Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 9

Accident Analysis and Prevention 50 (2013) 587–595

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Accident Analysis and Prevention


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/aap

Results of a randomized controlled trial assessing the efficacy of the Supervising


for Home Safety program: Impact on mothers’ supervision practices
Barbara A. Morrongiello ∗ , Daniel Zdzieborski, Megan Sandomierski, Kimberly Munroe
Psychology Department, University of Guelph, Guelph, Ontario N1G 2W1, Canada

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Unintentional injury constitutes a major health risk for young children, with many injuries occurring in
Received 27 March 2012 the home. Although active supervision by parents has been shown to be effective to prevent injuries,
Received in revised form 3 June 2012 evidence indicates that parents do not consistently apply this strategy. To address this issue, a random-
Accepted 5 June 2012
ized controlled trial was conducted to evaluate the impact of the Supervising for Home Safety program on
parent supervision practices in the home and when unobtrusively observed in a naturalistic laboratory
Keywords:
setting. Using a participant-event monitoring procedure, parents of children aged 2 through 5 years com-
Childhood injury
pleted supervision recording sheets weekly both before and after exposure to the intervention program;
Prevention
Supervision
Control parents completed the same measures but received a program focusing on child nutrition and
Intervention active lifestyles. Unobtrusive video recordings of parent supervision of their child in a room containing
RCT contrived hazards also were taken pre- and post-intervention. Results indicated that groups did not differ
in demographic characteristics. Comparisons of post- with pre-intervention diary reported home super-
vision practices revealed a significant decrease in time that children were unsupervised, an increase in
in-view supervision, and an increase in level of supervision when children were out of view, with all
changes found only for the Intervention group. Similarly, only parents in the Intervention group showed
a significant increase in attention to the child in the contrived hazards context, with these differences
evident immediately after and 3 months after exposure to the intervention. These results provide the
first evidence that an intervention program can positively impact caregiver supervision.
© 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction in the home (Gielen et al., 2002; Watson et al., 2005), active supervi-
sion by caregivers also has emerged as an important risk-reducing
1.1. Childhood injury strategy for young children (Morrongiello et al., 2009a).

In the United States and Canada, as in most developed coun- 1.2. Caregiver supervision: definition, measurement, and findings
tries, unintentional injury is the leading cause of death for children
between 1 and 19 years of age (Canadian Institute of Child Health, Although there are challenges in defining supervision (see
2002; Grossman, 2000; National Center for Injury Prevention and Morrongiello, 2005, for extensive discussion), three dimensions
Control [NCIPC], 2007; World Health Organization [WHO], 2005). (proximity, attention, and continuity) have been shown to relate
In fact, it has been predicted that by the year 2020 injury will be to children’s risk of injury (Morrongiello et al., 2004a,b, 2006a,b,
the leading single reason for loss of healthy years of human life 2009a,b,c). Hence, a popular definition of supervision is that it refers
(WHO, 1996). Not only is injury the leading cause of child mortality, to the extent to which caregivers provide sustained attention and
but injury related visits to emergency departments and hospital- proximity to children (Gitanjali et al., 2004; Morrongiello, 2005).
izations are commonplace, with estimates indicating that as many Maximum active supervision and lowest risk of injury presum-
as one in four children in the United States are seen annually for ably occurs when a supervisor is attentively watching, the child
medical treatment due to injury (Scheidt et al., 1995). For young is in close proximity (i.e., within reach), and these behaviors are
children under six years of age, many injuries occur in the home sustained over time.
(McDonald et al., 2003; Shanon et al., 1992). In addition to environ- With regard to measuring supervision, developing methods that
mental modifications that reduce access to and eliminate hazards yield reliable and valid data can be quite challenging (Morrongiello,
2005). Observational indices can be very useful but parents’ knowl-
edge that they are being observed can lead to distortions in their
∗ Corresponding author. behavior, producing results that represent best behavior rather than
E-mail address: bmorrong@uoguelph.ca (B.A. Morrongiello). typical behavior. An innovative way to manage this issue is by

0001-4575/$ – see front matter © 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2012.06.007
588 B.A. Morrongiello et al. / Accident Analysis and Prevention 50 (2013) 587–595

unobtrusively video recording caregiver supervision and doing so evaluating the impact of the Supervising for Home Safety program
when the child and parent are together in the presence of contrived on caregivers’ actual supervision practices; the Control condition
hazards (i.e., presenting items that look like real injury hazards but targeted healthy eating habits and physical activity (i.e., a healthy
that have been modified so that they pose no real threat of injury, lifestyle).
such as placing blue dyed water in a Windex bottle, or providing The study was limited to mothers because they are often the
scissors that look sharp but have been glued shut). This methodol- primary supervisor of young children at home (Morrongiello et al.,
ogy has proved very useful for studying how caregivers supervise 2009a), past research has not found any supervision differences
and react to children’s hazard interaction attempts (Cataldo et al., between mothers and fathers (Morrongiello and Dawber, 1998;
1992; Morrongiello and Dawber, 1998), but one limitation is that Morrongiello et al., 2009b), and many supervision measures have
not all hazard contexts can be represented using this method. been validated for mothers but not fathers. Both the Intervention
A popular approach to measuring supervision in more diverse and Control conditions utilized a video to present key material
contexts involves utilizing self-report measures of caregiver behav- because past research indicates that parents favor this medium
ior (Morrongiello, 2005). Checks of the validity of self-report data (Barone et al., 1986; Metchikian et al., 1999). To assess home super-
indicate good agreement between what caregivers report and how vision, a participant-event recording procedure was used in which
they behave. For example, research comparing self-reports with parents were trained to record aspects of their own supervision
observations in a laboratory setting (Kochanska et al., 1989), as behavior in real time and in real life situations (Ferguson, 2005;
well as with observations in a supermarket (Holden et al., 1992), Morrongiello et al., 2004a,b; Peterson et al., 1991), both before and
has found that maternal self-reports provide accurate and valid after exposure to the intervention. Unobtrusive video recordings of
indications of numerous caregiver parenting behaviors. Similarly, the parent and child in a contrived hazards room within a labora-
a study comparing maternal self-reports about supervision with tory setting were completed at three time points (pre-intervention;
unobtrusively observed supervision in park settings also supports post-immediate; post-3 months later). It was hypothesized that
the validity of using self-reports about supervision (Morrongiello after intervention exposure, diary recordings of home-supervision
and House, 2004). Finally, there is very good agreement (89%) practices would show a decrease in the time the child was left unsu-
between periodic reports of supervision obtained via random tele- pervised (i.e., the parent does not know where child is and what
phone calls and diary entries completed in the 5 min preceding a s/he is doing for more than 5 min), an increase in the time the child
call (Morrongiello et al., 2006a). Overall, these diverse results sug- is in-view, and improvement in the quality of supervision when
gest that using a participant event-recording methodology in which the child is out-of-view. In the contrived hazards room, parents in
parents complete diary records of supervision can provide accurate the Intervention but not the Control group were expected to show
information about caregiver supervision in a variety of contexts. an increase from pre- to post-intervention in attentiveness to their
The application of these methods has yielded important insights child.
into how caregivers routinely supervise, and how these practices
relate to childhood injury. Although greater continuity in attention
2. Methods
and closer proximity are associated with fewer injuries for young
children, studies examining how parents routinely supervise young
2.1. Study overview
children at home indicate that they are often unable or unwilling to
provide sustained active supervision. For example, in a prospective
2.1.1. Design
study in which parents completed diaries to track home supervi-
In this efficacy RCT, parent volunteers were recruited from the
sion over several weeks, it was found that young children regularly
community and then randomly assigned to either an Intervention
spend up to 8% of their awake time unsupervised (i.e., supervisor
or Control group (see flow diagram in Fig. 1; CONSORT, 2010). The
does not know where the child is or what s/he is doing, and has not
aim was to recruit broadly and obtain a representative sample of
checked on the child in over 5 min). Moreover, children are out-
parents of young children in the community. The same home super-
of-view of their supervisor an additional 20% of the time, and the
vision measures (i.e., diary records of home supervision practices)
level of supervision is much poorer for out-of-view than in-view
and unobtrusive supervision measures (i.e., video recordings of the
times (Morrongiello et al., 2006a). Poorer supervision is associated
parent and child in a contrived hazards room) were taken pre- and
with more frequent minor injuries (Morrongiello et al., 2004a,b,
post-intervention in each group.
2006a,b), as well as medically attended injuries (Morrongiello et al.,
2009a). The pattern of these findings suggests, therefore, that par-
ents routinely supervise in ways that can elevate young children’s 2.1.2. Intervention condition
risk of injury. What is notably lacking, however, are evidence-based The Supervising for Home Safety intervention program took 4
programs that are effective to promote active supervision. The cur- weeks to complete and began with presentation of the Watchful
rent study addresses this gap in the literature. Parents, Safe Children video (20 min) and a Post-Video Structured
Discussion (40 min) in a laboratory setting, followed by one month
1.3. Present study of the parent practicing solutions in the home that addressed
self-identified barriers to actively supervising, as well as track-
The Supervising for Home Safety program was developed for par- ing their self-talk and strategies related to close supervision. The
ents of children 2 through 5 years (Morrongiello et al., 2009c). program was manualized and included extensive training mate-
The program comprises a video presentation, structured follow- rials to ensure consistent and rigorous program delivery, with
up discussion that is tailored to the individual, and a one-month random checks on fidelity of program delivery (e.g., checking
series of activities that serve to change habitual patterns of supervi- audiotapes of program delivery sessions). The intervention was
sion and promote more active strategies. Recent research confirms implemented within two weeks of parents completing the base-
the efficacy of the program for positively impacting parent atti- line pre-intervention diary based supervision recordings. Diary
tudes towards injury prevention, beliefs about the need to actively based recording of post-intervention supervision practices began
supervise young children, and commitment to actively supervising, once the mother completed the one-month intervention program.
with these effects persisting 1 year post-intervention (Morrongiello Observations of parent supervision in the contrived hazards room
et al., in press). The aim of the present study was to extend this were completed pre-intervention, and at two time points post-
research by conducting a randomized controlled trial (RCT) and intervention (immediately after, and three months later).
B.A. Morrongiello et al. / Accident Analysis and Prevention 50 (2013) 587–595 589

Parent volunteers assessed for


ELIGIBILITY
(N = 670)
Did not meet
eligibility criteria
(N = 442)
Randomly Assigned
(N = 228)

INTERVENTION CONTROL
Group Group
(N = 116) SEE DEMOGRAPHIC TABLE (N = 112)
(TABLE 1)

Lab Visit #1 Lab Visit #1


(N = 116) (N = 112)
PRE-INTERVENTION MEASURES

8 weeks of home 8 weeks of home


recording recording

Lab Visit #2 Lab Visit #2


+ 1 month + 1 month
INTERVENTION DELIVERED
(N = 102) (N = 95)

8 weeks of home 8 weeks of home


recording recording

Lab Visit #3 Lab Visit #3


(N = 96) POST-INTERVENTION FOLLOW-UP (N = 90)
MEASURES

Fig. 1. Flow Diagram for the Study.

2.2. Sample size groups, a computer generated random numbers table was used and
a concealed method of randomization was used to eliminate any
Because of the lack of intervention research in this area, there potential biases in treatment effects (Schulz, 1995; Schulz et al.,
was no direct estimate of effect size to guide a power analysis. 1995). Assignment to group was done by someone not involved in
Therefore, past research that aimed to impact other types of par- data collection.
ent safety practices was considered instead (Kendrick et al., 2008).
Based on this literature, we anticipated obtaining medium effect
sizes, and with Type I error rate set at .05 and power of .80, this 2.4. Participants
resulted in needing a sample size of 65 parents per group (Cohen,
1992). Because no differences due to child age or sex were obtained The final sample comprised 186 mothers, including 96 hav-
in the initial research that led to development of the interven- ing a son in the target age range of 2.0–5.5 years (M = 3.70 years,
tion (Morrongiello et al., 2009c) or in the RCT assessing the impact SD = 1.02 years; minimum = 2.09 years; maximum = 5.36 years)
of the intervention on mothers’ injury and supervision appraisals and 90 having a daughter in this age range (M = 3.67 years,
(Morrongiello et al., in press), and given that we planned to bal- SD = 1.03 years; minimum = 2.07 years; maximum = 5.32 years).
ance groups for age and sex, these factors were not considered in Intervention (n = 96) and Control (n = 90) groups were approxi-
estimating sample size. mately equal with respect to child sex and age. An additional 20
parents in the Intervention group and 22 in the Control group
were recruited but then unable to participate due to scheduling
2.3. Randomization conflicts.
Of the participating mothers, the majority (93%) were Caucasian,
Parent volunteers were recruited throughout the local commu- 95% were in two-parent homes, and 68% of the mothers worked
nity (e.g., general information letters distributed at daycare centers outside the home (M = 32.39 h/week, SD = 10.89 h). Table 1 pro-
and kindergarten classes, posters hung at locations where child- vides a summary of income and education demographics for the
centered activities occurred) and then randomly assigned to the Intervention and Control groups separately, as well as for the sam-
Intervention or Control group via a stratified randomization pro- ple overall. Procedures were approved by the university Research
cedure (cf. Kernan et al., 1999) to ensure comparability across Ethics Board and consent was obtained from all participants at
groups based on the child’s sex and age. In assigning parents to every point of contact.
590 B.A. Morrongiello et al. / Accident Analysis and Prevention 50 (2013) 587–595

Table 1 (2) An In-View Recording Sheet was completed every time a “child
Summary demographics for the Intervention (n = 96) and Control (n = 90) groups, and
in view of supervisor” entry was made on the Master Time Use
overall sample (N = 186). Mean values are shown with standard deviations within
parentheses. Information Sheet. Parents indicated the room the child was in,
who was supervising, and whether the supervisor was doing
Attribute Control Intervention Overalla
something with the child (“doing” or “not doing”). If “doing” was
Child age (years) selected, then parents indicated what type of activity the super-
% male 3.76 (1.04) 3.64 (1.01) 3.70 (1.02) visor was engaged in with the child (e.g., helping or teaching
% female 3.69 (1.07) 3.64 (0.99) 3.67 (1.03)
the child, playing with or entertaining the child, sharing non-
Highest education for mothers play time together such as talking). If “not doing” was selected,
% high school 5.4 6.6 5.9
then mothers indicated the level of supervision being exhibited
% university/college 54.2 65.9 60.0
% graduate 40.4 27.5 34.1
(i.e., supervisor has the child within constant view, watching
him/her intermittently, not watching at all but listening, not
Family income
watching at all or listening at all, or do not know).
% at or under $40,000 4.3 5.5 4.9
% between $40,000 and $60,000 10.8 8.8 9.8 (3) An Out-of-View Recording Sheet was completed if the mother
% between $60,000 and $80,000 21.5 22.0 21.7 indicated that the child was out-of-view of the supervisor. The
% $80,000 and above 63.4 63.7 63.6 mother indicated the rooms the child and supervisor were in,
a
One-way Analyses of Variance were conducted comparing groups for each factor the activity each was engaged in, and the level of supervision
separately and results revealed no significant group differences for any demographic being exhibited.
factor.
2.6.2. Contrived hazard rooms
2.5. Inclusion and exclusion criteria To unobtrusively measure supervision pre- and post-
intervention (immediate, long term), three rooms (same size
Inclusion criteria were: (1) one child in the family is between 2.0 and general layout) were set up as waiting areas and at each lab
and 5.5 years of age; (2) child and participating parent are fluent visit, parents spent 10 min with their child in one of these rooms.
in English; (3) if there is an older child in the home, this child is at The order of room assignment was randomized such that at the
least 2 years older than the target child because smaller intervals conclusion of the study the parent and child had visited each room.
between cohabiting children can elevate injury risk for the younger In addition to child appropriate toys, each room contained 12
child (Nathens et al., 2000); (5) mother (biological, stepmother, contrived hazards (i.e., objects that appeared hazardous but that
adoptive) who the child predominantly resides with agrees to be were modified so they posed no real risk of injury). There were
the participating parent; (6) if adopted, the child has lived with the three hazards for each of four types of injury (falls, cuts, burns, poi-
mother a majority of time for at least the last 3 months; and (7) soning), with different hazard items in each room. Video cameras
mother will be available and agrees to participate repeatedly over were hidden in the ceiling and provided unobtrusive recording of
the full study period. Exclusion criteria were: (1) the child is not parent and child behaviors and verbalizations. None of the videos
developing normally as reported by the mother; (2) twins; and (3) were viewed until study completion when the parent was debriefed
if the family previously participated in any of the projects related about the recordings and consent was obtained; all parents granted
to this research including pilot testing of measures and procedures. consent for their videotapes to be included in the study.

2.7. Videos
2.6. Materials
2.7.1. Intervention video
2.6.1. Home Supervision Recording Sheets The theoretical basis for and steps taken in developing the 20-
To measure home supervision for the pre-intervention period, min Watchful Parents, Safe Children video that the Intervention
four days were selected over the course of eight weeks (max = 1 group received is fully described elsewhere (Morrongiello et al.,
in a week; recording days represented different days of the week, 2009c). This was developed with messaging based on reviews of
and were selected to occur approximately every other week) for theoretical and empirical literature. The messaging strategies were
which the parent was to complete the supervision recording sheets, selected based on childhood injury literature and research on fac-
as in past research on home supervision (Morrongiello et al., tors shown to affect attitude change (Petty et al., 1997; Wegener
2006a, 2006b). For post-intervention measurement, after parents et al., 1994), processing of health information (Petty and Cacioppo,
had practiced solutions and tracked their self-talk and strategies 1986, p. 123; Wegener et al., 1994), readiness for behavior change
about close supervision for one month, another four supervision (Janz and Becker, 1984; Rosenstock, 1974), attention to warnings
recording days were similarly selected for the next eight weeks. and risk information (Morrongiello et al., 2006a,b; Morrongiello
Three types of supervision recording sheets were used (cf. and Kiriakou, 2004), and parent responsiveness to parenting inter-
Morrongiello et al., 2006a,b). ventions (Brown et al., 2005; Kerr and Stattin, 2000; Steinberg et al.,
1994). Experts at Safe Kids Canada also shared their test-marketing
(1) A Master Time Use Information Sheet was completed to record results regarding effective ways to promote the impact of safety
how the mother and child spent time at home together start- messaging on parents’ safety attitudes and practices.
ing from the moment the child was awake in the morning Based on these diverse sources of information, the video com-
until her/his bedtime, with the major focus on supervision. The prised 12 messaging strategies: (1) graphic images of childhood
mother created a new entry and recorded the corresponding injury to evoke emotions of fear; (2) text on the screen that was also
clock time whenever the child’s activity, room, supervisor (i.e., read aloud to promote information processing using both visual and
person most responsible for the child), or type of supervision auditory modalities; (3) sound effects (e.g., children crying, sirens)
changed, as well as when the parent and/or child left the home. to evoke strong emotions in parents; (4) Canadian child-injury
For each entry, the mother indicated who was supervising and statistics depicted using graphs; (5) mention of long-term social
whether the child was in or out of view of the supervisor. If the and emotional difficulties as a result of childhood injures; (6) pre-
child was in view, the mother indicated whether the supervisor sentation of information about individual types of injury (i.e., falls,
and child were doing something together. burns, drowning, and poisoning); (7) testimonials by mothers (one
B.A. Morrongiello et al. / Accident Analysis and Prevention 50 (2013) 587–595 591

per injury type) about when their child was unsupervised for ‘just supervising her own child at home; these were written down in
a second’ that led to the child dying or being seriously injured; (8) point form. Then she was asked to generate realistic strategies
after the testimonials, viewers were asked to reflect on how similar that she would consider either implementing to address each bar-
their supervisory practices were to this mother; (9) developmental rier or implementing more often if she had done so before. The
milestones were discussed to emphasize that children’s behav- examiner then provided mothers with a printed laminated card
ioral competencies and cognitive curiosity continuously change, with the mnemonic ‘ALTER’ that provided a structured approach
making their behavior unpredictable; (10) supervising in ways to to identifying changes that could be made to address barriers to
promote readiness to intervene but not dependency; (11) no task is actively supervising (i.e., ALTER: A = activities of the child or par-
as important as a parent’s duty to keep their child safe; and (12) sug- ent, L = location of the child or parent, T = timing of an activity,
gested strategies to improve supervisory behaviors to evoke a sense E = environment change, use R = resources). Using the ALTER card
of empowerment and personal problem solving about barriers to the examiner and mother then brainstormed to identify other sug-
actively supervise and keep their child in view. gestions for overcoming her unique barriers, with the goal of having
The video was organized to evoke different emotions through- the mother practice using the ALTER approach in problem solving
out the viewing. The initial tone was positive and playful and the about supervision barriers. The feasibility of each of these sugges-
messaging emphasized that children are constantly developing tions was then discussed with mothers in order to identify ones
and, therefore, can behave unpredictably. The tone then shifted that she believed were both feasible and likely to be implemented.
to become more serious and fear-evoking, emphasizing: injury Possible solutions were then written down next to each barrier
vulnerability and severity, long-term consequences of injury, and to illustrate that one could address each barrier and more actively
children’s specific risks for falls, drowning, poisonings, and burns. supervise if desired. Parents were given the barriers/solutions sheet
The tone shifted again to become more positive, and parents were and the ALTER card to take home as a reminder of the solutions to
presented with messages aimed at empowering them to keep their barriers that were discussed and that would allow them to actively
child in view and to promote a sense of self-efficacy to do so. The supervise their young child. They were then asked to sign a one-
positive tone continued and mothers were then presented with month ‘Practice Solutions’ contract indicating that over the course
ways to help them actively supervise and keep their child in view of the next four weeks they would use the ALTER card and prac-
when faced with common barriers to doing so. They were also tice the solutions generated. Lastly, they were presented a sheet
encouraged to formulate their own strategies for keeping their child on which they were to write down other solutions they tried and
in view, and the video concluded with empowering messages and all instances of ‘self-talk’ about what they said to themselves that
reminders to actively supervise by keeping the child in view. motivated them to make a change so they could keep their child in
view and/or near them; the self-talk data are not considered herein.
2.7.2. Control video
The Control group video (Healthy Lifestyles, Healthy Children) 2.9. Procedure
was identical in all ways (e.g., density of photos, length, emotional
tone) except that the content focused on child nutrition and active Parents who volunteered were invited to participate in a lon-
lifestyles. It too was finalized based on extensive input from parents gitudinal research study examining parenting practices and how
of young children. these change and affect children’s health as the child develops. They
understood there would be a variety of types of activities for them
2.8. Post-Video Structured Discussion: tailoring the intervention to complete (some at home and some in the lab, including diary
recordings about parenting on select days) and repeated contacts
Immediately following video viewing, the parent rated how over six months, but were not told that the focus was on supervision
emotionally arousing she found the video. The examiner then or that an intervention was part of the research.
conducted a structured interview with the mother that focused During an initial laboratory visit (see Fig. 1), parents granted
on active supervision for Intervention parents and on child written consent and completed questionnaires about family demo-
nutrition and exercise for Control parents. This interview was graphics and their child’s health and development; these data are
audio-recorded and, to ensure consistency and fidelity of pro- not pertinent to this publication and are not considered herein.
gram delivery, a random sample of these for each interviewer was Questionnaires were begun during a 10-min ‘waiting period’ in a
checked biweekly by a supervisor not involved in data collection. contrived hazards room (pre-intervention) and completed at the
The format of the Intervention and Control discussions was the end of the session while the child played with a research assistant.
same, but only procedural details and results from the Intervention A home visit was then scheduled to occur within two weeks. Dur-
discussion will be presented herein. ing this home visit, mothers were given calendar pages that showed
Each mother in the Intervention group was asked to create a their supervision recording days, a binder containing supervision
radio message in which she was to act as a spokesperson advocat- diary recording sheets, and detailed instructions on how to com-
ing for active parent supervision of young children in the home. To plete these. The mothers also received training in how and when
do this, the mother was asked to generate and write out up to three to complete the sheets (i.e., every time there was an entry on the
key messages for convincing other parents to actively supervise Master Time Use Information Sheet the mother was also to complete
their children and to compile these into a radio ad. The mother then either the In View or Out-of-view Recording Sheet, and all sheets had
recited her radio ad three times, and after each recital, the examiner to be time synchronized with all the time accounted for until the
provided positive encouragement. Each radio message was audio child’s bedtime; see Morrongiello et al., 2006a,b, for further details).
recorded and played back for the mother to hear so refinements Mothers were told that we were interested in how parents and
could be made over practices. This aspect of the program was based children spend their time at home and how parents balance pro-
on evidence showing that public endorsement of behavior that moting independence with monitoring their child. On a recording
one is not practicing leads to feelings of dissonance (i.e., induced day, sheet completion was to begin when they and their child were
hypocrisy) and negative self-concept that are unpleasant but effec- both awake and was to end when their child went to bed at night;
tive to evoke instantiation of the desired behavior (Morrongiello recording was to cease during periods when the mother or child
and Marks, 2008; Son Hing et al., 2002). left the house or were sleeping. Parents received a call from the
The mother was then asked to identify three barriers that researcher assistant the day before each recording day to remind
she personally experiences that prevent her from actively them of this and to address any questions.
592 B.A. Morrongiello et al. / Accident Analysis and Prevention 50 (2013) 587–595

The intervention program began when parents made their sec- supervision provided when the child was out-of-view (0 = do not
ond lab visit, which took place after they completed the supervision know, 1 = not supervising, that is, not checking or listening in at
diary recording forms for an eight-week baseline period; parents all, 2 = only going to check on the child when she hears something
brought the binder with the completed diary recording forms to that indicates the child needs to be checked, 3 = checking every
this visit. Upon arrival, the mother was asked to view a 20-min 10 min or longer, 4 = checking every 8–9 min, 5 = checking every
video that had to do with some aspect of child health. She watched 6–7 min, 6 = checking every 4–5 min, 7 = checking every 2–3 min,
this video on a 19-in. flat-screen computer monitor, while listen- 8 = listening in constantly).
ing to the sound through speakers; a research assistant was in For in-view and out-of-view data, mean supervision scores were
the room but faced away from the parent and appeared distracted calculated by averaging entries across all days. Higher scores indi-
with paperwork. After viewing the video, the parent immediately cated more active supervision.
rated the level of emotional arousal evoked by the video (1 = not
at all emotionally arousing, 7 = extremely emotionally arousing). 2.10.2. Contrived hazards room supervision
The examiner then conducted and audio-recorded the Post-Video Videos were coded for a total of 10 min, starting from the point
Structured Discussion. The parent was told they could keep the at which the door of the room was closed. The primary measure was
ALTER card and was asked, for the next month, to: (1) imple- attention to the child. This was based on how closely the mother
ment the agreed upon strategies indicated on the barriers/solutions watched the child, with scores ranging from 0 to 3 and assigned for
listing; (2) initial the ‘Practice Solutions’ contract each day as a each of the 10 min (0 = not attentive at all, 3 = continuously atten-
reminder of what was discussed and agreed to; and (3) write down, tive); these 10 scores were averaged over time to provide an index
on a provided sheet, ‘self-talk’ about things they said to themselves of caregiver attention during the session, with higher scores indi-
to motivate them to change their supervisory practices and keep cating more active supervision. Because 10 min of videotape took
their child in view and/or near them. Lastly, the parent and child up to 12 h to fully code, the number of participants was reduced
spent 10 min in the contrived hazards room, with the parent again by randomly selecting 69 Intervention and 68 Control participants
given questionnaires to complete about their child’s health and after all the data were collected from the 186 participants. A power
development. analysis indicated this sample size would provide sufficient power
The self-talk recording sheets and initialed ‘Practice Solutions’ (see Participants). All videos were coded by the same coder who
contract were picked up at the parent’s home after one month. At was blind to each participant’s group assignment when coding.
this time, parents were given new Home Supervision Recording For estimating reliability, 25% of the videos (randomly selected)
Sheets for their binder on which to record their home supervi- were coded by a second independent coder, with excellent agree-
sion for another four days that were selected by the researcher to ment obtained (92% agreement overall; Kappa = .89). The data of
occur over the course of the next eight consecutive weeks. After the primary coder were analyzed.
this eight-week recording interval the parent made a final visit to
the lab, at which time she returned the Home Supervision Record- 2.11. Analytic approach
ing Sheets and spent 10 min with her child in the contrived hazards
room (post-intervention: 3 months) while completing some ques- For Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) tests, a Greenhouse–Geisser
tionnaires. Throughout the study, upon completion of each point adjustment was applied to the degrees of freedom when the need
of face-to-face contact with parents, they were given a gift card or for it was indicated in tests of sphericity. Multivariate outliers
monetary compensation. were removed when appropriate (cf. Tabachnick and Fidell, 1989),
resulting in some slight variation in sample size across ANOVAs.
2.10. Data coding and measures taken A Bonferroni correction for family wise error rate was applied for
all paired-comparisons and the results reported reflect the level of
2.10.1. Home Supervision Recording Sheets significance obtained after applying the correction.
Only entries indicating that the mother was the supervisor were
included in the analysis (97% of all entries), and any entries made 3. Results
when the child was sleeping or when the mother indicated “don’t
know” for the reported level of supervision were excluded from 3.1. Sample demographics
analysis (<1% of all entries). Hence, the time estimates computed
were based on when the child and mother were both at home and Demographic information for each group and the sample overall
awake, and she was the primary supervisor. is given in Table 1. A one-way ANOVA, with group as a between-
The Master Time Use Information Sheets were used to determine participants factor was conducted separately for each category and
the amount of time in different supervision circumstances (e.g., no significant differences were noted (p > .05 for all tests). Hence,
child in-view versus out-of-view) and with different supervisors. the randomization procedure was effective in creating two groups
The In View Recording Sheets were used to determine how much of parents having comparable demographic characteristics.
of the time a child and supervisor were in the same room and
“Doing Something” versus “Not Doing Something” together. Super- 3.2. Was the intervention effective in improving home
vision when “Not Doing Something” together was coded as 1 = not supervision?
supervising (i.e., not watching or listening for the child at all),
2 = not watching but listening intermittently, 3 = watching him/her To determine if there were group differences in the amount of
intermittently and/or listening constantly, 4 = have him/her within time parents reported the children were awake and at home with
constant view, 0 = do not know (mother was not the supervisor and them, a split-plot ANOVA was conducted with group (2: Inter-
does not know the nature of the supervision being provided but she vention, Control) as a between-participants factor and time (2:
knows the child and supervisor were in the same room). Supervi- pre-intervention, post-intervention) as a within-participants fac-
sion when “Doing Something” together was coded as maximum tor on the total amount of time reported. Results revealed only
supervision (i.e., 4). a significant main effect of time, F(1, 185) = 22.37, p < .001, par-
The Out-of-View Recording Sheets were used to determine the tial Á2 = .12. Thus, the daily amount of time children and mothers
nature of the activities of the child (i.e., to differentiate nap were awake and home together was comparable across groups,
time from awake time periods) and supervisor, and the level of but declined over time from the pre- to post-intervention interval
B.A. Morrongiello et al. / Accident Analysis and Prevention 50 (2013) 587–595 593

Table 2 in the level of out-of-view supervision for the Control group


Mean (SD) scores for different indices of supervision for Intervention and Control
(p > .05), but in the Intervention group there was a significant
groups at pre- and post-intervention periods.
increase from pre- to post-intervention, t(86) = 2.10, p < .05, Cohen’s
Measure Group Pre-intervention Post-intervention d = .45.
% Time Intervention 8.45 (6.05) 2.90 (5.33)
Unsuperviseda , b Control 6.96 (6.46) 6.28 (8.42)

% Time Intervention 83.02 (14.24) 86.83 (12.30) 3.3. Did the intervention impact unobtrusively observed
in-viewb Control 83.24 (13.00) 81.35 (13.96) supervision?
Level of supervision
In-viewb , c Intervention 3.76 (0.19) 3.78 (0.18) A split-plot ANOVA with group (2: Intervention, Control)
Control 3.74 (0.19) 3.75 (0.24) as a between-participants factor and time (3: pre-intervention,
Out-of-viewb , d Intervention 6.13 (1.79) 6.89 (1.54) post-intervention: immediate, post-intervention: 3 months) as a
Control 6.40 (1.80) 6.03 (2.07)
within-participants factor was applied to the attention-to-child
a
Defined as ‘parent has no knowledge of where child is and what s/he is doing scores for the randomly selected smaller sample. Results revealed
for at least 5 consecutive minutes’; measured as percentage of total time. a significant group x time interaction, F(2, 135) = 3.62, p < .05,
b
Designates a significant group × time interaction was obtained (p < .05).
c
Max score = 4 (i.e., constantly watching).
partial Á2 = .05. Follow-up tests confirmed that, relative to pre-
d
Max score = 8 (i.e., constantly listening). intervention levels of attention, parents in the Intervention group
showed a significant increase in attention to the child both immedi-
ately after the intervention [t(67) = 5.22, p < .01] and 3 months later,
(overall M = 15.44 and 13.21 h/day, SD = 7.11 and 7.45 h, respec- t(67) = 3.84, p < .01 (M = 1.27, 1.50, 1.56, SD = .56, .56, .60, respec-
tively), which was approximately three months. tively). In contrast, parents in the Control group did not show any
To determine if the intervention was effective in promoting significant changes in attention to the child over these same time
supervision generally, the percentage of time the child was unsu- intervals (M = 1.24, 1.30, 1.30, SD = .52, .44, .63, respectively), p > .05.
pervised (i.e., parent reports not knowing where the child is and
what s/he is doing for at least 5 min) was considered; these data
appear in Table 2. A split-plot ANOVA was conducted with group (2: 4. Discussion
Intervention, Control) as a between-participants factor and time (2:
pre-intervention, post-intervention) as a within-participants fac- Although it has been established that level of supervision influ-
tor. Results revealed a significant group x time interaction, F(1, ences risk of childhood injury (Morrongiello et al., 2004a,b, 2009a),
83) = 4.78, p < .05, partial Á2 = .06; see data in Table 2. Follow-up to date there are no proven interventions to promote active super-
tests confirmed that there were no group differences in the per- vision of young children by parents. Brown et al. (2005) found
centage of time unsupervised at pre-intervention [F(1, 83) = 1.20, that exposing parents to a video of their child interacting with
p > .05], but for the post-intervention interval, children of mothers contrived hazards increased their judgments about what level of
in the Intervention condition were unsupervised significantly less supervision was adequate and resulted in a reduction in home
often than children in the Control condition, F(1, 83) = 4.81, p < .05. hazards, however, no actual measures of supervision practices
To determine if the main intervention message of ‘keep your were taken. Addressing this gap, Morrongiello et al. (in press)
child in view’ had an impact on the percentage of time the child recently developed and evaluated the impact of the Supervising
was kept in view, a split-plot ANOVA was conducted, with group for Home Safety intervention program on parents’ appraisals of
(2: Intervention, Control) as a between-participants factor and time young children’s risk of injury and need for supervision. The pro-
(2: pre-intervention, post-intervention) as a within-participants gram was shown to positively impact parent injury risk appraisals
factor. Results revealed a significant time × condition interaction, relative to a Control group, and these changes persisted through
F(1, 182) = 7.34, p < .01, partial Á2 = .14. As seen in Table 2, and con- one year post-intervention. Although intentions to implement
firmed with follow-up tests, in-view time was at comparable levels a practice can predict actually doing so (Andrews et al., 2008;
across both groups at the pre-intervention period, F(1, 181) = .45, Webb et al., 1996), obtaining direct evidence of behavior change
p > .05, but reached higher levels in the Intervention than Control provides the best test of this and was addressed in the current
group at the post-intervention period, F(1, 181) = 4.44, p < .05. Inter- study.
vention parents also showed a significant increase from pre- to Changes in several aspects of parent supervision practices
post-intervention in the percentage of time they kept the child in occurred in response to the Supervising for Home Safety program.
view [t(85) = 2.51, p < .01, Cohen’s d = .29], but there was no such Following the intervention, parents showed a decrease in the time
increase in the Control group, p > .05. their child was completely unsupervised, an increase in the time
To determine if groups differed in the level of supervi- they kept their child in view, and an increase in the level of super-
sion provided when children were in-view and out-of-view, vision provided when the child was out of view. Moreover, when
split-plot ANOVAs with group (2: Intervention, Control) as parent supervision was unobtrusively observed, it was found that
a between-participants factor and time (2: pre-intervention, mothers showed a significant increase in attention to their child,
post-intervention) as a within-participants factor were applied and these effects persisted up to three months after the interven-
separately to the average level of supervision reported by par- tion concluded. Hence, the results of this study affirm the efficacy of
ents when the child was in-view and out-of-view (see Table 2). the Supervising for Home Safety program for evoking improvements
For the in-view data, scores approached a ceiling level in both in caregiver supervision practices.
the Intervention and Control groups and there were no group dif- Extending this research to explore more cost-effective deliv-
ferences (p > .05). For out-of-view supervision, the interaction of ery approaches, such as delivering the program to small groups
group × time was marginally significant, F (1, 166) = 3.10, p < .06, of parents, would be an important next step. Indeed this is cur-
partial Á2 = .04. There were no group differences at pre-intervention rently underway in our research laboratory. Indeed, given that
in the level of supervision provided when the child was out-of- safety interventions targeting parents have been shown to pro-
view, but at post-intervention, parents in the Intervention group duce the best effects when delivered within a parenting program
showed higher levels of supervision than in the Control group, (Kendrick et al., 2008), this might be a way not only to promote
t(166) = 2.99, p < .01. Moreover, there was no significant increase broader dissemination of the Supervising for Home Safety program,
594 B.A. Morrongiello et al. / Accident Analysis and Prevention 50 (2013) 587–595

but to further enhance impact of the program on parent supervisory Kernan, W.N., Viscoli, C.M., Makuch, R.W., Brass, L.M., Horwitz, R.I., 1999. Stratified
practices. We are currently engaged in this research. randomization for clinical trials. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 52 (1), 19–26,
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0895-4356(98)00138-3.
Kerr, M., Stattin, H., 2000. What parents know, how they know it, and several forms
4.1. Limitations and directions for future research of adolescent adjustment: further support for a reinterpretation of monitoring.
Developmental Psychology 36 (3), 366–380, http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-
1649.36.3.366.
Despite the important results of this study, several limitations Kochanska, G., Kuczynski, L., Radke-Yarrow, M., 1989. Correspondence between
should be noted and addressed in future research. First, the sample mothers’ self-reported and observed child-rearing practices. Child Development
60 (1), 56–63, Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/1131070.
is mostly Caucasian, well educated, and affluent. These demo-
McDonald, E., Gielen, A., Trifiletti, L., Andrews, J., Serwint, J., Wilson, M.,
graphic characteristics may have contributed to the successful 2003. Evaluation activities to strengthen an injury prevention resource
implementation of the program, and these findings, therefore, may center for urban families. Health Promotion Practice 4 (2), 129–137,
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1524839902250761.
not generalize to those with other demographic characteristics.
Metchikian, K.L., Mink, J.M., Bigelow, K.M., Lutzker, J.R., Doctor, R.M., 1999. Reducing
Future research should evaluate the impact of the intervention home safety hazards in the homes of parents reported for neglect. Child & Family
on more diverse populations, including lower income groups. Sec- Behavior Therapy 21 (3), 23–34, http://dx.doi.org/10.1300/J019v21n03 02.
ond, only mothers were included in the sample and, as such, these Morrongiello, B.A., 2005. Caregiver supervision and child-injury risk. I:
issues in defining and measuring supervision. II: findings and direc-
findings may not reflect how fathers or other caregivers would tions for future research. Journal of Pediatric Psychology 30 (7), 536–552,
react to the intervention. Although many similarities have been http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/jsi041.
noted between mothers and fathers in studies on child safety Morrongiello, B.A., Dawber, T., 1998. Toddlers’ and mothers’ behaviors in
an injury-risk situation: implications for sex differences in childhood
and supervision (Morrongiello and Dawber, 1999; Morrongiello injuries. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 19 (4), 625–639,
et al., 2009b), one cannot assume that the current intervention http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0193-3973(99)80059-8.
would impact fathers’ supervision practices in the same way as Morrongiello, B.A., Dawber, T., 1999. Parental influences on toddlers’ injury risk
behaviors: are sons and daughters socialized differently? Journal of Applied
it did mothers’ practices. Conducting research to directly eval- Developmental Psychology 20 (2), 227–251, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0193-
uate this important question is a necessary next step. Finally, 3973(99)00015-5.
examining supervision practices over a longer period of time is Morrongiello, B.A., House, K., 2004. Measuring parent attributes and
supervision behaviors relevant to child injury risk: examining the
essential for evaluating the sustainability of increased supervision
usefulness of questionnaire measures. Injury Prevention 10, 114–118,
in reaction to the intervention. In order to reduce children’s risk http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/ip.2003.003459.
of injury, it would be important not only that the intervention Morrongiello, B.A., Kiriakou, S., 2004. Parents’ home-safety practices for preventing
six types of childhood injuries: what do they do, and why? Journal of Pediatric
improve a parent’s supervision practices but that these changes be
Psychology 29 (4), 285–297, http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/jsh030.
maintained beyond the three month interval that was evaluated Morrongiello, B.A., Marks, L., 2008. Practice what you preach: induced hypocrisy
herein. as an intervention strategy to reduce children’s intentions to risk take on play-
grounds. Journal of Pediatric Psychology 33, 117–128.
Morrongiello, B.A., Ondejko, L., Littlejohn, A., 2004a. Understanding toddlers’ in-
References home injuries. I: context, correlates and determinants. Journal of Pediatric
Psychology 29, 415–431, http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/jsh046.
Andrews, J.A., Hampson, S.E., Barckley, M., Gerrard, M., Gibbons, F.X., 2008. The effect Morrongiello, B.A., Ondejko, L., Littlejohn, A., 2004b. Understanding toddlers’
of early cognitions on cigarette and alcohol use during adolescence. Psychology in-home injuries. II: examining parental strategies, and their efficacy, for
of Addictive Behaviors 22 (1), 96–106, http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0893- moderating child-injury risk. Journal of Pediatric Psychology 29, 433–446,
164X.22.1.96. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/jsh047.
Barone, V.J., Greene, B.F., Lutzker, J.R., 1986. Home safety with families being Morrongiello, B.A., Corbett, M., McCourt, M., Johnston, N., 2006a. Understanding
treated for child abuse and neglect. Behavior Modification 10 (1), 93–114, unintentional injury-risk in young children. I: the nature and scope of caregiver
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/01454455860101006. supervision of children at home. Journal of Pediatric Psychology 31 (6), 529–539,
Brown, K.J., Roberts, M.C., Mayes, S., Boles, R.E., 2005. Effects of parental viewing of http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/jsj045.
children’s risk behavior on home safety practices. Journal of Pediatric Psychology Morrongiello, B.A., Corbett, M., McCourt, M., Johnston, N., 2006b. Understanding
30 (7), 571–580, http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/jsi044. unintentional injury-risk in young children. II: the contribution of caregiver
Canadian Institute of Child Health, 2002. The Health of Canada’s Children: A CICH supervision, child attributes, and parent attributes. Journal of Pediatric Psychol-
Profile, 3rd ed. Canadian Institute of Child Health, Ottawa, ON. ogy 31 (6), 540–551, http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/jsj073.
Cataldo, M.F., Finney, J.W., Richman, G.S., Riley, A.W., Hook, R.J., Brophy, C.J., Nau, Morrongiello, B.A., Corbett, M., Brison, R.J., 2009a. Identifying predictors
P.A., 1992. Behavior of injured and uninjured children and their parents in of medically-attended injuries to young children: do child or par-
a simulated hazardous setting. Journal of Pediatric Psychology 17 (1), 73–80, ent behavioral attributes matter? Injury Prevention 15 (4), 220–225,
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/17.1.73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/ip.2008.019976.
Cohen, J., 1992. A power primer. Psychological Bulletin 112 (1), 155–159, Morrongiello, B.A., Walpole, B., McArthur, B., 2009b. Brief Report: Young children’s
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155. risk of unintentional injury: a comparison of mothers’ and fathers’ supervi-
CONSORT, 2010. The Consort Checklist was consulted in designing the study. Avail- sion beliefs and reported practices. Journal of Pediatric Psychology 34 (10),
able at: www.consort-statement.org. 1063–1068, http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/jsp011.
Ferguson, E., 2005. The use of diary methodologies in health and clinical psychology. Morrongiello, B.A., Zdzieborski, D., Sandomierski, M., Lasenby-Lessard, J., 2009c.
In: Miles, J., Gilbert, P. (Eds.), A Handbook of Research Methods for Clinical and Video messaging: what works to persuade mothers to supervise young chil-
Health Psychology. Oxford, New York, pp. 111–123. dren more closely in order to reduce injury risk? Social Science & Medicine 68
Gielen, A.C., McDonald, E.M., Wilson, M.E.H., Hwang, W., Serwint, J.R., Andrews, J.S., (6), 1030–1037, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.12.019.
Wang, M., 2002. Effects of improved access to safety counseling, products, and Morrongiello, B.A., Sandomierski, M., Zdzieborski, D., McCollam, H. Results of a
home visits on parents’ safety practices: results of a randomized trial. Archives Randomized, Controlled Trial assessing the efficacy of the Supervising for
of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine 156 (1), 33–40. Home Safety program: does it increase parental appraisals of injury risk and
Gitanjali, S., Brenner, R., Morrongiello, B.A., Haynie, D., Rivera, M., Cheng, T.L., commitment to actively supervising their young children? Health Psychology,
2004. The role of supervision in child injury risk: definition, conceptual and http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0028214, in press.
measurement issues. Injury Control and Safety Promotion 11 (1), 17–22, Nathens, A.B., Neff, M.J., Goss, C.H., Maier, R.V., Rivara, F.P., 2000. Effect of older
http://dx.doi.org/10.1076/icsp.11.1.17.26310. sibling and birth interval on the risk of childhood injury. Injury Prevention 6,
Grossman, D.C., 2000. The history of injury control and the epidemiology of child 219–222.
and adolescent injuries. The Future of Children 10 (1), 23–52, Retrieved from National Center for Injury Prevention and Control (NCIPC), 2007. Web-
http://www.jstor.org/stable/1602824. based Injury Statistics Query Reporting System. Retrieved from
Holden, G., Ritchie, K., Coleman, S., 1992. The accuracy of maternal self- http://www.cdc.gov/ncipc/wisqars (06.09.07).
reports: agreement between reports on a computer simulation compared with Peterson, L., Cook, S.C., Little, T., Schick, B., 1991. Mom lets me go there: the role of
observed behavior in the supermarket. Early Development and Parenting 1, environment and supervision in children’s minor injuries. Children’s Environ-
109–119. ments Quarterly 8 (3–4), 15–23.
Janz, N.K., Becker, M.H., 1984. The health belief model: a decade later. Health Educa- Petty, R.E., Cacioppo, J.T., 1986. The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. In:
tion & Behavior 11 (1), 1–47, http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/109019818401100101. Berkowitz, L. (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, vol. 19. Aca-
Kendrick, D., Barlow, J., Hampshire, A., Stewart-Brown, S., Polnay, L., 2008. Parenting demic Press, New York, pp. 123–205.
interventions and the prevention of unintentional injuries in childhood: sys- Petty, R.E., Wegener, D.T., Fabrigar, L.R., 1997. Attitudes and atti-
tematic review and meta-analysis. Child: Care, Health and Development 34 (5), tude change. Annual Review of Psychology 48, 609–647,
682–695, http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2214.2008.00849.x. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.48.1.609.
B.A. Morrongiello et al. / Accident Analysis and Prevention 50 (2013) 587–595 595

Rosenstock, I.M., 1974. Historical origins of the health belief model. Health Education Steinberg, L., Fletcher, A., Darling, N., 1994. Parental monitoring and peer influ-
Monographs 2, 1–8. ences on adolescent substance use. Pediatrics 93 (6), 1060–1064, Retrieved from
Safe Kids Canada, 2007. Child & Youth Unintentional Injury: 10 Years http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/93/6/1060.
in Review 1994–2003. Retrieved from http://www.safekidscanada.ca/ Tabachnick, B.G., Fidell, L.S., 1989. Using Multivariate Statistics. Harper & Row, New
SKCForPartners/custom/NationalReportUpdatedENG.pdf (05.10.09). York.
Scheidt, P.C., Harel, Y., Trumble, A.C., Jones, D.H., Overpeck, M.D., Bijur, P.E., 1995. Watson, M., Kendrick, D., Coupland, C., Woods, A., Futers, D., Robinson,
The epidemiology of nonfatal injuries among US children and youth. American J., 2005. Providing child safety equipment to prevent injuries: ran-
Journal of Public Health 85 (7), 932–938. domised controlled trial. British Medical Journal 330 (7484), 178–181,
Schulz, K.F., 1995. Subverting randomization in controlled trials. The http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bmj.38309.664444.8F.
Journal of the American Medical Association 274 (18), 1456–1458, Webb, J.A., Baer, P.E., Getz, J.G., McKelvey, R.S., 1996. Do fifth graders’ attitudes
http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.1995.03530180050029. and intentions toward alcohol use predict seventh-grade use? Journal of the
Schulz, K.F., Chalmers, I., Hayes, R.J., Altman, D.G., 1995. Empirical evidence of bias: American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 35 (12), 1611–1617,
dimensions of methodological quality associated with estimates of treatment http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199612000-00012.
effects in controlled trials. The Journal of the American Medical Association 273 Wegener, D.T., Petty, R.E., Klein, D.J., 1994. Effects of mood on high elaboration atti-
(5), 408–412, http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.1995.03520290060030. tude change: the mediating role of likelihood judgements. European Journal of
Shanon, A., Bashaw, B., Lewis, J., Feldman, W., 1992. Nonfatal childhood injuries: a Social Psychology 24 (1), 25–43, http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2420240103.
survey at the Children’s Hospital of eastern Ontario. Canadian Medical Associa- World Health Organization (WHO), 1996. The Global Burden of Disease. WHO,
tion Journal 146 (3), 361–365. Geneva.
Son Hing, L., Li, W., Zanna, M., 2002. Inducing hypocrisy to reduce prejudicial World Health Organization (WHO), 2005. Child and Adolescent Injury
responses among aversive racists. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology Prevention: A Global Call to Action. WHO, Geneva, Available at:
38, 71–78. http://www.who.int/violence injury prevention/media/news/29 11 2005/en/.

Вам также может понравиться