Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 14

Available online at www.sciencedirect.

com

Electronic Commerce Research and Applications 7 (2008) 386398


www.elsevier.com/locate/ecra

eWOM overload and its eect on consumer behavioral


intention depending on consumer involvement
Do-Hyung Park a,*, Jumin Lee b
a
LG Electronics Inc., South Korea
b
Kyung Hee Cyber University, 1-Hoegi-Dong, Dongdaemun-Gu Seoul, 130-701, South Korea

Received 11 April 2007; received in revised form 20 September 2007; accepted 15 November 2007
Available online 31 December 2007

Abstract

Online consumer reviews involving experiences, evaluations and opinions on products from previous consumers play two roles an
informant and a recommender. These two roles aect a consumers intention. However, there is a conict between two roles when a large
number of reviews are oered. From the perspective of the recommender role, consumers have more favorable attitude toward a product
as a greater number of positive reviews are oered while consumers may be confronted with too much information, which results in infor-
mation overload. This study investigates what consequences the tradeo between two roles has on purchasing intention depending on
consumer involvement.
2008 Published by Elsevier B.V.

Keywords: Electronic word-of-mouth; Online consumer review; Information overload; Elaboration likelihood model; Involvement

1. Introduction dence that consumer reviews have become important for


product sales [2,9,10].
Since the development of the World Wide Web (WWW) An online consumer review as a route for social inu-
on the Internet in the early 1990s, an increasing number of ence [13] plays two roles as an informant and a recom-
companies have been trying to carry out electronic com- mender [27]. As an informant, online consumer reviews
merce (EC) [8]. Through EC, a close customer relationship deliver additional user-oriented information. As a recom-
can be formed, and much of the operating overhead includ- mender, they provide either a positive or negative signal
ing time and money can be saved. Recently, the WWW is of product popularity. Since consumers request product
used as a new marketing channel to show recommenda- information and its recommendation information simulta-
tions from previous consumers [20]. The online consumer neously for purchasing a product, in order to learn about it
review, one type of electronic word-of-mouth (eWOM), and reduce uncertainty [5,33], online consumer reviews
involves positive or negative statements made by consum- playing these two roles can completely meet consumers
ers about a product for sale in Internet shopping malls. information needs.
This consumer-created information is helpful for deci- However, what happens when consumers confront a
sion-making on purchases because it provides consumers large number of reviews? It is easily observed that some
with indirect experiences [27]. There is recent empirical evi- popular products have too many reviews in online shop-
ping malls. For example, Chevalier and Mayzlins study
*
[9] reported that the average number of reviews per book
Corresponding author. Address: LG Electronics Inc. LG Twin Towers in May 2004 was about 68.31 and the standard deviation
20, Yeouido-dong, Yeongdeungpo-gu, Seoul, 150-721, South Korea. Tel.:
+82 17 362 2261.
was 205.42. From the perspective of online consumer
E-mail address: prehero@gmail.com (D.-H. Park), jumin04@gmail. reviews as recommenders, a greater number of positive
com (J. Lee). reviews seem better for a product. However, from the

1567-4223/$ - see front matter 2008 Published by Elsevier B.V.


doi:10.1016/j.elerap.2007.11.004
D.-H. Park, J. Lee / Electronic Commerce Research and Applications 7 (2008) 386398 387

perspective of online consumer reviews as information pro- distinguished two types of inuence processes informa-
viders, consumers may be confronted with too much infor- tional and normative. Informational inuence occurs when
mation, which results in information overload. Therefore, an individual accepts information from others as evidence
as the number of reviews becomes large, there may be a about reality, e.g., evidence about what others believe to
tradeo between the negative eect from decreasing review be the true qualities of a product. Normative inuence
informativeness caused by eWOM overload, and the posi- occurs when the individual conforms to expectations of
tive eect from an increase in the strength of the signal of another person or group. Early research regarding social
product popularity. Focusing on overall positive reviews, inuence on consumer behavior [35] concluded that nor-
this study tries to nd the consequences of the tradeo. mative pressures were operative in public/group settings
This study proposes that consumer involvement should and that individuals tended to conform to group opinions.
be a key moderator to determine the consequences of the Online consumer reviews, one type of eWOM communi-
tradeo. According to the elaboration likelihood model cation, aects consumer behavior through these two
(ELM), individuals who are highly involved with a product routes. Online consumer reviews deliver others normative
are more likely to engage in thoughtful and eortful pro- beliefs, indicating whether other consumers like or dislike a
cessing of persuasive arguments. Individuals who are less product. In accordance to the valence of reviews, norma-
involved are not aected by the argument contents, but tive inuences can occur in a dierent way. When reviews
rather by non-content elements (peripheral cues) [29]. about a product are overall positive, potential consumers
High-involvement consumers may consider the informant are likely to think the product is desirable in conformity
role as being more important than the recommender role to the reviewers. In contrast, when they read overall nega-
because such consumers are willing to elaborately process tive reviews of a product, consumers may reject or dislike it
the focal messages to get additional product information because disagreeing with others is likely to cause psycho-
from online consumer reviews rather than use them as a logical discomfort.
signal of product popularity. On the other hand, low- The number of reviews can aect the level of confor-
involvement consumers may consider the recommender mity. Asch [1] found that conformity increased as a direct
role as being more important than the informant role function of the size of the reference group. Campbell and
because such consumers are not likely to elaborately Fairey [7] concluded that group size was a factor that
engage in message-processing, therefore they rely on them enhanced normative pressure. They proposed that
as a simple sign of product popularity. Finally, consumer increases in group size should have stronger eects on con-
involvement determining which part is more important formity. In the context of online consumer reviews, con-
may moderate the consequences from the tradeo between sumers regard reviewers as one group of consumers. If
the informant and recommender role, and explain the the number of positive reviews increases, the size of the ref-
change in consumer purchasing intention from the tradeo. erence group recommending a product will increase as well
In this study, controlling the review valence as overall because one previous buyer usually creates an online con-
positive, we suggest several hypotheses and conduct an sumer review. Increases in the number of reviews are likely
experiment to address the above issues. Focusing on the to lead consumers to rationalize their purchasing decision
two factors of review structure (the type and the number by telling themselves, many other people also bought
of reviews), we investigate how they aect the perceived the product, so it can be a popular product, in conformity
review informativeness (informant role) and popularity of with previous consumers, inferring that the product is
a product (recommender role) and when information over- becoming popular and its sales increases. For example, a
load occurs in the context of online consumer reviews. Fur- consumer is likely to infer that a product is more popular
thermore, we investigate what consequences the tradeo when he/she reads many positive reviews than when he/
between the informant role and the recommender role she reads a few reviews. That is supported by the empirical
has on purchasing intention depending on consumer results of previous studies on online consumer reviews,
involvement. indicating that the number of ratings has a signicant
impact on sales [9,10]. Therefore, we propose the following
2. Theoretical background and hypotheses hypothesis.
Hypothesis 1. The greater the number of positive online
2.1. The dual role of online consumer reviews
consumer reviews, the greater the perceived product
popularity.
Research on the use and inuence of recommendations
on consumers has typically been subsumed under word- Online consumer reviews play an informant role, so they
of-mouth (WOM). Consumers may follow others opinion have informational inuence like consumer conformity.
as a result of overt conformity pressures from peer groups, Online consumer reviews mainly provide user-oriented
in response to concerns about what others may think of information describing a product in terms of usage situa-
them, or they may react to their product choice and usage tions, and measure the product performance from a users
[3], or because others have provided credible information perspective [5]. The informational inuence of online con-
regarding a products value [11]. Deutsch and Gerard [13] sumer reviews can vary with the type of reviews. Since a
388 D.-H. Park, J. Lee / Electronic Commerce Research and Applications 7 (2008) 386398

reviewer who already bought a product can freely express time, and this can result in confusion, cognitive strain,
his/her evaluation without any standard format, each and other dysfunctional consequences [23]. The previous
review is dierent. Some reviews such as I cant believe I studies showed that a users eectiveness suers and his/
got this. Im proud of this are subjective, emotional and her perception of informativeness decreases when too much
have no support for arguments. These kinds of reviews information is readily available [17,18].
are simple-recommendation reviews. On the contrary, Usually, an online consumer review is created after a
some reviews provide attribute-value information such as product is sold. As a result, an increase in sales leads to an
This product is twice as fast as similar products and even increase in the number of online consumer reviews. In addi-
cheaper, which are specic, clear and having reasons for tion, at present, there is no computer tool to control the
arguments. quantity of online consumer reviews. When buying a prod-
In this study, we categorized the reviews into two types. uct, consumers have a tendency to process all available infor-
Attribute-value reviews are rational, objective and concrete mation. As a result, a large number of reviews impose a
based on the specic facts about a product. On the other heavy burden on consumers, so they may read some of them
hand, simple-recommendation reviews are emotional, sub- carefully and use skimming or scanning reading strategy to
jective, and abstract based on the consumer feeling about a catch the general idea of unselected reviews. During this
product. Much research on message contents shows that information processing, they are likely to be concerned
strong messages which are rational and objective are more about the detailed information that they miss, resulting that
eective than weak ones which are emotional and subjec- they become less satised, less condent, and more confused
tive [28,30]. According to these studies, it is predicted that when provided with too much information. Even though
attribute-value reviews are perceived as being more infor- there are overall positive reviews only, consumers may want
mative than simple-recommendation reviews. a moderate quantity of information to process completely
The impact of the number of reviews on perceived infor- not a large quantity of information to process incompletely.
mativeness of the review information set may be dierent In addition, they may even blame online sellers for this con-
according to the type of reviews. Generally, from the per- fusion from information overload caused by the lack of man-
spective of message quantity, when more reviews are aging reviews. Finally, it is predicted that these result in
oered, consumers perceive the review information set as unfavorable consequences such as a decrease in perceived
being more informative [30]. An increase in the number informativeness of the review information set.
of reviews leads to an increase in the number of dierent Traditional approaches for measuring the amount of
arguments supporting product recommendations on the information provided to consumers involve simple counts
basis of specic facts. However, an increase in the number of the number of alternatives and attributes in a choice
of simple-recommendation reviews usually delivers more set [1719]. These counts are then used to make predictions
various feelings about the product not more specic rea- about decision processes and the quality of consumer
sons for the purchase. Finally, it is predicted that the choices. However, Schneiders study [34] argues that over-
impact of the number of online consumer reviews on the load usually occurs when the nature of the information is
perceived informativeness of the review information set is uncertain, ambiguous, novel, complex, or intense. Lurie
stronger with attribute-value reviews than with simple-rec- [21] argues that the likelihood of information overload
ommendation reviews. depends on multiple structural factors of information such
as information formats or types. Information structure has
Hypothesis 2. Attribute-value reviews are perceived to be
important implications for information acquisition, the
more informative than simple-recommendation reviews.
amount of information processing, and decision quality.
Hypothesis 3. The impact of the number of reviews on per- In this study, two dimensions of review structure are con-
ceived informativeness of the review information set is sidered to predict information overload: the number of
stronger for attribute-value reviews than simple-recom- reviews and the type of reviews. In the context of online
mendation reviews. consumer reviews, information quantity may not be mea-
sured simply by counting reviews. Information overload
should be viewed from a qualitative point of view because
2.2. Information overload in the context of online consumer there are various types of reviews from simple recommenda-
reviews tion to attribute-value information. Each type of review
needs a dierent level of cognitive resources. Attribute-
Todays consumers face richer information environ- value reviews require more cognitive resources than sim-
ments than ever before [21]. Too much information brings ple-recommendation reviews. Simple-recommendation
about the information overloading phenomenon [23]. reviews provide emotional and subjective opinions with
Information overload is the phenomenon of too much the consequence that they can be understood easily at a
information overwhelming a consumer, causing adverse glance. On the other hand, it takes more eorts and a longer
judgmental decision making. Consumers limited process- time to catch the general idea of attribute-value reviews. If
ing capacity can become cognitively overloaded if they the quantity of information (the number of reviews) is the
attempt to process too much information in a limited same, the consumers who read attribute-value reviews feel
D.-H. Park, J. Lee / Electronic Commerce Research and Applications 7 (2008) 386398 389

more loaded than the ones who read simple-recommenda- on purchasing intention. The research scope of this study is
tion reviews. It is expected that information overload comes the context of overall positive reviews, so we propose the
earlier for consumers with attribute-value reviews than for positive-side hypothesis.
those with simple-recommendation reviews. Hence, we
Hypothesis 5. Perceived product popularity positively
propose the following hypothesis.
aects purchasing intention.
Hypothesis 4. Information overload comes earlier with
attribute-value reviews than with simple-recommendation Hypothesis 6. For overall positive reviews, perceived infor-
reviews. mativeness of the review information set positively aects
purchasing intention.
2.3. The eects of the dual roles of online consumer reviews
on purchasing intention 2.4. The tradeo between the informant and the
recommender in information overload
Both the perceived popularity and informativeness of the
review information set can inuence purchasing intention. The eects of the informativeness of reviews and the pop-
Previous studies suggest that consumers have the social ularity of a product on purchasing intention can be moder-
desirability to conform to the expectations of others, so they ated by the level of involvement according to the
often determine their behavior by observing the behavior of elaboration likelihood model (ELM) [30]. ELM is a social
others [36]. In addition to the inuence of signicant others psychology model concerning the information processing
such as spouses, relatives, and friends, consumers are often of persuasive messages. ELM posits that individuals who
swayed by the sheer weight of popular opinion [32]. Market- have the motivation and the ability to process a message
ers have long been aware of the power of social expectations, are more likely to process persuasion attempts via the central
and popularity claims are often used in product advertise- route. In other words, they are more likely to engage in
ments (e.g., four out of ve dentists recommend). Bettman thoughtful and eortful processing of persuasive arguments
et al. [4] argued the need to justify ones decisions to others and attend to the persuasive arguments, and then generate
leads consumers to choose popular options that provide easy their own thoughts in relation to the arguments. However,
rationales or justications. Also, according to the social individuals lacking motivation or ability are more likely to
desirability hypothesis from the study of [32], the perceived process the information via the peripheral routes, which
popularity of a target positively aects brand preference, are mental shortcuts, by focusing on non-content cues.
including brand choice, future purchase intent, monetary ELM researchers have found that issue-relevant arguments
allocation, and overall evaluation. Therefore, it is predicted and product-relevant attributes were more inuential under
that the perceived popularity of a product shown via online high-involvement conditions while peripheral cues were
consumer reviews gives evidence of social desirability, and more inuential under low-involvement conditions [28].
therefore increases purchasing intention. When consumers are involved in the low-involvement
The relationship between the perceived informativeness process of online consumer reviews, they engage in periphe-
and purchasing intention is studied in the advertisement ral processing by focusing on non-content cues such as a sig-
informativeness literature. Previous studies suggest that the nal showing the product popularity. They are not likely to
more informative the information set is, the more favorable consider the review content. Thus, they are more aected
associations consumers have, resulting in an increase in by the perceived product popularity rather than the per-
behavioral intention [30]. We expect that, if positive argu- ceived informativeness of the review information set. On
ments about a product dominate online consumer reviews, the other hand, consumers in high involvement are more
the perceived informativeness of the review information set likely to process persuasion attempts via the central route
positively aects purchasing intention. Even though con- so that review content is important for them. That is, they
sumers know that there are a few weak points from a few neg- put more weights on the perceived informativeness of the
ative reviews, consumers can understand many more good review information set rather than the perceived product
points from many positive reviews, so they follow the posi- popularity.
tive recommendation in conformity with many positive
reviewers. However, if the number of negative reviews about Hypothesis 7. For low-involvement consumers, the impact
a product increases, consumers will learn its many disadvan- of the perceived product popularity on purchasing intention
tages as well as advantages. That leads them to perceive the is greater than the impact of the perceived informativeness of
review information set as being informative, but such per- the review information set on purchasing intention.
ceived informativeness has a negative eect on purchasing
intention. Finally, it is predicted that review valence moder- Hypothesis 8. For high-involvement consumers, the
ates the relationship between perceived informativeness of impact of the perceived informativeness of the review
the review information set and consumer purchasing inten- information set on purchasing intention is greater than
tion. If the total review valence is positive (negative), the per- the impact of the perceived product popularity on pur-
ceived informativeness will have a positive (negative) eect chasing intention.
390 D.-H. Park, J. Lee / Electronic Commerce Research and Applications 7 (2008) 386398

Consumers experience information overload from a 23.6 years. Random assignment to each of the cells
large number of reviews, resulting in a decrease in the per- was performed. A reward of US$5 was given to each
ceived informativeness of the review information set. The of the participants. Most of them already had purchase
decrease is regarded as an undesirable consequence of the experiences in online shopping malls. In particular, many
informant role of online consumer reviews. On the other of them had purchased electronic goods online.
hand, the recommender role of online consumer reviews The PMP (Portable Multimedia Player) was chosen as
creates more desirable outcomes such as an increase in the experiment product. PMP is a portable next generation
the perceived product popularity along with the number multimedia player that plays digital music and video les.
of reviews. The consequences of the tradeo between the There are three reasons for choosing this as the experiment
informant role and the recommender role can be moder- product. First, electronic products are frequently pur-
ated according to the level of consumer involvement. chased in online shopping malls. Second, consumers tend
Low-involvement consumers consider perceived product to rely on the comments from previous users due to the fact
popularity as being more important than perceived infor- that electronic products are complicated. For consumers,
mativeness of the review information set. Thus, even the information obtained from sellers may not be enough
though information overload occurs, an increase in the per- for decision-making. Third, PMP is a brand-new product
ceived popularity may dominate a decrease in the perceived for general consumers. Thus, consumers processed the sug-
informativeness of reviews. It is predicted that the purchas- gested information with no stereotypes about the brand
ing intention of low-involvement consumers increases and its product category.
along with the number of all reviews. On the other hand,
when high-involvement consumers are given a large num- 3.2. Independent variables
ber of attribute-value reviews, a decrease in the perceived
informativeness of reviews may dominate an increase in 3.2.1. Online consumer reviews
the perceived popularity because the impact of the per- We created 60 reviews based on real reviews from
ceived informativeness of the review information set on online shopping malls. Each online consumer review
purchasing intention is greater than that of the perceived included a title, a poster name and contents. We con-
product popularity. Thus, it is predicted that the purchas- trolled the length of the reviews regardless review type.
ing intention of high-involvement consumers decreases The length of each review was set at 3 lines with a font
when information overload occurs. size of 10.
A focus group interview (20 participants who did not
Hypothesis 9. The purchasing intention of high-involve-
participate in the main experiment) was used to decide
ment consumers initially increases then decreases, gradu-
the level of the number of reviews. They were asked to
ally, with the number of attribute-value reviews, drawing
estimate how many reviews they considered to be small,
an inverted U shape.
moderate, or large. When surng Internet shopping
malls, these members generally read 56 reviews with
Hypothesis 10. The purchasing intention of high-involve-
34 lines. They sometimes read about 10 reviews. Along
ment consumers improves with the number of simple-rec-
these interview results, 3 was selected as small, 9 was
ommendation reviews.
selected as moderate, and 27 was selected as large. We
can nd that some products have more than 100 reviews
Hypothesis 11. The purchasing intention of low-involve-
in Internet shopping malls. Nevertheless, the condition of
ment consumers improves with the number of attribute-
a large number of reviews can be sucient in terms of
value reviews.
giving much information to cause information overload.
The attribute-value reviews contained product-relevant
Hypothesis 12. The purchasing intention of low-involve-
attribute/benet information. On the other hand, the
ment consumers improves with the number of simple-rec-
simple-recommendation reviews consisted of emotional,
ommendation reviews.
subjective recommendations. These reviews presented
subjective feelings, interjections (Wow! How wonderful!
3. Research design and method etc.) and non-relative information. We classied the 60
reviews into either attribute-value information or simple
3.1. Design, participants, and experimental system recommendation. Table 1 shows the examples. Before
the main experiment, a pretest was conducted to check
We employed a 2  3  2 factorial design. The three whether these reviews were perceived as we intended.
independent variables were review type (simple recommen- We asked other 20 participants (who did not participate
dation and attribute-value information), review quantity in the main experiment) to classify each review according
(small, moderate, and large number of reviews), and to the review type. The reviews that all participants
involvement (low and high). selected unanimously as either attribute-value informa-
Three hundred thirty four college students participated tion or simple recommendations were used for the main
in this experiment voluntarily. Their average age was experiment.
D.-H. Park, J. Lee / Electronic Commerce Research and Applications 7 (2008) 386398 391

Table 1
Review examples
Attribute-value review Simple-recommendation review
The picture on the 3.5 in. LCD monitor supports the true colors and the Woooooooow! For many days, Ive searched and compared every PMP
contrast between darks on such a small screen. This PMP converts and nally bought one. Im really enjoying it and it is tough to put it down.
recorded TV les and syncs with Windows Media Player 10. It plays Every friend envies my PMP. At this moment Im writing a review, but I
songs at top level quality with 5.1 channels. want to play my PMP!!!!!

3.2.2. Involvement of this study. The individual dierences including personal-


Consumer involvement is manipulated by giving a dier- ity, cognitive style and personal web experiences, were con-
ent scenario in terms of the amount of goal directedness trolled by randomly assigning the participants to the
[22,24]. Participants in high-involvement groups were experimental conditions [16]. The individual dierences of
asked to imagine a scenario where they should buy a general attitude toward reviews were measured by four
PMP product for their business since they worked in the items, which are reported in the appendix [27].
multimedia industry. These instructions created a high level All groups should equally perceive the quality of prod-
of goal directedness with respondents focusing their atten- uct advertisement and the degree of review positiveness
tion on PMP-related issues [22]. As opposed to the high- for the product. Four measurements for the product adver-
involvement situation, the role-playing instructions for tisement and two measurements for review positiveness are
the low-involvement situation completely lacked goal in the appendix. Other variables to change the eects of
directedness. Participants in the low-involvement condition online consumer reviews should be controlled. The experi-
were simply asked to imagine that they see the product ment product was brand-new, so product familiarity was
webpage while browsing Internet shopping sites for fun. controlled in the experiment. Also, the brand eect was
Through this manipulation, the participants in the high- no problem since we hid the brand name and information
involvement condition read and processed the product about the brand. Prior knowledge was investigated in the
information more carefully while the participants in the survey by an item with anchors ranging from Ive never
low-involvement condition did not. heard of it to I know it well. Prior knowledge is used
as a covariate variable for hypotheses testing.
3.3. Dependent variables
3.5. Experimental procedure
Consumers could assess the popularity of the product
through online consumer reviews playing a recommender At the start of the experiment, the participants were told
role. That is, consumers could infer the products popular- that all the instructions were provided in the survey and
ity from how many people bought the product and how that they should read the instructions carefully and com-
they evaluated it. To measure this variable, participants plete the experiment independently. First, the participants
were asked how many other people had bought this prod- were manipulated to imagine dierent scenarios in terms
uct and how popular the product was [36]. of involvement. Then, a product advertisement including
Consumers acquire additional user-oriented product some attribute information and reviews was given to par-
information from reviews playing an informant role. Thus, ticipants through the virtual site that looks similar to the
consumers dierently perceive the informativeness of the popular website in Korea, (see Fig. 1). The number and
review information set depending on the type and the num- type of online consumer reviews were provided according
ber of reviews. To measure this variable, four items regard- to each groups conditions. The time limit for reading the
ing the perceptions of review information (informative; stimuli was about 5 min. After the stimuli were gone, the
able to help you understand the product; useful to under- participants were asked to ll in the same questionnaire
stand the product; able to oer necessary information) including purchasing intention, manipulation checks, recall
were asked [14,26]. checks, and demographics.
Purchasing intention was measured on two seven-point
scales. The corresponding questions were: How likely is 4. Research results
it that you will buy this product? and How likely is it that
you will recommend this product to your friends? [24]. 4.1. Manipulation and control checks

3.4. Control variables All variables for manipulation and control checks are fac-
tor analyzed. Seven factors were generated. The participants
An online shopping experiment could be aected by the responses on the two items designed to check their percep-
characteristics of the participants and stimulus. Random tion of the quantity of reviews (Eigenvalue = 1.789 and
assignments were used to control the eects of possible Cronbachs alpha = 0.879) were averaged. An ANOVA
confounding variables and improve the internal validity analysis indicated the presence of the main eect of the num-
392 D.-H. Park, J. Lee / Electronic Commerce Research and Applications 7 (2008) 386398

Fig. 1. A screenshot of the virtual site.

ber of reviews (F(2, 331) = 817.935, p < .01, mean = 5.49, the experiment product using eight attributes. Among 8
3.29 and 1.67). The participants responses on the manipula- attributes, 5 were correct ones and 3 are incorrect ones. Par-
tion checks relevant to the type of reviews (Eigen- ticipants in the high-involvement condition had a greater
value = 4.475 and Cronbachs alpha = 0.967) were also number of correct answers than those in the low-involve-
examined. The ANOVA test indicated that the type of ment condition (F(1, 332) = 280.687, p < .01, mean = 7.01
reviews was manipulated as we intended (F(1, 332) = and 5.18).
50.703, p < .01, mean = 3.82 and 3.06). Finally, the involve- Among groups, there is no signicant dierences of the
ment manipulation was checked by using recall scores quality of product advertisement (Eigenvalue = 3.526,
[29,30]. Participants were asked to check the attributes of Cronbachs alpha = 0.951, F(11, 322) = 0.307, ns) and the
D.-H. Park, J. Lee / Electronic Commerce Research and Applications 7 (2008) 386398 393

degree of positiveness of reviews on the product (Eigen- information set and purchasing intention were factor ana-
value = 1.897, Cronbachs alpha = 0.938, F(11, 322) = lyzed. The factor analysis revealed that three factors (two
1.084, ns). General attitude toward reviews (Eigen- items for perceived product popularity: Eigen-
value = 2.432, Cronbachs alpha = 0.768) and prior product value = 1.997, Cronbachs alpha = 0.98; four items for per-
knowledge were signicantly dierent among groups, so ceived informativeness of the review information set:
these were used as covariate variables for hypothesis testing Eigenvalue = 3.698, Cronbachs alpha = 0.98; two items
(F(11, 322) = 1.851, p < .05; F(11, 322) = 3.396, p < .01). for purchasing intention: Eigenvalue = 1.681, Cronbachs
alpha = 0.75) were generated. The descriptive statistics of
4.2. Hypothesis testing perceived product popularity and perceived informative-
ness of the review information set are in Table 2. An
The eight items for measuring the perceived product ANCOVA was performed on the perceived product popu-
popularity, the perceived informativeness of the review larity (see Table 3). The type of reviews and the number of
reviews were used as independent variables. Results
Table 2 showed only the main eect of review quantity
Descriptive statistics of perceived popularity and perceived (F(2, 326) = 617.177, p < .01) was signicant while the
informativeness
eects of other variables including covariates were not sig-
The type of reviews The number of Perceived Perceived nicant, indicating that Hypothesis 1 is accepted. The
reviews popularity informativeness
results are shown in the graph on the left in Fig. 2.
Simple- 3 (n = 56) 1.42 (0.61) 1.53 (0.63) We performed an ANCOVA on the perceived informa-
recommendation 9 (n = 56) 3.35 (0.85) 2.59 (1.46)
tiveness of the review information set (See Table 3). Each
reviews 27 (n = 54) 5.29 (0.81) 3.36 (2.02)
covariate variable was not signicant. The analysis indi-
Attribute-value 3 (n = 56) 1.65 (0.69) 1.72 (0.78) cated the presence of two signicant main eects of the
reviews 9 (n = 56) 3.51 (0.89) 4.33 (0.87)
number of reviews (F(2, 326) = 77.901, p < .001) and the
27 (n = 56) 5.23 (0.78) 3.17 (0.99)
type of reviews (F(1, 326) = 19.407, p < .001). The review
Note: Number of subjects (n), mean of purchasing intention (standard
quantity  review quality interaction eect was also signif-
deviation).
icant (F(2, 326) = 19.063, p < .001). The results are shown
in the graph on the right in Fig. 2. Attribute-value reviews
Table 3 are perceived more informative than simple-recommenda-
ANCOVA results tion reviews, indicating that Hypothesis 2 is accepted. Also,
Eect Perceived Perceived from a few to moderate number of reviews, the impact of
popularity informativeness the number of reviews on perceived informativeness of
F Sig. F Sig. the review information set is stronger for attribute-value
Prior knowledge 1.672 0.197 0.575 0.449 reviews than simple-recommendation reviews. Thus,
General attitude toward reviews 2.114 0.147 1.426 0.233 Hypothesis 3 is accepted.
The number of reviews 617.177 0.001 77.901 0.001 To test Hypothesis 4, we investigated the change of the
The type of reviews 1.793 0.181 19.407 0.001 perceived informativeness along the number of reviews. As
The number of reviews  the type of 0.864 0.423 19.063 0.001
seen in the graph on the right in Fig. 2, the perceived infor-
reviews
mativeness of attribute-value review set initially increases,

The Type of Reviews Attribute-value reviews


Simple-recommendation reviews
6.0 6.0
Perceived Product Popularity

5.0 5.0
Perceived Informativeness

P< 0.05

P< 0.05
4.0 4.0 P< 0.05

P< 0.05
P< 0.05
3.0 3.0
P< 0.05
P< 0.05
2.0 2.0 P< 0.05

1.0 1.0
few moderate large few moderate large
The Number of Reviews The Number of Reviews

Fig. 2. The interaction eect of the type of reviews  the number of reviews for perceived popularity and perceived informativeness.
394 D.-H. Park, J. Lee / Electronic Commerce Research and Applications 7 (2008) 386398

Table 4
Descriptive statistics of purchasing intention
Low involvement High involvement
Simple-recommendation Reviews Attribute-value reviews Simple-recommendation reviews Attribute-value reviews
3 Reviews n = 28 n = 28 n = 28 n = 28
2.55 (0.67) 2.64 (0.51) 2.78 (0.417) 2.75 (0.50)
9 Reviews n = 28 n = 28 n = 28 n = 28
3.50 (0.65) 3.61 (0.53) 3.12 (0.75) 4.11 (0.63)
27 Reviews n = 28 n = 28 n = 26 n = 28
4.38 (0.81) 4.02 (0.88) 3.12 (0.75) 3.50 (0.62)
Note: Number of subjects (n), mean of purchasing intention (standard deviation).

then decreases gradually, with the number of reviews in an To nd the eects of perceived informativeness (the
inverted U shape. On the other hand, the perceived infor- informant role) and popularity (the recommender role)
mativeness of the simple-recommendation review set only on purchasing intention, we conducted regression analyses.
increases with the number of reviews. Even though partic- The independent variables were perceived informativeness
ipants in both groups were exposed to the same number of and popularity, and the dependent variable was purchasing
reviews, only the participants who read attribute-value intention. The descriptive statistics of purchasing intention
reviews experienced information overload. That is, infor- is in Table 4. Prior knowledge and general attitude toward
mation overload came earlier to participants who read reviews were also included in the regression model to
attribute-value reviews. In order to support our arguments control the level of expertise and personal attitude toward
strongly, the process measure of information overload was reviews. The results of this analysis are presented in
tested using one seven-point item: there is too much infor- Table 5. Results indicated that the eects of perceived pop-
mation in the review information set. The result showed ularity and informativeness on purchasing intention were
that participants in a large number of attribute-value positive and signicant for all participants. Thus, Hypoth-
reviews perceived that there was much more information eses 5 and 6 are accepted. To nd the moderating role of
in the review information set than the other participants. consumer involvement, we conducted additional regression
That is, ANOVA with Fishers test of least signicance dif- analyses depending on the level of involvement. As we
ference indicates that participants in a large number of expected, the strengths of the two independent variables
attribute-value reviews (M27 attribute-value reviews = 4.98) per- were dierent depending on the level of involvement. For
ceived there is much greater information loads than partic- participants in the low-involvement condition, the eect
ipants in a moderate number of attribute-value reviews of the perceived popularity (Beta = 0.496, p < 0.001) was
(M9 attribute-value reviews = 2.59, p < 0.001) and participants stronger than that of the perceived informativeness
in a large number of simple-recommendation reviews (Beta = 0.201, p < 0.001). On the other hand, the eect of
(M27 simple-recommendation reviews = 2.89, p < 0.001). Thus, the perceived informativeness (Beta = 0.389, p < 0.001)
Hypothesis 4 is accepted. was stronger than that of the perceived popularity

Table 5
Regression results
Full Low involvement High involvement
ANOVA: F-value (p-value) 43.097 (p < 0.001) 35.475 (p < 0.001) 12.963 (p < 0.001)
R-square 0.344 0.465 0.244
Constant b 2.179 1.974 2.572
t-value (p-value) 20.787 (p < 0.001) 13.892 (p < 0.001) 15.982 (p < 0.001)
Perceived popularity b 0.157 0.275 0.074
Beta 0.310 0.496 0.166
t-value (p-value) 6.215 (p < 0.001) 6.638 (p < 0.003) 2.328 (p < 0.021)
Perceived informativeness b 0.214 0.119 0.217
Beta 0.383 0.201 0.389
t-value (p-value) 7.780 (p < 0.001) 2.740 (p < 0.007) 5.402 (p < 0.001)
Prior knowledge b 0.059 0.232 0.176
Beta 0.033 0.117 0.116
t-value (p-value) 0.745 (p < 0.457) 1.987 (p < 0.049) 1.637 (p < 0.104)
General attitude toward reviews b 0.002 0.025 0.033
Beta 0.002 0.028 0.040
t-value (p-value) 0.046 (p < 0.964) 0.485 (p < 0.629) 0.577 (p < 0.565)
D.-H. Park, J. Lee / Electronic Commerce Research and Applications 7 (2008) 386398 395

(Beta = 0.166, p < 0.021) for participants in the high- ing intention, but think the additional reviews have no new
involvement condition. The results mean that low-involve- information for their decision making. In addition, they
ment consumers consider the recommender role as being put less weight on the signal of popularity which becomes
more important than the informant role, but high-involve- apparent along with the number of reviews. Finally, high-
ment consumers consider the informant role as being more involvement consumers change their purchasing intention
important than the recommender role. Since involvement is to some degree, but retain it despite a continuous increase
related to the motivation of information processing, high- in the number of reviews. Our Hypothesis 10 is rejected.
involvement consumers are willing to elaborately process For low-involvement consumers, the purchasing inten-
the focal messages to get additional product information tion increases with the number of reviews regardless of
from online consumer reviews rather than use them as a the type of reviews. An increase in the perceived popularity
signal of product popularity. In contrast, low-involvement dominates a decrease in the perceived informativeness, and
consumers are not likely to elaborately engage in message- the result is that the purchasing intention of low-involve-
processing, therefore rely on them as a simple sign of prod- ment consumers improves with the number of all reviews.
uct popularity. Thus, Hypotheses 7 and 8 are accepted. However, the degree of the increase in the purchasing
When a large number of attribute-value reviews are intention is greater when simple-recommendation reviews
oered, consumers experience information overload. Infor- are oered than when attribute-value reviews are oered
mation overload results in a decrease in the perceived infor- because participants in the low-involvement condition also
mativeness of the review information set. However, the experienced information overload caused by a large num-
perceived product popularity increases with the number ber of attribute-value reviews. Finally, Hypotheses 11
of reviews regardless of the occurrence of information and 12 are accepted.
overload. To nd which one dominates, we examined the
purchasing intention for each condition. The results are 5. Conclusion
in Fig. 3. For high-involvement consumers, the purchasing
intention has an inverted U shape as the number of attri- Four major ndings emerge from this research. First,
bute-value reviews increases. From the moderate number this study hypothesized and validated the eects of two fac-
to the large number of reviews, a decrease in the perceived tors of the review structure (the number and the type of
informativeness dominates an increase in the perceived reviews) on the dual roles of online consumer reviews
popularity. Thus, Hypothesis 9 is accepted. When simple- (the informant role and the recommender role). The num-
recommendation reviews are given to high-involvement ber of reviews increases the perceived popularity of a prod-
consumers, purchasing intention slightly increases until uct (the recommender role). The type of reviews and the
there are a moderate number of reviews. However, there number of reviews increase the perceived informativeness
is no further change with a large number of reviews. The of the review information set (the informant role), and
persuasiveness of messages is important for high-involve- the interaction eect also exists.
ment consumers, but simple-recommendation reviews are Second, this study shows information overload can
less persuasive in changing their purchasing intention occur in the context of eWOM. Previous studies on
greatly. High-involvement consumers may use the rst sev- WOM suggest that the greater the number of positive
eral simple-recommendation reviews to form their purchas- WOM messages is, the better it is for the product

Attribute-value reviews
The Type of Reviews
Simple-recommendation reviews
4.5 4.5
Low Involvement High Involvement
P< 0.05 P< 0.05
4.0 4.0 P< 0.05
Purchasing Intention
Purchasing Intention

P< 0.05 P< 0.05


3.5 P< 0.05 3.5
P< 0.05

Not
3.0 P< 0.05 3.0
P< 0.05 Significant

2.5 2.5

2.0 2.0
few moderate large few moderate large

The Number of Reviews The Number of Reviews

Fig. 3. The interaction eect of the type of reviews  the number of reviews  involvement for purchasing intention.
396 D.-H. Park, J. Lee / Electronic Commerce Research and Applications 7 (2008) 386398

[9,27,31]. These studies have not considered information is related to the motivation of information processing,
overload. Since WOM plays the informant role as well as high-involvement consumers are willing to elaborately pro-
the recommender role, consumers can experience informa- cess the focal messages to get additional product informa-
tion overload. Schneiders study [34] argues that overload tion from online consumer reviews rather than use them as
usually occurs when the nature of the information is uncer- a signal of product popularity. In contrast, low-involve-
tain, ambiguous, novel, complex, or intense. In addition to ment consumers are not likely to elaborately engage in
such nature of information, this study argues that, even message-processing, therefore rely on them as a simple sign
though the information quantity (total length of given of product popularity. The results indicate that low-
reviews) is the same, the information type also can be a fac- involvement consumers consider the recommender role as
tor aecting the information overload. Information over- being more important than the informant role, but high-
load comes earlier when the messages requiring more involvement consumers consider the informant role as
cognitive resources are oered. The results emphasize being more important than the recommender role. Thus,
how to manage reviews from the qualitative perspective the tradeo between the informant role and the recom-
as well as quantitative perspective in order to prevent con- mender role is resolved from these relations. For low-
sumers from experiencing information overload. involvement consumers, an increase in the perceived popu-
One more nding of this study is that the consequences larity dominates a decrease in the perceived informative-
of information overload are not always bad. Online con- ness of reviews. On the other hand, for high-involvement
sumer reviews play a recommender role as well as an infor- consumers, a decrease in the perceived informativeness of
mant role, so a large number of reviews deliver the signal of reviews dominates an increase in the perceived popularity.
product popularity with a variety of product information. Hence, when information overload occurs, the purchasing
This study shows that low-involvement consumers focusing intention of low-involvement consumers increases while
on perceived popularity overcome the information over- that of high-involvement consumers decreases.
load, resulting in an increase of their purchasing intention. There are some limitations to this study. First, this study
Previous studies usually reported the negative conse- focuses only on overall positive reviews. There are three
quences of information overload such as a decrease in deci- reasons that we investigate only positive reviews. First,
sion quality [1719]. However, this study argues the the inclusion of negative reviews creates many confounding
consumer characteristics such as consumer involvement issues such as negativity, credibility of review sources, or
can moderate the outcomes of information overload by two-side eects. The research method of this study is an
determining which part of the information is the focus. experimental method. It is quite hard to nd a relationship
Online sellers have used some tools to help with eWOM among 4 or 5 variables. So, we controlled other variables
overload. They have used a tool that shows a summary of except the number of reviews, the type of reviews, and con-
the information that represents how many reviews there are sumer involvement. Second, in reality, there are a few neg-
and how good the overall evaluation is. In addition, a tool ative reviews. Dellarocas and Narayan [12] reported that
to show the rst line and hide the remaining lines of each 99.1% of customer feedback on eBay is positive, followed
review may reduce information overload. Nevertheless, by negative (0.6%) and neutral (0.3%). Chevalier and
online sellers still need to make more eort to manage Mayzlin [9], who investigated the eect of consumer
online consumer reviews. We propose several new ways reviews on sales of books at Amazon.com and Barnesand-
to overcome eWOM overload. First, online sellers can give noble.com, also reported that reviews are overwhelmingly
reviews with a standardized format. If the review format is positive at both sites. These studies can support our
standardized, consumers can understand reviews more eas- research scope. In addition, positive WOM and negative
ily and spend less time to collect information they want. WOM have been studied separately in most studies on
Second, online sellers can give personalized reviews by word-of-mouth. Finally, this study tries to address the
using personal information that consumers input when reg- main research question: What happens if there are many
istering membership. Since each consumer has a dierent positive reviews? It is natural that the increase in the num-
evaluation scheme, online sellers can meet their informa- ber of positive reviews (messages of positive WOM) has a
tion needs by giving personalized reviews. Also, the use positive eect on consumer evaluation of a product. This
of a specialized search engine only for the review informa- study argues this natural state cannot be true in a certain
tion set can be useful for consumers to collect what they case, where information overload occurs.
want. Third, online sellers can present the qualitative sum- Consumers may focus on the qualitative comments of
mary information as well as the quantitative summary negative comments and the quantitative summary of positive
information. If online sellers organize the review contents comments. This study and other studies on online consumer
by product attributes or pros/cons, and deliver this orga- reviews report that the qualitative comments also aect con-
nized review summary with the qualitative statistics such sumer behavioral intention by showing the eect of review
as average star-rating scores, consumers are less likely to quality or review length [9,27]. However, we agree that con-
experience eWOM overload. sumers are more likely to focus on the qualitative aspect for
Finally, we show the moderating role of consumer negative reviews and the quantitative aspect for positive
involvement on the WOM processing. Since involvement reviews. This issue can be a good research topic.
D.-H. Park, J. Lee / Electronic Commerce Research and Applications 7 (2008) 386398 397

There is another issue for future research in terms of the also showed that multiple sources with dierent back-
credibility of reviews. Online consumer reviews are created grounds enhance the persuasiveness of arguments. These
by unknown former purchasers, so consumers can wonder studies can support that the number of reviews each cre-
whether reviews are true or not. When consumers read ated by an individual consumer enhances the persuasive-
overall positive reviews, such doubt may be stronger ness of reviews, even when reviews are overall positive.
because, as you pointed, consumers can worry that compa- The increase in the persuasiveness of the reviews continues
nies remove negative reviews or only list the most positive to increase the perceived popularity of a product, too. The
ones. However, we think that such doubt can become eects of online consumer reviews can be generalized
weaker as the number of reviews increases. That is, con- through these further studies.
sumers are more worried about whether the reviews are
true or manipulated or not when there are a few positive Acknowledgements
reviews only, rather than there are many positive reviews.
Moore and Reardon [25] showed that multiple product The comment and guidance of Ingoo Han and Min-
endorsers presenting strong supporting arguments enhance hi Hahn are very gratefully acknowledged. The authors
the persuasiveness of advertising appeal. In a hypothetical would also like to thank the editor and reviewers for
jury trial, for example, Calder et al. [6] demonstrated that valuable comments. We thank Byungyoul Park, Jun-
participants generate more thoughts in favor of the defense ghee Kang, Kiho Lee, and Joung-A Min for idea gen-
(prosecution) after they were exposed to a greater number eration and helpful input on an earlier version of this
of defense (prosecution) arguments. Harkins and Petty [15] work.

Appendix

Please indicate the degree to which you would agree with the following statements by choosing a number from 1 to 6,
where 1 indicates strongly disagree and 6 indicates strongly agree. (The same scale was used for each of the questions
below.)

General Attitude toward Reviews (1) When I buy a product online, I always read reviews that are
presented on the website.
(2) When I buy a product online, the reviews presented on the website
are helpful for my decision making.
(3) When I buy a product online, the reviews presented on the website
make me condent in purchasing the product.
(4) If I dont read the reviews presented on the website when I buy a
product online, I worry about my decision.
The Type of Reviews (1) Each review has sucient reasons supporting the opinions.
Each review is (2) objective/(3) product-relevant/(4) rational.
(5) Each review has attribute-value information.
The Number of Reviews (1) The number of reviews is large.
(2) The quantity of review information is great.
Review Positiveness (1) The reviewers in the given page positively evaluate the product.
(2) In general, the reviewers in the given page recommend the product.
Product Advertisement The product information is (1) objective/(2) understandable/(3) credible/
(4) clear.

References [4] J.R. Bettman, E.J. Johnson, J.W. Payne, Consumer decision making,
in: T.S. Robertson, H.H. Kassarjian (Eds.), Handbook of Consumer
[1] S.E. Asch, Eects of group pressure upon the modication and Behavior, Prentice Hall, Englewood Clis, NJ, 1991.
distortion of judgments, in: H. Guetzkow (Ed.), Groups, Leadership [5] B. Bickart, R.M. Schindler, Internet forums as inuential sources of
and Men, Carnegie Press, Pittsburgh, PA, 1951, pp. 177190. consumer information, Journal of Interactive Marketing 15 (3) (2001)
[2] S. Ba, P.A. Pavlou, Evidence of the eect of trust in electronic 3140.
markets: price premiums and buyer behavior, MIS Quart. 26 (3) [6] B.J. Calder, C.A. Insko, B. Yandell, The relation of cognitive and
(2002) 243267. memorial processes to persuasion in a simulated jury trial, Journal of
[3] W.O. Bearden, R.L. Rose, Attention to social comparison informa- Applied Social Psychology 4 (1) (1974) 6293.
tion: an individual dierence factor aecting consumer conformity, [7] J.D. Campbell, P.J. Fairey, Informational and normative routes to
Journal of Consumer Research 16 (4) (1990) 461471. conformity: the eect of faction size as a function of norm extremity
398 D.-H. Park, J. Lee / Electronic Commerce Research and Applications 7 (2008) 386398

and attention to the stimulus, Journal of Personality and Social [22] D. Maheswaran, B. Sternthal, The eects of knowledge, motivation,
Psychology 57 (1989) 457468. and type of message on ad processing and product judgments, Journal
[8] P.Y.K. Chau, P.J.H. Hu, B.L.P. Lee, A.K.K. Au, Examining of Consumer Research 17 (1) (1990) 6673.
customers trust in online vendors and their dropout decisions: an [23] N.K. Malhotra, Reections on the information overload paradigm in
empirical study, Electronic Commerce Research and Applications 6 consumer decision making, Journal of Consumer Research 10 (4)
(2) (2007) 171182. (1984) 436440.
[9] J.A. Chevalier, D. Mayzlin, The eect of word of mouth on sales: [24] J. Meyers-Levy, L.A. Peracchio, Moderators of the impact of self-
online book reviews, Journal of Marketing Research 43 (3) (2006) reference on persuasion, Journal of Consumer Research 22 (4) (1996)
345354. 408423.
[10] E.K. Clemons, G.G. Gao, L.M. Hitt, When online reviews meet [25] D.J. Moore, R. Reardon, Source magnication: the role of multiple
hyperdierentiation: a study of the craft beer industry, Journal of sources in the processing of advertising appeals, Journal of Marketing
Management Information Systems 23 (2) (2006) 149171. Research 24 (November) (1987) 412417.
[11] J.B. Cohen, E. Golden, Informational social inuence and product [26] S. Negash, T. Ryanb, M. Igbariab, Quality and eectiveness in web-
evaluation, Journal of Allied Psychology 56 (1972) 5459. based customer support systems, Information & Management 40 (8)
[12] C. Dellarocas, R. Narayan, A statistical measure of a populations (2003) 757768.
propensity to engage in post-purchase online word-of-mouth, Statis- [27] D.-H. Park, J. Lee, I. Han, The eects of on-line consumer reviews on
tical Science 21 (2) (2006) 277285. consumer purchasing intention: the moderating role of involvement,
[13] M. Deutsch, H. Gerard, A study of normative and informational International Journal of Electronic Commerce 11 (4) (2007) 125148.
social inuences upon individual judgment, Journal of Abnormal and [28] R.E. Petty, J.T. Cacioppo, D. Schumann, Central and peripheral
Social Psychology 57 (1955) 629636. routes to advertising eectiveness: the moderating role of involve-
[14] S.M. Edward, H. Li, J.H. Lee, Forced exposure and psychological ment, Journal of Consumer Research 10 (2) (1983) 135146.
reactance: antecedents and consequences of the perceived intrusive- [29] R.E. Petty, J.T. Cacioppo, R. Goldman, Personal involvement as a
ness of pop-up ads, Journal of Advertising 31 (3) (2002) 8396. determinant of argument-based persuasion, Journal of Personality
[15] S.G. Harkins, R.E. Petty, Information utility and multiple source and Social Psychology 41 (5) (1981) 847855.
eect, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 52 (2) (1987) 260 [30] R.E. Petty, J.T. Cacioppo, The eects of involvement on response to
268. argument quantity and quality: central and peripheral routes to
[16] W. Hong, J.Y.L. Thong, K.Y. Tam, The eects of information format persuasion, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 46 (1)
and shopping task on consumers online shopping behavior: a (1984) 6981.
cognitive t perspective, Journal of Management Information [31] J.E. Phelps, R. Lewis, L. Mobilio, D. Perry, N. Raman, Viral
Systems 21 (3) (2004) 149184. marketing or electronic word-of-mouth advertising: examining con-
[17] J. Jacoby, D.E. Speller, C.K. Berning, Brand choice behavior as a sumer responses and motivations to pass along email, Journal of
function of information load, replication and extension, Journal of Advertising Research 44 (4) (2005) 333348.
Consumer Research 1 (6) (1974) 3341. [32] A. Rindeisch, J.J. Inman, Explaining the familiarity-liking relation-
[18] J. Jacoby, D.E. Speller, C.A. Kohn, Brand choice behavior as a ship: mere exposure, information availability, or social desirability?
function of information load, Journal of Marketing Research 11 (1) Marketing Letters 9 (1) (1998) 519.
(1974) 6369. [33] D.L. Rosen, R.W. Olshavsky, A protocol analysis of brand choice
[19] K.L. Keller, R. Staellin, Eects of quality and quantity of information strategies involving recommendations, Journal of Consumer Research
on decision eectiveness, Journal of Consumer Research 14 (2) (1987) 14 (3) (1987) 440444.
200213. [34] S.C. Schneider, Information overload: causes and consequences,
[20] T.R. Lee, J.M. Li, Key factors in forming an e-marketplace: an Human Systems Management 7 (2) (1987) 143153.
empirical analysis, Electronic Commerce Research and Applications 5 [35] M. Venkatesan, Experimental study of consumer behavior conformity
(2) (2006) 105116. and independence, Journal of Marketing Research 3 (1966) 384387.
[21] N.H. Lurie, Decision making in information-rich environments: the [36] J.H. Zhu, Z. He, Perceived characteristics, perceived needs, and
role of information structure, Journal of Consumer Research 30 (4) perceived popularity, Communication Research 29 (4) (2002) 466
(2004) 473486. 495.
-

www.xuebalib.com

24 IP

Вам также может понравиться