Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 4

-RXUQDORI$IIHFWLYH'LVRUGHUV  

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Aective Disorders


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jad

Acceptance alone is a better predictor of psychopathology and well-being 0$5.


than emotional competence, emotion regulation and mindfulness

Ilios Kotsoua, , Christophe Leysb, Pierre Fossionc
a
Faculty of Psychology, Universit Libre de Bruxelles, Brussels, Belgium & Chaire Mindfulness, Bien-Etre au travail et Paix conomique, Grenoble Ecole de Management,
France
b
Faculty of Psychology, Universit Libre de Bruxelles, Brussels, Belgium
c
Brugmann Hospital, Universit Libre de Bruxelles, Brussels, Belgium

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Emotional competence, emotion regulation, mindfulness and acceptance have all been strongly associated to
Anxiety emotional disorders and psychological well-being in multiple studies. However little research has compared the
Depression unique predictive ability of these dierent constructs. We hypothesised that they will all share a large proportion
Acceptance of common variance and that when compared to the broader constructs emotional competence, emotion reg-
Emotional competence
ulation and mindfulness, acceptance alone would predict a larger proportion of unique variance
Emotion regulation
Methods: 228 participants from a community sample completed anonymously measures of anxiety, depression,
Mindfulness
happiness, acceptance, mindfulness, emotional competence and emotion regulation. We then ran multiple re-
gressions to assess and compare the predictive ability of these dierent constructs.
Results: For measures of psychological distress, the acceptance measure uniquely accounted for between 4 and
30 times the variance that the emotional competence, emotion regulation and mindfulness measures did.
Limitations: These results are based on cross-sectional designs and non-clinical samples, longitudinal and ex-
perimental studies as clinical samples may be useful in order to assess the potential protective power of ac-
ceptance over time. Another limitation is the use of self-report questionnaires.
Conclusions: Results conrmed our hypothesis, supporting the research on the importance of acceptance as a
central factor in the understanding of the onset and maintenance of emotional disorders.

1. Introduction disorders. A well-established one is conceptualised as emotional com-


petence (Petrides et al., 2016). Emotional competence (EC) refers to
For after all, the best thing one can do when it is raining is let it individual dierences in the way individuals are able to identify, un-
rain. derstand, regulate, and use their emotions (Mikolajczak, 2009). A high
level of emotional competence is related to improved psychological and
Longfellow (1906)
physical health (Martins et al., 2010).
Psychological health is a central issue in our societies. According to Emotion regulation is one of the EC that has received extensive
a recent systematic review and analysis encompassing data from 63 attention (Gross, 2002) and there is strong evidence that anxiety, de-
countries, one in ve respondents reported a disorder in the year pre- pression and stress are multifactorial disorders that are at least partly
ceding the assessment, and almost a third of the respondents have ex- linked to maladaptive emotion regulation patterns (Cisler and Olatunji,
perienced a psychological health disorder some time in their life (Steel 2012; Joormann and Gotlib, 2010). Acceptance and mindfulness are
et al., 2014). Lifetime prevalence of emotional disorders, such as an- two other well-studied predictors. Meta-analysis has shown that ac-
xiety or depression, is considered as high as 29%, for anxiety or 21% for ceptance has a positive eect on various indicators such as anxiety,
mood disorders (Kessler et al., 2005). In this paper, psychological depression or addiction (A-tjak et al., 2015). Conversely, a meta-ana-
health is dened both by it pathological dimension (measured by three lysis has shown that mindfulness-based therapy can be considered as an
indexes of emotional disorders: anxiety, depression and stress) and its eective intervention for treating anxiety and depression (Goyal et al.,
positive dimension, measured by happiness. 2014).
There are several empirically based predictors of emotional Because EC, emotion regulation, mindfulness and acceptance all


Correspondence to: Department of Psychology, Universit Libre de Bruxelles, Avenue F.D. Roosevelt 50, 1050 Bruxelles, Belgium.
E-mail address: ilios.kotsou@ulb.ac.be (I. Kotsou).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2017.09.047
Received 28 March 2017; Received in revised form 22 June 2017; Accepted 24 September 2017
$YDLODEOHRQOLQH6HSWHPEHU
(OVHYLHU%9$OOULJKWVUHVHUYHG
I. Kotsou et al. -RXUQDORI$IIHFWLYH'LVRUGHUV  

seem to be eective but oer dierent perspectives on emotions and Attention Awareness Scale (Jermann et al., 2009). The MAAS is a 15-
psychological health, we wanted to compare their respective ability to item ( = 87) instrument measuring the general tendency to pay at-
predict symptom severity (stress, anxiety, depression) and psycholo- tention to present-moment experiences in daily life (which can be
gical well-being (happiness). To our knowledge, no prior study has considered to be one of the dimensions of the multidimensional con-
examined this issue yet. struct of mindfulness), using a 6- point Likert-type scale (almost always
A growing body of research is now suggesting that acceptance is a to almost never).
central trans-diagnostic process that explains a large proportion of Emotional competence (EC-T) was assessed with the Prole of
variance in diverse mood disorders (Kashdan et al., 2006). Following Emotional Competence (PEC; Brasseur et al., 2013). The PEC is a 50-
Hayes and colleagues psychological exibility model, acceptance can item ( = 91) tool that measures 10 facets of Emotional competence.
be seen as a trans-diagnostic process that is central in psychopathology The scale can also be used with a single score.
(Hayes et al., 2006). Therefore, we hypothesised that acceptance would Emotion regulation was measured with one of the PEC subscales,
have a unique incremental predictive validity over the three other Regulation of own emotions (ER), which consisted of 5 items ( =
predictors (mindfulness, emotional competence and emotional regula- 75) As described in the introduction, this measure is supposed to be an
tion). We hypothesised that a) the four predictors combined will sig- important predictor of psychopathology and well-being, and thus it was
nicantly predict psychopathology and psychological well-being, b) of particular interest to us.
each of the four predictors will separately predict psychopathology and
psychological well-being, c) all 4 predictors will share important 3. Results
common variance, but compared to the three other processes, accep-
tance will predict a larger proportion of unique variance. 3.1. Correlations

2. Method Descriptive statistics are provided in Table 0 in Supplemental ma-


terials. Pearson's Correlations are reported in Table 1. We used SPSS 24
2.1. Participants for all analysis. As predicted, all four predictors correlate signicantly
with psychopathology namely anxiety, depression and stress - and
Participants were recruited by an internet announcement posted by positively with happiness, and each association was in the predicted
a non for prot organization that organizes mindfulness interventions. direction.
The sample consisted of 228 participants (175 women, 53 men) from a
French-speaking community sample. The participants were 2569 years 3.2. Multiple regression analysis
of age (mean age = 43.70 years, SD = 10.04). 67% were living with a
partner, 33% were single. 63% had a high level of education (master or We then ran multiple regressions to assess the total variance (R2)
higher). that could be attributed individually to the four centered predictors. In
accordance with our hypothesis, we then separately compared AAQ
2.2. Procedure with each of the three others predictors, the total EC-T, ER and MAAS
with HADS-A, HADS-D, PSS and SHS as dependent variables. We
Ethical standards related to privacy, anonymity and informed con- wanted to assess the overlap in prediction between the variables (the
sent, in accordance with the ethics code of American Psychological common variance) and the unique contribution in variance that can be
Association, were respected. The questionnaires were completed in attributed to each construct, above and beyond the other factors (sr2).
French anonymously online and the participants were given no re- When introduced individually in regression analysis, MAAS and
tribution for participation in order to avoid the risk of undue induce- HADS-A, R2=.15 [.07;.23]; MAAS and HADS-D, R2=.07 [.01;.13];
ment. Participants were voluntary, they were aware they were parti- MAAS and PSS, R2=.22 [.13;.31]; and MAAS and SHS, R2=.07
cipating in a study and could withdraw from the questionnaire at any [.01;.13]. We did the same analysis for EC-T and HADS-A, R2=.08
time. The study received the authorisation of the ethical committee of [.01;.15]; EC-T and HADS-D, R2=.07 [.01;.13]; EC-T and PSS, R2=.15
the university. [.07;.23]; and EC-T and SHS, R2=.12 [.04;.20]. Then we computed the
regression for ER and HADS-A, R2=.17 [.08;.26]; ER and HADS-D,
2.3. Materials R2=.10 [.03;.17]; ER and PSS, R2=.25 [.15;.35]; and ER and SHS,
R2=.21 [.12;.30]. Lastly we computed regression between AAQ and
Anxiety and depression. Anxiety and depression were measured HADS-A, R2=.31 [.21;.41]; AAQ and HADS-D, R2=.24 [.14;.34]; AAQ
with the HADS, Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (Bocran and and PSS, R2=.39 [.29;.49]; and AAQ and SHS, R2=.44 [.34;.54].
Dupret, 2014). The HADS comprises fourteen items: seven items ( = We then systematically compared AAQ to the total score of EC-T,
77) assessing anxiety (HADS-A) and seven items ( = 79) measuring
depression (HADS-D). Each item is coded from 0 to 3, giving a score Table 1
varying between 0 and 21 for each scale. Variable characteristics.
Stress. Perceived stress was evaluated via the Perceived Stress Scale
Variables n M SD Skewness Kurtosis
(PSS; Cohen et al., 1983). The PSS is a 10-item scale ( = 87) designed
to measure the degree to which individuals appraise their life as HAD-A 228 1.43 .56 .77 .11 .54
stressful. HAD-D 228 .78 .49 .79 .77 .29
Happiness. Happiness was assessed using the SHS, the French ver- PSS 228 2.06 .62 .87 .08 .42
SHS 228 4.33 1.24 .89 .29 .51
sion of the Subjective Happiness Scale (Kotsou and Leys, 2017). The
AAQ 228 3.87 1.18 .90 .05 .25
measure is composed of 4 items ( = 89) scored on a 7-point Likert- MAAS 228 3.34 .74 .87 .13 .66
type scale and provides a general assessment of whether one is a happy EC-T 228 3.31 .45 .91 .05 .03
or an unhappy person. ER 228 2.59 .74 .75 .20 .38
Acceptance. The Acceptance and Action Questionnaire (AAQ-II) was
HADS-A = Hospital Anxiety and Depression Anxiety Subscale, HADS-D = Hospital
used to asses acceptance (Bond et al., 2011). It is a 7-item ( = 90)
Anxiety and Depression Subscale, PSS = Perceived Stress Scale, SHS = Subjective
measure scored on a 7-point scale (1 = strongly agree, 7 = strongly Happiness Scale, AAQ = Acceptance and Action Questionnaire, MAAS = Mindful
disagree). A high score on the AAQ means a low level of acceptance. Attention Awareness Scale, EC-T = Emotional Competence Total Score, ER = Regulation
Mindfulness. Mindfulness was assessed with the MAAS, the Mindful of Own Emotions Subscale.


I. Kotsou et al. -RXUQDORI$IIHFWLYH'LVRUGHUV  

Table 2 of overlap between the dierent predictors. Given that AAQ, MAAS, EC-
Common and unique contribution of AAQ, EC-T, ER and MAAS as predictors of psycho- T and ER are all constructs related to the way we deal with our emo-
pathology and happiness.
tions, we expected that they would share a large proportion of common
Acceptance and emotional competence single score variance. Yet, results revealed that the AAQ combined with any other
predictor (the MAAS, EC-T or ER) shared barely a sixth to less than a
AAQ+EC-T Common EC-T (sr2) AAQ (sr2) half of common variance.
R2[IC] variance
Altogether, these results show that acceptance, as measured with
Psychopathology:
HADS-A .32** [.22;.42] .07** .01 .24** the AAQ, is a robust predictor of psychological health such as anxiety,
HADS-D .25** [.15;.35] .06** .01 .18** depression, stress and happiness. These results are consistent with a
PSS .41** [.31;.51] .11** .03** .27** growing body of evidence suggesting that emotional avoidance (or its
Well-being: reversed form, emotional acceptance) is a key process in aective dis-
SHS .45** [.36;.54] .12** .01* .32**
orders (Kashdan et al., 2006) that uniquely predicts several positive
Acceptance and emotion regulation
therapeutic outcomes (Kashdan and Rottenberg, 2010). We believe the
AAQ + ER (R2) Common ER (sr2) AAQ (sr2)
current investigation supports models suggesting that emotional
variance
Psychopathology avoidance is a central and common factor in psychopathology that is
HADS-A .34** [.24;.44] .14** .03* .17** involved in the onset and maintenance of aective disorders (Barlow
HADS-D .26** [.16;.36] .10** .01 .17** et al., 2004). Emotional avoidance can therefore be seen as a main
PSS .44** [.34;.54] .20** .05** .19** factor of psychological vulnerability and emotional acceptance as a
Well-being
SHS .45** [.36;.54] .18** .02* .25**
factor of emotional resiliency, leading individuals to be less prone to
Acceptance and mindfulness develop emotional disturbances.
AAQ + MAAS Common MAAS (sr2) AAQ (sr2)
(R2) variance 5. Limitations and future directions
Psychopathology
HADS-A .34** [.24;.44] .14** .2* .18** Related to limitations, rst, this study is based on nonclinical sam-
HADS-D .24** [.14;.34] .7** .00 .17**
ples. Further studies may assess the same hypothesis with clinical
PSS .43** [.33;.53] .17** .05** .21**
Well-being
samples. Second, these results are based on cross-sectional designs,
SHS .43** [.33;.53] .7** .00 .36** longitudinal and experimental studies may be useful in order to assess
the potential protective power of acceptance over time. As an example,
R2 = adjusted R2, proportion of outcome variables variance explained by predictors; sr2 it would be interesting to measure the evolution of acceptance before
= semi-partial r2 (proportion of variance attributable only to a specic predictor). and after interventions related to mindfulness or emotional competence
** p < .001.
to conrm its relevance. Third, to measure mindfulness, we used the
* p < .05.
MAAS that only evaluates one dimension of mindfulness, attention to
the present moment. We may have dierent results using other mea-
the total score of ER and the total score of MAAS. These combined total
sures of mindfulness comprising multiple dimensions, some being more
scores signicantly predicted all scores for anxiety, depression, stress
related to emotions. Lastly, we only used self-reported measures, and
and happiness. The AAQ uniquely predicted more variance then the
thus further research should replicate these results with behavioral or
three other predictors for all outcomes. Table 2 presents the part of the
biological outcomes.
variance predicted by the combined predictors (column 2), common to
Notwithstanding these limitations, we believe that this research
the two predictors (column 3) and the part of variance that is unique to
advances our understanding of the processes involved in emotional
each of them (columns 4 & 5). In a nutshell, the results show that the
psychopathology and that some theoretical and applied conclusions can
AAQ predicted signicantly more unique variance than the other pre-
be drawn. At the theoretical level, our results suggest that many current
dictors for each of the outcomes.
theoretical approaches could very well boil down to a main common
process, the ability to accept one's emotions in order to enhance one's
4. Discussion psychological exibility. At a clinical level, our results suggest that
regardless of the type of intervention, it is important to bolster the
The aim of this study was to compare emotional acceptance with acceptance abilities of patients with psychopathology.
emotional competence, emotion regulation and mindfulness, in their
ability to predict psychological distress (stress, anxiety, depression) and Acknowledgements
psychological well-being (happiness). As expected, when introduced
individually these predictors all displayed signicant relationships with We thank Betty Chang for her help in order to correct the paper
each outcome. For the AAQ, these relations (except for one that was
moderate) were large in size, although the relations were small to Appendix A. Supporting information
moderate in the case of MAAS and EC-T, and moderate in the case of
ER. MAAS accounted for between 7 to 22% of the variance for the Supplementary data associated with this article can be found in the
dierent outcomes, ER for between 10 to 25%, EC-T for between 7 to online version at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2017.09.047.
15% while AAQ accounted for 2444% of the variance, showing the
greater predictive ability of AAQ. References
When AAQ was compared with each of the three others predictors,
for each measure of psychological distress, the AAQ uniquely accounted Brasseur, S., Grgoire, J., Bourdu, R., Mikolajczak, M., 2013. The prole of emotional
for between 4 and 10 times the variance that the EC-T, the ER and the competence (PEC): Development and validation of a self-reported measure that ts
dimensions of emotional competence theory. PLoS One 8 (5), e62635.
MAAS did. The EC-T failed to predict unique variance for anxiety and A-tjak, J.G., Davis, M.L., Morina, N., Powers, M.B., Smits, J.A., Emmelkamp, P.M., 2015.
depression, when ER and MAAS failed accordingly for depression. For A meta-analysis of the ecacy of acceptance and commitment therapy for clinically
the experience of happiness, the AAQ accounted for 30 times more relevant mental and physical health problems. Psychother. Psychosom. 84 (1),
3036. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000365764.
unique variance than the ECT-T did, almost 12 times more than the ER, Barlow, D.H., Allen, L.B., Choate, M.L., 2004. Toward a unied treatment for emotional
while the MAAS failed to predict any unique variance. disorders. Behav. Ther. 35 (2), 205230. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2016.11.
With regards to common variance, associations showed some degree 005.


I. Kotsou et al. -RXUQDORI$IIHFWLYH'LVRUGHUV  

Bocran, C., Dupret, E., 2014. A validation study of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression 02699930903407948.
Scale (HADS) in a large sample of French employees. BMC Psychiatry 14 (1). http:// Kashdan, T.B., Rottenberg, J., 2010. Psychological exibility as a fundamental aspect of
dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12888-014-0354-0. health. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 30 (7), 865878. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2010.
Bond, F.W., Hayes, S.C., Baer, R.A., Carpenter, K.M., Guenole, N., Orcutt, H.K., Zettle, 03.001.
R.D., 2011. Preliminary psychometric properties of the acceptance and action Kashdan, T.B., Barrios, V., Forsyth, J.P., Steger, M.F., 2006. Experiential avoidance as a
questionnaireII: a revised measure of psychological inexibility and experiential generalized psychological vulnerability: comparisons with coping and emotion reg-
avoidance. Behav. Ther. 42 (4), 676688. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2011.03. ulation strategies. Behav. Res. Ther. 44 (9), 13011320. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.
007. brat.2005.10.003.
Cisler, J.M., Olatunji, B.O., 2012. Emotion regulation and anxiety disorders. Curr. Kessler, R.C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Merikangas, K.R., Walters, E.E., 2005.
Psychiatry Rep. 14 (3), 182187. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11920-012-0262-2. Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the na-
Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., Mermelstein, R., 1983. A global measure of perceived stress. J. tional comorbidity survey replication. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 62 (6), 593. http://dx.
Health Soc. Behav. 385396. doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.593.
Goyal, M., Singh, S., Sibinga, E.M., Gould, N.F., Rowland-Seymour, A., Sharma, R., Kotsou, I., Leys, C., 2017. Echelle de bonheur subjectif (SHS): proprits psychomtriques
Shihab, H.M., 2014. Meditation programs for psychological stress and well-being: a de la version franaise de lchelle (SHS-F) et ses relations avec le bien-tre psy-
systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Intern. Med. 174 (3), 357368. http:// chologique, laect et la dpression. Can. J. Behav. Sci./Revue canadienne des sci-
dx.doi.org/10.1001/jamainternmed.2013.13018. ences du comportement 49 (1), 1.
Gross, J.J., 2002. Emotion regulation: aective, cognitive, and social consequences. Longfellow, H.W., 1906. Tales of a Wayside Inn. Houghton, Miin.
Psychophysiology 39 (3), 281291. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/ Martins, A., Ramalho, N., Morin, E., 2010. A comprehensive meta-analysis of the re-
S0048577201393198. lationship between emotional intelligence and health. Personal. Individ. Dier. 49
Hayes, S.C., Luoma, J.B., Bond, F.W., Masuda, A., Lillis, J., 2006. Acceptance and com- (6), 554564. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.05.029.
mitment therapy: model, processes and outcomes. Behav. Res. Ther. 44 (1), 125. Mikolajczak, M., Quoidbach, J., Kotsou, I., Nelis, D., 2009. Les comptences
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2005.06.006. motionnelles [Emotional competencies]. Dunod, Paris, France.
Jermann, F., Billieux, J., Lari, F., dArgembeau, A., Bondol, G., Zermatten, A., Van der Petrides, K., Mikolajczak, M., Mavroveli, S., Sanchez-Ruiz, M.-J., Furnham, A., Prez-
Linden, M., 2009. Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS): psychometric prop- Gonzlez, J.-C., 2016. Developments in trait emotional intelligence research. Emot.
erties of the French translation and exploration of its relations with emotion reg- Rev. 8 (4), 335341. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1754073916650493.
ulation strategies. Psychological Assess. 21 (4), 506514. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ Steel, Z., Marnane, C., Iranpour, C., Chey, T., Jackson, J.W., Patel, V., Silove, D., 2014.
a0017032. The global prevalence of common mental disorders: a systematic review and meta-
Joormann, J., Gotlib, I.H., 2010. Emotion regulation in depression: relation to cognitive analysis 19802013. Int. J. Epidemiol. 43 (2), 476493. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/
inhibition. Cogn. Emot. 24 (2), 281298. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ ije/dyu038.



Вам также может понравиться