Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 16

INT J LANG COMMUN DISORD, JANUARY–FEBRUARY 2013,

VOL. 48, NO. 1, 25–40

Review
How should children with speech sound disorders be classified? A review
and critical evaluation of current classification systems
R. Waring and R. Knight
Department of Language and Communication Sciences, City University London, London, UK
(Received January 2012; accepted September 2012)

Abstract
Background: Children with speech sound disorders (SSD) form a heterogeneous group who differ in terms of the
severity of their condition, underlying cause, speech errors, involvement of other aspects of the linguistic system
and treatment response. To date there is no universal and agreed-upon classification system. Instead, a number of
theoretically differing classification systems have been proposed based on either an aetiological (medical) approach,
a descriptive–linguistic approach or a processing approach.
Aims: To describe and review the supporting evidence, and to provide a critical evaluation of the current childhood
SSD classification systems.
Methods & Procedures: Descriptions of the major specific approaches to classification are reviewed and research
papers supporting the reliability and validity of the systems are evaluated.
Main Contribution Three specific paediatric SSD classification systems; the aetiologic-based Speech Disorders
Classification System, the descriptive–linguistic Differential Diagnosis system, and the processing-based Psy-
cholinguistic Framework are identified as potentially useful in classifying children with SSD into homogeneous
subgroups. The Differential Diagnosis system has a growing body of empirical support from clinical population
studies, across language error pattern studies and treatment efficacy studies. The Speech Disorders Classification
System is currently a research tool with eight proposed subgroups. The Psycholinguistic Framework is a potential
bridge to linking cause and surface level speech errors.
Conclusions & Implications There is a need for a universally agreed-upon classification system that is useful to
clinicians and researchers. The resulting classification system needs to be robust, reliable and valid. A universal
classification system would allow for improved tailoring of treatments to subgroups of SSD which may, in turn,
lead to improved treatment efficacy.

Keywords: childhood speech sound disorders, phonology, classification.

What this paper adds


Children with speech sound disorders (SSD) comprise more than 70% of clinicians’ caseloads and form an extremely
heterogeneous group. One of the greatest difficulties facing the classification of such children is that there is no
universal and agreed-upon system for SSD. There is support for the division of speech disorder into those with a
known aetiological cause versus unknown or ‘functional’ speech disorders. However, how to further divide the resulting
large, heterogeneous group of speech disorders of unknown origin remains controversial. To date there is no detailed
comparative discussion and evaluation of alternative childhood SSD classification systems. This paper describes the
classification systems, reviews existing empirical evidence and evaluates the major classification systems against five
criteria. This paper highlights the need for a clinically useful classification system that provides an explanation of SSD
of unknown origin while directing clinicians towards the best treatment approaches to implement for the resulting
SSD subgroups.

Address correspondence to: Rebecca Waring, Department of Language and Communication Sciences, City University London, Northampton
Square, London EC1V 0HB, UK; e-mail: Rebecca.Waring.1@city.ac.uk
International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders
ISSN 1368-2822 print/ISSN 1460-6984 online  C 2012 Royal College of Speech and Language Therapists

DOI: 10.1111/j.1460-6984.2012.00195.x
26 R. Waring and R. Knight
Introduction existing classification methods reflect the differing the-
oretical views of SSD, be it an aetiological (medical)
Speech sound disorders (SSD) are the most common
approach, a descriptive–linguistic approach or a pro-
paediatric communication disorder, affecting between
cessing approach (Tyler 2010). Figure 1 illustrates how
10% and 15% of preschoolers and 6% of school-aged
the current classification systems can be conceptualized
children (American Speech–Language–Hearing Associ-
according to theoretical approach.
ation (ASHA) 2000, McLeod and Harrison 2009). The
minority of SSD cases are attributable to a known cause,
Classification
such as a cognitive impairment, sensori-neural hear-
ing loss, cleft lip and/or palate or cerebral palsy. For There is no single ‘natural and right classification sys-
most children, the cause of their speech impairment is tem’ (Cantwell and Baker 1988: 522) in the health
unknown (Broomfield and Dodd 2004a, Shriberg and sciences. Classification systems can, however, be objec-
Kwiatkowski 1991). Children with an SSD of unknown tively evaluated. Cantwell and Baker (1988) and Taylor
origin are typically diagnosed between 2 and 4 years of (2011) proposed the following evaluation criteria: (1)
age, and present with restricted speech sound systems reliability—the degree to which all clinicians using the
without any apparent sensory, structural, neurological system would arrive at the same diagnosis for a given
or psychological impairment (Gierut 1998). Moreover, client; (2) validity—the extent to which a system mea-
SSD of unknown origin can persist beyond the preschool sures what it purports to measure and includes face (clin-
years, placing some children at risk academically, socially icians’ intuition), descriptive (uniqueness of categories),
and vocationally (McCormack et al. 2009). predictive (ability to predict outcome) and construct (re-
Over the last decade, researchers have increasingly lationship to theory) validity; (3) coverage—the extent
acknowledged the heterogeneity of speech sound im- to which the system can accurately differentially diag-
pairment (Tyler 2010). There is now consensus that nose all presenting individuals into distinct subgroups
children with SSD do not form a homogeneous group while ensuring sensitivity (identifying true positives),
(Baker 2006, Bowen 2009, Dodd 2005, Grunwell 1981, specificity (identifying true negatives), and reducing in-
Shriberg 1997, Stackhouse and Wells 1997). Children correct diagnoses (false-positives and false-negatives);
with SSD differ in severity, underlying cause, speech er- and (4) feasibility—the extent to which a classification
ror characteristics, involvement of other aspects of the system can be successfully used by the professionals for
linguistic system, response to treatment, and mainte- whom it was designed. This paper presents a descrip-
nance factors (Dodd 2011). Interest in the classification tion and review of the evidence, and a critical evaluation
of childhood SSD has grown since the 1990s, but to using the criteria listed above, for three major specific
date there is no universal and agreed-upon system. The childhood SSD classification systems, namely Shriberg’s

Figure 1. Current major classification systems for childhood speech sound disorders grouped by theoretical approach.
How should children with speech sound disorders be classified? 27
(2010) Speech Disorders Classification System (SDCS), tems, mainly due to familiarity of the classification sys-
Dodd’s (2005) Differential Diagnosis, and Stackhouse tems amongst health professionals and for insurance
and Wells (1997) Psycholinguistic Framework. billing purposes (McCauley 2004). Table 1 provides a
summary of the DSM-IV-TR and ICD-10 SSD classifi-
cation codes. The ICF-CY offers an alternative classifica-
Aetiological (medical) approach
tion for SSD. The ICF recognizes five domains, namely:
Aetiologically based classification systems are a- Body Structure; Body Function; Activities and Partic-
theoretical and start from a position of pathology rather ipation; Environmental Factors; and Personal Factors.
than normality. The central assumption of the aetio- According to the ICF, if there is a known sensory, neu-
logical approach is that an underlying clinical entity or rological or craniofacial impairment (e.g. hearing loss,
medical condition is responsible for a child’s presenting cerebral palsy, cleft lip and/or palate), the speech impair-
SSD. There are two types of aetiological-based classifica- ment is classified at the Body Structure level. If there is
tion systems: broad-based, which provide a classification no known cause, the speech impairment is classified at
system for more than communication disorders, such as the Body Function level.
the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM-IV-TR) These broad-based classification systems have played
(American Psychiatric Association 2000); the World an important role in establishing a framework for
Health Organization’s (WHO) International Classifica- categorizing childhood SSD by providing a common
tion of Diseases and Related Health Problems (ICD-10) reference frame for clinicians. Most importantly, the
(WHO 2007); and SSD-specific classification systems. coding systems of DSM-IV and ICD-10 have ensured
clinicians differentiate between SSD only, SSD and lan-
guage disorders, and SSD and other pervasive disorders.
Broad-based classification systems
The ICF-CY has encouraged (and continues to encour-
Currently there are three broad-based aetiologically age) the pursuit of a better understanding of the short-
based systems available for classifying childhood SSD: and long-term effects of childhood SSD on education,
DSM-IV-TR (2000), ICD-10 (2010), and the Inter- employment, social development and quality of life.
national Classification of Functioning, Disability and Unfortunately, DSM-IV-TR, ICD-10 and ICF-CY
Health (ICF) (WHO 2001) including the subsequent are inadequate classification systems for differentially
Children and Youth (CY) Version (2007). DSM-IV and diagnosing childhood SSD. The three classification
ICD-10 are the most clinically utilized classification sys- systems are too broad in their subgroup definitions,

Table 1. DSM-IV-TR and ICD-10 categories and codes for childhood SSD

Classification Name and Differentially


system Category Subdivision code Criteria diagnosed from:
DSM-IV-TR Disorders usually first Communication Phonological disorder 1. Failure to use age- 1. general medical or
diagnosed in infancy, disorders 1.4 315.39 and sensory condition;
childhood or dialect-appropriate 2. expressive language
adolescence 1 speech sounds. Errors disorder (315.31)
due to sound 3. mixed
production or receptive-expressive
difficulties with disorder (315.32)
representations, 4. stuttering (307.0)
organization and use 5. communication
(i.e. articulation and disorder not
phonological-based otherwise specified
disorders (307.4)
ICD-10 Mental and behavioural Disorders of Specific disorders of Child’s speech is below 1. aphasia not otherwise
disorders V psychological speech and language expected for mental specified (NOS)
development F80 age, whilst language R47.0
F80–F89 Specific speech skills are in the 2. apraxia R48.2
articulation disorder appropriate range 3. hearing loss
F80.0 H90–H91
Other developmental 4. mental retardation
disorders of speech F70–F79
and 5. with language
language—lisping developmental
F80.8 disorder +expressive
F80.1 + receptive
F80.2
28 R. Waring and R. Knight
resulting in children with SSD of unknown origin SSD are grouped according to the presence or absence
ending up in one of two subgroups: either speech of at least one structural, sensory or neurophysiological
sound production problems (‘articulation’ errors) and factor. An unknown category is provided as a default
cognitive–linguistic-based problems (phonological er- category. Ruscello’s subgroups are presented in table 3.
rors) subgroups, or known versus unknown aetiology A significant weakness with aetiological classification
subgroups. Specific childhood SSD classification sys- systems is the failure to subdivide the large and hetero-
tems have been developed to address this heterogeneous geneous SSD group of unknown origin. The lack of
subgroup problem. true differentiation reduces the explanatory power and
clinical utility of the aetiological classification system
to account for SSD and aid the management of chil-
Brief history of specific aetiological classification dren with speech impairment. Shriberg and colleagues
systems attempt to address these issues with the proposed SDCS.

The first childhood SSD classification systems were


strongly influenced by the medical approach (Bowen Speech Disorders Classification System (SDCS):
2009). The early classification systems focused on de-
a description
lineating three causes of SSD: a known cause; a putative
or supposed cause; and an unknown cause. An early The SDCS was developed to address the issue of how
example of an aetiological classification system is T. In- to classify children with SSD of unknown origin. The
gram’s classification system (Ingram 1959). Ingram’s six SDCS has been developed over the last 30 years; is
speech disorder subgroups appear in table 2. based on the data of several hundred subjects with SSD
Recently, Ruscello (2008) described a four-subgroup of unknown origin; and has evolved from five sub-
aetiological SSD-specific classification system. A key fea- groups (Shriberg 1994) to seven subgroups (Shriberg
ture of this classification system is the division of chil- 2006), to the current eight subgroups of SSD (Shriberg
dren with SSD into those with a known versus unknown et al. 2010a). The SDCS’s underpinning premise is that
aetiological basis for speech impairment. Children with an unvarying relationship exists between an identifiable

Table 2. T. Ingram’s subgroups of speech disorders adapted from Ingram (1959, 1972)

Speech disorder subtype Cause Percentage occurrence


Disorders of vocalization (dysphonia) Chronic or recurrent laryngitis, over-use of 3–5% children referred to hospital clinics
the voice
Disorders of respiratory coordination Defect in the accurate coordination of 3–4%, with 1% persistent
(dysrhythmia) respiratory and articulatory mechanisms
Disorders of speech sound production with Due to neurological abnormalities or local Not reported
demonstrable dysfunction or structural abnormalities (e.g. cerebral palsy)
abnormality of tongue, lips and palate
(dysarthria)
Disorders of speech sound production not ‘Mental defect’ (intellectual disability), Not reported
attributable to dysfunction or structural ‘hearing defect’ (hearing loss), true
abnormalities dysaphasia, psychiatric disorder, adverse
environmental factors, combination of
these
Developmental speech disorder syndrome Unknown ?‘auditory imperception’; not due Not reported
(specific developmental speech disorder) to intelligence, home background,
structure or function of oral mechanism
Mixed speech disorders, comprising two or Not reported
more of the above categories

Table 3. Summary of Ruscello’s (2008) Sound Systems Disorders (SSD) classification system

SSD subtype Cause Percentage occurrence


Oral structure defect Major and/or minor oral structure defects such as cleft Not reported
lip and palate, jaw malocclusion, tongue
malformation, missing teeth
Sensory deficit Hearing loss Not reported
Motor speech disorders: apraxia, dysarthria or Neurological deficit leading to difficulties with motor Not reported
both planning and/or motor execution
Sound system disorder of unknown origin ?Mislearning, linguistic-based Largest subgroup
How should children with speech sound disorders be classified? 29
Table 4. Summary of the Speech Disorders Classification System (SDCS) by typology and aetiology

Typology Subgroup aetiology Cause Processes affected Percentage occurrence


Normal/normalized speech – – – –
Speech delay Genetic Polygenic/environmental Cognitive–linguistic 56% M > F
Otitis media with effusion Polygenic/environmental Auditory–perceptual 30% M = F
Psychosocial Polygenic/environmental Psychosocial 12% M > F
Motor speech disorders Apraxia Monogenic? Oligogenic? Issues with speech motor control < 1% M >> F
Dysarthria Monogenic? Oligogenic? ?
Not otherwise specified Monogenic? Oligogenic? ?
Speech errors /r/ Environmental Speech attunement ?M>F
/s/ Environmental ?F>M
Note: Adapted from Shriberg (2010).

genetic anomaly and a specific type of speech behaviour. the causes of SSD of unknown origin. A detailed eval-
Shriberg (2010) asserted that a genetic variation (in uation of the SDCS raises a number of issues, how-
conjunction with possible environmental factors) is the ever, with the overall validity, coverage and clinical
primary origin of each aetiologic subgroup, except for feasibility.
residual speech errors which are caused by environmen- A significant difficulty with the SDCS is the unique-
tal factors alone. Table 4 provides a summary of the eight ness of each subgroup category. The lack of empirically
SSD of unknown origin subgroups. supported sensitive and specific diagnostic markers com-
promises the descriptive validity of the eight subgroups,
particularly the three speech delay subgroups. Shriberg
SDCS evidence (2010) has suggested that the speech delay subgroups
Over the last decade, Shriberg and colleagues have at- may be overlapping and not distinct subgroups. This
tempted to identify diagnostic markers, or distinct er- is a significant concern as these groups (according to
ror patterns which distinguish the SDCS proposed sub- the SDCS) comprise approximately 98% of the SSD of
groups. Atypical sound errors (i.e. those that fall outside unknown origin cases. Collapsing the speech delay sub-
the ‘natural’ processes of assimilation, cluster reduction, groups would result in only 2% of the SSD of unknown
final consonant deletion, liquid simplification, palatal cases being differentially diagnosed from the larger, pre-
fronting, stopping, velar fronting and unstressed sylla- sumably heterogeneous group.
ble deletion) have been investigated as potential diagnos- A clinically useful classification should allow clini-
tic markers (Shriberg et al. 2003). In addition, a range cians to predict the severity, nature, course and likely
of speech indices (including competence, precision and responses to intervention. A significant problem with
stability) have been developed from the calculation of the aetiological SDCS is that the diagnostic labels pro-
various measures based on a large database (Shriberg vide little information about the nature and severity of
et al. 2010a). Finally, case history risk factors have also a child’s speech difficulties, or the type of treatment in-
been examined to determine potential subgroup differ- dicated. Shriberg has provided some preliminary ‘clues’
ences. According to Shriberg (2010), there is a prelimi- as to what direction intervention should take based on
nary pattern of risk factors and diagnostic markers that the explanatory processes involved in each subgroup;
differentiate the groups but ‘few of the markers have however, to date no research has been conducted that
demonstrated sufficient diagnostic accuracy’ (p. 26). matches diagnostic label to specific interventions to de-
Further research is required to provide sensitive and termine if there is a differentiated response to treatment.
specific diagnostic markers if the subgroups are to be This type of research would provide additional support
empirically validated. Table 5 provides a summary of for the predictive validity of the SDCS.
current specific subgroup patterns. Establishing single causal factors to account for the
SSD of unknown origin subgroups is a critical problem
SDCS evaluation with the SDCS (McCauley 2004, Tyler 2010). There is
little support for the theoretical construct that a one-to-
The SDCS has attracted support in the United States one relationship exists between a genetic anomaly and
and Australia since its inception. The support for the a subgroup of SSD. Most researchers view multiple and
SDCS is due in part to its perceived high face valid- complex genetic variants and environmental factors, in
ity. The major divisions of speech delay, motor speech addition to maintenance factors, as causing speech (and
disorder and residual speech errors seem to resonate language) impairments (Newbury and Monaco 2010).
with clinicians who informally recognize these sub- How the SDCS’s theoretical underpinnings stand up
groups and who favour a medical approach to explaining over time will depend on future genetic research.
30 R. Waring and R. Knight
Table 5. Summary of proposed Speech Disorders Classification System (SDCS) diagnostic markers by subgroup

Number of proposed
Subgroup markers Major diagnostic pattern features
Speech delay—genetic (SD-GEN) 5 Predominately omission errors with few distortion errors
Reduced language test scores
Reduced performance on non-word repetition
Speech delay—otitis media with effusion 10 Frequent middle ear infections;
(SD-OME) Backing of fricatives
Initial consonant deletion
Glottal stops
Insertion of /h/
Speech delay—psychosocial (SD-PSI) 3 Reduced social skills test scores;
Low percentage consonants correct—revised (PCCR) and
percentage vowels correct—revised (PVCR) scores
Motor speech disorder—apraxia of speech 10 Late talkers
(MSD-AOS) Vowel errors
Inconsistent errors
Inappropriate lexical stress
Motor speech disorder—dysarthria (MSD-DYS) 10 Reduced diadochokinetic (DDK) scores
Slower speech rate
Nasality
Reduced vocal quality
Motor speech disorder—not otherwise specified Not reported Speech, prosody and voice behaviours that are consistent
(MSD-NOS) with motor speech impairment, i.e. reduced speech rate,
imprecision of consonant, but are not specific for
apraxia or dysarthria
Speech errors: /s/ and /r/ Not reported Lisping or lateralized /s/
/w/ for /r/ substitution
Note: Adapted in part from Shriberg (2010) and Shriberg et al. (2010a).

Practically, the therapeutic value of genetic subgrouping come clinically useful, accessible analysis software will
will be irrelevant if it does not alter clinical treatment. be required at the very least.
To date, there is only speculative information about how Finally, Shriberg et al. (2010b) have reported high
gene therapy techniques may influence the treatment of inter- and intra-judge agreement for coding diagnos-
childhood SSD (Lewis 2010). tic markers. How well clinicians would reach the same
The SDCS’s coverage is also questionable because diagnosis for each child with SSD using the SDCS is
clinical intuition suggests that some children might be unknown.
unclassifiable under the SDCS. For instance, how would
a child presenting with no history of family speech and Descriptive–linguistic approach
language difficulties, otitis media with effusion (OME), The descriptive–linguistic approach to classification in-
or psychosocial issues, intact motor speech skills but volves describing children’s SSD according to the error
atypical speech sound errors (initial consonant deletion, patterns exhibited (Kamhi 1989). The approach is de-
backing of stops and devoicing) be classified? Fox et al.’s velopmental and relies on identifying and describing
(2002) study investigated the relationship between risk how a child’s speech differs from that of a child at the
factors (such as a history of hearing loss, family history) same age with typical development.
and speech disorders. The investigators were unable to
classify more than half of their subjects using Shriberg’s Brief history of descriptive–linguistic classification
(1994) version of the SDCS. Individuals who could not
systems
be classified presented with more than one risk factor
or with none of the risk factors. Further evidence of the The emergence of the descriptive–linguistic approach
SDCS’s exhaustiveness is required. in the 1960s/1970s was revolutionary and fundamen-
The feasibility of the current SDCS is difficult to tally changed the theoretical framework for assessing
judge as it is a research tool and is not yet intended for and diagnosing children with SSD (Grunwell 1997):
clinical use. However, the SDCS’s dependency on nar- rather than children with unintelligible speech being
row phonetic transcription, the use of complex formulae classified as ‘multiple misarticulators’ with numerous
to ascertain many of the codings, and the necessary time individual sound errors, children were seen as having
to complete each assessment would make the current sound class problems. Order was found in disorder
SDCS an unwieldy clinical classification tool. To be- (Grunwell 1997), and a shift in conceptualizing speech
How should children with speech sound disorders be classified? 31
Table 6. Grunwell’s (1985) Categories of phonological disorders

Category Description
Persistent normal processes Typical error patterns of younger children remain after an age when they should have disappeared
Chronological mismatch Uneven speech development so that earlier patterns co-occur with characteristics of later speech development
Unusual processes Use of rare or atypical error patterns
Systematic sound preferences Overuse of one sound for a large range of target consonants
Variable use of processes Multiple realizations for the same target consonant

impairments from occurring at the ‘mouth’ to occur- five subgroups of SSD. The central premise of Dodd’s
ring in the ‘mind’ began. Importantly, distinctions were classification system is that subgroups can be identified
made between normal, delayed and deviant speech de- by surface-level pattern errors that reflect underlying
velopment. The delayed-deviant dichotomy is apparent subgroup-specific processing deficits. Dodd developed
in Grunwell’s (1985) five category classification system the Differential Diagnosis system from a theoretical ba-
for children with phonological disorders (table 6). sis using broadly recognized subgroups, namely ‘phono-
Ingram (1997) also postulated a speculative and logical delay’ (Leonard 1973, Grunwell 1987, Ingram
tentative descriptive–linguistic typology of phonolog- 1976), ‘deviant’ development (Ingram 1976, Grunwell
ical impairment based on single-subject phonological 1985, Leahy and Dodd 1987), and inconsistent error
analyses. Ingram proposed four types of phonological patterns (Grunwell 1985), combined with her own clin-
impairment, which are summarized in table 7. ical experience. Table 8 provides a summary of the pro-
These descriptive–linguistic classification systems posed subgroups.
were the antithesis of the aetiological approach which
was still highly influential on clinical practice through-
out the 1960s, 1970s and beyond (Bowen 2009). A ma- Evidence for the Differential Diagnosis system
jor criticism of descriptive–linguistic classification sys- Over the last two decades, Dodd and colleagues have
tems is that emphasis is placed solely on the speech provided a range of empirical evidence to support the
output of the children, obscuring the influence of other validity and clinical utility of the classification model
non-language factors such as cognitive functioning, so- (Dodd 2011). Research has focused on demonstrating
cial and environmental factors (Kamhi 1989). Another that: (1) all children with SSD of unknown origin can
frequently cited criticism is that clinicians use phono- be classified by surface error patterns into the five pro-
logical process terminology as explanations rather than posed subgroups; (2) the subgroups have distinct deficits
the descriptions they were intended to be (Locke 1983). or ‘profiles’ that underlie children’s speech difficulties;
Finally, in terms of classifying all children with SSD, the (3) matching subgroups to intervention techniques re-
classification models do not explicitly recognize children sults in increased treatment efficacy; and (4) that non-
with purely articulation (phonetic) impairments or mo- English-speaking children with SSD of unknown origin
tor planning/programming-based speech impairments can be classified into the same proposed subgroups, and
resulting in an overlap of categories and diminished sub- with similar prevalence across a range of languages. Each
group uniqueness. Dodd’s (2005) differential diagnosis evidence type is reviewed briefly below.
of SSD model addresses these issues.
Differential Diagnosis system classification by surface error
Description of the Differential Diagnosis system patterns
Dodd (2005 proposed a classification model anchored Two studies investigated the potential of assigning chil-
in the descriptive linguistic approach that consists of dren with SSD to Dodd’s five subgroups based on the

Table 7. Summary of Ingram’s (1997) descriptive linguistic typology of phonological impairment

Type Description
Phonological delay Children show phonological patterns of younger, typically developing children, and
have vocabularies consistent with phonological level
Developmentally distinct phonology Children have acquired relatively large vocabularies but express the words with
patterns used in the very earliest stages of speech development
Socially influenced phonological patterns Children use uncommon phonological pattern due to an awareness of speech
difficulties and try their own extreme measures to improve
Supra-laryngeal developmental delays Children with advanced development of the voice feature relative to place distinctions,
e.g. a child develops /b/, /p/, /d/ before the more typical sequence /b/,/d/, /g/
32 R. Waring and R. Knight
Table 8. Dodd’s five subgroups

Type Subtype Features Percentage occurrence


Phonetic Articulation disorder Substitutions or distortions of the same sound in 12.5
isolation, words and sentences, during imitation,
elicitation and spontaneous speech tasks
Phonemic Phonological delay Presence of usual phonological error patterns that 57.5
are typical of younger children
Consistent atypical Consistent use of one or more unusual, 20.6
phonological disorder non-developmental error patterns such as
backing or initial consonant deletion. A child
may also display some developmental error
patterns that are delayed or age appropriate
Inconsistent phonological Variability/inconsistency in speech production, as 9.4
disorder indicated by multiple error forms for the same
lexical item while having no oro-motor
difficulties
Motor planning, Childhood apraxia of speech Multiple deficits involving phonological planning, <1
programming and (CAS) phonetic programming and motor programming
execution implementation

analysis of surface error patterns. Using identical group have difficulties at the cognitive–linguistic level, pos-
allocation rules, Dodd et al. (1989) and Broomfield and sibly with linguistic rule abstraction. Further studies
Dodd (2004b) assigned a combined total of 246 children by Dodd and McIntosh (2008), Crosbie et al. (2009),
whose primary difficulty was SSD of unknown origin and Dodd (2011) demonstrated that children with
into articulation disorder, phonological delay, consistent consistent atypical phonological disorder perform less
atypical phonological disorder or inconsistent phono- well than typically developing, phonologically delayed,
logical disorder subgroups based on error pattern analy- and inconsistent phonological disordered children on
sis. The two studies reported comparable incidence per- tasks of rule abstraction and cognitive flexibility. Finally,
centages. No children were diagnosed with childhood Bradford and Dodd (1994) demonstrated that it was
apraxia of speech (CAS). possible to differentiate children with inconsistent
phonological disorder from other children with SSD
using motor tasks.
Differential Diagnosis system underlying subgroup
It is our interpretation of the data reported herein
processing profiles that this is support for the distinct underlying deficits
Dodd and colleagues investigated the linguistic hypothesis of the proposed subgroups. However, a num-
knowledge (phonological legality, phonological aware- ber of methodological issues need to be considered. First,
ness), cognitive (executive function), output process- each study employed a separate group of subjects and
ing (phonological planning, phonetic planning), and no study investigated the phonological awareness skills,
motor-execution skills of children within the four major rule abstraction and flexibility, and motor skills in the
subgroups (i.e. excluding the CAS subgroup) to provide same groups of children. It is unclear if similar results
subgroup processing profiles. These studies compared would be replicated if the same children were measured
two, three or four subgroups, and typically developing on all profiling tasks. Second, vocabulary age was not
control groups. taken into account when comparing the SSD subgroups.
Dodd et al.’s (1989) and Holm et al.’s (2008) find- Vocabulary size has been implicated in contributing to
ings suggested differences in phonological awareness the variance in phonological awareness task performance
skills between the proposed subgroups. Results indi- (Preston and Edwards 2010). Dodd’s lack of informa-
cated that children with inconsistent phonological dis- tion on vocabulary age and expressive language skills
order have poor syllable segmentation skills, a prefer- makes it impossible to discount the role vocabulary and
ence for phonologically legal nonsense words, and intact language differences may have had on differing task per-
alliteration awareness. Children with consistent atyp- formance, especially phonological awareness tasks, be-
ical phonological disorder have no preference for le- tween the proposed subgroups. Third, Dodd’s classifica-
gal or illegal nonsense words, and the poorest rhyme tion system critically depends on the multisyllabic word
and alliteration awareness of the subgroups. Dodd and inconsistency task which involves repeating the same
colleagues argued that children with inconsistent phono- set of 25 words over three separate trials. The validity
logical disorder have a deficit in phonological assem- of this task has not been determined and other consis-
bly, while children with atypical phonological disorder tency measures, such as those described by Preston and
How should children with speech sound disorders be classified? 33
Koenig (2011), are under investigation. Fourth, Dodd origin. Again, all children were classified into one of
et al. (1989) employed a self-constructed legality judg- the four proposed subgroups. In a larger scale study,
ment task. No validity data are available for this task. Fox and Dodd (2001) classified 100 German-speaking
Replication, using an alternative legality judgment task, children with SSD into Dodd’s (2005) proposed sub-
such as the legal versus illegal words task (Stackhouse groups. The combined results reveal that all the chil-
et al. 2007), would make a useful comparison. Finally, dren could be classified and similar subgroup prevalence
Dodd’s profile studies would benefit from replicated prevailed.
studies, conducted by different research groups, using The cross-language findings support using Dodd’s
larger sample sizes. classification system regardless of the phonological sys-
tem being learnt. Further studies investigating the
cognitive–linguistic profiles of children with SSDs who
Differential Diagnosis system intervention studies
speak languages other than English are needed to de-
Dodd and Bradford (2000) described three case studies termine if the same proposed speech processing deficits
of children classified with consistent atypical phono- are identifiable in each of the subgroups. Such findings
logical disorder who received three therapy treat- would provide compelling support for Dodd’s classifi-
ments (phonological contrast, core vocabulary and cation system.
PROMPT). Crosbie et al. (2005) used a multiple base-
line design with alternating treatment to compare the
Differential Diagnosis system evaluation
effectiveness of phonological contrast therapy and core
vocabulary therapy with 18 children. Results of both Dodd’s classification system is a potentially clinically
studies indicated differential responses to treatment with useful classification tool. The system is theory driven,
children with inconsistent phonological disorder re- incorporating theories of normal (Macken and Ferguson
sponding best to core vocabulary treatment, and chil- 1983) and abnormal linguistic development (Grunwell
dren with consistent atypical phonological disorder re- 1985) along with more recent psycholinguistic theories
sponding best to phonological contrast therapy. The (Stackhouse and Wells 1997). The theoretical basis of
results should be treated cautiously, however, due to Dodd’s Differential Diagnosis provides strong construct
small sample sizes and the possible cumulative effects of validity. The classification system also seems to have high
treatment (Dodd and Bradford 2000). predictive validity. For instance, Dodd has constructed
Broomfield and Dodd (2011) conducted a random- speech sound profiles that summarize the major features
ized control trial to measure the effectiveness of speech of the subgroups, the impact of speech intelligibility
and language therapy for children with speech/language and recommendations for treatment. Coverage is also
impairment. Children with articulation disorder and/or adequate given that to date no child has been left un-
phonological impairment were included in the study. classified by the system.
Recognized treatments such as Metaphon (Howell and The categorization of children with SSD using
Dean 1991) and Core Vocabulary (Holm et al. 2005) Dodd’s system has become more feasible with the publi-
were matched to individual participants based on age cation of the Diagnostic Evaluation of Articulation and
and diagnostic profile. Results showed that children who Phonology (DEAP) (Dodd et al. 2002), which is a stan-
received targeted interventions showed significant im- dardized test based on Dodd’s theoretical model. The
provement over children who received no intervention. test appears time efficient with the inclusion of a screen-
These preliminary findings suggest that there may be ing task which directs the assessment and differential
merit in further investigating the matching of specific diagnosis process. Moreover, DEAP is readily accessible
treatments to SSD subgroups. to clinicians in Western countries. The clinical uptake
of DEAP provides evidence of some face validity (Joffe
and Pring 2008, Pring, personal correspondence, 2012).
Differential Diagnosis system cross-language studies
Investigations quantifying the adoption of Dodd’s Dif-
The concept of universality among children with dif- ferential Diagnosis classification system, as opposed to
ferent linguistic backgrounds was studied by Dodd and the use of individual subtests, are needed to establish face
associates. So and Dodd (1994) described the phono- validity. Moreover, evidence regarding the reliability of
logical systems of 17 monolingual Cantonese-speaking diagnosis between clinicians is also required. Presently
children with SSD aged between 3 years 6 months and evidence is cited about test–retest reliability and inter-
6 years 4 months. Results indicated that all the chil- rater reliability; however, how well all clinicians using
dren could be classified into the articulation disorder, the DEAP arrive at the same diagnosis is unknown.
phonological delay, consistent deviant disorder or in- Dodd and colleagues’ body of research provides the
consistent disorder subgroups. Zhu and Dodd (2000) groundwork for establishing the validity of the Differ-
classified 33 Mandarin speakers with SSD of unknown ential Diagnosis classification system. More evidence
34 R. Waring and R. Knight
regarding the sensitivity and specificity of the DEAP with planning speech output), (4) articulatory plan-
is required. Further investigations are needed into the ning (difficulty with sequencing speech sounds), and
underlying cognitive–linguistic profiles of the proposed (5) motor execution (impairment of motor execution
subgroups, of both English and non-English speakers, due to neurological dysfunction). Winitz’s model was
given that a central tenet of the classification system is promising because it provided categories of SSD. Un-
that speech errors are symptomatic of underlying sub- fortunately, the majority of children with SSD of un-
group specific processing deficits. Finally, further em- known origin fall into the phonological subgroup. Con-
pirical evidence supporting the division between incon- sequently, his classification is not sufficiently sensitive
sistent phonological disorder and childhood apraxia of to discriminate between the groups of children most
speech is needed, with emphasis on establishing the va- in need of classification, i.e. children with cognitive–
lidity of the DEAP speech consistency task. linguistic based SSD.
Grundy (1989) proposed a simple input, storage and
output model that divided SSD of unknown origin into
Processing approaches
an articulatory (impairment of phonetic production)
The psycholinguistic processing approach employs and a phonological component (a linguistic disorder).
models of speech processing in children to explain ‘how’ According to Grundy, phonological disorder arises from
speech impairment arises. Kamhi (1989) described the ‘the productive, or the perceptive, or the organizational
psycholinguistic processing approach as a bridge be- mechanisms of speech’ (p. 257). This account is inad-
tween aetiological classification and linguistic descrip- equate in terms of both classification of SSD and as a
tions. The central premise of the psycholinguistic pro- description of breakdown.
cessing approach is that children’s speech impairments The second type of psycholinguistic approach is the
are due to a breakdown in the speech processing chain. connectionist models. Connectionist models are com-
This breakdown may occur at one or more of the follow- puter driven and are currently not clinically usefully due
ing levels: (1) peripheral hearing; (2) auditory discrimi- to the time-intense nature of designing and running
nation of phonemic distinctions; (3) storing words accu- models with children’s speech data (Baker et al. 2001).
rately; (4) planning speech output; and/or (5) executing For a detailed description and discussion of connection-
speech. Psycholinguistic speech processing models vary ist models, see the tutorial by Baker et al. (2001).
considerably in their complexity; however, the applica-
tion to individuals is the same: a series of hypotheses are
developed and systematically tested to find where the Description of Stackhouse and Wells’s
breakdown(s) is occurring. Thus, the psycholinguistic
Psycholinguistic Framework
speech processing approach can be used with any child
regardless of whether or not there is a known aetiological Stackhouse and Wells’s (1997) Psycholinguistic Frame-
cause (e.g. cerebral palsy, Down syndrome, hearing loss, work is a box and arrow model which links speech pro-
SSD of unknown origin). cessing theory and clinical practice. The Stackhouse and
Wells Framework is driven by three central tenets. First,
typical speech development depends on a normally func-
Brief history of processing approaches
tioning speech processing system. Second, SSD result
Two broad types of psycholinguistic approaches have from a breakdown at one or more points in the speech
been proposed. The first type is pictures of boxes and processing system. Third, SSD can be remediated by
arrows, with the boxes representing levels of processing targeting the faults in the speech processing system.
and the arrows representing processing routes. Box and The framework is a developmental, linear model that
arrow models differ greatly in complexity with some includes input, representation and output abilities that
models (Smith 1973) having only one or two boxes purportedly underlie speech production, and five phases
between input and output, and others having multi- of development: (1) pre-lexical phase, (2) whole word
ple boxes and real-time and time-free processing arrows phase, (3) systematic simplification phase, (4) assembly
(Hewlitt 1990). phase, and (5) metaphonological phase.
Examples of early box and arrow models are Winitz The Stackhouse and Wells Framework was not in-
(1975), Menn (1978), Grundy (1989), and Hewlitt tended to be a classification system; rather, it was de-
(1990). Winitz (1975) described five levels of break- veloped to provide individual profiles of the underlying
down in the speech processing chain: (1) auditory input speech processing abilities and deficits of children with
(hearing, discrimination or impoverished environment), and without speech and/or reading impairments. The
(2) phonological (impairment in attention, reasoning, inclusion of developmental phases lends the approach
memory or motivation leading to problem abstracting to describing a child with an SSD by the phase level
phonological rules), (3) systematic phonetic (difficulty at which he/she is habituated. A child could receive a
How should children with speech sound disorders be classified? 35
number of descriptions over time if he/she were to ha- Psycholinguistic Framework treatment case studies
bituate at more than one level (Stackhouse and Wells
Numerous quasi-experimental studies (Bryan and
1997).
Howard 1992, Pascoe et al. 2005, 2006, Waters et al.
1998) and non-experimental case studies (Corrin 2001a,
Psycholinguistic Framework evidence 2001b, Nathan and Simpson 2001, Nathan et al. 2004,
Rees 2008, Stackhouse and Wells 1993, 1997, Stack-
Numerous studies have investigated the key hypotheses
house et al. 2006, Vance 1997, Waters 2001) have in-
that (1) children with SSD have specific deficits at one
vestigated the effectiveness of tailoring intervention to
or more points in the speech processing chain which dif-
match strength and/or deficit areas to improve speech
ferentiates them from typically developing children; and
output and/or literacy skills. The Katy single-subject
(2) targeting deficits leads to improvements in speech
study is an example of tailoring intervention (Pascoe
output.
et al. 2005). Katy presented at 6 years 5 months
with mild ataxic cerebral palsy, persistent unintelligi-
Specific deficits ble speech and a long history of speech therapy. An
initial assessment revealed a percentage consonants cor-
Stackhouse et al. (2002), conducted a longitudinal study
rect (PCC) of 22%, percentage vowels correct (PVC) of
of 47 children with SSD of unknown origin (boys = 31,
74.1%, percentage phonemes correct (PPC) of 41.9%
girls = 16). The study compared children with SSD of
and a connected speech PCC of 25%. Her speech con-
unknown origin with typically developing children to
sisted predominately of CV and CVCV syllable struc-
test whether children with SSD have a deficit at one or
tures. Psycholinguistic assessment revealed that Katy had
more points in the speech processing chain. Each child
difficulty with retrieving stored motor patterns, creation
with SSD was matched on the basis of chronological
of on-line motor programs and motor planning. Katy re-
age, gender and non-verbal IQ to a typically developing
ceived 30 h of treatment which involved three treatment
child from the same pool of nurseries/schools. Each pair
phases: (1) targeting a specific set of single words; (2)
was assessed on a range of speech processing tasks (pic-
targeting a wide range of single words; and (3) targeting
ture naming; real word repetition; non-word repetition;
words in connected speech (using controlled sentences).
speech rate; mispronunciation detection; auditory dis-
The post-intervention assessment revealed: (1) an ex-
crimination of words, non-words, same/different; rhyme
pansion of word-final phonetic inventory; (2) improved
production; rhyme detection) and non-verbal IQ, lan-
production of final consonants in single words; (3) an
guage, phonological awareness, and literacy tests at three
increase in PCC; and (4) following specific targeting of
age points (t1 = 4 years, t2 = 5 years and t3 = 6 years).
connected speech, a decrease in final consonant deletion
Typically developing children performed signifi-
in connected speech. Pascoe et al. (2006) presented a
cantly better than the children with SSD on the ma-
further five detailed tailored intervention case studies.
jority of speech, language and phonological awareness
Combined, the longitudinal, single-case design and
measures. The typically developing children also per-
case studies provide support for the Stackhouse and
formed better on literacy tasks at t2 and t3. The authors
Wells (1997) Psycholinguistic Framework. Specifically
reported that the children with SSD showed different
the studies highlight the complex nature of SSD, in-
profiles in performance across tasks, with some children
cluding how children with the same (aetiological) di-
showing age-appropriate skills on some tasks. The find-
agnosis can present with differing breakdown point(s)
ings demonstrated the heterogeneity of SSD and the
in the speech processing chain; and how breakdowns
possibility of processing strength and weakness profiles.
can be mapped to reflect habituation at varying levels of
Numerous profiles of children with SSD have been
proposed speech development. Additional and stronger
published which have demonstrated how children given
rated empirical evidence (i.e. single-case design rather
the same diagnostic label can perform markedly differ-
than case studies) is needed.
ent on speech processing tasks (Stackhouse et al. 2006,
Stackhouse and Snowling 1992, Stackhouse and Wells
1997). Stackhouse et al. (2007) provided the speech
Evaluation of the Psycholinguistic Framework
processing profiles of two 4-year-old children diagnosed
with ‘phonological delay’. Zara was found to have spe- The Stackhouse and Wells (1997) Psycholinguistic
cific speech output difficulties (i.e. difficulties with nam- Framework allows for the categorization of children with
ing, word and non-word repetition and self-monitoring) SSD of unknown origin into profiles using a psycholin-
with intact input skills, while Tom was found to guistic approach. The framework has high construct
have pervasive input and output difficulties, includ- validity. It is based on years of psycholinguistic and
ing discrimination, naming and word and non-word cognitive neuropsychological research (Stackhouse and
repetition. Wells 1997), including Waterson (1987) and Hewlitt
36 R. Waring and R. Knight
(1990). The framework does, however, have some into the eight putative SDCS subgroups, the classifi-
theoretical shortcomings. First, breakdown hypotheses cation system would become clinically useful. Further
are restricted to input and output mechanisms. It is pos- validation is required before the SDCS is used as a clin-
sible that deficits are arising from a more central level ical tool.
such as the learning of phonological constraints (Dodd Dodd’s Differential Diagnosis is a clinically feasible,
2005). Second, deficits in the speech processing model inclusive classification system that divides children with
are treated as a cause of the SSD. It is possible that SSD of unknown origin into discrete subgroups. More
the speech processing difficulties are a consequence or research is needed to profile the cognitive–linguistic dif-
co-morbid symptom of another underlying deficit, for ference between the subgroups. The validity of Dodd’s
example Zelazo and Muller (2002) and Dodd (2011) classification system would be strengthened by replica-
argued that higher order executive function deficits (e.g. tion studies, conducted by different research groups.
deficits in rule abstraction, flexibility) can negatively im- Stackhouse and Wells’s (1997) Psycholinguistic
pact on the speech processing chain. Framework demonstrates the complexity of SSD, and
The Stackhouse and Wells framework is an inclusive illuminates the speech processing differences between
diagnostic system designed to be used with all children children with the same purported aetiology. Perhaps
with SSD. It is sensitive to differences in speech process- when more data are collected, a trend in strengths
ing between children with and without SSD. Stackhouse and weakness between and within the phases will
and Wells argued that all children with SSD should be emerge.
regarded as having a unique pattern of strengths and
weaknesses. The uniqueness position impacts negatively
Comparing and contrasting the classification systems
on the predictive validity of the framework. If every child
is regarded as unique, it becomes difficult to predict how Superficially, the three classifications systems appear
a child will improve over time or respond to treatment. markedly different, yet a close inspection reveals numer-
The framework has high face validity, especially ous similarities. All three classification systems recognize
as the assessment procedures encourage an holistic three common subgroups: (1) an articulation-based sub-
approach, incorporating medical, linguistic, develop- group; (2) a motor planning/programming subgroup;
mental, educational and psycholinguistic perspectives. and (3) a phonological subgroup characterized primar-
Moreover, the framework’s developmental phases (i.e. ily by simplification processes. Moreover, the SDCS and
pre-lexical, whole word, systemic simplification, assem- the Stackhouse and Wells Framework recognize a sub-
bly and metaphonological) are clinically intuitive. The group with auditory-based input deficits, while the Dif-
clinical feasibility of the framework is potentially high ferential Diagnosis system and the Stackhouse and Wells
once therapists overcome their initial anxieties (Gard- Framework recognize children with inconsistency but
ner 2009) and become familiar with psycholinguistic- no oro-motor difficulties. How to differentiate the large
oriented tasks such as those detailed in the Compendium phonological-based subgroup still remains controversial.
of Auditory and Speech Tasks (Stackhouse et al. 2007). Further evidence is required to determine if there are
Finally, potential variations in diagnosis between clini- qualitative differences between children with phono-
cians seem possible, leading to reduced reliability of the logical delay, consistent typical and consistent atypi-
framework. cal phonological errors as proposed by the Differential
Diagnosis classification system, or whether the SDCS
Discussion speech-delay genetic, otitis media and psychosocial sub-
groups are valid. Alternatively, further research using the
Summary of the classification systems
Stackhouse and Wells Framework may find a trend in
Three classification systems were critiqued in detail in strengths and weakness amongst children.
this paper: Shriberg’s (2010) Speech Disorders Clas- All three classification systems are driven to identify
sification System (SDCS), Dodd’s (2005) Differential the basis of SSD of unknown origin. In each classifi-
Diagnosis system, and Stackhouse and Wells’s (1997) cation system, underlying cognitive–linguistic processes
Psycholinguistic Framework. An evaluation summary are identified as playing a role in SSD. The difference is
appears in table 9. that the SDCS proposes that genetic variations lead to
The SDCS is still primarily a research tool driven cognitive–linguistic changes for some of the subgroups,
by a search for genetic factors associated with speech while the Differential Diagnosis system and the Stack-
disorders. The value of the theoretical underpinnings of house and Wells Framework propose that cognitive–
the SDCS may not be fully apparent until the repercus- linguistic deficits are what differentiate children with
sions of genetic research impact upon speech pathology, SSD.
sometime in the future. If researchers are able to identify The three classification systems also share the same
specific markers that can be readily employed by clini- dual purposes: (1) to improve the efficacy of treatment
cians to classify children with SSD of unknown origin for children with SSD of unknown origin; and (2) to
How should children with speech sound disorders be classified? 37
Table 9. Summary of evidence for the three classification systems

Reliability Validity Coverage Feasibility Future requirements


Speech disorders High inter- and Atheoretical—starting from Potentially Currently research Further evidence on
classification intra-judge a position of pathology overlapping groups; only tool; clinical the exhaustiveness
system agreement reported rather than normality not all children can feasibility unknown of the classification
for narrow phonetic Supporting evidence from be classified into a Clinical value is system; matching
transcription and the identification of single group dependent on of subgroups to
prosody-voice diagnostic makers future intervention to
coding which is Evidence level: Multiple repercussions of determine if there is
used to describe quasi-experimental genetic research on a differentiated
and classify studies treatment of SSD treatment response
children with SSD would aid
(Shriberg et al. predictive validity
2010b) of SDCS
Differential Reliability improved Theoretically driven. All children can be Specific standardized Replicated studies,
diagnosis with publication of Supporting evidence diagnosed; test (DEAP) which conducted by
standardized tests from: possibility of guides assessment different research
(DEAP); high Classification by surface overlap between and differential groups, using larger
test–retest error patterns groups (i.e. diagnosis process; sample sizes
reliability and Evidence level: articulation and test widely available
inter-rater Non-experimental study consistent atypical to clinicians in
reliability on Subgroup profiling profiles phonological Western countries
quantitative test Evidence level: disorder; and
measures reported Quasi-experimental articulation and
in the DEAP studies phonological delay)
manual Intervention studies
Evidence level: random
control trial; quasi
experimental studies;
case studies
Cross-language studies
Evidence level:
Non-experimental study
Psycholinguistic Reduced reliability Theoretically driven. Inclusive—all Specific tasks Further empirical
framework due to potential Supporting evidence children can be published (some evidence from
variations in from: profiled for with normative single-case
diagnosis between Specific deficits and profile strengths and data) to match treatment design
clinicians and studies weaknesses; framework rather than case
variations in Evidence level: differentiates studies to provide
assessment tasks Quasi-experimental between typically stronger predictive
administered studies; case studies developing and validity
Intervention studies children with SSD;
Evidence level: Quasi each child regarded
experimental studies; as unique
non-experimental
studies; expert
committee
report/clinical
experience of respected
authorities
Note: Levels of evidence as utilized by Williams et al. (2010).

allow for future research into the basis of SSD. The ference in focus has influenced the types of empirical
SDCS is currently more research focused with emphasis studies conducted to date.
on identifying genetic variations related to speech and Finally, all three classification systems recognize the
language impairments rather than treatment focused. need for a broad-based assessment for children with
Conversely, the Differential Diagnosis and Stackhouse SSD which takes into account the strengths of the
and Wells Framework are more treatment focused and medical, linguistic, psycholinguistic, psychological and
regard the aim of diagnosis and classification as iden- educational perspectives. The difference rests in how
tifying processing strengths and weaknesses which can much weighting each classification system gives to po-
be exploited in intervention, and used to predict later tential diagnostic elements such as: underlying medical
speech and literacy difficulties. This fundamental dif- condition; family history; types of sound errors; speech
38 R. Waring and R. Knight
processing; and importance of memory, and higher ex- BOWEN, C., 2009, Children’s Speech Sound Disorders (Chichester:
ecutive functions. For example, the SDCS and the Dif- Wiley-Blackwell).
ferential Diagnosis system both give substantial weight BRADFORD, A. and DODD, B., 1994, The motor planning abilities
of phonologically disordered children. International Journal
to the importance of the nature of the speech sound of Language and Communication Disorders, 29(4), 349–369.
errors as indicators of subgroup while the Stackhouse BROOMFIELD, J. and DODD, B., 2004a, Children with speech
and Wells Framework proposes that sound error pat- and language disability: caseload characteristics. International
terns are not indicative of a specific breakdown, and Journal of Language and Communication Disability, 39, 303–
can arise from any point(s) in the speech processing 324.
BROOMFIELD, J. and DODD, B., 2004b, The nature of referred sub-
chain. types of primary speech disability. Child Language Teaching
and Therapy, 20, 135–151.
BROOMFIELD, J. and DODD, B., 2011, Is speech and language ther-
The future apy effective for children with speech/language impairment?
There is universal agreement that children with SSD A report of an RCT. International Journal of Language and
Communication Disorders, 46, 628–640.
of unknown origin are heterogeneous. An agreed-upon BRYAN, A. and HOWARD, D., 1992, Frozen phonology thawed: the
classification system is needed to facilitate communica- analysis and remediation of a developmental disorder of real
tion between professionals and to allow further testing word phonology. European Journal of Disorders of Communi-
of diagnostic and treatment hypotheses (Taylor 2011). cation, 23(3), 245–365.
The challenge ahead is to construct an inclusive, uni- CANTWELL, D. P. and BAKER, L., 1988, Issues in the classification of
child and adolescent psychopathology. Journal of the American
versally agreed-upon classification system that meets the Academy of Child Psychiatry, 27, 521–533.
needs of clinicians and researchers. At a minimum, the CORRIN, J., 2001a, From profile to programme: steps 1–2. In J. Stack-
resulting classification system will need: house and B. Wells (eds), Children’s Speech and Literacy Dif-
ficulties: Book 2. Identification and Intervention (Chichester:
r to classify all children with SSD of unknown ori- Wiley), pp. 96–132.
CORRIN, J., 2001b, From profile to programme: steps 3–6. In J.
gin into discrete subgroups;
r to have accurate, specific and sensitive diagnostic Stackhouse and B. Wells (eds), Children’s Speech and Literacy
Difficulties: Book 2. Identification and Intervention (Chich-
markers; ester: Wiley), pp. 133–163.
r to have universal applicability; CROSBIE, S., HOLM, A. and DODD, B., 2005, Interventions for
r to improve clinical management by directing children with severe speech disorder: a comparison of two
approaches. International Journal of Language and Communi-
treatment differentiation;
r to be feasibly implemented in the clinical setting; cation Disorders, 40, 467–492.
CROSBIE, S., HOLM, A. and DODD, B., 2009, Cognitive flexibility in
and children with and without speech disorder. Child Language
r to contribute towards an explanation of childhood Teaching and Therapy, 25, 250–270.
SSD. DODD, B., 2005, Differential Diagnosis and Treatment of Children
with Speech Disorder, 2nd edn (London: Whurr).
DODD, B., 2011, Differentiating speech delay from disorder. Does
Acknowledgements it matter? Topics in Language Disorders, 31(2), 96–111.
DODD, B. and BRADFORD, A., 2000, A comparison of three therapy
The authors thank Barbara Dodd for her detailed and insightful methods for children with different types of developmental
comments on earlier drafts of this paper. Declaration of interest: phonological disorder. International Journal of Language and
The authors report no conflicts of interest. The authors alone are Communication Disorders, 35, 189–209.
responsible for the content and writing of the paper. DODD, B., HUA, Z., CROSBIE, S, HOLM, A. and OZANNE, A., 2002,
Diagnostic Evaluation of Articulation and Phonology (London:
Psychological Corporation).
References DODD, B., LEAHY, J. and HAMBLY, G., 1989, Phonological disorders
in children: underlying cognitive deficits. British Journal of
AMERICAN PSYCHIATRIC ASSOCIATION, 2000, Diagnostic and Statis- Developmental Psychology, 7, 55–71.
tical Manual of Mental Disorders—Fourth Edition, Text Revi- DODD, B. and MCINTOSH, B., 2008, The input processing, cogni-
sion (DSM-IV-TR). (Washington, DC: American Psychiatric tive linguistic and oro-motor skills of children with speech
Publ.). difficulty. International Journal of Speech–Language Pathology,
AMERICAN SPEECH–LANGUAGE–HEARING ASSOCIATION (ASHA), 10, 169–178.
2000, Communication Facts (Rockville, MD: American FOX, A. V. and DODD, B., 2001, Phonologically disordered German-
Speech–Language–Hearing Association). speaking children. American Journal of Speech–Language
BAKER, E., 2006, Management of speech impairment in children: Pathology, 10, 291–307.
the journey so far and the road ahead. Advances in Speech– FOX, A. V., HOWARD, D. and DODD, B., 2002, Risk factors for speech
Language Pathology, 8(3), 156–163. disorders in children. International Journal of Language and
BAKER, E., CROOT, K., MCLEOD, S. and PAUL, R., 2001, Tutorial Pa- Communication Disorders, 37(2), 117–131.
per: Psycholinguistic models of speech development and their GARDNER, H., 2009, Psycholinguistic profiling and intervention. In
application to clinical practice. Journal of Speech, Language, C. Bowen (ed.), Children’s Speech Sound Disorders (Chichester:
and Hearing Research, 44, 685–702. Wiley-Blackwell), pp. 138–143.
How should children with speech sound disorders be classified? 39
GIERUT, J. A., 1998, Treatment efficacy: functional phonological MCCORMACK, J., MCLEOD, S., MCALLISTER, L. and HARRISON,
disorders in children. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing L. J., 2009, A systematic review of the association between
Research, 41, 85–100. childhood speech impairment and participation across the
GRUNDY, K., 1989, Developmental speech disorders. In K. Grundy lifespan. International Journal of Speech–Language Pathology,
(ed.), Linguistics in Clinical Practice (London: Academic Press, 11(2), 155–170.
pp. 255–289). MCLEOD, S. and HARRISON, L. J., 2009, Epidemiology of speech and
GRUNWELL, P., 1981, The Nature of Phonological Disability in Chil- language impairment in a nationally representative sample of
dren (London: Academic Press). 4- to 5-year-old children. Journal of Speech, Language, and
GRUNWELL, P., 1987, Clinical Phonology, 2nd edn (London: Croom- Hearing Research, 52, 1213–1229.
Helm). MENN, L., 1978, Phonological units in beginning speech. In A. Bell
GRUNWELL, P., 1985, Phonological Assessment of Children’s Speech and J. B. Hooper (eds), Syllables and Segments (New York,
(PACS) (Windsor: NFER-Nelson). NY: Elsevier-North Holland), pp. 157–172.
GRUNWELL, P., 1997, Developmental phonological disability: order NATHAN, L. and SIMPSON, S., 2001, Designing a literacy programme
in disorder. In B. W. Hodson and M. L. Edwards (eds), for a child with a history of speech difficulties. In J. Stackhouse
Perspectives in Applied Phonology (Gaithersburg, MD: Aspen), and B. Wells (eds), Children’s Speech and Literacy Difficulties:
pp. 61–104. Book 2. Identification and Intervention (Chichester: Wiley),
HEWLITT, N., 1990, Processes of development and production. In pp. 249–298.
P. Grunwell (ed.), Developmental Speech Disorders (London: NATHAN, L., STACKHOUSE, J., GOUNLANDRIS, N. and SNOWLING,
Whurr), pp. 15–38. M., 2004, The development of early literacy skills among
HOLM, A., CROSBIE, S. and DODD, B., 2005, Treating inconsistent children with speech difficulties: a test of the “critical age hy-
speech disorders. In B. Dodd (ed.), Differential Diagnosis and pothesis”. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research,
Treatment of Children with Speech Disorder, 2nd edn (London: 47, 377–391.
Whurr), pp. 182–201. NEWBURY, D. F. and MONACO, A. P., 2010, Genetic advances in the
HOLM, A., FARRIER, F. and DODD, B., 2008, Phonological aware- study of speech and language disorders. Neuron, 68, 309–320.
ness, reading accuracy and spelling ability of children with PASCOE, M., STACKHOUSE, J. and WELLS, B., 2005, Phonologi-
inconsistent phonological disorder. International Journal of cal therapy within a psycholinguistic framework—promoting
Language and Communication Disorders, 43(3), 300–322. change in a child with persisting speech difficulties. Inter-
HOWELL, J. and DEAN, E. C., 1991, Treating Phonological Disorders national Journal of Language and Communication Disorders,
in Children—Metaphon: Theory to Practice (London: Whurr). 40(2), 189–220.
INGRAM, D., 1976, Phonological Disability in Children (London: PASCOE, M., STACKHOUSE, J. and WELLS, B., 2006, Children’s Speech
Edward Arnold). and Literacy Difficulties: Book 3. Persisting Speech Difficulties
INGRAM, D., 1997, The categorization of phonological impairment. in Children (Wiley: Chichester).
In B. W. Hodson and M. L. Edwards (eds), Perspectives in PRESTON, J. and EDWARDS, M. L., 2010. Phonological awareness
Applied Phonology (Gaithersburg, MD: Aspen), pp. 19–41. and types of sound errors in preschoolers with speech sound
INGRAM, T. T. S., 1959, Specific developmental disorders of speech disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research,
in childhood. Brain, 82(3), 450–467. 53, 44–60.
INGRAM, T. T. S., 1972, Diagnosis, treatment and management of PRESTON, J. and KOENIG, L. L., 2011, Phonetic variability in residual
speech defects in childhood. Proceedings of the Royal Society of speech sound disorders; exploration of subtypes. Topics in
Medicine, 65, 404–409. Language Disorders, 31(2), 168–184.
JOFFE, V. and PRING, T., 2008, Children with phonological prob- REES, R., 2008, Deaf children’s acquisition of speech skills: a psy-
lems: a survey of clinical practice. International Journal of cholinguistic perspective through intervention. Unpublished
Language and Communication Disorders, 43, 154–165. doctoral thesis, University College London.
KAMHI, A. G., 1989, Developmental child language disorders. In M. RUSCELLO, D. M., 2008, Treating Articulation and Phonological Dis-
Lahey (ed.), Disorders of Communication: An Introduction to orders in Children (St Louis, MO: Elsevier).
Intervention (London: Taylor & Francis), pp. 69–103. SHRIBERG, L. D., 1994, Five subtypes of developmental phonological
LEAHY, J. and DODD, B., 1987, The development of disordered disorders. Clinics in Communication Disorders, 4, 38–53.
phonology: a case study. Language and Cognitive Processes, 2, SHRIBERG, L., 1997, Developmental phonological disorders: one or
115–132. many? In B. W. Hodson and M. L. Edwards (eds), Perspectives
LEONARD, L., 1973, Referential effects on articulatory learning. Lan- in Applied Phonology (Gaithersburg, MD: Aspen), pp. 105–
guage and Speech, 16, 44–56. 132.
LEWIS, B. A., 2010, Genetic influences on speech sound disorders. SHRIBERG, L. D., 2006, Research in idiopathic and symptomatic
In R. Paul and P. Flipson Jr (eds), Speech Sound Disorders in childhood apraxia of speech. Paper presented at the 5th In-
Children: In Honor of Lawrence D. Shriberg (San Diego, CA: ternational Conference of Speech Motor Control, Nijmegen,
Plural), pp. 51–70. the Netherlands, June 2006.
LOCKE, J. L., 1983, Clinical phonology: the explanation and treat- SHRIBERG, L. D., 2010, Childhood speech sound disorders: from
ment of speech sound disorders. Journal of Speech and Hearing postbehavorism to the postgenomic era. In R. Paul and P.
Disorders, 48, 339–341. Flipsen, Jr (eds), Speech Sound Disorders in Children—in
MACKEN, M. and FERGUSON, C., 1983, Cognitive aspects of phono- Honor of Lawrence D. Shriberg (San Diego, CA: Plural), pp.
logical development: model, evidence and issues. In K. Nel- 1–34.
son (ed.), Children’s Language (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum), pp. SHRIBERG, L. D., FOURAKIS, M., HALL, S. D., KARLSSON, H. B.,
255–282. LOHMEIER, H. L., MCSWEENEY, J., POTTER, N. L., SCHEER-
MCCAULEY, R., 2004, Speech sound disorders in children: descrip- COHEN, A. R., STRAND, E. A., TILKENS, C. M. and WILSON,
tion and classification. In R. Kent (ed.), The MIT Encyclopedia D. L., 2010a, Extensions to the speech disorders classification
of Communication Disorders (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), system (SDCS). Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 24(10),
pp. 218–219. 795–824.
40 R. Waring and R. Knight
SHRIBERG, L. D., FOURAKIS, M., HALL, S. D., KARLSSON, H. B., TYLER, A. A., 2010, Subgroups, comorbidity, and treatment impli-
LOHMEIR, H. L., MCSWEENEY, J., POTTER, N. L., SCHEER- cations. In R. Paul and P. Flipsen, Jr (eds), Speech Sound
COHEN, A. R., STRAND, E. A., TILKENS, C. M. and WILSON, Disorders in Children—in Honor of Lawrence D. Shriberg (San
D. L., 2010b, Perceptual and acoustic reliability estimates for Diego, CA: Plural), pp. 71–92.
the Speech Disorders Classification System (SDCS). Clinical VANCE, M., 1997, Developing phonological representations in a
Linguistics and Phonetics, 24(10), 825–846. child with an auditory processing deficit. In S. Chiat, J.
SHRIBERG, L. D., KENT, R. D., KARLSSON, H. B., MCSWEENY, J. L., Law and J. Marshall (eds), Language Disorders in Children
NADLER, C. J. and BROWN, R. L., 2003, A diagnostic marker and Adults: Psycholinguistic Approaches to Therapy (London:
for speech delay associated with otitis media with effusion: Whurr), pp. 17–41.
backing of obstruents. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 17, WATERS, D., 2001, Using input processing strengths to overcome
529–547. speech output difficulties. In J. Stackhouse and B. Wells (eds),
SHRIBERG, L. D. and KWIATKOWSKI, J., 1991, Developmental phono- Children’s Speech and Literacy Difficulties: Book 2. Identification
logical disorders 1: A clinical profile. Journal of Speech and and Intervention (Chichester: Wiley), pp. 164–203.
Hearing Research, 37, 1100–1126. WATERS, D., HAWKES, C. and BURNETT, E., 1998, Targeting speech
SMITH, N., 1973, The Acquisition of Phonology: A Case Study (Cam- processing strengths to facilitate pronunciation change. Inter-
bridge: Cambridge University Press). national Journal of Language and Communication Disorders,
SO, L. and DODD, B., 1994, Phonologically disordered Cantonese- 33(suppl.), 469–474.
speaking children. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 8, 235– WATERSON, N., 1987, Prosodic Phonology: The Theory and its Applica-
255. tion to Language Acquisition and Speech Processing (Newcastle
STACKHOUSE, J. and SNOWLING, M., 1992, Developmental verbal upon Tyne: Grevatt & Grevatt).
dyspraxia II: a developmental perspective on two case studies. WILLIAMS, A. L., MCLEOD, S. and MCCAULEY, R. J., 2010, Intro-
European Journal of Disorders of Communication, 27(1), 35– duction to interventions for speech sound disorders in chil-
54. dren. In A. L. Williams, S. McCloud and R. J. McCauley
STACKHOUSE, J., NATHAN, L., GOULANDRIS, N. and SNOWLING, M., (eds), Interventions for Speech Sound Disorders in Children
2002, The Relationship Between Speech Disorders and Literacy (Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.), pp. 1–27.
Problems: Identification of the At Risk Child. Report on a 4 WINITZ, H., 1975, From Syllable to Conversation (Baltimore, MD:
Year Longitudinal Study to Research and Development (London: University Park Press).
North Thames Regional Health Authority). WORLD HEALTH ORGANIZATION, 2001, ICF: International Classifi-
STACKHOUSE, J., PASCOE, M. and GARDNER, 2006, Intervention for cation of Functioning, Disability and Health (Geneva: World
a child with persisting speech and literacy difficulties: a psy- Health Organization).
cholinguistic approach. Advances in Speech–Language Pathol- WORLD HEALTH ORGANIZATION (WHO WORKGROUP FOR DEVEL-
ogy, 8(3), 231–244. OPMENT OF VERSION OF ICF FOR CHILDREN AND YOUTH),
STACKHOUSE, J., VANCE, M., PASCOE, M. and WELLS, B., 2007, 2007, International Classification of Functioning, Disability
Compendium of Auditory and Speech Tasks (Chichester: Wiley). and Health—Version for Children and Youth: ICF-CY (Geneva:
STACKHOUSE, J. and WELLS, B., 1993, Psycholinguistic assessment of World Health Organization).
developmental speech disorders. European Journal of Disorders ZELAZO, P. and MULLER, U., 2002, Executive function in typical
of Communication, 28, 331–348. and atypical development. In U. Goswami (ed.), Handbook
STACKHOUSE, J. and WELLS, B., 1997, Children’s Speech and Literacy of Childhood Cognitive Development (Oxford: Blackwell), pp.
Difficulties: A Psycholinguistic Framework (London: Whurr). 445–469.
TAYLOR, E., 2011, How to classify? Journal of the American As- ZHU, H. and DODD, B., 2000, Putonghua (modern standard
sociation of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 50(2), 103– Chinese)-speaking children with speech disorder. Clinical
105. Linguistics and Phonetics, 14, 165–191.

Вам также может понравиться