Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 35

fPrevention and Health Promotion: Research, Social Action, Practice And Training: Volume 9, Issue 1 1

Prevention and Health Promotion: Research, Social Action, Practice and Training is a publication of the Prevention Section of
the Society for Counseling Psychology. The publication is dedicated to the dissemination of information on prevention theory,
research, practice and training in counseling psychology, stimulating prevention scholarship, promoting collaboration between
counseling psychologists engaged in prevention, and encourages student scholars. The publication focuses on prevention in specific
domains (e.g., college campuses) employing specific modalities (e.g., group work), and reports summaries of epidemiological and
preventive intervention research. All submissions to the publication undergo blind review by an editorial board jury, and those
selected for publication are distributed nationally through electronic and hard copies.

Managing Editor
Erin E. Ayala, PhD, Albany Medical Center

Editorial Board Chair


Sally M. Hage, PhD, Springfield College

Members of the Editorial Board


Aimee Arikian, The Emily Program
Kimberly Burdine, Texas Tech University
Stephanie Chapman, Baylor College of Medicine
Robert Conyne, University of Cincinnati
Simon Chung, Toronto, Canada
Michael Gale, University at Albany
Arthur Horne, University of Georgia
Cara Maffini, San Jose State University
LeRoy Reese, Morehouse School of Medicine
Chandra Story, Oklahoma State University
Ellen Vaughan, Indiana University Bloomington

Editorial Assistant
Julianne Yavorski, Oklahoma State University

Submission Guidelines
The Prevention Section of the Society of Counseling Psychology publishes Prevention and Health Promotion: Research, Social
Action, Practice and Training. This is a blind peer-reviewed publication presenting scholarly work in the field of prevention that is
distributed nationally. Contributions can focus on prevention theory, research, practice or training, or a combination of these topics.
We welcome student submissions. As a publication of the Prevention Section of Division 17, presentations and awards sponsored by
the section will be highlighted in these issues. We will also publish condensed reviews of research or theoretical work pertaining to the
field of prevention. All submissions need to clearly articulate the prevention nature of the work. Submissions to this publication need
to conform to APA style. All submissions must be electronically submitted. Please send your documents prepared for blind review
with a cover letter including all identifying information for our records. Submissions should be emailed to Erin Ayala, Managing
Editor, at ayalae@mail.amc.edu.

Prevention and Health Promotion: Research, Social Action, Practice And Training: Volume 9, Issue 1 2
PREVENTION AND HEALTH PROMOTION
RESEARCH, SOCIAL ACTION, PRACTICE AND TRAINING
Primary Prevention: Is It an Idea Whose Time Has Come?
Myrna B. Shure 4

Examining Associations Between Residential Status, Protective Behavioral Strategy Use, and Alcohol-
Related Negative Consequences Among College Students

Kray A. Scully, Danielle P. Cottonham, Margo C. Villarosa, Saarah D. Kison, and Michael B. Madson 15

Suicide Prevention Gatekeeper Training for Resident Assistants: A Between Groups Control Design
Study
Colleen Thomas and Martin Swanbrow Becker 27

Comparison between International and Domestic Graduate Students on Alcohol, Sexual Assault, and
Prevention Efforts: An Exploratory Study
Lei Wang, Leslie A. Fasone, and Ellen L. Vaughan 29

Teaching Interpersonal Communication Skills: Comparing the Fidelity of Online Simulations


Amy E. Dawson, Bianca L. Bernstein, Jennifer M. Bekki, and Kerrie G. Wilkins 32

Prevention and Health Promotion: Research, Social Action, Practice And Training: Volume 9, Issue 1 3
RESEARCH, SOCIAL ACTION, PRACTICE AND TRAINING

Invited Contribution
Primary Prevention: Is It an Idea Whose Time Has Come?
Myrna B. Shure

Drexel University

Myrna B. Shure, PhD, is a developmental psychologist and Professor Emeritus at Drexel University in Philadelphia. She is the creator
of the I Can Problem Solve (ICPS) programan evidence-based social and emotional learning/violence prevention program for use in
schoolsand Raising a Thinking Child (RATC), also evidence-based for use by parents. Dr. Shure received the 2015 Lifetime
Achievement Award from the Society of Counseling Psychologys Prevention Section in the American Psychological Association in
recognition of her continued contributions to the field. For additional information, email Dr. Shure at mshure@drexel.edu or access
her website at www.thinkingchild.com.

I am deeply honored and humbled to be the co- had. Although most children would ask for it, the answer was
recipient of the 2015 Lifetime Achievement Award from the almost always some form of no. Some children would then
Society of Counseling Psychology Prevention Section of the grab the toy, hit the child, or tell the teacher and let the teacher
American Psychological Association. This recognition has solve the problem for them+. Then I noticed four-year old
propelled me to reflect on my lifes work, barriers I met along Zachary, who, when he was refused a wagon that Peter was
the way, the role of the school guidance counselor, the future playing with, did something quite different. He asked, Why
of prevention research and practice, and some of my own cant I have it? Peter replied, I need it, Im pulling the
satisfactions that may inspire early career psychologists and rocks. Zachary then offered, I can help you. We can pull
others who are now entering the field. the rocks together. Peter said Okay, both children were
With increasing violence, substance abuse, teen satisfied with the solution, and no teacher had to intervene,
pregnancy, and sexual behaviors that could lead to AIDS and suggest they share, or take the toy away so neither could have
other sexually transmitted diseases, as well as various it.
psychological dysfunction, such as depression, there is now an I met George Spivack in 1968, who was studying
urgent need for prevention prevention that goes beyond how adolescents were navigating their interpersonal worlds.
increased staff and security guards in our schools, metal As I expressed my interests in the topic, we became a team
detectors, and lectures about the ills of unsafe sex, bullying, and began assessing Means-Ends and Consequential Thinking.
and other serious outcomes (Hawkins et al., 2015). Today, With his colleague Murray Levine, George studied how
there are a number of model primary prevention programs adolescents planned step-by-step; more specifically, how they
being conducted in the schools and with parents, under the reached a stated interpersonal goal, how they anticipated
egis of social-emotional learning (SEL; see the Collaborative potential obstacles that could interfere with reaching that goal,
for Academic, Social, and Emotional Learning [CASEL] and how to gain an appreciation of the fact that it takes time to
website at www.casel.org). I am going to describe how one reach the goal. A test (called the Means-Ends Problem
such program, now called I Can Problem Solve, was born. Solving (MEPS) test) was administered in story form; the
It all began when I was a nursery school teacher, in subjects were given the beginning of the story (e.g., John just
the early 1960s, when I noticed that the most important moved into the neighborhood and wanted to make lots of
problem for four-year-olds was wanting a toy another child friends) and the end of the story (John had lots of friends), and

Prevention and Health Promotion: Research, Social Action, Practice And Training: Volume 9, Issue 1 4

were asked to fill in the middle. Spivack and Levine (1963) lost object (Platt & Spivack, 1970).
found that youngsters in a residential treatment home were The rational for studying means end thinking was
significantly more deficient when compared to normal based on our hypothesis that An individual who is
adolescents in both Means-Ends Thinking and in the tendency overwhelmed by his environment possesses a narrower
to weigh the pros and cons when making a decision, for repertoire of means-ends thinking and is preoccupied with the
example, to go to a party or study for an exam they would end goal rather than the means to obtain it. If he is not adept
need to take the next day -- a skill called Consequential at thinking through ways to solve problems, or if unforeseen
Thinking. obstacles interfere, he may make impulsive mistakes, become
When I began working with George, I administered frustrated and aggressive, or evade the problem entirely by
the MEPS test to fifth-graders and found that, even within a withdrawing. In any case, his/her initial need remains
homogeneous classroom of relatively normal children (not unabated, and if such failures occur repeatedly, varying
diagnostically disturbed), those showing more aggression, degrees of maladaptive behavior may ensue (Shure &
other impulsive behaviors, or shy/withdrawn behaviors were Spivack, 1972, p. 348).
more deficient in Means-End Thinking than their classmates Upon completion of our first project, I said to
who did not show these behaviors (Shure & Spivack, 1972). George, I was a nursery school teacher. I wonder if we could
For example, a story of an impulsive child depicted a boy study how preschoolers navigate their interpersonal worlds.
(John) who introduced himself to the most popular child, who It became clear right away that means-ends thinking and
in turn introduced him to his friends, which led to him having weighing pros and cons was developmentally too sophisticated
lots of friends. This story consisted of two means, no for preschoolers; however, we did find that the ability to name
obstacles, and no appreciation that making friends takes time. different, unconnected, alternative solutions to an
In contrast, a story of a behaviorally adjusted child depicted interpersonal problem (Alternative Solution Thinking), and to
the boy (John) offering to bring the leader to a basketball court think about what might happen next if an act were carried out
and teaching him how to shoot baskets. It turned out that the (Consequential Thinking), could distinguish behaviorally
leader didnt like basketball (an obstacle), so John asked him adjusted and prosocial children from their more impulsive and
what he did like. The leader said he liked hockey, so being the withdrawn classmates (Shure, Spivack, & Jaeger, 1971). For
good athlete he was, John learned how to play hockey. Three example, an impulsive child might provide two or three
months later (recognition of time), John showed the leader solutions for obtaining a toy from another child, or to keeping
how to shoot hockey pucks, and as a result, the leader thought a mother from being angry after having broken property of
John was great and brought his friends to the rink. At the end, value, while an adjusted child could more likely give six or
John had lots of friends. What a different story between these seven solutions to those problems. Typical solutions to the toy
two children. The example stories showed how impulsive and problem included take it, hit him, tell the teacher, and
withdrawn youngsters were more concerned with immediate share it, while more creative ones included put his name on
gratification and exhibited less rational thought with regard to it and hell think its his and youll have more fun if you
steps taken toward goal completion. Importantly, these play with me. To the damaged object story, such as a broken
findings held when IQ was controlled, suggesting that IQ was flower pot, typical solutions included say Im sorry, fix
not the explanatory factor. The same findings emerged when it, and get a new one, while more creative ones included,
normal adults were compared with adult psychiatric inpatients, pretend shes sleeping so mommy cant whoop her, and my
regardless of whether the problem revolved around friendship favorite, get her mommy a drink and shell feel better.
choice, heterosexual relations, employment, or how to find a When the children were asked what might happen

Prevention and Health Promotion: Research, Social Action, Practice And Training: Volume 9, Issue 1 5

next if, for example, a child grabs a toy from another, correlational studies alone. Thus, we examined the linkage
impulsive or withdrawn children gave significantly fewer between ICPS ability and behavioral adjustment by
consequences than their more adjusted peers. Typical experimentally altering ICPS skills (via intervention) and
consequences were: hell grab it back, hell hit him, or observing changes in childrens display of behaviors related to
hell tell the teacher. One more creative response was, these thinking skills. The purpose of the study was to learn
hell eat marshmallows in front of him and when he wants whether ICPS ability would mediate such behaviors, and if
one, hell say no, you took my truck. Interestingly, in a that was the case, we could pinpoint the ICPS skills that play
second scenario, in which a child took his (or her) mothers the most significant role in the behavioral adjustment of four-
umbrella when she wasnt looking, typical responses included: and five-year-old children.
hell get in trouble, and shell hide it so he cant get it. It Our first major study took place over a two year
was here that we learned young children could be empathic, period, beginning with 113 preschool children trained in ICPS
evidenced by statements such as, when it rains, shell get wet skills and 106 controls. The participants were divided into
and catch a cold thinking of the mother vs. thinking of four groups during their second (kindergarten) year: (1)
him/herself. trained both years; (2) trained in preschool only control
While performing these correlational studies, we kindergarten; (3) control preschool -, trained in kindergarten;
implicitly assumed Spivacks theoretical position that and (4) control both years (Shure & Spivack, 1980; Shure &
Interpersonal Cognitive Problem Solving (ICPS) skills served Spivack, 1982). The program was implemented by following
as an antecedent condition for healthy social adjustment a script, developed by Shure and Spivack, and presented in
(Spivack & Shure, 1982). More specifically, Spivack asserted full in Spivack and Shure (1974). The program consisted of
that an individual who can think through and solve problems, daily 20-minute games and dialogues for a period of about
weigh the pros and cons of an act, and appreciate underlying three months, through which teachers and children interacted
causal dynamics of an interpersonal solution will be less likely in small groups. The concepts taught were essentially the
to make impulsive mistakes and will suffer less frustration same in both the nursery and kindergarten years, although the
from failure and circumstances leading to maladaptive content was more sophisticated for the older children (Shure &
functioning. Spivack, 1980). Early games consisted of word concepts that
The First Training Programs: Preschool and were later used in problem solving. For example, the words is
Kindergarten and not, and same and different, were played with in fun ways
The correlational research studies discussed prior such as, This is a table. It is not a chair. Can you show me
suggested that the behavior of children who behave and think something different that is not a chair? When a real problem
differently was mediated by a set of Interpersonal Cognitive would come up, for example, one child hitting another, the
Problem Solving (ICPS) skills. We wanted to know whether teacher would ask, Is hitting a good idea or not a good idea?
we could guide behavior by enhancing the deficient ICPS Can you think of something different to do that is a good
skills rather than focusing directly on the behaviors idea? Instead of telling the child what to do, or what might
themselves. Contrary to the assumption of educators and happen if he does something in particular, the teacher engages
clinicians that relieving emotional tension leads to the ability in what we call ICPS Dialoguing to encourage the childs
to think straight, we tested the reverse idea that is, if one use of his or her newly acquired problem solving skills to
could think straight, emotional tensions would be relieved. solve real life problems. Other words included if-then and
Whether availability of ICPS skills is an antecedent might/maybe to be used in later consequential thinking (e.g.,
condition to adjustment, however, cannot be ascertained by If I hit him, then he might hit me back.) and before/after

Prevention and Health Promotion: Research, Social Action, Practice And Training: Volume 9, Issue 1 6

(beginning in kindergarten) to be used instead of the rhetorical controls, the children who were behaviorally adjusted and
Why did you hit him? (e.g., Did you hit him before or after received ICPS skill training in preschool were less likely to
you took his toy?). Subsequent games focused on begin engaging in early high-risk behaviors in kindergarten
recognition and awareness of childrens own and others and beyond, suggesting that ICPS intervention is not only
feelings, which is awareness that could enrich the range and beneficial as a treatment program, but can also be used as a
nature of solutions and consequential thinking. Through prevention program.
games and dialogues, the aim was to help children develop a ICPS for Use by Parents
problem solving style of thinking. Children were never told After our early research that examined the
what the solutions or consequences to an act could be. Rather, effectiveness of teacher-implemented ICPS in the classroom,
they were encouraged to generate their own ideas, and then to we took ICPS into the home. The concepts of the program
think about how they and others might feel, and what they were the same as they were for teachers, but the lesson-games
might do. In addition, teachers and their aides were trained to were modified to be conducted with a single child at home.
use these concepts, ICPS Dialoguing, during the day when Over a ten-week period, mothers attended weekly group
children were having actual problems in the classroom. The meetings (40 mothers, 10 per group), during which they role-
goal of training was to teach children how to think, not what to played the lessons and practiced dialoguing techniques. The
think, so they could choose and evaluate for themselves what program script, outlined in Shure and Spivack (1978), was
to do and what not to do. designed to teach both the mother (or mother surrogate) and
We learned from this study that as children become her child how to think in ways that would help them
more sensitive to their own and others feelings, more aware successfully resolve interpersonal problems. Exercises
of potential consequences to their actions, and better able to designed to teach the mother concepts in concert with those
think of alternative solutions to real-life problems, they she would in turn transmit to her child were interspersed
display improved peer relations and genuine concern for throughout the meetings. For example, when the mothers were
others in distress. The program also led to significantly teaching their children to think about their own and others
decreased early high-risk behaviors that research shows feelings, and how to consider the effects of their actions on
predicts later violence, substance abuse, teen pregnancy, others, they were also encouraged to think about their own
school dropout, and some forms of psychopathology (e.g., feelings and how the things they do might affect others
Hawkins, Catalano, & Miller, 1992; Parker & Asher, 1987). (including their children). In the same manner, the mothers
Specifically, such high-risk behaviors include overt were taught to guide their children toward thinking of
aggression, inability to wait, inability to share and take turns, alternative solutions to problems relevant to a four-year-old
over-emotionality in the face of frustration, and lack of (e.g., He hit me). They were also encouraged to think
concern for others. We found that these behaviors decreased through and solve problems that their children might have
after as little as one three-month exposure to ICPS training in brought to them or created for them (e.g., Johnny doesnt
preschool, and the ICPS skills gained held until the end of listen to me). Just as the mothers learned not to tell solutions
kindergarten. to their children, they were encouraged to think of their own
Importantly, the children who showed the most ICPS solutions in handling problems that arose between them and
skill gains, especially solution skills, also showed the most their children.
behavioral gains. This direct link suggests that the ICPS skills The results of the parenting study indicated that ICPS
mediated the behavioral gains (Shure & Spivack, 1980). Also, training significantly improved impulsive and inhibited
when compared to never-trained behaviorally adjusted behaviors of inner-city, low-income African-American four-

Prevention and Health Promotion: Research, Social Action, Practice And Training: Volume 9, Issue 1 7

year-olds. Further, similarly to the teacher-implemented situations in the lesson-games to real life.
program, alternative solutions were the strongest mediator of The curriculum was conducted with African-
behavior change, with consequential thinking coming in American fifth- and sixth-graders from the inner-city and
second. We learned that children exposed to ICPS training in implemented three times a week for four months. Briefly,
one environment (the home) improved their behavior in a results suggested that for this age and SES group, one
different environment (the school) as well. This finding was exposure to ICPS in grade five enhanced the measured
particularly important because the behavior improvements solution, consequential and means-ends thinking skills, as well
were reported by teachers unaware of the training procedures as positive and prosocial behaviors; however, a second
and goals. We also learned that inner-city mothers, many of exposure in grade six was required to reduce negative
whom also displayed deficient ICPS skills at the beginning of impulsive and inhibited behaviors. Importantly, no such
the program, were able to successfully improve their own interpersonal or behavior gains were seen either year in the
skills in addition to those of their children in time as short as comparison group trained in impersonal critical thinking
ten weeks. In addition, interviews showed that typical pre- skills. In fact, the impersonal skills-trained group that focused
training telling-the-child-what-to-do childrearing style on reasoning skills, deductive logic, and generating
changed to a childrearing style that included significantly impersonal alternative thinking (e.g., how water might have
more guidance in helping children think through and solve disappeared from a can taken to the desert), became more
their own problems. While childrearing styles and ICPS skills impulsive from grade five to six, further strengthening our
did have some impact on the childrens behavior, they also conclusion with the younger children that ICPS training is a
had a direct impact on the childrens thinking skills skills viable model of prevention for a variety of age groups.
which in turn played a significant role in the childrens However, full behavioral impact for latency-aged low-income
ultimate behavioral adjustment (Shure & Spivack, 1979). children may take longer than the briefer one-time exposure to
ICPS for Older Children the curriculum required for younger children in the preschool-
Adding to the concepts taught to younger children, and kindergarten-aged years (Shure & Healey, 1993).
implemented around age-appropriate problems, we Revision of the Training Manuals: From Research to
implemented more sophisticated skills of perspective-taking Practice
(an appreciation that someone else might have thoughts and Although our interventions were originally designed
feelings different from ones own), recognition of peoples to test research hypotheses about how (and if) interpersonal
potential motivations for behavior (e.g., he didnt say hello cognitive problem solving (ICPS) skills could impact
because he doesnt like me vs. he didnt see me), behavior, teachers we trained suggested some revisions to
sensitivity to problems as interpersonal, recognition of causes make the lesson-games more user-friendly and to prepare the
of problems, and listening and awareness. These and other manuals for mass-dissemination. After over two decades of
prerequisite skills enriched childrens ability to generate further research (in Shure, 1996), with the help of teachers, the
alternative solutions to real-life problems, anticipate potential manuals were transformed into their present form. Originally
consequences to an act, and help develop more sophisticated called Interpersonal Cognitive Problem Solving (ICPS), with
skills of the earlier-described means-ends thinking. As was the above-mentioned studies published under that name, I was
the case with the younger children, teachers learned how to told that the name was too much of a mouthful and a name
apply ICPS Dialoguing when real problems arose, in order that children could use was needed. In the middle of the night,
to help children associate their thinking with their behavior, I woke up with an Aha! moment, and realized that a better
and to apply the concepts learned through the fictional name would be I Can Problem Solve, which would allow us to

Prevention and Health Promotion: Research, Social Action, Practice And Training: Volume 9, Issue 1 8

keep the abbreviation of ICPS. questions to actual problems. Although we do not play these
The manuals for mass-dissemination, from preschool kinds of games with the older children, we still use them in
through grade 6, were published in 1992. In the new manuals, dialoguing when actual problems arise. The importance of the
the elements that comprised ICPS Dialoguing were broken dialoguing approach was clear when we saw how the children
into manageable bites one concept at a time. In our earlier responded to it; they felt involved in the conversation as
years, we asked teachers to ask all of the questions from the opposed to being lectured to, told what to do, or even yelled at
beginning, which was too much to absorb and apply when and sent to time out a solution that teachers and parents tell
talking with children. For example, after having played me only lasts for a few minutes.
formal lesson-games with the prerequisite word pairs is/not Our feelings about dialoguing were validated when
and same/different with preschool and kindergarten children, Weissberg and Gesten (1982) found significant behavior gains
so they could later be asked to think of a different way to solve when dialoguing was added to a problem solving program
a problem, the teacher could now focus on daily use of just the they developed for early school-aged children, and when
word pairs until they felt comfortable with them. First, in a teachers of school-aged children reported that dialoguing
non-problem situation, the teacher might say, Johnny is might be the key to independent problem solving thinking.
painting. Who is doing the same thing as Johnny? Who is not Also, the children felt so proud when they succeeded in
painting? Who is doing something different? And soon the problem solving; for example, one four-year-old came home
question could become a little harder, such as, Who is not from school and proclaimed, Mommy, I solved a problem
doing the same thing as Johnny? When a real problem would today.
arise, the teacher would first ask each child what happened In the late 1980s, Donald Meichenbaum, a clinical
from their point of view, and now she could ask, Do you two psychologist at the University of Waterloo, a founder of
see what happened the same way or a different way? Having cognitive behavior therapy, currently a consultant to the
associated the words embedded in this question with fun made Melissa Institute in Florida (a think tank for the prevention of
it more palatable for children to respond. With many more violence in schools) said to me, You really ought to integrate
examples of this slow build-up presented in the manuals, your interpersonal concepts into the curriculum. At the time,
teachers could now more easily absorb the full steps of ICPS we wanted to test the impact of interpersonal cognitive skills
Dialoguing, with such questions as How do you feel?, on behavior and we realized the importance of examining any
What happened when , and Can you think of a different added value of ICPS interventions on academic achievement
way to solve this problem so that you wont feel that way and as well. Again, with the help of teachers, I incorporated each
that wont happen? Shortly thereafter, I revised our program of the ICPS concepts into subjects, such as math (e.g., Would
for parents to be used by parent educators in English, called you be happier with a half a piece of pizza or a whole piece of
Raising a Thinking Child Workbook (Shure, 1995) and in pizza?), history (e.g., What made Neil Armstrong feel
Spanish, called Enseando a Nuestros Nios a Pensar (Shure, proud?), and science (e.g., What might happen if it never
2000) based on a trade book I wrote for parents called rains?), with specific examples that were chosen according to
Raising a Thinking Child (Shure, 1994). what the children were studying. We found academic
I have often been asked why we teach words like is achievement gains in reading and math for fifth- and sixth-
and not and same and different when even three- and four- graders (Shure, 1980), and in second graders reading and
year-olds already know them. The reason is to guide the social studies standardized achievement test scores, especially
children to think of these words in a problem solving way and among those trained by teachers in kindergarten and again, by
to associate the fun of playing with them and the dialoguing their mothers in grade 1 (Shure, 1993).

Prevention and Health Promotion: Research, Social Action, Practice And Training: Volume 9, Issue 1 9

It is not yet known whether the integration of ICPS the existence of a training program) and got significant gains
directly accounts for gains in the academic curriculum, or if it from peer- and independent observer ratings as well (Shure,
instead has an indirect gain due to another confounding factor. 1993; Shure & Healey, 1993).
Perhaps the ability to appreciate anothers point of view I also learned that to successfully conduct research, it
(perspective-taking) in a problem situation may play a part in is important to meet the needs of the school principal and not
the process of thinking through plans and/or solutions to focus on the needs of the research project. In our early
problems, but the latter ability to solve problems is what research, I had the consent of a principal in a school where we
relieves any tensions and reduces interpersonal conflict. It would study kindergarten children first trained in preschool.
also seems logical to conclude that regardless of actual IQ At the beginning of that year, a new principal who was
level, once behaviors mediated through ICPS skills do assigned to the school refused our entry because
improve, youngsters can better absorb the task-oriented psychologists just use the schools for their own ends and
demands of the classroom, and subsequently do better in contribute little or nothing to the school or to the children. I
school. Whatever the case, academic gains have clearly been knew that explaining that we needed those children for our
documented with other social and emotional (SEL) programs research design because they were already trained in preschool
as well (Durlak et al., 2011), as demonstrated by the results of would fall on deaf ears. Instead, I focused on staff
a meta-analysis of 213 school-based universal SEL programs development and what the program could do for the teachers
involving 270, 034 kindergarten through high-school students. and the children (luckily this was to be a training and not a
Researchers report that when compared to non-trained control school), and then he became interested. Fortunately,
controls, SEL-trained students demonstrated significantly he allowed us to proceed, and as it turned out, he became one
improved social and emotional skills, attitudes, behavior, and of our greatest advocates.
academic performance that reflected an 11-percentile-point It was in the implementation of ICPS as a service that
gain in achievement (p. 405). We now know that poor I found to be most difficult. I had a contract with 40 teachers
academic achievement is in itself a high-risk marker for later in the Philadelphia Public Schools during the very year that
serious problems (e.g., Hawkins, Catalano, & Miller, 1992), the No Child Left Behind Act was being implemented.
and ICPS and other SEL programs can play a significant role Teachers, concerned about their students reading and math
in preventing the problems from happening. test scores, their own jobs, and the school itself felt that
Barriers to ICPS although ICPS was important, they had too much else to do to
During our hypothesis-testing years, researchers have time for talking about feelings, solutions, and
thought that the behavior change we reported was a result of consequences to behavior. As a result, only a handful of
teachers who did the training also rating their students teachers conducted the program with fidelity, some in bits and
behaviors, suggesting bias in the interpretation of the validity pieces here and there, and some not at all. I think that today,
of ICPS. To test whether that was the case, we studied with the evidence that ICPS and other similar social and
whether the same children who improved in their problem emotional programs significantly enhances reading and math
solving skills were the children who most improved in their scores, teachers and school administrators would be more
behaviors (a direct link) and we found that to be the case open to systematic inclusion of these kinds of programs into
(Shure & Spivack, 1982). Since the teachers were unaware of their day. As Ruth Cross, a former principal of the Mill Street
how the children tested in their ICPS skills, a case against Elementary School in Naperville, Illinois, and an ICPS-trainer,
rater bias could be made. To further test rater bias, we added told me, Unless teachers understand how to integrate the
peer ratings and independent observers (the latter unaware of ICPS skills with what they already do, they will see the

Prevention and Health Promotion: Research, Social Action, Practice And Training: Volume 9, Issue 1 10

program as an add on and will not use it. This has turned more empathic, learn to wait and not fly off the handle when
out to be important with the emphasis on academics and the they cant have what they want right away, get along better
need to be flexible in presenting these kinds of programs to a with their peers, and do better academically. It was also
school or school system (August, Gewirtz, & Realmuto, gratifying to watch shy, inhibited children become more
2010). outgoing, stand up for their rights, and express their emotions
Role of the School Guidance Counselor in appropriate ways. Ill always remember four-year-old
The school guidance counselor can play a very Tanya, for example, who before ICPS would stand outside the
significant role in the implementation of ICPS in classrooms, doll corner watching, but being too shy to get in and play. The
and remove or reduce some of the barriers mentioned above teacher, meaning well, would offer, Tanya would like to pack
that teachers encounter, such as time for ICPS. As a member the suitcase. Even if the children invited Tanya in, she was
of a school leadership team, the counselor can: not ready. After six weeks of ICPS, Tanya watched for a few
Participate in monitoring that provides positive minutes, then said, If you need a fireman, Im a fireman.
guidance, support, and encouragement to teachers; One boy shouted, The house is on fire. That time it was
Monitor program fidelity (checklist is available); Tanyas idea, not the teachers.
Discuss solutions to barriers or issues that arise in the I will also always remember a sixth-grader, who,
classroom (e.g., finding time for ICPS); when asked after the program was completed why we did

Guide teachers when necessary by modeling the ICPS, responded, We have to learn to think for ourselves.

lessons with the children, or applying the problem People wont always be around to help us. These kinds of

solving approach when real problems arise during the responses, and changes in childrens behavior was our

day; ultimate goal, and unquestionably, our greatest reward.

Participate in team leadership meetings at specified Future of Prevention Research and Practice

time periods (e.g., once a month) to discuss the ICPS ICPS is a universal prevention program, designed to

initiative; and train entire classrooms of children. The advantage is to catch

Reinforce the teachers lessons by repeating needed any false negatives. This is a possibility in that at least among

lessons and using ICPS dialogues individually with inner-city preschoolers, as our earlier-mentioned research

difficult, or high-risk children. found, were less likely to begin showing behavior problems a

One counselor conducted the lessons within earshot year later, in kindergarten. Were the initially adjusted non-

of a teacher who was at first reluctant to get involved, and trained controls who did begin to show behavior problems in

when the teacher saw how the children responded, she became kindergarten among those false negatives in preschool?

interested and conducted the entire program with every group Researchers can investigate risk factors that are yet to be

of students she had from that time on. Another counselor ran identified, so that school counselors and other student support

training groups with parents of kindergarten children using the personnel can add targeted programming for those youngsters.

parent training manual (Shure, 1995), and one of the parents One risk-factor, for example, identified by

herself became a trainer of future groups. Ehrenreich, Beron, and Underwood (2016) is a form of

Satisfactions aggression called relational, or social, aggression. A child

Having experienced the earlier-mentioned displaying this kind of aggression (not studied by us in our

frustrations and working to circumvent them, I want to convey earlier research) excludes peers, spreads rumors, hurts

the immense satisfaction I have experienced by watching feelings, makes mean faces, and engages in other behaviors

children who initially displayed impulsive behaviors become that affect relationships. Recently, Boyle and Hassett-Walker

Prevention and Health Promotion: Research, Social Action, Practice And Training: Volume 9, Issue 1 11

(2008) found ICPS to be effective in reducing both physical settings where time is limited, new ways to maximize that
and relational aggression in rural low-income, mostly time can be tried. As teachers and school administrators
Hispanic, kindergarten- and first-graders. When studying continue to view programs for social and emotional learning
these behaviors across childhood and adolescence, Ehrenreich, as a necessity, not just a luxury, it is important for us to
Beron, and Underwood (2016) found that while physical consider how we can best foster system-level support
aggression across childhood and adolescence confers risk for mechanisms to give these programs a home needed to help
maladjustment, social aggression also confers risk for students thrive?
maladjustment as youngsters enter transition into young For the next generation of researchers and
adulthood. Their suggestion to develop prevention programs practitioners, perhaps these thoughts will stimulate further
to reduce social aggression, and the impact Boyle and Hassett- thought and action in this, now burgeoning, field of primary
Walker (2008) found ICPS creates added-value to the prevention.
variables previously investigated. Final Thoughts
In addition to identifying risk factors, defined as Having earlier expressed some barriers and
variables associated with a high probability of onset, greater frustrations along my road to implementing ICPS to reduce
severity, and longer duration of major mental health high-risk behaviors and promote protective factors in children,
problems (Coie et al., 1993, p. 1013), we also need to I am also very gratified to see how the field of social and
identify protective factors, defined as conditions that emotional learning (SEL), as one form of prevention, has
improve peoples resistance to risk factors and disorders grown over the years to where it is today. Enough evidence
(Coie et al., 1993, p. 1013). Two known protective factors, exists, including replications of ICPS and other SEL
resilience and prosocial behaviors, are also significantly programs, for the value of this kind of addition to the standard
impacted by ICPS (Shure & Aberson, 2012; Shure & Healey, curriculum (reviewed in Shure & Aberson, 2012). Thousands
1993). As Bell (1992) noted, researchers must seek to identify of schools are now implementing some form of SEL. The
who is at risk for particular dysfunctions, what are transition goal that every preschool, elementary, and high-school
points that may heighten that risk, and to address the issue of classroom would include the programs into their curriculum,
how to improve prediction to permit intervening at optimal may now come true, more than ever. Primary prevention, an
developmental stages. Identifying further interpersonal idea as Stephen Goldston (1980) once dreamed, whose time
cognitive problem solving skills at various ages might inform has come.
future research in timing of intervention to reduce and prevent
both immediate (proximal) and longer range (distal) outcomes.
References
And I am often asked, What are the kids like now?, meaning
in high-school and beyond. We learned that we can nip high- August, G. J., Gewirtz, A., & Realmuto, G. M. (2010).
risk behaviors in the bud when children are young. Did we Moving the field of prevention from science to
really prevent violence, substance abuse, unsafe sex, and some service: Integrating evidence-based preventive
forms of psychopathology later on? With hope for future interventions into community practice through
funding opportunities, we may be able to find out. adapted and adaptive models. Applied and Preventive
For continuing modification with a broader audience, Psychology, 14, 72-85. doi:
practitioners can continue to refine, modify, and test various 10.1016/j.appsy.2008.11.001
SEL programs in various settings, including schools, after-
school groups, and mental health agencies. In schools or other

Prevention and Health Promotion: Research, Social Action, Practice And Training: Volume 9, Issue 1 12

Bell, R. Q. (1992). Multiple-risk cohorts and segmenting risk Platt, J., & Spivack, G. (1970, April). Real-life problem-
as solutions to the problem of false positives in risk solving thinking in neuropsychiatric patients and
for the major psychoses. Psychiatry, 55, 370-381. controls. Paper presented at the meeting of the
Boyle, D., & Hassett-Walker, C. (2008). Reducing overt and Eastern Psychological Association, Atlantic City, NJ.
relational aggression among young children: The Shure, M. B. (1992a). I Can Problem Solve (ICPS): An
results from a two-year outcome evaluation. Journal Interpersonal cognitive problem solving program
of School Violence, 7, 27-42. doi: [preschool]. Champaign, IL: Research Press.
10.1300/j202v07n01_03 Shure, M. B. (1992b). I Can Problem Solve (ICPS): An
Coie, J. D., Watt, N. F., West, S. G., Hawkins, J. D., Asarnow, Interpersonal cognitive problem solving program
J. R., Markman, H. J., Ramey, S. L., Shure, M. B., & [kindergarten/primary grades]. Champaign, IL:
Long, B. (1993). The science of prevention: A Research Press.
conceptual framework and some directions for a Shure, M. B. (1992c). I Can Problem Solve (ICPS): An
national research program. American Psychologist, interpersonal cognitive problem solving program
48, 1013-1022. doi: 10.1037/0003-066x.48.10.1013 [intermediate elementary grades]. Champaign, IL:
Durlak, J. A., Weissberg, R. P., Dymnicki, A. B., Taylor, R. Research Press.
D., & Schellinger, K. B. (2011). The impact of Shure, M. B. (1993). Interpersonal problem solving and
enhancing students social and emotional learning: A prevention. A comprehensive report of research and
meta-analysis of school-based universal training. MH-40801. Washington, DC: National
interventions. Child Development, 82, 405-432. doi: Institutes of Mental Health.
10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01564.x Shure, M. B. (1994). Raising a thinking child. New York:
Ehrenreich, S. E., Beron, K. J., & Underwood, M. K. (2016). Henry Holt; 1996, Pocket Books [Paperback edition].
Social and physical aggression trajectories from Shure, M. B. (1995). Raising a thinking child workbook.
childhood through late adolescence: Predictors of Champaign, IL: Research Press.
Psychosocial maladjustment at age 18. Shure, M. B. (1996). I Can Problem Solve (ICPS): An
Developmental Psychology, 52, 457-462. doi: interpersonal cognitive problem solving program for
10.1037/dev0000094 children. In M. C. Roberts (Ed.), Model programs in
Goldston, S. E. (1980). An overview of primary prevention child and family mental health (47-62), Mahwah, NJ:
programming. In D. C. Klein and S. E. Goldston Erlbaum.
(Eds.), Primary Prevention: An Idea Whose Time Shure, M. B. (2000). Enseando a nuestros nios a pensar.
Has Come, DHHS Publication No. ADM 80-447. Champaign, IL: Research Press.
Hawkins, J. D., Catalano, R. F., & Miller, J. Y. (1992). Risk Shure, M. B., & Aberson, B. (2012). Enhancing the process of
and protective factors for alcohol and other drug resilience through effective thinking. (2nd Ed), In S.
problems in adolescence and early adulthood: Goldstein & R. Brooks (Eds.), Handbook of
Implications for substance abuse prevention. resilience in children (pp. 481-506), New York, NY:
Psychological Bulletin, 112, 64-105. doi: Kluwer Academic/Plenum.
10.1037//0033-2909.112.1.64 Shure, M. B., & Healey, K. N. (1993, August). Interpersonal
Parker, J. G., & Asher, S. R. (1987). Peer relations and later problem solving and prevention in urban school
personal adjustment: Are low-accepted children at children. Paper presented at the meeting of the
risk? Psychological Bulletin, 102, 357-389. American Psychological Association, Toronto.

Prevention and Health Promotion: Research, Social Action, Practice And Training: Volume 9, Issue 1 13

Shure, M. B., & Spivack, G. (1972). Means-ends thinking, Weissberg, R. P., & Gesten, E. L. (1982). Considerations for
adjustment and social class among elementary developing effective school-based social problem-
school-aged children. Journal of Consulting and solving (SPS) training programs. School Psychology
Clinical Psychology, 38, 348-353. Review, 11, 56-63.
Shure, M. B., & Spivack, G. (1978). Problem solving
techniques in childrearing. San Francisco, CA:
Jossey-Bass.
Shure, M. B., & Spivack, G. (1979). Interpersonal problem
solving thinking and adjustment in the mother-child
dyad. In M. W. Kent & J. E. Rolf (Eds.). Primary
prevention of psychopathology: Vol. III. Social
competence in children (pp. 201-219). Hanover, NH:
University Press of New England.
Shure, M. B., & Spivack, G. (1980). Interpersonal problem
solving as a mediator of behavioral adjustment in
preschool and kindergarten children. Journal of
Applied Developmental Psychology, 1, 29-44.
Shure, M. B., & Spivack, G. (1982). Interpersonal problem
solving in young children: A cognitive approach to
prevention. American Journal of Community
Psychology, 10, 341-356.
Shure, M. B., Spivack, G., & Jaeger, M. (1971). Problem-
solving thinking and adjustment among
disadvantaged preschool children. Child
Development, 42, 1791-1803.
Spivack, G., & Levine, M. (1963). Self-regulation in acting-
out and normal adolescents. Report M-4531.
Washington, DC: National Institutes of Mental
Health.
Spivack, G., & Shure, M. B. (1974). Social adjustment of
young children: A cognitive approach to solving
real-life problems. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Spivack, G., & Shure, M. B. (1982). Interpersonal problem-
solving and clinical theory. In B. Lahey & A. E.
Kazdin (Eds.), Advances in child clinical psychology.
Vol. 5. (pp. 323-372). New York: Plenum.

Prevention and Health Promotion: Research, Social Action, Practice And Training: Volume 9, Issue 1 14

RESEARCH, SOCIAL ACTION, PRACTICE AND TRAINING

Examining Associations Between Residential Status, Protective


Behavioral Strategy Use, and Alcohol-Related Negative Consequences
Among College Students
Kray A. Scully
Danielle P. Cottonham
Margo C. Villarosa
Saarah D. Kison
Michael B. Madson
University of Southern Mississippi

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Michael B. Madson, University of Southern
Mississippi, 118 College Drive #5025, Hattiesburg, MS 39406-5025. E-mail: michael.madson@usm.edu

Abstract Introduction

Alcohol consumption, alcohol-related negative consequences, Alcohol use by college students is considered an
and use of protective behavioral strategies (PBS) are important increasingly serious public health problem, as alcohol is the
to consider when preventing college student alcohol use given most reportedly used psychoactive substance among college
the serious public health implications. Further, there is a need students (American College Health Association [ACHA],
to examine contextual variables, such as where students live, 2008). This is alarming due to the range of alcohol-related
as possible explanations for drinking behaviors and targets for negative consequences, such as hangovers, assaults, and
prevention initiatives. The current study explored the degree to sexual victimization, that can occur as a result of college
which college students residential status and PBS use predicts student drinking (White & Hingson, 2013). Protective
alcohol-related negative consequences while accounting for behavioral strategy (PBS) use has been identified as a safe
consumption differences among 705 traditional age college drinking approach that may reduce harm associated with
students. Living in residential halls accounted for the fewest college students alcohol use (Linden, Kite, Braitman, &
alcohol-related negative consequences, while living in Greek Henson, 2014). One contextual factor that might help to
housing accounted for the most alcohol-related negative explain college students safe and harmful drinking
consequences. Use of both factors of PBS (i.e., serious harm experiences is residential status; where students live may
reduction and controlled consumption) were related to fewer influence how and when they engage in alcohol use behaviors.
negative consequences. Research and prevention implications The current study assessed the degree to which college
are discussed. students residential status was related to alcohol-related
negative consequences while examining the preventative
ability of PBS use.

Prevention and Health Promotion: Research, Social Action, Practice And Training: Volume 9, Issue 1 15

According to the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse empirical support (Kenney & LaBrie, 2013; Linden et al.,
and Alcoholism (NIAAA; 2012), over 80% of college students 2014; Madson et al., 2013b; Martin et al., 2012). The use of
have consumed alcohol during a 30-day period, with nearly PBS consists of direct planning for controlled drinking (e.g.,
half participating in heavy episodic drinking at least once over setting a limit on the number of drinks one consumes) and
a two-week period. Alcohol use at these levels is concerning indirect actions to minimize serious harm when drinking (e.g.,
to university prevention personnel because of the associated using a designated driver; Howard, Griffin, Boekeloo, Lake, &
alcohol-related negative consequences, which have resulted in Bellows, 2007). Direct/controlled consumption strategies tend
approximately one-third of college students meeting the to be more strongly related to reduced alcohol consumption;
criteria for alcohol abuse (Borsari, Murphy, & Barnett, 2007). whereas indirect/serious harm reduction strategies are more
All degrees of alcohol consumption can lead to a variety of strongly related to fewer alcohol-related negative
physical (e.g., assault and physical injury), mental (e.g., consequences (DeMartini et al., 2013; Madson, Arnau, &
memory loss), and academic (e.g., failing a test or missing Lambert, 2013a). Factors such as mental health (e.g.,
class) consequences for college student drinkers and their non- depression, social anxiety), race, and sex are linked with
drinking peers (Boekeloo, Novik, & Bush, 2011; Hingson, degrees of PBS use, such that students who are less
Zha, & Weitzman, 2009; Martin et al., 2012; Wechsler, Lee, psychologically healthy, White, and male report fewer PBS
Hall, Wagenaar, & Lee, 2002). Almost one-third of college use than those who are psychologically healthy, African-
student drinkers reported experiencing at least one alcohol- American or Hispanic, and female (LaBrie, Kenney, & Lac,
related negative consequence in the past 30 days (ACHA, 2010; Madson & Ziegler-Hill, 2013; Villarosa, Madson,
2008). Additionally, non-drinking students have reported Zeigler-Hill, Noble, & Mohn, 2014). Paralleling harmful
experiencing a greater number of secondhand alcohol-related drinking behaviors, safe drinking strategies, such as PBS, can
negative consequences, such as disruption of studies and be learned in a college students environment (Durkin, Wolfe,
sleep, property damage, and verbal, physical, and sexual & Clark, 2005). Based on social learning theory, it may be
violence (Palmer, McMahon, Rounsaville, & Bell, 2010; important to consider contextual factors, such as where
Wechsler et al., 2002a). The college environment is students live, to better understand what predicts students
conducive to alcohol use behaviors, and, by extension, increased alcohol use and decreased PBS use, and by
alcohol-related negative consequences (Akers, 1998), which in extension, leads students to experience more or less alcohol-
part may be explained by concepts of social learning theory, related negative consequences. Consequently, learning more
such as the development of alcohol expectancies, norm about contextual factors within the scope of social learning
perceptions, and modeling (Madson, Moorer, Zeigler-Hill, can better inform safe drinking prevention efforts on college
Bonnell, & Villarosa, 2013b; Neighbors et al., 2008). Given campuses.
increases in primary and secondary alcohol-related negative Residential status serves as a possible explanation for
consequences (Hingson et al., 2009; ACHA, 2008), many the rates of alcohol-related negative consequences and
researchers and college health professionals have focused on variations in PBS use among college students (Cacciola &
identifying safe drinking strategies in an attempt to ameliorate Nevid, 2014; Hummer, LaBrie, & Pederson, 2012). Akers
alcohol-related negative consequences on college campuses (1998) proposed that risky behaviors, such as participation in
(Madson & Zeigler-Hill, 2013; Martens et al., 2004; Martin et harmful drinking behaviors, are learned from ones peer
al., 2012). network. Researchers suggest that the college years are
The use of protective behavioral strategies (PBS) is marked by an expansion of ones social networks, which
one approach to safe drinking that has received increased involves the introduction of new ideas and attitudes (Wrzus,

Prevention and Health Promotion: Research, Social Action, Practice And Training: Volume 9, Issue 1 16

Hnel, Wagner, & Neyer, 2013). According to social learning the relationship between a wider array of residential statuses
theory, as applied to college student alcohol use, drinking (i.e., with parents, apartments off-campus) and alcohol-related
behaviors may be facilitated by factors such as positive negative consequences needs to be further explored, as well as
evaluations/expectations of drinking and perceived norms their relationship to PBS use.
engendered by ones peer network including the peers with The current study sought to examine the degree to
whom they live (Durkin et al., 2005). Thus, residential status which residential status and PBS use accounted for alcohol-
may play a crucial role in the development of a college related negative consequences in traditional age college
students attitudes towards drinking, meriting the exploration students above and beyond alcohol consumption. Researchers
of this factor as a possible explanation for alcohol-related suggest that consumption variances can encapsulate
negative consequences and PBS use. differences among demographic variables, such as sex and
Further exploration of residential status is also race (Borsari et al., 2007; Madson & Ziegler-Hill, 2013),
needed due to the dearth of literature regarding this factor. therefore, we controlled for alcohol use in the present study.
Thus far, the majority of alcohol-related research exploring Further, the current study explored the differences in PBS use
residential status has focused solely on Greek organizations among the different residential statuses. Because alcohol
and Greek housing or residence halls (Chauvin, 2012; consumption behaviors are normative in a college students
Hummer et al., 2012; Ragsdale et al., 2012; Scott-Sheldon, environment, it was hypothesized that residential status would
Carey, & Carey, 2008). Previously, most findings have shown be a significant predictor, above the effects of alcohol
that students who engage in more alcohol consumption and consumption, of alcohol-related negative consequences in
experience more alcohol-related negative consequences on college students, such that students living in Greek housing
college campuses have been those with Greek life affiliation and apartments off-campus would experience the most
and those living in Greek housing (Bersamin, Paschall, Saltz, alcohol-related negative consequences, while those living in
& Zamboanga, 2012; Ragsdale et al., 2012; Scott-Sheldon et residential halls and with parents would experience the least.
al., 2008). Page and OHagerty (2006) found that there was Since participation in safe drinking behaviors may also be
greater risk for heavy-drinking and alcohol-related problems influenced by ones surroundings, it was hypothesized that
among members of fraternities and sororities than among students living in housing with higher social interaction (i.e.,
students living in residence halls. However, the findings of residential halls and Greek housing) would report fewer PBS
the relationship between living in residence halls and alcohol than more isolated settings (i.e., apartment off-campus and
use have been mixed. Akmatov, Mikolasjczyk, Meier, and with parents).
Kramer (2011) showed that living in residential halls has been Methods
related to problematic drinking behaviors; conversely, Boyd et Participants and Procedures
al. (2008) found that less alcohol consumption was associated Participants included 705 traditional age (M = 20.21,
with fewer primary and secondary alcohol-related negative SD = 1.94) college students from a mid-sized southeastern
consequences for students living in residential halls. Until university. Those eligible for the study consumed alcohol at
recently, little attention has been given to other residential least once within 30 days prior to completing the survey. A
environments, such as living with parents (Cacciola & Nevid, majority of participants (80.5%) were female with a relatively
2014) or in apartments off-campus (Wechsler, Dowdall, even distribution across academic status30% freshmen,
Davenport, & Castillo, 1995). Additionally, we were unable 24.9% sophomores, 21.5% juniors, and 23.6% seniors. Most
to find any studies exploring the association between students identified as White, non-Hispanic (61.7%), or African
residential status and PBS use among college students. Thus, American (33.7%). The majority of participants reported

Prevention and Health Promotion: Research, Social Action, Practice And Training: Volume 9, Issue 1 17

living in a residential hall (46.5%), or in an apartment off- Protective behavioral strategies. The Protective
campus (40.4%), with 7.4% living in Greek housing, and 5.7% Behavioral Strategy Scale Revised (PBSS-R; Madson et al.,
of participants living with parents. Upon providing an 2013b) was used to measure participants use of protective
Institutional Review Board approved informed consent, strategies while consuming alcohol. The 18-item PBSS-R
participants completed measures using a secure online survey assesses PBS use across two domains: indirect/Serious Harm
system assessing alcohol consumption, alcohol-related Reduction (SHR) and direct/Controlled Consumption (CC).
negative consequences, and PBS use in partial fulfillment of Items on SHR include using a designated driver and
class credit. avoiding drinking games, while items on the CC subscale
Measures include determine not to exceed a set number of drinks and
Typical alcohol consumption. The Daily Drinking stop drinking at a predetermined time. Participants were
Questionnaire (DDQ) was used to assess typical weekly asked to score the items using a Likert-type scale ranging from
alcohol consumption. Participants reported the number of 1 (never) to 6 (always). Subscale scores range from 12 to 72
drinks they consumed and time spent drinking for each day for CC and from 6 to 36 for SHR, with higher scores
during a typical week (Collins, Parks, & Marlatt, 1985). representing greater PBS use. The PBSS-R has been shown to
Participants were then classified into one of three drinking have convergent validity (Madson et al., 2013b). In the
categories based on guidelines offered by Collins et al. (1985): present study, the PBSS-R subscales demonstrated acceptable
light (3 drinks or less drinks/week), moderate (4-11 drinks per internal consistencies (CC - = 0.87 and SHR - = 0.80).
week), and heavy drinkers (more than 12 drinks per week). Data Analytic Approach
Alcohol-related negative consequences. The 20- Three separate hierarchical regression analyses were
item Young Adult Alcohol Problems Screening Test conducted to assess residential statuses (i.e., residential halls,
(YAAPST) was used to assess alcohol-related negative Greek housing, apartments off campus, with parents) and PBS
consequences within the past year (Kahler, Strong, Read, use (i.e., PBS-SHR, PBS-CC), as predictors of alcohol-related
Palfai, & Wood, 2004). Participants responded to the first 6 negative consequences, as well as residential status as a
items, such as Have you gotten into physical fights when predictor of each type of PBS use. Because of its high
drinking? and Have you gotten in trouble at work or school response rate, college students living in residential halls were
because of drinking?, using a Likert-type scale that ranged embedded in the constant for comparison purposes (Field,
from 0 (no, never) to 9 (yes, 40 or more times) and responded 2013). For all analyses, consumption or participants DDQ
to the remaining 14 items, such as Have you neglected your scores were utilized as a control variable in Step 1. The
obligations, your family, your work, or school work? and remaining residential statuses were entered as predictor
Has your drinking gotten you into sexual situations which variables in Step 2 for each analysis.
you later regretted?, using a Likert-type scale ranging from 0 Results
(no, never) to 4 (yes, 3 or more times). Scores range from 0 to Means, standard deviations, and intercorrelations for
110 with higher scores indicating a greater frequency of the measures are presented in Table 1. The mean number of
experiencing alcohol-related negative consequences (Walters drinks consumed weekly was 9.44 (SD = 11.75). Moderate
& Baer, 2006). The YAAPST has been found to have drinkers represented the majority of the sample (n = 296,
satisfactory criterion validity, concurrent validity, and 41.2%). Two hundred forty-eight participants (34.5%) were
construct validity (Hurlbut & Sher, 1992). Internal classified as light drinkers and 175 participants (24.3%) were
consistency with this sample was adequate ( = 0.80). classified as heavy drinkers (Collins et al., 1985). Consistent
with prior literature, men reported drinking more alcohol,

Prevention and Health Promotion: Research, Social Action, Practice And Training: Volume 9, Issue 1 18

t(703) = 6.35, p < 0.001, and experiencing more alcohol- There were no significant associations found between PBS-
related negative consequences than women, t(703) = 4.47, p < SHR and living in Greek housing and with parents. Similarly,
0.001, further supporting the need to account for consumption residential status predicted variance in PBS-CC use,
differences in the analysis. Additionally, means and standard accounting for 5% of unique variance after accounting for
deviations for PBS use across both factors, for all residential consumption differences. Specifically, living in Greek
statuses, are presented in Table 2. housing (B = 42.58, t = -2.85, p < 0.01) and in apartments off-
Residential Status and PBS Use as Predictors of Alcohol- campus (B = 44.50, t = -3.33, p < 0.01) predicted significantly
Related Negative Consequences decreased use of PBS-CC with the strength of that prediction
Results for the first hierarchical regression analysis being stronger for living in Greek housing. Conversely, living
can be found in Table 3. After accounting for alcohol in residential halls (B = 48.87, t = 70.08, p < 0.001) predicted
consumption and residential status, PBS use accounted for an increased use of PBS-CC. No relationships were found
additional 13% of the variance in alcohol-related negative between students living with their parents and PBS-SHR or
consequences. College student residential status positively PBS-CC use.
predicted alcohol-related negative consequences after Discussion
accounting for consumption differences. All living conditions The present study aimed to explore the relationships
significantly predicted alcohol-related negative consequences, among residential status, PBS use, and alcohol-related
such that students living in Greek housing (B = 14.22, t = 3.08, negative consequences in college students while accounting
p < .01), with parents (B = 13.34, t = 2.13, p < .05), and in off- for gender differences in alcohol consumption patterns. Based
campus apartments (B = 12.68, t = 3.35, p < .001) reported on our results, it appears that residential status might serve an
higher rates of alcohol-related negative consequences. important role in predicting whether or not college students
Specifically, living in Greek housing predicted the most experience adverse alcohol-related outcomes, in part because
alcohol-related negative consequences, while living in of its significance to students social networks and potential
residential halls (B = 10.38, t = 22.52, p < 0.001) predicted the modeling of behavior. Moreover, results from this study
least alcohol-related negative consequences. Conversely, both further support PBSs utility as a safe drinking behavior
factors of PBS use had inverse associations with alcohol- college students can use to reduce alcohol-related negative
related negative consequences. Increases in PBS-SHR (B = consequences. Alcohol researchers and practitioners may
10.24, t = -2.05, p < .05) and PBS-CC (B = 10.15, t = -8.37, p benefit in a concerted effort to increase PBS exposure,
< .001) use predicted fewer alcohol-related negative especially for students living in Greek housing and off-
consequences. campus, such that these safety strategies become a learned part
Residential Status as a Predictor of Protective Behavioral of college students normal alcohol use behaviors.
Strategy Use Alcohol-related Negative Consequences
Results for the second set of hierarchical regression Regarding alcohol-related negative consequences,
analyses can be found in Table 4. Residential status living in residential halls predicted the fewest alcohol-related
significantly predicted PBS-SHR use and accounted for 6% of negative consequences, whereas those living in Greek housing
the unique variance after controlling for alcohol consumption. predicted the most alcohol-related negative consequences.
Living in apartments off-campus (B = 30.72, t = -4.01, p < Drinking is often perceived as a normative behavior in Greek
0.001) significantly predicted decreased use of PBS-SHR life, which can result in students engaging in riskier drinking
while living in residential halls (B = 32.33, t = 118.70, p < behaviors and ultimately contribute to increased alcohol-
0.001) significantly predicted increased use of PBS-SHR. related negative consequences (Barry, 2007; Goodwin, 1989;

Prevention and Health Promotion: Research, Social Action, Practice And Training: Volume 9, Issue 1 19

Sher, Barthalow, & Nanda, 2001). Moreover, research depending on the drinking policies of the university.
suggests there is an inherent risk for those living in Greek Further, living in apartments off-campus and with
housing because they experience more injury and harm as a parents was predictive of more alcohol-related negative
result of alcohol consumption (Walters, Roudsari, Vader, & consequences than living in residence halls. A unique finding
Harris, 2007). Because participation in harmful drinking was that living with ones parents while attending college was
behaviors is viewed as normative for college students in Greek predictive of the second highest rate of alcohol-related
housing and modeled by other students, adverse outcomes negative consequences. This could be a product of
may result in a pattern of learned behaviors, attitudes, and consequences that may result from increased parental
ideas in this environment (Durkin et al., 2005; Scott-Sheldon monitoring (Patock-Peckham & Morgan-Lopez, 2006) and
et al., 2008). Such behavior could also be a result of a lack of engaging in drinking behaviors that tend to be more normative
enforced policies in procedures that are present at the to the college environment. The higher number of alcohol-
university level. Perhaps, a deficiency of supervision and related consequences may also be attributed to the salience of
regulation at apartment complexes and Greek housing may peer influence to those living with their parents (Valliant &
contribute to higher consumption and alcohol-related negative Scanlan, 1996). Future studies are needed to investigate the
consequences (Wechsler et al., 1995). Thus, other non-policy factors associated with more alcohol-related consequences
related prevention efforts may need to be considered for experienced by college students living at home. Additionally,
students living in these environments. increased campus-wide prevention efforts have been effective
College students living on campus in residential halls in reducing maladaptive drinking behaviors across residential
reported experiencing the least amount of alcohol-related statuses (Wechsler et al., 2002b). Perhaps, future prevention
negative consequences. Although this result lends support to endeavors could include initiatives such as safe drinking
residential halls as a protective factor (Boyd et al., 2008; Page policies with college student groups and organizations in
& OHagerty, 2006), some researchers have found residence environments to enhance external supervision.
halls to be a risk factor for developing harmful drinking Protective Behavioral Strategies
behaviors (OHare, 1990; Willoughby & Carroll, 2009). A novel aspect of this study was exploring the degree
These inconsistencies may be attributable to varying campus to which residential status predicted PBS use. We found that
drinking policies (e.g., zero alcohol tolerance policies vs. less college students residential hall living arrangements were
drinking restrictive policies). For instance, the current study related to increased use of PBS-SHR and PBS-CC. Such
collected data from a campus with a relatively restrictive findings suggest that living in residential halls may be a
alcohol policy (i.e., no alcohol in the residence halls even for preventive and protective factor in and of itself, which is
students 21 and older), which may have contributed to the contrary to Willoughby and Carrolls (2009) findings about
lower levels of drinking for students living in the residence residence halls potentially being a risk factor for harmful
halls. Further, this finding may be explained by vicarious behavior. Moreover, living in apartments off-campus
learning of peer alcohol use norms (Ward & Gryczynski, predicted less use of PBS-SHR use and PBS-CC use while
2009), wherein participation in alcohol use behaviors may be Greek housing was only predictive of lowered PBS-CC use.
discouraged or encouraged by a college students immediate This finding may be due to the risky drinking culture created
environment. Future research should compare the differential in college environments (Chauvin, 2012) and the dearth of
effects of residential status on drinking behaviors among prevention and intervention strategies available to those living
campuses with diverse drinking policies. Such findings could off-campus and in Greek housing, thus making college
inform whether prevention efforts should be tailored students less inclined to utilize PBS. It can be speculated that

Prevention and Health Promotion: Research, Social Action, Practice And Training: Volume 9, Issue 1 20

problematic alcohol use behaviors are encouraged and present study restrict the generalizability of the results. We
accepted as normal by college students in these residential acknowledge that a simple categorization of residential status
statuses (Roemer & Walsh, 2014). Moreover, with the lack of into four separate variables does not completely capture the
PBS exposure in these environments, college students may not complexity of the construct. Future research may benefit from
be able to learn safe drinking strategies that can protect examining residential status as a covariate or moderator in
themselves thus, keeping safe drinking from becoming a associations between alcohol use behaviors and environmental
normative behavior. Therefore, availability and accessibility variables, such as drinking context. These analyses are
to PBS needs to be addressed for college students as a whole. exploratory in nature and are intended to generate further
Implications research on residential status and college student peer-related
Our findings may have implications for alcohol use variables (e.g., perceived norms, expectancies, resistance to
prevention and intervention efforts. One possible proactive peer influence) in an effort to better understand how the
policy would be the creation of a university-based or factors may better predict student drinking behaviors.
community overseen safe-ride home program, particularly for Moreover, while consumption was controlled in the data
Greek students and those living off-campus. A safe-ride home analyses, over 80% of respondents were female, which limits
program involves the use of designated drivers to provide safe the scope of this studys findings. This study employed a
transportation for students who consume alcohol. The cross-sectional design, subsequently limiting inferential
presence of such a program could encourage safer drinking conclusions. The use of more longitudinal designs assessing
behaviors as well as possibly reduce alcohol-related negative drinking behaviors as students change living locations over the
consequences related to personal injury and drunk driving course of an academic career may help address this limitation.
(Rivara et al., 2007). Similarly, based on our findings, it This research was conducted at one midsized university with a
appears that increased exposure to PBS for those living off no alcohol campus policy in the southern United States, which
campus and in Greek housing is needed. Many colleges and may limit generalizability of findings. Future studies may
universities require incoming freshmen students to complete address this limitation by including multiple universities
an alcohol education course before being successfully across the country with different types of campus alcohol
admitted to the institution. The implementation of an annual, policies.
online alcohol psychoeducational program for all students References
could increase students exposure to PBS and, consequently,
Akers, R. L. (1998). Social Learning and Social Structure: A
increase PBS use, likely reducing students susceptibility to
General Theory of Crime and Deviance. Boston:
alcohol-related negative consequences. Moreover, future
Northeastern University Press.
research should take into account other alcohol-related
Akmatov, M. K., Mikolasjczyk, R. T., Meier, S., & Kramer,
variables, like drinking context and self-regulation, regarding
A. (2011). Alcohol consumption among university
the roles they play concerning residential status and the
students in North RhineWestphalia, Germany
aforementioned alcohol-related factors. Future research might
results from a multicenter cross-sectional study.
also benefit from exploring the mediating effects of PBS on
Journal of American College Health, 59, 620-626.
the association between residential status and alcohol-related
doi: 10.1080/07448481.2010.520176
negative consequences.
The American College Health Association (ACHA), American
Limitations
college health association national college health
Although our findings contribute to the literature on
assessment spring 2008 reference group data report
college student drinking, some limitations associated with the

Prevention and Health Promotion: Research, Social Action, Practice And Training: Volume 9, Issue 1 21

(Abridged). (2008). Journal of American College indirect protective strategies in young adults.
Health, 57, 477-488. doi: 10.3200/jach.57.5.477-488 Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 27, 615-626. doi:
Barry, A. E. (2007). Using theory-based constructs to explore 10.1037/a0033636
the impact of Greek membership on alcohol-related Durkin, K. F., Wolfe, T. W., & Clark, G. A. (2005). College
beliefs and behaviors: A systematic literature review. students and binge drinking: An evaluation of social
Journal of American College Health, 56, 307-315. learning theory. Sociological Spectrum, 25, 255-272.
doi: 10.3200/jach.56.3.307-316 doi: 10.1080/027321790518681
Bersamin, M. M., Paschall, M. J., Saltz, R. F., & Zamboanga, Field, A. P. (2013). Discovering statistics using IBM SPSS
B. L. (2012). Young adults and casual sex: The statistics: And sex and drugs and rock 'n' roll (4th
relevance of college drinking settings. Journal of Sex ed.). Los Angeles: Sage.
Research, 49, 274-282. doi: Goodwin, L. (1989). Explaining alcohol consumption and
10.1080/00224499.2010.548012 related experiences among fraternity and sorority
Borsari, B., Murphy, J. G., & Barnett, N. P. (2007). Predictors members. Journal of College Student Development,
of alcohol use during the first year of college: 30, 448458.
Implications for prevention. Addictive Behaviors, 32, Hingson, R. W., Zha, W., & Weitzman, E. R. (2009).
2062-2086. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.01.017 Magnitude of and trends of alcohol-related mortality
Boyd, C. J., McCabe, S. E., Cranford, J. A., Morales, M., and morbidity among U.S. college students ages 18-
Lange, J. E., Reed, M. B., Ketchie, J. M., Scott, 24, 1998-2005. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and
M. S. (2008). Heavy episodic drinking and its Drugs, 16, 12-20. doi: 10.15288/jsads.2009.s16.12
consequences: The protective effects of same-sex, Howard, D. E., Griffin, M., Boekeloo, B., Lake, K., &
residential living-learning communities for Bellows, D. (2007). Staying safe while consuming
undergraduate women. Addictive Behaviors, 33, 987- alcohol: A qualitative study of the protective
993. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2008.03.005. strategies and informational needs of college
Cacciola, E. E. T., & Nevid, J. S. (2014). Alcohol freshmen. Journal of American College Health, 56,
consumption in relation to residence status and 247-254. doi: 10.3200/jach.56.3.247-254
ethnicity in college students. Psychology of Addictive Hummer, J. F., LaBrie, J. W., & Pedersen, E. R. (2012). First
Behaviors, 28, 1278-1283. doi: 10.1037/a0038362 impressions on the scene: The influence of the
Chauvin, C. D. (2012). Social norms and associations with immediate reference group on incoming first-year
college binge drinking. Sociological Inquiry, 82, 257- students' alcohol behavior and attitudes. Journal of
281. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-682X.2011.00400.x College Student Development, 53, 149-162. doi:
Collins, R., Parks, G. A., & Marlatt, G. (1985). Social 10.1353/csd.2012.0008
determinants of alcohol consumption: The effects of Hurlbut, S. C., & Sher, K. J. (1992). Assessing alcohol
social interaction and model status on the self- problems in college students. Journal of American
administration of alcohol. Journal of Consulting and College Health, 41, 49-58. doi:
Clinical Psychology, 53, 189-200. doi: 10.1080/07448481.1992.10392818
10.1037/0022006X.53.2.189 Kahler, C. W., Strong, D. R., Read, J. P., Palfai, T. P., &
DeMartini, K. S., Palmer, R. S., Leeman, R. F., Corbin, W. R., Wood, M. D. (2004). Mapping the continuum of
Toll, B. A., Fucito, L. M., OMalley, S. S. (2013). alcohol problems in college students: A Rasch model
Drinking less and drinking smarter: Direct and

Prevention and Health Promotion: Research, Social Action, Practice And Training: Volume 9, Issue 1 22

analysis. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 18, 322- Behaviors, 18, 390-393. doi: 10.1037/0893-
333. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.18.4.322 164x.18.4.390
Kenney, S. R., & LaBrie, J. W. (2013). Use of protective Martin, R. J., Cremeens, J. L., Umstattd, M. R., Usdan, S. L.,
behavioral strategies and reduced alcohol risk: Talbott-Forbes, L., & Garner, M.M. (2012). Drinking
Examining the moderating effects of mental health, behavior, protective behavioural strategies and school
sex, and race. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 27, performance in students. Drugs: Education,
997-1009. doi: 10.1037/a0033262 Prevention, and Policy, 19, 64-71. doi:
LaBrie, J. W., Kenney, S. R., & Lac, A. (2010). The use of 10.3109/09687637.2011.560910
protective behavioral strategies is related to reduced National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism
risk in heavy drinking college students with poorer (NIAAA), College drinking. (2012). Understanding
physical and mental health. Journal of Drug the Impact of Alcohol on Human Health and Well
Education, 40, 361-378. doi: 10.2190/DE.40.40.c Being. Retrieved from the National Institute of
Linden, A. N., Kite, B. A., Braitman, A. L., & Henson, J. M. Health: http://www.niaaa.nih.gov/alcohol-
(2014). Protective behavioral strategy use and health/special-populations-co-occurring-
motivations for drinking: Exploring alternatives for disorders/college-drinking
drinking strategies. Addictive Behaviors, 39, 469-472. Neighbors, C., O'Connor, R., Lewis, M., Chawla, N., Lee, C.,
doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.10.008 & Fossos, N. (2008). The relative impact of
Madson, M. B., Arnau, R. C., & Lambert, S. J. (2013a). injunctive norms on college student drinking: the role
Development and psychometric evaluation of the of reference group. Psychology of Addictive
revised Protective Behavioral Strategies Scale. Behaviors, 22, 576-581. doi: 10.1037/a0013043
Psychological Assessment, 25, 556-567. doi: OHare, T. M. (1990). Drinking in college: Consumption
10.1037/a00317688 patterns, problems, sex-differences
Madson, M. B., Moorer, K. D., Ziegler-Hill, V., Bonnell, M. and legal drinking age. Journal of Studies on Alcohol,
A., & Villarosa, M. (2013b). Alcohol expectancies, 51, 536-541. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1990.51.536
protective behavioral strategies, and alcohol-related Page, R.M., & OHagerty, M. (2006). Type of student
outcomes: A moderated mediation study. Drugs: residence as a factor in college students alcohol
Education, Prevention, and Policy, 20, 286-296. doi: consumption and social normative perceptions
10.3109/09687637.2013.766788 regarding alcohol use. Journal of Child & Adolescent
Madson, M. B., & Ziegler-Hill, V. (2013). Protective Substance Abuse, 15, 15-31. doi:
behavioral strategies, alcohol consumption, and 10.1300/j029/v15n03_02
negative alcohol-related consequences: Do race and Palmer, R. S., McMahon, T. J., Rounsaville, B. J., & Ball, S.
sex moderate these associations? Journal of Ethnicity A. (2010). Coercive sexual experiences, protective
in Substance Abuse, 12, 242-258. doi: behavioral strategies, alcohol expectancies and
10.1080/15332640.2013.798848 consumption among male and female college
Martens, M. P., Taylor, K. K., Damann, K. M., Page, J. C., students. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 25,
Mowry, E. S., & Cimini, M. D. (2004). Protective 1563-1578. doi: 10.1177/0886260509354581
behavioral strategies when drinking alcohol and their
relationship to negative alcohol-related consequences
in college students. Psychology of Addictive

Prevention and Health Promotion: Research, Social Action, Practice And Training: Volume 9, Issue 1 23

mediating effects of drinking motives. Psychology of
Patock-Peckham, J. A., & Morgan-Lopez, A. A. (2006). Addictive Behaviors, 28, 710-718. doi:
College drinking behaviors: Mediational links 10.1037/a0036501
between parenting styles, impulse control, and Walters, S. T., & Baer, J. S. (2006). Talking with college
alcohol-related outcomes. Psychology of Addictive students about alcohol: Motivational strategies for
Behaviors, 20, 117-125. doi: 10.1037/0893- reducing abuse. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
164x.20.2.117 Walters, S. T., Roudsari, B. S., Vader, A. M., & Harris, T. R.
Ragsdale, K., Porter, J. R., Mathews, R., White, A., Gore- (2007). Correlates of protective behavior utilization
Felton, C, & McGarvey, E. L. (2012). Liquor before among heavy-drinking college students. Addictive
beer, youre in the clear: Binge drinking and other Behaviors, 32, 2633-2644. doi:
risk behaviours among fraternity/sorority members 10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.06.022
and their non-Greek peers. Journal of Substance Use, Ward, B. W., & Gryczynski, J. (2009). Social learning theory
17, 323-339. doi: 10.3109/14659891.2011.583312 and the effects of living arrangement on heavy
Rivara, F. P., Relyea-Chew, A., Wang, J., Riley, S., Boisvert, alcohol use: Results from a national study of college
D., & Gomez, T. (2007). Drinking behaviors in students. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and
young adults: the potential role of designated driver Drugs, 70, 364-372. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2009.70.364
and safe ride home programs. Injury Prevention, 13, Wechsler, H., Dowdall, G. W., Davenport, A., & Castillo, S.
168-172. doi: 10.1136/ip.2006.015032 (1995). Correlates of college student binge
Roemer, A., & Walsh, Z. (2014). Where you live matters: The drinking. American Journal of Public Health, 85,
roles of living arrangement and self-esteem on 921-926. doi: 10.2105/ajph.85.7.921
college students hazardous drinking Wechsler, H., Lee, J. E., Hall, J., Wagenaar, A. C., & Lee, H.
behaviors. Addiction Research & Theory, 22, 474- (2002a). Secondhand effects of student alcohol use
480. doi: 10.3109/16066359.2013.877454 reported by neighbors of colleges: The role of alcohol
Scott-Sheldon, L. A. J., Carey, K. B., & Carey, M. P. (2008). outlets. Social Science and Medicine, 55, 425-435.
Health behavior and college students: Does Greek doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(01)00259-3
affiliation matter? Journal of Behavioral Medicine, Wechsler, H., Lee, J. E., Kuo, M., Seibring, M., Nelson, T. F.,
31, 61-70. doi: 10.1007/s10865-007-9136-1 & Lee, H. (2002b). Trends in college binge drinking
Sher, K. J., Bartholow, B. D., & Nanda S. (2001). Short- and during a period of increased prevention efforts:
long-term effects of fraternity and sorority Findings from 4 Harvard School of Public Health
membership on heavy drinking: A social norms College Alcohol Study surveys: 19932001. Journal
perspective. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 15, of American College Health, 50, 203-217. doi:
4251. doi: 10.1037/0893-164x.15.1.42 10.1080/07448480209595713
Valliant, P.M., & Scanlan, P. (1996). Personality, living Willoughby, B. J., & Carroll, J. S. (2009). The impact of
arrangements, and alcohol use by first year university living in co-ed resident halls on risk-taking in college
students. Social Behavior and Personality, 24, 151 students. Journal of American College Health, 58,
156. doi: 10.2224/sbp.1996.24.2.151 241-246. doi: 10.1080/07448480903295359
Villarosa, M. C., Madson, M. B., Zeigler-Hill, V., Noble, J. J., Wrzus, C., Hnel, M., Wagner, J., & Neyer, F. J. (2013).
& Mohn, R. S. (2014). Social anxiety symptoms and Social network changes and life events across the life
drinking behaviors among college students: The

Prevention and Health Promotion: Research, Social Action, Practice And Training: Volume 9, Issue 1 24

span: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 53 80. doi: 10.1037/a0028601

Table 1

Means, Standard Deviations, and Intercorrelations for the DDQ, YAAPST, PBSS-SHR, & PBSS-CC.

M (SD) DDQ YAAPST PBSS-SHR PBSS-CC

DDQ 9.44 (11.75) --


YAAPST 11.72 (10.27) .46* --
PBSS-SHR 31.51(5.16) -.22* -.31* --
PBSS-CC 45.92(13.30 -.32* -.47* .49* --
Note: DDQ- Daily Drinking Questionnaire; YAAPST- Young Adult Alcohol Problems Screening Test; PBSS-SHR-
Protective Behavioral Strategies Scale- Serious Harm Reduction; PBSS-CC- Protective Behavioral Strategies Scale-
Controlled Consumption.

* p < 0.01

Table 2

Means and Standard Deviations for PBSS-R-SHR and PBSS-CC Across Residential Statuses

PBSS-R-SHR PBSS-R-CC
M (SD) M (SD)

Residential Halls 32.46 (4.98) 48.35 (14.32)


Greek Housing 31.13 (4.68) 41.57 (11.27)
Apartments Off-Campus 30.60 (5.34) 44.21 (12.20)
With Parents 30.88 (4.79) 44.22 (11.35)
Note: PBSS-R-SHR- Protective Behavioral Strategies Scale- Revised- Serious Harm Reduction; PBSS-R-CC-
Protective Behavioral Strategies Scale-Revised- Controlled Consumption

Prevention and Health Promotion: Research, Social Action, Practice And Training: Volume 9, Issue 1 25

Table 3

Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Experienced Alcohol-Related Negative Consequences among College Students
(N=705)

YAAPST

Predictor B SE B R2

Step 1: .21***
DDQ .40 .03 .46***
Step 2: .34***
Apartment OC 2.30 .69 .11**
Greek Housing 3.84 1.25 .10**
With Parents 2.97 1.39 .07*
PBS-SHR -.23 .30 -.30***
PBS-CC -.14 .70 -.07*
Note: DDQ- Daily Drinking Questionnaire; YAAPST- Young Adult Alcohol Problems Screening Test; PBS-R-
SHR- Protective Behavioral Strategies-Revised Serious Harm Reduction; PBS-R-CC- Protective Behavioral
Strategies-Revised- Controlled Consumption.

* p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.00

Table 4

Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analyses for Protective Behavioral Strategy Use among College Students
(N=705)

PBSS-R SHR PBSS-R CC

Predictor B SE B R2 B SE B R2

Step 1: .04*** .10***


DDQ -.10 .02 -.22*** -.36 .40 -.31***
Step 2: .06*** .12***
Apartment OC -1.62 .40 -.15*** -3.37 1.01 -.12**
Greek Housing -.94 .74 -.05 -5.29 2.86 -.10**
With Parents -1.46 .83 -.07 -3.68 2.07 -.06
Note: PBSS-R-SHR- Protective Behavioral Strategies Scale Revised- Serious Harm Reduction; PBSS-R-CC-
Protective Behavioral Strategies Scale Revised- Controlled Consumption.

* p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p <0.001

Prevention and Health Promotion: Research, Social Action, Practice And Training: Volume 9, Issue 1 26

RESEARCH, SOCIAL ACTION, PRACTICE AND TRAINING

Suicide Prevention Gatekeeper Training for Resident Assistants: A


Between Groups Control Design Study
Colleen Thomas
Martin Swanbrow Becker
Florida State University

Colleen Thomas and Martin A. Swanbrow Becker, Department of Educational Psychology and Learning Systems,
Florida State University. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Martin A. Swanbrow
Becker, Department of Educational Psychology and Learning Systems, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL,
32306. E-mail: mswanbrowbecker@fsu.edu

Abstract

Suicide remains a leading cause of death among college Post-semester surveys assessed for the impact of the
students (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention [CDC], training on RAs knowledge, attitudes, and perceived
2013), despite efforts to train campus gatekeepers, such as competency. Post-semester surveys also measured the
Resident Assistants (RAs), in suicide prevention. Research is frequency of RAs suicide prevention behaviors and
warranted to better understand the effectiveness of these interactions with distressed residents during the semester.
trainings and to determine how they might be adapted for Paired samples t-tests were used to assess within group
improved potency. This study represents the first known change. All RAs, regardless of condition, reported greater
between groups experimental design examining the efficacy perceived competency to intervene with a suicidal student at
of suicide prevention training for RAs on a college campus. post-semester. However, the treatment group reported
Participants were 160 RAs from a large southeastern significantly improved attitudes towards seeking professional
university. Approximately 60% of the RAs were female, and help, whereas the control groups attitudes worsened.
the ethnic representation of the sample was 64.6% Caucasian, Independent samples t-tests yielded no between group
9.3% Black, 8.1% Latino, 4.3% Asian, 9.9% mixed ethnicity, differences of frequency of intervention behaviors at post-
and 3.8% identified as Other. RAs in the treatment group semester; thus, the RAs trained in suicide prevention did not
participated in a one-hour suicide prevention gatekeeper report more interventions with distressed students as
training amidst their RA orientation trainings, while the compared to the control group. These results suggest that a
control group received a one-hour training in stress and time one-hour suicide prevention gatekeeper training was
management. All RAs completed pre-training surveys, which sufficient for improving participants knowledge about the
measured their: (a) baseline knowledge of the warning signs warning signs of suicide, perceived competency to intervene,
of suicide and intervention strategies with suicidal students, and attitudes towards help-seeking. However, the training did
(b) perceived competency to identify and help students in not lead to an increase in gatekeepers suicide prevention
distress, and (c) attitudes towards seeking professional help. behaviors and interventions with students in distress, and

Prevention and Health Promotion: Research, Social Action, Practice And Training: Volume 9, Issue 1 27

therefore many students in distress remain disconnected from
professional help. Future research should focus on adapting
suicide prevention gatekeeper trainings (e.g., emphasizing
role plays, adding booster sessions, addressing barriers to
intervention) to increase their effectiveness in increasing the
frequency of gatekeepers suicide prevention behaviors.

References

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). (2013).


Ten leading causes of death by age group, United
States-2013. Retrieved July 26, 2015 from
http://www.cdc.gov/injury/wisquars/pfd/leading_caus
es_of_death_by_age.

Prevention and Health Promotion: Research, Social Action, Practice And Training: Volume 9, Issue 1 28

RESEARCH, SOCIAL ACTION, PRACTICE AND TRAINING

Comparison Between International and Domestic Graduate


Students on Alcohol, Sexual Assault, and Prevention Efforts:
An Exploratory Study
Lei Wang
Leslie A. Fasone
Ellen L. Vaughan
Indiana University Bloomington

Lei Wang, Department of Counseling and Educational Psychology, Indiana University Bloomington; Leslie A.
Fasone, Department of Applied Health Science & Division of Student Affairs, Indiana University Bloomington;
Ellen L. Vaughan, Department of Counseling and Educational Psychology, Indiana University Bloomington.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Lei Wang, 201 N Rose Ave, Room 4054,
Bloomington, IN 47405. E-mail: lw55@indiana.edu, Tel: 573-777-2001

Abstract study. Participants included 52 (15%) IGS and 305 (85%)


DGS studying at a large public Midwestern university in
International students are a growing population on
spring 2014. The mean age of IGS was 27.87 (SD = 5.22) and
college campuses in the U.S. (Institute of International
the mean age of DGS was 28.92 (SD = 7.34). Approximately
Education, 2015); however, their mental health concerns have
58% of IGS and 60% of DGS were women. Eleven percent of
often been overlooked (Mori, 2000). There is limited
IGS and 15% of DGS self-identified as being part of the LGB
empirical research conducted on this population with regards
community. Most of the students resided off-campus (83%
to alcohol use, with the existing studies focused on the
IGS and 90% DGS). Participants were asked to report their
relationship between academic stress and alcohol use (Misra
frequency of alcohol use, incidents of
& Castillo, 2004). In addition, there are no studies on sexual
physical/emotional/sexual violence they have experienced, the
assault and prevention efforts among international students.
frequency of witnessing problematic behaviors (e.g., someone
This study aims to explore the differences between
being discriminated against, someone being hazed to join an
international graduate students (IGS) and domestic graduate
organization, someone who had too much to drink, someone
students (DGS) in terms of engagement in drinking behaviors,
who has been taken advantage of sexually), situations in
witnessing risky behaviors among peers, and ways to
which they believe it is important to intervene, and their
intervene to reduce risk.
confidence level of intervening in those situations. Chi-
Data from the American College Health Association
square and one-way ANOVA results revealed significant
National College Health Assessment II was utilized in this
differences between IGS and DGS. In terms of alcohol use,

Prevention and Health Promotion: Research, Social Action, Practice And Training: Volume 9, Issue 1 29

IGS were found to consume less alcohol in the last 30 days (F homesickness (Olivas & Li, 2006), etc., which may all
= 5.59, p < .05) and lower rates of driving after drinking any attribute to IGS not knowing how to intervene or even
alcohol ( = 15.86, p < .001). IGS reported encountering discouraging them to be an active bystander. Although this is
more incidents of sexual penetration attempted without their not an exhaustive list, we add that certain cultural values and
consent (F = 5.95, p < .05) and witnessing more struggles with the need to maintain documentation/visa status may also be
severe stress, anxiety or depression (F = 10.62, p = .001), potential barriers that prohibit them from intervening.
drinking too much (F = 8.68, p < .05), and abusing drugs (F =
References
6.01, p < .05) than DGS. Compared to DGS, IGS believed
that it was more important to intervene when someone has too Abbey, A. (2002). Alcohol-related sexual assault: A
much to drink (F = 8.78, p < .05), abused drugs (F = 4.77, p < common problem among college students.
.05), or been taken advantage of sexually (F = 12.72, p < Journal of Studies on Alcohol, Supplement,
.001). IGS felt more confident in their abilities to help 14, 118-128. doi:10.15288/jsas.2002.s14.118
someone who needed assistance in sexual assault/harassment Abbey, A., Zawacki, T., Buck, P. O., Clinton, A. M.,
situations (F = 4.07, p < .05) than DGS did. & McAuslan, P. (2001). Alcohol and sexual
Based on the findings of this study, it may be assault. Alcohol Research and Health, 25, 43-
helpful for prevention specialists to develop programming 51.
on providing resources to intervene in different situations with Institute of International Education (2015). Open
IGS. Although the IGS in this sample engage in fewer risky Doors Data: International Students: All
behaviors, they do report experiencing more attempts of non- Places of Origin. Retrieved June 8, 2015,
consensual sexual penetration as well as observing from http://www.iie.org/Research-and-
problematic behaviors. Several studies in the past have Publications/Open-Doors/Data/International-
examined alcohol-related sexual assault along domestic Students/All-Places-of-Origin/2012-14.
college students (e.g., Abbey, 2002; Abbey et al., 2001). Leong, F. T., & Mallinckrodt, B. (1990). Cross-
However, due to the cultural differences (Leong & cultural variations in stress and adjustment
Mallinckrodt, 1990) between their home countries and the among Asian and Caucasian graduate
U.S. as well as acculturative stress (Zhang & Goodson, 2011), students. Journal of Multicultural Counseling
it may be especially helpful for practitioners to be aware of and Development, 18, 19-28.
ways to inform IGS of resources available and to equip them doi:10.1002/j.2161-1912.1990.tb00433.x
with legal and institutional knowledge by taking into account Misra, R., & Castillo, L. G. (2004). Academic stress
their levels of English proficiency and acculturation to U.S. among college students: Comparison of
culture. Information may also be received better by IGS by American and international students.
teaching methods of intervening that are congruent with their International Journal of Stress Management
cultural values and beliefs. In addition, practitioners should 11, 132-148. doi:10.1037/1072-
also be sensitive to the unique stressors of IGS, including 5245.11.2.132
language barriers (Chen, 1999), lack of social support Mori, S. C. (2000). Addressing the mental health
(Pedersen, 1991), academic stress (Misra & Castillow, 2004), concerns of international students. Journal of
perceived language discrimination (Wei, Wang, & Ku, 2012), Counseling and Development, 78, 137-144.

Prevention and Health Promotion: Research, Social Action, Practice And Training: Volume 9, Issue 1 30

doi:10.1002/j.1556-6676.2000.tb02571.x
Olivas, M., & Li, C. (2006). Understanding stressors
of international students in higher education:
What college counselors and personnel need
to know. Journal of Instructional Psychology,
33, 217-222.
Pedersen, P. (1991). Counseling international students.
Counseling Psychologist, 19, 10-58.
doi:10.1177/0011000091191002
Wei, M., Wang, K. T., & Ku, T. (2012). A
development and validation of the Perceived
Language Discrimination Scale. Cultural
Diversity And Ethnic Minority Psychology,
18, 340-351. doi:10.1037/a0029453
Zhang, J., & Goodson, P. (2011). Predictors of
international l students psychosocial
adjustment to life in the United States: A
systematic review. International Journal of
Intercultural Relations, 35, 139-162.
doi:10.1016/j.ijintrel.2010.11.011

Prevention and Health Promotion: Research, Social Action, Practice And Training: Volume 9, Issue 1 31

RESEARCH, SOCIAL ACTION, PRACTICE AND TRAINING

Teaching Interpersonal Communication Skills:


Comparing the Fidelity of Online Simulations
Amy E. Dawson
Bianca L. Bernstein
Jennifer M. Bekki
Kerrie G. Wilkins
Arizona State University

Amy E. Dawson, Counseling Psychology, Arizona State University; Bianca L. Bernstein, Counseling and
Counseling Psychology, Arizona State University; Jennifer M. Bekki, Department of Engineering, Arizona State
University; Kerrie G. Wilkins, Counseling Psychology, Arizona State University This material is based upon work
supported by the National Science Foundation under Grant Number 0910384. Correspondence concerning this paper
should be addressed to Amy E. Dawson, Counseling Psychology, Arizona State University, Payne Hall 311-I,
Tempe, AZ 85287-0811. Email: amy.e.dawson@asu.edu.

Introduction presenting technical content within online learning


Interpersonal communication skills (ICS) have been environments. Simulations, in particular, are growing in
identified as one of the 21st century skills that students popularity because they allow for addressing educational
should acquire in order to succeed in higher education and objectives explicitly, while still allowing learners to engage
future employment (Pellegrino & Hilton, 2012, pg. 34). actively in the study of complex and real life phenomena
Communication skills are essential to working effectively in (Suave, Kaufman, & Marquis, 2007). However, research on
teams, responding to feedback, and navigating academic and the use and effectiveness of simulations for teaching soft
professional complications (Bernstein, 2011; Rugarcia, skills such as communication is in its infancy. Further,
Felder, Woods, & Stice, 2000). In the realm of positive little is known about the design features that might be
psychology and resiliency based prevention programs, especially relevant to teaching ICS, such as the fidelity or
protective factors such as communication skills are realism of simulations.
identified as candidates for assisting at-risk individuals The purpose of the study reported here was to assess
avoid later difficulties (Coie, Watt, West, Hawkins, the value of visual and audio realism in an interactive
Asarnow et al., 1993; Romano, Koch, & Wong, 2012). Yet, simulation for teaching active listening, a key aspect of ICS.
educators struggle with how best to provide substantial We compared the effectiveness of simulations that featured
learning and practice opportunities in a scalable way either live actors or animated characters paired with human
(Duggan & Adcock, 2007). A promising means to offer or animated voices. We hypothesized that participants who
widely accessible instruction and practice in ICS is in an experienced a more realistic live actor condition would
online interactive learning environment (Duggan & Adcock, benefit more from the program than those exposed to
2007). Multiple types of media have been explored for animated simulations.

Prevention and Health Promotion: Research, Social Action, Practice And Training: Volume 9, Issue 1 32

Methods 1 (very unlikely) to 5 (very likely) on the likelihood of
Participants included undergraduates (196 women, 76 achieving a stated desired outcome. Participants also
men) recruited from a large university in the Southwest. Ages completed a 15-item Overall Experience measure ( = .86),
ranged from 19-60 years of age, M = 27.6; SD = 7.76, and rated from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree),
race/ethnicity self-reports included 7% African American, designed to capture the worth or significance that they placed
7% Asian or Pacific Islander, 17% Hispanic, 2% Native on the skills taught and their experience with the learning
American or Alaskan Native, 55% Caucasian/White, .02% environment. Responses on items within each scale were
other, and 9% multiethnic. averaged and used as the dependent variables for each
Participants were randomly assigned to one of three analysis.
Active Listening Communication Simulation (ALS) Results
conditions as they enrolled in the study. In the ALS, A series of 2x3 ANOVAs compared participant
described in full detail elsewhere (Prim et al., 2013), performance on each of the three measures by Gender and
participants viewed video clips of a doctoral student and her Condition. There was a significant main effect for Condition
advisor discussing the advisors feedback about the students on the Knowledge outcome (2 = .029). Participants in
research. At various points the video stopped and the condition 2 (animated with live actor voice) had significantly
participant was asked to decide what the students best higher scores on Knowledge (M = 2.18) than participants in
response would be. Based on the users selection, a second Condition 1 (live actor) and Condition 3 (animated with
video was displayed and the dialogue continued until the next computerized voice) (M = 1.98, M = 1.98 respectively): F (2,
decision point was reached. The three simulations featured 263) = 3.88, p = .022. There were no significant differences
exactly the same content and dialogue. Condition 1 (N = 74) between conditions 1, 2, and 3 on Skills or Overall Experience.
showed live actors using their own voices, condition 2 (N = No main effects were found for Gender on Knowledge and
98) paired animated characters with the live actors voices, Skills. On Overall Experience, men rated their experience
and condition 3 (N = 97) paired animated characters with significantly higher (M = 2.30) than did women (M = 2.06): F
computerized voices. (1,263) = 8.12, p = .005, 2 = .03.
Participants were instructed to complete the Conclusions
simulation, an outcome survey and a short demographic The preliminary results demonstrate that minimal
survey. Two scales from the Science Technology differences in relevant outcomes were generated based on the
Engineering Mathematics-Active Listening Skills type of medium used in an online interactive environment to
Assessment (STEM-ALSA; Wilkins, Bernstein, Bekki, teach interpersonal communication skills. Contrary to our
Harrison & Atkinson, 2012) were used to measure targeted hypothesis, the live actor condition was actually less effective
outcomes. The 12-item Knowledge Scale ( = .87) was than the animated character condition for increasing
designed to measure self-reported knowledge of active Knowledge of ICS. It may be that experiencing the full
listening skills. Responses were rated on a 5-point Likert- complexity of contiguous verbal and nonverbal communication
type scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly of live actors burdens a learners cognitive load (Pass & Van
agree). An 18-item Skills Scale ( =.55) measured Merrienboer, 1994). Vocal quality transmits salient nonverbal
participants ability to apply active listening skills. Similar to information (Scherer, Ladd, & Silverman, 1984), and therefore
a Situational Judgment Test (Lievens & Sackett, 2012), condition 2 (animated character with live actor voices)
participants read interpersonal communication scenarios and participants may have benefitted from focusing on the vocal
rated response options on a five-point Likert-type scale from cues without being distracted by live actor nonverbal behaviors.

Prevention and Health Promotion: Research, Social Action, Practice And Training: Volume 9, Issue 1 33

Similarly, it is possible that the communication lesson was Coie, J. D., Watt, N. F., West, S. G., Hawkins, J. D., Asarnow,
more distilled in the animated plus live actor voice condition J. R., Markman, H. J., Ramey, Sharon L., Shure, M.
and therefore easier to understand and learn. B., & Long, B. (1993). The science of prevention: A
Replication of our findings and more research on the conceptual framework and some directions for a
use of simulations for interpersonal communication skills national research program. American Psychologist, 48,
training are needed. Nonetheless, the results raise the 1013.
possibility that animations, especially paired with live actor Duggan, M. H., & Adcock, A. B. (2007). Animated agents
voices, can provide scalable ICS learning and practice teaching helping skills in an online environment: A
opportunities for a fraction of the cost required to develop pilot study. Journal of Interactive Online Learning, 6,
simulations with live actors. The learning of any skill, including 56-71.
communication techniques, requires numerous iterations of Flay, B. R., Biglan, A., Boruch, R. F., Castro, F. G.,
practice according to the power law of practice (Pellegrino & Gottfredson, D., Kellam, S., ... & Ji, P. (2005).
Hilton, 2012, pg. 80). At first, this rehearsal results in rapid Standards of evidence: Criteria for efficacy,
improvements in performance, and then slow, steady effectiveness and dissemination. Prevention
progression follows (Pellegrino & Hilton, 2012). Interpersonal Science, 6, 151-175.
communication skills training available via an online format Lievens, F., & Sackett, P. R. (2012). The validity of
allows learners to return to the simulations and practice the interpersonal skills assessment via situational
scenarios as many times as desired or needed in order to master judgment tests for predicting academic success and job
the skills. Furthermore, while research has shown (cf., Salas et performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 97, 460.
al., 2008) that team training interventions can increase Paas, F. G., & Van Merrinboer, J. J. (1994). Instructional
interpersonal competencies, such as communication, these are control of cognitive load in the training of complex
labor and time-intensive and geographically limited. In cognitive tasks. Educational Psychology Review, 6,
contrast, online training in communication skills, an important 351-371.
type of protective factor (Romano & Hage, 2000), can reach a Pellegrino, J. W., & Hilton, M. L. (2012). Education for life
larger audience and thereby increase the probability of assisting and work: Developing transferable knowledge and
at-risk individuals improve communication in challenging skills in the 21st century. Washington, DC: The
situations. Given the difficulties in providing widely accessible National Academies Press.
instruction and practice in ICS, these findings may have Prim, D., Bernstein, B. L., Way, A., Hita, L., Liddell, T.,
significant implications for the future of ICS training and new Sarma, A., & Bekki, J. M. (2013). Development of an
applications of educational technology. internet-delivered communication curriculum for
graduate women in STEM. Proceedings of the
American Society of Engineering Education (ASEE)
References
(7544).

Bernstein, B. L. (2011). Managing barriers and building Romano, J. L., & Hage, S. M. (2000). Prevention and

supports in science and engineering doctoral counseling psychology revitalizing commitments for

programs: Conceptual underpinnings for a new online the 21st century. The Counseling Psychologist, 28,

training program for women. Journal of Women and 733-763.

Minorities in Science and Engineering, 17, 2950. Romano, J. L., Koch, J., & Wong, Y. J. (2012). Prevention in
counseling psychology: Promoting education, health,

Prevention and Health Promotion: Research, Social Action, Practice And Training: Volume 9, Issue 1 34

and well-being across the life cycle. In Fouad, N. A.,
Carter, J. A., & Subich, L. M. (Eds.), APA handbook
of counseling psychology, Vol. 1: Theories, research,
and methods. (pp. 345-367). Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
Rugarcia, A., Felder, R. M., Woods, D. R., & Stice, J. E.
(2000). The future of engineering education I. A vision
for a new century. Chemical Engineering
Education, 34, 16-25.
Salas, E., DiazGranados, D., Klein, C., Burke, C. S., Stagl, K.
C., Goodwin, G. F., & Halpin, S. M. (2008). Does
team training improve team performance? A meta-
analysis. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human
Factors and Ergonomics Society, 50, 903-933.
Sauve, L., Renaud, L., Kaufman, D., & Marquis, J. (2007).
Distinguishing between games and simulations: A
systematic review. Educational Technology & Society,
10, 247-256.
Scherer, K. R., Ladd, D. R., & Silverman, K. E. (1984). Vocal
cues to speaker affect: Testing two models. The
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 76,
1346-1356.
Wilkins, K. G., Bernstein, B. L., Bekki, J. M., Harrison, C. J.,
& Atkinson, R. K. (2012). Development of the science
technology engineering and mathematicsactive
listening skills assessment (STEM-ALSA).
Proceedings of the Frontiers in Education Conference
(FIE), 2012, 1-6.
Zemsky, R. (1997). Skills and the economy: An employer
context for understanding the school-to-work
transition. In Lesgold, A., Feuer, M.J., & Black, A.M.
(Eds.), Transitions in work and learning: Implications
for assessment (pp. 34-61). Washington, DC: National
Academy Press.

Prevention and Health Promotion: Research, Social Action, Practice And Training: Volume 9, Issue 1 35

Вам также может понравиться