Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 8

Rising from a Supine Position to Erect Stance

Description of Adult Movement and a Developmental Hypothesis


ANN F. VANSANT

Standing up from a supine position is important for physical independence. This


study was designed to describe movements within specific body regions used to
stand up from a supine position. Another purpose was to identify motor devel-
opmental sequences for the upper extremities, lower extremities, and axial region
for this rising task. Thirty-two young adults were videotaped while rising from a
supine position 10 times. Descriptive categories were formed to portray move-
ments of the upper extremities, lower extremities, and axial region. Subjects
varied greatly in the movement patterns they used to rise. Only 25% of the
subjects demonstrated a similar combination of movements during rising. That
combination involved symmetrical use of the limbs and trunk while flexing forward
from a supine position, moving through sitting to squatting, then standing. An
ordering of categories was found for each body region that was proposed as a
developmental sequence of movement patterns for this task. The variability of
subjects' movements while rising provides clinicians with numerous movement
combinations that might be used when teaching patients to stand from a supine
position.
Key Words: Functional training and activities; Kinesiology/biomechanics, general;
Movement; Pediatrics, development.

The ability to stand up from the floor their evaluations and selection of move- The form of movements used to rise
is a significant part of physical inde- ment patterns they will teach. from a supine position change during
pendence. Movement patterns used to The first purpose of this study was to the preschool years.3-5 Little is known,
stand up are a concern of physical ther- describe adults' movements in the task however, about movement patterns
apists when evaluating their patients' of standing up from a supine position. I used to right the body after the period
performances and instructing them to believed that my method of movement of early childhood. Righting movements
perform this task. analysis might provide physical thera- are assumed to reach mature form and
The motor reeducation theories and pists with a more detailed description of then remain unchanged during later
techniques of Bobath and Bobath1 and this task that eventually might lead to childhood and the adult years. These
Knott and Voss2 prescribe specific more refined evaluation and treatment assumptions, however, have never been
movement patterns to be used when of young adults who experience diffi- studied formally.
teaching individuals to rise from a su- culty in standing up.
pine position. No formal studies, how- The second purpose of the study was BACKGROUND
ever, have been reported in the literature to hypothesize specific developmental
that describe clearly movements used by sequences for this task. Rising from a Without studies of adults' rising
healthy adults in this task. When train- supine position is an excellent task for movements reported in the literature, I
ing disabled adults to stand up from a studying life span motor development. turned to the published research on
supine position, therapists must rely From a theoretical perspective, rising righting in infants and young children.
either on these authorities or their own from a supine position is a "righting" Schaltenbrand3 and McGraw4 have de-
informal observations as a reference for task.3,4 Righting encompasses all of the scribed development within the task of
varied movements used in the process rising from a supine position. Both re-
of assuming erect stance. Motor abilities searchers implied that developmental
A. VanSant, PhD, is Associate Professor, Depart-
such as rolling from a supine to a prone change in this task was complete in early
ment of Physical Therapy, Medical College of Vir- position, moving to sitting, getting up childhood. According to Schaltenbrand,
ginia, Virginia Commonwealth University, PO Box on all fours, and ultimately standing up the adult form of rising appears by the
224, MCV Station, Richmond, VA 23298-0001
(USA). She was a doctoral candidate, Department from a supine position, have been con- age of 4 to 5 years and is characterized
of Physical Education and Dance, School of Edu- sidered righting tasks.1,3 Development of by symmetry of body action.3 McGraw's
cation, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madi- righting abilities during the first year of
son, WI, when this study was conducted. description and illustration of a mature
This study was completed in partial fulfillment life represents progression toward phys- form of rising did not portray symmetry
of the requirements for Dr. VanSant's doctoral ical independence. The ability to rise of body action, despite study of children
degree, University of Wisconsin-Madison.
This study was supported in part by a grant from
from a supine position without pulling up to 6 years of age.4
the Foundation for Physical Therapy and was pre- up on someone or something is com- This discrepancy over mature form
sented at the Sixty-First Annual Conference of the monly acquired early in the second year may have resulted from the varied
American Physical Therapy Association, New Or-
leans, LA, June 16-20, 1985. of life5 and, under normal circum- points of emphasis when describing ris-
This article was submitted June 26, 1986; was stances, can be expected to be main- ing action or from differences in the
with the author for revision 27 weeks; and was tained until the end of the human life
accepted April 29, 1987. Potential Conflict of Inter- samples of children studied. Nonethe-
est: 4. span. less, a single mature form of rising from

Volume 68 / Number 2, February 1988 185


a supine position cannot be described change that precedes a mature state. The activity. The protocol for this study was
on the basis of previous reports. mature state is characterized by behav- approved by the university's Human
Early researchers described righting ioral stability. Subjects Committee, and written in-
movements in quite general terms. That Change in behavior that occurs in a formed consent was obtained from each
is, a particular aspect of therisingmove- mature individual is traditionally attrib- subject.
ment was selected and used to charac- uted to learning. Age-related behaviors
terize movements of the body as a that are not a result of specific learning Design
whole. Schaltenbrand, for example, em- experiences, however, possibly may ap-
phasized differences in the amount of pear in adults. Unless an individual had The research was conducted as a de-
body rotation used to accomplish right- specific training in a particular motor scriptive survey. From a developmental
ing from a supine position,3 and Mc- task, the motor performance demon- perspective, the study represented a sin-
Graw concentrated on description of the strated at a particular point in time as- gle age-group, cross-sectional design.
characteristics of automaticity and voli- sumably is representative of naturally
tion in infants' and young children's occurring, untrained performance. I de- Equipment
righting movements.4 When develop- signed this study to describe naturally A Beta format portable videocassette
mental differences are described in occurring righting behavior. I did not recorder and tuner (VCR) were used
terms of action of the whole body, pre- train the subjects, and I assumed that in conjunction with a videocamera to
cision is lost.6 Descriptions of move- they were never taught how to rise from record each subject's performance. The
ments are often incomplete. Movements the floor. videocamera was located 7.6 m from the
of the extremities, for example, were not A survey of individuals of different center of a 1.2- × 1.8-m exercise mat.
consistently reported when successive ages is the most common approach to The camera was positioned on a tripod
developmental levels in therightingtask identifying developmental sequences.8 such that the optical axis was approxi-
were described.3,4 Cross-sectional research designs are mately perpendicular to the long side of
Roberton tried to classify children's based on the assumption that develop- the mat at a height of 1 m above the
throwing movements by using whole- mental change is age related. The behav- floor. The camera obtained a side view
body descriptions of developmental iors of different age groups are used to of each subject at the beginning of tap-
steps for the task of throwing.6 She order a developmental sequence. ing. A television monitor was used to
found that children might demonstrate An additional assumption is possible, view the tapes during data analysis.
upper extremity action corresponding to however, that allows study of a single
a description of one level of develop- age group for the purpose of identifying Procedure
ment and at the same time demonstrate developmental sequences. That assump-
trunk action corresponding to a differ- tion proposes that motor development I recorded each subject performing 10
ent developmental step. Because de- is an orderly process. Developmental successive trials of rising from a supine
scriptions of developmental levels for change in motor behavior is believed to position. I instructed each subject to lie
throwing were inadequate, Roberton occur in a sequence of identifiable steps. supine on the mat and on my signal
adopted a "component approach" to A logical corollary predicts that at any "Go" to stand up as quickly as possible.
movement description.6 She broke total point in the life span, individuals should I used the preliminary instruction to
body action down into constituent parts demonstrate motor behavior character- stand quickly to facilitate automaticity
and then described movement within istic of their level within a develop- in the subjects' movements. The sub-
these specific regions of the body. Ap- mental sequence. If an individual is in jects received no other instructions con-
plying this method in a longitudinal transition between steps of a develop- cerning how the movements were to be
study of development of the overarm mental sequence, behaviors representa- performed, although I occasionally pro-
throw in children, Roberton discovered tive of adjacent developmental steps vided them with indiscriminate praise
that developmental change occurred at should be evident.6 Individuals who such as "Good" or "Great" to acknowl-
different rates in different components demonstrate variability in motor pat- edge their efforts. Intervals between
of body action.7 That is, the children terns while performing several trials of trials were self-paced by the subjects, but
demonstrated change in the movement a task, therefore, may be demonstrating in no instance exceeded one minute.
pattern used in one component of body movement patterns that represent adja-
action, while the movement pattern cent steps in a developmental sequence. Data Reduction
used in another component remained
stable across the same time interval. In METHOD Body action was divided into three
addition, different children developed at components: 1) the upper extremities
Subjects
different rates within each component (UEs), 2) the axial (head-trunk) region,
of body action. Thirty-two adults (17 women, 15 and 3) the lower extremities (LEs). Con-
A major assumption of my study is men) comprised the study sample. I ac- centrating first on the axial region only,
that motor development is a lifelong cepted subjects into the study who were I played back the videotapes using both
process: Developmental or age-related at least 20 years old and no older than slow-motion and stop-action capabili-
change in movement patterns that may 35 years. Their mean age was 28.6 years. ties of the VCR system and wrote de-
not be directly attributed to learning The sample was one of convenience, scriptions of movements of the head and
may occur throughout the human life with subjects recruited from the campus trunk for each subject's performance
span. Such an assumption allows the use of the University of Wisconsin-Madi- during the first trial. I then repeated this
of adult subjects in a developmental son. I eliminated any subject who re- procedure for the 2nd through 10th
study. Traditionally, the term develop- ported acute or chronic physical or med- trials in succession and compared my
ment refers to age-related behavioral ical conditions that could limit physical written descriptions for similarities and

186 PHYSICAL THERAPY


RESEARCH
differences. Where similarities of action TABLE 1 two possibilities is likely to be the devel-
were apparent, I wrote more general Number of Trialsa in Each Category opmental sequence for younger or older
descriptions of head and trunk move- of Movement individuals. I used this procedure to
ments that could serve as categorical identify a developmental sequence for
Subject Category
descriptions of axial component action. each of the three components and then
I reviewed the videotapes as the cate- Number X X' XX XX' referred to the works of Schaltenbrand3
gories were formed to ensure their ac- 1 6 4 and McGraw4 to help select the likely
curacy. This process was continued until 2 9 1 developmental sequences for younger
all trials across all subjects could be clas- 3 8 2 subjects.
sified into one of the categorical descrip- 4 6 4
tions of axial component action. 5 6 4 RESULTS
I repeated the process to reduce the 6 4 6
data for each of the other two compo- 7 4 6 Categories of Component Action
8 9 1
nents, the UEs and the LEs. After action Similarities and differences in the sub-
9 9 1
categories were formed for each com- 10 8 1 1 jects' rising movements resulted in five
ponent, I reviewed the videotape and 11 3 7 categories of UE action, four categories
used the categories to classify move- 12 1 8 1 of LE action, and four categories of axial
ments within each component across all 13 1 9 movement. The action categories are
trials and all subjects. a delineated for the UE, LE, and axial
Each subject performs 10 trials of the
Data Analysis movement task. components in Tables 3, 4, and 5,
respectively.
Reliability of categorical descriptions.
I trained two raters to analyze righting Specifically, I constructed a table for
Reliability of
movements with reference to the com- each component that included each cat-
Categorical Descriptions
ponent categorical descriptions. After egory of action and the number of trials
training, each rater independently clas- each subject demonstrated in each cat- I attained greater than 90% of exact
sified 50 randomly selected trials of the egory (Tab. 1). I then rearranged the agreement with each of the two trained
subjects' performances. I compared the categories of action (represented by col- raters when we independently catego-
raters' classifications to my original clas- umns of the table) until an order was rized component action in a randomly
sification of these trials by calculating identified in which each subject varied selected set of 50 trials. I attained greater
percentages of exact agreement. If less only between adjacent categories (Tab. than 95% of exact agreement when I
than 90% of exact agreement was ob- 2). Using this method, the reverse order recategorized component action in this
tained for any component, I met with also is always a potential ordering for same set of 50 trials. The percentages of
the raters to clarify the reasons for disa- the developmental sequence. That is, if exact agreement are reported in Ta-
greement. We worked together to refine action categories XX, X', XX', and X ble 6.
the categorical descriptions of compo- are one order in which all subjects vary
nent action or to generate decision rules between adjacent steps, then the order- Description of Righting
to improve the consistency of classifying ing X, XX', X', and XX also is a poten- in Adults
component action. I then randomly se- tial ordering of the developmental se-
lected another set of 50 trials, and we quence. The researcher either must refer The frequency with which each cate-
repeated this process until 90% or to previous developmental studies of the gory of movement appeared across trials
greater of exact agreement was obtained task or must hypothesize which of the is presented for the UE, LE, and axial
for all three components. I also reclas- components in Tables 3, 4, and 5, re-
sified a randomly selected set of 50 trials spectively. In the UE component, a
to determine intrarater agreement. TABLE 2 symmetrical pushing pattern was most
Order in Which Subjects Vary Among common. The most frequently observed
Description of righting in adults. The Adjacent Categoriesa
percentage of trials observed in each action of the axial region was symmet-
category of component action was cal- Subject Category rical flexion followed by extension. The
culated to portray the frequency of oc- Number LEs most commonly demonstrated an
XX X' XX' X
currence of each type of component ac- asymmetrical squatting pattern.
tion. I also tabulated "profiles," the 1 4 6 Within this sample of 32 adults, 21
2 9 1
combinations of UE, LE, and axial com- different combinations of component
3 2 8
ponent categories, displayed by each 4 4 6
action appeared across the 320 trials of
subject on each trial and then deter- 5 4 6 rising. Thirteen of these combinations,
mined the mode profile for each subject. 6 6 4 or profiles, occurred as the mode per-
The frequency with which different sub- 7 6 4 formance of at least one subject. The
ject mode profiles occurred across the 8 1 9 different subject mode profiles and their
sample was determined, and these data 9 1 9 frequency of occurrence are listed in
were then used to characterize adult 10 1 1 8 Table 7.
body action in rising. 11 3 7 The most common profile in rising
12 8 1 1 was characterized by symmetry of
Developmental sequences. I used the
13 1 9
records of those subjects who varied movement within each component.
within a component to identify a devel- a
Order of development is proposed to be These subjects pushed symmetrically
opmental sequence for that component. XX, X', XX', and X. with the UEs as they flexed their heads

Volume 68 / Number 2, February 1988 187


and trunks symmetrically forward and TABLE 3
flexed their LEs assuming a symmetrical Percentage of Occurrence Across Trials (N = 320) for Upper Extremity (UE)
squat pattern. Extension of the LEs and Component Categories
axial region brought the body to erect
stance as the UEs were lifted from the Occurrence
Category Description
support surface (Fig. 1). Although this (%)
profile was most common, it was ob- A—Push and reach to One hand is placed on the support 12.2
served as the mode in only 8 of the 32 symmetrical push surface beside the pelvis. The
subjects. Another eight subjects differed other UE reaches across the
from this symmetrical profile only in body, and the hand is placed on
the support surface. Both hands
LE action. These individuals either
push against the support surface
demonstrated an asymmetrical squat to an extended elbow position.
(Fig. 2) or lost their balance when at- The UEs are then lifted and used
tempting to rise from a symmetrical for balance.
squatting position, which resulted in a B—Push and reach One hand is placed on the support 27.5
stepping action. surface beside the pelvis. The
Four subjects rose by flexing and ro- other UE reaches out to assist in
balance throughout the move-
tating their trunk to one side, while
ment. The supporting UE pushes
pushing with one UE and reaching with into extension and is then lifted,
the other (Fig. 3). The LEs moved assisting in balance.
through a half-kneeling pattern in as- C—Symmetrical push to Both hands are placed on the sup- 10.6
suming the standing position. Another push and reach port surface, one on each side of
four subjects rolled to face the support the pelvis. Both hands push
surface, with one UE reaching across the against the support surface as
body while the other pushed against the the trunk moves forward. One
mat keeping the abdomen from con- hand leaves the support surface
tacting the surface. The LEs were either before the other to assist in
brought to an asymmetrical squat pat- balance.
D—Symmetrical push Both hands are placed on the sup- 46.6
tern or moved through a half-kneeling port surface, one on each side of
pattern. Both UEs then pushed on the the pelvis. Both hands push
support surface, elevating the trunk to- against the support surface be-
ward a horizontal position with the ven- fore the point when the UEs are
tral surface of the trunk facing the mat. lifted simultaneously and used to
The UEs were then lifted as the axial assist in balance.
region and LEs were extended vertically. E—Symmetrical reach The UEs reach forward, leading the 3.1
trunk, and are used to assist
Of the remaining body action profiles,
in balance throughout the
four were seen as the mode in at least movement.
two individuals, and another three were TOTAL 100.0
idiosyncratic. Eight additional body ac-
tion profiles were observed in the sample
but were not the mode performance of The records of nine subjects who category C appeared to be using a com-
any subject. demonstrated variability in head-trunk bination of action seen in categories B
action across 10 trials of rising were and D. That is, they began the move-
Developmental Sequences for analyzed to identify a developmental ment by pushing symmetrically, as in
Each Component sequence for this component. The hy- category D, but then switched to push-
pothesized order of development for the ing with just one UE later in the move-
Variability in component action axial component is presented in Table ment as in category B. I interpreted
within subjects permitted me to identify 5, with category A proposed to be the category C as a transitional pattern that
a developmental sequence for the LE earliest appearing step in the sequence may appear in individuals moving be-
and axial components. For each com- and successive steps labeled in alphabet- tween categories B and D, but believed
ponent, the subjects demonstrating var- ical order. category C was not necessarily a devel-
iability across trials of rising are listed in Eight subjects demonstrated variabil- opmental step. I, therefore, merged cat-
Table 8. Thirteen subjects demonstrated ity in UE action. The movement cate- egory C with category B. Combining
variability in LE action across their 10 gories could not be arranged into an categories B and C into a single descrip-
trials. Analysis of this variability re- order in which each subject varied only tive category enabled me to hypothesize
vealed a sequence in which no subject between adjacent steps. A sequence was a developmental sequence in which no
varied to other than adjacent categories. found in which two subjects varied be- individual varied between other than
That sequence is presented in Table 4, tween categories B and D without dem- adjacent steps. The proposed four-step
with category A proposed to be the ear- onstrating category C. At this point, I sequence of development for UE action
liest appearing of the developmental reinspected the categorical descriptions in the rising task is presented in Table
steps identified for LE action in this and videotapes of all subjects demon- 9. This sequence presents a revised de-
task. Successive steps follow in alpha- strating categories B, C, or D in the UE scription of UE category B that incor-
betical order. component. Individuals demonstrating porates asymmetrical movements of the

188 PHYSICAL THERAPY


RESEARCH

TABLE 4
Implications for
Percentage of Occurrence Across Trials (N = 320) for Lower Extremity (LE) Clinical Practice
Component Categories
The relatively high degree of intersub-
Occurrence ject variability, evidenced by 21 differ-
Category Description
(%) ent combinations of component action,
A—Half kneel The LEs are brought toward the trunk 15.9 demonstrates the many forms of rising
assuming an asymmetrical crossed- that are possible. The results of my study
leg position with one foot and the refute the notion that all adults perform
opposite thigh contacting the sup- therisingtask using the same movement
port surface. Body weight is trans- patterns. From a practical perspective,
ferred from the thigh to the knee of this variability provides the physical
the same LE, as the body is ro- therapist with options when selecting
tated over the LEs into a half-kneel-
movement patterns to be used when
ing position. Weight is then trans-
ferred to the opposite foot as the
teaching the task of rising. Which com-
LEs extend. bination of movements might be an ap-
B—Asymmetrical squat One or both LEs are brought toward 40.9 propriate set for a specific patient is now
the trunk, assuming an asymmetri- a question of interest. Should a patient
cal or crossed-leg position with the be trained to perform the rising task
soles of the feet contacting the using the profile most commonly en-
support surface. The LEs (or LE if countered in their age group? Is one
one remained extended) push(es) form of rising more "efficient" than an-
up to an extended position. Cross- other? Are certain body dimensions re-
ing or asymmetry may be corrected
lated to the use of component move-
during the extension phase by cir-
cumduction or stepping action.
ment categories? Do biomechanical
C—Symmetrical squat with The LEs are flexed synchronously 16.9 constraints restrict an individual's
balance step and symmetrically, placing the choice of combinations of component
soles of the feet on the support action during the rising task? Surely,
surface. Foot placement is adjusted knowing the answers to such questions
before extension or at the end of could provide a degree of specificity for
straightening by stepping or physical therapists concerned with mo-
hopping. tor reeducation that is yet unrealized.
D—Symmetrical squat The LEs are brought symmetrically 26.3
into flexion with the heels approxi- Developmental Implications
mating the buttocks. Weight is
transferred from buttocks to the Theoretically, the component ap-
feet, and the LEs then extend proach offers a more insightful model
vertically. of motor development than previous ap-
TOTAL 100.0 proaches. Previous studies ofrisingwere
based on a model that views neuromo-
tor development as a process of heir-
UEs, including those that begin with a when faced with the task of retraining archical integration of reflexes that
symmetrical push pattern but end with individuals to perform these functional ultimately come under volitional or cor-
asymmetrical use of the UEs. motor skills. In addition, the component tical control.3,5 When the cortex exerted
method could be used more extensively control over motor behavior, develop-
in pathokinesiology to describe and ment was considered to be complete.
DISCUSSION characterize movement disorders result- The model of neuromotor organization
ing from neuromuscular or musculo- suggested by the studies of Kuypers
Utility of Component Approach skeletal dysfunction. Indeed, Brunns- and colleagues proposes that axial and
The results of this study illustrate the trom has proposed that recovery from limb regions may be primarily con-
usefulness of using the component stroke may proceed at different rates trolled through medial and lateral neu-
method of movement analysis for de- within different regions of the body.9 roanatomical structures, respectively.10
scription of motor behavior in tasks of The results of the study also imply Subsequent primate studies based on
interest to physical therapists. The level that biomechanical studies of motor Kuypers' model suggested that motor
of detail in description seems to be ap- tasks such as rising from a supine posi- development may be viewed as progres-
propriate as a beginning step toward tion would best be performed within sive dissociation of limb movements
characterizing movement patterns used component action categories or a single from an early linkage with axial move-
to accomplish such fundamental motor body action profile. The kinesiological ment.11 The component method of
tasks as rising from a supine position, differences between the various cate- movement analysis is ideally suited to
assuming a sitting position, or rolling. gories of component action are too great examination of the relationship between
This method may lead to more precise to be ignored, particularly if movements movement patterns occurring in differ-
descriptions of body movements for are to be described in terms of kinematic ent regions of the body during the proc-
tasks that previously have been de- variables such as angular displacements, ess of development.
scribed only in general terms. For phys- velocities, and accelerations of body The findings of this study raise several
ical therapists, such detail is necessary segments. additional issues. First, applying the

Volume 68 / Number 2, February 1988 189


TABLE 5
adjacent criterion to order action cate-
Percentage of Occurrence Across Trials (N = 320) for Axial Component Categories gories demonstrated by adults into
developmental sequences has not been
Occurrence reported previously. Although Roberton
Category Description
(%) successfully used this criterion to iden-
A—Full rotation, abdomen up The head and trunk flex and ro- 14.4 tify developmental sequences within
tate to the side. Rotation con- components for the task of throwing,
tinues until the ventral surface she initially identified the movement
of the trunk faces, but does patterns that were to become develop-
not contact, the support sur-
mental steps by using children as sub-
face. The pelvis is then ele-
vated to or above the level of
jects.6 Whether the component action
the shoulder girdle. The back categories evident in adult subjects are
extends from this position representative of developmental steps in
vertically, with or without ac- movement used to right the body and
companying rotation of the whether the proposed sequences are or-
trunk. dered correctly can only be answered by
B—Partial rotation Flexion and rotation of the head 19.7 further study. Although longitudinal
and trunk bring the body to a study is the ultimate validation proce-
side-facing position, with the dure for the sequences, a more practical
trunk inclined slightly forward
approach might be a cross-sectional
of the vertical plane. The
trunk extends vertically, with
study of individuals of different ages.
or without accompanying The purpose of a cross-sectional study
rotation.
would be to determine whether the ac-
C—Symmetrical, interrupted The head and trunk begin to flex 19.7 tion categories rise and fall in frequency
by rotation forward symmetrically. The in the order predicted.8
symmetrical movement is in- Assuming that these categories do rep-
terrupted by rotation to one resent developmental differences, an-
side or by extension with ro- other issue arises. Do these various
tation. Forward movement forms of component action observed in
then continues until the head
and trunk are forward of the
vertical plane. The trunk may
rotate counterclockwise dur-
TABLE 6
ing extension to the upright
Percentages of Exact Agreement Across
position. A frontal or diagonal
Trials (N = 50) by Component
facing may result.
D—Symmetrical The head and trunk move for- 46.2 Component
ward symmetrically past the
Rater UE LE Axial
vertical plane; the back then
extends symmetrically to the (%) (%) (%)
upright position. 1 vs 2 92 94 100
1 vs 3 94 94 100
TOTAL 100.0
1 vs 1 98 96 96

Fig. 1. Most common form of rising to a standing position: upper extremity component, symmetrical push; axial component, symmetrical; lower
extremity component, symmetrical squat.

Fig. 2. Second most common form of rising to a standing position: upper extremity component, symmetrical push; axial component, symmetrical;
lower extremity component, asymmetrical squat.

190 PHYSICAL THERAPY


RESEARCH
adults possibly represent lack of progress TABLE 7
to advanced symmetrical form in rising Profiles Demonstrated as Mode Performance by Subjects (N = 32)
or might they represent developmental
regression? Are these movement pat- Component Number
terns specific to young adults and, there- UE Axial LE of Subjects
fore, different from those seen in
Symmetrical push symmetrical symmetrical squat 8
younger and older individuals? Studies Symmetrical push symmetrical asymmetrical squat 5
of both older and younger subjects could Asymmetrical push and partial rota- half kneel 4
begin to answer these questions. reach tion
Symmetrical push symmetrical symmetrical squat, 3
with balance
CONCLUSIONS steps
Asymmetrical push and full rotation, half kneel 2
The component approach to move- reach to symmetrical abdomen
ment description is a useful method of push up
describing fundamental movement pat- Asymmetrical push and full rotation, asymmetrical squat 2
terns of interest to physical therapists for reach to symmetrical abdomen
both theoretical and practical reasons. push up
Great variability exists in the patterns of Symmetrical push to symmet- asymmetrical squat 2
push and reach rical, in-
movement used by adults in the rising
terrupted
task. Differences in adult movements by rota-
used in rising from a supine to a stand- tion
ing position may well represent different Symmetrical reach symmetrical asymmetrical squat 1
developmental steps within components Asymmetrical push and symmet- symmetrical squat 1
of body action. Only further cross-sec- reach rical, in-
tional and longitudinal studies of this terrupted
movement task in individuals of various by rota-
ages will support or refute the proposed tion
component developmental sequences. Asymmetrical push and symmet- asymmetrical squat 1
Until additional studies are performed reach rical, in-
terrupted
to further identify factors that might
by rota-
affect which component action pattern tion
is most appropriate for any patient, Asymmetrical push and full rotation, asymmetrical squat 1
physical therapists are faced with select- reach abdomen
ing from a wide range of possible move- up
ment pattern combinations when teach- Asymmetrical push and partial rota- symmetrical squat 1
ing patients to rise from the floor. reach tion with balance
steps
Asymmetrical push and symmet- symmetrical squat 1
reach rical, in- with balance
Acknowledgments. I thank Mary
terrupted steps
Baldwin, MS, and Randy Richter, MS,
by rota-
for their assistance in determining the tion
objectivity of the component categories.

Fig. 3. Third most common form of rising to a standing position: upper extremity component, asymmetrical push and reach; axial component,
partial rotation; lower extremity component, half kneel.

Volume 68 / Number 2, February 1988 191


REFERENCES
TABLE 8
Individual Patterns of Variability in Body Action Components 1. Bobath B, Bobath K: Cerebral palsy. In Pear-
son PH, Williams CE (eds): Physical Therapy
Subject Number of Trials in Category Services in the Developmental Disabilities.
Component Springfield, IL, Charles C Thomas, Publisher,
Number A B C D Ea 1972, pp 31-177
UE 4 9 1 2. Knott M, Voss DE: Proprioceptive Neuromus-
cular Facilitation: Patterns and Techniques, ed
7 3 2 5
2. Baltimore, MD, Williams & Wilkins, 1968
14 1 9
3. Schaltenbrand G: The development of human
23b 3 7
motility and motor disturbances. Archives of
25 1 3 6 Neurology and Psychiatry 18:720-730, 1927
26 1 9 4. McGraw MB: Neuromuscular Maturation of the
29 1 9 Human Infant. New York, NY, Hafner Press,
31 b 1 9 1945
Axial 1 2 8 5. Bayley N: The development of motor abilities
4 6 4 during the first three years. Monogr Soc Res
7 5 5 Child Dev 1:1-26, 1935
12 4 6 6. Roberton MA: Stability of stage categorizations
across trials: Implications for the "stage theory"
13 2 8
of overarm throw development. Journal of Hu-
14 1 9 man Movement Studies 3:49-59, 1977
23 3 7
7. Roberton MA: Longitudinal evidence for devel-
25 3 7 opmental stages in the forceful overarm throw.
31 1 9 Journal of Human Movement Studies 4:167-
LE 4 4 6 175,1978
7 9 1 8. Roberton MA, Williams K, Langendorfer S: Pre-
9 2 8 longitudinal screening of motor development
10 4 6 sequences. Research Quarterly for Exercise
and Sport 51:724-731, 1980
13 4 6
14 6 4 9. Brunnstrom S: Movement Therapy in Hemiple-
gia: A Neurophysiological Approach. New
20 6 4 York, NY, Harper & Row, Publishers Inc, 1970
22 1 9
10. Kuypers HGJM: Anatomy of the descending
27 1 9 pathways. In Brooks VB (ed): Handbook of
28 1 1 8 Physiology, Section 1: The Nervous System:
30 3 7 Motor Control. Baltimore, MD, Williams & Wil-
1 kins, 1981, vol 2, pp 597-666
31 8 1
32 1 9 11. Lawrence DG, Hopkins DA: The development
of motor control in the Rhesus monkey: Evi-
a
Category E does not apply to the axial or the LE component. dence concerning the role of corticomotoneu-
b ronal connections. Brain 99:235-254, 1976
Subject varied to nonadjacent steps in this proposed ordering of categories.

TABLE 9
Proposed Developmental Sequence for the Upper Extremity (UE) Component

Proposed Order of Categories Description

A—Push and reach to One hand is placed on the support surface


symmetrical push beside the pelvis. The other UE reaches
across the body, and the hand is placed
on the support surface. Both hands push
against the support surface to an ex-
tended elbow position. The UEs are then
lifted and used for balance.
B—Push and reacha One or both hands are placed beside the
pelvis on the support surface. One hand
continues to support and push against
the support surface as the other reaches
forward to assist in balance.
C—Symmetrical push Both hands are placed on the support sur-
face, one on each side of the pelvis. Both
hands push against the support surface
before the point when the UEs are lifted
synchronously and used to assist in
balance.
D—Symmetrical reach The UEs reach forward, leading the trunk,
and are used to assist in balance
throughout the movement.
a
Category B represents a combination of categories B and C presented in Table 3. Category
A is the same as presented in Table 3. Categories C and D were labeled as categories D and
E, respectively, in Table 3.

192 PHYSICAL THERAPY

Вам также может понравиться