Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 12

Behavioral Interventions

for Infant Sleep Problems: A


Randomized Controlled Trial
Michael Gradisar, PhD,a Kate Jackson, PhD, ClinPsyc,a Nicola J. Spurrier, PhD,b Joyce Gibson, MNutrDiet,c
Justine Whitham, PhD,a Anne Sved Williams, MBBS, FRANZCP,d,e Robyn Dolby, PhD,b David J. Kennaway, PhDf

OBJECTIVES: To evaluate the effects of behavioral interventions on the sleep/wakefulness abstract


of infants, parent and infant stress, and later child emotional/behavioral problems, and
parent-child attachment.
METHODS: A total of 43 infants (616 months, 63% girls) were randomized to receive either
graduated extinction (n = 14), bedtime fading (n = 15), or sleep education control (n =
14). Sleep measures included parent-reported sleep diaries and infant actigraphy. Infant
stress was measured via morning and afternoon salivary cortisol sampling, and mothers
self-reported mood and stress. Twelve months after intervention, mothers completed
assessments of childrens emotional and behavioral problems, and mother-child dyads
underwent the strange situation procedure to evaluate parent-child attachment.
RESULTS: Significant interactions were found for sleep latency (P < .05), number of
awakenings (P < .0001), and wake after sleep onset (P = .01), with large decreases in sleep
latency for graduated extinction and bedtime fading groups, and large decreases in number
of awakenings and wake after sleep onset for the graduated extinction group. Salivary
cortisol showed small-to-moderate declines in graduated extinction and bedtime fading
groups compared with controls. Mothers stress showed small-to-moderate decreases
for the graduated extinction and bedtime fading conditions over the first month, yet no
differences in mood were detected. At the 12-month follow-up, no significant differences
were found in emotional and behavioral problems, and no significant differences in secure-
insecure attachment styles between groups.
CONCLUSIONS: Both graduated extinction and bedtime fading provide significant sleep benefits
above control, yet convey no adverse stress responses or long-term effects on parent-child
attachment or child emotions and behavior.

aSchool of Psychology, and cDepartment of Nutrition and Dietetics, Flinders University, Adelaide, South Australia, WHATS KNOWN ON THIS SUBJECT: Recommended
Australia; bPublic Health and Health Promotion, SA Health, South Australia, Australia; dDepartment of Psychiatry, guidelines exist for the treatment of nocturnal
and fSchool of Paediatrics and Reproductive Health, University of Adelaide, Adelaide, South Australia, Australia;
wakefulness for infants, including graduated
and eWomens and Childrens Health Network, Adelaide, South Australia, Australia
extinction and bedtime fading. Such interventions
Dr Gradisar provided co-conception and design of the study, supervision of most of the analyses have a solid evidence base for improving infant sleep,
and conducted the rest of analyses, signicant contribution to the interpretation of analyses, yet little is known about their contraindications.
and was main contributor to writing of manuscript; Dr Jackson provided co-conception and
design of the study, collected most of the data (pretreatment to 3 months), conducted most of the WHAT THIS STUDY ADDS: Sustained improvements in
analyses, provided signicant input to the literature reviewed, was a minor contributor to writing sleep latency were found for both treatment groups,
of the manuscript, and provided critical evaluation of manuscript drafts; Dr Spurrier provided but not controls, yet no signicant differences
co-conception and design of the study, critical evaluation of manuscript drafts, and interpretation occurred in the infant salivary cortisol, parental
of ndings; Ms Gibson provided input into study design, collected most of the 12-month follow-up stress and mood, and attachment proles between
data, and provided interpretation of ndings and critical evaluation of manuscript drafts; all groups.
Dr Whitham provided input into the study design, conducted data scoring and analyses of 12-month
To cite: Gradisar M, Jackson K, Spurrier NJ, et al. Behavioral Interventions
for Infant Sleep Problems: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Pediatrics.
2016;137(6):e20151486

Downloaded from by guest on September 13, 2017


PEDIATRICS Volume 137, number 6, June 2016:e20151486 ARTICLE
Nocturnal wakefulness is normal TABLE 1 Timing of Screening Measures, the Primary Outcome Variable, and Secondary Outcomes
during early infant development Pretreatment 1 wk 1 mo 3 mo 12 mo
(ie, first 12 months).13 Night Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (screen for
wakings allow infants to signal postnatal depression)
parents provision of sustenance Infant Development Inventory (screen typical
and comfort.1,3,4 At 2 months of age, development)
Sleep diary (primary outcome [sleep])
sleep homeostatic pressure develops,
Actigraphy (objective measure of sleep)
potentially compressing nighttime Salivary cortisol (secondary outcome: infant
awakenings.2 Large declines in stress)
nocturnal wakefulness occur (on DASS-21 (secondary outcome: parent stress/
average) over the first 6 months mood)
Child Behavior Checklist 1.55.0 y (secondary
of age, and plateau thereafter,2,5
outcome: child behavior/emotion)
occurring after 24-hour circadian Strange situation procedure (secondary outcome:
rhythm stabilization at 3 to 6 child-parent attachment)
months of age.6,7 Infants nighttime Child Behavior Checklist and DASS-21 were blind scored by JW; strange situation procedure was blind scored by RD;
awakenings typically diminish by actigraphy data were automatically scored using the ActiWare (v.5, Philips Respironics, Bend, OR) computerized algorithm
the end of the first year of life3; after manual correction of bedtimes from sleep diaries.28 Sleep diaries were scored by KJ.

however, from 6 months, 16% to


21% of infants continue experiencing (eg, 2 minutes) and then gradually stress, attachment, and emotional
nocturnal wakefulness such that extended up to 6 minutes on the and behavioral problems, as well
parents report their child has a first night (graduated extinction).21 as maternal mood and stress, over
sleep problem.8,9 Sleep problems Despite a longstanding solid evidence 3 months of treatment and at a
may be due to reduced sleep base for improving infant sleep and 12-month follow-up. The primary
homeostatic pressure2 and/or a maternal mood and stress both in the outcome was infants sleep (sleep
coercive behavior trap, whereby short- and long-term,21,22 concerns latency, number of wakings, wake
parents nocturnal responses are after sleep onset), and secondary
have nonetheless been raised. The
more reinforcing than sleep.1 Aside outcomes were infants cortisol,
immediate stress experienced
from an array of known daytime parents stress and mood, child-
by parents while undertaking
impairments that follow from sleep parent attachment, and child
extinction-based methods can often
restriction in adults,10 serious behavior. Descriptions and the timing
lead to ceasing such techniques.23,24
consequences have been reported of these measures are presented in
However, arguably more important
for families who have an infant with Table 1.
(and central to the current article)
a sleep problem. Mothers of infants
is that the stress associated with
with a sleep problem are more likely
extinction-based treatments might
to use physical punishment,11,12 and METHODS
are at an increased risk of developing elevate cortisol levels that could
depression (eg, >2.0 odds ratio).9,1315 have long-term consequences Participants
Also worth noting is that mothers of infant helplessness, and later A total of 43 infants (mean age 10.8
with emotional disturbances (ie, insecure parent-child attachments 3.5 months, 616 months, 63% girls)
stress, depression) are more likely to and child emotional and behavioral and their mothers (mean age 33.3
report intrusive thoughts of harming problems.3,25 This antithesis to 4.8 years) and fathers (age 35.5
their infant,16,17 and some even using extinction-based methods 6.4 years) were randomly assigned
commit filicide.1820 Thus, evidenced- has strong support, to the extent to receive graduated extinction (n
based treatments that rapidly that more gentle approaches are = 14), bedtime fading (n = 15), or
resolve infants sleeplessness while sought. Bedtime fading is one such sleep education control (n = 14).
minimizing family distress are vital. technique.21 Based on the physiologic Inclusion criteria were the following:
theory of sleep homeostasis,26,27 1 parent identifying their child
Several interventions exist for bedtime fading indirectly compresses having a sleep problem (parents
infants sleeplessness. Some with sleep by gradually limiting time in responded with yes to the question
the strongest evidence are based on bed, usually by delaying the infants Do you think your child has a sleep
psychological learning theory (ie, bedtime by 15 minutes each night.21 problem?)29; doctor/health nurse
operant conditioning), where the The current studys aim is to compare check 1 month where the infant
parents response to their infants effects from graduated extinction was healthy and attained expected
nocturnal cries are totally ignored and bedtime fading against a sleep weight gain; and typical infant
(extinction), or initially delayed education control on infants sleep, development (Infant Development

Downloaded from by guest on September 13, 2017


2 GRADISAR et al
Inventory). Exclusion criteria were TABLE 2 Demographic Characteristics of Participating Families, by Treatment Group
the following: significant postnatal Graduated Bedtime Fading Education Control
depression (Edinburgh Postnatal Extinction
Depression Scale scores 15), and Child
indication of suicidality. Most parents Age, mean SD, mo 10.8 3.7 11.8 3.4 9.61 3.3
were in a marriagelike relationship Boys, n (%) 5 (39) 5 (31) 5 (42)
Mother
(92.7%), had higher education
Age, mean SD, y 31.38 4.4 34.44 5.1 33.82 4.3
qualifications (77.2%), and were Education status, n (%)
middle- to high-income earners Did not complete high school 1 (8.3)
(>AUD$80000; 48.7%). Mothers Completed high school 2 (15.4) 3 (18.8) 1 (8.3)
were mainly responsible for their Completed higher education 11 (84.6) 13 (81.3) 10 (83.3)
Father
infants sleep (69.3%; 26.9% shared
Age, mean SD, y 33.38 5.4 35.67 7.0 37.73 6.2
responsibility). Table 2 presents Education status, n (%)
the sample demographics. Parents Did not complete high school 1 (7.1) 3 (27.3)
provided informed consent to Completed high school 2 (15.4) 5 (35.7)
participate. The study was approved Completed higher education 11 (84.6) 8 (57.1) 8 (72.2)
Family
by the Flinders University Social
Parental status, n (%)
and Behavioral Research Ethics Two parents 13 (100) 14 (87.5) 11 (91.7)
Committee. Single parent 2 (12.5) 1 (8.3)
Household income per annum, n
Procedure (%)*
<50 000 4 (26.7) 3 (25.0)
Parents contacted researchers 50 00080 000 4 (33.3) 5 (33.3) 4 (33.3)
in response to advertisements at >80 000 8 (66.7) 6 (40.0) 5 (41.7)
pediatric outpatient clinics, child Data missing for n = 1 for each of graduated extinction and bedtime fading groups. , no data.
care centers, health professional
private practices, newspapers, and afternoon (mean SD time 3:38 PM limiting the infants nocturnal
word-of-mouth. Parents completed 1.24 hours) on 2 consecutive days sleep opportunity (Supplemental
a questionnaires and a 90-minute by using sterile cotton-tipped eye Table 4). For the sleep education
interview (conducted by K.J.) spears.30 Blinded frozen samples control, parents were provided
assessing the infants medical and (20C) were thawed and centrifuged sleep information from a statewide
sleep history. Eligible families before cortisol analysis in duplicate child health service (www.cyh.com;
were randomly assigned via a by enzyme-linked immunosorbent Supplemental Table 4). Families
predetermined computerized block assay (Salimetrics, LCC, State College, received a booklet describing their
randomization held by K.J. (random PA). The group origin of the samples intervention, and 24/7 cell phone
allocation was not concealed from was blinded to the laboratory, and support (by K.J.). Of calls received,
K.J.). Due to ethics committee samples were within acceptable these included clarification of
requirements, parents were allowed validity criteria (the intra-assay techniques, delaying treatment, and
to swap conditions. This occurred coefficient was <10%, and the clarifying measurements (saliva,
for 2 families (1 from graduated interassay coefficients at 3.0 nM actigraphy), with no perceived
extinction to sleep education control; and 27.5 nM were 13.5% and 9.3%, differences in the number of calls
1 from sleep education control to respectively).31 between groups (unfortunately, no
graduated extinction). systematic data were recorded for
Parents subsequently underwent
these calls; thus, these data are based
During pretreatment, families their individualized treatment
on our retrospective recall).
completed sleep measures (7-day session. Graduated extinction
sleep diary; ankle-worn activity involved a set schedule of gradually Parents participated with their child
monitor [AW64 Minimitters; delaying parents response to their in the strange situation procedure
Philips Respironics, Bend, OR]), infants cry (see Supplemental Table to assess child-parent attachment.32
and maternal depressed mood 4). Parents were instructed to put This procedure used 8 standardized
and stress (depression and stress their infant to bed awake and leave interactions (including separations
subscales of the Depression Anxiety the room within 1 minute. When and reunions) between the parent,
Stress ScaleShort Form [DASS- reentering the room, they comforted child, and a stranger (the stranger
21]). Parents collected infant saliva their child, but avoided picking the was blind to childrens group
samples in the morning (mean child up and turning the lights on. allocation), which were videotaped
SD time 8:59 AM 1.27 hours) and Bedtime fading involved gradually and blind scored (by R.D.) according

Downloaded from by guest on September 13, 2017


PEDIATRICS Volume 137, number 6, June 2016 3
developing infants, nor some of
the important secondary outcomes
tested in the current trial (eg, salivary
cortisol). Large effect sizes (Cohens
d > 0.80) for graduated extinction
(compared with controls) have been
reported for sleep.21 Based on a
power of 0.80, and a probability level
of .05, at least 21 participants per
group would be needed to detect a
large effect size.

RESULTS
Figure 1 presents the flow of
participants through each stage of
the study.

Infant Sleep
Figure 2 presents descriptive
statistics for infants sleep over the
3-month treatment and follow-up
period. Linear mixed model
regression analyses of sleep diaries
showed significant interactions for
sleep latency, P < .05, wake after
sleep onset, P < .0001, number of
awakenings, P = .01, and total night
sleep time, P < .01. From pretreatment
to the 3-month follow-up, sleep
latency showed large declines
for infants in both the graduated
extinction (12.7 minutes; d = 0.87)
and bedtime fading (10 minutes;
d = 1.04) groups, but no change for
the control group (+2.0 minutes; d =
0.11). There was a very large decline
FIGURE 1
Participant ow through the randomized controlled trial. in the number of awakenings for
infants in the graduated extinction
group (d = 1.98), yet no changes for
to gold standard criteria. Recordings demonstrate changes within each
infants in both the bedtime fading (d
of each child-parent dyad were coded intervention. A time-of-day effect
= 0.10) and control (d = 0.13) groups.
as either secure, insecure (avoidant), was found between morning
Large improvements in wake after
insecure (resistant), and insecure and afternoon cortisol values
sleep onset were found for infants in
(disorganized). (pretreatment, P < .01), thus morning
the control (31.7 minutes; d = 0.93)
and afternoon cortisol values are and bedtime fading (24.6 minutes;
Analysis analyzed separately. Fishers exact d = 0.99) groups, with a very large
Linear mixed-model regressions test was used to assess differences in improvement found for the graduated
were used to test for significant attachment styles between groups. extinction group (44.4 minutes;
interactions on primary (sleep diary) A series of 1-way analyses of variance d = 2.02). Finally, total sleep time
and secondary (infant cortisol; assessed between-group differences showed a moderate increase in the
maternal stress/mood) outcome on the Child Behavior Checklist. graduated extinction condition (+0.32
variables. Within-group Cohens No effect sizes were available for hour), little change in the bedtime
d effect sizes were calculated to using bedtime fading on typically fading condition (+0.09 hour), and

Downloaded from by guest on September 13, 2017


4 GRADISAR et al
FIGURE 2
Means (95% condence interval [CI]) during and after treatment of infants in graduated extinction, bedtime fading, and sleep education control for (A)
sleep onset latency, (B) wake after sleep onset, (C) number of awakenings, and (D) total nocturnal sleep time.

a moderate increase in the control Afternoon cortisol showed a large and a large reduction in mothers
condition (+0.36 hour). Actigraphy decline in the graduated extinction stress in the bedtime fading group
showed no significant interactions for group (d = 0.89), a moderate decline (d = 0.86). Analysis of maternal
wake after sleep onset, P > .05, and in the bedtime fading group (d = mood demonstrated no significant
total sleep time, P > .05. 0.61) and a small decline in the interaction, P > .05. Mothers mood
control group (d = 0.39). improved from pretreatment to the
Infant and Parental Stress 12-month follow-up in all conditions,
Morning and afternoon infant cortisol A significant interaction was found with small effects found for those in
values for each group over time are for maternal stress, P < .01 (Fig 3). the graduated extinction (d = 0.34)
presented in Fig 3. No significant There were moderate improvements and control (d = 0.39) conditions,
interactions occurred for morning in all groups from pretreatment to yet a large effect for mothers in the
cortisol, P > .05, yet there was for 12-month follow-up (graduated bedtime fading group (d = 0.83).
afternoon cortisol values, P < .01. extinction: d = 0.51; bedtime fading:
From pretreatment to the 12-month d = 0.62; control: d = 0.64). However, Twelve-Month Follow-up: Child
follow-up, morning cortisol showed over the initial month of treatment, Attachment and Emotional-
Behavioral Problems
a small decline in the graduated mothers stress in the control group
extinction group (d = 0.23), a remained somewhat unchanged (d = Table 3 presents the percentage
moderate drop in the bedtime fading 0.16), whereas there was a moderate of parent-child attachment
group (d = 0.62), yet no change stress reduction for mothers in the classifications in each group. No
for the control group (d = 0.17). graduated extinction group (d = 0.67) significant differences were found

Downloaded from by guest on September 13, 2017


PEDIATRICS Volume 137, number 6, June 2016 5
FIGURE 3
Means (95% CI) during and after treatment of infants in graduated extinction, bedtime fading, and sleep education control for (A) infant salivary cortisol
morning samples, (B) infant salivary cortisol afternoon samples, (C) self-reported maternal stress, and (D) self-reported maternal mood.

between secure and insecure fading produced large decreases in a central research question in the
attachment styles between groups, sleep latency compared with the current study was: Do extinction-
P > .05. There were no significant control group. The control groups based techniques produce
differences between groups for any sleep did show improvements in psychophysiological stress that leads
emotional or behavioral problems on nocturnal wakefulness and total to later problematic emotions and
the Child Behavior Checklist (all P > sleep, suggesting developmental behavior, and thus insecure parent-
.05; Table 3). maturity and/or improvements from child attachment?
sleep education.35 No significant
sleep changes were found by using Child-Parent Stress, Emotions,
DISCUSSION objective actigraphy, suggesting Behaviors, and Attachment
Compared with controls, graduated sleep diaries and actigraphy measure Relatively minor stressors (eg, brief
extinction produced large decreases different phenomena (eg, infants parental separation) elevate cortisol
in nocturnal wakefulness (time taken absence of crying by parents vs levels in newborn infants38; however,
to fall asleep, number of awakenings, infants movements, respectively), the cortisol stress response diminishes
minutes awake after sleep onset), further suggesting infants may from 4 months of age.30,38,39
yet a novel aspect of this trial was still experience wakefulness but This may explain the lack of
evaluating bedtime fading, about do not signal to parents.36 We significant cortisol elevation in the
which there has been relatively little do not interpret these data as graduated extinction condition in
research,22 especially in typically the infant giving up,25,37 but the current study, especially as our
developing infants.33,34 Bedtime instead self-soothing.36 However, sample was older than 4 months of

Downloaded from by guest on September 13, 2017


6 GRADISAR et al
TABLE 3 Child Behavior Checklist Scores (Mean [95% CI] and Between-Group Effect Sizes [d]) at 12-Month Follow-up, by Treatment Group; Attachment
Classications (n [%]) by Group
Graduated Bedtime Fading, Education P d (Graduated d (Graduated d (Bedtime Fading
Extinction, n = 13 n = 15 Control, n = 12 Extinction versus Extinction versus versus Education
Bedtime Fading) Education Control)
Control)
Problem scale scores
Internalizing
problems
Raw scores 5.00 (2.77.3) 6.06 (3.98.2) 5.08 (1.88.4) .77 0.27 0.02 0.21
T scorea 60, n 1 (6.3) 1 (8.3) .59
(%)
Externalizing
problems
Raw scores 10.6 (6.414.8) 10.6 (7.713.5) 10.2 (5.514.9) .98 0.01 0.06 0.06
T score 60, n (%) 1 (7.7) 1 (6.3) 2 (16.6) .67
Total problems
Raw scores 10.8 (6.814.7) 10.9 (8.213.6) 12.1 (7.616.6) .84 0.02 0.19 0.20
T score 60, n (%)
Attachment
classications
Secure 7 (54) 9 (60) 5 (62)
Insecure (avoidant) 1 (8) 1 (7) 1 (13)
Insecure (resistant) 2 (15) 3 (20) 1 (13)
Insecure 3 (23) 2 (13) 1 (13)
(disorganized)
, not applicable.
a If T score in the borderline or clinical range; d > 0.50 = moderate effect, d > 0.20 = small effect; data missing for graduated extinction group (n = 1) and sleep education control group

(n = 1).

age (range 616 months; mean 10.8 behaviors comparable across groups. those of Price et al.41 are the only
months). We cannot conclude no This lack of findings concur with a studies that now form a preliminary
cortisol response occurred, as we did recent 6-year follow-up assessment evidence base that suggests brief
not collect real-time cortisol data (ie, of a large randomized controlled trial, behavioral sleep treatments may help
plasma) during nocturnal treatment where no differences in problematic young children sleep, yet do not lead
implementation. Our diurnal cortisol behaviors and mental health were to later emotional and behavioral
data indicate the active treatments found between children who received problems, or later parent-child
did not result in chronically elevated behavioral sleep interventions and insecure attachment.25
levels over time (ie, values were those in the control group.41
within normative limits),40 which is Limitations and Future Research
necessary for hypothalamic-pituitary- The final argument against using Directions
adrenocortical dysregulation.38 extinction-based methods for infant
sleep problems is the potential for Although the generalizability of
This is a crucial point when insecure child-parent attachment.25 our findings to the population is
considering the chain of arguments No significant differences were reduced with a small sample size,
forming the hypothesis that found in attachment styles between they nevertheless support those of a
graduated extinction may lead to groups, which suggests a lack of larger follow-up study of behavioral
problematic emotions and behaviors evidence between infants sleep and sleep interventions for infants (n =
in later child development.25 This attachment.5 For parental stress, 326).41 Although stress was measured
hypothesis requires a significant and mothers in both intervention groups at different time points across
chronic cortisol elevation resulting reported less stress than mothers interventions, we did not measure
from graduated extinction, yet is in the control group. The lack of acute stress during interventions.
further disconfirmed by examining support for dysfunctional child- Changes in attachment across the
our long-term emotional and parent relationships (ie, disinhibited study would also have been interesting,
behavioral findings. No significant attachment, child-parent closeness yet we note that 12 months of age is
difference in childrens emotions and and conflict, global relationship) after the prime age for the strange situation
behaviors could be found between behavioral sleep interventions has also procedure.32 The current study
groups 12 months after intervention, been found in a recent 6-year follow-up contrasted only 2 behavioral sleep
with internalizing and externalizing study.41 Altogether, our findings and interventions. Future trials are needed

Downloaded from by guest on September 13, 2017


PEDIATRICS Volume 137, number 6, June 2016 7
to compare other interventions (eg, clinical practice (eg, bedtime fading effects on the child or family. Further
extinction with parental presence, followed by graduated extinction). replication studies by independent
cosleeping via room-sharing). Due Our data suggest introducing bedtime groups are needed to confirm and
to ethical obligations and a coroners fading will provide quick results raise confidence in these findings.
inquiry in our home state,42 bed- for improving sleep-onset latency.
sharing was not an option to evaluate Graduated extinction may then be
because of infant mortality risks, introduced to reduce nocturnal ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
and we have seen similar warnings wakefulness during the night (if The authors thank the families who
internationally.43,44 Worth noting is needed). Our data suggest sleep generously donated their time and
that, to our knowledge, we have been education alone may not be enough energy into this project, Dr Patricia
unable to find a similar coronial finding to help most families with an infant McKinsey Crittenden for her expert
of an infant death due to graduated who has a sleep problem. advice regarding the conduct of
extinction. Thus, we wish to highlight strange situation assessments, Ms
to researchers the crucial need to build Laura Jarema for bravely being the
the evidence base for establishing CONCLUSIONS stranger, and Mr Pawel Skuza and
safe and effective treatments for those This randomized controlled trial of Megan Gunnar for their conceptual
families who perceive their infant to behavioral interventions for infant and statistical advice for cortisol
have a sleep problem. sleep problems found meaningful collection and analysis. We also
effects for both graduated extinction thank the Flinders University Social
Clinical Implications and bedtime fading. Compared with and Behavioral Ethics Committee
Although graduated extinction is the control group, large reductions for their impartial evaluation of
based on learning theory,1 sleep in nocturnal wakefulness resulted this project in response to an online
homeostatic pressure would increase from each treatment. Despite petition to cease this approved
over successive nights of less assertions that extinction-based research.
sleep.26,27 Likewise, bedtime fading methods may result in elevated
would increase sleep homeostatic cortisol, emotional and behavioral
pressure, quickening sleep latency, problems, and insecure parent-infant
reducing the time between stimuli attachment, our data did not support ABBREVIATION
(eg, cot) and the response (ie, sleep), this hypothesis. Coupled with the
DASS-21:Depression Anxiety
and thus infants re-learn to initiate findings from Price and colleagues,41
Stress ScaleShort
sleep. This theoretical overlap allows behavioral interventions appear to
Form
these treatments to combine in improve sleep without detrimental

follow-up data and the minority of analyses under supervision, provided some input to the literature reviewed and was a minor contributor to writing of
manuscript, and provided critical evaluation of manuscript drafts; Dr Sved Williams provided assistance with design of the study, interpretation of ndings, and
critical evaluation of manuscript drafts; Dr Powell-Davies provided input into the study design, scored the infant-parent attachment, and provided interpretation
of ndings and critical evaluation of manuscript drafts; Dr Kennaway provided input into study design, expert input into salivary cortisol data collection, scoring
of salivary cortisol, interpretation of ndings, and critical evaluation of manuscript drafts; and all authors approved the nal manuscript as submitted.
This trial has been registered at the Australian New Zealand Clinical Trials Registry (identier ACTRN12612000813886)
DOI: 10.1542/peds.2015-1486
Accepted for publication Mar 21, 2016
Address correspondence to Michael Gradisar, PhD, c/o Flinders University, School of Psychology, GPO Box 2100, Adelaide, SA, 5001, Australia. E-mail: grad0011@
inders.edu.au
PEDIATRICS (ISSN Numbers: Print, 0031-4005; Online, 1098-4275).
Copyright 2016 by the American Academy of Pediatrics
FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have indicated they have no nancial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.
FUNDING: Supported by Australian Rotary Health Fund, Channel 7 Childrens Research Fund, Faculty of Social and Behavioral Sciences.
POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST: The authors have indicated they have no potential conicts of interest to disclose.

Downloaded from by guest on September 13, 2017


8 GRADISAR et al
REFERENCES
1. Blampied NM, France KG. A b J Public Health Nations Health. infants and young children. Sleep.
ehavioral model of infant sleep 1965;55(12):18801888 2006;29(10):12771281
disturbance. J Appl Behav Anal. 23. Sadeh A, Tikotzky L, Scher A. Parenting
13. Hiscock H, Wake M. Infant sleep
1993;26(4):477492 and infant sleep. Sleep Med Rev.
problems and postnatal depression:
2. Jenni OG, Borbly AA, Achermann P. a community-based study. Pediatrics. 2010;14(2):8996
Development of the nocturnal sleep 2001;107(6):13171322 24. Middlemiss W, Granger DA, Goldberg
electroencephalogram in human
14. Dennis CL, Ross L. Relationships WA, Nathans L. Asynchrony of mother-
infants. Am J Physiol Regul Integr
among infant sleep patterns, infant hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal
Comp Physiol. 2004;286(3):R528R538
maternal fatigue, and development axis activity following extinction of
3. Scher A. Attachment and sleep: a of depressive symptomatology. Birth. infant crying responses induced
study of night waking in 12-month- 2005;32(3):187193 during the transition to sleep. Early
old infants. Dev Psychobiol. Hum Dev. 2012;88(4):227232
15. Hatton DC, Harrison-Hohner J, Coste
2001;38(4):274285
S, Dorato V, Curet LB, McCarron DA. 25. Blunden SL, Thompson KR, Dawson
4. Rottenberg J, Bylsma LM, Vingerhoets Symptoms of postpartum depression D. Behavioural sleep treatments
AJJM. Is crying benecial? Curr Dir and breastfeeding. J Hum Lact. and night time crying in infants:
Psychol Sci. 2008;17:400404 2005;21(4):444449, quiz 450454 challenging the status quo. Sleep Med
Rev. 2011;15(5):327334
5. Weinraub M, Bender RH, Friedman 16. Fairbrother N, Woody SR. New mothers
SL, et al. Patterns of developmental thoughts of harm related to the 26. Borbly AA. A two process model of
change in infants nighttime newborn. Arch Women Ment Health. sleep regulation. Hum Neurobiol.
sleep awakenings from 6 through 2008;11(3):221229 1982;1(3):195204
36 months of age. Dev Psychol.
17. Jennings KD, Ross S, Popper 27. Achermann P, Borbly AA.
2012;48(6):15111528
S, Elmore M. Thoughts of harming Mathematical models of
6. Jenni OG, Deboer T, Achermann P. infants in depressed and sleep regulation. Front Biosci.
Development of the 24-h rest-activity nondepressed mothers. J Affect 2003;8:s683s693
pattern in human infants. Infant Behav Disord. 1999;54(12):2128
28. So K, Adamson TM, Horne RSC.
Dev. 2006;29(2):143152 18. Erikson L, Mazerolle P, Wortley R, The use of actigraphy for assessment
7. Joseph D, Chong NW, Shanks ME, Johnson H. Maternal and paternal of the development of sleep/wake
et al. Getting rhythm: how do babies licide: case studies from the patterns in infants during the rst
do it? Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. Australian Homicide Project. Child 12 months of life. J Sleep Res.
2015;100(1):F50F54 Abuse Review. 2016;25(1):1730 2007;16(2):181187
8. Martin J, Hiscock H, Hardy P, 19. Trautmann-Villalba P, Hornstein C. 29. Ferber R. Solve Your Childs Sleep
Davey B, Wake M. Adverse associations Children murdered by their mothers Problems. 2nd ed. New York, NY:
of infant and child sleep problems and in the postpartum period [in German]. Fireside; 2006
parent health: an Australian population Nervenarzt. 2007;78(11):12901295
30. Gunnar MR, White BP. Salivary
study. Pediatrics. 2007;119(5):947955 20. Bartels L, Patricia E. Mothers who kill: cortisol measures in infant and child
9. Wake M, Morton-Allen E, Poulakis Z, the forensic use and judicial reception assessment. In: Singer LT, Zeskind PS,
Hiscock H, Gallagher S, Oberklaid F. of evidence of postnatal depression eds. Biobehavioral Assessment of the
Prevalence, stability, and outcomes and other psychiatric disorders in Infant. New York, NY: Guilford Press;
of cry-fuss and sleep problems in Australian licide cases. Melbourne 2001:167189
the rst 2 years of life: prospective University Law Review 2013;15:37.
Available at: http://www5.austlii.edu. 31. Salimetrics. Inter- and intra-assay
community-based study. Pediatrics.
au/au/journals/MelbULawRw/2013/15. coefcients of variability. Salimetrics,
2006;117(3):836842
html. Accessed February 24, 2015 USA. Available at: www.salimetrics.
10. Banks S, Dinges DF. Behavioral and com/assets/documents/Spit_Tips_-_
21. Mindell JA, Kuhn B, Lewin DS, Meltzer
physiological consequences of Inter__Intra_Assay_Coefcients_of_
LJ, Sadeh A; American Academy of
sleep restriction. J Clin Sleep Med. Variability.pdf. Accessed September 4,
Sleep Medicine. Behavioral treatment
2007;3(5):519528 2015
of bedtime problems and night
11. Johnson CM. Infant and toddler sleep: wakings in infants and young children. 32. Ainsworth MDS, Biehar MC, Waters
a telephone survey of parents in one Sleep. 2006;29(10):12631276 E, Wall S. Patterns of Attachment:
community. J Dev Behav Pediatr. A Psychological Study of the Strange
22. Morgenthaler TI, Owens J, Alessi C,
1991;12(2):108114 Situation. New York, NY: Psychology
et al; American Academy of Sleep
Press; 1978
12. Korsch BM, Christian JB, Gozzi Medicine. Practice parameters for
EK, Carlson PV. Infant care and behavioral treatment of bedtime 33. Piazza CC, Fisher WW. Bedtime
punishment: a pilot study. Am problems and night wakings in fading in the treatment of pediatric

Downloaded from by guest on September 13, 2017


PEDIATRICS Volume 137, number 6, June 2016 9
insomnia. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. their parents? Sleep Med Rev. sleep intervention: randomized trial.
1991;22(1):5356 2011;15(5):335337 Pediatrics. 2012;130(4):643651
34. Piazza CC, Fisher WW, Sherer M. 38. Larson MC, White BP, Cochran A, 42. South Australian Coroners Court.
Treatment of multiple sleep problems Donzella B, Gunnar M. Dampening Finding of Inquest (Inquest Number
in children with developmental of the cortisol response to 25/2009). Adelaide, Australia: State
disabilities: faded bedtime with handling at 3 months in human Coroners Court; 2010
response cost versus bedtime infants and its relation to sleep,
scheduling. Dev Med Child Neurol. circadian cortisol activity, and 43. Brandon M, Bailey S, Belderson P,
1997;39(6):414418 behavioral distress. Dev Psychobiol. Larsson B. The role of neglect in child
35. Jones CHD, Owens JA, Pham 1998;33(4):327337 fatality and serious injury. Child Abuse
B. Can a brief educational 39. Lewis M, Ramsay DS. Developmental Review. 2014;23(4):235245
intervention improve parents change in infants responses to stress.
44. Kemp JS, Unger B, Wilkins D, et al.
knowledge of healthy childrens Child Dev. 1995;66(3):657670
sleep? A pilot test. Health Educ J. Unsafe sleep practices and an analysis
40. Ivars K, Nelson N, Theodorsson A, of bedsharing among infants dying
2013;72:601610 Theodorsson E, Strm JO, Mrelius
suddenly and unexpectedly: results
36. Sadeh A. Assessment of intervention E. Development of salivary cortisol
for infant night waking: parental circadian rhythm and reference of a four-year, population-based,
reports and activity-based home intervals in full-term infants. PLoS One. death-scene investigation study of
monitoring. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2015;10(6):e0129502 sudden infant death syndrome and
1994;62(1):6368 41. Price AMH, Wake M, Ukoumunne OC, related deaths. Pediatrics. 2000;106(3).
37. Sadeh A, Mindell JA, Owens J. Why Hiscock H. Five-year follow-up of harms Available at: www.pediatrics.org/cgi/
care about sleep of infants and and benets of behavioral infant content/full/106/3/E41

Downloaded from by guest on September 13, 2017


10 GRADISAR et al
Behavioral Interventions for Infant Sleep Problems: A Randomized Controlled
Trial
Michael Gradisar, Kate Jackson, Nicola J. Spurrier, Joyce Gibson, Justine Whitham,
Anne Sved Williams, Robyn Dolby and David J. Kennaway
Pediatrics; originally published online May 24, 2016;
DOI: 10.1542/peds.2015-1486
Updated Information & including high resolution figures, can be found at:
Services /content/early/2016/05/21/peds.2015-1486.full
Supplementary Material Supplementary material can be found at:
/content/suppl/2016/05/18/peds.2015-1486.DCSupplemental.
html
References This article cites 38 articles, 7 of which can be accessed free
at:
/content/early/2016/05/21/peds.2015-1486.full.html#ref-list-1

Subspecialty Collections This article, along with others on similar topics, appears in
the following collection(s):
Psychiatry/Psychology
/cgi/collection/psychiatry_psychology_sub
Sleep Medicine
/cgi/collection/sleep_medicine_sub
Permissions & Licensing Information about reproducing this article in parts (figures,
tables) or in its entirety can be found online at:
/site/misc/Permissions.xhtml
Reprints Information about ordering reprints can be found online:
/site/misc/reprints.xhtml

PEDIATRICS is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly


publication, it has been published continuously since 1948. PEDIATRICS is owned, published,
and trademarked by the American Academy of Pediatrics, 141 Northwest Point Boulevard, Elk
Grove Village, Illinois, 60007. Copyright 2016 by the American Academy of Pediatrics. All
rights reserved. Print ISSN: 0031-4005. Online ISSN: 1098-4275.

Downloaded from by guest on September 13, 2017


Behavioral Interventions for Infant Sleep Problems: A Randomized Controlled
Trial
Michael Gradisar, Kate Jackson, Nicola J. Spurrier, Joyce Gibson, Justine Whitham,
Anne Sved Williams, Robyn Dolby and David J. Kennaway
Pediatrics; originally published online May 24, 2016;
DOI: 10.1542/peds.2015-1486

The online version of this article, along with updated information and services, is
located on the World Wide Web at:
/content/early/2016/05/21/peds.2015-1486.full

PEDIATRICS is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly


publication, it has been published continuously since 1948. PEDIATRICS is owned,
published, and trademarked by the American Academy of Pediatrics, 141 Northwest Point
Boulevard, Elk Grove Village, Illinois, 60007. Copyright 2016 by the American Academy
of Pediatrics. All rights reserved. Print ISSN: 0031-4005. Online ISSN: 1098-4275.

Downloaded from by guest on September 13, 2017

Вам также может понравиться