Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 8

REGULAR ARTICLES

Frontal Systems
Dysfunction in Children
With Attention-Deficit/
Hyperactivity Disorder
and Learning Disabilities
J. Wayne Lazar, Ph.D.
Yitzchak Frank, M.D.

Tests of attention, inhibition, working memory,


motor learning, and problem solvingfunctions T he concept of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disor-
der (ADHD) has evolved over the last 30 years.
DSM-II1 defined a disorder called hyperkinetic reaction
associated with the brains frontal systemswere of childhood (or adolescence). DSM-III2 described the
administered to 26 children with attention-deficit/ disorder as a developmentally inappropriate degree of
hyperactivity disorder and learning disabilities inattention, impulsivity, and hyperactivity; and DSM-
(ADHD`LD), 22 children with LD only, and 10 IV3 described it in terms of inattention and/or
with ADHD only. Percentages of abnormal test hyperactivity-impulsivity. Theories on the pathophys-
results and significantly impaired scores were iology of the disorder include the suggestion that it is
caused by malfunctioning of the brain dopaminergic
higher for the two groups with LD than for the system.4
ADHD-only group. The groups differed signifi- Symptoms of children with ADHD include hyperki-
cantly on some tests of attention-inhibition- nesis and distractibility, variability of task performance,
cueing, working memory, and problem solving, disorganization, an inability to plan or follow through
with the ADHD`LD and LD groups performing on a plan, an inability to shift set and reprogram activ-
worse than the ADHD-only group. Abnormalities ities when needed, and deficient rule-governing behav-
ior. These symptoms have been associated with abnor-
of frontal systems tests are not exclusive ADHD malities of the brains frontal lobe systems, particularly
characteristics and are also present in LD chil- the executive systems.5,6 Because there has not been a
dren, implying a strong connection between cen- definitive anatomic localization of all these symptoms
ters of processing and centers of executive to the frontal lobes, we will use the term frontal systems,
functions. rather than frontal lobe, functions.
(The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical The frontal lobes are part of a broad network with
Neurosciences 1998; 10:160167) connections to almost all parts of the nervous system.
Therefore, possibly all cognitive systems are sensitive to
frontal lobe pathology. The involvement is in terms of

Received April 28, 1997; revised July 21, 1997; accepted July 24, 1997.
From the Divisions of Neuropsychology and Pediatric Neurology, De-
partment of Neurology, North Shore University Hospital, New York
University Medical School, Manhassett, New York. Address corre-
spondence to Dr. Frank, North Shore University Hospital, Department
of Neurology, 300 Community Drive, Manhassett, NY 11030.
Copyright q 1998 American Psychiatric Press, Inc.

160 VOLUME 10 NUMBER 2 SPRING 1998


LAZAR AND FRANK

executive controlfor instance, the ability to explore, to and no major psychiatric abnormalities (such as conduct
monitor and shift the direction of attention, to initiate disorder or depression). Informed consent was obtained
and direct language, to organize methods of memori- from parents.
zation, to temporarily discriminate items in memory, ADHD was clinically diagnosed by strict DSM-III-R
and to inhibit interference during recall.7,8 criteria,16 based on behavioral questionnaires (described
Pontius9 was one of the first to note the essential below) completed by parents and teachers and on par-
analogy between frontal lobe dysfunction and Minimal ent interviews. The diagnosis was made by a team of
Brain Dysfunction. Mattes10 presented evidence that hy- experienced clinicians including a child neurologist
perkinesis was linked to a frontal systems dysfunction. (Y. F.), a neuropsychologist (J. W. L.), and a child psy-
Recently, Barkley11 emphasized the importance of defi- chiatrist.
ciency in rule-governing behavior, excessive task per- Criteria for a diagnosis of LD were reading, mathe-
formance variability, and disorganization in ADHD. matics, or written expression achievement abnormalities
Although laboratory measures of impulsiveness and at or below the 16th percentile, as determined by na-
response inhibition and other frontal systems func- tional standardized or special education testing.
tions differentiate children with ADHD from normal Thirty-six children were diagnosed with ADHD; 26 of
children, such measures do not consistently differentiate these children had LD. Twenty-six children did not have
between ADHD and other clinical groups.12,13 It is there- ADHD; of these, 22 had LD. Four children could not be
fore necessary to include clinical control groups in stud- classified as ADHD or LD and were not included in
ies of frontal systems dysfunction in ADHD. A signifi- the study. The three study groups were, therefore,
cant deficiency of most such studies dealing with the ADHD`LD (group 1), LD alone (group 2), and ADHD
role of frontal systems dysfunction in ADHD is the lack alone (group 3).
of a control group of children with learning disabilities
(LD). In a review of studies of frontal systems dysfunc- Measures: Behavioral, Cognitive, and Frontal Systems
tion in ADHD by Barkley et al., only 3 of the 22 studies Parents completed the Child Behavior Checklist
used children with achievement difficulties or LD as (CBCL)17 and the Conners Parents Questionnaire.18 The
controls.14 Learning disability is a common comorbidity Teacher Response Form (TRF)19 and the Conners Teach-
of ADHD.15 Children with LD commonly have cognitive ers Questionnaire18 were completed by the teacher or
abnormalities (language, visual, spatial). The suggested guidance counselor who was best acquainted with the
association between frontal systems dysfunction and child.
ADHD may, in fact, be related to LD and its cognitive A neuropsychological battery of tests is routinely
abnormalities and not specifically to ADHD. used in our neurobehavioral program for children with
The purpose of this study is to examine frontal sys- school-related problems. It includes the Raven Progres-
tems dysfunctions in ADHD children and to assess sive Coloured20 or Standard Matrices test,21 the Token
whether such dysfunctions are specific to ADHD or are Test for Children,22 the Boston Naming Test,23 the
equally present in children with learning disabilities. We CELF-R Word Structure subtest,24 the WRAML Story
used tests for frontal systems functions that are appro- Memory subtest,25 the Stanford-Binet-IV Sentence Mem-
priate for use in children, and grouped them into four ory subtest,26 and the Visual-Motor Integration Test.27 It
sets: 1) attention, inhibition, and response after cueing; also includes the Stanford-Binet-IV Bead Memory sub-
2) working memory and the ability to maintain a se- test26 and the Selective Reminding Learning Test,28
quence; 3) problem solving and the ability to establish, which can be considered frontal systems measures by
maintain, and change set and to monitor personal be- the criteria discussed below and are included in the
havior; and 4) motor and verbal learning. frontal systems measures for the present study.
An average of the results of the first four measures
listed above was calculated to provide a general mea-
METHODS sure of nonverbal reasoning and language skills, which
we have called General.
Subjects Tests reflecting frontal systems functions that are ap-
A frontal systems test battery was administered to 81 propriate for use in children were selected through a
consecutive patients evaluated in our neurobehavioral review of the literature and were grouped into four do-
clinic at North Shore University Hospital. Sixty-two pa- mains: Attention-Inhibition-Cueing, Working Memory,
tients met the inclusionary criteria: age 6 to 13 years, Problem Solving, and Learning.
average intelligence, no major neurological abnormali- Tests related to attention and inhibition5 and response
ties (such as Tourettes syndrome or a seizure disorder), after cueing29 are the Gordon Diagnostic System sub-

JOURNAL OF NEUROPSYCHIATRY 161


FRONTAL DYSFUNCTION IN ADHD/LD CHILDREN

tests: the Delay subtest (efficiency, number correct, and ploratory nature of the study and the correlated nature
total responses), the Vigilance subtest (number correct of the measures.40
and commission errors), and the Distractibility subtest
(number correct and commission errors).30 Other tests
related to inhibition only are the Rampart drawing of RESULTS
the Dementia Rating Scale31 with specifically developed
scoring rules for errors and the Matching Familiar Fig- There were no significant differences of age, gender, or
ures Test with number correct and error measures.32 Full Scale, Verbal, or Performance IQ among the exper-
Working memory and the ability to maintain a se- imental groups (Table 1).
quence are tested by the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Table 2, Table 3, and Table 4 show z-score means and
ChildrenRevised (WISC-R) or WISC-III Digit Span sub- standard deviations of frontal systems tests grouped un-
test33,34 with separate analyses of digits forward and der Attention-Inhibition-Cueing, Working Memory, and
digits backward;35 the Illinois Test of Psycholinguistic Problem Solving and Learning, respectively. The mean
Abilities (ITPA) Visual Sequential Memory subtest;36 the z-scores for many measures of Attention-Inhibition-
Stanford-Binet-IV Bead Memory subtest;26 the Kaufman Cueing and of Problem Solving are 1 or less.
Assessment Battery for Children (KABC) Hand Move- The number of tests with a mean z-score of 1 or less
ment subtest37 used on dominant and nondominant is 10 (45%) of the 22 tests for which standardized norms
hands; and the Token Test for Children, parts three and
four.22
The ability to solve problems and, more specifically, TABLE 1. Age, gender, and IQ of subjects in the three study
the ability to establish, maintain, and change set and the groups: means, standard deviations, and P-values for
analysis of variance
ability to monitor personal behavior5 are examined by
Group 1 Group 2 Group 3
the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test38 with categories and ADHD~LD LD ADHD
perseverative error measures. Variable (n$26) (n$22) (n$10) P
Learning ability is tested by the Serial Hand Move- Age 8.9551.74 9.8452.08 9.7052.00 0.2267
ment Learning Test with a series of six or eight hand Gender (male) 0.8950.32 0.7050.47 0.9150.30 0.1363
movements, constructed specifically for this study from Full Scale IQ 94.6514.44 94.2510.65 102.2514.82 0.2184
Verbal IQ 98.4516.1 99.4510.8 100.9515.4 0.9033
the KABC Hand Movements subtest. The test reflects the Performance IQ 96.4515.2 96.1515.9 102.9515.0 0.4996
ability to learn a complex motor sequence.5 The Buschke
Selective Reminding Learning Test provides measures Note: ADHD4attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder;
LD4learning disabilities.
of verbal learning and consistency.

Data Analysis TABLE 2. Z-score means and standard deviations of tests of


Mean z-scores were calculated for each variable for the Attention-Inhibition-Cueing for the three study groups
three experimental groups. In addition, we calculated Group 1 Group 2 Group 3
the proportion of individuals who were impaired on Measure ADHD~LD LD ADHD
each test, defined as those who scored at or below 1 Delaya
standard deviation of the mean. In a normative sample, Efficient 11.6951.02 11.7551.08 11.1850.94
16% of the subjects may score less than 1 standard de- Correct 11.0250.75 10.9451.07 10.1850.99
Response 11.1651.68 11.6951.18 10.8951.19
viation, and we can expect that 16% of our subjects Vigilancea
would score below that point if their distribution were Correct 11.4851.43 10.9051.39 10.3251.03
the same as the distribution of the normative group. Bi- Commission 11.7151.42 11.6251.22 11.1851.28
Distractibilitya
nomial statistics were used to assess the likelihood that Correct 11.7251.39 11.2851.57 10.0650.95
the proportion of patients scoring at or below that point Commission 12.1351.27 11.1151.42 11.1151.48
was greater than would be expected by chance. Kagenb
Error 10.9551.31 10.7751.10 0.0751.26
Comparison of the study groups (ADHD`LD, LD Time 10.4550.89 10.3850.67 10.7450.45
alone, ADHD alone) was performed by analyzing each Rampartc 3.3653.36 2.8253.57 2.8252.75
variable with a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA)
Note: ADHD4attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder;
and Student-Newman-Keuls post hoc mean compari- LD4learning disabilities.
sons (significance set at the 0.05 level) done by SPSS/ a
Gordon Diagnostic System.30
b
PC` Release 6.0.1.39 Multiple ANOVAs were chosen Matching Familiar Figures Test.32
c
Rampart drawing.31 Number of errors in raw score form.
over a single multivariate analysis because of the ex-

162 VOLUME 10 NUMBER 2 SPRING 1998


LAZAR AND FRANK

are available for the ADHD`LD group, 7 of 22 (32%) (0.16) according to the binomial test, are calculated for
for the LD-only group, and 3 of 22 (14%) for the ADHD- each test (Table 5 through Table 7). The proportion of
only group. The two groups with LD (groups 1 and 2) subjects with impaired test scores is greater than chance
appear, therefore, more impaired than the ADHD-only for many measures of Attention-Inhibition-Cueing and
group (group 3). of Problem Solving. The percentage of tests with a sig-
The proportion of subjects with impaired test scores nificant proportion of impaired test scores for tests of
(z-scores of 1 or less), and the probability that this pro- Attention-Inhibition-Cueing is 89, 89, and 33 for groups
portion is larger than would be expected by chance 1, 2, and 3, respectively; for Working Memory, 67, 44,
and 0, respectively; for Problem Solving, 100, 100, and
0, respectively. There is, therefore, more impairment in
TABLE 3. Zscore means and standard deviations of tests of the two groups with learning disabilities (groups 1 and
Working Memory for the three study groups 2) than in the ADHD-only group (group 3) for all the
Group 1 Group 2 Group 3 categories of tests. Verbal Learning was normal for the
Measure ADHD~LD LD ADHD three groups.
Digitsa The Rampart Drawing test from Table 2 and the motor
Backward 10.5651.00 10.5250.91 0.2050.95 learning measures from Table 4 were not included in this
Forward 10.4650.72 10.8050.61 10.2750.90
Total 10.5750.90 10.7150.74 10.0650.79 summary because norms were not available to make
Bead Memoryb 0.8750.97 10.9050.87 10.6551.69 them comparable to the other measures. Verbal Learn-
ITPA Visual Sequential ing was normal (at mid-average) for all groups.
Memoryc 10.2151.40 0.0751.49 0.0351.41
Tokend Table 8 shows the results of a one-way ANOVA com-
Part 3 10.2350.79 10.1051.12 10.3651.40 paring the mean z-scores of the three experimental
Part 4 10.4551.07 10.5751.30 10.1651.23 groups, along with results of post hoc Student-
KABCe
Dominant 10.9650.72 10.6050.96 10.2150.34 Newman-Keuls mean comparisons tests. Only tests with
Nondominant 11.0050.86 10.8050.86 10.4250.90 probability values of less than 0.05 are presented. The
Note: ADHD4attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder;
LD4learning disabilities.
a
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for ChildrenRevised33 or III.34 TABLE 5. Proportion of subjects with impaired measures for
b
Stanford-BinetIV Bead Memory subtest.26 each test of Attention-Inhibition-Cueing and the
c
Illinois Test of Psycholinguistic Abilities.36 probability that this proportion is larger than chance
d
Token Test for Children.22 exceeding 0.16 (P-values in parentheses)
e
Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children Hand Movement Group 1 Group 2 Group 3
subtest.37 Variable ADHD~LD LD ADHD
Delaya
Efficient 0.78 0.81 0.54
(0.0000)* (0.0000)* (0.0037)*
TABLE 4. Z-score means and standard deviations of tests of Correct 0.52 0.52 0.18
Problem Solving and Learning for the three study (0.0000)* (0.0001)* (0.5453)
groups Response 0.48 0.71 0.36
Group 1 Group 2 Group 3 (0.0000)* (0.0000)* (0.0846)
Measure ADHD~LD LD ADHD Vigilancea
Correct 0.56 0.54 0.09
WCST Categoriesa (0.0000)* (0.0000)* (0.4547)
(Problem Solving) 11.2751.50 11.6851.78 0.0651.04 Commission 0.74 0.64 0.54
WCST Errorsa (0.0000)* (0.0000)* (0.0037)*
(Problem Solving) 11.5651.61 11.7851.66 10.5551.25 Distractibilitya
Learn Sixb Correct 0.68 0.53 0.20
(Motor Learning) 9.0454.65 9.7154.44 8.9153.39 (0.0000)* (0.0002) (0.4920)
Learn Eightb Commission 0.76 0.74 0.50
(Motor Learning) 13.3853.06 11.7653.81 10.4553.80 (0.0000)* (0.0000)* (0.0130)*
Long-term storagec Kagenb
(Verbal Learning) 0.8851.08 1.0251.17 1.0251.92 Error 0.43 0.43 0.18
Consistent long-term (0.0000)* (0.0017)* (0.5453)
retrievalc Time 0.21 0.17 0.27
(Verbal Learning) 0.4951.62 0.5651.54 1.0552.85 (0.2995) (0.5141) (0.2521)

Note: ADHD4attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; Note: ADHD4attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder;


LD4learning disabilities. LD4learning disabilities.
a
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test.38 a
Gordon Diagnostic System.30
b b
Number of trials in raw score form. Matching Familiar Figures Test.32
c
Selective Reminding Learning Test.28 *Binomial P-value,0.05.

JOURNAL OF NEUROPSYCHIATRY 163


FRONTAL DYSFUNCTION IN ADHD/LD CHILDREN

groups differed significantly on the Correct answers of ing were often impaired (z-score means less than 1) for
the Distractibility test and on the Kagen Error test (At- the three study groups, and the number of individuals
tention-Inhibition-Cueing); on the KABC dominant and with impaired performance on these measures, as well
the Digits Backward tests (Working Memory); and on as on measures of working memory and motor learning,
the Categories part of the WCST (Problem Solving). The was often higher than would be expected by chance. We
significant post hoc mean differences demonstrate that therefore confirm that ADHD, LD, and ADHD`LD
the groups with LD (groups 1 and 2) perform more subjects do not perform well on tests that examine ex-
poorly than the group of ADHD only (group 3). ecutive and other frontal systems functions.
Table 8 also shows the results of a one-way ANOVA Our study demonstrates, though, that abnormalities
of the general measure of cognition (composed of an of these functions are not specifically related to ADHD.
average of the results of the Raven, Token, Boston Nam- The two groups of LD patients (LD and ADHD`LD)
ing, and CELF-R tests). The groups with learning dis- performed worse than the ADHD-only group. Also, the
abilities (groups 1 and 2) had a significantly worse per- ADHD`LD group did not differ significantly from the
formance than the ADHD-only group (group 3; LD-only group, showing no evidence for an
P,0.014). ADHD`LD synergism. Therefore, there is a stronger
LD effect than an ADHD effect on executive frontal sys-
tems functions.This result is not due to the choice of
DISCUSSION tests, to IQ difference among the groups, to lack of sta-
Measures of response inhibition and sustained atten- tistical power, or to our ADHD definition. Our measures
tion, ability to respond after cueing, and problem solv- were chosen to test the full range of frontal systems
functions. They were not chosen to differentiate children
with learning disabilities from children with ADHD.
TABLE 6. Proportion of subjects with impaired measures for There was no significant difference in general intelli-
each test of Working Memory and the probability that gence between the groups. Absence of ADHD effect is
this proportion is larger than chance (P-values in not related to lack of statistical power, because a signifi-
parentheses)
cant difference was found between the small ADHD
Group 1 Group 2 Group 3 group (group 3) and the LD and ADHD`LD groups,
Variable ADHD~LD LD ADHD
whereas no significant difference was found between
Digitsa the larger ADHD`LD and LD groups. We also recal-
Backward 0.36 0.30 0.09
(0.0048)* (0.0624) (0.4547)
culated group differences after applying more stringent
Forward 0.25 0.43 0.09 criteria for ADHD diagnosis (by increasing the number
(0.1489) (0.0017)* (0.4547)
Total 0.43 0.39 0.18
(0.0001)* (0.0066)* (0.5453)
Bead Memoryb 0.36 0.56 0.40 TABLE 7. Proportion of subjects with impaired measures for
(0.0048)* (0.0000)* (0.0614) each test of Problem Solving and Verbal Learning and
ITPA Visual the probability that this proportion is larger than
Sequential Memoryc 0.32 0.26 0.37 chance (P-values in parentheses)
(0.0191)* (0.1498) (0.0846) Group 1 Group 2 Group 3
d
Token Variable ADHD~LD LD ADHD
Part 3 0.22 0.15 0.11
(0.3028) (0.5990) (0.5652) WCSTa
Part 4 0.30 0.16 0.11 Categories
(0.0624) (0.6380) (0.5652) (Problem Solving) 0.64 0.68 0.18
KABCe (0.0000)* (0.0000)* (0.5453)
Dominant 0.56 0.27 0.09 Errors
(0.0000)* (0.1270) (0.4547) (Problem Solving) 0.64 0.73 0.27
Nondominant 0.56 0.59 0.27 (0.0000)* (0.0000)* (0.2521)
(0.0000)* (0.0000)* (0.2521) Long-term storageb
(Verbal Learning) 0.08 0.04 0.20
(0.1873) (0.0976) (0.4920)
b
Note: ADHD4attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; Consistent long-term retrieval
LD4learning disabilities. (Verbal Learning) 0.23 0.17 0.30
a
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for ChildrenRevised or -III.33,34 (0.2367) (0.5141) (0.2064)
b
Stanford-Binet IV Bead Memory subtest.26
c
Illinois Test of Psycholinguistic Abilities.36 Note: ADHD4attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder;
d
Token Test for Children.22 LD4learning disabilities.
e a
Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children Hand Movement Wisconsin Card Sorting Test.38
subtest.37 b
Selective Reminding Learning Test.28
*Binomial P-value,0.05. *Binomial P-value,0.01.

164 VOLUME 10 NUMBER 2 SPRING 1998


LAZAR AND FRANK

of affirmative answers on the DSM-III-R questions in clinic-referred small groups of ADD`H boys, ADD
needed for ADHD diagnosis from 6 up to 11). There was H boys, LDnon-ADD boys, and normal control sub-
still no significant consistent difference between the jects. Significant differences were found between normal
ADHD and the non-ADHD groups. control subjects and the other groups on two Stroop
The high frequency of LD among ADHD children measures, namely, words and interference. Omission er-
makes differences between an unselected group of rors on a continuous performance task differentiated
ADHD and normal children impossible to interpret as ADD`H boys from normal control subjects, and ADD
due to ADHD alone. Most studies of executive and other H boys from the LD boys and normal control subjects.
frontal systems functions in ADHD do not control for Robins44 studied the responses of children with
the presence of LD. Barkley et al.14 reviewed 22 such ADHD, LD, and ADHD`LD on 12 neuropsychological
studies. Only three had a control group of children with measures including subtests of the WISC-R, the Beery,
LD, usually reading disability: Felton et al.,41 using lin- Trail Making, Matching Familiar Figures Test, Rey Au-
guistic fluency and semantic fluency tests, found no dif- ditory-Verbal Learning Test, and Gordon Delay and Vig-
ferences between reading-disabled groups with or with- ilance subtests. Significant differences between the
out ADD with hyperactivity (ADD`H). McGee et al.42 groups on a multiple discriminant-function analysis
found that ADD`H 13-year-old children with reading were related to the constructs of self-regulation and
disabilities had lower scores on the Rey-Osterrieth Com- planning/accuracy/speed. The ADHD and ADHD`
plex Figure Test than did children with only ADD`H, LD groups were more impulsive, less accurate, and
again demonstrating a reading disability rather than more variable in terms of self-regulation than the LD
ADHD effect. Douglas and Benezra,43 again using the sample. The three groups did not differ on other exec-
Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figures Test, did not find a sig- utive measures including those of sustained attention,
nificant difference between the performance of children auditory memory for digits, or verbal learning.
with reading disabilities and children with ADD`H. August and Garfinkel45 raised the question of
These three studies, although using somewhat different whether or not defective cognitive mechanisms are spe-
test batteries, demonstrate that the presence of LD is at cific to ADHD. They compared three groups of ADHD
least an equal factor to ADHD, and possibly a more im- children and normal control subjects by using several
portant one, in correlating with frontal systems func- cognitive measures. One ADHD group included chil-
tions. dren who also had a reading/spelling disability (the
Barkley et al.14 also reported their own study com- Cognitive group). The other two groups of ADHD chil-
paring results of 26 measures of frontal lobe functions dren had either low or high scores on the Conduct
Disorder-Aggressive scale of the Yale Childrens Inven-
tory and comprised the Mild Behavioral and Severe
TABLE 8. Mean z-scores, group differences (P-value) and post Behavioral Groups, respectively. The Mild Behavioral
hoc comparisons for frontal systems tests and for the
General cognitive composite according to one-way
group did not meet the clinical cutoff on the Conduct
analysis of variance Disorder-Aggressive scale and will be considered for the
Mean Z-Scores
purpose of this discussion an ADHD group without
Post Hoc
Group 1 Group 2 Group 3 Comparisons conduct-aggression behavior or reading/spelling dis-
Variable ADHD~LD LD ADHD P of Groups abilities. The Cognitive group differed significantly
Kagen Errora from the Mild Behavioral group on several measures.
(Inhibition) 11.01 10.72 10.25 0.0413 None The Cognitive group made significantly more errors
KABC Dominantb
(Working Memory) 11.00 10.60 10.22 0.0400 1,3 than the Mild Behavioral group on a continuous perfor-
Digits Backwardc mance test, had more omissions, commissions, and total
(Working Memory) 12.60 12.11 11.52 0.0277 1,3, 2,3
Distractibility Correctd errors on a maze test (typically ADHD and frontal mea-
(Attention) 11.78 11.26 10.23 0.0264 1,3 sures), and performed more poorly on a test of word
WCST Categoriese
(Problem Solving) 10.91 11.53 10.05 0.0203 2,3 knowledge, rapid word reading, and recognition of
Generalf 10.60 10.52 0.23 0.0140 1,3, 2,3 fragmented words. In other words, the group with
Note: ADHD4attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; LD4learning reading/spelling disability performed worse than the
disabilities.
a
ADHD-only group, a finding consistent with our re-
Matching Familiar Figures Test.32
b
Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children Hand Movement subtest.37 sults. It is of note that this study did not attempt to
c
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for ChildrenRevised or -III.33,34 control the findings for intelligence, discussing the point
d
Gordon Diagnostic System.30
e
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test.38 that matching or controlling for intelligence may be an
f
Average of the results of the Raven, Token, Boston Naming, and CELF-R excessive measure. Certain cognitive deficits of reading-
tests (see text under Measures).
disabled students may contribute to a lower general in-

JOURNAL OF NEUROPSYCHIATRY 165


FRONTAL DYSFUNCTION IN ADHD/LD CHILDREN

telligence, but this does not mean that they have deficits toward specific language deficiencies in children with
in all areas as a correction for general intelligence would reading and spelling problems.
imply. Our study has some limitations, chiefly the small
Korkman and Pesonen46 compared three groups of number of ADHD-only (group 3) patients, the inclusion
childrenADHD only, LD only, and ADHD`LDon of multiple forms of LD (reading, mathematics, or writ-
19 cognitive measures. Individual ANOVAs showed sig- ten expression), and the usual referral bias of a specialty
nificant differences among the three groups on measures clinic. Nevertheless, our results suggest that abnormal-
ities detected in laboratory tests of impulsive response,
of attention and response control, storytelling, grapho-
attention, and problem solving, which are considered
motor skill, and digit span. Although post hoc analyses
frontal systems or executive functions, are not exclusive
were not reported, t statistics between the LD-only and ADHD characteristics and are equally (or more) preva-
the ADHD-only groups demonstrated that the ADHD lent in children with LD. This result would be expected,
group performed better on the measures of auditory given the complexity of the laboratory tests and the
analysis of speech, storytelling, and digit span, whereas complicated integration of cognitive processes needed
the LD group performed better on tests of response con- for their successful completion; the close association be-
trol. Consistent with our study, there were no significant tween brain centers of processing and those of ex-
differences between the two groups on attention mea- ecutive functions that may be impaired in LD and
sures. ADHD, respectively; and the broad characterization of
These authors also report significant differences in executive control as discussed by Stuss and Benson.5,6
Verbal IQ scores among their groups, with the ADHD- In addition, our results suggest that tests assessing only
only group having the highest, as is the case in our frontal systems or executive functions cannot be used to
differentiate ADHD from LD.
study. They did not correct for this difference, noting
that it may distort true conditions given the tendency The authors thank Susan Barth for her technical support.

References

1. American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical 13. Matier-Sharma K, Perachio N, Newcorn JH, et al: Differential
Manual of Mental Disorders, 2nd edition. Washington, DC, diagnosis of ADHD: are objective measures of attention, impul-
American Psychiatric Association, 1968 sivity and activity level helpful? Child Neuropsychology 1995;
2. American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical 1:118127
Manual of Mental Disorders, 3rd edition. Washington, DC, 14. Barkley RA, Grodzinsky G, DuPaul GJ: Frontal lobe functions in
American Psychiatric Association, 1980 attention deficit disorder with and without hyperactivity: a re-
3. American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical view and research report. J Abnorm Child Psychol 1992; 20:163
Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition. Washington, DC, 188
American Psychiatric Association, 1994 15. Shaywitz SE, Shaywitz BA: Attention deficit disorder: current
4. Shaywitz BA, Yager RD, Klopper JH: Selective brain dopamine perspectives. Pediatr Neurol 1987; 31:129135
depletion. Science 1976; 191:305308 16. American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical
5. Stuss DT, Benson DF: Neuropsychological studies of the frontal Manual of Mental Disorders, 3rd edition, revised. Washington,
lobes. Psychol Bull 1984; 95:328 DC, American Psychiatric Association, 1987
6. Stuss DT, Benson DF: The frontal lobes and control of cognition 17. Achenbach TM, Edelbrock C: Manual for the Child Behavior
and memory, in The Frontal Lobes Revisited, edited by Perce- Checklist and Revised Child Behavior Profile. Burlington, VT,
man E. New York, IRBN Press, 1987, pp 141158 University of Vermont, 1983
7. Stuss DT, Alexander MP, Palumbo CL, et al: Organizational 18. Conners CK: Rating scales for use in drug studies with children.
strategies of patients with unilateral or bilateral frontal lobe in- Psychopharmacotherapy With Children (special issue) 1973; 24
jury in word list learning tasks. Neuropsychology 1994; 8:355 29
373 19. Achenbach TM, Edelbrock C: Manual for the Teachers Report
8. Wickelgren I: Getting a grasp on working memory. Science 1997; Form and Teacher Version of the Child Behavior Profile. Bur-
275:15791582 lington, VT, University of Vermont, 1986
9. Pontius AA: Dysfunction patterns analogous to frontal lobe sys- 20. Raven JC: Guide to Using the Coloured Progressive Matrices.
tem and caudate nucleus syndromes in some groups of minimal London, England, HK Lewis; New York, Psychological Corpo-
brain dysfunction. J Am Med Womens Assoc 1973; 26:285292 ration, 1965
10. Mattes JA: The role of frontal lobe dysfunction in childhood hy- 21. Raven JC: Guide to the Standard Progressive Matrices. London,
perkinesis. Compr Psychiatry 1980; 21:358369 England, HK Lewis; New York, Psychological Corporation, 1960
11. Barkley RA: Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: A Hand- 22. DiSimoni F: Token Test for Children. Boston, Teaching Resources
book for Diagnosis and Treatment. New York, Guilford, 1990 Corporation, 1978
12. Barkley RA: The ecological validity of laboratory and analogue 23. Kaplan EF, Goodglass H, Weintraub S: The Boston Naming Test.
assessment methods of ADHD symptoms. J Abnorm Child Psy- Malvern, PA, Lea and Febiger, 1983
chol 1991; 19:149177 24. Semel E, Wiig EH, Secord W: CELF-R: Clinical Evaluation of

166 VOLUME 10 NUMBER 2 SPRING 1998


LAZAR AND FRANK

Language Fundamentals, 2nd edition, revised. San Antonio, TX, of Psycholinguistic Abilities. Los Angeles, Western Psychologi-
Psychological Corporation, 1986 cal Services, 1982
25. Sheslow D, Adams W: WRAML: Wide Range Assessment of 37. Kaufman AS, Kaufman NL: KABC: Kaufman Assessment Bat-
Memory and Learning: Administration Manual. Wilmington, tery for Children: Administration and Scoring Manual. Circle
DE, Jastak Associates, 1990 Pines, MN, American Guidance Service, 1983
26. Thorndike RL, Hagen EP, Sattler JM: Stanford-Binet Intelligence 38. Heaton RK: A Manual for the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test.
Scale, 4th edition. Chicago, Riverside, 1986 Odessa, FL, Psychological Assessment Resources, 1981
27. Beery KE: Administration, Scoring and Teaching Manual for the 39. Norusis MJ: SPSS/PC` V2.O Base Manual. Chicago, SPSS, 1988
Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration. Chicago, Fol- 40. Moltulsky H: Intuitive Biostatistics. New York, Oxford Univer-
lett, 1982 sity Press, 1995
28. Buschke H: Selective reminding for analysis of memory and 41. Felton RH, Wood FB, Brown IS, et al: Separate verbal and nam-
learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 1973; ing deficits in attention deficit disorder and reading disability.
12:543550 Brain Lang 1987; 31:171184
29. Verfaellie M, Heilman KM: Response preparation and response 42. McGee R, Williams S, Moffitt T, et al: A comparison of 13-year-
inhibition after lesions of the medial frontal lobe. Arch Neurol old boys with attention deficit and/or reading disorder on neu-
1987; 44:12651271 ropsychological measures. J Abnorm Child Psychol 1989; 17:37
30. Gordon Diagnostic System. DeWitt, NY, Gordon Diagnostic Sys- 53
tems, 1986 43. Douglas EI, Benezra E: Supraspan verbal memory in attention
31. Mattis S: Dementia Rating Scale. Odessa, FL, Psychological As- deficit disorder with hyperactivity, normal and reading disabled
sessment Resources, 1988 boys. Child Psychol 1990; 18:617638
32. Kagen J: Matching Familiar Figures Test. Cambridge, MA, Har- 44. Robins PM: A comparison of behavioral and attentional func-
vard University, 1995 tioning in children diagnosed as hyperactive or learning dis-
33. Wechsler D: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 2nd edi- abled. J Abnorm Child Psychol 1992; 20:6582
tion. New York, Psychological Corporation, 1974 45. August JA, Garfinkel BD: Behavioral and cognitive subtypes of
34. Wechsler D: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 3rd edi- ADHD. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1989; 28:739748
tion. New York, Psychological Corporation, 1991 46. Korkman M, Pesonen AE: A comparison of neuropsychological
35. Gardner RA: The Objective Diagnosis of Minimal Brain Dys- test profiles of children with attention deficit hyperactivity dis-
function. Cresskill, NJ, Creative Therapeutics, 1979 order and/or learning disorder. Journal of Learning Disabilities
36. Kirk SA, McCarthy JJ, Kirk WD: Examiners Manual: Illinois Test 1994; 27:383392

JOURNAL OF NEUROPSYCHIATRY 167

Вам также может понравиться