Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 10

Behavioural Brain Research 305 (2016) 164173

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Behavioural Brain Research


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/bbr

Research report

Modality specicity in the cerebro-cerebellar neurocircuitry during


working memory
H.B. Tommy Ng a , K.-L. Cathy Kao a , Y.C. Chan b,c , Efe Chew b,c , K.H. Chuang d ,
S.H. Annabel Chen a,e,
a
Division of Psychology, School of Humanities and Social Sciences, Nanyang Technological University, 637332, Singapore
b
Division of Neurology, University Medicine Cluster, National University Health System, Singapore
c
Department of Medicine, Yong Loo Lin School of Medicine, National University of Singapore, Singapore
d
The Queensland Brain Institute, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia
e
Centre for Research and Development in Learning (CRADLE), Nanyang Technological University, 637459, Singapore

h i g h l i g h t s

We examined hemispheric lateralization patterns in working memory.


Left lateralized IFG and IPL were activated during verbal working memory.
Right lateralized cerebellar lobular VI and VIII were also activated in verbal working memory.
A left-cortical right-cerebellar network underlies verbal working memory.
No evidence for a right-cortical left-cerebellar network in visual working memory.

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Previous studies have suggested cerebro-cerebellar circuitry in working memory. The present fMRI study
Received 28 December 2015 aims to distinguish differential cerebro-cerebellar activation patterns in verbal and visual working mem-
Received in revised form 22 February 2016 ory, and employs a quantitative analysis to deterimine lateralization of the activation patterns observed.
Accepted 24 February 2016
Consistent with Chen and Desmond (2005a,b) predictions, verbal working memory activated a cerebro-
Available online 27 February 2016
cerebellar circuitry that comprised left-lateralized language-related brain regions including the inferior
frontal and posterior parietal areas, and subcortically, right-lateralized superior (lobule VI) and infe-
Keywords:
rior cerebellar (lobule VIIIA/VIIB) areas. In contrast, a distributed network of bilateral inferior frontal
Cerebellum
Cerebro-cerebellar and inferior temporal areas, and bilateral superior (lobule VI) and inferior (lobule VIIB) cerebellar areas,
Functional magnetic resonance imaging was recruited during visual working memory. Results of the study veried that a distinct cross cerebro-
Verbal working memory cerebellar circuitry underlies verbal working memory. However, a neural circuitry involving specialized
Visual working memory brain areas in bilateral neocortical and bilateral cerebellar hemispheres subserving visual working mem-
ory is observed. Findings are discussed in the light of current models of working memory and data from
related neuroimaging studies.
2016 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction comprehension [7]. Within the theoretical framework, mecha-


nisms of the phonological loop and visuo-spatial sketchpad are
Working memory may be conceptualized as a neural system that thought to support verbal and visual working memory, respec-
temporarily maintains, stores, and manipulates information for tively.
complex cognitive tasks such as reasoning, learning, and language Data from neuroimaging studies have consistently demon-
strated the involvement of left inferior frontal and left inferior
parietal regions in verbal working memory, areas of the brain that
Corresponding author at: Division of Psychology, School of Humanities and have been linked to phonological rehearsal and storage, respec-
Social Sciences, HSS-04-19, 14 Nanyang Drive, Nanyang Technological University, tively [20,21,3,36,11,35,116,55]. Although cerebellar activations
637332, Singapore. were also observed in early neuroimaging studies [96,99,56], their
E-mail addresses: annabelchen@ntu.edu.sg, tommymr25@yahoo.co.uk,
ENGZ0001@e.ntu.edu.sg (S.H.A. Chen).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2016.02.027
0166-4328/ 2016 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
H.B.T. Ng et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 305 (2016) 164173 165

Fig. 1. Event timing of a single trial of the Sternberg working memory tasks. Stimuli presented during high-load and low-load condition of (a) verbal working memory and
(b) visual working memory tasks. (c) Timing of one cycle of high-load and low-load trials interleaved by a 1.6 s rest interval.

exact function in verbal working memory was not well understood Although the laterality effect was observed in a number of
(see [56]). subsequent studies [18,60,61,73], a caveat was that hemispheric
In a previous fMRI study, Desmond et al. [28] identied two lateralization was assessed via visual comparison of the number of
cerebellar regions that were differentially activated during verbal activated voxels and activation intensity (as indicated by the nor-
working memory: one located bilaterally in the superior hemi- malized Z value) in homologous brain regions, without the rigour
sphere (lobule VI/Crus I), and another in the inferior aspect (lobule of statistical testing. To address this, one of the objectives of the
VIIB) of the right hemisphere. Based on the canonical cortico-ponto- present study was to evaluate the laterality effect of verbal working
cerebellar network [81,59] that forms a closed loop circuitry via the memory by means of a laterality index measurement [10,26,29,40],
thalamus [123,2,79]; for review see [102], the authors proposed which statistically evaluates voxel beta-values of homologous
a cerebro-cerebellar model of verbal working memory to account regions of interest. Specically, an unbiased threshold-free com-
for superior cerebellar contribution to phonological rehearsal and putation of voxel t-values within regions of interest was used to
encoding and inferior cerebellar contribution to phonological stor- calculate the laterality index. This approach has been shown to
age processes. In a later event- related fMRI study, Chen and minimize computational biases that are associated with statistical
Desmond [17] found concomitant activation of the superior cere- thresholding [75]; for review see [113].
bellum and left frontal regions during encoding, and concomitant Another objective of the present study was to verify the later-
activation of the right inferior cerebellum and left parietal regions ality effects of working memory processes in the visual system. In
during maintenance, supporting earlier speculations that a later- the animal literature, an abundance of neurophysiological evidence
alized left cortical-right cerebellar neurocircuitry supports verbal converged on the involvement of ventral prefrontal and occipi-
working memory. totemporal brain areas in pattern, object, and face recognition and
recall [135,83,82,101,109,106]. These results are in good concor-
166 H.B.T. Ng et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 305 (2016) 164173

dance with studies demonstrating visual recognition decits in load condition) was presented above the cross for 3 s. During this
monkeys with ventromedial prefrontal cortex and inferior tem- time, subjects rehearsed the letters in sequential order subvocally
poral cortex lesions [95,85,4,54]. Findings from human lesion (for and continued to do so during a 3 s maintenance period where no
review see [34,94,93,90,88,84] and neuroimaging [24,50,100]; for stimuli were shown on the screen. After the maintenance period,
review see [130,104] studies also provide evidence for the involve- a probe letter was presented for 2 s and the subjects determined
ment of the abovementioned brain regions in visual working if it matched one of the letters in the array by pressing a button
memory. However, despite the wealth of data, it remains unclear under the right index nger for match and a button under the
whether or not cerebral hemispheric lateralization persists in pro- right middle nger for not-match.
cesses underlying visual working memory. In the visual working memory (VISWM) task, the same xa-
While there have been reports of left mid- frontal region tion cross preceded (1.2 s) the presentation of a horizontal array of
preponderance during object working memory tasks [115,117], either three non-repeated geometrical patterns (high-load condi-
there are by far more evidence for supporting right cere- tion) or three identical geometrical pattern (low-load condition).
bral dominance during visuo-spatial working memory tasks During a 4 s encoding phase, subjects viewed and retained the
[77,52,23,132,131,125,136]; for review see [87,120]. The incon- visual patterns in their mind and continued to do so during a 1.5 s
sistent laterality effects observed at the neocortex prevail at the maintenance period where no stimuli were shown on the screen.
cerebellar level, with a number of studies reporting left cerebel- After the maintenance period, a probe pattern was presented below
lar dominance during face [51,62], spatial [45], and abstract object the cross for 2.5 s and the subjects determined if it matched one of
[49,57] working memory tasks, and to a lesser extent, preponder- the patterns in the array by a button press in the same manner as
ance of right cerebellar activations during visuospatial working that in the VERWM task. Subjects were instructed not to use verbal
memory tasks [97,126]. strategies for the VISWM task. In both the verbal and visual working
Given the inconsistency in previous ndings, the current study memory tasks, duration of a single trial was 9.2 s (see Fig. 1).
aims to determine if laterality topography exists for working mem-
ory in the cerebrum and cerebellum for verbal and visual stimuli 2.3. Behavioural data analysis
through a quantitative analysis.
Paired-samples t-tests evaluated within-condition (high load
versus low load) differences in performance accuracy and reaction
2. Materials and methods
times.

2.1. Participants
2.4. MRI data acquisition

Forty two (22 males, 20 females) neurologically healthy young-


Whole-brain MRI data were acquired on a 3 T scanner (Siemens
adults took part in the study. Their age ranged from 2131 years
Trio, Erlangen, Germany) equipped with a 32-channel array coil.
with a mean of 22.83 years (S.D. = 1.95 years). All the subjects were
To minimize movements, foam paddings were placed between the
right-handed (Laterality Quotient (LQ) >60) according to Edinburgh
subjects head and scanners head rest. The acquisition protocol
Handedness Inventory (EHI LQ: Mean = 96.83, S.D. = 6.14; [92] . The
consisted of localizer, structural, and functional scans. High res-
study protocol was approved by the Nanyang Technological Univer-
olution (1 mm isotropic) T1- weighted images were acquired in
sity Institutional Review Board and the National Healthcare Group
the sagittal plane with magnetization prepared rapid acquisition
(NHG) Domain Specic Review Board (DSRB)(A). The experimental
gradient echo sequence. T2*-weighted echo planar images were
procedures were carried out in accordance with the 1964 Decla-
acquired in the oblique axial plane with the following parameters:
ration of Helsinki. Subjects were paid for their participation after
TR = 2500 ms; TE = 29 ms; 48 slices; ip angle = 90 ; FOV = 225 mm;
successful completion of the experiment. Informed consent was
64 64 matrix giving an in-plane voxel size of 3.5 3.5 mm2 ; slice
obtained from individual subjects prior to the start of each experi-
thickness = 3.5 mm. The acquisition plane was rotated 25 with
ment.
respect to the posterior vertical axis of the brainstem to optimize
signal measurements from the cerebellum and neocortices [18].
2.2. Task description MRI data collection was synchronized with the behavioural task via
interfacing with the E-Prime software. Prior to each run, subjects
We employed a block design for two working memory tasks were provided instructions via the scanners intercom system.
(verbal and visual), based on the Sternberg paradigm. The tasks
were presented to the subjects in alternating blocks of high- and 2.5. Functional data analysis
low- load in separate runs. Each cycle consisted of 1 block of high-
load and 1 block of low-load that had 2 trials each. Each block was fMRI data were analyzed using the Statistical Parametric Map-
interleaved by a 1.6 s interval and block duration was 20 s. There ping (SPM8) software package (Wellcome Department of Cognitive
were 10 cycles in each run adding up to a total of 20 high-load and Neurology, Institute of Neurology, University College London, UK,
20 low-load trials. Each run lasted 400 s. 2 runs of each task were www.l.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). Structural and EPI images were aligned
performed and task order was counterbalanced across subjects. to the AC-PC plane. An interleaved bottom-up slice timing correc-
Stimuli were pseudo-randomly presented on a Fujitsu Lifebook tion was applied to the EPI images to correct for temporal lags in
computer (Fujitsu, Tokyo, Japan) with E-Prime version 2.0 (Psychol- image acquisition. Head motions were corrected by aligning EPI
ogy Software Tools, Inc., Pittsburgh, US) software. The images were images of run 1 and run 2 to the rst EPI image of run 1. This rou-
projected onto a screen that was located at the back of the scanner tine generated a mean EPI image, which was used as a source image
bore, and subjects viewed them via a mirror mounted on the cage for registering the realigned functional images to the individual
coil. Before the experiment began, subjects practised the tasks to subjects anatomical T1 image.
familiarize with the experimental procedures. The experimental effects were estimated with the general linear
In the verbal working memory (VERWM) task, a red xation model within SPM8. Two [high-load > low-load (VERWM); high-
cross indicating the start of a trial was presented in the middle of the load > low-load (VISWM)] contrast images were calculated for each
screen for 1.2 s. Following that, a horizontal array of either six non- subject, yielding 42 images per task. It was assumed that these con-
repeated letters (high-load condition) or six identical letters (low- trast images reected parametric recruitment of neural resources
H.B.T. Ng et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 305 (2016) 164173 167

with signicance level at p < 0.05 were reported. Except for cere-
bellar activations that were superimposed onto the SUIT template,
all other brain activations were superimposed onto the MNI tem-
plate. Locations of cerebral and cerebellar activation maxima were
determined using the Automated Anatomical Labeling [AAL; [129]]
software and probabilistic cerebellar atlas [31], respectively.
Lateralization of brain activations was evaluated with AveLI [75].
In this method, voxel beta-values are used to compute laterality
index (LI) of two homologous regions of interest (ROIs). Specically,
subordinate LIs (sub-LIs) are computed from task-specic positive
beta-values using the following equation:

Lt Rt
sub LI =
Lt + Rt

where Lt and Rt are the summations of voxel beta-values at and


above an arbitrary threshold in the left and right ROIs, respectively.
AveLI is subsequently calculated by averaging the sub-LIs in the
ROIs as follows:

(sub LI)
AveLI =
VN
where VN is the total number of positive t-values voxels in the ROIs.
In the current study, we used AAL template [129] in Wake For-
est University PickAtlas [71] and the SUIT cerebellum template [30]
to create bilateral ROIs for AveLI analyses in the cerebral cortex
and cerebellum, respectively. Based on previous literature, ROIs for
the VERWM task were dened in the inferior frontal gyrus (see
[17], inferior parietal lobule (see [105], cerebellar lobule VI/CrusI,
and cerebellar lobule VIIB/VIII (see [18]. For the VISWM task, ROIs
were dened in the inferior frontal gyrus (see [23], inferior tem-
poral gyrus (see [103], cerebellar lobule VI/CrusI (see [126], and
cerebellar lobule VIIB/VIII (see [49].

3. Results
Fig. 2. For display purposes, activation maps superimposed on the canonical T1-
weighted rendered brain were thresholded at T = 4.7 [P < 0.05 (FWE corrected) at 3.1. Behavioural data
cluster-level]. (a) During verbal working memory, left hemispheric dominance
was observed in frontal, parietal, occipital, and occipitotemporal regions. (b) Dur- In the VERWM task, performance accuracy (percent) was signif-
ing visual working memory, bilateral activations of the same brain regions were icantly (t (40) = 5.55, P < 0.001) higher in the low-load (M = 98.90,
observed. The colour bar refers to activation intensity expressed in T values. Lighter
colour represents increased activation intensity.
SD = 1.94) compared to high-load (M = 95.24, SD = 4.18) condition.
Reaction times (milliseconds) were signicantly (t (40) = 16.51,
P < 0.001) greater during high-load (M = 939.64, SD = 166.38) com-
during the two load conditions. The images were then normalized pared to low-load (M = 653.55, SD = 114.27) condition. Similarly in
separately to t the ICBM152 whole-brain template with default the VISWM task, subjects registered signicantly (t (40) = 12.05,
parameters in SPM8 and the Spatially Unbiased Infra-tentorial P < 0.001) higher percentage of correct responses in the low-load
Template (SUIT). This was done to overcome the limitations of (M = 97.99, SD = 2.64) compared to high-load (M = 87.44, SD = 5.85)
normalizing cerebellar activations to MNI space. Accordingly, the condition. Reaction times (milliseconds) were signicantly (t
MNI template, which is derived from 152 T1-weighted scans, lacks (40) = 14.03, P < 0.001) greater during high-load (M = 864.07,
information of important anatomical landmarks (e.g. primary and SD = 165.07) compared to low-load (M = 699.56, SD = 142.56) con-
intrabiventer ssures) crucial for aligning structures in the cere- dition.
bellum. It has been previously suggested that such a normalization Across tasks, subjects performed better in the verbal task com-
procedure would result in substantial stretching of the cerebellum pared to the visual task (t (40) = 7.02, P < 0.001) although they took
in the z-direction, pushing activations in superior aspect of the a longer time to make their responses (t (40) = 4.10, P < 0.001).
cerebellum to the visual cortices and obscuring those in the inferior
cerebellum [30,31]. 3.2. Imaging data
In the SUIT normalization, individual T1- weighted images were
cropped to isolate the cerebellum from the other brain structures. 3.2.1. Activations in MNI space
These structural images were then warped to t the SUIT space and Fig. 2, Table 1, and Table 2 show regional activation proles and
the resulting deformation parameters were applied to the func- loci of activation maxima during the working memory tasks.
tional images for resampling at 3.5 3.5 3.5 mm3 resolution. All In the VERWM task, left hemispheric dominance was observed
contrast images were smoothed with a 6 mm Full Width Half Max- in inferior and middle frontal regions, posterior parietal regions,
imum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel. and occipitotemporal regions. Apart from the brain regions
After normalization, random effects analyses were conducted depicted in Fig. 2, other activated brain areas not visible from the
on task-specic contrasts with one-sample t-tests. Cluster-level rendered brain included the putamen, caudate, pallidum, insular,
activations that survived family-wise error (FWE) rate correction cingulum, and hippocampus.
168 H.B.T. Ng et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 305 (2016) 164173

Table 1 Table 4
Brain regions activated during the working memory tasks, high-load > low-load. Means and standard deviations (SDs) of the laterality indices.

Brain structures MNI coordinates (mm) ROIs VERWM VISWM

Nvox SPM{Z} X Y Z Mean SD Laterality Mean SD Laterality

Verbal working memory IFG 0.32 0.17 Left 0.02 0.23 Bilateral
Precentral L 2688 7.57 51 0 46 IPL 0.59 0.20 Left
Precentral L 6.56 49 2 28 ITG 0.04 0.26 Bilateral
Precentral L 6.55 41 6 30 CL VI 0.37 0.40 Right 0.06 0.36 Bilateral
Calcarine L 8351 7.07 11 91 0 CL Crus I 0.12 0.46 Bilateral 0.08 0.39 Bilateral
Occipital Inf R 6.91 21 92 4 CL VIIB 0.58 0.45 Right 0.05 0.51 Bilateral
Putamen L 2234 7.20 19 4 14 CL VIIIA 0.31 0.41 Right 0.12 0.45 Bilateral
Caudate R 6.73 17 0 18
Abbreviations: IFG: inferior frontal gyrus, IPL: inferior parietal lobule, ITG: infe-
Pallidum R 6.15 11 0 0
rior temporal gyrus, CL: cerebellar lobule, ROI: region of interest, VERWM: verbal
Parietal Sup L 966 6.98 23 64 46
working memory, VISWM: visual working memory.
Insula L 427 6.95 33 24 2
Supp Motor Area L 1181 6.88 3 12 50
Supp Motor Area L 6.78 7 16 44
Cingulum Mid R 6.75 9 20 38 In the VISWM task, brain activations were obtained bilaterally in
Insular R 342 6.13 31 22 4 the inferior and middle frontal regions, posterior parietal regions,
Hippocampus R 166 5.51 25 28 6 occipital regions, and occipitotemporal regions. Activated brain
Hippocampus L 202 5.38 27 36 4
areas not visible from the rendered brain included the cingulum,
Angular R 214 5.27 33 58 46
insular, hippocampus, and thalamus.
Activation maxima are reported using a single subject MNI template brain [129],
P < 0.05 (FWE corrected) at cluster-level. Abbreviations: Inf: inferior; L: Left; Mid:
middle; Nvox : number of voxels in a contiguous cluster; Oper: opercularis; R: right; 3.2.2. Activations in SUIT space
Sup: superior; Supp: supplementary. Fig. 3 and Table 3 show cerebellar activation proles and loci of
Table 2
activation maxima during the working memory tasks, respectively.
xxx. Although the VERWM task appeared to recruit bilateral superior
cerebellar regions, cluster-level thresholding produced activation
Brain structures MNI coordinates (mm)
maxima only in the right hemisphere. Within this region, two
Nvox SPM{Z} X Y Z activation maxima were obtained; locus of the stronger maxima
Visual working memory lay anterior, inferior, and lateral to the weaker one. In the same
Fusiform L 17637 7.61 33 52 22 hemisphere, a separate activation maxima was obtained in lob-
Occipital Mid L 7.45 25 88 10 ule VIIB/VIII of the inferior posterior cerebellum. Of note, there
Fusiform R 7.38 37 46 22
was no signicant cerebellar lobule VIIB/VIII activation in the left
Supp Motor Area L 1482 7.48 1 16 46
Cingulum Mid R 6.75 9 24 32 hemisphere.
Insula R 464 6.87 37 22 6 In the VISWM task, robust activations were obtained bilater-
Precentral L 1699 6.32 45 6 32 ally in the superior posterior region of cerebellar lobule VI, which
Precentral L 6.21 35 4 62 became a contiguous cluster through the adjoining vermal activa-
Precentral L 5.59 31 6 54
Insular L 171 5.59 33 18 4
tion in the anterior cerebellum. Smaller clusters of voxels were also
Frontal Inf Oper R 781 5.59 53 16 38 obtained in the inferior posterior aspects of both cerebellar cortices.
Frontal Inf Oper R 5.37 49 10 28 The intensity and size of the activation maxima in left cerebellar
Frontal Inf Tri R 5.33 53 38 24 lobule VIIB was greater compared to that in the right hemisphere.
Hippocampus L 43 5.45 27 36 2
Frontal Mid R 417 5.39 37 2 64
Precentral R 5.36 33 2 50 3.2.3. AveLI
Parietal Sup R 135 5.34 11 78 50 Table 4 shows the laterality effect of brain activations during the
Thalamus R 32 5.28 23 30 2
working memory tasks. According to Seghier [113] and Matsuo et al.
Activation maxima are reported using a single subject MNI template brain [129], [75], AveLI values greater than 0.2 and lesser than 0.2 represent
P < 0.05 (FWE corrected) at cluster-level. Abbreviations: Inf: inferior; L: Left; Mid:
left and right lateralization of brain activations, respectively.
middle; Nvox : number of voxels in a contiguous cluster; Oper: opercularis; R: right;
Sup: superior; Supp: supplementary; Tri: triangularis.
A robust laterality effect was observed in the VERWM task. Acti-
vations in the IFG (0.32 0.17; Mean SD) and IPL (0.59 0.20)
Table 3 were lateralized to the left cortical hemisphere whereas those in
Cerebellar brain regions activated during the working memory tasks, high-
lobule VI (0.37 0.40), lobule VIIB (0.58 0.45) and lobule VIIIA
load > low-load.
(0.31 0.41) were lateralized to the right cerebellar hemisphere.
Brain structures MNI Coordinates (mm) Activations in cerebellar lobule Crus I was bilateral.
Lobule Nvox SPM{Z} X Y Z In contrast, in the VISWM task, AveLI values approximating
zero (0.040.12) indicated that cortical (inferior frontal gyrus,
Verbal working memory
Superior posterior cerebellum VI 961 R Inf 21 63 26 inferior temporal gyrus) and cerebellar (lobule VI/CrusI and lob-
Superior posterior cerebellum VI R 6.87 11 73 23 ule VIIB/VIIIA) brain regions were quasi- equitably activated across
Inferior posterior cerebellum VIIB/VIII R 6.44 25 70 51 hemispheres.
Visual working memory
Superior posterior cerebellum VI 930 L 7.13 35 59 23
4. Discussion
Superior posterior cerebellum VI R 7.12 28 70 19
Anterior cerebellum/Vermis IX 6.75 0 56 33
Inferior posterior cerebellum VIIB 22 L 5.70 25 73 51 The present study conrms earlier ndings supporting a lateral-
Inferior posterior cerebellum VIIB 9 R 4.65 25 73 51 ized cerebro-cerebellar (left neocortical-right cerebellar) network
Activation maxima are reported using the probabilistic cerebellar atlas [31], P < 0.05 of verbal working memory. In contrast, quantitative ndings
(FWE corrected) at cluster-level. Abbreviations: L: left; Nvox : number of voxels in a showed bilateral activations in the cerebral and cerebellar regions
contiguous cluster; R: right. during visual working memory. These results did not provide evi-
H.B.T. Ng et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 305 (2016) 164173 169

Fig. 3. Coronal section views of cerebellar activations during the working memory tasks. For display purposes, activation maps were thresholded at T = 4.7 [P < 0.05 (FWE
corrected) at cluster-level]. (a) During verbal working memory, bilateral superior cerebellar activations were obtained from slices y = 78 to y = 57. In the same y slices,
inferior cerebellar activations were obtained only in the right hemisphere. (b) During visual working memory, bilateral superior cerebellar activations were obtained from
slices y = 80 to y = 57. Bilateral inferior cerebellar activations were obtained from slices y = 78 to y = 61. The colour bar refers to activation intensity expressed in T values.
Lighter colour represents increased activation intensity.

dence to suggest a distinct right neocortical-left cerebellar network strates that were uniquely activated or brain regions that were
for visual working memory. activated more in extent and intensity in one task relative to the
The activations observed in the region representative of the other. In contrast, a within-task (high-load minus low-load con-
Brocas area and posterior aspect of the left parietal cortex during dition) parametric contrast reects neural underpinnings of the
verbal working memory are in concordance with previous nd- task that was performed. Previously, Thomason et al. [125] had
ings [17,18,96,21,105]. Specically, the left inferior parietal lobule employed laterality index measurement to determine hemispheric
(BA 40) has been linked to processes related to phonological stor- specialization of phonological loop processes and found left cere-
age. Although cluster maxima was obtained in the proximity of bral lateralization in prefrontal and parietal regions. In the present
the Brocas area (see Fig. 2, Table 1), the contrast images in the study, we compared high- and low-load verbal working memory
present study did not reveal any cluster maximum in the left infe- within-subjects and found not only left lateralization of the afore-
rior parietal lobule. However, based on laterality effect analysis mentioned neocortical brain regions but also right lateralization
that showed strong dominance of the left inferior parietal lobule of cerebellar substrates in lobule VI and lobule VIIB/VIII. To our
(see Table 4), it is likely that this brain region might be part of knowledge, this is the rst quantitative demonstration of a cerebro-
a larger contiguous node that was localized to the superior pari- cerebellar network in verbal working memory, conrming ndings
etal lobule (see Table 1). A number of previous investigations have of earlier studies that implicated right-sided lateralization of cere-
observed superior parietal activations during verbal working mem- bellar involvement in phonological loop routines [17,18,60,61,73].
ory [99,128,25,89,74]. While its exact function is unclear, the brain Studies of primates have shown that neural connections orig-
region is thought to be involved in the storage of verbal informa- inating from neocortical frontal regions terminate predominantly
tion [3], memory retrieval [15], and processes related to attentional along medial aspects of the pontine nucleus [134,111,112], which
demands [118]. In the current Sternberg paradigm, it is likely that in turn project to superior portions of the cerebellar cortex
the subjects iteratively attended to the alphabets on the screen including lobule VI and Crus I [47,80,81,32,59]. A distinct cir-
to subvocally rehearse the visual information for memory main- cuitry originating from the parietal and temporal regions has
tenance and storage. In general, our data are in agreement with also been identied to synapse along lateral portions of the
current models of verbal working memory that hypothesize the pontine nucleus [134,67,133,76,14], which in turn project to
contribution of the Brocas area to articulatory rehearsal and the inferior aspects of the paramedian lobule [(hemispheric lobule
posterior parietal region to phonological storage [28,17], processes VIIB); [12,13,43]]. Taking together these ndings and our cur-
of which sustain the phonological loop assumed to be fundamental rent data, they form a strong neuroanatomical and functional
to the perception and production of language [6,86,8]. basis for implicating a crossed coupling between left inferior
Hellige [53] and Banich [9] proposed that hemispheric spe- frontal region and right superior cerebellar region in articulatory
cialization of cognitive functions is innate in the human brain. A rehearsal and a separate crossed coupling between left inferior
number of fMRI studies have reported dominance of the left cere- parietal region and right inferior cerebellar region in phonological
bral hemisphere during verbal working memory tasks [16,58,44] store [17,18,27,96,108,107,3,28,118]. Future research employing
and right cerebral hemisphere during nonverbal working mem- effective connectivity analysis should provide insights into the inte-
ory tasks [124,72]. In these studies, hemispheric specialization was gration between functionally specialized brain areas.
determined by either contrasting the activation maps of working Of note, our ndings are not consistent with that of two recent
memory versus control tasks [16,124], spatial working memory neuroimaging studies that reported the absence of cerebellar hemi-
versus object working memory tasks [72], or verbal working mem- spheric lateralization in verbal working memory [49,126]. In the
ory versus nonverbal working memory tasks [72]. The results of earlier study that employed an n-back paradigm [49], subtrac-
these between-task contrasts informed us about the neural sub- tion analyses of 2-back minus 0-back yielded bilateral cerebellar
170 H.B.T. Ng et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 305 (2016) 164173

activation in lobule VI and lobule VIIIA/VIIB. It was argued that etal region postulated by Baddeley [8] to be involved in inner
the lack of cerebellar lateralization could be due to the nature of scribe processes. While mechanisms of these processes are not
the n-back task that emphasized manipulation of information and well understood, a parsimonious prediction would be that visual
executive control in excess of stimulus maintenance, thus super- imagery plays a part in keeping the stimuli in working memory.
seding processes of the phonological loop that might have been A number of neuroimaging studies have demonstrated precuneal
induced. Although reasonable, the assumption did not prevail when activations, a component brain region of the superior parietal lob-
a Sternberg paradigm emphasizing item encoding and mainte- ule, during visual mental imagery [63,37,42,41]. Lundstrom et al.
nance was used in a subsequent fMRI study [126]. We suggest that [69] further showed that these brain activities are related to accu-
the lack of cerebellar hemispheric lateralization might be due to rate item recognition and recall, coherent with the high accuracy
inequitable articulatory rehearsal between the load conditions. In scores in the current study.
that study, high-load condition comprised a matrix of six letters Apart from these brain regions, additional activations were
and low-load condition comprised a matrix of one letter and ve also obtained in the fusiform gyrus of the inferior temporal cor-
# symbols. In the latter condition, it is not reported whether the tex (see Table 2), as reported in a number of other investigations
subjects rehearsed subvocally only the letter or every item in the [38,83,82,100]. While its function is not as clear-cut as those of
matrix including the # symbols, or if they did rehearse at all dur- the phonological loop, there is evidence to show that the inferior
ing the maintenance phase of the task. In the present study, the temporal cortex is centrally involved in visual working memory,
number of items presented was identical across conditions and as lesion or cooling of this brain region signicantly impaired per-
all items were alphabets (see Fig. 1) and subjects were asked to formance of delayed matched to sample working memory tasks
continue subvocal rehearsal even during the maintenance phase. [54,39,90].
This, we reasoned has the effect of equating motoric processes Unlike the distinct laterality effect that was observed in the
underlying articulatory rehearsal for encoding and maintenance, verbal working memory task, we failed to obtain evidence for hemi-
without which subtraction analyses might elicit additional cere- spheric lateralization in visual working memory, as the contrast
bellar resources for motoric processes in the high-load condition. images revealed activations in bilateral frontal and inferior tempo-
Alternatively, the additional cerebellar activations might reect ral regions (see Fig. 2), and subcortically in bilateral superior and
greater motoric demands for rehearsing different alphabets under inferior cerebellar regions (see Fig. 3). The zero-centering laterality
the high-load condition or response-time variability between the indices conrmed that visual working memory engaged specialized
two conditions [114]. brain areas in both neocortical and cerebellar hemispheres (see
While brain regions associated with motoric and cognitive Table 4). Previous neuroimaging studies have demonstrated the
functions may be topographically located in the same cerebellar involvement of bilateral frontal and inferior temporal brain regions
lobules (i.e. VI and VIIb/VIII), meta-analytic data of Stoodley and in working memory of novel faces [52,24], and more recently
Schmahmann [120] suggests a dissociation of functional locality. with event-related fMRI, Ranganath et al. [103] showed that active
Accordingly, lobular VI and VIIb/VIII activations associated with maintenance of visual information is supported by activation of
motoric processes are located anterior (y coordinate between 60 object representations in inferior temporal cortices, and that mem-
to 70) to those that are associated with working memory (y ory retrieval is effected by top-down signals originating from the
coordinate between 70 to 80). Our data concur well with their frontal cortices. Therefore, lesions in and along the pathway con-
ndings in that two distinct regions of cerebellar lobule VI were necting these two brain regions would severely impair visual recall
activated during the verbal working memory task, i.e. one in the [33,46,48,127].
anterior aspect and another in the posterior aspect of the lobule Consistent with our nding, two recent fMRI studies reported,
(Table 3). Recently, Peterburs et al. [98] were able to attribute a among other cerebellar areas, bilateral activation of lobule VI and
motoric cerebellar network to oculomotor demands and a separate lobule VIIB during working memory of abstract object [49,126].
load effect cerebellar network to memory demands during stim- Of note, in the Thurling et al. [126] study, stronger lobule VI
ulus encoding. In the case of lobule VIIb/VIII, the activation was and lobule VIIB activations were obtained in the right cerebel-
localized to the posterior aspect of the brain structure. lar hemisphere. Through post- experiment feedback, the authors
Apart from the phonological loop, working memory models posited that the use of verbal strategies might have activated the
have also envisaged a separate system (visuo-spatial sketchpad) phonological loop, thereby reducing demand of the visuo-spatial
that enables the human brain to hold and manipulate non-verbal sketchpad. This may offer a plausible explanation for the ndings
information for deciphering semantic and geographical properties in our study. In a recent event-related fMRI study, Sobzak-Edmans
of objects in the environment [1,22]. As proposed by Baddeley et al. [119] reported that stored visual representations could pos-
and colleagues [7,5,68], the visuo-spatial sketchpad comprises two sibly involve maintenance mechanisms that employ verbal coding.
mechanisms: an inner scribe for image rehearsal and a visual cache Alternatively, the quasi-equitable cerebellar activations obtained
for storage, processes of which operate in tandem to support visual in lobule VI and VIIB might not be the result of phonological loop
working memory. In a review of data from lesion and neuroimag- recruitment but rather a reection that a distributed network is
ing studies, Baddeley [8] proposed candidate brain structures that needed to support processes of the visuo-spatial sketchpad. Cere-
might be involved in visuo-spatial working memory: for inner bellar activations obtained in the midline vermal regions could be
scribe, inferior frontal gyrus (BA 47), middle frontal gyrus, and a associated with online oculomotor control [121,78,64], as subjects
region overlapping the superior parietal lobule (BA 7) and ante- in the current study might have made saccadic eye movements
rior extrastriate occipital cortex (BA 19); for visual cache, inferior from one geometrical pattern to another during encoding.
aspect of the occipitotemporal region and the cuneus (BA 17). The Data from neurophysiological and behavioural experiments in
current activations obtained in the aforementioned brain areas (see monkeys have shown that object recognition involves a multistage
Table 2) support Baddeleys conjecture and concur with data from process leading from localized feature analysis in the primary visual
animal [4,54], human lesion [94,93,90] and neuroimaging [50,100] cortex, through a sequence of association areas, to more global
studies of visual working memory. object recognition in the inferior temporal cortex [70]. This might
Although bilateral parietal activations were observed during explain why an extensive recruitment of brain resources is needed
visual working memory (Fig. 2b), only a maximum in the right supe- to support processes of not only object recognition but also mem-
rior parietal lobule was revealed under the current thresholding ory retrieval. Alternatively, we could suppose that if subjects in the
(Table 2). Interestingly, this brain area lies near the occipitopari- present study had chosen to remember the letter array in the verbal
H.B.T. Ng et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 305 (2016) 164173 171

working memory task as a visual pattern, the same way visual stim- Consent
uli were maintained in visual working memory, it is possible that
an equally extensive bilateral network might have been activated. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants
However, this process is cost intensive and the brain knowing of a included in the study.
more efcient system would convert the graphemes to phonemes
before entering them into the phonological loop for encoding and References
maintenance, thus recruiting the left cerebro-right cerebellar net-
work. We speculate that the lack of an alternative cost-effective [1] J. Andrade, Working Memory in Context, Psychology Press, Hove, Sussex,
system in visual working memory might have resulted in the exten- 2001.
[2] C. Asanuma, W.T. Thach, E.G. Jones, Distribution of cerebellar terminations
sive recruitment of bilateral neocortical and cerebellar structures.
and their relation to other afferent terminations in the ventral lateral
The present study is not without limitations. First, similar to the thalamic region of the monkey, Brain Res. 286 (1983) 237265.
trial setup of Thurling et al.s [126] study, timing and item param- [3] E. Awh, J. Jonides, E.E. Smith, E.H. Schumacher, R.A. Koeppe, S. Katz,
Dissociation of storage and rehearsal in verbal working memory: evidence
eters were differentiated across tasks to match for performance
from positron emission tomography, Psychol. Sci. 7 (1996) 2531.
accuracy. Results from our pilot study showed that by increas- [4] J. Bachevalier, M. Mishkin, Visual recognition impairment follows
ing encoding and reducing maintenance timing, and reducing the ventromedial but not dorsolateral prefrontal lesions in monkeys, Behav.
number of items in the visual task, accuracy rate was compara- Brain Res. 20 (1986) 249261.
[5] A.D. Baddeley, Working Memory, Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK, 1986.
ble between the two tasks (t (9) = 1.53, P = .16). Of note, subjects in [6] A.D. Baddeley, S.E. Gathercole, C. Papagno, The phonological loop as a
our pilot study did not receive practice prior to the behavioural language learning device, Psychol. Rev. 105 (1998) 158183.
experiment. In contrast, subjects practised the tasks until they [7] A.D. Baddeley, G.J. Hitch, Working memory, in: G.A. Bower (Ed.), The
Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory,
attained eighty percent accuracy prior to fMRI scanning. We posit Academic, New York, 1974, pp. 4789.
that additional exposure to the stimuli might have aided, to a [8] A.D. Baddeley, Working memory: looking back and looking forward, Nat.
greater extent, performance of the verbal task compared to the Rev. Neurosci. 4 (2003) 829839.
[9] M.T. Banich, Neuropsychology: The Neural Bases of Mental Function,
visual task, biasing the interpretation that the observed laterality Houghton-Mifin., Boston, 1997.
effect was a consequence of task difculty. While that could be a [10] J.R. Binder, S.M. Rao, T.A. Hammeke, J.A. Frost, P.A. Bandettini, A.
plausible explanation, it is noteworthy to highlight that behavioural Jesmanowicz, J.S. Hyde, Lateralized human brain language systems
demonstrated by task subtraction functional magnetic resonance imaging,
confounds of disparate task performance have been reported in
Arch. Neurol. 52 (1995) 593601.
previous studies [91,45,19,126], implicating the difculty of match- [11] T.S. Braver, J.D. Cohen, L.E. Nystrom, J. Jonides, E.E. Smith, D.C. Noll, A
ing performance accuracy across different modality of working parametric study of prefrontal cortex involvement in human working
memory, Neuroimage 5 (1997) 4962.
memory.
[12] P. Brodal, The ponto-cerebellar projection in the rhesus monkey: an
Second, while the current analyses are sufcient for answer- experimental study with retrograde axonal transport of horseradish
ing questions about hemispheric lateralization in working memory, peroxide, Neuroscience 4 (1979) 193208.
they lack the temporal resolution for evaluating specic processes [13] P. Brodal, The cerebropontocerebellar pathway: salient features of its
organization, Exp. Brain Res. 6 (1982) 108133.
of working memory. Future research employing fast event-related [14] P. Brodal, J.G. Bjaalie, Salient anatomic features of the
fMRI will help us better understand underlying cerebro-cerebellar cortico-ponto-cerebellar pathway, Prog. Brain Res. 114 (1997) 227249.
circuitry of encoding and maintenance in human working memory. [15] J.H. Callicot, V.S. Mattay, A. Bertolino, K. Finn, R. Coppola, J.A. Frank, T.E.
Goldberg, D.R. Weinberger, Physiological characteristics of capacity
In summary, the results of the present study provide quan- constraints in working memory as revealed by functional MRI, Cereb. Cortex
titative evidence for an underlying cerebro-cerebellar circuitry 9 (1999) 2026.
in verbal working memory. As postulated in earlier studies [16] B.J. Casey, J.D. Cohen, P. Jezzard, R. Turner, D.C. Noll, R.J. Trainor, J. Giedd, D.
Kaysen, L. Hertz-Pannier, J.L. Rapaport, Activation of prefrontal cortex in
[18,60,61,73], the phonological loop recruited left-lateralized children during nonspatial working memory task with functional MRI,
language-related brain areas in the inferior frontal and posterior Neuroimage 2 (1995) 221229.
parietal regions, and right-lateralized cerebellar areas in lobular VI [17] S.H.A. Chen, J.E. Desmond, Temporal dynamics of cerebro-cerebellar
network recruitment during a cognitive task, Neuropsychologia 43 (2005)
and VIIB/VIII regions. In contrast, a lack of hemispheric dominance
12271237.
in visual working memory suggests that a distributed network of [18] S.H.A. Chen, J.E. Desmond, Cerebrocerebellar networks during articulatory
brain areas in the cerebral and cerebellar hemispheres is needed to rehearsal and verbal working memory tasks, Neuroimage 24 (2005)
332338.
sustain this modality of working memory.
[19] Y.N. Chen, S. Mitra, Distinctions between spatial and verbal working
memory: a study using event-related potential, Chang Gung Med. J. 32
(2009) 380389.
Conict of interest [20] J.D. Cohen, S.D. Forman, T.S. Braver, B.J. Casey, D. Servan-Schreiber, D.C. Noll,
Activation of the prefrontal cortex in a nonspatial working memory task
with function MRI, Hum. Brain Mapp. 1 (1994) 293304.
All the authors in the manuscript declare no conict of interest.
[21] J.D. Cohen, W.M. Perlstein, T.S. Braver, L.E. Nystrom, D.C. Noll, J. Jonides, E.E.
Smith, Temporal dynamics of brain activation during working memory task,
Nature 386 (1997) 604608.
Funding [22] A.R.A. Conway, C. Jarrold, M.J. Kane, A. Miyake, J.N. Towse, Variation in
Working Memory, Oxford University Press, New York, 2007.
[23] S.M. Courtney, L.G. Ungerleider, K. Keil, J.V. Haxby, Object and spatial visual
This work was supported by a Tier 2 Academic Research Fund working memory activate separate neural systems in human cortex, Cereb.
(MOE2011-T2-1-031) that was approved by the Ministry of Educa- Cortex 6 (1996) 3949.
tion, Singapore. [24] S.M. Courtney, L.G. Ungerleider, K. Keil, J.V. Haxby, Transient and sustained
activity in a distributed neural system for human working memory, Nature
386 (1997) 608611.
[25] S. Crottaz- Herbette, R.T. Anagnoson, V. Menon, Modality effects in verbal
Ethical approval working memory: differential prefrontal and parietal responses to auditory
and visual stimuli, Neuroimage 21 (2004) 340351.
All procedures performed in studies involving human partici- [26] C.A. Cuenod, S.Y. Bookheimer, L. Hertz-Pannier, T.A. Zefro, W.H. Theodore,
D. Le Bihan, Functional MRI during word generation, using conventional
pants were in accordance with the ethical standards of Nanyang equipment: a potential tool for language localization in the clinical
Technological University Institutional Review Board and National environment, Neurology 45 (1995) 18211827.
Healthcare Group (NHG) Domain Specic Review Board (DSRB)(A), [27] J.E. Desmond, J.A. Fiez, Neuroimaging studies of the cerebellum: language,
learning and memory, Trends Cogn. Sci. 2 (1998) 355362.
as well as, with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amend-
[28] J.E. Desmond, J.D. Gabrieli, A.D. Wagner, B.L. Ginier, G.H. Glover, Lobular
ments or comparable ethical standards. patterns of cerebellar activation in verbal working- memory and
172 H.B.T. Ng et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 305 (2016) 164173

nger-tapping tasks as revealed by functional MRI, J. Neurosci. 17 (1997) impaired spatial working memory in schizophrenia, Neuropsychologia 49
96759685. (2011) 28362847.
[29] J.E. Desmond, J.M. Sum, A.D. Wagner, J.B. Demb, P.K. Shear, G.H. Glover, J.D.E. [58] W.M. Kelley, F.M. Miezin, K.B. McDermott, R.L. Buckner, M.E. Raichle, N.J.
Gabrieli, M.J. Morrell, Functional MRI measurement of language Cohen, J.M. Ollinger, E. Akbudak, T.E. Conturo, A.Z. Snyder, S.E. Petersen,
lateralization in Wada- tested patients, Brain 118 (1995) 14111419. Hemispheric specialization in human dorsal frontal cortex and medial
[30] J. Diedrichsen, A spatially unbiased atlas template of the human cerebellum, temporal lobe for verbal and nonverbal memory encoding, Neuron 20
Neuroimage 33 (2006) 127138. (1998) 927936.
[31] J. Diedrichsen, J.H. Balsters, J. Flavell, E. Cussans, N. Ramnani, A probabilistic [59] R.M. Kelly, P.L. Strick, Cerebellar loops with motor cortex and prefrontal
atlas of the human cerebellum, Neuroimage 46 (1) (2009) 3946. cortex of a nonhuman primate, J. Neurosci. 23 (2003) 84328444.
[32] R.P. Dum, P.L. Strick, An unfolded map of the cerebellar dentate nucleus and [60] M.P. Kirschen, S.H. Chen, P. Schraedley- Desmond, J.E. Desmond, Load- and
its projection to the cerebral cortex, J. Neurophysiol. 89 (2003) 634639. practice-dependent increases in cerebro-cerebellar activation in verbal
[33] M.J. Eacott, D. Gaffon, Inferotemporal- frontal disconnection: the uncinate working memory, Neuroimage 24 (2005) 462472.
fascicle and visual associative learning in monkeys, Eur. J. Neurosci. 4 (1992) [61] M.P. Kirschen, S.H.A. Chen, J.E. Desmond, Modality specic
13201332. cerebro-cerebellar activations in verbal working memory: an fMRI study,
[34] M.J. Farah, Visual Agnosia: Disorders of Object Recognition and What They Behav. Neurol. 23 (2010) 5163.
Tell Us About Normal Vision, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1990. [62] H. Koshino, R.K. Kana, T.A. Keller, V.L. Cherkassky, N.J. Minshew, M.A. Just,
[35] J.A. Fiez, M.E. Raichle, Linguistic processing, in: J. Schmahmann (Ed.), The fMRI investigation of working memory for faces in autism: visual coding and
Cerebellum and Cognition International Review of Neurobiology, vol. 41, underconnectivity with frontal areas, Cereb. Cortex 18 (2008) 289300.
Academic Press, San Diego, CA, 1997, pp. 233254. [63] S.M. Kosslyn, N.M. Alpert, W.L. Thompson, V. Maljkovic, S.B. Weise, C.F.
[36] J.A. Fiez, E.A. Raife, D.A. Balota, J.P. Schwarz, M.E. Raichle, S.E. Petersen, A Chabris, S.E. Hamilton, S.L. Rauch, F.S. Buonanno, Visual mental imagery
positron emission tomography study of the short-term maintenance of activates topographically organized visual cortex: PET investigations, J.
verbal information, J. Neurosci. 16 (1996) 808822. Cognitive Neurosci. 5 (1993) 263287.
[37] P.C. Fletcher, C.D. Frith, S.C. Baker, T. Challice, R.S.J. Frackowiak, R.J. Dolan, [64] R.J. Krauzlis, The control of voluntary eye movements: new perspectives,
The minds eye-precuneus activation in memory-related imagery, Neuroscientist 11 (2005) 124137.
Neuroimage 2 (1995) 195200. [67] G.R. Leichnetz, D.J. Smith, R.F. Spencer, Cortical projections to the
[38] J.M. Fuster, J.P. Jervey, Neuronal ring in the inferotemporal cortex of the paramedian tegmental and basilar pons in the monkey, J. Comp. Neurol. 228
monkey in a visual memory task, J. Neurosci. 2 (1982) 361375. (1984) 388408.
[39] D. Gaffan, E.A. Murray, Monkeys (Macaca fascicularis) with rhinal cortex [68] R.H. Logie, Visuo-spatial Working Memory, LEA Publishers, Hove, Sussex,
ablations succeed in object discrimination learning despite 24-hr intertrial 1995.
intervals and fail at matching to samples despite double sample [69] B.N. Lundstrom, M. Ingvar, K.M. Petersson, The role of precuneus and left
presentations, Behav. Neurosci. 106 (1992) 3038. inferior frontal cortex during source memory episodic retrieval, Neuroimage
[40] W.D. Gaillard, S.Y. Bookheimer, L. Hertz- Pannier, T.A. Blaxton, The 27 (2005) 824834.
noninvasive identication of language function. Neuroimaging and rapid [70] R. Malach, J.B. Reppas, R.R. Benson, K.K. Kwong, H. Jiang, W.A. Kennedy, P.J.
transcranial magnetic stimulation, Neurosurg. Clin. N. Am. 8 (1997) Ledden, T.J. Brady, B.R. Rosen, R.B.H. Tootell, Object-related activity revealed
321335. by functional magnetic resonance imaging in human occipital cortex, Proc.
[41] G. Ganis, W.L. Thompson, S.M. Kosslyn, Brain areas underlying visual mental Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 92 (1995) 81358139.
imagery and visual perception: an fMRI study, Cognitive Brain Res. 20 [71] J.A. Maldjian, P.J. Laurienti, J.B. Burdette, R.A. Kraft, An automated method
(2004) 226241. for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fMRI
[42] O. Ghaem, E. Mellet, F. Crivello, N. Tzourio, B. Mazoyer, A. Berthoz, M. Denis, data sets, Neuroimage 19 (2003) 12331239.
Mental navigation along memorized routes activates the hippocampus, [72] D.S. Manoach, M.S. Cain, M.G. Vangel, A. Khurana, D.C. Goff, R. Stickgold, A
precuneus, and insula, Neuroreport 8 (1997) 739744. failure of sleep-dependent procedural learning in chronic, medicated
[43] M. Glickstein, N. Gerrits, I. Kralj- Hans, B. Mercier, J. Stein, J. Voogd, Visual schizophrenia, Biol. Psychiatry 56 (2004) 951956.
pontocerebellar projections in the macaque, J. Comp. Neurol. 349 (1994) [73] C.L. Marvel, J.E. Desmond, The contributions of cerebro-cerebellar circuitry
5172. to executive verbal working memory, Cortex 46 (2010) 880895.
[44] A.J. Golby, R.A. Poldrack, J.B. Brewer, D. Spencer, J.E. Desmond, A.P. Aron, [74] C.L. Marvel, J.E. Desmond, From storage to manipulation: how the neural
J.D.E. Gabrieli, Material-specic lateralization in the medial temporal lobe correlates of verbal working memory reect varying demands on inner
and prefrontal cortex during memory encoding, Brain 124 (2001) speech, Brain Lang. 120 (2012) 4251.
18411854. [75] K. Matsuo, S.H.A. Chen, W.Y.I. Tseng, AveLI: a robust lateralization index in
[45] O. Gruber, D. Yves von Cramon, The functional neuroanatomy of human functional magnetic resonance imaging using unbiased threshold-free
working memory revisited. Evidence from 3-T fMRI studies using classical computation, J. Neurosci. Methods 205 (2012) 119129.
domain-specic interference tasks, Neuroimage 19 (2003) 797809. [76] J.G. May, R.A. Andersen, Different patterns of corticopontine projections
[46] S.A. Gutnikov, Y.Y. Ma, D. Gaffan, Temporo-frontal disconnection impairs from separate cortical elds within the inferior parietal lobule and dorsal
visualvisual paired association learning but not congural learning in prelunate gyrus of the macaque, Exp. Brain Res. 63 (1986) 265278.
Macaque monkeys, Eur. J. Neurosci. 9 (1997) 15241529. [77] G. McCarthy, A.M. Blamire, A. Puce, A.C. Nobre, G. Bloch, F. Hyder, P.
[47] D.E. Haines, E. Dietrichs, G.A. Mihailoff, E.F. McDonald, Cerebellar- Goldman- Rakic, R.G. Shulman, Functional magnetic resonance imaging of
hypothalamic axis: basis circuits and clinical observations, Int. Rev. human prefrontal cortex activation during a spatial working memory task,
Neurobiol. 41 (1997) 83107. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 91 (1994) 86908694.
[48] I. Hasegawa, T. Fukushima, T. Ihara, Y. Miyashita, Callosal window between [78] R.C. Miall, H. Imamizu, S. Miyauchi, Activation of the cerebellum in
prefrontal cortices: cognitive interaction to retrieve long-term memory, co-ordinated eye and Hand tracking movements: an fMRI study, Exp. Brain
Science 281 (1998) 814818. Res. 135 (2000) 2233.
[49] H. Hautzel, F.M. Mottaghy, K. Specht, H.-W. Muller, B.J. Krause, Evidence of a [79] F.A. Middleton, P.L. Strick, Cerebellar output channels, Int. Rev. Neurobiol.
modality-dependent role of the cerebellum in working memory? An fMRI (1997) 41614182.
study comparing verbal and abstract n-back tasks, Neuroimage 47 (2009) [80] F.A. Middleton, P.L. Strick, Dentate output channels: motor and cognitive
32082. components, Prog. Brain Res. 114 (1997) 553566.
[50] J.V. Haxby, L. Petit, L.G. Ungerleider, S.M. Courtney, Distinguishing the [81] F.A. Middleton, P.L. Strick, Cerebellar projections to the prefrontal cortex of
functional roles of multiple regions in distributed neural systems for visual the primate, J. Neurosci. 21 (2001) 700712.
working memory, Neuroimage 11 (2000) 380391. [82] E.K. Miller, C.A. Erickson, R. Desimone, Neural mechanisms of visual working
[51] J.V. Haxby, L.G. Ungerleider, B. Horwitz, J.M. Maisog, S.I. Rapoport, C.L. memory in prefrontal cortex of the Macaque, J. Neurosci. 16 (1996)
Grady, Face encoding and recognition in the human brain, Proc. Natl. Acad. 51545167.
Sci. U. S. A. 93 (1996) 922927. [83] E.K. Miller, L. Li, R. Desimone, A neural mechanisms for working and
[52] J.V. Haxby, L.G. Ungerleider, B. Horwitz, S.I. Rapoport, C.L. Grady, recognition memory in inferior temporal cortex, Science 254 (1991)
Hemispheric differences in neural system for face working memory: a 13771379.
PET-rCBF study, Hum. Brain Mapp. 3 (1995) 6882. [84] B. Milner, Visual recognition and recall after right temporal-lobe excision in
[53] J.B. Hellige, Hemispheric Asymmetry: Whats Right and Whats Left, Harvard man, Epilepsy Behav. 4 (2003) 799812.
University Press, Cambridge, MA, 1993. [85] M. Mishkin, F. Manning, Non-spatial memory after selective prefrontal
[54] J.A. Horel, D.E. Pytko-Joiner, M.L. Voytko, K. Salsbury, The performance of lesions in monkeys, Brain Res. 143 (1978) 313323.
visual tasks while segments of the inferotemporal cortex are suppressed by [86] A. Miyake, P. Shah, Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active
cold, Behav. Brain Res. 23 (1987) 2942. Maintenance and Executive Control, University Press, New York:
[55] J. Jonides, E.H. Schumacher, E.E. Smith, R.A. Koeppe, E. Awh, P.A. Cambridge, 1999.
Reuter-Lorenz, C. Marshuetz, C.R. Willis, The role of the parietal cortex in [87] F.M. Mottaghy, Interfering with working memory in humans, Neuroscience
verbal working memory, J. Neurosci. 18 (1998) 50265034. 139 (2006) 8590.
[56] J. Jonides, E.H. Schumacher, E.E. Smith, E.J. Lauber, E. Awh, S. Minoshima, [88] N.G. Muller, L. Machado, R.T. Knight, Contributions of subregions of the
R.A. Koeppe, Verbal working memory load affects regional brain activation prefrontal cortex to working memory: evidence from brain lesions in
as measured by PET, J. Cognitive Neurosci. 9 (1997) 462475. humans, J. Cognitive Neurosci. 14 (2002) 673686.
[57] S.S. Kang, S.R. Sponheim, M.V. Chafee, A.W. MacDonald III, Disrupted [89] N.S. Narayanan, V. Prabhakaran, S.A. Bunge, K. Christoff, E.M. Fine, J.D.E.
functional connectivity for controlled visual processing as a basis for Gabrieli, The role of the prefrontal cortex in the maintenance of verbal
H.B.T. Ng et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 305 (2016) 164173 173

working memory: an event-related fMRI analysis, Neuropsychology 19 [114] D.J. Simmonds, S.G. Fotedar, S.J. Suskauer, J.J. Pekar, M.B. Denckla, S.H.
(2005) 223232. Mostofsky, Functional brain correlates of response time variability in
[90] F. Newcombe, G. Ratcliff, H. Damasio, Dissociable visual and spatial children, Neuropsychologia 45 (2007) 21472157.
impairments following right posterior cerebral lesions: clinical, [115] E.E. Smith, J. Jonides, M. Gazzaniga, Working memory in humans:
neuropsychological and anatomical evidence, Neuropsychologia 25 (1987) neuropsychological evidence, in: The Cognitive Neurosciences, MIT Press,
149161. Cambridge, MA, 1995, pp. 10091020.
[91] L.E. Nystrom, T.S. Braver, F.W. Sabb, M.R. Delgado, D.C. Noll, J.D. Cohen, [116] E.E. Smith, J. Jonides, Neuroimaging analyses of human working memory,
Working memory for letter, shapes, and locations: fMRI evidence against Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 95 (1998) 1206112068.
stimulus-based regional organization in human prefrontal cortex, [117] E.E. Smith, J. Jonides, R.A. Koeppe, E. Awh, E.H. Schumacher, S. Minoshima,
Neuroimage 11 (1999) 424446. Spatial vs. object working memory: PET investigations, J. Cognitive
[92] R.C. Oldeld, The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh Neurosci. 7 (1995) 337356.
inventory, Neuropsychologia 9 (1971) 97113. [118] E.E. Smith, J. Jonides, C. Marshuetz, R.A. Koeppe, Components of verbal
[93] A.M. Owen, R.G. Morris, B.J. Sahakian, C.E. Polkey, T.W. Robbins, Double working memory: evidence from neuroimaging, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A.
dissociations of memory and executive functions in working memory tasks 95 (1998) 876882.
following frontal lobe excisions, temporal lobe excisions or [119] M. Sobczak- Edmans, T.H.B. Ng, Y.C. Chan, E. Chew, K.H. Chuang, S.H.A. Chen,
amygdalo-hippocampectomy in man, Brain 119 (1996) 15971615. Temporal dynamics of visual working memory, Neuroimage 124 (2016)
[94] A.M. Owen, B.J. Sahakian, J. Semple, C.E. Polkey, T.W. Robbins, Visuo-spatial 10211030.
short-term recognition memory and learning after temporal lobe excisions, [120] C.J. Stoodley, J.D. Schmahmann, Functional topography in the human
frontal lobe excisions or amygdalo-hippocampectomy in man, cerebellum: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies, Neuroimage 44
Neuropsychologia 33 (1995) 124. (2009) 489501.
[95] R. Passingham, Delayed matching after selective prefrontal lesions in [121] M. Takagi, D.S. Zee, R.J. Tamargo, Effects of lesions of the oculomotor vermis
monkeys, Brain Res. 92 (1975) 89102. on eye movement in primate: saccades, J. Neurophysiol. 80 (1998)
[96] E. Paulesu, C.D. Frith, R.S.J. Frackowiak, The neural correlates of the verbal 19111931.
component of working memory, Nature 362 (1993) 342345. [123] W.T. Thach, E.G. Jones, The cerebellar dentatothalamic connection: terminal
[97] L. Pessoa, E. Gutierrez, P.A. Bandettini, L.G. Ungerleider, Neural correlates of eld, lamellae, rods and somatotopy, Brain Res. 169 (1979) 168172.
visual working memory: fMRI amplitude predicts task performance, Neuron [124] K.M. Thomas, S.W. King, P.L. Franzen, T.F. Welsh, A.L. Berkowitz, D.C. Noll, V.
35 (2002) 975987. Birmaher, B.J. Casey, A developmental functional MRI study of spatial
[98] J. Peterburs, D.T. Cheng, J.E. Desmond, The association between eye working memory, Neuroimage 10 (1999) 327338.
movements and cerebellar activation in a verbal working memory task, [125] M.E. Thomason, E. Race, B. Burrows, S. Whiteld-Gabrieli, G.H. Glover, J.D.E.
Cereb. Cortex (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhv187. Gabrieli, Development of spatial and verbal working memory capacity in the
[99] M.E. Petrides, B. Alivisatos, E. Meyer, A.C. Evans, Functional activation of the human brain, J. Cognitive Neurosci. 21 (2009) 316332.
human frontal cortex during the performance of verbal working memory, [126] M. Thurling, H. Hautzel, M. Kuper, M.R. Stefanescu, S. Maderwald, M.E. Ladd,
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 90 (1993) 878882. D. Timmann, Involvement of the cerebellar cortex and nuclei in verbal and
[100] B.R. Postle, C.E. Stern, B.R. Rosen, S. Corkin, AN fMRI investigation of cortical visuo-spatial working memory: a 7 T fMRI study, Neuroimage 62 (2012)
contributions to spatial and nonspatial visual working memory, 15371550.
Neuroimage 11 (2000) 409423. [127] H. Tomita, M. Ohbayashi, K. Nkahara, I. Hasegawa, Y. Miyashita, Top-down
[101] G. Rainer, W.F. Asaad, E.K. Miller, Memory elds of neurons in the primate signal from prefrontal cortex in executive control of memory retrieval,
prefrontal cortex, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 95 (1998) 1500815013. Nature 401 (1999) 699703.
[102] N. Ramnani, The primate cortico-cerebellar system: anatomy and function, [128] T. Tsukiura, T. Fujii, T. Takahashi, R.T. Xiao, M. Inase, T. Iijima, A. Yamadori, J.
Nat. Rev. 7 (2006) 511522. Okuda, Neuroanatomical discrimination between manipulating and
[103] C. Ranganath, M.X. Cohen, C. Dam, M. DEsposito, Inferior temporal, maintaining processes involved in verbal working memory: a functional
prefrontal, and hippocampal contributions to visual working memory MRI study, Cognitive Brain Res. 11 (2001) 1321.
maintenance and associative memory retrieval, J. Neurosci. 24 (2004) [129] N. Tzourio- Mazoyer, B. Landeau, D. Papathanassiou, F. Crivello, O. Etard, N.
39173925. Delcroix, B. Mazoyer, M. Joliot, Automated anatomical labeling of activations
[104] C. Ranganath, M. DEsposito, Directing the minds eye: prefrontal, inferior in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI
and medial temporal mechanisms for visual working memory, Curr. Opin. single-subject brain, Neuroimage 15 (2002) 273289.
Neurobiol. 15 (2005) 175182. [130] L.G. Ungerleider, S.M. Courtney, J.V. Haxby, A neural system for human
[105] S.M. Ravizza, M.R. Delgado, J.M. Chein, J.T. Becker, J.A. Fiez, Functional visual working memory, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 95 (1998) 883890.
dissociations within the inferior parietal cortex in verbal working memory, [131] M. van Asselen, R.P.C. Kessels, S.F.W. Neggers, L.J. Kappelle, C.J.M. Frijns, A.
Neuroimage 22 (2004) 562573. Postma, Brain areas involved in spatial working memory, Neuropsychologia
[106] E.T. Rolls, Functions of the primate temporal lobe cortical visual areas in 44 (2006) 11851194.
invariant visual object and face recognition, Neuron 27 (2000) 205218. [132] H. Walter, V. Bretschneider, G. Gron, B. Zurowski, A.P. Wunderlich, R.
[107] B. Rypma, V. Prabhakaran, J.E. Desmond, G.H. Glover, J.D. Gabrieli, Tomczak, M. Spitzer, Evidence for quantitative domain dominance for
Load-dependent roles of frontal brain regions in the maintenance of verbal and spatial working memory in frontal and parietal cortex, Cortex 39
working memory, Neuroimage 9 (1999) 216226. (2003) 897911.
[108] E. Salmon, M. Van der Linden, F. Collette, G. Delore, P. Maquet, C. [133] J.T. Weber, C.T. Yin, Subcortical projections of the inferior parietal cortex
Degueldre, A. Luxen, G. Franck, Regional brain activity during working (area 7) in the stump-tailed monkey, J. Comp. Neurol. 224 (1984) 206230.
memory tasks, Brain 119 (1996) 16171625. [134] R. Wiesendanger, M. Wiesendanger, D.G. Ruegg, An anatomical
[109] S.P. Scalaidhe, F.A.W. Wilson, P.S. Goldman- Rakic, Face-selective neurons investigation of the corticopontine projection in the primate
during passive viewing and working memory performance Rhesus (Macaca-fascicularis and Saimiri sciureus)-II. The projection from frontal
monkeys: evidence for intrinsic specialization of neuronal coding, Cereb. and parietal association areas, Neuroscience 4 (1979) 747766.
Cortex 9 (1999) 459475. [135] F.A.W. Wilson, S.P. Scalaidhe, P.S. Goldman- Rakic, Dissociation of object and
[111] J.D. Schmahmann, D.N. Pandya, Anatomic organization of the basilar pontine spatial processing domains in primate prefrontal cortex, Science 260 (1993)
projections from prefrontal cortices in rhesus monkey, J. Neurosci. 17 (1997) 19551958.
438458. [136] K. Yamanaka, B. Yamagata, H. Tomioka, S. Kawasaki, M. Mimura,
[112] J.D. Schmahmann, D.N. Pandya, The cerebrocerebellar system, Int. Rev. Transcranial magnetic stimulation of the parietal cortex facilitates spatial
Neurobiol. 41 (1997) 3160. working memory: near-infrared spectroscopy study, Cerebal. Cortex 20
[113] M.L. Seghier, Laterality index in functional MRI: methodological issues, (2010) 10371045.
Magn. Reson. Imaging 26 (2008) 594601.

Вам также может понравиться