Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 15

Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment 40 (2011) 150 164

Regular article

Predictors of addiction treatment providers' beliefs in the disease and


choice models of addiction
Christopher Russell, (M.Sc.), John B. Davies, (Ph.D., F.B.Ps.S.), Simon C. Hunter, (Ph.D.)
School of Psychological Sciences and Health, University of Strathclyde, Glasgow, G1 1QE Scotland, UK

Received 8 January 2010; received in revised form 16 September 2010; accepted 22 September 2010

Abstract

Addiction treatment providers working in the United States (n = 219) and the United Kingdom (n = 372) were surveyed about their beliefs
in the disease and choice models of addiction, as assessed by the 18-item Addiction Belief Scale of J. Schaler (1992). Factor analysis of item
scores revealed a three-factor structure, labeled addiction is a disease, addiction is a choice, and addiction is a way of coping with life,
and factor scores were analyzed in separate hierarchical multiple regression analyses. Controlling for demographic and addiction history
variables, treatment providers working in the United States more strongly believe addiction is a disease, whereas U.K.-based providers more
strongly believe that addiction is a choice and a way of coping with life. Beliefs that addiction is a disease were stronger among those who
provide for-profit treatment, have stronger spiritual beliefs, have had a past addiction problem, are older, are members of a group of addiction
professionals, and have been treating addiction longer. Conversely, those who viewed addiction as a choice were more likely to provide
public/not-for-profit treatment, be younger, not belong to a group of addiction professionals, and have weaker spiritual beliefs. Additionally,
treatment providers who have had a personal addiction problem in the past were significantly more likely to believe addiction is a disease the
longer they attend a 12-stepbased group and if they are presently abstinent. 2011 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Addiction; Treatment providers; Beliefs; Disease; Choice

1. Introduction which undermines their values and best intentions (White,


2001). Regardless of the scientific credibility of the disease
The question what is addiction? has long polarized the and choice (or free will) models, research has shown that
medical, social science, legal, and spiritual communities into clients of addiction services tend to adopt the addiction
those who view addiction as a disease (Benowitz, 2008; ideology of their treatment service (Koski-Jannes, 2004).
Jellinek, 1960; Ketcham, Asbury, Schulstad & Ciaramicoli, Therefore, the extent to which addiction treatment providers
2000; Kalivas & Volkow, 2005; Koob & Nestler, 1997; believe their clients' addictive behaviors are diseased or
Leshner, 1997; Lyvers, 1998; Maltzman, 1994; Vaillant, chosen should be expected to have a strong bearing on how
1990) and those who view addiction as a cognizant choice clients will attribute the causes of their problems, seek to
(Fingarette, 1988a, 1988b; Heyman, 2009; Merry, 1966; resolve these problems, and believe in their capacity to
Szasz, 1972; Playfair, 1991; Room, 1983; Schaler, 2000). achieve a desired change. Extending research by Schaler
Many professional and lay conceptions of addiction can be (1992), we examined addiction treatment providers' beliefs
traced back to this dichotomy in causationdrug-addicted about addiction and investigated the factors explaining
individuals are either responsible/moral agents who perpe- variance in beliefs, with a specific interest in the importance
trate acts of mayhem on themselvesor victims of a disorder of country in which treatment is provided.

1.1. Dichotomous and trichotomous thinking about addiction


Corresponding author. School of Psychological Sciences and Health,
University of Strathclyde, 40 George Street, Glasgow, G1 1QE Scotland, UK. The disease and choice models of addiction are not the
E-mail address: christopher.russell@strath.ac.uk (C. Russell). only perspectives of addiction in existence; they are only the

0740-5472/10/$ see front matter 2011 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jsat.2010.09.006
C. Russell et al. / Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment 40 (2011) 150164 151

two chosen for scrutiny in this study. Several other become addicted to drugs and alcohol, their capacity for
perspectives of addictionsuch as an illness, disorder, control during consumption, and their prospects for change
malady, allergy, ailment, sickness, condition, habit, func- without medical treatment. That the different sets of
tional attribution, and social construction among others fundamental assumptions driving each model are philosoph-
can be viewed as implicitly ascribing or alluding to the ically irreconcilable also necessitates, we argue, proponents
respective disease/choice model assumptions about addic- of one model to be equally passionate critics of the other.
tion as a compelled versus chosen act, an involuntary versus The disease model describes addiction/substance depen-
voluntary act, and a problem inherent to the drug versus a dency as a primary, progressive, chronic relapsing disease
problem inherent to the mind of the user. Alternatively, some that is either genetically transmitted or acquired through
theorists refute the suggestion that addiction can be fit to a excessive consumption (Leshner, 1997; Ketcham et al.,
diseasechoice dichotomy, arguing addiction to be a 2000). Here, initial drug use occurs voluntarily. As repeated
complex, messy intertwining of the user's biology and drug use changes neural and brain function, however, the
sociology that subsumes elements of the disease and choice user progressively loses control over their initial voluntary
model without contradiction. behavior to the point that further drug seeking and use
Consequently, a diseaseintermediatechoice trichotomy become acts of compulsion, not choice (Ochoa, 1994; Foulds
has emerged. White's (2001) degrees of freedom perspec- & Ghodse, 1995). Thus, getting drug users who are in the
tive, for example, argues addiction as a process disease early or latter stages of an addiction into treatment with
should be discussed not in terms of complete control or medical experts often represents their best hope for arresting
complete loss of control, but in terms of degrees of but never curing the addiction (Milam & Ketcham, 1983). In
diminishment and enhancement of volitional control. The response to criticism, however, that a large body of scientific
problem with an intermediate perspective, however, is that it evidence on alcoholism and alcohol problems has contra-
must logically presume there exists a critical, discrete point dicted the view of addiction as an incurable, unitary, all-or-
along the freedom continuum at which drug use becomes no nothing disorder caused solely by hereditary physical
longer governed by phenomenological wants but by abnormalities (Miller, 1993, p. 133), a more scientifically
physiological needs. This tipping point has survived as a defensible disease model has been sought in recent years.
core hypothesis of 19th-century disease conceptualizations Miller proposed that research, treatment, and education
of inebrietyJoseph Parrish suggested in 1888 that a line about alcoholism should be based on a disease model that
could be crossed where drunkenness evolves into a disease describes alcohol problems on continua of severity and an
that is no longer under the conscious control of the drinker etiological model comprising interactions of drug properties,
(cited by White, 2000)through to the modern disease drug user, and drug setting.
conceptthe non-addicted brain is distinctly different from The alternative model describes addiction as a motivated
the addicted brainA metaphorical switch in the brain seems choice. Here, drug taking is at all times something
to be thrown as a result of prolonged drug use. Initially, drug individuals do voluntarily, usually when life is going badly
use is voluntary, but when that switch is thrown, the or to avoid coping with problems in living (Schaler, 2000).
individual moves into the state of addiction characterized by When these problems in living are resolved, individuals
compulsive drug seeking and use (Leshner, 1997, p. 46).1 normally find that the addiction resolves with them, while
To create an intermediate perspective would therefore be other individuals mature out of their addiction in time (Peele,
redundant for the purposes of asking whether drug seeking Brodsky, & Arnold, 1991) or learn to control their
and use are willed or determined. Thus, although the validity consumption (Heather & Robertson, 1989). In this way,
of a trichotomous model of the governing factors in addiction is seen as more to do with the environments people
addiction and the mechanisms of change at the boundary live in than with brain pharmacology (Alexander, Hadaway,
of each state will continue to be debated, this study was & Coambs, 1980; Cohen, Liebson, Faillace, & Allen, 1971;
concerned only with treatment providers' beliefs in the Robins, Helzer & Davis, 1975). With regard to the issue of
disease and choice models. control, choice proponents argue that not only do drug users
never lose control over their drug use but that the best way to
1.2. The disease and choice models curb problem drug use is to make and implement better
decisions, which does not require them to seek medical
The disease and choice models of addiction emerged from treatment. Choice proponents tend to allow discussion of
different assumptions about the origins of behavior; namely, addiction as a metaphorical disease but refute that it is a
whether behavior is determined by physical mechanism or literal brain disease (McMurran, 1994). They note that a
willed by an emergent force that transcends direct physical large body of scientific evidence contradicts disease model
mechanism (Davies, 1997). Consequently, they hold diver- claims regarding heritability, loss of control, and effective-
gent but equally powerful assumptions about how people ness of treatment, and they denounce the disease model's
inference of a critical discrete event discriminating addicted
1
Allan I. Leshner was the director of National Institute of Drug Abuse and nonaddicted drug users' as myth. They argue that drug
(NIDA) at time of publication. users are always free to choose to stop and that drug users'
152 C. Russell et al. / Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment 40 (2011) 150164

difficulty in effecting change should not be mistaken for a physiology, not psychology, determines whether one drinker
lack of freedom to do so. will become addicted and another will not) to the Big Book
of AA (Alcoholics Anonymous World Services, 1939), the
1.3. Milestones in the evolution of the disease model fellowship's core publication. Alcoholics Anonymous,
through its public relations campaigns, has been instrumen-
Treatment providers' support for each of the competing tal in spreading and popularizing the disease concept of
models may vary depending on whether treatment is alcoholism while avoiding discourse of alcoholism as a
provided in the United States or not. Although the idea of literal disease.
alcoholism as a disease did not originate in the United States, Addiction as a disease in the United States gained
the modern disease concept of alcoholism has been 200 years momentum in the mid-1990s with significant increases in
in the making2, during which time the United States has public funding of research into the genetic and neurobio-
presided over the most significant events in changing public logical foundations of addiction (Institute of Medicine
conceptions of drunkenness and drug use from voluntary [IOM], 1996). This research agenda, accompanied by a
choices to involuntary compulsions (Levine, 1978; Peele, public education campaign that used a basic vocabulary to
1989). Benjamin Rush first medicalized the problem of teach a basic level of understanding about brain reward
drunkenness in the early 19th century, his definition of a circuitry, sought to move addiction is a disease from the
disease of the will becoming a central message of the status of an ideological proclamation by policy activists and
American Temperance Movement (Levine, 1978). an organizing metaphor for individuals seeking to resolve
The term inebriety was introduced in the late 19th century alcohol and other drug problems to a science-grounded
to explain the seeking and problem use of a variety of drugs conclusion (White, 2007). In recent years, former and
as due to a common underlying pathology. Interest turned to current directors of the National Institute of Drug Abuse,
the effects of drugs' effects on the host, and doctors began to Alan Leshner (1997) and Nora Volkow, respectively, have
hypothesize that the inebriate's apparent loss of control and used high-profile, highly respected academic outlets to
other symptoms could be traced to rogue hereditary and/or summarize 20 years of evidence from neurosciences and
self-impaired biological mechanisms that mark a primary behavioral sciences, which they claim prove addiction is a
disease of the nervous system. In particular, the work of the brain disease. Leshner, additionally, called for public policy,
Drs. Parrish (1883) and Crothers (1893)prominent leaders education, and addiction treatment to catch up with these
of the American Association for the Cure of Inebriety scientific facts. Volkow's keynote speech to the Annual
described inebriety as a disease that is curable in the sense Conference of the American Psychiatric Association in 2007
that other diseases are curable and as inherited or acquired followed on from a special issue of Nature Neuroscience
through excessive consumption. This disease concept of (multiple authors, 2005) in which a group of renowned
inebriety began a movement to treat inebriates at specialized neuroscientists reported the latest evidence on the neurobi-
institutions in medical and scientific ways similar to other ology of addiction. Their findings described addiction as a
diseases (i.e., through the development of vaccines). During fundamentally neurobiological disorder.
this period, Dr. Norman Kerr (1888) was advocating a Finally, as a vehicle for the dissemination of this new
comparable disease concept in England. Consequently, neuroscientific evidence base to the public, the IOM (1996)
disease thinking about inebriety soon spread throughout recommended that education about addiction should explain
the United States and United Kingdom. in basic language that drugs can alter neural or brain
Public thinking about the disease of drunkenness took off function, how these changes impair the user's ability to make
in 1935 with the inception and rapid growth of Alcoholics choices about using drugs, and that treatment is effective.
Anonymous (AA), a spiritual self-help fellowship made up The brain hijacking metaphorthe concept that these
of self-described recovering alcohol-dependent individuals drugs can capture control of brain mechanisms that control
committed to helping one another maintain sobriety (Kurtz, motivations and emotionswas proposed as an effective
1988). Although AA literature does not refer to alcoholism device to increase understanding about a common effect of
as a literal disease, Kurtz (2002) states that AA and members some drugs on the brain. This device has since featured
of AA do use medical termsillness, sickness, malady prominently in major media pieces such as a TIME Magazine
and the disease concept to reflect their belief about the (2007) feature article entitled, How We Get Addicted and a
solution to alcoholismabstinenceand to convey the 14-part NIDA-funded TV special by HBO Documentary
hopelessness of alcohol-dependent individuals to change Films (2007) entitled, Addiction: Why Can't They Just Stop?
themselves. Ragge (1998), for example, traces seven Today, the American Medical Association (Morse &
features of the modern disease concept of alcoholism (e.g., Flavin, 1992), American Psychiatric Association (2000), and
beliefs that an intense physical craving is responsible for NIDA (2009) continue to define as the essence of addiction
alcohol-dependent individuals' loss of control and that uncontrollable, compulsive drug seeking and use. Conse-
quently, the use of the word addiction in public discourse
2
Levine (1978) and White (2000) provide a comprehensive history of has come to describe the activities that people engage in
the disease concept of alcoholism. because they are physically unable to avoid doing so
C. Russell et al. / Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment 40 (2011) 150164 153

(Levine, 1978; Mercadante, 1996; Schaler, 2000). Although beliefs explained by demographic and personal and
the disease model now dominates addiction discourse professional addiction history variables.
internationally, the prominent role played by the United
States' psychiatric, medical, research, media, and spiritual
communities in shaping the modern disease concept of 2. Materials and method
addictive behaviors suggests that support for the view of
addiction as a disease may be stronger within the U.S. versus 2.1. Recruitment methods
nonU.S. treatment communities.
Treatment providers were recruited in three ways. First,
1.4. Previous research a survey pack was sent to designated persons at each of
the 21 and 94 regional Drug and Alcohol Action Teams
Investigation of these questions was motivated by Schaler (DAATs) in Scotland and England, respectively. As part of
(1992), who found that treatment providers tended to believe regional National Health Service Health Boards, DAATs
that addiction is a disease from which only about 25% of are responsible for the topdown and bottomup commu-
people recover without medical or 12-stepbased treatment. nication of substance misuse-related data between addic-
Treatment providers who reported stronger beliefs that tion treatment services and local and central government
addiction is a disease were significantly more likely to be and, therefore, have excellent access to voluntary and
women, members of the National Association for Alcohol- statutory addiction treatment providers within their region.
ism and Drug Abuse Counselors (NAADAC), present or past Drug and Alcohol Action Teams were asked to forward
members of AA, certified addiction counselors/therapists, the survey pack to managers of local addiction treatment
abstinent at present, and have stronger spiritual beliefs as services. In turn, managers were asked to forward the
defined in AA philosophy. Strength of spiritual beliefs, as survey to all staff who are directly involved in the
measured by the Spiritual Belief Scale (SBS; Schaler, 1992) provision of addiction treatment.
accounted for most variance (41%) in disease beliefs. Second, survey packs were sent via e-mail to 785 persons
However, although Schaler's sample comprised treatment who could be identified as chief executive officers/managers
providers working in the United States, Canada, and of addiction treatment services on the Web sites of several
Australia, differences across these locations were not large associations and online databases of addiction
investigated. This precludes offering conclusions about treatment professionals. These were NAADAC, the Associ-
support for the disease model in the United States relative ation for Addiction Professionals, Federation of Drug and
to out-with the United States and the hypothesized Alcohol Professionals (a U.K. branch of NAADAC),
significance of the country of treatment as a predictor of European Federation of Therapeutic Communities, Europe-
addiction beliefs. Data were also not collected on the profit an Association for the Treatment of Addiction, Association
status of treatment provided. of Intervention Specialists, Recover Now, Time for New
Furthermore, there are a number of reasons to suspect that Beginnings, Sober Recovery, Addiction Treatment Center
addiction beliefs may have changed since 1992. These Directory, Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services
include high staff turnover rates, new pharmacological and Administration, Substance Abuse Treatment Facility Locator
psychotherapeutic treatment approaches to addiction, policy (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services), and
changes regarding public funding of addiction treatment and Alcohol Focus Scotland. Treatment providers typically
insurance coverage, new laboratory and field evidence on provide contact information for public viewing on these
treatment effectiveness including the much publicized Web sites for the benefit of persons seeking help for an
findings of Project MATCH (1997) and UK Alcohol addiction problem, although they are assumed to not oppose
Treatment Trial (2005), the aforementioned U.S.-led re- being contacted in this way by other interested parties.
search drive to emphasize the neuronal mechanisms and Indeed, no treatment providers objected to being contacted in
heritability of addiction, and the transmission of the basic this way and many were quite happy to know they were
facts of addiction neuroscience to the public, policy makers, accessible in this way.
and treatment providers. Thus, we examined whether a Third, survey packs were circulated to subscribers of the
similar factorial structure emerged from ABS scores and following e-newsletter mailing lists: (1) U.K. lists: Alcohol
whether factors found by Schaler to explain variance remain Misuse, Drug Misuse Research, Drug Day Programmes,
potent 18 years on. Wired In, and Therapeutic Community Open Forum; (2) U.
S./Canadian lists: Addict-L, Addiction Medicine, and
1.5. Current study aims Apolnet; (3) European lists: Therapeutic Communities and
European Working Group on Drugs Oriented Research; and
The purpose of this study was to assess (a) whether belief (4) international lists: the Kettil Bruun Society and Gambling
in the disease model of addiction is stronger among treatment Issues International. These lists cover approximately 2,500
providers who work in the United States versus out-with the subscribers in total. The e-survey was closed 2 months after
United States and (b) the variance in disease and choice the final survey pack was sent out.
154 C. Russell et al. / Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment 40 (2011) 150164

2.2. Sample characteristics Table 1


Demographic and professional characteristics of addiction treatment
providers by country: count (percentage within country)
The survey received 854 responses. Of these, 164 were
excluded because the Addiction Belief Scale (ABS) was Variable United States United Kingdom Total
incomplete (n = 160) or because respondents were not N 219 372 591
providers of addiction treatment/were no longer actively Age, M (SD) 47.61 (10.60) 44.03 (10.68) 45.35 (10.78)
Sex
treating clients (n = 14). We had initially planned to compare
Male 95 (43.4) 145 (39.0) 240 (40.6)
the strength of disease beliefs in the United States versus Female 124 (56.6) 227 (61.0) 351 (59.4)
several countries. However, the majority of survey responses Profit status
came from treatment providers working in the United Private/for-profit 70 (32.0) 118 (31.7) 188 (31.8)
Kingdom (n = 372) and the United States (n = 219), with Public/not-for-profit 149 (68.0) 254 (68.3) 403 (68.2)
Years as treatment provider
the remaining 99 respondents representing 21 other
01 13 (5.9) 28 (7.6) 41 (7.0)
countries. Thus, comparing disease beliefs in the United 25 53 (24.2) 99 (26.8) 152 (25.8)
States with those in several countries was not possible and 610 50 (22.8) 104 (28.1) 154 (26.1)
creating a U.S.-versus-not-U.S. variable was considered 1115 36 (16.4) 59 (15.9) 95 (16.1)
misleading, given that 79% of the not U.S. respondents 1620 23 (10.5) 41 (11.2) 64 (10.9)
21+ 44 (20.1) 39 (10.5) 83 (14.1)
came from the United Kingdom. Purely due to the
Certified
geographical distribution of our sample, it was decided to Yes 115 (52.8) 111 (30.2) 226 (38.6)
exclude the 99 non-U.K. and non-U.S. respondents so as to No 103 (47.2) 256 (69.8) 359 (61.4)
compare the strength of disease beliefs of treatment Professional group member
providers working in the United States versus the United Yes 99 (45.4) 74 (20.3) 153 (31.4)
No 119 (54.6) 290 (79.7) 332 (68.6)
Kingdom. This left a final sample of 591. Due to the
Problems treated
opportunistic sampling method, it was impossible to Alcohol 191 (87.2) 320 (86.0) 511 (86.5)
calculate a survey return rate. Professional characteristics Illicit drugs 185 (84.5) 327 (87.9) 512 (86.6)
of the sample by country are summarized in Table 1. Prescription drugs 172 (78.5) 271 (72.8) 443 (75.0)
Nicotine/tobacco 84 (39.2) 64 (17.2) 148 (25.0)
Gambling 82 (37.4) 63 (16.9) 145 (24.5)
2.3. Materials
Video gaming 27 (12.3) 18 (4.8) 45 (7.6)
Sex/Pornography 39 (17.8) 26 (7.0) 65 (11.0)
Treatment providers were invited by e-mail to complete Food 34 (15.5) 29 (7.8) 63 (10.7)
an e-survey of their addiction beliefs. This e-mail gave a Shopping 28 (12.8) 19 (5.1) 47 (8.0)
brief explanation of the study, confirmed that local ethical Internet use 30 (13.7) 19 (5.1) 49 (8.3)
Treatment methods used
approval had been granted, assured respondents of confi-
Psychotherapeutic a 190 (88.0) 326 (91.6) 516 (90.2)
dentiality, and gave contact information for the primary and Pharmacotheraputic b 135 (62.5) 210 (59.0) 335 (60.3)
secondary authors. The survey comprised three parts: the 12-Step , c 99 (45.8) 125 (35.1) 224 (39.2)
ABS, the SBS, and questions about their personal and Note: Country Variable differences were tested using the 2 statistic for
professional addiction history. Other information collected categorical variables and an independent groups t test on the one
included age (in years) and sex. continuous variable (age).
a
Psychotherapeutic methods included reported use of at least one of
2.3.1. The Addiction Belief Scale cognitivebehavioral therapy, individual and group counseling, person
center therapy, motivational interviewing, biopsychoscoial models, stress
The 18 items of this scale comprise statements about
management, art therapy, equine therapy, family systems approach, couples
addiction as described in the disease (nine items) and choice therapy, occupational therapy, rational emotive therapy, emotion-focused
(nine items) models regarding etiology, the need for therapy, mindfulness, meditation, psychodynamic therapy, Jungian therapy,
treatment, and addicted individuals' capacity for self-control, Rogerian therapy, narrative therapy, systems theory, motivational enhance-
insofar as these assumptions can be dichotomized. An ment therapy, anger management therapy, nations healing, trauma therapy,
grief and loss therapy, acupuncture, gestalt therapy, humanistic therapy,
example of a statement that reflects the disease model is
stages of change approach, Bowinian approach, shiatsu, and yoga.
Physiology, not psychology, determines whether one b
Pharmacotherapeutic methods included reported use of substitute
drinker will become addicted to alcohol and another will prescribing to support maintenance, detoxification, reduction, or abstinence
not (item 11). An example of an item reflecting the choice (at least one of methadone, buprenorphine [Subutex], lofexidine,
model is People can stop relying on drugs or alcohol as they naltrexone, chlordiazepoxide, disulfiram, acamprosate, baclofen, thiamine,
benzodiazepine, disulfiram [Antabuse], acamprosate [Campral], diazepam,
develop new ways to deal with life (item 6). Respondents
and vitamin B).
rate on a five-point Likert scale the extent to which they c
12-Step methods included reported use of 12-Step Model, 12-Step
agree with each statement (1 = strongly disagree to 5 = Facilitation, Minnesota Model, and AA/NA model.
strongly agree) and the nine choice model items are reverse p b .05.
p b .01.
scored. The highest possible score is 90 (minimum = 18),
p b .001.
with a conceptual median of 54. A score higher or lower than
54 on the ABS indicates a belief in the disease or choice
C. Russell et al. / Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment 40 (2011) 150164 155

model of addiction, respectively. Schaler (1995) reported professional group of addiction treatment providers, whether
strong internal consistency for the ABS ( = .91, they are a certified counselor or therapist for treating an
standardized item, = .91, n = 266) and a three-factor addiction, and which types of addiction problems they treat.
structure described as power ( = .91, n = 274), Regarding personal addiction history, respondents were
dichotomous thinking ( = .83, n = 285), and addiction asked if they have personally had a problem with an
as a way of coping with life ( = .47, n = 286). High addiction in the past. If yes was indicated, they were then
construct validity was evidenced by a strong negative asked several follow-up questions: whether they have ever
correlation (r = .67, p = .01) between respondents' ABS attended a treatment agency in the past; whether they have
scores and their beliefs about the percentage of individuals attended in the past or presently do attend AA, Narcotics
able to recover from an addiction without any form of Anonymous (NA), or any other 12-stepbased program;
medical or 12-steptype treatment, that is, the stronger their number of years in total they have been a member of a 12-
belief in addiction as a disease (higher ABS score), the lower stepbased program; and whether they are abstinent at
the percentage of individuals they believed are able to present. Respondents who indicated no to the past
recover without treatment. The full ABS and factorial addiction problem question did not answer these five
analysis can be found in the work of Schaler (1995). follow-up questions. Finally, an empty text box at the end
Despite reporting a three-factor structure, Schaler initially of the survey allowed respondents to comment on the survey.
scored ABS items in accordance with a single factor, bipolar
in nature, with endorsement of the disease model at one end
and endorsement of the choice model at the other end. As 3. Results
such, the nine items designed to represent beliefs in the
choice model were reverse scored. To determine whether this 3.1. Power analysis
scoring system was appropriate for current data, a factor
analysis of current ABS data was conducted to check Power analyses were performed to determine whether the
whether addiction beliefs loaded on a single disease planned multiple regression analysis would be sufficiently
choice factor (and so choice items can be reverse scored and powered to detect meaningful effects (f2 for multiple
ABS total scores used as a dependent measure of belief in the regression, see Cohen, 1988) given a sample of 591. The
disease model) or whether addiction beliefs conform to a analysis showed that when N = 591 and = .05 and with 10
multifactorial structure. Results of this analysis are reported predictors, power = 1.00. Thus, it was concluded that the
in Section 3.2. present analysis was sufficiently powered.

2.3.2. The Spiritual Belief Scale 3.2. Factor analysis of the ABS
The eight items of this scale measure spiritual thinking as
defined in the philosophy of AA as belief in a metaphysical Separate factor analyses were conducted to compare the
power that can influence personal experience. Items were ABS factor structures for the U.K.- (n = 372) and U.S.-based
adapted from how spirituality is discussed in the Big Book of (n = 219) samples. Despite the ABS's apparent bipolar
AA to form statements about God and spiritual health. content on a single dimension of addiction beliefs, extremely
Items reflect the four spiritual characteristics of AA similar four-factor solutions were found for each country.
release, gratitude, humility, and toleranceidentified by Within the U.K. sample, five disease items (1 [.73], 2 [.75], 3
Kurtz (1988). Respondents rate on a five-point Likert scale [.76], 5 [.61], and 10 [.57]) loaded on factor 1; four disease
the extent to which they agree with each statement (1 = items (9 [.53], 11 [.57], 14 [.68], and 17 [.49]) and one choice
strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree). Higher scores item (12 [.66]) loaded on factor 1; six choice items (4 [.66],
indicate stronger spiritual beliefs. The highest possible score 7 [.60], 8 [.50], 13 [.54], 15 [.61], and 16 [.48]) loaded on
is 40 (minimum = 8). Schaler (1996) reported strong internal factor 3; and two choice items (6 [.70], 18 [.62]) loaded on
consistency for the SBS ( = .92, standardized item, = .91, factor 4. The only differences in the composition of the four
n = 280) and a two-factor structure described as release factors extracted from U.S.-based scores were that item 12
gratitudehumility (six items, = .95, n = 281) and switched from being negatively correlated with disease items
tolerance (two items, = .53, n = 290). The full SBS and in factor 2 to strongly positively correlated with the choice
factorial analysis can be found in the work of Schaler (1996). items in factor 3 and items 13 and 15 switched from factor 3
(all choice items) to factor 4 (all choice items).
2.3.3. Addiction history questions Given these extremely similar factor solutions found in
Questions regarding respondents' professional addiction separate analyses, all ABS scores (N = 591) were factor
history were whether they are an addiction treatment analyzed using varimax rotation with Kaiser normalization.
provider, job title, the country and state/county in which Three factors were extracted, which we labeled addiction is
they provide treatment; the profit status of their treatment a disease, addiction is a choice, and addiction is a way of
facility, number of years experience as an addiction coping with life, respectively. These factors together
treatment provider, whether they are a member of any explained 50.13% of common variance. Factor 1 had an
156 C. Russell et al. / Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment 40 (2011) 150164

eigenvalue of 6.08 and explained 33.80% of variance. Ten Table 2


items loaded on this factor, nine of which were designed to Correlation matrix and internal consistency values () for ABS subscales
and ABS total score
represent the disease model of addiction3 (items 1 [.67; 2
[.74], 3 [.66], 5 [.70], 9 [.72], 10 [.68], 14 [.65], and 17 [.66]) Factor 1 a Factor 2 b Factor 3 c ABS total d
and one item designed to represent the choice model of Factor 1 .43 .21 .76
addiction (12: Alcoholics can learn to moderate their Factor 2 .36 .22
Factor 3 . .24
drinking or cut down on their drug use), which was strongly
ABS
negatively correlated (.55). Disagreement with item 12
implied the belief that alcohol-dependent individual are
a
Cronbach's = .79, 10 items, N = 591 (addiction is a disease).
b
Cronbach's = .71, 6 items, N = 591 (addiction is a choice).
unable to learn to moderate drinking/drug use, which is c
Cronbach's = .54, 2 items, N = 591 (addiction is a way of coping
consistent with the disease perspective. The item with the with life).
strongest correlation value reads, Addicts cannot control d
Cronbach's = .67, 18 items, N = 591.
themselves when they drink or take drugs. p b .001.
Factor 2 had an eigenvalue of 1.83 and explained 10.19%.
Six of the remaining eight items designed to represent the = yes), member of a group of addiction treatment profes-
choice model loaded on this factor (items 4 [.58], 7 [.72], sionals (0 = no, 1 = yes), had a personal addiction problem in
8 [.50], 13 [.54], 15 [.62], and 16 [.54]). The item with the the past (0 = no, 1 = yes), the profit status of treatment
strongest correlation value reads, Addiction has more to do provision (0 = public/not-for-profit, 1 = private/for-profit),
with the environments people live in than the drugs they are and SBS score. The country in which treatment is provided
addicted to. (0 = United Kingdom, 1 = United States) was added at step 2.
Factor 3 had an eigenvalue of 1.11 and explained 6.14% Finally, to assess any moderation of an effect of profit status
of variance. The two remaining choice model items (6 [.65] on ABS score by country, an interaction term for profit status
and 18 [.69]) loaded on this factor. The item with the and country was regressed on ABS score at step 3. Data
strongest correlation reads, Drug addiction is a way of life satisfied assumptions of linearity, multicollinearity, and
people rely on to cope with the world. However, the homoscedasticity of residuals. Mean scores and standard
overall pattern mirrored that revealed by the factor analysis deviations for the three ABS factors are presented in Table 3.
of the entire sample: Items designed to represent the disease
and choice models correlated positively with their own kind 3.4. Variables explaining variance in treatment providers'
and correlated negatively with items representing the beliefs that addiction is a disease
alternative model.
Therefore, scores were summed for each of the three The final regression model accounted for 35.6% of
factors extracted by the main factor analysis (addiction is a variance in treatment providers' beliefs in the disease model
disease, addiction is a choice, and addiction is a way of of addiction (see Table 4). Step 1 produced a significant
coping with life) and used as criterion variables in model, F(8, 565) = 26.47, p b .001, and accounted for 27.3%
subsequent regression analyses. These factors had maximum of variance in factor 1 scores. Six of the eight variables made
scores of 50, 30, and 10, respectively, with higher scores significant contributions.
reflecting stronger beliefs in each factor. Each factor had Score on the SBS and age were both positively associated
good to very good internal consistency and correlated ( = .40, p b .001, and = .15, p b .001, respectively) with
strongly with each other, as shown in Table 2. ABS score. These indicate that belief in the disease model
strengthens with level of spiritual thinking and with age.
3.3. Hierarchical multiple regression models Providing private/for-profit treatment was positively associ-
ated ( = .10, p b .01) with factor 1 score. Those who
To investigate the variables that explain variance in provide addiction treatment for-profit more strongly believe
addiction treatment providers' beliefs about addiction, three (M = 28.96, SD = 6.53) that addiction is a disease than those
separate hierarchical multiple linear regression analyses were who provide public/not-for-profit treatment (M = 26.39, SD
conducted, each in three steps, with score on factor 1 = 6.15). Being a member of a professional group of addiction
(addiction is a disease), factor 2 (addiction is a choice), treatment providers was positively associated ( = .11, p b
and factor 3 (addiction is a way of coping with life) of the .01) with factor 1 score. Professional group members more
ABS used, respectively, as the criterion variables. To control strongly believe (M = 29.61, SD = 6.09) that addiction is a
for their effects, eight variables were entered at step one of disease than nonmembers (M = 26.27, SD = 6.17). Number
each regression equation: sex (0 = male, 1 = female), age, of years of experience as a treatment provider was positively
number of years as an addiction treatment provider, associated with factor 1 score ( = .13, p b .01), with disease
certification as an addiction treatment provider (0 = no, 1 beliefs strongest (M = 27.60, SD = 7.39) among those who
have provided addiction treatment for the longest (21+
years). Finally, having had a personal problem with
3
Correlation values in brackets. addiction in the past was positively associated ( = .10,
C. Russell et al. / Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment 40 (2011) 150164 157

Table 3
Mean scores and standard deviations for the three factors of the ABS
Variable n Disease Choice Way of coping with life
Sex
Male 269 27.63 (6.72) 16.83 (3.63) 6.76 (1.47)
Female 322 26.86 (6.07) 17.05 (3.45) 7.17 (1.49)
Country
United Kingdom 372 24.97 (5.77) 17.96 (3.22) 7.40 (1.35)
United States 219 31.02 (5.53) 15.24 (3.39) 6.28 (1.48)
Profit status
Private/for-profit 188 28.97 (6.53) 15.85 (3.46) 6.55 (1.75)
Public/not-for-profit 403 26.39 (6.15) 17.47 (3.45) 7.19 (1.31)
United States Profit Status
Private/for-profit 70 33.96 (4.54) 13.13 (2.55) 5.41 (1.35)
Public/not-for-profit 149 29.64 (5.43) 16.23 (3.28) 6.68 (1.36)
United Kingdom Profit Status
Private/for-profit 118 26.02 (5.68) 17.46 (2.88) 7.22 (1.62)
Public/not-for-profit 254 24.49 (5.75) 18.19 (3.35) 7.48 (1.20)
Years treating addiction problems
01 41 27.15 (6.35) 16.83 (3.60) 7.17 (1.58)
25 152 27.32 (6.35) 17.17 (3.76) 7.21 (1.56)
610 154 27.19 (6.34) 17.64 (3.11) 7.12 (1.34)
1115 95 27.20 (5.83) 16.29 (3.09) 6.81 (1.45)
1620 64 27.42 (6.25) 16.39 (3.44) 6.53 (1.74)
21+ 83 27.60 (7.39) 16.47 (4.14) 6.76 (1.32)
Member of professional group
Yes 173 29.61 (6.09) 15.70 (3.23) 6.59 (1.49)
No 409 26.27 (6.17) 17.46 (3.50) 7.16 (1.47)
Past addiction problem
Yes 199 29.15 (6.39) 16.06 (3.79) 6.76 (1.63)
No 392 26.27 (6.17) 17.40 (3.31) 7.09 (1.41)
Attended treatment in the past a
Yes 135 29.69 (6.45) 15.70 (3.71) 6.70 (1.65)
No 64 28.02 (6.17) 16.81 (3.87) 6.91 (1.59)
Attended 12-step group in the past a
Yes 145 30.17 (6.29) 15.37 (3.69) 6.61 (1.68)
No 54 26.41 (5.89) 17.93 (3.45) 7.17 (1.44)
Attend 12-step group at present a
Yes 94 31.60 (5.57) 14.40 (3.44) 6.46 (1.75)
No 105 26.96 (6.31) 17.54 (3.48) 7.04 (1.47)
Years as member of 12-step group a
0 10 24.40 (4.86) 17.30 (3.13) 7.50 (1.08)
01 63 25.70 (5.91) 18.40 (3.31) 7.21 (1.45)
25 10 24.40 (6.93) 18.00 (3.89) 6.90 (2.18)
610 17 30.88 (5.94) 14.88 (4.23) 7.12 (1.76)
1115 20 30.70 (5.55) 16.35 (3.94) 6.95 (1.54)
1620 23 32.13 (4.70) 14.39 (2.39) 6.48 (1.28)
2125 13 32.00 (5.40) 14.77 (2.20) 6.15 (1.68)
25+ 42 32.83 (5.07) 13.38 (2.96) 6.00 (1.75)
Abstinent at present a
Yes 145 29.90 (6.32) 15.61 (3.69) 6.69 (1.64)
No 53 23.78 (3.74) 19.39 (2.81) 7.35 (1.50)
Note: The highest possible scores for factor 1 = 50, factor 2 = 30, and factor 3 = 10. Higher scores on each factor reflect stronger addiction beliefs.
a
Question answered only by the 199 respondents who indicated they have had a personal addiction problem in the past.
p b .05.
p b .01.
p b .001.

p b .05) with factor 1 score. Those who have had a personal 564) = 68.55, p b .001. After partialing out variance
addiction problem more strongly believe (M = 29.15, SD = explained by variables in step 1, providing addiction
6.39) that addiction is a disease than those who have not had treatment in the United States was positively associated (
an addiction problem (M = 26.23, SD = 6.16). = .32, p b .001) with factor 1 score. Providers of addiction
Step 2 in the model accounted for a significant increase treatment in the United States more strongly believe (M =
of 8.0% explained variance in factor 1 scores, Fchange(1, 31.02, SD = 5.53) in the disease model of addiction than
158 C. Russell et al. / Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment 40 (2011) 150164

Table 4
Separate hierarchical multiple linear regression analyses using scores on three factors extracted from the ABS as criterion variables
Disease Choice Way of coping with life
Step Predictor Step 1 Step 2 Step 3 Step 1 Step 2 Step 3 Step 1 Step 2 Step 3
1 Sex .03 .02 .01 .00 .04 .03 .12 .07 .08
Age .15 .14 .13 .10 .08 .08 .12 .10 .10
Years treating addiction problems a .13 .13 .14 .02 .01 .02 .02 .02 .02
Certified .00 .02 .03 .01 .01 .02 .04 .01 .00
Professional group membership .11 .06 .05 .11 .06 .05 .05 .00 .02
Past addiction problem .09 .08 .07 .06 .06 .06 .03 .02 .03
Profit status .10 .13 .08 .14 .17 .08 .16 .19 .09
SBS .40 .28 027 .24 .13 .12 .14 .03 .01
Disease: F(8, 565) = 26.47, p b .001, R2 = .273
Choice: F(8, 565) = 12.89, p b .001, R2 = .154
Way of coping with life: F(8, 565) = 9.45, p b .001, R2 = .118
2 Country .32 .28 .30 .22 .33 .24
Disease: Fchange(1, 564) = 68.55, p b .001, R2change = .080
Choice: Fchange(1, 564) = 48.71, p b .001, R2change = .067
Way of coping with life: Fchange(1, 564) = 57.80, p b .001, R2change = .082
3 Country Profit Status .10 .18 .21
Disease: Fchange(1, 563) = 3.84, p = .051, R2change = .004
Choice: Fchange(1, 563) = 10.80, p b .001, R2change = .015
Way of coping with life: Fchange(1, 563) = 14.40, p b .001, R2change = .020
a
Ordinal scale variable.
p b .05.
p b .01.
p b .001.

those who provide addiction treatment in the United providers whom are not members of a group of addiction
Kingdom (M = 24.97, SD = 5.77). professionals more strongly believe (M = 17.46, SD = 3.50)
Step 3 in the model yielded a nonsignificant increase of that addiction is a choice than group members (M = 15.70,
0.4% explained variance, Fchange(1, 563) = 3.84, p N .05. SD = 3.23).
Therefore, the country of treatment did not moderate the Step 2 in the model accounted for a significant increase
effect of profit status on treatment providers' beliefs that of 6.7% explained variance in factor 2 score, Fchange(1, 564)
addiction is a disease. = 48.71, p b .001. After partialing out variance explained
by variables in step 1, providing addiction treatment in the
3.5. Variables explaining variance in treatment providers' United States was negatively associated ( = .30, p b
beliefs that addiction is a choice .001) with factor 2 score. Providers of addiction treatment
in the United Kingdom more strongly believe (M = 17.96,
The final regression model accounted for 23.6% of SD = 3.22) that addiction is a choice than those who
variance in treatment providers' beliefs that addiction is a provide addiction treatment in the United States (M = 15.24,
motivated choice. Step 1 produced a significant model, F(8, SD = 3.39).
565) = 12.89, p b .001, and accounted for 15.4% of Step 3 in the model yielded a significant increase of 1.5%
variance in factor 2 score. Four of the eight variables made explained variance in factor 3 score, Fchange(1, 563) = 10.80,
significant contributions. p b .001. To interrogate this interaction, simple effects
In complete contrast to factor 1 scores, score on the SBS analyses were conducted (i.e., repeating the analysis with
and age were both negatively associated ( = .24, p b .001, neither the Country Profit Status interaction term nor main
and = .10, p b .05, respectively) with factor 2 scores. effects of country), first for U.S.-based treatment providers (n
These indicate that beliefs that addiction is a choice weaken = 219) and then for U.K.-based treatment providers (n = 372).
as strength of spiritual thinking increases and with age. This revealed a significant negative association between
Providing private/for-profit treatment was negatively asso- profit status and factor 2 score among U.S.-based treatment
ciated ( = .14, p b .01) with factor 2 score. Those who providers ( = .40, p b .001) after controlling for seven
provide public/not-for-profit addiction treatment more variables entered at step 1, Fchange(1, 207) = 31.42, p b .001,
strongly believe (M = 17.47, SD = 3.45) that addiction is a R2 change = .117. No significant association was found
choice than those who provide private/for-profit treatment between profit status and factor 2 score among U.K.-based
(M = 15.70, SD = 3.46). Being a member of a professional providers (see Table 5). This indicates that country of
group of addiction treatment providers was negatively treatment moderates the relationship between profit status
associated ( = .11, p b .01) with factor 2 score. Treatment and treatment providers' beliefs that addiction is a choice,
C. Russell et al. / Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment 40 (2011) 150164 159

Table 5
Decomposition of Country Profit Status interaction effects found for factors 2 and 3: Results from U.S.-based (and U.K.-based) treatment providers
Choice Way of coping with life
Step Predictor Step 1 Step 2 Step 1 Step 2
1 Sex .02 (.08) .04 (.08) .05 (.06) .11 (.07)
Age .19 (.05) .10 (.05) .10 (.18 ) .01 (.18 )
Years treating addiction problems a .13 (.08) .12 (.08) .10 (.03) .10 (.03)
Certified .06 (.03) .09 (.03) .10 (.06) .13 (.06)
Professional group membership .00 (.14 ) .07 (.13 ) .08 (.00) .01 (.01)
Past addiction problem .09 (.08) .07 (.07) .03 (.01) .05 (.02)
SBS .15 (.16 ) .05 (.16 ) .23 (.07) .12 (.07)
Choice, U.S.-based: F(7, 208) = 3.74, p b .001, R2 = .082
Choice, U.K.-based: F(7, 350) = 4.07, p b .001, R2 = .075
Way of coping with life, U.S.-based: F(7, 208) = 2.84, p b .01, R2 = .088
Way of coping with life, U.K.-based: F(7, 350) = 2.32, p b .05, R2 = .044
2 Profit status .40 (.07) .41 (.09)
Choice, U.S.-based: Fchange(1, 207) = 31.42, p b .001, R2change = .117
Choice, U.K.-based: Fchange(1, 349) = 1.74, p N .05, R2change = .005
Way of coping with life, U.S.-based: F(1, 207) = 32.56, p b .001, R2 = .124
Way of coping with life, U.K.-based: F(1, 349) = 2.92, p N .05, R2 = .008
a
Ordinal scale variable.
p b .05.
p b .01.
p b .001.

with only U.S.-based providers of public/not-for-profit United States was negatively associated ( = .33, p b .001)
treatment reporting significantly stronger beliefs (M = with factor 3 score. Providers of addiction treatment in the
16.23, SD = 3.28) than private/for-profit providers (M = United Kingdom more strongly believe (M = 7.40, SD = 1.35)
13.13, SD = 2.55) that addiction is a choice. that addiction is a choice than those who provide addiction
treatment in the United States (M = 6.28, SD = 1.48).
3.6. Variables explaining variance in treatment providers' Step 3 in the model yielded a significant increase of 2.0%
beliefs that addiction is a way of coping with life explained variance in factor 3 score, Fchange(1, 563) = 14.40,
p b .001. To interrogate this interaction, simple effects
The final regression model accounted for 22.0% of analyses were again conducted, first for U.S.-based sample
variance in treatment providers' beliefs that addiction is a (n = 219) and then the U.K.-based sample (n = 372). This
way of coping with life. Step 1 produced a significant model, revealed a significant positive association between profit
F(8, 565) = 9.45, p b .001, and accounted for 11.8% of status and factor 3 score in the United States ( = .47, p b
variance in factor 3 score. Four of the eight variables made .001) after controlling for seven variables entered at step 1,
significant contributions. Fchange(1, 207) = 32.56, p b .001, R2change = .124. Again, no
Score on the SBS and age were again both negatively significant association was found between profit status and
associated ( = .14, p b .001, and = .12, p b .05, factor 3 score in the U.K. sample. This indicates that country
respectively) with factor 3 score. These indicate that beliefs of treatment moderates the relationship between profit status
that addiction is a way of coping with life weaken as strength and treatment providers' beliefs in the disease model, with
of spiritual thinking increases and with age. Providing only U.S.-based treatment providers reporting significantly
private/for-profit treatment was negatively associated ( = stronger beliefs (M = 6.68, SD = 1.36) than private/for-profit
.16, p b .001) with factor 3 score. Those who provide providers (M = 5.41, SD = 1.35) that addiction is a way of
public/not-for-profit addiction treatment more strongly coping with life.
believe (M = 7.19, SD = 1.49) that addiction is a way of
coping with life than those who provide private/for-profit 3.7. Hierarchical multiple regression models applied to
treatment (M = 6.55, SD = 1.75). Being female was positively data provided only by treatment providers who have
associated ( = .12, p b .01) with factor 3 score. Female had a past addiction problem
treatment providers more strongly believe (M = 7.17, SD =
1.49) that addiction is a way of coping with life than male Three separate hierarchical multiple linear regression
treatment providers (M = 6.76, SD = 1.47). analyses were then conducted to investigate the variance in
Step 2 in the model accounted for a significant increase of addiction beliefs of treatment providers who have had a past
8.2% explained variance in factor 2 scores, Fchange(1, 564) = addiction problem (n = 199) explained by five personal
57.80, p b .001. After partialing out variance explained by addiction history variables. Three ABS factor scores were
variables in step 1, providing addiction treatment in the again used as criterion variables. To control for their effects,
160 C. Russell et al. / Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment 40 (2011) 150164

sex, age, number of years as an addiction treatment provider, providers who have had a personal addiction problem and
certification status as an addiction treatment provider, are presently abstinent more strongly believe (M = 29.90,
membership status of a group of addiction treatment SD = 6.32) that addiction is a disease than those who are
professionals, country of treatment, profit status of treatment, not presently abstinent (M = 23.78, SD = 3.74).
and SBS score were all entered at step 1. A further five
variables were entered at step 2 of the equation: attended 3.9. Variables explaining variance in addiction is a choice
treatment in the past (0 = no, 1 = yes), attended 12-step beliefs of treatment providers who have had a past
based group in the past (0 = no, 1 = yes), attend 12-step addiction problem
based group at present (0 = no, 1 = yes), number of years in
12-stepbased group, and abstinence status at present (0 = Controlling for the effects of the eight variables entered at
not abstinent, 1 = abstinent). Power analysis confirmed that step 1, step 2 in the model accounted for a significant increase
with a sample of 199, = .05, and with 13 predictor of 5.3% explained variance in factor 1 scores, Fchange(5, 179)
variables, power = .89, meaning this regression model was = 6.13, p b .001. Only one variable made a significant
sufficiently powered to detect meaningful factor effects. contribution. Number of years as a member of a 12-step
group was negatively associated ( = .26, p b .001) with
3.8. Variables explaining variance in addiction is a disease factor 2 score. The longer treatment providers are members
beliefs of treatment providers who have had a past of a 12-stepbased group, the less they come to view
addiction problem addiction as a choice.

Controlling for the effects of the eight variables entered 3.10. Variables explaining variance in addiction is a way
at step 1, step 2 in the model accounted for a significant of coping with life beliefs of treatment providers who have
increase of 12.2% explained variance in factor 1 scores, had a past addiction problem
Fchange(5, 179) = 7.48, p b .001 (see Table 6). Two of the
five variables made significant contributions. Number of Controlling for the effects of the eight variables entered
years as a member of a 12-step group was positively at step 1, step 2 in the model accounted for a nonsignificant
associated ( = .24, p b .001) with factor 1 score. The increase of 1.8% explained variance in factor 3 scores,
longer treatment providers are members of a 12-stepbased Fchange(5, 179) = 0.64, p N .05. Thus, treatment providers
group, the more strongly they come to believe addiction is a who have had a personal addiction problem in the past did
disease. Being abstinent at present was also positively not significantly vary in their beliefs about addiction as a
associated ( = .16, p b .05) with factor 1 score. Treatment way of coping with life across the step 2 variables.

Table 6
Separate hierarchical multiple linear regression analysis using scores on three factors extracted from the ABS as criterion variables: Conducted on data provided
by treatment providers who reported having had a personal problem with addiction in the past (n = 199)
Disease Choice Way of coping with life
Step Predictor Step 1 Step 2 Step 1 Step 2 Step 1 Step 2
1 Sex .00 .00 .07 .08 .09 .08
Age .12 .10 .11 .07 .08 .07
Years treating addiction problems a .07 .10 .07 .09 .02 .04
Certified .07 .07 .06 .07 .01 .01
Professional group membership .09 .06 .12 .07 .06 .05
Country .19 .14 .24 .17 .34 .31
Profit status .08 .06 .08 .06 .19 .18
SBS .48 39 .26 .12 .02 .06
Disease: F(8, 184) = 15.54, p b .001, R2 = .403
Choice: F(8, 184) = 7.57, p b .001, R2 = .248
Way of coping with life: F(8, 184) = 5.29, p b .001, R2 = .187
2 Attended treatment in the past .00 .03 .03
Attended 12-stepbased group in the past .12 .06 .06
Attend 12-stepbased group at present .08 .14 .00
Years membership of 12-stepbased group .24 .26 .14
Present abstinence status .16 .12 .04
Disease: Fchange(5, 179) = 7.48, p b .001, R2change = .122
Choice: Fchange(5, 179) = 6.13, p b .001, R2change = .110
Way of coping with life: Fchange(5, 179) = 0.64, p N .05, R2change = .018
a
Ordinal scale variable.
p b .05.
p b .01.
p b .001.
C. Russell et al. / Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment 40 (2011) 150164 161

4. Discussion individuals are able to control themselves by influential


national health bodies such as the National Health Service
4.1. Key findings (2010): addiction is not having control over doing, taking,
or using something, to the point that it becomes harmful.
Addiction treatment providers in the United States and Not having control implies addiction compels action
United Kingdom were surveyed on their beliefs about the regardless of the will of the individual. Thus, it appears
etiology of addiction, the need to receive treatment, and the that those working at the frontline of U.K. addiction
addicted individuals' capacity for self-control during drug treatment view and, therefore, likely explain addiction to
use. Seven variables were significant in explaining variance their clients in ways that contradict the disease-based
in addiction beliefs. After controlling for the variance definitions and media messages of authoritative health
accounted for by eight variables, treatment providers' bodies. However, we have no evidence that treatment
strength of beliefs in the disease model of addiction was providers treat clients in line with their beliefs when their
significantly predicted by the country in which treatment is beliefs conflict with their institution's addiction ideology.
provided. Those who provide addiction treatment in the This is certainly a next step in this research.
United States more strongly believe that addiction is a The U.S.-based sample's tendency to favor the disease
disease than those who provide addiction treatment in the model, however, was expected. Relative to U.K. treatment
United Kingdom, whereas U.K.-based treatment providers providers, U.S. treatment providers both endorsed the view
more strongly believe that addiction is a choice than U.S.- of addiction as a disease and rejected the view of addiction as
based treatment providers. Those more likely to believe that a choice and as a way of coping with life. Disease model
addiction is a disease also tend to provide for-profit beliefs appear to have persisted as the dominant view of
treatment, have stronger spiritual beliefs, have had a personal addiction in the United States since Schaler's (1992) initial
problem with addiction in the past, are members of a group use of the ABS, although three methodological issues
of addiction professionals, have been treating addiction suggest caution when comparing these studies. First,
problems for longer, and are older. In contrast, those who although assumed by Schaler to be very high, Schaler does
believe addiction is a choice tend to provide public/not-for- not report the U.S.-based proportion of his sample; second,
profit treatment, have weaker spiritual beliefs, be younger, Schaler's methodology involved mailing and requesting the
and not be members of a group of addiction professionals. return of paper copies of his survey, whereas the current
The country in which treatment is provided moderates the sample were recruited and provided data online; third, the
effect of treatment profit status on providers' beliefs about current study did not use a repeated-measures design; it is
addiction as a choice and as a way of coping with life, with highly unlikely that any treatment providers provided data
those providing public/not-for-profit treatment in the United for both our study and Schaler's study, and tracking down
States more strongly believing that that addiction is a choice Schaler's sample was impossible. Thus, we can only
and a way of coping with life than U.S.-based providers of tentatively conclude that the disease model has prevailed
private/for-profit treatment. as the dominant of the two models of addiction within U.S.
Finally, treatment providers who have had a personal treatment services across the past 20 years.
problem with addiction in the past are more likely to believe That no prior research on U.K.-based treatment providers'
addiction is a disease if they have attended a 12-step group disease/choice model beliefs exists, however, prevents any
for longer and are presently abstinent. Beliefs that addiction conclusions about whether the addiction beliefs of our U.K.
is a choice weaken among these treatment providers the sample reflect a snapshot in an increasing, stable, or
longer they remain members of a 12-stepbased group. decreasing trend of disease model support. Current findings
Overall, results suggest treatment providers' beliefs about provide a context for assessing the stability of disease model
what addiction is largely fit a diseasechoice model support over time, perhaps with developments and marked
dichotomy, that agreement with one model predicts changes to scientific and political perspectives on addiction
disagreement with the other, and that addiction etiology and a context for assessing addiction beliefs internationally.
and course are understood very differently by U.S. and U.K. One explanation for the discrepancy in belief systems
treatment communities. between the United States and United Kingdom may lay in
the sizeable difference between these country's public
4.2. Conflicting beliefs in the United States versus the funding of addiction research. In a recent national report,
United Kingdom about what addiction is Colin Blakemore, then head of the United Kingdom's
Medical Research Council (MRC) reported that, In 2003 to
The assumed global dominance of the disease model of 2004 [the MRC] spent 2 million in total out of a 450
addiction was not found; rather, the concept of addiction million budget on addiction research. The total budget of the
meant very different things to the sampled U.K. and U.S. three NIH [U.S. National Institutes of Health] institutes that
treatment communities. The relative strength of the United work in this area is $2.9 billion so even if one takes a
Kingdom's choice model endorsement is very surprising conservative estimate of how much of that is actually
given the unequivocal rejection of the idea that addicted devoted to addiction research it comes out to about five
162 C. Russell et al. / Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment 40 (2011) 150164

hundred times higher than in the UKin other words about clients. People often enter addiction treatment because they
a hundred times more per head of the population (The seek definitive answers as to why they find self-control of
Science and Technology Committee, 2006). In the same drug use so elusive and to know what is wrong with them.
report, former chair of the Advisory Council on the Misuse Ambivalence on the etiology of addiction reported by some
of Drugs Technical Committee, Professor David Nutt, groups of treatment providers and the stronger committal of
estimated the expenditure differential to be 1,000-fold in the U.S. and U.K. treatment communities to the disease and
favor of the United States. Future research should ask choice models, respectively, may, therefore, facilitate and
whether there exists a significant association between this obstruct clients' change process in different ways.
research expenditure differential, a differential in breadths of On one side, treatment providers with strong beliefs in
evidence bases regarding addiction etiology and treatment either model are more likely to send a clear and unambiguous
effectiveness, and the differential in disease beliefs about message to clients about what addiction is and what it is not.
addiction reported by the current U.S. and U.K. samples of Defining the problem and giving clients clear direction as to
treatment providers. what they should do and expect in the short and long term
should enhance clients' perceived self-efficacy and optimism
4.3. Methodological limitations for change. In contrast, providers who reserve judgment or
show ambivalence as to what causes addiction (which
The e-survey methodology was inexpensive and allowed implies endorsement of an eclectic treatment approach) may
faster and wider access to and response from our sample than send mixed messages to clients about the nature of their
could have been achieved by mailing paper versions of a problems and how best to deal with them. Thus, it may be
survey or conducting face-to-face/telephone interviews. The argued from a pragmatic standpoint that is it better for
manual demands of generating a sample of 591 treatment treatment providers to convey a definitive perspective of
providers in such ways would have been impractical for this addiction to their clients, whichever that perspective may be.
study, although we do acknowledge that e-surveys may On the other side, treatment providers who are strongly
induce a sampling bias, and so caution is suggested in committed to either model may be less flexible to change
generalizing results to the wider U.S. and U.K. treatment when their beliefs are challenged by scientific evidence or
communities. For example, results may not accurately the anecdotes of other therapists and clients. In this way,
describe the beliefs of treatment providers who declined to noncommitted providers should be more open to weighing
participate, had difficulty in navigating the online format and up contrasting empirical and anecdotal evidence and
so did not complete the survey (and so, were excluded from adapting treatment to reflect current thinking on addiction.
analyses), and those for whom electronic contact details were The strongly disease- and choice model-committed U.S. and
unavailable/unknown and so could not be invited to U.K. treatment communities, however, may be less willing
participate. Researchers who wish to make comparisons to revise their treatment philosophy in the face of evidence
involving current findings should also appreciate that the which suggests a revision should be considered. Among the
dynamics of completing e-surveys versus paper-and-pencil strongest disease beliefs in the current sample were reported
surveys may be different. by treatment providers who have had a personal addiction
The larger sample of U.K.-versus-U.S.-based providers problem in the past but are abstinent at present; these groups
may be partially attributed to the researchers' greater of treatment provider may be most likely to stick with the
knowledge of and access to U.K. treatment services. treatment methods that have worked for clients and
Although the U.K. sample was boosted by enlisting the themselves in the past, regardless of client differences in
help of U.K. DAATs to distribute survey packs, we made symptomatology, environments, and reasons for drug use.
every effort to offset this imbalance through an exhaustive Finally, a common criticism of the diseasechoice debate
recruitment of U.S.-based treatment providers through is that absolute truths about addiction are irrelevant so long
voluntary, statutory, and private association Web sites and as people do recover. The success of treatment may
online databases. Additionally, approximately 1,750 (70%) therefore depend on the degree of congruence between
of the 2,500 subscribers of targeted e-newsletters are treatment providers' and clients' beliefs about addiction
believed to be based in the United States. Nonetheless, (Keene & Raynor, 1993). Assuming that disease-based
recruitment would have benefited from collaboration with messages will be less effective if clients ultimately believe
researchers experienced in accessing U.S. treatment services. that they are not diseased (and likewise for choice-based
messages), the ABS may be used to match therapists and
4.4. Implications of treatment providers' ambivalent and clients on belief compatibility at intake. If the success of
strong beliefs about addiction addiction treatments is shown to depend on therapists
fostering clients' clear and uncompromised addiction beliefs
Irrespective of either model's validity, current findings of whatever kind, then we may be justified in abandoning the
indicate a potential for a diversity of addiction beliefs to exist search for absolute truths about addiction and instead
within treatment services, which has implications for how focusing on the importance of subjective experiences and
effectively treatment providers work with each other and with lay conceptualizations of addiction to the change process.
C. Russell et al. / Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment 40 (2011) 150164 163

Acknowledgments Kurtz, E. (1988). AA: The story (a revised edition of Not-God: A History of
Alcoholics Anonymous). New York: Harper & Row.
Kurtz, E. (2002). Alcoholics Anonymous and the disease concept of
The authors are grateful to all participants who gave up alcoholism. Alcoholism Treatment Quarterly, 20, 539.
their valuable time to help with this research and to Rowdy Leshner, A. I. (1997). Addiction is a brain disease, and it matters. Science,
Yates, Douglas Cameron, Moira Plant, Martin Plant, Stanton 278, 4547.
Peele, Ted Russell, Gemma McGill and three anonymous Levine, H. (1978). The discovery of addiction: Changing conceptions of
reviewers for their suggestions at various stages. Dedicated habitual drunkenness in America. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 39,
143174.
to Martin Plant. Lyvers, M. (1998). Drug addiction as a physical disease: The role of physical
dependence and other chronic drug-induced neurophysiological changes
References in compulsive drug self-administration. Experimental and Clinical
Psychopharmacology, 6, 107125.
Alcoholics Anonymous World Services. (1939). Alcoholics Anonymous. Maltzman, I. (1994). Why alcoholism is a disease. Journal of Psychoactive
New York: Author. Drugs, 26, 1331.
Alexander, B. K., Hadaway, P. F., & Coambs, R. B. (1980). Rat Park McMurran, M. (1994). The psychology of addiction. London: Taylor &
chronicle. British Columbia Medical Journal, 22, 5456. Francis.
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical Mercadante, L. (1996). Victims and sinners: Spiritual roots of addiction and
manual of mental disorders, (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. recovery. Louisville, KY: Westminster John Knox Press.
Benowitz, N. L. (2008). Clinical pharmacology of nicotine: Implications for Merry, J. (1966). The loss of control myth. Lancet, 1, 12571258.
understanding, preventing, and treating tobacco addiction. Clinical Milam, J., & Ketcham, K. (1983). Under the influence: A guide to the myths
Pharmacology and Therapeutics, 83, 531541. and realities of alcoholism. New York: Bantam Books.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences. Miller, W. R. (1993). Alcoholism: Toward a better disease model.
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 7, 129136.
Cohen, M., Liebson, I. A., Faillace, L. A., & Allen, R. P. (1971). Moderate Morse, R. M., & Flavin, D. K. (1992). The definition of alcoholism. Journal
drinking by alcoholics: A schedule-dependent phenomenon. Journal of of the American Medical Association, 268, 10121014.
Nervous and Mental Disease, 153, 434444. National Health Service. (2010). NHS choices, your choices, your life.
Crothers, T. D. (1893). The disease of inebriety from alcohol, opium and Addictions. Retrieved January 3, 2010, from http://www.nhs.uk/
other narcotic drugs. New York: E. B. Treat. conditions/Addictions/Pages/Introduction.aspx.
Davies, J. B. (1997). Pharmacology versus social process. Competing or National Institute on Drug Abuse. (2009). NIDA InfoFacts: Understanding
complimentary views on the nature of addiction. Pharmacology & drug abuse and addiction. Retrieved October 10, 2009, from http://www.
Therapeutics, 80, 265275. nida.nih.gov/PDF/InfoFacts/Understanding08.pdf.
Fingarette, H. (1988a). AlcoholismThe mythical disease. Public Interest, Nature Neuroscience (multiple authors). (2005). Focus on neurobiology of
91, 322. addiction. [Special issue] Nature Neuroscience, 8, 11051489.
Fingarette, H. (1988b). Heavy drinking: The myth of alcoholism as a Retrieved April 1, 2010, from http://www.nature.com/neuro/focus/
disease. Berkeley: University of California Press. addiction/index.html.
Foulds, J., & Ghodse, A. H. (1995). The role of nicotine in tobacco smoking: Ochoa, E. L. M. (1994). Nicotine-related brain disorders: The neurobiolog-
Implications for tobacco control policy. Journal of the Royal Society of ical basis of nicotine dependence. Cellular and Molecular Neurobiology,
Health, 115, 225230. 14, 195225.
HBO Documentary Films. (2007). Addiction: Why can't they just stop? Parrish, J. (1883). Alcoholic inebriety: From a medical standpoint.
Retrieved April 1, 2010, from http://www.hbo.com/addiction/thefilm/. Philadelphia: P. Blakiston.
Heather, N., & Robertson, I. (1989). Controlled drinking. London: Methuen. Peele, S. (1989). The diseasing of America. Lexington, MA: Lexington
Heyman, G. (2009). Addiction: A disorder of choice. Cambridge, MA: Books.
Harvard University Press. Peele, S., Brodsky, A., & Arnold, M. (1991). The truth about addiction and
Institute of Medicine. (1996). Dispelling the myths about addiction: recovery: The life process program for outgrowing destructive habits.
Strategies to increase understanding and strengthen research. Commit- New York: Simon & Schuster.
tee to Identify Strategies to Raise the Profile of Substance Abuse and Playfair, W. (1991). The useful lie. Wheaton, IL: Crossway Books.
Alcoholism research. Division of Neuroscience and Behavioral Health Project MATCH Research Group. (1997). Matching alcoholism treatment to
and Division of Health Promotion and Disease Prevention, Institute of client heterogeneity: Project MATCH post-treatment drinking outcomes.
Medicine. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 58, 729.
Jellinek, E. M. (1960). The disease concept of alcoholism. New Brunswick, Ragge, K. (1998). The real A.A.: Behind the myth of 12-step recovery.
NJ: Hillhouse. Tucson, AZ: Sharp Press.
Kalivas, P. W., & Volkow, N. D. (2005). The neural basis of Addiction: A Robins, L. N., Helzer, J. E., & Davis, D. H. (1975). Narcotic use in Southeast
pathology of motivation and choice. American Journal of Psychiatry, Asia and afterward: An interview study of 898 Vietnam returnees.
162, 14031413. Archives of General Psychiatry, 32, 955961.
Keene, J., & Raynor, P. (1993). Addiction as soul sickness: The influence of Room, R. (1983). Sociological aspects of the disease concept of
client and therapist beliefs. Addiction Research and Theory, 1, 7787. alcoholism. In R. G. Smart, et al (Ed.), Research advances in alcohol,
Kerr, N. (1888). Inebriety. London: H. K. Lewis. Vol. 7. (pp. 4791) New York: Plenum Press.
Ketcham, K., Asbury, W., Schulstad, M., & Ciaramicoli, A. (2000). Beyond Schaler J. (1992). Addiction beliefs of treatment providers: Factors
the influence: Understanding and defeating alcoholism. New York: explaining variance. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Disserta-
Bantam Books. tion Services, Order 9327490.
Koob, G. F., & Nestler, E. J. (1997). The neurobiology of drug addiction. Schaler, J. (1995). The addiction belief scale. International Journal of the
Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 9, 482497. Addictions, 30, 117134.
Koski-Jannes, A. (2004). In search of a comprehensive model of addiction. Schaler, J. (1996). Spiritual thinking in addiction treatment providers. The
In P. Rosenqvist, J. Blomqvist, A. Koski-Jannes, & L. Ojesjo (Eds.), Spiritual Belief Scale. Alcoholism Treatment Quarterly, 14, 733.
Addiction and life course (pp. 4970). Helsinki: NAD. Schaler, J. A. (2000). Addiction is a choice. Chicago, IL: Open Court.
164 C. Russell et al. / Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment 40 (2011) 150164

Szasz, T. (1972). Bad habits are not diseases: A refutation of the claim that Vaillant, G. (1990). We should retain the disease concept of alcoholism.
alcoholism is a disease. Lancet, 2, 8384. Harvard Medical School Mental Health Newsletter, 6, 46.
TIME Magazine. (2007). How we get addicted. Retrieved March 27, 2010, White, W. L. (2000). Addiction as a disease: The birth of a concept.
from http://www.time.com/time/magazine/article/0,9171,1640436,00.html. Retrieved March 26, 2010, from http://www.bhrm.org/papers/
The Science and Technology Committee. (2006). Select committee on science Counselor1.pdf.
and technology fifth report. Retrieved April 1, 2010, from http://www. White, W. L. (2001). A disease concept for the 21st century. Retrieved
publications.parliament.uk/pa/cm200506/cmselect/cmsctech/1031/ January 3, 2010, from http://www.bhrm.org/papers/Counselor4.pdf.
103102.htm. White, W. L. (2007). In search of the neurobiology of addiction recovery:
UKATT. (2005). Cost effectiveness of treatment for alcohol problems: A brief commentary on science and stigma. Retrieved March 25,
Findings of the randomized UK Alcohol Treatment Trial (UKATT). 2010, from http://www.facesandvoicesofrecovery.org/pdf/White/
British Medical Journal, 331, 544548. White_neurobiology_2007.pdf.

Вам также может понравиться