Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 11

School Psychology Review,

2005, Volume 34, No. 3, pp. 41 5-424

RESEARCH TO PRACTICE
Yoga as an Intervention for Children With Attention
Problems

Heather L. Peck
Bethany Public Schools, Bethany, Connecticut

Thomas ]. Kehle and Melissa A. Bray


University of Connecticut

Lea A. Theodore
Queens College

Absiract. A multiple baseline design across three grade level groups with a com-
parison group was employed to investigate the effectiveness of yoga for improv-
ing time on task with 10 elementary school children who evidenced attention prob-
lems. A yoga videotape, published by Gaiam. was used that required the children
to follow an adult instructor and three children who engaged in deep breathing,
physical postures, and relaxation exercises for 30 minutes, twice a week, for a
period of 3 weeks. Time on task was defined as the percentage of intervals ob-
served that the students were orientating toward the teacher or task, and perform-
ing the requested classroom assignments. The results indicated effect sizes that
ranged from 1.5 to 2.7 as a function of the intervention. Effect sizes at follow-up
decreased, but ranged from .77 to 1.95. Peer comparison data indicated that class-
mates' time on task remained essentially unchanged throughout the three phases
of the study.

Developing effective interventions for gi.sts (2003) recognizes that inattentive be-
children with attention difficulties has been an haviors exist along a continuum from mild
important area of psychological research, as it to severe and advocates for school psycholo-
is a prevalent problem in schools today giststo address abroad range of attention prob-
(DuPaul & Stoner, 1994). Problems of inat- lems. not solely those students diagnosed with
tention occur in varying degrees, with the ADHD.
most severe form often resulting in a diag- Students with attention problems often
nosis of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity fail to finish assignments, are easily distracted,
Disorder-Inattentive Type ( A D H D ) . The have difficulty listening to directions, concen-
National Association of School Psycholo- trating, and organizing their work, and most

Address correspondence regarding this article to Thomas J. Kehle. Department of Educational Psychol-
ogy. University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT 06269-2064; E-mail: Thomas.Kehle@uconn.edu
Copyright 2005 by the National Association of School Psychologists, ISSN 0279-6015

415
School Psychology Review, 2()t)5. Volume 34, No.

often require supervision to accomplish tusks stimulant medications have negative side-ef-
(Dawson. 1995). Not surprisingly, some re- fects for certain children, such as insomnia.
searchers have found that as many as 80% of appetite loss(Hai!eMariam. Bradley-Johnson,
children with attention problems also display & Johnson. 2002), obsessive-compulsive
academic performance problems {Cantwell & symptoms (Borcherding. Keysor. Rapoport.
Baker. 1991). Typically, school-based prob- Elia, & Amass, 1990), and growth suppression
lems such as academic underachievement are (Zeiner, 1995). Because of this, parents often
often related to poor study and organizational search for alternative treatment options.
skills as well as impulsivity. inattention, and Yoga is one such alternative that shows
hyperaetivity in the classroom (Faraone et al., promise as an intervention for a variety of so-
1993). Further, children with attention prob- cial, emotional, behavioral, and academic dif-
lems often demonstrate poor soeial skills sueh ficulties (Nardo & Reynolds. 2002). Although
as aggression and failure to adhere to rules that previous experimental research on yoga's ef-
often result in impaired peer relationships fectiveness specifically for attention problems
(Ernhardt & Hinshaw. 1994). In an innovative is somewhat limited, a few studies have been
study. Mannuz/a. Klein, Abikotf, and Moulton conducted. One of the early studies that em-
(2{)()4) compared 207 Caucasian boys with ployed yoga as a treatment for ADHD and op-
ADHD with matched controls and noted that positional behaviors was successful in reduc-
the diagnosis of ADHD was a precursor to de- ing inattention and impulsive and oppositional
velopment of oppositional defiant disorder, behavior (Redfering & Bowman. 1981). A
conduct disorder, and adult antisocial person- yoga program that incorporated meditation
ality disorder. decreased children's hyperaetivity, inattention,
There are several interventions identified and anxiety, and improved their peer relation-
in the literature tor addressing the difficulties ships and sleep patterns (Harrison. Manocha,
of children with attention problems. They in- & Rubia. 2004). Similarly, Jensen and Kenny
clude behavior modification and cognitive be- (2004) reported that boys with ADHD de-
havioral treatments (Abramowitz & O'Leary, creased their hyperaetivity. impulsivity. and
1991). and medication for those children with inattention after completing a yoga program.
an ADHD diagnosis. Behavioral inter\entions, Yoga incorporates physical postures.
such as token reinforcement programs, contin- breath control, mental concentration, and deep
gency contracting, and response cost, are com- relaxation to positively affect mental states
monly used with these children and have (Zipkin, 1985). It produces similar effects as
been found to be effective in improving relaxation in that it tends to result in feelings
classroom behavior (Barkley, 1990). Chil- of calmness. Yoga also tends to promote self-
dren with attention problems often respond control, attention and concentration, self-effi-
well to these interventions because they pro- cacy, body awareness, and stress reduction
vide frequent, immediate feedback and in- (Nardo & Reynolds, 2002).
corporate secondary, potent reinforcers. Simi- The coordination of body movements
larly, cognitive behavioral interventions have and stretching in combination with deep
been shown lo be effective, but also are rela- breathing improves the body's overall circula-
tively time-consuming and require consider- tion. This results in a release of tension as well
able resources for proper implementation as increasing levels of blood and oxygen
(Abramowitz & O'Leary). throughout the entire body that in turn affect
Treatment with stimulant medication is the central and autonomic nervous systems
another common intervention for children with (Brosnan, 1982; Laivani. 1999).
severe attention problems (Pelham. 1993). The autonomic nervous system, which
Stimulants have been found to have a positive primarily regulates involuntary activity sueh
effect on classroom behavior, social interac- as heartbeat and respiration, consists of the
tions, and academic performance {DuPaul. sympathetic and parasympathetic systems
Stoner, Tilly, & Putnam, 1991). However. (Seamon & Kenrick. 1994). Although these

416
Yoga for Attenlion Problems

systems generally control the same muscles tension, and improved concentration
and glands., they work in opposition to each (Brosnan, 1982), especially for attention
other (Carlson. 1998). The sympathetic sys- problems, would be conducive to learning
tem utilizes stored energy and prepares the (Hopkins & Hopkins, 1979). However, the ef-
body for "fight or flight" by increasing heart fect of yoga on direct measures of children's at-
rate and blood sugar level (Carlson; Seamon tention in the classroom has not been studied.
& Kenrick). In contrast, the parasympathetic Therefore, the purpose of this study was to ex-
division conserves energy and is active in re- amine the effectiveness of yoga on improving
laxed situations. Activities of this division in- attention in primary-grade children.
clude a decrease in heart rate and activation of
the digestive system (Carlson). In essence, the Methods
process of yoga deactivates the sympathetic
division and stimulates the parasympathetic Participants and Setting
system resulting in a sense of calm, emotional
Ten elementary school students with at-
balance, tranquility, and increased concentra-
tention problems (aged 6-10) across three grade
tion (Brosnan, 1982).
levels (Grades 1. 2, and 3) volunteered to par-
Although there is a dearth of current sys- ticipate in the present investigation. Specifi-
tematic studies in this area, some research stud- cally, they were recruited by the school psycholo-
ies have examined the effectiveness of yoga gist who made initial contact with the parents
on various aspects of students' functioning. and secured permission. The school psycholo-
Yoga has been shown to improve children's gist recruited students whose teachers had
concentration, as measured by scores on sought school psychology consultation services
coding (Hopkins & Hopkins, 1979) and due to the students' attention problems.
static motor performance tasks (Telles, The participants were not diagnosed with
Hanumanthaiah, Nagarathna, & Nagendra, ADHD, which is in accord with the National
1993). Additionally, the practice ot yoga has Association of School Psychologists' (2003)
been shown to reduce middle school children's position that school psychologists need to ad-
state anxiety, heart rate, headaches, and gen- dress a broad range of attention problems, not
eral tension and stress symptoms (Kalayil. exclusively those students diagnosed with
1988). Sloviicek, Tucker, and Pantoja (2003) ADHD. Criteria for participation in the investi-
examined the relationship of yoga instruction gation included documented attention problems
in an inner-city K-8 school to several signifi- in the classroom as evidenced by less than 80%
cant outcome variables. Although one cannot time-on-task, which has been cited as evidence
attribute yoga as the causal factor in this study ot anention problems (Rhode, Jenson, & Reavis,
due to the lack of a control group, students' 1993), during two structured observations con-
participation levels in yoga were all posi- ducted by the school psychologist, written par-
tively related to academic performance, ent permission, and verbal student assent.
positive student attitudes about themselves,
To calculate their percentage of time on
student physical fitness levels, and student
task, the students were observed in their class-
behavior. Moreover, the therapeutic poten-
rooms during their morning class work using
tial of yoga has been suggested for children
a structured Behavioral Observation Form
with psychomotor deficits {Hopkins &
(BOF; Rhode et al., 1993). Time on task was
Hopkins, 1976) that are often also exhibited
defined as the percentage of intervals observed
by children with attention deficits. A par-
where the students had eye contact with the
ticularly attractive aspect of yoga when
teacher or assigned task, and performed the
implemented with children is their reported
requested classroom assignments. The BOF
feelings of well-being and enhanced body
employed 10-minute observations using mo-
awareness (Hopkins & Hopkins, 1979). It
mentary time sampling with lO-second inter-
would .seem sensible that children's height-
vals. At least one of the two screening obser-
ened body awareness, calmness, reduced
vations per student was completed with an

417
School Psycholof^y Review, 2005, Volume .34, No. 3

additional observer (the school psychology vidual seatwork in their classrooms, most of-
intern), and adequate interobserver agreement ten in language arts.
was established (above 80%).
The students were from a suburban Dependent Variable
middle and upper middle class town in the
Time on task was defmed as the percent-
northeastern United States. Ot the 10 partici-
age of intervals ob.served where the students
pants. 3 were male and 7 were female; I was
had eye contact with the teacher or assigned
Hispanic and 9 were Caucasian; 2 were
task, and performed the requested classroom
mainstreamed special education students (1
assignments. The students were observed in
learning disability and 1 speech/language im-
their classrooms by the school psychologist
pairment) and 8 were regular education stu-
and/or intern using the structured BOF as com-
dents. The control students were a composite
pleted during the initial screening for inclu-
of the same gender children in each
sion in the study (Rhode et al., 1993) to calcu-
participant's classroom, with similar demo-
late their percentage of time on task. The BOF
graphic characteristics to the participants ac-
employed lO-minute observations using mo-
cording to the school's overall population.
mentary time sampling with lO-second inter-
vals. The student's behavior was observed and
Design recorded at the end of the lO-second interval.
A multiple baseline design across three The participants were observed approximately
grade levels with a follow-up phase that in- two times per week across all phases of the
cluded 10 children and composite controls was study (baseline lasted approximately 3, 5, and
employed. The students were grouped for the 7 weeks for Grades 3, 2, and 1, respectively;
intervention by grade level, as the experi- the intervention phase took place for 3 weeks
menter previously found children to be most for ail grades; follow-up data were collected
engaged in yoga exercises when they were for approximately 3 weeks for all grades). At
done in a small group as opposed to indi- the same time that each participant was ob-
vidually. Consequently, the grade levels served, the same gender children in the class-
were treated as the unit of analysis. Grade 1 room were observed to serve as comparison
included 3 students (Students H. 9. and 10). peers (the observer chose a different compari-
Grade 2 included 3 students (Students 5. 6. son child during each 10-second interval to
and 7), and Grade 3 included 4 students (Stu- form a composite). The school psychologist
dents I, 2, 3, and 4). The phases included and intern were both trained in observational
baseline, intervention, and follow up. Prior procedures as part of their graduate programs
to beginning the baseline phase, the school in school psychology.
psychologist scheduled with the cla.ssroom
teachers the time of the school day that the Interobserver Agreement
yoga exercises would be taking place for Twenty percent of observations across
each grade level. Because the observations the three phases of the study were coded by
during the intervention phase would be tak- both the experimenter and another observer
ing place immediately following the yoga to establish the degree of interobserver
exercises, the observations during the agreement. Percentage of agreement was
baseline, as well as follow-up, phases were calculated to be 91 (number of intervals with
completed at the same time of day to be con- the same rating divided by total number of
sistent across phases. The exact time of day of intervals multiplied by 100). Although the
the intervention varied by grade level groups school psychologist supervised the yoga
according to the most accommodating time for exercises and observed the students in the
the classroom teachers, although all grade lev- classroom, it was not believed that the chil-
els participated at some point during the morn- dren evidenced reactivity to her presence in
ing. During the observations, the children were that she often spent time in the classroom.
engaged in either whole group lessons or indi- Additionally, the investigator and additional

418
Yoga for Attention Problems

observer adhered strictly to the operationally three children engaged in deep breathing,
defined criteria for time on task to obtain valid physical postures, and relaxation exercises.
observational data, given the fact that they were This was done for 30 minutes, twice a week,
not blind to the hypothesis of the study or the for a period of 3 weeks. The students followed
treatment phase of the participants. along with the videotape in the school
psychologist's office grouped by grade level.
Treatment Integrity The school psychologist supervised the chil-
dren during the treatment. No behavioral in-
To ensure that the intervention was con-
terventions were necessary, as the children
sistently and accurately employed across stu-
complied with the directions and remained
dents, all components of the intervention were
engaged with the exercises.
compared to a checklist protocol. They were
checked oft only if completed as intended. The The videotapes depicted movement be-
protocol included the following: (a) all partici- tween various physical postures performed in
pants in the grade level group were present in an age-appropriate game-like manner. At the
the school psychologist's office; (b) all partici- outset of the exercises the children were in-
pants were dressed appropriately for the yoga structed to engage in simple warm-up exercises
exercises (i.e., comfortable clothes, shoes off); (i.e., jumping up and down like a "shooting
and (c) researcher played yoga videotape and rocket"). The video instructor described the
participants followed along with deep breath- postures using animals or objects in nature.
ing, physical postures, and relaxation exercises. There were various poses including the frog,
downward dog, and bridge. For example, the
Social Validity frog pose, from the age 3 to 6 videotape, re-
quired the children to squat down and make
All students completed a social validity "ribbet" noises. The instructor frequently re-
questionnaire, based on Bray and Kehle's minded the children to take slow deep breaths
(1996) index, upon completion of the follow- while engaging in the postures. Finally, the
up phase. The three-item scale assessed how videotape concluded with guided imagery
much the students liked engaging in the yoga where the children laid on their backs on the
fitness exercises, the frequency and duration floor envisioning peaceful images such as be-
of the yoga, and the overall effect of the treat- ing a seed that sprouted into a beautiful fiower.
ment on their ability to focus in the classroom.
The ratings ranged from 1 = "1 Disliked It Very Results
Much" to 5 = "I Liked It Very Much." Depend-
ing on the age of the students, the questions Structured Behavioral Observation
were read to them. Procedure

Treatment Historically, visual inspection has been


the most commonly used method to determine
The "Yoga Fitness for Kids" (Gaiam, single-case intervention effects (Busk &
2003; http://www.gaiam.com/retaii/product. Marascuilo. 1988); however, it has also been
asp?product%5Fid=91%2DOI92+MSTR) shown to be unreliable at times due to indi-
commercial videotape from Living Arts was vidual difterences and bias among evaluators
the treatment variable. Two versions of this (Wampold & Furlong, 1981). Thus, the addi-
videotape were used. The first grade children tional calculation of effect sizes for each grade
were shown the version designed for ages 3-6 level group was achieved by using Busk and
with an adult instructor. The second and third Serlin's (1992) Approach One: No Assump-
grade children were shown the version de- tions method. These effect sizes were used as
signed for ages 7-12 with the same instructor. an indicator of the practical magnitude and
No other materials were required. The video- importance of the yoga treatment. The effect
tapes required the children to follow the vid- sizes reported in the study were interpreted in
eotape that depicted this adult instructor and concert with Cohen's (1992) guidelines. That

419
School Psychology Review, 2005, Volume 34, No. 3

Table 1
Participants' and Composite Peers' Average Effect Sizes as a Function of
Grade Level and Phase of Study
Standard Effect Effect
Grade & Average Average Average Deviation Size Size
Condition Baseline Intervention Follow-up Baseline Intervention Foliow-up

3 Treatment 68.92 84.58 80.77 6.15 2J5 1.95

3 Comparison 87.88 87.21 87.65 4.64 -.14 -.05

2 Treatment 60.94 81.31 72.75 7.48 2.72 1.58

2 Comparison 82.81 80.97 82.17 4.29 -.43 -0.15

1 Treatment 65.48 85.28 75.56 13.07 1.51 .11

1 Comparison 83.97 84.89 84.11 10.82 .09 .01

is, a small effect should be .20 or greater, a did not return to baseline levels after the with-
moderate effect .50 or greater, and a large ef- drawal of the treatment would indicate that a
fect .80 or greater. "behavior trap" occurred (Hartmann &
The effect size for the average of each Atkin.son, 1973). Therefore, it was assumed
grade level group was derived hy calculating that the behavior wa.s maintained by environ-
the difference between the mean ot the baseline mental contingencies. Further, peer compari-
and the mean of the intervention phases divided .son data indicated that classmates' time on task
by the standard deviation of the ba.seline phase. remained essentially unchanged during the
Additionally, for the follow-up data, the effect three phases of the study (Table 1). Follow-
size was derived by calculating the difference ing intervention, on-task levels for the yoga
between the mean of the baseline and the mean participants were commensurate with those
of the follow-up phases divided by the stan- of comparison peers. However, the slightly
dard deviation of the baseline phase. The group positive baseline trends, together with the
data collected across baseline, intervention, and existence of a substantial number of over-
follow-up phases are depicted in Table 1 and lapping data points (see Figure I) suggest cau-
Figure 1. tion in the interpretation of the findings.
Effect sizes for the average for each Treatment Integrity
grade level group ranged from 1.51 to 2.72
indicating a large effect. Effect sizes at follow- At the end of each treatment session the
up decreased, but remained moderate to large data collector completed the Treatment Integ-
ranging from .77 to 1.95 for grade leve! groups. rity Checklist. It was determined that all ele-
Figure 1 depicts relatively stable levels of on- ments of the intervention were implemented
task behavior at baseline for Grades 3 and 2, with 100% accuracy.
with more variability in Grade I initially. One
Social Validity
session of yoga practice was required before
producing an effect on student behavior dur- The average of Grades I and 2 were both
ing the treatment phase across grade level 5.0 suggesting they "liked it a lot." However,
groups. On-task levels decreased slightly at the average of Grade 3 was 3.5. This 3.5 rank-
follow-up but generally remained higher than ing was an anomaly in that one child in Grade
baseline levels. The observation that the data 3 responded with a 1 meaning "disliked it very

420
Yoga for Attention Problems

Baseline Treatment Follow-up

100

80

60

40
3rd Grade
20

0
A

° 100 n
I 80-
^ 60 -

I 40-
2nd Grade
c 20
o
S 0
Jo

100

80

60

40
1st Grade
20-

0 1 1 1 r

Figure 1. Average percentage of time on task across grade levels.

421
School Psychology Review, 2005, Volume 34, No. 3

much" to Item 2, which asked her to rate the lated to classroom performance and academic
following statement: Doin^ the yo^i>a twice a achievement, as well as social validity from
week for three weeks. The experimenter asked the perspective of the classroom teacher. Fi-
the participant about her response and she in- nally, studies with a longer term follow-up
dicated that she wanted to do yoga much more would provide more data regarding the
than just twice a week. sustainability of the effects of the intervention.

Discussion Limitations

As stated earlier, the use of yoga to ad- Although the observations were con-
dress attention problems in children not for- ducted at the same time of day across phases,
mally classified as students with attention defi- the students were occasionally engaged in dif-
cit hyperactivity disorder is in concert with ferent activities related to their normal class-
NASP's position. Assuming that the results of room routine (e.g., whole group activities vs.
this study are replicated, yoga may become a independent seatwork). The type of activity
promising alternative or complement to behav- may have had an effect on the variability of
ioral and medical interventions that are com- the data. Additionally, the investigator served
monly used for children with attention prob- dual roles as both implementer of the interven-
lems. Of particular significance was that the tion and observer of the participants, which
practice of yoga facilitated by employing the introduces a potential bias that impacts the in-
videotapes was not only easily implemented, ternal validity of the findings. To reduce the
but was also enjoyed by the children. This in- potential bias, the investigator strictly adhered
tervention has the potential to be implemented to the operationally defined criteria for time
not only in small groups in the school on task. However, a future study could employ
psychologist's office but also with an entire an observer who is blind to the purpose of the
classroom. For example, the class could en- study and the phase of the participants. Fur-
gage in brief yoga exercises to help them fo- ther, comparison students did not present a
cus prior to taking a test or to settle into work control for attention problems, rather only a
after recess. comparison for demographics. Future research
could identify a control group of students with
Although there is a dearth of recent re-
similarly poor rates of attention to task.
search in this area, the findings of this study
are in concert with other studies that have These results make a contribution to the
shown the benefits of yoga for children. These literature on improving children's attention
include the improvement of children's concen- within educational settings. However, the mag-
tration, as measured by scores on coding nitude of these effects were calculated using
(Hopkins & Hopkins. 1979) and static motor Busk and Serlin's (1992) Approach One: No
performance tasks (Telles et al, 1993); the re- Assumptions Model. Although effect sizes pro-
duction of children's state anxiety, heart rate, vide a valuable supplement to visual analysis,
headaches, and general tension and stress the different techniques used to calculate ef-
symptoms (Kalayil, 1988); and the correlation fect sizes in single case research can yield quite
with academic performance, positive student different results (Parker et al., 2005). Conse-
attitudes about themselves, physical fitness quently, interpretation of the results of this
levels, and behavior (Slovacek et al., 2003). study should be made with caution.
Future research should include replica-
tion with different ages, and with different de- References
grees of severity of inattention. Also, future Abramowitz. A. J., & OXeary. S. G. (1991). Behavioral
studies should examine the effects of yoga on inlervenlions for the classroom: Implications for stu-
other indices of social competence such as pro- dents with ADHD. School Fsychoh^v Review. 20. 220-
234.
moting children's social skills, stress manage-
Barkley. R. A. (1990). Attention-deficit hyperactivity dis-
ment, and general well-being. In addition, stud- order: A handbook of diagnosis and treatment. New
ies should incorporate outcome measures re- York: Guilford Pres.s.

422
Yoga for Attention Problems

Bray. M. A.. & Kehle, J. J. (1996). Self-modeling as an Hanmann. D. P, & Atkinson. C. (1973). Having your cake
intervenlion tor stuttering. SchnnI Psychology Review. and eating it too: A note on some apparent contradic-
25. 35K-3ft9. tions between therapeutic achievements and design re-
Borchcrding. B. G., Keysor. C. S.. Rapoport. J. L.. Elia, quirements in N = I studies. Behavior Therupv. 4, 589-
J., & Amas;s, J. (1990), Motor/viH:al tics and compul- 591.
sive behaviors on siimulam drugs; Is there a common Hopkins. L. J., & Hopkins, J. T. (1976). Yoga in psycho-
vulncnbWWyl Psyvhiatric Re.^earch. 33. 83-94. motor training./^rademif Therapy. II, 461-465.
Brosnan. B. (1982). Yofiu for hamlicupped people. Liin- Hopkins. J. T , & Hopkins, L. J. (1979). A study of
don; Souvenir Press. yoga and concentration. Academic Therap\. 14,
Busk. P. L.. & Marascuilo. L. A. (1988). Autocorrelation 341-345.
in singltr-subjecl research: A counlerargumeni to the Jensen, P S.. & Kenny, D. T (20(14). The effects of yoga
myth of no autocorrelation. Behavioral Assessment. 10. on the attention and behavior of boys with ADHD.
229-242. Journal of Attention Disorders. 7. 206-216,
Busk. ?. L.. & Serlin. R. C. (1992). Mcla-analysis for Kalayil, J, A. (1988). A controlled comparison of progres-
single-case research. In T. R. Kriilochwill & J. R. Levin sive relaxation and yoga meditation as methods to re-
(Eds.), Single-case research tlesinn and analysis: New lieve stress in middle grade school children. Disserta-
directions for psychology and education (pp. 187-212). tion Abstracts International, 49. 3626.
Hillsdale. NJ: Lawrence Eribaum Associates. L-A\-An\.\.{\^9). Classic yoga for stress relief New York:
Caiitwell. D. P.. & Baker, L. (1991). Association between Sterling Publishing Co.. Inc.
attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and learning Mannuzza, S.. Klein. R.. Abikoff, H.,& Muulton, J. (2004).
disorders. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 24.88-95. Significance of childhood behavior problems to later
Carlson, N. R. (1998). Physiology of behavior (6'^ ed.). development of conduct disorders among children with
Boston: Allyn and Bacon. ADHD: A prospective follow-up study. Journal of
Cohen, J. (1992). Statistical power analysis for the be- Abnormal Child P.sychology. 32. 565-573.
havioral .sciences (2"'' ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Nardo. A. C , & Reynolds, C. (3002, February). Social,
Erlbaum Associates. emotional, behavioral, and cognitive benefits of yoga
Dawson. M. M. (1995). Best practices in planning inier- for children: A nontraditional role for.school p.u'cholo-
veniions for students with attention disorders. In A. gists to consider. Paper presented at the annual meet-
Thomas & J. Grimes (Eds.). He.-it practices in school ing of the National Association of School Psycholo-
psychology-m (pp. 987-998). Washington. DC: Na- gists, Chicago. IL.
tional AssiKiation of School Psychologists. National Association of School Psychologists. (2003, July).
DuPaul, G, J., & Sloner. G. l,\'i9A). Assessment and treat- Position statement on .students with attention problems.
ment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in the Retrieved on August 8, 2004 from http://
schools. New York: Guitlbrd Press. www.nasponline.org/information/pospaper_add.html.
DuPaul, G. J.. Stoner, G., Tilly, W. D.. & Putnam. D. Parker, R, I., Brossart. D. F., Vannest, K. J., Long, J, R..
(1991). Interventions for attention problems. In G. De-Alba, R. G.. Baugh. F. G.. & Sullivan. J. R. (2005).
Stoner, M. R. Shinn. & H. M. Walker (Eds.). Interven- Effect si7e.s in single case research: How large is large?
tions for achievement and behavior problems (p'p.b'i^- School P.mhology Review. 34. 116-132.
713). Silver Spring. MD: National Association of Pelham, W. E. (1993}. Phannacotherapy for children with
School Psychologists. Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. School Psy-
Ernhardt, D.. & Hinshaw. S. P. (1994). Initial socio- chology Review. 22. 199-227.
metric impressions of attention deficit hyperactiv- Redfering, D, L., & Bowman. M. J. (1981). Effect of a
ity disorder and comparison boys: Predictions from meditative yoga relaxation exercise on non-attending
social behaviors and from nonbehavioral variable.s. behaviors of bchaviorally disturbed children. Clinical
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psvchologv. 62, Child Ps\chology. 10. 126-127.
833-842. Rhode. G., Jenson. W. R.. & Reavis. H. K. (1993). The
Faraone. S. V, Biedernian. J., Lehman. B. K.. Spencer. T.. tough kill book. Longmont. CO: Sopris West.
Norman. D..Seidman,L. J., Kraus, l..Perrin,J..Chen, Seamon. J. G.. & Kenrick. D. T. (1994). Psychology. New
W. J..&Tsuang, M.T. 11993). Intellectual performance Jersey: Prentice Hall.
and school failure in children with attention deficit Slovacek, S. P, Tucker, S. A., & Pantoja, L, (2003). A
hyperactivity disorder and iheir siblings. Journal of study of the Yoga Ed program at the Accelerated
Abnormal P.sychology. 102. 616-623. School. Unpublished manuscript. Program Evaluation
Gaiam, Inc. Retrieved Septetnber 4. 2003. from http:// and Research Collaborative, California State Univer-
www.gaiam.coi}]/retai{/produa.asp.'product% 5Fid= sity at Los Angeles,
9I%2DOI92+MSTH (U\'mg Arts). Telles, S.. Hanumanthaiah. B., Nagarathna, R., &
HaileMariam. A., Bradley-Johnson. S.. & Johnson. C. M. Nagendra, H. R, (1993). Improvement in static motor
(2002). Pediatricians' preferences for ADHD informa- performance following yogic training of school chil-
tion from schiH)ls. School P.svchologv Review. JI. 94- dren. Ff»ii-e/7fu(i/(;n(/iW(*wr.%i7/.v. 76. 1264-1266.
105. Wampold, B. E.. & Furlong, M. J. (1981). The heuris-
Harrison. L. J.. Manocha, R., & Rubia, K. (2(X)4). Sahaja tics of visual inspection. Behavioral Assessment.
yoga meditation as a family treatment for children with 3. 79-92.
attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder. Clinical Child Zeiner. P. (1995). Body growth and cardiovascular func-
Psychology and Psychiatry, 9. 479-497. tion after extended treatment (1.75 years) with meth-

423
School Psychology Review, 2005, Volume 34, No.

ylphenidate in boy.s with attention-deficit hyperactiv- Zipkin, D. (1985). The relaxation techniques for handi-
ity disorder. you/vK capped children: A review of literature. Journalof Spe-
harmacology. 5. 129-138. cial Education, 19. 283-289.

Heather L. Peck received her PhD in school psychology from the University of Connecti-
cut in 2(K)I and is a practicing school psychologist. Her primary research interest involves
the use of yoga and relaxation and guided imagery to promote students' attention to task
and physical health.

Thomas J. Kehle received his PhD in school psychology from the University of Kentucky
in 1972andisaprofessoranddirectorof school psychology at the University of Connecti-
cut. His primary research interests involve interventions to promote students" academic
and social functioning.

Melissa A. Bray received her PhD in school psychology from the University of Connecti-
cut in 1997 and is an associate professor at the University of Connecticut. Her primary
research interests involve interventions to address communication disorders, disruptive
classroom behavior, and health-related issues.

Lea A. Theodore received her PhD in school psychology from the University of Connecti-
cut in 2002 and is an assistant professor at Queens College. Her primary research interests
involve the useof group contingencies to reduce classroom disruptive behavior, and inter-
vention to enhance physical health.

424

Вам также может понравиться