Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 8

Neuropsychologia 82 (2016) 171178

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Neuropsychologia
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neuropsychologia

The role of metrical information in apraxia of speech. Perceptual


and acoustic analyses of word stress
Ingrid Aichert n, Mona Spth, Wolfram Ziegler
EKN Clinical Neuropsychology Research Group, Institu of Phonetics and Speech Processing, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universitt Mnchen, Germany

art ic l e i nf o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Several factors are known to inuence speech accuracy in patients with apraxia of speech (AOS), e.g.,
Received 31 August 2015 syllable structure or word length. However, the impact of word stress has largely been neglected so far.
Received in revised form More generally, the role of prosodic information at the phonetic encoding stage of speech production
8 December 2015
often remains unconsidered in models of speech production. This study aimed to investigate the inu-
Accepted 10 January 2016
Available online 11 January 2016
ence of word stress on error production in AOS. Two-syllabic words with stress on the rst (trochees) vs.
the second syllable (iambs) were compared in 14 patients with AOS, three of them exhibiting pure AOS,
Keywords: and in a control group of six normal speakers. The patients produced signicantly more errors on iambic
Apraxia of speech than on trochaic words. A most prominent metrical effect was obtained for segmental errors. Acoustic
Word stress
analyses of word durations revealed a disproportionate advantage of the trochaic meter in the patients
Trochee
relative to the healthy controls. The results indicate that German apraxic speakers are sensitive to me-
Iamb
Prosody trical information. It is assumed that metrical patterns function as prosodic frames for articulation
planning, and that the regular metrical pattern in German, the trochaic form, has a facilitating effect on
word production in patients with AOS.
& 2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction of syllables and, in cases of irregular stress, the main stress posi-
tion (Roelofs and Meyer, 1998). As soon as a words segmental and
Adult speakers rely on acquired implicit knowledge about how metrical properties have been retrieved, syllabication rules are
to move their articulators for the production of speech sounds. applied to attach the segments, from left to right, to the metrical
This knowledge is considered to be embodied in phonetic plans template. The output of this association process, a syllabied
which provide the basis of uent, automated speech. Apraxia of phonological word, constitutes the input to the phonetic encoding
speech (AOS), an acquired neurogenic motor speech disorder, is stage of word production. The phonetic encoding component of
conventionally dened as resulting from a deconstruction of such Levelt et al.'s (1999) speech production model dispenses with any
information (e.g., Code 1998; for a review see Ziegler et al., 2012). prosodic mechanisms. In this theory, the frequent phonological
It may therefore provide a window into the internal make-up of syllables of syllabied words activate corresponding phonetic
learned speech motor behavior. syllables stored in a mental syllabary. The syllabary is considered
This article deals with the architecture of speech motor plans as as a repository of holistic, overlearned motor plans containing the
inferred from speech errors of patients with AOS. More specically, gestural specications for the lower-level speech motor system
it addresses the question of how articulation planning for vowels (Cholin et al., 2006; Cholin, 2008). Metrical structure plays no role
at this stage. As a consequence, if the phonetic encoder embodies
and consonants interacts with the rhythmical structure of speech.
overlearned, automated aspects of speech motor behavior, the
This question is particularly controversial in psycholinguistic and
motor patterns implementing, for instance, a trochaic vs. a iambic
phonetic theories of speech production. The speech production
rhythm, are in Levelt's theory not a part of this skill. Hence,
model developed by Levelt and coworkers (Levelt et al., 1999;
there is also no interaction between segmental and prosodic in-
Cholin et al., 2004), for instance, takes a perspective in which the
formation within the phonetic encoding component of this model.
segments of an utterance, i.e., its vowels and consonants, and the
In contrast, phonetic studies provide abounding evidence for an
metrical structure of the utterance are represented on different
interplay of segmental with metrical information. Generally, the
tiers. The metrical frame of an intended word species the number realization of sounds is considered to be strongly inuenced by the
prosodic framework of an utterance. To mention only a few ex-
n
Corresponding author. amples: In several studies stress-related strengthening effects,
E-mail address: ingrid.aichert@ekn-muenchen.de (I. Aichert). i.e., more extensive and prolonged articulations in stressed vs.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.01.009
0028-3932/& 2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
172 I. Aichert et al. / Neuropsychologia 82 (2016) 171178

unstressed syllables, were described (e.g., De Jong, 1995). Another Wambaugh et al. (2006b) and Ballard et al. (2015)). Some of these
prosodic effect related to the strengthening concept is domain- techniques rely on internal rhythmical cues, such as nger tapping
nal lengthening: A study by Wightman et al. (1992), for instance, (Wambaugh and Martinez, 2000), while others use external cues,
reported a more extreme lengthening at the end of higher pro- e.g. tactile (Rubow et al., 1982) or acoustic (Shane and Darley,
sodic domains (e.g., intonational phrases) as compared to lower 1978; Dworkin et al., 1988; Wambaugh and Martinez, 2000;
prosodic domains (e.g., syllables). Similar effects have also been Brendel and Ziegler, 2008). Furthermore, rhythmic speech and
described domain-initially (e.g., Fujimura, 1990). Several electro- singing are basic elements of Melodic Intonation Therapy applied in
palatographic studies have for instance reported that consonants the treatment of severely impaired non-uent aphasics (e.g.,
are produced with greater articulatory magnitude in domain-in- Helm-Estabrooks et al., 1989; Sparks and Deck, 1994). Stahl et al.
itial as compared to domain-medial positions at different levels (2011) compared the contribution of singing vs. rhythm in patients
(e.g., Fougeron and Keating, 1997; Fougeron, 2001). These in- with non-uent aphasia, most of whom were diagnosed with AOS,
vestigations are predominantly concerned with surface properties and suggested that rhythm rather than melody was crucial in the
of speech sounds and speech movements and refrain from draw- recovery of speech in these patients.
ing inferences about the underlying phonetic encoding mechan- In sum, the results from treatment studies demonstrate that
isms. However, it can plausibly be assumed that at least some of the symptoms of AOS can be modulated positively by interven-
these surface phenomena are mediated by highly automated in- tions focused on the rhythmical aspects of speaking. Notably, the
terlinks between segmental and metrical properties at the pho- effects of these approaches are apparently not conned to the
netic encoding level and that they reect learned aspects of the suprasegmental symptoms of apraxic speech (e.g. uency), but
motor organization of speech. This assumption receives support by they also encroach on articulatory accuracy at a segmental level.
more recent investigations. In a tongue twister experiment with What is still lacking, however, is an experimental approach to-
unimpaired speakers (Croot et al., 2010) it was shown that sen- wards understanding how prosodic structure impacts speech
tence-level prominence, i.e. accent, protects against errors. Croot motor planning in patients with AOS.
and colleagues inferred from their results that segmental content Based on the premise that AOS provides a window into the
and prosodic structure of an utterance are integrated during the internal make-up of learned speech motor behavior, we here seek
phonetic encoding stage. In reading experiments, Sulpizio et al. evidence for an interaction of segmental and prosodic information
(2015) reported an inuence of word stress on reading latency and in speech planning by analyzing the factors inuencing the oc-
pronunciation accuracy and concluded that stress assignment af- currence of speech errors in apraxic speakers. In a recent in-
fects the reading process during articulatory planning. vestigation including 33 patients with AOS we applied a nonlinear
As regards the characteristics of the speech impairment in regression model to predict apraxic speech errors on words and
patients with AOS, there is general agreement that segmental as- nonwords as a function of gestural decomposition, syllable struc-
pects of speech motor programming are affected in these in- ture, syllable number, and metrical rhythm (Ziegler and Aichert,
dividuals. The cardinal symptoms of the disorder, i.e., phonemic 2015). In this study we had discovered, among several other
errors and phonetic distortions, are considered segmental in nat- things, that two-syllabic trochaic words, which constitute the
ure because they affect the vowels and consonants of spoken regular metrical pattern in German (Domahs et al., 2008; Wiese
utterances (for an overview see McNeil et al., 2009). Moreover, a 2000; e.g., puma, engl. puma), were produced with fewer errors
wide range of factors related to the segmental structure of words than iambic words (e.g., men, engl. menu). However, since the
are known to inuence the error pattern of patients with AOS, materials used in this experiment were not devised to examine
including, for instance, sound class or syllable complexity (e.g., metrical inuences on speech in a very specic way, this nding
Canter et al., 1985; Odell et al., 1990; Romani and Galluzzi, 2005; was obtained as a side effect which deserves closer investigation.
Aichert and Ziegler, 2004a; Laganaro, 2008; Staiger and Ziegler, The present study was therefore designed to directly explore
2008; Galluzzi et al., 2015). In contrast, inuences of prosodic the inuence of word stress in a new sample of apraxic speakers
properties (e.g., word stress) on accuracy in apraxic speakers have by using highly specic and tightly controlled speech materials.
been neglected so far, even though dysuency and prosodic im-
pairment are regularly mentioned among its clinical signs. Kent
and Rosenbek (1982, 1983) emphasized that syllable segregation 2. Method
and a lengthening of steady-state segments and transitions (ar-
ticulatory prolongation) constitute characteristic symptoms of AOS 2.1. Participants
(for a similar pattern in cases of Primary Progressive Aphasia with
AOS see Ballard et al., 2014). Furthermore, a attening of the in- Twenty-one patients with a diagnosis of apraxia of speech
tensity contours across syllable chains and of stress contrasts was (AOS) according to clinical records were recruited from twelve
described (Kent and Rosenbek, 1982, 1983; Itoh and Sasanuma, clinical centers in Germany. Before inclusion they were screened
1984; Marquardt et al., 1995). In the Treatment guidelines for for participation by a thorough assessment of their spontaneous
acquired apraxia speech of the Academy of Neurologic Commu- speech and a word repetition test administered by the rst author.
nication Disorders and Sciences (Wambaugh et al., 2006a) two out Inclusion criteria were (1) a conrmed diagnosis of AOS, (2) no or
of ve primary clinical characteristics are related to the supra- only very mild dysarthria, (3) no or only minor aphasic impair-
segmental characteristics of the disorder (p. xvii). However, the ment in word repetition, (4) intact auditory discrimination, and
nature of the prosodic symptoms is controversial: some authors (5) ability to participate in an auditory repetition test involving
assume that they result from a genuinely prosodic impairment two-syllabic words. Conrmation of the clinical diagnosis of AOS
(e.g., Kent and Rosenbek, 1982, 1983; Boutsen and Christman, was grounded on the following criteria (e.g., Ziegler, 2008):
2002), but others suggest that they reect a compensation of a (i) inconsistent occurrence of phonetic distortions and presence of
foremost segmental decit (e.g., Lebrun, 1990; Marquardt et al., perceived phonemic errors, (ii) suprasegmental disturbances such
1995). as intrasyllabic pauses, syllable segregation, or lengthening of
Remarkably, the interplay between articulation and prosody speech sounds and sound transitions, and (iii) visible/audible
has been an issue in AOS therapy more than in basic research. A groping, self-corrections, and effortful speaking.
number of rhythm-based treatment methods have been proposed From the initial patient sample of 21, seven participants failed
and considered successful in AOS therapy (for an overview see to match the inclusion criteria. Four patients suffered from a
I. Aichert et al. / Neuropsychologia 82 (2016) 171178 173

Table 1 Table 2
Demographic and clinical characteristics of the patient sample (n14). Organization of the word list, with an example for each word type.

Age in years: median (range) 64 (2687) Trochees (N 24) Lambs (N 24)

Months post onset: median (range) 8 (1144) CV (N 16) Puma, engl. puma Kopie, engl. copy
Etiology Ischemic (11), hemorrhagic (2), CVC (N 16) Muskel, engl. muscle Kostm, engl. costume
traumatic (1) CCVC/CVCC (N 16) Plastik, engl. plastic Kompost, engl. compost
Severity of aphasiaa No (3), mild (5), moderate (5), se-
vere (1)
Severity of AOSb 4 Mild, 9 moderate, 1 severe
Auditory word discrimination (LeMoc, 14 Unimpaired 2.2. Materials and procedure
test 2)
Auditory word-picture matching (LeMoc, 12 Unimpaired, 2 at threshold
The materials consisted of 48 two-syllabic nouns with stress on
test 23)
Verbal namingd (LeMoc, test 30) 6 Unimpaired, 2 at threshold, the rst (trochees) or second syllable (iambs), respectively. The
6 moderate-severe word list was grouped into six sublists of 8 words each, including
words with simple CV and CVC structures and with complex syl-
a
Aachener Aphasie Test; Huber et al. (1983). lables in the stressed word position. All words were mono-
b
Based on the percentage of segmentally correct items in the experimental
materials (mild: 470%/moderate: 3070%/severe: o 30% correct responses).
morphemic nouns of low lexical frequency (o10 spoken/written
c
LeMo; De Bleser et al. (2004). per Million; CELEX database, Baayen et al., 1995). Trochees and
d
Responses with errors on only one phoneme (e.g. iambs were matched for phoneme density (mean phoneme
) were scored as number per word in trochees: 5.25; in iambs: 5.17). The organi-
correct.
zation of the materials and examples are shown in Table 2.
Test words were also controlled for syllable frequency. Syllable
frequency counts were taken from a German syllable frequency
notable dysarthric or aphasic impairment, and in three patients database (Aichert et al., 2005) based on the CELEX-corpus (Baayen
speech apraxia was too mild given the low demands of the single et al., 1995). T-tests failed to reveal signicant differences in syl-
word repetition task administered here. lable frequency between the stressed syllables of the trochees and
Therefore, 14 patients were nally included. All participants were the iambs (p4 .05) and the unstressed syllables of the two item
right-handed native speakers of German, most of them suffered from groups (p 4.05). A position-wise analysis revealed that fre-
an ischemic infarct. There was no clinically apparent difference be- quencies of the unstressed syllables in both positions were sig-
tween the 11 patients who had ischemic infarcts and the three pa- nicantly higher compared to the stressed syllables in the corre-
sponding position of the respective metrical cognates (position 1:
tients with other aetiologies (for an overview of demographic and
t2.72; po .01; position 2: t 4,63; p o.001).
clinical characteristics of the patient sample see Table 1).
The study was conducted in a single 60-min session during
According to the Aachener Aphasie Test (AAT; Huber et al.,
which patients were administered the word list as well as the
1983), three patients had no aphasia, hence were classied as pure
subtests from the Lemo battery (De Bleser et al., 2004, see above).
AOS. Only one patient was diagnosed with a severe aphasia. Most The list was administered twice in two separate runs of an audi-
patients had undisturbed auditory single-word processing abilities tory repetition task, resulting in a total of 96 items per patient and
as revealed by subtests 2 (auditory word discrimination) and 23 1344 items overall. Items were presented in a pseudo-randomized
(auditory word-picture matching) from the LeMo battery (Lexikon order.
modellorientiert, De Bleser et al., 2004). Hence, auditory impair-
ment could be ruled out as a factor inuencing repetition accuracy. 2.3. Data analysis
An oral picture naming test (LeMo, test 30) revealed intact
word retrieval in six patients. Two patients were mildly disturbed, Patients' responses were recorded on video- and audiotape
and six patients displayed moderate to severe word-nding dif- (video camera: Panasonic NV-GS180, external microphone: be-
culties (Table 1). Regarding the error types in the naming task, the yerdynamic TG-X-58). For auditory evaluation, each word was
transcribed phonetically by the rst author. Analyses were con-
patients with naming decits produced mainly null reactions
ducted using both the auditory and the visual (mouth movement)
(50%) or semantic errors (35%). Of note, these two error types were
information. Symptoms which were not captured by the core IPA-
not observed in the experimental word repetition task. symbols (e.g., phonetic distortions or prosodic deviations such as
Based on the results of these neurolinguistic data, a core as- phoneme lengthening, intra- and intersyllabic pauses) were
sumption of this study is that the patients' performance on the marked with diacritics. Additionally, acoustic measures of word
experimental task used here, i.e., word repetition, was primarily duration were obtained using wideband spectograms of the re-
affected by their apraxic impairment, with a negligible aphasic or corded wav-les (Praat, 5.1.05; Boersma and Weenink, 2009).
dysarthric contribution. Furthermore, there was no relationship
between the severity of AOS and aphasia severity. More speci- 2.3.1. Response accuracy
cally, the patient with the most severe AOS was only mildly The patients' responses were rst analyzed with respect to
aphasic, and the patients with moderate AOS ranged across all correct/incorrect productions. Whole words were considered as
degrees of aphasia severity, from no aphasia to severe aphasia. error units, i.e., multiple errors on a word were counted as a single
word error. In case of multiple attempts, the rst response was
Severity of AOS was ranked on the basis of rates of overall seg-
transcribed. Errors were classied as segmental and prosodic, re-
mental errors made in the present repetition experiment. In four
spectively. Segmental errors included phonetic distortions and
cases the apraxic impairment was classied as mild ( o30% er-
perceived phonemic errors (i.e., substitutions, additions or omis-
rors), in nine cases as moderate (3070% errors), and in two cases sions). Prosodic errors comprised phoneme lengthenings, schwa-
as severe (4 70% errors). insertions and intersyllabic or intersegmental pauses. Reactions
Six neurologically healthy control speakers (four female, two that missed any phonological relationship to the target word (i.e.,
male; age, mean: 51, range: 3673) were also examined. fragmentary responses) were marked as unclassied and were
174 I. Aichert et al. / Neuropsychologia 82 (2016) 171178

excluded from the statistical analysis. Furthermore, we also 3. Results


documented the occurrence of visible or audible groping behavior
for each response. 3.1. Response accuracy: word-based errors
In a second step, segmental errors were analyzed on a syllable-
by-syllable basis, i.e., errors on the rst and second syllable of each Since the healthy controls made no errors in the repetition task
word were counted separately. they were not included in the error analyses. In the patient group,
25 out of 1344 responses (9 trochees, 16 iambs; 1,9%) were ex-
2.3.2. Word duration cluded as unclassied. The unclassied reactions were exclusively
Word durations were measured by determining the onset of due to fragmentary responses following articulatory groping.
the initial and the offset of the nal phoneme in the acoustic There were no null reactions and no semantic errors in the re-
petition task.
signal. Whereas phonetically distorted reactions were included in
Fig. 1 depicts mean error rates by stress type (trochee, iamb)
this analysis, items whose phonemic content was altered (i.e., by
and syllable structure (CV, CVC, and complex). Word-based seg-
single or multiple phonemic errors or by fragmentary responses)
mental and prosodic errors and groping behaviors are plotted in
were not considered. the left, middle, and right panels, respectively. Overall, the amount
Word duration measurements were also performed for the six of segmental errors signicantly outnumbered the amount of
control speakers. prosodic errors and groping behaviors, respectively. This pattern
also held for each individual subject.
2.3.3. Statistics As a general tendency, higher error rates occurred on the
All statistics were performed using R (R Core Team, 2015). iambic as compared to the trochaic items. Average segmental error
Generalized Linear Mixed Models were calculated using lme4 rates across the three syllable types were .33 in the trochees vs. .55
(Bates et al., 2015). in the iambs. Prosodic error rates were .10 vs. .14, groping rates .22
vs. .24. There was also an expected increase of error rates with
increasing syllable complexity.
2.4. Reliability Generalized linear mixed models (logit-link) were computed for
each error type separately, with the xed effects factors METER and
2.4.1. Reliability of diagnosis (syllable) STRUCTURE and the random effects factors PARTICIPANT, ITEM and
To validate the diagnosis of AOS, a second experienced speech RUN. In order to account for the possibility that participants and
therapist who had no pre-information about the patients and who items might differ in their sensitivity to METER, random slopes were
was not aware of the research question was presented stretches of also specied for by-PARTICIPANT and by-ITEM effects of METER. Good-
spontaneous speech and extracts from a word repetition task of all ness-of-t was estimated using the Hosmer and Lemeshow test (HL
14 patients. Additionally, four patients with aphasic-phonological test) for logistic models (Hosmer and Lemeshow, 1980).
symptoms were included in the classication rating. In a rst step, The results are plotted in Table 3. For each model, signicant
the second rater was asked to classify each patient as either speech deviation from the empirical data could be rejected by the HL test.
apraxic or aphasic-phonological. In a second step she was asked to Goodness-of-t was poorest for the groping data. There were
evaluate how certain she was in her classication (certain, slightly signicant main effects of STRUCTURE across all error types, and a
uncertain, very uncertain). Among the 14 patients included in this signicant inuence of METER for the segmental and the prosodic
errors. There was also a signicant interaction of METER x STRUCTURE
study, the independent rater was in accordance with the original
in all three error variables. Plots of the random slopes of the by-
diagnosis of AOS in 13 cases (certain: 10, slightly uncertain: 3). In
PARTICIPANT effects of METER revealed that the advantage of the tro-
one patient who had pure AOS with only mild symptoms the rater
chaic pattern regarding segmental and prosodic errors was
went for a classication as aphasic-phonological, qualifying her homogeneous across the apraxic participants, since coefcients
decision as very uncertain. In all four patients with aphasic- were all positive and varied within a very small range (segmental:
phonological disorder her classication was in accordance with 1.21.8; prosodic: 1.42.3).
the prior diagnosis (certain: 2, slightly uncertain: 2). Closer inspection of Fig. 1 reveals that the advantage of tro-
chees was much smaller for the words containing consonant
2.4.2. Reliability of accuracy scores clusters than for the CV- and the CVC-words. A straightforward
In order to determine the reliability of the accuracy scores, the explanation of this result is that in the trochees the clusters oc-
rst 22 words of both runs (N 44 words) from four randomly curred in the word onset (e.g., plastic), whereas in the complex
selected patients were analyzed by the same speech therapist who iambs the cluster was in the onset or the coda of the second syl-
also did the classication rating. There was substantial agreement lable (e.g.,so'pran, com'post), - an imbalance which was due to the
(Kappa statistics, Landis and Koch, 1977) between the two tran- fact that iambs with complex word onsets are rare in German.
scribers for the segmental ( .785, po .001) and the supraseg- Hence, the results plotted in Fig. 1 suggest that the benet of the
mental whole-word errors ( .646, po .001). Furthermore, there trochaic pattern was partly outweighed by onset complexity,
was almost perfect agreement for the evaluation of the groping especially in the case of the prosodic and the groping errors.
To compensate for this obvious inuence of onset position, a
behavior ( .950, p o.001). Regarding the syllable-wise analysis
further series of GLM logit models was conducted in which only
of the segmental errors we also found a signicant agreement
words with CV- and CVC-syllables were included. Table 4 plots the
between the ratings for the rst syllable ( .768, p o.001/sub-
results of these analyses, demonstrating that METER again had a
stantial agreement) and the second syllable ( .864, po .001/al- signicant effect on segmental and prosodic errors. In the absence
most perfect agreement). of the consonant cluster words, an inuence of syllable structure
Regarding acoustic measurements, 28 words which were sui- was only seen on the prosodic errors, and the interaction of METER
table for word duration analysis (i.e., phonemically correct pro- with STRUCTURE disappeared. Again, the random slope coefcients of
ductions) from the four patients were re-analyzed (N 112 items). the by-PARTICIPANT effects of METER were uniformly positive and
A high correlation between the values measured by the two ex- ranged within a small interval in the two analyses.
aminers was found (Pearson Correlation, r .998, po .001). Although the inuence of the word stress pattern turned out to
I. Aichert et al. / Neuropsychologia 82 (2016) 171178 175

Fig. 1. Mean error rates (word-based) as a function of stress and syllable structure. Left: segmental errors; middle: prosodic errors; right: groping behavior. The stressed
syllables had a CV-, CVC-, or a complex structure with a consonant cluster (CC).

Table 3
Generalized linear mixed logit models of the inuence of metrical pattern (trochaic vs. iambic) on segmental and prosodic word level errors and on the occurrence of
groping.

HL-test Segmental errors 2(8) 9.8; p .28 Prosodic errorsa 2(8) 10.5; p .23 Groping 2(8) 15.2; p .06

Wald z df sig Wald z df sig Wald z df sig


METER 4.39 1 *** 2.47 1 * 1.70 1 n.s.
STRUCTURE 4.12 2 *** 5.21 2 *** 3.31 2 ***
METER  STRUCTURE  2.18 2 *  2.42 2 *  2.00 2 *

The structure of the stressed syllable (CV, CVC, complex) was modeled as a further xed factor. Participants, items and runs (rst vs. second) were included as random effects
factors. HL-test: Hosmer & Lemeshow (p-values4 .05 indicate that the model should not be rejected). Signicance levels: *** p o .001; ** p o .01; * p o .05; ns not signicant.
a
Since 8 patients made no prosodic errors in one or several conditions, the model was calculated for only the six remaining participants.

be homogeneous across the apraxic participants in almost all A generalized linear mixed logit model with two xed effects
analyses, whole-word errors were also analyzed for each single factors (STRESS, POSITION) and the random effects factors PARTICIPANT and
ITEM explained the data signicantly (LH (8) 10.56, p .23). Both
2
participant. In these analyses, words with complex clusters were
excluded and the CV- and CVC-conditions were collapsed. Pear- the POSITION (Wald-z(1)  6.2, p o.001) and the STRESS effect (Wald-
sons chi-squared tests were performed, with an alpha-level of .05. z(1) 3.1, p o.01) became signicant, and there was also a sig-
Regarding segmental errors, 9/14 patients were signicantly less nicant STRESS x POSITION interaction (Wald- z(1) 6.7, p o.001).
accurate on iambic than on trochaic words, among them were two
of the three patients with pure AOS. Three of them also produced 3.3. Word duration
signicantly more prosodic errors on the iambic as compared to
the trochaic words. Additionally, one further patient (the third In the control group, all word productions were phonemically
patient with pure AOS) also showed a facilitating trochee-effect in correct und could therefore be included in the acoustic analyses. In
his groping behavior. Reverse effects were not observed in any of the patient group, only 65% of all items (n 875) could be analyzed
the 14 participants on any of the three error variables. for word duration. There were more trochaic (n 505) than iambic
words (n 370) remaining in the analysis, which reects the
3.2. Response accuracy: syllable-based errors higher vulnerability of iambic words. Yet, the distribution of ana-
lyzable items across the three syllable structure types was similar
In order to investigate whether the metrical effects reported in the trochaic and the iambic words, with at least 110 items per
above were ascribable to syllable position or syllable stress, syl- syllable structure remaining in each word stress category.
lable-wise error counts were analyzed. Fig. 2 depicts mean error Fig. 3 depicts average word duration as a function of metrical
rates as a function of syllable position (rst vs. second) and stress pattern and syllable complexity for the healthy participants (left
(stressed vs. unstressed). panel) and the AOS patients (right panel). As can be seen in Fig. 3,
On the average there was an overall tendency towards more iambic words tended to have longer durations than trochaic words
errors on the rst relative to the second syllable (28% vs. 20%) and (mean difference across groups and syllable structures: 99 ms).
also a slightly greater vulnerability of stressed vs. unstressed syl- Words with complex syllables were produced with the longest
lables (26% vs. 23%). Interestingly, however, the stressed rst syl- durations, and this effect was more pronounced in the trochaic
lable of the trochees was produced more accurately than the un- than in the iambic words.
stressed rst syllable of the iambs, demonstrating that the me- A linear mixed model was calculated with the xed effects
trical effect outweighed the syllable-stress effect by far. Moreover, factors METER (trochees vs. iambs), STRUCTURE (CV, CVC) and GROUP
the nal stressed syllables of the iambic words were more error- (controls vs. AOS patients) and the random effects factors PARTICI-
prone than the initial stressed syllables of the trochees, indicating PANT, ITEM, and RUN. Due to the position asymmetry of consonant
that the overall benet of the nal vs. the initial syllable was re- clusters in trochees vs. iambs (see above), the analysis was con-
versed in the stressed condition, i.e., the metrical effect outplayed ned to the CV and CVC stimuli. Signicant main effects of METER (F
the disadvantage of the initial position. (1, 35.1) 22.5, p o.001) and of STRUCTURE (F(1, 35.1) 4.3, p o.05)
176 I. Aichert et al. / Neuropsychologia 82 (2016) 171178

Table 4
Generalized linear logit models as in Table 3 above, with the difference that syllables with consonant clusters were excluded, i.e., the factor STRUCTURE includes only two levels
(CV, CVC).

HL-test Segmental errors 2(8) 13.6; p .09 Prosodic errorsa 2(8) 8.0; p .43 Groping 2(8) 24.8; p o.01

Wald z df sig Wald z df sig Wald z df sig


METER 4.72 1 *** 2.85 1 ** 1.37 1 n.s.
STRUCTURE 1.85 1 n.s. 2.83 1 ** 1.46 1 n.s.
METER  STRUCTURE  .77 1 n.s.  1.84 1 n.s.  .88 1 n.s.

Signicance levels: ***p o .001; ** p o .01; *p o .05; ns not signicant.


a
Since 8 patients made no prosodic errors in one or several conditions, the model was calculated for only the six remaining participants.

both the healthy control speakers and the apraxic patients, but the
effect was signicantly stronger in the patients. As regards groping
behavior, there was no signicant inuence of word stress at the
group level (though individual patients did show a metrical effect).
Instead, groping for articulations appeared to be more related to
word onset complexity.
Syllable-wise error counts revealed that the impact of a words
metrical pattern on segmental accuracy was not ascribable to
syllable-related factors like position, stress, or frequency. It is
widely accepted that apraxic speakers are susceptible to failure at
word onsets and less vulnerable at the end of a word (e.g., Aichert
and Ziegler, 2013a; Canter et al., 1985; Croot, 2002). However, in
this study the rst syllable of the trochees was produced more
accurately than both syllables of the iambs. Therefore, the metrical
effect emerged to be stronger than the position effect. To give an
example: the stressed syllable [ju:] in the trochee judo (engl. judo)
Fig. 2. Segmental errors per syllable (in %), broken down by syllable position (rst was produced with a considerably lower error rate (45%) than the
vs. second) and syllable stress (stressed: lled squares; unstressed: open circles). unstressed syllable [ju:] in the iamb ju'wel (engl. jewel, error rate:
Labels indicate how the four syllable types combine in the two metrical patterns. 64%), despite the fact that the two syllables have the same seg-
ments. Furthermore, we found that stressed syllables were not
were obtained. There was also a signicant main effect of GROUP (F per se more error-prone than unstressed syllables, as would have
(1, 14.9) 5.5, p o.05), i.e., patients with AOS had signicantly been predicted by Odell et al. (1991). To the contrary, the rst,
longer word durations than the controls, with an average differ- unstressed syllables of the iambic words were even produced with
ence of 136 ms. Most importantly, a signicant GROUP x METER in- the highest error rates. This nding deserves particular mention
teraction was found (F(1, 690.0) 13.0, po .001), indicating that because the rst syllables of the iambic words had higher syllable
the disadvantage of the iambic pattern was stronger in the pa- frequencies than the rst syllables of the trochaic words. Ob-
tients than in the healthy participants. viously, the inuence of the regular metrical pattern even out-
Taken together, these acoustic data provide additional and played the syllable frequency effect that has been described in
objective evidence for a metrical inuence on specically the several earlier studies (Aichert and Ziegler, 2004a; Laganaro, 2008;
prosodic aspects of apraxic speech. The fact that the metrical effect Staiger and Ziegler, 2008).
was still there after exclusion of the more vulnerable items (i.e., Notably, however, the trochee effect did not swamp all seg-
items with phonemic errors) argues for the robustness of the mental inuences, especially not the consonant cluster effect
effect. observed earlier (e.g., Aichert and Ziegler, 2004a). In the experi-
ment described here, the presence of consonant clusters in the
stressed syllables attenuated the trochaic advantage, presumably
4. Discussion because the clusters occurred in the word onsets of the trochees,
but word-medially or -nally in the iambs. Onset clusters were
This is the rst systematic investigation of how word stress particularly vulnerable to errors, and especially for the prosodic
affects error production in apraxia of speech. A group of 14 pa- and groping errors they nullied or even reversed the metrical
tients with left hemisphere strokes whose single word production effect.
was predominantly inuenced by their apraxic speech impairment Overall, the results presented here show that segmental and
was examined by a word-repetition task involving disyllabic prosodic factors interact closely in apraxia of speech and should
words with a trochaic (i.e., stressedunstressed) and an iambic therefore not be considered independent in their inuence on
(i.e., unstressedstressed) metrical pattern. The words were sys- speech motor planning. Hence, our study provides new evidence
tematically varied for the structure of stressed syllables and were for a nonlinear architecture of speech motor representations, in
paralleled for lexical frequency, syllable frequency, and phoneme which the articulatory primitives of speech motor planning, i.e.,
number. As a general result, trochees (which constitute the pre- phonetic gestures for the articulation of vowels and consonants,
dominant metrical pattern in German) were less error prone than are dominated by the rhythmical patterning of words (cf. Ziegler,
iambs. The benet of the regular ( trochaic) pattern was parti- 2005, 2009; Ziegler and Aichert, 2015). This perspective is at var-
cularly pronounced with regard to rates of segmental errors, but iance with theories suggesting that the phonetic encoding me-
was also found in prosodic error counts (prolonged sounds and chanism operates on linear strings of phonemes or syllables. In the
sound transitions, within-word pauses). Furthermore, an impact of inuential model proposed by Levelt et al. (1999), for instance,
metrical patterning was also observed on word durations. Dura- metrical properties of utterances are accessed independently from
tions turned out to be longer for iambic than for trochaic words in the segmental content during word form (phonological) encoding
I. Aichert et al. / Neuropsychologia 82 (2016) 171178 177

Fig. 3. Word duration as a function of metrical and syllabic structure. Left: healthy controls; right: AOS patients.

and play no role at the subsequent phonetic encoding stage. There composed of trochaic words (e.g., eine laute Pauke, engl. a loud
is also some neurolinguistic evidence supporting such a dualist timbal) and poems with an underlying regular metric (e.g., Goe-
view of segmental vs. prosodic encoding, as for instance data from the's "The Sorcerer's Apprentice"). Anecdotally, after the rst ex-
an earlier study in which we reported on two aphasic patients ercises with these materials the patient was struck by experien-
with a mutually dissociating pattern of segmental and prosodic cing an immediate increase in uency. After 20 treatment sessions
errors (Aichert and Ziegler, 2004b), or other reports of patients we observed a clearly increased speaking rate.
whose impairment was purely prosodic since they mis-stressed Taken together, these data show that the regular metrical
words with an irregular metrical pattern (Cappa et al., 1997; La- pattern in German, the trochaic form, facilitates articulatory ac-
ganaro et al., 2002). A weak interaction of segmental with prosodic curacy and articulatory uency in patients with AOS. We assume
information was demonstrated in aphasic patients who showed a that trochees are particularly stable phonetic patterns in German.
tendency to omit unstressed syllables and to make more seg- Furthermore, the results suggest that segmental and prosodic as-
mental errors on unstressed as compared to stressed syllables pects of speech motor planning are intertwined, which ts well
(Nickels and Howard, 1999). with earlier suggestions of a hierarchically nested organization of
In plain contrast, the AOS patients of the present study ex- phonetic plans (Ziegler and Aichert, 2015). Finally, the results
hibited no bias for unstressed syllables to be universally more presented here may also contribute to discussions about the units
vulnerable, but rather showed that segmental accuracy is a func- to be chosen as treatment targets in AOS therapy.
tion of the overall metrical organization of a word. Furthermore,
there was no case where a word was mis-stressed, i.e., an iambic
target word realized with a trochaic stress pattern or vice versa. Acknowledgments
This contrasts with reports of inappropriate word stress patterns
in related aetiologies, such as childhood apraxia of speech (e.g., The rst author of this study was supported by a grant from the
Shriberg et al., 2003) and foreign accent syndrome (e.g., Miller German Research Council (Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft
et al., 2006). Our result may be explained by assuming that the [DFG], Grant Zi 469/141). We are grateful to the patients and to
assignment of stress is already completed when the phonological the healthy control speakers for their participation. Furthermore,
word form is fed into the phonetic encoding process. we thank Bernadette Vgele for contributing to the reliability
The facilitating effect of the trochaic (i.e., the predominant, analyses.
regular, unmarked) stress pattern in our patients with AOS sup-
ports studies of normal speakers which also pointed at a close link
between segmental and prosodic aspects and an advantage of the References
trochee in speech motor planning (e.g., Croot et al., 2010; Sulpizio
et al., 2015). We assume that, at least for German, the strong-weak Aichert, I., Ziegler, W., 2004a. Syllable frequency and syllable structure in apraxia of
stress pattern constitutes a particularly stable architecture, in speech. Brain Lang. 88, 148159.
Aichert, I., Ziegler, W., 2004b. Segmental and metrical encoding in aphasia: two
which the lower-level gestural and syllabic motor routines for case reports. Aphasiology 18, 12011211.
words are organized more coherently than in words with a weak Aichert, I., Ziegler, W., 2010. Therapie bei chronischer Sprechapraxie: Vorge-
strong pattern. hensweise am Beispiel eines Patienten mit reiner Sprechapraxie. Forum Logop.
3, 613.
From a clinical perspective, the results of this study suggest
Aichert, I., Ziegler, W., 2013a. Word position effects in apraxia of speech: group data
that the facilitating effect of the regular word stress might also be and individual variation. J. Med. Speech Lang. Pathol. 20, 711.
helpful in the treatment of apraxic speakers. Choosing words with Aichert, I., Ziegler, W., 2013b. Segments and syllables in the treatment of apraxia of
speech: An investigation of learning and transfer effects. Aphasiology 27,
a regular stress pattern may enhance speech motor learning in
11801199.
patients with AOS. In a recent learning experiment in which we Aichert, I., Marquardt, C., Ziegler, W., 2005. Frequenzen sublexikalischer Einheiten
investigated the effectiveness of syllable-based learning in AOS des Deutschen: CELEX-basierte Datenbanken. Neurolinguistik 19, 5581.
Baayen, R.H., Piepenbrock, R., Gulikers, L., 1995. The CELEX Lexical Database (CD-
(Aichert and Ziegler, 2013b) we found that the learning of single
ROM). Linguistic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA.
syllables led to improvements on (untrained) two-syllabic trochaic Ballard, K.J., Savage, S., Leyton, C.E., Vogel, A.P., Hornberger, M., Hodges, J.H., 2014.
words. Obviously, the transfer from single syllables to trochees Logopenic and nonuent variants of primary progressive aphasia are differ-
required only little extra motor planning for these patients. In a entiated by acoustic measures of speech production. PLoS One 9, 114.
Ballard, K.J., Wambaugh, J.L., Duffy, J.R., Layeld, C., Maas, E., Mauszycki, S., McNeil, M.R.,
treatment report of a German patient with chronic, pure AOS 2015. Treatment for acquired apraxia of speech: A systematic review of intervention
(Aichert and Ziegler, 2010) we used metrically structured phrases research between 2004 and 2012. Am. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 24, 316337.
178 I. Aichert et al. / Neuropsychologia 82 (2016) 171178

Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., Walker, S., 2015. lme4: Linear mixed-effects Landis, J.R., Koch, G.G., 1977. The measurement of observer agreement for categorial
models using Eigen and S4. R package version 1.1-8, URL: http://CRAN.R-pro data. Biometrics 33, 159174.
ject.org/package lme4. Lebrun, Y., 1990. Apraxia of speech: a critical review. J. Neurolinguistics 5, 379406.
Boersma, P., Weenink, D., 2009. Praat: Doing Phonetics by Computer (Version Levelt, W.J.M., Roelofs, A., Meyer, A.S., 1999. A theory of lexical access in speech
5.1.05) [Computer Software]. Retrieved from www.praat.org. production. Behav. Brain Sci. 22, 138.
Boutsen, F.R., Christman, S.S., 2002. Prosody in apraxia of speech. Semin. Speech Marquardt, T.P., Duffy, G., Cannito, M.P., 1995. Acoustic analysis of accurate word
Lang. 23, 245255. stress patterning in patients with apraxia of speech and Broca's aphasia. Am. J.
Brendel, B., Ziegler, W., 2008. Effectiveness of metrical pacing in the treatment of Speech Lang. Pathol. 4, 180185.
apraxia of speech. Aphasiology 22, 77102. McNeil, M.R., Robin, D.A., Schmidt, R.A., 2009. Apraxia of speech: denition and
Canter, G.J., Trost, J.E., Burns, M.S., 1985. Contrasting speech patterns in apraxia of differential diagnosis. In: McNeil, M.R. (Ed.), Clinical Management of Sensor-
speech and phonemic paraphasia. Brain Lang. 24, 204222. imotor Speech Disorders, 2nd Ed. Thieme, New York, NY, pp. 249268.
Cappa, S.F., Nespor, M., Ielasi, W., Miozzo, A., 1997. The representation of stress: Miller, N., Lowit, A., O'Sullivan, H., 2006. What makes acquired foreign accent
evidence from an aphasic patient. Cognition 65, 113. syndrome foreign? J. Neurolinguistics 19, 385409.
Cholin, J., 2008. The mental syllabary in speech production: an integration of dif- Nickels, L., Howard, D., 1999. Effects of lexical stress on aphasic word production.
ferent approaches and domains. Aphasiology 22, 11271141. Clin. Linguist. Phon. 13, 269294.
Cholin, J., Schiller, N.O., Levelt, W.J.M., 2004. The preparation of syllables in speech Odell, K., McNeil, M., Rosenbek, J.C., Hunter, L., 1990. Perceptual characteristics of
production. J. Mem. Lang. 50, 4761. consonant production by apraxic speakers. J. Speech Hear. Disord. 55, 345359.
Cholin, J., Levelt, W.J.M., Schiller, N.O., 2006. Effects of syllable frequency in speech Odell, K., McNeil, M.R., Rosenbek, J.C., Hunter, L., 1991. Perceptual characteristics of
production. Cognition, 205235. vowel and prosody production in apraxic, aphasic, and dysarthric speakers. J.
Code, C., 1998. Major review: models, theories and heuristics in apraxia of speech. Speech Hear. Res. 34, 6780.
Clin. Linguist. Phon. 12, 4765. Roelofs, A., Meyer, A.S., 1998. Metrical structure in planning the production of
Core Team, R., 2015. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R spokeln words. J. Exp. Psychol.: Learn., Mem., Cogn. 24, 922939.
Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria, URL http://www.R-pro Romani, C., Galluzzi, C., 2005. Effects of syllabic complexity in predicting accuracy
ject.org/. of repetition and direction of errors in patients with articulatory and phono-
Croot, K., 2002. Diagnosis of AOS: denition and criteria. Semin. Speech Lang. 23, logical difculties. Cognit. Neuropsychol. 22, 817850.
267280. Rubow, R.T., Rosenbek, J.C., Collins, M., Longstreth, D., 1982. Vibrotactile stimulation
Croot, K., Au, C., Harper, A., 2010. Prosodic structure and tongue twister errors. In: for intersystemic reorganization in the treatment of apraxia of speech. Arch.
Fougeron, C., Kuehnert, B., d'Imperio, M., Valle, N. (Eds.), Papers in Laboratory
Phys. Med. Rehabil. 63, 150153.
Phonology 10: Variation, Phonetic Detail and Phonological Representation. De
Shane, H.C., Darley, F.L., 1978. The effect of auditory rhythmic stimulation on ar-
Gruyter Mouton, Berlin, pp. 433459.
ticulatory accuracy in apraxia of speech. Cortex 14, 444450.
De Bleser, R., Cholewa, J., Stadie, N., Tabatabaie, S., 2004. LeMo Lexikon mod-
Shriberg, L.D., Campbell, T.F., Karlsson, H.B., Brown, R.L., McSweeny, J.L., Nadler, C.J.,
ellorientiert. Einzelfalldiagnostik bei Aphasie, Dyslexie und Dysgraphie. Else-
2003. A diagnostic marker for childhood apraxia of speech: The lexical stress
vier, Muchen.
ratio. Clin. Linguist. Phon. 17, 549574.
De Jong, K.J., 1995. The supraglottal articulation of prominence in English: linguistic
Sparks, R., Deck, J., 1994. Melodic intonation therapy. In: Chapey, R. (Ed.), Language
stress as localized hyperarticulation. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 97, 491504.
Intervention Strategies in Adult Aphasia. Williams & Wilkins, Baltimore, MD,
Domahs, U., Wiese, R., Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I., Schlesewsky, M., 2008. The
pp. 368379.
processing of German word stress: evidence for the prosodic hierarchy. Pho-
Stahl, B., Kotz, S.A., Henseler, I., Turner, R., Geyer, S., 2011. Rhythm in disguise: why
nology 25, 136.
singing may not hold the key to recovery from aphasia. Brain 134, 30833093.
Dworkin, J.P., Abkarian, G.G., Johns, D.F., 1988. Apraxia of speech: the effectiveness
Staiger, A., Ziegler, W., 2008. Syllable frequency and syllable structure in the
of a treatment regimen. J. Speech Hear. Disord. 53, 280294.
Fougeron, C., 2001. Articulatory properties of initial segments in several prosodic spontaneous speech production of patients with apraxia of speech. Aphasiology
domains. J. Phon. 29, 109135. 22, 12011215.
Fougeron, C., Keating, P.A., 1997. Articulatory strengthening at edges of prosodic Sulpizio, S., Spinelli, G., Burani, C., 2015. Stress affects articulatory planning in
domains. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 101, 37283740. reading aloud. J. Exp. Psychol.: Hum. Percept. Perform. 41, 453461.
Fujimura, O., 1990. Methods and goals of speech production research. Lang. Speech Wambaugh, J.L., Martinez, A.L., 2000. Effects of rate and rhythm control treatment
33, 195258. on consonant production accuracy in apraxia of speech. Aphasiology 14,
Galluzzi, C., Bureca, I., Guariglia, C., Romani, C., 2015. Phonological simplications, 851871.
apraxia of speech and the interaction between phonological and phonetic Wambaugh, J.L., Duffy, J.R., McNeil, M.R., Robin, D.A., Rogers, M.A., 2006a. Treat-
processing. Neuropsychologia 71, 6483. ment guidelines for acquired apraxia of speech: a synthesis and evaluation of
Helm-Estabrooks, N., Nicholas, M., Morgan, A., 1989. Melodic Intonation Therapy the evidence. J. Med. Speech Lang. Pathol. 14 14, xvxxxiii.
Manual. Pro-Ed., Austin, TX. Wambaugh, J.L., Duffy, J.R., McNeil, M.R., Robin, D.A., Rogers, M.A., 2006b. Treat-
Hosmer, D.W., Lemeshow, S., 1980. A goodness-of-t test for the multiple logistic ment guidelines for acquired apraxia of speech: Treatment descriptions and
regression model. Commun. Stat. A10, 10431069. recommendations. J. Med. Speech Lang. Pathol. 14, xxxvixvii
Huber, W., Poeck, K., Weniger, D., Willmes, K., 1983. Aachener Apasie Test (AAT). Wiese, R., 2000. The Phonology of German. Oxford University Press, Oxford.
Gttingen, Hogrefe. Wightman, C.W., Shattuck-Hufnagel, S., Ostendorf, M., Price, P.J., 1992. Segmental
Itoh, M., Sasanuma, S., 1984. Articulatory movements in apraxia of speech. In: durations in the vicinity of prosodic phrase boundaries. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 91,
Rosenbek, J.C., McNeil, M.R., Aronson, A.E. (Eds.), Apraxia of Speech: Physiology, 17071717.
Acoustics, Linguistics, Management. College-Hill Press, San Diego, pp. 135165. Ziegler, W., 2005. A nonlinear model of word length effects in apraxia of speech.
Kent, R.D., Rosenbek, J.C., 1982. Prosodic disturbance and neurologic lesion. Brain Cognit. Neuropsychol. 22, 603623.
Lang. 15, 259291. Ziegler, W., 2008. Apraxia of speech. In: Goldenberg, G., Miller, B. (Eds.), Handbook
Kent, R.D., Rosenbek, J.C., 1983. Acoustic patterns of apraxia of speech. J. Speech of Clinical Neurology 88. Elsevier, London, pp. 269285.
Hear. Res. 26, 231249. Ziegler, W., 2009. Modelling the architecture of phonetic plans: evidence from
Laganaro, M., 2008. Is there a syllable frequency effect in aphasia or in apraxia of apraxia of speech. Lang. Cognit. Process. 24, 631e661.
speech or both? Aphasiology 22, 11911200. Ziegler, W., Aichert, I., 2015. How much is a word? Predicting ease of articulation
Laganaro, M., Vacheresse, F., Frauenfelder, U.H., 2002. Selective impairment of planning from apraxic speech error patterns. Cortex 69, 2439.
lexical stress assignment in an Italien-speaking aphasic patient. Brain Lang. 81, Ziegler, W., Aichert, I., Staiger, A., 2012. Apraxia of speech: concepts and con-
601609. troversies. J. Speech, Lang., Hear. Res. 55, 14851501.

Вам также может понравиться