Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 15

American Journal of Medical Genetics Part C (Seminars in Medical Genetics) 169C:135–149 (2015)

I N T R O D U C T I O N

Down Syndrome: Cognitive and Behavioral


Functioning Across the Lifespan
JULIE GRIECO, MARGARET PULSIFER, KAREN SELIGSOHN, BRIAN SKOTKO,
AND ALISON SCHWARTZ

Individuals with Down syndrome (DS) commonly possess unique neurocognitive and neurobehavioral profiles
that emerge within specific developmental periods. These profiles are distinct relative to others with similar
intellectual disability (ID) and reflect underlying neuroanatomic findings, providing support for a distinctive
phenotypic profile. This review updates what is known about the cognitive and behavioral phenotypes
associated with DS across the lifespan. In early childhood, mild deviations from neurotypically developing
trajectories emerge. By school-age, delays become pronounced. Nonverbal skills remain on trajectory for mental
age, whereas verbal deficits emerge and persist. Nonverbal learning and memory are strengths relative to verbal
skills. Expressive language is delayed relative to comprehension. Aspects of language skills continue to develop
throughout adolescence, although language skills remain compromised in adulthood. Deficits in attention/
executive functions are present in childhood and become more pronounced with age. Characteristic features
associated with DS (cheerful, social nature) are personality assets. Children are at a lower risk for
psychopathology compared to other children with ID; families report lower levels of stress and a more positive
outlook. In youth, externalizing behaviors may be problematic, whereas a shift toward internalizing behaviors
emerges with maturity. Changes in emotional/behavioral functioning in adulthood are typically associated with
neurodegeneration and individuals with DS are higher risk for dementia of the Alzheimer's type. Individuals with
DS possess many unique strengths and weaknesses that should be appreciated as they develop across the
lifespan. Awareness of this profile by professionals and caregivers can promote early detection and support
cognitive and behavioral development. © 2015 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.

KEY WORDS: Down syndrome; neurocognitive; neurobehavioral; development

How to cite this article: Grieco J, Pulsifer M, Seligsohn K, Skotko B, Schwartz A. 2015. Down syndrome:
Cognitive and behavioral functioning across the lifespan. Am J Med Genet Part C 169C:135–149.

INTRODUCTION alterations in both neuronal prolifera- pregnancy due to the rate of both
tion and differentiation [Pulsifer, 1996], spontaneous and elective abortions
John Langdon Down initially identified manifesting in alterations in cognitive [Hsu, 1998; Ethan and Canfield, 2002;
Down syndrome (DS) approximately and behavioral functioning across the Skotko, 2009]. The diagnosis of DS is
150 years ago [Down, 1866]. DS is the lifespan. DS is estimated to occur once typically made through genetic karyo-
genetic manifestation of trisomy of in every 700–800 live births with a type testing, with post-natal confir-
chromosome 21 [Rahmani et al., global incidence of more than 200,000 mation via the identification of
1989; Delabar et al., 1993; Korenberg cases per year [Constestabile et al., characteristic syndrome-based physical
et al., 1994; Constestabile et al., 2010]. 2010]; however, this may be an under- and/or medical features [Siegel and
DS is associated with neuropathological estimate of the incidence during Smith, 2010]. The distal part of the

Julie A. Grieco, PsyD, is a post-doctoral neuropsychology fellow at the Psychology Assessment Center at the Massachusetts General Hospital. She
also holds an appointment as a Clinical Fellow at Harvard Medical School.
Margaret B. Pulsifer, Ph.D. is a clinical neuropsychologist at the Psychology Assessment Center at the Massachusetts General Hospital. She is also an
Assistant Professor in Psychology in the Department of Psychiatry at Harvard Medical School.
Karen J. Seligsohn, Ph.D. is a clinical neuropsychologist at the Psychology Assessment Center at the Massachusetts General Hospital. She is also an
Instructor in Psychology in the Department of Psychiatry at Harvard Medical School.
Brian G. Skotko, M.D, MPP, is a Board-certified medical geneticist and co-director of the Down Syndrome Program at Massachusetts General
Hospital. He is also an Assistant Professor in the Department of Pediatrics at Harvard Medical School.
Alison T. Schwartz, M.D, is Board-certified in pediatrics and internal medicine and is co-director of the Down Syndrome Program at Massachusetts
General Hospital. She is also an Instructor in the Department of Pediatrics at Harvard Medical School.
*Correspondence to: Julie A. Grieco, PsyD, Massachusetts General Hospital, One Bowdoin Square, 7th Floor, Boston, MA 02114.
E-mail: jagrieco@mgh.harvard.edu
DOI 10.1002/ajmg.c.31439
Article first published online 18 May 2015 in Wiley Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com).

ß 2015 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.


136 AMERICAN JOURNAL OF MEDICAL GENETICS PART C: SEMINARS IN MEDICAL GENETICS INTRODUCTION

long arm of chromosome HSA21 is cognitive development to assessment Oller and Siebert, 1988; Steffens et al.,
recognized as the “Down syndrome for cognitive decline. When declines 1992; Thoradottir et al., 2002]; how-
critical region” and it is specifically are observed on testing, they often ever, infants show reduced vocal reac-
associated with intellectual disability reflect a loss of previously acquired skills tivity and responsiveness to the
(ID) [Rahmani et al., 1989; Delabar relative to one’s own prior abilities environment [Kasari et al., 1990; Jah-
et al., 1993; Korenberg et al., 1994; [Nadel, 2003; Pennington et al., 2003; romi et al., 2008]. Delayed acquisition of
Constestabile et al., 2010]. Phenotypic Constestabile et al., 2010]. a child’s first words is observed [Laws
manifestations of DS have been identi- Within the domains of cognitive and Bishop, 2003; Levy and Eilam,
fied and are discussed here. functioning, individuals with DS dem- 2013]. Early in life, single word vocabu-
onstrate a consistent pattern of weak- lary, the intentional use of communica-
nesses in the processing of verbal tion, and the pragmatic aspects of
COGNITIVE FUNCTIONING
information relative to visual information language are commonly recognized as
[Gibson, 1978; Pueschel et al., 1987; strengths [Owens and MacDonald,
General Intelligence
Pulsifer, 1996; Rondal and Edwards, 1982; Coggins et al., 1983; Tannock,
ID is recognized as one of the most 1997; Miller, 1999; Abbeduto et al., 1988; Beeghly et al., 1990; Roberts
prominent features of DS [Vicari et al., 2001]. Children with DS continue to et al., 2007; Couzens et al., 2011]. As the
2005; Constestabile et al., 2010] and DS make gains in nonverbal cognitive abil- language demands increase, a delay in
is the most common genetic etiology of ities [Couzens et al., 2011; Channell et al., the use of multi-word phrases and
ID [Pulsifer, 1996; Anderson et al., 2014a], whereas the growth of verbal atypical patterns of communication is
2013; Daunhauer et al., 2014]. The abilities tends to decelerate throughout demonstrated [Oliver and Buckley,
severity of ID among individuals with adolescence and into adulthood [Carr, 1994; Sigman and Ruskin, 1999].
DS falls on a spectrum that ranges from 2000, 2005; Kittler et al., 2004; Naess Consistent delays in language skills are
profound to borderline intellectual et al., 2011]; however, this does not observed once the child reaches age five
functioning (Intelligence Quotient [IQ] indicate that individuals’ raw scores [Guralnick, 2002; Rondal, 2006].
¼ 30–70; mean IQ ¼ 50) [Vicari et al., become stagnant [Chapman et al., 2002]. Language profiles of school-aged
2005; Constestabile et al., 2010], with Longitudinal studies through mid- children reveal a significant delay in
most individuals demonstrating moder- dle adulthood reveal continued declines expressive language relative to receptive
ate to severe ID [Nelson et al., 2005; in standardized test scores with increas- language development, with the greatest
Lott and Dierssen, 2010]. Cognitive ing age [Dameron, 1963; Share et al., delays in expressive syntax and phono-
functioning often changes across the 1964; Carr, 1970, 1988, 2000, 2005; logical processing [Chapman et al.,
lifespan and is moderated by several co- Dicks-Mireaux, 1972; Shonkoff et al., 2002; Chapman, 2006]. More specifi-
morbid factors such as sensory impair- 1992; Crombie and Gunn, 1998; Miller, cally, language syntax (including use of
ments, seizures, autism, sleep disruption, 1998; Hauser-Cram et al., 1999; Cou- verbs, nouns, pronouns, grammatical
and other medical and psychiatric zens et al., 2011]. Cognitive declines in morphemes, and sentence structure) is
conditions [Gasquoine, 2011]. middle to late adulthood are frequently particularly challenging both in terms of
Cognitive growth persists through associated with a dementia of the verbal expression and comprehension
childhood, adolescence and early adult- Alzheimer type, although neurodege- [Beeghly et al., 1990; Hulme and
hood [Carr, 2005; Couzens et al., 2012] nerative changes can take place even in Mackenzie, 1992; Fowler et al., 1994;
and is followed by a gradual loss of the absence of clinical signs of dementia. Rondal, 1994; Kernan and Sabsay, 1996;
abilities [Oliver et al., 1998; Carr, 2005] Accelerated volume loss is observed Rondal and Edwards, 1997; Miller,
commonly associated with a dementia across the frontal, temporal, and parietal 1999; Chapman and Hesketh, 2000 ;
process [Devenny et al., 2000; Maatta lobes [Teipel et al., 2004; Haier et al., Eadie et al., 2002; Vicari et al., 2002;
et al., 2006]. While standardized test 2008; Beacher et al., 2010; Anderson Abbeduto et al., 2003; Vicari, 2004;
scores appear to drop [Dameron, 1963; et al., 2013] and reduced connectivity is Abbeduto and Chapman, 2005; Miolo
Share et al., 1964; Carr, 1970, 1988, demonstrated, reflecting impaired abil- et al., 2005; Chapman, 2006; Estigarriba
2000, 2005; Dicks-Mireaux, 1972; ity to integrate information from distant et al., 2012; Levy and Eilam, 2013].
Shonkoff et al., 1992; Crombie and brain regions into coherent distributed Aspects of language therefore develop
Gunn, 1998; Miller, 1998; Hauser- networks [Lott and Dierssen, 2010; unevenly and the discrepancy continues
Cram et al., 1999; Couzens et al., Anderson et al., 2013]. Specific cogni- to grow with age [Chapman et al., 1991;
2011], analysis of raw scores reveals the tive domains will be discussed below. Rondal, 1994; Miller, 1995; Chapman,
acquisition of skills at a slower rate of 2003; Joffe and Varlokosta, 2007; Con-
cognitive growth, rather than loss of stestible et al., 2010].
Language
skills [Carr, 1988; Crombie and Gunn, Relative strength in single word
1998; Couzens et al., 2011, 2012]. In Early language milestones (e.g., bab- receptive vocabulary is also present
middle to late adulthood, test interpre- bling) are typically met within an age- during adolescence; however, the ability
tation shifts from an emphasis on expected range [Smith and Oller, 1981; to comprehend more complex language
INTRODUCTION AMERICAN JOURNAL OF MEDICAL GENETICS PART C: SEMINARS IN MEDICAL GENETICS 137

syntax may plateau in late childhood or development. Weakness in verbal proc- EF also include the ability to integrate
early adolescence [Chapman et al., essing is believed to be secondary to what a person wants to do with what
2002, 2006]. Syntactic weakness is deficits in the phonological loop, an they can do, self-monitor behavior, and
most prominent during late child- aspect of verbal working memory, direct energy toward achieving a future
hood/early teenage years [Miller, which likely contributes to problems goal [Alvarez and Emory, 2006; Wil-
1999], particularly when viewed in with structural language and weaknesses loughby et al., 2014].
comparison with typically developing in phonological processing, and in Older studies of EF in individuals
(TD) peers. In adulthood, language sentence imitation [Broadley et al., with DS demonstrate deficits on tasks of
deficits in articulation, phonological 1995; Pulsifer, 1996; Chapman and attention, perceptual speed, reaction
processing, and morphosyntax remain Hesketh, 2001; Jarrold and Baddeley, time, and motor control [Berkson,
diminished; however, semantic, prag- 2001; Laws, 2002; Hodapp and Dykens, 1960; Meyers et al., 1961; Clausen,
matic, and communicative intent are 2004; Laws and Gunn, 2004; Brock and 1968; Nettlebeck and Brewer, 1976;
relatively preserved [Pulsifer, 1996; Jarrold, 2005; Fidler, 2005; Miolo et al., Logan, 1985] relative to adolescents with
Rondal and Comblain, 1996; Estigar- 2005; Purser and Jarrold, 2005; Chap- matched MA [Meyers et al., 1961].
riba et al., 2012] and generally consistent man, 2006; Vicari and Carlesimo, 2006; Weaknesses in simultaneous and succes-
with or slightly above mental age (MA) Silverman, 2007; Couzens et al., 2011]. sive processing and organization of motor
(mean intellectual performance for a Language development is therefore responses are also observed when com-
specific age) [Pulsifer, 1996]. Overall, dependent on and contributes to more pared with peers with ID of other
comprehension of language remains global cognitive functions. etiologies [Snart et al., 1982; Lincoln
limited and individuals often attempt When comparing individuals with et al., 1985]. Recent studies demonstrate
to rely on lexical and situational cues to DS to other individuals with ID of more varied results among individuals
make meaning of what is said to them differing etiologies, linguistic develop- evaluated early in the lifespan, whereas
[Rondal and Comblain, 1996]; how- ment is delayed beyond other groups studies in adulthood show evidence of
ever, even these aspects of language [Abbeduto et al., 2008] and it qualita- deficits more consistently. For example,
processing are weak relative to TD peers tively differs in nature [Polisenska and in childhood some studies show aspects
[Bello et al., 2014]. As individuals Kapalkova, 2014]. Relative to individ- of preserved EF skills relative to individ-
continue to mature, speech comprehen- uals with Williams and fragile X uals matched for MA [Vicari et al., 2000;
sion and production slow further, there syndromes, individuals with DS have Pennington et al., 2003; Lanfranchi et al.,
are higher rates of dysfluencies (hesi- even greater delays in morphosyntactic 2010; Costanzo et al., 2013], whereas
tation, pauses), word discrimination skill development [Estigarriba et al., other studies demonstrate impairment in
becomes more difficult, and speech 2012; Levy and Eilam, 2013] and poor aspects of EF for individuals with DS,
organization/word retrieval problems articulation and speech intelligibility even relative to other individuals with ID
emerge [Rondal and Comblain, 1996]. [Hulme and Mackenzie, 1992; Fowler [Trezise et al., 2008; Costanzo et al.,
Some of these difficulties may be et al., 1994; Miller, 1999; Abbeduto and 2013]. Discussion of salient findings by
attributed in part to age-related changes Chapman, 2005; Chapman, 2006]. In EF domain is outlined below.
in hearing, auditory discrimination, and contrast, language pragmatics and con-
less efficient respiratory support for versational style appear consistent with
Attention
speech [Rondal and Comblain, 1996]. MA [Beeghly et al., 1990; Rondal,
Impaired processing in aspects of 1994; Rondal and Edwards, 1997] and Children with DS show impairment in
language comprehension can adversely may be viewed as areas of strength many aspects of attention (e.g., auditory
impact other aspects of cognition such as relative to individuals with other ge- sustained attention, visual selective at-
learning and memory. Lexical access, netic-related ID. tention) that extend beyond that ex-
syntactic awareness, and phonological pected for MA [Rowe et al., 2006;
skills are reduced in individuals with DS, Porter et al., 2007; Trezise et al., 2008;
Attention/Executive Functions
particularly if articulation does not Kogan et al., 2009; Lanfranchi et al.,
develop to an automatic level or if Executive functions (EF), at the lower 2010; Rhodes et al., 2010; Lee et al.,
representations of phonemic sequences level involve the regulatory components 2011; Costanzo et al., 2013]. Selective
fail to be consolidated into long-term of behavior and cognition including attention deficits persist through adult-
semantic memory [Gathercole and aspects of attention, inhibition, and hood [Cornish et al., 2001; Rowe et al.,
Baddeley, 1989; Gathercole et al., processing speed [Alvarez and Emory, 2006; Breckenridge et al., 2013] and
1991; Adams and Gathercole, 1995, 2006]. Higher level EF include higher contribute to difficulty prioritizing,
2000; Grossberg and Myers, 2000; Laws, ordered cognitive processes of informa- staying engaged with a task, and con-
2002; Laws and Gunn, 2004; Miolo tion processing that include strategic sistently responding in the same manner
et al., 2005; Conners et al., 2008]. planning, impulse control, organized to certain situations, thus limiting one’s
Similarly, cognitive functioning in other search, flexibility of thought and action ability to function and ultimately reside
domains also impacts language [Alvarez and Emory, 2006]. Higher level independently.
138 AMERICAN JOURNAL OF MEDICAL GENETICS PART C: SEMINARS IN MEDICAL GENETICS INTRODUCTION

Inhibition and Baddeley, 1997; Jarrold et al., 2002; accuracy to MA matched controls
Conners et al., 2008]. [Vicari et al., 2000; Pennington et al.,
Poor response inhibition is evident
In school-aged children and ado- 2003; Rowe et al., 2006].
across the developmental lifespan,
lescents, further studies have suggested
emerging in toddlers and continuing
that reduced verbal working memory
through adulthood [Cornish et al., Set-Shifting/Multitasking
may reflect the absence of active
2007; Edgin et al., 2010; Lanfranchi
rehearsal [Jarrold et al., 2000; Vicari, Shifting mental set is particularly chal-
et al., 2010]. Greater difficulty is evident
2004; Silverman, 2007]; however, lenging for children and adults with DS
on verbally mediated inhibition tasks
Broadly and colleagues [1995] propose [Rowe et al., 2006; Costanzo et al.,
relative to visually mediated ones [Mu-
that a subvocal rehearsal strategy for 2013], especially on verbally-mediated
nir et al., 2000; Pennington et al., 2003;
storing and recalling verbally presented tasks [Hippolyte et al., 2009; Lanfranchi
Borella et al., 2013; Costanzo et al.,
items is employed. Interestingly, when et al., 2010; Landry et al., 2012].
2013]. Poor inhibition of irrelevant
examined longitudinally syntax com- Simultaneous processing is a core under-
information is also demonstrated in
prehension is associated with working lying weakness relative to TD-MA
toddlers, school-aged children, and
memory (auditory and visual) [Chap- matched children [Lanfranchi et al.,
adolescents [Cornish et al., 2007; Bor-
man et al., 2002], demonstrating the 2004, 2009].
ella et al., 2013], suggesting a general-
relationship between working memory
ized deficit in inhibitory control.
(across both modalities) and its inter-
Self-Monitoring
relatedness with continued language
Processing Speed acquisition. Individuals with DS, from childhood
From childhood and persisting through adulthood, have poor monitor-
Evaluation of reaction times show
across the lifespan, visuo-spatial short- ing of their verbal comprehension or are
mixed results, with speed of reaction
term memory remains relatively spared unable to execute and create a plan to
time shown to be consistent with
compared to verbal working memory, request clarification of instructions [Ab-
intellectual functioning [Silverman and
on tasks with brief amounts of informa- beduto et al., 1997, 2008]. Children,
Kim, 1997], but slower reaction time
tion or when visual and spatial compo- adolescents, and adults with DS often
relative to individuals with MA matched
nents are assessed separately [Jarrold and fail to signal that they do not understand
individuals with ID [Brunamonti et al.,
Baddeley, 1997; Jarrold et al., 2000, [Abbeduto et al., 1997, 1998, 2008] and
2011].
2002; Numminen et al., 2001; Seung when compared with TD-MA matched
and Chapman, 2004; Lanfranchi et al., controls, adolescents, and young adults
Short-Term/Working Memory 2004; Vicari, 2004; Rowe et al., 2006; with DS show lower frequency of
Visu-Petra et al., 2007]. As the amount signaling [Abbeduto et al., 2008]. Self-
Studies across the lifespan have consis- of information that needs to be proc- monitoring in adults remains vulner-
tently demonstrated that auditory work- essed increases (e.g., combined visual able, as poor monitoring for intrusion
ing memory is less developed than and spatial components), performance is errors and difficulty preventing irrele-
visuospatial working memory [McDade compromised in children and adoles- vant information from interfering with
and Adler, 1980; Marcell and Weeks, cents relative to MA matched controls cognitive processing of relevant infor-
1988; Hulme and Mackenzie, 1992; [Lanfranchi et al., 2004; Visu-Petra mation has been demonstrated [Kittler
Bower and Hayes, 1994; Marcell et al., et al., 2007], likely secondary to limi- et al., 2006].
1995; Seung and Chapman, 2000; tations in working memory storage
Jarrold et al., 2002; Lanfranchi et al., capacity.
2004, 2009; Hick et al., 2005; Miolo Comparison Studies of EF
et al., 2005; Chapman, 2006; Abbeduto Overall, individuals with DS show
et al., 2008; Conners et al., 2008; Planning/Organization
impairments in EF that extend beyond
Frenkel and Bourdin, 2009; Lott and Children with DS show difficulty those observed in individuals with ID of
Dierssen, 2010; Levy and Eilam, 2013]. executing a strategy to problem-solve unknown etiology and of those with
Deficits in verbal working memory [Lanfranchi et al., 2010]. For many comparable MA [Rowe et al., 2006;
extend beyond that observed in other individuals with DS, poorly organized Lanfranchi et al., 2010; Costanzo et al.,
individuals with ID, as well as beyond approaches to learning new skills and 2013]. These deficits appear even in
that which is expected for individuals difficulty rehearsing new information young children with DS [Gregory and
with hearing and speech articulation contribute to slowed developmental Hodges, 1996]. Relative to young
difficulties, with differences evident progress [Gilmore and Cuskelly, 2009; children with other genetic disorders
even in young, school-aged children Lott and Dierssen, 2010]. As children associated with ID, children with DS
that persist throughout adulthood [Mar- with DS mature, they take longer to show poorer sustained attention and
cell et al., 1988; Marcell and Weeks, execute actions in planning tasks, but are verbal inhibition [Brown et al., 2003;
1988; Bower and Hayes, 1994; Jarrold able to perform similarly with regard to Cornish et al., 2007; Porter et al., 2007;
INTRODUCTION AMERICAN JOURNAL OF MEDICAL GENETICS PART C: SEMINARS IN MEDICAL GENETICS 139

Scherif and Steele, 2011]. Young chil- relatively preserved even in the face of of TD children and also from that of
dren with DS also process information cognitive decline [Jarrold et al., 1999; individuals with similar levels of intel-
more slowly with more errors than Fidler et al., 2006; Costanzo et al., lectual functioning. Similar to many
children with Fragile X syndrome or 2013]. children with ID, children with DS
Williams Syndrome [Cornish et al., While early studies demonstrated show reduced learning capacities with
2007]. Children with DS show signifi- deficits in nonverbal ability relative to regard to both short-term and long-
cantly more difficulty with shifting and matched MA peers [Meyers et al., 1961], term memory [Rast and Meltzoff, 1995;
verbal aspects of memory than children more recent studies consistently show Carlesimo et al., 1997; Vicari et al.,
with Williams syndrome and TD-MA that visuospatial functioning (e.g., visual 2000, 2005; Brown et al., 2003; Clark
matched controls [Costanzo et al., processing, visuospatial short-term and Wilson, 2003; Constestible et al.,
2013]. memory, and visuoconstruction) devel- 2010]. Certain aspects of learning are
Individuals with DS show pervasive ops to a level commensurate with MA identified as strengths. Children with
deficits across modality in working [Wang and Bellugi 1994; Hodapp and DS perform favorably in paradigms of
memory when compared with individ- Zigler, 1997; Jarrold et al., 1999; Klein observational learning (observing the
uals with Williams syndrome and with and Mervis, 1999; Laws, 2002; Gath- actions of others) [Reed et al., 2011] and
TD children [Carney et al., 2013]. ercole and Alloway, 2006; Kogan et al., when associating objects with rewards
Working memory deficits in children 2009; Couzens et al., 2011]. Further [Kogan et al., 2009]; whereas, they have
and adolescents with DS were greater analyses of trends within the visuo- greater difficulty with instrumental
with regard to verbal stimuli [Lanfranchi spatial domain indicate that individuals learning (manipulating the environment
et al., 2010]. On parent and teacher with DS tend to approach visuo-spatial to meet their needs) [Wishart, 1993;
rating scales, young children with DS are information using a global approach to Ohr and Fagen, 1994; Reed et al.,
reported to show significant deficits in analyzing information [Bellugi et al., 2011]. Their higher social motivation
aspects of EF [Lee et al., 2011] when 1999; Carretti et al., 2013]. When and responsiveness to positive reinforce-
compared with TD children matched mistakes in nonverbal processing are ment likely make socially oriented
for MA [Daunhauer et al., 2014]. made, they are often characterized by a learning more successful. Visual learning
Deficits observed are in working mem- failure to perceive, or to accurately is also stronger than verbal learning, a
ory (nine times more likely than TD perceive, details within the information finding that is consistent with the
children to experience problems either [Bellugi et al., 1999; Carretti et al., strength in nonverbal ability relative to
at school or at home) and planning (six 2013]. Of note, nonverbal processing is verbal ability characterized in the cog-
to seven times more likely to experience often compromised when greater inte- nitive profile.
problems); parents also endorse signifi- gration and appreciation for more
cant problems with inhibitory control, complex information is necessary, sec-
Long-term Memory
whereas teachers do not [Daunhauer ondary to weakness in EF [Carretti et al.,
et al., 2014]. Individuals with DS show a 2013]. Individuals with DS have deficits on
pattern of global executive dysfunction, When comparing children with DS tasks of explicit verbal and nonverbal
similar to that observed in individuals and those with fragile X syndrome, long-term memory [Carlesimo et al.,
with Fragile X syndrome [Kirk et al., individuals with DS show strength in 1997; Vicari et al., 2000; Nadel, 2003;
2005; Hooper et al., 2008]. spatial learning and object discrimina- Kogan et al., 2009]. Problems are
tion tasks, but have greater difficulty believed to occur at the levels of
with visual-perceptual and visual-spatial encoding and retrieval [Carlesimo
Visuo-Spatial Ability
reversal learning tasks [Kogan et al., et al., 1997] and are adversely impacted
In contrast to the commonly held view 2009]. Similarly, children with DS have by attention deficits [Brown et al., 2003;
that nonverbal skills are a strength in weakness in visual-perceptual abilities in Clark and Wilson, 2003; Krinsky-
individuals with DS, a recent review by the context of relatively preserved McHale et al., 2008] and high process-
Yang and colleagues [2014] found that visual-spatial abilities when compared ing demands [Lanfranchi et al., 2004;
nonverbal, visuospatial processing shows to children with Williams syndrome Rowe et al., 2006; Visu-Petra et al.,
an uneven profile of skills, with some [Vicari et al., 2005]. These findings may 2007]. In addition, deficits in memory
aspects of visuo-spatial abilities com- reflect the aforementioned vulnerability consolidation may exist secondary to
mensurate with general cognitive ability in tasks with higher EF demands. temporal lobe and hippocampal dys-
[Yang et al., 2014]; whereas, other function [Carlesimo et al., 1997; Pen-
aspects are below expected develop- nington et al., 2003; Belichenko et al.,
Learning
mental level [Yang et al., 2014]. In some 2004, 2009; Lott and Dierssen, 2010;
instances, visuo-spatial skills therefore Individuals with DS show the capacity Kleschevnikov et al., 2012]. Therefore,
remain an area of strength relative to to learn and acquire new skills; however, memory deficits may be primary in
weaker verbal skills and there is some the rate of learning and the range of skills nature and do not exist solely as a
evidence that visuo-spatial skills remain acquired often differs from the trajectory manifestation of deficits in language
140 AMERICAN JOURNAL OF MEDICAL GENETICS PART C: SEMINARS IN MEDICAL GENETICS INTRODUCTION

processing. These deficits exist across the distraction can occur subtly and is often high level of social motivation, can
lifespan [Ellis et al., 1989; Caltagirone not perceived as intentionally opposi- begin to decline with age. For example,
et al., 1990], but become more pro- tional; however, as the behavioral reg- older individuals with DS show signifi-
nounced with age. ulation demands increase with a typical cantly fewer and briefer smiles than
developmental trajectory, these behav- younger children [Fidler et al., 2005].
iors become more apparent. Parents and Fortunately, social reasoning skills
SOCIAL/EMOTIONAL/
teachers report a higher rate of non- amongst adults with DS remain rela-
BEHAVIORAL
compliance relative to siblings and TD tively preserved [Hippolyte et al., 2010].
FUNCTIONING
children [Pueschel et al., 1991; Cuskelly A survey of adolescents and adults with
Individuals with DS are commonly and Dadds, 1992; Coe et al., 1999]. Poor DS (n ¼ 284) shows that 99% of the
perceived as “charming,” “affectionate,” task persistence and stubbornness are participants express feeling happy with
“cheerful”, and “sociable” with other also commonly reported by caregivers their lives and express love for their
observed personality assets that include and educators [Kasari and Freeman, families [Skotko et al., 2011b].
kindness, humor, and forgiveness 2001]. These latter personality charac- Many individuals are able to live in
[Down, 1866; Dykens, 2007; Fidler teristics, when present, relate to reduced group settings with limited support,
et al., 2008]. These perceptions are levels of achievement when examined although studies show that most indi-
founded in behavioral research. For longitudinally [Gilmore and Cuskelly, viduals remain in their family’s home
example, children with DS seek out 2009]. Task persistence that is observed [Thomson et al., 1995; Pulsifer, 1996;
social interaction more frequently than early in childhood remains stable Dyke et al., 2013; Foley et al., 2013].
children with nonspecific intellectual throughout childhood and into early Greater levels of functional independ-
impairment and also demonstrate more adolescence [Gilmore and Cuskelly, ence have been found to predict
positive facial expressions relative to 2009]. participation in mainstream employ-
typically developing peers [Kasari The research also shows higher rates ment or occupational training when
et al., 1995; Kasari and Sigman, 1996; of externalizing behaviors are com- compared with individuals in sheltered
Jahromi et al., 2008]. Relative to other monly reported in early childhood and employment or alternatives to employ-
children with ID, children with DS are amongst school-aged children. For ex- ment situations [Foley et al., 2013]. It is
at a lower risk for psychopathology ample, higher rates of hyperactivity, important to note that parental advocacy
[Dykens and Kasari, 1997; Stores et al., impulsivity, tantrums, agitation, stub- and resourcefulness, as well as govern-
1998] and families of children with DS bornness, disruptiveness/argumenta- ment policy and procedures, serve as
report lower levels of stress [Hodapp and tiveness, repetitive movements, and mediating environmental factors that
Dykens, 2004; Fidler et al., 2008] and a sensory dysregulation are reported facilitate level of independence and
more positive outlook on life because of [Dykens and Kasari, 1997; Capone opportunities [Dyke et al., 2013].
their child with Down syndrome et al., 2006; Siegel and Smith, 2010]. Relative to individuals with ID secon-
[Skotko et al., 2011a]. For some individuals, disruptive behav- dary to other genetic disorders, individ-
Social development in DS appears iors may be secondary to deficits in uals with DS show fewer maladaptive
to unfold in a similar fashion to that of a expressive language communication behaviors [DiNuovo and Buono, 2011],
TD child; however, there are important [Skotko et al., 2013]. When comparing consistent with older studies reporting
qualitative differences in the develop- children with DS to those with non- fewer adaptive behavior problems [Dyk-
ment of emotional recognition, social specific ID, greater and more intense ens and Kasari, 1997; Chapman and
referencing, joint attention, spontane- displays of physical and vocal frustration Hesketh, 2000]. However, recent com-
ous gesturing, and lower levels of are observed [Jahromi et al., 2008] and parison shows that basic adaptive skills
mastery motivation [Mundy et al., may relate to a disproportionate weak- are reduced in individuals with DS than
1988; Kneips et al., 1994; Ruskin ness in verbal expression. Compulsive individuals with Prader–Willi, Fragile
et al., 1994; Franco and Wishart, behaviors are also a component of the X, and Williams syndrome [DiNuovo
1995; Legerstee and Weintraub, 1997; behavioral phenotype of DS, evident and Buono, 2011].
Glenn et al., 2001; Fidler et al., 2005; more prominently in children with DS While many adults with DS con-
Cebula et al., 2010]. Strengths emerge in than other children with ID [Evans and tinue to function relatively well, depres-
imitation abilities and in pro-social, Gray, 2000; Maatta et al., 2006]. sion is observed in higher rates than
empathic behaviors [Down 1866; Kasari As maturation occurs, individuals other individuals with ID in adulthood
et al., 2003]. While social motivation is a can experience higher rates of internal- [Collacott et al., 1992; Maatta et al.,
strength in many contexts, studies show izing symptoms (e.g., social withdrawal, 2006]. Psychiatric problems such as
that children with DS often use social depression, anxiety, secretive behavior), attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder,
distraction behaviors as a means of whereas externalizing behaviors tend to obsessive-compulsive disorder, and self-
preventing the completion of a re- decline [Cooper and Collacott, 1994; injury behaviors may impact a consid-
quested task [Pitcairn and Wishart, Evans and Gray, 2000; Dykens, 2007; erable number of individuals with DS
1994; Kasari and Freeman, 2001]. Social Siegel and Smith, 2010]. Traits, such as a [Maatta et al., 2006; Siegel and Smith,
INTRODUCTION AMERICAN JOURNAL OF MEDICAL GENETICS PART C: SEMINARS IN MEDICAL GENETICS 141

2010]. However, debilitating mental with DS than in other adults with ID amongst the first observable signs [Chic-
health conditions, including bipolar [Zigman and Lott, 2007; Krinsky- oine et al., 1994; Devenny et al., 1996;
and schizophrenia, are relatively rare McHale and Silverman, 2013]; however, Zigman et al., 1996; Oliver et al., 1998;
[Collacott et al., 1992; Craddock and prevalence rates of dementia amongst Krinsky-McHale et al., 2002; Ball et al.,
Owen, 1994; Patti and Tsiouris, 2006]. individuals with DS vary greatly in the 2006; Krinsky-McHale and Silverman,
In adulthood, the emergence and se- literature (8–100%) [Zigman et al., 2013]. In some cases, impairments in EF
verity of psychological problems can be 1996]. This variability is likely the are associated with the emergence of
associated with the beginning stages of manifestation of various sample charac- dementia [Rowe et al., 2006; Deb et al.,
dementia [Haverman et al., 1994; teristics within each study, the method- 2007; Ball et al., 2008], particularly
Maatta et al., 2006; Dykens, 2007]. In ology used, and the time period in which compromised attention and planning
fact, depression with apathy and with- the studies were conducted. While many ability [Das et al., 1995]. Other symp-
drawal is believed to be a prodromal individuals with DS are not impacted by toms associated with changes in the
feature of dementia in DS [Burt et al., dementia until after age 50 [Devenny frontal lobes (apathy, depression, im-
1995] and marked changes in person- et al., 1996, 2000; Roizen and Patterson, paired adaptive functioning, and reduced
ality and behavior are early indicators of 2003; Couzens et al., 2011], by age 60 communication) may indicate under-
cognitive decline and dementia in the approximately 75% of individuals with lying neurodegeneration secondary to
DS population [Nelson et al., 1995; DS experience symptoms of AD [Roizen AD [Zigman et al., 1996; Lott and Head,
Holland et al., 1998, 2000; Ball et al., and Patterson, 2003] and beyond age 60 2001; Ball et al., 2006; Constestible et al.,
2008; Adams and Oliver, 2010]. Higher there is often severe cognitive decline 2010].
rates of depressed mood, restlessness, [Wisniewski et al., 1985; Lai and Early detection of AD is associated
hyperactivity, sleep disturbance, exces- Williams, 1989; Chapman and Hesketh, with progressive impairment in selective
sive uncooperativeness and auditory 2000]. The incidence rates of initial attention as early as 2 years prior to a
hallucinations are observed with aging diagnosis are also reported: 49 years dementia diagnosis and it is generally
[Cooper and Prasher, 1998; Maatta ¼ 8.9%; 50–54 years ¼ 17.7%; 55–59 accompanied by episodic memory loss
et al., 2006; Dykens, 2007]. Cognitive years ¼ 32.1%; 60 ¼ 25.6% [Coppus [Krinsky-McHale et al., 2008]. Older
deterioration, as well as co-morbid et al., 2006]. The average age of diagnosis studies propose impaired visual short-
emotional and behavioral changes, of dementia is estimated to be around 55 term memory as an early sign [Dalton and
commonly mandate more intensive years with a median survival rate of 7 Crapper-McLachlan, 1986]; however,
supports [Pulsifer, 1996]. years following diagnosis [McCarron subsequent studies demonstrate deficits
et al., 2014]. Life expectancy has been in this domain that are present through-
extended to well over 60 years secondary out youth and adolescence [Lanfranchi
UNIQUE
to advancements in medical care [Glasson et al., 2004; Rowe et al., 2006; Visu-
CHARACTERISTICS OF THE
et al., 2002; Roizen and Patterson, 2003; Petra et al., 2007]; thus, this factor is not
DOWN SYNDROME
Bittles and Glasson, 2004; Bittles et al., considered predictive. A clear differ-
POPULATION
2006; Constestabile et al., 2010]. Older entiation between individuals with DS
age is identified as a strong risk factor to diagnosed with dementia and those
Dementia
develop dementia; whereas, gender and without dementia is observed (mean
Individuals with DS are at an increased severity of ID are not identified as age ¼ 47 years), particularly in the cog-
risk for developing early-onset Alz- predictors of developing dementia in nitive domains of memory and EF even
heimer disease (AD), as this is associated DS [Krinsky-McHale and Silverman, after controlling for age and degree of ID
with several genetic factors that are 2013]. [Ball et al., 2008].
overexpressed on chromosome 21 [Lai One of the main challenges in
and Williams, 1989; Evenhuis, 1990; providing care for aging individuals
Sensory Impairment
Visser et al., 1997; Lott, 2012]: (1) with DS is the task of differentiating
overexpression of amyloid precursor age-related decline from progressive Sensory impairments are commonly
protein resulting in B-amyloid plaques cognitive decline associated with de- observed in individuals with DS and
[Prasher et al., 1998; Bush and Beail, mentia. The vast majority of individuals can adversely affect learning and cogni-
2004; Constestabile et al., 2010; Krinsky with DS have neuropathological changes tive functioning [Maatta et al., 2006]. It is
and Silverman, 2013]; (2) degeneration associated with AD; however, some do estimated that between 38 and 78% of
of cholinergic neurons [Schliebs and not exhibit symptoms of dementia individuals with DS experience hearing
Arendt, 2006; Kleschevnikov et al., [Chicoine et al., 1994; Devenny et al., loss and approximately 80% of children
2012], and (3) disruption in the short- 1996; Zigman et al., 1996; Oliver et al., ages 5–12 years show vision problems,
lasting biochemical synaptic events that 1998; Krinsky-McHale et al., 2002; Ball such as refractive errors, strabismus, and/
serve to consolidate memory informa- et al., 2006; Krinsky-McHale and Silver- or nystagums [Roizen and Patterson,
tion [Lott and Dierssen, 2010]. AD man, 2013]. Of those who are diagnosed 2003]. Augmentative supports and thera-
occurs more frequently in individuals with dementia, there is a variation pies are key components to supporting
142 AMERICAN JOURNAL OF MEDICAL GENETICS PART C: SEMINARS IN MEDICAL GENETICS INTRODUCTION

cognitive development and functioning et al., 2007; Molloy et al., 2009; Siegel language, syntax, articulation, phono-
and are needed in higher frequency in the and Smith, 2010], which possesses its logical processes, and verbal working
DS population. own set of unique challenges. Diagnos- memory as the most vulnerable areas of
ing a behavioral disorder, such as ASD, is functioning. Variable functioning across
difficult to define in the DS population aspects of EF are observed. Optimal
Seizures
due to the behavioral diagnostic criteria performance is evident on succinct,
In individuals with DS, seizures occur at a of the syndrome that may overlap with concrete tasks supported by explicit
higher rate than is observed in the general phenotypical social communication pat- multisensory presentation. Weaknesses
population [Roizen and Patterson, 2003] terns associated with DS [Reilly, 2009]. emerge as the complexity, length, and
and often emerge during two main However, when co-morbid diagnosis is demands for integration increase, re-
developmental periods: infancy and clearly present, children are often more gardless of the modality in which the
advanced age. Infantile spasms are the severely cognitively impaired and show information is presented. Co-morbid
most commonly observed type of seizure greater delay in the development of factors described above (e.g., sensory
[Lott, 2012] and may persist into early language and adaptive behavior skills impairment, sleep disruption, etc.)
childhood; it is estimated that 5–13% of [Capone et al., 2005; Molloy et al., can further contribute to cognitive
children with DS have co-morbid seiz- 2009]. Additionally, dual-diagnosis is and behavioral difficulties and must
ures [Arya et al., 2011; Lujic et al., 2011; marked by a distinctive pattern of be recognized when examining
Lott, 2012]. These events can be partic- unusual stereotypic behavior, anxiety, functioning.
ularly detrimental, especially when they and social withdrawal [Carter et al., Specific patterns of cognition are
occur frequently, as they can disrupt 2007]. Together, these factors result also demonstrated across the lifespan
neuronal development and subsequently in reduced levels of functional both relative to matched MA-TD peers
contribute to further delays or disrup- independence. and to individuals with various genetic
tions in development. In later adulthood, syndromes associated with ID. Interest-
generalized seizures typically emerge in ingly, examination of cognition in
Other Common Medical Factors
the context of cognitive decline and individuals with DS is unique and differs
cortical atrophy [Lott, 2012]. The em- Additional conditions with higher in- from both of the aforementioned
phasis in this phase shifts to concern cidence within this population include groups. The results from these studies
surrounding a loss of functional skills and celiac disease, hypothyroidism, leuke- provide support for the observation of a
the advanced decline of cognitive func- mia, congenital heart defects, and unique cognitive phenotype for indi-
tioning [Lott and Dierssen, 2010]. diabetes [Roizen and Patterson, 2003; viduals with DS that is distinct from
Visootsak et al., 2011; Lott, 2012]. Left other individuals even with matched
untreated, these conditions may result in level of cognitive functioning. It is
Sleep Disruption
changes in cognition, behavior, and important to consider the demands of
Many individuals with DS experience energy level. Underdetection is com- the tasks when designing studies and
high rates of sleep problems, including mon, as a recent study showed that only interpreting results of others’ studies in
obstructive sleep apnea, reduced REM 9.8% of patients with DS were clinically relation to specific cognitive pheno-
sleep, and poor sleep initiation and up to date on the recommended screen- types, such as a relative weaknesses in
maintenance [Stores et al., 1998; Harvey ings from the American Academy of language processing for individuals in
and Kennedy, 2002; Dykens, 2007; Pediatrics [Skotko et al., 2013]. Atten- the DS population as such confounding
Capone et al., 2013]. These difficulties tion to and proper screening for a factors may inaccurately influence our
are associated with significantly poorer common co-morbid condition are interpretations [Channell et al., 2014b].
performance on tasks of attention and warranted. Importantly, recent neuroimaging and
EF [Beebe and Gozal, 2002; Chen et al., functional imaging studies (that are
2013; Breslin et al., 2014], as well as briefly described here) have demon-
DISCUSSION
memory storage/consolidation [Breslin strated both structural and functional
et al., 2014], and may exist in the context The literature of cognitive and behav- correlates consistent with the defined
of co-morbid depression [Capone et al., ioral functioning of individuals with DS cognitive pattern providing support for
2013]. Proper sleep hygiene and inter- reveals a pattern of development that is the unique phenotypic patterns ob-
ventions are an essential component to characterized by unique strengths and served across the lifespan.
optimizing cognitive functioning. weaknesses that emerge in stages Cognitive functioning directly in-
throughout the progression of the life- forms the ability to learn academic skills,
span. Key features of the DS phenotype as well as to apply knowledge to novel
Autism Spectrum Disorder
include relative strengths in nonverbal information to function independently.
Approximately 6–10% of children have abilities and social motivation. In con- Individuals with DS are highly socially
co-morbid autism spectrum disorder trast, weaknesses in language are motivated and show strengths in obser-
(ASD) [Kent et al., 1999; Lowenthal consistently observed, with expressive vational learning and nonverbal
INTRODUCTION AMERICAN JOURNAL OF MEDICAL GENETICS PART C: SEMINARS IN MEDICAL GENETICS 143

capabilities. However, many individuals factor is the variability among the language impairments. Amsterdam: John
Benjamins. pp 53–72.
with DS remain limited in their ability composition of participants within Abbeduto L, Murphy MM, Cawthon SW. 2003.
to function independently [Maatta et al., each study, the methodology used to Receptive language skills of adolescents and
2006]. As such, functional living skills examine functioning, and the limita- young adults with Down or Fragile X
syndrome. Am J Ment Retard 108:149–160.
should be incorporated in school cur- tions in reliability and validity of the Abbeduto L, Murphy MM, Kover ST, Giles ND,
riculum at an early age to support the tools used to quantify abilities. Karadottir S, Amman A, Bruno L, Kim JS,
development of these skills. The role of In conclusion, appreciating the Schroeder S, Anderson JA, Nollin KA.
2008. Signaling noncomprehension of
social, behavioral, and emotional func- unique cognitive and behavioral phe- language: A comparison of Fragile X and
tioning must also be considered. Com- notypes associated with DS will help Down syndrome. Am J Ment Retard
monly observed strengths and challenges professionals and parents understand 113:214–230.
Abbeduto L, Pavetto M, Kesin E, Weissman MD,
that are depicted within each devel- individuals with DS more fully, as well Karadottir S, O’Brien A. 2001. The lin-
opmental period should be assessed. The as inform treatments and instructional guistic and cognitive profile of Down
provision of behavioral and emotional methods for learning. Improved con- syndrome: Evidence from a comparison
with Fragile X syndrome. Down Synd Res
supports and opportunities for social- ceptualization of areas of strengths and Pract 7:9–15.
ization may improve overall level of weaknesses will help professionals and Abbeduto L, Short-Meyerson K, Benson G,
independence and self-care skill devel- families support the growth of the Dolish J. 1997. Signaling of noncompre-
hension by children and adolescents with
opment, and enhance overall quality of individual with DS to their highest level mental retardation. Effects of problem type
life. of independence, ultimately optimizing and speaker identity. J Speech Lang Hear
Intervention to improve function- their functioning and quality of life. Res 40:20–32.
Abbeduto L, Short-Meyerson K, Benson G,
ing and slow decline are underway and Dolish J, Weissman M. 1998. Understanding
several different approaches such as referential expressions: Use of common
Conflict of Interest
pharmacological treatments [Capone, ground by children and adolescents with
mental retardation. J Speech Lang Hear R
2010; Costa, 2011; De la Torre and Margaret Pulsifer, Ph.D, services in a 41:348–362.
Dierssen, 2012; Kleschevnikov et al., non-paid capacity on the Board of Adams AM, Gathercole SE. 1995. Phonological
2012], complementary and alternative Directors for the Massachusetts Down working memory and speech production in
preschool children. J Speech Hear Res
therapies and therapeutics diets [Salman, Syndrome Congress, a non-profit or- 38:403–414.
2002; Roizen, 2005], and cognitive ganization. Brian Skotko, M.D, MPP, Adams AM, Gathercole SE. 2000. Limitations in
rehabilitation strategies are being exam- serves in a non-paid capacity on the working memory: Implications for language
development. Int J Lang Commun Disord
ined [Broadley and MacDonald, 1993; Board of Directors or Scientific Advi- 35:95–116.
Comblain, 1994; Messer and Hasan, sory Boards for the Massachusetts Down Adams D, Oliver C. 2010. The relationship
1994; Laws et al., 1996; Conners et al., Syndrome Congress, Band of Angels between acquired impairments of executive
function and behavior change in adults with
2008]. The application of behavioral Foundation, and the National Center Down syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res
strategies, particularly applied behavior for Prenatal and Postnatal Down Syn- 54:393–405.
analysis techniques, also shows favorable drome Resources, all non-profit organ- Alvarez JA, Emory E. 2006. Executive function
results by increasing on-task learning izations. Dr. Skotko is the Co-Director and the frontal lobes: A meta-analytic
review. Neuropsych Rev 16:17–42.
behaviors and reducing challenging of the Massachusetts General Hospital Anderson JS, Nielsen JA, Ferguson MA, Burback
behaviors in order to support learning Down Syndrome Program and occa- MC, Cox ET, Dai L, Gerig G, Edgin JO,
[Feelin and Jones, 2005]. sionally gets remunerated from Down Korenberg JR. 2013. Abnormal brain syn-
chrony in Down syndrome. Neuroimage:
Considerations when interpreting syndrome non-profit organizations for Clin 2:703–715.
the information outlined above include speaking engagements about Down Arya R, Kabra M, Gulati S. 2011. Epilepsy in
the need for caution when applying this syndrome. He receives support for children with Down syndrome. Epileptic
Disord 13:1–7.
phenotypic pattern to all individuals clinical drug trials involving people Ball SL, Holland AJ, Hon J. 2006. Personality and
with DS, as the pattern is not intended to with Down syndrome from Hoff- behavior changes mark the early stages of
define each individual’s unique mann-La Roche, Inc. He has a sister Alzheimer’s disease in adults with Down’s
syndrome: Findings from a prospective
strengths and weaknesses. Alternatively, with Down syndrome. Authors Julie population-based study. Int J Geriatr Psy-
it should be used as a framework from Grieco, PsyD, Karen Seligsohn, Ph.D, chiatry 21:661–673.
which to guide assessment and inform and Allison Schwartz, M.D, have no Ball SL, Holland AJ, Treppner P, Watson PC,
Huppert FA. 2008. Executive dysfunction
interventions. When interpreting these actual or potential conflicts of interest to and its association with personality and
studies, one must also appreciate the report. behavior changes in the development of
relationship between performance and Alzheimer’s disease in adults with Down
syndrome and mild to moderate learning
competence. Many individuals do not
REFERENCES disabilities. Brit J Clin Psychol 47:1–29.
consistently demonstrate their full ca- Beacher F, Daly E, Simmons A, Prasher V, Morris
pacities on formal testing and therefore Abbeduto L, Chapman RS. 2005. Language R, Robinson C, Lovestone S, Murphy K,
results may not reflect their true level of development in Down syndrome and Fragile Murphy DGM. 2010. Brain anatomy and
X syndrome: Current research and implica- ageing in non-demented adults with
ability [Wishart and Duffy, 1990; Wish- tions for theory and practice. In: Fletcher P, Down’s syndrome: An in vivo MRI study.
art, 1995; Silverman, 2007]. Another Miller JF, editors. Developmental theory and Psychol Med 40:611–619.
144 AMERICAN JOURNAL OF MEDICAL GENETICS PART C: SEMINARS IN MEDICAL GENETICS INTRODUCTION

Beebe DW, Gozal D. 2002. Obstructive sleep Williams syndrome and Down syndrome. Cebula KR, Moore DG, Wishart JG. 2010. Social
apnea and the prefrontal cortex: Towards a Neuropsychologia 41:1037–1046. cognition in children with Down’s syn-
comprehensive model linking nocturnal Brunamonti E, Pani P, Papazachariadis O, Onorati drome: Challenges to research and theory
upper airway obstruction to daytime cogni- P, Albertini G, Ferraina S. 2011. Cognitive building. J Intellect Disabil Res 54:113–134.
tive and behavioral deficits. J Sleep Res control of movement in Down syndrome. Channell MM, Conners FA, Barth JM. 2014b.
11:1–16. Res Devel Disabil 32:1792–1797. Emotion knowledge in children and ado-
Beeghly M, Weiss-Perry B, Cicchetti D. 1990. Burt DB, Loveland KA, Chen Y, Chuang A, Lewis lescents with Down syndrome: A new
Beyond sensorimotor functioning: Early KR, Cherry L. 1995. Aging in adults with methodological approach. Am J Intellect
communicative and play development of Down syndrome: Report from a longitudi- Dev Disabil 119:405–421.
children with Down syndrome. In: Cicci- nal study. Am J Ment Retard 100:262–270. Channell MM, Thurman AJ, Kover ST, Abbeduto
chetti D, Beegley M, editors. Children with Bush A, Beail N. 2004. Risk factors for dementia L. 2014a. Patterns of change in nonverbal
Down syndrome: A developmental perspec- in people with Down syndrome: Issues in cognition in adolescents with Down syn-
tive. New York: Cambridge University assessment and diagnosis. Am J Ment Retard drome. Res Devel Disabil 35:2933–2941.
Press. pp 329–368. 109:83–97. Chapman R. 2003. Language and communication
Belichenko PN, Belichenko PV, Kleschevnikov Caltagirone C, Nocentini U, Vicari S. 1990. in individuals with Down syndrome. In:
AM, Salehi A, Reeves RH, Mobley WC. Cognitive functions in adult Down’s syn- Abbeduto L, editor. Language and commu-
2009. The “Down syndrome critical re- drome. Int J Neurosci 54:221–230. nication in mental retardation. San Diego,
gion” is sufficient in the mouse model to Capone GT. 2010. The emergence of pharmaco- CA: Academic Press. pp 1–34.
confer behavioral neurophysiological and therapies for cognitive function in Down Chapman R. 2006. Language learning in Down
synaptic phenotypes characteristic of Down syndrome. Am J Med Genet Part A syndrome: The speech and language profile
syndrome. J Neurosci 29:5938–5948. 152A:3026–3027. compared to adolescents with cognitive
Belichenko RV, Masliah E, Kleschevnikov AM, Capone GT, Aidikoff JM, Taylor K, Rykiel N. impairment of unknown origin. Down
VIllar AJ, Epstein CJ, Salehi A, Mobley WC. 2013. Adolescents and young adults with Synd Res Pract 10:61–66.
2004. Synaptic structural abnormalities in Down syndrome presenting to a medical Chapman R, Hesketh LJ. 2000. Behavioral
the Ts65Dn mouse model of Down syn- clinic with depression: Co-morbid obstruc- phenotype of individuals with Down syn-
drome. J Comp Neurol 480:281–298. tive sleep apnea. Am J Med Genet Part A drome. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev
Bello A, Onofrio D, Caselli MC. 2014. Nouns and 161A:2188–2196. 6:84–95.
predicates comprehension and production in Capone G, Goyal P, Ares W, Lannigan E. 2006. Chapman R, Hesketh L. 2001. Language,
children with Down syndrome. Res Devel Neurobehavioral disorders in children, ado- cognition, and short-term memory in
Disabil 35:761–775. lescents, and young adults with Down individuals with Down syndrome. Down
Bellugi U, Adolphs R, Cassidy C. 1999. Towards syndrome. Am J Med Genet C Semin Synd Res Pract 7:1–7.
the neural basis for hypersociability in a Med Genet 142C:158–172. Chapman R, Hesketh L, Kistler D. 2002.
genetic syndrome. Neuroreport 22:197– Capone GT, Grados MA, Kaufmann WE, Predicting longitudinal change in language
207. Bernad-Ripoll S, Jewell A. 2005. Down production and comprehension in individ-
Berkson G. 1960. An analysis of reaction time in syndrome and comorbid autism-spectrum uals with Down syndrome: Hierarchica
normal and mentally deficient young men. J disorder: Characterization using the aber- modeling. J Speech Lang Hear R 45:902–
Ment Defic Res 4:69–77. rant behavioral checklist. Am J Med Genet A 915.
Bittles AH, Bower C, Jussain R, Glasson EJ. 2006. 134:373–380. Chapman R, Schwartz S, Kay-Raining Bird E.
The four ages of Down syndrome. Eur J Carlesimo GA, Marotta L, Vicari S. 1997. Long- 1991. Language skills of children and
Public Health 17:221–225. term memory in mental retardation: Evi- adolescents with Down syndrome. I. Com-
Bittles AH, Glasson EJ. 2004. Clinical, social, and dence for a specific impairment in subjects prehension. J Speech Lang Hear R 34:1106–
ethical implications of changing life expect- with Down’s syndrome. Neuropsychologia 1120.
ancy in Down syndrome. Dev Med Child 35:71–79. Chen CC, Spano J, Edgin JO. 2013. The impact of
Neurol 46:282–286. Carney DPJ, Brown JH, Henry LA. 2013. sleep disruption on executive function in
Borella E, Carett B, Lanfranchi S. 2013. Inhibitory Executive function in Williams and Down Down syndrome. Res Devel Disabil
mechanisms in Down syndrome: Is there a syndrome. Res Devel Disabil 34:46–55. 34:2033–2039.
specific or general deficit? Res Devel Disabil Carr J. 1970. Mental and motor development in Chicoine B, McGuire D, Hebein S. 1994.
34:65–71. young mongol children. J Ment Defic Res Development of a clinic for adults with
Bower A, Hayes A. 1994. Short-term memory 14:205–220. Down syndrome. Ment Retard 32:100–106.
deficits and Down’s syndrome: A compara- Carr J. 1988. Six weeks to twenty-one years old: A Clark D, Wilson GN. 2003. Behavioral assessment
tive study. Down Syn Res Pract 2:47–50. longitudinal study of children with Down’s of children with Down syndrome using the
Breckenridge K, Atkinson J, Braddick O. 2013. syndrome and their families. J Child Psychol Reiss psychopathology scale. Am J Med
Attention in Williams syndrome and Psyc 29:407–431. Genet 118:210–216.
Down’s syndrome: Performance on the Carr J. 2000. Intellectual and daily living skills of Clausen J. 1968. Behavioral characteristics of
new early childhood attention battery. Br J 30-year-olds with Down’s syndrome: Con- Down syndrome subjects. Am J Ment Defic
Dev Psychol 31:257–269. tinuation of a longitudinal study. J Appl 73:118–126.
Breslin J, Spano G, Botzin R, Anand P, Nadel L, Intellect 13:1–16. Coe DA, Matson JL, Russell DW, Slifer KJ,
Edgin J. 2014. Obstructive sleep apnea Carr J. 2005. Stability and change in cognitive Capone GT, Baglio C, Stallings S. 1999.
syndrome and cognition in Down syn- ability over the life span: A comparison of Behavior problems of children with Down
drome. Dev Med Child Neur 56:657–684. populations with and without Down’s syndrome and life events. J Autism Dev
Broadley I, MacDonald J. 1993. Teaching short- syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res 49:915– Disord 29:149–156.
term memory skills to children with Down 928. Coggins TE, Carpenter RL, Owings NO. 1983.
syndrome. Down Synd Res Pract 1:56–62. Carretti B, Lanfranchi S, Mammarella IC. 2013. Examining early intentional communication
Broadley I, MacDonald J, Buckley S. 1995. Spatial-simultaneous and spatial-sequential in Down’s syndrome and nonretarded
Working memory in children with Down’s working memory in individuals with Down children. Br J Disord Commun 18:98–107.
syndrome. Down Syn Res Pract 3:3–8. syndrome: The effect of configuration. Res Collacott RA, Cooper SA, McGrother C. 1992.
Brock J, Jarrold C. 2005. Serial order reconstruc- Dev Disabil 34:669–675. Differential rates of psychiatric disorders in
tion in Down syndrome: Evidence for a Carter JC, Capone GT, Gray RM, Cox CS, adults with Down’s syndrome compared
selective deficit in verbal short-term mem- Kaufmann WE. 2007. Autistic-spectrum with other mentally handicapped adults. Br J
ory. J Child Psychol Psych 46:304–316. disorders in Down syndrome: Further Psyc 161:671–674.
Brown JH, Johnson MH, Paterson SJ, Gilmore R, delineation and distinction from other Comblain A. 1994. Working memory in Down’s
Longhi E, Karmiloff-Smith A. 2003. Spatial behavioral abnormalities. Am J Med Genet syndrome: Training the rehearsal strategy.
representation and attention in toddlers with Part B 144B:87–94. Down Synd Res Pract 2:123–126.
INTRODUCTION AMERICAN JOURNAL OF MEDICAL GENETICS PART C: SEMINARS IN MEDICAL GENETICS 145

Conners FA, Rosenquist CJ, Arnett L, Moore MS, Down syndrome. Res Dev Disabil 16:461– and Down syndrome. J Speech Lang Hear R
Hume LE. 2008. Improving memory span in 478. 55:1600–1612.
children with Down syndrome. J Intellect Daunhauer LA, Fidler DJ, Hahn L, Will E, Lee Ethan MK, Canfield MA. 2002. Impact of
Disabil Res 52:244–255. NR, Hepburn S. 2014. Profiles of everyday including elective pregnancy terminations
Constestabile A, Benefenat F, Gasparini L. 2010. executive functioning in young children before 20 weeks gestation on birth defect
Communication breaks-Down: From neu- with Down syndrome. Am J Intel Dev rates. Teratology 66:S32–S35.
rodevelopmental defects to cognitive dis- Disabil 119:303–318. Evans DW, Gray FL. 2000. Compulsive-like
abilities in Down syndrome. Prog Neurobiol De la Torre R, Dierssen M. 2012. Therapeutic behavior in individuals with Down syn-
91:1–22. approaches in the improvement of cognitive drome: Its relation to mental age level,
Cooper SA, Collacott RA. 1994. Clinical features performance in Down syndrome: Past, adaptive and maladaptive behavior. Child
and diagnostic criteria of depression in present, and future. Prog Brain Res 197:1– Dev 71:288–300.
Down’s syndrome. Br J Psychiatry 14. Evenhuis HM. 1990. The natural history of
165:399–403. Deb S, Hare M, Prior L. 2007. Symptoms of dementia in Down syndrome. Arch Neurol-
Cooper SA, Prasher VP. 1998. Maladaptive dementia among adults with Down’s syn- Chicago 47:263–267.
behaviors and symptoms of dementia in drome: A qualitative study. J Intellect Disabil Feelin KM, Jones EA. 2005. Addressing challeng-
adults with Down’s syndrome compared Res 51:726–739. ing behavior in children with Down
with adults with intellectual disability of Delabar JM, Thophile D, Rahmani Z, Chetouh Z, syndrome: The use of applied behavior
other etiologies. J Intellect Disabil Res Blouin JL, Prieur M, Noel B, Sinet PM. analysis for assessment and intervention.
42:293–300. 1993. Molecular mapping of twenty-four Presentation: International Conference on
Coppus A, Evenhuis HM, Verberne GJ, Visser F, features of Down syndrome on chromosome Developmental Issues in Down Syndrome.
van Gool P, Eikelenboom P, can Duijin C. 21. Eur J Hum Genet 1:114–124. Portsmouth UK.
2006. Dementia and mortality in persons Devenny DA, Krinsky-McHale SJ, Sersen G, Fidler DJ. 2005. The emerging Down syndrome
with Down’s syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Silverman WP. 2000. Sequence of cognitive behavioral phenotype in early childhood:
Res 50:768–777. decline in dementia in adults with Down’s Implications for practice. Infant Young
Cornish K, Munir F, Wilding J. 2001. A neuro- syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res 44:654– Child 18:86–103.
psychological and behavioral profile of 665. Fidler DJ, Hepburn S, Rogers S. 2006. Early
attention deficits in Fragile X syndrome. Devenny DA, Silverman VP, Hill AL, Jenkins E, learning and adaptive behavior in toddlers
Rev Neurol 33:S24–S29. Sersen EA, Wisniewski KE. 1996. Normal with Down syndrome: Evidence for an
Cornish K, Scerif C, Karmiloff-Smith A. 2007. ageing in adults with Down’s syndrome: A emerging behavioural phenotype? Down
Tracing syndrome- specific trajectories of longitudinal study. J Intellect Disabil Res Syn Res Pract 9:37–44.
attention across the lifespan. Cortex 43:672– 40:208–221. Fidler DJ, Most DE, Philofsky AD. 2008. The
685. Dicks-Mireaux MJ. 1972. Mental development of Down syndrome behavioural phenotype:
Costa ACS. 2011. On the promise of pharmaco- infants with Down syndrome. Am J Ment Taking a developmental approach. Down
therapies targeted at cognitive and neuro- Defic 77:26–32. Syn Res Pract Online 37–44.
degenerative components of Down DiNuovo S, Buono S. 2011. Behavioral pheno- Foley KR, Jacoby P, Girdler S, Bourke J, Pikora T,
syndrome. Devel Neurosci 33:414–427. types of genetic syndromes with intellectual Lennox N, Einfeld S, Llewellyn G, Par-
Costanzo F, Varuzza C, Menghini D, Addona F, disability: Comparison of adaptive profiles. menter TR, Leonard H. 2013. Functioning
Gianesini T, Vicari S. 2013. Executive Psy Res 3:440–445. and post-school transition outcomes for
functions in intellectual disabilities: A com- Down JLH. 1866. Observations on an ethnic young people with Down syndrome. Child
parison between Williams syndrome and classification of idiots. Lond Hosp Clin Lect Care Health Dev 39:789–800.
Down syndrome. Res Devel Disabil Rep 3:265–262. Fowler A, Gelman R, Gleitman L. 1994. The
34:1770–1780. Dyke P, Bourke J, Llewellyn G, Leonard H. 2013. course of language learning in children with
Couzens D, Cuskelly M, Haynes M. 2011. The experiences of mothers of young adults Down syndrome. In: Taer-Flusberg H,
Cognitive development and Down syn- editor. Constraints on language acquisition:
with an intellectual disability transitioning
drome: Age-related change on the Stan- Studies of atypical children. Hillsdale NJ:
from secondary school to adult life. J
ford-Binet test (fourth edition). Am J Intel Intellect Dev Disabil 38:149–162. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp 91–140.
Dev Disabil 116:181–204. Dykens EM. 2007. Psychiatric and behavioral Franco F, Wishart JG. 1995. The use of pointing
Couzens D, Haynes M, Cuskelly M. 2012. and other gestures by young children with
disorders in persons with Down syndrome.
Individual and environmental characteristics Down syndrome. Am J Ment Retard
Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev 13:272–
associated with cognitive development in 278. 100:160–182.
Down syndrome: A longitudinal study. J Frenkel S, Bourdin B. 2009. Verbal, visual, and
Dykens EM, Kasari C. 1997. Maladaptive behav- spatio-sequential short-term memory: As-
Appl Res Intellect 25:396–413. ior in children with Prader-Willi syndrome, sessment of the storage capacities of children
Craddock N, Owen M. 1994. Is there an inverse Down syndrome and non-specific mental
relationship between Down’s syndrome and and teenagers with Down’s syndrome. J
retardation. Am J Ment Retard 102:228–
bipolar affective disorder? Literature review Intellect Disabil Res 53:152–160.
237. Gasquoine PG. 2011. Cognitive impairment in
and genetic implications. J Intell Disabil Res Eadie PA, Fey ME, Douglas JM, Parsons CL. common, noncentral nervous system med-
38:613–620. 2002. Profiles of grammatical morphology ical conditions of adults and the elderly. J
Crombie MP, Gunn P. 1998. Early intervention, and sentence imitation in children with Clin Exp Neuropsych 33:486–496.
families, and adolescence with Down syn- specific language impairment and Down Gathercole SE, Alloway TP. 2006. Practitioner
drome. Inter J Disabil Dev Edu 45:253–281. syndrome. J Speech Lang Hear R 45:720– review: Short-term and working memory
Cuskelly M, Dadds M. 1992. Behavioural prob- 732. impairments in neurodevelopmental disor-
lems in children with Down’s syndrome and Edgin JO, Pennington BF, Mervis CB. 2010. ders: Diagnosis and remedial support. J
their siblings. J Child Psychol Psy 33:749– Neuropsychological components of intel- Child Psychol Psyc 47:4–15.
761. lectual disability: The contributions of Gathercole SE, Baddeley AD. 1989. The role of
Dalton AJ, Crapper-McLachlan DR. 1986. Clin- immediate, working, and associative mem- phonological memory in the development
ical expression of Alzheimer’s disease in ory. J Intellect Disabil Res 54:406–417. of complex verbal skills. In: vonEuler C,
Down’s syndrome. Psychiatr Clin North Ellis NR, Woodley-Zanthos P, Dulaney Cl. 1989. editor. Brain and reading. London: Macmil-
Am 9:659–670. Memory for spatial location in children, lian Press.
Dameron LE. 1963. Development of intelligence adults, and mentally retarded persons. Am J Gathercole SE, Willis C, Baddeley AD. 1991.
of infants with mongolism. Child Dev Ment Retard 93:521–526. Differentiating phonological memory and
34:733–738. Estigarriba B, Martin GE, Roberst JE. 2012. awareness of rhyme: Reading and vocabu-
Das JP, Divis B, Alexander J. 1995. Cognitive Cognitive, environmental, and linguistic lary development in children. Br J Psych
decline due to aging among persons with predictors of syntax in Fragile X syndrome 82:387–406.
146 AMERICAN JOURNAL OF MEDICAL GENETICS PART C: SEMINARS IN MEDICAL GENETICS INTRODUCTION

Gibson D. 1978. Down syndrome: The psychol- and research, 3rd ed. Mahwah NJ: Erlbaum. development in atypical children. Hillsdale,
ogy of mongolism. Cambridge, England: pp 115–136. New Jersey: Erlbaum. pp 109–130.
Cambridge University Press. Holland AJ, Hon J, Huppert FA, Stevens F. 2000. Kent L, Evans J, Paul M, Shapr M. 1999.
Gilmore L, Cuskelly M. 2009. A longitudinal Incidence and course of dementia in people Comorbidity of autistic spectrum disorders
study of motivation and competence in with Down’s syndrome: Findings from a in children with Down syndrome. Dev Med
children with Down syndrome: Early child- population-based study. J Intel Disabil Res Child Neurol 41:153–158.
hood to early adolescence. J Intellect Disabil 44:138–146. Kernan KT, Sabsay S. 1996. Linguistic and
Res 53:484–492. Holland AJ, Hon J, Hubbert AJ, Stevens F, Watson cognitive ability of adults with Down
Glasson EJ, Sullivan SG, Hussain R, Petterson BA, P. 1998. Population-based study of the syndrome and mental retardation of un-
Montgomery PD, Bittles AH. 2002. The prevalence and presentation of dementia in known etiology. J Commun Disord 29:401–
changing survival profile of people with adults with Down’s syndrome. Br J Psyc 422.
Down’s syndrome: Implications for genetic 172:493–498. Kirk JW, Mazzocco MM, Kover ST. 2005.
counseling. Clin Gen 62:390–393. Hooper SR, Hatton D, Sideris J, Sullivan K, Assessing executive dysfunction in girls
Glenn S, Dayus B, Cunningham C, Horgan M. Hammer J, Schaaf J. 2008. Executive with Fragile X or Turner syndrome using
2001. Mastery motivation in children with functions in young males with fragile X the Contingency Naming Test. Dev Neuro-
Down syndrome. Down Synd Res Pract syndrome in comparison to mental age- psychol 28:755–777.
7:52–59. matched controls: Baseline findings from a Kittler P, Krinsky-McHale SJ, Devenny DA. 2004.
Gregory CA, Hodges JR. 1996. Clinical features longitudinal study. Neuropsychol 22:36–47. Sex differences in performance over 7 years
of frontal lobe dementia in comparison to Hsu LY. 1998. In: Milunsky A, editor. Genetic on the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for
Alzheimer’s disease. J Neural Transm Suppl disorders and the fetus. Baltimore: Johns Children- Revised among adults with
47:103–123. Hopkins Univ Press. pp 179–248. intellectual disability. J Intellect Disabil Res
Grossberg S, Myers CW. 2000. The resonant Hulme C, Mackenzie S. 1992. Working memory 48:114–122.
dynamics of speech perception: Interword and severe learning difficulties. New Jersey: Kittler P, Krinsky-McHale SJ, Devenny DA. 2006.
integration and duration-dependent back- Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp 1–141. Verbal intrusions precede memory decline in
ward effects. Psychol Rev 107:735–767. Jahromi LB, Gulsrud A, Kasari C. 2008. Emo- adults with Down syndrome. J Intellect
Guralnick MJ. 2002. Involvement with peers: tional competence in children with Down Disabil Res 50:1–10.
Comparisons between young children with syndrome: Negativity and regulation. Am J Klein BP, Mervis CB. 1999. Contrasting patterns
and without Down’s syndrome. J Intel Ment Ret 113:32–43. of cognitive abilities of 9- and 10- year olds
Disabil Res 46:379–393. Jarrold C, Baddeley AD. 2001. Short-term with Williams syndrome or Down syn-
Haier RJ, Head K, Head E, Lott IT. 2008. memory in Down syndrome: Applying the drome. Dev Neuropsychol 16:177–196.
Neuroimaging of individuals with Down’s working memory model. Down Synd Res Kleschevnikov AM, Belichenko PV, Salehi A, Wu
syndrome at-risk for dementia: Evidence for Pract 7:17–23. C. 2012. Discoveries in Down syndrome:
possible compensatory events. NeuroImage Jarrold C, Baddeley AD. 1997. Short-term Moving basic science to clinical care. Prog
39:1324–1332. memory for verbal and visuospaital infor- Brain Res 197:199–221.
Harvey MT, Kennedy CH. 2002. Polysomno- mation in Down’s syndrome. Cog Neuro- Kneips LJ, Walden TA, Baxter A. 1994. Affective
graphic phenotypes in developmental dis- psyc 2:101–122. expressions of toddlers with and without
abilities. Int J Dev Neurosci 20:443–448. Jarrold C, Baddely AD, Hewes AK. 1999. Down syndrome in a social referencing
Hauser-Cram P, Warfield ME, Shonkoff JP, Krauss Genetically dissociated components of context. Am J Ment Retard 99:301–312.
MW, Upshur CC, Sayer A. 1999. Family working memory evidence from Down’s Kogan CS, Boutet I, Cornish K, Graham GE,
influences on adaptive development in and William’s syndrome. Neuropsychologia Berry-Kravis E, Drouin A, Milgram NW.
young children with Down syndrome. Child 37:637–651. 2009. A comparative neuropsychological
Dev 70:979–989. Jarrold C, Baddeley AD, Hewes AK. 2000. test battery differentiates cognitive signatures
Haverman MJ, Maaskant MA, van Schrojenstein Verbal short-term memory deficits in of Fragile X and Down syndrome. J Intellect
Lantman HM, Urlings HF, Kessels AG. Down syndrome: A consequence of prob- Disabil Res 53:125–142.
1994. Mental health problems in elderly lems in rehearsal. J Child Psychol Psyc Korenberg JR, Chen XN, Schipper R, Sun Z,
people with and without Down syndrome. J 40:233–244. Gonsky R, Gerwehr S, Carpenterc N,
Intel Disabil Res 38:341–355. Jarrold C, Baddeley AD, Phillips C. 2002. Verbal Daumer C, Dignan P, Disteche C, Graham
Hick RF, Botting N, Conti-Ramsden G. 2005. short-term memory in Down syndrome: A JM, Hugdins L, McGillvray B, Miyazaki K,
Short-term memory and vocabulary de- problem of memory, audition, or speech? J Ogaswara N, Park JP, Pagon R, Pueschel S,
velopment in children with Down syn- Speech Lang Hear Res 45:531–544. Sack G, Say B, Schuffenhauer S, Soukup
drome and children with specific language Joffe V, Varlokosta S. 2007. Patterns of syntactic Yamanaka T. 1994. Down syndrome phe-
impairment. Dev Med Child Neurol development in children with Williams notypes: The consequences of chromosomal
47:532–538. syndrome and Down’s syndrome: Evidence imbalance. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
Hippolyte L, Iglesias K, Barisnikov K. 2009. A from passives and wh- questions. Clin 91:4997–5001.
new emotional Stroop-like task: Application Linguist Phon 21:705–727. Krinsky-McHale SJ, Devenny DA, Kittler P,
to the Down syndrome population. Arch Kasari C, Freeman SFN. 2001. Task-related social Silverman W. 2008. Selective attention
Clin Neuropsych 24:293–300. behavior in children with Down syndrome. deficits associated with mild cognitive
Hippolyte L, Iglesias K, van der Linden M, Am J Ment Retard 106:253–264. impairment and early stage Alzheimer’s
Barisnikov K. 2010. Social reasoning skills in Kasari C, Freeman SFN, Bass W. 2003. Empathy disease in adults with Down syndrome.
adults with Down syndrome: The role of and response to distress in children with Am J Ment Ret 113:369–386.
language, executive functions and socio- Down syndrome. J Child Psychol Psyc Krinsky-McHale SJ, Devenny DA, Silverman WP.
emotional behavior. J Intellect Disabil Res 44:424–431. 2002. Changes in explicit memory associ-
54:714–726. Kasari C, Freeman S, Mundy P, Sigman MD. 1995. ated with early dementia in adults with
Hodapp RM, Dykens EM. 2004. Genetic and Attention regulation by children with Down Down’s syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res
behavioural aspects: Application to malad- syndrome: Coordinated joint attention and 46:198–208.
aptive behavior and cognition. In: Rondal J, social referencing looks. Am J Ment Retard Krinsky-McHale SJ, Silverman W. 2013. Demen-
Hodapp RM, Soresi S, Dykens E, Nota E, 100:128–136. tia and mild cognitive impairment in adults
editors. Intellectual disabilities: Genetics, Kasari C, Mundy P, Yirmiya N, Sigman M. 1990. with intellectual disability: Issues of diag-
behavior, and inclusion. London: Whurr. Affect and attention in children with Down nosis. Devel Disabil 18:31–42.
pp 13–48. syndrome. Am J Ment Retard 95:55–67. Lai F, Williams RS. 1989. A prospective study of
Hodapp RM, Zigler E. 1997. New issues in the Kasari C, Sigman M. 1996. Expression and Alzheimer disease in Down syndrome. Arch
developmental approach to mental retarda- understanding of emotion in atypical devel- Neurol-Chicago 46:849–853.
tion. In: MacLean W, editor. Ellis’ handbook opment: Autism and Down syndrome. In: Landry O, Russo N, Dawkins T, Zelazo PD,
of mental deficiency, psychological theory Lewis M, Sullivan MW, editors. Emotional Burack JA. 2012. The impact of verbal and
INTRODUCTION AMERICAN JOURNAL OF MEDICAL GENETICS PART C: SEMINARS IN MEDICAL GENETICS 147

nonverbal development on executive func- Maatta T, Tervo Maatta, Taanila A, Kaski M, memory skills in children with Down
tion in Down syndrome and Williams Ilvanainen M. 2006. Mental health, behavior syndrome: A meta-analytic review. Res
syndrome. J Devel Disabil 18:26–35. and intellectual abilities of people with Develop Disabil 32:2225–2234.
Lanfranchi S, Cornoldi C, Vianello R. 2004. Down syndrome. Down Synd Res Pract Nelson L, Johnson JK, Freedman M, Lott I, Groot
Verbal and visuospatial working memory 11:37–43. J, Chang M. 2005. Learning and memory as
deficits in children with Down syndrome. Marcell M, Harvery C, Cotheran L. 1988. An a function of age in Down syndrome: A
Am J Ment Retard 109:456–466. attempt to improve auditory short-term study using animal-based tasks. Prog Neuro-
Lanfranchi S, Jerman O, Dal Pont E, Alberti A, memory in Down’s syndrome individuals Psychoph 29:443–453.
Vianello R. 2010. Executive function in through reducing distractions. Res Devel Nelson L, Lott I, Touchette P, Satz P, D’Elia L.
adolescents with Down syndrome. J Intel Disabil 9:405–417. 1995. Detection of Alzheimer disease in
Disabil Res 54:308–319. Marcell M, Ridgeway M, Sewell D, Whelan M. individuals with Down syndrome. Am J
Lanfranchi S, Jerman O, Vianello R. 2009. 1995. Sentence imitation by adolescents and Ment Retard 99:616–622.
Working memory and cognitive skills in young adults with Down’s syndrome and Nettlebeck T, Brewer N. 1976. Effects of
individuals with Down syndrome. Child other intellectual disabilities. J Intel Disabil stimulus-response variables on the choice
Neuropsychol 15:397–416. Res 39:215–232. reaction time of mildly retarded adults. Am J
Laws G. 2002. Working memory in children and Marcell M, Weeks S. 1988. Short-term memory Ment Defic 81:85–92.
adolescents with Down syndrome: Evidence difficulties and Down’s syndrome. J Ment Numminen H, Service E, Ahonen T, Ruoppila I.
from a colour memory experiment. J Child Defic Res 32:153–162. 2001. Working memory and everyday
Psychol Psyc 45:1–11. McCarron M, McCallion P, Reilly E, Mulryan N. cognition in adults with Down syndrome.
Laws G, Bishop DVM. 2003. A comparison of 2014. A prospective 14-year longitudinal J Intel Disabil Res 45:157–168.
language abilities in adolescents with Down follow-up of dementia in persons with Ohr PS, Fagen JW. 1994. Contingency learning in
syndrome and children with specific lan- Down syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res 9-month-old infants with Down syndrome.
guage impairments. J Speech Lang Hear R 58:61–70. Am J Ment Retard 99:74–84.
46:1324–1329. McDade H, Adler S. 1980. Down syndrome and Oliver B, Buckley S. 1994. The language develop-
Laws G, Gunn D. 2004. Phonological memory as a short-term memory impairment: A storage ment of children with Down’s syndrome:
predictor of language comprehension in or retrieval deficit?. Am J Ment Defic First words to two-word phrases. Down Syn
Down syndrome: A five-year follow-up 84:561–567. Res Pract 2:71–75.
study. J Child Psychol Psyc 45:326–337. Messer DJ, Hasan PJ. 1994. Early communication Oliver C, Crayton L, Holland A. 1998. A four year
Laws G, MacDonald J, Buckley S. 1996. The and cognition in children with Down’s prospective study of age-related cognitive
effects of a short training in the use of a syndrome. Down Syn Res Pract 2:3–10. change in adults with Down’s syndrome.
rehearsal strategy on memory for words and Meyers CE, Dingman HF, Attwell AA. 1961. Psychol Med 28:1365–1377.
pictures in children with Down syndrome. Comparative abilities of normal and retar- Oller K, Siebert JM. 1988. Babbling in prelin-
Down Syn Res Pract 4:70–78. dates of mental age 6 years on a factor-type guistic retarded children. Am J Ment Retard
Lee NR, Fidler DJ, Blakeley-Smith A, Daunhauer test battery. Am J Ment Defic 66:250–258. 92:369–375.
L, Robinson C, Hepburn S. 2011. Care- Miller EC. 1998. A longitudinal investigation of Owens RE, MacDonald JD. 1982. Communica-
giver-report of executive functioning in a the effects of congenital heart disease on the tive uses of the early speech of nondelayed
population-based sample of young children development of young children with Down and Down syndrome children. Am J Ment
with Down syndrome. Am J Intel Devel syndrome. Boston College Dissertations and Defic 86:503–510.
Disabil 116:290–304. Theses: AA I9901372. Patti PJ, Tsiouris JA. Psychopathology in adults
Legerstee M, Weintraub J. 1997. The integration Miller JF. 1995. Individual differences in vocabu- with Down syndrome: Clinical findings
of person and object attention in infants with lary acquisition in children with Down from an outpatient clinic Int J Disabil
and without Down syndrome. Infant Behav syndrome. In: Epstein J, Hassold T, Lott I, Human Dev 52006; 357–364
Dev 20:71–82. Nadel L, Patterson Q, editors. Etiology and Pennington BF, Moon J, Edgin J, Stedron J, Nadel
Levy Y, Eilam A. 2013. Pathways to language: A pathogenesis in Down syndrome. New I. 2003. The neuropsychology of Down
naturalistic study of children with Williams York: Wiley-Liss, Inc. pp 93–103. syndrome: Evidence for hippocampal dys-
syndrome and children with Down syn- Miller JF. 1999. Profiles of language development in function. Child Dev 74:75–93.
drome. J Child Lang 40:106–138. children with Down syndrome. In: Miller F, Pitcairn TK, Wishart JG. 1994. Reactions of young
Lincoln AJ, Courchesne E, Kilman BA. 1985. Leddy M, Leavitt LA, editors. Improving the children with Down syndrome to an impos-
Neuropsychological correlates of informa- communication of people with Down syn- sible task. Br J Dev Psychol 12:485–489.
tion-processing by children with Down drome. Baltimore: Paul H Brookes. pp 11–40. Polisenska K, Kapalkova S. 2014. Language
syndrome. Am J Ment Defic 89:403–414. Miolo G, Chapman RS, Sindberg HA. 2005. profiles in children with Down syndrome
Logan GD. 1985. On the ability to inhibit simple Sentence comprehension in adolescents and children with language impairment:
thoughts and actions: II. Stop-signal studies with Down syndrome and typically devel- Implications for early intervention. Res Dev
of repetition priming. J Exp Psychol Learn oping children: Role of sentence voice, Disabil 35:373–382.
11:675–691. visual context, and auditory-verbal short- Porter MA, Coltheart M, Langdon R. 2007. The
Lott IT. 2012. Neurological phenotypes of Down term memory. J Speech Lang Hear Res neuropsychological basis of hypersociability
syndrome across the life span. Prog Brain 48:172–188. in Williams and Down syndrome. Neuro-
Res 197:101–121. Molloy CA, Murray DS, Kinsman A, Castillo H, psychologia 45:2839–2849.
Lott IT, Dierssen M. 2010. Cognitive deficits and Mitchell T, Hickey FJ, Patterson B. 2009. Prasher VP, Farrer MJ, Kessling AM. 1998.
associated neurological complications in Differences in the clinical presentation of Molescular mapping of Alzheimer-type
individuals with Down’s syndrome. Lancet Trisomy 21 with and without autism. J dementia in Down’s syndrome. Ann Neurol
9:623–633. Intellect Disabil Res 53:143–151. 43:380–383.
Lott IT, Head E. 2001. Down syndrome and Mundy P, Sigman M, Kasari C, Yirmiya N. 1988. Pueschel SM, Gallagheer PL, Zartler AS. 1987.
Alzheimer’s disease: A link between devel- Nonverbal communication skills in Down Cognitive and learning processes in children
opment and aging. Ment Retard Dev Disabil syndrome children. Child Dev 59:235–249. with Down syndrome. Res Dev Disabil
Res Rev 7:172–178. Munir F, Cornish K, Wilding J. 2000. A neuro- 8:21–37.
Lowenthal R, Paula CS, Schwartzman JS, Brunoni D, psychological profile of attention deficits in Pueschel SM, Lousi S, McKnight P. 1991. Seizure
Mercadante MT. 2007. Prevalence of pervasive young males with Fragile X syndrome. disorders in Down syndrome. Arch Neurol
developmental disorder in Down’s syndrome. J Neuropsychologia 38:1261–1270. 48:318–320.
Autism Dev Disord 37:1394–1395. Nadel L. 2003. Down’s syndrome: A genetic Pulsifer MB. 1996. The neuropsychology of
Lujic L, Bosnjak VM, Delin S, Duranovic V, disorder in biobehavioral perspective. Genes mental retardation. J Int Neuropsych
Krakar G. 2011. Infantile spasms in children Brain Behav 2:159–166. 2:159–176.
with Down syndrome. Collegium Antropol Naess KB, Lyster SH, Hulme C, Melby-Lervag M. Purser HRM, Jarrold C. 2005. Impaired verbal
35:213–218. 2011. Language and verbal short-term short-term memory in Down syndrome
148 AMERICAN JOURNAL OF MEDICAL GENETICS PART C: SEMINARS IN MEDICAL GENETICS INTRODUCTION

reflects a capacity limitation rather than Seung HK, Chapman RS. 2000. Digit span in with Down’s syndrome. Disabil Society
atypically rapid forgetting. J Exp Child individuals with Down syndrome and typi- 10:325–340.
Psychol 91:1–23. cally developing children: Temporal aspects. J Thoradottir ET, Chapman RS, Wagner L. 2002.
Rahmani Z, Blouin JL, Creau-Goldberg N, Speech Lang Hear Res 43:609–620. Complex sentence production by adoles-
Watkins PC, Mattei JF, Poissonnier M, Seung HK, Chapman RS. 2004. Sentence cents with Down syndrome. Appl Psychol
Prieur M, Chetrouh Z, Nicole A, Aurias memory of individuals with Down’s syn- 23:163–183.
A, Sinet PM, Delabar JM. 1989. Critical role drome and typically developing children. J Teipel SJ, Alexander GE, Schapiro MB, Moller HJ,
of the D21S55 region on chromosome 21 in Intellect Disabil Res 48:106–171. Rapoport SI, Hampel H. 2004. Age-related
the pathogenesis of Down syndrome. Proc Share J, Koch R, Webb A, Graliker B. 1964. The cortical grey matter reductions in non-
Natl Acad Sci USA 86:5959–5962. longitudinal development of infants and demented Down’s syndrome adults deter-
Rast M, Meltzoff AN. 1995. Memory and young children with Down’s syndrome mined by MRI with voxel-based morph-
representation in young children with (Mongolism). Am J Ment Defic 68:689– ometry. Brain 127:811–824.
Down syndrome: Exploring deferred imi- 692. Trezise KL, Gray KM, Sheppard DM. 2008.
tation and object permanence. Dev Psycho- Shonkoff JP, Hauser-Cram P, Krauss MW, Upshur Attention and vigilance in children with
path 7:393–407. CC. 1992. Development of infants with Down syndrome. J Appl Res Intellect
Reed P, Staytom L, Stott S, Truzoli R. 2011. disabilities and their families: Implications 21:502–508.
Comparison of conditioning impairments in for theory and service delivery. Monogr Soc Vicari S. 2004. Memory development and
children with Down syndrome, autistic Res Child 57:153. intellectual disabilities. Acta Paediatr Suppl
spectrum disorders and mental age-matched Siegel MS, Smith WE. 2010. Psychiatric features 93:60–64.
controls. J Intel Disabil Res 55:988–997. in children with genetic syndromes: Toward Vicari S, Bellucci S, Carlesimo GA. 2000. Implicit
Reilly C. 2009. Autism spectrum disorders in functional phenotypes. Child Adolesc Psy- and explicit memory: A functional dissoci-
Down syndrome: A review. Res Autism chiatric Clin N Am 19:229–261. ation in persons with Down syndrome.
Spect Disord 3:829–839. Sigman M, Ruskin E. 1999. Continuity and Neuropsychologia 38:240–251.
Rhodes SM, Riby DM, Park J, Fraser E, Campbell change in the social competence of children Vicari S, Bellucci S, Carlesimo GA. 2005. Visual
LE. 2010. Executive neuropsychological with autism, Down syndrome, and devel- and spatial long-term memory: Differential
functioning in individuals with Williams opmental delays. Monogr Soc Res Child pattern of impairments in Williams and
syndrome. Neuropsychologia 48:1216– 64:1–114. Down syndromes. Dev Med Child Neurol
1226. Silverman W. 2007. Down syndrome: Cognitive 47:305–311.
Roberts JE, Price J, Malkin C. 2007. Language phenotype. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Vicari S, Carlesimo GA. 2006. Short-term
and communication development in Down Rev 13:228–236. memory deficits are not uniform in Down
syndrome. Ment Retard Dev Disabil 13:26– Silverman W, Kim H. 1997. Reaction times verify and Williams syndromes. Neuropsychol
35. nondemented status of adults with Down Rev 16:87–94.
Roizen NJ. 2005. Complementary and alternative syndrome. Down Syndr Q 2:1–10. Vicari S, Caselli MC, Gagliardi C, Tonucci F,
therapies for Down syndrome. Ment Retard Skotko BG. 2009. With new prenatal testing, will Volterra V. 2002. Language acquisition in
Dev Disabil Res Rev 11:149–155. babies with Down syndrome slowly dis- special populations: A comparison between
Roizen NJ, Patterson D. 2003. Down’s syndrome. appear? Arch Dis Child 94:823–826. Down and Williams syndrome. Neuro-
Lancet 361:1281–1289. Skotko BG, Davidson EJ, Weintraub GS. 2013. psychologia 40:2461–2470.
Rondal JA. 1994. Exceptional cases of language Contributions of a specialty clinic for Visootsak J, Mahle WT, Kirshbom PM, Hud-
development in mental retardation: The children and adolescents with Down syn- dleston L, Caron-Besch M, Ransom A,
relative autonomy of language as a cognitive drome. Am J Med Genet Part A 161A:430– Sherman SL. 2011. Neurodevelopmental
system. In: Tager-Flusberg H, editor. Con- 437. outcomes in children with Down syndrome
straints of language acquisition. Hillsdale Skotko BG, Levine SP, Goldstein R. 2011a. and congenital heart defects. Am J Med
New Jersey: Erlbaum. pp 155–174. Having a son or daughter with Down Genet Part A 155:2688–2691.
Rondal JA. 2006. Specific language profiles in syndrome: Perspectives from mothers and Visser FE, Aldenkamp AP, van Huffelen AC,
Down syndrome and other genetic syn- fathers. Am J Med Genet Part A 155:2335– Kuilman M, Overweg J, van Wijk J. 1997.
dromes of mental retardation. In: Rondal J, 2347. Prospective study of the prevalence of
Perera J, editors. Down syndrome: Neuro- Skotko BG, Levine SP, Goldstein R. 2011b. Self- Alzheimer-type dementia in institutional-
behavioral specificity. West Sussex, England: perceptions from people with Down syn- ized individuals with Down syndrome. Am J
Wiley. pp 101–123. drome. Am J Med Genet Part A 155:2360– Ment Retard 101:400–412.
Rondal JA, Comblain A. 1996. Language in adults 2369. Visu-Petra L, Benga O, Tincas L, Miclea M. 2007.
with Down syndrome. Down Syn Res Pract Smith BL, Oller DK. 1981. A comparative study of Visuo-spatial processing in children and
4:3–14. pre-meaningful vocalizations produced by adolescents with Down’s syndrome: A
Rondal JA, Edwards S. 1997. Language in mental normally developing and Down’s syndrome computerized assessment of memory skills.
retardation. London: Whurt. infants. J Speech Hear Disord 46:46–51. J Intellect Disabil Res 51:942–952.
Rowe J, Lavender A, Turk V. 2006. Cognitive Snart F, O’Grady M, Das JP. 1982. Cognitive Wang PP, Bellugi U. 1994. Evidence from two
executive function in Down’s syndrome. Br processing by subgroups of moderately genetic syndromes for a dissociation be-
J Clin Psychol 45:5–17. mentally retarded children. Am J Ment tween verbal and visual-spatial short-term
Ruskin EM, Kasari C, Mundy P, Sigman M. 1994. Defic 86:465–472. memory. J Clin Exp Neuropsych 16:317–
Attention to people and toys during social Steffens ML, Oller K, Lynch M, Urbano R. 1992. 322.
and object mastery in children with Down Vocal development in infants with Down Willoughby M, Holochwost SJ, Blanton ZE, Blair
syndrome. Am J Ment Retard 99:103–111. syndrome and infants who are developing C. 2014. Executive functions: Formative
Salman M. 2002. Systematic review of the effect of normally. Am J Ment Retard 97:235–246. versus reflective measurement. Measure-
therapeutic dietary supplements and drugs Stores R, Stores G, Fellows B, Buckley S. 1998. ment 12:69–95.
on cognitive function in subjects with Down Daytime behavior problems and maternal Wishart JG. 1995. Cognitive abilities in children
syndrome. Eur J Pediatr Neuol 6:213–219. stress in children with Down’s syndrome with Down syndrome: Developmental in-
Scherif G, Steele A. 2011. Neurocognitive their siblings and non-intellectually disabled stability and motivational deficits. Prog Clin
development of attention across genetic and other intellectually disabled peers. J Intel Biol Res 393:57–91.
syndromes: Inspecting a disorder’s dynamics Disabil Res 42:228–237. Wishart JG. 1993. The development of learning
through the lens of another. Prog Brain Res Tannock R. 1988. Mothers’ directiveness in their difficulties in children with Down’s syn-
189:285–301. interactions with their children with and drome. J Intel Disabil Res 37:389–403.
Schliebs R, Arendt T. 2006. The significance of without Down syndrome. Am J Ment Wishart JG, Duffy L. 1990. Instability of perform-
the cholinergic system in the brain during Retard 93:154–165. ance on cognitive tests in infants and young
aging and in Alzheimer’s disease. J Neural Tr Thomson GOB, Ward KM, Wishart JG. 1995. children with Down syndrome. Br J Edu
S 113:1625–1644. The transition to adulthood for children Psychol 60:10–22.
INTRODUCTION AMERICAN JOURNAL OF MEDICAL GENETICS PART C: SEMINARS IN MEDICAL GENETICS 149

Wisniewski KE, Wisniewski HM, Wen GY. 1985. syndrome: Is it really a strength? Res Dev Zigman W, Silverman W, Wisniewski HM. 1996.
Occurrence of neuropathological changes Disabil 35:1473–1500. Aging and Alzheimer disease in Down
and dementia of Alzheimer’s disease in Zigman WB, Lott IT. 2007. Alzheimers disease in syndrome: Clinical and pathological
Down’s syndrome. Ann Neurol 17:278–282. Down syndrome neurobiology and risk. changes. Met Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev
Yang Y, Conners FA, Merrill EC. 2014. Visuo- Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev 13:237– 2:73–79.
spatial ability in individuals with Down 246.

Вам также может понравиться