Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 16

British Journal of Management, Vol.

21, 438–452 (2010)


DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8551.2009.00672.x

The Prevalence of Destructive


Leadership Behaviour
Merethe Schanke Aasland, Anders Skogstad, Guy Notelaers, Morten
Birkeland Nielsen and Ståle Einarsen
Department of Psychosocial Science, Christies Gate 12, University of Bergen, N-5015 Bergen, Norway
Corresponding author email: merethe.aasland@psysp.uib.no

This study investigates the prevalence of the four types of destructive leadership
behaviour in the destructive and constructive leadership behaviour model, in a
representative sample of the Norwegian workforce. The study employs two estimation
methods: the operational classification method (OCM) and latent class cluster (LCC)
analysis. The total prevalence of destructive leadership behaviour varied from 33.5%
(OCM) to 61% (LCC), indicating that destructive leadership is not an anomaly.
Destructive leadership comes in many shapes and forms, with passive forms prevailing
over more active ones. The results showed that laissez-faire leadership behaviour was
the most prevalent destructive leadership behaviour, followed by supportive–disloyal
leadership and derailed leadership, while tyrannical leadership behaviour was the least
prevalent destructive leadership behaviour. Furthermore, many leaders display
constructive as well as destructive behaviours, indicating that leadership is not either
constructive or destructive. The study contributes to a broader theoretical perspective
on what must be seen as typical behaviour among leaders.

Introduction 2006). While some researchers claim that destruc-


tive or abusive leadership constitutes a low base-
So far, research on destructive forms of leader- rate phenomenon (e.g. Aryee et al., 2007), others
ship has mainly focused on its detrimental effects believe it to be a substantial problem in many
on subordinates (Bies and Tripp, 1998; Tepper, organizations, in terms of both its prevalence and
2000; Zellars, Tepper and Duffy, 2002). However, consequences (Burke, 2006; Hogan, Raskin and
few studies have investigated the prevalence of Fazzini, 1990). In organizational climate research
such destructive leadership in contemporary from the mid-1950s to 1990, 60%–75% of all
working life (e.g. Schat, Frone and Kelloway, employees typically reported that the worst
aspect of their job was their immediate supervisor
(Hogan, Raskin and Fazzini, 1990). In the USA,
The present project is a collaborative project between job pressure has been cited in 75% of workers’
the University of Bergen and Statistics Norway, which compensation claims in which mental stressors
collected the data. The project was made possible by
joint grants from two Norwegian employer associations were the main cause of absenteeism, and 94% of
(the Confederation of Norwegian Enterprise and the those claims were allegedly caused by abusive
Norwegian Association of Local and Regional Autho- treatment by managers (Wilson, 1991). Thus, a
rities), and the Norwegian government (the National growing body of evidence documents that leaders
Insurance Administration) and their FARVE programme. behave in a destructive manner, be it towards
We would like to thank Bengt Oscar Lagerstrm and
Maria Hstmark of Statistics Norway and Stig Berge their subordinates (Bies and Tripp, 1998; Tepper,
Matthiesen of the Faculty of Psychology, University of 2000, 2007), towards the organization itself or
Bergen, for their contribution to the data collection. towards both (Kellerman, 2004; Vredenburgh

r 2009 British Academy of Management. Published by Blackwell Publishing Ltd, 9600 Garsington Road, Oxford
OX4 2DQ, UK and 350 Main Street, Malden, MA, 02148, USA.
Destructive Leadership Behaviour 439

and Brender, 1998). The prevalence of destructive employees to engage in such activities (Altheide
leadership has serious implications for theories et al., 1978).
on leadership and for measures of leadership, as Considering the breadth of the concepts used
well as for the recruitment, training and devel- to describe destructive leaders, it seems clear that
opment of leaders. The aim of the present study destructive leadership is not one type of leader-
is thus to contribute to the growing body of ship behaviour, but instead involves a variety of
literature on destructive leadership by investigat- behaviour. Taking this diversity into account, the
ing the prevalence of four forms of destructive present study uses the overarching concept of
leadership behaviour derived from the Destruc- ‘destructive leadership’, defined as ‘systematic
tive and Constructive Leadership (DCL) and repeated behaviour by a leader, supervisor or
behaviour model (Einarsen, Aasland and Skog- manager that violates the legitimate interest of
stad, 2007) employing a representative sample of the organization by undermining and/or sabota-
the Norwegian workforce. ging the organization’s goals, tasks, resources,
and effectiveness and/or the motivation, well-
being or job satisfaction of subordinates’ (Einar-
Conceptualizations of destructive leadership
sen, Aasland and Skogstad, 2007, p. 208). Hence,
Many concepts have been used to describe destructive leadership is about systematically
destructive forms of leadership, such as ‘abusive acting against the legitimate interest of the
supervision’ (Tepper, 2000) and ‘petty tyranny’ organization, whether by abusing subordinates
(Ashforth, 1994), referring to leaders who behave or by working against the attainment of the
in a destructive manner towards subordinates, by organization’s goals, including any illegal beha-
intimidating subordinates, belittling or humiliat- viour. The definition emphasizes repeated de-
ing them in public or exposing them to non- structive behaviour as opposed to a single act
verbal aggression (Aryee et al., 2007). Concepts such as an isolated outburst of anger or
such as authoritarian (Adorno et al., 1950; Bass, spontaneous misbehaviour. However, if mistakes
1990a), Machiavellian (Christie and Geis, 1970), or outbursts of anger become repeated, they
autocratic (Kipnis et al., 1981), narcissistic represent destructive leadership according to the
leadership (Kets de Vries and Miller, 1985) and definition irrespective of their intentions or
personalized charismatic leadership (House and antecedents. Furthermore, destructive leadership
Howell, 1992) emphasize similar but not over- is about behaviour that violates, or is in opposi-
lapping behaviours. However, these concepts tion to, what is considered to be the legitimate
mainly focus on control and obedience, and less interest of the organization. Including legitimate
on the abusive aspect of leadership. interest is in accordance with Sackett and
Leaders may also behave destructively in a way DeVore’s (2001) definition of ‘counterproductive
that primarily affects the organization (Keller- workplace behavior’, narrowing what an organi-
man, 2004; Lipman-Blumen, 2005), potentially zation may expect from its leaders to what must
leading to negative consequences for the execu- be seen as legitimate, legal, reasonable and
tion of tasks, quality of work, efficiency and justifiable behaviour in a given cultural setting.
relations with customers and clients (Padilla, Hence, what is perceived as destructive behaviour
Hogan and Kaiser, 2007). Concepts frequently may vary between cultures and societies and also
used to describe such behaviour are ‘flawed over time (Einarsen, Aasland and Skogstad, 2007).
leadership’ (Hogan, 1994), ‘derailed leadership’ In accordance with the above definition, the
(McCall and Lombardo, 1983; Shackleton, 1995), DCL model (see Figure 1) describes four main
the ‘dark side of leadership’ (Conger, 1990), kinds of destructive leadership behaviour target-
‘toxic leadership’ (Lipman-Blumen, 2005) and ing either subordinates and/or the organization
‘impaired managers’ (Lubit, 2004). Such leaders (Einarsen, Aasland and Skogstad, 2007). Cate-
neglect, or even actively prevent, goal attainment gorizing leadership behaviour as subordinate-
in the organization by, for example, sabotaging oriented and organization-oriented is not new.
subordinates’ task execution, by working towards However, while many models of leadership
alternative goals than those of the organization behaviour, such as the managerial grid developed
(Conger, 1990), by stealing resources such as by Blake and Mouton (1985) and the full range of
materials, money or time, or by encouraging leadership model (Avolio, 1999; Bass and Riggio,

r 2009 British Academy of Management.


440 M. S. Aasland et al.

Pro-subordinate

Supportive–disloyal Constructive

Leadership Leadership

Behaviour Behaviour
Laissez-faire

Anti-organization Leadership Pro-organization

Behaviour
Derailed Tyrannical

Leadership Leadership

Behaviour Behaviour

Anti-subordinate

Figure 1. A model of destructive leadership behaviour

2006), are based on the assumption that leader- wards the fulfilment of the organization’s goals,
ship behaviour can be seen on a continuum from setting clear and unambiguous objectives, mak-
low to high with regard to constructive leadership ing or supporting strategic decisions and imple-
behaviour, the present model views leadership menting legitimate organizational change.
behaviour on a continuum from highly ‘anti’ to By crosscutting the two dimensions, the DCL
highly ‘pro’ (Aasland, Skogstad and Einarsen, model presents five categories of leadership
2008). behaviour, one of which is constructive, three of
Hence, the subordinate dimension describes which are actively destructive – tyrannical,
leadership behaviour ranging from anti-subordi- derailed, and supportive–disloyal leadership be-
nate behaviour to pro-subordinate behaviour. haviour (Einarsen Aasland and Skogstad, 2007) –
Anti-subordinate behaviour illegitimately under- and one of which is passive: laissez-faire leader-
mines or sabotages the motivation, well-being or ship, situated in the middle of the proposed
job satisfaction of subordinates, involving beha- model. Constructive leadership is in accordance
viour such as harassment and mistreatment of with the legitimate interest of the organization,
subordinates (Einarsen, Aasland and Skogstad, showing both pro-subordinate and pro-organiza-
2007). Pro-subordinate behaviour fosters the tion behaviour to some degree. Constructive
motivation, well-being and job satisfaction of leadership is about displaying behaviour that
subordinates, including taking care of and involves supporting and enhancing the goal
supporting them in accordance with organiza- attainment of the organization, making optimal
tional policies. Organization-oriented behaviour use of organizational resources, as well as
may also range from anti-organization behaviour enhancing the motivation, well-being and job
to pro-organization behaviour, where the former satisfaction of subordinates. Such leadership
violates the legitimate interest of the organization behaviour can also be described using concepts
by working in opposition to the organization’s such as transactional (Bass et al., 2003), trans-
goals, values and optimal use of resources, by formational (Bass et al., 2003), charismatic (e.g.
stealing from the organization, by sabotaging the Conger and Kanungo, 1987) and empowering
organization’s goals, or even by being involved in leadership (e.g. Conger and Kanungo, 1988).
corruption (Einarsen, Aasland and Skogstad, However, the focus of the present paper is on
2007). Anti-organizational behaviour can also destructive leadership behaviours.
be described as counterproductive workplace Tyrannical leadership behaviour is about dis-
behaviour directed at the organization (Fox and playing pro-organizational behaviour combined
Spector, 1999; Sackett and DeVore, 2001). Pro- with anti-subordinate behaviour. Strictly speak-
organizational behaviour is about working to- ing, such leaders may behave in accordance with

r 2009 British Academy of Management.


Destructive Leadership Behaviour 441

the legitimate goals, tasks and strategies of the reflect an overriding concern with establishing
organization. However, they typically obtain camaraderie with subordinates and ensuring their
results not through, but at the expense of, well-being. However, while country club manage-
subordinates (Ashforth, 1994; Ma, Karri and ment involves a minimum of focus on production
Chittipeddi, 2004). Tyrannical leaders may hu- and efficiency, thus being low on the organiza-
miliate, belittle and manipulate their subordi- tion-oriented dimension, leaders who behave in a
nates in order to ‘get the job done’. Because supportive but disloyal manner behave destruc-
tyrannical leaders may behave constructively in tively towards the organization, thus behaving in
terms of organization-oriented behaviour while an anti-organizational manner (Aasland, Skog-
at the same time displaying anti-subordinate stad and Einarsen, 2008). Hence, the absence of
behaviour, subordinates and superiors may eval- constructive leadership behaviour is not the same
uate the leader’s behaviour quite differently. What as the presence of destructive leadership beha-
upper management may see as a strong focus on viour (Kelloway, Mullen and Francis, 2006).
task completion may at the same time be seen by However, destructive leadership is not necessa-
subordinates as abusive leadership or even bully- rily limited to such active and manifest behaviour
ing (Einarsen, Aasland and Skogstad, 2007). as described above. Buss (1961) describes aggres-
Derailed leadership is about displaying both sive behaviour along three principal axes, namely
anti-organizational and anti-subordinate beha- physical versus verbal, active versus passive and
viour. Such leaders may bully, humiliate, manip- direct versus indirect aggression. Consequently,
ulate or deceive, while simultaneously engaging destructive leadership behaviour may also in-
in anti-organizational behaviour such as absen- clude passive and indirect behaviour (Skogstad
teeism, fraud or otherwise stealing resources from et al., 2007). Kelloway and colleagues (2005) also
the organization (Aasland, Skogstad and Einar- acknowledge this in their study of ‘poor leader-
sen, 2008; McCall and Lombardo, 1983). Conger ship’, in which they differentiate between an
(1990) focuses on similar themes in his study of active, abusive leadership style and a passive one.
‘the dark side’ of leadership, in which he Avoidant or passive leadership, which is also
recognizes that leaders may use their charismatic referred to as ‘laissez-faire leadership’ (Bass,
qualities for personal gain and abusively turn 1990b), represents a leadership style in which
against what is good for both followers and the the leader has been appointed to and still
organization. physically occupies the leadership position, but
Supportive–disloyal leadership consists of pro- in practice has abdicated the responsibilities and
subordinate behaviour combined with anti-orga- duties assigned to him or her (Lewin, Lippitt and
nizational behaviour. Such leaders motivate and White, 1939). Such leaders may avoid decision-
support their subordinates, while simultaneously making, show little concern for goal attainment
stealing resources from the organization, be it and seldom involve themselves with their sub-
materials, time or financial resources (Altheide ordinates, even when this is necessary (Bass,
et al., 1978; Ditton, 1977). Supportive–disloyal 1990b). Ashforth (1994) emphasizes the impor-
leaders may give employees more benefits then tance of passive destructive behaviour in his
they are entitled to at the expense of the conceptualization of the petty tyrant, including
organization, encourage low work ethics and ‘lack of consideration’ and ‘discouraging initiative’
misconduct and lead their subordinates to be as two of six dimensions. Thus, the systematic
inefficient, or towards other goals than those of absence of positive behaviour is conceptualized
the organization, all of this while behaving in a as destructive leadership behaviour. Indepen-
comradely and supportive manner. They may dently of the causes of passive or laissez-faire
also commit embezzlement or fraud, or encou- leadership behaviour, be it a result of incompe-
rage subordinates to enrich themselves through tence, lack of knowledge or strategic intent to
such anti-organizational behaviour (Einarsen, harm, it clearly violates the legitimate interest of
Aasland and Skogstad, 2007). This form of the organization as well as legitimate expecta-
leadership behaviour has some features in com- tions of subordinates, and it may thus harm both
mon with the leadership style that Blake and the organization and the subordinates (Frischer
Mouton (1985) termed ‘country club manage- and Larsson, 2000; Hinkin and Schriesheim, 2008;
ment’, as both forms of leadership behaviour Skogstad et al., 2007).

r 2009 British Academy of Management.


442 M. S. Aasland et al.

Aim of the study Measures


A small but growing body of studies exists on Data were collected by a questionnaire measuring
destructive forms of leadership behaviour. How- demographic variables, exposure to bullying,
ever, these studies are mostly limited to the observed leadership behaviour of the respon-
characteristics of such destructive leadership and dent’s immediate superior, job satisfaction, sub-
its effects on subordinates. Apart from two jective health complaints and various aspects of
studies investigating the prevalence of leadership the psychosocial working environment. Only
aggression (Hubert and van Veldhoven, 2001; demographic variables and questions related to
Schat, Frone and Kelloway, 2006), we know little leadership behaviour are included in the present
about how prevalent various forms of destructive study.
leadership behaviour are. Such knowledge is of Leadership behaviour was measured using 22
great importance, especially since efforts to items from the destructive leadership scale
develop effective interventions against such be- (Einarsen et al., 2002). Tyrannical leadership
haviour may depend on the prevalence of the behaviour was measured using four items (Cron-
phenomenon (Zapf et al., 2003). Moreover, bach’s alpha 5 0.70). Examples of items included
further theoretical developments specifically re- ‘has humiliated you, or other employees, if you/
lating to destructive leadership, as well as to they fail to live up to his/her standards’ and ‘has
leadership in general, depend on an estimate of spread incorrect information about you or your
the prevalence of destructive leadership beha- co-workers, in order to harm your/their position
viour. Nuanced information in this regard may in the firm’. Derailed leadership behaviour was
alter our perception of leadership as a phenom- measured by four items (Cronbach’s alpha 5 0.71),
enon and lay the foundation for how much examples of items being ‘has used his/her position
attention should be devoted to this aspect of in the firm to profit financially/materially at the
leadership in future leadership training and company’s expense’ and ‘regards his/her staff more
development (Burke, 2006). Hence, the aim of as competitors than as partners’. Supportive–
the present study is to investigate, on the basis of disloyal leadership behaviour was measured by four
a representative sample of subordinates, the items (Cronbach’s alpha 5 0.65). Examples of
prevalence of the four forms of destructive items measuring this type of leadership behaviour
leadership behaviour laid out in the DCL model. are ‘has behaved in a friendly manner by encoura-
ging you/your co-workers to extend your/their
lunch break’ and ‘has encouraged you to enjoy
Method extra privileges at the company’s expense’. Laissez-
faire leadership behaviour was measured by four
Procedure/Sample
items (Cronbach’s alpha 5 0.72) from the Multi-
Questionnaires were sent by regular mail to a factor Leadership Questionnaire (Bass and Avolio,
representative sample of 4500 employees, ran- 1990), an example being ‘has avoided making
domly drawn from the Norwegian Central decisions’. To prevent response set among the
Employee Register during spring 2005, with two participants, constructive leadership behaviour in
reminders. The sampling criteria were employees the form of employee-centred, production-centred
between 18 and 65 years of age, employed during and change-centred leadership was included using
the last six months in a Norwegian company with six items from Ekvall and Arvonen (1991)
five or more employees, and with mean working (Cronbach’s alpha 5 0.87), examples being ‘gives
hours of more than 15 hours per week. A total of recognition for good performance’ and ‘en-
57% responded (N 5 2539), which is somewhat courages innovative thinking’. Items measuring
above average for surveys of this kind (Baruch constructive leadership behaviour were distribu-
and Holtom, 2008). ted randomly among the items measuring de-
The mean age was 43.79 years (SD 5 11.52), structive forms of leadership.
(range 19 to 66). The sample is representative of Four response categories were employed
the working population in Norway, except for a (‘never’, ‘sometimes’, ‘quite often’ and ‘very
minor overrepresentation of women (52% versus often or nearly always’), and the respondents
47%; Hstmark and Lagerstrm, 2006). were asked to report on leadership behaviour

r 2009 British Academy of Management.


Destructive Leadership Behaviour 443

which they had experienced during the last six not considered to be exposed to destructive
months. leadership; otherwise, the respondent is consid-
To ensure the internal validity of the scales ered to be exposed.
measuring the destructive forms of leadership, a Although a common method of reporting
series of exploratory factor analyses was con- prevalence rates, some weaknesses have been
ducted. The model that yielded the best fit to the pointed out concerning the OCM (Notelaers
data was a five-factor solution (w2 5 467.10; et al., 2006). First, the cut-off point provided by
df 5 199; comparative fit index (CFI) 5 the OCM is an arbitrary choice that reduces a
0.95; goodness of fit index (GFI) 5 0.88: consis- complex phenomenon to a simple either–or
tent Akaike information criterion (CAIC) 5 phenomenon. Second, the number of items used
933.01; root mean square error of approximation may influence the prevalence rate (Agervold,
(RMSEA) 5 0.026) supporting the internal 2007). Third, subordinates who are frequently
validity of the scale, which measures four exposed to a wide range of specific destructive
destructive forms of leadership behaviour in leadership behaviour, but where each specific
addition to constructive leadership behaviour. behaviour only occurs ‘sometimes’, are not
Table 1 shows the fit statistics for all the factor regarded as being exposed to destructive leader-
solutions. ship. Of course, a low level of exposure to many
different types of destructive leadership beha-
viour may still reflect a systematic pattern in the
Data analysis
leader’s behaviour.
Two methods are used to estimate the prevalence To compensate for these potential weaknesses,
rate of destructive leadership: the operational we applied LCC analysis, which is a systematic
classification method (OCM) and latent class way of classifying research subjects into homo-
cluster (LCC) analysis. The former defines a geneous groups based on similarities in their
specific criterion that classifies respondents as responses to particular items, in our case the
either exposed or not exposed to destructive items describing the behaviour of their immediate
leadership, based on their reports of their supervisor. LCC analysis thus identifies mutually
immediate superiors’ behaviour, a method com- exclusive groups based on the distribution of
monly used in research on workplace bullying observations in an n-way contingency table of
(e.g. Nielsen et al., 2009). As the definition of discrete variables (i.e. observed destructive leader
destructive leadership emphasizes repeated and behaviour). A goal of traditional LCC analysis is
systematic behaviour, the classification criterion to determine the smallest number of latent
employed was exposure to one or more types of classes, T, which is sufficient to explain (account
destructive leadership behaviour during the last for) the associations observed between the
six months, ‘quite often’ or ‘very often or nearly manifest variables (the reported leadership beha-
always’. Destructive leadership behaviour that is viour) (Magidson and Vermunt, 2004). The
reported ‘quite often’ or ‘very often or nearly analysis typically begins by fitting the T 5 1 class
always’ is coded as 1, whereas all other frequen- (only one group of destructive leadership beha-
cies are coded as 0. All instances are then added viour is reported) baseline model, which speci-
up and, when the sum is zero, the respondent is fies mutual independence among the variables.

Table 1. Fit statistics for the factor analyses solutions

Model Satorra-Bentler df RMSEA CFI GFI CAIC


scaled w2

One factor: Leadership behaviour 3949.76 209 0.093 0.81 0.52 4329.39
Two factor: Constructive and destructive leadership behaviour 1654.05 208 0.058 0.88 0.64 2042.32
Three factor: Constructive, supportive–disloyal, destructive 821.56 206 0.038 0.93 0.81 1227.08
(laissez-faire, derailed and tyrannical) leadership behaviour
Four factor: Constructive, supportive–disloyal, laissez-faire, 572.40 203 0.030 0.94 0.82 1003.80
destructive (derailed and tyrannical) leadership behaviour
Five factor: Constructive, supportive–disloyal, laissez-faire, 467.10 199 0.026 0.95 0.88 933.01
derailed, tyrannical leadership behaviour

r 2009 British Academy of Management.


444 M. S. Aasland et al.

Table 2. Means, standard deviations and inter-correlations for all continuous measures (N 5 2539)

Variables M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8

1 Age 43.79 11.52 –


2 Number of employees in enterprise 279 835.9 0.00 –
3 Mean working hours per week 37.49 10.36 0.01 0.05* –
4 Tyrannical leadership 0.11 0.26 0.10** 0.02 0.02 –
5 Derailed leadership 0.21 0.38 0.04 0.01 0.07** 0.60** –
6 Supportive–disloyal leadership 0.29 0.38 0.10** 0.09** 0.08** 0.03 0.01 –
7 Constructive leadership 1.44 0.66 0.05* 0.03 0.04* 0.21** 0.29** 0.35** –
8 Laissez-faire leadership 0.57 0.52 0.01 0.03 0.02 0.38** 0.54** 0.08** 0.37** –

*Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level.


**Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level.

Assuming that this null model does not provide Prevalence of destructive leadership behaviour
an adequate fit to the data, a one-dimensional using the OCM
LCC model with T 5 2 classes (that distinguishes
Destructive behaviour by superiors proved to be
between destructive and non-destructive leader-
quite common, as 83.7% reported exposure to
ship behaviours) is then fitted to the data. This
some kind of such behaviours. Yet, according to
process continues by fitting successive LCC
the operational criterion, 33.5% of the respon-
models to the data, increasing the number of
dents reported exposure to at least one destruc-
classes each time – thus implicitly introducing
tive leadership behaviour ‘quite often’ or ‘very
multidimensionality – until the simplest model
often or nearly always’ during the last six months
that provides an adequate fit is found (Goodman,
(Table 3). Employing this criterion, 21.2% were
1974; McCutcheon, 1987), and a model is found
exposed to one or more instances of laissez-faire
in which the latent variable can explain all of the
leadership behaviour, while 11.6% reported one
associations among the reported behaviours (cf.
or more instances of supportive–disloyal leader-
Magidson and Vermunt, 2004).
ship behaviour. Furthermore, the prevalence of
Different from traditional cluster methods
derailed leadership was 8.8%, with the prevalence
(such as K-means clustering), LCC analysis is
rate of tyrannical leadership behaviour being
based on a statistical model that can be tested
3.4% (Table 3).
(Magidson and Vermunt, 2002a). In conse-
quence, determining the number of latent classes
is less arbitrary than when using traditional
cluster methods (Notelaers et al., 2006). It can LCC analysis concerning groups of targets
thus be seen as a probabilistic extension of K- The LCC analysis showed six separate clusters of
means clustering (Magidson and Vermunt, reported leadership behaviour (see Table 4). For
2002b). The LCC analysis will thus determine each of the six groups of respondents, Table 4
whether different groups exist among the respon- summarizes the profile output,1 retaining condi-
dents with respect to exposure to destructive tional probabilities by calculating the average
leadership behaviour based on similarities in their conditional probability of each group responding
response patterns (Notelaers et al., 2006). As the ‘never’, ‘sometimes’, ‘quite often’ or ‘very often or
sample size in the present study is large, the level nearly always’ to the items measuring the different
of significance was set to po0.01. forms of destructive leadership behaviour.
The first cluster, which we labelled ‘no-destruc-
tiveness’, included 39% of the respondents in the
sample. This cluster is characterized by a high
Results conditional probability of answering ‘never’ to
any of the items measuring the four kinds of
The inter-correlations, means and standard de-
viations for all the continuous measures used in 1
The profile output can be obtained by contacting the
the study are reported in Table 2. first author of the paper.

r 2009 British Academy of Management.


Destructive Leadership Behaviour 445

Table 3. Exposure to constructive and destructive leadership behaviour ‘quite often’ or more often

Number of instances of behaviour exposed to

0 1 2 3 4 or morea

Constructive behaviour 29.1 12.4 10.4 11.7 35.9


Tyrannical behaviour 96.6 2.4 0.7 0.2 0.1
Derailed behaviour 91.2 6.1 1.7 0.7 0.3
Supportive–disloyal behaviour 88.3 8.8 2.3 0.4 0.1
Laissez-faire behaviour 78.8 13.1 4.8 2.4 0.9
All destructive behaviourb 66.6 17.9 7.8 3.5 4.3

Frequency in per cent for rows (N 5 2539).


a
The constructive behaviour list consists of six items. The tyrannical, derailed, popular but disloyal lists each consist of four items.
b
‘All destructive behaviour’ is the sum of all tyrannical, derailed, supportive–disloyal and laissez-faire items (16 items).

Table 4. Average conditional probabilities (for all items concerned) expressed as percentages

Cluster 1 Cluster 2 Cluster 3 Cluster 4 Cluster 5 Cluster 6


Non- Laissez- Sometimes laissez-faire, Sometimes Supportive– Highly
destructiveness faire sometimes supportive– destructive disloyal abusive
disloyal

Size 0.39 0.19 0.17 0.11 0.10 0.06


Never 0.91 0.75 0.665 0.57 0.78 0.42
Sometimes 0.09 0.21 0.29 0.36 0.17 0.33
Quite often 0 0.04 0.04 0.06 0.04 0.16
Very often/nearly always 0 0.01 0 0.01 0.01 0.09

destructive leadership behaviour. Hence, these show some level of both these two kinds of
subordinates claim not to observe any kind of behaviour. This cluster contained 17% of the
destructive behaviour on the part of their respondents. Nearly every item associated with
immediate superior. laissez-faire leadership is reported less frequently
A second cluster of respondents, comprising in this cluster than in the ‘laissez-faire’ cluster.
19% of the respondents, was labelled ‘laissez- However, these respondents have a high prob-
faire’ given their higher conditional probability of ability of answering ‘sometimes’ to items measur-
reporting exposure to items measuring laissez- ing both laissez-faire and supportive–disloyal
faire leadership and their low conditional prob- leadership behaviour. Examples of items with a
abilities of reporting any tyrannical, derailed or high average probability of being reported ‘some-
supportive–disloyal leadership behaviour. Al- times’ by these respondents are ‘has behaved in a
most every item associated with laissez-faire friendly manner by encouraging you/your co-
leadership is reported more frequently in this workers to extend your/their lunch break’ (68%
cluster compared with the ‘no-destructiveness’ ‘sometimes’, 15% ‘quite often’), ‘has encouraged
cluster: ‘has avoided making decisions’ (62% you, or your co-workers, to take extra coffee/
‘sometimes’ and 12% ‘quite often’), ‘is likely to cigarette breaks as a reward for good work effort’
be absent when needed’ (61% ‘sometimes’ and (61% ‘sometimes’, 8% ‘quite often’), ‘is likely to
17% ‘quite often’), ‘has avoided getting involved be absent when needed’ (60% ‘sometimes’ and
in your work’ (58% ‘sometimes’ and 24% ‘quite 11% ‘quite often’), ‘has avoided making deci-
often’) and ‘has steered away from showing sions’ (56% ‘sometimes’ and 6% ‘quite often’)
concern about results’ (43% ‘sometimes’ and and ‘has avoided getting involved in your work’
6% ‘quite often’). (52% ‘sometimes’ and 6% ‘quite often’). The
The third cluster was labelled ‘sometimes probability of these respondents reporting any
laissez-faire and sometimes supportive–disloyal’, tyrannical or derailed leadership behaviour is
as these subordinates report that their superiors very low.

r 2009 British Academy of Management.


446 M. S. Aasland et al.

The fourth cluster, which was labelled ‘some- behaviour seems to dominate, but respondents
times destructive’, comprised 11% of the respon- also report excessive exposure to tyrannical and
dents. These respondents face ‘sometimes’ derailed leadership behaviour. Examples of items
destructive leadership behaviour in a variety of that are frequently reported by respondents in
forms, be it laissez-faire, tyrannical or derailed this cluster are ‘has avoided getting involved in
leadership. Items measuring laissez-faire leader- your work’ (40% ‘quite often’, 23% ‘very often
ship behaviour are more frequently reported than or nearly always’), ‘is likely to be absent when
in the ‘sometimes laissez-faire and sometimes needed’ (35% ‘quite often’, 29% ‘very often or
supportive–disloyal’ cluster, and items measuring nearly always’), ‘attributes the company’s success
tyrannical and derailed leadership behaviour are to his/her own abilities rather than the abilities of
also more frequently reported, examples being the employees’ (29% ‘quite often’, 19% ‘very
‘has humiliated you, or other employees, if you/ often or nearly always’), ‘has avoided making
they fail to live up to his/her standards’ (45% decisions’ (34% ‘quite often’, 22% ‘very often or
‘sometimes’, 7% ‘quite often’), ‘regards his/her nearly always’), ‘regards his/her staff more as
staff more as competitors than as collaborators’ competitors than as collaborators’ (27% ‘quite
(43% ‘sometimes’, 10% ‘quite often’), ‘has made often’, 12% ‘very often or nearly always’), ‘has
it more difficult for you to express your views at a made it more difficult for you to express your
meeting, either by giving you too little speaking views at a meeting, either by giving you too little
time or by placing you last’ (37% ‘sometimes’, speaking time or by placing you last’ (19% ‘quite
6% ‘quite often’) and ‘has imitated or made faces often’, 11% ‘very often or nearly always’), ‘has
(e.g. rolled eyes, stuck out tongue etc.) at you or steered away from showing concern about
other employees to show that he/she is not results’ (19% ‘quite often’, 11% ‘very often or
satisfied with your/others’ work efforts’ (36% nearly always’) and ‘has humiliated you, or other
‘sometimes’, 7% ‘quite often’). employees, if you/they fail to live up to his/her
The fifth cluster of respondents, comprising standards’ (15% ‘quite often’, 6% ‘very often or
10% of the respondents, only report exposure nearly always’). The LCC analysis shows that as
to ‘supportive–disloyal’ leadership behaviour. many as 61% of all subordinates experience
Nearly every item associated with supportive– systematic exposure to varying forms and com-
disloyal leadership is reported more frequently in binations of destructive leadership, with laissez-
this cluster than in the ‘sometimes laissez-faire faire leadership again being the dominant form.
and sometimes supportive–disloyal’ cluster: ‘has Laissez-faire leadership may exist in isolation
encouraged you, or your co-workers, to take with either a low or a high frequency, or it may
extra coffee/cigarette breaks as a reward for good occur in combination with either supportive–
work effort’ (60% ‘sometimes’, 27% ‘quite disloyal leadership or tyrannical and derailed
often’), ‘has behaved in a friendly manner by leadership, again in more or less intense form.
encouraging you/your co-workers to extend
your/their lunch break’ (60% ‘sometimes’, 26%
‘quite often’), ‘has encouraged you to enjoy extra Discussion
privileges at the company’s expense’ (35% ‘some-
times’, 10% ‘quite often’) and ‘has encouraged The present study shows that destructive leader-
you or your co-workers to do private work/run ship behaviour is very common. Depending on
private errands during working hours’ (35% the estimation method, between 33.5% and 61%
‘sometimes’, 4% ‘quite often’). of all respondents report their immediate super-
The sixth cluster was labelled ‘highly abusive’ iors as showing some kind of consistent and
and comprised 6% of the respondents in the frequent destructive leadership during the last six
study. Respondents in this cluster report high months, while only about 40% report no
exposure to laissez-faire, tyrannical and derailed exposure whatsoever to such leadership beha-
leadership behaviour. Their response patterns are viour. Thus, destructive leadership behaviour is
characterized by a higher average probability of not a low base-rate phenomenon, but something
reporting that such behaviour occurs ‘quite often’ that most subordinates will probably experience
and/or ‘very often or nearly always’ than all the during their working life. The observed preva-
other clusters. Again, laissez-faire leadership lence rates are far higher than those reported by

r 2009 British Academy of Management.


Destructive Leadership Behaviour 447

Schat, Frone and Kelloway (2006) and Hubert ing the last 12 months, while the present study
and van Veldhoven (2001). Schat, Frone and has a time frame of only six months. In the study
Kelloway (2006) found that, during the last 12 by Hubert and van Veldhoven (2001), only 2.1%
months, 13.5% of the respondents were exposed of the respondents reported frequent (‘often’ or
to aggression from their superior, and, likewise, ‘always’) exposure to aggression from their
Hubert and van Veldhoven (2001) found an leader. Thus, it can be concluded that the
average prevalence rate of about 11%. However, prevalence rate of tyrannical leadership beha-
these two studies only investigated one form of viour in the present study is comparable to these
destructive leadership behaviour, namely aggres- studies.
sive behaviour. Consequently, they did not About 9% of the respondents reported that their
measure passive forms of destructive leadership immediate leader displayed derailed leadership
nor destructive forms of leadership that may also behaviour, which, in addition to anti-subordinate
include constructive elements. Furthermore, behaviour, also involves anti-organizational be-
these studies did not measure destructive beha- haviour, potentially making these leaders more
viour targeting the organization. Hence, measur- visible to upper management, compared with
ing a broader range of destructive leadership leaders who display tyrannical leadership beha-
behaviour, as was the case in the present study, viour. The prevalence rate of exposure to this
will probably yield higher prevalence rates than form of leadership behaviour could therefore be
measuring a narrower range of behaviour. expected to be lower than for tyrannical leader-
Participants in this study reported exposure to ship behaviour. That was not the case, however.
all four forms of destructive leadership behaviour Again the explanation may be that we measure
described in the presented conceptual model. leadership behaviour over a six-month period, as
While some 3.5% had been exposed to tyrannical the potential positive effect tyrannical leadership
leadership behaviour during the last six months, behaviour may have on the organization’s goal
some 9% reported exposure to derailed leader- attainment is claimed to decline, and eventually
ship behaviour. About 11.5% had been exposed disappear, over time (Ma, Karri and Chittipeddi,
to supportive–disloyal leadership behaviour. 2004), eventually developing into derailed leader-
Laissez-faire leadership behaviour had the high- ship behaviour. Furthermore, experiencing anti-
est prevalence rate at 21.2%, all according to the subordinate behaviour such as bullying, humilia-
OCM. tion or harassment may have such a profound
As, to our knowledge, no other studies have effect on some subordinates that organiza-
investigated the prevalence of tyrannical leader- tion-oriented behaviour, be it constructive or
ship behaviour as operationalized in the present destructive, is of minor importance to them. This
study, it is difficult to evaluate whether a assumption is supported by Baumeister and
prevalence rate of about 3.5% should be con- colleagues (2001), who found that bad experi-
sidered high, average or low. However, if we look ences have a much stronger impact than good
at the item level, this form of leadership ones, resulting in a ‘horns’ effect, whereby the
behaviour overlaps with behaviour classified as leader is only perceived in negative terms.
abusive (Bies and Tripp, 1998) or aggressive Supportive–disloyal leadership behaviour had
leadership (Hubert and van Veldhoven, 2001; a prevalence rate of about 11.5% according to
Schat, Frone and Kelloway, 2006). Thus, one can the OCM, and it was the second most prevalent
compare the prevalence rate for tyrannical form of destructive leadership in the present
leadership behaviour with the prevalence rates study. This form of leadership behaviour involves
obtained in the study by Schat, Frone and behaving in a comradely and supportive manner
Kelloway (2006) and the study by Hubert and towards subordinates, while at the same time
van Veldhoven (2001). Although Schat, Frone violating the legitimate interests of the organiza-
and Kelloway found a prevalence rate of 13.5% tion by, for example, working towards alternative
for people who reported some kind of exposure goals to those of the organization, stealing from
to aggressive behaviour from their superior, only the organization or encouraging subordinates to
5.5% reported frequent exposure to such beha- engage in such activities (Einarsen, Aasland and
viour. Furthermore, Schat, Frone and Kelloway Skogstad, 2007). To our knowledge, this form of
measured exposure to aggressive behaviour dur- destructive leadership behaviour has not been

r 2009 British Academy of Management.


448 M. S. Aasland et al.

investigated in earlier studies on destructive a medium frequency, while some 6% of the


leadership. However, a recent qualitative, cross- respondents reported high exposure to all these
cultural study focusing on observations of senior three destructive forms of leadership. Hence,
leaders’ wrongdoing in ten different countries laissez-faire leadership seems to be much more
(Birkland, Glomb and Ones, 2008) showed that prevalent than indicated by the use of the OCM.
anti-organizational behaviour was reported more The present study focuses on exposure to
often than anti-subordinate behaviour, support- destructive leadership behaviour during a six-
ing the high prevalence rates of both derailed and month period. Hence, the behaviours reported in
supportive–disloyal leadership in the present the different clusters are not necessarily experi-
study. Moreover, this form of destructive leader- enced simultaneously. Laissez-faire leadership be-
ship may be underestimated in the present study haviour may over time evolve into more active
as leaders at different levels in an organization forms of destructive leadership behaviour, in the
have a continual opportunity to misuse their same way that tyrannical leadership behaviour
power and position for personal gain (Conger, may evolve into derailed leadership behaviour over
1990), relatively often without being noticed by time (Ma, Karri and Chittipeddi, 2004). However,
subordinates. With such extensive opportunities longitudinal studies are needed to investigate
to conceal these forms of destructive behaviour, whether such an escalation process is involved.
we believe that a prevalence rate of some 11.5% According to the LCC analysis, about 17% of
should be considered a conservative estimate and, the respondents reported consistent and systema-
as such, as representing a serious problem for tic exposure to supportive–disloyal leadership
organizations. behaviour in combination with laissez-faire lea-
In the present study, laissez-faire leadership dership behaviour. Moreover, 10% reported
behaviour showed the highest prevalence rate, more frequent exposure to supportive–disloyal
with 21.2% of the respondents reporting that they leadership behaviour alone. Hence, low-fre-
experienced at least one such leadership beha- quency supportive–disloyal leadership behaviour
viour ‘quite often’ or ‘very often or nearly always’ seems fairly common in Norwegian working life.
during the last six months. Lack of adequate Supportive–disloyal leadership behaviour may
leadership, which is the case for laissez-faire even be perceived as just ‘being nice’, and not
leadership, is passive behaviour that may be as constituting destructive behaviour. However,
harder to detect and intervene against than more over time, even low-frequency supportive–dis-
active forms of destructive leadership behaviour, loyal leadership may entail considerable expense
and it may also be more easily tolerated by senior for the organization. Additionally, our results
managers (Skogstad et al., 2007). However, show that highly abusive leaders display a range
according to the LCC analysis, destructive leader- of different forms of behaviour, be it laissez-faire,
ship is not an either–or phenomenon, but more a derailed or tyrannical behaviour. A total of 17%
compound phenomenon with respect to both its of the respondents report such compound nega-
nature and frequency. Based on this analysis, tive behaviour, even though only 6% report it
laissez-faire leadership behaviour is reported in happening on a very frequent basis.
three different forms, either as a stand-alone Thus, the results substantiate our assumption
phenomenon, in combination with supportive– that destructive leadership is a phenomenon
disloyal leadership behaviour or as part of a comprising different classes of behaviour. Focus-
compound phenomenon in which respondents ing on aggression by a leader (Hubert and van
report exposure to both tyrannical and derailed Veldhoven, 2001; Schat, Frone and Kelloway,
leadership behaviour as well as to laissez-faire 2006) or abusive supervision only (Tepper, 2007)
leadership. Nineteen per cent of the respondents may therefore result in a too constricted focus,
reported some laissez-faire leadership behaviour resulting in a partial picture of the complex
according to this analysis, while an additional phenomenon of destructive leadership. Further-
17% were exposed to a low-frequency combina- more, moderate correlations exist between the
tion of laissez-faire and supportive–disloyal lea- different forms of destructive leadership behaviour
dership behaviour. Moreover, 11% of the and constructive leadership behaviour (Table 2),
respondents reported being exposed to tyrannical, indicating that destructive leadership is not some-
derailed and laissez-faire leadership behaviour at thing that exists apart from constructive leader-

r 2009 British Academy of Management.


Destructive Leadership Behaviour 449

ship but is an integrated part of the behavioural supportive–disloyal leadership behaviour sub-
repertoire of leaders. A similar conclusion can be scale of the destructive leadership scale is low,
drawn from an English study on harassment by thus questioning the internal consistency of this
managers (Rayner and Cooper, 2003) in which, of subscale (Hinkin, 1998). Consequently, the re-
72 managers evaluated by subordinates, only sults obtained using this subscale should be
11.1% were exclusively perceived as being a interpreted with caution, and the subscale should
‘tough manager’, with 9.7% of the managers be investigated further in future studies.
being exclusively perceived as ‘angels’. In the same The present study is based on subordinates’
study, about 28% of the managers were evaluated appraisal. Phillips and Lord (1986) distinguish
as either ‘tough managers with supporters’, between two types of accuracy in such leader
‘middlers with victims’ or as ‘angels with victims’. appraisals, namely classification accuracy and
Thus, leaders behave both destructively and behavioural accuracy. Classification accuracy
constructively, demonstrating different behaviour refers to the ability to adequately classify a
and different combinations of behaviour in rela- person’s behaviour, while behavioural accuracy
tion to different subordinates, who again may is the ability to detect the presence of a specific
react differently (Rayner and Cooper, 2003). This type of behaviour (Bretz, Milkovich and Read,
has implications for leadership research, which, in 1992). If classification accuracy is lacking, sub-
the past, has mainly seen leadership as being either ordinates may be influenced by a ‘halo’ (Thorn-
constructive or destructive, with the latter being dike, 1920) or ‘horns’ effect (Bligh et al., 2007).
an anomaly. Furthermore, experiencing both The halo effect consists of an overall positive
constructive and destructive behaviour from the evaluation of a leader based, for example, on a
same source may have more distressing effects on single positive characteristic or action (Thorn-
the target than being exposed to destructive dike, 1920), while the horns effect refers to the
behaviour alone (Duffy, Ganster and Pagon, reverse situation in which an overall negative
2002; Major et al., 1997). appraisal is made based on one salient failure or
negative characteristic (Bligh et al., 2007), which
may prevent subordinates from noticing the
Methodological issues
variety and nuances in their leader’s behaviour.
A notable strength of the present study is its use However, Cardy and Dobbins (1994) emphasize
of a representative sample of the Norwegian that subordinates are valid sources of informa-
workforce (Hstmark and Lagerstrm, 2006). tion, as they are often well placed to observe and
Previous research in this field has mainly been evaluate their leader’s performance and leader-
carried out in small non-representative samples, ship style. Moreover, as negative experiences and
giving rise to uncertainty about the general- information are processed more thoroughly than
izability of the findings. Furthermore, the present are positive information and experiences (Bau-
study employs two methods of calculating pre- meister et al., 2001), subordinates’ appraisals of
valence rates. While the OCM implicitly assumes destructive leadership behaviour probably have
that only two groups of respondents exist (those high behavioural accuracy.
exposed and those not exposed), the LCC
method identifies several different groups with
respect to observations of destructive leadership Conclusion
in one’s immediate superior (Notelaers et al.,
2006), taking both the nature and the frequency Destructive forms of leadership behaviour are
of the behaviour into account. Research on highly prevalent, at least in their less severe
bullying in the workplace shows that the LCC forms, including the passive form of laissez-faire
method displays better construct and predictive leadership. Considering the negative effects of
validity than the OCM (Notelaers et al., 2006). destructive leadership for both subordinates and
Thus, the LCC method seems to result in a more the organization documented in several studies
convincing and comprehensive estimate of the (Bamberger and Bacharach, 2006; Mitchell and
prevalence of destructive leadership. Ambrose, 2007; Tepper, 2000), destructive lea-
A limitation of the present study that should be dership constitutes a serious problem in con-
mentioned is that the Cronbach’s alpha of the temporary working life. Furthermore, destructive

r 2009 British Academy of Management.


450 M. S. Aasland et al.

leadership behaviour comes in many shapes and Johnson and J. D. Douglas (eds), Crime at the Top: Deviance
forms, categorized along two basic dimensions, in Business and the Professions, pp. 90–124. Philadelphia, PA:
namely pro-organizational versus anti-organiza- J. B. Lippincott.
Aryee, S., Z. X. Chen, L. Sun and Y. A. Debrah (2007).
tional behaviour and pro-subordinate versus ‘Antecedents and outcomes of abusive supervision: test of a
anti-subordinate behaviour, meaning that a leader trickle-down model’, Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, pp.
over a period of time may display constructive as 191–201.
well as destructive behaviour. Ashforth, B. (1994). ‘Petty tyranny in organizations’, Human
A high prevalence rate of destructive leader- Relations, 47, pp. 755–778.
Avolio, B. J. (1999). Full Leadership Development. Building the
ship behaviour has important implications for Vital Forces in Organizations. London: Sage.
theory development regarding destructive leader- Bamberger, P. A. and S. B. Bacharach (2006). ‘Abusive
ship in particular, but also for leadership research supervision and subordinate problem drinking: taking
in general. Leaders who behave in a destructive resistance, stress, and subordinate personality into account’,
manner are not exceptional, nor can they be Human Relations, 59, pp. 1–30.
Baruch, Y. and B. C. Holtom (2008). ‘Survey response rate
referred to as a few deviants, at least not as levels in organizational research’, Human Relations, 61, pp.
experienced by their subordinates. Moreover, 1139–1160.
destructive leadership behaviour is not a phe- Bass, B. M. (1990a). ‘Authoritarianism, power orientation,
nomenon that exists apart from constructive Machiavellianism, and leadership’. In B. M. Bass (ed.), Bass
leadership, but must be viewed as an integral & Stogdill’s Handbook of Leadership. Theory, Research, and
Managerial Applications, pp. 124–139. New York: Free Press.
part of what constitutes leadership behaviour. Bass, B. M. (1990b). ‘Laissez-faire leadership versus motivation
Including this ‘dark side’ of leadership, a more to manage’. In B. M. Bass (ed.), Bass & Stogdill’s Handbook
accurate and nuanced understanding of the very of Leadership. Theory, Research and Managerial Applications,
phenomenon of leadership behaviour may pp. 544–559. New York: Free Press.
emerge, which in turn may contribute to the Bass, B. M. and B. J. Avolio (1990). Transformational Leader-
ship Development: Manual for the Multifactor Leadership
general understanding of both the nature and Questionnaire. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
effectiveness of leadership, and to the develop- Bass, B. M. and R. E. Riggio (2006). Transformational
ment and management of leaders (Burke, 2006). Leadership. London: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Leaders may behave destructively for a variety Bass, B. M., B. J. Avolio, D. I. Jung and Y. Berson (2003).
of reasons, be it their personality, incompetence, ‘Predicting unit performance by assessing transformational
and transactional leadership’, Journal of Applied Psychology,
perceived injustice or threat to their identity, 88, pp. 207–218.
financial reasons, low organizational identifica- Baumeister, R. F., E. Bratslavsky, C. Finkenauer and K. D.
tion etc. Future studies should investigate the Vohs (2001). ‘Bad is stronger than good’, Review of General
antecedents of the different forms of destructive Psychology, 5, pp. 323–370.
leadership behaviour identified in this paper, as Bies, R. J. and T. M. Tripp (1998). ‘Two faces of the powerless.
Coping with tyranny in organizations’. In R. M. Kramer and
such knowledge could help us prevent such
M. A. Neale (eds), Power and Influence in Organizations, pp.
behaviour among leaders and develop tools for 203–219. London: Sage.
organizational reactions and the rehabilitation of Birkland, A., T. M. Glomb and D. S. Ones (2008). ‘A cross-
leaders who act in breach of the legitimate cultural comparison of senior leader wrongdoing’. Paper
interest of the organization. presented at the 23rd Annual Conference of the Society for
Industrial and Organizational Psychology, 11 April, San
Francisco, CA.
Blake, R. R. and J. S. Mouton (1985). The Managerial Grid III.
References Houston, TX: Gulf Publishing.
Bligh, M. C., J. C. Kohles, C. L. Pearce, J. E. G. Justin and J. F.
Aasland, M. S., A. Skogstad and S. Einarsen (2008). ‘The dark Stovall (2007). ‘When the romance is over: follower
side of leadership: defining and describing destructive forms perspectives of aversive leadership’, Applied Psychology: An
of leadership behaviour’, Organizations and People, 15, pp. International Review, 56, pp. 528–557.
20–28. Bretz, R. D. J., G. T. Milkovich and W. Read (1992). ‘The
Adorno, T. W., E. Frenkel-Brunswik, D. J. Levinson and R. N. current state of performance appraisal research and practice:
Sanford (1950). The Authoritarian Personality. New York: concerns, directions, and implications’, Journal of Manage-
Harper. ment, 18, pp. 321–352.
Agervold, M. (2007). ‘Bullying at work: a discussion of Burke, R. J. (2006). ‘Why leaders fail. Exploring the dark side’.
definitions and prevalence, based on an empirical study’, In R. J. Burke and C. L. Cooper (eds), Inspiring Leaders, pp.
Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 48, pp. 161–172. 239–248. London: Routledge.
Altheide, D. L., P. A. Adler, P. Adler and D. A. Altheide Buss, A. H. (1961). The Psychology of Aggression. London:
(1978). ‘The social meaning of employee theft’. In J. M. Wiley.

r 2009 British Academy of Management.


Destructive Leadership Behaviour 451

Cardy, R. L. and G. H. Dobbins (1994). Performance Appraisal: Hubert, A. B. and M. van Veldhoven (2001). ‘Risk sectors
Alternative Perspective. Cincinnati, OH: South-Western Pub- for undesirable behaviour and mobbing’, European
lishing. Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 10, pp.
Christie, R. and F. L. Geis (1970). Studies in Machiavellianism. 415–424.
New York: Academic Press. Kellerman, B. (2004). Bad Leadership. What it is, How it
Conger, J. A. (1990). ‘The dark side of leadership’, Organiza- Happens, Why it Matters. Boston, MA: Harvard Business
tional Dynamics, 19, pp. 44–55. School Press.
Conger, J. A. and R. N. Kanungo (1987). ‘Toward a behavioral Kelloway, E. K., J. Mullen and L. Francis (2006). ‘Divergent
theory of charismatic leadership in organizational settings’, effects of transformational and passive leadership on employ-
Academy of Management Review, 12, pp. 637–647. ee safety’, Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 11, pp.
Conger, J. A. and R. N. Kanungo (1988). ‘The empowerment 76–86.
process: integrating theory and practice’, Academy of Kelloway, E. K., N. Sivanathan, L. Francis and J. Barling
Management Review, 13, pp. 471–482. (2005). ‘Poor leadership’. In J. Barling, E. K. Kelloway and
Ditton, J. (1977). Part-time Crime: An Ethnography of Fiddling M. R. Frone (eds), Handbook of Work Stress, pp. 89–112.
and Pilferage. London: Billings. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Duffy, M. K., D. C. Ganster and M. Pagon (2002). ‘Social Kets de Vries, F. R. and D. Miller (1985). ‘Narcissism and
undermining in the workplace’, Academy of Management leadership: an object relations perspective’, Human Relations,
Journal, 45, pp. 331–351. 38, pp. 583–601.
Einarsen, S., M. S. Aasland and A. Skogstad (2007). Kipnis, D., S. Schmidt, K. Price and C. Stitt (1981). ‘Why do I
‘Destructive leadership behaviour: a definition and a like thee: is it your performance or my orders?’, Journal of
conceptual model’, Leadership Quarterly, 18, pp. 207–216. Applied Psychology, 66, pp. 324–328.
Einarsen, S., A. Skogstad, M. S. Aasland and A. M. S. B. Lewin, K., R. Lippitt and R. K. White (1939). ‘Patterns of
Lseth (2002). ‘‘Destruktivt lederskap: årsaker og konsek- aggressive behaviour in experimentally created social cli-
venser’ [‘Destructive leadership: predictors and conse- mates’, Journal of Social Psychology, 10, pp. 271–301.
quences’]’. In A. Skogstad and S. Einarsen (eds), Ledelse på Lipman-Blumen, J. (2005). The Allure of Toxic Leaders. Why
Godt og Vondt. Effektivitet og Trivsel [Leadership for Better We Follow Destructive Bosses and Corrupt Politicians – and
or Worse. Efficiency and Job Satisfaction], pp. 233–254. How We Can Survive Them. Oxford: Oxford University
Bergen: Fagbokforlaget. Press.
Ekvall, G. and J. Arvonen (1991). ‘Change-centered leadership: Lubit, R. (2004). ‘The tyranny of toxic managers: applying
an extension of the two-dimensional model’, Scandinavian emotional intelligence to deal with difficult personalities’,
Journal of Management, 7, pp. 17–26. Ivey Business Journal, 68, pp. 1–7.
Fox, S. and P. E. Spector (1999). ‘A model of work frustration– Ma, H., R. Karri and K. Chittipeddi (2004). ‘The paradox of
aggression’, Journal of Organizational Behavior, 20, pp. 915– managerial tyranny’, Business Horizons, 4, pp. 33–40.
931. Magidson, J. and J. K. Vermunt (2002a). ‘Latent class modeling
Frischer, J. and K. Larsson (2000). ‘Laissez-faire in research as a probabilistic extension of K-means clustering’, Quirk’s
education – an inquiry into a Swedish doctoral program’, Marketing Research Review, 20, pp. 77–80.
Higher Education Policy, 13, pp. 131–155. Magidson, J. and J. K. Vermunt (2002b). ‘Latent class models
Goodman, L. A. (1974). ‘The analysis of systems of qualitative for clustering: a comparison with K-means’, Canadian
variables when some of the variables are unobservable. Part Journal of Marketing Research, 20, pp. 37–44.
1: A modified latent structure approach’, American Journal of Magidson, J. and J. K. Vermunt (2004). ‘Latent class models’.
Sociology, 79, pp. 1179–1259. In D. Kaplan (ed.), The SAGE Handbook of Quantitative
Hinkin, T. R. (1998). ‘A brief tutorial on the development of Methodology for the Social Sciences, pp. 175–198. Thousand
measures for use in survey questionnaires’, Organizational Oaks, CA: Sage.
Research Methods, 1, pp. 104–121. Major, B., J. M. Zubek, M. L. Cooper, C. Cozzarelli and C.
Hinkin, T.R and C. A. Schriesheim (2008). ‘An examination of Richards (1997). ‘Mixed messages: implications of social
‘‘nonleadership’’. From laissez-faire leadership to leader conflict and social support within close relationships for
reward omission and punishment omission’, Journal of adjustment to a stressful life event’, Journal of Personality and
Applied Psychology, 93, pp. 1234–1248. Social Psychology, 72, pp. 1349–1363.
Hogan, R. (1994). ‘Trouble at the top: causes and consequences McCall, M. W. J. and M. M. Lombardo (1983). Off the Track: Why
of managerial incompetence’, Consulting Psychology Journal, and How Successful Executives Get Derailed. Technical Report
46, pp. 9–15. No. 21. Greensboro, NC: Center for Creative Leadership.
Hogan, R., R. Raskin and D. Fazzini (1990). ‘The dark side of McCutcheon, A. (1987). Latent Class Analysis. Newbury Park,
charisma’. In K. E. Clark and M. B. Clark (eds), Measures of CA: Sage.
Leadership, pp. 343–354. West Orange, NJ: Leadership Mitchell, M. S. and M. L. Ambrose (2007). ‘Abusive super-
Library of America. vision and workplace deviance and the moderating effects of
Hstmark, M. and B. O. Lagerstrm (2006). ‘Underskelse om negative reciprocity beliefs’, Journal of Applied Psychology,
arbeidsmilj: destruktiv atferd i arbeidslivet’ [‘A study of 92, pp. 1159–1168.
work environment: destructive behaviours in working life’]. Nielsen, M. B., A. Skogstad, S. B. Matthiesen, L. Glas, M. S.
Dokumentasjonsrapport 44, Statistisk sentralbyrå/Statistics Aasland, G. Notelaers and S. Einarsen (2009). ‘Prevalence of
Norway, Oslo. workplace bullying in Norway: comparisons across time and
House, R. J. and J. M. Howell (1992). ‘Personality and estimation methods’, European Journal of Work and Organi-
charismatic leadership’, Leadership Quarterly, 3, pp. 81–108. zational Psychology, 18, pp. 81–101.

r 2009 British Academy of Management.


452 M. S. Aasland et al.

Notelaers, G., S. Einarsen, H. De Witte and J. K. Vermunt Skogstad, A., S. Einarsen, T. Torsheim, M. S. Aasland and H.
(2006). ‘Measuring exposure to bullying at work: the validity Hetland (2007). ‘The destructiveness of laissez-faire leader-
and advantages of the latent class cluster approach’, Work ship behavior’, Journal of Occupational Health Psychology,
and Stress, 20, pp. 288–301. 12, pp. 80–92.
Padilla, A., R. Hogan and R. B. Kaiser (2007). ‘The toxic Tepper, B. J. (2000). ‘Consequences of abusive supervision’,
triangle: destructive leaders, susceptible followers, and Academy of Management Journal, 43, pp. 178–190.
conducive environments’, Leadership Quarterly, 18, pp. Tepper, B. J. (2007). ‘Abusive supervision in work organiza-
176–194. tions: review, synthesis, and research agenda’, Journal of
Phillips, J. S. and R. G. Lord (1986). ‘Notes of the practical and Management, 33, pp. 261–289.
theoretical consequences of implicit leadership measurement’, Thorndike, E. L. (1920). ‘A constant error on psychological
Journal of Management, 12, pp. 31–41. rating’, Journal of Applied Psychology, 4, pp. 25–29.
Rayner, C. and C. L. Cooper (2003). ‘The black hole in Vredenburgh, D. and Y. Brender (1998). ‘The hierarchical
‘‘bullying at work’’ research’, International Journal of abuse of power in work organizations’, Journal of Business
Management and Decision Making, 4, pp. 47–64. Ethics, 17, pp. 1337–1347.
Sackett, P. R. and C. J. DeVore (2001). ‘Counterproductive Wilson, B. (1991). ‘U.S. businesses suffer from workplace
behaviors at work’. In N. Anderson, D. S. Ones, H. K. trauma. Here’s how to protect the mental health of your
Sinangil and C. Viswesvaran (eds), Handbook of Industrial, work force – and the fiscal health of your company’,
Work and Organizational Psychology, Vol. 1, pp. 145–164. Personnel Journal, 70, pp. 47–50.
London: Sage. Zapf, D., S. Einarsen, H. Hoel and M. Vartia (2003). ‘Empirical
Schat, A. C. H., M. R. Frone and E. K. Kelloway (2006). findings on bullying in the workplace’. In S. Einarsen, H. Hoel,
‘Prevalence of workplace aggression in the U.S. workforce: D. Zapf and C. L. Cooper (eds), Bullying and Emotional
findings from a national study’. In E. K. Kelloway, J. Barling Abuse in the Workplace. International Perspectives in Re-
and J. J. Hurrell (eds), Handbook of Workplace Violence, pp. search and Practice, pp. 103–126. London: Taylor & Francis.
47–90. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Zellars, K. L., B. J. Tepper and M. K. Duffy (2002). ‘Abusive
Shackleton, V. (1995). ‘Leaders who derail’. In V. Shackleton supervision and subordinates’ organizational citizenship
(ed.), Business Leadership, pp. 89–100. London: Thomson. behavior’, Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, pp. 1068–1076.

Merethe Schanke Aasland is a licensed clinical psychologist, currently working as a research fellow
and a PhD student at the University of Bergen completing her PhD thesis on destructive leadership
in organizations. She is also a member of the Bergen Bullying Research Group. Main research topics
are leadership, counterproductive behaviours and workplace bullying.

Dr Anders Skogstad is a Professor at the Faculty of Psychology, University of Bergen, and member
of the Bergen Bullying Research Group. He is a specialist in work and organizational psychology,
Norwegian Psychological Association. His main research topics are destructive leadership in
organizations, psychosocial factors at work, counterproductive behaviours and workplace bullying.

Guy Notelaers is currently funded by the Norwegian Research Council to deliver a PhD on a stress-
oriented approach to explain workplace bullying, and is a member of the Bergen Bullying Research
Group. His main research topics are methods and statistics in the area of work and organizational
research.

Morten Birkeland Nielsen has a PhD in psychology and is currently employed as a postdoctoral
research associate at the University of Bergen. He is a member of the Operational Psychology
Research Group and the Bergen Bullying Research Group. His main research topics are workplace
bullying, destructive leadership, whistle-blowing and research methodology.

Dr Ståle Einarsen is a Professor at the Faculty of Psychology, University of Bergen, and managing
director of the Bergen Bullying Research Group. His main research topics are destructive leadership
in organizations, creativity and innovation, workplace bullying and sexual harassment.

r 2009 British Academy of Management.


Copyright of British Journal of Management is the property of Wiley-Blackwell and its content may not be
copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written
permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

Вам также может понравиться