Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 4

BRIEF COMMUNICATIONS

imitating gestures, and they concluded that ideational


Ideationai Atxaxia: apraxia may occur independent of ideomotor apraxia.
A Deficit in ?o01 These results could be accounted for by several fac-
tors. De Renzi and colleagues used transitive gestures
Selection and Use when testing for ideational apraxia and intransitive ges-
C. Ochipa, MA,*$ L. J. G. Rothi, PhD,*$
tures when testing for ideomotor apraxia. Ideomotor
and K. M. Heilman, MD+$ apraxics have been shown to be more impaired when
making transitive movements than when making in-
transitive movements 123, suggesting that these move-
We report a 67-year-old left-handed man who exhibited ments are mediated differently 13, 151. Second, the
an ideational apraxia in both clinical and nonclinical ideomotor task required the imitation of one gesture,
natural settings following a right hemisphere infarction. while the ideational task required the selection of an
His inability to use tools could not be explained by a item to verbal command, which may have been in-
motor production deficit (ideomotor apraxia), because
fluenced by language comprehension problems. Third,
he made content errors and could not match tools with
objects. His deficit could not be attributed to an agnosia the nature of the errors produced by de Renzi and
or language comprehension deficit, because he could colleagues’ patients was not described. If ideational
name tools and point to tools on command. Based on our apraxia is a conceptual deficit, content errors (as op-
testing, it appeared that this patient had a loss of knowl- posed to motor production errors) shouid be ob-
edge related to tool use. served. Fourth, their patients were not tested for an
Ochipa C , Rothi LJG, Heilman KM. Ideational
agnosia.
apraxia: a deficit in tool selection and use. We also define ideational apraxia as a loss of concep-
Ann Neurol 1989;25:190-193 tuai knowledge related to tool use (not necessarily due
to a performance deficit), and we describe a left-
handed patient with a right hemisphere infarction who
The error patterns in ideational apraxia have been de- exhibited such an ideational apraxia. This patient’s ide-
scribed as the perseveration, omission, or misordering ational apraxia could not be accounted for on the basis
of acts that are components of multistage complex ac- of an agnosia, impaired language comprehension, or an
tions with objects [4, 10, li]. Poeck [lo] maintains ideomotor apraxia.
that ideational apraxics are impaired not in their use of
single objects, but only in the correct sequencing of Case History and Methods
acts such that an intended goal cannot be achieved. Case History
The term ideational apraxia proposed by Liepmann [b, A 67-year-old left-handed, high school-educated, hyperten-
71, however, impiies not a sequencing deficit but a sive, diabetic man had sudden onset of a left hemiparesis
conceptual defect. Morlaas E81 and De Renzi and col- accompanied by speech difficulty. A computed tomographic
leagues [i] agreed and operationally defined ideational scan conducted 1 week after onset revealed an infarction in
apraxia as a defect in tool utilization. the distribution of the right middle cerebral artery involving
An impairment in the use of tools may, however, the frontal, inferior parietal, and superior temporal lobes
result from either a conceptual defect (ideational (Figure). While he was in the hospital, we investigated his
apraxia) or a defect in motor production (ideomotor language and praxis, because his performance with actual
apraxia). Zangwill [lb] suggested that ideational tools was impaired.
apraxia, as defined by tool use, may be a severe form of
ideomotor apraxia wherein a defect in motor program- Metbods
ming results in impaired performance with actual ob- The patient’s language abilities were evaluated 1 week and 1
jects. De Renzi and colleagues [i] found that 11 of month after onset with the Western Aphasia Battery [5J.
their 140 patients with left hemisphere damage were Praxis testing was conducted at approximately 1 month after
more impaired when using actual objects than when onset. The patient performed al1 tasks with his nonparalyzed,
nondominant right upper extremity. The same stimulus set
of common household tools and objects was used for al1
tasks. The patient was asked to point to these objects on
From the *Audiology-SpeechPathology Service and the tNeurol-
ogy Service, Veterans Administration Medicai Center, Gainesville, command, to name objects held before him, to point to
FL, and the SDepanment of Neurology, Coiiege of Medicine, Uni- objects described by function (e.g., “Point to the one used
versity of Florida, Gainesviile, FL. for cutting”), to describe the function of objects upon visual
Received Apr 22, 1988, and in revised form Jun 30. Accepted for presentation, and to identify verbally objects described by
publication Jui 2, 1988. their function (e.g., “What do you use for cutting?”). Tests
Address correspondence to Dr Heilman, Box J-236, J. Hillis Miller for ideomotor apraxia included those of pantomime to verbal
Heaith Center, Gainesviile, FL 32610. command, pantomime imitation, and actual object use. The
Computed tomography scans demonstrating right hemisphere hy-
podense area.

Brief Communication: Ochipa et al: Ideationai Apraxia 191


Tabie 1. Western Aphasaa Battery Scores a t One Month Table 2. Praxz“ Test Pe$oòrmance
Post Onset
Number Correct
Patient SubscoredMaximum (n = 20)
Test Points Possible
~ ~~ ~

Tool identification 19
Spontaneous speech 11/20 Tool identification by function 7
Information content 7/10 Tool naming 17
Fluency 4/10 Tool function description 3
Comprehension 7.55110 Tool use 2
Yeslno questions 60160
Auditory word recognition 37/60 Tool use with object 4
Sequential commands 54/80 Tool selection 3
Repetition 5.8110 Pantomime to command O
Naming 7/10 Pantomime imitation 4
Object naming 48/60
Word fluency 4/20
Sentence completion 8/10
Responsive speech 10110
Aphasia quotient 62.7
foils (tools other than those typically used for a particu-
lar activity), he often chose the incorrect tool (e.g., he
was observed to eat with a toothbrush and brush his
teeth with a spoon and a comb).
patient was also presented with both tools and the objects His defective tool use could not be attributed to an
that typically receive the tools’ action and was nonverbally agnosia because he could name objects upon visual
requested to demonstrate the tools’ use. Gesture compre- presentation ( 17 / 2 0 correct). Furthermore, his deficit
hension was assessed by asking the patient to name each of could not be attributed to language comprehension
the 15 gestures on the Florida Apraxia Screening Test (as difficulties because he could point to objects on com-
produced by the examiner) { 131. The Action Naming Test mand (1900 correct). However, the patient’s perfor-
197 was also administered as a measure of the patient’s ability mance when he was required to identify objects by
to label actions verbaily.
To determine if our patient had preserved knowledge of
their function was relatively poor ( 7 / 2 0 correct), and
tool-object relationships, and to further ensure that his er- he was unable to describe verbally the function of visu-
rors were not production errors from an ideomotor apraxia, ally presented objects (3/20 correct). He was also un-
he was given a task wherein he had to select the correct tool able to identify verbally objects described by their
for a target action. For example, he was presented with a function (3/10 correct). In contrast, gesture compre-
partialiy completed task (e.g., a partially sawed board) and an hension (as tested by the Florida Apraxia Screening
array of four tools (e.g., saw, hammer, key, needle) and was Test) was relatively spared (12115 correct), and scores
nonverbally requested to select the appropriate tool to com- on the Action Naming Test 197 were within normai
plete the task. limits.
Finally, che patient was videotaped on the hospital ward Pantomime to verbal command was profoundly im-
whiìe participating in activities of daily living (i.e., eating, paired ( 0 / 2 0 correct). Unlike patients with ideomotor
personal grooming, etc.). Foils (tools and objects other than
those typically used for a particular activity) were presented
apraxia who make production errors (e.g., spatiai,
along with those typically used for a specific activity. For orientation, body part as object) 1141, this patient’s
example, a toothbrush and a comb were placed along with performance on the pantomime task was characterized
eating utensils at the side of a dinner tray. The patient was by content errors (no response or irrelevant move-
given no instructions during these videotaped activities. ments). Only a slight improvement was noted upon
imitation of gestures ( 4 / 2 0 correct). The patient was
Resdts unable to demonstrate tool function when actuaily
One week after onset the patient had a global aphasia. holding tools ( 2 / 2 0 correct). Performance improved
By 1 month post onset, the patient’s condition had only slightly ( 4 / 2 0 correct) when the patient was given
evolved to a Broca’s aphasia with gains in auditory both the tool (e.g., hammer) and the object that re-
comprehension, naming, and repetition. Spontaneous ceives the tool’s action (e.g., a piece of wood contain-
speech remained reduced in quantity and was charac- ing a partiaily driven nail). In addition, he was unable
terized by articulatory struggle. Comprehension was to select the correct tool to complete a task (3/20 cor-
relatively good (Table 1). rect), suggesting an impairment in the appreciation of
Throughout the patient’s hospital stay, he was noted the associative relationship between tools and the ob-
on multiple occasions spontaneously to use objects in- jects they act upon. See Table 2 for a summary of the
appropriately. When the patient was provided with patient’s scores on praxis tasks.

192 Annals of Neurology Vol 25 No 2 February 1989


Discussion patient, Roeltgen and Heilman’s patient could not
This patient’s inability to use actual objects cannot be name tools or point to tools when named, but he could
explained by a production deficit (ideomotor apraxia) point to tools when their action was described and
because his errors were content errors and because he could also use the tools correctly. In this case the pa-
couid not match tools with the objects on which they tient’s “what” left hemisphere was impaired, and his
are used. His deficit could not be attributed to an “how” right hemisphere was intact.
agnosia or ianguage comprehension deficit because he In both our patient and in Roeltgen and Heilman’s
could both name tools and point to tools when they patient, “how” knowledge (which tool to use and
were named by the examiner. It appeared that this which movement to use with the tool) seemed to be
patient had lost the knowledge of tool function. This represented in the hemisphere contralaterai to the pre-
loss of knowledge of tool function was aiso evident in ferred left hand, while the “what” system was, at least
his language performance. Although he codd point to in part, mediated by the hemisphere ipsilaterai to the
a tool named by the examiner, he could not point to preferred hand. In right handers both the “what” and
the tools when they were described by function. Simi- “how” systems may be mediated by the same left hemi-
larly, he couid name tools but couid not describe tool sphere, and it therefore may be more difficuit to disas-
function. Although the patient’s nonfluent speech may sociate ideational apraxia from an agnosia, ideomotor
have influenced results, it appeared that when he was apraxia, and severe aphasia.
externally provided with a gesture or a picture of an
action, his performance was relatively intact. In con-
trast, when he was presented with the object and re- This work was supported by che Medicai Research Service of the
quested to describe verbaliy or demonstrate its func- Veterans Administration and the Memory Disorders Clinic, Aging
tion, his performance was severely impaired. Adult Services Program, Department of Health and Rehabilitative
If ideationai apraxia is truly a conceptual deficit, pa- Services, State of Florida.
tients should exhibit impaired object use in natural Presented at the 16th Annual Meeting of the International
settings. Although De Renti and colleagues [i] did Neurologicai Society, New Orleans, LA, January 27-30, 1988.
not systematically investigate this aspect of perfor-
mance, they did report that their patients adequately References
performed activities of daily living on the hospital 1. De Renti E, Pieczuro A, Vignolo A: Ideational apraxia: a quan-
ward. They suggested that in most cases ideational titative study. Neuropsychologia 1968;6:41-52
apraxia was limited to the artificiai test situation. Our 2. Goodgiass H, Kaplan E Disturbance of gesture and pantomime
in aphasia. Brain 1963;86:703-720
patient differed in that he was observed spontaneously
3. Haaland KY, Flaherty D: The different types of limb apraxia
to use single objects inappropriately in natural settings. errors made by patients with left vs. right hemisphere damage.
This man’s inability to use objects appropriately in Brain Cogn 1984;3:370-384
both naturai and test settings, in the presence of intact 4. Hecaen H, Albert M L Human Neuropsychology. New York,
recognition abilities, suggested a deficit of tool knowl- Wiley, 1978
5. Kertesz A: The Western Aphasia Battery. New York, Grune
edge. and Stratton, 1982
There are at least two possibilities as to why idea- 6. Liepmann H: Drei Aufsatze aus dem Apraxiegebeit. Berlin,
tionai apraxia has not been more commonly reported. Karger, 1908
In the natural setting not only do we not provide pa- 7. Liepmann H: Uber Storungen des Handekns bei Gehirnkran-
tients with foils, but aiso the ward personnel often ken. Berlin, Karger, 1905
8. Morlaas J: Contribution a l’etude de I’apraxie. Paris, iegrand,
provide the brain-impaired patients with the correct 1928
utensil or tool. The second possibility relates to brain 9. Nicholas M, Obler L, Albert M, Goodglas H: Lexical retrieval
organization. Our patient was left handed and had a in healthy aging. Cortex 1985;21(4):595-606
right hemisphere lesion that induced both ideational 10. Poeck K Ideational apraxia J Neurol 1983;230:1-5
apraxia and a Broca’s type aphasia Perhaps at least part 11. Poeck K, Lehmkuhl G Ideatory apraxia in a left-handed patient
with right-sided brain lesion. Cortex 1980;16:273-284
of this man’s ability to name tools and to point to tools 12. Roeltgen DP, Heiiman KM: Apractic agraphia in a patient with
named by the examiner (what we term “what” knowl- normal praxis. Brain 1983;18:35-46
edge) was mediated by his left hemisphere, but his 13. Rothi LJG, Heilman KM: Acquisition and retention of gesnues
knowledge of tool use arid movement programs by apraxic patients. Brain Cogn 1984;3:426-437
(“how” knowledge) was mediated by his right hemi- 14. Rothi LJH, Heilman KM, Mack L, et al. Ideomotor apraxia:
error pattern analysis. Aphasiology 1988;2:381-387
sphere. 15. Watson RT, Fleet WS, Rothi LG, Heilman KM: Apraxia and
A complementary patient was published by Roelt- the supplementary motor area. Arch Neurol 1986;43:787-792
gen and Heilman 1121. This patient was also left 16. Zangwill OL: L’apraxie ideatoire. Rev Neurol (Paris) 1960;
handed but had a left hemisphere lesion. Unlike our 102~595-603

Brief Communication: Ochipa et ai: Ideational Apraxia 193

Вам также может понравиться