Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 18

Available online at www.sciencedirect.

com
Journal of
CONSUMER
PSYCHOLOGY
Journal of Consumer Psychology 22 (2012) 37 54

Managing sensory expectations concerning products and brands:


Capitalizing on the potential of sound and shape symbolism
Charles Spence
Crossmodal Research Laboratory, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, South Parks Road, Oxford, OX1 3UD, UK

Received 28 June 2011; received in revised form 13 September 2011; accepted 14 September 2011
Available online 10 November 2011

Abstract

In this article, the evidence demonstrating the existence of a variety of robust crossmodal correspondences between both sounds (phonetic
speech sounds, tones, and other parameters of musical expression) and shapes, and the sensory attributes (specically the taste, avor, aroma,
and oral-somatosensory attributes) of various foods and beverages is reviewed. The available research now clearly suggests that marketers can en-
hance their consumers' product experiences by ensuring that the sound symbolism of the brand name, as well as any shape symbolism of/on the
labeling, and even the very shape of the packaging itself, sets up the right (i.e., congruent) product-related sensory expectations in the mind of the
consumer. In this review, the rapidly-growing literature on the topic of sound and shape symbolism is critically evaluated. Potential caveats, lim-
itations, and problems of interpretation with previous studies are highlighted. The question of whether this approach to sensory marketing should
be considered as implicit (or functionally subconscious) is also addressed. Finally, some of the relative strengths and weaknesses of this approach
to modulating a consumer's product-related expectations (relative to various other approaches) are considered.
2011 Society for Consumer Psychology. Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Sound symbolism; Shape symbolism; Crossmodal correspondences; Congruency; Sensation transference; Multisensory; Packaging; Branding; Labeling

Introduction softness is influenced by olfactory cues (Churchill, Meyners,


Griffiths, & Bailey, 2009; Dematt, Sanabria, Sugarman, &
Most of our everyday experiences, at least the pleasant ones, Spence, 2006; Laird, 1932), and crispness turns out to be as
are multisensory. A consumer's brand and product experiences much a matter of what we hear, as about what we actually feel
are no exception, as many sensory marketers are increasingly in the mouth (see Spence & Shankar, 2010; Spence, Shankar, &
coming to realize (e.g., Hultn, 2011; Hultn, Broweus, & van Blumenthal, 2011; Zampini & Spence, 2004).
Dijk, 2009; Krishna, 2010; Lindstrom, 2005; Spence, 2002). One aspect of multisensory perception that has started to
That said, introspection often tells us that we see color only gain increasing importance over the last couple of years relates
with our eyes, that we feel softness exclusively with our finger- to the topic of crossmodal correspondences (Spence, 2011a,
tips, and that we taste the crunch of the potato chip only with 2011b, 2011c). Generally-speaking, crossmodal correspon-
our mouths. However, the empirical evidence that has emerged dences can be defined as a tendency for a feature, or attribute,
from the psychology and neuroscience laboratories over the last in one sensory modality to be matched (or associated) with a
few years tells a very different story. In fact, it has now become sensory feature, or attribute in another sensory modality (Parise
increasingly apparent that what we see, and how we feel about & Spence, in press a). One ubiquitous crossmodal correspon-
it, are also influenced by what we happen to be smelling at the dence is that between larger objects (no matter whether seen
time (Dematt, sterbauer, & Spence, 2007; Li, Moallem, Paller, or felt) and lower-pitched sounds, and smaller objects and
& Gottfried, 2007; Spence, 2008). Similarly, our perception of higher-pitched sounds (Parise & Spence, 2009; Walker &

Fax: +44 1865 310447.


E-mail address: charles.spence@psy.ox.ac.uk.

1057-7408/$ - see front matter 2011 Society for Consumer Psychology. Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jcps.2011.09.004
38 C. Spence / Journal of Consumer Psychology 22 (2012) 3754

Smith, 1985). In this case, at least, the correspondence reflects a successful, brandnames think K-Mart, IKEA, TK Maxx (in
fundamental law of physics. The two classes of crossmodal cor- UK, TJ Maxx in North America), etc.
respondence that I wish to look at in this article are commonly- Other commentators, meanwhile, have been tempted to attri-
referred to as sound and shape symbolism. Sound symbolism is bute the historic failure of certain brands, such as the Ford
the name given to the association that people experience be- Edsel, named after Edsel Ford, a relation of the company's foun-
tween specific sounds (including speech sounds) and particular der (Wallace, 1975), and launched back in 1958, to the poor
stimulus attributes (e.g., as when they associate words contain- choice of brand name by the marketers concerned (Hartley,
ing the i sound with smallness). Shape symbolism refers to the 1992; see also Cheskin, 1972; http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Edsel,
similar crossmodal mapping that exists between abstract shapes especially Section 3.3, downloaded on 14/07/2011; Vanden
and other sensory attributes (e.g., as between sharp pointy Bergh et al., 1987). In this regard, it is interesting to note how
shapes and bitterness or carbonation in foods and beverages). many of Ford's successful car models in more recent times have
Put simply, when the different sensory attributes of a product, had names ending in the letter a think Ford Cortina, Ford Fi-
or its packaging, or of the environment in which that product is esta, Ford Granada, Ford Sierra, and Ford Ka. Other seemingly
purchased, used, or experienced match (or correspond) crossmod- non-coincidental combinations of letters appearing in brand
ally, then this can impact positively on the consumer's overall names include the letters 'i', and often also 'd', and l found in
multisensory consumer experience. In this chapter, I want to re- the majority of the budget supermarket and store chains currently
view the evidence demonstrating that our rapidly growing under- doing so well in the UK (e.g., Aldi, Lidl, Londis, Iceland,
standing of the psychological mechanisms underlying crossmodal and Primark; Lotto may be one of the few exceptions to this gen-
correspondences, and in particular sound and shape symbolism, eral pattern). Is this more than mere coincidence, one might ask? I
can be used to set up the appropriate sensory expectations in the would argue that in many cases it is. What is more, scientific re-
mind of the consumer which, in turn can enhance the consumer's search is now starting to provide some intriguing insights into
multisensory experience of both products and brands. the putative principles underlying many of these examples of
In order to try and keep this article manageable, I will focus on sound symbolism in the marketplace.
the use of sound and shape symbolism in the food and beverage For many years now, researchers have argued that there is a
sectors. However, it should be noted that the findings outlined non-arbitrary relationship between the words, or rather, the speech
here are equally applicable to a number of different classes of sounds, that we happen to use to name objects and the properties
product, everything from portable electronic goods to prescription that those objects possess (see Hinton, Nichols, & Ohala, 1994;
medications, and from fast-moving consumer goods (FMCGs) to Nuckolls, 2003; Robson, 2011, for reviews). As will become ap-
major household investments, such as cars (e.g., Abel & Glinert, parent below, many of the consonant and vowel sounds found in
2008). Interestingly, many of these crossmodal effects appear to English, not to mention in other languages, appear to have different
operate on an implicit level (i.e., without the consumer necessarily crossmodal associations linked to them. Of course, there is always
being aware of what is going on). Hence, it is not uncommon to a danger here that one can read too much into apparent examples of
find that, when asked, consumers deny being influenced by sound symbolism, seeing patterns where all there really is, is mere
sound and shape symbolism, and yet carefully controlled studies coincidence (as may well be the case in the case of Ford cars end-
of their behavior, in both the laboratory and in the marketplace, ing in -a; see also Saussure, 1916). Fortunately, however, statisti-
tell a very different story (see also Cheskin, 1972). cal techniques now exist with which to determine the significance
of such patterns of crossmodal correspondence (see Spence,
2011c), both in the psychology or linguistics laboratory, and,
Sound symbolism in the marketplace more importantly, out there in the marketplace.

Marketers have long been aware of the non-arbitrary relation-


ship that exists between a brand's success in the marketplace and When i = small: Mil, mal, and the success of the Mini!
the speech sounds that are contained in the brand name itself. So,
for example, 30 years ago, Ira Schloss who was, at the time, assis- I will illustrate the principle of sound symbolism with an exam-
tant research director of the Advertising Research Foundation, ple: There are good grounds to believe that brands that are associ-
noted that the letter k was significantly over-represented ated with small objects, and/or companies that are associated with
among the top 200 brand names during the late 1970s think low prices, would do well to include the [i] sound in their brand
Kraft, Kellogg's, Kodak, KFC, etc. (Schloss, 1981). He also name. Indeed, this may be the reason why, as we just saw, the let-
pointed out that there were far fewer brands starting with the letter ter i appears in so many of the names of those successful budget
s than would have been expected by chance alone, given the dis- supermarket chains. It likely also explains, at least in some small
tribution of words in the dictionary starting with this letter. Fol- part, the continued success of the Mini car brand. Could a large
low-up studies published a few years later came to essentially car ever have succeeded with such a small-sounding name? 1
the same conclusion regarding the success of brand names begin- (In fact, the word mini actually means tiny in French as an
ning with the k sound (Vanden Bergh, 1990; Vanden Bergh,
Adler, & Oliver, 1987; see also Abel & Glinert, 2008). Indeed, 1
True, the Innity car marque has more i's than a Mini, but in this case,
the publication of such findings may help to explain the promi- the semantic meaning of the word may override the sound symbolic associa-
nence of this letter in so many of today's most well-known, and tions present in the word itself.
C. Spence / Journal of Consumer Psychology 22 (2012) 3754 39

adjective and a prefix, as in the English word miniskirt, but is used Consonants are often grouped into categories such as stop con-
much more commonly than in English). sonants and fricatives. In this review, I will focus on those words
The first researcher to pick-up on the association between the that start with (or contain) plosive stops, that is, words with any
letter i and smallness was Edgar Sapir back in 1929. He demon- one of the following letters b, hard c (as in Coke), d, g,
strated that the majority of people (80%), when given the k, p, q, and t. Note that the abrupt popping sound of a plo-
choice, chose the nonsense word Mil to describe a small table, sive stop consonant involves the complete closure of the articula-
and the nonsense word Mal to describe a large table (rather tors. In the English language, air is prevented from escaping from
than the other way round). Sapir went on to highlight the fact the oral cavity when making a plosive sound by placing the ton-
that the same sound symbolic relationship was observable in the gue against the roof of the mouth (alveolar ridge, palate, or
speakers of various different languages, including in a group of velum) or lips (or glottis, if you include glottal stops). This is fol-
Chinese participants (see also Hinton et al., 1994; Newman, lowed by a sudden explosive release of air from the mouth. By
1933). These results were taken to show that this particular exam- contrast, nasal stops, otherwise known as nasals, are continuants.
ple of sound symbolism probably had more to do with the speech They involve the continuous release of air from the nose rather
sounds themselves than with any semantic meaning that these par- than the mouth, as in the case of m and n. Note that nasals
ticular vowel sounds might have (e.g., as in English words such as are stops by virtue of the complete closure of the articulators
millimeter, miniscule, tiddler, tinsy, etc. vs. words such as large, that is involved (rather than as a result of the manner of air re-
grand, etc.). lease). The sudden release of air that is characteristic of plosive
Subsequent research has demonstrated that the mapping of i to stop consonants therefore contrasts with the slow and continuous
small (and a to large) constitutes one of the more robust examples release of air in both fricative consonants (as in s, f, v, and z)
of sound symbolism to have been discovered to date (Hinton et al., and nasal stop consonants. Generally-speaking, fricatives tend to
1994; Newman, 1933). In fact, over the years since Sapir's (1929) produce higher-frequency sounds than stop consonants. Words
ground-breaking study was first published, sound symbolism ef- containing fricative (as compared to stop) consonants will, all
fects associated with a wide variety of different vowels and conso- other things being equal, be perceived as smaller, faster, lighter
nants have been documented (see Hinton et al., 1994; Nuckolls, (in weight), and more feminine (Klink, 2000).
2003). What is more, these effects have been demonstrated in Another important classification, but one that will not be fo-
many different cultures (e.g., see Davis, 1961; though see also cused on here, is that between voiced and voiceless consonants.
Diffloth, 1994; Ultan, 1978). Voiced consonants such as the fricatives z and v involve the
vibration of the vocal chords, whereas voiceless fricatives do
Phonetics and sound symbolism not. In the case of plosives, the vibration of the vocal fold begins
earlier for voiced consonants such as b, g, and d than for
One problem for research in the area of sound (or phonetic) voiceless consonants such as p, k, and t. Brand names con-
symbolism is that the literature is both broad and diverse (e.g., taining voiceless (as compared to voiced) stop consonants are
see Hinton et al., 1994). Hence, in order to keep things manage- perceived as smaller, sharper, more feminine, faster, and lighter
able in this review, I want to focus primarily on the sound symbol- in weight, than words containing voiced consonants (see Abel
ism effects associated with the distinction between front and back & Glinert, 2008; Klink, 2000). Meanwhile, brand names contain-
vowel sounds, and with the class of stop consonants known as plo- ing a voiceless (as opposed to a voiced) fricative, will be per-
sives (see Ladefoged, 1993; MacKay, 1978). Consonants and ceived as faster, more feminine, and softer.
vowels are discriminated from one another by their manner of ar-
ticulation: The front/back vowel distinction, for example, refers to Sound symbolism and brand name development
the location of the tongue in the oral cavity during pronunciation.
Front vowels are those where the highest point of the tongue is in Marketers, and others charged with the development of novel
the front of the mouth (e.g., as when pronouncing the i in hit or brand names for products/companies, have apparently taken the
bin). By contrast, back vowels are produced when the highest implications of the large body of research on the topic of sound
point of the tongue is in the back of the mouth instead (e.g., as symbolism very much to heart (e.g., see Abel & Glinert, 2008;
when pronouncing the u sound in bun or the o sound in Begley, 2002; Belli, 2001; Collins, 1977; Douglas, 1981; Lowrey
home). The front/back distinction can be thought of as a continu- & Shrum, 2007; McNeil, 1982, pp. 228230; Vanden Bergh et al.,
um going from the frontal vowel sounds in words like beat and 1984; Shrum & Lowrey, 2007). Indeed, brand names are now
bit through bet, bait, bat, boat, bought, posh, but, widely recognized as constituting an important component of a
put, and finally boot (these examples are taken from Yorkston brand's overall capital (King & Cook, 1990; McNeil, 2003; Zin-
& Menon, 2004). Generally-speaking, frontal vowels are higher in khan & Martin, 1987). Over the last decade or so, a number of
pitch than back vowels. According to the results of one study, studies have been published demonstrating that the consonant
brand names containing such frontal (as compared to back) vowels and vowel sounds incorporated into a variety of putatively new
(think Tiffany and Pepsi) will be perceived as smaller, faster, ligh- (but actually fictitious) brand names are associated with very dif-
ter (as compared to darker), lighter (as compared to heavier), ferent attributes in the minds of consumers. In fact, the sound sym-
milder, softer, weaker, thinner, colder, prettier, friendlier, more bolism approach has been used by marketers to study the
feminine, and, finally, more bitter (Klink, 2000; cf. Walker & development of brand names for products in many different sec-
Smith, 1985). tors everything from consumer electronics through to
40 C. Spence / Journal of Consumer Psychology 22 (2012) 3754

prescription medication (Abel & Glinert, 2008; McNeil, 2003). with those present in the name of the attribute that they were
Typically, in order to assess the meaning that people associate supposed to be judging (the /I/ sound appears in both the pre-
with particular speech sounds, researchers give participants a ferred brand name bilad and in the taste descriptor bitter).
pair of meaningless names, that vary in just a single speech The question of just how many of the results reported by
sound, and ask them to pick one of the two that better represents Klink, Yorkston and Menon (2004), and others in this area
(or matches) the concept, or attribute, under consideration. Here, can be explained in this manner is one that will require further
though, I will restrict the discussion to those results that are rele- analysis/research in order to resolve unequivocally.
vant specifically to the naming of food and beverage products. A second potential limitation with the interpretation of the
For example, in one study, Yorkston and Menon (2004) dem- two studies just mentioned concerns the fact that they were car-
onstrated that people's impressions of a new food product could ried out on undergraduate students. As a consequence, it is hard
be shaped by the vowel sounds that were contained in the product to know how generalizable the findings would be to other
name itself. Undergraduates were more likely to believe that an populations and/or to other age groups (e.g., see Henrich,
ice cream would taste creamier, smoother, and richer when it Heine, & Norenzayan, 2010; Jones, 2010). Once again, a simi-
was given the brand name Frosch than when it was called lar criticism can also be raised against many of the other studies
Frisch instead. Obviously, neither of these brand names is, in- that have attempted to assess the implications of sound symbol-
and-of-itself, particularly meaningful. The idea here though is ism research for marketing purposes (e.g., Heath, Chatterjee, &
that there is some form of sound symbolic relationship (or cross- Russo France, 1990; Lowrey & Shrum, 2007; Ngo, Misra, &
modal correspondence; see Spence, 2011c) between the o sound Spence, 2011; Spence & Gallace, 2011).
in the middle of the word Frosch that is more congruent with a Third, while Klink's (2000) study provided support for sev-
creamier/smoother/richer-tasting product than the i sound (a eral different kinds of sound symbolism, there was a lack of
frontal vowel) embedded in the middle of the word Frisch. theoretical explanation as to why many of these effects were
In another study, Richard Klink (2000) presented a group of observed. The problem here is that without having any insight
business graduate students with pairs of fictional brand names for into the underlying neuroscientific causes of the crossmodal
a wide range (nearly 20) of different products. While many of the correspondences that were documented, it is hard to know
results of this study have been described already, I want here to when exactly (in real life) the principles should be expected
focus on those results involving participants' evaluation of three to apply, and whether or not they could be extended to other
food and beverage items: lemonade, ketchup, and beer. When brands/classes of product not explicitly tested in these studies.
given the choice, the students thought that a lemonade with a It is, for example, unclear on the basis of Klink's empirical
brand name having a higher-frequency frontal vowel sound (such findings whether it is specifically increased bitterness that is as-
as the /I/ as in bilad) was more likely to taste bitter than a fictional sociated with words containing frontal vowels, such as i, or
brand name containing a lower-frequency back vowel (such as the whether instead it might be an increase in the intensity of any
/o/ sound in bolad). Similarly, when it came to ketchup, the stu- tastant (i.e., no matter whether it happens to be bitterness,
dents' responses indicated that they thought that a brand name con- sweetness, sourness, saltiness, or even savory- or umami-ness;
taining an initial lower-frequency back vowel sound (such as the see de Arauju, Kringelbach, Rolls, & Hobden, 2003) that ex-
/o/ in nodax) was more likely to be thicker than a brand name con- hibits this particular sound symbolic correspondence.
taining an initial higher-frequency front vowel (such as the /I/
sound in nidax). Meanwhile, the name godan (containing an initial Crossmodal correspondences between tastes/flavors and
back vowel) was associated with a darker beer than the word gidan (speech) sounds
(containing a more frontal vowel instead).
At this point, however, it is important to note that there are In early research in this area, Kristian Holt-Hansen (1968)
several potential problems with the interpretation of the two stud- from the University of Copenhagen, demonstrated the existence
ies just-mentioned (Klink, 2000; Yorkston & Menon, 2004). First of a specific crossmodal correspondence between the taste/flavor
off, it should be born in mind that none of the participants in ei- of beer and the pitch of a simultaneously-presented sound. The
ther study actually got to taste or evaluate (i.e., physically interact participants in this study were given a glass of beer and a tone
with) any of the food or drink items whose name they were being generator to play with. They were instructed to taste the beer
asked to judge. Consequently, it is possible that what participants and turn the knob until the frequency of the tone matched the fla-
were actually doing in these studies was simply matching the vor of the beer. The participants chose tones in the 510520 Hz
sounds present in the putative brand names with the sounds pre- range for regular Carlsberg Lager while preferring 640670 Hz
sent in the words used to describe the product attributes, rather tones for the Elephant brand beer. This difference in participants'
than with the imagined physical (or sensory) attributes them- responses was presumably driven by differences in the sensory
selves (cf. Simner, Cuskley, & Kirby, 2010). attributes (e.g., bitterness) of the two beers that they were evalu-
To put this potential criticism more concretely, let's take ating. Bizarre though these results might sound, they have subse-
Klink's (2000) finding that people expect a lemonade called quently been replicated, at least in part, by Rudmin and Cappelli
bilad to taste more bitter than one called bolad: This result (1983; see also Holt-Hansen, 1976).
would be predicted if the participants given this seemingly non- In 2009, Crisinel and Spence used a simplified version of the
sensical task were using the heuristic (to guide their respond- Implicit Association Test (IAT; see Greenwald, McGhee, &
ing), of matching the vowel sounds present in the brand name Schwartz, 1998, 2003; Greenwald, Nosek, & Banaji, 2003),
C. Spence / Journal of Consumer Psychology 22 (2012) 3754 41

in order to demonstrate that people match bitter foods with low- The final scale, spectral balance, consisted of white noise vary-
pitched sounds and sour-tasting foods with sounds having a ing from low- to high-frequency.
higher-pitch (see also Crisinel & Spence, 2010a, b). In fact, The participants in Simner et al.'s (2010) study mapped sour
the IAT has been shown to provide a quick and easy means tastes onto sounds that had a higher-frequency spectral balance, a
of testing for the existence of various crossmodal correspon- higher F1, and a higher F2 than the sweet tastes (cf. Crisinel &
dences (see also Dematt et al., 2006, 2007; Parise & Spence, Spence, 2009). Participants' ratings of the salty and bitter tastes
2012). That said, one limitation with Crisinel and Spence's fell somewhere in-between the other two tastes. In terms of the
study is that the names of foods that were either sour (lime, voice discontinuity scale, the sweet taste was judged as being
lemon juice, vinegar, etc.) or bitter (e.g., coffee, tonic water, smoother (or more continuous) than any of the other tastants.
beer, etc.) were presented, rather than the actual foods them- When presented at the higher concentration, the tastants were
selves. As Simner et al. (2010) have subsequently pointed mapped onto sounds that were more discontinuous. These sounds
out, this leaves open the possibility that what Crisinel and also had a higher-frequency spectral balance, a higher F1, and a
Spence's (2009, 2010a) participants may actually have been higher F2. While the marketing implications of Simner et al.'s find-
doing is matching the phonetic attributes of the specific words ings are not so easy to draw, the results are nevertheless important
used to label the sour and bitter foods rather than necessarily in demonstrating that certain phonetic features map onto specific
sour- and bitter-tasting foods themselves. 2 tastants, not to mention differing concentrations of those tastants.
Crisinel and Spence (2010b) addressed this potential criticism Taken together, then, the results of several studies, from both
of their original findings by directly testing the crossmodal corre- the psychology laboratory and the marketing classroom, now
spondence between actual tastes and flavors and sounds varying in support the claim that there is a robust sound symbolic associa-
both their pitch and in the class of instrument used to make them. tion between increases in the intensity of a tastant (any tastant,
The participants in this study were given plastic cups containing as it happens) and higher-frequency sounds (such as in frontal
12 different solutions. They tasted each solution in turn with vowels; see Klink, 2000; Simner et al., 2010).
the order of presentation of the solutions counterbalanced across Let us, then, move on from basic tastes and flavors to more
participants. The participants were asked to match the taste/flavor complex oral-somatosensory food and drink experiences and see
of each solution with the sound made by one of four different clas- how they are mapped onto specific speech sounds. In research con-
ses of musical instrument (piano, brass, string, or woodwind) play- ducted here at the Crossmodal Research Laboratory in Oxford, we
ing one of 13 different notes going from C2 (64.4 Hz) to C6 have demonstrated that people map sourness (e.g., as in the acidic
(1046.5 Hz) in intervals of two tones. Among a number of other taste of cranberry juice) onto words starting with a harder plosive
findings, Crisinel and Spence's results highlighted the fact that sound (Tuki and Takete), while pairing foods with a creamier tex-
people tend to match bitter- and umami-tastes with lower-pitched ture (e.g., as in a Brie cheese) with more rounded speech sounds
sounds and sweet and sour tastes with higher-pitched notes. Fur- instead (such as Lula and Maluma; see also Gallace et al., 2011;
thermore, bitter tastes were predominantly matched with brass in- Spence & Gallace, 2011). The participants in our studies rated
struments while sweet tastes were matched with the sound of the chocolates with a crunchy texture, or center (e.g., Mars Maltesers),
piano instead. More recently, Crisinel and Spence (in press a,b, and chocolate with mint chips, onto words containing harder-
2011) have conducted further studies in which participants have sounding plosives (like t and k) than regular milk chocolate,
had to match a variety of different foodstuffs, oral-somatosensory or chocolates having a caramel center (such as Cadbury Caramel
textures, and wine-related odors to musical stimuli varying in pitch Nibbles; Ngo et al., 2011; Spence & Gallace, 2011; see also Ngo
and class of instrument. & Spence, 2011). Other crispy and crunchy foods, such as crisps,
Elsewhere, researchers have assessed the relationship be- otherwise known as potato chips, were also paired with words con-
tween taste and audition using stimuli that are much more taining harder plosive sounds such as kiki, tuki, takete, and
closely related to actual speech sounds. So, for example, the decter, 3 as compared to more rounded sounds of bouba,
participants in a study by Simner et al. (2010) had to select lula, maluma, and bolobo. In fact, some years ago now,
from several different auditory scales the sounds that best- Vickers (1984, p. 162) noted that there was even a correspondence
matched each of the four basic tastes (sweet, sour, bitter, and between the sensory qualities of dry foodstuffs and the words used
salty). The tastants were presented at one of two different con- to describe those attributes, specifically crispy and crunchy (the
centrations, either medium or high. The auditory scales F1, word crispy having a higher frequency than the word crunchy; see
F2, voice discontinuity, and spectral balance varied in ways also Dijksterhuis et al., 2007).
that, to some extent (at least for the first three scales), matched Taken together, then, the results reported in this section dem-
the phonetic qualities of human speech. In particular, while var- onstrate that there are a number of robust crossmodal correspon-
iations in F1 correspond to variations in vocal height, F2 corre- dences between the phonetic attributes of speech sounds, and the
sponds (at least for the range of stimuli used in Simner et al.'s tastes, flavors, and oral-somatosensory attributes of food and
study) to the front/backness of vowel sounds. Voice discontinu-
ity maps onto the interruptions in vocal quality that occur, for 3
example, when one goes from smooth to more staccato sounds. According to Jools Simner (pers. comm., 09/05/2011), there is actually
nothing particularly hard about these speech sounds, phonetically speaking,
thus it may turn out that what is critical here is the angularity of the graphemes
2
Note that we have already come across this criticism in relation to the inter- that happen to be associated with these phonetic sounds (see also Westbury,
pretation of Klink (2000) and Yorkston and Menon's (2004) studies. 2005).
42 C. Spence / Journal of Consumer Psychology 22 (2012) 3754

drink items. Note also that in all of the studies just-mentioned O'Mahony, Goldenberg, Stedmon, & Alford, 1979). What
(Gallace et al., 2011; Ngo et al., 2011; Simner et al., 2010; Spence is more, another prominent attribute of many lemonades is their
& Gallace, 2011), and in contrast to all of the marketing studies sourness. Now all of the studies reported thus far in this review
described earlier, the participants were given actual food products have shown that sourness is associated with higher-pitched
or solutions to taste/evaluate, rather than just the names of those sounds. Hence, I would argue that it is at least conceivable that
products. Klink's somewhat unusual finding (when compared to the results
Other researchers have gone even further in terms of the match- of all subsequent studies) may reflect the fact that what his partic-
ing of more complex musical stimuli to both tastes and flavors ipants were responding to when imagining the taste of lemonade
(see Spence, 2011b, for reviews). So, for example, Mesz et al. was its sourness, and not its bitterness.
(2011) have recently demonstrated that trained musicians generate What is more, according to at least two putative accounts of the
reliable and consistent musical representations when asked to origin of the sound symbolic relationship between basic tastes and
improvize in response to any one of the four basic tastes: The mu- speech sounds, bitterness should be associated with lower-pitched
sicians' sour improvizations were generally high-pitched and (rather than higher-pitched) speech sounds. One of these accounts
dissonant; Their sweet improvizations, by contrast, were softer, is based on the literature on our affective behavioral responses to
slower, and more consonant; The musicians' bitter improviza- basic tastants. It turns out that newborn humans, monkeys, and
tions were low-pitched and legato, while their salty improviza- even rats all show the same kind of stereotypical oro-facial hedon-
tions were staccato, and dense in wide musical intervals (that is, ic responses: Lip protrusion in response to bitter tastants, lip-
they consisted of successive notes that had widely-differing smacking (and licking) in response to sweetness (see Fig. 1).
pitches). More importantly, nave listeners were able to decode The tongue protrudes upward in response to pleasant tastes and
the tastes that the musicians were trying to communicate in their downward in response to aversive tastes (e.g., Berridge, 2000;
musical compositions with a high degree of accuracy. It would Steiner et al., 2001). Sour and salty tastes have also been shown
therefore appear as though certain styles of musical improvization, to give rise to idiosyncratic, albeit somewhat less distinctive, oro-
and not just the auditory qualities of specific musical notes, can be facial gestures. One suggestion here, then, is that the sound that is
used to reliably connote the various basic tastes in the minds of made while air is exhaled when either of these seemingly-innate
consumers (see also Crisinel, Cosser, King, Jones, Petrie, & orofacial responses is adopted would likely have a different funda-
Spence, 2012). Can the same be done for flavors? mental frequency. Consequently, one might expect that bitter
In an, as-yet unpublished, piece of industry-funded research tastes would soon come to be associated with lower-pitched
(the research in question being funded by Symrise), Kai Bronner sounds whereas sweet tastes would be associated with sounds hav-
and his colleagues in Germany demonstrated that people match ing a somewhat higher frequency instead. That is, there might be
different musical parameters, or qualities, to flavors (citrus and va- good scientific grounds for expecting there to be a particularly ro-
nilla; see Bronner, submitted for publication; Bronner et al., 2008). bust sound symbolic correspondence between front-back vowel
Citrus was associated with music that had a more syncopated sounds and foods and beverages varying on the bitter-sweet con-
rhythm and a brighter and sharper timbre. By contrast, vanilla tinuum (Ngo et al., 2011; cf. Fnagy, 1963).
was mapped onto music that was rather slow (adagio), and tended Note, though, that the oro-facial affective account of why
to have a soft, dull timbre (see also Spence, 2011b). The same re- lower-pitched sounds (especially speech sounds) should be
sults were observed no matter whether these two flavors were ac- matched with bitter tastes is not the only one. It could equally
tually presented to participants or just imagined by them. It is, well be argued here that what participants are doing when they as-
though, perhaps worth noting at this point that sourness is such a sociate higher-pitched sounds with sweet tastes and lower-
central attribute of many citrus flavors that it is unclear whether pitched sounds with bitter tastes is simply matching them in
it might not have been the acidic taste associated with citrus that terms of their hedonic valence (see Spence, 2011c; though see
was driving these effects, rather than the flavor of citrus itself. also Crisinel & Spence, in press). Both sweet tastes and higher
pitched tones are typically judged as being more pleasant than bit-
Are bitter tastes low- or high-pitched? ter tastes and low-pitched tones (there is though, of course, likely
to be some upper level/concentration beyond which this claim no
The attentive reader should by now have spotted that there is longer holds true, that is, when the sweetness becomes sickly).
an apparent contradiction in the results of the various studies of What is more, this appears to be true from a very early age in
sound symbolism that have been reported thus far. On the one human development (see Raymond, 2000). However, the key
hand, the majority of the studies reviewed just now support the point to note here is that both of the accounts of the origins of
existence of a crossmodal correspondence between bitterness sound symbolism just mentioned predict that bitter tastes should
and lower-pitch (e.g., Crisinel & Spence, 2010a; Crisinel et al., be mapped onto lower-pitched tastes, and not onto higher-pitched
2012; Mesz et al., 2011; Simner et al., 2010). However, earlier sounds, as was reported by Klink (2000).
on it was reported that words containing a higher-frequency fron-
tal vowel sound led people to expect that a lemonade would be Capitalizing on the crossmodal correspondences between
bitter rather than names that contained a lower-pitched back tastes/flavors and speech sounds
vowel sound instead (Klink, 2000). As yet, there is no clear ex-
planation for this discrepancy. It is, though, perhaps worth noting Following on from these studies, research at the Crossmodal
that people often confuse sour and bitter tastes (e.g., Research Laboratory here in Oxford has now demonstrated that
C. Spence / Journal of Consumer Psychology 22 (2012) 3754 43

Fig. 1. Hedonic and aversive affective orofacial responses to sweet (upper row) and bitter (lower row) tastants in three different species rat, primate, and human.
Note the homologous tongue protrusion in response to a liked taste and the gaping response following the presentation of a disliked bitter taste.
Figure reprinted from Pecia & Smith, 2010, Fig. 1, with permission.

there are products out there in the marketplace where, at least combined with semantic content in order to generate brand
according to the rules of sound symbolism, the phonetic features names that are both better liked and better positioned (see
of the brand name are actually inappropriate. Ngo et al. (2011), also McNeil, 2003; though see also Pinker, 2007, pp. 303
for example, recently showed how at least one currently-available 304). Finally, certain speech sounds may succeed in a brand
brand of chocolate, named Koko (from Cadbury), may set-up the name simply because they are distinctive from those that have
wrong taste expectations in people's minds. The participants in gone before. Such sonic distinctiveness may also help a brand
this laboratory-based study matched the repeated plosive sound to differentiate itself from the competition (see Abel & Glinert,
(e.g., k) in the nonsense words koko or tuki with a food 2008). Thus, the research reviewed here documenting the po-
that was bitter (e.g., high in cocoa), whereas it turns out that tential use of sound symbolism in product naming should be
this product, a milk chocolate truffle, isn't especially bitter. It seen as just one part of an overall effective brand naming
will be interesting to see, then, in the coming years, whether the strategy.
general success of brand names beginning with the k sound
(e.g., Schloss, 1981; Vanden Bergh, 1990; Vanden Bergh et al., Shape symbolism in the marketplace
1987; see also Abel & Glinert, 2008) will provide enough of an
advantage for the Koko brand to overcome the incongruency Having highlighted the existence of a number of robust cross-
in terms of the product's sensory attributes set-up by the repeated modal correspondences between sounds and tastesflavors, I
plosive sound in the brand name. now want to move on to look at another form of crossmodal corre-
It is, of course, important to remember here that the success spondence that should be of interest to marketers, namely that
of a brand name may depend on a number of different factors. between shape (or angularity) and tastes, flavors, and the
Brand names that are more attention-capturing, memorable, oral-somatosensory attributes of foods and beverages. Spence
and/or easier to process are more likely to be successful (e.g., and Gallace (2011) have demonstrated that people associate
Alter & Oppenheimer, 2006; Erlich, 1995; Ewald & Moskowitz, the oral-somatosensory carbonation (in a sample of sparkling
2007; Keller et al., 1998; LaBroo et al., 2008; Lowrey et al., water) with more angular shapes, while associating still water
2003; Robertson, 1989; Vanden Bergh et al., 1987). Many with more rounded shapes instead (Chandrashekar et al.,
brand names also convey some kind of semantic information, 2009). Meanwhile, Deroy and Valentin (2011) have recently
or claim, regarding a product's functionality and/or effective- demonstrated a crossmodal correspondence between three dif-
ness (Kohli & LaBahn, 1995). Relevant here then is research ferent kinds of beer and various 2D and 3D shapes (the partic-
from Klink (2001) showing that sound symbolism can be ipants were given a total of 34 shapes to choose from).
44 C. Spence / Journal of Consumer Psychology 22 (2012) 3754

In another conceptually-similar experiment, Ngo et al. (2011)


highlighted the existence of a crossmodal correspondence be-
tween the angularity of visually-presented shapes and the bitter-
ness of chocolate. The participants in this study were given three
samples of commercially-produced chocolate to taste that varied
in their cocoa content (30% milk chocolate, 70% dark chocolate,
and 90% dark chocolate). They had to taste each sample and
then place a mark at the appropriate point along an analog line
scale anchored at one end by a rounded shape and at the other
end by an angular shape (see Fig. 2A). The results revealed
that as the cocoa content in the chocolate samples went up,
so the participants tended to mark a point that was closer to
the angular-shaped end of the line (see Fig. 2B). They also
tended to map bitterness onto sharper-sounding words as well
(that is, onto words starting with a plosive sound, such as Tuki
and Takete as opposed to words like Maluma and Lula).
Note that these shape and sound symbolism effects were very ro-
bust, demonstrable in a paper-and-pencil test with each of 20 par-
ticipants only tasting each (of the three) chocolate sample once (see
also Ngo & Spence, 2011).

The consequences of manipulating crossmodal correspon-


dence for consumer perceptions

So far, everything that we have seen in this section of the review


supports the claim that people exhibit reliable crossmodal corre- Fig. 2. A) The paper-and-pencil response scale used by participants in Ngo et al.'s
spondences between particular tastes and flavors and certain attri- (2011) recent studies of shape and sound symbolism. The participants tasted three
butes of auditory stimuli, be they pure tones, phonetic stimuli, samples of chocolate that varied in their cocoa content and placed a mark on the
corresponding point on the scales; B) The results show both sound and shape sym-
and/or the parameters of more complex musical expression. People bolism effects with increasing bitterness (e.g., as the cocoa content increases in the
also exhibit robust crossmodal correspondences between abstract chocolate) being matched with more angular shapes and words starting with a
shapes and the carbonation of beverages and the bitter taste of harder plosive sound (t in this case). The units on the Y-axis indicate the number
cocoa in dark chocolate products. However, what many marketers of cm along the 10 cm line scale, with 0 reflecting the center-point.
really want to know is whether presenting pairs of stimuli that cor- Figure adapted and reprinted from Ngo et al., 2011, Figs. 1 and 2, with permission.
respond crossmodally in terms of the matching of sounds or shapes
to tastes and/or flavors can actually result in people rating food and To date, there has been far less research on this particular ques-
drink items more favorably than if no such correspondence had tion. However, the evidence that is available suggests that people's
been presented, or worse still, if the stimuli that happen to be pre- responses to food/drink items are more favorable from both the
sented are crossmodally incongruent. The idea here is that sound consumer's perspective and from that of the marketer when
symbolism in brand naming and shape symbolism in labeling crossmodally corresponding stimuli are presented (e.g., see Gal
and packaging design could eventually be used in order to set-up et al., 2007; Holt-Hansen, 1968, 1976; Spence, 2011a, b; though
the appropriate sensory expectations in the minds of consumers. see also Crisinel et al., 2012).
This approach is premised on the idea that people like food
and drink products more if they meet their expectations than if Crossmodal correspondences modulate taste, aroma, and/or
they do not, that is, if they are surprised (see Peterson & Ross, flavor perception
1972; Pinson, 1986; Piqueras-Fiszman & Spence, in press
a, b; though see also Garber et al., 2000). Food scientists have As yet, there isn't a great deal of research on the question of
shown that when a food or beverage item fails to meet our expec- whether sound or shape symbolism can influence product percep-
tations, we are likely to evaluate it, both immediately and for a tion, but what there is, is promising (see DeRosia, 2008). Take, for
long time thereafter, more negatively than if our expectations example, early work by Holt-Hansen (1968, 1976). This research
had been met (e.g., see Cardello, 1994; Deliza & MacFie, 1997; provides anecdotal evidence that when the frequency of the tone
Deliza et al., 2003; Schifferstein, 2001; Yeomans et al., 2008). playing in the background matched the taste/flavor of the beer
In fact, we may be especially sensitive to disconfirmed expectation that the participants were evaluating, they rated the beer-drinking
when it comes to food and drinks, since we actually take these experience, and even the very taste of the beer itself, as more en-
stimuli into our mouths, and hence need to avoid the risk of poi- joyable than when listening to a tone whose frequency did not
soning (see Koza et al., 2005; Piqueras-Fiszman & Spence, match the taste (though see Rudmin & Cappelli, 1983). That
in press b; Wheatley, 1973). said, the subjective reports of Holt-Hansen's participant's study
C. Spence / Journal of Consumer Psychology 22 (2012) 3754 45

are sufficiently florid to make one wonder quite what was in the
beer bottles that they were sampling from (see especially Table
2, Holt-Hansen, 1976, p. 1026).
Another, rather more convincing, example here comes from
an as-yet unpublished study by Gal et al. (2007). They had par-
ticipants judge which of three simultaneously-presented (and
similarly-sized) shapes occupied the largest surface area. The
participants then tasted a piece of cheddar cheese. After having
evaluated three angular shapes, the participants rated the
cheese as tasting 7% sharper than if they had just evaluated Fig. 3. Three different types of visual stimuli used in Seo et al.'s (2010) study of
crossmodal correspondences between food aromas and shape. The middle sym-
three rounded shapes instead. 4 While these results are most cer-
bol was congruent with the odor of phenylethanol (PEA), while the abstract
tainly promising, it is worth noting that Gal and his colleagues symbol on the right was congruent with the odor of 1-butanol. These com-
had to test rather a large number of participants (N 200) in order pounds smell of violet and parmesan cheese, respectively.
to obtain an effect that only just reached statistical significance This figure is reprinted from Seo et al., 2010, Fig. 2, with permission.
(p = .03). Furthermore, it is clearly unlikely that people, when
consuming a packaged food or drink item under more natural-
istic conditions, would necessarily pay anything like as much
attention to the shapes on the label as the participants did in participants' experience of the odor, and not just on their subse-
this study. That said, there are many consumers out there. quent ratings of that experience.
Hence, even a very subtle effect of shape symbolism on food Finally, in a very recent study, Crisinel et al. (2012) system-
perception demonstrated in the laboratory could potentially be atically altered the taste of a bittersweet cinder toffee simply by
important out in the marketplace. changing the crossmodal congruency (or correspondence) of
Elsewhere, Seo et al. (2010) have reported an intriguing the auditory soundtrack that happened to be presented over
study in which the participants had to say which of eight food headphones at the same time as people were eating this partic-
odors (guava, honey melon, mint, parmesan cheese, pepper, ular sweet. The participants in this study rated the bitter taste in
truffle, vanilla, and viola) best matched each of 19 different ab- the food as being more prominent when a lower-pitched sound-
stract shapes (or symbols), varying from more mounded to scape was presented and the sweeter taste was rated as more
more angular. Once again, a reliable crossmodal correspon- prominent when a high-pitched soundscape was played over
dence was observed, this time between the odors and specific the headphones instead. Importantly, the soundscapes used in
abstract shapes. In the second part of their study, Seo and col- this study were based on the findings of Crisinel and Spence's
leagues went on to demonstrate significant differences in odor (2010b) psychophysical research (described earlier).
pleasantness and intensity ratings for two of the odors (those Taken together, the results reported in this section of the review
smelling of violet and of parmesan cheese) when presented to- support the view that it may be possible to enhance a consumer's
gether with an abstract visual shape that was congruent than perception of a particular food or drink item by ensuring that any
when they were presented with an incongruent shape, or else shape information displayed on the product packaging is as con-
with no visual stimulus at all (see Fig. 3). In particular, the gruent as possible with the taste, flavor, and/or oral-somatosensory
odor of violet was rated as more intense and more pleasant attributes of the food or drink product contained within. At this
while the parmesan odor was rated as more unpleasant (see point, those familiar with the history of marketing may be
also Crisinel & Spence, in press a, 2011). reminded of the seminal early work of Louis Cheskin (1957,
The brain responses of participants in Seo et al.'s (2010) study 1972); and recently repopularized by writers such as Malcolm
were also measured using event-related potentials (ERPs). This Gladwell, 2005, pp. 160161; see also Hine, 1995, pp. 210
technique allowed the researchers to demonstrate that this partic- 213. According to Cheskin, people reported liking products such
ular crossmodal correspondence between abstract visual shapes as women's skin cream more (at least at the time that the study
and odors was influencing their participants' brain responses. In was conducted) when those products were presented in packaging
particular, the early N1 component had both a higher amplitude on which circles were visible rather than on packaging covered
and a shorter latency on congruent as compared to incongruent with angular triangles instead. One might also wonder whether
trials. These crossmodal interactions occurred within a matter of the prominent red circle that appears in the middle of 7-Up logo
a few hundred milliseconds of stimulus onset. As such, Seo et was also based on one of Cheskin's suggestions (he certainly
al.'s results can be taken to suggest that the manipulation of the looked at the effects of changing the overall color scheme on the
crossmodal correspondence between the abstract visual shape 7-Up can and its effects on flavor perception). There is, after all,
and the odor (contrasting matching, or congruent, with non- apparently a crossmodal correspondence between sweetness and
matching, or incongruent) had a genuinely perceptual effect on roundness (see Dichter, 1971).
It is, however, important to note that the sensation transfer-
ence effects reported by Cheskin (1957, 1972) were of a very
4
Results such as these clearly suggest that there is some underlying truth to general nature. Indeed, it would be too easy to come away
the metaphoric use of language captured in phrases such as a sharp cheese from his early research believing that almost regardless of the
(e.g., Marks, 1991; Spence et al., submitted for publication; Williams, 1976). product category involved (e.g., food or home-and-personal-
46 C. Spence / Journal of Consumer Psychology 22 (2012) 3754

Table 1
Summary of the various different crossmodal correspondences between the taste, flavor, and oral-somatosensory attributes of foods and beverages and various
parameters of auditory and visual stimuli that have been documented to date. The numerous empty cells in this table highlight some of the many crossmodal
correspondences yet to be investigated. (Hypothesized correspondences for which there is currently little, contradictory, or else an absence of, empirical evidence
are followed by a questionmark.).
Modality Attribute Sound Tone-pitch Instrument Musical Shape Color
symbolism symbolismc symbolismd parameterse symbolismf symbolismg
Vowelsa Consonantsb
Oral-somatosensation
Carbonation Front Plosives Angular Red?
Creamy, smooth, Back Rounded
rich texture
Thickness Back Plosives?
Crunchy/Crispy Plosives, voiced Angular
Taste
Bitterness Front?/Back? Plosives? Low-pitch Brass Legato Angular Green?
Sweetness Front Fricatives? High-pitch Piano Softer, slower, and Rounded Red
consonant
Sourness (acidity) Voiceless? High-pitch Dissonant and fast Angular Yellow
Saltiness Staccato, and dense White
in wide musical intervals
Umami (savory) Low-pitch
Flavor
Citrus Syncopated rhythm, Angular
and bright, sharp timbre
Vanilla Piano, woodwind Slow (adagio), and soft,
dull timbre
Fruit High-pitch Piano, woodwind
a,b Gallace et al. (2011); Klink (2000); Ngo et al. (2011); Spence and Gallace (2011); Yorkston and Menon (2004).
c,d Belkin et al. (1997); Crisinel and Spence (2009, 2010a, b, in press, 2011, submitted for publication); Holt-Hansen (1968, 1976); Mesz et al. (2011); Rudmin
and Cappelli (1983); Simner et al. (2010).
e Bronner (submitted for publication); Bronner et al. (2008); Mesz et al. (2011).
f Deroy and Valentin (2011); Dichter (1971); Gal et al. (2007); Lyman (1989); Ngo et al. (2011); Spence and Gallace (2011); Spence et al.
g Maga (1974); O'Mahony (1983); though see also Spence et al. (2010).

care, HPC), circles are better than more angular forms on pack- does indeed suggest that this may, in fact, be the case. Both San
aging in terms of positively influencing people's responses to Pellegrino and Apollinaris, for example, contain very prominent
products (cf. Dazkir & Read, in press). Speaking more general- angular shapes on their labels/packaging (see Fig. 4). 5 The
ly, people appear to prefer rounded over angular forms (Bar & shape symbolism approach may also extend to other carbonated
Neta, 2006; see also Carbon, 2010). However, the results de- beverages, such as beer. In this regard, it is interesting to note
scribed here support the view that such a positive effect of that Heineken also uses a prominent red star in their logo, consis-
rounded forms might be much more product or attribute specif- tent with the carbonation of the larger products they purvey. 6
ic than that. In particular, when it comes to food and beverage Given the prominent use of angular shapes on the front labels
items, it would appear as though bitter, carbonated, sour, and (not to mention the cap of the San Pellegrino bottle), it would be
crunchy food and beverage items are actually better matched
5
with more angular shapes on the packaging, while sweeter and Even those brands that do not incorporate angular shapes sometimes utilize
creamier-tasting food and drink items are better-matched with angular imagery think of the thistle that adorns the front of the sparkling
more rounded forms instead (see Table 1). Angularity also mineral water bottle from Highland Spring. Meanwhile, it should be noted the
still water from the manufacturer of San Pellegrino, Acqua Panna, contains no
seems to be a feature of at least certain cigarette packets such angular imagery on the packaging. Note that the red star on the Heineken
think of the pyramid on the Camel packet (Day, 1985) and the an- logo actually dates back to the red star that was a traditional Dutch brewing
gular contrast between red and white that make the Marlboro symbol (see The Dutch touch, 2011).
6
brand so distinctive. Color symbolism. Is it mere coincidence, one might ask, that the angular
shapes on the packaging of San Pellegrino, Appolinaris, Saskia (carbonated wa-
ter), and Heineken all happen to be red? Surely not. One possibility here is that
Shape symbolism: labeling there may also be a crossmodal correspondence between the color red and car-
bonation (or some other product attribute). Some years ago, O'Mahony (1983)
One direct prediction from Spence and Gallace's (2011) re- reported that people associate red with sweetness rather than with any of the
sults (described earlier) is that the labeling on the containers for other primary tastes (see Table 1). Although interesting, a full discussion of
the topic of color symbolism in food and drink packaging/logo design falls be-
carbonated beverages, such as sparkling water, should be more yond the scope of the present article (see Garber et al., 2008; Maga, 1974; Ngo
likely to incorporate angular imagery than the containers used et al., submitted for publication; Piqueras-Fiszman & Spence, in press a;
for still water. A casual inspection of the supermarket shelf Spence, 2010; Spence et al., 2010; Wheatley, 1973).
C. Spence / Journal of Consumer Psychology 22 (2012) 3754 47

bitter (see also Zhang et al., 2006). While the Costa coffee
chain's logo shows coffee beans, many of the hot beverages
(e.g., the cappuccino) served in-store currently are topped with
cocoa powder that has been sprinkled in the shape of a star. Per-
haps this all-too-visible shape sets up an expectation about the
taste of what lies below the frothy milk topping.
Given the frequency with which wine writers use descriptors
such as sharp or rounded when describing their impressions
of the taste of wine (e.g., Lehrer, 2009, pp. 140141; Peynaud,
1987, pp. 168171), it would seem only natural that the shape
symbolism approach could also profitably be used to aid in the de-
sign of wine labels. Round labels for rounded wines, more angu-
Fig. 4. Several brands of mineral water already use angular shapes on their lar labels for sharper, more acidic, wines. 8 However, here, it is
packaging. Take, for example, the San Pellegrino and Apollinaris bottles important to note that some wines, unlike many other food and
shown here. The San Pellegrino bottle has a total of four red stars displayed beverage products, are designed for long-term storage in the bot-
on the packaging, three on the two front labels and one on the screw-cap.
Here it is argued that angular shapes are more common on sparkling than on
tle. What is more, for many such quality wines, the sensory attri-
still mineral water bottles. butes actually change quite dramatically as they age (e.g., as the
tannins soften in a young red wine, say). Hence, unless a wine pro-
ducer can predict the likely taste/flavor qualities at the time of con-
easy to imagine that the graphic designers who worked this iconic
sumption, it may be hard for them to get the shape symbolism on
example of product labeling/packaging must intuitively have pick-
the wine label right.
ed-up on the existence of a crossmodal correspondence between
The shape symbolism approach to marketing outlined here
angularity and carbonation (some decades before Khler, 1929,
might be appropriate, then, for the producers of wines, such
first introduced the notion of shape symbolism to the field of Ex-
as Beaujolais, that are designed to be drunk young. It might
perimental Psychology). 7 For, according to the San Pellegrino
also be more appropriate for those mass-marketed blended
website, the stars have been an integral part of the label since at
wines (or packaged coffees) where the producers strive for a
least as far back as 1907 (see Spence & Gallace, 2011). That
consistent flavor profile. In the case of wine, this will usually
said, it is difficult to determine causality here. It could equally
be one that is sweet, and hence congruent with a more rounded
well be argued that those food and drink products whose pack-
label/imagery on the label. Fortunately, the technology for wine
aging, serendipitously or otherwise, happens to incorporate ab-
labels has progressed to such an extent in recent years that the in-
stract imagery that sets-up the appropriate taste/flavor/oral-
troduction of innovative label designs is now much cheaper, and
somatosensory expectations in the minds of consumers will
hence more practical, than ever before (e.g., see Kidd, 1999, p.
be more likely to succeed in the marketplace, and hence still
82). Hence, I would argue that the wine producers, or at the
be around many years after their introduction.
very least the wine marketers, are now in a position to capitalize
By contrast, crossmodal incongruency in branding, packag-
on the emerging science of shape symbolism together with the in-
ing, and/or labeling design should be much less likely to succeed
novations in packaging technology in order to allow for a more
(e.g., see Ngo et al., 2011; Spence & Piqueras-Fiszman, in press;
strategic use of shape symbolism in the marketplace (see Fig. 5).
Schifferstein & Spence, 2008). As such, incongruency is far more
From the evidence reviewed here, it would appear that research
likely to disappear from the shelves soon after its introduction into
on the topic of shape symbolism can provide useful insights with
the marketplace (though see also Piqueras-Fiszman & Spence, in
regard to the most appropriate shapes for any abstract imagery
press b). Counterevidence to the argument outlined here would
presented on the labels of food and drink packaging, not to men-
then come from a product that has been in the marketplace for
tion the very shape of labels themselves. The shape symbolism ap-
many years but where the shape, or for that matter sound, symbol-
proach is likely to be particularly effective for those mass-
ism is inappropriate (or incongruent) with the product's sensory
marketed food and beverage products whose flavor profile at the
qualities (i.e., with its taste, aroma, flavor, or other oral-somato-
time of consumption can be safely predicted. Once again, though,
sensory attributes).
it must be remembered that shape symbolism is only one small
Coffee would seem to be another category in which one might
part of successful label design. Product images (Cheskin, 1972;
expect to see shape-taste symbolism in action. Bitterness, what is
Van Ittersum et al., 2003), descriptive labels (Charters et al.,
euphemistically called strength on most packets, is a prominent
1999), and bold attention-capturing colors (Garber et al., 2008)
part of the coffee drinking experience (e.g., Calvio et al., 1996;
undoubtedly also play an important role as well.
Geel et al., 2005). While I haven't yet had the chance to perform
a systematic analysis of the use of shape symbolism in coffee 8
Here, it is important to note that there may also be gender differences in the
labeling/packaging design, one might already speculate that appeal of different label shapes. So, for example, many years ago, Louis Che-
the rounded shape of the Starbucks logo surreptitiously sug- skin worked on a project with Fleishmann's Gin in which the corners of the la-
gests to consumers that their product offering will not be too bel were rounded specically because it was believed that the change would be
more appealing to women (who, at the time this change was made, accounted
7
And a century before Spence and Gallace (2011) provided the rst empirical for almost 40% of shoppers at the liquor store; see http://en.wikipedia.org/
evidence to demonstrate the existence of this crossmodal correspondence. wiki/Louis_Cheskin downloaded on 07/05/2011).
48 C. Spence / Journal of Consumer Psychology 22 (2012) 3754

Tetra
prisma Edge Tetratop gemina
pak
Centred spout

Beverage
carton
packs

Tetra brik Tetra Gable top Elopak Diamond SIG


Combi
combifit
& SIG &
Elopak
(top panel+ big bloc
combi
bloc cap);Curve
(corner)

Fig. 6. Samples of innovative packaging shapes currently available in the mar-


ketplace. [Picture courtesy of Elopack].

Fig. 5. Would you expect a wine with such an angular label to have a sharp While it might, at first, seem far-fetched to suggest that com-
(acidic/sour) or more rounded taste? The last few years have seen label de- panies would strategically change the shape of their product
signers increasingly experimenting with a wide variety of novel shapes and pat- packaging in order to modify the response of their consumers,
terns on wine bottles. [Picture copyright Betina Piqueras-Fisman]. the evidence now available shows that this may be precisely
what they should be doing. Take, for example, a recent study
Shape symbolism: packaging form by Becker et al. (2011). They have taken a first step in terms
of demonstrating the impact of changes in packaging shape
Finally, as well as manipulating the abstract shapes dis- (specifically changes in angularity) on the perception of the
played on the labels and packaging, not to mention the very foodstuff contained within (see also Ares & Deliza, 2010;
shapes of the labels themselves (see Cutler, 2006), many food Overbeeke & Peters, 1991). Becker et al. conducted a study
companies are now starting to think about modifying the very in which the participants had to evaluate a yogurt presented in
shape of the packaging in which their products are presented a range of different packaging formats (see Fig. 7). The product
on the supermarket shelves. While some marketers have sug- ratings given by those participants who were sensitive to de-
gested that this could be thought of as a form of tactile branding sign were shown to be influenced by changing the shape, spe-
(Lindstrom, 2005), others have argued that it may be more ap- cifically shape curvature, of the packaging in which it was
propriate to think of it as amodal branding instead (see Spence presented (though see also Piqueras-Fiszman et al., 2011). In
& Gallace, 2011). The term amodal here refers to the fact that particular, the taste of the yogurt was rated as being more
the shape of the packaging can actually be perceived by several
senses, and not just by touch. Indeed, the shape of a package
will most likely be seen long before it is felt. Crucially, recent
developments in packaging technology also mean that it has be-
come much cheaper for companies to alter the shape of their
packaging than ever before. Just take the tetrapak/brik carton
format: Unsurprisingly, the last few years have seen an explo-
sion of novel packaging shapes launched into the marketplace
(Anon., 2010; see Fig. 6, for some innovative examples). The
question therefore facing many companies/packaging manufac-
turers (e.g., Norway's Elopak or Faraday Packaging in the UK)
is precisely which packaging shape is most appropriate for
which type of product (cf. Durgee, 2003)?
At a first approximation, the research outlined here could be
taken to suggest that milk products and creamy yogurts should
probably be presented in more rounded packaging, whereas
fruit juices, and other products with a more sharp, acidic, and/or
Fig. 7. The yogurt containers (varying in angularity) that were displayed on the
bitter taste should be presented in more angular packaging in- computer monitor before the participants in Becker et al.'s (2011) study had to
stead. Grapefruit juice, for example, being both acidic and bitter, evaluate a sample of yogurt served from an aluminum sampling dish.
would likely benefit from being sold in more angular packaging. Figure reprinted from Becker et al., 2011, Fig. 2, with permission.
C. Spence / Journal of Consumer Psychology 22 (2012) 3754 49

intense after the participants had viewed an angular, rather than form of shape symbolism could also be used in the chocolate
more rounded, package. market. Already individual chocolate pieces for children are
In terms of further addressing the way in which individual dif- sold in both rounded (e.g., Cadbury Dairy Milk Buttons) and
ferences might affect people's sensitivity to packaging, it could star-shaped forms (e.g., Milky Way Magic Stars). The research
also be worth investigating those who score higher on The outlined here clearly suggests that the rounded shape is more ap-
need for touch scale (see Peck & Childers, 2003). One obvious propriate for milk chocolate. Indeed, anecdotally I have heard
prediction here being that such individuals would be expected from several individuals who report finding the angular shape
to be differentially affected by any changes in packaging shape of the Magic Stars to be wrong (i.e., incongruent). Recent tech-
as compared to those consumers who score lower on this scale nological innovations in 3-D chocolate printing mean that things
(see Krishna & Morrin, 2008; Spence & Gallace, 2011). could soon go much further in terms of delivering innovatively-
Once again, the idea that features of a product's packaging can shaped chocolates to the marketplace (see Anon., submitted for
be used to modulate that consumer expectations isn't altogether publication; Moskvitch, 2011).
new (e.g., Cheskin, 1957). However, what is new, at least as far
as I am aware, is that we now have a better understanding of at Does the use of sound/shape symbolism constitute a form of
least some of the scientific principles underlying the crossmodal implicit marketing?
correspondences that have been documented between shapes,
or angularity (no matter whether that angularity is seen Having demonstrated the potential role that sound/shape sym-
and/or felt), and taste/flavor perception. Of course, it is impor- bolism can play in terms of modifying people's perception of,
tant to note that the shape symbolic associations outlined here and response to, of a variety of different food and beverage
likely work in tandem with any semiotic/semantic associations items, the final question to consider here is whether or not this ap-
that specific packaging shapes/formats may have picked-up proach to setting consumer expectations should be considered as
over the years that they have been in the marketplace. Think operating at an implicit (or functionally subconscious) level. So,
here only of the association that many consumers hold between for instance, everyone will see the red stars when they pour them-
round (as opposed to rectangular) plastic tubs and quality in ice selves a glass of water from a San Pellegrino bottle. That said, I
cream (Cheskin, 1957; Spence, 2007). Or, for UK consumers, would argue that sound/shape symbolism can be considered as
the association between the regular tetrapak format and freshness, typically operating at an implicit or functionally subconscious
that has been capitalized on so successfully by The New Covent level. For although consumers can see, hear, and/or feel (in the
Garden Soup Co. (see Fig. 8). It seems unlikely that the subtle ef- case of packaging shape) the inducing stimulus, they are typically
fects of shape symbolism in food and beverage packaging will be unaware both of its meaning in terms of crossmodally setting
capable of overriding such semantic/semiotic effects (see Spence their product expectations, and its likely influence on their per-
& Piqueras-Fiszman, in press). Nevertheless, they can perhaps ception of the product contained within (see Becker et al., 2011;
operate effectively in tandem with them. Gal et al., 2007; Holt-Hansen, 1976; Piqueras-Fiszman et al.,
Finally, here, in the coming years we may even see an in- 2011; Spence, 2011b; Yorkston & Menon, 2004). A similar
creased consideration of the shape of the food product itself. claim for the implicit nature of crossmodal correspondence ef-
Chances are if you think of a piece of cheddar cheese, like fects has been made in the case of sound symbolism in brand
those sold in the supermarket, then you will bring a rectangular naming (see McNeil, 2003). That said, it remains an open ques-
block to mind. Is it any coincidence one might ask that the tion what may happen to consumer perceptions of a brand,
sharp edges of the cheese correspond crossmodally to the sharp when they become aware of the forethought that has gone into
taste (or flavor) that such cheeses often have. Perhaps, then, establishing the right sensory expectations.
milder cheeses should come in a more rounded form. A similar
Conclusions

Although research on the topic of sound and shape symbolism


has a long history in the fields of experimental psychology and lin-
guistics, it is only in the last decade or so that researchers have re-
ally started to realize the potential relevance of this area of study
for the fields of product naming, branding, and packaging design.
The latest findings from this newly-revitalized field of research
(see Robson, 2011) now demonstrate that the sounds present in
the brand name, the abstract imagery on a product's packaging,
or even the shape of the label or packaging itself, can be used to
Fig. 8. The New Covent Garden Food Co. soup package. This tetrapak captures set-up sub-conscious expectations in the minds of consumers.
the associations with milk of freshness (attributable to the fact that fresh milk These expectations, which concern the likely sensory attributes
was, for many years, sold in the same container in the UK). Shape symbolism of food and beverage items will then influence, albeit subtly,
in packaging design is unlikely to override such semantic associations (or
image molds, see Stern, 1981) that consumers hold with a particular packaging their perception of, and hence their liking for, a particular product.
format, especially when they are well entrenched. By making sure that the expectation about the likely taste, smell,
Figure reprinted from Spence & Gallace, 2011, Fig. 2, with permission. and flavor of food and beverage products is met, one can
50 C. Spence / Journal of Consumer Psychology 22 (2012) 3754

significantly increase the likelihood that consumers will enjoy That is, they are shared by people over the world. What this means
those products more (Cardello, 1994; Pinson, 1986; Schifferstein, in practice is that certain examples of shape/sound symbolism
2001; Yeomans et al., 2008; though see also Piqueras-Fiszman & should work particularly well in the global marketplace. Al-
Spence, in press b). Importantly, this form of expectation setting though the relevant research has yet to be conducted, the impli-
appears to operate at an implicit level. The ultimate aim here, cation of previous studies of sound and shape symbolism is
then, is to capitalize on our growing understanding of the phenom- that they tend to be shared cross-culturally (e.g., Davis, 1961;
ena of sound and shape symbolism in order to more appropriately Diffloth, 1994; Hinton et al., 1994). Whether the same is true
guide consumer expectations in the marketplace. for the crossmodal correspondence between sounds, shapes
To recap, the sound/shape-taste/flavor/oral-somatosensory and the taste and flavor of food and beverage items will obvi-
correspondences that have been documented to date include the ously be an important question for future research. 9 It should
finding that sourness, bitterness, crunchiness, and carbonation, be noted here that such universality is not a property of brand
correspond crossmodally with angular shapes and sharper/harder names that contain semantic content (Klink, 2003; see also
plosive stop consonants as well as with higher-pitched frontal Lowenthal, 1981). Indeed, one important limitation with se-
vowels. By contrast, sweet, still, and creamy food and beverage mantically meaningful brand names is precisely that they
products appear to share a crossmodal correspondence with may only work in those markets were the potential customers
rounder shapes and speech sounds, as well as with back vowels. happen to speak the right language.
Table 1 provides a summary of many of the sound, shape, and Second, sound/shape symbolism may also have the advantage
color symbolism effects relevant to the taste and flavor of food over other more semantic/explicit forms of packaging/branding in
and drink items that have been documented to date. that it operates at an implicit (or functionally nonconscious) level
Of course, rather than using the sound/shape symbolism ap- (McNeil, 2003), not to mention the fact that its occurrence appears
proach to correctly predict the sensory experience of a food or to be both automatic and effortless (Yorkston & Menon, 2004).
beverage etc., there will always be some temptation to use What this means in practice is that consumers will likely not
sound/shape symbolism in order to set-up expectations that show any conscious rejection, or evaluation, of the information
are actually higher (better) than what is actually being delivered that is being provided to them (via the sound/shape symbolism
by a product. However, while there may certainly be some pos- route). This may not be such an issue for those product attributes
sibility to enhance the consumer experience in this way (cf. that consumers value positively, such as carbonation in a beverage
Woods et al., 2011), one has to be very careful not to go too or bitterness (in coffee), say. However, it may be much more rel-
far, and give rise to disconfirmed expectations (Schifferstein, evant when it comes to product attributes that consumers desire,
2001). Researchers have now shown that such disconfirmed ex- but which at the same time happen to be associated with less pos-
pectation, when the product experience does not meet the con- itive sensory qualities. Take, for example, the case of low-fat
sumer's product expectation, can give rise to long-lasting foods. Many consumers expect that such foods will be associated
negative consequences for product perception and consumption with a lower quality taste (sensory/hedonic experience). Indeed,
(Yeomans et al., 2008; though see also Piqueras-Fiszman & several studies have now shown that explicitly labeling a food
Spence, in press a,b). as low-fat or healthy often has a negative consequence on con-
At this point, however, one might reasonably ask whether sumers' product evaluation, no matter what the taste (e.g.,
there is really any need to use (or benefit associated with) Wansink et al., 2004; see also Does the label change the
such subtle sound/shape symbolism manipulations in the mar- taste?, 1962; Solheim, 1992; Wansink & Park, 2002). Should
ketplace. Why should one use sound/shape symbolism when there be some crossmodal correspondence between oral fat (or
one can place actual product images on the packaging and/or texture) and sound/shape, then it may be possible to convey the
choose brand names with semantic meaning, and or which are es- message to consumers that a food is low in fat (or healthy) in a
pecially attention-capturing, memorable, and/or which consumers manner that is functionally subliminal (or implicit). In turn, the
find it easy to process perceptually (e.g., Alter & Oppenheimer, hope would be that by adopting this approach, it doesn't engage
2006; Keller et al., 1998; LaBroo et al., 2008; Nilsen, 1979; a consumer's conscious (and in this case negative) evaluation in
Prez Hernndez, in press; Van Ittersum et al., 2003; Vanden quite the same way that an explicit low-fat (or healthy) label does.
Bergh et al., 1987)? Well, in practice, and as I have tried to Finally, the fact that many sound/shape symbolism effects ap-
make clear throughout this review, sound and shape symbolism pear to be present from very soon after birth (i.e., prior to the de-
should be considered as but one element in a successful brand velopment of language; e.g., Maurer, Pathman, & Mondloch,
naming, and product and packaging design strategy. The claim 2006; Walker et al., 2010; see also Reardon & Bushnell, 1988)
here is certainly not that sound/shape symbolism can be used to re- means that this approach to setting expectations could perhaps
place all these other approaches, but that by combining this ap- be particularly effective when it comes to food and beverage
proach with other effective strategies, the likelihood of a food or items that are targeted at infants. Even in adults, brand names
beverage brand's success in the marketplace will be increased that utilize the appropriate sound and shape symbolism are likely
(e.g., Klink, 2001; Vanden Bergh et al., 1987). to be learned more rapidly and remembered for longer (Kantartzis,
That said, it is also important to note that the sound/shape sym- Imai, & Kita, 2011; Kovic, Plunkett, & Westermann, 2009;
bolism approach may have a number of unique advantages rela-
tive to other more explicit/semantic forms of labeling/branding. 9
In fact, we are currently conducting research in Namibia to address this very
First, many shape/sound symbolism effects appear to be universal. question (see Bremner, Davidoff, & Spence, submitted for publication).
C. Spence / Journal of Consumer Psychology 22 (2012) 3754 51

Robson, 2011). At this point, one can only speculate as to whether Bronner, K. (submitted for publication). What is the sound of citrus? Research
one's household pets also show shape and/or sound symbolism on the correspondences between the perception of sound and taste/flavour.
Unpublished manuscript.
effects. Bronner, K., Bruhn, H., Hirt, R., & Piper, D. (2008). Research on the interaction
In conclusion, the sound and shape symbolism approach to set- between the perception of music and flavour. Poster presented at the 9th
ting and modifying consumer expectations for food and drink Annual Meeting of the International Multisensory Research Forum
products would appear to offer an effective means for marketers (IMRF), 15-19th July. Hamburg, Germany.
Calvio, A. M., Zamora, M. C., & Sarchi, M. I. (1996). Principal components
to implicitly (or subconsciously) guide their potential consumers'
and cluster analysis for descriptive sensory assessment of instant coffee.
expectations about the likely taste, aroma, flavor, and/or oral- Journal of Sensory Studies, 11, 191210.
somatosensory attributes of the products that they market. To the Carbon, C.-C. (2010). The cycle of preference: Long-term dynamics of aesthetic
extent that the marketers are successful in achieving this goal, con- appreciation. Acta Psychologica, 134, 233244.
sumers are likely to enjoy their product offerings more. While the Cardello, A. V. (1994). Consumer expectations and their role in food accep-
gaps shown in Table 1 clearly highlight the fact that more research tance. In H. J. H. MacFie, & D. M. H. Thomson (Eds.), Measurement of
food preferences (pp. 253297). London: Blackie Academic &
is needed, I would argue that there are already enough well- Professional.
supported insights/findings that are already actionable. While Chandrashekar, J., Yarmolinsky, D., von Buchholtz, L., Oka, Y., Sly, W., Ryba,
some marketers may have serendipitously (or intuitively) stumbled N. J. P., & Zuker, C. S. (2009). The taste of carbonation. Science, 326,
across certain of these phenomena over the years (e.g., Cheskin, 443445.
1957, 1972; Dichter, 1971; see Spence & Gallace, 2011), we Charters, S., Lockshin, L., & Unwin, T. (1999). Consumer responses to wine
bottle back labels. Journal of Wine Research, 10, 183195.
now have a much firmer scientific basis on which to base, limit, ex- Cheskin, L. (1957). How to predict what people will buy. New York: Liveright.
tend, and effectively utilize such findings. Cheskin, L. (1972). Marketing success: How to achieve it. Boston, MA: Cah-
ners Books.
Churchill, A., Meyners, M., Griffiths, L., & Bailey, P. (2009). The cross-modal ef-
Acknowledgments fect of fragrance in shampoo: Modifying the perceived feel of both product and
hair during and after washing. Food Quality and Preference, 20, 320328.
Collins, L. (1977). A name to conjure with. European Journal of Marketing, 11,
Many thanks to Betina Piqueras-Fiszman, Anne-Sylvie Crisi- 340363.
nel, Jools Simner, Nancy Puccinelli, and Mary Ngo for many Crisinel, A. -S., Cosser, S., King, S., Jones, R., Petrie, J., & Spence, C. (2012).
helpful comments on an earlier draft of this manuscript. A bittersweet symphony: Systematically modulating the taste of food by
changing the sonic properties of the soundtrack playing in the background.
Food Quality and Preference, 24, 201204.
Crisinel, A. S., & Spence, C. (2009). Implicit association between basic tastes
References and pitch. Neuroscience Letters, 464, 3942.
Crisinel, A. S., & Spence, C. (2010a). A sweet sound? Exploring implicit asso-
Abel, G. A., & Glinert, L. H. (2008). Chemotherapy as language: Sound sym- ciations between basic tastes and pitch. Perception, 39, 417425.
bolism in cancer medication names. Social Science & Medicine, 66, Crisinel, A. -S., & Spence, C. (2010b). As bitter as a trombone: Synesthetic corre-
18631869. spondences in non-synesthetes between tastes and flavors and musical instru-
Alter, A. L., & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2006). Predicting short-term stock fluctu- ments and notes. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 19942002.
ations by using processing fluency. Proceedings of the National Academy of Crisinel, A. S., & Spence, C. (in press a). A fruity note: Crossmodal associations be-
Sciences USA, 103, 93699372. tween odors and musical notes Chemical Senses, doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjr085.
Anon. (2010). Maximum appeal. Active and Intelligent Packaging World, 9(3), Crisinel, A.-S., & Spence, C. (in press b). Assessing the impact of pleasantness
48. ratings on crossmodal associations between food samples and musical
Anon. (submitted for publication). Impact of shape on the sensory properties of notes. Food Quality and Preference.
individual dark chocolate pieces. Food Science and Technology. Crisinel, A. S., & Spence, C. (2011). Crossmodal associations between fla-
Ares, G., & Deliza, R. (2010). Studying the influence of package shape and col- voured milk solutions and musical notes. Acta Psychologica, 138, 155161.
our on consumer expectations of milk desserts using word association and Cutler, L. (2006). Wine label design: What makes a successful label. Wine Busi-
conjoint analysis. Food Quality and Preference, 21, 930937. ness Monthly 15 August.
Bar, M., & Neta, M. (2006). Humans prefer curved visual objects. Psychologi- Davis, R. (1961). The fitness of names to drawings: A cross-cultural study in
cal Science, 17, 645648. Tanganyika. British Journal of Psychology, 52, 259268.
Becker, L., Van Rompay, T. J. L., Schifferstein, H. N. J., & Galetzka, M. Day, K. (1985). Packaging emerges as a key selling tool from cigarettes to
(2011). Tough package, strong taste: The influence of packaging design candy, designers prove that looks rival content. : Los Angeles Times
on taste impressions and product evaluations. Food Quality and Preference, March 17. Downloaded from http://articles.latimes.com/1985-03-17/busi-
22, 1723. ness/fi-35588_1_consumer on 21/04/2011.
Begley, S. (2002). StrawBerry is no BlackBerry: Building brands using sound. Dazkir, S., & Read, M. (in press). Furniture forms and their influence on our emo-
Wall Street Journal August 26, B1. Downloaded from http://www.stanfor- tional responses toward interior environments. Environment & Behavior.
d.edu/class/linguist34/Unit_08/blackberry.htm on 28/04/2011. de Arauju, I. E. T., Kringelbach, M. L., Rolls, E. T., & Hobden, P. (2003). Rep-
Belkin, K., Martin, R., Kemp, S. E., & Gilbert, A. N. (1997). Auditory pitch as a resentation of umami taste in the human brain. Journal of Neurophysiology,
perceptual analogue to odor quality. Psychological Science, 8, 340342. 90, 313319.
Belli, S. (2001). Which one is takete? Which one is maluma? La psycolonguis- Deliza, R., & MacFie, H. J. H. (1997). The generation of sensory expectation by
tica applicata alla cominicazione pubblicitaria (2nd Ed). Milan, Italy: external cues and its effect on sensory perception and hedonic ratings: A re-
Franco Angeli. view. Journal of Sensory Studies, 2, 103128.
Berridge, K. C. (2000). Measuring hedonic impact in animals and infants: Mi- Deliza, R., MacFie, H., & Hedderley, D. (2003). Use of computer-generated im-
crostructure of affective taste reactivity patterns. Neuroscience and Biobe- ages and conjoint analysis to investigate sensory expectations. Journal of
havioral Reviews, 24, 173198. Sensory Studies, 18, 465486.
Bremner, A., Davidoff, J., & Spence, C. (submitted for publication). What hap- Dematt, M. L., sterbauer, R., & Spence, C. (2007). Olfactory cues modulate
pened when Bouba and Kiki went to Namibia? Psychological Science. judgments of facial attractiveness. Chemical Senses, 32, 603610.
52 C. Spence / Journal of Consumer Psychology 22 (2012) 3754

Dematt, M. L., Sanabria, D., & Spence, C. (2006). Cross-modal associations Holt-Hansen, K. (1968). Taste and pitch. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 27,
between odors and colors. Chemical Senses, 31, 531538. 5968.
Dematt, M. L., Sanabria, D., & Spence, C. (2007). Olfactory-tactile compati- Holt-Hansen, K. (1976). Extraordinary experiences during cross-modal percep-
bility effects demonstrated using the Implicit Association Task. Acta Psy- tion. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 43, 10231027.
chologica, 124, 332343. Hultn, B. (2011). Sensory marketing: The multi-sensory brand-experience
Dematt, M. L., Sanabria, D., Sugarman, R., & Spence, C. (2006). Cross-modal concept. European Business Review, 23, 256273.
interactions between olfaction and touch. Chemical Senses, 31, 291300. Hultn, B., Broweus, N., & van Dijk, M. (2009). Sensory marketing. Basing-
DeRosia, E. D. (2008). The effectiveness of nonverbal symbolic signs and met- stoke, UK: Palgrave Macmillan.
aphors in advertisements: An experimental inquiry. Psychology and Mar- Jones, D. (2010). A WEIRD view of human nature skews psychologists' stud-
keting, 25, 298316. ies. Science, 328, 1627.
Deroy, O., & Valentin, D. (2011). Tasting shapes: Investigating cross-modal Kantartzis, K., Imai, M., & Kita, S. (2011). Japanese soundsymbolism facili-
correspondences. Chemosensory Perception, 4, 8090. tates word learning in Englishspeaking children. Cognitive Science, 35,
Dichter, E. (1971). The strategy of selling with packaging. Package Engineer- 575585.
ing Magazine, July, 16a-16c. Keller, K. L., Heckler, S. E., & Houston, M. J. (1998). The effects of brand name
Diffloth, G. (1994). i: big, a: small. In L. Hinton, J. Nichols, & J. J. Ohala suggestiveness on advertising recall. Journal of Marketing, 62(January), 4857.
(Eds.), Sound symbolism (pp. 107114). Cambridge: Cambridge University Kidd, I. (1999). The power of packaging. The Australian and New Zealand Indus-
Press. try Journal of Oenology, Viticulture, Finance, and Marketing, 14(1), 8183.
Dijksterhuis, G., Luyten, H., de Wijk, R., & Mojet, J. (2007). A new sensory King, A. M., & Cook, J. (1990). Brand names: The invisible assets. Manage-
vocabulary for crisp and crunchy dry model foods. Food Quality and Pref- ment Accounting, 72(5), 4145.
erence, 18, 3750. Klink, R. R. (2000). Creating brand names with meaning: The use of sound
Does the label change the taste? (1962). Printers Ink, 278 (1), 5557. symbolism. Marketing Letters, 11(1), 520.
Douglas, P. (1981). Developing and testing brand names. In W. Stern (Ed.), Klink, R. R. (2001). Creating meaningful new brand names: A study of seman-
Handbook of package design research (pp. 269283). New York: Wiley tics and sound symbolism. Journal of Marketing: Theory and Practice,
Interscience. 9(Spring), 2734.
Durgee, J. F. (2003). Visual rhetoric in new product design. Advances in Con- Klink, R. D. (2003). Creating meaningful brands: The relationship between
sumer Research, 30, 367372. brand name and brand mark. Marketing Letters, 14, 143157.
Erlich, J. (1995). Giving drugs a good name. The New York Times Magazine, Khler, W. (1929). Gestalt psychology. New York: Liveright.
September 3, 3637. Kohli, C., & LaBahn, D. W. (1995). Creating effective brand names: A study of
Ewald, J., & Moskowitz, H. (2007). The pushpull of marketing and advertis- the naming process. Institute for the Study of Business Markets: PA ISBM
ing and the algebra of the consumer's mind. Journal of Sensory Studies, 22, Report 121995.
126175. Kovic, V., Plunkett, K., & Westermann, G. (2009). The shape of words in the
Fnagy, I. (1963). Die Metaphern in der Phonetik [The metaphors in phonet- brain. Cognition, 114, 1928.
ics]. : The Hague. Koza, B. J., Cilmi, A., Dolese, M., & Zellner, D. A. (2005). Color enhances
Gal, D., Wheeler, S. C., & Shiv, B. (2007, unpublished manuscript). Cross- orthonasal olfactory intensity and reduces retronasal olfactory intensity.
modal influences on gustatory perception. Available at SSRN: http://ssrn. Chemical Senses, 30, 643649.
com/abstract=1030197. Krishna, A. (Ed.). (2010). Sensory marketing: Research on the sensuality of
Gallace, A., Boschin, E., & Spence, C. (2011). On the taste of Bouba and products. London: Routledge.
Kiki: An exploration of word-food associations in neurologically normal Krishna, A., & Morrin, M. (2008). Does touch affect taste? The perceptual
participants. Cognitive Neuroscience, 2, 3446. transfer of product container haptic cues. Journal of Consumer Research,
Garber, L. L., Burke, R. R., & Jones, J. M. (2000). The role of packaging ap- 34, 807818.
pearance in consumer purchase consideration and choice. Boston, MA: Mar- Labroo, A. A., Dhar, R., & Schwartz, N. (2008). Of frog wines and frowning
keting Science Institute Working Paper Series. watches: Semantic priming, perceptual fluency, and brand evaluation. Jour-
Garber, L. L., Jr., Hyatt, E. M., & Boya, .. (2008). The mediating effects of nal of Consumer Research, 34, 819831.
the appearance of nondurable consumer goods and their packaging on con- Ladefoged, P. (1993). A course in phonetics (3rd Ed.). New York: Harcourt
sumer behavior. In H. N. J. Schifferstein, & P. Hekkert (Eds.), Product ex- Brace Jovanovich.
perience (pp. 581602). London: Elsevier. Laird, D. A. (1932). How the consumer estimates quality by subconscious sen-
Geel, L., Kinnear, M., & De Kock, H. L. (2005). Relating consumer preferences to sory impressions: With special reference to the role of smell. Journal of Ap-
sensory attributes of instant coffee. Food Quality & Preference, 16, 237244. plied Psychology, 16, 241246.
Gladwell, M. (2005). Blink: The power of thinking without thinking. USA: Lit- Lehrer, A. (2009). Wine & conversation (2nd. Ed.). Oxford: Oxford University
tle, Brown, & Company. Press.
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring in- Li, W., Moallem, I., Paller, K. A., & Gottfried, J. A. (2007). Subliminal smells
dividual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. can guide social preferences. Psychological Science, 18, 10441049.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 14641480. Lindstrom, M. (2005). Brand sense: How to build brands through touch, taste,
Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and smell, sight and sound. London: Kogan Page.
using the Implicit Association Test: I, an improved scoring algorithm. Jour- Lowenthal, A. M. (1981). Design research for bilingual packaging. In W. Stern
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 197216. (Ed.), Handbook of package design research (pp. 399402). New York:
Hartley, R. F. (1992). Marketing mistakes (5th Ed.). New York: Wiley. Wiley Interscience.
Heath, T. B., Chatterjee, S., & Russo France, K. (1990). Using the phonemes of Lowrey, T. M., & Shrum, L. J. (2007). Phonetic symbolism and brand name
brand names to symbolize brand attributes. In W. Bearden, & A. Parasura- preference. Journal of Consumer Research, 34, 406414.
man (Eds.), The American Marketing Association (AMA) educator's pro- Lowrey, T. M., Shrum, L. J., & Dubitsky, T. M. (2003). The relation between
ceedings: Enhancing knowledge development in marketing, Vol. 3. (pp. brand-name linguistic characteristics and brand-name memory. Journal of
842), Chicago: American Marketing Association. Advertising, 32(3), 717.
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the Lyman, B. (1989). A psychology of food, more than a matter of taste. New
world? The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 61135. York: Avi, van Nostrand Reinhold.
Hine, T. (1995). The total package: The secret history and hidden meanings of MacKay, I. R. A. (1978). Introducing practical phonetics. Boston: Little,
boxes, bottles, cans, and other persuasive containers. New York: Little Brown. Brown and Co..
Hinton, L., Nichols, J., & Ohala, J. J. (Eds.). (1994). Sound symbolism. Cam- Maga, J. A. (1974). Influence of color on taste thresholds. Chemical Senses and
bridge: Cambridge University Press. Flavor, 1, 115119.
C. Spence / Journal of Consumer Psychology 22 (2012) 3754 53

Marks, L. E. (1991). Metaphor and the unity of the senses. In H. T. Lawless & Piqueras-Fiszman, B., & Spence, C. (2011). Crossmodal correspondences in
B. P. Klein (Eds.), Sensory science theory and applications in foods product packaging: Assessing color-flavor correspondences for potato
(pp. 185205). New York: Marcel Dekker. chips (crisps). Appetite, 57, 753757.
Maurer, D., Pathman, T., & Mondloch, C. J. (2006). The shape of boubas: Sound- Piqueras-Fiszman, B. & Spence, C. (in press b). Sensory incongruity in the food
shape correspondences in toddlers and adults. Developmental Science, 9, and beverage sector: Art, science, and commercialization. Petit Propos
316322. Culinaire.
McNeil, D. G., Jr. (2003). The science of naming drugs (sorry, Z is already Raymond, J. (2000). The world of senses. Newsweek Special Issue, 136(17A),
taken). The New York Times, December, 28, 10. 1618 Fall-Winter.
McNeil, J. U. (1982). Consumer behaviour: An integrative approach. Boston, Reardon, P., & Bushnell, E. W. (1988). Infants' sensitivity to arbitrary pairings
MA: Little, Brown. of color and taste. Infant Behavior & Development, 11, 245250.
Mesz, B., Trevisan, M., & Sigman, M. (2011). The taste of music. Perception, Robertson, K. (1989). Strategically desirable brand name characteristics. Jour-
40, 209219. nal of Consumer Marketing, 6(4), 6171.
Moskvitch, K. (2011). Printer produces personalised 3D chocolate: BBC News Robson, D. (2011). Language's missing link. New Scientist, 1971(16 July),
downloaded from http://www.bbc.co.uk/go/em/fr/-/news/technology- 3033.
14030720 on 17/07/2011. Rudmin, F., & Cappelli, M. (1983). Tonetaste synesthesia: A replication. Per-
Newman, S. S. (1933). Further experiments in phonetic symbolism. The Amer- ceptual and Motor Skills, 56, 118.
ican Journal of Psychology, 45, 5375. Sapir, E. (1929). A study in phonetic symbolism. Journal of Experimental Psy-
Ngo, M., Misra, R., & Spence, C. (2011). Assessing the shapes and speech chology, 12, 225239.
sounds that people associate with chocolate samples varying in cocoa con- Saussure, F. de (1916). Cours de linguistique gnrale. Paris: Payot. [Translat-
tent. Food Quality & Preference, 22, 567572. ed as Course in general linguistics (1966). Edited by C. Bally and A.
Ngo, M. K., Piqueras-Fiszman, B., & Spence, C. (in press a). On the colour and Sechehaye in collaboration with A. Reidlinger (trans.) and notes by W. Bas-
shape of still and sparkling water: Implications for product packaging. Food kin. New York: McGraw-Hill.]
Quality & Preference. Schifferstein, H. N. J. (2001). Effects of product beliefs on product perception
Ngo, M. K., & Spence, C. (2011). Assessing the shapes and speech sounds that and liking. In L. Frewer, E. Risvik, & H. Schifferstein (Eds.), Food, people
people associate with different kinds of chocolate. Journal of Sensory Stud- and society: A European perspective of consumers' food choices
ies, 26, 421428. (pp. 7396). Berlin: Springer Verlag.
Nilsen, D. L. F. (1979). Language play in advertising: Linguistic invention in Schifferstein, H. N. J., & Spence, C. (2008). Multisensory product experience. In
product naming. In J. E. Alatis, & G. R. Tucker (Eds.), Language in public H. N. J. Schifferstein & P Hekkert (Eds.), Product experience (pp. 133161).
life (pp. 137143). Georgetown: Georgetown University Press. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Nuckolls, J. B. (2003). The case for sound symbolism. Annual Review of An- Schloss, I. (1981). Chickens and pickles: Choosing a brand name. Journal of
thropology, 28, 225252. Advertising Research, 21(December), 4749.
O'Mahony, M. (1983). Gustatory responses to nongustatory stimuli. Percep- Seo, H. -S., Arshamian, A., Schemmer, K., Scheer, I., Sander, T., Ritter, G., &
tion, 12, 627633. Hummel, T. (2010). Cross-modal integration between odors and abstract
O'Mahony, M., Goldenberg, M., Stedmon, J., & Alford, J. (1979). Confusion in symbols. Neuroscience Letters, 478, 175178.
the use of the taste adjectives sour and bitter. Chemical Senses and Fla- Shrum, L. J., & Lowrey, T. M. (2007). Sounds convey meaning: The implica-
vour, 4, 301318. tions of phonetic symbolism for brand name construction. In T. M. Lowrey
Overbeeke, C. J., & Peters, M. E. (1991). The taste of desserts' packages. Per- (Ed.), Psycholinguistic phenomena in marketing communications
ceptual and Motor Skills, 73, 575580. (pp. 3958). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Parise, C., & Spence, C. (2009). When birds of a feather flock together: Syn- Simner, J., Cuskley, C., & Kirby, S. (2010). What sound does that taste?
esthetic correspondences modulate audiovisual integration in non- Cross-modal mappings across gustation and audition. Perception, 39,
synesthetes. PLoS One, 4(5), e5664. 553569.
Parise, C. V., & Spence, C. (in press a). Audiovisual crossmodal correspon- Solheim, R. (1992). Consumer liking for sausages affected by sensory quality
dences. To appear in J. Simner & E. Hubbard (Eds.), Oxford handbook of and information on fat content. Appetite, 19, 285292.
synaesthesia. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Spence, C. (2002). The ICI report on the secret of the senses. London: The
Parise, C. V., & Spence, C. (2012). Assessing the associations between brand Communication Group.
packaging and brand attributes using an indirect performance measure. Spence, C. (2007). Creating innovative packaging that appeals to all the senses.
Food Quality and Preference, 27, 1723. Invited presentation given as the PACE: Packaging and Converting Execu-
Pecia, S., & Smith, K. S. (2010). Hedonic and motivational roles of opioids in tive Forum meeting. Paris (pp. 811). February.
food reward: Implications for overeating disorders. Pharmacology Bio- Spence, C. (2008). Sensing the future. In L. Miles (Ed.), AQR directory & hand-
chemistry and Behavior, 97, 3446. book 2008 (pp. 5660). St. Neots, Cambridgeshire: AQR.
Peck, J., & Childers, T. L. (2003). Individual differences in haptic information Spence, C. (2010) zrzkels t rzkszervvel [Colour associations]. IPM
processing: The Need for Touch scale. Journal of Consumer Research, InterPress Magazin, October, 108112. (In Hungarian).
30, 430442. Spence, C. (2011a). Sound design: How understanding the brain of the
Prez Hernndez, L. (2011). Cognitive tools for successful branding. Applied consumer can enhance auditory and multisensory product/brand devel-
Linguistics, 32, 369388. opment. In K. Bronner, R. Hirt, & C. Ringe (Eds.), Audio Branding
Peterson, R. A., & Ross, I. (1972). How to name brands. Journal of Advertising, Congress Proceedings 2010 (pp. 3752). Baden-Baden, Germany:
12, 2934. Nomos Verlag.
Peynaud, E. (1987). The taste of wine: The art and science of wine appreciation Spence, C. (2011b). Wine and music. The World of Fine Wine, 31, 96104.
(Trans. M. Schuster). London: Macdonald & Co. Spence, C. (2011c). Crossmodal correspondences: A tutorial review. Attention,
Pinker, S. (2007). The stuff of thought: Language as a window into human nature. Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 971995.
London: Allen Lane. Spence, C., & Gallace, A. (2011). Tasting shapes and words. Food Quality &
Pinson, C. (1986). An implicit product theory approach to consumers' inferential Preference, 22, 290295.
judgments about products. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 3, Spence, C., Levitan, C., Shankar, M. U., & Zampini, M. (2010). Does food
1938. color influence taste and flavor perception in humans? Chemosensory Per-
Piqueras-Fiszman, B., Alcaide, J., Roura, E., & Spence, C. (2011). Is it the plate ception, 3, 6884.
or is it the food? The influence of the color and shape of the plate on the per- Spence, C., Ngo, M., Percival, B., & Smith, B. (submitted for publication).
ception of the food placed on it. Food Quality & Preference, doi: Crossmodal correspondences: Assessing the shape symbolism of foods
10.1016/j.foodqual.2011.05.009. having a complex flavour profile. Journal of Sensory Studies.
54 C. Spence / Journal of Consumer Psychology 22 (2012) 3754

Spence, C., & Piqueras-Fiszman, B. (in press). The multisensory packaging of Walker, P., & Smith, S. (1985). Stroop interference based on the multi-
beverages. To appear in M. G. Kontominas (Ed.), Food packaging: Proce- modal correlates of haptic size and auditory pitch. Perception, 14,
dures, management and trends. Hauppauge NY: Nova Publishers. 729736.
Spence, C., & Shankar, M. U. (2010). The influence of auditory cues on the per- Wallace, D. (1975). Naming the Edsel. Automotive Quarterly Magazine, XIII
ception of, and responses to, food and drink. Journal of Sensory Studies, 25, (2; Second Quarter), 182191.
406430. Wansink, B., & Park, S. -B. (2002). Sensory suggestiveness and labeling:
Spence, C., Shankar, M. U., & Blumenthal, H. (2011). Sound bites: Auditory Do soy labels bias taste? Journal of Sensory Studies, 17, 483491.
contributions to the perception and consumption of food and drink. In In F. Wansink, B., van Ittersum, K., & Painter, J. E. (2004). How diet and
Bacci & D. Mecher (Eds.), Art and the senses (pp. 207238). Oxford: Ox- health labels influence taste and satiation. Journal of Food Science,
ford University Press. 69, S340S346.
Steiner, J. E., Glaser, D., Hawilo, M. E., & Berridge, K. C. (2001). Comparative Westbury, C. (2005). Implicit sound symbolism in lexical access: Evidence
expression of hedonic impact: Affective reactions to taste by human infants from an interference task. Brain and Language, 93, 1019.
and other primates. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 25, 5374. Wheatley, J. (1973). Putting colour into marketing. Marketing, October, 2429
Stern, W. (Ed.). (1981). Handbook of package design research. New York: 67.
Wiley Interscience. Williams, J. M. (1976). Synesthetic adjectives: A possible law of semantic
The Dutch touch (2011). http://175proof.com/triedandtested/the-dutch-touch/ last change. Language, 52, 461478.
downloaded on 14/04/2011. Woods, A. T., Lloyd, D. M., Kuenzel, J., Poliakoff, E., Dijksterhuis, G. B.,
Ultan, R. (1978). Size-sound symbolism. In J. Greenberg, C. A. Ferguson, & & Thomas, A. (2011). Expected taste intensity affects response to sweet
E. A. Moravcsik (Eds.), Universals of human language. Phonology, 2. drinks in primary taste cortex. Neuroreport, 22, 365369.
(pp. 527568). Stanford: Stanford University Press. Yeomans, M., Chambers, L., Blumenthal, H., & Blake, A. (2008). The
Vanden Bergh, B. G. (1990). The recurring kase of the special K. Journal of role of expectancy in sensory and hedonic evaluation: The case of
Advertising Research, 30(5), RC9RC12. smoked salmon ice-cream. Food Quality and Preference, 19,
Vanden Bergh, B. G., Adler, K., & Oliver, L. (1987). Linguistic distinction among 565573.
top brand names. Journal of Advertising Research, 24(August/September), Yorkston, E. A., & Menon, G. (2004). A sound idea: Phonetic effects of brand
3944. names on consumer judgements. Journal of Consumer Research, 31,
Vanden Bergh, B. G., Collins, J., Schultz, M., & Adler, K. (1984). Sound ad- 4351.
vice on brand names. Journalism Quarterly, 61, 835840. Zampini, M., & Spence, C. (2004). The role of auditory cues in modulating
Van Ittersum, K., Candel, M. J. J. M., & Meulenberg, M. T. G. (2003). The influence the perceived crispness and staleness of potato chips. Journal of Sensory
of the image of a product's region of origin on product evaluation. Journal of Science, 19, 347363.
Business Research, 56, 215226. Zhang, Y., Feick, L., & Price, L. J. (2006). The impact of self-construal on aes-
Vickers, Z. M. (1984). Crispness and crunchiness A difference in pitch? thetic preference for angular versus rounded shapes. Personality and Social
Journal of Texture Studies, 15, 157163. Psychology Bulletin, 32, 794805.
Walker, P., Bremner, J. G., Mason, U., Spring, J., Mattock, K., Slater, A., & John- Zinkhan, G. M., & Martin, C. R., Jr. (1987). New brand names and infer-
son, S. P. (2010). Preverbal infants' sensitivity to synesthetic cross-modality ential beliefs: Some insights on naming new products. Journal of
correspondences. Psychological Science, 21, 2125. Business Research, 15, 157172.

Вам также может понравиться