Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 6

Psychobiology and Behavioral Strategies

Effect of Carbohydrate and Prolonged


Exercise on Affect and Perceived Exertion
SUSAN H. BACKHOUSE1, NICOLETTE C. BISHOP2, STUART J. H. BIDDLE2, and CLYDE WILLIAMS2
1
Carnegie Research Institute, Carnegie Faculty of Sport and Education, Leeds Metropolitan University, Leeds, UNITED
KINGDOM; and 2School of Sport and Exercise Sciences, Loughborough University, Loughborough, UNITED KINGDOM

ABSTRACT
BACKHOUSE, S. H., N. C. BISHOP, S. J. H. BIDDLE, and C. WILLIAMS. Effect of Carbohydrate and Prolonged Exercise on Affect
and Perceived Exertion. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc., Vol. 37, No. 10, pp. 1768 1773, 2005. Introduction: It has been reported that
perceptions of exertion are attenuated during prolonged cycle exercise, following CHO ingestion. However, no studies to date have
examined the influence of such feedings on psychological affect during prolonged exercise, even though affect and perceived exertion
are different constructs. Purpose: To examine the influence of regular CHO beverage ingestion on affect (pleasure displeasure) and
perceived exertion during prolonged cycle exercise. Methods: In a randomized, double-blind, counterbalanced design, nine endurance
trained males cycled for 2 h at 70% VO2max on two occasions, separated by 1 wk. On each occasion, they consumed either a water
placebo (PLA) or a 6.4% carbohydrate-electrolyte solution (CHO) immediately before they cycled (5 mLkg1 body mass) and every
15 min thereafter (2 mLkg1 body mass). Pleasure displeasure was assessed before, during, and after the prolonged bout of cycling.
Results: During exercise, reported pleasure initially improved and was subsequently maintained in the CHO trial, in contrast to a
decline reported in the PLA trial. Ratings of pleasure displeasure were more positive during recovery in the CHO trial compared with
the PLA trial (P 0.05) and the only significant increase (P 0.05) in pleasure occurred 15 min postexercise in the CHO trial only.
RPE increased (P 0.05) over the course of the bout of cycling and was lower (P 0.05) 75 min into exercise in the CHO trial.
Immediately postexercise, plasma glucose concentration was higher in the CHO compared with the PLA trial (P 0.05). A main effect
of trial was found for plasma cortisol concentration, with higher values reported in PLA trial. Conclusion: Results suggest that CHO
ingestion enhanced feelings of pleasure during and following prolonged cycling and highlighted the importance of assessing not only
what, but also how a person feels. Key Words: AFFECT, RPE, PLEASUREDISPLEASURE, CHO INGESTION, CYCLING

T
he benefits of CHO ingestion during prolonged (90 previously the focus has been on what a person feels, as
min) continuous exercise have been well docu- measured by the RPE scale. Therefore, in accordance with
mented (11). The literature is consistent in showing the assertion of Hardy and Rejeski (19) that the RPE on its
that CHO feedings can delay fatigue in a variety of exercise own provides limited information about the subjective ex-
protocols by maintaining blood glucose and in some cases periences of individuals during exercise, what and how
sparing muscle glycogen levels, both being important a person feels were examined during a prolonged exercise
sources of energy for working muscles. In addition, the session. This was facilitated by the administration of the
notion that CHO ingestion influences perceptions of exer- Borg (7) RPE scale, a measure of what a person feels, and
tion during prolonged cycling exercise has been well doc- the Feeling Scale (FS) (19) as the measure of how a
umented in the recent literature (8,20,29). person feels.
To our knowledge, research has yet to examine the in- The administration of both scales in the present study
fluence of CHO ingestion on affective states (how a provides a more encompassing representation of the subjec-
person feels) during prolonged cycling. As noted earlier, tive exercise experience because affect and perceived exer-
tion are not isomorphic constructs (19). In support of this
assertion are previous studies (1,19) that have reported that
Address for correspondence: Dr. Susan H. Backhouse, Carnegie Research the correlations between the FS and RPE scale change
Institute, Carnegie Faculty of Sport and Education, Leeds Metropolitan
University, Headingley Campus, Leeds, LS6 3QS, UK; E-mail:
across a range of exercise intensities, but generally never
S.Backhouse@leedsmet.ac.uk. exceed a moderate level (approximately 0.6, leaving a
Submitted for publication December 2004. large proportion of the variance in each unaccounted for).
Accepted for publication May 2005. Furthermore, a linear response to incremental exercise is
0195-9131/05/3710-1768/0 often reported using the RPE scale, which differs from the
MEDICINE & SCIENCE IN SPORTS & EXERCISE curvilinear responses elicited using the FS (14). It is not
Copyright 2005 by the American College of Sports Medicine surprising that a linear relationship is often reported because
DOI: 10.1249/01.mss.0000181837.77380.80 the Borg RPE scale was originally designed to facilitate
1768
linearity of RPE values with HR (6). This supports the leagues (1) reported that ratings of pleasure progressively
importance of administering additional subjective measures declined during a 2-h constant-paced run at 70% VO2max,
during exercise studies because, as stated previously (19), whereas RPE progressively increased. This is in contrast to
two individuals may report the same perception of exertion the positive affect that Morgan (24) suggests is experienced
on the RPE scale (e.g., hard, 15), but this may be accom- by some runners after the first hour of a run (based on
panied by feelings of pleasure in one individual and dis- anecdotal accounts). The present study, although focusing
pleasure in another. One can also distinguish affect from predominantly on the effect of CHO ingestion, also enables
perceived exertion based on the neuroanatomical and neu- a further examination of such discrepancies in prior re-
rophysiological mechanisms involved as they differ beyond search.
the level of the brainstem. It appears that the thalamus Therefore, the purpose of this study was to examine the
insulasomatosensory cortex forms the basis of perceived effects of CHO ingestion and prolonged (i.e., 120 min)
exertion (10) and the thalamusamygdalanucleus accum- exercise on pleasure displeasure and perceived exertion.
bens axis forms the basis of affective responses (9,22).
Therefore, even though the relationship between CHO in-
METHODS
gestion and RPE has been documented previously, such
conceptual differences support the need for further investi- This study was performed as part of an investigation
gations into the role of CHO ingestion on subjective states examining the role of CHO ingestion on immune measures,
during exercise. the data from which have already been published (4).
A number of studies have been reported in the literature Participants. Nine endurance trained males (mean
that have examined the influence that glucose-containing SEM; age 25 2 yr; height 191 4 cm; body mass 76.8
drinks exert on affective states at rest (2,3). Benton and 2.8 kg; VO2max, 64.7 2.7 mLkg1min1) volunteered
Owens (3) concluded that higher blood glucose levels (that to participate in this study. All participants were fully in-
were within the normal range) were associated with lower formed of the nature and purpose of the study before signing
reported tension. Negative affect has also been associated a statement of informed consent. The study had the approval
with low blood glucose in insulin-dependent diabetics (17) of the ethical advisory committee of Loughborough
and in individuals experimentally manipulated into a hypo- University.
glycemic state (15). It has also been reported that CHO Measures of affect and perceived exertion. The
ingestion resulted in improved affective states during pro- FS (19) was used as a measure of the affective dimension of
longed training periods in field hockey players (21). Two pleasure displeasure. Commonly used for the assessment of
recent studies (30,32) have considered the relationship be- affective responses during exercise, it is an 11-point single-
tween CHO feedings and affect among other factors during item bipolar rating scale. The scale ranges from 5 to 5.
intermittent high-intensity exercise. However, these inves- Anchors are provided at the 0 point (neutral) and at all odd
tigations by Welsh et al. (30) and Winnick et al. (32) integers, ranging from very good (5) to very bad
focused on specific states, such as fatigue, and inconsistent (5). Participants were asked to rate how they felt at that
patterns were reported. Few studies have examined the particular moment. The FS was administered preexercise,
possible link between CHO feedings and affective states every 15 min throughout the prolonged cycle, upon cessa-
during exercise, which is unfortunate because whether one tion of exercise, and 5, 15, and 30 min postexercise (Fig. 1).
feels good or bad during exercise is clearly an important The RPE scale (7) was used as a measure of perceived
factor. Indeed, it could determine the outcome of a compet- exertion during exercise and was administered every 15 min
itive event and also impact on task persistence (1). There- during the trial (Fig. 1). The scale ranges from 6 to 20, with
fore, given the relationships previously documented in the anchors ranging from very, very light to very, very
literature, it was hypothesized that affective states may be hard. The FS was presented first, followed by the RPE
influenced by CHO ingestion during prolonged exercise. scale.
Based on studies at rest, we speculated that the effects might Preliminary procedures. Maximal oxygen uptake
be due to changes in blood glucose level and therefore was estimated by means of a continuous incremental exer-
activation of the autonomic nervous system in an attempt to
return blood glucose concentration to normal levels (2). It
was also hypothesized that, in accordance with previous
studies investigating CHO ingestion and RPE during cy-
cling exercise (8,13), an attenuation in RPE would be ob-
served following CHO ingestion.
Despite more than three decades of research concerning
the exerciseaffect relationship, there is, and continues to
be, a void in the literature on prolonged exercise. There is a
strong advocacy that exercise makes people feel better;
however, aside from a study by Acevedo et al. (1), affective
changes during and following prolonged constant-paced ex-
ercise have not been identified. Indeed, Acevedo and col- FIGURE 1Schematic representation of the experimental protocol.

CHO INGESTION, AFFECTIVE STATES, AND CYCLING Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise 1769
cise test on an electrically braked cycle ergometer (Lode Further blood samples were taken immediately postexercise
Excalibur, Groningen, The Netherlands) to volitional and at 1 h postexercise. No food was consumed during this
fatigue. From the VO2work relationship, the work rate period, and fluid ingestion was as prescribed.
equivalent to 70% VO2max was interpolated. Please refer Blood analyses. Blood samples were collected into
to Bishop et al. (4) for further details of the preliminary monovette tubes (Sarstedt, Leicester, UK), containing lith-
procedures. ium heparin and were centrifuged at 1500 g at 4C for 10
Nutritional control. Participants reported for each ex- min within 15 min of sampling. The plasma obtained was
perimental trial following an overnight fast of between 10 immediately stored at minus 70C for later analysis for
and 12 h. This was to ensure that participants began each glucose and cortisol using hexokinase (No. 16-50 Kit,
trial with an empty stomach, thus eliminating any negative Sigma, Poole, UK) and 125I radioimmunoassay (ICN Phar-
effect that a previous meal might have had, both on exercise maceuticals, Costa Mesa, CA) methods, respectively. Ra-
metabolism and gastric emptying. For the 48 h before the dioactivity was measured using an automated gamma
experimental trial, participants refrained from any strenuous counter (Cobra II, Packard Instruments Co. Inc.). For further
physical activity. Participants consumed their normal diet, details of the analysis procedures, the reader is referred to
but weighed and recorded food eaten in a food diary during Bishop et al. (4).
the 48 h before the first trial. This diet was then replicated Statistical analysis. A series of two-way ANOVA for
during the corresponding period before any other further repeated measures on two factors (experimental condition
trials. This information was later analyzed using COMP- and sampling time) was used to examine the affective,
EAT (Nutrition Systems, London). In addition, caffeine and physiological, metabolic, and perceived exertion data. For
alcohol were also prohibited during the 48 h before the trial the FS ratings, separate ANOVA were conducted on the pre-
because both have been found to have transient effects on to postexercise time points (pre, post 0, post 15, post 30) and
affective states (27). during-exercise time points (from 15 to 120 min). This
Experimental procedures. In a randomized, double- approach allowed direct comparisons during exercise be-
blind, counterbalanced design, participants completed two tween the FS and RPE scale. Significant main effects were
exercise trials, each separated by at least 7 d. On each further analyzed using paired t-tests and the Bonferroni
occasion, participants consumed either a carbohydrate adjustment for the number of pairwise comparisons was
(6.4%) solution (CHO) flavored with lemon or a placebo employed. GreenhouseGeisser epsilon corrections were
solution (PLA) that was artificially sweetened and flavored used when the sphericity assumption was violated. Statisti-
with lemon. Each drink was identical in flavor and appear- cal significance was set at the 0.05 level, apart from the
ance, and participants were not aware of the content of the Bonferroni analyses. Values are presented as means (SEM).
drinks in each trial. These solutions were ingested immedi-
ately before exercise (5 mLkg1 body mass), every 15 min
during (2 mLkg1 body mass), and 5 min postexercise (5
RESULTS
mLkg1 body mass).
Participants reported to the laboratory at 8:00 a.m. on Diet. A nutrient analysis of the 2-d food records before
each occasion following an overnight fast. Upon arrival, each of the two exercise sessions revealed no differences in
they responded to the FS. They then emptied their bladders the energy intake and nutrient composition between condi-
before body mass (in shorts only) was measured. Partici- tions. The mean energy intake of the participants was 2322
pants were then seated quietly for 15 min after which a kcald1, with the proportion of energy being 64.1%
blood sample was taken from an antecubital vein by veni- 3.7% from CHO, 19.6% 3.0% from fat, and 17.3 0.8%
puncture. They then performed a 2-h cycle ergometer ride from protein.
on an electronically braked ergometer (Lode Excalibur) Physiological responses to the exercise proto-
(Fig. 1). Participants began cycling at 70% VO2max and, col. HR and %VO2max did not differ between trials with
although the intensity was monitored, they completed the the mean VO2max during exercise being 74.2 1.3% in the
exercise without continual adjustment of the resistance. The CHO trial compared with 75.2 1.4% in the PLA trial, thus
FS was administered at 15-min intervals. Expired air sam- demonstrating that the participants were exercising at the
ples were obtained using the standard Douglas bag method same relative exercise intensity in both conditions. HR
at 20, 50, 80, and 110 min during the exercise bout to during exercise ranged from 158 to 170 bpm.
determine exercise intensity. A paramagnetic oxygen ana- Plasma glucose. Plasma glucose concentration was
lyzer (Servomex 1420B, Crowborough, UK) and an infrared higher immediately postexercise in the CHO trial (6.1 0.3
carbon dioxide analyzer (Servomex 1415B) were used along mmolL1) than the PLA trial (5.4 0.3 mmolL1),
with a dry gas meter (Harvard Apparatus, Edenbridge, UK) and there was a significant interaction for treatment time
for determination of VO2 and VCO2. HR were recorded (F(2, 16) 7.6; P 0.05). This showed that in the CHO trial,
every 15 min during exercise using short-range telemetry blood glucose concentration increased from preexercise to
(Sportester, Polar Electro, Kempele, Finland). immediately postexercise, whereas in the PLA trial, the
Upon cessation of the exercise task, participants imme- concentration remained stable from preexercise to post 0,
diately responded to the FS. Participants once again re- and by 1 h postexercise had fallen to a concentration lower
sponded to this measure 5, 15, and 30 min postexercise. than that observed preexercise (Table 1).
1770 Official Journal of the American College of Sports Medicine http://www.acsm-msse.org
TABLE 1. Changes in blood glucose and cortisol concentration during the 120-min cycle (mean SEM).
CHO PLA
Pre Post 0 Post 1 h Pre Post 0 Post 1 h
Glucose (mmolL1) 5.2 0.1 6.1 0.3 5.3 0.2 5.4 0.1 5.4 0.3 5.0 0.2
Cortisol (nmolL1) 528 23 588 74 477 60 576 46 811 153 641 99
Cortisol: main effect for trial, P 0.05; glucose: main effect for trial, time and trial time interaction, P 0.05.

Plasma cortisol. Plasma cortisol concentrations in- direction; however, only one was significant (90 min, PLA
creased by 11% during the CHO trial (528 23 to 588 trial; 0.75, P 0.05). The remainder displayed low to
74 nmolL1) and by 24% (576 46 to 811 153 moderate (range: 0.02 to 0.63), nonsignificant correla-
nmolL1) in the PLA trial, but this increase across both tions, demonstrating that the FS and RPE are conceptually
trials was not statistically significant due to large variability distinct constructs.
(Table 1). Overall, there was a main effect for trial, with
values being higher in the PLA trial (F(1, 8) 8.7; P 0.05)
DISCUSSION
compared with the CHO trial.
Pleasure displeasure. Analysis on the FS revealed In the present study, we observed that ratings of pleasure
that during exercise, an overall main effect for treatment displeasure changed as a result of prolonged cycling and that
was observed (F(1, 8) 8.5; P 0.05), but not time. Overall CHO supplementation appeared to influence this response.
ratings of pleasure were higher during the CHO trial, com- Repeated in-task assessment of pleasure displeasure led to
pared with the PLA trial. Pleasure ratings became more the observation that CHO ingestion noticeably influenced
positive and were maintained throughout exercise in the how participants felt during and following exercise as
CHO trial, whereas in the PLA trial, ratings became less measured by the FS. This study highlights the importance of
positive (Fig. 2). Analysis of the pre- to postexercise assessing this psychological parameter in addition to the
changes revealed a significant interaction effect (F(4, 32) often administered RPE scale because what the partici-
2.77; P 0.05) with ratings of pleasure being higher overall pants felt was reduced by CHO ingestion to a much lesser
in the CHO trial compared with the PLA trial, and pleasure extent. Furthermore, we found no significant correlations
improved from preexercise to 15 min postexercise in the between the FS and RPE scale over the course of the trial,
CHO trial only (Fig. 2). supporting the assertion that affect and perceived exertion
Rating of perceived exertion. RPE increased over are not isomorphic constructs (19).
time (F(7, 56) 21.1; P 0.001) across both conditions. Based on previous studies at rest, we hypothesized that
There was an interaction of treatment time (F(7, 56) 2.6; affect would be positively maintained in the CHO trial. Our
P 0.05), with RPE being significantly lower only at 75 data supported this hypothesis as pleasure displeasure was
min in the CHO trial compared with the PLA trial (Fig. 3). observed to shift in opposite directions when the CHO and
Correlations. The correlations between the FS and PLA trials were compared, with participants in the CHO
RPE scale were weak. All correlations were of negative trial reporting a more pleasurable exercise experience.

FIGURE 2Pleasure displeasure (FS) be-


fore, during, and following the 120-min bout
of cycling (overall main effect of condition
during exercise; P < 0.05).

CHO INGESTION, AFFECTIVE STATES, AND CYCLING Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise 1771
tion. Indeed, a number of studies have reported an asso-
ciation between increased cortisol levels and increases in
negative affective states (16) and further investigations
may be warranted.
As highlighted, the relationship between CHO ingestion
and RPE during prolonged exercise has been well docu-
mented (8,20,29), and findings indicate that CHO availabil-
ity attenuates RPE during the latter stages of exercise
(29,31). This study offers tentative support for such findings
as RPE was lower 75 min into exercise compared with the
PLA trial. However, such a reduction was not sustained and
it is not clear why the effect was limited to 75 min into
exercise. In agreement with previous studies involving cy-
cling (12), blood glucose concentration was influenced by
CHO supplementation, with higher concentrations noted
following exercise in the CHO trial. Although blood sam-
FIGURE 3Rating of perceived exertion during the 120-min bout of
cycling; *P < 0.05, CHO vs PLA. ples were not collected during exercise, they were collected
as soon as the exercise bout was complete and therefore still
Indeed, the ingestion of CHO during exercise prevented offer partial support for the assertion by Coggan and Coyle
the observed reduction in pleasure noted in the PLA trial (12) that, during cycling exercise, reductions in RPE fol-
(Fig. 2). We also hypothesized that CHO ingestion during lowing CHO ingestion may be a function of elevated cir-
exercise would attenuate RPE, but this was limited to the culating levels of blood glucose.
75th minute of exercise. Consequently, future studies This study was also designed to determine the impact of
should continue to employ both scales in order to obtain prolonged exercise on the temporal dynamics of pleasure
a more complete understanding of the subjective exercise displeasure. Unlike the RPE scale, FS ratings can be bidirec-
experience. tional and research is consistently observing something of a
Although this study was not designed to uncover the rebound pattern immediately following exercise that has
mechanisms by which CHO feedings improve affect, a induced negative responses during exercise (1,5,18). The
number of mechanisms have been previously described at present study offers further support for this phenomenon (Fig.
rest (2) and the findings of this study could offer support 2). It appears that the trajectory of pleasure displeasure during
for one of the propositions during exercise. The overall and following exercise exhibits two distinct phases. The first
differences during exercise in pleasure displeasure be- phase involves the decline during exercise and the second
tween the CHO and PLA trials could be linked to the phase an improvement following exercise (1,5,25). In the PLA
physiological changes observed following the 120-min trial, if only pre- to postexercise changes had been considered,
cycle. Immediately postexercise, blood glucose concen- the decline in reported pleasure during the exercise bout would
trations were higher (P 0.05) following the CHO trial. have been masked. We also found that prolonged cycling
This is consistent with findings that low blood glucose elicited positive affective changes 15 min postexercise, but this
concentrations are associated with negative affective was restricted to the CHO trial only. Therefore, it appears that
states (15,17). Although participants in the PLA trial did nutritional intervention during prolonged exercise may be a
not become hypoglycemic, glucose concentrations were key determinant of the exercise experience and should be
markedly lower. However, the adequacy of this mecha- considered in future studies.
nism in explaining the link between CHO ingestion and In summary, our results suggest that CHO ingestion can
affect during exercise remains to be elucidated. In addi- attenuate the reductions in pleasure that occur when only
tion, there was a main effect for trial on plasma cortisol fluid and electrolytes are ingested. Therefore, athletes,
concentration, with overall values being higher following coaches, and sport and exercise scientists can extrapolate
the PLA trial than the CHO trial. This is consistent with from this study the observation that CHO ingestion during
the findings of Utter et al. (29) who reported lower exercise also influences the athletes psychological state, as
cortisol concentrations during the later stages of a 2.5-h assessed by the FS, adding to the literature on physiological,
bout of cycling at approximately 75% VO2max. According performance, and exertional effects. RPE was also reduced
to Morgan et al. (23), such findings may indicate that the in the CHO trial, but only at 75 min. In sum, these results
PLA trial was more stressful, both perceptually and phys- show that CHO ingestion exerts differential effects on affect
iologically. Cortisol is often secreted in response to emo- and perceived exertion and emphasize the importance of
tional stress and unpleasant sensations (26) and the re- assessing not only what but also how a person feels
duction in pleasure noted in the PLA trial would support during exercise.
this suggestion. Rudolph and McAuley (28) have empha-
The authors thank GlaxoSmithKline (Coleridge, UK) for supplying
sized that the role that cortisol might play in affective the test drinks. This work was undertaken while S. H. Backhouse
responses to acute exercise has received minimal atten- was at Loughborough University.

1772 Official Journal of the American College of Sports Medicine http://www.acsm-msse.org


REFERENCES
1. ACEVEDO, E.O., D. L. GILL A. H. GOLDFARB, and B. T. BOYER. 18. HALL, E. E., P. EKKEKAKIS, and S. J. PETRUZZELLO. The affective
Affect and perceivedexertion during a two-hour run. Int.J. Sport beneficence of vigorous exercise revisited. Br. J. Health Psychol.
Psychol. 27:286292, 1996. 7:4766, 2002.
2. BENTON, D. Carbohydrate ingestion, blood glucose and mood. 19. HARDY, C. J., and W. J. REJESKI. Not what, but how one feels: the
Neurosci. Behav. Res. 26:293308, 2002. measurement of affect during exercise. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol.
3. BENTON, D. and D. OWENS. Is raised blood glucose associated with 11:304317, 1989.
the relief of tension? J. Psychosom. Res. 37:723735, 1993. 20. KANG, J., R. J. ROBERTSON, F. L. GOSS, et al. Effect of carbohydrate
4. BISHOP, N. C., N. P. WALSH, and G. A. SCANLON. Effect of pro- substrate availability on ratings of perceived exertion during pro-
longed exercise and carbohydrate on total neutrophil elastase longed exercise of moderate intensity. Percept. Mot. Skills. 82:
content. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 35:13261332, 2003. 495506, 1996.
5. BIXBY, W. R., T. W. SPALDING, and B. D. HATFIELD. Temporal 21. KREIDER, R. B., D. HILL, G. HORTON, M. DOWNES, S. SMITH and B.
dynamics and dimensional specificity of the affective response to ANDERS. Effects of carbohydrate supplementation during intense
exercise of varying intensity: differing pathways to a common training on dietary patterns, psychological status and performance.
outcome. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 23:171190, 2001. Int. J. Sport Nutr. 5:125135, 1995.
6. BORG, G. An Introduction to Borgs RPE-scale. Ithaca, NY: Mou- 22. LEDOUX, J. E. Emotion in the amygdala. In: The Amygdala:
vement, 1985. Neurobiological Aspects of Emotion, Memory and Mental Dys-
7. BORG, G. A. Psychophysical bases of perceived exertion. Med. Sci. function. J. P. Aggelton (Ed.). New York: Wiley-Liss, 1992, pp.
Sports Exerc. 14:377381, 1982. 339 351.
8. BURGESS, M. L., R. J. ROBERTSON, J. M. DAVIS, and J. M. NORRIS. 23. MORGAN, C. A. I., S. WANG, A. RASMUSSON, G. HAZLETT, G. G.
RPE, blood glucose, and carbohydrate oxidation during exercise: ANDERSON, and D. S. CHANNEY. Relationship among plasma cor-
effects of glucose feedings. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 23:353359, tisol, catecholamines, neuropeptide Y & human performance dur-
1991. ing exposure to uncontrollable stress. Psychosom. Med. 63:412
9. CACIOPPO, J. T., G. G. BERNTSON, and W. L. GARDNER. The affect
422, 2001.
system has parallel and integrative processing components: form
24. MORGAN, W. P. Affective beneficence of vigorous physical activ-
follows function. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 76:839855, 1999.
ity. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 17:94100, 1985.
10. CAFERELLI, E. Peripheral contributions to the perception of effort.
25. PARFITT, G., D. MARKLAND, and C. HOLMES. Responses to physical
Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 14:382 389, 1982.
exertion in active and inactive males and females. J. Sport Exerc.
11. COGGAN, A. R., and E. F. COYLE. Carbohydrate ingestion during
prolonged exercise: effects on metabolism and performance. Ex- Psychol. 16:178186, 1994.
erc. Sport Sci. Res. 19:140, 1991. 26. POWERS, S. K., and E. T. HOWLEY. Hormonal responses to exercise.
12. COGGAN, A. R., and E. F. COYLE. Metabolism and performance In: Exercise Physiology Textbook. Dubuque, IA: Brown & Bench-
following carbohydrate ingestion late in exercise. Med. Sci. Sports mark, 1994, pp. 69 108.
Exerc. 21:5965, 1989. 27. ROGERS, P. J., S. EDWARDS, M. W. GREEN, and P. JAS. Nutritional
13. COGGAN, A. R., and E. F. COYLE. Reversal of fatigue during influences on mood and cognitive performance: the menstrual
prolonged exercise by carbohydrate infusion or ingestion. J. Appl. cycle, caffeine and dieting. Proc. Nutr. Soc. 51:343351, 1992.
Physiol. 63:23882395, 1987. 28. RUDOLPH, D. L., and E. MCAULEY. Cortisol and affective responses
14. EKKEKAKIS, P., E. E. HALL and S. J. PETRUZZELLO. Practical markers to exercise. J. Sport Sci. 16:121128, 1998.
of the transition from aerobic to anaerobic metabolism during 29. UTTER, A. C., J. KANG, D. C. NIEMAN, et al. Effect of carbohydrate
exercise: rationale and a case for affect-based exercise prescrip- ingestion and hormonal responses on ratings of perceived exertion
tion. Prev. Med. 38:149159, 2004. during prolonged cycling and running. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol.
15. GOLD, A. E., K. M. MACLEOD, B. M. FRIER, and I. J. DEARY. 61:9299, 1999.
Changes in mood during acute hypoglycemia in healthy partici- 30. WELSH, R. S., J. M. DAVIS, J. R. BURKE, and H. G. WILLIAMS.
pants. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 68:498504, 1995. Carbohydrates and physical/mental performance during intermit-
16. GOLD, P. W., D. L. LORIAUX, A. ROY, et al. Responses to cortico- tent exercise to fatigue. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 34:723731, 2002.
tropin-releasing hormone in the hypercortisolism of depression 31. WILBER, R. L., and R. J. MOFFAT. Influence of carbohydrate in-
and Cushings disease. Pathophysiologic and diagnostic implica- gestion on blood glucose and performance in runners. Int. J. Sport
tions. N. Engl. J Med. 314:13291335, 1986. Nutr. 2:317327, 1992.
17. GONDER-FREDERICK, C. F., D. J. COX, S. A. BOBBITT, and J. W. 32. WINNICK, J. J., J. M. DAVIS, R. S. WELSH, M. D. CARMICHAEL, E. A.
PENNEBAKER. Mood changes associated with blood glucose fluc- and J. A. BLACKMON. Carbohydrate feedings during team sport
tuations in insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus. Health Psychol. exercise preserve physical and CNS function. Med. Sci. Sports
8:4549, 1989. Exerc. 37:306315, 2005.

CHO INGESTION, AFFECTIVE STATES, AND CYCLING Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise 1773

Вам также может понравиться