Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 14

Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci (2015) 15:5568

DOI 10.3758/s13415-014-0315-6

The dynamic influence of emotional words on sentence processing


Jinfeng Ding & Lin Wang & Yufang Yang

Published online: 12 August 2014


# Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2014

Abstract In the present study, we aimed to examine how the to refer to the emotional manipulation of the words. Both
emotionality of words influences online sentence process- positive and negative words were taken as emotional words.
ingspecifically, the influence of emotional words on the The privileged status of emotional words has been extensively
processing of following words in sentences. We manipulated explored in behavioral studies (for a review, see Vuilleumier
the emotionality of verbs as well as the orthographic correct- & Huang, 2009). Recently, the use of event-related potentials
ness of their following (neutral) object nouns, so that the (ERPs) has led to considerable progress in our understanding
orthographic violation of the (neutral) nouns occurred in either of emotional words processing. ERP is a technique used for
emotional or neutral sentences. Event-related potentials studying the time course of brain activities in response to
(ERPs) were recorded to both the nouns and the verbs. We certain stimuli. It includes several components, and the differ-
found that the orthographic violation of the nouns elicited a P2 ent components reflect different cognitive processes.
and an N400 effect in the emotionally neutral sentences, but Most ERP studies on emotional language processing have
an LPC effect in the emotionally charged sentences. We also used isolated emotional words as stimuli. These studies have
found that the emotional verbs elicited a larger N1, a larger P2, compared the ERP responses to emotional and neutral words
and a larger N400 than did the neutral verbs. The ERP results in different tasks (for reviews, see Citron, 2012; Kissler,
suggest that emotional words capture more attention than Assadollahi, & Herbert, 2006). Some early ERP components,
neutral words, which further affects early orthographic analy- such as the P1, N1, P2, and early posterior negativity (EPN),
sis of the following words. Our findings demonstrate a dy- were found to be larger in response to emotional words.
namic influence of emotional words on sentence processing. Larger P1s/N1s for emotional than for neutral words have
been reported, which interacted with word frequency (Scott,
Keywords Emotionality . Orthographic processing . ODonnell, Leuthold, & Sereno, 2009). These effects might
be attributed to the top-down signals imposed by the amyg-
Language comprehension . ERP
dala on sensory cortex (Vuilleumier, 2005) and indicate that
the emotionality of words affects very early visual perception
Language can be used to express and elicit emotion during and attention allocation. The P2 and EPN effects have similar
communication. Generally, emotion is characterized along temporal latencies and are both taken as a reflection of
two dimensions: valence (pleasant vs. unpleasant) and arousal automatic attention orientation to emotional stimuli during
(arousing vs. calm; Russell, 1980). Very pleasant or unpleas- early stages of meaning encoding. In addition, Kanske and
ant stimuli are usually highly arousing. In the present study, Kotz (2007) reported an N400 effect, indicating facilitated
we used emotionality, represented as both valence and arousal, semantic processing of emotional relative to neutral words.
Moreover, a late positive complex (LPC) difference was found
in response to emotionality, reflected by the larger amplitudes
J. Ding : L. Wang (*) : Y. Yang (*)
for emotional words (Carreti et al., 2008; Fischler & Bradley,
Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology,
Chinese Academy of Sciences, Chaoyang District, Lincui Road 16, 2006; Kissler, Herbert, Winkler, & Junghofer, 2009; Schacht
Beijing 100101, China & Sommer, 2009b), and the direction of the effect has varied
e-mail: wangl@psych.ac.cn in different studies. Larger amplitudes for positive than for
e-mail: yangyf@psych.ac.cn
negative and neutral words (Herbert, Junghofer, & Kissler,
J. Ding 2008; Herbert, Kissler, Junghfer, Peyk, & Rockstroh, 2006;
University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China Kissler et al., 2009) or for negative than for neutral words
56 Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci (2015) 15:5568

(Hofmann, Kuchinke, Tamm, V, & Jacobs, 2009; Schacht & for emotional words might result from the nature of the
Sommer, 2009a) have been reported for this component. preceding contexts (e.g., the length of the preceding context:
Overall, the LPC effect reflects sustained elaborate processing Len et al. [using long story contexts] vs. Fields & Kuperberg
of the emotionally salient stimuli, and it is influenced by [using short sentences] vs. Martn-Loeches et al. [using a few
stimulus properties (e.g., lexical category, word frequency, words]) and tasks (e.g., Holt et al. [using explicit emotion
or word length) and tasks. Another ERP component, the classification] vs. De Pascalis et al. [using correctness judg-
P600, is similar to the LPC in terms of latency and topograph- ment] vs. Wang et al. [using reading for comprehension]).
ical distribution. They both start approximately 500 ms after Therefore, relative to neutral words, emotional words ob-
critical-word onset and have a central-posterior scalp distribu- tain enhanced processing both in isolation and in sentence
tion. The P600 effect has been found in response to syntactic, contexts, as indicated by increased perceptual processing,
semantic, or orthographic violations and has been associated deeper semantic analysis, and more elaborate emotion evalu-
with reanalysis processing. The emotional LPC effect has ation (as suggested by the above-mentioned ERP effects). The
been supposed to belong to the P300 family, which reflects a enhanced processing of emotional words also results in supe-
reevaluation of the stimulus (Citron, 2012; Holt, Lynn, & rior memory of emotional relative to neutral information
Kuperberg, 2009). However, it remains controversial whether (Conway et al., 1994; Doerksen & Shimamura, 2001;
the P600 belongs to the P300 family (Coulson, King, & Kensinger, Brierley, Medford, Growdon, & Corkin, 2002;
Kutas, 1998; Frisch, Kotz, von Cramon, & Friederici, 2003; Kensinger & Corkin, 2003; Maratos, Allan, & Rugg, 2000;
Osterhout & Hagoort, 1999; Schacht, Sommer, Shmuilovich, Phelps, LaBar, & Spencer, 1997). Moreover, the emotional
Martenz, & Martn-Loeches, 2014). It should be noted that effect on memory was not restricted to the emotional infor-
the EPN and LPC have been robustly responsive to emotional mation itself, but extended to its accompanying or following
features across these studies that have used isolated emotional information. There is debate as to whether emotionality en-
words as stimuli. hances or impairs accompanying neutral information process-
All of the above-mentioned studies have used isolated ing. The enhancement effect is explained on the basis of
words as stimuli. When the emotional words have been pre- priority-binding theory, in which emotion-linked stimuli are
sented in sentence or discourse contexts, different patterns of given processing priority (MacKay et al., 2004). According to
ERP effects have been reported. For instance, Holt et al. this theory, neutral information co-occurring with emotional
(2009) found larger N400s for emotional than for neutral information will be better processed and memorized.
words, indicating that the emotional words were difficult to Evidence has come from studies that showed superior mem-
integrate into neutral contexts. However, De Pascalis, Arwari, ory for the color (Doerksen & Shimamura, 2001; Kensinger &
DAntuono, and Cacace (2009), Martn-Loeches et al. (2012), Corkin, 2003) and spatial location (MacKay & Ahmetzanov,
and L. Wang, Bastiaansen, Yang, and Hagoort (2013) reported 2005) of emotional information relative to neutral informa-
smaller N400s for positive than for negative words, indicating tion, as well as for the neutral words preceding or following
facilitated semantic integration of pleasant as compared to emotional words relative to neutral words in the rest of the
unpleasant words, probably because healthy people are more contexts (Brierley, Medford, Shaw, & David, 2007; Guillet &
likely to expect pleasant information than to expect unpleasant Arndt, 2009; Kensinger et al., 2002; Medford et al., 2005). On
information (Kanske & Kotz, 2007). In addition to the N400 the contrary, the impairment effect is explained according to
effect, a larger late positivity was found for negative than for the attention-narrowing hypothesis (Christianson, 1992;
neutral words (Bayer, Sommer, & Schacht, 2010) or for Easterbrook, 1959), which implies superior memory for emo-
emotional words than for neutral words (Delaney-Busch & tional information at the expense of neutral information in the
Kuperberg, 2013; Holt et al., 2009,in their Exp. 1, emotion same contexts or scenes. For instance, MacKay et al. (2004)
evaluation). However, a negativity bias, reflected by a larger found reduced recall of neutral words when the neutral words
LPC for negative than for positive and neutral words, was also followed taboo words relative to when they followed neutral
revealed (Delaney-Busch & Kuperberg, 2013; Fields & words. Similarly, Hadley and MacKay (2006) found that
Kuperberg, 2012; Holt et al., 2009). All together, the late neutral words were recalled worse in a mixed list of neutral
positive effect might reflect a reallocation of attentional re- and taboo words than in a pure list of neutral words. Mather
sources to reevaluate the emotional words (Hajcak, Weinberg, and Sutherland (2011) outlined an arousal-biased competition
MacNamara, & Foti, 2012) for further integration, and it (ABC) theory to explain the contradictions in the emotional
might be more sensitive to emotional valence (Bayer et al., memory studies. The ABC theory proposes that emotional
2010). A few studies have also reported early ERP effects arousal makes high-priority information easier to be perceived
(such as N1/P2) that reflect a very rapid establishment of and more memorable, but reduces the processing of low-
emotional expectations (Len, Daz, de Vega, & Hernndez, priority information. The priority of information can be deter-
2010) or enhanced perceptual processing of emotional words mined by both bottom-up perceptual salience and top-down
(L. Wang et al., 2013). The heterogeneity of the ERP effects factors such as goal relevance and expectations. Therefore, the
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci (2015) 15:5568 57

effect of emotionality on the memory of accompanying neu- induced by single emotional words, because the processing of
tral information could be attributed to the priority of the emotional discourse occurs on a relatively large time scale.
neutral information, which might be determined by the rela- Therefore, it remains unclear whether emotion conveyed by
tionship between the emotional and neutral information. single emotional words would modulate the processing of
When neutral words were paired or presented with emotional following information in the same sentences. In addition, the
words in succession, their processing priority was low, due to orthographic analysis of words, which serves as the basis of
the lack of bottom-up or top-down enhancement. However, semantic and syntactic analysis in written language compre-
when the neutral and emotional words appeared in the same hension, relates to the perceptual analysis of words. It might
sentence, the neutral word obtained high processing priority, be modulated differently from other levels of language pro-
due to its close semantic relatedness with the emotional word. cessing (such as semantic and syntactic levels) by emotional
In the present study, we further explored the influence of context.
emotional words on the memory of their following neutral In the present study, we aimed to examine the dynamic
words when they were presented in the same argument influence of emotional words on sentence processing. That is,
structure. how does the emotionality of words influence the online
In addition to the memory effect, we were also interested in processing and offline retrieval of the following neutral words
the influence of emotional words on the online encoding of in the same sentences, and how are the emotional words
concurrent neutral words. It has been shown that the memory themselves processed online and retrieved offline? Previous
trace is generally related to the depth of information analysis studies (e.g., Delaney-Busch & Kuperberg, 2013; Kissler
(Craik & Lockhart, 1972; Craik & Tulving, 1975). In addition, et al., 2009; Martn-Loeches et al., 2012) have shown that
emotional words affect the memory of concurrent neutral emotional valence affects the processing of emotional words
words. A logical question, then, is how the emotionality of both in isolation and in a context. However, little is known
words affects the online processing of their subsequent neutral about whether it also plays a role in modulating the processing
words. That is to say, do the emotional words only obtain of the following words. The present study was designed as
privileged processing themselves, or will the emotional effect exploratory research to investigate the influence of emotional
spill over into the online processing of their following words on the processing of the neutral words that followed in
information? the same sentences. We manipulated the emotionality of the
Hinojosa, Mndez-Brtolo, and Pozo (2012) investigated verbs and the orthographic correctness of their directly follow-
the influence of emotional words on the processing of the ed nouns. We did not include Emotional Valence as a factor,
following neutral words in a priming study. They reported due to the difficulty of matching words on different word
larger amplitudes in the N1 and LPC components for the properties (such as arousal, frequency, concreteness, and
neutral words preceded by highly arousing positive words, imageability). In the attentional blink and priming paradigms,
as compared with those preceded by less arousing positive individual words are presented in sequence. In contrast, the
words. The findings suggest that the arousal of emotional verbs and nouns in the present study were in the same argu-
words influences the allocation of attentional resources at ment structure, which might increase the influence of the
early perceptual processing and the postlexical analysis of verbs emotionality on the following nouns. Therefore, the
the subsequent neutral words. In order to examine how emo- emotionality of the contexts was manipulated by using emo-
tional context modulates the semantic and syntactic process- tional (including positive and negative) or neutral verbs in the
ing of following words, Jimenez-Ortega et al. (2012) present- sentences. Meanwhile, the orthographic correctness of the
ed participants with emotional or neutral discourses, which (neutral) object nouns following the verbs was manipulated
were followed by sentences that contained semantic or syn- by changing strokes or radicals in a subtle way (e.g., to change
tactic violations. They found no modulation of emotional the Chinese characters to ). An ERP component,
context on the following semantic integration of the words, the P2component, has been related to early orthographic anal-
as indicated by similar N400 and P600 effects in response to ysis (Barnea & Breznitz, 1998; Luck & Hillyard, 1994;
semantic violations following the emotional and the neutral Sereno, Rayner, & Posner, 1998). Also, the violation of or-
contexts. However, they found a modulation of emotional thographic features was found to elicit P2 and N400 effects
context on the syntactic processing of the following words. (Liu, Jin, Qing, & Wang, 2011; Meng, Tian, Jian, & Zhou,
The syntactic violations elicited a left anterior negativity 2007) or a late positive effect (Laszlo & Federmeier, 2009;
(LAN) and a P600 effect in the emotional context, but only Vissers, Chwilla, & Kolk, 2006). We compared the ERPs
a P600 effect in the neutral context. The authors therefore elicited by orthographically violated and correct words in the
concluded that the emotional discourses induced different emotional and neutral verb contexts separately. When the
emotional states (i.e., arousal levels), which led to modula- currently processed information is salient, more attentional
tions of language processing. However, the emotional states resources will be devoted to such information, leading to a
induced by the emotional discourses might differ from those boosted ERP effect for the emotional verbs. Moreover,
58 Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci (2015) 15:5568

according to different theories, emotional words influence seven-point Likert scales (with 7 indicating the most positive,
different processing stages of neutral words. According to most arousing, most concrete, and most imageable).
the priority-binding theory, the neutral nouns following emo- Furthermore, we checked the frequency of the critical verbs
tional verbs should obtain processing priority so that the (Cai & Brysbaert, 2010). We selected the words that were also
orthographic and lexico-semantic features of the neutral words included in the CAWS and compared the in-house valence and
could be processed effectively, leading to a P2 effect and an arousal ratings with the ratings in the CAWS. The results
N400 effect in response to the orthographic violations. showed that the ratings in the pretests were highly correlated
According to the attention-narrowing hypothesis, on the other with those in the CAWS for both valence and arousal (r=.98,
hand, the following neutral nouns should receive limited p<.001, and r=.86, p<.001, respectively, for valence and
attentional resources, since more resources should be captured arousal), indicating that the test of 20 participants provided
by the preceding emotional verbs. This would result in de- us with reliable rating results. On the basis of the rating results,
layed or impaired orthographic analysis, leading to a late ERP we selected positive verbs whose valence ratings were higher
effect (e.g., a late positive effect) or even the absence of one. than 4, and negative verbs whose valence ratings were lower
We also compared the ERPs elicited by emotional and neutral than 3.8. Both the positive and the negative verbs were highly
verbs, from which we would expect both early (e.g., N1/P2) arousing [mean (SD)=4.23 (0.80)]. For the neutral verbs, we
and late (e.g., N400/LPC) ERP effects if the emotional words selected verbs whose valence ratings were around 4 and
automatically capture attention at early processing stages and whose arousal ratings were lower than 3.6 [mean (SD)=2.69
are integrated into contexts differently from neutral informa- (0.32)].
tion. In addition, we asked participants to recognize the emo- Then we constructed sentence pairs based on the selected
tional and neutral verbs, as well as the following neutral verbs and nouns. All of the sentences had the same structure,
nouns, in an incidental memory task presented after the ERP subject + verb + object + other words, with the previously
measures were collected. On the basis of previous findings selected verbs serving as verbs and the previously chosen
(e.g., Brierley et al., 2007; Guillet & Arndt, 2009; Hadley & nouns serving as objects. The critical object nouns never
MacKay, 2006; MacKay et al., 2004), we expected to find a appeared at the ends of the sentences. In each sentence pair,
higher accuracy for emotional verbs and different accuracies the two sentences only differed in the verbs, which were either
for nouns following neutral and emotional verbs. emotional or neutral. The objects of the sentences in each
sentence pair were the same. We took the verbs and the
following nouns (i.e., the objects) as our critical words.
Method Another 20 participants (mean age 22 years, range 1827;
eight males, 12 females) rated the sentences plausibility,
Participants emotional valence, arousal, and imageability on seven-point
Likert scales (with 7 indicating the most plausible, most
A group of 22 university students (mean age 22 years, range positive, most arousing, and most imageable). On the basis
1926; 12 males, ten females) participated in the experiment of the plausibility ratings, we chose 160 sentence pairs that
as paid volunteers. All were right-handed native Chinese were relatively plausible [means (SDs)=4.50 (0.61) and 4.53
speakers with normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity. (0.63), respectively, for the emotional and neutral sentences].
They all reported no dyslexia or neurological impairment, In all, 84 out of the 160 emotional sentences contained neg-
and they read and signed a written consent form. The data of ative verbs. Then we created an orthographically violated
two participants (both males) were excluded because of poor condition by changing strokes or radicals from the critical
signal-to-noise ratios. The analysis reported here was per- nouns in all of the sentence pairs. The alterations of strokes
formed on the data from 20 participants (mean age 22 years, or radicals rendered the character unpronounceable and mean-
range 1926; 10 males, 10 females). ingless. However, the orthographically violated words looked
very similar to the correct ones and conformed to the rules of
Materials Chinese orthography. Therefore, the alterations of strokes and
radicals should be perceived as repairable. A within-subjects
We selected 200 emotional verbs (including 100 positive and design was created with a combination of two variables
100 negative verbs), 200 neutral verbs, and 200 neutral nouns, emotionality of the verbs (emotional, neutral) and orthograph-
which were partly from the Chinese Affective Words System ic correctness of the nouns (correct, violated)which created
(CAWS; Y. Wang, Zhou, & Luo, 2008). Then we asked 20 four conditions: emotionalcorrect, emotionalviolated, neu-
participants (who did not participate in the electroencephalog- tralcorrect, and neutralviolated. Table 1 presents examples
raphy [EEG] experiment; mean age 24 years, range 1928; of the experimental stimuli.
nine males, 11 females) to rate the verbs and nouns for Table 2 presents the rating results for the selected verbs and
valence, arousal, concreteness, and imageability using sentences. We used paired-samples t tests to analyze the
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci (2015) 15:5568 59

Table 1 Examples of the experimental stimuli

The examples were originally in Chinese. The critical verbs are in boldface, and the critical object nouns are underlined

differences between the emotional and neutral verbs and for the emotional and neutral conditions; t(159)=0.87,
sentences separately for each variable. Since we were inter- p=.388].
ested in the emotionality effect (including valence and arous-
al), the positive and negative conditions were collapsed into Procedure
one emotional condition in the tests. The results showed that
the emotional and neutral verbs were significantly different in Participants were seated comfortably in front of a computer
valence and arousal, but not in frequency, concreteness, and screen. All of the words were shown in white on a black
imageability. In addition, the emotional and neutral sentences background in Song typeface. The words subtended visual
were significantly different in valence and arousal, but not in angles of 4.58 and 6.87, respectively, for the compound
plausibility and imageability. two-character and compound three-character words. A trial
In the end, we also measured the cloze probabilities of the started with a 300-ms fixation cross in the center of the screen.
neutral nouns in the emotional and neutral sentence contexts. Each word of the sentence was presented for 300 ms and was
In the cloze probability test, another 20 participants (mean age followed by a 200-ms blank screen. Then, 1,000 ms after the
21 years, range 1825; ten males, ten females) were asked to presentation of the last word, the next trial began. Participants
complete the sentences, which were presented up to where the were told that there would be a comprehension test after the
critical neutral nouns would appear. The results showed that experiment, so they needed to read and comprehend the
the critical neutral nouns showed equally low cloze probabil- sentences carefully.
ities between the emotional and neutral conditions [means After the presentation of all sentences, an incidental recog-
(SDs)=1.06 % (3.09 %) and 1.44 % (4.86 %), respectively, nition test was presented. In this test, we presented 80 emo-
tional verbs, 80 neutral verbs, as well as 80 neutral ortho-
graphically correct nouns that were shown in the sentences. In
Table 2 Statistical analysis results for critical verbs and sentences order to balance the ratio of old and new words, we also added
240 new words in the test. These were 80 emotional verbs, 80
Item Emotional Neutral t
Mean (SD) Mean (SD) neutral verbs, and 80 neutral nouns. The distractor verbs were
significantly different in valence [means (SDs)=4.85 (0.48),
Positive verb valence 4.92 (0.67) 4.06 (0.24) 10.97*** 4.13 (0.20), and 3.11 (0.56), respectively, for the positive,
Negative verb valence 2.42 (0.67) 4.00 (0.25) 21.08*** neutral, and negative distractor new verbs; F(2, 157) =
Verb arousal 4.23 (0.80) 2.69 (0.32) 24.54*** 196.69, p<.001; post hoc analysis ps<.001] and arousal
Verb frequency 2.48 (0.55) 2.54 (0.72) 0.77 [means (SDs)=4.06 (0.51) and 3.19 (0.27), respectively, for
Verb concreteness 3.28 (0.55) 3.35 (0.44) 1.47 the emotional and neutral distractor new verbs; F(1, 158)=
Verb imageability 3.95 (0.95) 3.85 (0.80) 1.20 183.60, p<.001]. In contrast, the new nouns were neutral with
Sentence plausibility 4.50 (0.61) 4.53 (0.63) 0.50 low arousal [means (SDs)=4.24 (0.29) and 3.01 (0.30), re-
Positive sentence valence 4.43 (0.47) 4.29 (0.46) 2.43* spectively, for the valence and arousal of the distractor new
Negative sentence valence 3.28 (0.62) 4.09 (0.38) 12.56*** nouns]. In the recognition task, a trial began with a 300-ms
Sentence arousal 4.07 (0.54) 3.67 (0.46) 8.45*** fixation cross followed by a word presented for 1,000 ms.
Sentence imageability 4.66 (0.68) 4.61 (0.65) 0.99 After the critical word disappeared, participants were asked to
judge whether or not they had read the word in the sentences
The positive and negative verbs were taken as the emotional verbs and before by pressing the F and J keys, to indicate old and
were each paired with different neutral verbs and nouns. The last five
rows reflect ratings obtained for only correct stimuli. SD, standard devi-
new, respectively.
ation. * Significant at .05 level, ** Significant at .01 level, *** Significant at We used a Latin square design to divide the 160 items (four
.001 level conditions per item) into four versions, with no repetition of
60 Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci (2015) 15:5568

items within one version. Each version contained 160 ERP data analysis
sentences (40 sentences per condition). In addition, 80 filler
sentences were constructed to balance the positions of the On the basis of the previous literatures (e.g., Len et al., 2010;
orthographically violated neutral nouns. Among the 80 fillers, Scott et al., 2009; L. Wang et al., 2013) and visual inspection
40 fillers contained orthographically violated words that were of the waveforms, we chose the following time windows to
located at either the beginning or the end part of the sentences. statistically analyze the effects of the critical verbs and nouns:
Also, half of the 80 filler sentences contained emotional verbs. N1 (90170 ms), P2 (180280 ms), N400 (300500 ms), and
These filler sentences were combined with each version of the LPC (5001,000 ms). For the critical nouns, because the N400
experimental lists. Then, the 240 sentences (160 experimental time window of the critical verbs served as the baseline, the
sentences + 80 fillers) in each list were evenly divided into baselines of the critical nouns might be significantly different
four blocks, with each block containing 60 sentences. between the emotional and neutral conditions. In addition, the
Participants read the 240 sentences in a pseudorandom correct and violating words might elicit different ERPs in the
order. No more than three sentences of the same condition early time windows (such as the P2 effect to the orthographic
were presented in succession. In the comprehension task, each violations in the neutral condition). Therefore, it was difficult
block lasted about 4 min. In the word recognition task, the 480 to dissociate the emotional effect of the verbs and that on the
recognition words (consisting of 80 critical emotional verbs, following nouns using a poststimulus baseline. Moreover, we
80 critical neutral verbs, 80 critical neutral nouns, and 240 had a strong a-priori hypothesis that neutral words would be
new words) were evenly divided into eight blocks, with each processed differently following emotional and neutral words.
block lasting about 3 min. Between two blocks, there was a 2- Therefore, repeated measures analyses of variance (ANOVAs)
to 3-min break. The whole experiment took about one and a were conducted with Orthographic Correctness (correct, vio-
half hours, including the participants preparation, instruc- lated), Laterality (left, midline, right), and Anteriority (anteri-
tions, and a short practice of four trials. or, central, posterior) as within-subjects factors in the emo-
tional and neutral conditions separately. For the critical verbs,
Emotionality (emotional, neutral), Laterality (left, midline,
EEG recording and preprocessing right), and Anteriority (anterior, central, posterior) served as
within-subjects factors. Fig. 1 shows the selected electrodes
EEG was recorded using 64 AgAgCl electrodes mounted on and the division. Any (marginally) significant interactions
an elastic cap with a sampling rate of 500 Hz. Impedances with emotionality or orthographic correctness were followed
were kept below 5 k. To monitor eye blinks and movements, by simple-effects tests. When Mauchlys test of sphericity was
horizontal and vertical electrooculograms were recorded from significant, the GreenhouseGeisser correction was applied,
the left and right orbital rims and supra- and suborbital to the and in these cases the original degrees of freedom are reported
left eye. During recording, the EEG data were referenced to with corrected p values. In addition, the F values of some
the right mastoid, and an electrode localized between the FPz nonsignificant results are shown when the comparison was
and Fz electrodes served as the ground. Since we were inter- critical to our manipulation.
ested in the online processing of the neutral words following
the emotional or neutral verbs, and since the EEG signals
might get too noisy during the memory task due to the fatigue
of the participants after the preparation, practice, and compre-
hension task (around 1 h), EEG was only recorded during the Results
sentence comprehension task.
The EEG data were analyzed with Neuroscan 4.3 offline. Behavioral results
After correcting for eye blinks (Semlitsch, Anderer, Schuster,
& Presslich, 1986), a band-pass filter of 0.130 Hz (24 dB/oct We found that the recognition accuracies of the verbs and the
slope, half-power cutoff) was applied to the data. The EEG nouns were significantly higher than chance level [as indicat-
data were segmented from 200 ms before to 1,000 ms after the ed by one-sample t tests against .50: mean (SD)=.63 (.14),
onset of the critical words (including both verbs and nouns). t(19)=4.26, p<.001, for the emotional verbs; mean (SD)=.66
Then, the data were baseline-corrected to the mean amplitude (.17), t(19) = 4.30, p < .001, for the neutral verbs; mean
from 200 ms before to the critical words onset. After an (SD)=.63 (.15), t(19)=3.97, p=.001, for the object nouns
automatic artifact rejection (80V) procedure, approximate- following the emotional verbs; and mean (SD)=.59 (.14),
ly 39 trials per condition remained. The ERPs were then re- t(19)=2.83, p=.011, for the object nouns following the neutral
referenced offline to the algebraic average of two mastoids. verbs], but no significant difference was apparent be-
Finally, the averaged ERPs were computed in each condition tween the emotional and neutral verbs [t(19) = 1.69,
at each electrode for each participant. p = .107]. Nor was the difference between the object
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci (2015) 15:5568 61

over the anterior and central regions [F(1, 19)=0.11, p=.743,


p2 =.006; F(1, 19)=1.53, p=.231, p2 =.075]. No significant
orthographic correctness effect was found in the emotional
condition.
The statistical analysis of the N400 component in the
neutral condition revealed a significant main effect of ortho-
graphic correctness [F(1, 19)=5.38, p=.032, p2 =.221],
showing a larger N400 for the orthographically violated words
than for the correct words. We also observed a significant
interaction between orthographic correctness and laterality
[F(2, 38)=3.54, p=.039, p2 =.157]. Simple-effects tests re-
vealed that the orthographically violated words elicited a
larger N400 than did the correct words over the left hemi-
sphere [F(1, 19)=5.31, p=.033, p2 =.219] and midline [F(1,
19)=6.50, p=.020, p2 =.255], but not over the right hemi-
sphere [F(1, 19)=2.86, p=.107, p2 =.131]. However, no sig-
nificant effect in response to orthographic correctness was
found in the emotional condition.
In the 500- to 1,000-ms time window, we found that the
orthographically violated words yielded a larger positivity
than did the correct words [main effect of orthographic cor-
Fig. 1 Electrode layout on the scalp. Nine representative electrodes, rectness, F(1, 19)=9.79, p=.006, p2 =.340] in the emotional
circled with thicker lines, were selected for displaying grand average condition, but not in the neutral condition [main effect of
waveforms. The electrodes chosen for the statistical analysis are grouped orthographic correctness, F(1, 19) = 0.004, p = .952, p2
into nine regions: left anterior, left central, left posterior, middle anterior,
middle central, middle posterior, right anterior, right central, and right <.001].
posterior Overall, in the neutral sentence contexts, the orthographi-
cally violated words elicited a smaller P2 over the posterior
nouns following the emotional and neutral verbs signif- region and a larger N400 over the left and middle regions (as is
icant [t(19)=1.52, p=.145]. shown in the scalp topographies in Fig. 2b) than did the
correct words. However, in the emotional sentence contexts,
the orthographically violated words elicited a larger LPC than
ERP results did the correct words over broad regions (as is shown in the
scalp topographies in Fig. 3b).
Figures 2 and 3 show the grand average waveforms elicited by
the critical nouns at nine representative electrodes (F3/C3/P3, Emotionality effects of the verbs In the time window of 90
Fz/Cz/Pz, and F4/C4/P4, circled in Fig. 1) in the neutral and 170 ms, we found a significant three-way interaction among
emotional conditions, respectively. The waveforms elicited by emotionality, laterality, and anteriority [F(4, 76) = 3.43,
the emotional and neutral verbs are presented in Fig. 4. Since p = .025, p2 = .153]. We then performed three two-way
the focus of the present study was the influence of emotional ANOVAs (EmotionalityLaterality) for the anterior, central,
words on the processing of following information, we first and posterior regions separately. Only one significant interac-
present the emotionality influence on the processing of the tion between the two factors was found over the anterior
nouns, then that on the verbs. region [F(2, 38)=4.99, p=.012, p2 =.208; F(2, 38)=2.00,
p=.149, p2 =.095; and F(2, 38)=0.53, p=.593, p2 =.027,
Orthographic effects of the nouns in the neutral and emotional respectively, for the anterior, central, and posterior regions].
contexts In the time window of 90170 ms, no significant Simple-effects tests showed that the emotional verbs elicited a
effect of orthographic correctness emerged in either the emo- larger negativity than did the neutral verbs over the left
tional or the neutral condition. anterior region [F(1, 19)=4.63, p=.044, p2 =.196], but
In the time window of 180280 ms, we found a significant not over the middle and right anterior regions [F(1,
interaction between orthographic correctness and anteriority 19) = 0.66, p = .426, p 2 = .034, and F(1, 19) = 0.01,
in the neutral condition [F(2, 38)=8.29, p=.003, p2 =.304]. p=.921, p2 <.001, respectively, for the middle and right
Simple-effects tests showed that the orthographically violated anterior regions].
words elicited a smaller P2 than did the correct words over the In the time window of 180280 ms, we found a significant
posterior region [F(1, 19)=9.18, p=.007, p2 =.326], but not interaction between emotionality and anteriority [F(2, 38)=
62 Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci (2015) 15:5568

Fig. 2 Orthographic correctness effects of the critical nouns in the neutral represent orthographically violated words. (b) Topographies showing the
condition. (a) Grand average event-related potential (ERP) waveforms average amplitude voltage differences between the orthographically vio-
evoked by the critical nouns in the neutral condition at nine representative lated and correct words
electrodes. The solid lines represent correct words, and the dotted lines

12.46, p=.050, p2 =.187]. Simple-effects tests revealed that the emotional verbs yielded a larger N400 than did the neutral
the emotional verbs yielded a larger P2 than did the neutral verbs over bilateral central and posterior regions.
verbs over the anterior region [F(1, 19) =4.37, p =.050, In the time window of 5001,000 ms, we did not find any
p2 =.187], but not over the central and posterior regions significant effect of emotionality or any other interaction.
[F(1, 19)= 1.30, p = .269, p2 = .064, and F(1, 19)= 1.01, Overall, as compared with the neutral verbs, the emotional
p=.327, p2 =.050, respectively]. verbs yielded a larger N1 over the left anterior region, a larger
In the time window of 300500 ms, we found a significant P2 over the anterior region, and a larger N400 over the left
main effect of emotionality [F(1, 19) = 11.63, p = .003, anterior, bilateral central, and posterior regions (as is shown in
p2 =.380], showing a larger N400 for the emotional than for the scalp topographies in Fig. 4b).
the neutral verbs. We also found a significant three-way
interaction between emotionality, laterality, and anteriority
[F(4, 76)=4.20, p=.004, p2 =.181]. The following two-way
ANOVAs revealed a significant interaction between emotion- Discussion
ality and laterality in the anterior region [F(2, 38)=5.79,
p=.013, p2 =.234; F(2, 38)=2.32, p=.112, p2 =.109; and In this study, we aimed to examine the dynamic influence of
F(2, 38)=1.60, p=.215, p2 =.078, respectively, for the ante- the emotionality of verbs on the orthographic analysis of their
rior, central, and posterior regions]. Simple-effects tests indi- following nouns during sentence comprehension. In
cated that the emotional verbs yielded a larger N400 than did sentences, object nouns followed emotional or neutral verbs
the neutral verbs over the left anterior region [F(1, 19)=9.02, and were orthographically correct or violated. We found that
p=.007, p2 =.322], but not over the middle and right anterior the orthographically violated nouns elicited a smaller P2 and a
regions [F(1, 19)=1.55, p=.228, p2 =.076, and F(1, 19)< larger N400 than did the correct nouns when they followed the
0.001, p=.960, p2 <.001]. The main effect of emotionality neutral verbs, whereas a late positive effect (5001,000 ms)
and the lack of an interaction between emotionality and was observed when they followed the emotional verbs. In
laterality over the central and posterior regions suggested that addition, the emotional verbs elicited larger N1s, P2s, and
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci (2015) 15:5568 63

Fig. 3 Orthographic correctness effect of the critical nouns in the emo- dotted lines represent orthographically violated words. (b) Topographies
tional condition. (a) Grand average event-related potential (ERP) wave- showing the average amplitude voltage differences between the ortho-
forms evoked by the critical nouns in the emotional condition at nine graphically violated and correct words
representative electrodes. The solid lines represent correct words, and the

N400s than did the neutral verbs. However, we found no a larger N400 in response to pseudohomophones (e.g., bouks
difference between the emotional and neutral verbs or nouns vs. books). The observed P2 and N400 effects in the present
in a subsequent incidental memory test. study suggest that the orthographic violation caused process-
ing difficulty at an early stage, which in turn had a conse-
Emotionality modulated the orthographic processing quence on the lexico-semantic analysis of the words.
of following words On the contrary, the orthographic violation elicited a late
positive effect in the emotional sentence contexts. The ab-
In the neutral sentence contexts, the orthographically violated sence of early ERP effects in the emotional condition indicates
words yielded a smaller P2 and a larger N400 than did the impaired orthographic analysis of the neutral nouns following
correct words. The P2 is generally related to orthographic and emotional verbs, due to limited processing resources. In the
phonological information processing (Barnea & Breznitz, context of the present study, the LPC effect might reflect
1998; Luck & Hillyard, 1994; Sereno et al., 1998), whereas reanalysis of the orthographic feature. The LPC effect might
the N400 is a well-established component that associates with not be orthographic-specific, given that previous studies have
lexico-semantic processing (Kutas & Federmeier, 2011). Our also found such an LPC effect in response to syntactic viola-
results are consistent with those of a previous study (Meng tions (Hagoort, Brown, & Groothusen, 1993; Osterhout &
et al., 2007), in which the researchers replaced the second Holcomb, 1995) and semantic violations (Kuperberg, 2007;
characters in Chinese compounds with homophones [i.e., Nieuwland & Van Berkum, 2005). Therefore, the late positive
(pronounced feng1jing3, meaning scene) was replaced with effect was taken as a reflection of general reanalysis of the
(pronounced feng1jing3, no meaning)] and were able inputs (Vissers et al., 2006). This reprocess could occur at the
to report both a P2 and an N400 effect. The P2 effect indicates phonological, lexical, conceptual, or syntactic level.
that the orthographic features were processed early in sentence Nevertheless, the orthographic violation elicited both a P2
comprehension, whereas the N400 effect indicates that the and an N400 effect in the neutral condition, and these were
orthographic violation affected the lexico-semantic processing taken as reflecting enhanced orthographic analysis and the
of the violated words. Similarly, Vissers et al. (2006) and Van increased difficulty of integrating such violating words into
de Meerendonk, Indefrey, Chwilla, and Kolk (2011) reported sentences, respectively. However, it is important to note that
64 Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci (2015) 15:5568

Fig. 4 Emotionality effects of the critical verbs (regardless of the cor- conditions. The solid lines represent emotional verbs, and the dotted lines
rectness of the following nouns). (a) Grand average event-related poten- represent neutral verbs. (b) Topographies showing the average amplitude
tial (ERP) waveforms evoked by the critical verbs at nine representative voltage differences between the emotional and neutral verbs
electrodes. The waveforms were averaged across the correct and violated

this interpretation does not necessarily imply that reanalysis 2007; Barnard et al., 2005). Using the AB paradigm,
does not occur if the violation has caused processing difficulty Batterink, Karns, Yamada, and Neville (2010) found that
at earlier stages of orthographic processing. In agreement with semantically unrelated and syntactically incorrect targets with-
Van de Meerendonk et al. (2011), the observed effect in in the AB period elicited a reduced and no effect, respectively.
response to orthographic violations could be related to the A resource depletion account states that resources should in
resources available for processing the representational con- large part be allocated to the first target at the expense of the
flicts between an expected orthographically correct word and second (Di Lollo, Kawahara, Ghorashi, & Enns, 2005;
the input of an orthographically violated word. Shapiro, Raymond, & Arnell, 1994; Ward, Duncan, &
The distinct ERP effects evoked by the orthographic vio- Shapiro, 1996). In the present study, the emotional words
lations in the emotional and neutral contexts indicate a dy- captured and held more attentional resources than did the
namic influence of the emotionality of words on the process- neutral words. When the orthographic violations occurred
ing of following words at the perceptual and lexico-semantic following the emotional words, the attentional resources oc-
levels. The results are in line with data obtained from a well- cupied by the previous emotional words cannot be released
known phenomenon called the attentional blink (AB). The immediately, so orthographic analysis was diminished (as
AB is a phenomenon in which the report accuracy of the indicated by the absence of the early ERP effects) and led to
second target is low when it is presented less than 500 ms a reanalysis of the orthographic features in the later time
after the first target in rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) window (as indicated by the LPC effect).
streams. In particular, the deficit of report accuracy to the In the present study, the sentences were presented in a
second target was bigger when the first target was a high- word-by-word manner with an interstimulus interval of
arousing emotional word relative to neutral word (Arnell, 200 ms. The short interstimulus interval may have resulted
Killman, & Fijavz, 2004; Barnard, Ramponi, Battye, & in our lack of ERP effects of orthographic correctness.
Mackintosh, 2005; Mathewson, Arnell, & Mansfield, 2008), However, the absence of an early effect to the orthographic
even when these high-arousing emotional words served as violation in the emotional sentences could not be solely at-
distracters prior to the target (Arnell, Killman, & Fijavz, tributed to the stimulus presenting mode, because the early
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci (2015) 15:5568 65

effect was observed in the neutral sentences with a similar effect might be attributed to the top-down control of amygdala
presenting mode. Therefore, the LPC effect in response to the on cortical pathways (Vuilleumier, 2005). The anterior P2
orthographic violations in the emotional condition reflects effect has temporal latencies similar to those of another well-
repaired processing of the orthography due to the salience of established emotional ERP effectthe EPN effect in single
the preceding emotional words, which narrowed the attention emotional-word processing. Although they are both taken as
allocated to the subsequent neutral words. reflections of automatic attention orientation to emotional
Nevertheless, different results have been reported in previ- stimuli during early stages of meaning encoding or of the
ous studies regarding the influence of preceding information conscious processing of affective content (for a review, see
on the processing of the following words. Moreno and Citron, 2012), the EPN effect usually has a bilateral temporal
Vzquez (2011) investigated the effect of negatively and occipital scalp distribution. In addition, studies exploring
positively biased sentence frames on the semantic analysis emotionality effects in sentences have rarely reported an
of following neutral information. They reported similar N400 EPN effect in response to emotional words, which might be
effects in response to nonsense words in both negative and related to the use of re-reference channels (average reference
positive conditions. The lack of emotional modulation might vs. linked mastoid reference; Junghfer, Peyk, Flaisch, &
have been due to the high expectation of the contexts. In the Schupp, 2006) or the presentation mode of words (e.g., short
study of Fischler and Bradley (2006), participants were asked vs. long interstimulus intervals; Herbert et al., 2008; Herbert
to comprehend adjectivenoun word pairs as a phrase and to et al., 2006).
make a coherence judgment. Fischler and Bradley failed to In addition to the early N1 and P2 effects, the emotional
find any emotional effects for either the first adjectives (pleas- words also elicited a larger N400 than the neutral words did.
ant, neutral, and unpleasant) or the second neutral nouns. The present result is in line with the study of Holt et al. (2009),
However, Hinojosa et al. (2012) reported larger N1 and LPC who explained that the emotional words in the neutral contexts
amplitudes to neutral words preceded by positive words that differed in their neural representations, relative to the neutral
were high as compared to low in arousal. The enhancement words in the neutral contexts. As a result, the integration of the
effects might be attributed to the more visual attention devoted emotional words into neutral contexts was more difficult than
to the highly arousing stimuli. In the present study, we found integration of the neutral words, resulting in a larger N400 for
that the orthographically violated nouns elicited smaller P2 the emotional words. In addition, Bayer et al. (2010) reported
and larger N400 amplitudes than the correct nouns when they a larger N400 for high-arousal than for low-arousal words of
followed the neutral verbs, but larger LPC amplitudes when the same valence. However, Kanske and Kotz (2007) and L.
they followed the emotional verbs. The discrepancies could be Wang et al. (2013) reported smaller N400s for positive than
due to a number of important differences between the present for negative words. Therefore, the observed N400 effects
study and previous studies, such as the stimuli (the present seem to relate to both emotional valence and arousal.
study using emotional and neutral conditions vs. Hinojosa et al. In the present study, we did not observe any late positive
using positive and negative conditions; the present study using effect in response to emotional words. The LPC effect has
sentences vs. Fishler & Bradley using word pairs), tasks (the been related to emotional evaluation (Citron, 2012) and is task
present study using comprehension vs. Fishler & Bradley using relevant (Fischler & Bradley, 2006). When the task did not
coherence decision), as well as paradigms (the present study involve emotional evaluation (e.g., a structural decision task),
using sentence reading vs. Hinojosa et al. using priming). the late positive effect was found to be attenuated or eliminat-
ed (Schacht & Sommer, 2009b). In our study, participants
Emotional words processing in sentence contexts were told to comprehend the sentences, so no explicit emo-
tional evaluation of the verbs was necessary. Moreover, the
Emotional verbs elicited a larger N1 over the left anterior occasionally occurring orthographic violations might have
region and a larger P2 over the anterior region than did neutral even distracted their attention away from emotional evalua-
verbs. These effects are consistent with previous studies on tion. All of these might be reasons why we did not observe a
isolated emotional-words processing (Hofmann et al., 2009; late positive effect. It should be noted that the emotionality of
Schapkin, Gusev, & Kuhl, 2000). When emotional words the words manipulated in the present study represented both
were presented in contexts, two studies also reported N1/P2 emotional valence and arousal, so further studies will be
effects in response to emotional salience (Len et al., 2010; L. required to specify the roles of emotional valence and arousal
Wang et al., 2013). The N1 effect suggests that emotion affects in modulating word processing in contexts.
early visual perception of the stimuli. In order to account for
such an early emotional effect, a pathway between amygdala No emotional memory effect
and visual cortex has been proposed (Morris et al., 1998;
Pessoa, McKenna, Gutierrez, & Ungerleider, 2002; The recognition accuracy of the emotional words was not
Sabatinelli, Bradley, Fitzsimmons, & Lang, 2005). The N1 significantly higher than that of the neutral words in our study.
66 Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci (2015) 15:5568

Previous studies have found that people recognize or recall Arnell, K. M., Killman, K. V., & Fijavz, D. (2007). Blinded by emotion:
Target misses follow attention capture by arousing distractors in
emotional words at a higher accuracy rate than neutral words
RSVP. Emotion, 7, 465477. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.7.3.465
in both word lists (Kensinger & Corkin, 2003; Phelps et al., Barnard, P., Ramponi, C., Battye, G., & Mackintosh, B. (2005). Anxiety
1997) and sentences (Brierley et al., 2007; Guillet & Arndt, and the deployment of visual attention over time. Visual Cognition,
2009). One might argue that the presentation of words in 12, 181211. doi:10.1080/13506280444000139
Barnea, A., & Breznitz, Z. (1998). Phonological and orthographic pro-
sentences would increase memory for the words. In retrospect,
cessing of Hebrew words: Electrophysiological aspects. Journal of
most of previous studies using isolated words as stimuli G e n e t i c P s y c h o l o g y, 1 5 9 , 4 9 2 5 0 4 . d o i : 1 0 . 1 0 8 0 /
(Doerksen & Shimamura, 2001; Kensinger et al., 2002; 00221329809596166
Phelps et al., 1997) have employed a free recall task. The Batterink, L., Karns, C. M., Yamada, Y., & Neville, H. (2010). The role of
awareness in semantic and syntactic processing: An ERP attentional
recall accuracy was lower than the recognition accuracy found
blink study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 25142529. doi:
in the present study. However, the recognition accuracy of 10.1162/jocn.2009.21361
isolated words in another study (Maratos et al., 2000) was Bayer, M., Sommer, W., & Schacht, A. (2010). Reading emotional words
found to be higher (86 %, on average) than that of the present within sentences: The impact of arousal and valence on event-
related potentials. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 78,
study (60 %, on average). Moreover, some studies that have
299307. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.09.004
presented words in sentences (Brierley et al., 2007; Medford Brierley, B., Medford, N., Shaw, P., & David, A. S. (2007). Emotional
et al., 2005) also obtained higher recognition accuracy than memory for words: Separating content and context. Cognition and
the present study. Therefore, the presentation of words in Emotion, 21, 495521. doi:10.1080/02699930600684963
Cai, Q., & Brysbaert, M. (2010). SUBTLEX-CH: Chinese word and
sentences did not increase the recognition accuracy, and the
character frequencies based on film subtitles. PLoS ONE, 5,
low recognition accuracy in the present study could be due to e10729. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0010729
the incidental recognition task and the number of items. In our Carreti, L., Hinojosa, J. A., Albert, J., Lpez-Martn, S., De La Gndara, B.
study, the participants were instructed to carefully compre- S., Igoa, J. M., & Sotillo, M. (2008). Modulation of ongoing cognitive
hend sentences, so no particular efforts were made to memo- processes by emotionally intense words. Psychophysiology, 45, 188
196. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00617.x
rize the words in the sentences. Also, the participants had 240 Christianson, S.-. (1992). Emotional stress and eyewitness memory: A
old and 240 new words to judge in the incidental recognition critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 284309. doi:10.1037/
task, which might have caused a floor effect (as indicated by 0033-2909.112.2.284
the low accuracies in all conditions). Citron, F. M. (2012). Neural correlates of written emotion word process-
ing: A review of recent electrophysiological and hemodynamic
neuroimaging studies. Brain and Language, 122, 211226. doi:10.
Conclusions 1016/j.bandl.2011.12.007
Conway, M. A., Anderson, S. J., Larsen, S. F., Donnelly, C. M.,
McDaniel, M. A., McClelland, A. G., & Logie, R. H. (1994). The
The orthographic violations of object nouns elicited P2 and formation of flashbulb memories. Memory & Cognition, 22, 326
N400 effects when they followed neutral verbs, whereas a late 343. doi:10.3758/BF03200860
positive effect was found when the violations followed emo- Coulson, S., King, J. W., & Kutas, M. (1998). Expect the unexpected:
Event-related brain response to morphosyntactic violations.
tional verbs. Therefore, the emotionality of words modulates
Language and Cognitive Processes, 13, 2158. doi:10.1080/
at least the perceptual analysis of the following words. The 016909698386582
findings support the attention-narrowing hypothesis in the Craik, F. I. M., & Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: A frame-
context of language comprehension. In addition, the emotion- work for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal
Behavior, 11, 671684. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(72)80001-X
al verbs elicited both early (N1 and P2) and late (N400) effects
Craik, F. I. M., & Tulving, E. (1975). Depth of processing and the retention
relative to the neutral verbs during sentence comprehension, of words in episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
indicating influences of emotionality on both early perceptual General, 104, 268294. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.104.3.268
analysis and the subsequent semantic integration of the words. Delaney-Busch, N., & Kuperberg, G. (2013). Friendly drug-dealers and
Taken together, emotional words have a dynamic influence on terrifying puppies: Affective primacy can attenuate the N400 effect
in emotional discourse contexts. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral
sentence processing. Neuroscience, 13, 473490. doi:10.3758/s13415-013-0159-5
De Pascalis, V., Arwari, B., DAntuono, L., & Cacace, I. (2009).
Author note This research was supported by awards from the National Impulsivity and semantic/emotional processing: An examination
Natural Science Foundation of China to Y.Y. [Grant No. 31070989] and of the N400 wave. Clinical Neurophysiology, 120, 8592. doi:10.
to L.W. [Grant No. 31200849]. 1016/j.clinph.2008.10.008
Di Lollo, V., Kawahara, J.-I., Ghorashi, S. S., & Enns, J. T. (2005). The
attentional blink: Resource depletion or temporary loss of control?
Psychological Research, 69, 191200. doi:10.1007/s00426-004-
References 0173-x
Doerksen, S., & Shimamura, A. P. (2001). Source memory enhancement
for emotional words. Emotion, 1, 511. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.1.1.5
Arnell, K. M., Killman, K., & Fijavz, D. (2004). Blinded by emotions: Easterbrook, J. A. (1959). The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the
Target misses follow attentional capture by arousing distractors in organization of behavior. Psychological Review, 66, 183201. doi:
RSVP. Journal of Vision, 4(8), 359. doi:10.1167/4.8.359 10.1037/h0047707
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci (2015) 15:5568 67

Fields, E. C., & Kuperberg, G. R. (2012). Its all about you: An ERP study Kissler, J., Assadollahi, R., & Herbert, C. (2006). Emotional and semantic
of emotion and self-relevance in discourse. NeuroImage, 62, 562 networks in visual word processing: insights from ERP studies.
574. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.05.003 Progress in Brain Research, 156, 147183. doi:10.1016/S0079-
Fischler, I., & Bradley, M. (2006). Event-related potential studies of 6123(06)56008-X
language and emotion: words, phrases, and task effects. Progress Kissler, J., Herbert, C., Winkler, I., & Junghofer, M. (2009). Emotion and
in Brain Research, 156, 185204. doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(06) attention in visual word processing: An ERP study. Biological
56009-1 Psychology, 80, 7583. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.03.004
Frisch, S., Kotz, S. A., von Cramon, D. Y., & Friederici, A. D. (2003). Kuperberg, G. R. (2007). Neural mechanisms of language comprehen-
Why the P600 is not just a P300: The role of the basal ganglia. sion: Challenges to syntax. Brain Research, 1146, 2349. doi:10.
Clinical Neurophysiology, 114, 336340. doi:10.1016/S1388- 1016/j.brainres.2006.12.063
2457(02)00366-8 Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2011). Thirty years and counting:
Guillet, R., & Arndt, J. (2009). Taboo words: The effect of emotion on finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain
memory for peripheral information. Memory & Cognition, 37, 866 potential (ERP). Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 621647. doi:10.
879. doi:10.3758/MC.37.6.866 1146/annurev.psych.093008.131123
Hadley, C. B., & MacKay, D. G. (2006). Does emotion help or hinder Laszlo, S., & Federmeier, K. D. (2009). A beautiful day in the neighbor-
immediate memory? Arousal versus priority-binding mechanisms. hood: An event-related potential study of lexical relationships and
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and prediction in context. Journal of Memory and Language, 61, 326
Cognition, 32, 7988. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.32.1.79 338. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2009.06.004
Hagoort, P., Brown, C., & Groothusen, J. (1993). The syntactic positive Len, I., Daz, J. M., de Vega, M., & Hernndez, J. A. (2010). Discourse-
shift (SPS) as an ERP measure of syntactic processing. Language based emotional consistency modulates early and middle compo-
a nd C og n i t i v e P ro c e ss e s, 8 , 4 3 9 48 3 . d o i : 1 0 . 1 0 8 0 / nents of event-related potentials. Emotion, 10, 863873. doi:10.
01690969308407585 1037/a0019983
Hajcak, G., Weinberg, A., MacNamara, A., & Foti, D. (2012). ERPs and Liu, B., Jin, Z., Qing, Z., & Wang, Z. (2011). The processing of phono-
the study of emotion. In S. J. Luck & E. S. Kappenman (Eds.), logical, orthographical, and lexical information of Chinese charac-
Oxford handbook of event-related potential components (pp. 441 ters in sentence contexts: An ERP study. Brain Research, 1372, 81
474). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. 91. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2010.11.068
Herbert, C., Junghofer, M., & Kissler, J. (2008). Event related potentials Luck, S. J., & Hillyard, S. A. (1994). Electrophysiological correlates of
to emotional adjectives during reading. Psychophysiology, 45, 487 feature analysis during visual search. Psychophysiology, 31, 291
498. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00638.x 308. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1994.tb02218.x
Herbert, C., Kissler, J., Junghfer, M., Peyk, P., & Rockstroh, B. (2006). MacKay, D. G., & Ahmetzanov, M. V. (2005). Emotion, memory, and
Processing of emotional adjectives: Evidence from startle EMG and attention in the taboo stroop paradigm an experimental analogue of
ERPs. Psychophysiology, 43, 197206. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986. flashbulb memories. Psychological Science, 16, 2532. doi:10.
2006.00385.x 1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00776.x
Hinojosa, J. A., Mndez-Brtolo, C., & Pozo, M. A. (2012). High arousal MacKay, D. G., Shafto, M., Taylor, J. K., Marian, D. E., Abrams, L., &
words influence subsequent processing of neutral information: Dyer, J. R. (2004). Relations between emotion, memory, and atten-
Evidence from event-related potentials. International Journal tion: Evidence from taboo Stroop, lexical decision, and immediate
of Psychophysiology, 86, 143151. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho. memory tasks. Memory & Cognition, 32, 474488. doi:10.3758/
2012.06.001 BF03195840
Hofmann, M. J., Kuchinke, L., Tamm, S., V, M. L., & Jacobs, A. M. Maratos, E. J., Allan, K., & Rugg, M. D. (2000). Recognition memory for
(2009). Affective processing within 1/10th of a second: High arousal emotionally negative and neutral words: An ERP study.
is necessary for early facilitative processing of negative but not Neuropsychologia, 38, 14521465. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(00)
positive words. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 00061-0
9, 389397. doi:10.3758/9.4.389 Martn-Loeches, M., Fernndez, A., Schacht, A., Sommer, W., Casado,
Holt, D. J., Lynn, S. K., & Kuperberg, G. R. (2009). Neurophysiological P., Jimnez-Ortega, L., & Fondevila, S. (2012). The influence of
correlates of comprehending emotional meaning in context. Journal emotional words on sentence processing: Electrophysiological and
of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 22452262. doi:10.1162/jocn.2008. behavioral evidence. Neuropsychologia, 50, 32623272. doi:10.
21151 1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.09.010
Jimenez-Ortega, L., Martn-Loeches, M., Casado, P., Sel, A., Fondevila, Mather, M., & Sutherland, M. R. (2011). Arousal-biased competition in
S., de Tejada, P. H., & Sommer, W. (2012). How the emotional perception and memory. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6,
content of discourse affects language comprehension. PLoS ONE, 7, 114133. doi:10.1177/1745691611400234
e33718. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0033718 Mathewson, K. J., Arnell, K. M., & Mansfield, C. A. (2008). Capturing
Junghfer, M., Peyk, P., Flaisch, T., & Schupp, H. T. (2006). and holding attention: The impact of emotional words in rapid serial
Neuroimaging methods in affective neuroscience: Selected method- visual presentation. Memory & Cognition, 36, 182200. doi:10.
ological issues. Progress in Brain Research, 156, 123143. doi:10. 3758/MC.36.1.182
1016/S0079-6123(06)56007-8 Medford, N., Phillips, M. L., Brierley, B., Brammer, M., Bullmore, E. T.,
Kanske, P., & Kotz, S. A. (2007). Concreteness in emotional words: ERP & David, A. S. (2005). Emotional memory: separating content and
evidence from a hemifield study. Brain Research, 1148, 138148. context. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 138, 247258. doi:10.
doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2007.02.044 1016/j.pscychresns.2004.10.004
Kensinger, E. A., Brierley, B., Medford, N., Growdon, J. H., & Meng, X., Tian, X., Jian, J., & Zhou, X. (2007). Orthographic and
Corkin, S. (2002). Effects of normal aging and Alzheimers phonological processing in Chinese dyslexic children: An ERP
disease on emotional memory. Emotion, 2, 118134. doi:10. study on sentence reading. Brain Research, 1179, 119130. doi:
1037/1528-3542.2.2.118 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.08.046
Kensinger, E. A., & Corkin, S. (2003). Memory enhancement for emo- Moreno, E. M., & Vzquez, C. (2011). Will the glass be half full
tional words: Are emotional words more vividly remembered than or half empty? Brain potentials and emotional expectations.
neutral words? Memory & Cognition, 31, 11691180. doi:10.3758/ Biological Psychology, 88, 131140. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.
BF03195800 2011.07.003
68 Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci (2015) 15:5568

Morris, J. S., Friston, K. J., Bchel, C., Frith, C. D., Young, A. W., Calder, Scott, G. G., ODonnell, P. J., Leuthold, H., & Sereno, S. C. (2009). Early
A. J., & Dolan, R. J. (1998). A neuromodulatory role for the human emotion word processing: Evidence from event-related potentials.
amygdala in processing emotional facial expressions. Brain, 121, Biological Psychology, 80, 95104. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.
4757. doi:10.1093/brain/121.1.47 03.010
Nieuwland, M. S., & Van Berkum, J. J. (2005). Testing the limits of the Semlitsch, H. V., Anderer, P., Schuster, P., & Presslich, O. (1986). A
semantic illusion phenomenon: ERPs reveal temporary semantic solution for reliable and valid reduction of ocular artifacts, applied to
change deafness in discourse comprehension. Cognitive Brain the P300 ERP. Psychophysiology, 23, 695703. doi:10.1111/j.1469-
Research, 24, 691701. doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.04.003 8986.1986.tb00696.x
Osterhout, L., & Hagoort, P. (1999). A superficial resemblance does not Sereno, S. C., Rayner, K., & Posner, M. I. (1998). Establishing a time-line
necessarily mean you are part of the family: Counterarguments to of word recognition: Evidence from eye movements and event-
Coulson, King and Kutas (1998) in the P600/SPS-P300 debate. related potentials. NeuroReport, 9, 21952200. doi:10.1097/
Language and Cognitive Processes, 14, 114. doi:10.1080/ 00001756-199807130-00009
016909699386356 Shapiro, K. L., Raymond, J. E., & Arnell, K. M. (1994). Attention to
Osterhout, L., & Holcomb, P. J. (1995). Event-related potentials and visual pattern information produces the attentional blink in rapid
language comprehension. Electrophysiology of Mind, 171215. serial visual presentation. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
doi:10.1234/12345678 Human Perception and Performance, 20, 357371. doi:10.1037/
Pessoa, L., McKenna, M., Gutierrez, E., & Ungerleider, L. (2002). Neural 0096-1523.20.2.357
processing of emotional faces requires attention. Proceedings of the Van de Meerendonk, N., Indefrey, P., Chwilla, D. J., & Kolk, H.
National Academy of Sciences, 99, 1145811463. doi:10.1073/pnas. H . ( 2 0 11 ) . M o n i t o r i n g i n l a n g u a g e p e r c e p t i o n :
172403899 Electrophysiological and hemodynamic responses to spelling
Phelps, E. A., LaBar, K. S., & Spencer, D. D. (1997). Memory for violations. NeuroImage, 54, 23502363. doi:10.1016/j.
emotional words following unilateral temporal lobectomy. Brain neuroimage.2010.10.022
and Cognition, 35, 85109. doi:10.1006/brcg.1997.0929 Vissers, C. T. W., Chwilla, D. J., & Kolk, H. H. (2006). Monitoring in
Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of language perception: The effect of misspellings of words in highly
Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 11611178. doi:10.1037/ constrained sentences. Brain Research, 1106, 150163. doi:10.
h0077714 1016/j.brainres.2006.05.012
Sabatinelli, D., Bradley, M. M., Fitzsimmons, J. R., & Lang, P. J. (2005). Vuilleumier, P. (2005). How brains beware: Neural mechanisms of emo-
Parallel amygdala and inferotemporal activation reflect emotional tional attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 585594. doi:10.
intensity and fear relevance. NeuroImage, 24, 12651270. doi:10. 1016/j.tics.2005.10.011
1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.015 Vuilleumier, P., & Huang, Y.-M. (2009). Emotional attention uncovering
Schacht, A., & Sommer, W. (2009a). Emotions in word and face pro- the mechanisms of affective biases in perception. Current Directions
cessing: early and late cortical responses. Brain and Cognition, 69, in Psychological Science, 18, 148152. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.
538550. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2008.11.005 2009.01626.x
Schacht, A., & Sommer, W. (2009b). Time course and task Wang, L., Bastiaansen, M., Yang, Y., & Hagoort, P. (2013). ERP
dependence of emotion effects in word processing. evidence on the interaction between information structure and
Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, 2843. emotional salience of words. Cognitive, Affective, &
doi:10.3758/CABN.9.1.28 Behavioral Neuroscience, 13, 297310. doi:10.3758/s13415-
Schacht, A., Sommer, W., Shmuilovich, O., Martenz, P. C., & Martn- 012-0146-2
Loeches, M. (2014). Differential task effects on N400 and P600 Wang, Y., Zhou, L., & Luo, Y. (2008). The pilot establishment and
elicited by semantic and syntactic violations. PLoS ONE, 9, e91226. evaluation of Chinese Affective Words System. Chinese Mental
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0091226 Health Journal, 22, 608612.
Schapkin, S. A., Gusev, A. N., & Kuhl, J. (2000). Categorization of Ward, R., Duncan, J., & Shapiro, K. (1996). The slow time-course of
unilaterally presented emotional words: An ERP analysis. Acta visual attention. Cognitive Psychology, 30, 79109. doi:10.1006/
Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 60, 1728. cogp.1996.0003

Вам также может понравиться