Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 9

Food Quality and Preference 21 (2010) 225233

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Food Quality and Preference


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/foodqual

Perception of traditional food products in six European regions using free


word association
Luis Guerrero a,*, Anna Claret a, Wim Verbeke b, Geraldine Enderli c, Sylwia Zakowska-Biemans d,
Filiep Vanhonacker b, Sylvie Issanchou c, Marta Sajdakowska d, Britt Signe Granli e, Luisa Scalvedi f,
Michele Contel f, Margrethe Hersleth e
a
IRTA Monells, Finca Camps i Armet, E-17121 Monells, Spain
b
Ghent University, Department of Agricultural Economics, Coupure links 653, B-9000 Gent, Belgium
c
INRA, UMR 1129 FLAVIC, F-21000 Dijon, France
d
Warsaw University of Life Sciences SGGW (WULS-SGGW), Department of Organization and Consumption Economics, Nowoursynowska Street 166, 02-787 Warsaw, Poland
e
Noma Mat As, Osloveien 1, 1430 s, Norway
f
PEGroup, Viale Gorizia 25/C, 00198 Rome, Italy

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: A total of 721 consumers were interviewed in order to obtain and compare consumer-driven associations
Received 31 October 2008 to the word Traditional, in a food context, in six European regions. Participants, who were individually
Received in revised form 10 May 2009 interviewed, had to state the rst words that came into their mind when the word Traditional was ver-
Accepted 2 June 2009
bally presented. Frequencies of occurrence of associations were obtained and analysed by means of sim-
Available online 6 June 2009
ple correspondence analysis. The different word associations obtained were classied in 55 classes and
then grouped in ten principal dimensions by triangulation. In general, southern European regions tended
Keywords:
to associate the concept of Traditional more frequently with broad concepts such as heritage, culture or
Traditional food products
Free word association
history. Central and Nordic European regions tended to focus mainly on practical issues such as conve-
European regions nience, health or appropriateness. As a nal outcome of the analyses, a consensus conceptual map of tra-
ditional food products was obtained. The empirical ndings of this qualitative exploratory free word
association test provide valuable insights for product positioning, innovation and new developments in
the traditional food market.
2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction In order to better understand and predict the likelihood of suc-


cess or failure of different innovations in traditional foods it is cru-
Traditional food is an important element of the European cul- cial to know the meaning that the word Traditional has in
tural heritage. Production and sale of traditional food products pro- consumers minds and what values they relate to this concept.
vide a decisive economic input to many regions (European Some denitions of traditional food exist already (Bertozzi, 1998;
Commission, 2007) and can contribute to diversication of rural EU, 2006; EuroFIR, 2007; Jordana, 2000; Ministero Agricoltura,
areas and preventing them from depopulation. 1999; Trichopoulou, Soukara, & Vasilopoulou, 2007; Truefood,
European consumers demand for safe and tasteful traditional 2006), although all of them are coming from the experts perspec-
food products (Cayot, 2007), but also for a higher variety, more tive. The only published denition of traditional food from the con-
convenient, more nutritive and healthier options that t better sumers point of view is the one provided by Guerrero et al. (2009).
with the present needs in modern societies. In this context, there According to these authors European consumers perceived a tradi-
are some unexploited opportunities for traditional food producers tional food product as a product frequently consumed or associated
to maintain and even increase their market share by introducing with specic celebrations and/or seasons, normally transmitted from
innovations in their products, given that some of the current tech- one generation to another, made accurately in a specic way accord-
nologies involved in the production of traditional food products ing to the gastronomic heritage, with little or no processing/manipula-
still rely on traditional manufacturing practices with low compet- tion, distinguished and known because of its sensory properties and
itiveness and poor efciency (Fito & Toldra, 2006). associated with a certain local area, region or country. This deni-
tion was obtained by means of qualitative research using focus
groups discussions carried out in six European regions. Focus group
discussion is a qualitative exploratory research technique that pro-
* Corresponding author. Tel.: +34 972 630052; fax: +34 972 630373.
E-mail address: lluis.guerrero@irta.es (L. Guerrero). vides a rational and cognitive approach to a specic topic.

0950-3293/$ - see front matter 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.foodqual.2009.06.003
226 L. Guerrero et al. / Food Quality and Preference 21 (2010) 225233

However, in some cases, the results may be affected by stereotypes trained and experienced researchers and skilled interpreters. This
(Krueger, 1988) even when led by an experienced moderator. So- may explain why projective techniques have been heavily criti-
cial barriers and pressure may bias the expression of feelings, cised. The degree of subjectivity involved in the interpretation of
thoughts and self-reported behaviour. In some cases and when the responses obtained together with the high cost (time and
interviewed, subjects tend to provide answers that are socially money) required for performing them and the difculties for test-
desirable and stereotypical, thus trying to be polite and comply ing the reliability and validity of the results obtained might explain
with the supposed answer that the interviewer expects (Will, Ea- why these techniques have not been more frequently applied.
die, & Macaskill, 1996). The aim of this study was to identify European consumers asso-
Projective techniques, although qualitative in nature too, might ciations to the concept of Traditional in a food context in six Euro-
provide complementary information since they reveal the internal pean regions by means of free word association. In addition,
thoughts and feelings of a person, thus yielding a more spontane- different approaches for analysing the obtained information were
ous and affective answer from consumer. Projective techniques are compared and discussed. The robustness of the results was also
based on the use of vague, ambiguous, unstructured stimulus ob- checked by comparing the ndings with a previous denition of
jects or situations in which the subject projects his/her personality, traditional food products obtained by means of focus groups dis-
attitude, opinions and self-concept to give the situation some cussions in the same six regions (see Guerrero et al., 2009).
structure (Donoghue, 2000). In the scientic literature, there are
different commonly used projective techniques, both for personal-
ity and clinical evaluation, such as Rorschach Inkblot Test, The- 2. Materials and methods
matic Apperception Test, Drawing Techniques (Garba, Wood,
Lilienfeld, & Nezworski, 2002) or Free Word Association (Colangelo, 2.1. Participants
Stephenson, Westbury, & Buchanan, 2003) among others.
Word association allows the evaluation of conceptual struc- This study was carried out in six different regions from six Euro-
tures, as well as ascertaining belief or attitude changes in psychol- pean countries: Flanders in Belgium, Burgundy (Dijon) in France,
ogy and sociology (Ross, 2003) and even in food science. According Lazio region in Italy, the counties of Akershus and stfold in Nor-
to Dean et al. (2006) word association has proven to be useful in way, Mazovia (Warsaw) in Poland and Catalonia in Spain.
eliciting the affective element behind the concepts involved. The Consumers were selected within each area or region using a
elicitation of word associations is a relatively simple procedure, convenient intentional and reasoned sampling with predetermined
which is one of its attractions (Schmitt, 1998). Rozin, Kurzer, and quotas (Pedret, Sanier, Garca, & Morell, 2003). Convenience sam-
Cohen (2002) stated that after the rst free association, both the pling is often used in exploratory research where the researcher
target word and prior associations inuence any subsequent asso- is interested in getting an inexpensive approximation to a specic
ciation. Based on this assumption only the rst association elicited topic through involving participants who meet specic recruit-
by each consumer would provide new and relevant information. ment criteria with relevance for the subject under investigation.
However, Schmitt (1998) stated that subjects will not necessarily This non-probability method is recommended during preliminary
give the most relevant elicitation initially. In some cases they research activities to get a gross estimate of the results, without
may give an idiosyncratic response rst and a very pertinent as a incurring the cost or time required to select a random sample
second one. According to the latter author, allowing for multiple (Pla, 1999). Convenience sampling is probably used more often
responses gives the individual more chances to supply the more than any other kind of sampling in behavioural science research
relevant associations and to capture better the richness of a sub- (Graveter & Forzano, 2008). On average 120 participants in each re-
jects association network. gion (total n = 721 consumers) were recruited from previous dat-
Word association has proven to be useful also in the domain of abases and/or through different advertising systems. Participants
food research. Roininen, Arvola, and Lhteenmki (2006) and Ares, did not have any relationship with the research centre/university
Gimnez, and Gmbaro (2008) concluded that this technique was where the test was carried out.
an efcient and rapid method for obtaining information on con- The rst criterion for selecting the participants was their
sumer perceptions of local foods and conventional and functional involvement in deciding about food shopping and preparation of
yogurts, respectively. Rozin et al. (2002) studied the free associa- food at home. Only consumers who stated to be involved in these
tion patterns to the word food depending on gender, generation two activities were selected. Second, the different quotas for
and culture. They observed that the results of free associations had selecting consumers were age (a minimum of 15% of consumers
some stability over time, as well as some validity. Despite apparent in each decade from 20 to 60 years old) and gender (a minimum
simplicity and usefulness of this technique, very few studies have of 25% of individuals of each gender within each age group). Table
been published based on word associations in the food area. 1 shows the distribution of the recruited consumers per quota and
Donoghue (2000) stated that the main disadvantages of projec- region. In each country, all the participants belonged to the same
tive techniques are in general the complexity of the data and the geographical area or region. At the recruitment stage, participants
expertise required to interpret them. According to this author, were not informed about the specic objective of the study. No
the responses have little meaning without a careful evaluation by mention was made of the word Traditional when recruiting them.

Table 1
Characteristics of the participants recruited per quota and region (%).

Region Gender (%) Age groups (%) n


Males Females 2030 3140 4150 5160
Flanders (Belgium) 55.4 44.6 26.4 18.2 23.1 32.2 121
Burgundy (Dijon, France) 47.6 52.4 26.2 24.3 24.3 25.2 103
Lazio region (Italy) 47.9 52.1 35.6 24.8 19.8 19.8 121
Akershus and stfold counties (Norway) 38.2 61.8 23.5 19.6 35.3 21.6 102
Mazovia (Warsaw, Poland) 27.4 72.6 33.9 23.4 18.5 24.2 124
Catalonia (Spain) 35.3 64.7 27.3 26.0 22.0 24.7 150
L. Guerrero et al. / Food Quality and Preference 21 (2010) 225233 227

2.2. Free word association test data. In this analysis, each cell in the data le indicated the relative
frequency that a word, class or dimension (columns) was men-
Consumers were interviewed individually. Interviewers (one or tioned in each region (rows). Relative frequencies of elicitation
two in each region) explained the procedure for carrying out the for each word, class or dimension per gender and age were also in-
test to the participant and provided him/her with some examples cluded in all cases as supplementary variables (rows) to avoid
(house, holiday. . .) in order to relax the individual and to train redundant information. All the analyses were performed by means
him/her. Once the interviewer assumed that the participant had of the software XLSTAT 2006, v.4 (Addinsfot SARL, Paris, France).
properly understood the dynamics of the experiment the word
association test started. To focus on a food context, participants
were rst asked to concentrate on food-related issues and matters 3. Results and discussion
when providing their answer. They were asked to provide single-
word responses and avoid brands, specic dishes or foods in their 3.1. Number of elicited words
answers, since these words could be highly country/region specic
and could complicate the comparison between regions. The word The total number of valid elicited words was 1743, which repre-
association test consisted of verbally presenting the word Tradi- sents an average of 2.42 associations per participant. Most of the
tional (the stimulus) to the participants and wait for the rst ver- participants (82.8%) were able to provide two or three different
bal response that came to his/her mind. Consumers were asked to associations and only 8.5% could not associate anything to the word
elicit up to three different words (Schmitt & Meara, 1997). A max- traditional during the available time (30 s). Verbal uency, dened
imum of 30 seconds were allowed for giving each valid association. as the ability to produce words under specic constraints and with-
The total duration of the test including the introductory and train- in a xed time interval (Lezak, Howieson, Loring, Hannay, & Fischer,
ing phase ranged between 10 and 15 min. 2004), has frequently been related to semantic knowledge (Kemper
& McDowd, 2008, chap. 11). Semantic knowledge refers to the com-
2.3. Data analysis ponent of long-term memory containing knowledge of objects,
facts, and concepts as well as words and their meanings (Tulving,
All valid words elicited by each participant were retained for 1972). In consequence, the relatively high number of words elicited
data analysis according to Schmitt (1998). Although this procedure in the free word association test seems to indicate that, in general,
violates the assumption of independence between the different consumers had a clear image formation of the word traditional, gi-
associations (up to three associations coming from the same indi- ven that the access to their internal mental representation of the
vidual may occur), this violation was not considered in the present stimulus occurred relatively easily. Usually mentally working with
exploratory-qualitative approach as suggested by Schmitt (1998). abstract concepts is more difcult than working with objects or ac-
The words elicited in each region were rst translated to Eng- tions (Prabhu, 1987). The degree of abstractness of the word Tradi-
lish and then entered into an Excel le template made up of seven tional could have made the word association test quite
columns (participant, region, gender, age, rst association, second complicated, but this did not seem to be the case in the present
association and third association) and 721 rows (one per partici- study probably because of the familiarity and the emotional values
pant). All the translations were performed and/or veried by the associated with the word Traditional for most consumers.
researchers involved in each region, who also performed or at- No signicant differences (p > 0.05) were found in the number of
tended the individual interviews. A back-translation process (Bris- words elicited by gender or by age group, although verbal uency
lin, 1970; Maneesriwongul & Dixon, 2004) was applied for those may be related with both age (Tomer & Levin, 1993; Mathuranath
words difcult to translate. et al., 2003) and gender (Kimura, 1992; Weiss, Kemmler, Deisen-
Frequencies of elicitation were obtained for each region, each hammer, Fleischhacker, & Delazer, 2003). According to Herrmann,
gender and each age group following three different approaches: Walter, Ehlis, and Fallgatter (2006) functional brain activation dur-
rst, using the raw data (the different words elicited); second, ing a cognitive task is mainly inuenced by age but not by gender,
grouping raw data (words elicited) in different classes and third, showing older individuals less performance in verbal uency tasks.
grouping the classes in different dimensions. The grouping proce- The absence of differences between age groups in the present study
dure was based on the personal interpretation of the meaning of can be explained by the peculiarity of the stimulus word used. Gen-
the different words; words were grouped together by means of a erally, older people are more concerned about traditions and their
semantic analysis. All the grouping processes were performed by transmission from one generation to another (Laureati, Pagliarini,
triangulation: three different researchers built up their own classes Calcinoni, & Bidoglio, 2006). Furthermore, older people might be
and dimensions in an independent way. The denitive classes and more familiar with and knowledgeable about traditional foods.
dimensions and the name or label assigned to each of them was ob- According to Schwartz and Rubel (2005) age and culture explain
tained by consensus between the three researchers over the three the higher amount of the variance observed in samples from differ-
independent classications generated previously. The researchers ent countries for traditional values. These facts may have balanced
involved in this task had a minimum of 5 years of experience in the number of elicited words among younger and older consumers.
semantic analyses applied to different qualitative techniques Regarding countries or regions, no signicant differences (p >
(including free word association). Triangulation, or the application 0.05) were observed in the average number of elicited words (rang-
of multiple data collection and/or analysis techniques with the ing from 2.92 valid words/participant in Spain to 2.50 valid words/
same objective, provides a relatively potent means of assessing participant in Norway) except for Italian consumers, where the aver-
the degree of convergence as well as elaborating on divergences be- age number of valid elicited words per participant was only 0.92.
tween results obtained (Modell, 2005; Wadsworth, 2000). This difference was mainly due to the higher number of non-valid
The existence of statistical differences between the total num- associations (brands, foods and/or dishes) obtained in the Italian
ber of words elicited per region, age and gender was tested by sample.
means of a Chi-Square statistic. Previously, these gures were con-
verted to relative frequencies depending on the number of partic- 3.2. Approach by using the raw data
ipants available in each region, age or gender group. Additionally,
simple correspondence analysis was performed in order to better From the total of 1743 words elicited 741 (42.5%) were different
visualise the information contained in the matrices of frequency and only 28 (1.6%) words were quoted by at least 10 of the 721
228 L. Guerrero et al. / Food Quality and Preference 21 (2010) 225233

consumers. Some of the most frequent elicited words were Family mealtime routines. The meaning of the word Old is not obvious.
(n = 63), Old (n = 49), Habit (n = 45), Christmas (n = 43) and Grand- Old may have a neutral, a positive or a negative meaning. Positive
mother (n = 31). Frequency of elicitation has been related with as something authentic, well established and proven to be decent,
the strength or importance of a concept in the consumers minds that has to be preserved, versus negative as something out-dated,
(Guerrero, Colomer, Gurdia, Xicola, & Clotet, 2000). Accordingly, not very useful nowadays. In order to facilitate the interpretation
consumers related the concept of traditional in a food context to of the different associations obtained, Roininen et al. (2006) also
their family and familial situations (Family and Grandmother) and collected an affective rating for each elicited word from very neg-
with repeated practice (Habit), although it was also linked to spe- ative/bad to very positive/good, which has not been done in our
cial occasions such as Christmas. Family and traditions are closely study.
related, given that family is the most natural and common way Concerning the comparison between countries or regions, some
of transmitting norms and values from one generation to another similarities and differences were obtained (Fig. 1). Habit was the
and to build a cultural identity (Abad & Sheldon, 2008). Family tra- most frequent cited word in Belgium and Spain, while Christmas
ditions can bring a great sense of belonging by helping link chil- was the most frequent elicited word in Norway. In all these three
dren with generations of the past and present and provide regions traditional was mainly associated with what people
continuity and security, giving all family members a sense of normally eat, although in Norway consumers mainly focused on
belonging and commitment (Nelms, 2005). For these reasons it is what people normally eat in special occasions. The gastronomic
not surprising that Family appears as the most frequent association heritage was particularly important in France, where words such
to traditional in the raw data. According to Nelms (2005) habit is as Cooking or Dish had a noticeable high frequency. For Polish con-
also an important element of traditions, because it may create sumers traditional was mostly linked to sensory properties (Tasty)
new ones such as bedtime, special foods, different activities or and Family, while for Italians the most frequent association was

Belgium France
16 18

16
Frecuency of elicitation (n)
Frecuency of elicitation (n)

14
14
12
12
10
10
8
8
6
6
4
4
2 2

0 0
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28

Italy Norway
9 25

8
Frecuency of elicitation (n)

Frecuency of elicitation (n)

7 20

6
15
5

4
10
3

2
5
1

0 0
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28

Poland Spain
18 30

16
Frecuency of elicitation (n)

Frecuency of elicitation (n)

25
14

12 20

10
15
8

6 10

4
5
2

0 0
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28

Fig. 1. Most frequent words (n P 10 for all the participants) elicited in each region in the word association test using the word Traditional as stimulus. 1 = Ancient,
2 = Christmas, 3 = Cooking, 4 = Country, 5 = Culture, 6 = Dinner, 7 = Dish, 8 = Family, 9 = Feast, 10 = Good, 11 = Grandmother, 12 = Habit, 13 = Healthy, 14 = Holidays,
15 = Home, 16 = Home-made, 17 = Kitchen, 18 = Meal, 19 = Natural, 20 = Old, 21 = Old-fashioned, 22 = Quality, 23 = Recipe, 24 = Regional, 25 = Restaurant, 26 = Rural,
27 = Simple, 28 = Tasty.
L. Guerrero et al. / Food Quality and Preference 21 (2010) 225233 229

Homemade. These results stress the heterogeneity in food culture 3.3. Approach by grouping the raw data in different classes
within Europe in agreement with Askegaard and Madsen (1998),
who afrmed that Europe cannot be regarded homogeneously as In order to overcome the possible bias mentioned in the previ-
a unique food culture, since noticeable differences exist not only ous approach, the 741 different words obtained were grouped in
at a national level but also from a more regional/local point of view classes by triangulation (Modell, 2005; Wadsworth, 2000). Table
in food preferences, habits, food-related behaviour and attitudes. 2 shows some examples of the words grouped in the same class.
Despite the differences pointed out some similarities between A total of 55 classes were obtained by consensus between the three
regions were also observed. In general and from a qualitative point independent grouping processes performed (triangulation).
of view, the perception of the word traditional was quite similar in Results of the simple correspondence analysis performed on
all regions. Words such as Family, Good, Grandmother, Healthy, Nat- this data set with 55 classes are shown in Fig. 3. Southern European
ural, Regional, Restaurant or Simple were elicited in all regions. regions (in France, Italy and Spain) were located closer to more
Fig. 2 shows the result of the simple correspondence analysis broad classes difcult to interpret such as Heritage, History or Cul-
carried out for the words elicited at least by 10 consumers over ture while Central and Northern European regions were closer to
the six regions. It is important to note that this statistical technique more practical and concrete concepts such as Convenience, Appro-
highlights the main differences between regions (Greenacre & Bel- priateness or Health. The presence or absence of traditional prod-
sius, 1994). Again gastronomic associations (Restaurant, Cooking, ucts linked to collective quality labels (PDO, PGI and TSG) in
Meal, Recipe and Dish) were positioned closer to France (FR); Tasty, these countries may explain the distinction between Southern
Family and Dinner close to Poland (PL); Old-fashioned, Quality, Res- and Central-Nordic European countries. According to Jordana
taurant and Culture close to Belgium; Home, Good, Habit closer to (2000) the number of foods traditionally produced in Northern
Spain (SP); Christmas, Rural, Country and Good close to Norway
(NO); and Home-made, Natural and Old close to Italy (IT).
Several aspects that may inuence the conclusions obtained af- Table 2
fect the approach based on the analysis of the most frequent words Examples of elicited words grouped in the same class by triangulation.
in the raw data. When using the most frequent words in the raw Class Words
data the existence of synonym words was not considered, e.g.
Old Antique, old, not modern, made for a long time, aged,
the word Habit was retained but words such as Daily, Often, Usual,
not new. . .
Regularly, etc. that could probably have a similar meaning were not Celebration Marriage, birthday, feast (party), festive, feast meal, village
taken into account due to their lower frequency of elicitation. In festival, church festival, public holiday. . ..
addition, in cross-cultural studies when analysing the data based Habitual/typical Daily, often, regular, usual, everyday things of life, routine,
normal, ordinary (common), frequent. . .
on the frequency of elicited words themselves instead of their
Ancestors Grandmother, grandparents, mother, ancestral, roots, parents,
meaning, the translation from the original language to English mum, generation. . .
may amplify the differences between countries or regions, since Country/region European, local, Norwegian, Polish, regional, origin, terroir,
the same original meaning for a certain word could be translated country. . .
in two different words depending on the region where it was elic- Culture Culture, folklore, folk, dance, regional culture, food culture. . .
Farm/eld/rural Farm, eld, rustic, rural, agriculture, own kitchen garden,
ited and depending on the researcher who made the translation.
farming, cows, from the farm, harvest. . .

Feast
Recipe
Dish

Holidays
Meal FR Restaurant
Cooking BE

Regional Old-fashioned
Tasty Quality
PL
F2 (22.95 %)

Dinner Kitchen
Simple Culture
Family Grandmother
Healthy

Natural
IT Habit
Old
Homemade
Rural
Country Home
SP
NO Good
Christmas

Ancient

F1 (26.55 %)

Fig. 2. Simple correspondence analysis of the most frequent elicited words for all the consumers (n P 10). BE: Flanders (Belgium), FR: Burgundy (Dijon, France), IT: Lazio
region (Italy), NO: Akershus and stfold counties (Norway), PL: Mazovia (Warsaw, Poland), SP: Catalonia (Spain).
230 L. Guerrero et al. / Food Quality and Preference 21 (2010) 225233

Holidays
Convenience Shopping Season
Health Appropriateness
Aroma PL
NO
F2 (21.61%)
Taste Farm/field/rural
Natural/fresh BE
Boredom
Laborious
Celebration
FR
IT Good Conservative
Heritage History SP
Culture
Handmade

Distrust/risk Everlasting

F1 (29.02%)

Fig. 3. Simple correspondence analysis of the 55 different classes obtained by triangulation. BE: Flanders (Belgium), FR: Burgundy (Dijon, France), IT: Lazio region (Italy), NO:
Akershus and stfold counties (Norway), PL: Mazovia (Warsaw, Poland), SP: Catalonia (Spain). Only classes having a squared cosine (quality of the projection) higher or equal
to 0.5 in one of the rst two dimensions are shown.

European countries is very narrow compared to Southern European (includes classes such as Variety, Boring or Choice), Habit (includes
countries, where this range is rather wide. Consequently, the num- classes such as Habitual/typical), Origin (includes classes such as
ber of collective quality labels is also much lower in Northern Eur- Country/region), Basic/simple, Special occasions (includes classes
ope (Becker, 2009). The number of EU certication labels per such as Celebration, Holidays or Christmas) and Marketing (includes
country in 2007 was 155 in France, 164 in Italy and 111 in Spain classes such as Expensive, Store/shop or Distribution).
versus 10 in Belgium or 1 in Poland (Rosati, 2007). This fact may Fig. 4 shows the frequencies of elicitation for each dimension in
explain why more broad concepts are stronger associated to South- each region. As expected and because of the simplication of the
ern European countries, with a higher experience, knowledge and information the similarities between regions increased in this ap-
culture of traditional food products. proach of data analysis. All the dimensions were present in all
In Fig. 3 Poland was closer to sensory classes (Aroma and Taste) the regions except for Variety in Italy. The differences between re-
and Health; Norway was close to Farm/eld/rural, Shopping and gions were quantitative as it is also shown in Fig. 5 obtained after
Appropriateness; France and Italy were close to Heritage, Laborious, performing a simple correspondence analysis. Sensory and Health
Good and History; Spain was close to Culture, History, Celebration, dimensions were closer to Poland and Italy; Heritage was close to
Conservative and Good; and nally Belgium was close to Natural/ Spain; Special occasions, Basic/simple and Origin were close to Nor-
Fresh, Laborious and Celebration. Again, the results point out the way; Elaboration was close to France; and Habit, Marketing and
existence of differences in food culture within Europe. Neverthe- Variety were closer to Belgium. As noticed previously differences
less, it is important to point out that these differences are mainly in food culture across European countries may explain these
quantitative (frequencies of elicitation for the different classes be- results.
tween regions) since all the regions had a similar perception from This approach was the only one where a signicant difference
a qualitative perspective (the majority of the classes were present between genders (v2 = 39.408, p < 0.001) was observed. Women
in all the regions). By grouping the raw data in different classes all were closer to the Heritage, Health, Origin or Sensory dimensions,
the initial information was taken into account, thus allowing putt- while men were closer to Elaboration, Habit, Marketing and Variety.
ing together words expressed differently but having a similar Differences between genders are quite common in food related as-
meaning. The main disadvantage is the existence of a subjective pects such as attitudes (Gurdia, Guerrero, Gelabert, Gou, & Arnau,
grouping practice, although the reliability of this process may be 2006; Kiefer, Rathmanner, & Kunze, 2005; van den Bree, Przybeck,
increased when it is made by triangulation (Donoghue, 2000; & Cloninger, 2006), behaviour (Burton, Smit, & Lightowler, 2007;
Modell, 2005; Wadsworth, 2000) as it was done in the present Otis & Pelletier, 2008) or health concern (Perez-Cueto, Verbeke, La-
study. chat, & Remaut-De Winter, 2009; Rozin, Bauer, & Catanese, 2003).
The main advantage of this approach is its simplicity, although
3.4. Approach by grouping the different classes in generic dimensions the double grouping process (in classes rst and in dimensions sec-
ond) increases the subjectivity of the results obtained. In addition,
In order to perform a further reduction of the information facil- some difculties may be observed when trying to obtain a reduced
itating the interpretation of the results obtained, the 55 classes cre- number of classes because it was not always easy to group the dif-
ated in the previous approach were grouped in different ferent classes together under a common dimension or concept. It is
dimensions by triangulation (Modell, 2005; Wadsworth, 2000). A also important to notice that using this approach the more subtle
total of 10 dimensions were obtained by consensus between the differences between regions may disappear.
three independent grouping processes performed. These dimen-
sions were Sensory (includes classes such as Tasty, Taste, or Fla- 3.5. Robustness of the information obtained
vour), Health (includes classes such as Healthy, Unhealthy, Heavy/
copious or Nutritious), Elaboration (includes classes such as Hand- In order to check the robustness of the words associated to the
made, Home-made, Elaboration or Laborious), Heritage (includes concept of traditional obtained by means of different approaches,
classes such as Ancestors, Old, Family, Culture or Everlasting), Variety the nal ten dimensions created were compared with the afore-
L. Guerrero et al. / Food Quality and Preference 21 (2010) 225233 231

Belgium France

60 60

50 50
Frecuency of elicitation (n)

Frecuency of elicitation (n)


40 40

30 30

20 20

10 10

0 0
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

Italy Norway

20 60

18
50
16
Frecuency of elicitation (n)

Frecuency of elicitation (n)


14
40
12

10 30

8
20
6

4
10
2

0 0
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

Poland Spain

100 100

90 90

80 80
Frecuency of elicitation (n)

Frecuency of elicitation (n)

70 70

60 60

50 50

40 40

30 30

20 20

10 10

0 0
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

Fig. 4. Frequency of elicited dimensions in each region in the word association test using the word Traditional as stimulus. 1 = Elaboration, 2 = Heritage, 3 = Origin,
4 = Health, 5 = Special occasions, 6 = Habit, 7 = Sensory, 8 = Marketing, 9 = Basic/simple, 10 = Variety.

mentioned denition of the concept of traditional food obtained in 4. Conclusions


the same six regions (Guerrero et al., 2009). These authors found by
means of focus groups that seven different factors made up the In general, the participants in the present study had a clear im-
concept of traditional: Habit, Special occasions, Heritage (transmit- age formation of the word Traditional despite its abstractness and
ted from one generation to another), Made in a specic way (elabo- independently of their age or gender. Although this image was
ration), Sensory properties, Simplicity and Linked to a certain origin. quantitatively (frequency of elicitation of each raw word, class or
Each of these seven factors was observed in the present study as dimension) different depending on the European region observed,
well. In addition, the word association test provided supplemen- important qualitative similarities were detected. From a qualita-
tary information since three additional dimensions emerged from tive perspective the concept of Traditional in a food context is
this test: Health (positive and negative aspects), Marketing and made up of ten main dimensions: sensory, health, elaboration, her-
Variety. Despite its limitations, the word association technique itage, variety, habit, origin, simplicity, special occasions and mar-
proved to be a robust qualitative research technique to identify keting. These dimensions conrm a previous denition for the
various dimensions of traditional food and to broaden the meaning concept of Traditional food products obtained in the same six re-
of the concept of traditional as compared with the results of other gions using focus group discussions and demonstrate the robust-
qualitative approaches such as focus groups. Similarly, Rozin et al. ness of a free word association test in the food domain as well as
(2002) also concluded that free word association test was a valid its value as a reliable research tool that even provides additional
methodology in food related aspects. information (three new dimensions: Health, Marketing and Variety)
232 L. Guerrero et al. / Food Quality and Preference 21 (2010) 225233

BASIC/SIMPLE

SENSORY IT SPECIALNO
Women ORIGIN OCCASIONS
PL HEALTH HERITAGE
SP
FR
ELABORATION
Men HABIT
F2 (21.12%)

BE
MARKETING

VARIETY

F1 (46.69%)

Fig. 5. Simple correspondence analysis of the 10 different dimensions obtained by triangulation. BE: Flanders (Belgium), FR: Burgundy (Dijon, France), IT: Lazio region (Italy),
NO: Akershus and stfold counties (Norway), PL: Mazovia (Warsaw, Poland), SP: Catalonia (Spain).

as compared to some other qualitative techniques. However, free Donoghue, S. (2000). Projective techniques in consumer research. Journal of Family
Ecology and Consumer Sciences, 28, 4753.
word association may reveal different results depending on the
EU (2006). Council Regulation (EC) No. 509/2006 of 20 March 2006 on agricultural
consumer sample selected and the approach used for data analysis. products and foodstuffs as traditional specialities guaranteed. Ofcial Journal of
Consequently, these results need to be conrmed and validated in the European Union L 93/1.
a quantitative study involving larger and representative consumer EuroFIR (2007). FOOD-CT-2005513944, EU6th framework food quality and safety
programme. Website: <http://www.euror.net>. Visited June 2008.
samples. In any case, the empirical ndings obtained from the free European Commission (2007). European research on traditional food. Website:
word association test provide valuable insight for product posi- <http://ftp.cordis.europa.eu/pub/fp7/kbbe/docs/traditional-foods.pdf>. Visited
tioning, innovation and new developments in the traditional food October 2008.
Fito, P., & Toldra, F. (2006). Innovations in traditional foods. EFFOST 2005
market. conference. Trends in Food Science and Technology, 17(9), 470.
Garba, H. N., Wood, J. M., Lilienfeld, S. O., & Nezworski, M. T. (2002). Effective use of
Acknowledgments projective techniques in clinical practice: Let the data help with selection and
interpretation. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 33(5), 454463.
Graveter, F. J., & Forzano, L. A. B. (2008). Research methods for the behavioural sciences.
This study was supported by the TRUEFOOD Traditional Uni- Cengage Learning EMEA, International Editions 3e. UK: Gardners Books.
ted Europe Food an Integrated Project nanced by the European Greenacre, M., & Belsius, J. (1994). Correspondence analysis in the social sciences.
Amsterdam: Academic Press.
Commission under the 6th Framework Programme (Contract No. Gurdia, M. D., Guerrero, L., Gelabert, J., Gou, P., & Arnau, J. (2006). Consumer attitude
FOOD-CT-2006-016264). towards sodium reduction in meat products and acceptability of fermented
The information in this document reects only the authors sausages with reduced sodium content. Meat Science, 73(3), 484490.
Guerrero, L., Colomer, Y., Gurdia, M. D., Xicola, J., & Clotet, R. (2000). Consumer
views and the European Commission is not liable for any use that
attitude towards store brands. Food Quality and Preference, 11(5), 387395.
may be made of the information contained therein. Guerrero, L., Guardia, M. D., Xicola, J., Verbeke, W., Vanhonacker, F., Zakowska, S., et al.
(2009). Consumer-driven denition of traditional food products and innovation
in traditional foods. A qualitative cross-cultural study. Appetite, 52(2), 345354.
References Herrmann, M. J., Walter, A., Ehlis, A. C., & Fallgatter, A. J. (2006). Cerebral
oxygenation changes in the prefrontal cortex: Effects of age and gender.
Abad, N. S., & Sheldon, K. M. (2008). Parental autonomy support and ethnic culture Neurobiology of Aging, 27(6), 888894.
identication among second-generation immigrants. Journal of Family Jordana, J. (2000). Traditional foods: Challenges facing the European food industry.
Psychology, 22(3), 652657. Food Research International, 33(34), 147152.
Ares, G., Gimnez, A., & Gmbaro, A. (2008). Understanding consumers perception Kemper, S., & McDowd, J. M. (2008). Dimensions of cognitive aging: Executive
of conventional and functional yogurts using word association and hard function and verbal uency. In S. M. Hofer & D. F. Alwin (Eds.), Handbook of
laddering. Food Quality and Preference, 19(7), 636643. cognitive aging. Interdisciplinary perspectives (pp. 744). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage
Askegaard, S., & Madsen, T. K. (1998). The local and the global: Exploring traits of Publications, Inc..
homogeneity and heterogeneity in European food cultures. International Kiefer, I., Rathmanner, T., & Kunze, M. (2005). Eating and dieting differences in men
Business Review, 7, 549568. and women. The Journal of Mens Health and Gender, 2(2), 194201.
Becker, T. (2009). European food quality policy: The importance of geographical Kimura, D. (1992). Sex differences in the brain. Scientic American, 267, 118125.
indications, organic certication and food quality insurance schemes in Krueger, R. A. (1988). Focus groups: A practical guide for applied research. Newbury
European countries. Estey Centre Journal of Internal Law and Trade Policy, Park, CA: Sage Publications.
10(1), 111130. Laureati, M., Pagliarini, E., Calcinoni, O., & Bidoglio, M. (2006). Sensory acceptability
Bertozzi, L. (1998). Tipicidad alimentaria y dieta mediterrnea. In A. Medina, F. of traditional food preparations by elderly people. Food Quality and Preference,
Medina, & G. Colesanti (Eds.), El color de la alimentacin mediterrnea. 17(12), 4352.
Elementossensoriales y culturales de la nutricin (pp. 1541). Barcelona: Icaria. Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., Loring, D. W., Hannay, H. J., & Fischer, J. S. (2004).
Brislin, R. W. (1970). Back-translation for cross-cultural research. Journal of Cross- Neuropsychological assessment (4th ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
Cultural Psychology, 1, 185216. Maneesriwongul, W., & Dixon, J. K. (2004). Instrument translation process: A
Burton, P., Smit, H. J., & Lightowler, H. J. (2007). The inuence of restrained and methods review. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 48, 175186.
external eating patterns on overeating. Appetite, 49(1), 191197. Mathuranath, P. S., George, A., Cherian, P. J., Alexander, A., Sarma, S. G., & Sarma, P. S.
Cayot, N. (2007). Sensory quality of traditional foods. Food Chemistry, 101(1), (2003). Effects of age, education and gender on verbal uency. Journal of Clinical
154162. and Experimental Neuropsychology, 25(8), 10571064.
Colangelo, A., Stephenson, K., Westbury, C., & Buchanan, L. (2003). Word Ministero Agricoltura (1999). Decreto Legislativo 30 Aprile 1998 n. 173. Decreto 785
associations in deep dyslexia. Brain and Cognition, 53, 166170. Ministero Agricoltura 8 settembre 1999 n. 350, Italy.
Dean, M., Arvola, A., Vassallo, M., Lhteenmki, L., Raats, M. M., Saba, A., et al. (2006). Modell, S. (2005). Triangulation between case study and survey methods in
Comparison of elicitation methods for moral and affective beliefs in the theory management accounting research: An assessment of validity implications.
of planned behaviour. Appetite, 47(2), 244252. Management Accounting Research, 16, 231254.
L. Guerrero et al. / Food Quality and Preference 21 (2010) 225233 233

Nelms, B. C. (2005). Giving children a great gift: Family traditions. Journal of Schmitt, N. (1998). Quantifying word association responses: What is native-like?
Pediatric Health Care, 19, 345346. System, 26(3), 389401.
Otis, N., & Pelletier, L. G. (2008). Womens regulation styles for eating behaviors and Schmitt, N., & Meara, P. (1997). Researching vocabulary through a word knowledge
outcomes: The mediating role of approach and avoidance food planning. framework. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 19(1), 1736.
Motivation and Emotion, 32(1), 5567. Schwartz, S. H., & Rubel, T. (2005). Sex differences in value priorities: Cross-cultural
Pedret, R., Sanier, L., Garca, I., & Morell, A. (2003). Investigaci de Mercats I. and multimethod studies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(6),
Barcelona: Fundaci per a la Universitat Oberta de Catalunya. Editorial UOC. 10101028.
Perez-Cueto, F., Verbeke, W., Lachat, C., & Remaut-De Winter, A. M. (2009). Changes Tomer, R., & Levin, B. E. (1993). Differential effects of aging on two verbal uency
in dietary habits following temporal migration: The case of international tasks. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 76(2), 465466.
students in Belgium. Appetite, 52(1), 8388. Trichopoulou, A., Soukara, S., & Vasilopoulou, E. (2007). Traditional foods: A
Pla, M. (1999). El rigor en la investigacin cualitativa. Atencin Primaria, 24(5), science and society perspective. Trends in Food Science and Technology, 18(8),
295300. 420427.
Prabhu, N. S. (1987). Second language pedagogy. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Truefood (2006). Newsletter of integrated project traditional United Europe food
Roininen, K., Arvola, A., & Lhteenmki, L. (2006). Exploring consumers perceptions (Truefood), Year 1, n. 1, October 2006.
of local food with two different qualitative techniques: Laddering and word Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In E. Tulving & W. Donaldson
association. Food Quality and Preference, 17(12), 2030. (Eds.), Organization of memory. New York: Academic Press.
Rosati, M. (2007). I prodotti certicati in Italia ed in Europa (pp. 743744). van den Bree, M. B. E., Przybeck, T. R., & Cloninger, C. R. (2006). Diet and
Osservarotio Socio Economico QUALIVITA. Available at the Website: <http:// personality: Associations in a population-based sample. Appetite, 46(2),
www.qualivita.it/FrontEnd/OSS2007/studio.aspx>. Visited October 2008. 177188.
Ross, T. P. (2003). The reliability of cluster and switch scores for the controlled oral Wadsworth, G. (2000). Hearing midlife voices: Assessing different methods for
word association test. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 18(2), 153164. researching womens experiences of menopause and midlife. Womens Studies
Rozin, P., Bauer, R., & Catanese, D. (2003). Food and life, pleasure and worry, among International Forum, 23(5), 645654.
American college students: Gender differences and regional similarities. Journal Weiss, E., Kemmler, G., Deisenhammer, E. A., Fleischhacker, W. W., & Delazer, M.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(1), 132141. (2003). Sex differences in cognitive functions. Personality and Individual
Rozin, P., Kurzer, N., & Cohen, A. B. (2002). Free associations to food: The effects of Differences, 35, 863875.
gender, generation, and culture. Journal of Research in Personality, 36(5), Will, V., Eadie, D., & Macaskill, S. (1996). Projective and enabling techniques
419441. explored. Marketing Intelligence and Planning, 14(6), 3844.

Вам также может понравиться