Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 12

Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 (2010) 11–22

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Environmental Psychology


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jep

Towards a developmental theory of place attachment


Paul Morgan*
Child Protection Unit, Sydney Children’s Hospital, High Street, Randwick NSW 2031, Australia

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Place theory offers no explanation of the developmental processes by which place attachment arises.
Available online 13 August 2009 Drawing on recent findings in human attachment theory, this study offers a developmental model of the
process by which place attachment emerges from a childhood place experience. A pattern of positively
Keywords: affected experiences of place in childhood are generalised into an unconscious internal working model of
Place attachment place which manifests subjectively as a long-term positively affected bond to place known as place
Attachment theory
attachment. Qualitative analysis of adult remembrance of childhood place experience provides support
Child development
for this model and finds important parallels in the developmental processes underpinning place
attachment and human attachment as well as some differences.
! 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction social place theorists as politically regressive in ignoring the social


forces by which the meaning of place is contested (Creswell, 2004).
The concept of place refers to the subjective experience of A second tradition, which Patterson and Williams (2005) name
embodied human existence in the material world. It is a paradoxical psychometrics, explores the relationship between the physical envi-
concept with a meaning that is readily grasped, but difficult to ronment and the human psyche by attributing numeric measures to
define. In their review of place attachment literature, Low and psychosocial phenomena such as place attachment and then ana-
Altman (1992) state that while place is an integrating concept, lysing this data using quantitative techniques. Grounded in the epis-
there is no systematic theory of place, and numerous commenta- temology of scientific empiricism, this tradition has been criticised for
tors since have echoed their concerns about the lack of conceptual reducing holistic phenomena to a mechanistic set of interacting
coherence in place research. Patterson and Williams (2005) suggest objective elements, and failing to provide any account of the subjec-
that no systematic theory of place has emerged because the domain tive aspects of the human experience of place (Malpas, 1999). The
of place research is composed of multiple research traditions based third tradition, social constructivism, while happy to embrace
on very different, often incompatible epistemological foundations subjectivity, sees it as a socially constructed phenomenon (Massey,
and philosophical assumptions about the nature of reality. Some 1994). Constructivist place theorists have been criticised for seeking
aspects of place research are best dealt with quantitatively, while to explain place solely in terms of the social processes and failing to
for other aspects a qualitative approach is more appropriate. They account for the embodied, individuated nature of subjective experi-
argue that if researchers grounded in any one research paradigm ence and the link that the body creates between subjectivity and the
recognise the limits to that paradigm and adopt an attitude of objective material world (Malpas, 1999). Following Patterson and
openness to alternative paradigms, their ‘critical pluralist’ frame- Williams (2005), this study recognises that a broad discussion of the
work provides an overarching coherence to the field. phenomenon of place attachment will draw on contributions to place
This study recognises three broad approaches to place theory, literature from each of these research approaches.
which often appear to be incompatible. Phenomenological and
humanistic approaches explore the deeper significance of place to
human existence and the subjective, emotional quality of people’s 2. Place attachment
relationship to places. This tradition has been criticised by posi-
tivistic place researchers for the lack of an empirical basis, and by Most authors recognise an emotional or affective component in
the concept of place attachment. But the word emotion, like place,
has an easy-to-understand, hard-to-define quality making place
attachment if anything, more conceptually elusive than place itself.
Giuliani and Feldman (1993) identify 11 different definitions of
* Tel.: þ61 2 9382 1412. place attachment in a single review collection of articles. In this
E-mail address: paul.morgan@sesiahs.health.nsw.gov.au study place attachment refers to the experience of a long-term

0272-4944/$ – see front matter ! 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2009.07.001
12 P. Morgan / Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 (2010) 11–22

affective bond to a particular geographic area and the meaning offers children. Hart (1979) and Olwig (1989) link place attachment
attributed to that bond. Where a person lives in a particular locale to opportunities the physical environment offers children for the
over an extended period, that person will often develop feelings of realisation of the developmental drive for mastery. Some authors
affection for, and a sense of belonging, or being of that place, so that attribute developmental significance to the self-directed and
place becomes ‘one anchor of his or her identity’ (Hay, 1998). pleasurable nature of children’s place-based play and exploration
Indeed Proshansky, Fabian, and Kaminoff (1983) coined the term (Cobb, 1977; Hart, 1979; Porteous, 1990; Schachtel, 1959).
place identity to signify the importance of the physical environ- Sebba (1991) argues that the developmental drive towards
ment in shaping the human sense of self. Early studies tended to sensory integration and the drive to obtain information about the
conceive of place attachment as static. More recently a dynamic environment underpin a heightened attentiveness to place in
view has emerged, so that while place attachment is understood as childhood. Also, a developmental shift in early adolescence from
enduring, it is also seen as changing over time (Hay,1998). the primacy of sensory to cognitive engagement with the world is
For many individuals, childhood place experience plays an accompanied by a dimming of sensory perception. Prior to the
important role in adult identity (Cobb, 1977; Cooper, 1992; Hester & emergence of abstract thinking in adolescence, sensory perception
O’Donnell, 1987; Pearce, 1977). Film and literature offer numerous is more vivid and pleasurable. Consequently, memory of childhood
examples of adult identity being profoundly shaped by childhood place is fixed in the context of an intense and ecstatic sensory
place experience. Hay (1998), investigating place attachment over awareness (Cobb, 1977; Sebba, 1991). For these researchers, place
the entire human life span found that feelings of connection or attachment to a greater or lesser extent is established through the
belonging to place increased as people aged, and that place developmental processes of childhood. Some argue that children’s
attachments formed in childhood were stronger than those formed observed preference for natural settings over manmade environ-
later in life. Strong bonds to place are only possible when individ- ments represents a universal developmental need (Cobb, 1977;
uals remained in their place of origin for the duration of childhood. Hart, 1979; Pearce, 1977).
This finding reflects a widespread agreement in the literature that However, it is difficult to reconcile this position with Chawla’s
the foundations of place attachment are laid down in middle (1986) finding that the experience of a positively affected place
childhood (Sobel, 1990). attachment is not universal. Place attachment quality and strength
Descriptive studies also indicate a qualitative difference between varies widely with some adults experiencing either no or negative
adult and childhood experience of place. Adult accounts of place feelings about their place of origin. The memory of childhood place is
attachment tend to highlight their feelings for place, the meanings central to adult identity for some, but for others, place has little
attributed to those feelings, and an awareness of the sociocultural bearing on their sense of self. Chawla identified seven different
influence on place attachment (Massey, 1994; Twigger-Ross & qualitative categories of place attachment, but explanation of
Uzzell, 1996). On the other hand, enquiries into children’s attitude differences in the formative process responsible for the different
towards place describe an unselfconscious, taken-for-granted strengths and categories of place attachment are vague.
approach to place, where the physical environment is valued for
what you can do in it, rather than in and of itself or for social 3. Attachment theory1
meanings (Hart, 1979; Hay, 1998; Moore, 1986). Descriptive studies
have identified common themes of children’s engagement with Two decades before Patterson and Williams (2005) offered their
place. These include: children’s preference for natural over man- ‘critical pluralist’ resolution of the problem of conflicting traditions
made environments (Jones & Cunningham, 1999); children’s of place research, Daniel Stern (1985) identified a similar episte-
sensuous engagement with place (Cobb, 1977; Sebba, 1991); mological impasse confronting developmental psychology. In order
exploration and place-play as inherently pleasurable, self-directed to progress, developmental psychology required an accurate
learning activities (Sebba, 1991); and children’s use of place for working hypothesis of infantile subjective experience. Stern argued
emotional regulation (Dovey, 1990; Kirkby, 1989; Korpela, 1989). that such a hypothesis of infantile subjective reality needed to
Qualitative studies of adults’ retrospective accounts of child- include both developmental psychology’s observed infant and the
hood places note the important meaning that memories of child- subjectively reconstructed infantile experience of psychoanalysis,
hood places take on later in life. The memories of those places can but that neither approach alone provided an adequate account of
evoke powerful feelings and exert great influence over adult human psychological development. He pointed out that some of the
identity (Cooper, 1992; Porteous, 1990; Rubenstein & Parmelee, tenets of psychoanalysis had been disproved by empirical observa-
1992). Where adults have migrated away from their place of origin, tions while developmental psychology, restricted to observation,
they can be prone to spending significant amounts of time revealed little of the ‘felt quality of lived social experience’.
reflecting on memories of childhood place rather than engaging
with their immediate surrounds. ‘Our places of origin shape us To relate observed behaviour to subjective experience, one must
whether we like it or not’ (Chawla, 1992). This importance attrib- make inferential leaps. As soon as we try to make inferences
uted to the autobiographical memory of place should not be about. the actual experience of the real infant – that is, to build
confused with veracity. Sebba found a marked disjunction between in qualities of subjective experience such as a sense of self – we
adult reports of a remembered childhood preference for outdoor are thrown back to our own subjective experience as the main
settings (96.5%) and children’s reported preferences for outdoor
settings (46%), indicating that adult remembrance of childhood
1
experience is subject to a degree of reconstruction and reinter- Strictly speaking, the use of the term attachment theory is a misnomer, as it
represents only part of this new convergence. This field of research and theory,
pretation. Nonetheless, the strong affects commonly experienced
currently in the throws of a Kuhnian revolution, is so wide ranging and rapidly
during adult remembrance of childhood place experience led expanding, that it is yet to attract a broadly accepted identifying name. Some
Chawla to argue that ‘this backward glance is an in important researchers have named it ‘infant brain research’. However, this name overlooks the
dimension of [place] attachment (Chawla, 1992). very significant social aspects of the field and restricts the age range of research. For
However, the processes that link adult identity with childhood its part ‘attachment theory’ does not explicitly include the considerable neuro--
scientific elements of the field. The term attachment theory is used here because it
place experiences are unclear. A number of qualitative studies of has wide recognition, because it includes those aspects of the field most pertinent
children’s use of place attribute place attachment to the support to this study, and because it has a complimentarity vis-à-vis the term place
and stimulation of human developmental processes that place attachment.
P. Morgan / Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 (2010) 11–22 13

source of inspiration. Here, then, is the problem: the subjective abilities are present, attachment is secure, and large numbers of
life of the adult is the main source of inference about the infant’s synaptic connections develop. However, where the attachment
felt quality of social experience. A degree of circularity is relationship is significantly disrupted, synaptic connectivity and
unavoidable. Each view of the infant [adult clinical reconstruc- psychological functioning are markedly impaired (Greenspan, 1999;
tion and observational] has features the other lacks. Rutter et al., 1997; Siegel, 1999; Zeanah, Boris, & Larrieu, 1997).
(Stern, 1985, pp. 13–17) Attachment relationships also influence the way memory is
structured (Siegel, 1999, pp. 29–33). Implicit memory is composed
Like Patterson and Williams, Stern noted that epistemologically of internal working models, the generalised representations of
conflicted approaches show a high degree of complementarity, and attachment interactions. Implicit memories do not require
the integration of objectivist, scientific approaches with subjective conscious attention for their formation, and normally lie outside of
clinical methods resulted in a new holistic conception of how the conscious awareness. They are fixed in childhood, and are charac-
human mind/brain develops. terised by the absence of subjective sense of remembrance. Explicit
This overarching coherence is widely understood to have origi- autobiographical memory, the memory of self across time, is
nated in the work of John Bowlby (1969, 1974, 1980). His attachment characterised by a subjective sense of recollection and narrative
theory, developed from naturalistic observations of infants and structuring. The storage of explicit memory involves a process
mothers, described attachment as infant behaviours that elicit adult known as ‘cortical consolidation’. Each act of explicit remembering
proximity and care-giving responses. These instinctive behaviours involves the reorganisation of existing memory traces into new,
have their roots in neurophysiological structures of the body. In unpredictable associative linkages. Internal working models have
a break with behaviourist orthodoxy of the period, Bowlby also an important influence over cortical consolidation, shaping both
explained attachment behaviours as being motivated by subjective the content and narrative structuring of autobiographical memory.
emotional states. He argued that long-term emotional bonds to Themes reflecting internal working models bring coherence and
particular individuals are a basic part of human nature. An attach- continuity to explicit memory (Siegel, 1999).
ment bond to someone endows feelings of security and wellbeing in
the presence of that person. Attachment behaviours are triggered by 4. Emotion and the self: emergent phenomena
feelings of anxiety and distress experienced by young children on
separation from parental caregivers (Bowlby, 1974). More recently attachment theory has expanded to include an
By using subjective understanding of emotional states to explain identity theory, self psychology (Cicchetti & Beeghly, 1994). It
observed infant behaviours, Bowlby integrated supposedly incom- describes how the psychological structure of the self emerges from
patible epistemologies to provide a holistic scientific theory of great the intersubjective context of the attachment relationship,
scope and explanatory power. Previously under the dominance of providing a biological developmental basis to social identity theo-
behaviourism, emotions had been dismissed as subjective ries. Infants communicate their biological needs by becoming dis-
phenomena, unmeasurable, and irrelevant to the objective scientific tressed. By attending to infantile biological needs and providing
study of the human psychology. However, with the development of soothing, attachment figures regulate emotional distress. With
a procedure known as the ‘Strange Situation’, attachment theory’s time and frequent repetition, infants establish an internal working
recognition of emotional states was validated using a predictive model of this care-giving role and through this process, learn to
empirical methodology (Colin, 1996). Attachment theory legiti- regulate emotional arousal for themselves. The process of
mised the scientific study of internal states and repositioned emotional regulation that emerges in the attachment relationship
emotions as central to understanding the human mind. plays a major role in establishing internal coherence in the infant
In this transactional model of human development, the (Schore, 1994).
simplistic nature versus nurture debate has been replaced by Brain imaging studies show that the processes of emotional
a model, where human development is shaped by a complex regulation and integration of mental functions both utilise the
interplay of environmental and genetic factors occurring in the same neural structures within the limbic system (Siegel, 1998).
context of the attachment relationship (Rutter, 2002; Sameroff & Emotions are now understood as emergent phenomena arising
Fiese, 2000; Siegel, 2001). From birth, infant and caregiver are from the integration of all domains of physical and mental activity
engaged in a reciprocal system of sensory stimulation and non- within the body (Damasio, 1998; Sroufe, 1996). The subjective sense
verbal communication of emotional states, and these sequences of of self (identity) arises from the experience of integration, the
interaction usually culminate in the shared experience of mutual holistic, internal organisation that emerges from the generalisation
delight (Schore, 1994). Frequent daily experiences of care-giving of repeated experiences of affect regulation within the attachment
and emotional attunement with the attachment figure are gener- relationship (Ciompi, 1991; Schore, 1994; Siegel, 1999; Sroufe,
alised in the infant’s mind into mental representations of self and 1996). Three aspects of attachment interactions are essential for the
caregiver that become unconscious psychological structures known emergence of a healthy sense of self: shared pleasure, soothing of
as internal working models (Bowlby, 1980). These internal models distress and repetition (Schore, 1994).
are enduring psychological structures and form the template for all
subsequent social relationships. Internal working models manifest 5. Lack of a developmental theory of place attachment
subjectively as the long-lasting emotional bond known as love
(Sroufe, 1990). The last thirty years have witnessed enormous advances in the
Attachment interactions also shape the phenotypic expression of field of developmental psychology and neurobiology. Attachment
brain structure. The vast majority of connections between human theory now provides a detailed, systematic account of the biolog-
brain cells are established postnatally. Advanced brain imaging ical, psychological and social processes that shape human devel-
techniques show the normal development of brain microstructure opment and has achieved the status of scientific orthodoxy. By
in infancy is highly dependant on the quality of the attachment contrast, place theorists offer no systematic explanation of how the
relationship (de Haan, Belsky, Reid, Volein, & Johnson, 2004; Schore, complex relationship between place, identity, affect and cognition
1997). The caregiver’s capacity to attune to the infant’s emotional develops throughout childhood. Place theory has failed to capitalise
state, and to engage in complex sequences of reciprocal behavioural on progress in developmental science. Lack of dialogue between
interactions are fundamental to these developments. Where these developmental psychology and environmental psychology’s place
14 P. Morgan / Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 (2010) 11–22

theory is apparent from the very limited referencing across these psychometric tool has not been widely repeated in environmental
fields in scientific journals. The few attempts to build a coherent psychology. However, the role of emotion is seen as central to
developmental theory of place attachment (Chawla, 1992; Hart, attachment theory. Place attachment theory could profitably make
1979; Moore, 1986) draw on theories that predate the recent use of both the detail and the epistemological approach of attach-
advances in developmental science. ment theory.
Low and Altman (1992) noted a growing interest in exploring Attachment theory recognises that subjective affective states
the ‘social relations that a place signifies’, and since the late 1980s drive observed human behaviour. ‘There is no action and no
the majority of studies have tended to focus on the social thought that is not affectively motivated. Motivation underpins
construction of place attachment. In that time most investigations agency and motivation is always emotional’ (Basch, 1988, pp. 68–
of the relationship between place and identity have relied heavily 69). The attachment theorist Lichtenberg (1989) has proposed that
on adult focused, cognitive and social frameworks, largely ignoring human behaviour is driven by a set of five ‘motivational systems’,
psychobiological developmental processes, as though place ‘designed to promote the fulfilment and regulation of basic needs.’
attachment arrives fully formed in adulthood (Jorgensen & Sted- Two of these systems, the attachment-affiliation system and the
man, 2001; Massey, 1994; Twigger-Ross & Uzzell, 1996). Place exploration-assertion system are of interest here. The attachment
attachment theory offers no systematic explanation of the forma- system motivates proximity and care seeking behaviours. The
tive processes by which place attachment in all its observed vari- exploration system motivates engagement with the environment.
ations emerges (Chawla, 1992). In spite of Chawla’s identification of According to Lichtenberg, the experience of exploration and agency
the need for research in this area, the absence of a systematic in the world produces positively affected sense of efficacy and
explanation of the process by which place attachment emerges competence. Lichtenberg (1989) suggests that behaviours promp-
remains a significant gap in the literature of place. ted by his exploration-assertion motivational system (play and
exploration) result in positive affect, motivating engagement with
6. Integrating attachment and place attachment theory the world other than the attachment figure.
The attachment system activates a positively affected care-
The environmental and developmental branches of psychology giving interaction between attachment figure and infant. Frequent
both stand to benefit from a greater dialogue between attachment repetition of this interaction results in patterning of the behaviour
theory and place theory. This study brings together recent devel- and associated emotional states, and the emergence of an uncon-
opments in these two fields. Attachment theory offers a theory that scious psychological structure (internal working model of the
provides useful insights for the elaboration of the developmental relationship) which manifests in conscious awareness as a long-
perspective that place theory currently lacks. However attachment term, specific affective bond towards the attachment figure. The
theory attributes no developmental significance to the child’s character of the internal working model (as template for all
relationship with the physical environment – place. Sameroff’s subsequent social relationships) is shaped by the generalised
(1975) transactional theory describes human development as quality of these attachment interactions. Where the positive quality
emerging from a relationship of mutual interaction between child of the interactions is significantly compromised by negative
and environment. The reciprocity and mutual influence between emotional states of the attachment figure, the attachment is
caregiver and child central to attachment theory exemplify trans- described as insecure (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978).
actional processes. Marvin, Cooper, Hoffman, and Powell (2002) have developed
In spite of place theorists identifying the important role that a model of child behaviour in relation to the attachment figure
physical environment plays an in human development (Chawla, resulting from the interplay of the attachment and exploration
1992; Hart, 1979; Moore, 1989; Proshansky et al., 1983), for motivational system. Their Circle of Security (COS) model describes
attachment theorists, transactional theory applies only to the social the child’s circular pattern of movement through the physical
environment, not the physical environment. The role of place in environment (called ‘the world’ in attachment theory), that begins
developmental processes remains largely overlooked by attach- and ends with the attachment figure. When the exploration system
ment theorists. Where the literature of children’s place describes is activated, the child moves away from the attachment figure to
a rich interactive relationship in which place nurtures and stimu- explore and play. When the child becomes distressed, anxious or
lates children’s development through interactions of play, explo- tired, the attachment system is activated and he seeks proximity to,
ration, sensory stimulation and emotional regulation, attachment and emotional regulation from, the attachment figure. The model
theory sees place only as a passive backdrop for the attachment emphasises the emotional states and developmental needs
relationship. In comparison with the detailed analysis attachment underpinning this circular pattern, and explains how a child’s
theory affords the human attachment relationship, the role of place developmental trajectory is shaped by their patterned experience
as a vital, interactive presence stimulating and supporting the of interaction with the attachment figure (Marvin et al., 2002).
child’s development remains uninvestigated. The physical environment has no role in this model. Motivation
Emotion is a crucial part of the relationship between person and to explore and play is located wholly in the child, rather than in
environment (Kaplan & Kaplan, 1984; Russell & Snodgrass, 1987; a relationship between child and environment. Striniste and Moore
Tuan, 1974; Wohlwill & Heft, 1987), and is central to the concept of (1989) contest such a non-transactional construction of the child’s
place attachment. However, emotion has proved ‘the most relationship with the physical environment. ‘Motivation [is] both
problem-fraught sector in contemporary psychology’ (Giuliani, a quality inherent to the child, which determines how the child will
2003), prompting Russell and Snodgrass (1987) to observe with use the environment, and a quality of the environment, which has
respect to environmental psychology, that ‘the relationship the potential to draw the child’s involvement’ (p. 25). Place is a vital,
between emotion and environment thus remains largely fascinating presence that draws in the child. Fascination is the
uncharted’. Because emotions do not easily lend themselves to the human response to environments or ‘circumstances that call on the
empirical strategy of measurement, psychometrics has largely effortless attention [and] are intrinsically compelling’ (Kaplan,
ignored them, favouring the investigation of behaviour and cogni- 1995, p. 172). The integration of this understanding of the physical
tion in order to progress psychological theory. Mehrabian and environment as an interactive presence influencing child behaviour
Russell (1974) developed a three dimensional model for measuring and attachment theory’s detailed interactional model of human
the emotional qualities of environments, but to date, use of this development points the way towards a developmental theory of
P. Morgan / Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 (2010) 11–22 15

place attachment. This study proposes a variation to the Circle of a child’s positively affected exploration/play/mastery and sensory
Security model to one in which the child’s pattern of behaviour is interactions with her environment is internalised into an uncon-
depicted as to and fro’ between two antithetical poles – physical scious internal working model of that relationship. The long-term
environment and attachment figure. affective bond known as place attachment is the conscious
Fig. 1 depicts this integrated developmental model of human subjective manifestation of that internal working model. This is the
attachment and place attachment. The diagram outlines the inter- process by which place attachment develops.
actional pattern emerging from integration of the attachment
system and the exploration-assertion motivational system. The 7. Research method
upper arc of the diagram represents activation of the exploration-
assertion motivational system. The child’s exposure to the physical This study explores if there is preliminary support for the
environment elicits arousal of this motivational system, resulting in developmental model of place attachment presented here, by
internal states such as fascination or excitement, and a consequent investigating if qualitative accounts provide grounds for the prop-
movement away from the attachment figure to exploration and osition that a long-term affective bond to place develops from
play interactions with the environment. These place interactions a childhood pattern of positively affected interactions with place.
generate positively affected senses of mastery, adventure, freedom The study uses qualitative analysis of semi-structured interviews of
as well as sensory pleasure. The lower part of the diagram repre- adults to look for indications of a process of internalisation and
sents the attachment motivational system. When interaction with generalisation of positively affected childhood place experiences
the outdoor environment elicits pain (through injury), or anxiety into internal working models of childhood place relationships
(through perceived threat or overlong absence from the attachment which manifest consciously as place attachment. It is argued that
figure), the attachment motivational system is engaged overriding qualitative accounts of subjective adult remembrance of childhood
the exploration system. The child seeks proximity to and comfort place experiences will contain common themes reflecting the
from the attachment figure. Interaction with the attachment figure influence of internal working models of childhood place experience
results in regulation of emotional arousal and a positively affected on explicit memory. Attachment theory also theorises that the
sense of connection. When the child’s need for connection and attachment relationship is central to the development of the self,
regulation is satisfied, environmental cues stimulate the explora- and the study also investigates whether there is further support for
tion motivational system causing it to override the attachment the model presented here (Fig. 1) in the form of participants’
system, reinitiating the cycle. This sequence results in a to and fro’ identification of a role for childhood place experience in the
movement between attachment figure and environment, and development of adult sense of identity that parallels the identity-
a cyclical pattern of emotional arousal, interaction and positive shaping role of the attachment relationship.
affect. In the study seven adults, five male and two females undertook
Using a pluralistic combination of attachment theory’s holistic a semi-structured interview. Participants in the study were selected
model of human development and place theory’s recognition of on the basis of having previously expressed adult place attachment
a transactional role for the physical environment in human devel- sentiments likely to indicate the presence of long-term affective
opment, this study proposes a developmental model in which place bonds. This approach reflects the phenomenological method of
attachment emerges from a child-environment interactional choosing ‘the best group of subjects available in our culture’ (Van
sequence of arousal, exploration/play and pleasure. An internal Kaam cit Crotty, 1996) to articulate the phenomenon being
working model of the attachment relationship develops from the researched. Participants were all middle aged (early forties to mid
patterning of repeated positively affected interactions with the sixties). In recognition of Riley’s (1992) emphasis on the concept of
attachment figure. In a similar process, the day-to-day pattern of readily accessible ‘ordinary’ places, and of the importance of

Fig. 1. Integrated model of human attachment and place attachment


16 P. Morgan / Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 (2010) 11–22

natural environments to children’s development (Striniste & 8. Results


Moore, 1989), and which abound in objects and processes which
elicit human fascination (Kaplan, 1995), participants were also The seven interviews yielded seven highly particular accounts of
selected on the basis of having grown up in locales where they had childhood place experience. In each interview, childhood place is
everyday access to natural environments either on suburban inextricably interwoven into broader biographical narrative. Indi-
fringes (4), or rural areas (3). vidual family members, family structure and culture, as well as the
The interviews were divided into three sections. In the first wider culture, and place itself, all interact to create a unique set of
section, participants were asked open questions about their places conditions within which a child experiences place. Adult place
and families of origin in order to position participants biographi- attachment cannot be understood without reference this broader
cally and geographically. These questions drew on exclusively on biographical frame, and a brief biography of each participant as
explicit memory. revealed in the interviews is included to contextualise exerts cited.
Phillip grew up in suburban Sydney with access to extensive
Can you tell me something about the place where you grew up?
open spaces of farm, bush, park and industrial land along the upper
Can you tell me about your childhood family?
reaches of Sydney Harbour. In his early twenties he spent some
Were there any particular special places that you remember?
years in Europe until he felt compelled to return home after
The second part of the interview process had some similarity encountering the unique smell of the Sydney flora at Kew Botanic
with the psychoanalytic approach of ‘bringing unconscious material Gardens. He still lives in his childhood home. Bluey grew up on
to consciousness’ (Frosh, 2002, p. 16), in order to arrive at the a small family farm of pasture and woodlands outside of Mel-
subjectively reconstructed childhood experience of adult psycho- bourne. This property became a refuge from a school life where he
analysis, which Stern (1985) identified as the ‘main source of infer- was unhappy and struggled academically. He lived on the property
ence about the infant’s felt quality of social experience’. The aim here until it was sold in his early twenties. He now lives in a suburb
differed somewhat from psychoanalysis in that the interviews were 25 km from the farm, which still has an important place in his
intended to reveal unconscious internal working models of place dream-life.
relationships rather than social relationships. To this end, questions Annie grew up on a large sheep grazing property in New South
focused on place memories and participants were encouraged to Wales. At age 12 much of the property was excised from her fam-
describe any involuntary place memories (Game, 2001) and asso- ily’s ownership and she was sent to boarding school. After school
ciations that came to mind, as these are likely to reflect unconscious she went to college, and lived in Europe for several years before
processes (Freud, 1976; Frosh, 2002). At the start of this part of the returning to care for her father. She now lives on another property
interviews participants were administered a passive relaxation 20 km from her childhood farm. Mick grew up on the outskirts of
exercise (Everly & Rosenfeld, 1981; Madders, 1981; Payne, 2005), Christchurch, New Zealand where he had access to the Port Hills,
a procedure intended to induce a mental state of unfocused reverie, a large grassy volcano remnant. When he was 11 his younger
which facilitates the emergence of unconscious material associated brother died in an accident as they played, and Mick became very
with childhood environments (Bachelard, 1969). Participants were socially withdrawn through his adolescence. As a young man he left
also encouraged to enter into the ‘felt quality’ of memories, in order New Zealand and now lives in Melbourne.
to highlight the subjective emotional and sensory qualities of Jim grew up on an orchard outside Adelaide. He left the family
childhood place memories and minimise analytic cognitive farm to attend boarding school in adolescence. He went on to
processes. Questions from this section included. university and a successful academic career in USA, but abandoned
this and returned to Australia to work as a farmer and environ-
Can you describe that place [you are remembering], how it mental consultant. He now lives in coastal bushland 1200 km from
looked, the sounds and smells? his childhood farm. Neal grew up in a suburb of Sydney with access
Can you tell me how you felt as a child in this place? to large tracts of forested parkland. His account of childhood local
neighbourhood was extremely limited, and focused instead on of
In the third section of the interview, respondents were invited to
holiday places away from home, and his bedroom. He now lives in
reflect on the meaning of their memories from an adult perspective,
the very urban environment of inner Sydney. Jane grew up in
particularly as to how they understood the relationship between
Canberra when it was a very small city with extensive open spaces.
their childhood experiences of place and adult identity.
At age 8 her family spent a year in England, and she experienced
clinical depression, missing her older brother, her dog and the clear
How do you think the experience of growing up in that place has
blue skies and trees of Canberra. When she returned her brother
shaped who you are now?
had left home, her dog had died, a much-loved tree was cut down,
Can you tell me what is it like for you as an adult to remember
and Canberra was never the same for her. Jane now lives in inner
your childhood experience of where you grew up?
Melbourne.
These questions were intended to explore cognitive meanings
that participants attributed to their childhood place memories. 8.1. Adult emotional connection to place
Data interpretation drew on phenomenologist, Giorgi’s method of
data analysis (Giorgi cit Von Eckartsberg, 1998) in which interviews Participants estimated that most place memories described in
were broken up into numerous ‘meaning units’, short sequences of the interview date from between the ages of eight and thirteen,
uniform meaning. Using the process of phenomenological reduc- with none dating from earlier than five. They expressed two
tion, broad themes emerging from the interviews were identified, broadly different degrees of emotional connection within the
and meaning units are coded according to those themes. Major interviews. Annie, Jane, Bluey, Phillip and Mick reported moments
themes, those with the highest aggregates of meaning units, were during the interview of intensely reliving childhood memories, to
interpreted as reflecting unconscious working models (Siegel, the point where the present-day context was experienced as less
1999), and detailed in the results. Finally, in a separate analysis, vivid, less real than the remembered experience. Such memories
longer interview sequences reflecting the arousal-interaction- were often involuntary. Jane: [That memory] came into my mind
pleasure dynamics of the proposed developmental model of place unbidden. I was intending to talk about other places, but this image
attachment (Fig. 1) were identified. was just there, too strong to ignore. These memories appeared in the
P. Morgan / Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 (2010) 11–22 17

form of a static image rather than a narrative structure, although in 8.2.2. Grief
most cases participants soon added a narrative context. Present day The most intense expressions of emotion connected with
surroundings faded to background when feeling states associated memories of childhood place were those of grief. Annie: [Losing the
with these images were experienced as being intensely relived in family farm] was like losing a limb. You know, I mean something vital
the present rather than being recalled. Recounted in the present had gone. With the exception of Annie, this grief was still keenly felt
tense, these memories lacked a subjective sense of remembrance. in adulthood. Mick and Jane experienced loss of childhood place
Although these experiences during the interview were relatively through migration, while Phillip expressed a deep sense of grief
brief they were very intense and participants attributed great over changes he has witnessed to his childhood place where he still
significance to them. Jane: I’ve always had those memories, but not lives. For Mick, Annie and Jane this place-related grief was inter-
with so much feeling. It’s like they’re in full colour rather than black woven with childhood losses associated with family. Bluey and
and white. Mick also reported experiencing feelings of intense emotional
This subjective emotional intensity was also conveyed in non- distress being soothed by time spent in childhood place. Neal and
verbal expressions such as gesture, facial expression, and vocal Jim denied any sense of grief associated with absence from their
tone. These moments of deep immersion in childhood memory childhood places.
were characterised by enormous intersubjective power. Sensory
impressions were described with great clarity and detail giving 8.2.3. Pleasure
these memories a vitality that was absent from other parts of the The emotion most frequently expressed throughout all the
interview when a more mundane adult cognitive sensibility interviews was that of pleasure/delight. Non-verbal expressions
predominated, and memories were described as distant pictures indicated that as they remembered during the interview, respon-
viewed from the outside rather than being relived from within. dents frequently re-experienced feelings of pleasure associated
Participants who experienced these moments of deep immersion with various childhood place experiences. Participants described
in childhood emotional states also reported strong adult place several different types of pleasure. PLAY: All participants enjoyed
attachments. Jane, Bluey, Phillip and Mick reported that as adults recounting memories of play-in-place. Jim: We made a racetrack in
they still experienced a strong sense of attachment to their there no one knew about. And it was curves, and rises and jumps and
childhood places. Annie said that she recalled having a strong stuff. And we would go and race bikes in there. I haven’t thought
emotional bond to her family farm as a child, but she no longer about that for a long time. That was a lot of fun. SENSORY PLEASURE:
felt this way. She did however report a very strong adult Jane: And this lovely sort of mellow kind of lambent quality to the light,
attachment to her current home, a similar landscape to that of as it is filtered by those beautiful green leaves of the willow. I just
her childhood. absolutely love it. It just fills me with the most buoyant sense of joy, like
Although they were known to have previously expressed I could just leap into the air. Participants expressed delight in
sentiments of adult place attachment, both Neal and Jim reported remembered childhood sensory experience of place, and stated
little sense of emotional connection with their childhood place they were re-experiencing the original feelings as they spoke.
experience. Their generally flat non-verbal expression, and These memories were often multisensory. Mick: The smells would be
a marked lack of sensory detail in their descriptions of childhood a mixture of sheep shit, pine, um a certain um crispness, crispness in
place added weight to these self reports. Neal said that he had the smells, and the sound is the wind in the tussocks. MASTERY: All
never felt a strong place attachment his childhood neighbour- respondents reported childhood feelings of pleasure associated
hood places, and in his interview he spoke only of special distant with a growing sense of agency, skill and achievement in the
places where holidays were spent. Neal: I wish I could recall more material world. Mick: The physical world is where I had most of my
of the detail of it.I don’t feel great attachment to [childhood pla- success and achievement as a kid, because I certainly had none
ces]. Jim did report a sense of place attachment. However, when academically and certainly very limited socially. ADVENTURE: Most
questioned further he could articulate little sensory or emotional respondents reported a delightful sense of adventure associated
detail to give substance to his attachment to either childhood or with exploration, imaginative play, risk and manageable feelings of
adult place. anxiety. Phillip: You could be anywhere in the world, you could
imagine yourself a pirate or you could play war games or any
adventurous sense of a story could be easily conjured up from these
8.2. Common themes of childhood place remembrance places and they always involved the outdoors. FREEDOM: An enjoy-
able sense of freedom was frequently expressed, usually in associ-
These reported differences in the intensity of emotional ation with the perceived absence of adult control. Annie: The sense
engagement with memories of childhood place offered one of movement through space, you know the emptiness, the wind, and
dimension in which narratives could be differentiated. Through the the freedom of being on a horse that was cantering along. You know
process of thematic analysis the following themes emerged from that’s the overwhelming memory of place that came back to me. just
the adult remembrances of childhood place experience. Excerpts a great feeling. In the absence of major trauma, childhood experi-
from the interviews cited typify the thematic expressions of ence of place appears to be inherently pleasurable. The process of
a number of participants. remembering childhood place was also described as enjoyable by
most participants, even where feelings of loss and grief were
8.2.1. Love experienced.
Annie: I was intensely engaged in the love I had for that farm. [It
was] really intense as a child. All respondents except Neal reported 8.2.4. Security
a love of childhood place. Five participants who still felt this love Mick: Being on the vine and snuggled into it, and feeling very
strongly, also described a reciprocal sense of having felt loved and contained and secure and comfy in it. Most respondents expressed
nurtured or nourished by their childhood place, and of having a positively affected sense of security associated with childhood
derived a sense of psychological wellbeing from this experience. place. This recalled sense of security-in-place appears to have
Phillip: I feel like I belong here. It’s like I just know this is my home, emerged from a strong sense of childhood familiarity with place
and um. I’m always going to be, I suppose nourished by it. Jane: described by six of the seven respondents. Mick: This tussock’s
There’s a kind of nurturing, nourishing space around you. familiar, the sounds are familiar, the bird-life’s familiar, the tracks are
18 P. Morgan / Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 (2010) 11–22

familiar, all the different brows, the contours and textures are familiar. hills. You’d have this [huge] valley to climb out of. You’d think ‘That’s
Implied here is a repeated, goal-less criss-crossing of childhood a long way up’. And away you’d go, put your head down and you’d
place, which Bluey named wandering, and which gave rise to this sweat it out for the next 45 minutes. And there was always [a sense of]
sense of familiarity. In marked contrast to other respondents, Neal’s ‘That’s great. We got there.’.I never felt defeated by that landscape,
accounts of childhood places were frequently coloured by feelings whereas in many other environments I felt quite defeated. It presented
of anxiety and lack of security. His account also differed in that it me with so many opportunities to engage in enough challenge that I
contained no sense of familiarity with childhood place or any felt stretched, but not so much that I was over-stretched. and [felt]
recollections of exploration and play-in-place independent of adult a sense of defeat. I think [that was responsible for my adult] sense of
supervision. The potential for the sense of place-security to be ‘must do’, my get-up-and-go. The physical world is where I had most of
illusory was powerfully brought home to Mick when his brother my success and achievement as a kid, because I certainly had none
was killed as a result of play-in-place. Along with the sense of academically and very limited socially.
personal security emerging from familiarity, the rapid pace of the Bluey attributes his adult aesthetic sensibility to a childhood
child’s developmental change, vis-a-vis the usually slow pace of fascination with and pleasure in the material form of place. I’d just
change to place, gave rise to a sense that childhood place itself is sort of wander, and there’d be points where I’d be literally observing
secure, permanent; a trust that place was always just out the every rock or old bit of car body, or anything that was intriguing me.
backdoor (Phillip). The sense that place itself is secure could also be Rocks, I just loved rocks, I was always fascinated by rocks.That
illusory as Annie found out when much of the family farm was lost. waterfall had good rocks on it. Mossy rocks.I’d feel them. And there’d
You know, . something vital had gone. be times of just wonder, you know like looking. I guess I was like I am
now with my [adult] intrigue of form. I love things that look intriguing.
8.2.5. Identity I love the material and how they’re placed. So yes, I was intrigued, I
Annie, Jane, Bluey, Phillip and Mick all attributed great impor- guess. There were definitely little pockets on the property that I liked
tance to the developmental influence of childhood place, stating more than the others. And I’d just. explore the rocks around you
that it helped form and still contributes to their adult identity. know, literally, in some areas, I’d know every rock.. I was very aware
Mick: Somehow those hills are connected to my soul.you know just of the environment. And I liked it, so that made me feel good to be
as I began to think about it a lot more, um [I realise] how bloody around that.
important it was. This is home. This is what shaped me. It was
shaping my identity. It was shaping who I was, who I am. This 9. Discussion
retrospective attribution of importance to the developmental
influence of childhood place was very different from participants’ While noting some variation in the place attachments reported
descriptions of their childhood attitude to place. These had a naı̈ve, by participants, this study found enough similarities in the majority
taken-for-granted quality, with place being a lot of fun or just out the of the accounts suggest a common process by which place attach-
back door. Unlike the participants who reported a strong emotional ment develops; and which shows many parallels with the devel-
connection to childhood place, both Neal and Jim denied that their opmental process described by attachment theory. Attachment
childhood place experiences made any significant contribution to theorists describe an interactional pattern in which the infant’s
their adult identity. distress (arousal) is soothed by the attachment figure’s care-giving
behaviours, and which culminates in a positively affected sense of
8.3. The arousal-interaction-pleasure model mutual connectedness. Frequent repetition of this sequence stim-
ulates and consolidates the infant’s internal developmental
The themes of adult recollections of childhood place experience processes, and gives rise to an unconscious internal working model
are captured in relatively succinct excerpts from participants of the attachment relationship, which is characterised by a long-
accounts. However, the more complex pattern of child interaction term affective bond (Ciompi, 1991; Lichtenberg, 1989; Schore, 1994;
with place depicted in the upper arc of Fig. 1 is only revealed in Sroufe, 1990). This study has proposed that a parallel pattern of
more extensive excerpts. The following passage from Bluey’s arousal-interaction-pleasure, shown in the upper arc of Fig. 1
account captures his place-induced excitement, his playful behav- characterises childhood place experience. Extended excerpts from
ioural response, and a strong positive affect associated with the interviews cited in 8.3 reflect this pattern of child-environment
sense of mastery resulting from that behaviour. [There] was a long interaction as well the developmental influence of childhood place
hill down to the house. I’d walk it and I’d [feel like] running, and I’d um experience on adult identity. These findings support the proposi-
get a real momentum up. And I’d start to leap. And that each time I’d tion that repeated enactments of the arousal-interaction-pleasure
leave the ground, I’d pick a point on the grass ahead of me to land, and pattern generate an internal working model of the child’s rela-
each leap I’d extend it more and more and the hill got steeper and tionship with environment, which manifests consciously as a long-
steeper, and by the point of the last bit of that hill I would just fly. I term affective bond to that environment known as place
would just be having such a momentum going, and I’d pick a point that attachment.
was way beyond what I could possibly reach. But I would always reach Five participants who reported strong place attachment senti-
it, and I’d feel like I’d be in the air, and I’d be like I’ve still got another ments also described involuntary place memories in the form of
two metres to go, and I’d make it, and I’d sort of make myself make it. vivid re-experiencing during the interview of emotional states and
So, that was always something I did [going down that hill. And the sensory perceptions associated with childhood place experiences.
feeling was] just fantastic, just fantastic. I was um. (smiles). I’d just These moments when childhood emotional states were re-lived
fly, literally fly and I would be doing something that I couldn’t do, but I during interviews in full colour rather than black and white
was doing it. demonstrated characteristics that Siegel (1999) attributes to
Other passages capture not only the pattern of arousal – play/ implicit memory. The absence of narrative structure and subjective
exploration – pleasure, but also link aspects of adult identity to this sense of recollection (reflected in use of the present tense in the
formative pattern. Mick reported deriving a great sense of interviews) suggest that such memories are part of implicit
achievement from ascending the 1000 metre high hills of his memory. As such, these potent, affect-laden images of childhood
childhood. He attributed his high levels of motivation characteristic place provide further support for the presence of internal working
of his adult personality to childhood experiences of mastery in the models of childhood place experience. The two participants who
P. Morgan / Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 (2010) 11–22 19

did not report these vivid involuntary memories during the inter- his place of origin. Neither reported any deep immersion in child-
view process expressed little or no sense of adult place attachment, hood experience suggestive of implicit memory during their
suggesting the absence of robust internal working models of interviews. Although both reported they experienced some place
childhood place. attachment as adults, their descriptions lacked noticeable
Siegel (1999) also suggests that unconscious internal working congruent non-verbal expressions that would have given them
models are reflected in the themes of explicit autobiographical greater substance. Also, neither Jim nor Neal reported any sense of
memory. This study argues that themes emerging from partici- grief associated with separation from childhood place. The lack of
pants’ remembrances of childhood place experience reflect internal detailed childhood place memories and strong place attachment
working models of childhood place experience and something of sentiments from the accounts of Neal and Jim suggest the absence
the processes by which they arise. The study found the five themes of strong internal working models of childhood place.
emerging through phenomenological reduction and these appear This range of results across all participants suggests some vari-
to support the proposed arousal-interaction-pleasure develop- ability in the developmental processes that give rise to adult place
mental model of place attachment. attachment. This finding accords with Chawla’s (1992) identification
The experience of pleasure as a result of childhood interactions of a qualitative spread of different types of adult place attachments.
with the environment is central to this developmental model of While it is possible that Neal and Jim’s accounts reflect a failure of the
place attachment. The presence of positive affect resulting from interview procedure to evoke implicit memory, the results are
childhood interactions with place is clearly demonstrated by the consistent with the explanation that their childhood place experi-
emergence of pleasure as the most prominent theme of the inter- ences were not generalised into robust internal working models with
views. It just fills me with the most buoyant sense of joy. That was strong subjective feelings of place attachment. This finding does not
a lot of fun. Participants associated pleasure with the activities of undermine the proposed model, but suggests that where childhood
place-exploration, place-play and sensory perception, as well as place experience is not consolidated into an internal working model,
with the subjective senses of mastery, freedom and adventure. The place attachment is weak. While Neal’s experience of childhood place
five participants who provided highly detailed recall of positively anxiety appears to have undermined the development of a robust
affected experiences of childhood place, also reported a strong place attachment, the reasons why Jim’s childhood place experiences
long-term affective bond to place, supporting the idea of a devel- did not consolidate into a strong place attachment remain unclear.
opmental link between positively affected childhood place expe- Attachment theory uses a continuum of anxiety-security to
rience and place attachment. evaluate the quality of attachment (Bowlby, 1974). The attachment
The developmental role of childhood place experience is further figure’s proximity and emotional attunement engenders feelings of
supported by the emergence of identity as a prominent theme of security and wellbeing. Threats to security give rise to anxiety in
the interviews. Attachment theory holds that the internal organi- the short term and attachment disorders if sustained. Findings of
sation resulting from emotional self-regulation and the uncon- this study suggest that for the majority of participants, a sense of
scious working model of the attachment relationship gives rise to security of place was associated with feelings of wellbeing, a sense
the psychological structure of the self (Schore, 1994; Siegel, 1999; of the soul’s being nurtured, and disruptions to place attachment
Sroufe, 1996). Suggesting a parallel to this developmental process, were associated with strong feelings of grief and anxiety, similar to
Bluey, Jane, Phillip and Mick attribute their adult place attachment those observed in young children in response to separation from
to their childhood place experience, and expressed the belief that attachment figures (Bowlby, 1980). The developmental model of
their childhood place experiences also contributed to their place attachment proposed here requires frequent re-enactment of
psychological development and adult identity. In extended the arousal-interaction-pleasure pattern in order that childhood
excerpts (Section 8.3), Bluey and Mick explicitly attribute the place experience be generalised and internalised into a strong
development of specific aspects of adult personality to childhood internal working model of place. The security/familiarity theme
place experience. Conversely participants with very limited child- provides support for this aspect of the proposed model. The sense
hood place memories, Neal and Jim, stated they did not believe that of familiarity and security reported by participants expressing
childhood place experiences had contributed to their adult identity. strong place attachment feelings are unlikely to have emerged
The emergence of this link between childhood place experience without those participants undertaking frequent interactions with
and adult identity as a prominent theme, along with some partic- the physical environment in childhood.
ipants’ reflective awareness of that link, further supports the Attachment theory argues that by settling distress through care-
developmental model offered here (Fig. 1). In a similar finding giving and soothing behaviours, the attachment figure acts as an
Korpela and Hartig (1996) concluded that experiences of positive external emotional regulator until the infant can internalise this
affect in child-environment interactions contribute to identity function for himself (Trevarthen, 1993; Schore, 1994). The restor-
consolidation (developmental) processes. ative (soothing) qualities of natural environments (Altman &
Five participants used the word ‘love’ (of place), accompanied by Wohlwill, 1983; Kaplan, 1995; Kaplan & Kaplan, 1989) are here
congruent non-verbal communications. This was understood as understood as being somewhat analogous to the soothing effect of
indicating a long-term affective bond to place (place attachment). human care-giving. Korpela (1989) took this analogy further,
Four of these participants also reported experiencing strong grief finding that place functions as external regulator for emotional
associated with loss of childhood place, providing further support states. Likewise, several participants in this study described using
from the interviews for the presence of a strong affective attach- childhood place to regulate emotional distress. Bluey was very clear
ment to place. The prominence of these two themes in adult that he used his time wandering the farm each weekend to recover
remembrances of childhood place experience, and participants’ from his negative experience of school and create a sense of calm
assertion that their love of place arose out of their childhood place self belief. Similarly, in a study of autobiographical writing, Chawla
experience, supports the proposal that an affective bond to place (1992) found that the most common reported benefit of fondly
(place attachment) emerges through childhood place experience. remembered childhood places was ‘that they form an internal
Conversely Jim, whose memories of childhood place experience centre of stability and calm’ in adulthood.
contained very limited affect or sensory detail, expressed only Alongside these parallels between human attachment and place
superficial place attachment. Neal whose place memories were attachment, there are some important differences. Human attach-
tinged with anxiety rather than pleasure denied any attachment to ment is universal – a fundamental requirement of human
20 P. Morgan / Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 (2010) 11–22

development. Like this study, Chawla (1986) found that place this can only be considered preliminary support, and there is need
attachment is not universal, or at least there appears to be a much for stronger evidence. An obvious limitation of the study is the very
greater range in its quality and perceived importance than there is small number of participants, a product of the qualitative orienta-
for human attachment. The need she identified for place theorists tion. Also, while the proposed model draws on Stern’s (1985)
to define the different types of place attachment still stands ‘inferential leap’ of integrating subjective and objective approaches,
unaddressed. Also, attachment theorists see the first three years of this study only attempts to replicate one of the two complimentary
life as most important in the attachment relationship, as this is epistemological approaches that Stern argued were necessary for
when brain development is greatest (Siegel, 1999). Participants in a holistic picture of child development. Empirical observations of
this study estimated that most of the place memories described in children-in-place have not been undertaken. Instead the theoret-
the interview date from between the ages of 8 and 13, with none ical model draws on observational studies previously described in
dating from an age younger than five. Place attachment emerges at the literature. However, in the light of the major developments in
a later age than human attachment (Sobel, 1990). However, there is attachment theory that have occurred since, there is need for an
nothing in the study to suggest that the circular interactional updating of Hart (1979) and Moore’s (1986) rich observational
dynamic depicted in Fig. 1 is not operational from a much earlier studies of children’s engagement with place.
age, but that the child’s sense of place attachment only emerges Also this study has limited its focus to the child’s relationship
long after the strong emotional attachment to the attachment with the physical environment. The model proposed here has not
figure. addressed social constructivist notions of place. This is a reflection
Another difference between the two is the absence of an iden- of the limited scope of the study rather than an implicit denial of
tified attachment figure in place attachment. In human attachment, the role of culture in place attachment. The model proposed here is
the attachment figure is another human being. A shared biological compatible with Moore’s (1986) modification to Bronfenbrenner’s
substrate allows for the broad intersubjective, deeply attuned ecological model of child development, which provides an over-
relationship necessary for the psychobiological development of the arching theoretical modal that incorporates both place and cultural
infant. The sophisticated intersubjective attunement underpinning influences on child development. A promising area for future
human attachment has no obvious parallel in place attachment. research is the longitudinal perspective of the interplay of these
Instead of a clearly identifiable, deeply attuned figure, place two influences over the course of human developmental.
attachment is more usefully conceptualised in terms of an attach- A further challenge to the inferences drawn from this study is
ment field. In attachment theory, this field is seen as arousing the the well-recognised unreliability of adult recollections of childhood
young child’s fascination, excitement and anxiety – the antithesis of events. This study argues that while explicit memory of childhood
the attachment figure who soothes, settles and regulates emotional events is unreliable, the subjective emotional states experienced
arousal (Marvin et al., 2004). Yet, despite this absence of an iden- during deep remembrance and the five emergent themes are
tified, attuned figure, four of the participants reported a subjective associated with unconscious internal working models. These
sense of reciprocity about their relationship with childhood place. implicit memory generalisations of repeated childhood experiences
They described feeling loved and nurtured by childhood place, of place are more robust than the reconstructed recollections of
adding support to the transactional view of place as a rich and explicit memory.
active presence supporting psychological development and Two participants in this study reported an adult place attach-
wellbeing. ment that did not appear to have developed from childhood
In the most thorough exploration to date of the links between experience, or at least not according to the developmental model
place attachment and human attachment, Giuliani (2003) looks to provided here. This finding suggests the possibility of at least one
Ainsworth’s definition of an affectional bond to compare the two. alternative to the model offered here. In spite of Chawla’s (1986)
Drawing on Ainsworth’s criteria of longevity, uniqueness of the identification of different types of place attachment, there is yet no
attachment figure, and the experience of security and pleasure in equivalent of attachment theory’s four experimentally validated
proximity and distress on separation, Giuliani finds that on balance, types of attachment (Goldberg, 2000), and certainly no mapping of
the differences between place attachment and human attachment the different processes that give rise to the various forms of place
outweigh the similarities. However, the model of place attachment attachment. Ultimately however, much of the evidence and theo-
development supported by the majority of the accounts in this retical detail are most likely to emerge through longitudinal studies
study suggests important similarities between the two forms of and these are sadly lacking (Hay, 1998). Hopefully this study can
attachment. Four of the participants in this study report place provide some impetus towards such work.
attachment to be as long lasting and as particular as human This study elicited involuntary childhood place memories with
attachment. Five participants reported the experience of security the characteristics of implicit memory from those participants
and pleasure in proximity and distress on separation from place. reporting a strong adult place attachment. The numerous expres-
Regarding Ainsworth’s criterion of uniqueness, four participants sions of remembered place-pleasure support the proposition that
expressed stronger feelings for remembered childhood places than outdoor environments elicit place-exploration and place-play
current adult place. The picture of place attachment emerging from behaviours in children which give rise to frequent positive affect
this study is that, while feelings of pleasure and security arising states. The frequent repetition of this child-environment trans-
from proximity to, and distress on separation from, place may be actional pattern (Fig. 1) is implied in the security and familiarity
more subtle and take longer to register than similar feelings asso- associated with childhood place by these same participants. Such
ciated with human attachment figures, such feelings are definitely repetition allows for the generalisation of this pattern into an
a part of the experience of place attachment. Overall, while internal working model of the relationship with childhood place
acknowledging differences between the two forms of attachment, which manifests subjectively as long-term positively affected bond
this study finds important similarities in the childhood experience to place. The implicit memory characteristics of the most vivid place
at the core of both human attachment and place attachment. memories reflect the presence of internal working models of
However, the study is limited in its scope. It sketches out a basic childhood place experience. Reports by these same participants of
theoretical model of the development of place attachment from the use of childhood place to regulate emotional distress, of a posi-
childhood experience of place. The retrospective, subjective tively affected childhood sense of place-security, and of intense grief
accounts of the interviews lend support to that model. However, on separation from place, all suggest strong parallels to the
P. Morgan / Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 (2010) 11–22 21

developmental role of the attachment relationship. These parallels Frosh, S. (2002). Key concepts in psychoanalysis. London: The British Library.
Game, A. (2001). Belonging: experience in sacred time and space. In J. May, &
along with a belief expressed by the same group of participants that
N. Thrift (Eds.), Timespace: Geographies of temporality. London: Routledge.
childhood place experience has shaped their adult identity supports Giuliani, M. V. (2003). Theory of attachment and place attachment. In M. Bonnes,
the argument for a developmental role for childhood place experi- T. Lee, & M. Bonaiuto (Eds.), Psychological theories for environmental issues (pp.
ence. The study also details the absence of such reports from 137–165). Aldershot: Ashgate.
Giuliani, M. V., & Feldman, R. (1993). Place attachment in a developmental and
participants who described a weak adult place attachment. These cultural context. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 13, 267–274.
results combine to provide preliminary support for the develop- Goldberg, S. (2000). Attachment and development. London: Arnold.
mental model of place attachment offered and the proposition that Greenspan, S. I. (1999). Developmentally based psychotherapy. Connecticut: Inter-
national Universities Press.
the process by which place attachment develops has strong simi- de Haan, M., Belsky, J., Reid, V., Volein, A., & Johnson, M. (2004). Maternal
larities to that by which human attachment develops. personality and infants’ neural and visual responsivity to facial expressions of
The parallels between human attachment and place attachment emotion. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(7), 1209–1218.
Hart, R. (1979). Children’s experience of place. New York, Irvington.
appear to be significant enough to be recognised by several Hay, R. (1998). Sense of place in a developmental context. Journal of Environmental
participants who describe a subjective sense of being shaped or Psychology, 18, 5–29.
‘parented’ by place. Similarly, the wide use of the term ‘Mother Hester, R., & O’Donnell, W. (1987). Inner and outer landscapes-integrating
psychotherapy with design. Mas, 5, 30–36.
Earth’ signifies a broader cultural recognition of close parallels Jones, M., & Cunningham, C. (1999). The expanding worlds of middle childhood. In
between the two forms of attachment. These links have been rec- E. K. Teather (Ed.), Embodied geographies: Space, bodies and rites of passage (pp.
ognised both transculturally and transhistorically (Malpas, 1999). 27–39). London: Routledge.
Jorgensen, B. S., & Stedman, R. C. (2001). Sense of place as an attitude: lakeshore
Giuliani (2003) argues the need for a theory of attachment and
owners’ attitudes toward their properties. Journal of Environmental Psychology,
affect that includes ‘persons, places and even animals and physical 21, 233–248.
objects’. By extending the dialogue between place theory and Kaplan, S. (1995). The restorative benefits of nature: toward an integrative frame-
attachment theory, this study has begun to address the absence of work. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 15, 169–182.
Kaplan, S., & Kaplan, R. (1984). Cognition and environment. New York: Praeger.
a developmental theory of place attachment as well as attachment Kaplan, S., & Kaplan, R. (1989). The experience of nature: A psychological perspective.
theory’s lack of attention to place as an interactive presence New York: Cambridge University Press.
contributing to human developmental processes. In identifying Kirkby, M. A. (1989). Nature as refuge in children’s environments. Children’s Envi-
ronments Quarterly, 6(1), 7–12.
significant parallels between children’s experience of place and the Korpela, K., & Hartig, T. (1996). Restorative qualities of favourite places. Journal of
human attachment relationship, particularly the way both Environmental Psychology, 16, 221–233.
phenomena stimulate human development, give rise to long-term Korpela, K. M. (1989). Place identity as a product of environmental self regulation.
Journal of Environmental Psychology, 9, 241–256.
affective bonds and contribute to adult identity, this study is a step Lichtenberg, J. D. (1989). Psychoanalysis and motivation. Hillsdale: The Analytic Press.
towards an integrated developmental theory. Low, S., & Altman, I. (1992). Place attachment: a conceptual inquiry. In I. Altman, &
S. Low (Eds.), Place attachment. New York: Plenum Press.
Madders, J. (1981). Stress and relaxation: self-help ways to cope with stress and relieve
Acknowledgements nervous tension, ulcers, insomnia, migraine and high blood pressure. London:
Martin Dunitz.
Malpas, J. E. (1999). Place and experience: A philosophical topography. Cambridge:
The author wishes to John Cameron for his generous and
Cambridge University Press.
informed comments on this research. Marvin, R., Cooper, G., Hoffman, K., & Powell, B. (2002). The circle of security
project: attachment-based intervention with caregiver-preschool dyads.
Attachment and Human Development, 4(1), 1–17.
References Massey, D. (1994). Space, place and gender. Cambridge: Polity Press.
Mehrabian, A., & Russell, J. (1974). The basic emotional impact of environments.
Ainsworth, M., Blehar, M., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A Perceptual and Motor Skills, 38, 283–301.
psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Moore, R. C. (1986). Childhood’s domain: Play and place in child development. London:
Erlbaum Associates. Croom Helm.
Altman, I., & Wohlwill, J. (Eds.). (1983). Behaviour and the natural environment. New Moore, R. C. (1989). Plants as play props. Children’s Environmental Quarterly, 6(1),
York: Plenum. 3–6.
Bachelard, G. (1969). The poetics of space. Boston: Beacon Press. Olwig, K. R. (1989). The childhood deconstruction of nature. Children’s Environment
Basch, M. F. (1988). Understanding psychotherapy: The science behind the art. New Quarterly, 6(1), 19–25.
York: Basic Books. Patterson, M. E., & Williams, D. R. (2005). Maintaining research traditions on place:
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Volume 1. Attachment. London: Penguin. diversity of thought and scientific progress. Journal of Environmental Psychology,
Bowlby, J. (1974). Attachment and loss: Volume 2. Separation: Anxiety and anger. 25, 361–380.
London: Pelican Press. Payne, R. A. (2005). Relaxation techniques: A practical handbook for the health care
Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and Loss: Volume 3. Loss: Sadness and depression. professional. Edinburgh: Elsevier Churchill Livingstone.
London: Penguin. Pearce, J. C. (1977). Magical child. New York: Dutton.
Chawla, L. (1986). The ecology of environmental memory. Children’s Environment Porteous, J. D. (1990). Landscapes of the mind: Worlds of sense and metaphor. Tor-
Quarterly, 3(4), 34–41. onto: Toronto University Press.
Chawla, L. (1992). Childhood place attachments. In I. Altman, & S. Low (Eds.), Place Proshansky, H. M., Fabian, A. K., & Kaminoff, R. (1983). Place identity: physical world
attachment (pp. 63–86). New York: Plenum Press. socialisation of the self. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 3, 57–83.
Cicchetti, D., & Beeghly, M. (1994). Perspectives on the study of the self in transition. Rubeinstein, R. L., & Parmelee, P. A. (1992). Attachment to place and the repre-
In D. Cicchetti, & M. Beeghly (Eds.), The self in transition: Infancy to childhood. sentation of the life course by the elderly. In I. Altman, & S. M. Low (Eds.), Place
Chicago: University of Chicago Press. attachment (pp. 139–163). New York: Plenum Press.
Ciompi, L. (1991). Affects as central organising and integrating factors: a new Russell, J. A., & Snodgrass, J. (1987). Emotion and the Environment. In D. Stokols, &
psychosocial/biological model of the psyche. British Journal of Psychiatry, 159, I. Altman (Eds.), Handbook of Environmental Psychology, Vol. 1 (pp. 245–279).
97–105. New York: Wiley.
Cobb, E. (1977). The ecology of imagination in childhood. London: Routledge and Rutter, M. (2002). The interplay of nature, nurture and developmental influences.
Keegan Paul. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 996–1000.
Cooper, M. C. (1992). Environmental memories. In I. Altman, & S. Low (Eds.), Place Rutter, M., Dunn, J., Plomin, R., Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Maughan, B., Ormel, J.,
attachment (pp. 87–112). New York: Plenum Press. Meyer, J., & Eaves, L. (1997). Integrating nature and nurture: implications of
Creswell, T. (2004). Place: A short introduction. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing. person-environment correlations and interactions in developmental psycho-
Crotty, M. (1996). Phenomenology and nursing research. South Melbourne: Churchill pathology. Development and Psychopathology, 9, 335–364.
Livingstone. Sameroff, A. J. (1975). Transactional models in early social relations. Human
Damasio, A. R. (1998). Emotion in the perspective of an integrated nervous system. Development, 18, 65–79.
Brain Research Reviews, 26, 83–86. Sameroff, A. J., & Fiese, B. H. (2000). Models of development and developmental
Dovey, K. (1990). Refuge and imagination: places of peace in childhood. Children’s risk. In C. H. Zeanah (Ed.), Handbook of infant mental health (pp. 3–19). New
Environment Quarterly, 7(4), 13–17. York: Guilford Press.
Everly, G. S., & Rosenfeld, R. (1981). The nature and treatment of the stress response. Schachtel, E. G. (1959). Metamorphosis: On the development of affect, perception,
New York: Plenum Press. attention, and memory. New York: Basic Books.
22 P. Morgan / Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 (2010) 11–22

Schore, A. N. (1994). Affect regulation and the origin of the self: The neurobiology of Stern, D. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant: A view from psychoanalysis and
emotional development. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Earlbaum. developmental psychology. New York: Basic Books.
Schore, R. (1997). Rethinking the brain. New York: Families and Work Institute. Striniste, N. A., & Moore, R. C. (1989). Early childhood outdoors: a literature review
Sebba, R. (1991). The landscapes of childhood: the reflection of childhood’s envi- related to the design of childcare environments. Children’s Environments Quar-
ronment in adult memories and in children’s attitudes. Environment and terly, 7(4), 5–12.
Behaviour, 22(4), 395–422. Trevarthen, C. (1993). The self born in intersubjectivity: the psychology of infant
Siegel, D. (1998). The developing mind: toward and neurobiology of interpersonal communicating. In U. Neisser (Ed.), The perceived self: Ecological and interpersonal
experience. The Signal: Newsletter for the World Association of Infant Mental sources of self-knowledge (pp. 121–173). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Health, 6(3), 1–11. Tuan, Y.-F. (1974). Topophilia. Englewood Cliffs: Prentiss-Hall.
Siegel, D. (1999). The developing mind. New York: Guildford Press. Twigger-Ross, C. L., & Uzzell, D. L. (1996). Place and identity processes. Journal of
Siegel, D. (2001). Toward an interpersonal neurobiology of the developing mind Environmental Psychology, 16, 205–220.
attachment relationships, mindsight and neural integration. Infant Mental Von Eckartsberg, R. (1998). Existential phenomenological research. In R. Valle (Ed.),
Health Journal, 22(1–2), 67–94. Phenomenological Inquiry in Psychology: Existential and Transpersonal Dimen-
Sobel, D. (1990). A place in the world: adults’ memories of childhood’s special sions (pp. 21–62). New York: Plenum Press.
places. Children’s Environments Quarterly, 6(4), 25–31. Wohlwill, J. F., & Heft, H. (1987). The physical environment and the development of
Sroufe, L. A. (1990). An organisational perspective on the self. In D. Cicchetti, & the child. In D. Stokols, & I. Altman (Eds.), Handbook of environmental psychology
M. Beeghly (Eds.), The self in transition: Infancy to childhood (pp. 281–307). (pp. 281–327). New York: Wiley.
Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Zeanah, C. H., Boris, N. W., & Larrieu, J. A. (1997). Infant development and devel-
Sroufe, L. A. (1996). Emotional development: The organisation of emotional life in the opmental risk: a review of the past ten years. Journal of American Academy of
early years. New York: Cambridge University Press. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 165–178.

Вам также может понравиться