinuous populations, adult Fort Collins chickadees in 2004 were more likely to introduce novel song type variants (e.g. Bird c consistently
left off the second half of the bee syllable in his fa-fee-bee) and were more likely to sing multiple song type variants (e.g. Bird d sang two
different versions of low fee-bee). Multiple song type variants were also used by adult chickadees in isolated habitat patches north of Fort Collins
(e.g. Bird es fa-fee-bee song type) and by juvenile chickadees singing near the time of natal dispersal (e.g. Bird fs chick-a-fee-bee song type).
A C O U S T I C VA R I AT I O N I N B I R D S O N G 191

diversity 1 for most (18/21) of the Islands North should have been more likely to come from birds
birds. The median measure of within-bird diver- that hatched in 2003 (i.e. SY birds), whereas 2004
sity in Islands North birds was 1.38, which was not ASY (i.e. those hatched prior to 2003) birds should
significantly different from the 1.20 value for Fort have been less likely to introduce novel song type
Collins birds in 2004 following the bottleneck variants because they became adults prior to the
(U 171, P 0.22, MannWhitney U-test, Fig. bottleneck.
12.5a, Fig. 12.6e). To summarize, acoustic variation To test this, I collected recordings in and around
in chickadee song originated in response to both Fort Collins during the breeding season of 2004.
population bottlenecks in regions of continuous After recording each birds repertoire using the
habitat as well as in small isolated populations. same sampling procedure described above, I
captured the bird by luring it into a mist net with
playback, and classified the bird as a SY or ASY bird
12.6 Acoustic variation within through observation of its outer tail feathers (Pyle
juveniles 1997). For each song included in the sample, I made
According to the model of song learning, the vocal the same acoustic measurements as described
output of juveniles should vary acoustically at the above. I then computed a within-bird diversity
time of natal dispersal, and juveniles should sing a measure for each bird as described above, and I also
few novel song type variants at that time period. To computed a novelty score that measures the
see if this was the case for black-capped chickadees proportion of the birds songs that were novel song
in Fort Collins, I sampled songs from four solitary type variants in 2004. By novel, I mean that no
juvenile chickadees at three separate study sites bird recorded at that study site in 2003 ever gave a
around the time of natal dispersal in late June and song that would be classified as the same song type
early July 2003. Each juvenile was recorded the day I variant. Novelty scores were averaged across song
first observed an individual in a study site, presum- type to get a single novelty score for each bird
ably soon after or during its dispersal. I recognized sampled. If a bird received a novelty score of 0.5,
juveniles because they sing with lower amplitude then half of songs uttered by that bird were novel
and more structural variability in their fee-bees, gar- song type variants in 2004.
gles, and chick-a-dee calls. Further details on identifi- Eight birds were sampled from five study sites
cation and recording methods are provided in (Table 12.1). Five of these birds were aged as SY
Gammon et al. (2005). Each juvenile contributed birds, and the other three were aged as ASY birds.
between four and 45 songs to my sample. All four Within-bird diversity values were high for all but
juveniles used several variants of each song type one of the birds (an ASY bird), and novelty scores
(Fig. 12.6f), and at least one of these song type vari- were high for about half of the birds (Table 12.1). All
ants was not shared by the adults recorded at that five SY birds sang a novel song type variant
study site. I also sampled several novel song type (i.e. novelty score 0), but only one of the three
variants in each of two chickadee family groups a ASY birds sang a novel song type variant. Given
day or two prior to their natal dispersal. Baker et al. the small sample size, the results are suggestive but
(2003) also found that juvenile song was highly not conclusive.
variable in black-capped chickadees, and similar
findings have been reported for other songbird
12.8 Synthesis of empirical results
species (Marler and Peters 1982; Tchernichovski et al.
2001). Thus, dispersing juveniles probably have the To summarize all of these studies, greater acoustic
capacity to introduce cultural innovations into a variation existed for adult black-capped chickadees
population if selective retention does not occur. in small, isolated populations (Islands North and
2004 Fort Collins) than in large contiguous popula-
tions (2003 Fort Collins). Prior to a population
12.7 Ages of singing adults in 2004 bottleneck in late 2003, the songs of Fort Collins
If song development is age-limited in chickadees, chickadees were remarkably stereotyped, but
then the novel song type variants recorded in 2004 acoustic variation both within and between birds
192 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

Table 12.1 Information about the songs of eight Fort Collins birds aged during the 2004 breeding season

Bird Site Song types Age Within-bird Novelty


diversity score

Wuss Dixon 3 ASY 1.00 0.00


Boss Cem 2 ASY 1.32 0.00
Buck Poudre 3 SY 1.41 0.03
Joey Zimdahl 3 SY 1.41 0.03
Jared Poudre 3 SY 1.59 0.30
Red Poudre 3 SY 1.32 0.33
Ventura Poudre 2 ASY 1.52 0.40
Anthony WSpCrk 3 SY 1.87 0.67

Birds with a high score for within-bird diversity used multiple variants of each song type, and birds with a
novelty score 0 used novel song type variants not found at that site in 2003.

increased substantially following the bottleneck. higher quality. It is unlikely for my study that all
Juvenile chickadees sang novel song type variants 2004 Fort Collins individuals were of low quality,
at the time of natal dispersal, and SY birds recorded however, because there was no suitable habitat
at several Fort Collins study sites in Spring 2004 nearby other than Fort Collins for higher quality
often used novel song type variants not previously individuals to disperse. Furthermore, a recent
recorded from adults at that study site. Thus it study failed to show any link between the quality
appears that Fig. 12.2d best represents what occurs of male black-capped chickadees and the quality of
for developing juveniles that disperse into small the habitat into which they dispersed (van Oort and
isolated populations. Otter 2005).
A third explanation for these patterns is that the
postdispersal social environment had an effect on
12.8.1 Potential explanations
the song development of young chickadees. This
At least three explanations could account for these explanation is consistent with data from this
data. The first is that these data represent simply a chapter and with data from previous studies in
sampling artifact and that the 12 chickadees song learning (McGregor and Krebs 1989; Nelson
recorded in 2003 do not accurately represent the and Marler 1994; Nordby et al. 2001; Chapter 11).
overall population. Because I sampled unbanded Juveniles dispersing into large contiguous popula-
birds, it is also possible that the 2003 sample may tions should discard novel song type variants so
contain mostly ASY birds with fully crystallized that the acoustic structure of each song type
songs whereas the 2004 sample may have becomes stereotyped within and between adult
contained mostly SY birds with songs retaining birds, whereas juveniles dispersing into small
some plasticity. Given that 35 to 45% of the Fort isolated populations should be more likely as
Collins populations were SY birds before and after adults to retain multiple song type variants, some
the bottleneck (M. C. Baker, unpublished data), of which are novel to that population.
however, this possibility seems unlikely.
Another potential explanation is that small,
12.8.2 How postdispersal social environment
isolated habitats are more likely to attract lower-
may affect song development
quality individuals and that these individuals may
have a more plastic song structure. Christie et al. Although a relationship between postdispersal
(2004) showed that male black-capped chickadees social environment and song development appears
of low quality possess a more variable frequency likely in black-capped chickadees, the mechanism
interval between whistled notes than males of of such a relationship remains unclear. What
A C O U S T I C VA R I AT I O N I N B I R D S O N G 193

aspects of a birds social environment following recorded from young males before and after coun-
dispersal would affect the amount of acoustic tersinging interactions to see if song plasticity
plasticity during its song development? According decreases. In their studies of song development in
to the model of song learning presented earlier zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata), lveczky et al.
(Fig. 12.1), acoustic plasticity in a young birds song (2005) found that acoustic variation in juveniles had
type gradually decreases throughout development. a neurological basis. After they inactivated a partic-
This reduction in plasticity may occur as a develop- ular group of neurons located in the basal ganglia
ing bird discards song type variants not shared of the songbird brain, variation in the vocal output
with its adult neighbors (McGregor and Krebs of juveniles decreased. The neural circuits involved
1989). Song sharing among neighbors is often in song learning are plastic to some degree
adaptive (Beecher et al. 1997; Molles and (Brenowitz and Beecher 2005) and may rewire
Vehrencamp 2001), which results in juveniles that themselves in response to particular social inter-
selectively retain song type variants that best match actions. Perhaps countersinging interactions stimu-
their adult neighbors (Nelson and Marler 1994; late the inactivation of the neural circuits of the
Nordby et al. 2001). basal ganglia in a developing bird, thus resulting in
I hypothesize that a juveniles tendency to retain decreased song plasticity, although this idea has not
shared song type variants is related to the amount been tested.
of social pressure to conform faced by that juvenile.
The magnitude of social pressure to conform likely
12.8.3 Future studies needed
varies within and between songbird species. When
social pressure is high, an individual retains only Because human activities often fragment songbird
those song type variants that are shared with its habitat, resulting in isolated habitat patches with
adult neighbors, but when social pressure is low, an smaller population sizes (Vitousek 1994), there are
individual retains greater plasticity in the acoustic plenty of opportunities to test the effects of isola-
structure of its song types. Brown and Laland tion and population size on song learning in future
(2002) used a similar explanation to explain the observational studies. It is also possible to manipu-
origin of cultural innovations in strategies used by late a young birds social environment in field and
guppies to escape predators. laboratory populations during the later stages of
Social pressure within a population might mani- song learning, and then monitor closely the sub-
fest itself to a developing bird in many ways, such jects song development as it proceeds. In a labora-
as the number of its adult neighbors, its spatial tory experiment with song sparrows (Melospiza
proximity to these neighbors, and the number of melodia), Nordby et al. (2000) placed young males in
countersinging interactions involving the young contact with four adult sparrows for several
bird or overheard by the young bird. Any or all of months following the age typical of natal dispersal
these factors might play a role. In the 2004 Fort in sparrows. In accordance with the model, acoustic
Collins and Islands North populations, chickadees variation within the young birds decreased until
experienced fewer adult neighbors, greater spatial their songs became stereotyped and matched the
isolation, and fewer countersinging interactions songs of their adult tutors. This type of study could
(unpublished data), and this may explain why the be extended by varying the number of adult tutors
acoustic structure of their song types never became later in development, by measuring the counter-
fully stereotyped. If countersinging interactions singing interactions between birds, by considering
indeed have an effect on the level of song plasticity, acoustic variation both within and between the
it would be interesting to compare in an adult the song types of birds and by tracking developmental
amount of acoustic variation present during a changes more closely (e.g. Tchernichovski et al.
countersinging interaction involving that bird 2004). Perhaps in some cases a juvenile requires
versus the amount of variation present during its only a single tutor song to learn accurately and pre-
broadcast singing (e.g. during the dawn chorus). It cisely, and novel song will arise only in the absence
would also be interesting to compare songs of any social tutors. This alone may account for
194 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

some of the novel song found in Islands North and resulting vocalizations. Investigators in the labora-
2004 Fort Collins birds considering that some of tory audio-tutored fledgling black-capped chicka-
these birds possessed no singing neighbors during dees (Shackleton and Ratcliffe 1993; Kroodsma et al.
the dawn chorus in which they were recorded. 1995; K. A. Otter unpublished data) and fledgling
Carolina chickadees (Kroodsma et al. 1995), and
then monitored subsequent vocal development. For
12.9 On the origin and maintenance of both species, high acoustic variation was found
acoustic variation in birdsong within juvenile song during early developmental
stages, just as I found for juveniles in the field. This
12.9.1 Acoustic variation across space and
variation decreased over developmental time until
time in Parids
the songs of males that were raised in the same
Socially learned vocalizations vary across space group converged in acoustic structure (Shackleton
and time for most songbirds (Catchpole and Slater and Ratcliffe 1993; Kroodsma et al. 1995).
1995) and for Parids in particular (Hailman 1989). Interestingly, some of the acoustic features distin-
Although the songs of Parids usually vary geo- guishing the songs of adult black-capped chicka-
graphically, within a local population conspecific dees in Fort Collins from typical fee-bee song
neighbors often share song types (e.g. blue tit, (e.g. introductory syllables, novel amplitude
Thielcke 1969; great tit, Gompertz 1961; McGregor breaks, and syllable recombination) also arose in
and Krebs 1989; willow tit, Haftorn 1993; and tufted the songs of black-capped chickadees reared in the
titmouse, Schroeder and Wiley 1983), thus showing laboratory with a limited social environment
a tendency towards cultural conformity. Kroodsma (Shackleton and Ratcliffe 1993; Kroodsma et al.
et al. (1999) found extensive cultural conformity in 1995; K. A. Otter unpublished data), suggesting
the acoustic structure of the fee-bee song of the that similar developmental mechanisms may be
black-capped chickadee throughout much of North involved. Similar acoustic variants have also been
America, yet they also sampled populations con- found in the songs of adult black-capped chicka-
taining variant song. These populations existed in dees in islands near Massachusetts (Kroodsma et al.
geographically isolated regions such as the islands 1999), Ontario (Christie et al. 2004), and several
off the coast of Massachusetts. The data from this other continental locations (unpublished data).
chapter suggest that novel song type variants such
as these are more likely to originate in small, iso-
12.9.3 Does the possession of novel song
lated populations. Perhaps the variant populations
material affect reproductive fitness?
of Kroodsma et al. (1999) began as small founder
populations, which facilitated the fee-bee song to One remaining question is whether retaining novel
diverge from its typical acoustic form. I predict that song type variants as an adult would affect the
novel song type variants are also likely to be found reproductive fitness of a male. The fact that con-
in other isolated populations of black-capped formity in the fee-bee song is maintained within
chickadees across North America not yet examined large contiguous populations (Kroodsma et al. 1999;
(e.g. isolated urban areas in the Great Plains). data from 2003 Fort Collins chickadees) suggests
that conformity may be adaptive. Conformity in
song structure could be adaptive in at least two
12.9.2 Development of song in Parids
ways: (i) conformity allows males to match the
The development of song in Parids has been stud- songs of their opponents during countersinging,
ied less frequently than variation in adult song. thus signaling an increased likelihood of escalation,
Haftorn (1993) monitored vocal development in the and (ii) females could respond preferentially to
field for the willow tit. He found that acoustic vari- song that conforms to a local pattern (e.g. Searcy
ation decreased over developmental time for all et al. 2002). With regard to the first possibility,
learned vocalizations, but he provided only limited male chickadees in Ontario responded differently
detail regarding the acoustic structure of the to playback of acoustically matching versus
A C O U S T I C VA R I AT I O N I N B I R D S O N G 195

non-matching song (Chapter 14), although Fort Acknowledgments


Collins chickadees did not discriminate between
matching and non-matching playback in a similarly Thanks to Mike Baker for advice during my Ph.D.
designed study (unpublished data). studies at Colorado State University. David Logue,
With regard to the second possibility, Ratcliffe Don Kroodsma, John Tipton, Ken Otter, Dan
and Otter (1996) found that female black-capped Mennill, and Chris Sturdy provided ideas, criti-
chickadees responded with fewer copulation solici- cisms and fruitful discussion. Land access for
tations to playback of novel song material such as recording chickadees was provided by the Belinski,
an irregular frequency interval between fee and bee. Lesser, Martin, McGraw, McGrew, Turek, and
In a similar study, Baker et al. (1987) found that Zimdahl families, and by Phil Carpenter, Tinka
female great tits solicited fewer copulations in Greenwood, and Jan Kollar. Michelle Bartz, Alice
response to unfamiliar versus familiar song types. Campbell, Nicole Ernst, Jean Hans, Sarah Niemuth,
It would be interesting to know if female chicka- Derek Perry, Jessica Prada, Kerry Robison and sev-
dees in the Fort Collins area also discriminate eral students from the CSU Ornithology class
against novel song material. Perhaps females in helped with audio recordings and other fieldwork.
small isolated populations discriminate less than I was supported during much of this research by a
females in large contiguous populations, particu- graduate research fellowship from the National
larly if females must also learn songs in order to Science Foundation.
discriminate among males. Females in isolated
populations may learn multiple song variants and References
accept males that produce these, rather than typical
Baker, M. C., Baker, M. S. A., and Gammon, D. E. (2003).
stereotyped song. Vocal ontogeny of nestling and fledgling black-capped
chickadees (Poecile atricapilla) in natural populations.
12.9.4 Implications for the origin and Bioacoustics, 13, 265296.
Baker, M. C., McGregor, P. K., and Krebs, J. R. (1987).
maintenance of dialects and repertoires
Sexual response of female great tits to local and distant
The existence of dialects (e.g. Nelson et al. 2004) and songs. Ornis Scandinavica, 18, 186188.
repertoires of song types (e.g. Krebs and Kroodsma Beecher, M. D., Nordby, J. C., Campbell, S. E., Burt, J. M.,
1980) are well documented for many songbird Hill, C. E., and OLoghlen, A. L. (1997). What is the
species, but we have only a limited understanding function of song learning in songbirds? In: Owings, D.
H., Beecher M. D., and Thompson, N. S., eds.
of what causes a new dialect or a new song type to
Perspectives in Ethology, Vol. 12, pp. 7797. Plenum
originate. The data from this chapter suggest that in
Press, New York, New York.
small, isolated populations the acoustic plasticity in Brenowitz, E. A. (1997). Comparative approaches to the
the vocal output of young birds fails to become avian song system. Journal of Neurobiology, 33, 517531.
stereotyped acoustically as adults. Thus, popula- Brenowitz, E. A. and Beecher, M. D. (2005). Song learning
tion bottlenecks and separation of populations into in birds: diversity and plasticity, opportunities and
isolated subpopulations could result in the intro- challenges. Trends in Neurosciences, 28, 127132.
duction of novel song material. Conversely, when Brown, C. and Laland, K. N. (2002). Social learning of a
populations remain large and contiguous, novel novel avoidance task in the guppy: Conformity and
song material should be less likely to originate. The social release. Animal Behaviour, 64, 4147.
introduction of novel song type variants could Catchpole, C. K. and Slater, P. J. B. (1995). Birdsong:
Biological Themes and Variations. Cambridge University
result in acoustic divergence that eventually leads
Press, Cambridge.
to new dialects and song types. Through investi-
Christie, P. J., Mennill, D. J., and Ratcliffe, L. M. (2004).
gating how the postdispersal social environment Pitch shifts and song structure indicate male quality in
affects song development, we can gain greater the dawn chorus of black-capped chickadees. Behavioral
insight into the mechanisms of acoustic divergence Ecology and Sociobiology, 55, 341348.
and a greater understanding of cultural evolution Gammon D. E. and Baker, M. C. (2004). Song repertoire
in general. evolution and acoustic divergence in a population
196 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

of black-capped chickadees. Animal Behaviour, 68, Molles, L. E. and Vehrencamp, S. L. (2001). Songbird
903913. cheaters pay a retaliation cost: evidence for auditory
Gammon, D. E., Baker, M. C., and Tipton, J. R. (2005). conventional signals. Proceedings of the Royal Society of
Cultural divergence within novel song in the London, Series B, 268, 20132019.
Black-capped chickadee (Poecile atricapillus). Auk, 122, Mountjoy, D. J. and Lemon, R. E. (1995). Extended song
853871. learning in wild European starlings. Animal Behaviour,
Gompertz, T. (1961). The vocabulary of the great tit. British 49, 357366.
Birds, 54, 369418. National Audubon Society (2002). The Christmas Bird
Haftorn, S. (1993). Ontogeny of the vocal repertoire in the Count Historical Results [Online]. Available http://
willow tit, Parus montanus. Ornis Scandinavica, 24, www.audubon.org/bird/cbc [accessed 3 June 2005].
267289. Nelson, D. A. (2000). Song overproduction, selective attri-
Hailman, J. P. (1989). The organization of major vocaliza- tion and song dialects in the white-crowned sparrow.
tions in the Paridae. Wilson Bulletin, 101, 305343. Animal Behaviour, 60, 887898.
Krebs, J. R. and Kroodsma, D. E. (1980). Repertoires and Nelson, D. A. and Marler, P. (1994). Selection-based learn-
geographical variation in bird song. In: Rosenblatt, J. S., ing in bird song development. Proceedings of the National
Hinde, R. A., Beer, C., and Busnel, M. C., eds. Advances Academy of Sciences, USA, 91, 1049810501.
in the Study of Behavior, Vol. 11, pp. 134177. Academic Nelson, D. A., Hallberg, K. I., and Soha, J. A. (2004).
Press, New York, New York. Cultural evolution of Puget Sound white-crowed spar-
Kroodsma, D. E. (1974). Song learning, dialects, and dis- row song dialects. Ethology, 110, 879908.
persal in the Bewicks Wren. Zeitschrift fr Nordby, J. C., Campbell, S. E., and Beecher, M. D. (2001).
Tierpsychologie, 35, 352380. Late song learning in song sparrows. Animal Behaviour,
Kroodsma, D. E. and Baylis, J. R. (1982). Appendix: A 61, 835846.
world survey of evidence for vocal learning in birds. In: Nordby, J. C., Campbell, S. E., Burt, J. M., and Beecher,
Kroodsma, D. E. and Miller, E. H., eds. Acoustic M. D. (2000). Social influences during song development
Communication in Birds, Vol. 2, pp. 311337. Academic in the song sparrow: a laboratory experiment
Press, New York, New York. simulating field conditions. Animal Behaviour, 59,
Kroodsma, D. E., Albano, D. J., Houlihan, P. W., and Wells, 11871197.
J. A. (1995). Song development by black-capped Nowicki, S., Peters, S., Searcy, W. A., and Clayton, C.
chickadees (Parus atricapillus) and Carolina chickadees (1999). The development of within-song type variation
(P. carolinensis). Auk, 112, 2943. in song sparrows. Animal Behaviour, 57, 12571264.
Kroodsma, D. E., Byers, B. E., Halkin, S. L., Hill, C., Minis, Nowicki, S., Searcy, W. A., and Peters, S. (2002). Brain
D., Bolsinger, J. R., Dawson, J. A., Donelan, E., development, song learning and mate choice in birds: a
Farrington, J., Gill, F. B., Houlihan, P., Innes, D., Keller, review and experimental test of the nutritional stress
G., Macaulay, L., Marantz, C. A., Ortiz, J., Stoddard, P. hypothesis. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 188,
K., and Wilda, K. (1999). Geographic variation in black- 10031014.
capped chickadee songs and singing behavior. Auk, 116, lveczky, B. P., Andalman, A. S., and Fee, M. S. (2005).
387402. Vocal experimentation in the juvenile songbird requires
Marler, P. and Peters, S. (1982). Subsong and plastic song: a basal ganglia circuit. PLoS Biology, 3, e153.
Their role in the vocal learning process. In: Kroodsma, Pyle, P. (1997). Identification Guide to North American Birds.
D. E. and Miller, E. H., eds. Acoustic Communication in Part I. Slate Creek Press, Bolinas, California.
Birds, Vol. 2, pp. 2550. Academic Press, New York, Ratcliffe, L. and Otter, K. (1996). Sex differences in song
New York. recognition. In: Kroodsma, D. E. and Miller, E. H.,
Marler, P. and Tamura, M. (1964). Culturally transmitted eds. Ecology and Evolution of Acoustic Communication in
patterns of vocal behavior in sparrows. Science, 146, Birds, pp. 339355. Cornell University Press, Ithaca,
14831486. New York.
McGregor, P. K. and Krebs, J. R. (1989). Song learning in Schroeder, D. J. and Wiley, R. H. (1983). Communication
adult great tits (Parus major): Effects of neighbours. with shared song themes in tufted titmice. Auk, 100,
Behaviour, 108, 139159. 414424.
McGregor, P. K., Dabelsteen, T., Shepherd, M., and Searcy, W. A., Nowicki, S., Hughes, M., and Peters, S.
Pedersen, S. B. (1992). The signal value of matched (2002). Geographic song discrimination in relation to
singing in great tits: evidence from interactive playback dispersal distances in song sparrows. American
experiments. Animal Behaviour, 43, 987998. Naturalist, 159, 221230.
A C O U S T I C VA R I AT I O N I N B I R D S O N G 197

Shackleton, S. A. and Ratcliffe, L. (1993). Development of Tchernichovski, O., Mitra, P. P., Lints, T., and Nottebohm,
song in hand-reared black-capped chickadees. Wilson F. (2001). Dynamics of the vocal imitation process:
Bulletin, 105, 637644. how a zebra finch learns its song. Science, 291,
Smith, S. M. (1991). The Black-capped Chickadee: Behavioral 25642569.
Ecology and Natural History. Cornell University Press, Thielcke, G. (1969). Geographic variation in bird vocaliza-
Ithaca, New York. tions. In: Hinde, R. A., ed. Bird Vocalizations, pp.
Stoddard, P. K., Beecher, M. D., and Willis, M. S. (1988). 311341. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Response of territorial male song sparrows to song Van Oort, H. and Otter, K. A. (2005). Natal nutrition and
types and variations. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, the habitat distributions of male and female black-
22, 125130. capped chickadees. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 83,
Tchernichovski, O., Lints, T., Deregnaucourt, S., and 14951501.
Mitra, P. P. (2004). Analysis of the entire song develop- Vitousek, P. M. (1994). Beyond global warmingecology
ment: methods and rationale. Annals of the New York and global change. Ecology, 75, 18611876.
Academy of Science, 1016, 348363.
This page intentionally left blank
CHAPTER 13

Information and the chick-a-dee


call: Communicating with a
complex vocal system
Jeffrey R. Lucas and Todd M. Freeberg

chickadees. We also review some of our own stud-


13.1 Introduction
ies on the chick-a-dee call of Carolina chickadees, as
The chick-a-dee call is one of the most commonly well as studies from others on various chickadee
used, and complex, vocal signals of chickadees species, all of which are beginning to address
and is given by all members of the genus Poecile. exactly what chickadees might be communicating
The complexity of the chick-a-dee call arises not when the birds vary the structure of their calls.
just from its structural characteristics, but also from Third, we draw some conclusions about chick-a-dee
its potential functions in social organization. call complexity and raise the need for comparative
Earlier work demonstrated that the call is unusual research to elucidate chick-a-dee call evolution and
because it has a computable syntax that can gener- function.
ate an open-ended number of unique call types Most of the work on North American Paridae
(e.g. Hailman et al. 1985, 1987; Hailman and Ficken vocal behavior has concentrated on chickadee
1986; Ficken et al. 1994). Hypothetically, this species. One reason for this concentration is the
means the call could convey an enormous amount large repertoires of diverse vocalizations possessed
of information. However, despite two decades since by these species (e.g. Hailman 1989; Hailman and
the generative properties of the call were described, Ficken 1996). The different vocal signals used by
we know little about how much information is chickadees and related species are believed to
actually conveyed by this call. In this sense, represent different communicative entitiesfor
Hailman et al. (1987, p. 63) were prescient in example, an individual producing fee-bees is
suggesting that the information encoded in these presumably communicating something different
calls presents a difficult challenge in decoding. than if it were producing chick-a-dee calls. Even
Nevertheless, some progress has been made in within the chick-a-dee call itself, structurally differ-
the study of this fascinating system, as we demon- ent chick-a-dee variants (e.g. calls differing in note
strate below. composition) likely hold different meanings to
The review is broken into three sections. First, we receivers. This raises the question of what is being
describe some general characteristics of chickadee communicated about the signaler or the signalers
vocal behavior and some of the major arguments immediate environmental context. Indeed what,
regarding definitions of communication and exactly, is communication?
information, as these ideas are important for Most definitions of communication (e.g.
interpreting the notions of complexity in the Burghardt 1970, p. 16; Wilson 1975, p. 176; Hailman
chick-a-dee call. Second, we review work of the 1977, p. 52) focus on three parts of communica-
Hailmans and Fickens on the structural complexity tiona feature (often a signal) of one individual
of chick-a-dee calls in black-capped and Mexican (the signaler) affects the behavior of another

199
200 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

individual (the receiver). However, two major argu- perspective was carried out on black-capped
ments have arisen over the definition. The first chickadees. Hailman et al. (1985) recorded roughly
argument relates to whether the signaler primarily 3500 calls from at least 20 individuals. The call was
benefits, or the signaler and receiver both benefit, described as being composed of four note types
from the communicative interaction (e.g. Dawkins (see Fig. 13.1 for Carolina chickadee examples).
and Krebs 1978; Owings and Morton 1998; Two note typesA and Bare relatively pure
Bradbury and Vehrencamp 1998). One can argue tones and span a considerable frequency range. A
that the target of selection argument is tangential notes tend to have a higher peak frequency, with a
to the question of how and whether communica- short ascending arm followed by an abrupt and
tion occurs, although clearly it is important to long descending arm (in Carolina chickadees, the
determining the types of communicative events ascending arm is typically much longer than the
that occur (e.g. distinctions between signals and descending arm in terms of frequency span). B
cues as discussed in Maynard Smith and Harper notes tend to be relatively lower in frequency and
2003). The second argument revolves around shorter in duration, and both the ascending and
the usefulness of the notion of information or descending arms are relatively similar in the fre-
information transfer. quencies they span.1 C notes are harsh sounding
A precise definition of information has proven notes with a fairly complex and noisy structure,
difficult to pin down, despite a general under- with a relatively lower pitch than A or B notes. D
standing of the concept. There are two major notes have a longer duration that the other three
viewsone is the view that if communication note types, and are relatively unmodulated in
involves one individual signaling about some- terms of frequency, with a characteristic harmonic-
thing to another individual, then that about like structure when seen in sonagram form.
something is the information. The other view Hailman et al.s (1985) sample of nearly 3500 calls
defines information not in the terminology of resulted in 362 calls that were distinct in note
meaning, but mathematically as a measure of composition. This large number of unique calls
ones freedom of choice when one selects a mes- illustrates one component of chick-a-dee call
sage in the simplest cases, to be measured by the complexitythe number of distinct calls (in terms
logarithm of the number of available choices of note composition) increases with sample size.
(Shannon and Weaver 1949, p. 9). Smith (1977, This is very different from, for example, the songs
p. 193) bridges these two ideas nicely by defining of many songbird species, in which the sampling of
information as an abstract property of entities and dozens or a few hundred songs will often com-
events that makes their characteristics predictable pletely capture the song repertoire of an individ-
to individuals with suitable sensory equipment for ualthe chick-a-dee call possesses the language-like
receiving the information. Information thus enables feature of being open-ended.
such individuals to make choices (see also
Halliday 1983). In the next section, we attempt to
13.2.2 Variability in note types
place these ideas of information in the context of
the chick-a-dee call by discussing the seminal work One aspect of the chick-a-dee complex that needs
on black-capped chickadees by the Hailmans and more attention is how many notes any one species
Fickens and our work on Carolina chickadees. actually uses in its callswe cant understand the
call complex completely without understanding its

13.2 Information and the 1


Note that Bloomfield et al. (2003) redefined Carolina chicka-
chick-a-dee call dee A notes in a series as A, B1, and B2 notes. Given the prece-
dence in the literature, we will define their B1 and B2 notes as
13.2.1 Basic note types of chick-a-dee calls A notes and suggest that only their B3 notes be called true B
notes. Of course, the only way to resolve this issue is to evalu-
The first detailed investigation of the chick-a-dee ate whether the birds themselves distinguish these notes as
call from the mathematically-based information we have defined themsee also Chapter 10.
THE CHICK-A-DEE CALL 201

kHz
10
A A A A A A A A A
8

(a) 6
4
2

0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 1.2 1.4 s

kHz
10
A A A A C C C
8
6
(b)
4
2

0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 1.2 1.4 s

kHz
10 A A C D D D
8
(c) 6
4
2

0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 1.2 1.4 s

kHz
10 B D D D
8
(d) 6
4
2

0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 1.2 1.4 s

kHz
10 A Dh D D D D
8
(e) 6
4
2

0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 1.2 1.4 s

kHz
10 A A C D D D D D D D D
8
6
(f)
4
2

0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 1.2 1.4 s

Figure 13.1 Sonagrams of Carolina chickadee calls that vary in syntax using A, B, C, and D note classifications. (a) String of A notes. (b) Four
A notes, three C notes. (c) Two A notes, one C note, three D notes. (d) One B note, three D notes. (e) One A note, one hybrid D note, four D
notes. (f) Two A notes, one C note, eight D notes. Sonagrams were generated using Avisoft SASLab Pro with an FFT length of 512, frame 75%,
and Blackman window.
202 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

elements. Clearly, virtually all chickadees have structurally similar to the soft dee described by
some variant of the A, B, C, D series of primary Smith (1972).
note types (Hailman 1989). However, there are Smith (1972) defined four different D notes in
some intriguing species differences. Mountain Carolina chickadees: dee, broken dee, harsh dee, and
chickadees (Bloomfield et al. 2004) and Mexican soft dee. Ficken et al. (1994) indicated that D notes in
chickadees (Ficken et al. 1994) have a common Mexican chickadees varied from banded (Smiths
hybrid note described by Ficken et al. (1994) as an A dee note) to uniformly noisy (Smiths harsh dee).
running into a D note. We found these notes in Similarly, mobbing black-capped chickadees
Carolina chickadees (e.g. Fig. 13.1e), but they were employ a noisy variant of the D note (Ficken and
rare enough that we concluded that they were Popp 1996), and female black-capped chickadees
anomalies. Given that this note occurs in 94% of use a broken dee when they solicit food from their
mountain chickadee calls analyzed by Bloomfield mates (Ficken et al. 1978).
et al. (2004), our notion of anomalous notes needs In some cases, there are species differences in the
to be revisited. Interestingly, Mexican chickadees structure of notes. Introductory notes of most
(Ficken et al. 1994) have, for the most part, lost one chickadees are pure tones (Carolina chickadees,
of the four primary notes from their repertoireB Smith 1972; black-capped chickadees, Ficken et al.
notes are quite rare in their chick-a-dee calls. Also, in 1978; mountain chickadees, Gaddis 1985; willow
mountain chickadees (Gaddis 1985) C notes and D tits, Haftorn 1993). However, the A note is a
notes appear to be a single graded series (see also frequency-modulated trill in Mexican chickadees
Hailman et al. 1985). Thus, for most chickadee (Ficken 1990; Ficken et al. 1994). The D note of
species, it is arguably an open question as to how Mexican chickadees is also noisier than its North
many distinct note types occur in the call. American counterparts (Ficken 1990), although as
New or hybrid notes notwithstanding, it is clear noted above, noisy D notes are found in a number
that in many species the A, C, and D notes have of other species as well.
several subtypes (Fig. 13.2). The A note in particu- A critical question related to our perception of
lar is quite varied. A spectral analysis by Nowicki note types is whether the birds themselves share
and Nelson (1990) showed that A and B notes in this perception. An acoustical analysis of black-
black-capped chickadees grade into each other. capped chickadee note types by Nowicki and
A note variants should therefore grade into each Nelson (1990) suggested that they should: A and B
other as well, but these variants are sufficiently dis- notes are statistically distinguishable from one
tinct that Smith (1972) defined at least four different another, although they tend to be a part of a graded
A-like notes in Carolina chickadees (high see, high series, and C and D notes are quite distinct. Sturdy
tee, loud tee, lisping tee), and our own analyses of et al. (2000) showed that black-capped chickadees
Carolina chickadee calls (T. M. Freeberg and J. R. can learn to discriminate between note types more
Lucas, unpublished data) support this conclusion rapidly than within variants of the same note type,
(Fig. 13.2). suggesting that the birds do indeed discriminate
C notes also vary. Smith (1972) described at least among note types and generalize within note types.
two, the chip and chick notes, that were elicited in Moreover, the birds behavior was consistent with
different situations: the chip is an aggressive call the suggestion of Nowicki and Nelson (1990) that
usually given in a string without D notes, and the A and B notes are more similar to one another than
chick note is a component of the more standard to the other note types. This is good news for the
chick-a-dee call given in a variety of circumstances. field, although Sturdy et al.s (2000) important con-
Similarly, Haftorn (1993) suggested that willow tits tribution needs to be extended to other chickadee
have two C-like notes, one used in mobbing calls species.
and the other in more standard chick-a-dee calls. We Arguably the most interesting aspect of the chick-
have sonagrams of a third C-like note in Carolina a-dee call complex is that it shares some similarities
chickadees that Arch McCallum (personal commu- to human language (Hailman et al. 1985, 1987;
nication) called a begging C, which appears Hailman and Ficken 1986). As we will discuss
THE CHICK-A-DEE CALL 203

(a)
Tseet notes, 2 beggingC notes, A note
kHz
10 Tseet C C A
B B

0.2 0.4 s

(b)
Tseet, 3 A notes plus chip (C-variant) string
kHz
10
Ts eet A A A Chip
8
6
4
2

0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 1.2 1.4 1.6 s

(c)
2 A notes, Abrupt T note
kHz
10
A A AT
8
6
4
2

0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0s

(d)
Rapid D string
kHz
10
8
6
4
2

0.2 0.4 0.6 s

(e)
Harsh D string
kHz
10
8
6
4
2

0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 1.2 1.4s

Figure 13.2 Sonagrams of Carolina chickadee calls illustrating note type diversity (partly based on Smith 1972). Sonagrams generated
as in Fig. 13.1.
204 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

below, the call system has a rudimentary


13.2.4 Identity
syntaxnotes that occur in any given call follow
simple rules of note ordering. In addition to syntax, One kind of information a signal can communicate
the literature on chick-a-dee calls has addressed a is identity. Identity can range from individual to
number of other non-syntactical components of group to species, although we will limit our discus-
information. We will address these alternative sion to the population level and below.
sources of information first, in part because they Signals communicating individual identity are
may influence how we think about syntax, or at well established (Bradbury and Vehrencamp 1998),
least about constraints on syntax. at least in relatively stable social groups (Slater et al.
2000). In black-capped chickadees, Nowicki (1989)
suggested that the D note plays a special role in
13.2.3 Signal redundancy
encoding identity and Hailman et al. (1987) sug-
The first component of information we will address gested that this may be why chick-a-dee calls tend to
is internal information, or the redundancy in a be more likely than chance to have at least one D
signal. Redundancy results from multiple signals, or note in themthe trailing D essentially acts as an
multiple components of a signal, encoding similar identification flag. More recent work by Charrier
information. Theoretically, redundancy increases et al. (2004) and Bloomfield et al. (2004) has made
the probability that receivers correctly perceive the this viewpoint even more intricate. These studies
information embodied in the signal, although showed that both mountain chickadees and black-
redundancy also reduces the total amount of infor- capped chickadees (and, by extension, other
mation that can be encoded in the signal (Bradbury species) have individually specific introductory
and Vehrencamp 1998). A more practical aspect of notes in addition to individually specific D notes,
redundancy is that we can potentially over-interpret although both studies concluded that the D note
experimental results if we ignore redundant cues. has the highest potential for identity coding.
Freeberg et al. (2003) showed that, in Carolina The implication is that identity coding need not
chickadees, the spectral structure of the first A, C, reside only in the D note, but playback experiments
and D notes in a string of similar notes is signifi- are needed to resolve this issue.
cantly correlated with the note composition in that Mammen and Nowicki (1981) were the first
call. This suggests that a chickadee might be able to authors to suggest that flock-level call properties
predict the syntax of the rest of the call even if it develop in black-capped chickadees. This possibil-
were able to hear only a fraction of that call, ity was strengthened by a playback experiment
although this statement must be tested. Nowicki showing that birds played foreign calls were less
and Nelson (1990) offer a complementary view- likely to visit a feeder than those played calls of
point: single A and B notes in a string of notes tend flock members (Nowicki 1983). Nowicki (1989)
to decline in frequency and amplitude (black- then put five birds together in an aviary flock and
capped chickadees: Hailman et al. 1985; Carolina demonstrated convergence in one component of
chickadees: Freeberg et al. 2003). Chickadees may the D note of the call. However, a potential con-
be able to use the change in frequency and ampli- found of the Mammen and Nowicki (1981) study is
tude as an index of syntax, again without hearing that syntax was not controlled for in the spectral
the entire call. analysis; indeed, their data suggest that birds from
The statistical analysis of Freeberg et al. (2003) different flocks tended to have calls with different
suggested that we can expect note properties to syntax. Given that syntax itself affects note struc-
change if syntax changes. We need to be careful ture (Freeberg et al. 2003), Mammen and Nowickis
about reading anything more complex into the call. (1981) results may have had artifacts of flock-
As such, conclusions about chick-a-dee calls could be specific differences in note composition. It is also
premature if spectral analyses fail to correct for syn- possible that the putative flock-level recognition
tax (e.g. Nowicki 1989; Mammen and Nowicki detected in these studies was actually due to
1981; Templeton et al. 2005). individual recognition. Nowicki (1989) found a
THE CHICK-A-DEE CALL 205

single trait converging in a single flock in the in chick-a-dee calls are not used equallyD notes are
laboratory; this is not, however, unequivocal far more common. This means that the actual infor-
evidence for convergence among flock members mation in terms of note use in chick-a-dee calls is
of a chick-a-dee call structure that marks flock lower than the maximum possible information. The
membership. uncertainty measured for actual use of the different
There is stronger evidence for population-level units of a communication system is referred to as
variation in the structure of chick-a-dee calls. For the zero-order uncertainty, and is calculated as:
example, Baker and Logue (2003) were able to
U0 Pi (log2 Pi)
identify the population of origin of 100% of black-
capped chickadees calls based on multitaper where Pi is the probability of each of the i units
spectral analysis (see Tchernichovski et al. 2000). occurring in the system. In the case of the chick-a-dee
Spectrogram cross-correlation was nearly as effec- call of black-capped chickadees, there is indeed a
tive. Similarly, Freeberg et al. (2003) demonstrated drop-off from UM to U0. However, a much greater
population-level differences in Carolina chickadees drop-off in uncertainty occurs when one assesses
in both A and D notes. These results mirror the the transition probabilities between pairs of notes.
demonstration of population-specific dialects in the This measure of first-order uncertainty, U1, repre-
gargle call of black-capped chickadees (Ficken et al. sents the uncertainty of a given unit to occur in a
1985; Miyasato and Baker 1999). sequence when a previous unit has already
occurred. It is calculated as:

13.2.5 Syntactical information U1 Pij (log2 Pj|i)

There are at least three ways that we can begin to where Pij is the probability of the i and j note occur-
understand the meaning of the chick-a-dee call ring in the ijth sequence, and Pj|i is the conditional
from the perspective of syntactical information. The probability of the j unit occurring given that the
first, pioneered by Hailman et al. (1985), is a struc- i unit has occurred. For the chickadee call this
tural analysis of syntax per se to determine the hypo- analysis would address the ability to predict that,
thetical information that may reside in a signaling for example, a D note will follow if a C note occurs
system. The second is a natural-history approach, in a call. Hailman et al. (1985) found that there is a
documenting calls produced in natural settings and considerable drop-off in information at this level of
what behavioral or other contextual correlates exist uncertaintyif a researcher (and, presumably, a
(e.g. Smith 1972). The third is an experimental chickadee receiver) detects one note type in a call,
analysis, using playbacks to test predicted informa- there is a good probability of predicting what the
tion content in the calls (e.g. Freeberg and Lucas next note type will be in the call.
2002; Clucas et al. 2004; Templeton et al. 2005). The preceding discussion leads into one of the
other major structural features of the chick-a-dee call.
13.2.5.1 Syntax Notes and pairs of notes do not occur with equal
Hailman et al. (1985) measured the amount of infor- probability. Instead, the chick-a-dee call obeys rules
mation that might be conveyed by the chick-a-dee of note ordering, a simple form of syntax. In black-
call system of black-capped chickadees. From capped chickadees, the two most common call
Shannon and Weavers (1949) theory of communi- structures are [A][D] and [B][C][D], with brackets
cation, a communicative system with four distinct indicating that the particular note type can occur
units (note types) has a maximum uncertainty of more than once. In other words, if the following
two bits of information (UM log2 N, where notes occurred in a nine-note call, BBCCCDDDD,
N number of units). The maximum information they would virtually always (e.g. over 99% of the
in a communicative system occurs when all of time) occur in the order [B][C][D]. Taken together,
the systems units are used with equal probability. the chick-a-dee call represents an interesting case of
As is the case with letters in the English language an open-ended communicative system that is
(e.g. relative use of e vs. x), however, note types nonetheless constrained by its note ordering rules.
206 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

In addition to the constraints upon the call (a survivorship plot) with the expected cumula-
imposed by the note ordering rules, other con- tive number of notes based on a semi-Markovian
straints appear to limit the diversity of potential call model. A departure from the simple semi-
structures (Hailman et al. 1987). For example, as the Markovian expectation implies some meaning in
number of A, B, and C notes increase in a call, the the note composition of the call beyond a simple
number of D notes that might occur decreases. repetition of notes. They found that A notes fit
Therefore, there seems to be a constraint on the expectations almost exactly. B notes did not: calls
overall number of notes that can occur in an with three or fewer notes fit expectations whereas
average call. However, calls with extremely large those with four or more B notes were too common.
numbers of D notes are more common than As with A notes, the probability of repeating a
expected by chance, suggesting that the constraints C note was constant, albeit somewhat less than
on introductory notes are relaxed when calls expected by chance. The survivorship curve for D
contain many D notes (Hailman et al. 1987). notes departed completely from semi-Markovian
This mathematic approach to the question of expectations, with too many short-D calls, too few
information in the chick-a-dee call was extended to intermediate-D calls, and too many long-D calls.
another species, Mexican chickadees (P. sclateri; Hailman et al. (1987) suggested that this distribu-
Ficken et al. 1994), and interesting comparative tion represented a compound of two or more
results emerge. The Mexican chickadee chick-a-dee separate processes, and potentially separate
call system is open-ended, the C note is more com- syntactical functions.
mon than the D note, and the B note is extremely We repeated Hailman et al.s (1987) analysis with
rare. Notes follow the A-B-C-D note-ordering rule a preliminary data set of 2153 Carolina chickadee
shared by black-capped chickadees. The most com- calls recorded in non-manipulated field settings in
mon call structures were [A][D], [C], and [A], and eastern Tennessee (nine sites) and central Indiana
calls tended to be shorter in note number than (six sites). The uncertainty measures for Carolina
black-capped chickadee calls. Ficken et al. (1994, chickadees for this sample of the field recordings
p. 80) indicate that, relative to the chick-a-dee call of showed a similar pattern to those reported for
black-capped chickadees, the rarity of B notes black-capped chickadees (Hailman et al. 1985) and
and the shorter note length of calls means that Mexican chickadees (Ficken et al. 1994), with a
the Mexican chickadees utterances tend to be marked reduction between zero-order and first-
syntactically simpler, although not necessarily order uncertainty (UM 2, U0 1.49, U1 0.63). A
semantically simpler This quote nicely cap- and D notes did not meet semi-Markovian expecta-
tures the two approaches to information discussed tions (Fig. 13.3). Long strings of A notes (six A
earlier. For information as a mathematically- notes/calls) were more common than expected by
defined measure, Mexican chickadee calls appear chance. The survivorship curve for D notes was
to convey less information than black-capped qualitatively similar to that of black-capped chicka-
chickadee calls. Mountain chickadee calls also dees: too many calls with a few notes, too few with
appear to convey less information than black- a large number of notes (1025), and too many with
capped or Carolina chickadees because their calls a very large number of notes.
are substantially shorter (threefour notes/call vs. We asked whether our chick-a-dee calls met
sixeight notes/call, respectively; Bloomfield et al. expectations of Mandlebrots modification of Zipfs
2004). For information as meaning, however, it is Law (see Hailman et al. 1985). Stated simply, Zipfs
an open question in these species as to which call law argues that the frequency of utterances should
system conveys more information. be reciprocally related to their frequency rankthe
Hailman et al. (1987) published an additional tenth most common utterance (word in a human
method of evaluating the syntactical properties of language, call syntax in the chick-a-dee call) should
black-capped chickadees calls. They compared occur with 1/10th the frequency of the most
the cumulative number of calls containing at common utterance. Human language meets this
least some number of A, B, C, and D notes criterion, but black-capped chick-a-dee calls do not.
THE CHICK-A-DEE CALL 207

A notes D notes
100.00 100.00
Cumulative percent of calls

10.00 10.00

1.00 1.00

0.10 0.10

0.01 0.01
0 5 10 15 0 10 20 30 40
No. A-notes/call No. D-notes/call

Figure 13.3 Survivorship plots of A notes and D notes of the Carolina chickadees. These are based on a sample of 2153 calls. The triangles
represent calls in the sample, the line is the predicted survivorship from a semi-Markovian model, based on the transitions between same-type
notes (e.g. in the left panel, the transition probability from A to another A note).

However, black-capped calls do fit a broader


form suggested by Mandelbrot (Hailman et al.
1985). Interestingly, our Carolina chickadees do not
fit Mandelbrots function (Fig. 13.4), at least based
Percent of use

1.0
on a least-squares best fit of the data. Nonetheless,
the general increase in cumulative call types with
an increase in the number of calls sampled
indicates that the call system is generative, or 0.1
open-ended.
As Hailman et al. (1987) showed in black-capped 1.0 10.0 100.0
chickadees, the probability that a call ends (instead Frequency rank
of continuing with a new note) increases with an
increase in the number of A and C notes Figure 13.4 Percent use of chick-a-dee variants as a function the
(Table 13.1). In Carolina chickadees, longer strings frequency rank (1 most common). Triangles are data from our set
of 2153 calls of Carolina chickadees. Line represents the best fit line:
of A notes (eightten) are more likely to transition p i(rk)s, where p percent use, r rank, and i, k, and s are
to C notes whereas shorter strings (onefive) are as fit constants (see Hailman et al. 1985).
likely to transition to B or D notes. B notes almost
always transition into D notes or end the call.
We need to add a caveat that the differences
between black-capped and Carolina chickadees instructive to see this analysis done on European
could result from differences in the field recording tits, particularly the marsh and willow tits, which
contexts. Assuming that this caveat does not are closely related to the North American chicka-
generate a bias in our data sets, the results suggest dees (Gill et al. 2005).
that Carolina chickadees use long strings of intro-
ductory notes in different ways (with different 13.2.5.2 Context
meanings?) than black-capped chickadees. It would Studies of correlations between chick-a-dee variants
be instructive to perform this analysis on mountain and behavior are critical because they give us some
and Mexican chickadees, given the species insight into the potential for syntactically-mediated
differences reported earlier. It would also be information transfer. Results to date clearly
208 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

Table 13.1 Transition probabilities for strings of A, B and C notes in chick-a-dee calls of Carolina chickadees; in each case, the number of
elements in a string of same-type notes is given, followed by the probability that the string ends in another note type or silence (A0, B0, or C0)

AA AB AC AD A0 BB BC BD B0 CC CD C0

1 0.11 0.32 0.36 0.21 1 0.06 0.82 0.12 1 0.81 0.19


2 0.20 0.20 0.29 0.31 2 0.5 0 0.5 2 0.77 0.23
3 0.10 0.22 0.29 0.39 3 0 0.5 0.5 3 0.48 0.52
4 0.08 0.26 0.08 0.58 4 0.33 0.67
5 0.06 0.14 0.28 0.52 5 0.63 0.37
6 0 0.11 0.05 0.84 6 0.36 0.64
7 0 0.11 0.35 0.54 7 0.43 0.57
8 0 0.14 0 0.86 8 0.50 0.50
9 0 0.17 0 0.83
10 0 0.20 0 0.80
11 0 0 1.0 0
1317 0 0 0 1.0

demonstrate that different chick-a-dee variants are but the same number of C and D notes. Baker and
given under different conditions. Becker (2002) showed a similar pattern, with more
The first extensive, though qualitative, study of B notes and fewer A notes given under more imme-
this type was published by Smith (1972) on diate risk (1 m vs. 6 m from a stuffed prairie falcon),
Carolina chickadees. Smith suggested that different but no difference in the number of C or D notes. In
note types had different meanings. For example, contrast, Templeton et al. (2005) showed that the
chip (a C-variant) notes are aggressive calls whereas number of D notes correlated strongly with the
chick notes (another C variant) are non-aggressive, intensity of risk represented by different species of
often heard around feeding stations. Haftorns predators. It is not clear why these results are so dif-
(1993) study of willow tits demonstrated similar ferent, although there were considerable method-
meaning in the C note, with one variant character- ological differences across the three studies.
istic of alarm calls and another typically embedded In addition to syntactical cues, some cues may be
in less aggressive calls. given by call rate itself, with higher rates indicating
Ficken et al. (1994) suggested that A notes are more intense conditions. Black-capped chickadee
given by Mexican chickadees moving in space, mobbing calls, for example, tend to be given at
whereas C notes are given in response to a higher rates under more immediate risk (Baker and
disturbing stimulus (e.g. when mobbing a screech Becker 2002). Carolina chickadee chick-a-dee call
owl tape) or when birds change directions. D notes rates are higher when the birds are light-weight
tend to be given by perched birds. These trends for and hungry than when they are heavy or sated
A and D notes are similar to those reported by Smith (Lucas et al. 1999)chick-a-dee calls may encode
(1972) for Carolina chickadees. Gaddis (1985) found information about signaler physiological condition.
that chick-a-dee variants were context specific in We (K. Bledsoe and J. Lucas, unpublished data)
mountain chickadees. For example B notes are have limited data on two Carolina chickadees that
given when birds leave food; A notes are given indicate just this: D note fundamental frequency
when birds fly up, and [A][D] strings are given in and duration correlated strongly with changes in
flocks. corticosterone levels. These results are consistent
There are at least three studies of the syntactical with Owings and Mortons (1998) assessment/
cues given in black-capped chickadee mobbing management model of communication, although
calls. Hurd (1996) suggested that mobbing calls had clearly we need more extensive studies of this
more introductory notes than non-mobbing calls aspect of the chick-a-dee complex.
THE CHICK-A-DEE CALL 209

13.2.5.3 Playback studies test for the potential role of seasonal context on
We can use playback experiments to test implica- receivers responses to the calls. The experiment
tions about syntactical information derived from tested whether chickadees respond to manipula-
field (or laboratory) observational studies. Here we tion of the two components of syntactical organiza-
focus on recent field-based playback studies tion, note composition and note order, but did not
(Sturdy et al. review laboratory-based studies of test any specific hypotheses about the meaning of
perception in Chapter 10). Freeberg and Lucas chick-a-dee variants. The birds showed no differen-
(2002) proposed that the C-note (chick variant) was tial response to calls with atypical note ordering
food related, based on preliminary field observa- that varied in note composition (CACACACA vs.
tions. They tested this by broadcasting either C-rich DCDCDCDC). Birds did, however, distinguish
or D-rich chick-a-dee calls at a temporary seed stand. between calls that varied in note composition if the
Consistent with the hypothesis, birds tended note order followed the species-typical A-B-C-D
to come to the stand and take seeds in response to ordering. Season and social context also affected
C-rich calls but never took a seed in response to the birds response to the playbacks. The number of
D-rich calls. Moreover, the rate of chick-a-dee calling introductory notes in the responding birds chick-a-
was significantly higher in playbacks with C-rich dee calls was affected by note composition in the fall
calls than those with D-rich calls. There are at least but not in the spring: AAAACCCC playback calls
two alternative explanations for this result. One is elicited A/B-rich responses, and D-rich playback
that the C note is indeed a food-associated note. calls elicited C-rich responses. But this latter
The second is that D-rich calls are aggressive calls response was evident only when white-breasted
that elicit an aggressive reaction by receivers (and, nuthatches and tufted titmice (two dominant het-
by comparison, C-rich calls are non-aggressive). erospecifics) were present. In the fall, the number of
While we cannot distinguish these alternatives with D notes in the call was also positively correlated
this experimental design, the results indicate that with the number of birds responding, but this rela-
chick-a-dee variants vary in their meaning to tionship was positive only when heterospecifics
receivers. were not present. Only the number of birds
Templeton et al. (2005) tested the relative function responding affected the number of D notes in the
of black-capped chickadee mobbing calls. They spring [note: Table 2 in Clucas et al. (2004) is correct,
found that the number of D notes was negatively but the discussion of this pattern in the text of that
correlated with predator wingspan and body paper and Figure 8 are not correct].
length (smaller, more dangerous, predators elicit Our results suggest that both note ordering and
more D notes). Smaller predators elicited D notes note composition affect the information content of
with a narrower band width and more narrowly the chick-a-dee complex. In addition, context (season
spaced overtones. Templeton et al. (2005) used play- and presence of dominant heterospecifics) matters,
backs of the mobbing calls in the absence of preda- as predicted by Hailman et al. 1985 (also see Leger
tors as an important test of the proposed 1993; Marler and Evans 1996).
information embodied in the calls. The birds gave
responses appropriate to the predator that was
13.3 Conclusions, concessions, and a
being mobbed when the calls were first recorded,
call for comparative work
suggesting the calls conveyed some quantitative
index of predation risk. The chick-a-dee call is complex both structurally and
Finally, Clucas et al. (2004) monitored Carolina functionally. The structure of chick-a-dee calls can
chickadees responses to artificially constructed depend upon numerous characteristics of the
calls that varied in both note composition signaler or its contextenergetic state, flock/local
(AAAACCCC, AAAADDDD, and CCCCDDDD) population, sex, feeding behavior, flight behavior,
and note ordering (AAAACCCC vs. CACACACA, and presence of avian predators. Thus, the chick-a-dee
CCCCDDDD vs. DCDCDCDC). The playback was call can potentially convey a great deal of
repeated in two seasons, spring and fall/winter, to information about the different states, behavioral
210 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

probabilities, events, etc. relevant to the signals Playback studies in field settings are one of the
sender. Furthermore, chick-a-dee calls of black- strongest ways of testing whether structural vari-
capped chickadees (Hailman et al.1985), Mexican ation in chick-a-dee calls conveys information (oper-
chickadees (Ficken et al. 1994), and Carolina chicka- ant conditioning experiments offer an important
dees (Fig. 13.4) are generative and possess fairly high complementary approach, see Chapter 10).
levels of uncertainty. Thus, in terms of information Unfortunately, playback studies are relatively rare
as a mathematical measure of binary choices, call for this call system in the Paridae. Those that have
structural rules result in an information-rich signal- been conducted, however, indicate that chick-a-dee
ing system. In addition, the number of a particular calls that vary in their note composition and/or in
note type seems to influence acoustic characteristics the structural characteristics of their notes can
and numbers of other notes, which suggests an inter- influence receiver behavior dramatically. In
esting form of redundancy in these calls. This redun- Carolina chickadees, receivers respond differently
dancy may increase a signalers ability to transmit, in a feeding context to calls containing a large
and a receivers ability to exploit, a message of a call, number of C notes relative to calls containing no C
while limiting the total number of different messages notes and a relatively large number of D notes
a single call might convey. As we noted earlier, our (Freeberg and Lucas 2002). In black-capped chicka-
claim about chick-a-dee call complexity is by no dees, individuals respond differently to playbacks
means novel to usHailman et al. (1985) first raised of chick-a-dee calls of flockmates relative to
it two decades ago in their work with black-capped chick-a-dee calls of non-flockmates (Nowicki 1983),
chickadees. In this final section of the chapter, we and acoustic variation in D note structure is
draw some conclusions about this complexity and thought to be the basis for this discrimination
why it might exist, point out some gaps in our (Mammen and Nowicki 1981; although see our
understanding of the call system, and suggest a need caveat about these studies described above).
to make broader comparative analyses of the call Playbacks of calls recorded in a high threat con-
system in other Paridae groups. text of a northern pygmy owl, Glaucidium gnoma,
A possible driving force for such a complex call resulted in more receivers approaching the play-
system in chickadees might be their complex and back speaker and also producing more chick-a-dee
fairly atypical social structure (at least for temper- calls, relative to playbacks of calls recorded in a
ate zone birds). In late summer and early fall, low threat context of a great horned owl, Bubo
chickadee femalemale pairs join other pairs (and virginianus (Templeton et al. 2005). The results from
often juveniles) to form a flock with a relatively these three playback studies in two chickadee
stable membership (Ekman 1989; Smith 1991). species indicate that variation in chick-a-dee call
Flocks remain together, generally defending their structure can potentially convey information about
territory from other flocks or individual intruders, a feeding context, identity, and degree of threat cor-
until the following early spring, when flocks break responding to predator size.
up into breeding pairs. Thus, chickadee social In terms of general structural characteristics of
organization changes over the course of a year from the note types and note ordering in chick-a-dee
femalemale pairs in the breeding season to larger, calls, all of the North American Paridae possess
stable social units in the fall and winter. Could it be a chick-a-dee-like call (Hailman 1989), although
that the demands of a complex social structure have work on Baeolophus (titmice) species is needed.
driven the evolution of a complex communicative Ficken (2000) indicates that chick-a-dee call note
system (see Blumstein and Armitage 1997; similarities extend beyond the chickadees and
Wilkinson 2003)? It is too early to answer this titmice in North America (even if the chick-a-dee
question definitively for chickadees (our first call itself does not), to include some of the species
concession). And although we still cannot unequivo- that occur in mixed-species flocks with those
cally state what structurally different calls mean to Paridae species, including golden-crowned
the birds themselves (our second concession), some kinglets, Regulus satrapa, and brown creepers,
interesting findings are emerging. Certhia americana.
THE CHICK-A-DEE CALL 211

Many of the European tit species possess a simi- system to provide a firm answer. We do hope,
lar call system (Hailman 1989). Perhaps the most however, that our review has been of sufficient
thoroughly studied European chick-a-dee call interestand informationto generate more
system is that of the willow tit. Haftorn (1993) observational and experimental work to help
describes a long-term study of the major vocaliza- elucidate the information-rich nature and evolution
tions of willow tits in central Norway, including the of this call system.
si-t call of this species that appears homologous
with the chick-a-dee call. In adults, the call seems to
Acknowledgements
be a medium range signal for flock members
moving through their territory and often out of We thank Chris Sturdy, Dave Gammon, Ben and
visual contact. The call system thus has a general Kerry Fanson, Mark Nolen, Ken Henry, and
function in social cohesion, similar to that of North Christine Jonason for reading previous versions of
American Paridae. It often occurs with two distinct the manuscript. Special thanks to Ken Otter for put-
note types, alarm (zi) notes and spitt notes, in ting together a terrific book and for putting in so
what are defined as mixed vocalizations, and its much effort to get this done.
structure appears to vary with context (e.g. mixed
calls containing alarm notes are produced more References
often in contexts of tits mobbing perched predators
or in surprising or other potentially threatening Baker, M. C. and Becker, A. M. (2002). Mobbing calls of
situationssee also Haftorn 2000). The pj call is black-capped chickadees: effects of urgency on call
production. Wilson Bulletin, 114, 510516.
produced primarily in agonistic contexts such as
Baker, M. C. and Logue, D. M. (2003). Population differ-
when two flocks interact, and is often combined
entiation in a complex bird sound: a comparison of
with the pure-tone introductory notes and/or the three bioacoustical analysis procedures. Ethology, 109,
D-like t notes of the si-t call into mixed 223242.
calls. Bloomfield, L. L., Charrier, I., and Sturdy, C. B. (2004).
Hailman (1989, p. 305) argued that Parid vocal- Note types and coding in parid vocalizations. II: The
izations represent one of the most interesting and chick-a-dee call of the mountain chickadee (Poecile
theoretically important communication systems gambeli). Canadian Journal of Zoology, 82, 780793.
known in the animal kingdom. We agree. We have Bloomfield, L. L., Sturdy, C. B., Phillmore, L. S., and
to conclude, though, that it is still largely an open Weisman, R. G. (2003). Open-ended categorization of
question as to how rich in information this system chick-a-dee calls by black-capped chickadees (Poecile atr-
icapilla). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 117, 290301.
may be. Studies aimed at documenting whether
Blumstein, D. T. and Armitage, K. B. (1997). Does sociality
different call structures are produced in different
drive the evolution of communicative complexity? A
contexts have been conducted in only a handful of comparative test with ground-dwelling sciurid alarm
species. There is a more serious paucity of informa- calls. American Naturalist, 150, 179200.
tion from playback studies to determine experi- Bradbury, J. W. and Vehrencamp, S. L. (1998). Principles of
mentally what different call structures might mean Animal Communication. Sinauer Associates, Sunderland,
to the birds themselves. In short, comparative Massachusetts.
studies are desperately neededstudies not just of Burghardt, G. M. (1970). Defining communication. In:
other members of the Paridae but also of the Johnston, J. W., Jr, Moulton, D. G., and Turk, A., eds.
penduline tits (Remizidae). We are not yet in a posi- Advances in Chemoreception, Vol. 1: Communication by
tion to answer fundamental questions about the chemical signals, pp. 518. Appleton-Century-Crofts,
New York.
evolution of this call system. For example to what
Charrier, I., Bloomfield, L. L., and Sturdy, C. B. (2004).
extent do phylogeny and ecology impact the struc-
Note types and coding in parid vocalizations. I: The
ture, and perhaps function, of this call system in chick-a-dee call of the black-capped chickadee (Poecile
different groups of Paridae? Phylogenies are being atricapillus). Canadian Journal of Zoology, 82, 769779.
established and clarified, but we dont have Clucas, B. A., Freeberg, T. M., and Lucas, J. R. (2004).
sufficient information about the chick-a-dee call Chick-a-dee call syntax, social context, and season affect
212 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

vocal responses of Carolina chickadees (Poecile caroli- Hailman, J. P. and Ficken, M. S. (1986). Combinatorial ani-
nensis). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 57, 187196. mal communication with computable syntax: chick-a-dee
Dawkins, R. and Krebs, J. R. (1978). Animal signals: infor- calling qualifies as language by structural linguistics.
mation or manipulation? In: Krebs, J. R. and Davies, Animal Behaviour, 34, 18991901.
N. B., eds. Behavioural ecology: an Evolutionary Approach, Hailman, J. P. and Ficken, M. S. (1996). Comparative
pp. 282309. Sinauer Associates, Sunderland, analysis of vocal repertoires, with reference to chicka-
Massachusetts. dees. In: Kroodsma, D. E. and Miller, E. H., eds. Ecology
Ekman, J. (1989). Ecology of non-breeding social systems and Evolution of Acoustic Communication in Birds, pp.
of Parus. Wilson Bulletin, 101, 263288. 136159. Comstock Publishing Associates of Cambridge
Ficken, M. S. (1990). Vocal repertoire of the Mexican University Press, Ithaca, New York.
chickadee. I. Calls. Journal of Field Ornithology, 61, Hailman, J. P., Ficken, M. S., and Ficken, R. W. (1985). The
380387. chick-a-dee calls of Parus atricapillus: a recombinant sys-
Ficken, M. S. (2000). Call similarities among mixed species tem of animal communication compared with written
flock associates. Southwestern Naturalist, 45, 154158. English. Semiotica, 56, 191224.
Ficken, M. S. and Popp, J. W. (1996). A comparative ana- Hailman, J. P., Ficken, M. S., and Ficken, R. W. (1987).
lysis of passerine mobbing calls. Auk, 113, 370380. Constraints on the structure of combinatorial chick-a-
Ficken, M. S., Ficken, R. W., and Apel, K. M. (1985). dee calls. Ethology, 75, 6280.
Dialects in a call associated with pair interactions in the Halliday, T. (1983). Information and communication. In:
black-capped chickadee. Auk, 102, 145151. Halliday, T. R. and Slater, P. J. B., eds. Animal Behaviour,
Ficken, M. S., Ficken, R. W., and Witkin, S. R. (1978). Vocal Vol. 2: Communication, pp. 4381. W. H. Freeman and
repertoire of the black-capped chickadee. Auk, 95, Company, New York.
3448. Hurd, C. R. (1996). Interspecific attraction to the mobbing
Ficken, M. S., Hailman, E. D., and Hailman, J. P. (1994). calls of black-capped chickadees (Parus atricapillus).
The chick-a-dee call system of the Mexican chickadee. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 38, 287292.
Condor, 96, 7082. Leger, D. W. (1993). Contextual sources of information
Freeberg, T. M. and Lucas, J. R. (2002). Receivers respond and responses to animal communication signals.
differently to chick-a-dee calls varying in note compos- Psychological Bulletin, 113, 295304.
ition in Carolina chickadees (Poecile carolinensis). Animal Lucas, J. R., Schraeder, A., and Jackson, C. (1999). Carolina
Behaviour, 63, 837845. chickadee (Aves, Paridae, Poecile carolinensis) vocaliza-
Freeberg, T. M., Lucas, J. R., and Clucas, B. (2003). tion rates: effects of body mass and food availability
Variation in chick-a-dee calls of a population of Carolina under aviary conditions. Ethology, 105, 503520.
chickadees, Poecile carolinensis: identity and redundancy Mammen, D. L. and Nowicki, S. (1981). Individual differ-
within note types. Journal of the Acoustical Society of ences and within-flock convergence in chickadee calls.
America, 113, 21272136. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 9, 179186.
Gaddis, P. K. (1985). Structure and variability in the vocal Marler, P. and Evans, C. (1996). Bird calls: just emotional
repertoire of the mountain chickadee. Wilson Bulletin, displays or something more? Ibis, 138, 2633.
97, 3046. Maynard Smith, J. and Harper, D. (2003). Animal Signals.
Gill, F. B., Slikas, B., and Sheldon, F. H. (2005). Phylogeny Oxford University Press, Oxford.
of titmice (Paridae): II. Species relationships based on Miyasato, L. E. and Baker, M. C. (1999). Black-capped
sequences of the mitochondrial cytochrome-B gene. chickadee call dialects along a continuous habitat corri-
Auk, 122, 121143. dor. Animal Behaviour, 57, 13111318.
Haftorn, S. (1993). Ontogeny of the vocal repertoire in the Nowicki, S. (1983). Flock-specific recognition of chick-a-dee
willow tit Parus montanus. Ornis Scandinavica, 24, calls. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 12, 317320.
267289. Nowicki, S. (1989). Vocal plasticity in captive black-
Haftorn, S. (2000). Contexts and possible functions of capped chickadees: the acoustic basis and rate of call
alarm calling in the willow tit, Parus montanus; the prin- convergence. Animal Behaviour, 37, 6473.
ciple of better safe than sorry. Behaviour, 137, 437449. Nowicki, S. and Nelson, D. A. (1990). Defining natural
Hailman, J. P. (1977). Optical Signals: Animal categories in acoustic signals: comparison of three meth-
Communication and Light. Indiana University Press, ods applied to chick-a-dee call notes. Ethology, 86, 89101.
Bloomington, Indiana. Owings, D. H. and Morton, E. S. (1998). Animal Vocal
Hailman, J. P. (1989). The organization of the major vocal- Communication: a New Approach. Cambridge University
izations in the Paridae. Wilson Bulletin, 101, 305343. Press, Cambridge.
THE CHICK-A-DEE CALL 213

Shannon, C. E. and Weaver, W. (1949). The Mathematical Sturdy, C. B., Phillmore, L. S., and Weisman, R. G. (2000).
Theory of Communication. University of Illinois Press, Call-note discrimination in black-capped chickadees
Urbana, Illinois. (Poecile atricapillus). Journal of Comparative Psychology,
Slater, P. J. B., Lachlan, R. F., and Riebel, K. (2000). The 114, 357364.
significance of learning in signal development: the Tchernichovski, O., Nottebohm, F., Ho, C. E., Pesaran, B.,
curious case of the chaffinch. In: Espmark, Y., and Mitra, P. P. (2000). A procedure for an automated
Amundsen, T., and Rosenqvist, G., eds. Animal Signals: measurement of song similarity. Animal Behaviour, 59,
Signalling and Signal Design in Animal Communication, 11671176.
pp. 341352. Tapir Academic Press, Trondheim, Templeton, C. N., Greene, E., and Davis, K. (2005).
Norway. Allometry of alarm calls: black-capped chickadees
Smith, S. M. (1991). The Black-Capped Chickadee: Behavioral encode information about predator size. Science, 308,
Ecology and Natural History. Cornell University Press, 19341937.
Ithaca, New York. Wilkinson, G. S. (2003). Social and vocal complexity in
Smith, S. T. (1972). Communication and other social bats. In: de Waal, F. B. M. and Tyack, P. L., eds. Animal
behavior in Parus carolinensis. Publications of the Nuttall Social Complexity: Intelligence, Culture, and Individualized
Ornithological Club, 11, 1125. Societies, pp. 322341. Harvard University Press,
Smith, W. J. (1977). The Behavior of Communicating: an Cambridge, Massachusetts.
Ethological Approach. Harvard University Press, Wilson, E. O. (1975). Sociobiology: the New Synthesis.
Cambridge, Massachusetts. Belknap Press of Harvard University Press, Cambridge,
Massachusetts.
This page intentionally left blank
CHAPTER 14

Status signaling and communication


networks in chickadees: Complex
communication with a simple song
Daniel J. Mennill and Ken A. Otter

14.1 Introduction two singing contexts: diurnal countersinging inter-


actions and the dawn chorus. We then evaluate
Black-capped chickadees produce an extensive var-
chickadee singing behavior within the new
iety of calls, including the versatile gargle call
communication network model and highlight
(Chapter 11) and the namesake chick-a-dee call
recent research on chickadees which has generated
(Chapter 13). In contrast to these complex calls, the
exciting insights into the ecology and evolution of
song of black-capped chickadees demonstrates a
sexual signaling in songbirds.
remarkable simplicity. Males sing a tonal, two-note
phrase: fee-bee1 (Fig. 14.1a). The fine structure of the
chickadee song is highly conserved across the
species range throughout North America, with
14.2 The fee-bee song: Variation
noteworthy exceptions described in a few isolated
on a two-note theme
populations (Hailman 1989; Kroodsma et al. 1999; In contrast to birds where each individual
Gammon and Baker 2004; Chapter 12). Although possesses a repertoire of song types, male black-
the black-capped chickadee song is simple, young capped chickadees typically have just one song
males inherit songs culturally, and nestlings raised type. However, male chickadees vary their songs
in absence of adult tutors do not develop normal by transposing the two-note fee-bee up and down a
adult song (Kroodsma et al. 1995). In this chapter, frequency continuum (Fig. 14.1b; Horn et al. 1992).
we explore the ways in which male black-capped Originally, black-capped chickadees were under-
chickadees use this simple, learned song to achieve stood to have two song variants: a normal song at
remarkably complex forms of communication. one frequency and a shifted song at another
After a brief description of the ways in which male (Ratcliffe and Weisman 1985; Hill and Lein 1987;
chickadees vary their songs over time, we evaluate Chapter 10). Today we understand that male
black-capped chickadees sing across a continuous
1
frequency range of approximately 860 Hz (Horn
Several mnemonics have been used to describe the chicka-
dees two-note song. Odum (1941) described the song as phoebe, et al. 1992; Christie et al. 2004b). Among songbirds,
while Desfayes (1964) suggested dee-dh. Brewer (1961) and frequency transposition of an otherwise invariant
then Ficken et al. (1978) opted for the more intuitive spelling of phrase is an uncommon strategy, although it does
fee-bee. Kroodsma et al. (1999) suggest the mnemonic hey sweetie,
which nicely describes the amplitude break in the second half of occur in other species of birds, including the
the chickadee song but also suggests that male song is directed male songs of stripe-breasted wrens Thryothorus
at females, which we know to be only half of the singing males thoracicus (D. J. Mennill, personal observation) and
audience. Among these options, we favor fee-bee, which is easily
extended to fee-bee-ee when the amplitude break in the second eastern whipbirds Phsophodes olivaceous (Mennill
syllable is of particular interest. and Rogers 2006). In other songbirds, including

215
216 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

(a) fee bee-ee

Amplitude
5.0
Frequency (kHz)
4.5
4.0
3.5
3.0
2.5
0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0
Time (seconds)
(b)
3.7
Frequency (Hz)

3.5

3.3

3.1

2.9
0 100 200 300 400 500 600
3.7
Frequency (Hz)

3.5

3.3

3.1

2.9
0 100 200 300 400 500 600
Number of songs

Figure 14.1 (a) Waveform and sound spectrogram of a black-capped chickadee fee-bee song. The fee note is higher in frequency and contains
a subtle downslurred glissando. The bee note is relatively constant in pitch and contains a very short amplitude break which is audible at close
range, i.e. fee-bee-ee. (b) Frequencies of all songs sung at dawn by two males, arranged along the x-axis in the order they were sung. Males
transpose the fee-bee song across a frequency continuum, singing bouts of song at one frequency and then pitch shifting their song by
intervals 80 Hz. Part b redrawn with permission from The Auk.

European nightingales Luscinia megarhynchos, (Chapters 10, 11, and 13) aid individual recognition.
Harris sparrows Zonotrichia querula, and Kentucky Christie et al. (2004a) analyzed nine songs recorded
warblers Opornis formosus, males emphasize certain from each of 46 male black-capped chickadees from
frequency components within their songs eastern Ontario and measured five fine structural
(reviewed in Mennill and Ratcliffe 2004) even features: (1) song length, (2) relative length of the fee
though they do not transpose the entire phrase. note, (3) relative loudness of the fee note, (4) the
The songs of black-capped chickadees, despite frequency ratio of the beginning to the end of the fee
their simplicity and extreme similarity throughout note (also known as the glissando frequency ratio),
most of North America, nevertheless show and (5) the frequency interval between the fee and
substantial variation in fine structure. This vari- the bee note. All five song features showed more
ation mayin conjunction with other vocal cues variation between males than within males.
S TAT U S S I G N A L I N G A N D C O M M U N I C AT I O N N E T W O R K S 217

Multivariate analyses demonstrated that the 14.3 Context of singing in chickadees


fine structure of chickadee songs is sufficiently
distinctive to allow individual identification Black-capped chickadees sing in two primary
(Christie et al. 2004a). In a test of laboratory-reared contexts: males sing an extended bout of song in the
chickadees, Phillmore et al. (2002) confirmed that early morning dawn chorus and males sing during
chickadees can indeed discriminate between the diurnal song bouts, which are often manifest as
songs of at least eight different males. By using a countersinging interactions between multiple
sound transmission experiment, Christie et al. males. Although birds sing throughout the year
(2004a) demonstrated that these cues of individual (Ficken et al. 1978), singing activity increases
identity persist over long broadcast distances, and dramatically in late winter when birds break out of
Phillmore et al.s (2002) laboratory test demon- their winter flocks and begin defending territories
strated that birds continue to distinguish between against their former flock-mates. In our eastern
different individuals songs when they are Ontario population, where the first egg is typically
degraded by distance. laid around April 30, diurnal countersinging inter-
In addition to providing information on singer actions are common as early as late February,
identity, the fine structure of black-capped chicka- especially on sunny days. Pronounced dawn
dee songs may convey cues of singer quality. choruses, on the other hand, do not begin until mid-
Males do not show rank-related differences in the April. Both diurnal countersinging interactions and
fine structural features of their songs (Christie et al. dawn choruses persist into the breeding season, but
2004a). However, high-ranking males maintain decrease substantially when parents begin offspring
more consistent frequency ratios between their fee care. Near identical patterns occur in our Northern
and bee notes when singing at low frequencies BC population, although egg-laying and the onset
(Christie et al. 2004b). Therefore, by listening to an of the other activities occur about a week later,
extended song bout which contains songs sung at suggesting the influence of photoperiod and
multiple frequencies, a listening chickadee may weather on controlling these behaviors (see Chapter
be able to identify the singer and also extract cues 4). Black-capped chickadee communication appears
to the quality of the singer. In this regard, the abil- to operate differently in these two contexts, so we
ity to alter song frequency could allow chickadees discuss each context separately. We first discuss
to signal individual condition, which is conveyed communication during diurnal countersinging
via repertoire size in other species of Parids interactions, where the intricacies of chickadee
(McGregor et al. 1981; Lambrechts and Dhondt communication are better understood, and then
1988; Lambrechts 1992). The difference, however, discuss communication during the dawn chorus.
appears to be in how that information is encoded.
In black-capped chickadees it appears to be the use
14.3.1 Singing context 1: Diurnal
of different frequencies and the control of fine
countersinging interactions
structure across those frequencies, rather than the
absolute number of song type variants, that indi- Before migratory birds return to North Americas
cate male quality. Frequency shifting and the ability temperate forest, the quiet of a crisp, sunny spring
to maintain consistent frequency ratios may be morning is often interrupted when a male black-
analogous to the ability to regulate and control capped chickadee begins to sing from a high song
strophe length in great tits (Lambrechts and post. A moment later, a distant male may respond
Dhondt 1988; Lambrechts 1992) or the incorpor- with his own songs and the forest becomes the site
ation of particular note types into songs (Forstmeier of a vocal duel. Intense dyadic countersinging inter-
et al. 2002). As we discuss below, frequency shifting actions are especially common during the period of
in chickadees also allows for other signaling territory establishment and egg-laying (Dixon and
patterns typically associated with repertoires, but it Stefanski 1970). Participants in these interactions are
is important to first investigate the contexts in typically neighboring territorial males (Shackleton
which chickadees sing. and Ratcliffe 1994), or else territorial males and
218 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

interlopers in search of mating opportunities (Smith 14.3.1.1 Variation in song frequency during
1989; personal observations). Contestants typically countersinging interactions
exchange songs from a substantial distance from Several experimental studies have helped elucidate
one another, although countersinging interactions the signal function of frequency variation during
sometimes escalate to extremely aggressive physical diurnal countersinging interactions. Both loop
encounters (Dixon and Stefanski 1970); in the playback and interactive playback techniques have
weeks prior to female fertility, song contests may been used to simulate the songs of a chickadee
escalate to the point where the two males, feet intruding into a male subjects territory. Horn et al.
locked, tumble to the ground in a burst of feathers, (1992) broadcast chickadee songs at low, moderate,
usually with a crowd of observers close at hand. or high frequencies (3130, 3380, or 3630 Hz
During countersinging interactions, contestants measured at start of bee note) from a loudspeaker
vary both the pitch of their songs and rhythm of positioned near the center of the territories of 24
their singing relative to their opponent (Fig. 14.2). male chickadees. Territorial males responded with
Both the relative timing and relative frequency songs that fell within 120 Hz of the frequency of the
of contestants songs are salient features of song playback-simulated intruder (Horn et al. 1992).
contests. Otter et al. (2002) expanded on this approach and

6.0
male 2
male 1 male 1 male 1
5.0

4.0

3.0

0 2 4 6 8 10
6.0
male 2 male 2 male 2
Frequency (kHz)

male 1 male 1
5.0

4.0

3.0

12 14 16 18 20 22
6.0
male 2 male 2 male 2
male 1 male 1 male 1
5.0

4.0

3.0

24 26 28 30 32 34
Time (seconds)

Figure 14.2 Sound spectrogram of a countersinging interaction between two neighboring territorial male black-capped chickadees. The
spectrogram is shown in three consecutive stretches, depicting 34 seconds of countersinging. The two opponents are not frequency matched;
male 1 sings at a frequency approximately 330 Hz lower than male 2. The two birds alternate songs until the final exchange (at second 32)
where male 2 overlaps the song of male 1.
S TAT U S S I G N A L I N G A N D C O M M U N I C AT I O N N E T W O R K S 219

gave playback to 24 territorial males using three demonstrated in many birds, including great tits
different treatments: (1) loop playback of a 3200 Hz (Krebs et al. 1981; McGregor et al. 1992). As with
song played every 5 seconds; (2) interactive type matching, our analyses of chickadee singing
playback of a 3200 Hz song played immediately strategies suggest that frequency matching is a
after every song sung by the territorial male; and directed signal of aggression. Thus, the ability to
(3) interactive playback of songs that matched the frequency shift songs compensates black-capped
frequency of the subjects songs (matching accur- chickadees for the lack of song-type variation found
acy: 24 11 Hz) and immediately followed the in other Parids. Innovative, interactive playback
subjects songs. These treatments incited increas- experiments with repertoire singing birds have
ingly close approaches from the territorial male, revealed a complex system of graded signals based
indicating that chickadees perceive frequency on the relative song choice of countersinging birds,
matching, in concert with one-to-one temporal which have helped the understanding of the
correspondence, as a threatening signal (Otter et al. evolutionary pressures responsible for the evolution
2002). Interestingly, males in treatment 2 often of repertoires (reviewed in Beecher and Brenowitz
shifted to frequency match the playbacks, thus 2005). Further research on the subtleties of chickadee
effectively becoming treatment 3 although not countersinging behavior is needed to answer ques-
through our intervention; these males showed tions about the importance of frequency variation
heightened levels of response compared to those during song contests. For example is high-frequency
that did not frequency match the playback. singing more or less aggressive than low-frequency
Mennill and Ratcliffe (2004c) employed a fully- singing, and are changes in song frequency signals of
interactive approach to engage 81 territorial males aggression per se?
in countersinging interactions with a playback-
simulated intruder. In half of the trials, the simu- 14.3.1.2 Variation in song timing during
lated intruder matched the frequency of the resident countersinging interactions
male (matching accuracy: 7 15 Hz). In the other Although frequency variation during diurnal coun-
half of the trials, the intruder sang at a frequency tersinging interactions is well studied, variation in
346 16 Hz higher than the resident male. All song timing is more poorly explored. Detailed stud-
males who received interactive playback responded ies of the function of song overlapping require tight
very aggressively to playback, exhibiting closer regulation of the timing of signals broadcast during
average approach distances than had been reported playback, and only since the proliferation of inter-
in any previous study. However, birds who were active playback software have researchers rigor-
frequency matched during playback retreated away ously applied an experimental approach to field
from the loudspeaker and spent more time singing studies of overlapping. The topic of overlapping of
from a greater distance from the simulated intruder, song was originally studied in association with the
indicative of an aversive response (Mennill and repetitive phrasing of great tit song strophes, which
Ratcliffe 2004c). Taken together, these three play- can lengthen or shorten a song bout. Male great tits
back studies indicate that frequency variation has increase variation in song timing (Dabelsteen et al.
important consequences during countersinging 1996) and stop songs short (Langemann et al. 2000)
interactions. Frequency matching during diurnal when overlapped by playback. Blue tits show
countersinging interactions appears to be a directed higher song rates when overlapped (Poesel 2004).
signal of aggression and functionally similar to Territorial males respond in like fashion in other,
song-type matching in other birds. non-Parid songbirds including nightingales,
Many birds who sing with a repertoire of discrete European robins Erithacus rubecula (Todt 1981), and
song types communicate aggressive signals by blackbirds Turdus merula (Brindley 1991).
tactically choosing songs that match their opponents Mennill and Ratcliffe (2004c) used interactive
song type (Vehrencamp 2001). Type matching, as playback to simulate a singing chickadee who
an aggressive countersinging strategy, has been either overlapped every song given by each
220 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

playback subject or avoided overlapping by on how reliably overlapping and frequency match-
singing 1.5 seconds after the playback subjects ing could be determined in each contest). Song
song was complete. In response to an overlapping contests that escalated tended to contain more song
opponent, territorial males sang significantly more overlaps (3.9 0.7 overlaps per contest; mean SE)
shortened songs, dropping the bee note from signifi- than song contests that did not escalate (3.0 0.7
cantly more songs when they were overlapped by overlaps per contest) but this difference was not
an opponent. Furthermore, overlapped males significant (Wilcoxon: Z 1.1, P 0.11, N 61).
sang with significantly more variation in the Frequency matching, on the other hand, showed a
silent interval between consecutive songs. These significant relationship with contest outcome:
responses of male black-capped chickadees to an 25 of 32 contests (78%) that escalated contained
overlapping opponent mirror the responses of frequency matching between the contestants,
other songbirds to overlapping opponents outlined whereas only 20 of 36 contests (56%) that did not
above. This cross-taxa similarity of response to escalate contained frequency matching (Fisher exact:
overlapping playback suggests that the very nature p 0.04; birds were considered to be frequency-
of overlapping, as a signal that masks an oppon- matched if the musically trained transcriber could
ents signal, is a more antagonistic countersinging detect no difference in their song frequency). These
approach than non-overlapping. Both the increase results mirror those of Shackleton and Ratcliffe
in shortened songs and the increase in variable (1994) who found that frequency matching was asso-
song timing exhibited by black-capped chickadees ciated with contest escalation in 27 interactions
may be strategies used by countersinging birds to recorded in 1991. Therefore, observations of natural
avoid having their songs masked by an opponent. contests support the idea that song overlapping and
To further quantify the occurrence of frequency frequency matching are directed signals of aggres-
matching and overlapping during chickadee coun- sion, important during the diurnal countersinging
tersinging interactions, we transcribed 68 naturally- interactions of black-capped chickadees. These
occurring song contests heard between March 21 results also suggest that song overlapping and
and May 6, 2001 and 2002 (D. J. Mennill, unpub- frequency matching serve distinct signal functions.
lished data). Using a different symbol for each Mennill and Ratcliffes (2004c) interactive playback
contestant, we noted the relative pitch and relative experiment showed that overlapping primarily
timing of each song by the relative position of the influenced the birds singing rhythm and frequency
symbols on graph paper. (One challenge with matching influenced the birds movement behavior.
documenting song overlap during naturally- Our observations of naturally occurring counter-
occurring countersinging interactions is that the singing interactions support this idea; song contests
observers perception of overlapping signals may that involved frequency-matching usually resulted
differ from the birds perceptions of those signals in the opponents approaching one another, whereas
owing to the slow speed of sound propagation song contests that involved overlapping did not.
through air. During the transcriptions we describe
here, the observer was typically in relatively close
14.3.2 Singing context 2: The dawn chorus
proximity to the countersinging males, such that
the perception of overlapping likely matched the In the early twilight before sunrise, when dark tree-
perception of overlapping by the counter-singing tops can just barely be distinguished from the dark
chickadees.) From these transcriptions, we extracted sky, male black-capped chickadees begin an
the following variables for each song contest: (1) the extended period of singing. The fee-bee of one male
number of song overlaps; (2) whether or not the con- penetrates the still dawn air, then two more males
testants were frequency-matched with one another; join the chorus, and soon the entire neighborhood
and (3) whether or not the countersinging contest comes alive with song. Each male may continue to
escalated (as indicated by the contestants approach- sing for as long as 70 min, singing an average of
ing each other and giving extended bouts of chick- 282 36 songs (Horn et al. 1992; Christie et al.
a-dee and gargle calls; sample sizes vary depending 2004b). Song output during the chorus varies with
S TAT U S S I G N A L I N G A N D C O M M U N I C AT I O N N E T W O R K S 221

male dominance status. Males with high-ranking consistently hear more than one male and often as
dominance status begin singing earlier, they sing for many as four males (Hansen et al. 2005). Second,
longer, and they sing at a higher rate than their low- males may benefit by remaining near their nest
ranking counterparts (Otter et al. 1997). Therefore, cavity for reasons that are independent of commu-
during the chorus, males broadcast cues to their nication with the female. Most notably, females
quality not only through the fine structure of their often solicit copulations, both withinpair and extra-
songs (see above) but also through song output pair, upon exiting the nest cavity at dawn. Males
(Otter et al. 1997). Male chorus output also reflects may find an advantage in remaining near the nest
male pairing status. Following experimental cavity so that they can either copulate with their
removal of the female, male chorus length partner or mate guard her when she emerges. This
dramatically increases (Otter and Ratcliffe 1993). idea could be tested by conducting a pair-wise
During the chorus, males pitch shift their song by an comparison of the position of males during the
interval of 80 Hz every 30.7 3.7 songs (Christie dawn chorus when their partner is fertile and when
et al. 2004b). The rate of pitch shifting varies dramat- she is not, or by comparing the chorus locations of
ically, both between males and between the choruses high-ranking males (whose females seldom follow a
from any particular male, where some chorus mixed reproductive strategy) versus low-ranking
performances featuring a pitch shift after nearly males (whose females often follow a mixed
every song while others contain no pitch shifts what- reproductive strategy). Third, males may sing the
soever. In general, however, male choruses consist of dawn chorus near their nest cavity to mask their
bouts of songs at similar frequencies punctuated by partners ability to hear the dawn chorus perform-
marked changes in frequency (Fig. 14.1b). ance of neighboring males. Analyses of recordings
Egg-laying female black-capped chickadees sleep made with microphones mounted inside nest boxes
inside their nest cavities, and males typically sing suggest that this sort of masking comes into play in
much of the dawn chorus in close proximity to their the dawn chorus of great tits (see below; K. A. Otter,
nest (Otter and Ratcliffe 1993; Gammon 2004; T. M. Peake, A. M. R. Terry, and P. K. McGregor,
Mennill and Ratcliffe 2004b). Males often pause or unpublished data).
stop singing upon the females emergence from the The interactive nature of chickadees diurnal
nest cavity (Otter and Ratcliffe 1993; Gammon countersinging contests is obvious, but the
2004). Upon the emergence of the female from the processes that govern the timing and frequency of
nest cavity, males often switch from singing to chorusing male chickadees songs are very poorly
giving variable see and gargle calls (Gammon 2004; understood. Upon hearing the apparent cacophony
personal observation). Both withinpair copulations of a busy black-capped chickadee chorus, which
(Otter and Ratcliffe 1993; Gammon 2004) and extra- includes the dissonant harmonies of multiple song
pair copulations (Smith 1988; Mennill et al. 2004) frequencies and the irregular overlaps and alterna-
commonly follow the end of the dawn chorus. tions between many males songs, a human listener
Based on the observation that males tend to give may be inclined to believe that chorus singing is
their dawn chorus in close proximity to the nest cav- non-interactive. To test whether the dawn chorus
ity, several authors have suggested that the male was interactive, Shackleton and Ratcliffe (1994)
chorus is directed at the female (Otter and simultaneously recorded three males in neighboring
Ratcliffe 1993; Gammon 2004). We encourage a territories during two mornings of dawn chorus.
cautious approach to this interpretation for several They found little evidence for correspondence in the
reasons. First, male dawn song is far louder than frequency of songs given by neighboring males.
necessary to communicate with a female inside the However, analysis of larger groups of birds over a
nest cavity; indeed, males accomplish this task much longer periods of time is required to rigor-
with the remarkably quiet faint fee-bee vocalization ously determine whether song frequency and
later in the breeding season (Smith 1991). Instead, timing show any correspondence between neigh-
males sing songs at a high amplitude, such that a bors during the chorus. As Burt and Vehrencamp
human observer listening to the dawn chorus can (2005) have demonstrated using multichannel array
222 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

recordings of banded wrens Thryothorus pleurostic- on malemale countersinging interactions to evalu-


tus, the dawn chorus can involve interactive behav- ate the quality of potential partners. Male chicka-
ior with complex processes of tactical song choice. dees might benefit from eavesdropping on the
countersinging interactions of other combatants;
this information could then be used to adjust his
14.4 Communication networks
own willingness to escalate in subsequent inter-
Conventionally, animal communication has been actions with one or the other of these rivals. We
understood as a dyadic process of information tested these ideas using multispeaker playback and
exchange involving one signaler and one receiver: a interactive playback approaches.
songbird may defend his territory by signaling to a
male receiver and he may attract a breeding partner
14.4.1 Male eavesdropping in black-capped
by signaling to a female receiver (e.g. Kroodsma
chickadees
and Byers 1999). The dyadic model of communica-
tion is useful for understanding pitch matching and To test whether male black-capped chickadees
overlapping during dyadic countersinging contests eavesdrop, Mennill and Ratcliffe (2004a) used a
of black-capped chickadees (see above). However, multispeaker experiment to evaluate the responses
chickadees share two characteristics, in common of territorial males to a countersinging contest
with many other songbirds, which are not easily between two simulated intruders. Playback was
accounted for within the dyadic model. First, chicka- given through three loudspeakers arranged in an
dee songs are long-range signals that may convey equilateral triangle with 24 m between each loud-
information across broad distances to multiple speaker. Playback occurred in three stages
receivers simultaneously. Second, each breeding (Fig. 14.3a). In stage 1, the territorial male was
neighborhood of chickadees consists of multiple attracted to within 5 m of a loudspeaker broadcast-
males and females who may all benefit by assessing ing chick-a-dee calls, thereby bringing the male to a
the quality of their neighbors. A communication position equidistant from the remaining two loud-
network model, which accounts for multiple speakers. In stage 2, fee-bee songs were broadcast
signalers and multiple receivers exchanging infor- from the other two loudspeakers, simulating a
mation simultaneously (McGregor 2005), may countersinging contest between two unknown
therefore provide an enhanced understanding of males. The two simulated males sang songs at the
communication in black-capped chickadees in same frequency, the same rate, and the same ampli-
particular, and in songbirds in general. tude; the only difference between the two loud-
Within a communication network, individuals speakers was that the songs of one male consistently
may transmit information in novel ways that overlapped the other (after Naguib and Todt 1997).
occur outside of the traditional dyadic model. During playback, an observer evaluated which
Eavesdropping is a network-based receiver behav- loudspeaker the subject approached first. In stage 3,
ior, whereby an individual gathers information a postplayback assay, songs were broadcast from
from a signaling interaction between conspecifics the loudspeaker that the male did not first approach
without being directly involved in that interaction in stage 2, to confirm that both loudspeakers were
(McGregor and Dabelsteen 1996; sensu social positioned in such a way as to elicit a territorial
eavesdropping Peake 2005). Eavesdropping on the response from the subject.
diurnal countersinging contests of male songbirds The responses of territorial males to the
may facilitate low-cost and direct comparisons of multispeaker playback demonstrated that male
the relative quality of countersinging contestants. black-capped chickadees do eavesdrop on the
Female black-capped chickadees, who are genet- countersinging interactions between others. High-
ically promiscuous (Otter et al. 1998; Mennill et al. ranking males preferentially approached the
2004) and are quick to divorce their partner if a overlapping loudspeaker (Fig. 14.3b). Because there
higher-status male becomes available (Otter and were no absolute differences in information
Ratcliffe 1996; Ramsay et al. 2000), might eavesdrop broadcast by the two loudspeakers, only relative
S TAT U S S I G N A L I N G A N D C O M M U N I C AT I O N N E T W O R K S 223

(a)
Stage 1: Chick-a-dee calls Stage 2: Simulate song Stage 3: Songs broadcast
attract resident male to first contest between speakers from the non-approached
speaker two and three speaker

1 1 1

2 3 2 3 2 3

(b) High-ranking Males Low-ranking Males

1.0 15/16

0.8
playback subjects
Proportion of

0.6 5/10

0.4 3/10
2/10
0.2
1/16
0/16
0.0
Approach Approach Remain Approach Approach Remain
overlapping overlapped at lure overlapping overlapped at lure
speaker speaker speaker speaker speaker speaker

Response to playback

Figure 14.3 (a) Schematic representation of a multispeaker playback experiment designed to test whether male black-capped chickadees
eavesdrop on the countersinging interactions of other males. Stage 2 simulated a countersinging interaction between two males where one bird
consistently overlapped the others songs. (b) High-ranking males preferentially approached the loudspeaker broadcasting overlapping songs,
demonstrating that males eavesdrop on malemale interactions. Reprinted with permission from Behaviour.

information in the form of song overlapping, the neither loudspeaker. These differential responses of
high-ranking males responses indicate that males high and low-ranking males support the idea that
extract relative information from rivals counter- responses to conspecific rivals varies with the
singing interactions. This result adds to a growing quality of the territorial male: high-ranking males,
body of research documenting male eavesdropping who are better able to dominate other birds during
in great tits and other species (see Peake 2005 for physical encounters, preferentially approach a
review). In contrast to the strong directional loudspeaker broadcasting a more aggressive signal.
response exhibited by high-ranking male black- Future playback studies expanding on this
capped chickadees, low-ranking males showed a multispeaker experimental design may shed
mixed response. Although low-ranking males insight into the signal function of pitch matching
approached the overlapping loudspeaker most and pitch shifting behavior, and may even help to
often, they sometimes approached the overlapped tease apart which signals are most salient to eaves-
loudspeaker and, in three cases, approached droppers.
224 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

who received aggressive playback, compared to


14.4.2 Female eavesdropping in
the broods of females paired to high-ranking
black-capped chickadees
males who received submissive playback or to
Using a modified version of Otter et al. (1999) inter- males who received no playback (Fig. 14.4c,
active playback study, Mennill et al. (2002, 2003) Mennill et al. 2002). In other words, after hearing
engaged male chickadees in countersinging con- their high-ranking partner lose a song contest to
tests with a simulated intruder to test whether an opponent, females paired to high-ranking males
female black-capped chickadees eavesdrop on changed from a monogamous mating strategy to a
malemale song interactions. The experiment promiscuous mating strategy. Surprisingly, the
involved playback to groups of four birds: two behavioral assay (stage 3) yielded no evidence that
pairs of breeding partners in neighboring territo- female black-capped chickadees eavesdrop on
ries, where the males had been flock-mates during malemale song contests; neither female behavior
the previous winter, one high-ranking and one low- nor male behavior showed any difference on the
ranking. Playback was conducted at the onset of morning following aggressive or submissive play-
female fertility and occurred in four successive back manipulations (Mennill et al. 2002, 2003).
stages (Fig. 14.4a). In stage 1, one male was Although no extraterritorial forays were observed
engaged in a 6-min countersinging contest with a during the observation period, one extrapair copu-
playback-simulated intruder. In stage 2, an hour lation was observed outside the observation period.
later, the other male was engaged in a 6-min coun- A female paired to a high-ranking male who had
tersinging contest with the same playback-simu- received aggressive playback had an extrapair
lated intruder. We used the same catalogue of songs copulation with a low-ranking male who had
to interact with both males, but with one male we received submissive playback (Mennill et al. 2004).
simulated an aggressive opponent (one who over- Genetic assignment of extrapair fathers demon-
lapped and pitch-matched all of the subjects songs) strated two more cases where females paired to
and to the other male we simulated a submissive aggressive-playback high-ranking males had extra-
countersinging opponent (one who avoided over- pair copulations with submissive-playback low-
lapping and sang at a frequency 300400 Hz higher ranking males. No other study of the black-capped
than the subject) (Fig. 14.4b). There were no chickadee mating system (Smith 1988; Otter et al.
absolute differences in the song output of the play- 1998; Mennill et al. 2004) has documented a female
back between aggressive and submissive trials, seeking an extrapair copulation from a low-ranking
only relative differences between the pitch and tim- male from the same winter flock as her high-
ing of the songs of the resident male and the simu- ranking partner. The three cases that followed
lated intruder. Stages 1 and 2 of playback were Mennill et al.s playback experiment therefore
repeated to each male on two subsequent days. In indicate that eavesdropping on malemale song
the final two stages of the experiment, we assessed contests influences not only the females decision
females responses to playback. In stage 3, a behav- about whether or not to follow a mixed reproduct-
ioral assay, we followed each pair for 30 min on the ive strategy (Mennill et al. 2002) but also whom to
day following playback in the early morning, a target as an extrapair sire (Mennill et al. 2003).
time when chickadee extrapair copulations are Females paired to high-ranking male chickadees
common (Smith 1988; Mennill et al. 2004). In stage are typically genetically monogamous (Otter et al.
4, a genetic assay, we used microsatellite paternity 1998; Mennill et al. 2004), yet these females increased
analyses to sample the broods of both females for the proportion of extrapair young in their broods
extrapair offspring. after hearing their partner lose an aggressive coun-
The genetic assay (stage 4) confirmed that female tersinging contest. In contrast, females paired to low-
black-capped chickadees eavesdrop on malemale ranking males continued to produce similar
vocal interactions. The proportion of nests contain- proportions of extrapair young regardless of the
ing extrapair young was significantly greater in the playback treatment given to their partner
broods of females paired to high-ranking males (Fig. 14.4c). Why were aggressive playback sessions
S TAT U S S I G N A L I N G A N D C O M M U N I C AT I O N N E T W O R K S 225

(a)
Stage 1: Playback to first male Stage 2: Playback to neighboring male

High-ranking males territory Low-ranking males territory High-ranking males territory Low-ranking males territory

Stage 3: Observe birds after playback Stage 4: Sample paternity of broods

High-ranking males territory Low-ranking males territory High-ranking males territory Low-ranking males territory

(b) Submissive playback Aggressive playback


4.5
Frequency (kHz)

4.0
3.5
3.0
2.5

0 1 2 3 4 0 1 2 3 4
Time (seconds)

(c) High-ranking Males Low-ranking Males


0.6
12/23
3/6
containing extra-male young

0.5
7/17 4/10
Proportion of nests

0.4

0.3

0.2
1/10 1/10
0.1

0.0
Aggressive Submissive No Submissive Aggressive No
Playback Playback Playback Playback Playback Playback
Playback treatment

Figure 14.4 (a) Schematic representation of an interactive playback experiment designed to test whether female black-capped chickadees
eavesdrop on malemale countersinging interactions. (b) During submissive playback interactions, interactive playback simulated an opponent
(black) who avoided overlapping the subject (white) and sang at a higher frequency than the subject. During aggressive playback interactions,
the playback-simulated intruder overlapped and pitch matched the subject. (c) High-ranking males who received aggressive playback
lost paternity significantly more often than high-ranking males who received submissive playback or no playback. Low-ranking
males lost paternity at equivalent levels regardless of the playback treatment they received. Parts b and c reprinted with permission from
Science.
226 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

sufficient to increase the proportion of extrapair who had received submissive playback, presumably
young in the broods of high-ranking males whereas to assess alternative mating opportunities (Otter
submissive playback sessions did not reduce the et al. 1999). Genetic analyses demonstrated that
proportion of extrapair young in the broods of low- these extraterritorial forays did not, however, trans-
ranking males? In complex systems, Gould (1998) late into extrapair fertilizations (Otter et al. 2001).
suggests that a great asymmetry exists between Otter et al.s (1999, 2001) experiments with great
the perception of constructive and destructive forces, tits and Mennill et al.s (2002, 2003) experiments
where the apparent severity of one negative event with black-capped chickadees demonstrate that
outweighs the product of many positive events. female Parids eavesdrop. Both experiments docu-
Such an asymmetry may influence the mate choice mented a change in female reproductive activities
decisions of eavesdropping female black-capped following manipulation of male signals. However,
chickadees. Females paired to high-ranking males the reproductive activities of females changed in
are accustomed to observing their partner dominate different ways for these two species. Great tit
contests over food resources throughout the winter females readily forayed into neighboring territories
and dominate territorial countersinging interactions but did not change their genetic mating strategy,
during the breeding season. Therefore, the poor whereas black-capped chickadee females did not
vocal performance of high-ranking males during readily foray outside of their territory and yet they
aggressive playback trials was unusual and may changed their genetic mating strategy. These differ-
have been sufficiently extraordinary to call into ences may be related to differences in the behav-
question previous female assessments. Females ioral ecology of chickadees and tits. Black-capped
paired to low-ranking males, in contrast, observe chickadees spend the winter in small cohesive
their partner dominate some contests (e.g. with flocks with stable flock membership where inter-
interlopers of even lower quality) and be dominated actions follow a strict linear dominance hierarchy
in other contests (e.g. with a higher-ranking neigh- (Smith 1991). In contrast, great tit flocks are larger,
bor). Consequently, the strong performance of low- flock membership is transitory, and rank relation-
ranking males during the playback manipulated ships are more labile (Dent et al. 2002). For female
song contests may not have been outside the normal great tits, surrounded by breeding males whose
experience of females paired to low-ranking males. relative quality is less resolved, eavesdropping may
Otter et al. (1999) evaluated female eavesdrop- be a means of initial assessment of male quality,
ping in great tits using a similar interactive play- which females follow up with forays into neigh-
back experiment. Two differences between the bors territories. If detailed assessments fail to
great tit studies and those described above for confirm the information gained through eaves-
chickadees may be salient in comparing results. As dropping, female great tits may forgo extrapair
great tits do not have cohesive flock membership or copulations (Otter et al. 2001). Female black-capped
strict dominance hierarchies, males were randomly chickadees, however, are expected to be intimately
assigned to receive aggressive or submissive play- familiar with the relative status of all males singing
back during playback interactions. Further, because within earshot. Consequently, information gained
great tits do not pitch shift and song type matching through eavesdropping may be especially import-
does not appear to have an additive effect to over- ant to female black-capped chickadees when that
lapping alone (Dabelsteen et al. 1996), overlapping information reveals quality differences that contrast
versus non-overlapping was the primary difference the females previous assessments. Having lost
between aggressive and submissive treatments. confidence in her mates status after hearing him
Like the results with chickadees, the evidence dominated in a song contest with an aggressive
from interactive playback sessions with great tits opponent, female chickadees may adopt the next
suggests that breeding females eavesdrop on the best strategy of seeking an extra-pair fertilization
vocal interactions of males. Female great tits paired from another nearby, high-ranking male (Mennill
to males who received aggressive playback readily et al. 2003). Despite their differences, these
forayed into the territories of neighboring males two studies of Parid eavesdropping behavior
S TAT U S S I G N A L I N G A N D C O M M U N I C AT I O N N E T W O R K S 227

demonstrate that female eavesdropping is an theory. Black-capped chickadees routinely con-


important means of female assessment of male struct their nests closer to the edges of their territor-
quality. ies than would be expected by chance (Ramsay
et al. 1999). Nests typically fall within 20 m of
territory edges, despite the large size of chickadee
14.4.3 Nest cavities and male singing
territories (Fig. 14.5; average territory diameter:
behavior
152 27 m; Mennill et al. 2004). Detailed investiga-
Communication network theory suggests that tion of food abundance and vegetation features
females should position themselves in order to show no marked differences between nest sites at
maximize assessment of the males around them territory edges versus comparison sites at territory
(Otter and Ratcliffe 2005). The nesting behavior of centers, suggesting that chickadees nest at territory
black-capped chickadees provides support for this peripheries based on conspecific attraction

100m
High

High

Low Low
High

Low

High
High

Low Low

Low High
Low

High

Queens University Biological Station


on Lake Opinicon

Figure 14.5 Territory map of part of the breeding population of black-capped chickadees at Queens University Biological Station in 2000. Nest
cavities are shown as circles. Arrows indicate the orientation of the nest cavity entrance. Territories were assessed by spot mapping the birds
movement patterns (see Mennill et al. 2004). The rank of the territorial male is shown for each territory.
228 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

(Ramsay et al. 1999). Female chickadees, who territory edges to facilitate early morning extrapair
remain inside of the nest cavity for much of the copulations with neighbors (Ramsay et al. 1999).
dawn chorus, may be better able to hear the dawn However, although extrapair copulations do often
chorus performance of neighboring males by occur shortly after female emergence from the nest
nesting near territory boundaries. Given that black- (Smith 1988), they do not usually occur in close
capped chickadee song transmits over a minimum proximity to the nest (Mennill et al. 2004), suggest-
distance of 80 to 100 m (Christie et al. 2004a; ing that the edge-nesting behavior of female
Fotheringham and Ratcliffe 1995), females maxi- chickadees maximizes acoustic contact rather than
mize the number of males audible to them during physical contact with neighbors.
the chorus by nesting near territory edges. Analyses Recordings made inside the nest boxes of great
of black-capped chickadee nest positions in 190 tits provide further support for the idea that roost-
territories over 5 years showed that nest sites fell ing female Parids may monitor the dawn chorus
within 100 m of more chorusing positions of neigh- performance of neighboring males while they roost
bors than did comparison sites at territory centers inside the nest cavity. Microphones were placed
(Mennill et al. 2004). Females may also nest at inside the nests of seven different females in a

(a) 25 m from source (b) 50 m from source


4.0 4.0
0 0
3.5 3.5

3.0 3.0

2.5 2.5

4.0 4.0
90 90
Frequency (kHz)

3.5 3.5

3.0 3.0

2.5 2.5

4.0 4.0
180 180
3.5 3.5

3.0 3.0

2.5 2.5
0 0.5 1.0 1.5 0 0.5 1.0 1.5
Time (seconds) Time (seconds)

Figure 14.6 Sound spectrograms of black-capped chickadee songs recorded by a microphone mounted inside an abandoned nest cavity. Songs
were broadcast through a loudspeaker positioned either 25 m from the nest cavity (a) or 50 m from the nest cavity (b). The nest was rotated so
that the entrance to the nest cavity was oriented facing the loudspeaker (0; top), at a right-angle to the loudspeaker (90; middle), or away from
the loudspeaker (180, bottom). The nest cavity demonstrated acoustically directional properties, such that a female chickadee roosting inside the
cavity will be better able to hear a male in front of her nest than a male singing behind her nest. Reprinted with permission from Journal of
Avian Biology.
S TAT U S S I G N A L I N G A N D C O M M U N I C AT I O N N E T W O R K S 229

population of breeding great tits at Strdam two major themes of variation, both of which are
Biological Station near Hillerd, Denmark. For all distinct from the pitch-shifting behavior of black-
seven recordings, the dawn chorus of both the pair capped chickadees. Mountain chickadees P. gambeli,
male plus at least one neighboring male were Carolina chickadees P. carolinensis, and Mexican
audible (K. A. Otter, T. M. Peake, A. M. R. Terry, and chickadees, P. sclateri, appear to have repertoires of
P. K. McGregor, unpublished data). The recordings discrete song types. The songs of mountain chicka-
further showed that songs from males singing more dees are superficially similar to black-capped
than 75 m away from the roosting female were chickadees, but each male has a repertoire of four to
clearly audible. Similarly, in black-capped chicka- seven song types, each consisting of two to four
dees, recordings made with microphones placed whistled notes (Wiebe and Lein 1999). Mountain
inside nest cavities could clearly detect males chickadees engage in countersinging interactions,
singing more than 50 m away (Mennill and during which males use songs with lower fre-
Ratcliffe 2004b). Although cavity nesting may quency terminal notes in the most aggressive
impede the transmission of some signals, these contests (Hill and Lein 1989). The songs of Carolina
findings of the acoustics of Parid nest cavities chickadees vary substantially from black-capped
demonstrate that cavity nesting does not impair chickadee songs, although males of both species
female assessment of distant male dawn chorus can learn the other species song (Kroodsma et al.
performances. 1995). Studies of pure Carolina chickadees show
Black-capped chickadee nest cavities have that their songs usually possess four whistled notes
acoustically directional properties. Recordings (Hailman 1989), although the number can vary
made with microphones placed inside a chickadee from one to 12 (Ward 1966), and individuals have at
nest demonstrate that a female is better able to hear least two distinct song variants (Lohr et al. 1991),
males singing in front of her nest cavity than males which may be important during countersinging
singing behind her nest cavity (Fig. 14.6; Mennill interactions (D. J. Mennill, personal observation).
and Ratcliffe 2004b). Analyses of the orientation of The songs of Mexican chickadees are poorly under-
132 chickadee nests in eastern Ontario demon- stood. Two types of songs have been described for
strated that chickadee nests have random compass Mexican chickadees (Ficken 1990), at least one of
orientation and are not specifically directed at the which appears to occur as complex song types, but
territories of high-ranking neighbors (e.g. Fig. 14.5). countersinging behavior has not been described.
However, the nest cavities of females who nested in Thus, most North American chickadees possess
the soft wood substrate of birch, Betula papyrifera, song repertoires akin to those of European
and aspen, Populus tremuloides, were oriented congeners, such as the marsh tit and willow tit.
towards their extrapair partners territory (n 17; Similarly, the songs of male North American titmice
Mennill and Ratcliffe 2004b). Consequently, the (genus Baeolophus) also exhibit repertoires. Tufted
drive to monitor the singing behavior of neighbors titmice sing with a repertoires of up to 14 song
may influence not only nest cavity location types (Gaddis 1983) and during countersinging
(Ramsay et al. 1999, Mennill et al. 2004) but also nest interactions males type-match their opponents
entrance orientation (Mennill and Ratcliffe 2004b). (Duguay and Ritchison 1998). Oak titmice B.
These intriguing results demonstrate how commu- inornatus have repertoires of up to 11 song types
nication network thinking may provide new (Dixon 1969) and certain song types are associated
insights into the breeding biology of songbirds. with countersinging interactions between males
(Gaddis 1983). From a communication network
perspective, North American Parids with song
14.5 Singing behavior in other
repertoires are expected to behave in a way similar
North American Parids
to Eurasian Parids with song repertoires. So far,
The male songs of North American Parids vary however, black-capped chickadees appear unique
substantially between each species. The other in the use of frequency shifting to achieve functions
North American Poecile chickadees demonstrate associated with multiple song types in other Parids.
230 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

The remaining two North American Poecile chicka- breeding males and females have become a model
dees, chestnut-backed chickadees and boreal chicka- system in animal communication. Our research with
dees, are (along with their Eurasian congener the black-capped chickadees, taken together with
Siberian tit) anomalous songbirds: none exhibit a research on communication networks in other Parids,
long-range, male song. Male boreal chickadees pro- demonstrates that male singing behavior is a net-
duce a varied warbling song in aggressive work-based process that can convey information
situations with other males (McLaren 1976) and about male quality to rival males and choosy females
chestnut-backed chickadees give modified gargle simultaneously.
calls in aggressive situations (Dahlsten et al. 2002), Many aspects of chickadee communication
but neither of these vocalizations travel sufficient require further research. Within the context of the
distances to be used in long-range advertising. dawn chorus, the interplay between broadcast
Hailman et al. (1994) studied the dawn behavior of communication and directed communication is
Siberian tits, and found that these birds center their poorly understood. Within the context of diurnal
activity around the nest, as do chorusing black- countersinging interactions, overlapping is
capped chickadees, but use chick-a-dee and gargle relatively well understood, although future studies
calls in place of song. Like black-capped chickadees, should evaluate how often birds overlap during
male Siberian tits cease their vocalizations when the natural contests and whether partial overlap is
female emerges from the nest cavity, but the lack of a functionally equivalent to complete overlap.
long-range signal coupled with the highly dispersed Communication through pitch variation, in
spacing of this species would seemingly preclude contrast, is poorly understood. During non-
the ability of a communication network to form matched countersinging, is singing at a lower
around dawn calling activity. Similar patterns of frequency a more or less aggressive communication
dawn activity using chick-a-dee calls have been noted strategy? Is pitch shifting a signal per se? The con-
in the boreal chickadee (C. T. Naugler, personal tinuity between the dawn chorus and diurnal
communication), but these are yet to be formally singing merits further investigation. Poesel et al.s
studied. These species therefore represent an inter- (2004) study of blue tits demonstrated that dawn
esting opportunity to test the importance of commu- chorus performance predicted male performance
nication networks for songbirds. If other female during malemale countersinging contests. Other
Parids use male chorus performance for assessing singing contexts in chickadees also merit more
male quality, do females in brown-headed chickadee careful study. During the late winter and early
species suffer a loss of network-based assessment? spring it is not uncommon to hear a lone male belt
Alternately, do females use different mechanisms of out a song bout that is unanswered by other males.
assessing the relative quality of their mates, such as Are these solo bouts unchallenged contest proclam-
the extreme levels of copulation solicitation by ations, or do these song bouts serve a separate
females seen in crested titmice (Lens et al. 1997)? function? These and other questions will allow us
to better understand the dyadic and network-based
dynamics of communication and the complex ways
14.6 Summary and future studies
in which chickadees use their simple song.
The two-note fee-bee song of black capped chickadees
ranks among the simplest learned songs of oscine
songbirds. Male chickadees vary the timing and pitch
Acknowledgements
of this vocalization during both extended dawn Laurene Ratcliffe, Peter McGregor, Tom Peake, and
chorus performances and interactive countersinging Andrew Terry cooperated in designing and inter-
exchanges with rivals. Since the publication of Susan preting many of the studies reported here. For
Smiths book on the behavioral ecology of black- helpful discussions we thank Stphanie Doucet and
capped chickadees in 1991, our knowledge of chicka- also Raleigh Robertson. We are deeply thankful to
dee singing behavior has expanded dramatically and the many agencies that funded our research on
the complex processes of communication between black-capped chickadees, most notably NSERC,
S TAT U S S I G N A L I N G A N D C O M M U N I C AT I O N N E T W O R K S 231

who has funded our ongoing chickadee research Forstmeier, W., Kempenaers, B., Meyer, A., and Leisler, B.
programs since 1993. (2002). A novel song parameter correlates with extra-
pair paternity and reflects male longevity. Proceedings of
the Royal Society of London Series B, Biology, 269,
References 14791485.
Beecher, M. D. and Brenowitz, E. A. (2005). Functional Fotheringham, J. R. and Ratcliffe, L. (1995). Song degrad-
aspects of song learning in songbirds. Trends in Ecology ation and estimation of acoustic distance in black-
and Evolution, 20, 143149. capped chickadees (Parus atricapillus). Canadian Journal
Brewer, R. (1961). Comparative notes on the life history of of Zoology, 73, 858868.
the Carolina chickadee. Wilson Bulletin, 73, 348373. Gaddis, P. K. (1983). Differential usage of song types by
Brindley, E. L. (1991). Response of European robins to plain, bridled and tufted titmice. Ornis Scandinavica, 14,
playback of song: neighbour recognition and overlap- 1623.
ping. Animal Behaviour, 41, 503512. Gammon, D. E. (2004). Black-capped chickadee dawn
Burt, J. M. and Vehrencamp, S. L. (2005). The dawn chorus chorus and subsequent sexual activity. Wilson Bulletin,
as an interactive communication network. In: 116, 252256.
McGregor, P. K., ed. Animal Communication Networks, Gammon, D. E. and Baker, M. C. (2004). Song repertoire
pp. 320343. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. evolution and acoustic divergence in a population of
Christie, P. J., Mennill, D. J., and Ratcliffe, L. M. (2004a). black-capped chickadees, Poecile atricapillus. Animal
Chickadee song structure is individually distinctive Behaviour, 68, 903913.
over long broadcast distances. Behaviour, 141, 101124. Gould, S. J. (1998). The great asymmetry. Science, 279,
Christie, P. J., Mennill, D. J., and Ratcliffe, L. M. (2004b). 812813.
Pitch shifts and song structure indicate male quality in Hailman, J. P. (1989). The organization of major vocaliza-
the dawn chorus of black-capped chickadees. tions in the Paridae. Wilson Bulletin, 101, 305343.
Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology, 55, 341348. Hailman, J. P., Haftorn, S., and Hailman, E. D. (1994). Male
Dabelsteen, T., McGregor, P. K., Shepherd, M., Whittaker, siberian tit Parus cinctus dawn serenades: suggestion for
X., and Pedersen, S. B. (1996). Is the signal value of over- the origin of song. Fauna Norv Ser C Cinclus, 17, 1526.
lapping different from that of alternating during Hansen, I. J. K., Otter, K. A., and van Oort, H. (2005).
matched singing in great tits? Journal of Avian Biology, Communication breakdown? Habitat influences on the
27, 189194. communication networks in black-capped chickadees.
Dahlsten, D. L., Brennan, L. A., McCallum, D. A., and Acta Ethol, 82, 111120.
Gaunt, S. L. L. (2002). Chestnut-backed Chickadee (Poecile Hill, B. G. and Lein, M. R. (1987). Function of frequency-
rufescens). In: Poole A. and Gill, F., eds. The Birds of North shifted songs of black-capped chickadees. Condor, 89,
America. Academy of Natural Sciences, Philadelphia. 914915.
Dent, P. K., van Oers, K., and van Noordwijk, A. J. (2002). Hill, B. G. and Lein, M. R. (1989). Natural and simulated
Realized heritability of personalities in the great tit encounters between sympatric black-capped chicka-
(Parus major). Proceedings of the Royal Society of London dees and mountain chickadees. Auk, 106, 645652.
Series B, Biology, 270, 4551. Horn, A. G., Leonard, M. L., Ratcliffe, L., Shackleton, S. A.,
Desfayes, M. (1964). An observation on the song of the and Weisman, R. G. (1992). Frequency variation in
black-capped chickadee. Wilson Bulletin, 66, 438439. songs of black-capped chickadees (Parus atricapillus).
Dixon, K. L. (1969). Patterns of singing in a population of Auk, 109, 847852.
the plain titmouse. Condor, 71, 94101. Krebs, J. R., Ashcroft, R., and Van Orsdol (1981). Song
Dixon, K. L. and Stefanski, R. A. (1970). An appraisal of matching in the great tit Parus major. Animal Behavior,
the song of the black-capped chickadee. Wilson Bulletin, 29, 918923.
82, 5362. Kroodsma, D. E. and Byers, B. E. (1991). The function(s) of
Duguay, J. P. and Ritchison, G. (1998). A contextual ana- bird song. American Zoologist, 31, 318328.
lysis of singing behavior in male tufted titmice. Journal Kroodsma, D. E., Albano, D. J., Houlihan, P. W., and Wells,
of Field Ornithology, 69, 8494. J. A. (1995). Song development by black-capped
Ficken, M. S. (1990). Vocal repertoire of the Mexican chickadees (Pars atricapillus) and Carolina chickadees
chickadee. II. Song and song-like vocalizations. Journal (P. carolinensis). Auk, 112, 2943.
of Field Ornithology, 61, 388395. Kroodsma, D. E., Byers, B. E., Halkin, S. L., et al. (1999).
Ficken, M. S., Ficken, R. W., and Witkin, S. R. (1978). Vocal Geographic variation in black-capped chickadee songs
repertoire of the black-capped chickadee. Auk, 95, 3448. and singing behavior. Auk, 116, 387402.
232 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

Lambrecht, M. M. (1992). Male quality and playback in Mennill D. J., Boag, P. T., and Ratcliffe, L. M. (2003).
the great tit. In: McGregor, P. K., ed. Playback and Studies The reproductive choices of eavesdropping female black-
of Animal Communication, pp. 135152. Plenum Press, capped chickadees, Poecile atricapillus. Naturwissenschaften,
New York. 90, 577582.
Lambrechts, M. M. and Dhondt, A. A. (1988). Male quality Mennill, D. J., Ramsay, S. M., Boag, P. T., and Ratcliffe,
and territory quality in the great tit Parus major. Animal L. M. (2004). Patterns of extrapair mating in relation to
Behaviour, 36, 595601. male dominance status and female nest placement in
Langemann, U., Tavares, J. P., Peake, T. M., and McGregor, black-capped chickadees. Behavioral Ecology, 15, 757765.
P. K. (2000). Response of great tits to escalating patterns Mennill, D. J., Ratcliffe, L. M., and Boag, P. T. (2002).
of playback. Behaviour, 137, 451471. Female eavesdropping on male song contests in song-
Lens, L., Van Dongen, S., Van den Broeck, M., Van birds. Science, 296, 873883.
Broeckhovan, V., and Dhondt, A. A. (1997). Why female Naguib, M. and Todt, D. (1997). Effects of dyadic vocal
crested tits copulate repeatedly with the same partner: interactions on other conspecific receivers in nightin-
evidence for the mate assessment hypothesis. Behavioral gales. Animal Behavior, 54, 15351543.
Ecology, 8, 8791. Odum, E. P. (1941). Annual cycle of the black-capped
Lohr, B., Nowicki, S., and Weisman, R. (1991). Pitch chickadee. Auk, 58, 314333.
production in Carolina chickadee songs. Condor, 93, Otter, K. and Ratcliffe, L. (1993). Changes in the singing
197199. behaviour of male black-capped chickadees (Parus atri-
McGregor, P. K. (2005). Animal Communication Networks. capillus) following mate removal. Behavioural Ecology
Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. and Sociobiology, 33, 409414.
McGregor, P. K. and Dabelsteen, T. (1996). Otter, K. and Ratcliffe, L. (1996). Female initiated divorce
Communication networks. In: Kroodsma, D. E. and in a monogamous songbird: abandoning mates for
Miller, E. H., eds. Ecology and Evolution of Acoustic males of higher quality. Proceedings of the Royal Society of
Communication in Birds, pp 409425. Cornell University London Series B, Biology, 263, 351354.
Press, Ithaca. Otter, K. and Ratcliffe, L. (2005). Enlightened decisions:
McGregor, P. K., Dabelsteen, T., Shepherd, M., and female assessment and communication networks. In:
Pedersen, S. B. (1992). The signal value of matched McGregor, P. K., ed. Animal Communication Networks,
singing in great tits: evidence from interactive playback pp. 133151. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
experiments. Animal Behavior, 43, 987998. Otter, K., Chruszcz, B., and Ratcliffe, L. (1997). Honest
McGregor, P. K., Krebs, J. R., and Perrins, C. H. (1981). advertisement and singing during the dawn chorus of
Song repertoires and lifetime reproductive success in black-capped Chickadees, Parus atricapillus. Behavioral
the great tit (Parus major). American Naturalist, 188, Ecology, 8, 167173.
149159. Otter, K., McGregor, P. K., Terry, A. M., Burford, F. R. L.,
McLaren, M. (1976). Vocalizations of the boreal chickadee. Peake, T. M., and Dabelsteen, T. (1999). Do female great
Auk, 93, 451463. tits (Parus major) assess males by eavesdropping? A field
Mennill, D. J. and Ratcliffe, L. M. (2004a). Do male black- study using interactive song playback. Proceedings of the
capped chickadees eavesdrop on song contests? A Royal Society of London Series B, Biology, 266, 13051310.
multi-speaker playback experiment. Behaviour, 141, Otter, K., Ratcliffe, L., Michaud, D., and Boag, P. T. (1998).
125139. Do female black-capped chickadees prefer high ranking
Mennill, D. J. and Ratcliffe, L. M. (2004b). Nest cavity males as extra-pair partners? Behavioural Ecology and
orientation in black-capped chickadees Poecile atricapil- Sociobiology, 43, 2536.
lus: do the acoustic properties of cavities influence Otter, K., Stewart, I. K., McGregor, P. K., Terry, A. M. R.,
sound reception in the nest and extra-pair matings? Dabelsteen, T., and Burke, T. (2001). Extra-pair paternity
Journal of Avian Biology, 35, 477482. in great tits in relation to manipulation of male signals.
Mennill, D. J. and Ratcliffe, L. M. (2004c). Overlapping Journal of Avian Biology, 32, 338344.
and matching in the song contests of black-capped Otter, K. A., Ratcliffe, L., Njegovan, M., and
chickadees. Animal Behaviour, 67, 441450. Fotheringham, J. (2002). The importance of frequency
Mennill, D. J. and Rogers, A. C. (2006). Whip it good! and temporal matching in aggressive vocal signalling of
Geographic consistency in male songs and variability black-capped chickadees: evidence from interactive
in female songs of the duetting Eastern Whipbird playback. Ethology, 108, 181191.
(Psophodes olivaceus). Journal of Avian Biology, 37, Peake, T. M. (2005). Eavesdropping in communication
93100. networks. In: McGregor, P. K., ed. Animal Communication
S TAT U S S I G N A L I N G A N D C O M M U N I C AT I O N N E T W O R K S 233

Networks, pp. 1338. Cambridge University Press, Shackleton, S. A. and Ratcliffe, L. (1994). Matched counter-
Cambridge. singing signals escalation of aggression in black-capped
Phillmore, L. S., Sturdy, C. B., Turyk, M. M., and Weisman, chickadees (Parus atricapillus). Ethology, 97, 310316.
R. G. (2002). Discrimination of individual vocalizations Smith, S. M. (1988). Extra-pair copulations in black-capped
by black-capped chickadees (Poecile atricapilla). Animal chickadees: the role of the female. Behaviour, 107, 1523.
Learning and Behavior, 30, 4352. Smith, S. M. (1989). Black-capped chickadee summer
Poesel, A. (2004). Singing in territorial defence and mate floaters. Wilson Bulletin, 101, 344349.
choice in the blue tit Parus caeruleus. Ph.D thesis, Smith, S. M. (1991). The Black-Capped Chickadee: Behavioral
University of Copenhagen, Denmark. Ecology and Natural History. Cornell University Press:
Ramsay, S. M., Otter, K., Mennill, D. J., and Ratcliffe, L. Ithaca.
(2000). Divorce and extrapair mating in female black- Todt, D. (1981). On functions of vocal matching: effect of
capped chickadees (Poecile atricapillus): Separate strate- counter-replies on song post choice and singing.
gies with a common target. Behavioural Ecology and Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie, 57, 7393.
Sociobiology, 49, 1823. Vehrencamp, S. L. (2000). Is song-type matching a con-
Ramsay, S. M., Otter, K., and Ratcliffe, L. M. (1999). ventional signal of aggressive intentions? Proceedings of
Nest site selection by female black-capped chicka- the Royal Society of London, Series B, 268, 16371642.
dees: settlement based on social attraction. Auk, 116, Ward, R. (1996). Regional variation in the song of the
604617. Carolina chickadee. Living Bird, 5, 127150.
Ratcliffe, L. and Weisman, R. G. (1985). Frequency shift in Wiebe, M. O. and Lein, M. R. (1999). Use of song types by
the fee-bee song of the black-capped chickadee. Condor, mountain chickadees (Poecile gambeli). Wilson Bulletin,
87, 555556. 111, 368375.
This page intentionally left blank
SYNOPSIS III

Complexities in vocal
communication
Todd M. Freeberg, Myron C. Baker, Laurie L. Bloomfield,
Isabelle Charrier, David E. Gammon, Jack P. Hailman,
Tiffany T.-Y. Lee, Jeffrey R. Lucas, Daniel J. Mennill,
and Christopher B. Sturdy

Overview chickadees, we have made significant advances in


understanding vocal communication in a number
The vocal systems of Parids are among the of chickadee species. Much of this work is
most complex communicative systems docu- described in the chapters in this section of the book.
mented in the avian world. Part of this complexity Research on fee-bee vocalizations shows the extent
stems from the large number of acoustically of species-level information (Chapter 12), eluci-
and functionally distinct vocal signals, each dates the causes and functions of absolute and
of which has its own acoustic and functional relative pitch changes (Chapters 10 and 14), and
diversity (Ficken et al. 1978; Hailman and demonstrates significant geographic variation
Ficken 1996; Chapter 13). Because of the vocal (Chapter 12). We are starting to understand the
complexity of most, if not all Parids, this syntax of gargles and the nature and functions
group could be a model system for understanding of local sharing and social transmission of gargle
how social and physical environmental pressures types (Chapter 11). We continue to decode the
might drive variation in signaling systems. In information conveyed in chick-a-dee calls, from
this synopsis, we briefly review some of the messages and meanings of different note types to
major advances that have been made in different markers of identity that reside in acoustic
understanding the vocal systems of chickadees parametersindividual, population, and species
and related species and then draw comparisons to (Chapters 10 and 13).
vocal systems in Parids outside North America. As the chapters in this section attest, the vocal-
Our primary focus, however, is to delineate izations of chickadees and titmice offer an exciting
questions that need to be addressed if we hope to and rich comparative system with which to
answer perhaps the most fundamental question address questions of mechanisms, development,
about this vocal complexity; namely, why does it function, and phylogeny of vocal behavior
exist in this group? (e.g. Tinbergen 1963). Of these four why ques-
tions for behavior, we know much more about
the function question than the other three, so
Vocal complexity of chickadees clearly one of our general aims should be to gain
and titmice greater understanding of mechanistic, ontogen-
Beginning with the seminal work of Ficken et al. etic, and phylogenetic influences on Parid vocal
(1978) on the vocalizations of black-capped communication.

235
236 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

followed by most passerines? We do not know, but


The song versus call distinction and
we suggest the following possibilities. First, gargle
social complexity
and chick-a-dee calls can convey individual, popula-
One intriguing feature of the vocal systems of North tion, and species identity (e.g. Ficken and Popp
American Parids is the seemingly reversed struc- 1995; Chapter 10), so perhaps this has taken
turefunction relationship of their songs and calls pressure off fee-bees to solely encode this informa-
(also intriguing is the fact that the major vocaliza- tion, resulting in structurally simpler signals.
tions of many European Parids do seem to fit into Alternatively, perhaps song is more complex
the typical song/call distinctions). As described than we have realized. For example, complexity in
recently by Marler (2004, p. 132): songs are usually the fee-bee vocalization may occur in different
longer and more complex acoustically, involving a dimensions than is typical of other passerines (e.g.
variety of different notes and syllables, ordered in frequency shiftingChapter 14). The complexity of
statistically reliable sequences; calls are often short, the fee-bee vocalization could also be augmented by
monosyllabic, with simple frequency patterning, the addition of other vocal signals, particularly
often delivered in what often appears to be a disor- gargles, to song displays. Another possibility is that
derly fashion. Functionally, whereas songs play a the complex social structure of many chickadee
somewhat restricted role, in territory establishment species (Chapter 9) may have generated selection
and maintenance, and mate attraction, the functions pressure for greater complexity in their year-round
of calls include not only reproduction, but also social signals (chick-a-dees and gargles) than would
predator alarm, the announcement and exchange of otherwise be the case.
food, and the maintenance of social proximity and The complexity in the vocal repertoire of
group composition and integration. chickadees and titmice seems to exist for the purpose
Whereas the fee-bee song and chick-a-dee call obey of conveying different kinds of information, and not
the functional criteria outlined in the quote above, as a result of selection for diversity per se, as in the
the structural characteristics of these two vocal case of sexual selection for larger song repertoires in
signals are quite reversed, with fee-bees being, male northern mockingbirds, Mimus polyglottos
acoustically, relatively simple and chick-a-dee calls (Howard 1974). Might the vocal complexity be
variable and diverse (but its combinatorial nature is driven by social complexity? Recent findings
by no means disorderly). The gargle is similarly indicate that chickadees in larger (and presumably
difficult to fit into this traditional songcall frame- therefore more complex) social groups produce
work, as it is structurally complex like the chick-a-dee chick-a-dee calls that contain greater information
call (though more stereotyped and not as combina- than chickadees in smaller social groups (Freeberg
torial), but appears to be used in fewer social 2006). Further support for this hypothesis is
contexts than the chick-a-dee call. The gargle shares a the fact that other groups with complex social
functional characteristic with the chick-a-dee call that structures, such as the Corvidae (jays, crows, and
makes them different from fee-beesgargles and magpies) and Psittaciforms (parrots), often seem to
chick-a-dees are used year-round (feature of calls), show considerable complexity in their vocal
whereas fee-bees are used almost exclusively in pair- repertoires (e.g. Brown and Farabaugh 1997;
ing and breeding (feature of songs). Furthermore, Bradbury 2003).
songs in oscine passerines are usually characterized One major unanswered question circles back to
as being learned, and calls as being unlearned (e.g. the title of this book and how little we actually
Catchpole and Slater 1999). Research has found know about titmouse vocal behavior. Titmouse
evidence of vocal learning for all three vocal signals: vocal repertoires seem to be comparable to chicka-
fee-bees (Shackleton and Ratcliffe 1993; Kroodsma dee vocal repertoires in terms of the diversity of
et al. 1995), gargles (Ficken and Popp 1995), and vocal types that occur and the functions they likely
chick-a-dees (Hughes et al. 1998). serve (Gaddis 1979; Hailman 1989). However,
Why do these species seem to defy the more titmice have been severely understudied relative to
typical songcall structurefunction characteristics chickadees for virtually all aspects of their vocal
S TAT U S S I G N A L I N G A N D C O M M U N I C AT I O N N E T W O R K S 237

behavior. Of the small number of studies on our thinking about communication extends from a
titmouse vocal behavior, most have addressed the sender signal receiver framework to that of
song and what social contexts promote singing senders and receivers interacting in complex
behavior (Gaddis 1983; Johnson 1987; Schroeder communication networks, our understanding of the
and Wiley 1983; Duguay and Ritchison 1998). Do real-world causes and consequences of vocal signal-
titmouse signals, like those of the chickadees that ing in these species will continue to grow
have been studied, fall with difficulty into the (McGregor 2005).
traditional songcall distinctions? Finally, vocal Within chickadees, there has perhaps also been a
behavior of the true tits (Paridae) has been studied bias against investigations of visual signals by the
much more extensively than that of the penduline apparent lack of sexual dimorphism in coloration
tits (Remizidae). One of our major aims should patterns. However, increased understanding of UV
therefore be to increase our understanding of the perception in birds and techniques for measuring
vocal systems of these under-studied groups, a these cues has resulted in recent work showing
point to which we return later. previously unsuspected variation in plumage
brightness in chickadees (Mennill et al. 2003). Not
only does variation in plumage brightness appear
Methodological advances
to convey information about male rank and repro-
We know much more about the vocal systems of ductive success (Doucet et al. 2005), but females
chickadees and titmice, and of songbirds in general, appear to be able to assess male social standing on
than we do about their visual (or other) display these visual cues in the absence of witnessing
systems. The primary reason is that it has been far dominance interactions, and apparently in the
easier to record, analyze, and manipulate sounds absence of other vocal signals (Woodcock et al.
produced by birds in the laboratory and field than 2005). Clearly, visual signaling in chickadees is an
it has been to conduct studies of other signaling avenue for future research.
systems, largely due to the availability of portable
recording and playback equipment (e.g. Baptista
What is to be done?
and Gaunt 1994). Video and other methods of
visual analysis (e.g. measuring UV) are becoming We end this synopsis by raising some additional
more accessible and providing some exciting questions we need to pursue in vocal communica-
avenues of research (see Chapter 2) , but have yet tion in Parids. We also try to link our understand-
to catch up to the decades of research on vocal ing of North American Parids to some of the work
behavior in tits. Playbacks of vocal signals have a that has been conducted with species outside North
long history in studies of birds in field and America.
laboratory settings (McGregor 1992). Various Previous research in Parids has demonstrated a
methodological advances are aiding our ability to severe geographic and taxonomic bias.
understand the behavior of receivers and signalers Phylogenetic relationships of Parids are being
with respect to vocal communication. For example, clarified, but in terms of gaining understanding of
the recent development of interactive playback vocal system evolution, we have a long way to go
designs has revealed behavioral outcomes that because vocal repertoires of so few of the species
might not otherwise have been obtainable. These have been adequately documented (see also
developments include work on black-capped Hailman and Ficken 1996). We have minimal infor-
chickadees and great tits (reviewed in Chapter 14). mation on dawn chorus behavior, on types of vocal-
Other methodological advances, such as multi- izations and their contexts of use, and on the
channel real-time spectrogram capabilities and relationship between vocal signaling and mating
multimicrophone recording arrays are allowing behavior in most of the Paridaefor example,
for very detailed information about multiple much of the Voice descriptions for Paridae
signalers interacting over time and space (Bower species provided in Harrap and Quinn (1995) are
and Clark 2005; Burt and Vehrencamp 2005). As taken from field guides and not from primary
238 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

science articles devoted to addressing vocalizations these different vocal signals is to understand the
for a given species. To cite a couple of examples, we neural underpinnings of the behaviors. For
know almost nothing about the vocal behavior of example, are the neural circuits for vocal develop-
the penduline, black, and grey tits in Africa. The ment and production the same for the three sig-
one example documented thus far of commonly nals? Decades of work have clarified the neural
occurring syntactic permutation in the chick-a-dee substrates for bird song (Nottebohm 1999), but
call (thus perhaps approaching more closely the only recently have neural influences on calls in
human language notion of syntax) exists in the call songbirds been addressed (Vicario et al. 2002).
of the black-lored tit, Parus xanthogenys, of India Neural regions underlying signals used seasonally
(Hailman 1994). However, very little is known would likely themselves show seasonal changes
about the vocal behavior of the other Parus (for example brain regions underlying spatial
genus members in India and east Asia (though memory change seasonally with respect to the time
much more is known about the European member periods of reliance on cached food items in these
of this genus, the great tit). In North America, species; Chapters 2, 3, and 4). Very interestingly,
we know relatively little about the vocal behavior recent work with black-capped chickadees found
of the brown-capped chickadee groupthe no seasonal changes in nuclei of the song system
Siberian tit and the chestnut-backed and boreal despite seasonal changes in song structure
chickadees. (Smulders et al. 2006). Seasonal influences in
From recent work with interactive playbacks, it is song system nuclei have been documented in blue
clear that how a chickadees signals are perceived is tits (Caro et al. 2005), but the extent to which these
not just dependent upon what it signals but also neural changes may differentially influence singing
how and when it signals. This raises the need for us and calling behavior is not known. Gene expression
to study countersinging and countercalling in these patterns in the brain may help our understanding
species. How does assessment of senders occur in of vocal variation in these species, particularly if
species such as chestnut-backed chickadees that calls induce such patterns in different ways in
seem to lack long-range signals, or in species that the brain relative to songs (e.g. Mello 2002).
rely more on the shorter-range gargles in their dawn Chickadees, tits, and titmice would thus seem to
choruses? Recent work indicates that environmen- offer a powerful experimental system with which
tal noise impacts frequency characteristics of song to address neurophysiological questions related to
in great tits (Slabbekoorn and Peet 2003); do back- vocal signaling, learning, and seasonal changes.
ground noises also impact the shorter-range gargles We have learned an enormous amount of infor-
and chick-a-dee calls? mation about the vocal complexity of chickadees in
Within even the well-studied chickadee species, the past three decades. The richness and variation
numerous questions remain to be answered. For in the vocal and social behavior of these species is
example are pitch-shifted versions of fee-bees such that they likely represent an ideal system for
perceived as being distinct from one another by tackling important questions about ecological and
receivers? How are intermediate pitch shifts evolutionary influences on vocal communication.
perceived? In the chick-a-dee call, the C and D Still, it is important to take a cautious view about
notes seem to have been most studiedwhat how much we really do understand about Parid
roles do the A and B notes play in communication? vocal complexitywe will likely need all the
In the gargle, do patterns of geographic variation conceptual and methodological advances at our
and local sharing occur in species other than disposal to gain a firm hold of this daunting
black-capped chickadees, and do these other system. We have learned enough at this point,
species follow the same rules for constructing however, to feel confident in arguing that the
gargles? payoffs of continued efforts to integrate proximate
Earlier we raised the difficulty of placing the fee- and ultimate approaches to understanding this
bee, gargle, and chick-a-dee neatly into song or complex system of vocal behavior could be quite
call bins. An exciting prospect that stems from large indeed.
S TAT U S S I G N A L I N G A N D C O M M U N I C AT I O N N E T W O R K S 239

Hailman, J. P. (1994). Constrained permutation in chick-


References
a-dee-like calls of a black-lored tit (Parus xanthogenys).
Baptista, L. F. and Gaunt, S. L. L. (1994). Advances in the Bioacoustics, 6, 3350.
study of avian sound communication. Condor, 96, Hailman, J. P. and Ficken, M. S. (1996). Comparative ana-
817830. lysis of vocal repertoires, with reference to chickadees.
Bower, J. L. and Clark, C. W. (2005). A field test of the In: Kroodsma, D. E., and Miller, E. H., eds. Ecology and
accuracy of a passive acoustic location system. Evolution of Acoustic Communication in Birds, pp. 136159.
Bioacoustics, 15, 114. Cornell University Press, Ithaca, New York.
Bradbury, J. W. (2003). Vocal communication in wild Harrap, S. and Quinn, D. (1995). Chickadees, Tits,
parrots. In: de Waal, F. B. M. and Tyack, P. L., eds. Nuthatches and Treecreepers. Princeton University Press,
Animal Social Complexity: Intelligence, Culture, and Princeton, New Jersey.
Individualized Societies, pp. 293316. Harvard University Howard, R. D. (1974). The influence of sexual selection
Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts. and interspecific competition on mockingbird song
Brown, E. D. and Farabaugh, S. M. (1997). What birds with (Mimus polyglottos). Evolution, 28, 428438.
complex social relationships can tell us about vocal learn- Hughes, M., Nowicki, S., and Lohr, B. (1998). Call learning
ing: vocal sharing in avian groups. In: Snowdon, C. T. and in black-capped chickadees (Parus atricapillus): the role
Hausberger, M., eds. Social Influences on Vocal Development, of experience in the development of chick-a-dee calls.
pp. 98127. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Ethology, 104, 232249.
Burt, J. M. and Vehrencamp, S. L. (2005). Dawn chorus Johnson, L. S. (1987). Pattern of song type use for territor-
as an interactive communication network. In: McGregor, ial defense in the plain titmouse Parus inornatus. Ornis
P. K., ed. Animal Communication Networks, pp. 320343. Scandinavica, 18, 2432.
Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Kroodsma, D. E., Albano, D. J., Houlihan, P. W., and Wells,
Caro, S. P., Lambrechts, M. M., and Balthazart, J. (2005). J. A. (1995). Song development by black-capped
Early seasonal development of brain song control nuclei chickadees (Parus atricapillus) and Carolina chickadees
in male blue tits. Neuroscience Letters, 386, 139144. (P. carolinensis). Auk, 112, 2943.
Catchpole, C. K. and Slater, P. J. B. (1999). Bird Song: Marler, P. (2004). Bird calls: a cornucopia for communica-
Biological Themes and Variations. Cambridge University tion. In: Marler, P. and Slabbekoorn, H., eds. Natures
Press, Cambridge. Music: the Science of Birdsong, pp. 132177. Elsevier,
Doucet, S. M., Mennill, D. J., Montgomerie, R., Boag, P. T., London and San Diego, California.
and Ratcliffe, L. M. (2005). Achromatic plumage varia- McGregor, P. K., ed. (1992). Playback and Studies of Animal
tion predicts the reproductive success of male black- Communication. Plenum Press, New York.
capped chickadees. Behavioral Ecology, 16, 218222. McGregor, P. K., ed. (2005). Animal Communication
Duguay, J. P. and Ritchison, G. (1998). A contextual ana- Networks. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
lysis of singing behavior in male tufted titmice. Journal Mello, C. V. (2002). Mapping vocal communication
of Field Ornithology, 69, 8594. pathways in birds with inducible gene expression. Journal
Ficken, M. S. and Popp, J. W. (1995). Long-term persist- of Comparative Physiology, A, 188, 943959.
ence of a culturally-transmitted vocalization of the Mennill, D. J., Doucet, S. M., Montgomerie, R., and
black-capped chickadee. Animal Behaviour, 50, 683693. Ratcliffe, L. M. (2003). Achromatic colour variation in
Ficken, M. S., Ficken, R. W., and Witkin, S. R. (1978). Vocal black-capped chickadees, Poecile atricapilla, black and
repertoire of the black-capped chickadee. Auk, 95, white signals of sex and rank. Behavioral Ecology and
3448. Sociobiology, 53, 350357.
Freeberg, T. M. (2006). Social complexity can drive vocal Mennill, D. J., Ratcliffe, L. M., and Boag, P. T. (2002).
complexity: Group size influences vocal information in Female eavesdropping on male song contests in song-
Carolina chickadees. Psychological Science. 17, 557561. birds. Science, 296, 873.
Gaddis, P. K. (1979). A Comparative Analysis of the Vocal Nottebohm, F. (1999). The anatomy and timing of vocal
Communication Systems of the Carolina Chickadee and the learning in birds. In: Hauser, M. D. and Konishi, M.,
Tufted Titmouse. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Florida. eds. The Design of Animal Communication, pp. 63110.
Gaddis, P. K. (1983). Differential usage of song types by MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts.
plain, bridled, and tufted titmice. Ornis Scandinavica, 14, Otter, K. A., McGregor, P. K., Terry, A. M. R., Burford, F. R.
1623. L., Peake, T. M., and Dabelsteen, T. (1999). Do female
Hailman, J. P. (1989). The organization of the major vocal- great tits (Parus major) assess males by eavesdropping?
izations in the Paridae. Wilson Bulletin, 101, 305343. A field study using interactive song playback.
240 V O C A L C O M M U N I C AT I O N

Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: system nuclei of the black-capped chickadee (Poecile
Biological Sciences, 266, 13051309. atricapillus). Journal of Neurology. 66, 9911001.
Schroeder, D. J. and Wiley, R. H. (1983). Communication Tinbergen, N. (1963). On aims and methods of ethology.
with shared song themes in tufted titmice. Animal Zeitschrift fr Tierpsychologie, 20, 410433.
Behaviour, 31, 11281138. Vicario, D. S., Raksin, J. N., Naqvi, N. H., Thande, N., and
Shackleton, S. A. and Ratcliffe, L. (1993). Development of Simpson, H. B. (2002). The relationship between
song in hand-reared black-capped chickadees. Wilson perception and production in songbird vocal imitation:
Bulletin, 105, 637644. what learned calls can teach us. Journal of Comparative
Slabbekoorn, H. and Peet, M. (2003). Birds sing at a Physiology, A, 188, 897908.
higher pitch in urban noise. Nature, 424, 267. Woodcock, E. A., Rathburn, M. K., and Ratcliffe, L. M.
Smulders, T. V., Lisi, M. D., Tricomi, E., Otter, K. A., (2005). Achromatic plumage reflectance, social
Chruszcz, B., Ratcliffe, L. M., and DeVoogd, T. J. dominance and female mate preference in black-capped
(2006). Failure to detect seasonal changes in the song chickadees (Poecile atricapillus). Ethology, 111, 891900.
SECTION IV

Landscape ecology, behavior, and


conservation issues

At first glance, it may seem counterintuitive to thermogenesis. They then consider these evolved
think of Parids in the context of conservation. The responses in chickadees and tufted titmice in
birds in this family, particularly the well-studied isolated woodlots in agricultural landscapes,
species, are apparent paragons of adaptability to where fragmentation and patch size greatly alter
human disturbance. However, it is the ability to the microclimate to which species are exposed. In
persist in human-altered landscapes that makes the final chapter of the section, Otter, van Oort, and
this group useful for understanding the subtle Fort (Chapter 17) address the potential for breeding
impacts of landscape-level changes on native in altered habitats, such as those found in managed
organisms. In particular, chickadees and titmice are habitat matrices that separate remnant, native
forest generalists that can be used to test some of forest. Using studies on both Eurasian tits and
the theories associated with forest fragmentation, North American chickadees, Otter et al. parallel
especially in relation to impacts on movement, studies investigating breeding in forests of
breeding, and winter survival. The three chapters divergent habitat quality with proposed manage-
in this section of the book address such topics of ment of matrix vegetation as alternative breeding
habitat fragmentation and landscape ecology. habitat for matrix-tolerating species.
In Chapter 15, Desrochers and Blisle use meta- The chapters in this section emphasize the advan-
analysis to study Parid habitat use at three spatial tage that can be gained by using common, generalist
scalesedge, patch, and the landscape. The focus species to test theoretical assumptions in landscape
then shifts to how landscape features, particularly ecology and conservation. The ability to closely
the behavioral response of black-capped and boreal monitor movement and breeding in Parids in a
chickadees to gaps and forest corridors, influence variety of habitats allows researchers to assess even
interpatch dispersion and its potential for metapopu- subtle impacts of habitat disturbance on species
lation dynamics. Olson and Grubb (Chapter 16) ecology. By understanding what impacts human-
review the literature on the physiological response altered landscapes have on resistant species such as
to cold among overwintering resident Parids, Parids, we gain better understanding of how these
such as controlled hypothermia and shivering same impacts may affect more sensitive species.

241
This page intentionally left blank
CHAPTER 15

Edge, patch, and landscape effects on


Parid distribution and movements
Andr Desrochers and Marc Blisle

15.1 Introduction understand challenges that Parids face when they


move through landscapes. A cursory look at
The fact that birds, despite their great vagility, are the Parid-versus-landscape literature shows that,
often absent from small or isolated patches of habi- although very similar in appearance and life
tat has puzzled ecologists for years, if not decades. history, Parids exhibit a great diversity of responses
Answering the isolation question for forest birds to the challenges posed by habitat pattern and
in general would be a daunting task, to say the isolation.
least. But we feel that Parids provide a good start- In this chapter, we ask the general questions, how
ing point and a true model system. The basic do Parids use landscapes and can we explain it by
ecology of most species of Parids is covered in the study of movements of individuals? We review
detail by outstanding works such as Birds of North available evidence, observational and manipula-
America (Poole et al. 19922003), Birds of the Western tive, on space use at three scales: edge, patch, and
Palearctic (Cramp et al. 19781997), and will landscape. We link evidence on Parid distribution
undoubtedly be treated in detail in an upcoming with general insights on forest bird movements, in
issue of the monumental series, Birds of the World an attempt to explain the former by the latter.
(del Hoyo et al. 1992present). Yet, there is very Throughout the chapter, we place the emphasis on
little information on individual movements in individuals, not populations (e.g. dispersal), which
Parids, with the exception of natal dispersal dis- receives more thorough treatment in Chapter 16.
tances, which were documented in both Europe
(Greenwood et al. 1979) and North America (Weise
and Meyer 1979) but have received less attention
15.2 Response to edges by Parids
since these initial studies. Further, studies of natal
dispersal have typically focused on their popula- Habitat edges are especially important in the study
tion and genetic consequences (e.g. Postma and van of animal movements, because edges define land-
Noordwijk 2005), rather than the immediate scape heterogeneity as well as movement opportun-
processes involved, such as what happens when ities and challenges. Thus, it is along edges that
an individual encounters a barrier between two birds will often face movement options, such as
habitat patches. flying into the open versus remaining under forest
Landscape ecologists interested in birds gener- cover. The way edges refract or reflect bird
ally focus on entire communities. As a result, movements should tell us much about how birds
there is a sizeable body of literature on relation- perceive their landscapes. Researchers have stud-
ships between Parids and edges, patches, and ied responses of birds to edges mostly by looking at
landscapes. While that literature is mainly based distribution patterns in relation to distances from
on species distribution data, it does provide edges, but also through the detailed analysis of
useful background information that may help us passive or experimentally-induced movements

243
244 L A N D S C A P E E C O L O G Y, B E H AV I O R , A N D C O N S E R VAT I O N

(reviewed by Desrochers et al. 1999 and Harris and explanations for edge use or avoidance by Parid
Reed 2002). Here we address these approaches, and flocks: (1) higher food abundance near edges, (2)
their relevance to the study of Parids, especially in avoidance of harsh microclimate, (3) avoidance of
the case of non-breeding flocks, whose home range predators, and (4) edges as movement corridors.
sizes are relevant to what is commonly understood
as landscapes, that is areas spanning more than
15.2.1 Food abundance
just a few hectares.
There is a plethora of studies, spanning over In a Pennsylvania, US, study on Parids versus
70 years (e.g. Leopold 1933), on the relative occur- edges, Rodewald and Brittingham (2002) showed
rence or abundance of birds near or far from habi- that black-capped chickadees and tufted titmice
tat boundaries (hereafter referred to as edges). tended to have slower movement rates when they
These studies have mostly focused on species were near forest edges (~4.7 m/min) than when
inhabiting forest habitats, and have been conducted they were away from edges (~5.6 m/min). Based
along strong gradients or hard edges created by on this evidence, they suggested that forest edges
agriculture, forestry, or urban sprawl. Several functioned more as feeding habitats than move-
mechanisms, most often unrelated to movement ment corridors. On the other side of the Atlantic,
per se, have been proposed to explain why some Hansson (1983) found that tits (coal, crested, great,
species are associated with, or avoid, habitat edges. and willow) occurred more frequently near clearcut
Some species may indeed be attracted to intrinsic edges than away from them, and interpreted this as
properties of habitat edges (e.g. shrubby vegetation resulting from higher arthropod abundance near
or the encounter of two complementary resources), clearcut edges. In support of his hypothesis, he
while the association (positive or negative) of other explained that the association between tree-
species to edges may simply reflect processes unre- gleaners and forest edges was more pronounced
lated to an edge effect per se, for example artefactual than that for other types of foragers (e.g. grani-
(Imbeau et al. 2003), or reflecting constraints on vores), which he attributed to the rich supply of
movements (Desrochers and Fortin 2000). insects in decaying trees along forest edges.
Parids often occur in open areas adjoining Thus far, Brotons and Herrando (2003) provide
forests, such as transmission-line corridors the best evidence (i.e. with data on food abun-
(Anderson et al. 1977; Pearson 1993), agricultural dance), that winter flocks of coal tits indeed
fields (Blisle and Desrochers 2002), and recent concentrated their activities near forest edges,
clearcuts (A. Hadley, personal communication). Of apparently to take advantage of higher food supply.
25 studies that we found reporting associations (or However, Desrochers and Fortin (2000) found no
the lack thereof) between edge proximity and the increase in foraging success of black-capped chicka-
occurrence or abundance of Parids, at least 22 dees near edges versus chickadees away from
focused on the breeding season (AprilJuly), a edges. To our knowledge, these are the only studies
period during which movements of individuals are examining in detail the question of edges as food
generally restricted to a few hectares. Studies of sources for Parids, and it is therefore impossible to
edge versus distribution of breeding Parids are of draw general conclusions yet on this phenomenon.
limited interest to the study of movements, because
associations between Parid territory locations and
15.2.2 Microclimate
edges may largely result from nesting (e.g. Doherty
and Grubb 2002a) rather than movement. The few In an early study in Pennsylvania, US, Yahner
published studies of edge use by Parids outside the (1987) found that black-capped chickadees avoided
breeding season provide more scope for insights on the edges of clearcuts, and interpreted this as a pos-
movement behavior, given the large extent of sible avoidance of harsh microclimate. Yahner was
movements of flocks relative to breeding pairs apparently unaware of the earlier work by Grubb
(e.g. reaching over 30 ha; Siffczyk et al. 2003). Those (1977) in New Jersey, in which Grubb showed that
studies provide at least four non-exclusive black-capped chickadees and tufted titmice indeed
PA R I D D I S T R I B U T I O N A N D M O V E M E N T S 245

avoided windward edges of forest patches under and Desrochers 2003). These conclusions fit well
cold and windy weather. Twelve years later, Dolby with field observations. Rodrguez et al. (2001)
and Grubb (1999) replicated the latter study in studied the effect of predation risk on the behavior
Ohio, and again they showed how Carolina of coal, crested, and willow tits at the edge of
chickadees and tufted titmice avoided forest edges patches of mature boreal forest in Sweden. Birds
on windward sides of forest patches when weather used the exposed side of the edge (matrix) mainly
was cold and windy (see also Chapter 16). Dolby during periods with low levels of predation
and Grubbs 1999 Ohio study area was highly pressure by European pygmy owls (Glaucidium
fragmented (10% forest cover), however, and passerinum). Although tits did not respond consist-
presumably provided no opportunity for its Parid ently to predator pressure, they clearly avoided
inhabitants to use forest edges as movement habitats presenting high predation risk and, as a
conduits. result, were more or less confined to mature forest
Parid flocks may not only avoid windward forest stands. In particular, coal tits behaved more
edges, but they may also have the ability to conservatively (lower exposure) than the other,
expect that forest edges, especially windward larger, two species of tits (Rodrguez et al. 2001). A
ones, will provide poor foraging conditions. similar pattern of predator avoidance was noted in
Indeed, Brotons et al. (2001) showed that black- Spain, especially with blue tits, which foraged
capped chickadees hoard food towards the forest deeper in the canopy when predator pressure was
interior when they have access to a feeder at ca. greatest (Tellera et al. 2001).
30 m from an edge, but they do not exhibit such a
pattern when the feeder is deeper into a forest.
15.2.4 Movement corridors
A concentration of Parid occurrences near forest
15.2.3 Predator avoidance
edges should be found if forest edges act as effect-
Parids foraging outside forest cover have been ive movement barriers for these birds, because
shown experimentally to adjust their use of open flocks would tend to move along edges in order to
habitats with respect to predation risk. Individuals reach distant habitat patches with the smallest
do not venture out of cover in the presence of a possible fitness cost. The first quantitative evidence
predator (Desrochers et al. 2002), and will leave the for edge use as movement conduits for flocks was
forest only if in reduced body condition or if the presented by Desrochers and Fortin (2000), who
open offers improved foraging conditions com- found that black-capped chickadee flocks in
pared to the forest (Todd and Cowie 1990; Turcotte Alberta, Canada, often followed forest edges to

90
% of movements parallel to edge

80
70
60
50
40
30
20
10
Figure 15.1 A corridor effect: Black-capped Chickadee
0 flocks tend to move along forest edges (Alberta, Canada).
0 50 100 150 200 From Desrochers and Fortin (2000). Printed with
Distance to nearest forest edge (m) permission from Oikos.
246 L A N D S C A P E E C O L O G Y, B E H AV I O R , A N D C O N S E R VAT I O N

avoid flying through open areas, and thereby spent understanding of the significance of edges,
more time along edges. In support of their interpret- especially in relation to movements. But they
ation, they showed that chickadee flocks tended to provide a minimalist interpretation of habitat types
move parallel to forest edges when they were near (and their roles for movements), by basically
them (Fig. 15.1). Given the high latitude of their reducing landscapes to binary objects (forest versus
study area (54N) and its similar forest cover type non-forest). Of course, the reality is more
(deciduous), it would be difficult to imagine that complicated. For example some habitats may be
microclimatic conditions at forest edges were used primarily for movement as opposed to
milder than in the earlier Eurasian studies of edges feeding or cover habitats. Additionally, the cost
versus microclimate (both located south of 45N). of traveling through different types of forest may be
The preceding studies on Parid flocks versus highly variable. In south-central Sweden, Rodrguez
edges provide first steps towards a better et al. (2001) showed that the movements of three

Fort Montmorency
Roads
Rivers
Open
a
are

Mature
dy

Regeneration
stu

Meters
e of

0 50 100 200 300 400


Edg

Figure 15.2 Movements of four flocks of boreal chickadees, at Fort Montmorency (Quebec, Canada), each followed on a single occasion
(JanFeb 2004). Flocks are denoted by different symbols. Each symbol represents a period of 30 seconds. Positions obtained by GPS while
following the flock on showshoes. Note use of regeneration stands and movements at the edge of open areas. Data courtesy of A. Hadley.
PA R I D D I S T R I B U T I O N A N D M O V E M E N T S 247

species of tits (coal, crested, and willow) were less McCollin (1998) argued that while some common
frequent in young plantations relative to mature species of European tits (blue, coal, crested, marsh,
forests, and therefore that a barrier effect was not and willow) had consistent interior distribution
limited to truly open areas. By contrast, movements patterns in woodlands/forests across studies,
of boreal chickadees at Fort Montmorency, Quebec, great tits were subject to conflicting interior and
Canada, were apparently not impaired by regener- edge classifications. Such variability would
ating forest stands (27 m), but they did travel along explain why leading researchers such as Villard
edges of open areas (Fig. 15.2). Boreal chickadees are (1998) started to question the validity of generaliza-
typically found in mature boreal forests (Ficken et al. tions such as forest-interior species or edge
1996), but they also venture into earlier successional species.
stages occasionally (A. Hadley, personal communi-
cation; Fig. 15.2).
15.3 Responses to patches
There are undoubtedly other studies document-
ing edge use by Parid flocks, but evidence Finding a link between bird responses to forest
published so far is inadequate to provide general- edges and their apparent sensitivity to patch size
izations regarding edge use, and especially its (Ambuel and Temple 1983) would be rewarding,
causes, by Parids. However, it seems likely that given the allometric relation between the propor-
microclimate (present and future), food abundance, tion of edge a patch has, and its size. Patch use, like
predator pressure, and barrier effects all affect the edge use, has been investigated by ornithologists
use of forest edges by Parids. Of those four factors, for decades, particularly under the paradigm of
the barrier effect seems the most difficult to study, Island Biogeography. While Island Biogeography
because we normally do not have information on and the study of patch use in general was mostly
where the birds are heading when they move, and concerned with species diversity (MacArthur and
how strongly they are motivated to get to their Wilson 1967), these fields of investigation are
destination (Desrochers et al. 1999). To circumvent relevant for those of us interested in movements,
this problem, we have devised techniques in recent because they originate largely from the idea of
years where movements are induced experimen- isolation. The association between occurrence or
tally. Early experimentation involved attracting abundance and patch size may simply reflect basic
birds (including black-capped chickadees) to a area requirements by forest birds with all-purpose
speaker located in sites where the birds had to territories, such as Parids (Galli et al. 1976). Given
choose between approaching via a short, but risky their sometimes large breeding territories, and still
open route (i.e. exposed to predators), or a long but larger flock home ranges, one would expect that
safer forested route (i.e. providing cover against Parids may often exhibit patch-area sensitivity. A
predators). This experiment established that black- second potential explanation for the association
capped chickadees were prepared to travel twice as between occurrence or abundance and patch size
far, using forest edges, to avoid open areas would be edge avoidance, which would lead to the
(Desrochers and Hannon 1997). Further work avoidance of very small patches.
showed that even though black-capped chickadees To get a general idea of edge associations in the
are willing to cross gaps, they will seldom venture Parids, we searched the published literature using
over 25 m from forest edges when a forested detour the Science Citation Index Expanded from January
is available (Blisle and Desrochers 2002). 1986 to June 2005 (ISI Web of Science) for studies
Although Blisle and Desrochers (2002) did not with edge, patch, landscape, or fragmenta-
find seasonal effects on responses to forest edges, it tion and Parid generic names as keywords.
is likely that food, microclimate, predation, or Additionally, one of us (AD) used a database on
movement barriers will take precedence depending studies of edge effects on entire avian communities
on the locality and the season, and, as a result, a and searched it for studies that included Parids. Of
single bird species may exhibit great variability in the 49 documented edge associations we found for
its association with habitat edges. For example 12 species of Parids (including observations for all
248 L A N D S C A P E E C O L O G Y, B E H AV I O R , A N D C O N S E R VAT I O N

seasons), only 10 pointed to the avoidance of edges, forest patches exhibited more antipredator behav-
mostly due to predator avoidance (see above), ior (e.g. foraged deeper in the canopy) than tits for-
which suggests that edge avoidance would be a aging in unfragmented forest (Tellera et al. 2001).
relatively uncommon explanation for responses of Furthermore, under cold and windy winter condi-
Parids to patch size. tions, forest birds such as Parids may experience
Our compilation of published work points to a negative microclimatic conditions over the entire
great diversity of apparent responses to patch size area of the smallest forest patches, especially decidu-
by Parids, both interspecifically and intraspecifi- ous ones (Dolby and Grubb 1999), which would
cally (Table 15.1). Nonetheless, it is interesting to explain the apparently greater area-sensitivity dur-
note that 55% of the 75 patch-area associations ing winter (see also Chapter 16). The intraspecific
documented in 24 studies were significantly (and diversity of responses to patch size may also result
strictly) positive. This proportion may be some- from variation in the spatial arrangement of habitat
what inflated because of publication bias, but patches among studies. Indeed, a group of small
since the large majority of studies cited in Table 15.1 patches can accommodate some individuals if spa-
involved several species of birds, without a focus tially close enough to limit travel cost (Dunning
on Parids, we believe that this compilation pro- et al. 1992; Hinsley 2000; see below). Such an influ-
vides an accurate picture, at least for the species ence, though, like the ones pertaining to the range
and regions concerned. Moreover, this proportion of patch sizes and regional amount of habitat
did not depend on whether patch-area sensitivity involved, are difficult to assess because the relevant
was assessed using a species incidence (53%) or its information is hard, if not impossible, to obtain
abundance (50%). It is also noteworthy that none of from the published accounts.
the 13 Parid species under consideration exhibited As with the study of edge use, manipulative
apparent preference for smaller patch sizes. experiments can help better quantify how Parids
However, all of the 10 species for which there was and other birds respond to patch attributes such as
more than one assessment of patch-area sensitivity, their size and isolation. In an intriguing study,
exhibited somewhat conflicting results, being either Grubb and Bronson (2001) showed that the site-
area-independent or area-sensitive, depending on tenacity of black-capped chickadees relocated to
the study. Long-tailed and marsh tits stood out as small forest patches previously cleared of con-
being the most reliably area-sensitive among the specifics was unaffected by patch size or isolation.
studies concerned (9/11 and 5/7, respectively; Their interpretation of those results was that dis-
Table 15.1), but none of the authors concerned pro- persing chickadees are not unduly reluctant to
posed an explanation for these birds patch-area cross substantial gaps between habitat patches
sensitivity. Finally, it is interesting to note that there (Grubb and Doherty 1999). However, as acknow-
were four times as many clear cases of area-sensi- ledged by Grubb and Bronson, the range of patch
tivity versus insensitivity in winter (i.e. 12/15 ver- sizes they used was small, which may limit the
sus 3/15), while cases of area-sensitivity versus scope of their conclusions to the most extreme cases
insensitivity were nearly equivalent in summer (i.e. of forest fragmentation where birds may be con-
29/60 versus 28/60). It must, however, be stressed strained to manage with such conditions.
that only four of the 24 studies reported winter
observations (Table 15.1). Finally, the figures we
15.4 Beyond the patch: response to
report probably best reflect the response of Parids
landscapes
versus patch size within agricultural landscapes,
where 76% of the 75 patch-area associations were Forest patches offer a series of measures that can be
obtained. related to patterns of distribution of birds, but they
How do we make sense of the variation in area- also provide a very incomplete picture of land-
sensitivity by Parids? Edge avoidance by Parids, scapes, especially in cases where patches have com-
although uncommon, may partly explain it. For plex shapes and connections between each other.
example in Spain, wintering tits foraging in small For example Whitcomb et al. (1981) reported that
Table 15.1 Distribution of Parids in relation to forest patch size

Species Season Location Matrix Variable Patch size Reference

Long-tailed tit Breeding England A A Bellamy et al. (2000) Ecography 23 130138


Long-tailed tit Breeding England A A Hinsley et al. (1995) J. Avian Biol. 26 94104
Long-tailed tit Breeding Japan A A Kurosawa and Askins (2003) Conserv. Biol. 17 695707
Long-tailed tit Breeding Spain A/N O Brotons et al. (2004) Landscape Ecol. 19 663675
Long-tailed tit Breeding Spain A O Daz et al. (1998) J. Appl. Ecol. 35 562574
Long-tailed tit Breeding Sweden A/F/N O 0 Enoksson et al. (1995) Landscape Ecol. 10 267275
Long-tailed tit Breeding The Netherlands A O 0 Opdam et al. (1985) Biol. Conserv. 34 333352
Long-tailed tit Breeding The Netherlands A O van Dorp and Opdam (1987) Landscape Ecol. 1 5973
Long-tailed tit Winter Spain A A/O Tellera and Santos (1995) Biol. Conserv. 71 6167
Long-tailed tit Winter Spain U O Fernndez-Juricic (2002) coscience 9 450458
Long-tailed tit Winter Spain A A/O Tellera et al. (2001) Oikos 95 253264
Tufted titmouse Breeding Connecticut F? A 0 Askins et al. (1987) Biol. Conserv. 39 129152
Tufted titmouse Breeding Massachussetts U A 0 Tilghman (1987) For. Ecol. Manage. 21 163175
Tufted titmouse Breeding N Carolina N A 0/ Sallabanks et al. (2000) Condor 102 748758
Tufted titmouse Breeding N Jersey, Maryland, A O Robbins et al. (1989) Wildl. Monogr. 103 134
N Carolina
Tufted titmouse Breeding South Carolina A/N O Kilgo et al. (1997) Wildl. Soc. Bull. 25 878885

PA R I D D I S T R I B U T I O N A N D M O V E M E N T S
Tufted titmouse Winter Massachussetts U A 0 Tilghman (1987) For. Ecol. Manage. 21 163175
Coal tit Breeding Japan A A 0 Kurosawa and Askins (2003) Conserv. Biol. 17 695707
Coal tit Breeding Spain A/N O Brotons et al. (2004) Landscape Ecol. 19 663675
Coal tit Breeding Spain A O Daz et al. (1998) J. Appl. Ecol. 35 562574
Coal tit Breeding Sweden A A 0 Berg (1997) Bird Stud. 44 355366
Coal tit Winter Spain A A/O Tellera and Santos (1995) Biol. Conserv. 71 6167
Coal tit Winter Spain U O Fernndez-Juricic (2002) coscience 9 450458
Blue tit Breeding England A A Bellamy et al. (2000) Ecography 23 130138
Blue tit Breeding England A A Hinsley et al. (1995) J. Avian Biol. 26 94104
Blue tit Breeding Spain A/N O 0 Brotons et al. (2004) Landscape Ecol. 19 663675
Blue tit Breeding Spain A O Daz et al. (1998) J. Appl. Ecol. 35 562574
Blue tit Breeding Sweden A/F/N O 0 Enoksson et al. (1995) Landscape Ecol. 10 267275
Blue tit Breeding Sweden A A Berg (1997) Bird Stud. 44 355366

249
250
L A N D S C A P E E C O L O G Y, B E H AV I O R , A N D C O N S E R VAT I O N
Table 15.1 continued

Species Season Location Matrix Variable Patch size Reference

Blue tit Breeding The Netherlands A O 0 Opdam et al. (1985) Biol. Conserv. 34 333352
Blue tit Breeding The Netherlands A O 0 van Dorp and Opdam (1987) Landscape Ecol. 1 5973
Blue tit Winter Spain A A/O 0 Tellera and Santos (1995) Biol. Conserv. 71 6167
Blue tit Winter Spain U O Fernndez-Juricic (2002) coscience 9 450458
Blue tit Winter Spain A A/O 0 Tellera et al. (2001) Oikos 95 253264
Crested tit Breeding Spain A/N O Brotons et al. (2004) Landscape Ecol. 19 663675
Crested tit Breeding Sweden A A 0 Berg (1997) Bird Stud. 44 355366
Crested tit Winter Spain A A/O Tellera and Santos (1995) Biol. Conserv. 71 6167
Crested tit Winter Spain A A/O Tellera et al. (2001) Oikos 95 253264
Great tit Breeding England A A Bellamy et al. (2000) Ecography 23 130138
Great tit Breeding England A A Hinsley et al. (1995) J. Avian Biol. 26 94104
Great tit Breeding Japan A A 0 Kurosawa and Askins (2003) Conserv. Biol. 17 695707
Great tit Breeding Spain A/N O 0 Brotons et al. (2004) Landscape Ecol. 19 663675
Great tit Breeding Spain A O Daz et al. (1998) J. Appl. Ecol. 35 562574
Great tit Breeding Sweden A/F/N O 0 Enoksson et al. (1995) Landscape Ecol. 10 267275
Great tit Breeding Sweden A A - Berg (1997) Bird Stud. 44 355366
Great tit Breeding The Netherlands A O 0 van Dorp and Opdam (1987) Landscape Ecol. 1 5973
Great tit Winter Spain A A/O Tellera and Santos (1995) Biol. Conserv. 71 6167
Great tit Winter Spain U O Fernndez-Juricic (2002) coscience 9 450458
Great tit Winter Spain A A/O Tellera et al. (2001) Oikos 95 253264
Willow tit Breeding Japan A A 0 Kurosawa and Askins (2003) Conserv. Biol. 17 695707
Willow tit Breeding Sweden A O 0 Berg (1997) Bird Stud. 44 355366
Willow tit Breeding The Netherlands A O 0 Opdam et al. (1985) Biol. Conserv. 34 333352
Willow tit Breeding The Netherlands A O van Dorp and Opdam (1987) Landscape Ecol. 1 5973
Marsh tit Breeding England A A Hinsley et al. (1995) J. Avian Biol. 26 94104
Marsh tit Breeding England U A Taylor et al. (1987) Acta Oecol. 8 293299
Marsh tit Breeding Japan A A Kurosawa and Askins (2003) Conserv. Biol. 17 695707
Marsh tit Breeding Sweden A/F/N O 0 Enoksson et al. (1995) Landscape Ecol. 10 267275
Marsh tit Breeding Sweden A O 0 Berg (1997) Bird Stud. 44 355366
Marsh tit Breeding The Netherlands A O Opdam et al. (1985) Biol. Conserv. 34 333352
Marsh tit Breeding The Netherlands A O van Dorp and Opdam (1987) Landscape Ecol. 1 5973
Varied tit Breeding Japan A A 0 Kurosawa and Askins (2003) Conserv. Biol. 17 695707
Black-capped chickadee Breeding Connecticut F? A 0 Askins et al. (1987) Biol. Conserv. 39 129152
Black-capped chickadee Breeding Illinois A A 0/ Blake and Karr (1987) Ecology 68 6471
Black-capped chickadee Breeding Massachussetts U A Tilghman (1987) For. Ecol. Manage. 21 163175
Black-capped chickadee Breeding Michigan N A Crozier and Niemi (2003) Can. J. Zool. 81 441452
Black-capped chickadee Breeding N Jersey, Maryland, F/U/N O 0 Robbins et al. (1989) Wildl. Monogr. 103 134
N Carolina
Black-capped chickadee Breeding Ontario A O 0 Austen et al. (2001) Condor 103 701714
Black-capped chickadee Breeding Rhode Island N A 0 Golet et al. (2001) Wilson Bull. 113 217227
Black-capped chickadee Breeding Saskatchewan A A Hobson and Bayne (2000) Wilson Bull. 112 373387
Black-capped chickadee Winter Massachussetts U A Tilghman (1987) For. Ecol. Manage. 21 163175
Carolina chickadee Breeding N Carolina N A 0 Sallabanks et al. (2000) Condor 102 748758
Carolina chickadee Breeding N Jersey, Maryland, F/U/N O 0 Robbins et al. (1989) Wildl. Monogr. 103 134
N Carolina
Carolina chickadee Breeding South Carolina A/N O Kilgo et al. (1997) Wildl. Soc. Bull. 25 878885
Boreal chickadee Breeding Saskatchewan A A 0 Hobson and Bayne (2000) Wilson Bull. 112 373387

PA R I D D I S T R I B U T I O N A N D M O V E M E N T S
Chestnut-backed chickadee Breeding California F A Rosenberg and Raphael (1986) Wildlife 2000, 263272

Responses can be positive (), neutral (0), or negative () associations. A positive response means greater rate of occurrence (Variable O) or abundance (Variable A) in larger forest
patches. Sampled forest patches were surrounded by one of the following matrices: agricultural (A), forestry (F), natural (N), or urban (U). We searched the published, peer-reviewed literature
based on the Science Citation Index Expanded (January 1986 to May 2005; ISI Web of Science) for studies with edge, patch, landscape, or fragmentation, and Parid generic names as
keywords. Additionally, one of us (AD) used a database on landscape use studies on entire avian communities and searched it for studies that included Parids.

251
252 L A N D S C A P E E C O L O G Y, B E H AV I O R , A N D C O N S E R VAT I O N

tufted titmice occurred in forest patches sometimes an edge to show a greater probability of emigration
smaller than their territory size. Those puzzling than individuals born toward the interior of the
occurrences were interpreted as involving non-ter- patch.
ritorial birds, but they may also have been birds In the last 20 years, Geographic Information
whose territories were spread over a number of dis- Systems (GIS) have provided more sophisticated
crete patches. In Finland, Siffczyk et al. (2003) found tools for analyzing bird distributions over spatial
that willow tit flocks responded to habitat loss by expanses beyond the patches or territories occupied
enlarging their home range size. by individual birds. Do analyses of Parid distribu-
Networks of forest patches may pose challenges tion at the landscape scale (e.g. within a radius of
for wintering Parids whose flock territories may 500 m) provide evidence for landscape-sensitiv-
encompass several forest patches. For example ity? As for edge and patch size effects, the answer
crossing gaps between forest patches may incur has to be, sometimes (Table 15.2). The only pattern
predation as well as physiological costs. By meas- common to all Parids is that they are not preferen-
uring how far into the open black-capped chicka- tially associated to landscapes with small amounts
dees were willing to go to obtain a sunflower seed, of habitat. Indeed, only three out of the 54 associ-
Turcotte and Desrochers (2003) concluded that ations between occurrence and amount of forest
chickadees in highly deforested landscapes experi- within a fixed radius documented in 28 studies
enced greater food stress than chickadees in well- were negative. Moreover, clearly neutral and
forested landscapes. They interpreted their results clearly positive responses to amount of forest
as the outcome of greater loss of energy caused by composed 46% and 41% of cases, respectively.
repeated flights across gaps in fragmented home Although the proportion of positive associations
ranges, combined by risk-awareness in open areas. did not depend on whether landscape-sensitivity
Having to cross gaps may reduce fitness not only was quantified using a species occurrence (41%) or
for wintering Parids, but also through increased its abundance (48%), it depended on matrix type.
cost (in energy and time) incurred during central- Positive associations were twice as frequent in
place foraging, as shown by modeling on great tits landscapes strictly managed for agriculture (43%
(Hinsley 2000). of 23 cases) than for forestry (20% of 10 cases). Yet,
Of course, the greatest movement challenge this difference may not only result from a matrix-
faced by forest birds such as Parids may come at the type effect, but also from a lower regional amount
time of natal dispersal. It is easy to imagine that of forest habitat within the agricultural landscapes
juvenile Parids, which often disperse at distances of used in the studies we compiled. As for patch-area
several kilometers or more (e.g. Verhulst et al. 1997), associations (see above), though, published
may experience major challenges posed by habitat accounts often lack the details that would allow us
isolation. For example in Belgium, Lens and to conduct a proper meta-analysis, and thereby
Dhondt (1994) found that juvenile crested tits born avoid confounding effects and reach clearer
in forest fragments dispersed later than con- conclusions.
specifics born in contiguous forests, and immigra- In spite of this drawback, it is interesting to note
tion into forest fragments was also delayed. Lens that 11 of the 14 species studied so far sometimes
and Dhondt interpreted this result as indicative of exhibited positive associations with the amount of
lower habitat quality in fragments, possibly associ- forest habitat in the landscape. Of 12 species for
ated to a barrier effect preventing birds to dis- which we have occurrence or abundance data from
perse early. In accord with the barrier effect, more than one study, however, only three had con-
Matthysen (2002) found in another Belgian study sistent relationships between occurrence or abun-
that the likelihood that blue and great tits emigrate dance and area of forest habitat within a fixed
out of their natal patch was independent of the dis- radius (500 m; Table 15.2). These include long-
tance separating their birth site from the forest tailed (5/5), crested (3/4), and marsh tits (3/4)
edge. Indeed, if edges were highly permeable, we which generally were more abundant in areas with
may have expected that individuals born close to greater habitat. It is noteworthy that long-tailed
Table 15.2 Distribution of Parids in relation to landscape composition and configuration (amount of edges and patch isolation)

Species Season Location Matrix Variable % Forest Configuration Reference

Long-tailed tit Breeding England A O Hedgerows Hinsley et al. (1995) J. Avian Biol. 26 94104
Long-tailed tit Breeding Spain A/N O Brotons et al. (2004) Landscape Ecol. 19 663675
Long-tailed tit Breeding Sweden A/F/N O Isolation () Enoksson et al. (1995) Landscape Ecol. 10 267275
Long-tailed tit Breeding Sweden F O Isolation () Jansson and Angelstam (1999) Landscape Ecol. 14 283290
Long-tailed tit Breeding The Netherlands A O Isolation () Opdam et al. (1985) Biol. Conserv. 34 333352
Long-tailed tit Breeding The Netherlands A O Isolation () van Dorp and Opdam (1987) Landscape Ecol. 1 5973
Tufted titmouse Breeding Maryland A A 0 Isolation (0) Lynch and Whigham (1984) Biol. Conserv. 28 287324
New Jersey
Tufted titmouse Breeding Maryland, F/U/N O 0 Isolation (0) Robbins et al. (1989) Wildl. Monogr. 103 134
N. Carolina
Tufted titmouse Breeding Oklahoma A A Coppedge et al. (2002) Ecol. Appl. 11 4759
Tufted titmouse Breeding S. Carolina A/N A/O 0 Isolation (0/) Kilgo et al. (1997) Wildl. Soc. Bull. 25 878885
Coal tit Breeding Spain A/N O 0 Brotons et al. (2004) Landscape Ecol. 19 663675
Coal tit Breeding Sweden A A Berg (1997) Bird Stud. 44 355366
Coal tit Breeding Sweden U O Mrtberg (2001) Landscape Ecol. 16 193203

PA R I D D I S T R I B U T I O N A N D M O V E M E N T S
Blue tit Breeding England A A Hedgerows () Hinsley et al. (1995) J. Avian Biol. 26 94104
Blue tit Breeding Spain A/N O 0 Brotons et al. (2004) Landscape Ecol. 19 663675
Blue tit Breeding Sweden A A - Berg (1997) Bird Stud. 44 355366
Blue tit Breeding Sweden A/F/N O 0 Isolation (0) Enoksson et al. (1995) Landscape Ecol. 10 267275
Blue tit Breeding The Netherlands A O 0 Isolation (0) van Dorp and Opdam (1987) Landscape Ecol. 1 5973
Siberian tit Breeding Finland A/F/N A Brotons et al. (2003) Landscape Ecol. 18 377393
Crested tit Breeding Finland A/F/N A Brotons et al. (2003) Landscape Ecol. 18 377393
Crested tit Breeding Spain A/N O 0 Brotons et al. (2004) Landscape Ecol. 19 663675
Crested tit Breeding Sweden A A Berg (1997) Bird Stud. 44 355366
Crested tit Breeding Sweden U O Mrtberg (2001) Landscape Ecol. 16 193203
Great tit Breeding England A A Hedgerows () Hinsley et al. (1995) J. Avian Biol. 26 94104

253
254
L A N D S C A P E E C O L O G Y, B E H AV I O R , A N D C O N S E R VAT I O N
Table 15.2 continued

Species Season Location Matrix Variable % Forest Configuration Reference

Great tit Breeding Spain A/N O 0 Brotons et al. (2004) Landscape Ecol. 19 663675
Great tit Breeding Sweden A A 0 Berg (1997) Bird Stud. 44 355366
Great tit Breeding Sweden F A/O 0 Edenius and Elmberg (1996) Landscape Ecol. 11 325338
Great tit Breeding Sweden A/F/N O 0 Isolation (0) Enoksson et al. (1995) Landscape Ecol. 10 267275
Great tit Breeding The Netherlands A O 0 Isolation (0) van Dorp and Opdam (1987) Landscape Ecol. 1 5973
Willow tit Breeding Sweden A O Berg (1997) Bird Stud. 44 355366
Willow tit Breeding Sweden F A/O 0 Edenius and Elmberg (1996) Landscape Ecol. 11 325338
Willow tit Breeding Sweden U O Mrtberg (2001) Landscape Ecol. 16 193203
Willow tit Breeding The Netherlands A O Isolation (0) Opdam et al. (1985) Biol. Conserv. 34 333352
Willow tit Breeding The Netherlands A O 0 Isolation (0) van Dorp and Opdam (1987) Landscape Ecol. 1 5973
Marsh tit Breeding Sweden A O - Berg (1997) Bird Stud. 44 355366
Marsh tit Breeding Sweden A/F/N O Isolation () Enoksson et al. (1995) Landscape Ecol. 10 267275
Marsh tit Breeding Sweden U O Mrtberg (2001) Landscape Ecol. 16 193203
Marsh tit Breeding The Netherlands A O Isolation () Opdam et al. (1985) Biol. Conserv. 34 333352
Marsh tit Breeding The Netherlands A O Isolation () van Dorp and Opdam (1987) Landscape Ecol. 1 5973
Black-capped chickadee Breeding Alberta F A Isolation () Schmiegelow et al. (1997) Ecology 78 19141932
Black-capped chickadee Breeding Saskatchewan A A 0 (3/4 years) Hobson and Bayne (2000) Wilson Bull. 112 373387
Black-capped chickadee Breeding Maine F A/O 0 Hagan et al. (1996) Conserv. Biol. 10 188202
Black-capped chickadee Breeding New Jersey, F/U/N O 0 Isolation (0) Robbins et al. (1989) Wildl. Monogr. 103 134
Maryland,
N. Carolina
Black-capped chickadee Breeding Ontario A O 0 Austen et al. (2001) Condor 103 701714
Black-capped chickadee Breeding S. Carolina F O 0 Mitchell et al. (2001) Ecol. Appl. 11 16921708
Black-capped chickadee Breeding Wisconsin N A Hedgerows () Hawrot and Niemi (1996) Auk 113 586598
Black-capped chickadee Winter Quebec A A /0/- Hedgerows (/0/) Turcotte and Desrochers (2005) Ecography 28 129140
Black-capped chickadee Year round Wisconsin A A - Howe (1984) Ecology 65 15851601
Carolina chickadee Breeding Maryland A A 0/ Isolation (0) Lynch and Whigham (1984) Biol. Conserv. 28 287324
Carolina chickadee Breeding N. Carolina, N A 0 Patch shape, Lichstein et al. (2002) Ecol. Appl. 12 836857
Tennessee isolation
Carolina chickadee Breeding New Jersey, F/U/N O 0 Isolation (0) Robbins et al. (1989) Wildl. Monogr. 103 134
Maryland,
N. Carolina
Carolina chickadee Breeding Oklahoma A A Coppedge et al. (2002) Ecol. Appl. 11 4759
Carolina chickadee Breeding S. Carolina F O 0 Mitchell et al. (2001) Ecol. Appl. 11 16921708
Mountain chickadee Breeding Wyoming F A 0/ Hedgerows (0/-) Keller (1992) Condor 94 5565
Boreal chickadee Breeding Saskatchewan A A 0 Hobson and Bayne (2000) Wilson Bull. 112 373387
Boreal chickadee Breeding Quebec F O 0 Patch shape (0) Drolet et al. (1999) Condor 101 699704
Chestnut-backed chickadee Breeding British Columbia N/W A Isolation (-) Martin et al. (1995) Oikos 72 115131
Chestnut-backed chickadee Breeding California F A 0 Rosenberg and Raphael (1986) Wildlife 2000: modeling
habitat relationships of terrestrial vertebrates 263272
Chestnut-backed chickadee Breeding Oregon F A Hedgerows (0) McGarigal and McComb (1995) Ecol. Monogr. 65 235260

Responses can be positive (), neutral (0) or negative (-). A positive response means greater rate of occurrence (Variable O) or abundance (Variable A) in larger forest patches. Sampled
forest patches were surrounded by an agricultural (A), forestry (F), natural (N) or urban (U) matrix. See caption of Table 15.1 for details on literature search.

PA R I D D I S T R I B U T I O N A N D M O V E M E N T S
255
256 L A N D S C A P E E C O L O G Y, B E H AV I O R , A N D C O N S E R VAT I O N

and marsh tits have been shown to be generally more, these results came from consideration of
sensitive to patch size as well (see Table 15.2). The spatial landscapes with a radius less than 0.5 km.
influence of the overall isolation of habitat patches By calculating the cost of moving through each
or the amount of hedgerows on the distribution of pixel of a grid-based representation of a landscape
Parids are for now difficult to assess, especially (e.g. a classified Landsat TM satellite image), it
given the diversity of responses and the lack of becomes possible to find the least-cost path
information on the level of intercorrelation among between any two points, such as two forest patches.
the variables used to describe landscape structure. This leads to many research opportunities as
Finally, all but two of the 28 compiled studies were least-cost paths can be used to weigh alternative
solely conducted during the breeding season. pathways, quantify patch isolation and assess
Given that landscape-sensitivity was found to vary landscape functional connectivity at relatively
for black-capped chickadees between the onset and small scales (e.g. a few home ranges; Fig. 15.4).
the middle of the winter in Quebec (Turcotte and Furthermore, least-cost paths could help us better
Desrochers 2005) and that Carolina chickadee win- predict Parid distribution along forest edges and in
ter survival increased with woodlot area in Ohio highly fragmented landscapes, as they could be
(Doherty and Grubb 2002b), there is certainly a used to predict how an individual should respond
need to put more effort into characterizing the to predation risk as a function of its distance to
effect of landscape structure on the distribution of all forest edges. In addition, such paths could be
Parids at this crucial time of the year. used to assess the movement cost of territories
There are obvious limits to inferences on land- composed of several small forest patches (i.e. the
scape responses based on species distributions cost of landscape supplementation sensu Dunning
alone. With increasing amounts of data on move- et al. 1992).
ment decisions as well as simulation tools, it has While our previous experiments on responses to
become possible to ask how birds such as Parids forest edges provide useful parameters for simula-
tend to respond to real-life, spatially explicit chal- tions, they still need to be confronted to empirical
lenges. Recently, we were able to induce move- data on large-scale movements. One way to study
ments by black-capped chickadees, under movement at spatial scales relevant to dispersal is
controlled conditions, and parameterize their to induce them by relocation experiments, thus
movement decisions near gaps in the canopy forcing birds to home by flying through selected
(Blisle and Desrochers 2002). We have evidence landscapes. Homing experiments have been
suggesting that there is a threshold distance of ca.
25 m into open areas beyond which black-capped
chickadees seldom fly when a detour under forest 90
cover is available. When no such detour is avail- 80
able, we found that the likelihood that black- 70
capped chickadees move between two woodlots 60
Relative cost

decreases at an increasing rate with the size of gap 50


which must be crossed (Desrochers and Hannon 40
1997; St. Clair et al. 1998). 30
Simulating the experiments above using a spa-
20
tially explicit, individual-based approach and the
10
SELES language (Fall and Fall 2001), M. Blisle and
0
A. Fall were able to parameterize a cost function for 0 100 200 300 400 500
back-capped chickadees moving within agricul- Distance to all forest edges (m)
tural landscapes (unpublished data). This simula-
Figure 15.3 Cost function for black-capped chickadees moving in
tion work suggests that the relative cost of moving the open within agricultural landscapes. The cost of moving one
1 m in open habitat increases exponentially with distance unit under forest cover was set to one (M. Blisle and
the distance to all forest edges (Fig. 15.3); what is A. Fall unpublished data).
PA R I D D I S T R I B U T I O N A N D M O V E M E N T S 257

180 uals in landscapes with more than 50% forest cover.


160 The most parsimonious explanation for this
Mean least cost (x103)

140 contrast is that habitat loss and fragmentation


120 impaired the movement of black-capped chicka-
100 dees. If so, low forest cover would affect the ability
80 of these birds to find and select resources of high
60 quality, including territories (Blisle et al. 2001).
40 Given that other forest bird species differ in their
20 response to landscape structure when subjected to
0 translocation experiments (Blisle and St Clair 2001;
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 Gobeil and Villard 2002), the latter should be
Forest cover (%) replicated with other species of Parids in order to
Figure 15.4 Influence of landscape composition on the assess the generality of our conclusions.
functional connectivity of agricultural landscapes for black-capped Some individuals may suffer more than others
chickadees as assessed by the cost function shown in Fig. 15.3. from habitat fragmentation. For example floaters
With the help of A. Fall and using the SELES language, we created have been recorded in wintering populations of
810 neutral landscapes composed of 100 100 square pixels
chickadees (Smith 1984) and tits (Ekman et al. 1981),
covering 625 m2. Landscapes were composed of forest and open
habitat only. Ninety landscapes were allocated to each of nine and shown to have daily fattening patterns sug-
forest cover categories with a contagion index of 0.95. We then gesting that they face lower food predictability than
placed at random 100 cyber-chickadees on the south side and asked residents (Broggi and Brotons 2001; see Chapters 16
them to choose at random a destination on the north side. and 17 for more on fat deposition versus habitat
Cyber-chickadees were finally asked to find the least cost path
quality). By definition, floaters will have much
between their start and end points. As expected, the cost increased
as forest cover decreased. Yet, it is only when forest cover reached larger home ranges than other individuals, and will
30% that some landscapes showed a substantial increase in therefore need to cope with habitat barriers more
cost. Interestingly, it is not only the mean cost of paths within a frequently. Like Rodrguez et al. (2001), we expect
given landscape that increased as forest cover decreased, but also that forest fragmentation will hurt floaters
the absolute variation in mean cost among landscapes. Indeed, some
more than flock residents (Smith 1984) and there-
landscapes with only 5% or 20% forest cover presented the same
mean level of functional connectivity as landscapes with 30% or fore may significantly alter social relationships.
70% forest cover, respectively. Furthermore, fragmentation may affect mating
strategies among flock members, for example by
conducted mostly for the study of navigation (Able imposing greater movement costs that counter the
1995), and they have also historically been done potential benefits of seeking copulations with
with wintering Parids (Odum 1941 and references conspecific neighbors (K. Otter, personal commu-
therein). The Parid homing work by Odum and nication). Finally, Pravosudova et al. (1999) provide
others showed that black-capped chickadees were an excellent example of how forest fragmentation
not site-tenacious during winter; however, they did may impact the genetic composition of tufted
not offer insights on the responses of these birds to titmouse flocks either through lower nesting suc-
gaps in the canopy. More recently, we investigated cess or parentoffspring relations after fledging.
homing by territorial, mated male black-capped The consequences of landscape changes such as
chickadees, specifically with the purpose of testing fragmentation on animal social organization
for a barrier effect caused by forest fragmentation remain virtually unknown and Parids offer excel-
within agricultural landscapes (Blisle et al. 2001). lent opportunities for advances in this field.
Overall, we found that homing time (over 14 km)
increased with decreasing forest cover. The greatest
15.5 Parids as leaders of other
discrepancy in homing rate was observed after
forest birds
30 h, when only 33% of the 18 individuals translo-
cated in landscapes with less than 50% forest cover Field naturalists have often observed that non-
had returned compared to 87% of the 23 individ- breeding Parids do not generally travel alone.
258 L A N D S C A P E E C O L O G Y, B E H AV I O R , A N D C O N S E R VAT I O N

Instead, they are often accompanied by other forest explain why Parids will often travel along forest
birds, resident or migrant. Thus, Parid movements edges, around open areas, especially outside the
may have implications for the movements of other breeding season, when flock movements usually
species, especially in the postfledging (dispersal) cover much larger areas than breeding territories.
and migration periods. Ornithologists have often We thought that edge avoidance would be a candi-
wondered whether Parids lead other species in date mechanism for the patch-area sensitivity
their movements, possibly because other species found in about half of Parid populations studied,
should benefit from their knowledge of local but it appeared irrelevant in the large majority of
dangers and opportunities. In Louisiana, cases. We propose the spatial arrangement of
Maryland, and Maine, US, Morse (1970) had patches as an alternative explanation for patch-area
evidence showing that black-capped chickadees, sensitivity by Parids. In support of this, we showed
Carolina chickadees, and tufted titmice generally that larger-scale (landscape) studies yielded twice
led other species in mixed flocks. In as many cases of area-sensitivity (within a fixed
Pennsylvania, US, Rodewald and Brittingham radius) by Parids in agricultural (highly frag-
(2002) also showed that black-capped chickadees mented) than in forestry (connected) landscapes.
and tufted titmice led mixed-species flocks most of Also, our simulation work clearly shows that
the time (68%). Additionally, Dolby and Grubb landscape pattern (with a fixed amount of habitat)
(2000) showed that white-breasted nuthatches (Sitta leads to substantial variation in functional
carolinensis) were less reluctant to forage outside connectivity, especially at low amounts of habitat
forest cover in the presence of tufted titmice, even cover, a situation typical of most distribution
after controlling for flock-size effects. Analogously, studies on forest birds.
Sieving et al. (2004) showed that the presence of We found no evidence of contrast between
tufted titmice facilitated the movement of other Eurasian and North American Parids, as far
birds, mostly migrants, across gaps in the forest as landscape distribution and movement are
canopy. However, J. Bourque and C. Aznar (unpub- concerned. In fact, intraspecific variation in the
lished data), in a series of playback experiments movement ecology of Parids appears as important
using chickadee mobbing calls, found that the as interspecific variation. The only patterns valid
presence of black-capped chickadees did not influ- among Parids were either trivial, that is lack of
ence gap-crossing behavior of other species. It is specific associations to small habitat patches, or of
difficult to tell at this time whether this pattern of little-understood significance, that is the role
leaderfollower is general with mixed-species played by Parids as leaders for the movements of
flocks outside of North America, because of the other species outside the breeding season.
small number of published studies of this phenom- Although differences in methodology among
enon to date. In Europe, it is also possible that studies may explain part of the variability among
leaderfollower patterns occur among different published results, it is likely that regional effects,
species of Parids, because non-breeding flocks such as climate, predator pressure, and landscape
generally comprise several species of Parids type, play a major role in shaping movement con-
(e.g. Mnkknen et al. 1996). straints and opportunities for Parids. Furthermore,
our own work and the literature we reviewed
here do not generally address differences in habitat
15.6 Conclusions
quality, which may prove to be key in understand-
Neither Eurasian nor North American Parids exhibit ing Parid movements (see Chapter 16). In short, the
consistent patterns in distribution and movement movement ecology of Parids is highly idiosyncratic.
in relation to edges, patches, and landscapes. Near Where do we go from here? As with the study of
habitat edges, movements of Parids often result larger problems of avian landscape ecology, the
from food distribution and microclimate, and an time is ripe for behavioral experiments and meta-
avoidance of open areas and edges mediated by analyses in search for more general patterns, taking
predator avoidance. Avoiding open areas may advantage of differences among populations
PA R I D D I S T R I B U T I O N A N D M O V E M E N T S 259

(Desrochers et al. 1999, Blisle 2005). Parids are Brotons, L. and Herrando, S. (2003). Effect of increased
probably the best-suited avian family to make food abundance near forest edges on flocking patterns
progress in this field, given their wide distribution of coal tit Parus ater winter groups in mountain conifer-
and adaptability to experimental setups, as well as ous forests. Bird Study, 50, 106111.
Brotons, L., Desrochers, A., and Turcotte, Y. (2001). Food
the abundance of baseline data. Since there is as
hoarding behaviour of black-capped chickadees (Poecile
much variability in landscape distribution within
atricapillus) in relation to forest edges. Oikos, 95,
as among Parid species, one focus should be on 511519.
intraspecific variation for widespread, and well- Cramp, S., Simmons, K. E. L., Perrins, C. M., and Brooks,
studied species, such as black-capped chickadees D. J., eds (19781997). Handbook of the Birds of the Western
and blue tits. Additionally, dozens of essentially Palearctic. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK.
unstudied species in mainland Asia and in Africa del Hoyo, J., Elliott, A., Sargatal, J., and Christie, D., eds
would provide an excellent opportunity to test (1992-present). Handbook of the Birds of the World. Lynx
some of the ideas presented in this chapter and Edicions, Barcelona, Spain.
generalize to new systems. Further, Parids offer the Desrochers, A. and Fortin, M.-J. (2000). Understanding
opportunity of studying how birds respond to avian responses to forest boundaries: a case study with
chickadee winter flocks. Oikos, 91, 376384.
given landscape structures throughout their annual
Desrochers, A. and Hannon, S. J. (1997). Gap crossing
cycle, and, hence, how landscape structure affects
decisions by forest songbirds during the post-fledging
trade-offs among conflicting demands. Published period. Conservation Biology, 11, 12041210.
work rarely provides details on landscape Desrochers, A., Blisle, M., and Bourque, J. (2002). Do
structure, and those details may be difficult, or mobbing calls affect the perception of predation risk by
impossible, to obtain. We may thus need to obtain forest birds? Animal Behaviour, 64, 709714.
new distribution data in a variety of regional Desrochers, A., Hannon, S. J., Blisle, M., and St Clair,
landscapes in order to progress in the study of C. C. (1999). Movement of songbirds in fragmented
Parids as models for the study of avian movement forests: Can we scale up from behaviour to explain
ecology. occupancy patterns in the landscape? In: N. J. Adams
and R. H. Slotow, ed. Proceedings of the 22nd International
Ornithological Congress, Durban, pp. 24472464. BirdLife
References South Africa, Johannesburg.
Able, K. P. (1995). Orientation and navigation: a perspec- Doherty, P. F., Jr and Grubb, T. C., Jr (2002a). Nest usurp-
tive on fifty years of research. Condor, 97, 592604. ation is an edge effect for Carolina chickadees Poecile
Ambuel, B. A. and Temple, S. A. (1983). Area-dependent carolinensis. Journal of Avian Biology, 33, 7782.
changes in the bird communities and vegetation of Doherty, P. F., Jr and Grubb, T. C., Jr (2002b). Survivorship
southern Wisconsin forests. Ecology, 64, 10571068. of permanent-resident birds in a fragmented forested
Anderson, S. H., Mann, K., and Shugart, H. H., Jr (1977). landscape. Ecology, 83, 844857.
The effect of transmission-line corridors on bird popu- Dolby, A. S. and Grubb, T. C. (1999). Effects of winter
lations. American Midland Naturalist, 79, 216221. weather on horizontal vertical use of isolated forest
Blisle, M. and Desrochers, A. (2002). Gap-crossing deci- fragments by bark-foraging birds. Condor, 101, 408412.
sions by forest birds: an empirical basis for parameter- Dolby, A. S. and Grubb, T. C. (2000). Social context affects
izing spatially-explicit, individual-based models. risk taking by a satellite species in a mixed-species for-
Landscape Ecology, 17, 219231. aging group. Behavioral Ecology, 11, 110114.
Blisle, M. and St Clair, C. C. (2001). Cumulative effects of Dunning, J. B., Danielson, B. J., and Pulliam, H. R. (1992).
barriers on the movements of forest birds. Conservation Ecological processes that affect populations in complex
Ecology, 5, 9. [online] URL: http://www.consecol.org/ landscapes. Oikos, 65, 169175.
vo15/iss2/art9. Ekman, J., Cederholm, G., and Askenmo, C. (1981).
Blisle, M., Desrochers, A., and Fortin, M.-J. (2001). Spacing and survival in winter groups of willow tit
Influence of forest cover on the movements of forest Parus montanus and crested tit P. cristatusa removal
birds: a homing experiment. Ecology, 82, 18931904. study. Journal of Animal Ecology, 50, 19.
Broggi, J. and Brotons, L. (2001). Coal tit fat-storing Fall, A. and Fall, J. (2001). A domain-specific language for
patterns during the non-breeding season: the role of models of landscape dynamics. Ecological Modelling,
residence status. Journal of Avian Biology, 32, 333337. 137, 121.
260 L A N D S C A P E E C O L O G Y, B E H AV I O R , A N D C O N S E R VAT I O N

Ficken, M. S., McLaren, M. A., and Hailman, J. P. (1996). Morse, D. H. (1970). Ecological aspects of some mixed-
Boreal chickadee. In: A. Poole and F. Gill, eds. The Birds species foraging flocks of birds. Ecological Monographs,
of North America, No. 254, pp. The Academy of Natural 40, 119168.
Sciences, The American Ornithologists Union, Odum, E. P. (1941). Winter homing behavior of the
Philadelphia, PA, Washington, D.C. chickadee. Bird-Banding, 12, 113119.
Galli, A. E., Leck, C. F., and Forman, R. T. T. (1976). Avian Pearson, S. M. (1993). The spatial extent and relative
distribution patterns in forest islands of different sizes influence of landscape-level factors on wintering bird
in central New Jersey. Auk, 93, 356364. populations. Landscape Ecology, 8, 318.
Gobeil, J.-F. and Villard, M.-A. (2002). Permeability of Poole, A., Stettenheim, P., and Gill, F., eds (19922003). The
three boreal forest landscape types to bird movements Birds of North America. The Academy of Natural
as determined from experimental translocations. Oikos, Sciences, The American Ornithologists Union,
98, 447458. Philadelphia, PA, Washington, D.C.
Greenwood, P. J., Harvey, P. H., and Perrins, C. M. (1979). Postma, E. and van Noordwijk, A. J. (2005). Gene flow
The role of dispersal in the great tit (Parus major): the maintains a large genetic difference in clutch size at a
causes, consequences and heritability of natal dispersal. small spatial scale. Nature, 433, 6568.
Journal of Animal Ecology, 48, 123142. Pravosudova, E. V., Grubb, T. C., Jr, Parker, P. G., and
Grubb, T. C., Jr (1977). Weather dependent foraging Doherty, P. F., Jr (1999). Patch size and composition of
behavior of some birds wintering in a deciduous wood- social groups in wintering tufted titmice (Baeolophus
land: horizontal adjustments. Condor, 79, 271274. bicolor). Auk, 116, 11521155.
Grubb, T. C., Jr and Bronson, C. L. (2001). On cognitive Rodewald, P. G. and Brittingham, M. C. (2002). Habitat
conservation biology: why chickadees leave a patch of use and behavior of mixed species landbird flocks
woodland. Journal of Avian Biology, 32, 372376. during fall migration. Wilson Bulletin, 114, 8798.
Grubb, T. C., Jr and Doherty, P. F., Jr (1999). On home- Rodrguez, A., Andrn, H., and Jansson, G. (2001).
range gap-crossing. Auk, 116, 618628. Habitat-mediated predation risk and decision making
Hansson, J. (1983). Bird numbers across edges between of small birds at forest edges. Oikos, 95, 383396.
mature conifer forests and clearcuts in central Sweden. Sieving, K. E., Contreras, T. A., and Maute, K. L. (2004).
Ornis Scandinavica, 14, 97103. Heterospecific facilitation of forest boundary crossing
Harris, R. and Reed, J. M. (2002). Behavioral barriers to by mobbing understory birds in North-central Florida.
non-migratory movements of birds. Annales Zoologici Auk, 121, 738751.
Fennici, 39, 275290. Siffczyk, C., Brotons, L., Kangas, K., and Orell, M. (2003).
Hinsley, S. A. (2000). The costs of multiple patch use by Home range size of willow tits: a response to winter
birds. Landscape Ecology, 15, 765775. habitat loss. Oecologia, 136, 635642.
Imbeau, L., Drapeau, P., and Mnkknen, M. (2003). Are Smith, S. M. (1984). Flock switching in chickadees: why be
forest birds categorised as edge species strictly asso- a winter floater? American Naturalist, 123, 8198.
ciated with edges? Ecography, 26, 514520. St Clair, C. C., Blisle, M., Desrochers, A., and Hannon,
Lens, L. and Dhondt, A. A. (1994). Effects of habitat frag- S. J. (1998). Winter responses of forest birds to habitat
mentation on the timing of crested tit Parus cristatus corridors and gaps. Conservation Ecology, 2, 13. [online]
natal dispersal. Ibis, 136, 147152. URL: http://www.consecol.org/vo12/iss2/art13.
Leopold, A. (1933). Game Management. Charles Scribners Tellera, J. L., Virgs, E., Carbonell, R., Prez-Tris, J., and
Sons, New York, NY, USA. Santos, T. (2001). Behavioural responses to changing
MacArthur, R. and Wilson, E. O. (1967). The Theory of landscapes: flock structure and anti-predator strategies
Island Biogeography. Princeton University Press, of tits wintering in fragmented forests. Oikos, 95, 253264.
Princeton, NJ, USA. Todd, I. A. and Cowie, R. J. (1990). Measuring the risk of
Matthysen, E. (2002). Boundary effects on dispersal predation in an energy currency: field experiments with
between habitat patches by forest birds (Parus major, foraging blue tits, Parus caeruleus. Animal Behaviour, 40,
P. caeruleus). Landscape Ecology, 17, 509515. 112117.
McCollin, D. (1998). Forest edges and habitat selection in Turcotte, Y. and Desrochers, A. (2003). Landscape-
birds: a functional approach. Ecography, 21, 247260. dependent response to predation risk by forest birds.
Mnkknen, M., Forsman, J. T., and Helle, P. (1996). Oikos, 100, 614618.
Mixed-species foraging aggregations and heterospecific Turcotte, Y. and Desrochers, A. (2005). Landscape-
attraction in boreal bird communities. Oikos, 77, dependent distribution of northern forest birds in
127136. winter. Ecography, 28, 129140.
PA R I D D I S T R I B U T I O N A N D M O V E M E N T S 261

Verhulst, S., Perrins, C. M., and Riddington, R. (1997). Whitcomb, R. F., Robbins, C. S., Lynch, J. F., Whitcomb,
Natal dispersal of great tits in a patchy environment. B. L., Klimkiewicz, M. K., and Bystrak, D. (1981). Effects
Ecology, 78, 864872. of forest fragmentation on avifauna of the Eastern
Villard, M.-A. (1998). On forest-interior species, edge deciduous forest. In: R. L. Burgess and D. M. Sharpe,
avoidance, area-sensitivity, and dogmas in avian con- eds. Forest Island Dynamics in Man-dominated Landscapes,
servation. Auk, 115, 801805. pp. 125205. Springer-Verlag, New York, NY, USA.
Weise, C. M. and Meyer, J. R. (1979). Juvenile dispersal Yahner, R. H. (1987). Use of even-aged stands by winter
and development of site-fidelity in the black-capped and spring bird communities. Wilson Bulletin, 99,
Chickadee. Auk, 96, 4055. 218232.
This page intentionally left blank
CHAPTER 16

Winter adaptations in chickadees


and titmice and the added effect
of habitat fragmentation
Jennifer R. Olson and Thomas C. Grubb, Jr

can be lost. Most mature adults are in the 10 to 20 g


16.1 Introduction
range and are therefore challenged by a high
Many North American Paridae have ranges that surface-to-volume ratio. Such birds have an inter-
expose them to temperate winter seasons. nal body temperature of approximately 40C, so
Chickadees and titmice possess several physiologi- exposure to extreme winter environments (50C)
cal and behavioral adaptations that allow them to can result in a 90C temperature gradient in less
manage the reduced food supply and high ther- than 2 cm between ambient conditions and body
moregulatory demands of this environment. Yet, core (Sharbaugh 2001). In order to survive in condi-
increased habitat fragmentation posed by agricul- tions of high thermoregulatory demand, chicka-
tural and urban expansion also presents a further dees and titmice have developed several
stress to these populations. Furthermore, the inter- physiological adaptations, including the use of noc-
action between responses to winter and responses turnal hypothermia and shivering thermogenesis.
to fragmentation is assumed to be a major factor
constraining populations that are presented with 16.2.1 Nocturnal hypothermia and
both of these tasks. With this chapter, we review seasonal metabolic adjustments
research on physiological adaptations to reduced
temperatures, and then address how microclimate Maintaining an appropriate body temperature
change induced by habitat fragmentation can affect is metabolically demanding and birds can partially
the response potential of over-wintering birds. overcome this challenge through the use of
nocturnal hypothermia. Nocturnal hypothermia
has been observed in several families of Order
16.2 Physiological adaptations to
Passeriformes, including Parids (Reinertsen 1996).
winter conditions
This moderate reduction in body temperature is not
Over-wintering in north-temperate regions can be as extreme as the torpor observed in hummingbirds
energetically challenging for small passerines that (Wolf and Hainsworth 1972) and poor-wills (Jaeger
must endure cold temperatures, reduced food 1949). Several species of European (Grossman and
supply, and a shorter foraging time. For example West 1977) and North American Parids exhibit
during harsh winters at the northern edge of their moderate nocturnal hypothermia in response to
range, black-capped chickadees are exposed to low ambient temperatures (Chaplin 1976), and the
normal low temperatures of 30C and as few as exact nocturnal energy savings to a hypothermic
5 hours of daylight (Sharbaugh 2001). bird depend on several factors, including ambient
Relative to their body size, chickadees and temperature, body temperature, body mass, and
titmice possess a large surface area over which heat the duration of hypothermia.

263
264 L A N D S C A P E E C O L O G Y, B E H AV I O R , A N D C O N S E R VAT I O N

In order to survive overnight in winter, birds which resulted in a 32 to 45% lower oxygen
must achieve a thermal balance where the meta- consumption than predicted for homeotherms of a
bolic heat production equals the heat lost. In low similar size (Chaplin 1976). Nocturnal body
ambient temperatures, such a balance is accom- temperatures of European great tits (Steen 1958)
plished by either increasing consumption of energy and willow tits (Reinertsen and Haftorn 1983)
stores or decreasing the core body temperature. showed similar patterns to New York chickadees
Reinertsen and Haftorn (1983) suggested that a and were 6 to 8C lower than their diurnal temp-
change in posture or erection of the feathers is an eratures. In addition, the stable hypothermic body
initial response to colder temperatures, followed by temperature of willow tits was strongly correlated
a reduction in body temperature until a thermal with ambient temperature.
balance is achieved. It is at this body temperature North American and Eurasian Parids apparently
that the bird will remain throughout the night. differ in their arousal from a hypothermic state.
Entering a hypothermic state can result in Chaplin (1976) found that black-capped chickadees
substantial energy savings throughout the night, captured in New York and kept at over-night ambi-
perhaps allowing for survival that otherwise would ent temperature of 0 to 5C were still hypothermic
not be possible. For example lowering the body in the early morning, but could regain normal body
temperature from 39C to 33C in an ambient temperatures with 1 to 2 minutes of intermittent
temperature of 30C will result in a 10% reduction flying. This does not conform to the data from
in oxygen consumption. For an 11.5 g bird, this European species that returned to normal body
yields an over-night energy savings of approxi- temperatures in the morning even if kept in dark
mately 925 calories (Reinertsen and Haftorn 1983, chambers (Chaplin 1976).
1986). Another study showed that reducing noctur- Wind intensity can also affect a birds use of
nal body temperature by only 8C will provide a hypothermia. Mayer et al. (1982) found that
chickadee with 72 additional minutes of fasting Carolina chickadees would reduce their body
endurance, an energy savings that could mean the temperatures to 35C at 5C ambient temperatures
difference between over-night survival and death in the absence of wind. Body temperatures were
(Sharbaugh 2001). reduced further (to 30C) when birds were exposed
The proximate factor that induces a hypothermic to 10C temperatures and a wind speed of
state is unknown, although the degree of hypother- 10 km/h, and this decrease in body temperature
mia is correlated with body mass and fat reserves. corresponded to a 33% energy savings when
Reinertsen (1996) suggests that hormones or compared to the metabolic requirements for a bird
plasma metabolites contribute to a sequence of maintaining a 40C body temperature under those
events transforming information on energy same conditions.
reserves into the appropriate thermal response. Seasonal variation in hypothermic states is also
There is some lack of consistency in the results of present in North American chickadee populations.
studies on Parid hypothermia, which might be For black-capped chickadees collected in South
attributed to several factors, including different Dakota (42N), a larger percentage of winter than
techniques of body temperature measurement and summer birds became hypothermic in cold stress
the period of time for which the birds were held tests (Cooper and Swanson 1994). Similar trends
captive prior to testing. Grossman and West (1977) were also evident in willow tits from Norway
observed an average decrease in body temperature (63N); birds acclimatized to winter conditions
of only 3C (to 38.5C) for winter-acclimatized showed a much larger decrease in body tempera-
black-capped chickadees in Fairbanks, Alaska ture than summer-acclimatized birds (Reinertsen
(65N) when the birds were measured at ambient and Haftorn 1983) (Fig. 16.1). In contrast, measure-
temperatures of 27C to 50C. However, birds ments from mountain chickadees and juniper
of the same species in New York have shown body titmice captured in Utah (4152N) showed that
temperatures approximately 10C below normal both species achieved considerable nocturnal
when exposed to ambient temperatures of 0C, hypothermia throughout the year (Cooper and
W I N T E R A D A P TAT I O N S A N D H A B I TAT F R A G M E N TAT I O N 265

40

38 (>12L)
Body temperature (C)

36
(10L, 14D)

34

(6L, 18D)

32

30 20 10 0 10 20
Ambient temperature (C)

Figure 16.1 Relationship of body temperature to ambient temperate for willow tits during different seasons with different light:dark (L:D)
cycles: summer (crosses), late autumn or early spring (open circles), and midwinter (filled circles). Each point represents the minimum value
recorded on a single night. Curves were fitted by hand. (From: Reinertsen and Haftorn 1983.)

Gessaman 2005). Depth of hypothermia also did physiology occurred despite the fact that there was
not vary seasonally for either species (Fig. 16.2), no seasonal variation in body mass or visible fat
and the authors suggest that the regions high (Cooper 2002). In contrast, the standard metabolic
climatic variability may be the source of this indif- rates of Alaskan black-capped chickadees did not
ference. Both species reduced their body tempera- differ between seasons (Sharbaugh 2001).
tures by 3 to 11C, which resulted in nocturnal
energy savings of up to 28% in titmice and up to
16.2.2 Thermogenesis by shivering
50% in chickadees. Similar results were seen in
Alaskan black-capped chickadees which also In addition to lowering nocturnal body tempera-
utilize nocturnal hypothermia in both winter and tures, chickadees also respond to cold ambient
summer months (Sharbaugh 2001). This could be temperatures with shivering thermogenesis, facili-
due, in part, to the relatively cold and short tated primarily by the pectoral muscles. Shivering as
summers experienced at this latitudeconditions a means of heat production has been documented in
favoring individuals that maintain their cold several avian species, including pigeons (Columba
acclimatization year-round. livia, Hohtola 1982), house finches (Carpodacus mexi-
Several studies also provide evidence that win- canus, Marsh et al. 1984), and Japanese quail (Coturnix
ter-acclimatized birds are more cold tolerant than japonica, Hohtola and Stevens 1986), and the inverse
summer birds. Standard metabolic rates of winter relationship between shivering magnitude and ambi-
chickadees were 18% greater than those of summer ent temperatures is well established.
birds, and winter birds also showed a significant Chaplin (1976) observed an increase in both
increase (36%) in maximal thermogenic capacity, frequency of shivering bursts and mean duration of
another likely adaptation to winter conditions bursts at lower ambient temperatures in winter-
(Cooper and Swanson 1994). These differences in acclimatized black-capped chickadees. When these
266 L A N D S C A P E E C O L O G Y, B E H AV I O R , A N D C O N S E R VAT I O N

44 20 Y = 0.013T2a 0.863Ta+15.142
42
Body temperature (C)

Amplitude (mV/sec)
40 15
38

36 10
34

32 Mountain chickadees 5
30 Summer
Winter
28 0
14:00 18:00 22:00 02:00 06:00 10:00 14:00 0 10 20 30

44 Ta(C)

42 Figure 16.3 Shivering amplitude (mV/sec) of six black-capped


Body temperature (C)

chickadees exposed for at least 2 hours to ambient temperatures of


40
2C, 10C, 15C, 20C, and 27C. One data point represents the
38 mean amplitude of 10 shivering bursts. (From: Chaplin 1976.)

36
34
satisfy the increased need for shivering thermogen-
32 Juniper titmice esis experienced by wintering birds.
30 Summer
Winter
28 16.3 Behavioral modifications and
14:00 18:00 22:00 02:00 06:00 10:00 14:00 ecological adaptations to winter
Time of day (hours) conditions
Figure 16.2 Daily body temperatures of summer- and winter- Permanent-resident woodland birds, such as
acclimatized mountain chickadees and juniper titmice. Each point chickadees and titmice, possess several behavioral
represents one individual. Lines represent a quadratic fit of the data.
modifications that may reduce energetic costs under
(From: Cooper and Gessaman 2005.)
conditions of high thermoregulatory demand. This
can be accomplished through adjustments in their
foraging and roosting locations in order to minimize
measurements were combined, there was a seven- exposure to harmful winds and temperatures.
fold increase in total shivering time as temperature However, as members of heterospecific flocks, these
decreased from 27C to 2C. Electromyographic birds must do so while minimizing interspecific
measurements of the pectoralis muscle also showed competition for food and roosting sites.
an increase in shivering amplitude with decreasing
temperature, corresponding to a dramatic increase
16.3.1 Over-wintering in heterospecific flocks
in heat production (Fig. 16.3). The fact that during
their normal daily activities chickadees spend so Most North American Parids over-winter as mem-
little of their time in continuous flight supports the bers of mixed-species flocks. Within these flocks,
hypothesis that the pectoral muscles have a vital chickadees and titmice serve functionally as
role as organs of heat production. Cooper (2002) nuclear species, facilitating the flocks formation
found that pectoral muscle mass of wintering and movement. Additional, satellite species, such
mountain chickadees and juniper titmice was 33% as woodpeckers and nuthatches, maintain a
and 18% greater, respectively, than during the more passive role as flock followers (Morse 1970;
summer. This seasonal variation could help to Austin and Smith 1972; Buskirk 1976; Hutto 1994;
W I N T E R A D A P TAT I O N S A N D H A B I TAT F R A G M E N TAT I O N 267

Dolby and Grubb 1999b; Chapter 15). Removing foraged at greater heights (Jablonski and Lee 2002).
chickadees and titmice from small woodlots A similar change in foraging location was also
resulted in satellite species occurring together less observed in socially subordinate coal tits when
often, further supporting the nuclear species role dominant crested tits and willow tits were removed
in flock cohesion (Dolby and Grubb 1999b). from Swedish forests (Alatalo et al. 1985). Coal tits
Although this multispecies social arrangement shifted their foraging sites in trees to include the
can provide individuals with both antipredator inner regions previously occupied by the more
defense and increased foraging efficiency (Morse dominant species, suggesting the presence of either
1970; Berner and Grubb 1985), the resulting inter- interference or exploitation competition. In a subse-
specific competition can also have a negative effect. quent study (Alatalo et al. 1987), the smaller species
For example Carolina chickadees and tufted titmice were removed, causing the dominant species to
forage together during the non-breeding season, expand their foraging regions, suggesting that in
with the titmice the more socially dominant forest tits, competition for limited resources can
species. When titmice were removed from a wood- occur even without interference from dominants.
lot, the more subordinate chickadees responded by In Cimprich and Grubbs experiment, the
increasing feather growth rate (an index of an removal of titmice from a woodlot may have also
energetically demanding process; Box 16.1) and reduced competition for roosting sites, providing
expanding their foraging niche (Cimprich and the remaining chickadees with more protection
Grubb 1994). This suggests the presence of inter- from harsh winter conditions. Although competi-
specific competition between the species for local tion for roosting sites is common among great tits
food resources. This response has also been and blue tits (Dhondt et al. 1991), the refusal of
observed in palearctic Parids. When varied tits chickadees to use artificial roost boxes in winter
were removed from flocks containing the smaller suggests that roosting sites in this system may not
great tits, the great tits expanded their niche and be a limiting resource.

Box 16.1 Ptilochronology: feather time and avian biology

Ptilochronology is a term describing a relatively The induced feather is retained until the next
new technique for indexing the nutritional molt. In most temperate- and boreal-zone birds,
condition of birds (Grubb 1989, 2006). If you tail feathers are replaced annually during the
pluck a feather and release the bird, then recatch so-called prebasic molt of late summer and
the same individual more than a month later and autumn. Thus, a bird plucked in late autumn will
pluck the newly grown replacement feather, the grow the induced feather and will carry this record
replacement (or induced) feather can provide a of nutritional status to be collected any time during
day-by-day record of the nutritional regime of the the next 6 to 8 months.
bird during the previous weeks. Individual growth The general technique for obtaining
bars (one dark band plus one light band) can be ptilochronology feather measurements starts with
counted to establish the number of days taken to capturing a bird and plucking a feather, typically
grow the feather. Matching induced feathers from the outermost right tail feather, or rectrix.
two birds from whom feathers had been pulled at Removing a feather causes no visible trauma to a
the same time, and then released in the same bird, likely because a fully grown feather is held in
habitat, could be used to determine the place in the follicle solely by non-living connective
relationships of species, sex, age, social dominance tissue. After being so plucked, the bird is released,
status, kinship, and other factors to nutritional recaptured after the 5 to 6-week growth period of
condition. The wider the growth bar, the better the the induced feather, and the induced feather
nutritional condition. pulled.
268 L A N D S C A P E E C O L O G Y, B E H AV I O R , A N D C O N S E R VAT I O N

For example temperatures can be 25C warmer at


16.3.2 Foraging behavior and food caching
1000 m than in lowland areas (Hotly 1973).
Inclement winter weather can have a deleterious Many Parids collect and store food for consump-
effect on the foraging of chickadees and titmice by tion at a later time. Although time from caching to
reducing the variety of locations a bird can exploit food retrieval varies (see Chapter 2), birds do
while keeping thermal stress at a minimum. In a accumulate caches during late autumn in prepar-
study examining the effect of several climatic ation for the colder months when nutritional
factors on the foraging rates of black-capped resources are more difficult to find and foraging
chickadees and tufted titmice in New Jersey, Grubb time is limited due to shorter, colder days. Caching
(1975) found that temperature and wind speed of food resources has many benefits to birds that
significantly affected the choice of substrate. In cold over-winter in harsh climates. The presence of a
and windy weather, birds adjusted their foraging predictable food source decreases a wintering birds
habits to minimize exposure to these elements. As risk of starvation while allowing it to maintain a
wind speed increased, both chickadees and titmice lower body mass, thereby reducing the risk of
reduced their foraging height and spent more time predation. In addition, caching birds are able to
foraging in shrub-type vegetation, both of which spend less time foraging, minimizing their exposure
would offer more protection from the wind. to low temperatures and high winds (Pravosudov
In addition to altering their foraging location, and Grubb 1998). Further discussion of food caching
black-capped chickadees and tufted titmice reduced in Parids can be found in Chapters 2 and 3.
foraging rates as ambient temperature decreased
and wind speed increased (Grubb 1978). For
16.3.3 Cavity roosting
example, in conditions of high wind (3.14.0 m/s),
tufted titmice spent 11.5% more time stationary and Chickadees and titmice utilize both natural and arti-
traveled only 15.3% as rapidly as in low winds ficial cavities as nocturnal roost sites (Bent 1947).
(0.11.0 m/s). An increase in temperature from The microclimate of a roosting cavity can minimize
10C to 20C quadrupled the foraging speed of thermoregulatory stress by providing shelter from
chickadees. The difference in foraging rate can be wind and precipitation, and by reducing radiative
partially explained by a change in foraging heat loss. North American and Eurasian Parids
substrate, and it may not be the weather conditions roost in a variety of locations, but most roosting
themselves that dictate the speed at which an indi- sites can be found in holes and recesses of dead or
vidual feeds. In higher winds, both chickadees and rotting trees (Bent 1947; Pitts 1976; Perrins 1979).
titmice spent more time foraging on the ground Most roosts observed by Pitts (1976) had only a sin-
where their movements were dominated by small, gle side opening, but others had multiple openings
short hops. The change in location was then more and did not offer any better protection than dense
directly responsible for modifying the foraging tech- vegetation. Willow tits and Siberian tits will also
nique to a method that was not as rapid (Fig. 16.4). roost in holes in the snow (Smith 1991), and this
For birds with access to supplementary food at behavior has also been witnessed in black-capped
bird feeders, as temperatures decrease, time spent chickadees (C. C. St Clair, personal communication).
participating in energetically demanding activities, Communal roosting is not employed by chicka-
such as foraging and flight, decreases and more time dees or titmice; however, several different chicka-
is spent at rest. During cold weather, black-capped dees may use the same roost over several nights
chickadees spent as much as 70% of the day at their (Pitts 1976). Occasionally, these roosts were also
roosting site, and 10% of the time not spent at occupied by other species, such as tufted titmice.
the roost was spent resting (Kessel 1976). Foraging Small cavities provide a relatively stable microcli-
ranges also decrease in size with temperature, mate for over-night roosting, especially in metaboli-
presumably to minimize time spent away from the cally challenging winter conditions. Mayer et al.
roost. Black-capped chickadee populations in Alaska (1982) found that by roosting in a cavity, a birds net
tend to become more concentrated at higher radiant heat loss could be reduced 60 to 100%, and
elevations due to the presence of thermal inversions. convective heat loss was essentially zero due to the
W I N T E R A D A P TAT I O N S A N D H A B I TAT F R A G M E N TAT I O N 269

30 Carolina 0.11.0 30 Tufted 0.11.0


(a) Conditions chickadee m/sec titmouse m/sec
sun shining or occluded 25 25
temperature 0.110.0C
20 20
1.12.0

Meters
m/sec
15 15 1.12.0
m/sec

10 10
2.13.0
m/sec 3 140 m/sec
05 05

10 20 30 40 50 60 10 20 30 40 50 60
Seconds Seconds

30 30
Carolina 10.1 To Tufted 10.1 To
(b) Conditions 20.0C
titmouse
chickadee 20.0C
sun shining or occluded 25 25
wind vel. 0.11.0 m/sec
20 20 9.9 To
0.0C
Meters

15 9.9 To 15
0.0C

10 10
19.9 To
10.0C
05 19.9 To 05
10.0C

10 20 30 40 50 60 10 20 30 40 50 60
Seconds Seconds

Figure 16.4 Wind-dependent (a) and temperature-dependent (b) foraging curves for black-capped chickadees and tufted titmice wintering in a
deciduous woodlot. Each sawtooth consists of a slope representing the average rate of individual movement, and a flat segment denoting the
time spent on an average stop. The diagonal common to all sawteeth indicates the average foraging speed over 1 min. (From: Grubb 1978.)

lack of any wind inside the roost site. In fact, wind Fasting endurance can become especially important
protection may be the primary benefit conveyed by when foraging is hindered by inclement weather.
cavity roosts. Cooper (1999) showed that when air
temperatures inside a cavity were 4.3 to 5.6C
16.4 Responses to habitat fragmentation
warmer than open sites for chickadees (1.76.3C
warmer for titmice), the standard operative tempera- The ranges of many North American Parids are at
ture (Tes)which incorporates solar and thermal least partially coincident with agricultural and
radiation, ambient temperature, and windspeed with urban habitat modifications, which often results in
a species resistance to heat losswas much higher: a fragmented or patchy distribution of the birds
4.5 to 14.8C warmer than open sites for chickadees (see also Chapter 15). Habitat fragmentation in
and 3.2 to 23.0C warmer for titmice. This dramatic avian populations has received much attention,
increase in Tes translates into significant over-night especially in light of recent conservation concerns.
energy savings. For example in Coopers study, the The negative effects of habitat fragmentation
wind reduction experienced by winter cavity roost- on avian communities are well documented
ing led to nocturnal energy savings of 37.6% for and include reduced food supply, higher nest
chickadees and 25.1% for titmice, which increased predation, and greater risk from predators of
fasting endurance by 7.3 and 5.7 hours, respectively. adults. In addition, smaller woodlots may contain
270 L A N D S C A P E E C O L O G Y, B E H AV I O R , A N D C O N S E R VAT I O N

lower-quality individuals. These consequences are size also affects the annual survivorship of
of concern regarding persistence of chickadee and permanent-resident birds. Combining previously
titmice populations. mentioned constraints of a fragmented landscape
(e.g. lower food supplies, higher predation rates,
etc.) with the increased thermoregulatory demands
16.4.1 Species richness and density of winter may reduce survivorship in such wood-
Several studies have shown species richness and lots. Temperature may magnify any effects of
within-species density to be strongly associated fragmentation alone because sufficient energy and
with woodlot size; in general, woodlots with nutritional resources may become even more
greater area will harbor more species and more difficult to find on cold, short winter days (Grubb
individuals. In studies of permanent-resident birds and Pravosudov 1994). Although studies have
in northern Ohio, including Carolina chickadees shown temperature to be positively correlated with
and tufted titmice, Doherty and Grubb (2000) great tit survivorship in Holland and Finland
found that woodlot area was the most important (see citations in Doherty and Grubb 2000), its effect
predictor of species presence and density. Smaller on black-capped chickadee populations was min-
woodlots tend to have lower food supplies, fewer imal (Loery and Nichols 1985; Loery et al. 1987).
refuges from predation, and greater thermoregula- Doherty and Grubb (2002) were the first to
tory costs due to less protection from harsh winter explore the relationship of survival in a fragmented
temperature and wind, all of which presumably landscape to factors influencing winter severity,
contribute to the observed trends in avian presence. including the presence or absence of supplemental
Researchers (Telleria and Santos 1995) working in food. Their study was executed in a highly-
winter landscapes have also detected highly signifi- fragmented agricultural county of Ohio, with
cant positive relationships between woodland approximately 10% forested area (Steiger et al. 1979)
fragment size and both species richness and density and two riparian corridors. Carolina chickadee
in Eurasian Parid species, including great tits, numbers responded more strongly to woodlot size
crested tits, and coal tits, as well as several other and supplemental food than the other species
woodland passerines. However, blue tits failed to tested. Results showed that the annual probability
show an increase in density with woodlot size. of survival was positively associated with woodlot
area in chickadees; however, a similar association
was not apparent in tufted titmice (Fig. 16.5).
16.4.2 Woodlot edge effect In addition, the presence of supplemental food
Forest fragmentation also contributes to an increase also increased survivorship, most notably for the
in edge habitat. Some avian species will nest along chickadees, a result that is consistent with observa-
woodland edges in addition to the interior, though tions from gray jays (Perisoreus canadensis, Waite
with different degrees of success. For example the 1990) and European nuthatches (Sitta europaea,
nests of Carolina chickadees (in addition to other Nilsson et al. 1993).
Poecile spp.) are often usurped by migratory house Due to their subordinate status within a het-
wrens (Troglodytes aedon). Due presumably to erospecific flock, chickadees are often forced to
the house wrens preference for edge habitat, forage in suboptimal areas (Pierce and Grubb 1981).
chickadee nesting success is significantly greater in The possible foraging locations in a smaller wood-
woodland interiors (Doherty and Grubb 2002). land patch are, therefore, even further reduced,
putting an even greater strain on this species to
consume the necessary resources for over-night sur-
16.5 Effects of fragmentation on vival. Chickadees are also less likely than other flock
wintering permanent resident birds species to cross gaps between woodlots in search of
more optimal foraging conditions (Cimprich and
16.5.1 Survivorship
Grubb 1994; Chapter 15). Logically, increasing a
In addition to being associated with species pres- woodlots size or providing supplemental food
ence and density, it stands to reason that woodlot would help to counteract these disadvantages.
W I N T E R A D A P TAT I O N S A N D H A B I TAT F R A G M E N TAT I O N 271

Annual probability of survival


1.0 1.0
Carolina chickadee Tufted titmouse
0.8 0.8
Food-supplemented
0.6 0.6
Food-supplemented
0.4 0.4
Unsupplemented
0.2 0.2 Unsupplemented
0.0 0.0
0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35
Woodlot area (ha)

Figure 16.5 Relationship between annual probability of survival, woodlot area, and the presence of supplemental food for Carolina chickadees
and tufted titmice. (From: Doherty and Grubb 2002.)

Woodlot size and supplemental food not only Snow cover can also impact the survivorship of
influence the annual survivorship of Carolina winter-resident birds. In fragmented woodlots,
chickadees in a fragmented landscape, but also the tufted titmice showed higher survivorship in years
nutritional condition of those that do survive with less snow cover (Doherty and Grubb 2000).
(Doherty and Grubb 2003). Food consumption has Similarly, when examining feathers for evidence of
a direct effect on the fitness of winter birds, and the nutritional condition, the researchers found that
health of individuals can be compromised without titmice had significantly wider growth bars in years
making them victims of over-night mortality. with less snow cover (Doherty and Grubb 2003).
Through the use of ptilochronology (Box 16.1), Relationships between snow cover and either
Doherty and Grubb (2003) were able to evaluate the survivorship or nutritional condition were not seen
nutritional condition of permanent-resident birds in any chickadees, woodpeckers, or nuthatches.
in winter and ascertain whether food availability Tufted titmice spend more time foraging on the
was having a direct effect on survivorship, as ground than the other three species (Rybcynski
opposed to other factors associated with a small 1977), so snow cover may not have had as strong an
woodlot size, such as increased predation (Nupe impact on birds that concentrate their foraging on
and Swihart 1998). Using the width of growth bars other substrates. Reduced snow cover due to the
on induced feathers as a surrogate for overall nutri- effects of global warming may be one mechanism
tional health, the researchers compared birds from promoting the northward range expansion of
both large and small woodlots, with and without tufted titmice (Harrap and Quinn 1996).
supplemental food.
Daily feather growth of Carolina chickadees was
16.5.2 Effects of wind and temperature
positively correlated with the woodlot area food
on isolated woodlots
interaction, such that birds from large, supplemented
woodlots had the widest growth bars. Similar results The effects of wind and temperature below the
were not observed in other species testedtufted thermoneutral zone become much more pro-
titmice, white-breasted nuthatches (Sitta carolinensis), nounced in isolated woodlots, reducing the
and downy woodpeckers (Picoides pubescens)all of suitability of such patches for permanent-resident
which maintain distinctly different foraging niches. birds. Edge effects become much more pronounced.
Previous studies on these species did record benefits In the most extreme conditions, such effects could
from food supplementation (Grubb 1989; Grubb and potentially penetrate deep enough into a woodlot
Cimprich 1990), however, that work was conducted to render it completely uninhabitable (Dolby and
in a landscape of more continuous woodland. The Grubb 1999a).
additional food in this study may not have been Dolby and Grubb (1999a) examined wind-chill
enough to counteract the harm from such a small effects specifically on small (mean area 5.3 ha)
woodlot. isolated wooded patches in rural Ohio, each
272 L A N D S C A P E E C O L O G Y, B E H AV I O R , A N D C O N S E R VAT I O N

containing one mixed-species flock. As observed in woodlots edge. The investigators found a signifi-
previous studies (Grubb 1975, 1978), both wind cant correlation between a wind/temperature
speed and temperature influenced the vertical interaction term and the distance of birds from the
foraging location of the birds within a woodlot. As woodlots edge. The relationship with wind speed
wind speed increased, both chickadees and titmice was most pronounced at the lowest temperatures
were found increasingly lower in the canopy. The observed (Fig. 16.6). A resident flock often adjusted
increased wind did not elicit the same response their location in the woodlot based not only on
from downy woodpeckers or white-breasted wind speed, but also on its direction. 62% of the
nuthatches. Chickadees and titmice tend to forage time, the researchers observed a majority of the
on smaller-diameter substrates and may not be able flock on the leeward side of the woodlot, presum-
to shelter themselves from the wind as easily as ably there to minimize exposure to the wind.
woodpeckers and nuthatches which are more likely These observations provide evidence for an add-
to respond by foraging on the leeward side of a tree itional challenge to the flock. If severe conditions
or by moving to substrates of larger diameter persist and the flock remains constricted to one sec-
(Grubb 1975, 1977). tion of a woodlot, the availability of food resources
In addition to adjustments in foraging height, may potentially become an exigent issue, further
birds observed in these small fragments also modi- reducing the suitable habitat. In addition, returning
fied their foraging location with respect to the to windward edges for the purpose of cache

(a) 15.0 to 6.5C (b) 6.0 to 2.0C


Distance from windward edge (m)

400 P = 0.008 400 P = 0.48


R2 = 0.52
300 300 R2 = 0.04

200 200

100 100

0 0
0 1 2 3 4 5 6 0 1 2 3 4 5 6

(c) 1.5 to 2.5C (d) 3.0 to 8.0C


Distance from windward edge (m)

400 P = 0.56 400 P = 0.61

R2 = 0.04 R2 = 0.03
300 300

200 200

100 100

0 0
0 1 2 3 4 5 6 0 1 2 3 4 5 6
Wind speed (m/sec) Wind speed (m/sec)

Figure 16.6 Linear regressions for the distance of Carolina chickadees and tufted titmice from the windward edges of woodlots against the
wind speed, blocked by temperature range (ad). (From: Dolby and Grubb 1999.)
W I N T E R A D A P TAT I O N S A N D H A B I TAT F R A G M E N TAT I O N 273

retrieval may become difficult, if not averted a woodland patch, occupants may be more likely
altogether. Other permanent resident species may to disperse into and remain in larger patches.
not be as distressed by low temperatures and high During the winter, a larger patch will offer greater
winds as the smaller chickadees, but woodpeckers protection from the wind effects experienced at a
and nuthatches were also present on the leeward woodlot edge. Mated pairs will also benefit by
side of woodlots. One explanation for this behavior remaining in a larger woodlot during the breeding
is that they followed chickadees and titmice to that season. As discussed earlier, nesting in a larger
location to maintain the antipredation and foraging patch provides an opportunity for the pair to
benefits of social flocking (Dolby and Grubb 1999a). maximize their distance from the woodlots
The available foraging substrate may also affect edge, minimizing the risk of nest usurpation by
flock distribution within a woodlot (Telleria and house wrens.
Santos 1995). Patch edges that contain dense vegeta- In addition, Groom and Grubb (in press)
tion may provide the same wind protection as the discovered that chickadees were more likely to
woodlots interior or the leeward side. Likewise, a reoccupy a woodlot during periods of relatively
lack of appropriate foraging substrate on the leeward benign wind chill. During conditions of high
side of the woodlot may reduce a species presence thermoregulatory demand, minimizing exposure to
even when conditions may warrant wind protection. deleterious abiotic factors, such as wind, is critical
to survival. Interpatch dispersal may, therefore, be
further regulated by the current microclimatic con-
16.5.3 Woodlot connectivity and
ditions, in addition to degree of woodlot isolation.
interpatch movement
Inclement weather may not only restrict the current
In a fragmented landscape, the degree of patch con- foraging habitat, but could also prevent individuals
nectivity can affect winter movement among from seeking more suitable habitat.
wooded habitat patches (see also Chapter 15). Also, For small birds, the task of surviving temperate
if adverse winter conditions result in individual winter seasons can be physiologically demanding,
mortality and, in the most severe cases, local patch and the additional stress created by a fragmented
extinctions, the reoccupation of a woodlot may be habitat can further reduce a populations annual
directly related to its connectivity within the land- survivorship and the nutritional condition of indi-
scape. The presence of travel corridors, such as viduals. Many chickadees and titmice have large
fencerows, may help to facilitate interpatch move- ranges that expose them to a variety of habitats,
ments, especially during times of inopportune seasonal intensities, and microclimates. While
weather conditions when protection from wind and several studies have documented energy-saving
precipitation, as well as the usual protection from physiological adaptations and behavioral modifica-
aerial predators, would be more advantageous. tions to better manage increased thermoregulatory
The impact of winter weather conditions on demands, most studies have been limited to a
movements among fragmented woodlands has single species or to multiple species in the same
been demonstrated by Groom and Grubb (in press), area. The intraspecific variation of these responses
who simulated local patch extinctions by removing across a broad geographical range has received less
Carolina chickadees from selected woodlots in an attention. Comparisons of this sort, along with a
agricultural landscape, and then monitoring better understanding of the physiological chain of
recolonization events. All 25 woodlots selected for events that induce these responses, will provide
the study were eventually recolonized, and birds insights into how a population may respond in
moved sooner into those patches that contained environments that differ in thermal and spatial
more wooded area and were connected via structure. The development and use of innovative
fencerows to another woodlot. If, as previous stud- techniques, such as ptilochronology, and improve-
ies have suggested (Grubb 1975, 1977, 1978; Dolby ments to current technology, such as radio tracking
and Grubb 1999a,1999b), abiotic factors, such as and thermal sensors, will continue to provide
wind, can reduce the amount of suitable habitat in additional information as well.
274 L A N D S C A P E E C O L O G Y, B E H AV I O R , A N D C O N S E R VAT I O N

Habitat fragmentation is likely to increase due to for blue tits. Acta XX Congressus Internationalis
agricultural and urban expansion. Global climate Ornithologici.
change in also underway, though the magnitude and Doherty, P. F. and Grubb, T. C., Jr (2000). Habitat and land-
consequences at the regional scale are still hotly scape correlates of presence, density, and species
richness of birds wintering in forest fragments in Ohio.
debated. In both matters, an effect on avian commu-
Wilson Bulletin, 112, 388394.
nities is likely to be observed. Combining a complete
Doherty, P. F. and Grubb, T. C., Jr (2002). Survivorship of
knowledge of ecological and physiological con- permanent-resident birds in a fragmented forested
straints will facilitate more effective conservation landscape. Ecology, 83, 844857.
and management of over-wintering Parid species. Doherty, P. F. and Grubb, T. C., Jr (2003). Relationship of
nutritional condition of permanent-resident woodland
birds with woodlot area, supplemental food, and snow
References cover. Auk, 120, 331336.
Alatalo, R. V., Gustafsson, L., and Linden, M. (1985). Dolby, A. S. and Grubb, T. C., Jr (1999a). Effects of winter
Interspecific competition and niche shifts in tits and the weather on horizontal and vertical use of isolated forest
goldcrest: an experiment. Journal of Animal Ecology, 55, fragments. Condor, 101, 408412.
977984. Dolby, A. S. and Grubb, T. C., Jr (1999b). Functional roles
Alatalo, R. V., Eriksson, D., and Gustafsson, L. (1987). in mixed-species foraging flocks: a field manipulation.
Exploitation competition influences the choice of for- Auk, 116, 557559.
aging sites by tits: experimental evidence. Ecology, 68, Groom, J. D. and Grubb, T. C., Jr (in press). Patch coloniza-
284290. tion dynamics by Carolina chickadees (Poecile carolinensis)
Austin, G. T. and Smith, E. L. (1972). Winter foraging in a fragmented landscape: a manipulative study. Auk
ecology of mixed insectivorous bird flocks in oak wood- Grossman, A. F. and West, G. C. (1977). Metabolic rate
land in southern Arizona. Condor, 74, 1724. and temperature regulation of winter acclimatized
Bent, A. C. (1947). Life histories of North American black-capped chickadees Parus attricapillus of interior
jays, crows, and titmice. U.S. National Museum Alaska. Ornis Scandinavica, 8, 127138.
Bulletin, 170. Grubb, T. C., Jr (1975). Weather-dependent foraging
Berner, T. O. and Grubb, T. C., Jr (1985). An experimental behavior of some birds wintering in a deciduous wood-
analysis of mixed-species flocking in birds of deciduous land. Condor, 77, 175182.
woodland. Ecology, 66, 12291236. Grubb, T. C., Jr (1977). Weather-dependent foraging
Buskirk, W. H. (1976). Social systems in a tropical forest behavior of some birds wintering in a deciduous wood-
avifauna. American Naturalist, 110, 293310. land: horizontal adjustments. Condor, 79, 271274.
Chaplin, S. B. (1976). The physiology of hypothermia in Grubb, T. C., Jr (1978). Weather-dependent foraging rates
the black-capped chickadee, Parus atricapillus. Journal of of wintering woodland birds. Auk, 95, 370376.
Comparative Physiology, 112, 335344. Grubb, T. C., Jr (1989). Ptilochronology: Feather growth
Cimprich, D. A. and Grubb, T. C., Jr (1994). Consequences bars as indicators of nutritional status. Auk, 106,
for Carolina chickadees of foraging with tufted titmice 314320.
in winter. Ecology, 75, 16151625. Grubb, T. C., Jr (2006). Ptilochronology: feather time and the
Cooper, S. J. (1999). The thermal and energetic significance biology of birds. Oxford University Press.
of cavity roosting in mountain chickadees and juniper Grubb, T. C., Jr and Cimprich, D. A. (1990).
titmice. Condor, 101, 863866. Supplementary food improves the nutritional condition
Cooper, S. J. (2002). Seasonal acclimatization in mountain of wintering woodland birds: Evidence from
chickadees and juniper titmice. Physiological and ptilochronology. Ornis Scandinavica, 21, 277281.
Biochemical Zoology, 74, 386395. Grubb, T. C., Jr and Pravosudov, V. V. (1994). Toward a
Cooper, S. J. and Gessaman, J. A. (2005). Nocturnal general-theory of energy management in wintering
hypothermia in seasonally acclimatized mountain birds. Journal of Avian Biology, 25, 255260.
chickadees and juniper titmice. Condor, 107, 151155. Harrap, S. and Quinn, D. (1996). Chickadees, Tits,
Cooper, S. J. and Swanson, D. L. (1994). Seasonal acclima- Nuthatches and Treecreepers. Princeton University Press,
tization of thermoregulation in the black-capped Princeton, New Jersey.
chickadee. Condor, 96, 638646. Hohtola, E. (1982). Thermal and electromyographic
Dhondt, A. A., Kempenaers, B., and De Laet, J. (1991). correlates of shivering thermogenesis in the pigeon.
Protected winter roosting sites as a limiting resource Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology, 73A, 159166.
W I N T E R A D A P TAT I O N S A N D H A B I TAT F R A G M E N TAT I O N 275

Hohtola, E. and Stevens, E. D. (1986). The relationship of adaptations fro protection of stored food. Condor, 91,
muscle electrical activity, tremor, and heat production 766776.
to shivering thermogenesis in Japanese quail. Journal of Pierce, V. and Grubb, T. C., Jr (1981). Laboratory studies of
Experimental Biology, 125, 119135. foraging in four bird species of deciduous woodland.
Hotly, J. G. (1973). Air quality in a subarctic community Auk, 98, 307320.
Fairbanks, Alaska. Arctic, 26, 292302. Pitts, T. D. (1976). Fall and winter roosting habits of
Hutto, R. L. (1994). The composition of mixed-species Carolina chickadees. Wilson Bulletin, 88, 603610.
flocks in a tropical deciduous forest in western Mexico. Pravosudov, V. V. and Grubb, T. C., Jr (1998). Management
Condor, 96, 105118. of fat reserves in tufted titmice Baeolophus bicolor in
Jablonski, P. G. and Lee, S. D. (2002). Foraging niche shifts relation to risk of predation. Animal Behaviour, 56, 4954.
in mixed-species flocks of tits in Korea. Journal of Field Reinertsen, R. E. (1996). Physiological and ecological
Ornithology, 73, 246252. aspects of hypothermia. In: Carey, C., ed. Avian
Jaeger, E. (1949). Further observations on the hibernation Energetics and Nutritional Ecology, pp 125157. Chapman
of the poor-will. Condor, 50, 105109. and Hall, New York.
Kessel, B. (1976). Winter activity patterns of black- Reinertsen, R. E. and Haftorn, S. (1983). Nocturnal
capped chickadees in interior Alaska. Wilson Bulletin, hypothermia and metabolism in the willow tit Parus
88, 3661. montanus at 63N. Journal of Comparative Physiology,
Loery, G. and Nichols, J. D. (1985). Dynamics of a black- 151B, 109118.
capped chickadee population, 19581983. Ecology, 66, Reinertsen, R. E. and Haftorn, S. (1986). Different meta-
11951203. bolic strategies of northern birds for nocturnal survival.
Loery, G., Pollock, K. H., and Nichols, J. D. (1987). Age- Journal of Comparative Physiology, 156, 655663.
specificity of black-capped chickadee survival rates. Rybcynski, R. (1977). Dynamic Aspects of Bird Flocking: The
Analysis of capture-recapture data. Ecology, 68, Influence of Weather and Patterns of Spatial Utilization.
10381044. Ph.D. dissertation, Cornell University, Ithaca, New York.
Marsh, R. L., Carey, C., and Dawson, W. R. (1984). Sharbaugh, S. M. (2001). Seasonal acclimatization to
Substrate concentrations and turnover of plasma extreme climatic conditions by black-capped chickadees
glucose during cold exposure in seasonally acclima- (Poecile atricapilla) in interior Alaska (64N). Physiological
tized house finches, Carpodacus mexicanus. Journal of and Biochemical Zoology, 74, 568575.
Comparative Physiology B, 154, 469476. Smith, S. M. (1991). The Black-capped Chickadee: Behavioral
Mayer, L., Lustick, S., and Battersby, B. (1982). The import- Ecology and Natural History. Cornell University Press,
ance of cavity roosting and hypothermia to the energy Ithaca, NY.
balance of the winter acclimatized Carolina chickadee. Steen, J. (1958). Climatic adaptation in some small north-
International Journal of Biometeorology, 26, 231238. ern birds. Ecology, 39, 625629.
Morse, D. H. (1970). Feeding behavior and predator Steiger, J. R., Brug, W. H., Parkingson, R. J., and Lemaster,
avoidance in heterospecific groups. BioScience, 27, D. D. (1979). Soil survey of Crawford County, OH. Soil
332339. Conservation Service, U.S. Department of Agriculture.
Nilsson, J.-A., Kallander, H., and Owe, P. (1993). A Washington, D.C., US.
prudent hoarder: Effects of long-tern hoarding in the Telleria, J. L. and Santos, T. (1995). Effects of forest frag-
European nuthatch, Sitta europaea. Behavioral Ecology, 4, mentation on a guild of wintering passerines: the role of
369373. habitat selection. Biological Conservation, 71, 6167.
Nupe, T. E. and Swihart, R. K. (1998). Effects of forest Waite, T. A. (1990). Effects of caching supplemental food
fragmentation on population attributes of white-footed in induced feather regeneration in wintering Gray Jays
mice and eastern chipmunks. Journal of Mammology, 79, Perisoreus canadensis: A ptilochronology study. Ornis
12341243. Scandinavica, 21, 122128.
Perrins, C. M. (1979). British Tits. William Collins and Co., Wolf, L. L. and Hainsworth, F. R. (1972). Environmental
Glasgow. influence on regulated body temperature in torpid
Petit, D. R., Petit, L. J., and Petit, K. E. (1989). Winter hummingbirds. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology,
caching ecology of deciduous woodland birds and 41, 167173.
This page intentionally left blank
CHAPTER 17

Habitat quality and reproductive


behavior in chickadees and tits:
Potential for habitat matrix use
in forest generalists
Ken A. Otter, Harry van Oort, and Kevin T. Fort

transition can create edge effects into the remnant


17.1 Introduction
patches (Laurance 2000; Davies et al. 2001). By
Habitat destruction and fragmentation is one of comparison, the maintenance of a matrix struc-
the most serious threats to biodiversity loss in turally similar to the remnant (e.g. a managed
conservation biology (Davies et al. 2001). Isolated exotic forest surrounding native forests) can
fragments may be too small to maintain viable reduce some of the microclimatic effects of abrupt
populations, leading to extirpation within rem- edges on remnant habitats (Noss and Cooperrider
nant patches of pristine habitat. If the remnant 1994; Davies et al. 2001) and can promote species
patches are also sufficiently isolated from one movement between habitat patches (Ricketts 2001;
another, dispersal and recolonization may be Cooper and Walters 2002; Tabarelli and Gascon
limited (Noss and Cooperrider 1994; Cooper and 2005; Castelln and Sieving 2006; Chapter 15).
Walters 2002). However, what distinguishes habi- Such studies have shown that managing matrix
tat fragments from classic island biogeography vegetation may enhance dispersal between breed-
theory is that the intervening gaps are often vege- ing patches. However, suggestions have also been
tated, rather than open expanses of ocean. This made that the matrix, if well managed, could be
intervening habitat, referred to as the matrix utilized for foraging and breeding by generalist
(Noss and Cooperrider 1994), differs in species species typical of remnant habitat (Gascon et al.
composition (Fig. 17.1a) or age and/or structure 1999; Davies et al. 2001; Tabarelli and Gascon 2005).
of the vegetation (Fig. 17.1b) so as to be Noss and Cooperrider (1994) suggest that even if
sufficiently distinct from the remnant habitat such disturbed matrices serve as sinks, they may
islands they surround. Matrix habitat is temporarily maintain populations associated with
considered less hospitable for remnant-dwelling the core habitat area.
species, yet terrestrial matrices may not be quite This chapter assesses the prospect of managed
as impermeable as open oceans (Ricketts 2001). matrix habitat to facilitate either supplementary or
The distinction is probably more akin to alternative breeding areas. It is presupposed that the
extremely poor versus high-quality habitat, than species likely to utilize these buffers or matrix
between inhospitable ocean versus habitable patches are generalist in their habitat requirements,
islands. and can compensate for the exotic and often
If the matrix is sufficiently distinct from the disturbed nature of these habitatsthat is they are
remnant habitat, as in the case of farmland or classified as matrix-tolerating species (Gascon et al.
clearcuts surrounding retained forest, the abrupt 1999). However, whether matrices function as sinks

277
278 L A N D S C A P E E C O L O G Y, B E H AV I O R , A N D C O N S E R VAT I O N

(a)

Remnant Habitat Matrix

Matrix

Remnant

(b)
Figure 17.1 Remnant habitats surrounded by a matrix
of contrasting vegetation. Matrix habitat that facilitates
movement between island remnant patches may consist
of vegetation that differs in species composition (e.g.
exotics) but is similar in density and size to the remnant
(a), or could be similar in vegetation composition to the
Matrix Remnant Habitat
remnant but differ in age or successional stage (b).

or are able to sustain populations is largely young regenerating forest) may be incorporated
untested. Using data on habitat-generalist species in into territories of birds residing in the remnant
the Paridae family, evidence would suggest that patches themselves (Gascon et al. 1999; Tubelis et al.
even small changes in the habitat characteristics 2004b). Despite the difference in vegetative
can have profound effects on reproduction, condi- composition, such a matrix may provide a large-
tion, and behavior. If Parids serve as an indicator pore filter for dispersal between remnant forest
of how slight habitat variation could affect less- patches (Gascon et al. 1999; Tabarelli and Gascon
adaptable species, the potential of a matrix to 2005; Castelln and Sieving 2006), and may even be
amplify the size of remnant patches (Tubelis utilized for reproduction by species that can adapt
et al. 2004b) may be less than is currently to the matrix (Gascon et al. 1999; Tubelis et al.
hypothesized. 2004b). Further the ability to utilize the matrix may
reduce a species potential for extirpation within
patches immediately following fragmentation. This
17.2 Fragmentation, the matrix,
conceptknown as a matrix-vulnerability index
and halo effects
is based on observations that forest species that
In fragmented landscapes, a matrix consisting of utilize the matrix following fragmentation are more
mature vegetation (e.g. mature exotic forest or likely to persist in the remnant patches than those
H A B I TAT Q UA L I T Y A N D R E P R O D U C T I V E B E H AV I O R 279

that do not venture into the matrix (Gascon et al. remnant habitat among such matrix-tolerating
1999). Tubelis et al. (2004b) found that species in species.
small remnants incorporate areas of the matrix into Tubelis et al.s (2004b) results suggest that forest
their breeding territories. The utilization of the generalists are more willing to utilize the matrix
matrix was greater when the matrix contained when faced with small remnant patches. It is also
more mature (although, in this case, exotic) vegeta- likely that creating a matrix with similar structural
tion, and thus structurally mimicked the remnant composition (but exotic species composition) is
patch. Such use of the matrix around remnant likely far better than abrupt transitions between rem-
patchesreferred to as the halo effectis argued nants and harvested areas. Similarly, several studies
to effectively increase the functional size of the have found that more vegetated matrices promote
remnant patch for habitat generalists (Fig. 17.2b). dispersal between remnant patches (Ricketts 2001;
The halo effect specifically describes the use of Cooper and Walters 2002; Tabarelli and Gascon
matrix adjoining good quality remnant patches, but 2005; Castelln and Sieving 2006; Chapter 15).
this concept has been extended to suggest that However, the utilization of matrix habitat as alterna-
some species may settle solely in the matrix, such tive breeding/foraging habitat, rather than simply
that it becomes an alternative breeding habitat as a dispersal corridor, makes an implicit assump-
(Gascon et al. 1999depicted in Fig. 17.2c). While tion that the matrix is somewhat equivalent to the
the ability to use matrix habitat (either to supple- remnant habitatuse of the matrix offsets the loss of
ment territories or as a substitute for breeding in the remnant. While this could be true, one can not
remnant patches) may allow species to persist assume that the willingness to use the matrix is
following disturbance, less is known about whether equivalent to the birds perceiving the matrix as com-
such matrix use compensates for the loss of pensatory. The fact that birds in larger remnants are

(a) Exclusive use of remnant

Matrix

Remnant

Territory

Figure 17.2 Where remnant patches are


sufficiently large, matrix-tolerating species will likely
occupy territories solely contained within the
remnant (a). In the small remnant patches, territories
may be expanded to utilize the matrix, either for
100 m foraging or breeding (b). As a result, the matrix may
extend the functional size of the remnant patch,
referred to as a halo effect (after Tubuelis et al.
2004b). The extension of this usage is when
matrix-tolerating species begin to utilize the matrix
(b) Halo effect (c) Alternative breeding habitat as alternative breeding habitat (c).
280 L A N D S C A P E E C O L O G Y, B E H AV I O R , A N D C O N S E R VAT I O N

less likely to utilize matrix habitat (Tubelis et al. reproductive behavior of blue tits occupying Oak
2004b) would indicate these may be perceived as forests in Mediterranean habitats of Southern
lower quality by the birds themselves. Even where France and Corsica. Although the predominant tree
matrix is partially, or completely, used in breeding species are oak, forests are dominated by one of two
territories, settlement in these patches may not species: deciduous downy oak (Quercus pubescens)
constitute successful breeding attempts. Viewing versus sclerophyll (evergreen) holm oak (Q. ilex).
these areas as potential sinks, but with prospects of This apparently subtle difference has enormous
helping sustain overall populations in the short term biological significance to the birds. Deciduous oak
(Noss and Cooperrider 1994) may de-emphasize drop their leaves annually, replenishing these with
retention of remnant breeding habitat. To evaluate new growth early each spring. The young leaves
the potential of the matrix to supplement or act as have lower levels of tannins than mature leaves,
alternative to remnant habitat, detailed studies of and are more palatable to folivorous caterpillars,
how breeding behavior and success are modified by the primary prey of the birds (Blondel and Dias
subtle differences in habitat quality are required. 1994). When available, these caterpillars are the
Such information is available from long-term studies preferred prey for provisioning nestlings (Blondel
on members of the Paridae family. 1985; Blondel et al. 1991; Tremblay et al. 2004), and
have the highest energetic yield for the young birds
in digestible proteins (Blondel et al. 1991).
17.3 Habitat variation and Evergreen oak shed only a small portion of their
reproductive success in the Paridae leaves annually, which are replaced later in the spring
compared with those of deciduous oaks. As a greater
17.3.1 Matrix of similar age or structure
portion of their leaves are mature and tannin rich,
but dissimilar species composition
evergreen oaks support a much smaller caterpillar
Species are typically more likely to be found in a population (Blondel et al. 1991; Banbura et al. 1994). In
matrix that consists of vegetation with similar addition, the delayed development of the new leaves
canopy heights compared to the remnant patches, each year results in differences in the peak timing of
despite the vegetation potentially being of different caterpillar abundance. Development of the caterpil-
species composition (Tubelis et al. 2004b; Gascon lar instars begins soon after bud-burst of the new
et al. 1999). One can see why this may facilitate leaves, which in evergreen oaks occurs nearly
movement of the animals among remnant patches, 3 weeks later in the spring compared to deciduous
as exposure while moving through such matrix may oaks; the result is that peak abundance of caterpillar
be far lower than moving across large habitat open- larvae to feed nestlings is delayed by the same period
ings (Chapter 15). From a structural standpoint, (Blondel et al. 1993). The timing of reproduction in
birds may also view these kinds of habitats as more passerines is closely associated with prey cycles
contiguous and less disruptive than those where the (Chapter 5), as there is highly stabilizing selection on
matrix is an abrupt structural transition (such as for- matching the timing between the peak feeding
est bordered by grasslande.g. Tubelis et al. 2004a). demands of young with the peak of prey abundance.
It is the secondary function of managing matrices for Mismatching nest timing and prey peak has been
supplemental breeding habitat, however, that is less shown to reduce breeding success in great tits (Seki
clearly understood. Research on great tits and blue and Takano 1998) and blue tits (Dias and Blondel
tits conducted over the past three decades may shed 1996; Blondel et al. 1999; Lambrechts et al. 2004) and
light on the ability of birds to compensate for vari- growth rates in willow tits (Thessing 2000).
ation in species composition within their habitats. Can birds that breed in evergreen oak forests
compensate for these differences from deciduous
17.3.1.1 Blue tits in evergreen versus oak forests? Studies of populations in isolated
deciduous oak forests evergreen oak patches on the island of Corsica
For over 20 years, Jacques Blondel and colleagues suggest they can. Blondel et al. (1991) found the
have been conducting detailed work on the seasonal timing of breeding in Corsica is delayed
H A B I TAT Q UA L I T Y A N D R E P R O D U C T I V E B E H AV I O R 281

by approximately the same time interval as the appear to be maladaptive (Thomas et al. 2001). This
difference in caterpillar abundance, as compared to is more likely, however, to be a constraint of contin-
deciduous oak habitat on the French mainland. uous gene flow between these populations (Dhondt
Subsequent studies have found similar timing et al. 1990; Blondel et al. 1999; Thomas et al. 2001)
shifts in isolated patches of evergreen versus resulting in a sourcesink scenario (Dias 1996).
deciduous oak forests that both occur on Corsica In the mainland habitats, the richer territories of
(Blondel et al. 1999; Tremblay et al. 2004). Despite the deciduous oak habitat would constitute habitat
the ability of the birds in these isolated forests to sourcesthe higher fledgling quality in these areas
alter timing to match the caterpillar peaks, the habi- are likely to result in greater fledging success than in
tats themselves are not equivalent. Birds in evergreen habitats (Dias 1996). Thus, the extreme
Corsican evergreen oak forests have lower clutch mismatching of clutch initiation and prey abun-
sizes than those anywhere else in Europe (Blondel dance in the mainland evergreen habitats is unlikely
et al. 1993; Blondel et al. 1998; Lambrechts et al. to change as long as there is potential emigration of
2004), possibly due to differences in the diet birds from the deciduous forest, as reproductive
composition fed to nestlings, which is lower in timing appears to be largely under genetic control
caterpillars versus arthropods and other prey (Blondel et al. 1990). Without the genetic isolation of
(Blondel et al. 1991; Banbura et al. 1994). Birds the evergreen populations from source populations,
occupying these evergreen forests may also be in there will be no potential to adapt to the very differ-
poorer condition than those in deciduous forests. ent circumstances in these areas. This could result in
Blue tit males in Corsican populations have lower sink populations that persist due to replenishment
levels of aggressiveness than mainland counter- from source populations (Dias 1996; although some
parts (Perret and Blondel 1993), pairs occupying ambiguity still remains on long-term population
evergreen forests are subordinate to those in decidu- effects of habitat qualitysee Lambrechts et al. 2004).
ous forests (Braillet et al. 2002), and adults are in Regardless, the inability to adapt to species-atypical
poorer physical condition (Lambrechts et al. 2004). local conditions is not unheard ofrather this
Despite indications that the birds view the habi- phenomenon has also been reported in clutch size
tats differently in terms of quality, Corsican popu- differences between great tits breeding in woodland
lations in isolated evergreen habitat appear to have and hedgerow settings (Dhondt et al. 1990).
adapted to local conditions, likely facilitated by The conclusion drawn is that structurally similar
genetic isolation (Blondel et al. 1999; Thomas et al. habitats differing in vegetative species composition
2001; Tremblay et al. 2004). However, contiguous may, nevertheless, differ markedly in habitat quality.
populations of blue tits breeding in southern France In all circumstances noted, these have the potential
occupying either evergreen or deciduous habitats to affect reproductive success. However, the extent
show a different pattern in reproductive timing. of this impact varies based on whether local adapta-
These two populations are not isolated from one tion occurs. Adaptation to local environments
another and birds occupying the evergreen habitats appears to require reproductive isolation from
on the mainland appear to mismatch their repro- populations in native habitat. Among neighboring
ductive timing with the peak in caterpillar abun- patches that differ in composition (such as would
dance for these habitats (Blondel et al. 1999; Thomas occur among remnant patches embedded in a
et al. 2001). These mainlandevergreen birds do not matrix) there is increased potential for a mismatch
differ in the timing of breeding from those in neigh- between genotype and the local environment for
boring deciduous forests, suggesting that the gene birds settling in the matrix. This will occur if
flow occurs across habitats maintains traits adapted remnant patches are higher in quality and function
for breeding in the richer, deciduous habitat as source populations. Emigration from these
(Blondel et al. 1999). The result is extremely poor patches, followed by settlement within the matrix, is
reproductive behavior of birds in evergreen forests likely to result in settling birds retaining genes
on the mainland; their lack of compensation for the adapted for breeding within the remnant. If optimal
difference in timing of prey availability would behavior for breeding within the matrix (such as
282 L A N D S C A P E E C O L O G Y, B E H AV I O R , A N D C O N S E R VAT I O N

shifting timing of reproduction) is required for suc- presumably limited by other resources, such as
cess, such local adaptations are unlikely to arise with defense of nesting areas. It is, therefore, possible that
the influx of recruits from neighboring remnant species may be less susceptible to small patch size in
patches. The result could be a mismatch between highly productive habitats (Nour et al. 1998). The
exhibited behavior and the optimal behavior for same may not be true in low-quality habitat.
breeding in the context of the matrix. If the matrix Tremblay et al. (2003) found that overall food avail-
also provides different or reduced prey availability ability, mistiming of clutches, and variation in clutch
(as is the case for evergreen oak habitat) the com- size resulted in lower fledging success in low-quality
pounding effect of settlement in these habitats could habitats than was the case in high-quality habitats.
be very low productivity. However, populations in Where the birds are able to adapt to low-quality
the matrix may appear to be sustained if sufficient habitat through reproductive isolation, however,
emigration occurs from remnant patches. The latter, Tremblay et al. (2004) found evidence that pairs can
however, requires that sufficient remnant habitat is adapt to both differences in prey availability and
retained to serve as sources. Even halo territories clutch timing. In comparing two isolated Corsican
that incorporate mixed habitat could result in populations (one deciduous and one evergreen
reduced prey availability compared to pure remnant habitat), birds in the deciduous habitat make more
habitat and reduce breeding potential. The ability of frequent foraging visits to the nest and have
few large remnant patches to sustain a large number shorter foraging flights away from the nest
of sink populations (either birds breeding in the compared to those in evergreen habitats. However,
matrix itself or in small remnant/matrix halos) will birds in evergreen habitat compensate for longer
be limited. foraging flights by bringing larger prey items. The
result is that the overall biomass of prey brought to
17.3.1.2 Comparison of blue tits to greats tits the nest was equivalent between the sites.
and other study sites Conversely, in two neighboring plots that differed
The data from Mediterranean blue tits would appear in species composition (a deciduous oak versus a
to have dire consequences for viewing non-native mixed oak/pine forest in Germany), Stauss et al.
matrix habitat for breeding or possibly even for- (2005) found that blue tits were less compensatory.
aging, despite its probable utility as a corridor. Birds in the deciduous forest also made shorter
However, not all of the research on tit species shows foraging trips during nestling feeding, but in this
major effects of breeding in disturbed or altered population the nestling growth rates and fledging
habitats. For example Nour et al. (1998) found no mass was much lower than in the mixed forest.
impact on feeding rates, prey volume, nestling con- Thus, the ability to adjust provisioning behavior to
dition, nor nestling survival in either great tits or compensate for poorer breeding habitat may also
blue tits breeding within small or large habitat frag- rely on reproductive isolation of the populations,
ments. Similarly, Verhulst and Tinbergen (2001) which allows for development of local adaptation.
failed to find a correlation between energetic work- In a practical sense, this would require manage-
load and mismatching of reproduction with caterpil- ment of matrices so large that emigration from
lar peak in their great tit population. Why are these remnant patches is unlikely.
results contradictory? It may simply stem from how
much the habitat differed in composition. The habi-
17.3.2 Matrix: Habitat of similar species
tat patches studied by Nour et al. (1998) do not
but dissimilar age
appear to have differed in species; all appear to be in
deciduous oak woodland. Tremblay et al. (2003) Research on great tits has long suggested that mar-
found that food supply in high-quality habitats may ginal habitats surrounding woodlots are lower in
exceed the requirements of breeding tits (i.e. there is quality than the woodlots themselves (Dhondt et al.
a saturation threshold beyond which breeding 1992; Riddington and Gosler 1995). However, less
success no longer increases linearly with food work has focused on the effect of occupying
supply). In these cases, population density is second-growth forest on European tits than has
H A B I TAT Q UA L I T Y A N D R E P R O D U C T I V E B E H AV I O R 283

been conducted on comparing birds in evergreen acquired as a municipal park in the 1980s. In 1986,
versus deciduous oak forests. Our work in northern portions of the park (approximately half the land
British Columbia (BC) on black-capped chickadees base) were cleared and replanted with various
addresses the former scenario. conifer species to serve as an example of a model
Northern Canadas sub-boreal forest is a mixture forest. The park now consists of native habitat in
of deciduous and coniferous species. In north early seral forest, 30 years in age, which will here-
central BC, the deciduous component of these after be referred to as young forest. Several small
forests consists predominantly of two species patches (the largest approximately 4 ha) of mature
favored for nest excavation by chickadees forest are interspersed throughout the larger area
trembling aspen (Populus tremuloides) and paper and surrounded by the younger forest. The canopy
birch (Betula papyrifera). Coniferous components are varies between 5 and 15 m in height, with the major-
largely hybrid spruce (Picea glauca Picea engel- ity of the canopy not exceeding 10 m. Most aspen
mannii), lodgepole pine (Pinus contorta), Douglas-fir and birch found in the forest is of a diameter and age
(Pseudotsuga menziesii), and subalpine fir (Abies marginal for use by chickadees and few decayed
lasiocarpa), the latter two being associated mostly snags of these species occur.
with the climax forests. Lodgepole pine and By comparison, university-endowed lands adja-
Douglas-fir have been the focus of heavy logging in cent to the park constitute mature, undisturbed
the resource-based economy of northern BC, result- forest that has not undergone any large-scale logging
ing in large tracts of the landscape being clear-cut for over 80 years. The forest is composed of a mixed
logged within the last half century. mosaic of mature aspen and birch stands inter-
Commercial forestry in the region creates a spersed with mature pine and Douglas-fir forest.
mosaic of habitats of varying age. Young regenerat- The canopy is relatively uniform at 25 to 30 m. In
ing forests in the region are often dominated by addition to these two main patches, two smaller
lodgepole pine and a deciduous component of patches (one young forest and one mature) are also
green alder (Alnus crispa) and numerous willow monitored. Our work compares the reproduction
species (Salix spp.) intermixed with the aspen and and behavior of neighboring populations of birds
birch. The large scale of the landscape and forestry occupying habitat of similar species composition,
regulations in northern BC allow harvesting while but largely divergent seral stage (mature versus
remnant forest patches still retain corridors of young forest). While not a perfect test of the halo
continuous, mature vegetation, but movement effect (as we focus primarily on birds nesting entirely
between patches is also facilitated by the matrix in one habitat or the other), the work that we
of regenerating forests (see also Chapter 15). conduct may shed light on the potential for regener-
However, commercial replanting focuses on conifer ating natural matrix to serve as alternative breeding
rather than deciduous species, the latter of which habitat. Between 2000 and 2004, the average number
constitute the primary species used by cavity of chickadee territories falling completely in either
excavating guilds in the region (Chapter 8). As young or mature forest was 15.2 and 17.4 respect-
forests are also reharvested before reaching full ively (range of 12 to 19 pairs/year for young forest,
maturity (e.g. 50-year rotations), coupled with the and 13 to 22 for mature). A low productivity year
loss of mature and dead aspen/birch forests, a during spring of 2004 (see below) resulted in low
greater proportion of the landbase could become breeding densities in 2005, with eight pairs in young
early seral forests. forest and 11 in mature.
Since 2000, we have studied two adjacent forest
patches near the University of Northern British 17.3.2.1 Chickadees breeding in mature versus
Columbia which differ in the degree of habitat young forestssettlement
disturbance (Fig. 17.3). The main disturbed site is a The two habitats are adjacent, and thus birds occu-
100-ha forest logged for agricultural pasture land pying the two sites are not genetically isolated.
in the late 1960s. The land was allowed to begin Recruitment into local populations is extremely low
natural regeneration in the late 1970s, and was (5% of nestlings banded since 2000) due to the high
284 L A N D S C A P E E C O L O G Y, B E H AV I O R , A N D C O N S E R VAT I O N

Figure 17.3 Black-capped chickadee study site in northern British Columbia. The study compares chickadees breeding in adjacent patches of
mature sub-boreal mixed forest that has not undergone disturbance for more than 80 years, against younger forest (30 years) that has
undergone disturbance in the recent past. Vegetation differences between forest types mimic differences between a matrix and remnant patch
depicted in Fig. 17.1b.

dispersal propensity of juvenile chickadees (Weise periodcould be used as an index of the relative
and Meyer 1979). Recruits that have been found are condition of settling juveniles. Despite this, there
not philopatric to natal habitat type; juveniles was no difference in the growth bar size of males
fledged from one habitat type are found as com- occupying mature versus young forest sites. This
monly in non-natal versus natal habitat (K. A. Otter, suggests that male settlement among habitats is not
H. van Oort, and K. T. Fort, unpublished data). influenced by condition. Females that settle in
Dispersal in this species occurs during the first fall mature habitat, however, had larger growth bars
(Smith 1991), and each year a large number of than those that settled in young habitat, suggesting
juvenile recruits from the surrounding forests females may pay more attention to habitat attrib-
occupy both sites. In contrast to juvenile behavior, utes during settlement (van Oort and Otter 2005).
settled birds are sedentary, often breeding in the Yet, unlike the results found by Braillet et al. (2002)
same territories their entire life (Smith 1991). Within in blue tits between evergreen and deciduous
the study area, minor shifting of territories between forests, there is no indication that males occupying
years does not include the movement of adults the mature forest site are inherently dominant to
from one habitat type to the other (K. A. Otter, birds in the young forest (Fort and Otter 2004a).
H. van Oort, and K. T. Fort, unpublished data); once Thus, males at least do not appear to settle based on
a bird has settled in either the young or mature a perception of differences in habitat quality.
forest, movement to the other site for subsequent Further, despite the populations being functionally
breeding has not been witnessed. distinct following settlement, the interchange of
Initial settlement does not appear to be biased juvenile birds between sites and from surrounding
by condition of juveniles. Using ptilochronology habitats prevents genetic isolation, and with it the
(see Box 16.1 in Chapter 16), van Oort and Otter potential to adapt to local habitat variation.
(2005) found a correlation between body condition
and growth bar size on tail feathers among first 17.3.2.2 Habitat quality and reproductive
wintering juveniles; this suggests that growth bar success in chickadees
size of tail featherswhich in this first wintering Despite little evidence of differential settlement
cohort reflect growth during the nestling/fledgling based on habitat type, the young and mature forest
H A B I TAT Q UA L I T Y A N D R E P R O D U C T I V E B E H AV I O R 285

sites are not equivalent in quality. Birds occupying 20002004 was 32.6). The spring of 2005 was similar
the young forest have higher rates of nest abandon- in temperature and timing of leaf burst to those in
ment than those in the mature forest sites (Fort and 2002 and 2003, and low breeding densities seemed
Otter 2004b). Like studies of blue tits, the severity of to result in high fledging success (Fig. 17.4) in both
this effect appears to vary between yearsin sites. Despite this, the tendency remained for the
productive years, the disparity between success- mature forest to have higher success. Thus, the
fully nesting pairs is dampened, but in low effect that settling in either habitat has on breeding
productivity years the disparity can be extreme. In success appears to be magnified in years of more
2000 and 2001, the pairs in the young forest were atypical weather.
only 64% as likely to fledge young as those nesting Even in years where success rates are more
in neighboring mature forest, and the effect appears comparable between habitats, habitat influences
to be most pronounced among birds that are the condition of resident birds. The data suggests
subordinate in winter dominance hierarchies (Fort that the birds perceive the habitats to differ in qual-
and Otter 2004b). Both 20002001 had prolonged ity, and this may be attributable to differences
periods of cold weather in the critical nesting in relative food availability. Chickadees in the
period (May and June), and may have been the younger forest carry higher winter fat loads than
cause of dramatic differences in abandonment those in the mature forest sites (van Oort and Otter
across habitats (Fort and Otter 2004b). Conversely, 2005); higher fat deposition is typical of lower-
the springs of 2002 and 2003 were warmer with less ranked birds in the Paridae (Ekman and Lilliendahl
precipitation than the preceding 2 years, and the 1992) and is more common when birds perceive
disparity in nesting success between sites was that local food resources are uncertain (Gosler
diminished (Fig. 17.4). In 2002, fledging success 1996).
was extremely high in both habitats; despite the If birds in the young forests are being subjected
pattern of lower fledging in young forest continu- to lower food reserves in the winter, then there is
ing, the effect was dampened by pairs in both habi- the possibility that these birds are more likely to
tats being highly likely to fledge. The pattern across enter the breeding season in poorer condition. Over
habitats reverses slightly in 2003, the only year that the past few years, we have used condition-
this occurred in the study. This, however, may have dependent behavioral traits of birds in either habi-
been associated with high local predation on tat to determine whether there is evidence for a
nesting pairs, as the majority of nest abandonment difference in condition between sites (Godfrey
was confined to one area of the mature site where 2003). The traits measured include the dawn
a sharp-shinned hawk, Accipiter striatus, was chorus, calling by females to solicit courtship feed-
known to be nesting. Spring of 2004, however, was ing, territorial behavior, parental provisions, and
unseasonably mild, with snowmelt and leaf-burst condition of adults.
3 weeks earlier than normal. The result was
unexpected61.9% (13/21) of pairs in the mature 17.3.2.3 Habitat quality and condition-dependent
forest sites fledged young, but only 31.3% (5/16 behaviors in chickadees
pairs) of birds in the younger forest fledged, the Dawn chorus: the extended period of dawn singing
lowest success rates in the 5 study years, with only in chickadees typically reflects male condition, as
48.6% of birds across habitats breeding successfully there is a strong correlation between the relative
(Fig. 17.4). Following 2004s low productivity year, song output of males and their social rank (Otter
the wintering population of chickadees was et al. 1997)dominant males start singing earlier,
reduced by nearly 50% in 2005approximately sing for longer periods, and attain higher average
70 to 75 birds in the study area during the winter and maximum song rates than their subordinate
compared to normal years of between 120 and 130. flock-mates. During the springs of 2000 to 2003, we
The result, in 2005, was the lowest density of breed- recorded the dawn choruses of dominant and
ing pairs across sites (N 19) seen during all years subordinate males in either habitat. In the mature
of the study (average number of breeding pairs in forest, van Oort et al. (2006) found that the relative
286 L A N D S C A P E E C O L O G Y, B E H AV I O R , A N D C O N S E R VAT I O N

Young forest Mature forest

1.00

0.90
Proportion of Nests that Fledged Young
0.80

0.70

0.60

0.50

0.40

0.30

0.20

0.10

0.00
2000 2001 2002 2003 2004 2005
Year

Figure 17.4 Variation in the proportion of successfully fledged nests between young and mature sites among black-capped chickadees
breeding in northern British Columbia. Although the pattern of lower fledging success in the young forest is relatively consistent, the
magnitude of the difference between habitats appears to vary across years. The disparity appears to be greatest in years associated
with atypical weather patterns (see text). High local predation on nesting adults by a sharp-shinned hawk may account for reversal pattern
in 2003.

song output during the dawn chorus matched clutches (Chapter 5). Tobias and Seddon (2002)
expectationsdominant males had higher song suggest that rates of solicitation calling among
output than subordinate males. However, this pat- female birds may reflect relative hunger level; this
tern was not found in the young forest sites. appeared testable in female chickadees, which
Overall, males in the young forest had lower song often produce several hundred calls during 20-min
output during the dawn chorus than males in the observation periods conducted during the egg-
mature forest, but the differences was driven by laying period (Otter, S. Atherton, and H. van Oort,
abnormally low song output among dominant unpublished data).
males in the young forest (van Oort et al. 2006). A Solicitation calling rates of female chickadees
direct comparison of the dominant males between drop dramatically if the pair is provisioned with
sites indicated that males in the young forest sang supplemental food (10 min of feeding on meal-
at lower rates than those in the mature forest. worms), declining by nearly 50% compared to
Food solicitation calling: during the energetically- prefeeding rates (Otter et al. in press). Control birds
demanding egg laying period, female chickadees did not differ in call rates before and after being
produce a vocalization that appears to be used in presented with a similar feeder, but not being
soliciting feedings from malesthe broken dee allowed to feed. This suggests that the call rates
(Ficken et al. 1978; Smith 1991). Such solicitation may reflect immediate hunger levels in female
calls appear in other Parids, and food provisioning chickadees. We then compared calling rates
to females by their mates during the egg-laying between birds occupying the two forest types.
period may influence the females ability to initiate Females in the young forest had significantly
H A B I TAT Q UA L I T Y A N D R E P R O D U C T I V E B E H AV I O R 287

higher call rates than birds in the mature forest, but fledging (van Oort et al. in press). Feeding rates
this relationship was driven almost entirely by were recorded by observations of color-banded
subordinate birds. There was little difference in the males provisioning young and show a trend that is
call rates of females mated to dominant males similar in pattern to that for female call rates noted
across sites. Further, the difference between the abovethere was no difference in the number of
calling rates of dominant versus subordinate feeding trips made by dominant versus subordin-
females in the mature forest was less then between ate ranked males in the mature forest, but a large
females mated to subordinate males in the young disparity between dominant and subordinate pairs
forest. This suggests that there may be differences in the young forests. Dominant males in the young
in the relative hunger level (and possibly food forest had much higher provisioning rates than
availability) between the sites, and that it may be their subordinate counterparts, the latter of whom
disproportionately affecting the subordinate pairs had the lowest provisioning rates of all categories
during courtship feeding. of nests. It is worth noting that the analysis was
Territoriality: Fort and Otter (2004a) used both done using those subordinate pairs who had
radiotelemetry and observations of intrusions successfully managed to breed in the young forest,
during daily surveys to show that intrusions into as many subordinate pairs did not succeed long
neighboring territories were both more common, enough to produce nestlings in these years.
and more tolerated by residents, in the young forest We caught males during the early nesting period
sites than in the mature forest. All observations soon after feeding observations had ended,
suggested that these intrusions differ from those measuring these males condition using morpho-
associated with extrapair behavior in this species logical and haematocrit measures. The latter
(Smith 1988), which are surreptitious in nature and compares the proportion of blood volume that is
often only involve the soliciting female. Rather, red blood cells, and has been used as an index of
intrusions commonly involved both male and workload in different habitats in great tits (Ots et al.
female together and pairs continued to use contact 1998). The haematocrit measures may also be
calls as they foraged. Pairs were often discovered sensitive for assessing workload in chickadees, as
by residents, and not only tolerated, but the dominant males with their associated higher
residents often joined the intruders and foraged in metabolic costs consistently had higher haematocrit
flock-like formation. Such behaviors were not levels than their subordinate counterparts (van
witnessed in the mature forest. Oort et al. in press). In addition, blood haematocrits
These behaviors suggest that resources may were nearly 10% higher among males in the young
be limited in the younger forests, forcing some forest sites compared to the mature forest sites. The
pairs outside of their defended territories. Providing increase in haematocrits was parallel across
that these interlopers do not appear to be attempt- ranksdominant males in both habitats had
ing to usurp the territory (e.g. for nest site locations), higher haematocrit levels than their subordinate
the low resources in the habitat may make defense neighbors, but both rank classes had higher haema-
uneconomical and intruders are tolerated. The tocrits in the young forests than their rank counter-
elevated food solicitation call rates of birds in this part in the mature forests (van Oort et al. in press).
habitat during these same periods would further This would appear to indicate that males, regard-
support this interpretation. However, this behavior less of their social rank, may be induced to work
may itself be risky for residents if there is the harder in the young forest, which makes the high
prospect for intraspecific brood parasitism (see feeding rates of dominant males in these sites even
below). more costly.
Parental feeding effort and adult condition: we Taken individually, there are potentially alterna-
compared the feeding rates of males during tive interpretations that could be made to each of
the incubation and nestling period over 2 years the behavioral differences noted above. For example
to determine whether differences seen in the win- high ranked males in the young forest may not sing
ter and early breeding season persist through to at dawn simply because there isnt much advantage
288 L A N D S C A P E E C O L O G Y, B E H AV I O R , A N D C O N S E R VAT I O N

to itHansen et al. (2005) found that fewer males egg-dumping appeared to be a subordinate pair
are audible in these habitats during the dawn whose own nesting attempt failed parasitized the
chorus, so perhaps simply no one is listening. nest of their dominant flock-mates. In mature
However, females in this species appear to assess forest, nest abandonment by subordinate pairs is
males as extra-pair partners or divorce partners low and tends to occur during the latter parts of
during the dawn chorus (Chapter 14), so the reduc- incubation (K. A. Otter, K. T. Fort, and H. van Oort,
tion in singing by dominant males may be unpublished data); by comparison, many subordi-
ultimately disadvantageous. Alternatively, males nate pairs in the young forest sites abandon prior
may have to balance energy reserves more in the to completion of a clutch (Fort and Otter 2004b).
young forests, being unable to allocate large effort Thus, the potential for intraspecific brood
in their own advertisement in order to ensure parasitism is high. Although there is no difference
adequate food supplies to their mates through in clutch size between the two habitats (Fort and
courtship feeding effort. The higher workloads Otter 2004b), it is still unknown whether egg
indicated by haematocrits during the nestling dumping in the young forest is high and what, if
feeding period would support this increased any, impact this has on both reproductive success
energy requirement of birds in the young forests. In and future survival condition of dominant males in
poor habitats, dominant males may be forced to these habitats. Thus, even when birds successfully
allocate limited reserves to parental effort as fledge nestlings in the young forest, there are
opposed to extra-pair advertisement. Such a trade- numerous potential impacts to the condition and
off may explain the lower solicitation call rates of behavior of these residents. Although these
females mated to dominant males in the young for- impacts may have significant consequences to the
est sites contrasting with low dawn song rates by birds themselves, many of these will not be
these dominant males. If dominant males in young detected by simply censuses for presence/absence
forests allocate more of the apparently limited of breeding birds in the matrix.
reserves offered in these sites to parental effort, it
may also explain the greater disparity between
fledging success and feeding rates of dominant and
17.4 Parids and insight into the use
subordinate pairs in this habitat compared to pairs
of matrix in conservation planning
in mature forest. Thus, dominant males may adopt
a more paternal role to ensure within-pair repro- Parids are ideal in testing the potential of matrix
ductive efforts rather than capitalizing on extra- habitat to sustain or enhance breeding populations
pair opportunities that are common in mature of forest generalist birds. Most members of the
forests (Otter et al. 1998; Mennill et al. 2004). family are generalist enough to occupy a wide
Possibly, the high nest abandonment rates of the variety of habitats, ranging from pristine wood-
subordinate neighbors make allocating limited lands to suburban yards. However, as indicated
resources to siring extra-pair young undesirable. above, all habitats are not perceived equally by
Comparisons are underway to determine whether these highly matrix-tolerating bird species. If
there are differing mating tactics with respect to success is merely equated with use of these habi-
extra-pair behavior across the two habitat types. tats, presence in the matrix could easily be miscon-
If dominant males in young forests focus on strued as sufficient compensation for a small
within-pair parental effort rather than extra-pair allocation of remnant habitat. However, as the
matings, this effort may still be thwarted. work on European tits indicates, minor differences
Chickadees are known to engage in intraspecific in the vegetative structure between habitats can
brood parasitism, but do so at an extremely low have enormous implications on prey availability.
rate in mature woodlands (only two of over 110 The ability to adapt to these local differences
Ontario nests tested had young unrelated to appears to rely on the genetic isolation of popula-
both parentsOtter et al. 1998, Mennill et al. 2004). tions, which is antithetical to the objectives of using
The scenario in the Ontario population promoting a matrix to facilitate interpatch movement. Even
H A B I TAT Q UA L I T Y A N D R E P R O D U C T I V E B E H AV I O R 289

when genetic differentiation occurs, it may simply Blondel, J. (1985). Breeding strategies of the blue tit and coal
allow minor adjustments to living in a resource- tit (Parus) in mainland and island Mediterranean habi-
poor environment, but reproductive success and tats: a comparison. Journal of Animal Ecology, 54, 531556.
individual condition may still be largely depressed. Blondel, J. and Dias, P. C. (1994). Summergreenness, ever-
greenness and life history variation in Mediterranean
Research on chickadees suggests that similar
blue tits. In: Arianoutsou, M. and Groves, R. H., eds.
habitat quality issues can arise with the use of a
PlantAnimal Interactions in Mediterranean-Type
matrix of similar vegetation, but much different Ecosystems, pp. 2536. Kluwer Academic Publishers,
seral stage, from the remnant habitats. Netherlands.
This does not negate the importance of managing Blondel, J., Dervieux, A., Maistre, M., and Perret, P. (1991).
matrix habitat. The matrix likely serves a very Feeding ecology and life history variation of the blue tit
important role in facilitating movement between in Mediterranean deciduous and sclerophyllous habi-
patches, and creating habitat with structural matur- tats. Oecologia, 88, 914.
ity and/or similar species composition to the Blondel, J., Dias, P. C., Maistre, M., and Perret, P. (1993).
remnant forests should promote this function. Our Habitat heterogeneity and life-history variation of
research has also yet to directly test the halo effect Mediterranean blue tits (Parus caeruleus). Auk, 110,
511520.
of the effects of breeding on the edge, and
Blondel, J., Dias, P. C., Perret, P., Maistre, M., and
whether birds in these circumstances perform and
Lambrechts, M. M. (1999). Selection-based biodiversity
behave similarly to birds breeding in either pure at a small spatial scale in a low-dispersing insular bird.
habitat, or whether they are intermediate in nature. Science, 285, 13991402.
However, caution needs to be emphasized so that Blondel, J., Maistre, M., Perret, P., Hurtrez-Bousscs, S., and
management planners dont simply perceive Lambrechts, M. M. (1998). Is the small clutch size of a
matrix and remnant patches as interchangeable. It Corsican blue tit population optimal? Oecologia, 117,
is important to emphasize that the retention of a 8089.
species in a disturbed landscape does not necessar- Blondel, J., Perret, P., and Maistre, M. (1990). On the genet-
ily mean that the species is unaffected by disturb- ical basis of the laying-date in an island population of
ance. Retention is merely the first step in blue tits. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 3, 469475.
Braillet, C., Charmantier, A., Archaux, F., Dos Santos, A.,
managementit should not be the final measure of
Perret, P., and Lambrechts, M. M. (2002). Two blue tit
impact.
Parus caeruleus populations from Corsica differ in social
dominance. Journal of Avian Biology, 33, 444450.
Castelln, T. D. and Sieving, K. E. (2006). An experimental
Acknowledgements test of matrix permeability and corridor use by an
endemic understory bird. Conservation Biology, 20,
Z.McDonell, C. Holschuh, I.-J. Hansen, E. Brunsch,
135145.
S. Atherton, M. Kellner, K. Litwinow, and innumer- Cooper, C. B. and Walters, J. R. (2002). Experimental
able volunteers have contributed greatly to the evidence of disrupted dispersal causing decline of
work at UNBC. We also greatly appreciated add- an Australian passerine in fragmented habitat.
itional comments on the manuscript provided by Conservation Biology, 16, 471478.
Robert Curry and Marc Blisle. Our research is Davies, K. F., Gascon, C., and Margules, C. R. (2001).
supported primarily through the Natural Sciences Habitat fragmentation: consequences, management,
and Engineering Research Council of Canada. and future research priorities. In: Soule, M. E. and
Orians, G. H., eds. Conservation Biology: Research
Priorities for the Next Decade, pp.8197. Island Press,
Washington, DC.
References Dhondt, A. A., Adriaensen, F., Matthysen, E., and
Banbura, J., Blondel, J., de Wilde-Lambrechts, H., Galan, Kempenaers, B. (1990). Nonadaptive clutch sizes in tits.
M.-J. and Maistre, M. (1994). Nestling diet variation in Nature, 348, 723725.
an insular Mediterranean population of blue tits Parus Dhondt, A. A., Kempenaers, B., and Adriaensen, F. (1992).
caeruleus: effects of years, territories and individuals. Density-dependent clutch size caused by habitat het-
Oecologia, 100, 413420. erogeneity. Journal of Animal Ecology, 61, 643648.
290 L A N D S C A P E E C O L O G Y, B E H AV I O R , A N D C O N S E R VAT I O N

Dias, P. C. (1996). Sources and sinks in population biology. provision rates diet and breeding success in two species
Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 11, 326330. of tit (great tit and blue tit). Oecologia, 114, 522530.
Dias, P. C. and Blondel, J. (1996). Breeding time, food Ots, I., Murumagi, A., and Horak, P. (1998).
supply and fitness components of blue tits Parus Haematological health state indices of reproducing
caeruleus in Mediterranean habitats. Ibis, 138, 644649. great tits: methodology and sources of natural vari-
Ekman, J. B. and Lilliendahl, K. (1992). Using priority to ation. Functional Ecology, 12, 700707.
food access: fattening strategies in dominance-structured Otter, K., Chruszcz, B., and Ratcliffe, L. (1997). Honest
willow tit ( Parus montanus) flocks. Behavioral Ecology, 4, advertisement and singing during the dawn chorus of
232238. black-capped chickadees, Parus atricapillus. Behavioral
Ficken, M. S., Ficken, R. W., and Witkin, S. R. (1978). Vocal Ecology, 8, 167173.
repertoire of the black-capped chickadee. Auk, 95, 3448. Otter, K., Ratcliffe, L., Michaud, D., and Boag, P. T.
Fort, K. T. and Otter, K. A. (2004a). Territorial breakdown (1998). Do female black-capped chickadees prefer high-
of black-capped chickadees Poecile atricapillus, in ranking males as extra-pair partners? Behavioral Ecology
disturbed habitats? Animal Behaviour, 68, 407415. and Sociobioliology, 43, 2536.
Fort, K. and Otter, K. A. (2004b). Effects of habitat disturb- Otter, K. A., Atherton, S. E., and van Oort, H. (in press).
ance on reproduction in black-capped chickadees Female food solicitation calling, hunger levels and habi-
(Poecile atricapillus) in Northern British Columbia. Auk, tat differences in the black-capped chickadee. Animal
121, 10701080 Behaviour.
Gascon, C., Lovejoy, T. E., Bierregaard, R. O., Jr, Malcolm, Perret, P. and Blondel, J. (1993). Experimental evidence of
J. R., Stouffer, P. C., Vasconcelos, H. L., Laurance, W. F., the territorial defense hypothesis in insular blue tits.
Simmerman, B., Tocher, M., and Boges, S. (1999). Matrix Experientia, 49, 9498.
habitat and species richness in tropical forest remnants. Remes, V. (2003). Effects of exotic habitat on nesting suc-
Biological Conservation, 91, 223229. cess, territory density, and settlement patterns in the
Godfrey, J. D. (2003). Potential use of energy expenditure blackcap (Sylvia atricapilla). Conservation Biology, 17,
of individual birds to assess quality of their habitats. 11271133.
Science for Conservation, 214, 1124. Ricketts, T. H. (2001). The matrix matters: effective isola-
Gosler, A. G. (1996). Environmental and social determin- tion in fragmented landscapes. American Naturalist, 158,
ants of winter fat storage in the great tit Parus major. 8799.
Journal of Animal Ecology, 65, 117. Riddington, R. and Gosler, A. G. (1995). Differences in
Hansen, I. J., Otter, K. A., and van Oort, H. (2005). reproductive success and parental qualities between
Communication breakdown? Habitat influences on the habitats in the great tit Parus major. Ibis, 137, 371378.
communication networks in black-capped chickadees. Seki, S.-I. and Takano, H. (1998). Caterpillar abundance in
Acta Ethologica, 8, 111120. the territory affects the breeding performance of great
Lambrechts, M. M., Caro, S., Charmantier, A., Gross, tit Parus major minor. Oecologia, 114, 514521.
N., Galan, M.-J., Perret, P., Cartan-Son, M., Dias, P. C., Smith, S. (1988). Extra-pair copulations in black-capped
Blondel, J., and Thomas, D. W. (2004). Habitat quality as chickadees: the role of the female. Behaviour, 107, 1523.
a predictor of spatial variation in blue tit reproductive Smith, S. (1991). The Black-capped Chickadee; Behavioral
performance: a multi-plot analysis in a heterogeneous Ecology and Natural History. Comstock Publishing
landscape. Oecologia, 141, 555561. Associates, Ithaca.
Laurance, W. F. (2000). Do edge effects occur over large Stauss, M. J., Burkhardt, J. F., and Tomiuk, J. (2005).
spatial scales? Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 15, Foraging flight distances as a measure of parental effort
134135. in blue tits Parus caeruleus differ with environmental
Mennill, D. J., Ramsay, S. M., Boag, P. T., and Ratcliffe, conditions. Journal of Avian Biology, 36, 4756.
L. M. (2004). Patterns of extrapair mating in relation to Tabarelli, M. and Gascon, C. (2005). Lessons from frag-
male dominance status and female nest placement in mentation research: improving management and policy
black-capped chickadees. Behavioral Ecology, 15, guidelines for biodiversity conservation. Conservation
757765. Biology, 19, 734739.
Noss, R. F. and Cooperrider, A. Y. (1994). Saving Natures Thessing, A. (2000). Growth and final size of willow tit
Legacy: Protecting and Restoring Biodiversity. Island Press, Parus montanus under different environmental condi-
Washington, DC. tions. Ardea, 88, 215224.
Nour, N., Currie, D., Matthysen, E., Van Damm, R., and Thomas, D. W., Blondel, J., Perret, P., Lambrechts, M. M.,
Dhondt, A. A. (1998). Effects of habitat framentation on and Speakman, J. R. (2001). Energetic and fitness costs
H A B I TAT Q UA L I T Y A N D R E P R O D U C T I V E B E H AV I O R 291

of mismatched resource supply and demand in season- Tubelis, D. P., Lindenmayer, D. B., and Cowling, A.
ally breeding birds. Science, 291, 25982600. (2004b). Novel patch-matrix interactions: patch width
Tobias, J. A. and Seddon, N. (2002). Female begging in influences matrix use by birds. Oikos, 107, 634644.
European robins: do neighbors eavesdrop for extrapair van Oort, H. and Otter, K. A. (2005). Natal nutrition and
copulations? Behavioral Ecology, 13, 637642. habitat distribution of male and female black-capped
Tremblay, I., Thomas, D., Blondel, J., Perret, P., and chickadees. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 83, 14951501.
Lambrechts, M. M. (2004). The effect of habitat quality van Oort, H., Otter, K. A., Fort, K., and Holschuh, C. I.
on foraging patterns, provisioning rate and nestling (2006). Habitat quality affects the reliability of a condi-
growth in Corsican blue tits Parus caeruleus. Ibis, 147, tion-dependent signal in black-capped chickadees.
1724. Ethology, 112, 772778.
Tremblay, I., Thomas, D. W., Lambrechts, M. M., Blondel, van Oort, H., Otter, K. A., Fort, K., and McDonell, Z. (in
J., and Perret, P. (2003). Variation in blue tit breeding press). Habitat, dominance, and the phenotypic quality
performance across gradients of habitat richness. of male black-capped chickadees. Condor.
Ecology, 84, 30333043. Verhulst, S. and Tinbergen, J. M. (2001). Variation in food
Tubelis, D. P., Cowling, A., and Donnelly, C. (2004a). supply, time of breeding, and energy expenditure in
Landscape supplementation in adjacent savannas and birds. Science, 294, 471.
its implications for the design of corridors for forest Weise, C. M. and Meyer, J. R. (1979). Juvenile dispersal
birds in the central Cerrado, Brazil. Biological and development of site-fidelity in the black-capped
Conservation, 118, 353364. chickadee. Auk, 96, 4055.
This page intentionally left blank
SYNOPSIS IV

Landscape ecology, behavior,


and conservation issues
Andr Desrochers, Ken A. Otter, Marc Blisle, and
Jennifer R. Olson

Introduction and relevance of Parids for students of landscape


ecology, behavior, and conservation. We proceed
Conservation does not require landscape ecology, under two frameworks. First, we will look at Parids
nor the study of behavior. Given the frequent focus as a system with its intrinsic value for the advance-
of conservation on landscapes, however, and the ment of ornithology, with a focus on dispersal and
fact that population patterns in landscapes can be habitat selection. Second, we will consider the
difficult to interpret without an understanding of value of Parids as a model group for those inter-
the behavior of individual animals, the synergy ested in broad conservation issues.
between those fields also cannot be denied. We
believe that, as a result, landscape ecology and
behavior contributed greatly to the advance of Focus on the Parids
science-based conservation, especially in the last
Global perspective
two decades. For example, empirical and simula-
tion studies of individuals responses to landscapes Ornithologists are still just breaking ground in
demonstrated the influence of habitat loss on understanding landscape level issues with North
dispersal, metapopulation dynamics, and species American and other Parids. We saw from a broad
viability. Conversely, conservation issues have range of geographic locations that Parids studied
contributed greatly to the study of landscape ecol- in the Eurasian and in the North American often
ogy and behavior, simply because they fed those respond to edges, patch size, forest fragmentation
fields with a rich variety of problems to solve, and and habitat heterogeneity, especially in landscapes
substantial amounts of money to address those dominated by agriculture (Chapter 15). The Parids
problems. themselves may not differ much in their response to
Among its trickle-down effects, the synergy Eurasian versus North American landscapes, but
between landscape ecology, behavior, and conser- the studies of Parids landscape ecology does differ
vation has spurred advances in the study of Parids, greatly between those regions, at least in emphasis.
in areas that were not previously investigated in Work in the Eurasian is much farther along in
detail in this family, especially landscape and understanding habitat effects on breeding than it is
matrix use. In return, Parids may hold much in North America, even though Eurasian studies are
promise for the fields of landscape ecology, behav- largely limited to comparing birds among different
ior, and conservation because of their potential as oak species forests. The fact that North American
a model system. In this synopsis, we highlight Parids seldom opt for nestboxes in most areas (rela-
some key points of the latter three chapters, and tive to Eurasian ones at least) remains a major
propose further thoughts to not only advance the obstacle for the study of reproductive responses of
study of Parids, but also to emphasize the value these birds to phenomena such as habitat loss or

293
294 L A N D S C A P E E C O L O G Y, B E H AV I O R , A N D C O N S E R VAT I O N

degradation. However, work on black-capped during the first fall that these birds make their
chickadees at Northern British Columbia shows that primary, large-scale movement and we have little
those obstacles can be overcome. In contrast to the idea how they do this, let alone how this is affected
work on breeding Parids, North American studies by things such as fragmentation and disturbance to
of movement by individuals or flocks (Ohio, the environment. Further, we do not understand the
Alberta, and Quebec) are farther ahead than those significance of sudden influxes of juvenile chickadees
from the Eurasian. Yet, as is the case with studies from outside local populationstypically called
on breeding Parids, some key questions remain irruptionsseen at banding stations, and whether
unanswered, such as, whether habitat fragmen- these relate to factors associated with landscape dis-
tation impacts Parids more through reducing turbance (e.g. forest fires) and/or with (cyclic) vari-
access to habitat or by altering habitat quality. ation in habitat productivity (Fig. VI.2). The detailed
As this book illustrates, there is a very strong studies of Parid movement and occurrence patterns
legacy of experimental work with all aspects of discussed in this section of the book show that minor
Parid ecology. In the next couple of decades, gaps in forests do constrain Parid movements, but at
researchers working on Parids will evidently fill the same time, they clearly indicate that Parids can
many of the knowledge gaps pertaining to north- eventually reach even remote patches of forest. As a
ern/temperate locations. However, we need to put result, it is impossible at the moment to infer just how
things in perspective, and this requires taking into much impediments to individual movements scale-
account all regions of the geographical distribution up at the population level. That is especially true in
of Parids. Even though North American Parids North America, as reflected by the lack of a thorough
seem to respond to landscapes in much the same discussion of dispersal and its outcomes in this book.
way as Eurasian ones, this does not guarantee that North American research on Parid dispersal basically
Parid responses to landscapes will be similar in amounts to an old study of black-capped chickadees
other parts of the World, where most Parid species in Wisconsin (Weise and Meyer 1979), and a handful
occur but where landscape issues remain largely of banding studies mostly on irruptions by Parids
unaddressed in this family (Fig. VI.1). Studies of of unknown origin and destination. By contrast,
African and south-east Asian Parids hold much European researchers have produced numerous pub-
promise not only for the advancement of compara- lications, led by landmark studies of Wytham Woods,
tive or meta-analytic studies, but also as opportun- UK, and Northern Belgium. Yet, very few of these
ities to measure the geographic scope of landscape publications addressed the influence of landscape
ecology hypotheses with fresh, independent structure on dispersal processes and their resulting
evidence. Our understanding of Parids as a family occupancy patterns.
will remain grossly incomplete until we tackle Although the latter studies in Europe benefit from
several unstudied species in Asia and Africa. their extraordinarily long durations and associated
Additionally, new studies in Africa or Asia may large number of band recoveries and reobservations,
uncover unsuspected threats to populations of rarer there is no reason to believe that there is a European
Parids, or point to potential solutions for vulnerable monopoly on the study of Parid dispersal. Long-
or threatened species. Fortunately, only four of the term studies of Parids are becoming well established
Worlds ca. 65 Parids are currently listed by the IUCN in North America (Ohio, Massachusetts, Ontario,
(2004). All four listed Parids (Parus holsti, P. nuchalis, British Columbia) and should offer nice avenues for
P. semilarvatus, Periparus amabilis) are little-known (for the data-hungry topic of natal dispersal. At the same
this family) and endemic to south-east Asia. time, exciting new avian research tools, such as
microscale telemetry equipment and stable isotopes,
or clever low-tech experiments, may provide more
Dispersal
efficient methods to tackle natal and breeding dis-
As with most animals, the biggest period of move- persal questions without the need for long-term
ment in Parids is also the most poorly understood, datasets, thus bridging a knowledge gap between
and that is the period during natal dispersal. It is Parid studies of different parts of the World.
H A B I TAT Q UA L I T Y A N D R E P R O D U C T I V E B E H AV I O R 295

(a)

Species
1625
1015
69
15
0

(b)

Studies
721
56
34
12
0

Figure VI.1 Geographic patterns of Parid diversity (a) and landscape studies including Parids (b). Geographic regions in North America are
divided by states (US) and provinces (Canada) in (b) so as to more directly compare spatial distributions of the number of active studies on
landscape issues among Parids in relation to species distribution.

700

600

500
Individuals/100 h

400
Figure VI.2 Southward irruptive movements of black-
300 capped (black dots) and boreal (white dots) chickadees recorded
visually between August and November in Tadoussac, Quebec.
200
Observation effort averaged ( 1 SD) 913 142 h/year. The
100 proportion of juveniles (i.e. birds in their hatching year)
averaged ( 1 SD) 97 3% (n 7 banding years) and
0 86 13% (n 6 banding years) for black-capped and boreal
1994 1996 1998 2000 2002 2004 2006 chickadees, respectively. These data have been generously
Year provided by the Observatoire doiseaux de Tadoussac.

Until we collect direct evidence on movements of


Habitat selection at the individual level
juvenile Parids during the dispersal periods
(mostly in fall but also in spring, after flock break- Like dispersal, the problem of habitat selection has
up), we will have to contend with weak inference generated much literature, but again with an
on dispersal processes, based on postdispersal emphasis on occupancy patterns rather than
habitat selection (i.e. occupancy patterns). processes. From a pattern point of view, dispersal
296 L A N D S C A P E E C O L O G Y, B E H AV I O R , A N D C O N S E R VAT I O N

and habitat selection may appear as very different (Chapter 17), only to enter the subsequent winter
problems (typically with the latter focusing on again in poor condition, following an extra cost of
distribution patterns, and the former focusing on molting. Furthermore, low access to food mediated
dispersal distances). But from a process point of by social dominance has been shown to constrain
view, those two problems are closely linked and individual Parids, such as black-capped chicka-
face the same difficulties. In the case of Parids, dees, entering the breeding season to settle in
flocking, when it occurs, makes things even more lower-quality territories (Smith 1991). In habitats
complex, for habitat selection becomes a two-stage that are relatively productive (such as deciduous
process: first joining a flock (and its range), and, oak forests in Europe or the mature forests
second, partitioning the flock range into breeding mentioned in Chapter 17), the difference in quality
territories. As a result, dispersal activity probably between territories of dominant and subordinate
occurs not only in the fall, but also in the spring, as pairs may be negligible, and allow opportunity for
in cases where subordinate individuals are expelled success among young and subordinate pairs. As
from the flock home range altogether. subordinate pairs often constitute the majority of
Contrary to the study of dispersal, the study of a local population, the effect of their gaining oppor-
Parid habitat selection has flourished both in the tunity to breed successfully is not a minor consid-
Eurasia and in North America. If the selection of a eration. However, in lower quality habitats
place to spend the autumn and the winter has been (evergreen oak, or young forestsChapter 17), the
the focus of earlier work (e.g. marsh tit in Sweden, dichotomy in quality between territories secured by
Nilsson and Smith 1988, and great tit in Japan, dominants and subordinates may be more extreme,
Saitou 1979), Chapter 17 illustrates renewed especially in low productivity years. The result may
emphasis on the consequences of habitat selection be a larger contrast in reproductive success and
on breeding. Current evidence portrays the choice condition of birds based on rank in marginal
of flocking locations (in the fall) largely as habitats, which may be even more exacerbated in
determined by social factors (social dominance, highly fragmented habitats because of the travels
floating, and the availability of potential mates). costs imposed by multiple patch use (Hinsley
Dispersal decisions made in the fall are likely of the 2000). As a consequence, Parids of contrasting
utmost importance to temperate and boreal Parids. social ranks may adopt differing long-term repro-
Consider Olson and Grubbs outlined microclimate ductive strategies in different habitats to compen-
effects on thermoregulatory physiology in winter- sate for variation in habitat quality, but these
ing Parids (Chapter 16)this illustrates the fantastic studies are still in their infancy.
challenges that birds wintering in temperate/north- Whether habitat selection processes with strong
ern areas face. In fact, the potential of Parids to help demographic consequences occur mostly in the fall
us understand how birds cope with extreme or in the spring remains unknown in Parids, just as
weather remains largely untapped. Significant with most songbirds. However, as with dispersal,
advances on the physiological front (e.g. corticos- Parids lend themselves well to experiments on
teroid hormone levels, Chapter 3) are beginning to habitat selection, for example they sometimes use
show just how stressful disturbed habitats can be to nestboxes and generally accept supplemental food,
Parids (Lucas et al. 2006). at least at certain times of the year.
If habitat change is altering the evolved physio-
logical response mechanisms to deal with already
Parids as a model group
harsh environmental conditions, these can have
knock-on effects well beyond the direct challenges Although Parids as a group do not strike us as
posed by winter weather. For example birds requiring specific conservation measures, we argue
coming out of the winter in poorer body condition, in this section that they could offer key knowledge
faced with reduced food availability, may burn to address avian landscape and conservation
themselves out in egg laying and the provisioning issues. First of all, they are typical passerines
of nestlings, or simply abandon breeding altogether in the sense that they are small, short-lived, and
H A B I TAT Q UA L I T Y A N D R E P R O D U C T I V E B E H AV I O R 297

use all-purpose territories. Of course, the fact they level, even within a locality. The fact that they also
are short-lived is a blessing and a curse: one can take to habitat islands and can be experimentally
track differences over the course of individuals removed to look at recolonization rates, etc., makes
lifetimes, but at the same time one cannot track them a nice species for testing ideas about island
changes to individuals associated with longer scale biogeography and the utility of different matrixes
changes in the environment (e.g. rebounds associ- and corridors for facilitating movement.
ated with restoration projects, etc.) using individ- They lend themselves well to field experiments,

uals as their own beforeafter controls. particularly because of their frequent use of feeders
What landscape/conservation problems could (and sometimes nestboxes), but also because they
Parids help us solve? Although the last two decades are not cryptic and they are generally easy to cap-
have provided a plethora of information on bird ture and mark. For example the fact that they are
conservation issues, most of it is idiosyncratic and easy to track (relative to other birds) has resulted in
therefore of limited value for those in search of them being one of the best studied groups for small
general patterns. The effect of landscapes on birds scale movement in relation to habitat gaps and
is a case in point. Although we have solid evidence edges.
that birds do not generally use forest patches inde-
To illustrate the potential role of Parids as a
pendently of what happens outside those patches
model group for the study of avian landscape
(based on presence/absence patterns), we have not
ecology, conservation, and behavior, consider the
yet developed a solid understanding of the
effect of urban development on birds in North
processes underlying patch use. Several processes
American. There are a few studies on urbanization
undoubtedly lead to the frequent avoidance of
effects on birds, but many of the results are con-
small or isolated forest patches, but it would be
flicting, as are a lot of the studies on the effect of
useful to know under what circumstances edge
habitat fragmentation. Most forest birds tend to
avoidance, movement issues, conspecific attraction,
desert developing suburbs, but chickadees will
other unsuspected factors, or a synergy among
generally remain in those areas, thus providing
those factors is the driving force behind landscape
good opportunities to understand new opportuni-
occupancy patterns in most birds.
ties and challenges posed by urbanization (which
One of the major impediments to generalization
are easier to interpret at the intraspecific than at the
here is the overwhelming diversity in the life
interspecific level). Unlike most forest birds, chicka-
history of the bird species investigated. In this
dees persist in towns and cities (either through
regard, Parids can offer huge insights to working
immigration or reproduction), thus offering inter-
on larger issues associated with alteration to the
esting opportunities for comparisons between town
landscape (and climate), because they offer a
and forest. Being cavity-nesters, chickadees get
diversity of responses to landscapes, but at the
heavily hit by urbanization in terms of nest-site
same time share a lot of life history traits, making
limitation, which presumably explains why they
the diversity of landscape response hopefully more
appear to take more readily to nestboxes in sub-
manageable.
urbs. At the same time, they benefit from bird feed-
Parids have several things to offer as a model
ers. Fragmentation and other urban sprawl also put
group:
Parids much more directly in competition with
They are extremely well studied, and there is a lot exotics (house sparrows) for cavities, and also
known about their behavior, evolution, and exposes them to nest predators, such as house
ecological requirements. wrens, that they do not contend with when living
There are large numbers of long-term studies away from edges. However, the difficulty of setting
in areas that have undergone changes to the up long-term studies in urban settings has probably
landscape during the course of monitoring. limited these studiesfocus on urban parks or
They use a variety of forested habitats, inter- large campuses (such as the University of British
specifically, but more importantly, at the species Columbia, Canada) may help remedy this.
298 L A N D S C A P E E C O L O G Y, B E H AV I O R , A N D C O N S E R VAT I O N

References nance in marsh tits Parus palustris. Journal of Animal


Ecology, 57, 917928.
Hinsley, S. A. (2000). The costs of multiple patch use by Saitou, T. (1979). Ecological study of social organization in
birds. Landscape Ecology, 15, 765775. the great tit, Parus major L. II. Formation of the basic
IUCN (2004). 2004 IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. flocks. Journal of the Yamashina Institute of Ornithology,
www.iucnredlist.org. 11, 137148.
Lucas, J. R., Freeberg, T. M., Egbert, J., and Schwabl, H. Smith, S. (1991). The Black-Capped Chickadee; Behavioral
(2006). Fecal corticosterone, body mass, and caching Ecology and Natural History. Comstock Publishing
rates of Carolina chickadees (Poecile carolinensis) from Associates, Ithaca.
disturbed and undisturbed sites. Hormones and Weise, C. M. and Meyer, J. R. (1979). Juvenile dispersal
Behaviour. 49, 634643. and development of site-fidelity in the black-capped
Nilsson, J.-. and Smith, H. G. (1988). Effects of dispersal chickadee. Auk, 96, 4055.
date on winter flock establishment and social domi-
CHAPTER 18

What drives differences between


North American and Eurasian
tit studies?
Andr A. Dhondt

18.1 Introduction 18.2 Natural history traits determine


the ease of study and the questions
In comparing studies of Parid ecology, evolution,
asked
and behavior that have been carried out in Eurasia
and North America it becomes clear that there The types of research questions addressed by North
seem to be two essential factors that have driven American and Eurasian Parid researchers often do
differences: differences in species biology and not coincide. Rather than differences in interests
the possibility to carry out long-term studies. across continents, this may simply reflect the kinds
Differences in Parid evolution and natural his- of studies that are possible with the Parids that
tory between North America and Eurasia occupy the different regions. In Table 18.1, I have
have often dictated the types of research questions briefly summarized important differences between
that can be addressed. Long-term studies, many great and blue tit against the more derived tits, such
of which have now gone on for 20 or more as those more prominent in North America. Note
years, have made it possible to obtain answers that excavating a hole (rather than using an existing
before questions were asked. one), caching food in the fall to be used during
The phylogeny of the Paridae that Gill et al. winter (requiring an enlarged hippocampus), and
(2005) have developed (see Fig. 2.1) can be used living in pair or group territories during winter
to map different traits on the cladogram, (which leads to winter limitation) are all synapo-
making it possible to determine which traits morphies. Caching food and remaining territorial
are ancestral and which traits are derived. year-round evolved before the Baeolophus,
Based on this, the two main species that have Lophophanes, Periparus, and Poecile split off from the
been studied in Eurasia (great and blue tit) Parus group. One can speculate that both traits
are at the base of the Parid tree, while all evolved simultaneously to enhance winter survival
North American species are the result of two in response to a period of colder climate, and that
invasions 3.5 and 4 MBP (million years before taxa that developed these traits would have been
present) (Gill et al. 2005, see also Chapter 1). preadapted to invade North America across the
North American Parids, therefore, belong to more Bering Strait.
derived clades. Great and blue tit, the primary Among the Baeolophus, Lophophanes, Periparus,
study species that European scientists have been Poecile group, cavity excavation evolved multiple
studying as model species for more than 50 years times independently. Lhrl (1966) pointed out that
may, after all, not be representative for all tits and excavation among Poecile-tits (and nuthatches)
chickadees. occurs among smaller taxa. He suggested that the

299
300 N O RT H A M E R I C A N A N D E U R A S I A N T I T S T U D I E S

Table 18.1 Some differences between great and blue tit, and the more derived Parid clades

Ancestral Derived
(great, blue) (willow, marsh, black-capped, Carolina, crested,
tufted, boreal, etc.)

Breeding territory
General purpose territory (Type A), usually 0.51 ha (35 ha in Bialowica) General purpose territory (Type A), usually 110 ha
Winter social organization
Winter flocks with site-specific dominance Winter group territory: local breeding pair usually joined by non-kin
juveniles in 1:1 sex ratio
* leads to summer limitation, and high breeding density * leads to winter limitation, and low breeding density
Winter ecology
Non-hoarding Hoarding; leads to enlarged hippocampus
* migrates or uses human-provided food in cold winters * allows survival in extreme winter conditions
Nesting
Secondary cavity nester; non-excavator Some taxa excavate, but no adaptive morphological traits; excavation
has emerged multiple times, typically in smaller taxa in a
branch (Lhrl 1966)
Mostly single loader, many feeding visits by adults per hour Multiple loaderusually few visits by adult per hour
Song
Song with individual repertoire and multiple song types with small-scale Limited song repertoire but complex vocalizations, limited geographic
geographic variation of song types found in populations; variation; song sometimes lost (crested tit, boreal chickadee,
song output high chestnut-backed chickadee, Siberian tit)
*many studies of song and male quality * many studies of syntax

inability of the somber tit, one of the larger species density of, especially, great tits can be very high
among these, to excavate limited its distribution to when excess numbers of nest boxes are provided in
areas where great tits are rare, and therefore natural high-quality, food-rich study plots. In a 12.5-ha
cavities available. Although this might explain why study plot inside a large woodland near Antwerp,
somber tits have a limited distribution, I would sug- up to 58 pairs of great tits and 48 pairs of blue tit
gest that the selective pressure that has led smaller nested in a given year. The highest combined
Poecile tits and nuthatches to excavate (or frequently density in a single year was eight pairs per ha (52
excavate) their nests is more likely the result of great tit and 48 blue tit pairs). When birds use nest
interspecific competition with larger species for lim- boxes, nests can be found by simply checking these,
ited numbers of cavities. Note that these excavating and observations on the content is easy. This has led
species did not develop adaptive morphological to numerous, long-term studies with large or very
changes for excavation, thereby limiting their ability large sample sizes, whereby hundreds of breeding
to excavate to locations in soft or rotting wood, so pairs were studied by one investigative team.
that cavities are not really safe from predation Furthermore, nest boxes make manipulative experi-
(Christman and Dhondt 1997). ments relatively easy, leading, for example, to
Great and blue tit never excavate their own cavity, experimental studies of optimal clutch size, effects
and therefore rely on existing cavities (and are part of interspecific competition, effects of food manipu-
of a cavity guild; see Chapter 8). They prefer nest lation, etc.
boxes to natural cavities for breeding. Furthermore, By comparison, North American studies
in winter they usually flock in large groups that have often focused on wintering and flocking
roam over an area an order of magnitude larger behavior, foraging, vocalizations, dominance,
than a breeding territory. As a result, breeding with fewer long-term breeding studies, and few of
N O RT H A M E R I C A N A N D E U R A S I A N T I T S T U D I E S 301

Table 18.2 Number of publications from Science Citation Index Expanded of North-American (N. Am.) and
Eurasian (EU) titmice; more ancestral clades are at the top, more derived clades at the bottom

Genus Species name Region Number of publications

Cyanistes blue tit EU 343


Parus great tit EU 1307
Lophophanes crested tit EU 43
Baeolophus bridled titmouse N.Am. 6
oak/plain titmouse N.Am. 5
juniper titmouse N.Am. 10
tufted titmouse N.Am. 39
Periparus coal tit EU 71
Poecile willow tit EU 193
marsh tit EU 94
Siberian/gray-headed EU N. Am. 25 1
somber EU 2
black-capped chickadee N.Am. 193
Carolina chickadee N.Am. 44
mountain chickadee N.Am. 40
Mexican, chestnut-backed, boreal N.Am. 10

Search was carried out on 28 December, 2005.

those addressing questions of optimal nesting studies of interspecies hybridization being more
behavior. common in North America.
Vocalizations are quite different between great Another factor that may influence the choice of
and blue tit, on the one hand, and the more derived study species and number of studies is simply
clades on the other. Great and blue (and coal) tits relative abundance of ones study species. Great
sing a lot, both in spring and in the fall. Each tits are one of the best-studied species because they
individual uses many different song types, and song are abundant, secondary-cavity nesters living in
is sung in strophes. This has led European scientists diverse habitats. When searching in the Science
to study, primarily, variations in repertoire size and Citation Index Expanded (ISI Web of Science) for
in song production, and to relate this variation to publications that mention any of the titmice that
differences in male quality (McGregor et al. 1981; can be found in Eurasia or in North America more
Lambrechts and Dhondt 1986; Kempenaers et al. papers mention the great tit (1307) than all other
1997). The song of North American chickadees is species combined (1119) (Table 18.2). The blue
less variable, but other types of vocalizations are tit, another species that is mostly studied using
more variable leading to many fascinating studies of nest boxes, is the second most frequently studied
syntax (Chapters 1014). tit. Clearly the ease of studying a bird species
Another difference between North American and that readily breeds in nest boxes at high densities is
Eurasian studies is related to the number of Parid one of the factors driving the large number of
species with overlapping geographic distribution. studies. Note that the importance of these studies
In Europe there can be six sympatric species, while goes way beyond just understanding Parid
in North America there are rarely more than two, biology, as will be illustrated below. Among the
but many species have parapatric distributions. derived species the largest number of publications
This difference has led to studies on interspecific mention willow tit (193) and black-capped
competition being frequent in Europe, while chickadee (193).
302 N O RT H A M E R I C A N A N D E U R A S I A N T I T S T U D I E S

18.3 The value of long-term studies: Note that in his book, Lack (1966) described all 13
Providing answers to questions not bird population studies that had continued for at
yet asked least 4 years. Today, we have a few studies that have
continued for 50 years, and a large number that have
David Lack started his long-term studies of great studied great and blue tit for 20 years. This makes it
tits in Wytham Woods near Oxford with the explicit possible to study, for example, how birds respond to
assumption that what was true for great tits would environmental changes (Visser et al. 2003).
be true for other species as well. Lack was primarily In the next sections, I illustrate the fact that long-
interested in what factors regulated numbers (Lack term data sets make it possible to answer questions
1954) and adaptive explanations for clutch-size that were unanticipated at the outset of data collec-
variation. Lack was inspired by Kluyvers studies tion using three examples: (1) changes in spring
in the Netherlands that had started in 1937. phenology; (2) dispersal and local adaptation; and
Kluyver also had access to data that had been (3) effects of habitat fragmentation.
collected by Wolda who had the foresight to initi-
ate nestbox inspections as early as 1912 (Kluyver
18.3.1 Spring phenology
1951, p.3). Kluyvers work, in its turn, must have
been influenced by the pioneering studies of When Lack and Kluyver began their studies on
Margaret Morse Nice (1937, 1943), as there are great tit breeding biology more than 50 years ago,
many similarities in the questions addressed in her questions on possible effects of global warming, or
papers on song sparrows and Kluyvers 1951 great of large-scale climate fluctuations (NOA or El Nino)
tit monograph. In 1959, Jan Hubl in Belgium where not on the agenda of biologists (see also
started collecting the data that I later analyzed in Chapter 5). However, when in the 1990s evidence
many of my publications. Hubl explicitly modeled for important changes in phenology arose, both the
his studies on Kluyvers work, to the extent that he English and the Dutch studies provided important
used the same Wageningen nest boxes. information.
I guess this is where the funding comes in. Long- McCleery and Perrins (1998) showed that between
term studies cant be done without long-term fund- 1947 and 1970 spring temperatures had gradually
ing. By 1970, Hubl had distributed about 800 nest become colder and that great tits initiated laying
boxes, in eight study sites in and around Ghent. All gradually later; after 1970 however, as spring
boxes were monitored for breeding by himself, two temperatures warmed up the tits began to lay earlier
university paid technicians, and students, and earlier. The interesting part of the story here is
wherebyas in other studiesdetailed informa- that the relationship between spring temperature
tion was obtained on nesting through weekly visits and lay-date (or first-egg-date) was not different
to all nest boxes, all young were banded and most during the cooling period, 19471970, and the warm-
adults caught. Furthermore, all boxes were checked ing up period, 19711997. Visser et al. (1998), using
for roosting birds twice per winter. When I started the Dutch data, confirmed that from 1972 onwards
my own population studies near Antwerp in 1979, great tits began to lay earlier, that this was an adap-
I hung as many boxes as needed for the field experi- tive response to the temperature-driven earlier cater-
ments on competition with the caveat that my pillar peak, but that nevertheless the tits were not
university-paid technician and I would be able to able to fully follow this change in phenology and
collect all information (including individual that the magnitude of selection differential
weights of nestlings, and identity of all adults). representing the extent to which great tits mistimed
The fact that I knew that I would have full-time layinggradually increased over a 20-year period.
assistance for tens of years made it possible to start Taking advantage of the large number of long-
a study that needed to be long-term and is still term studies, Visser et al. (2003) compared changes
ongoing under the leadership of Erik Mathyssen in lay-date over a 20-year period in 24 populations
and still involves the same technician who started of great and blue tit in 14 study sites between
with me in 1978. Corsica in the south and Finland in the north,
N O RT H A M E R I C A N A N D E U R A S I A N T I T S T U D I E S 303

and between the UK in the west and Russia in be most productive. The inability of the tits to
the east. They found very large differences evolve to an optimal clutch in a heterogeneous
in changes in lay-date between sites, whereby in landscape was caused by the high proportion of
some sites lay-dates did not change, while in females that immigrated into each study plot, often
others lay-dates advanced on average as much as from sites with a different optimum. If gene flow is
0.67 days per year. The analyses showed that, in low and local selection pressures very strong, then
general, lay dates changed where spring tempera- strongly differentiated populations may arise as
tures had changed, while they did not change illustrated by Blondel et al.s (1999) studies on
where there had been no increase in spring Corsican blue tits living in evergreen and decidu-
temperature (for example in Russia and Finland), ous oak habitats. On Vlieland, another island, the
or where the phenology of the entire food chain optimal clutch size is lower than that on the adja-
(evergreen treescaterpillarsbirds) is less tempera- cent mainland 20 km away. Kluyver selected
ture sensitive (Corsica). One of the more surprising Vlieland to study the role of density-dependent
results was that in two neighboring sites just survival in population regulation and started a new
50 km apart (Antwerp, N. Belgium; Breda, S. study there in 1955, because it was the most remote
Netherlands), where the patterns of spring of the Dutch Frisian islands and few immigrants
temperatures changes had been very similar, were to be expected. The island comprises one large
changes in lay-dates were very different: the wood of 214 ha, and four smaller ones covering
Belgian population showed the strongest change in 78 ha together (Kluyver 1971). Postma and Van
lay-date over the 20-year period (0.667 per year Noordwijk (2005) showed that the clutch size in the
for great tit and 0.594 for blue tit), while the large area was significantly smaller than in the
Dutch population showed one of the weakest smaller ones. In the large area, low immigration
(0.177 great tit, 0.337 blue tit). Further analyses (13%) combined with strong selection against
suggested that another life-history characteristic immigrant genes maintained that subpopulation at
(% second broods) seemed to have a major effect on its adaptive value, while in the smaller areas the
changes in first-brood lay-dates. In populations proportion of immigrants from the mainland
with few second broods, first-brood lay-dates (which carry genes for large clutches) was too
rapidly changed with changes in spring tempera- high (43%) and the higher immigration rate pre-
tures. In contrast, among populations in which vented local adaptation. In contrast to these three
second broods were frequent, first-brood lay-dates examples, the fourth study in Wytham Woods,
did not respond to changes in spring temperatures, UK, showed that dispersal was non-homogenous,
but the % second broods declined. thereby enhancing evolutionary differentiation at
surprisingly small scales (Garant et al. 2005).
Because great and blue tit breed in nest boxes it
18.3.2 Dispersal and local adaptation
was possible to collect, in all four long-term studies,
Dispersal, causing gene flow, counteracts local very detailed information (lay-date, clutch size,
adaptation through natural selection via its hom- fledging success, individual nestling weight and
ogenizing effect. Long-term studies on great and size at fledging, identity and survival of parents,
blue tit in four countries have greatly contributed to and recruitment of fledglings to breeding) over
refine our understanding of this central theoretical extended periods for large numbers of breeding
concept in evolutionary biology. Inspired by Chris pairs, providing sufficient power for all these
Perrins concluding remarks at the 1989 Corsica analyses.
meeting on hole-nesting birds (Perrins 1990),
Dhondt et al. (1990) analyzed their data and docu-
18.3.3 Effects of habitat fragmentation
mented that, in high-quality habitat, great and blue
tits laid clutches that were smaller than what would Habitat fragmentation is a hot topic today, with
be most productive, while in low quality habitat tits several chapters in the book addressing aspects of
laid clutches that were larger than what would this topic. According to the ISI Web of Science the
304 N O RT H A M E R I C A N A N D E U R A S I A N T I T S T U D I E S

first paper that mentions this term was published when a Dutch colleague visited me in Belgium in
in 1984, with more than half being published in the the late 1980s and asked me if I had any data on tit
4-year period 20022005 (1055 of 2009 or 52.5%) populations in fragmented habitats I said: Sure,
(Fig. 18.1). Clearly, when the long-term studies about 20 years worth of data. I realized only after
were initiated the question as to possible effects of establishing a new set of nest box plots near
habitat fragmentation on populations and commu- Antwerp in 1979 that the reason the birds I had
nities had not been asked. One of the advantages studied near Ghent did not behave like the birds I
of studying woodland birds in northern Belgium is had read about in the literature was that they
that most wooded areas are small and isolated. So lived in isolated habitat fragments. In the Ghent
fragments, nestling mortality tended to be high and
nestling weights tended to be low even in high-
quality oak habitat (Table 18.3). Furthermore, the
most productive clutch size was much larger in
250 plots within large woodlots than in plots in
fragments. We also found that during winter, birds
Number of publications

200 that lived in fragments tended to flock less, leading


to a lower divorce rate (Dhondt et al. 1996).
Fragment size and isolation also greatly impacts
150
local recruitment rates (Matthysen et al. 2001).
The study system described in Chapter 8 was
100 established as a long-term study in the 1990s about
200 km south of Prince George in central BC. The
50 intent was to look at fragment size and logging on
nest-web communities. As one of the unexpected
0
outcomes, Kathy Martin suddenly found herself
1985 1990 1995 2000 2005 with a picture of community structure and nesting
Year published success of various cavity nesters (chickadees,
woodpeckers, nuthatches, etc.) before and after
Figure 18.1 Number of publications mentioning fragmentation
a pandemic attack of mountain pine beetles. The
according to a search on the ISI Web of Science on 28 December
2005. The first paper that mentions this term appeared in 1984; current infestation is unprecedentedwithin
more than half were published in the 4-year period 20022005 the next 10 years it is anticipated that 80% of the
(1055 of 2009 or 52.5%). lodgepole pine in an area about the size of England

Table 18.3 Effect of woodlot size on great tit and blue tit reproduction

Great tit Blue tit

F/E 15-day mass (g) Most productive F/E 15-day mass (g) Most productive
clutch size clutch size

Plots in large woodlot near Antwerp


B 0.90 17.64 12 0.85 11.15 14
TL 0.90 17.58 12 0.97 11.16 14
Isolated fragments near Ghent
Z (G for blue tit) 0.79 15.65 8 0.85 n.d. 12
H 0.80 16.02 11 0.75 10.6 9

Data from Table 1 in Dhondt et al.. 1990. F/E: fledglings per egg in successful first broods. All plots are predominantly oak, except plot H which is
predominantly beech.
N O RT H A M E R I C A N A N D E U R A S I A N T I T S T U D I E S 305

will be completely decimated. Because she had addressed concerning cost and benefits of flock
10 years of breeding data of all species before the joining and flock switching, effects of winter food
pandemic hit her site, Kathy Martin can now look and temperature on over winter survival, and more
at how chickadees respond to changes in other recently effects of fragmentation, winter food and
species densities. Long-term studies, wherever temperatures, and interspecific competition
they are carried out, always contribute to answers (Chapter 16).
of questions not asked at the onset. As landscapes In that context, a large number of experiments
become more and more fragmented, through what- have been carried out to determine the extent to
ever cause, a deep understanding of factors that which the winter flock territories actually limit
influence habitat quality gain in importance and breeding population density. The first removal
the older data from long-term studies combined experiment with a winter-territorial species
with more recent studies focusing on fragmentation (Cederholm and Ekman 1976) was carried out just
effects (Chapters 1517) may help us alleviate some before the breeding season and, to the surprise of
of its effects. the authors, showed no replacements (Table 18.4).
The authors concluded that there were no surplus
birds and wrote (p. 212): As our removals were
18.4 Winter group territoriality: A
done late in spring they do not, however, exclude
derived trait that leads to winter
the possibility that territorial behaviour limits
limitation
numbers earlier on. the question should be pur-
A marked difference between great and blue tits sued by further removal experiments outside the
and the species belonging to the Periparus, Poecile, breeding season. With the exception of Otter and
Lophophanes, and Baeolophus clades concerns winter Ratcliffe (1996) all other removal experiments in
social organization (Ekman 1989). Great tits, and this group of species were done in fall and winter,
probably also blue tits (although not well studied!) and nearly all showed that in these winter-
have a relatively loose organization characterized territorial titmice very few, if any, surplus birds
by semistable basic flocks which often intermin- remained alive by the beginning of the breeding
gle and live in overlapping home ranges (Saitou season. Hogstad (1989b) carried out a series of
1978). The other species live in discrete units (pairs removal experiments between September and
or small groups) with a linear dominance hierarchy February. In each instance, he removed a single
and high coherence among members. These winter bird from territorial groups and after a few days
flocks reside within non-overlapping territories released the removed birds back into their flock. In
that are actively defended. In Ekmans (1989) October and November all removed birds were
review of non-breeding social behavior in Parids, replaced by non-territorial floaters and the
he lists just eight studies involving great and four rereleased birds expelled the replacement birds
involving blue tits (together 18.5% of 65 studies), showing that floaters were subordinate individu-
compared to 53 (81.5%) in the more derived clades. als. Later in the season removed birds were no
These latter are about equally divided between longer replaced, indicating that no non-territorial
Eurasian and North American species. Studies of birds remained in the population. His data,
non-breeding social organization are thus rare in graphed in Fig. 18.2, showed very clearly how the
great and blue tit, but much more frequent in win- number of floaters, available to replace vacancies,
ter-flocking species, especially when compared to declined throughout winter, so that there remained
the proportions mentioned in Table 18.2 (68% of all no surplus birds by the time the breeding seasons
studies refer to great and blue tit). It is also interest- began.
ing to underline that 26 of 42 studies in Europe In stark contrast to these results, all removal
were carried out in Scandinavia (primarily experiments involving great tits (none were done
Sweden). In North America, black-capped with blue tits) resulted in rapid and often complete
chickadees and tufted titmice were studied replacement of the individuals removed. In the
most frequently and fascinating questions were Polish study, which was carried out in primeval
306 N O RT H A M E R I C A N A N D E U R A S I A N T I T S T U D I E S

Table 18.4 Results of removal experiments in Parids

Autumn/ winter removals


Ekman et al. 1981 Crested tit Removed birds partially replaced by birds from neighboring flocks,
suggesting no floaters
Hogstad 1989a Willow tit Birds removed in September all replaced by floaters
Hogstad 1989b Willow tit Birds removed in January not replaced
Hogstad 1990 Willow tit Replacement declined from 100% in Sept to 0 % from the end of December
onwards (see Fig. 18.2)
Ekman et al. 1981 Willow tit Fall removed birds more than replaced by known non-territorial individuals,
showing floaters
Alatalo et al. 1985 Willow, crested No (few) replacements
Alatalo et al. 1987 Coal tit No (few) replacements
Samson and Lewis 1979 Black-capped chickadee No September birds replaced
Smith 1987 Black-capped chickadee High-ranking winter removed birds replaced by floaters
Dolby and Grubb 1998 Carolina chickadee No (few) replacements
Samson and Lewis 1979 Tufted titmouse Some September birds replaced
Cimprich and Grubb 1994 Tufted titmouse No (few) replacements
Dolby and Grubb 1998 Tufted titmouse No (few) replacements
Spring removals
Cederholm and Ekman 1976 Willow tit, crested tit No replacements
Otter and Ratcliffe 1996 Black-capped chickadee Female movements between territories; no replacement by floaters
Krebs 1971, Krebs 1977 Great tit Rapid replacement; in 1977 study twice within 3 weeks
Wesolowski et al. 1987 Great tit Rapid replacement in natural habitat without nest boxes
Lambrechts and Dhondt 1988 Great tit Rapid replacement

100
80
% replaced

60
40
20 Figure 18.2 Seasonal variation in the percentage of willow tits
0 that were replaced after removal of single individuals from flocks at
different times during fall and winter. The results clearly show that
6 Oct

17 Oct

9 Nov

25 Nov

10 Dec

29 Dec

16 Jan

17 Feb

the number of non-territorial birds available to replace removed


birds decreases rapidly in late fall and early winter. (Data from
Date of removal Hogstad 1990.)

forest in which the breeding density was five to ten cost/benefit analysis individuals must make when
times lower than in the other populations, the deciding to join an existing group as a subordinate,
authors still found evidence for territorial exclu- or to wander around as a floater until it becomes
sion. In one of Krebss (1977) experiments all birds possible to insert oneself at a higher dominance
were replaced in two successive removals at the level with a good chance to breed (Smith 1984). In
onset of the breeding season. that context, very exiting work has been carried out
Winter group territories with a clear dominance by Susan Smith and by Laurene Ratcliffes group at
hierarchy provide an interesting system to address Queens University (Chapters 1 and 9).
the generally important question concerning causes, When comparing North American species with
correlates, and effects of dominance, and the Eurasian ones, one wonders why black-capped
N O RT H A M E R I C A N A N D E U R A S I A N T I T S T U D I E S 307

chickadees, for example, live in larger winter flocks (Mennill et al. 2004), and will desert their own part-
than willow tits, their Eurasian counterparts, and to ner if more dominant males are experimentally
what extent black-capped chickadees, in areas in widowed (Otter and Ratcliffe 1996).
which winter feeding is extensive, would be winter A comparison of North-American and Eurasian
limited as I concluded above. Well-timed removal Parids suggests that hybridization is more frequent
experiments could provide an experimental answer when species have parapatric rather than when
as to the effect of winter feeding on the size of they have sympatric distributions.
chickadee breeding populations (Brittingham and
Temple 1988).
18.6 Conclusions
To a large extent differences in questions studied
18.5 Studies of hybrid zones
among great and blue tit (largely studied in
Studies of hybrid zones provide important insight Europe) on the one hand, and other Parids on the
in evolutionary dynamics. Among Parids, many other hand (largely studied in North America and
hybrids have been described both among Eurasian in northern Europe) are driven by the fact that
and among North American tits (Harrap and when entire populations breed at high densities in
Quinn 1995). Some hybrids have even been given artificial nest boxes it becomes much easier to
names because they were frequent, such as collect detailed information, especially on repro-
the Pleskes tit, a hybrid between blue and azure duction, on large number of pairs. Although any
tits. Nevertheless, detailed studies of causes study we carry out has the ambition to obtain
and effects of hybridization and studies of hybrid insights that are generally applicable, it has been
zones in titmice have been carried out primarily questioned to what extent this is true for all aspects
among North American species (Chapter 7), deep- of studies on great and blue tit, and what they are
ening our understanding of factors that contribute representative for.
to shifts in hybrid zones. In Eurasian studies, obser- Thus Wesolowski et al. (1987) found that in the only
vations on hybrid pairs have remained mainly study on great tits in a primeval forest in Europe,
anecdotal. In his review, Randler (2002) pointed birds defended territories of 3 to 5 ha rather than of
out that various mechanisms might be involved in 0.3 to 1 ha as found in western Europe in mostly
hybridization including one species being rare, secondary or planted woodlands. Even in those low
mistakes in mate recognition, and supernormal density conditions, however, removal experiments
stimuli resulting in females selecting the male of during nest building or egg laying showed that terri-
the wrong species. Very elegant and probing North torial behavior still limits great tit breeding density,
American studies (Bronson et al. 2003a, b, 2005; implying that great tits are also summer limited in
Chapter 7) have shown that this latter mechanism native habitat. Tomialojc and Wesolowski (2004)
(female choice) seems to drive hybridization showed that in Bialowica forest bird species diversity
in the black-capped/Carolina chickadee system. was larger, but overall bird breeding density lower
Equivalent studies in Europe involve primarily the than in Western European woodlands, and suggested
cavity-nesting Ficedula flycatchers (Alatalo et al., that predation played a larger role there and limited
1994; Saetre et al. 1997). breeding density overall.
It is interesting to underline that black-capped Mller (1989) made the point that most people
chickadee females prefer the dominant male as a who study nest box populations remove old nests,
partner, regardless of whether the male is a conspe- and that therefore nest box studies largely ignore
cific or a Carolina chickadee. The strong attraction the role of nest parasites. Richners elegant experi-
of black-capped females for dominant males has ments have confirmed the important role of hen
been shown in diverse ways by Ratcliffes group: fleas and documented dramatic effects on the birds
females prefer dominant males as extrapair part- (Richner et al. 1993; Fitze et al. 2004). Mller was
ners (Otter et al. 1998), will excavate their nest thus correct in stating that by removing old nests
closer to the territory boundary of a dominant male selection pressures are changed.
308 N O RT H A M E R I C A N A N D E U R A S I A N T I T S T U D I E S

Although numerous studies showed that nest- Brittingham, M. C. and Temple, S. A. (1988). Impacts of
box size, and the size and location of the nest box supplemental feeding on survival rates of black-capped
and of its entrance hole, influences which species chickadees. Ecology, 69, 581589.
use them (e.g. Wille 1967, Lhrl 1977), most pro- Bronson, C. L., Grubb, T. C., and Braun, M. J. (2003a). A
test of the endogenous and exogenous selection
fessional studies have used boxes with a single
hypotheses for the maintenance of a narrow avian
entrance-hole size only, thereby favoring the great
hybrid zone. Evolution, 57, 630637.
tit. In a situation with variable entrance holes, Bronson, C. L., Grubb, T. C., Sattler, G. D., and Braun, M. J.
interspecific competition pressures may be relaxed (2003b). Mate preference: a possible causal mechanism
so that combining boxes with different entrance for a moving hybrid zone. Animal Behaviour, 65, 489500.
hole sizes can have dramatic effects on the breeding Bronson, C. L., Grubb, T. C., Sattler, G. D., and Braun, M. J.
density of smaller species (Dhondt et al. 1991). (2005). Reproductive success across the black-capped
Thus although studying cavity nesters in natural chickadee (Poecile atricapillus) and Carolina chickadee
cavities (as in most studies in this book) entails high (P. carolinensis) hybrid zone in Ohio. Auk, 122, 759772.
costs, the benefit could be that they study the birds Cederholm, G. and Ekman, J. (1976). A removal experi-
in their natural environment in which they are ment on crested tit Parus cristatus and willow tit P.
montanus in the breeding season. Ornis Scandinavica, 7,
exposed to diverse selection pressures. Even with
207213.
these challenges, there exist a fair number of long-
Christman, B. J. and Dhondt, A. A. (1997). Nest predation
term studies in North America (as described in this in black-capped chickadees: How safe are cavity nests?
book), but sample sizes are often problematic Auk, 114, 769773.
because of the enormous effort required to find and Cimprich, D. A. and Grubb, T. C. (1994). Consequences for
access nests, trap adults, or even band young. Carolina-chickadees of foraging with tufted-titmice in
Furthermore, because of the large territory sizes, winter. Ecology, 75, 16151625.
the probability to recapture Poecile juveniles after Dhondt, A. A., Adriaensen, F., Matthysen, E., and
dispersal is lower (Weise and Meyer 1979), so that it Kempenaers, B. (1990). Non-adaptive clutch-sizes in
is difficult to know which traits favor survival. In tits: evidence for the gene flow hypothesis. Nature,
great and blue tit, juvenile survival data are also London, 348, 723725.
Dhondt, A. A., Adriaensen, F., and Plompen, W. (1996).
less than perfect, but enough data are available in
Between- and within-population variation in mate
some studies to draw very strong inferences on
fidelity in the great tit. In: Black, J.M., ed. Partnerships in
selection pressures (Garant et al. 2005; Postma et al. Birds, pp. 235248. Oxford University Press, Oxford.
2005). Dhondt, A. A., Kempenaers, B., and De Laet, J. (1991).
Protected winter roosting sites as a limiting resource
for blue tits. Acta XX Congressus Internationalis
References
Ornithologici, pp. 14361443.
Alatalo, R. V., Eriksson, D., Gustafsson, L., and Larsson, K. Dolby, A. S. and Grubb, T. C. (1998). Benefits to satellite
(1987). Exploitation competition influences the use of members in mixed-species foraging groups: an experi-
foraging sites by titsexperimental-evidence. Ecology, mental analysis. Animal Behaviour, 56, 501509.
68, 284290. Ekman, J. (1989). Ecology of non-breeding social-systems
Alatalo, R. V., Gustafsson, L., Lindn, M., and Lundberg, of Parus. Wilson Bulletin, 101, 263288.
A. (1985). Interspecific competition and niche shifts in Ekman, J., Cederholm, G., and Askenmo, C. (1981).
tits and the goldcrestan experiment. Journal of Animal Spacing and survival in winter groups of willow tit
Ecology, 54, 977984. Parus montanus and crested tit P. cristatusa removal
Alatalo, R. V., Gustafsson, L., and Lundberg, A. (1994). study. Journal of Animal Ecology, 50, 19.
Male coloration and species recognition in sympatric Fitze, P. S., Tschirren, B., and Richner, H. (2004). Life
flycatchers. Proceedings of The Royal Society, London Series history and fitness consequences of ectoparasites.
B-Biological Sciences, 256, 113118. Journal of Animal Ecology, 73, 216226.
Blondel, J., Dias, P. C., Ferret, P., Maistre, M., and Garant, D., Kruuk, L. E. B., Wilkin, T. A., McCleery, R .H.,
Lambrechts, M. M. (1999). Selection-based biodiversity and Sheldon, B. C. (2005). Evolution driven by differen-
at a small spatial scale in a low-dispersing insular bird. tial dispersal within a wild bird population. Nature, 433,
Science, 285, 13991402. 6065.
N O RT H A M E R I C A N A N D E U R A S I A N T I T S T U D I E S 309

Gill, F. B., Slikas, B., and Sheldon, F. H. (2005). Phylogeny P. caeruleus) in relation to study plot size and degree of
of titmice (Paridae): II. Species relationships based on isolation. Ecography, 23, 3342.
sequences of the mitochondrial cytochrome-b gene. McCleery, R. H. and Perrins, C. M. (1998) temperature
Auk, 122, 121143. and egg-laying trends. Nature, 391, 3031.
Harrap, S. and Quinn, D. (1995). Chickadees, Tits, McGregor, P. K., Krebs, J. R., and Perrins, C. M. (1981).
Nuthatches and Treecreepers. Princeton University Press, Song repertoires and lifetime reproductive success in
Princeton, NJ. the great tit (Parus major). American Naturalist, 118,
Hogstad, O. (1989a). The role of juvenile willow tits, 149159.
Parus montanus, in the regulation of winter flock Mennill, D. J., Ramsay, S. M., Boag, P. T., and Ratcliffe,
size: an experimental study. Animal Behavior, 38, L. M. (2004). Patterns of extrapair mating in relation
920925. to male dominance status and female nest placement
Hogstad, O. (1989b). Subordination in mixed-age bird in black-capped chickadees. Behavioral Ecology, 15,
flocksa removal study. Ibis, 131, 128134. 757765.
Hogstad, O. (1990). Winter floaters in willow tits Parus Mller, A. P. (1989). Parasites, predators and nest boxes
montanusa matter of choice or making the best of a facts and artifacts in nest box studies of birds. Oikos, 56,
bad situation? In: Blondel, J., et al.., eds. NATO ASI 421423.
Series, Vol. G 24, Population Biology of Passerine Birds, pp. Nice, M. M. (1937 and 1943). Studies on the life history of
415421. Springer-Verlag, Berlin Heidelberg. the song sparrow I and II. Transactions of the Linnean
Kempenaers, B., Verheyen, G. R., and Dhondt, A. A. Society of New York, 4 and 6, New York.
(1997). Extrapair paternity in the blue tit (Parus Otter, K. and Ratcliffe, L. (1996). Female initiated divorce
caeruleus): female choice, male characteristics and off- in a monogamous songbird: Abandoning mates for
spring quality. Behavioral Ecology, 8, 481492. males of higher quality. Proceedings of The Royal Society,
Kluijver, H. N. (1951). The Population Ecology of the London Series B-Biological Sciences, 263, 351355.
Great Tit, Parus m. major L. Ardea, 39, 1135. Otter, K., Ratcliffe, L., Michaud, D., and Boag, P. T. (1998).
Kluijver, H. N. (1971). Regulation of numbers in popula- Do female black-capped chickadees prefer high-
tions of great tits Parus m. major. In: Den Boer, J. and ranking males as extra-pair partners? Behavioral Ecology
Gradwell, G. R., eds. Proceedings of the Advanced Study and Sociobiology, 43, 2536.
Institute on Dynamics of Numbers in Populations Perrins, C. M. (1990). Concluding remarks: dispersal and
Oosterbeek 1970, pp. 507531. Pudoc, Wageningen. gene flow. In: Blondel, J., et al., eds. NATO ASI Series,
Krebs, J. R. (1971). Territory and breeding density in the Vol. G 24, Population Biology of Passerine Birds, pp.
great tit Parus major L. Ecology, 52, 222. 475480. Springer-Verlag, Berlin, Heidelberg.
Krebs, J. R. (1977). Song and territory in the great tits Parus Postma, E. and van Noordwijk, A. J. (2005). Gene flow
major. In: Stonehouse, B. and Perrins, C. M., eds. maintains a large genetic difference in clutch size at a
Evolutionary Ecology, pp. 4362. University Park Press, small spatial scale. Nature, 433, 6568.
Baltimore. Randler, C. (2002). Avian hybridization, mixed pairing
Lack, D. (1954). The Natural Regulation of Animal Numbers. and female choice. Animal Behaviour, 63, 103119.
Clarendon Press, Oxford. Richner, H., Oppliger, A., and Christe, P. (1993). Effect of
Lack, D. (1966). Population Studies of Birds. Clarendon an ectoparasite on reproduction in great tits. Journal of
Press, Oxford. Animal Ecology, 62, 703710.
Lambrechts, M. and Dhondt, A. A. (1986). Male quality, Saetre, G. P., Kral, M., and Bures, S. (1997). Differential
reproduction and survival in the great tit. Behavioural species recognition abilities of males and females in a
Ecology and Sociobiology, 19, 5763. flycatcher hybrid zone. Journal of Avian Biology, 28,
Lambrechts, M. and Dhondt, A. A. (1988). Male quality 259263.
and territory quality in the great tit Parus major. Animal Saitou, T. (1978). Ecological study of social organization
Behaviour, 36, 596601. in the great tit, Parus major L. I. Basic structure of
Lhrl, H. (1966). Zur Biologie der Trauermeise (Parus the winter flocks. Japanese Journal of Ecology, 28,
lugubris). Journal fur Ornithologie, 107, 167186. 199214.
Lhrl, H. (1977). Nistkologische und ethologische Samson, F. B. and Lewis, S. J. (1979). Experiments on
Anpassungserscheinungen bei Hhlenbrtern. Die population regulation in two North American Parids.
Vogelwarte, 29, 92101. Wilson Bulletin, 91, 222233.
Matthysen, E., Adriaensen, F., and Dhondt, A. A. (2001). Smith, S. M. (1984). Flock switching in chickadees: Why be
Local recruitment of great and blue tits (Parus major, a winter floater? American Naturalist, 123, 8198.
310 N O RT H A M E R I C A N A N D E U R A S I A N T I T S T U D I E S

Smith, S. M. (1987). Responses of floaters to removal Variable responses to large-scale climate change in
experiments on wintering chickadees. Behavioral Ecology European Parus populations. Proceedings of the Royal
and Sociobiology, 20, 363367. Society, London, Series B-Biological Sciences, 270, 367372.
Tomialojc, L. and Wesolowski, T. (2004). Diversity of the Visser, M. E., van Noordwijk, A. J., Tinbergen, J. M., and
Bialowieza Forest avifauna in space and time. Journal of Lessells, C. M. (1998). Warmer springs lead to mistimed
Ornithology, 145, 8192. reproduction in great tits (Parus major). Proceedings of the
Wesolowski, T., Tomialojc, L., and Stawarczyk, T. (1987). Royal Society, London Series B-Biological Sciences, 265,
Why low numbers of Parus major in Bialowieza Forest 18671870.
removal experiments. Acta Ornithologica, 23, 304316. Weise, C. M. and Meyer, J. R. (1979). Juvenile dispersal
Visser, M. E., Adriaensen, F., van Balen, J. H., Blondel, J., and development of site-fidelity in the black-capped
Dhondt, A. A., van Dongen, S., du Feu, C., Ivankina, chickadee. Auk, 96, 4055.
E. V., Kerimov, A. B., de Laet, J., Matthysen, E., Wille, H. (1967). Selectieve nestkasten in Belgi. Vogeljaar,
McCleery, R., Orell, M., and Thomson, D. L. (2003). 6, 435448.
Index

Note: The terms Eurasian Parids and North American Parids have not been indexed as they appear throughout.

A note 168, 238 Asia 87, 88, 89, 107, 238, 259, 294 Pennsylvania 103
information and chick-a-dee aspen (Populus tremuloides) 229 song and call recordings 98
call: azure tit (Parus cyanus) 86, 87, song patterns beyond and within
basic note types 200 106, 107 hybrid zone 99101
identity 205 song patterns in Pennsylvania
signal redundancy 204 B note 161, 163, 171, 238 contact zone 1034
syntactical information information and chick-a-dee call songs and calls, lack of congruence
205, 206, 207, 208, 209 200, 201, 202, 204, 205, 206, 207, between 102
variability in note types 201, 202, 208, 209 sound analysis 98
203 Baeolophus vivii, ix, x, xi, 151 study sites 96
vocal production and perception differences between North behavioral modifications 2669
160, 161, 163 American and Eurasian Belgium 123, 252, 294, 300,
aberrant song 1034, 105 studies 299, 305 302, 303, 304
absolute pitch 155, 156, 157, 158 information and the chick-a-dee between song 193
acoustic plasticity 193 call 210 between-bird diversity values
adaptation, local 303 neurobiology of spatial behavior 1889, 190
adaptive specialization hypothesis 9, 10 bilingual songs 2, 96, 100, 101, 103,
26, 28 status signaling and 104, 105, 107
Africa 86, 87, 89, 238, 259, 294 communication networks 229 birch (Betula papyrifera) 229
age 2, 57, 134 Balkans 878, 89 black tit 238
photoperiodism and annual cycle banded wren (Thryothorus black-capped chickadee (Poecile
49, 51 pleurostictus) 222 attricapillus) vi, x, xi, 15, 71,
postdispersal social environment bark beetle 120, 149 75, 1489, 1512, 238
and acoustic variation in barrier effect 247, 252, 257 behavioral aspects of hybridization
birdsong 192 bee note 157 95, 103, 104, 105, 106, 107
social dominance and fitness 141 begging C 202 background and methods
alarm notes 211 behavioral aspects of chickadee 96, 97, 98
Alaska 1, 75, 147 hybridization 95108 results 99, 100, 101, 102
behavioral aspects of hybridization behavior and genetics, caching, spatial memory and
106 relationships between 1023 hippocampus 26, 27, 28
caching, spatial memory and chick-a-dee calls across hybrid corticosterone in Alaska and
hippocampus 278 zone 104 Colorado chickadees 345
corticosterone in chickadees 345 chick-a-dee calls beyond and within differences between North
phylogeography of chestnut- hybrid zone 1012 American and Eurasian
backed chickadee 78, 79, 80, confounding variables (potentially) studies 301, 305, 3067
82, 83, 84, 85 in vocal patterns analysis dominance, corticosterone, spatial
winter adaptations and habitat 1045 memory and hippocampus 35
fragmentation 264, 265, 268 Eurasian Parids 1067 edge, patch and landscape effects
Alberta 11516, 139, 2456, 254, 294 field methods 967 2445, 2478, 2512, 2549
AMPA receptors 17 genetic composition of study female eavesdropping 2247
AP5 17, 18 populations 989 global perspective 294
area parahippocampalis 17 genetic methods 97 habitat quality and reproductive
Arizona 86, 106 hybrid index scores 978 behavior 283, 284, 286

311
312 INDEX

black-capped chickadee (Cont.) studies 299305, 307, 308 brood parasitism 288
habitat selection at individual edge, patch and landscape effects brown creeper (Certhia americana)
level 296 245, 247, 24950, 252, 253, 259 79, 210
information and the chick-a-dee in evergreen versus deciduous oak brown-capped chickadee vi, 75, 77,
call 199200, 202, 20410 forests 2802 106, 238
irruptive movements 295 habitat quality and reproductive bushtit (Psaltriparus minimus) 79
male eavesdropping 2223 behavior 284 bushy-tailed woodrat (Neotoma
methodological advances 237 neurobiology of spatial behavior cinerea) 115
neurobiology of spatial behavior 9, 17
1117 phylogeography 86, 87, 88, 90 C note 161, 163, 238
neurogenesis in hippocampus postdispersal social environment information and chick-a-dee call
1819 and acoustic variation 194 210
phylogeography 77, 78, 79, 85, 86, reproduction 55, 56, 61, 67 basic note types 200
89, 90 climatic variations 63, 64, 65 signal redundancy 204
postdispersal social environment extrinsic factors 57, 58, 59 syntactical information 205, 206,
and acoustic variation 183, future effects on adult fitness 207, 208, 209
1856, 1902, 1945 components 62, 63 variability in note types
reproduction 556, 58, offspring production and 201, 202, 203
603, 65, 67 recruitment 61 caching 3, 325, 268, 299
status signaling and communication population adaptations to interspecific differences 256
networks 215, 230 habitat 60 intraspecific differences 269
communication networks 222, social dominance and fitness 132, seasonality 29
223, 227, 228, 229 136, 139 see also food storing
context of singing 218, 219, 220, status signaling and California 78, 82, 83, 106, 112, 251,
221 communication networks 255
fee-bee song 216, 217 219, 230 calls 95
unpredictable food, corticosterone, winter adaptations and habitat see also vocal production and
spatial memory and fragmentation 267, 270 perception
hippocampus 30, 31 bokharensis group 86 Canada 60, 65, 75, 77, 78
vocal communication complexities boreal chickadee (Poecile hudsonica) see also specific places
235 1, 106, 230, 238, 295 Canary Islands 867, 88, 89, 90
vocal production and perception edge, patch and landscape effects canary (Serinus canaria) 179
153, 154, 155, 156, 157, 15860, 247, 251, 255 cannabinoid receptors 18, 19
161, 162, 164 phylogeography 77, 78, 79, 83, Cardueline finch 44
winter adaptations and habitat 845, 86, 89, 90 Carolina chickadee (Poecile
fragmentation 263, 264, 265, breeding 2834 carolinensis) 1, 2, 4, 75,
268, 269, 270 costs, future 623 148, 151
see also demography; gargle call; density 300 behavioral aspects of hybridization
photoperiodism; social territory 300 95, 103, 104, 105, 107
dominance and fitness see also reproduction background and methods 96, 97,
black-crested titmouse (Baeolophus bridled titmouse (Baeolophus 98
atricristatus) x, 106, 107 wollweberi) 16, 26, 106 results 99, 100, 101, 102
black-lored tit (Parus xanthogenys) British Columbia 1, 2, 76, 147, 149, caching, spatial memory and
238 255, 294 hippocampus 29
blackbird (Turdus merula) 16, 219 demography, nest web call characteristics, sharing
blood parasites 59 communities and changing of 174
blue tit (Cyanistes caeruleus) 4, 71, 149 habitat 111, 112, 113, differences between North
behavioral aspects of hybridization 116, 117 American and Eurasian
106, 107 differences between North studies 307
caching, spatial memory and American and Eurasian dominance, corticosterone, spatial
hippocampus 26 studies 3045 memory and hippocampus 35
comparison with great tit 282 habitat quality and reproductive edge, patch and landscape effects
demography, nest web behavior 283, 284, 286 245, 251, 255, 256, 258
communities and changing phylogeography 78, 79, 80, 81, 82, information and the chick-a-dee
habitat 116 83, 84, 85, 90 call 199, 210
differences between North reproduction 58 basic note types 200
American and Eurasian broken dee note 202, 286 identity 205
INDEX 313

signal redundancy 204 context 2078 D note 160, 161, 163, 171, 238
syntactical information 206, 207, identity 2045 behavioral aspects of hybridization
208, 209 note types, variability in 2004 96, 98, 102, 104, 105
variability in note types 201, 202, playback studies 209 information and the chick-a-dee
203 signal redundancy 204 call 200, 204, 205
phylogeography 77, 85, 86, syntax 2057 syntactical information 205, 206,
87, 89 chick-a-fee-bee note 186 207, 208, 209
postdispersal social environment chip note 202, 208 variability in note types 201,
and acoustic variation 194 cinereus group 86, 107 202, 203
reproduction 56 climatic variation 55 dark-eyed junco (Junco hyemalis)
social dominance and fitness 139 cyclical 634 1314, 16, 17, 26
status signaling and directional 645 dawn chorus 2202, 2856, 288
communication networks 229 clutch size 556, 60, 303 deciduous downy oak (Quercus
vocal production and perception coal tit (Periparus ater) 25 pubescens) 2801, 282, 283, 284,
160, 161 behavioral aspects of hybridization 296, 303
winter adaptations and habitat 106, 107 dee call 2, 148, 202
fragmentation 264, 267, 270, caching, spatial memory and demography of sympatric mountain
271, 272, 273 hippocampus 26 and
caterpillars 2801 differences between North black-capped chickadees under
cavity: American and Eurasian changing habitat condition
characteristics 11820 studies 301 11128
excavation 299300 edge, patch and landscape effects excavation history, nest tree and
roosting 2689 244, 245, 247, 249, 253 cavity characteristics 11820
and singing behavior 2279 neurobiology of spatial behavior facilitators 115
cell proliferation 367, 38 11, 12 interspecific interactions 11516
chestnut-backed chickadee (Poecile reproduction 59, 64 nest web, Parids as members of
rufescens) 1, 75, 106, 1478, winter adaptations and habitat 11115
230, 238 fragmentation 267, 270 population growth models 118
edge, patch and landscape effects Colorado 278, 345, 115 predators 115
251, 255 see also Fort Collins site level changes using population
see also phylogeography communication networks see status growth models 1208
chick note 202, 208, 209 signaling and communication study area and habitat change
chick-a-dee call 2, 4, 148, 151, 236, 238 networks 11617
behavioral aspects of hybridization condition 1356, 287 Denmark 229
96, 98, 102, 105 condition-dependent behaviors density of species 270
call characteristics, sharing of 167, 2858 dialects 195
168, 171 conformity 194 disjunct populations in
photoperiodism and annual Connecticut 249, 251 chestnut-backed chickadee 90
cycle 48 conservation 4 dispersal 90, 2945, 303
postdispersal social environment context 2078 diurnal countersinging interactions
and acoustic variation continuous assessment hypothesis 21720
186, 191 138, 139 dominance hierarchy 358, 75, 296
status signaling and Corsica 55, 59, 65, 2802, 3023 call characteristics, sharing of 174,
communication networks corticosterone 19, 2938, 73 179
215, 220, 222, 230 Corvids 9, 16, 26, 236 habitat quality and reproductive
vocal communication complexities crested tit (Lophophanes cristatus) behavior 287
235 1, 9, 25, 62, 106, 123 photoperiodism and annual cycle
vocal production and perception edge, patch and landscape effects 456, 49, 51
15960, 161, 162, 163, 164 244, 245, 247, 250, 252, 253 reproduction 62
see also chick-a-dee call and winter adaptations and habitat see also social dominance
information fragmentation 267, 270 Douglas-fir (Pseudotsuga menziesii)
chick-a-dee call and information cross-fostering 107 112, 116, 119, 283
199211 Crossbill (Loxia species) 44 downy woodpecker (Picoides
across hybrid zone 104 crow 236 pubescens) 111, 11315, 11823,
basic note types 200 Cyanistes ix, 1 1267, 149
beyond and within hybrid zone cynomolgus monkey (Macaca winter adaptations and habitat
1012 fascicularis) 13 fragmentation 271, 272, 273
314 INDEX

eastern bluebird (Sialia sialis) 56 finch see Cardueline; gray-crowned contexts of use 1745
eastern whipbird (Phsophodes rosy; house; zebra description of call 168
olivaceous) 215 Finland 106, 252, 253, 270, 3023 features of call and population
ecological adaptations 2669 first fight hypothesis 137, 138, 139 patterns 1714
edges 2437, 271 first-order uncertainty 206 persistence and change in calls
avoidance 248 fitness see social dominance and over time 173
food abundance 244 fitness year-to-year changes in
habitat quality and reproductive flycatcher (Ficedula) 307 repertoires of individuals
behavior 277 food availability 567, 65, 66, 244 1734
microclimate 2445 food solicitation calling 2867, 288 interactive playback experiment I
movement corridors 2457 food storing 914, 723 175
predator avoidance 245 cache sites, memory for 11 interactive playback experiment II
egg dumping 288 caches, memory for versus other 1767
El Nio Southern Oscillation 63 spatial locations 1112 ontogeny of call 16871
energy limitation hypothesis 59 seasonal changes 4950 synthesis of theory and empirical
estradiol 51 spatial memory in versus results 17880
Eurasian starling (Sturnus vulgaris) non-storing birds 1214 genetics 57, 8890, 989, 1023
59 in the wild 911 genotyping 80
European nightingale (Luscinia food supply, fluctuating 2 geographic location of population 3
megarhynchos) 216 food supply, unpredictable 2932 Germany 107, 282
European nuthatch (Sitta europaea) food/energy limitation 589 glacial refugia 89
270 foraging behavior 268 global distributions vivii
European robin (Erithacus rubecula) forest: global perspective 2934
219 mature 2834 glucocorticoids 25, 33
European starling (Sturnus vulgaris) patch size 24951 glutamate 17
434, 48, 49, 115, 179, 183 young 2834 golden-crowned kinglet (Regulus
evergreen holm oak (Quercus ilex) fragmentation 27880 satrapa) 210
2802, 284, 296, 303 see also habitat fragmentation gonad development 44, 45, 47, 48, 49,
excavation history 11820 France 55, 57, 5960, 645, 86, 88, 51, 57, 65
2801 gonadotropin-releasing hormone
faint fee-bee call 48, 221 frequency matching/shifting 217, (GnRH) system 44, 45
fee-bee song 4, 151, 152, 236, 238 219, 220, 229 gray jay (Perisoreus canadensis) 270
behavioral aspects of hybridization gray-crowned rosy finch (Leucosticte
96, 99, 100 gargle call 4, 151, 236, 238 tephrocotis) 162
call characteristics, sharing of 167, behavioral aspects of hybridization grey-headed chickadee (see also
168, 171 96 Siberian tit) x, xi, 25, 238
information and the chick-a-dee information and the chick-a-dee behavioral aspects of hybridization
call 199 call 205 106
photoperiodism and annual photoperiodism and annual cycle phylogeography 77, 85, 86, 87
cycle 48 48 great horned owl (Bubo virginianus)
postdispersal social environment postdispersal social environment 210
and acoustic variation 183, and acoustic variation 191 Great Marsh 967, 98, 99, 101, 103
186, 191, 194 status signaling and Great Plains 194
social dominance and fitness 137 communication networks great tit (Parus major) x, 71, 149, 237,
status signaling and 215, 220, 221, 230 238
communication networks vocal communication complexities behavioral aspects of hybridization
220, 222 235 106, 107
variation on two-note theme 21517 vocal production and perception call characteristics, sharing of 179
vocal communication complexities 155 comparison with blue tit 282
235 see also gargle call of black-capped demography, nest web
vocal production and perception chickadees; sub-gargle communities and changing
153, 154, 155, 162, 164 gargle call of black-capped chickadees habitat 116
fee-bee-bee call 99, 156 16780 differences between North
fee-bee-ee call 100 aviary experiments on gargle American and Eurasian
fee-fee-bee call 156 changes 1756 studies 299305, 307, 308
female eavesdropping in combined analysis of interactive edge, patch and landscape effects
black-capped chickadees 2247 playback tests 1778 244, 250, 252, 253, 254
INDEX 315

habitat quality and reproductive comparative studies 16 lidocaine 17


behavior 280, 281 interspecific differences 256 lifetime reproductive success 132
Japan 296 intraspecific differences 269 lodgepole pine (Pinus contorta) 112,
neurobiology of spatial behavior 9, neurogenesis in black-capped 116, 11922, 124, 149, 283, 304
12, 16, 17 chickadee 1819 long-tailed tit 248, 249, 252, 253, 256
photoperiodism and annual cycle seasonal changes 4950 long-term potentiation 15, 17
43, 47 spatial information, neural long-term studies 299, 3025
phylogeography 86, 88 processing of 1618 Lophophanes viii, 9, 299, 305
postdispersal social environment volume and seasonal variation Louisiana 258
and acoustic variation 194, 2930 low fee-bee note 186
195 house finch (Carpodacus mexicanus) LRS 143
reproduction 5562, 65, 67 265 luteinizing hormone 65
social dominance and fitness 132, house sparrow 19
135, 136, 139 house wren (Troglodytes aedon) 270 Machlolophus ix
status signaling and HVC 18, 29, 45, 48, 49, 71 magpie 236
communication networks 217, hybrid spruce (Picea glauca x Picea Maine 254, 258
219, 221, 223, 226, 229 engelmannii) 116, 119, 283 mainland populations and
vocal production and perception hybrid zones 307 chestnut-backed chickadee 90
164 hybridization 756 major group 86
winter adaptations and habitat see also behavioral aspects of male eavesdropping in black-capped
fragmentation 264, 267, 270 chickadee hybridization chickadees 2223
green alder (Almis crispa) 283 marsh tit (Poecile palustris) 11, 25
grey tit 238 Iberian Peninsula 87, 89 behavioral aspects of hybridization
Groundpecker (Parus humilis) 9 Idaho 81 106
identity 2045 caching, spatial memory and
habitat: Illinois 251 hippocampus 26
fragmentation 3035 immediate early genes 17 dominance, corticosterone,
see also winter adaptations and India 238 spatial memory and
habitat fragmentation internal information 204 hippocampus 35
management 4 interpatch movement 2734 edge, patch and landscape
quality and reproductive behavior irruptions 294, 295 effects 247, 248, 251, 252,
27789 island populations and 254, 256
fragmentation, matrix and halo chestnut-backed chickadee 90 information and the chick-a-dee
effects 27880 Islands North see Fort Collins call 207
matrix: habitat of similar species Italy 86, 88 marsh tit (Cont.)
but dissimilar age 2828 neurobiology of spatial behavior
matrix of similar age or structure Japan 249, 250, 251, 296 11, 16, 17
but dissimilar species Japanese chinquapin (Castanopsis photoperiodism and annual
composition 2802 cuspidata) 11 cycle 47
matrix, use of in conservation Japanese quail (Coturnix japonica reproduction 57, 60
planning 2889 japonica) 44, 265 social dominance and fitness 135
selection at individual level 2956 Japanese tit 107 status signaling and
hairy woodpecker (Picoides villosus) jay 16, 236 communication networks
83, 114, 118 gray 270 229
halo effects 27880 ground 9 Sweden 296
Harris sparrow (Zonotrichia querula) Stellers 83 unpredictable food, corticosterone,
216 juniper titmouse (Baeolophus ridgwayi) spatial memory and
harsh dee note 202 x, 60, 106, 264, 266 hippocampus 31
hatching/fledging success 61 Maryland 253, 254, 255, 258
Hawk Mountain Sanctuary 967, 98, Kentucky warbler (Opornis formosus) Massachussetts 132, 194, 249, 251,
99, 101, 103 216 294
hermit warbler 84 kinglets (Regulus spp) 79 matrix 4, 27880
hierarchy formation and maintenance habitat of similar species but
1379 landscapes 248, 2527 dissimilar age 2828
high zee note 168, 171 lay-date 3023 of similar age or structure but
hippocampus 3, 9, 1419, 20, 328, laying, timing of 72 dissimilar species composition
45, 71, 723 leader-follower patterns 2578 2802
316 INDEX

matrix (Cont.) see also demography of sympatric oak forest 282


use in conservation planning mountain and black-capped see also deciduous downy oak;
2889 chickadees evergreen holm oak
vulnerability index 2789 mountain pine beetle (Dendroctonus oak titmouse (Baeolophus inornatus) x,
Melaniparus viiiix ponderosae) 76, 117, 1214, 60, 106, 229
metabolic adjustments, seasonal 1267, 3045 offspring food supply,
2635 movement corridors 2457 synchronization with 601
methodological advances 237 offspring production and recruitment
Mexican chickadee (Poecile gambeli) natural history traits 299301 602
1, 5 Nepal 106 Ohio 245, 256, 270, 294
behavioral aspects of hybridization nest density 1218 Oklahoma 253, 255
106 nest tree characteristics 11820 Ontario 14950, 294
caching, spatial memory and nesting 300 edge, patch and landscape effects
hippocampus 26 Netherlands 56, 57, 249, 250, 251, 253, 251, 255
information and the chick-a-dee 254 habitat quality and reproductive
call 199, 202, 207, 208, 210 differences between North behavior 288
neurobiology of spatial American and Eurasian photoperiodism and annual cycle
behavior 16 studies 302, 303 456
phylogeography 77, 86, 89, 90 reproduction 55, 61, 64 postdispersal social environment
status signaling and winter adaptations and habitat and acoustic variation 1945
communication networks 229 fragmentation 270 reproduction 58, 63
Mexico 86 neurogenesis 3, 2930 social dominance and fitness 132, 139
Michigan 251 neuron number and seasonal status signaling and
Michoacan 86 variation 2930 communication networks
microclimate 2445 neuropeptide Y 17 216, 217, 229
Minnesota 139 neurophysiology 3 Oregon 79, 80, 82, 83, 85, 255
minor group 86 New Jersey 2445, 249, 251, 253, 254, over-wintering in heterospecific
Missouri 96, 1034 255, 268 flocks 2667
mitochondrial DNA 77 New Mexico 115 overlapping geographic distribution
mixed songs 105 New York 264 230, 301
mobbing calls 208, 209 Newfoundland 86, 89, 90 owl 210
mountain chickadee (Poecile sclateri) nightingales 219 pygmy 245
2, 1489 NMDA receptors 17, 18, 19
behavioral aspects of hybridization nocturnal hypothermia 2635 Palearctic Parids 86
105, 106 Nolde Forest 96105passim paper birch (Betula papyrifera) 283
caching, spatial memory and nomenclatural distinctions viixi parahippocampalis 15
hippocampus 29 normal song 215 Pardaliparus viii
corticosterone, food caching, North Atlantic Oscillation 55, 634 parental feeding effort 287
spatial memory and North Carolina 249, 251, 253, 254, 255 pair formation 46
hippocampus 32, 33, 34 North Cascades 81 parrot 236
dominance, corticosterone, spatial northern flicker (Colaptes auratus) Periparus amabilis 294
memory and hippocampus 357 114, 118 Periparus ater 10
edge, patch and landscape northern flying squirrel (Glaucomys Cyanistes caeruleus 10
effects 255 sabrinus) 115 Parus major affinis 86
information and the chick-a-dee northern mockingbird (Mimus Parus major weigoldicus 86
call 202, 204, 206, 208 polyglottos) 179, 236 Poecile rufescens barlowi 79
neurobiology of spatial northern pygmy owl (Glaucidium Poecile neglectus 79
behavior 17 gnoma) 210 Poecile rufescens 79
phylogeography 77, 789, 86, 89 Norway 211, 264 Periparus venustulus 10
reproduction 56, 67 Nova Scotia 86 patches 2478, 24951, 2778, 279
status signaling and novel song 1945 penduline tit 211, 237, 238
communication networks 229 novelty score 191 Pennsylvania 1034, 148, 244, 258
unpredictable food, corticosterone, nutcracker (Nucifaga caryocatactes) 16 Periparus viii, 9, 10, 299, 305
spatial memory and nuthatch 16, 115, 116, 266, 270, photoperiod 34, 301, 579,
hippocampus 30, 31 299300 65, 66, 72
winter adaptations and habitat see also red-breasted; photoperiodism and annual cycle of
fragmentation 264, 266 white-breasted black-capped chickadee 4352
INDEX 317

reproduction 468 vocal production and perception recruitment 612


seasonal changes in food storing 153 red knot (Calidris canutus) 323
4950 see also black-capped; red squirrel (Tamiasciurus hudsonicus)
seasonal changes in song and brown-capped 115, 120, 121, 122, 123, 127, 149
song-control system 489 Poland 3056 red-breasted nuthatch (Sitta
seasonal neural plasticity 445 population adaptations to local canadensis) 79, 11114, 11823,
seasonal reproduction 44 habitat 5960 1258, 149
seasonality 434 population genetics, factors red-naped sapsucker (Sphyrapicus
photorefractoriness 44, 45, 47, 48, 50, influencing 8890 ruber) 11315, 118, 1214,
51, 71 population growth models 118 126, 149
photosensitivity 44, 50, 65, 71 and site level changes 1208 relative pitch 154, 155, 156, 157, 158
photostimulation 44, 47, 48, 50 postdispersal social environment and Remizidae 211
phylogeography of chestnut-backed acoustic variation in birdsong removal experiments 3057
chickadee 7791 18395 repertoires 170, 195
Alaska 82 ages of adults since 2004 191 reproduction 5567
biogeography 789 bottleneck, effect of 18990 adult fitness components, future
comparison to other Parids 868 data analysis 1889 effects on 623
interior and coastal populations empirical results, synthesis of behavior see habitat quality and
812 1914 reproductive behavior
origin 78 Fort Collins and Islands North black-capped chickadee 468
population genetics, factors 1901 climatic variation, cyclical 634
influencing 8890 model and hypothesis 1845 climatic variation, directional 645
private alleles 835 novel song and reproductive extrinsic factors 579
Queen Charlotte Islands 823 fitness 1945 fitness 1945
recolonization patterns 856 origin and maintenance of dialects instrinsic factors 57
samples and genotyping 80 and repertoires 195 offspring production and
physiological adaptations sampling procedure 1878 recruitment 602
2636 song development 194 population adaptations to local
physiology 3 study system 1857 habitat 5960
pigeon (Columba livia) 13, 15, 265 variation across space and time research, history of 567
pileated woodpecker 114 194 success 1413
pine forest 117, 282 variation within juveniles 191 timing in males 656
see also lodgepole pine postglacial colonization and Rhode Island 251
pine grosbeak (Pinicola enucleator) chestnut-backed chickadee 89 richness of species 270
83 postural signals 4 Rocky Mountains 60
pitch: predator avoidance 245 Russia 303
absolute 155, 156, 157, 158 private alleles 835, 86
ratio 157 proactive interference 13 samples 80
relative 154, 155, 156, 157, 158 proximate environmental cues 43, 46 San Juan Islands 90
variation 230 Psittaciform 236 Saskatchewan 251, 254, 255
pj call 211 ptilochronology 267, 271, 273, 284 seasonal changes 1819, 38
place cells 15 Puget Sound 183 in food storing 4950
Pleskes tit 307 pygmy owl (Glaucidiium passerinum) hippocampus 4950
plumage 4, 1367 245 in song and song-control system
Poecile vi, vii, viii, x, xi, 151 489
call characteristics, sharing of 180 quaking aspen (Populus tremuloides) seasonal metabolic adjustments
differences between North 112 2635
American and Eurasian Quebec 246, 247, 255, 256, 294, 295 seasonal neural plasticity 445
studies 299, 300, 305, 308 Queen Charlotte Islands 79, 81, 823, seasonal reproduction and
information and the chick-a-dee 85, 90, 147 photoperiodims 44
call 199 seasonality 434
neurobiology of spatial behavior RA 71 see also photoperiod; winter
9, 10 rank acquisition, variation in 13940 second broods 303
phylogeography 77 rasp call 175 see-bay-see call 99
status signaling and rat 13 see-bay-see-bay-see call 99
communication networks recolonization patterns and chestnut- see-bee-see-bay call 96, 99, 100, 101
229, 230 backed chickadee 856 see-see-bay-bay-bay call 96, 101
318 INDEX

semi-Markovian model 206, 207 winter field methods 1323 fee-bee song: variation on
seniority 135 soft dee note 202 two-note theme 21517
settlement 2834 somber tit (Parus lugubris) 300 female eavesdropping in
sex 2, 135, 141 song 300 black-capped chickadees
Shannon-Wiener species diversity aberrant 1034, 105 2247
index 178, 188 behavior and nest cavities 2279 male eavesdropping in
sharp-shinned hawk (Accipiter and call, lack of congruence black-capped
striatus) 285 between 102 chickadees 2223
shifted song 215 and call recordings 98 nest cavities and singing behavior
shivering 2656 control nuclei 712 2279
si-t call 211 control system 45 singing context 1: diurnal
Siberia 106, 107 frequency, variations in during countersinging interactions
Siberian tit (Poecile cincta) countersinging interactions 21720
caching, spatial memory and 21819 singing context 2: dawn chorus
hippocampus 29 patterns beyond and within hybrid 2202
dominance, corticosterone, zone 99101 Stellers jay (Cyanocitta stelleri) 83
spatial memory and patterns in Pennsylvania contact stress 3
hippocampus 35 zone 1034 stripe-breasted wren (Thryothorus
edge, patch and landscape photoperiodism and annual thoracicus) 215
effects 253 cycle 49 sub-gargle 168, 169, 171
neurobiology of spatial and song-control system, seasonal sub-Saharan Africa vi
behavior 11 changes in 489 sub-song 168
reproduction 62 sparrow (Melospiza melodia) subalpine fir (Abies lasiocarpa) 283
status signaling and 85, 193 substance P 17
communication networks 230 timing, variation in during suppression hypothesis 3, 138, 139
winter adaptations and habitat countersinging interactions survivorship 62, 1401, 206, 207,
fragmentation 267, 268 21920 2701
Sierra Nevada 60, 127, 149 types 95 Sweden 65, 2457, 24951, 2534,
signal redundancy 204 versus call distinction and social 267, 296, 305
singing see song complexity 2367 Switzerland 58, 61
site level changes using population see also postdispersal social syntax 2057, 235
growth models 1208 environment and acoustic
Sittiparus ix variation in birdsong; vocal target of selection argument 200
size 1356 production and perception taxonomic distinctions vivii
snow cover 271 sound analysis 98 temperature 268, 269, 270
social dominance 19, 38 South Carolina 249, 251, 253, 255 effects of on isolated woodlots
winter 23 South Dakota 264 2713
see also social dominance and Spain 86, 88, 245, 248, 249, 250, 253, and reproduction 58, 65, 66
fitness in black-capped 254 Tennessee 206, 255
chickadees sparrow 16 territoriality 287, 299
social dominance and fitness in see also Harris; house; testosterone 51
black-capped chickadees white-crowned Texas 106
13144 spatial behavior, neurobiology of thermogenesis by shivering 2656
age 134 920 three-toed woodpecker (Picoides
future research 1434 food storing 914 dorsalis) 114, 118
hierarchy formation and hippocampus 1419 thrushes 16
maintenance 1379 spatial memory 328 Townsends warbler 84
plumage 1367 interspecific differences 256 tree swallow (Tachycineta bicolor) 56
rank acquisition, variation in seasonality 29 trembling aspen (Populus tremuloides)
13940 species biology 299 116, 119, 283
reproductive success 1413 spitt notes 211 triangle fights 138
seniority 135 spring field methods 133 tropical antbirds 65
sex 135 spring phenology 3023 true calls 2
size and condition 1356 spruce 112, 117 tufted titmouse (Baeolophus bicolor) x,
spring field methods 133 SR141716A 17 4, 5
survival 1401 status signaling and communication behavioral aspects of hybridization
vocalizations 137 networks 21531 106
INDEX 319

caching, spatial memory and warbler 16 winter adaptations and habitat


hippocampus 29 hermit 84 fragmentation 264, 265,
differences between North Kentucky 216 267, 268
American and Eurasian Townsends 84 wind:
studies 305 warbling song 230 effects on isolated woodlots 2713
dominance, corticosterone, spatial Washington 80, 83, 85 intensity 264
memory and hippocampus 35 whistled songs 2 speed 268, 269
edge, patch and landscape effects white spruce 116 winter:
244, 245, 249, 252, 253, 257, 258 white-breasted nuthatch (Sitta adaptations and habitat
information and the chick-a-dee carolinensis) 209, 258, 271, fragmentation 26374
call 209 272, 273 behavioral modifications and
postdispersal social environment white-crowned sparrow (Zonotryicha ecological adaptations 2669
and acoustic variation 194 leucophrys) 434, 183 effects on wintering permanent
status signaling and white-fronted tit (Parus semilarvatus) resident birds 2704
communication networks 229 294 physiological adaptations 2636
winter adaptations and habitat white-naped tit (Parus nuchalis) 294 responses to fragmentation
fragmentation 267, 268, 269, wild canary 49 26970
270, 271, 272 willow species (Salix spp.) 283 ecology 300
Turkestan 107 willow tit (Poecile montana) 25 field methods 1323
behavioral aspects of hybridization fitness 2
under-represented groups xi 106, 107 group territoriality 3057
United Kingdom 55, 56, 57, 601, caching, spatial memory and limitation 3057
294, 302, 303 hippocampus 29 social organization 300
edge, patch and landscape effects corticosterone, food caching, Wisconsin 255, 294
249, 250, 251, 253 spatial memory and within bird diversity measure 1889,
Utah 116, 264 hippocampus 32 190, 191
differences between North within song 193
Vancouver Island 90 American and Eurasian woodlot:
variable see note 48, 221 studies 301, 306, 307 connectivity 2734
varied tit (Poecile varius) 106 dominance, corticosterone, edge effect 270
Virginia 1034 spatial memory and isolated 2713
visual signals 45 hippocampus 35 size 2701
vocal communication 4 edge, patch and landscape effects woodpecker 116, 266
vocal patterns analysis, confounding 244, 245, 247, 250, 252, 254 pileated 114
variables (potentially) in 1045 habitat quality and reproductive see also downy; hairy; three-toed
vocal production and perception behavior 280 wren see banded; house;
15364 information and the chick-a-dee stripe-breasted
call perception 1613 call 202, 207, 208, 211 Wyoming 255
operant conditioning studies neurobiology of spatial behavior
1623 11 yellow tit (Parus holsti) 294
playback studies 1612 phylogeography 77, 85, 86, 88, 90 yellow-bellied tit (Periparus
call production 15961 postdispersal social environment venustulus) 106
song perception 1559 and acoustic variation 194
operant conditioning reproduction 65 zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata) 15,
experiments 1579 social dominance and fitness 131, 59, 162, 193
playback experiments 1567 135, 139, 141 zero-order uncertainty 205, 206
song production 1535 status signaling and zi note 211
vocalizations 95, 137, 301 communication networks 229 Zipfs Law 206

Вам также может понравиться