Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 11

Melodic accent : experiments and a tentative model

Thomassen, J.M.

Published in:
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America

DOI:
10.1121/1.387814

Published: 01/01/1982

Document Version
Publisher’s PDF, also known as Version of Record (includes final page, issue and volume numbers)

Please check the document version of this publication:

• A submitted manuscript is the author's version of the article upon submission and before peer-review. There can be important differences
between the submitted version and the official published version of record. People interested in the research are advised to contact the
author for the final version of the publication, or visit the DOI to the publisher's website.
• The final author version and the galley proof are versions of the publication after peer review.
• The final published version features the final layout of the paper including the volume, issue and page numbers.
Link to publication

Citation for published version (APA):


Thomassen, J. M. (1982). Melodic accent : experiments and a tentative model. Journal of the Acoustical Society
of America, 71(6), 1596-1603. DOI: 10.1121/1.387814

General rights
Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the public portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners
and it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights.

• Users may download and print one copy of any publication from the public portal for the purpose of private study or research.
• You may not further distribute the material or use it for any profit-making activity or commercial gain
• You may freely distribute the URL identifying the publication in the public portal ?
Take down policy
If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately
and investigate your claim.

Download date: 20. May. 2018


Melodic accent: Experiments and a tentative model
Joseph M. Thomassen
Institutefor Perception
Research,P.O. Box 513, 5600MB Eindhoven,TheNetherlands
{Received6 May 1980;acceptedfor publication24 January1982)

The perceptionof accentin tone sequences is a constructiveprocessin which physicalcuesare matched


againstanticipatedaccents. The anticipationof the observer canexperimentally be controlledby embedding
the shorttonesequence to be investigated in a contextwith a meter:methodof controlledanticipation.An
investigation of melodicaccentuation, resultingfrom the succession of frequencyintervals,revealedthat in
principleeverychangeof frequencylevelbetweentwo successive tonescanbe interpretedasaccentuation of
the terminaltoneof the change.The melodiccontourseemsto be mostimportant.The first of two intervalsin
oppositedirectionsoperatesas the strongestaccentuation,whereastwo intervalsin the samedirectionare
equallyeffective.The effectof relativemagnitudeis lesspronounced. Only in the caseof clearlydiverging
relativemagnitudes the largestintervalis the mostpowerful,particularlywhenthe intervalsare in the same
direction.The advantage of risesoverfallsis almostnegligible.The short-terminfluenceof physicalfactorson
momentaryaccentperception allowsfor a descriptionin termsof a "memorywindow"slidingalongthe tone
sequence. At eachmomentthe frequencies within the windowprovidethe physicalcuefor accentthat hasto
be matchedagainstanticipation.If the spanof the windowis minimal,i.e., threetones,accentperceptionin
sequences of fourtones,embedded according to themethodof controlledanticipation, hasbeenaccounted for
fairly well,thecorrelationcoefficient betweenpredictions andoutcomes being0.76.

PACS numbers: 43.75.Bc, 43.66.Mk

INTRODUCTION ception of "melodic accents." Melodic accent is the


main subject of the present investigation.
Listening to music is, in a way, trying to organize
There is some controversy about melodic accent in
the incoming stream of sounds. The tendency to organ-
the early literature in experimental psychology. Meu-
ize incoming information manifests itself, in particular,
mann,a referring to music, and Squire,a writing on
when the listener imposes an organization upon a stimulus
speech, thought that pitch differences could create
lacking objective indicators of an organization, e.g., the
accents in the same way as differences in loudness do.
perception of "subjective rhythm" in the tick of a clock.
Woodrow4 has tried to check this claim experimentally
From a complex rhythmic percept usually two inter-
and he concludedthat "pitch differences do not deter-
woven aspects are derived: accent and grouping. In
mine the rhythm at all." His approach, however, was
the example of subjective rhythm, perfectly equal and
grouping oriented and based on a tradeoff between
equidistant ticks (or tones) are subjectively arranged in
melodic and temporal factors; the outcomes of his ex-
groups and often the first element in a group is per-
periments reflect that in his setup the temporal factor
ceived as an accent. However, this is not a general
overruled the melodic factor. Ehrlich et al. s did find
rule, there is no fixed relation between accent and
group-determining effects of pitch differences. They
group beginning (see, for example, the analyses of
did not report any accentuating effects of pitch differ-
Cooper and MeyerS). Therefore it is useful to study
ences: The tendency of their subjects to tap louder on
tone sequences from an accent point-of view.
certain tones was not interpreted as reproduction of
Various physical factors can accomplish accentuation perceived accents. Thereafter the problem seems to
independently or in interaction. Dynamic accentua- have drawn little attention, except with Royer and
tions caused by momentary increases in sound level Garner. 6 They hypothesized that group beginnings
play an important role in music but accents can occur are always accents and subsequently they and their
without sound level variation; for instance in music followers turned their attention to grouping, abandon-
played on a harpsichord accentuations appear to result ing the accent concept.
mainly from temporal differentiation. (Soundlevel here
The generarive theory of melody developed by Sund-
is the quantitative measure that can be obtained with the
berg and Lindblom ? is based on Chomsky's linguistic
standard sound level meter set for A-frequency weight-
principles. Their approach consists in deriving a
ing and fast exponential time averaging.) It is also
"prominence contour" for the melody to be constructed
clear, however, that accents can occur without either
and relating timing, harmonic progression, and pitches
temporal or'dynamicfactors. A computer-controlled to the position in the prominence contour. Unfortunate-
synthesizer offers the possibility to keep the temporal
ly the pitch rules for melody generation only apply if the
and dynamic factors under strict control; by equalizing
underlying harmonies are known; in the case of unac-
all tone durations and time intervals between onsets an
companied melodies, just a few rudimentary rules are
isochronous tone sequence is obtained in which accents
given concerning the tonality defining function of first
can still be perceived. These are partly "harmonic"
and last tones and the principle of proximity. No indi-
in nature, due to the succession of complex tones with
cations are given about realizing a prominence contour
different spectral envelopes, and partly "melodic,"
by operations in the pitch domain.
due to the pitch sensation of the tones. When sinusoi-
dal tones are used, the only accentuating factor left is The principle of proximity has recently been investi-
the succession of frequency intervals leading to the per- gated as one of the factors determining the coherence

1596 J. Acoust.Soc.Am. 71(6), June 1982 0001-4966/82/061596-10500.80 (D 1982 AcousticalSocietyof America 1596

Downloaded 07 Aug 2012 to 131.155.151.138. Redistribution subject to ASA license or copyright; see http://asadl.org/journals/doc/ASALIB-home/info/terms.jsp
of a melody. The results of Bregman and Dannenbring useful to consider melodic accent in short tone sequen-
(see for instanceBregman8) and Van Noorden9 suggest ces (motifs) first, and then having established a rela-
indirectly that melodic accent (implying a note standing tionship between melodic structure and accent percep-
out in a sense) and coherence are related, though the tion, to proceed to longer tone sequences. This aim '
nature of this relationship is as yet unclear. It is pos- and the following observations led to the particular
sible, for example, that accentuated tones are heard as method of measurement used in the present experi-
accents, that is to say as part of the melody, as long ments.
as coherence is possible, whereas in the case of fission
First and last tones of a melodic sequence derive
they become conspicuoustones outside the melody (pos-
accent from their very positions, as observed earlier
sibly building a second melody).
by Oftmann.•ø In short sequencesthe first tone is
In the musicological literature melodic accent re- mostly the strongest, whereas with increasing length of
ceives hardly any more attention than in psychological the melody the last tone becomes more important. Un-
literature. For Western traditional music, a fully elab- published experiments with short tone sequences indeed
orated theory of harmony exists but there have been yielded a preference for the first tone.•
only a few attempts toward a systematic approach to
melody. The available textbooks mostly bear on ton- In order to get rid of the effect of this preference it is
ality, a concept closely related to harmony. Other as- necessary to embed the motif to be investigated in a
pects of melody are usually illustrated by giving ex- longer sequenceof tones (context), the melodic struc-
ture of which should have no influence on the accent
amples without arriving at clear concepts. An exception
in this respect is Oftmann, •ø who gives some intuitive perception within the motif. Melodic neutrality of the
rules for melodic accent, describes grouping, and indi- context was achieved by making all frequencies before
cates the conditions for the perception of a string of and after the motif equal to the frequencies of the first
tones as a coherent melody. Although Smits van Waes- and last motif tones, respectively. However, in longer
berghen claims to treat melody irrespective of har- tone sequences, a more general anticipation on the part
mony, much of his theory in essence rests on contrast of the observer comes into play.
of implied harmonies. He discusses examples of
Accents perceived early in the tone sequence are con-
melodic accentuation but as regards melodic accent
sidered by the observer to indicate the accent structure
this author does not develop a consistent concept.
of the whole sequence; he usually expects a meter, i.e.,
For our purposes, the problem can now be formally a periodic accent structure. Once such accent struc-
stated and defined as follows. Accent is to be con- ture is established, it tends to be continued in the mind
sidered as a concept in the perceptual domain that can of the listener. We can regard this continuation as an
be described without making use of physical properties anticipation to hear certain accent patterns. This antici-
of the tone sequence: When listening to a sequence of pation may be either confirmed or contradicted by the
tones, some tones are perceived to be more prominent current accentuation. Because of the subtleness of
than others and are said to have accent. It will be use- melodic accent and the fact that a certain amount of
ful to introduceseparateterms in the physicaldomain. anticipation seems always to be present--even a purely
The term accentuation is used to indicate the physical subjective meter may be responsible for anticipation--
cue that may elicit the impression of an accent. Re- it is necessary to control the observer's anticipation
stricting ourselves to pure tones, three physical pro- in a way that enables us to investigate the influence of
perties stand out. physical factors in accent perception. This controlled
anticipation can be obtained by means of dynamic ac-
(1) A tone that has a higher sound level than its neigh-
centuation, i.e., by increasing the sound level of cer-
bors is said to have dynamic accentuation.
tain tones. We postulate that in simple tone sequences
(2) Temporalaccentuation
resultsfrom oneor more a direct correspondence exists between dynamic accen-
operations in the time domain (e.g., a delayed onset of tuation and perceived accent. When a dynamic accentu-
tone) that lead to the perception of accent. ation is applied to the context, a simple and clear met-
ric accent structure can be elicited. To establish a
(3) Melodic accentuation is the accentuation given by fixed influence from first and last tones it seemed use-
the succession of frequency intervals of the sequence.
ful to have the first and last tones of the sequence ac-
In the present paper we are particularly interested in
melodic accentuation.
centuated. It appeared possible to establish a meter
subjectively, with as few as two accentuations preced-
The first problem we encountered was to find a ing the motif, the distance between these accentua-
reliable and efficient method of measuring melodic tions defining the period. It was decided to make use
accent. By searching for an operational definition of of a robust pattern of four accentuations preceding the
accent that does justice to the common notion, we motif and two accentuations afterwards. This last mea-
arrived at a measurement method which is presented sure enabled the observer to check whether he had been
below. able to continue the meter through the motif. For dy-
namic accentuation a 4-dB increase in sound level
I. METHOD OF CONTROLLED ANTICIPATION proved to be adequate. To control the influence of
temporal factors the sequences were all made isochron-
A long melody often has a complicated structure which ous, i.e., a fixed onset-onset time was used and a
hamperssystematicinvestigation.•herefore it seemed fixed tone duration.

1597 J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 71, No. 6, June 1982 JosephM. Thomassen: Melodic accent 1597

Downloaded 07 Aug 2012 to 131.155.151.138. Redistribution subject to ASA license or copyright; see http://asadl.org/journals/doc/ASALIB-home/info/terms.jsp
Institute and six music students) participated in the ex-
periment. The results indicated that sequences in
which accent (accentuation) and anticipation coincided
were indeed judged as being the most regular in 95% of
the 360 (= 10x 30 +6 x 10) trials.
(1)
The method was then applied to motifs with melodic
accentuation only. Again there was a fair degree of
(2) agreement among the subjects (eight members of the
Institute). In 87% of the 1152 comparisons the tone se-
quence in which anticipation coincided with the terminal
(3) tone of a single change of frequency level was preferred
to the sequence in which anticipation preceded this
o i •
change. Most subjects had participated in the previous
TIME(S)
experiment in which coincidence of dynamic accentua-
FIG. 1. The stimuli are all tsochronous sequences with fixed tion and anticipation was shown to result in judgments of
tone duration (100 ms). Frequency levels, in semitones, of regularity. In the present experiment, the mechanisms
the pure tones are shown relative to the starting tone (1 kHz). of melodic accentuation were unknown at the outset, but
The motif (a peak of four semitones) is indicated by an acco- it is reasonable to assume that these subjects again
lade. In the upper part of the figure the relative amplitude en- judged the sequences in which anticipation and accentua-
velope is represented. The rise and fall times of the trape-
tion coincided as the most regular. This implies that a
zoidal envelope are 10 ms. Dynamic accentuation is achieved
by an increase in sound level of 4 dB. The anticipation induced change of frequency level might be interpreted as melo-
by the meter is indicated by the shaded area. In sequences 1, dic accentuation. This was further investigated in the
2, and 3 anticipation occurs for tones 1, 2, and 3 of the motif, main experiments on melodic accentuation which will be
respectively. described next. In these experiments the "method of
controlled anticipation" was used with confidence be-
cause of the uniform behavior of the subjects.
We thus arrived at tone sequences of the form illus-
trated in Fig. 1. As an example, three different tone II. EXPERIMENT I: THREE-TONE MOTIFS
sequences are presented that can be obtained with a A. Procedure
three-tone motif, the second tone of which is four semi-
tones higher in frequency than the other two. The Tone sequences were presented in pairs, each pair
starting tone being fixed in frequency at 1 kHz, the fre- counting as one trialø All tone sequences were isochron-
quency intervals between the successive motif tones ous; there was a fixed time interval (216 ms) between
determine the whole course of frequency. the onsets of successive tones, and all tone durations
were equal (100 ms).
Henceforth we will indicate motifs by this succession
of intervals, giving their magnitudes in semitones and The sinusoidal tones had a trapezoidal amplitude
denotingupward/downwarddirection by plus/minus envelope with 10-ms rise and fall times. The sound
signs. For this three-tone motif the code reads (+4-4). level of a tone was equal either to a reference level
The period of the meter is chosen according to the (unaccentuated tones) or to a level 4 dB higher than the
length of the motif, leading here to a ternary meter. reference level (dynamically accentuatedtones). The
Mental continuation of the meter induces anticipation of tone sequences contained a periodic accent structure,
an accent on one of the tones of the motif (shaded). By starting with the first and finishing with the last (19th)
displacing the motif with respect to the meter it is pos- tone, the period being three tones (see Fig. 1).
sible to induce this anticipation for the 1st, 2nd, or 3rd
Each trial was preceded by a short attention signal.
tone of the motif. This results in the three tone se-
The onset-onset time between attention signal and
quences (1, 2, and 3) shown in Fig. 1.
stimulus was equal to one period of the meter, as was
In each case, the physical parameters of the motif the onset-onset time between last tone of the first se-
tones may either confirm or run counter to the anticipa- quence and first tone of the second sequence. Following
tion. Presumably this leads to either regular or ir- a stimulus a response time of at least 2 s preceded the
regular rhythm (meter), i.e., continuedor disturbed attention signal of the next stimulus.
periodicity of the accent structure. Subjects can then be
The three-tone motifs that were investigated and the
asked to indicate this regularity by comparing pairs of
order and mode of presentation of the stimuli differed
tone sequences or scaling separate sequences. The
somewhat for the two groups of subjects.
sequence in which there is coincidence of accentuation
and anticipation is expected to be judged as the most
regular. I. Group A

The direct correspondencebetweendynamic accentua- The main group of subjects consisted of members of
tion and accent was exploited to test this expectation; the Institute, including the author. The subjects all had
instead of a motif with frequency differences, we kept normal hearing andwere experienced experimental sub-
all frequencies equal and used a motif with sound level jects, though not selected on the basis of musical cap-
differences only. Sixteen subjects (ten members of the abilities.

1598 J. Acoust.Soc.Am., Vol. 71, No. 6, June 1982 JosephM. Thomassen:Melodicaccent 1598

Downloaded 07 Aug 2012 to 131.155.151.138. Redistribution subject to ASA license or copyright; see http://asadl.org/journals/doc/ASALIB-home/info/terms.jsp
There were five successive sessions. Motifs with
frequency intervals of magnitudes, 4, 8, and I semi-
tone(s), were presented in sessions 1, 2, and 3,
respectively. Within each session all possible combin-
COMPUTER
• FREQUENCY SYNTHESIZER

ations of intervals with the given magnitude were em-


ployed. For example, session I contained the eight
motifs (+40), (-40), (0+4), (0-4), (+4-4), (-4+4), ..rATTENUATOR
(+4 +4), and (-4-4). Thereafter motifs containing two
intervals of unequal magnitude (one and eight semi-
tones) were presented, the two intervals being in op-
posite directions in session 4, or in the same direction
in session 5. In addition, the motifs with the same pat-
ENVELOPE__•--
GENERATOR
GATE
--•-••ATTE
tern of intervals from session I were presented again FIG. 2. Block diagram of the equipment.
in sessions 4 and 5. This served as a check because
the composition of the subject group did not remain
The material was divided into blocks, two practice
constant, although at least 50% of the subjects had
blocks (block length 6) and six experimental blocks
always participated in earlier sessions.
(block length 10). The six subjects were tested at the
For each motif the three possible tone sequences same time in a quiet classroom. The reference sound
(anticipation occurring for 1st, 2nd, and 3rd motif level was adjusted to be acceptable to every subject.
tones, respectively) resulted in six pairs that were
presented three times each. The stimuli were arranged
B. Apparatus
in such a way that the succession of various motifs and The generation of the stimulus material was con-
various anticipations made an impression of random- trolled by a P9202 minicomputer, connected to a digi-
ness. The material was divided into blocks; the block tal tone generator (HP 3320 B). A modular interface
length was eight trials in sessions 1 to 3, and six trials (MARIE, cf. Moohenand de Jong•) made computer con-
in sessions 4 and 5. Every session was preceded by trol of the signal generation and shaping possible. The
two practice blocks. The blocks were separated by an relative sound level of the tones was adjusted by a dig-
extra pause of 3 s and every block was preceded by an itally controlled attenuator before the tones were fed
extra attention signal. Subjects had to make a forced into a Vario-S gate, an envelope generator determining
choice between the two tone sequences. The instruc- the trapezoidal amplitude envelope, all modules de-
tions read: "In this experiment you will have to com- veloped and made at the Institute (Fig. 2).
pare pairs of tone sequences. You have to indicate
The stimuli were recorded on tape with a Revox A77
which sequence (the 1st or 2nd) gives rhythmically
tape recorder and were presented diotically to the sub-
(metrically) the most regular impression. You have
jects in a sound-insulated booth (Amplifon Type G) or in
to respond, even if in doubt."
a quiet room. Subjects wore headphones (Sennheiser
The tape containing the stimuli was prepared with HD424) connected to the recorder via a manually adjust-
some automatic stops dividing an experimental session able attenuator (General Radio Type 1450-TA).
into three equal parts of about 10 min each. This gave
C. Results
the subjects the choice to pause or to proceed at will.
The subjects did the experiments one at a time in a I. Presentation of the data
sound-insulated booth. The reference sound level was
For each pair comparison, the difference in votes for
adjusted to 55 dB SL (sensation level).
the two sequences was summed over the subjects. This
2. Group B was done separately for groups A and B, numbers for
the second group subsequently being placed between
In order to test whether musically experienced per- square brackets. If a two-tailed Sign Test for N--24 (8
sons would produce different (or more pronounced) re- subjectsx 3 repetitions) IN =60 (6 subjectsx 10 repetit-
suits, part of the experiment was repeated with a group ions)] showsthat the difference in votes is significant
of music students from the Universit7 of Utrecht. They (P< 0.05) it is said that there is a consensusamong the
were paid for their services. Seven different motifs subjects. In the case of P< 0.25 we speak of a trend.
were treated, one motif with sound level differences On the whole there was consensusin 74.6%[69%]of the
only and six motifs with frequency differences only. 216 [42] pair comparisons,anda trend in 6.9%[17%]of
All possible motifs with intervals of four semitones the cases. The rest of the cases, 18.5%[15%], were
were considered except the motifs (0 +4), (0-4). mainly comparisons of equally regular or irregular se-
quences. There was no difference between experiment-
Motifs were treated one at a time. The six possible
al sessions. Subjects that deviated from the consensus
pairs were presented ten times in all, stimuli being
or the trend did not do so systematically. This degree
preceded by 12 pairs as practice (every possible stimu-
of agreement between subjects justifies the treatment of
lus twice). The stimuli were arranged in such a way
the results for all subjects in a group together.
as to give an impression of randomness. For each of
the six melodic motifs a different permutation of the Response consistency was defined by comparing the
pairs was substituted to exclude the possibility that responses of a given subject to the two tone sequence
the subjects would become familiar with the scheme. pairs that differed only in order of presentation. If the

1599 J. Acoust.
Soc.Am.,Vol. 71, No.6, June1982 Joseph
M. Thomassen:
Melodicaccent 1599

Downloaded 07 Aug 2012 to 131.155.151.138. Redistribution subject to ASA license or copyright; see http://asadl.org/journals/doc/ASALIB-home/info/terms.jsp
responses favored the same sequence, regardless of
order, this was counted as a consistent case. The per- 1 2 3 (• • 1:2--,-lO:9O
2:3--,-81:19
centage of cases that were consistent was calculated 3:1--,-50:50
over all sequences and sessionsø (-8 o ) 8)

If there was a preference for the first or the second


sequence in a pair there would be an inconsistency be-
tween the results for the two pairs with the same se-
quences in different order. Consistency increased with
the agreementbetweensubjects:87%[94%]of the con-
sensuscases were consistent, whereas 74% [88%]of the
trend casesand 67%[92%]of the remaining caseswere
consistent. In all, 83%[92%] of the responseswere
consistent.
1 3
We can obtain an impression about a possible pre-
ference P•. for the second sequence in a pair X-- Y by FIG. 3. Example of the graphical representation of the results.
subtracting the number of votes for a sequence in first
position V•(X) from the number of votes for that same
sequencein secondposition V•.(X). Dividing this differ-
to the reciprocal of the ratio of votes for sequences
ence by twice the total number of votes per pair, V, we
obtain the relative number of votes that shifts from first i and 2 in pair comparison 1--2. By means of the
dashed lines, it can be seen whether a pair compari-
to second sequence of a pair as a consequence of a pre-
son revealed a significant preference: the dashed lines
ferencefor the secondsequence:P•.=[V•.(X)- V•(X)]/
2V. mark the outcomes 32:16 and 16:32, which are signifi-
cant according to the Sign Test (N =48).
Adding the results for all pairs, we found a preference
Note that the preference for 2 in comparison 1--2
of only P• = 1.3% [P2= -0.8%]. A chi-squaretest leadsto
was significant. Similar points are constructed for
the same conclusion: There were 2650 [1239]votes for
the pairs 3-- 1 and 2--3. The constructed points are
the secondsequencecomparedwith 2534 [1281] votes
connected with the opposite corners. The connecting
for the first sequence; this is not a significant deviation
lines intersect at the triangle's center of gravity in the
of an evendistribution,X•'=2.60 [=0.7]< 5.02 (•p=1).
There was no significant preference for the first or the
case of perfectly "fitting" pair comparisons. If the
three intersecting points do not coincide, a small tri-
second sequence in a pair. Moreover, the number of
angle results with an area that indicates the divergence
pairs with the same sequences in different order as
of the pair comparisons. A center of gravity is then ob-
balanced, so that the results for mutually reversed
pairs were added. For the motifs that were presented tained by determining the geometrical center of gravity
of this small triangle, as is demonstrated in Fig. 3. So,
to both group A and B the outcome of the pair compari-
in this example the center of gravity is located nearest
sons for the two groups were added, because there was
to tone 2, indicating that this tone is accentuated rela-
hardly any difference between the responses of the
tive to tones 1 and 3.
musically trained and untrained subjects. Only the
motif (+4 +4) showed a clear difference between the
3. Outcomesof the pa/r cornpar/sons
groups.
The data are presented in terms of changes in fre-
2. Interpretation of the graphs
quency level. A change of frequency level is character-
By adding the results for mutually reversed pairs ized by magnitude and direction; the sequence of direc-
the number of pair comparisons for each motif was re- tions (signs) of successive frequency intervals defines
duced to three. The results of these three pairs can a melodic contour. The results are not arranged per
be given a combined meaning by a graphical presenta- session, but motifs with similar melodic contours are
tion in an equilateral triangle as shown in Fig. 3. The grouped together, mutually inverse motifs (motifs with
motif, in this example a descending interval of eight the signs of all frequency intervals inverted relative to
semitones, followed by a frequency repetition, is de- each other) being denoted by similar symbols.
noted by a code: (-8 0). The corners of the triangle
The results for the motifs with a single change of fre-
represent the three tone sequences with anticipation
quency level are plotted in Fig. 4(a) (motifs with the
of motif tones 1, 2, and 3.
change between first and second tone) and Fig. 4(b)
If we assume that "masses" proportional to the ac- (motifs with the change between second and third tone).
centuations of motif tones 1, 2, and 3 are located in the In all cases there was a significant preference for the
corners we can then determine a center of gravity for terminal tone of the frequency change. The data points
the triangle. This center of gravity describes in a for mutually inverse motifs are close together, those
compact way the relative accentuations of the tones. for the frequency rises being located somewhat more to
Each experimental pair comparison provides us with the corner in Fig. 4(a), whereas the same holds for
the ratio of two of the three "masses." For instance, frequency falls in Fig. 4(b). Note that in both Figs. 4(a)
the point P divides the line segment 12 into two seg- and (b) the results for frequency level changesof four
ments 1P and P2, the ratio of the lengths being equal semitones are the most significant.

1600 J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 71, No. 6, June 1982 JosephM. Thomassen: Melodic accent 1600

Downloaded 07 Aug 2012 to 131.155.151.138. Redistribution subject to ASA license or copyright; see http://asadl.org/journals/doc/ASALIB-home/info/terms.jsp
2 2 level between two successive tones has to be considered
z• +1 0 /• ß-1 0 z• 0+1 //• ß 0-1
as an accentuation of the terminal tone of the change.
• •4 0 /''• ß-40
.- 0 0+4 / • ' 0-4
In the absence of surrounding (i.e., competing) fre-
quency level changes, the change operates indeed as
strong accentuation, almost independently of its direc-
tion and magnitude (note however, the significance of
the results for intervals of four semitones as compared
with the intervals of one and eight semitones). How-
I (a) 3 I (b) 3 ever, the terminal tone of a change may be the initial
tone of the next change in the case of two successive
2 2
changes. If both changes were independent accentua-
z• +1-1 /• ß -1+1 z• +1+1 /• ß -1-1 tions, we would expect the number of votes for the
0 +4-4 / •.• *-4+4 0 +4+4 / • *-4-4 second and third tone to be equal, but this is not often
•+8-8 / \k ß-8+8 •+8+s / •, ß-8-8
the case as can be seen in Figs. 4(c) and (d). It is ob-
•*8-1 /' ;ll<•>.•,
*-8*1 •*8*1 ,•' -'...•*;8-1
vious that the melodic contour of motifs with frequency
level changes of equal magnitude largely determines dif-
ferences in accentuation. The accentuation of the first
of two successive changes in opposite directions is the
strongest except when the magnitude of the change is
I (c) 3 I (d) 3 one semitone: in that case the accentuations are equal.
Two changes in the same direction yield equally strong
accentuations, except again when the magnitude of the
FIG. 4. (a), (b) Motifs with a single frequency level change
at the beginning/end; (c) motifs with two frequency level changes is one semitone, which makes the second ac-
changesin oppositedirections; {d) motifs with two frequency centuation the strongest. Considering next the pair
level changes in the same direction. For each motif, denoted comparison 2•-3 for all three-tone motifs with two
by its code, a center of gravity is constructed as in Fig. 3. frequency changes of unequal magnitude, we find the
The closer this point lies toward a corner, the stronger the total number of votes for the terminal tone of frequency
corresponding motif tone is accentuated. The total number level changes of one and eight semitones to be 136 and
of votes per pair comparison: N = 156 for (+4 -4), (-4 +4),
248, respectively. This is a very significant deviation
{+4 +4), {-4 -4); N=108 for (+4 0), (-4 0); andN=48 for
all other motifs. The dashed lines indicate significance ac-
from the equal distribution 192:192 which had to be
cording to the Sign Test for N = 48. expected if there would be no effect-of relative magni-
tude (the pair comparison for each of the eight motifs
being presented six times to eight subjects). Although
there is an effect of relative magnitudeit is not strong
enough to produce systematic shifts in the preferences
The results for motifs with two frequency level due to the melodic contour. Thus the relative magni-
changes in opposite directions, plotted in Fig. 4(c), tudes of successive frequency level changes seem to be
show that there was a significant preference for the less important than the melodic contour.
secondtone in the motifs (+4-4), (-4 +4), (+8-8), and
(+8-1), whereasno significantpreferencewas found The difference between the accentuations brought
in the motifs (-8 +8), (-8 +1), (+ 1 -8), (-1 +8). Gen- about by frequency level rises and falls of the same
erally, the preference for the second tone was stronger magnitude seems to be small. Considering all three-
tone motifs we see that the material contains the same
for the motifs with a frequency rise at the beginning
as compared with the corresponding motifs with a fre- number of falls and rises. Counting the total number of
quency fall at the beginning. Note that for the motifs votes for the terminal tones of frequency changes, we
find 1040 votes for the falls and 1080 votes for the rises.
(+ 1-8) and (-1 +8) there is no clear shift towards the
third tone as compared with the motifs (+ 1-1) and This means a Small and insignificant advantage for the
(-1 +1). rises. Looking for systematic differences between
mutually inverse motifs we see in Figs. 4(a) and (c) that
The data points for motifs with two frequency level all motifs with a frequency rise at the beginning show a
changes in the same direction can be found in Fig. 4(d). stronger preference for the second tone than the cor-
As compared with Fig. 4(c) the cloud of data points has responding motifs with a fall at the beginning. However,
spread somewhat and has moved away from corner 2. in Fig. 4(b) falls seem to give stronger accentuations
Except for the motifs (+8 + 1)--second tone--, (+ 1 + 1) and in Fig. 4(d) there is no systematic difference. On
and (-1-1)--third tone--, no significant preference is the whole, rises and falls are equally effective.
found.
A possible interpretationof the results could be that
4. Discussion
the accentuation of the first
frequency change by its
Tones obtain a fair number of votes only when pre- precedence suppresses the accentuation of the second
ceded by a frequency change. This is a necessary con- change, and that it does somore strongly, the stronger
dition as is shown by the empty corners I in Figs. 4(a)- its own accentuation. The strength of accentuation then
(d), the first tone always being preceded by a tone of is determined in decreasing order of importance by di-
equal frequency. It seems that a change of frequency rectional difference (melodic contour), relative magni-

1601 J. Acoust.Soc.Am., Vol. 71, No. 6, June1982 JosephM. Thomassen:Melodicaccent 1601

Downloaded 07 Aug 2012 to 131.155.151.138. Redistribution subject to ASA license or copyright; see http://asadl.org/journals/doc/ASALIB-home/info/terms.jsp
rude, and a possible difference between rise and fall; role. It was decided to concentrate first on the influ-
the effect of relative magnitude can become more ence of the melodic contour. We can simulate the pro-
noticeable when there is no effect of directional differ- cess with a window containing only three tones by using,
ence. for instance, the experimental results from experiment
I, session I (A-subjects, three-tone motifs with inter-
III. MODEL vals of four semitones).

We will now develop a model, starting from intuitive Suppose the window contains the tones i, i + 1, and
considerations about accent perception and taking the i +2 related by the frequency level changes Ct+• and
results of the experiments into account. This will re- Ct+•., respectively. Now call the relative probabilities
sult in an algorithm to compute an accent strength for for tone i + 1 and tone i +2 to be perceived as an accent,
each tone in a melodic contour. Predictions made by respectively, Pt+• (Ct+•, Ct+•.)and Pt+•. (Ct+•,Ct+•.). We
the model about accent perception in four-tone motifs postulate both probabilities to be zero if the window con-
can then be verified. tains no frequency level changes, otherwise they are
positive and normalized:
While the anticipation of the observer is built up and
maintained over fairly long periods of time, acoustical =Ct+2 =0
factors are thought to exert their influence within a
short time. We consider a change of frequency level to
P,+•(Ct+•,
C,+•.)
+Pt+•.(Ct+•,
C,+•.)
= 1, else .{0,
Ct+
• ß (1)

be the basic acoustical cue for melodic accent: a fre- The values for the probabilities as derived from experi-
quency level change between two successive tones can ment I are given in Table Io
be conceived of as causing melodic accentuation of the
The postulated values mean that no points are allotted
second tone. However, two successive frequency
in • situation where no accentuation is present. The
changes bring about melodic accentuations that differ
results for one single frequency change contained in the
according to the relative directions and magnitudes of
window are idealized because they were virtually uni-
the changes. This implies that, in order to determine
vocal. In the case of two successive frequency changes
the presence of melodic accentuation of a certain tone in
the experimental values for the pair comparison 2--3
the sequence, at least three tones have to be stored in
are substituted for Pt +• and Pt +2.
the subject's memory. This "processing window" has
to be carried along the shift to the next tone of the se- Having applied this procedure of allotting points for
quence. In this way a series of equivalent impressions the window being in a certain position in the tone se-
are linked up. Thus we can describe the perceptual quence, a shift is made to the next position and the pro-
process in terms of a window sliding along the tone cedure is repeated. At the boundaries of the tone se-
sequence; the window will have a limited size, spanning quence the window contains only one or two tones. In
at least three tones, however. The frequency level the case of a single tone this tone is assigned the
changes between the tones in this window determine value 1.0. In the case of two tones, the tone immediate-
their probabilities of being perceived as an accentø ly after the frequency level change receives the value
The ultimate accent perception, however, is also in- 1o00, whereas both tones are assigned 0.50 if there is
fluenced by anticipation. There are good reasons for no frequency change between them. The product of the
assuming that the melodic contour makes an important values allocated to a tone i is considered to be a mea-
contribution to memory for melody (see for instance sure of its accent strength At. The accent strength of
Dowling and Fujitani•4). Moreover, our experiment each tone then indicates how much accentuation con-
with three-tone motifs indicates that the melodic con- tributes to the probability that this tone will be per-
tour could be the most important determinant of ceived as an accent, in the hypothetical case of no anti-
melodic accentsø Therefore it seems useful to intro- cipation. In this way a sequence of tones i (i = 1, .... n)
duce a distinction between effects of contour and effects is transformed into a sequence of accent strengths A t
of relative magnitude of successive frequency changes. (i = 1, o.. ,n) according to the formula
Apart from these effects, tonal relationshipsplay a At =Pt(C,-•, Ct) x Pt(Ct, C,+•) . (2)
As an example we apply this procedure to a four-tone
TABLE I. The valuesP/+•(C/+l,Ci+2) and .t:•i+2(Ci+l,
Ci+2) to motif containing only intervals of four semitones and
be assigned to the second and third tones of a motif containing
embedded according to the method of controlled antici-
frequency levels i to i + 2; these values are derived from the
results of experiment I. pation (Fig. 5).
The model, applied in this way to all three-tone
Ci , 1 Ci +2 Motif Pi ß! Pi ß2 motifs with intervals of four semitones, yields exactly
the values of Table I: the accent strengths of the sec-
( 0 0 ) 0.00 0.00 Postulated ond and the third tone of each motif are found to be the
(-• 4 0) 1.00 0.00 values Pt+• and Pt+2 at the corresponding table entry.
(0 • 4) 0.00 1.00 The model thus reproduces the data on which it is
(+4-4) 0.83 0.17 Experimental values based. By using the model to compute expected accent
(-4+4) 0.71 0.29 of pair comparison strengths for all possible four-tone motifs with inter-
(+4+4) 0.33 0.67 2 • 3 (session 1) vals of four semitones, predictions can be made for
(-4-4) 0.50 0.50 used. accent perception in four tones. These predictions
were tested in experiment II.

1602 J. Acoust.Soc. Am., Vol. 71, No. 6, June 1982 JosephM. Thomassen' Melodicaccent 1602

Downloaded 07 Aug 2012 to 131.155.151.138. Redistribution subject to ASA license or copyright; see http://asadl.org/journals/doc/ASALIB-home/info/terms.jsp
Tones

I .oo .oo i • /

I, , .oo 1.00 I
.3 3 .67 I r = .94 r =.99 r=1.00
P's I
I .83 .1 7 !
I 1.00 .00 !
x
A's .00 .33 .55 .17
FIG. 5. An example of the operation of the model.

r--.94 r =.80 r=.88

IV. EXPERIMENT II- FOUR-TONE MOTIFS


1
A. Setup
We considered four-tone motifs with intervals of
four semitones. Motifs that are trivial expansions of
three-tone motifs were excluded, thus leaving 12 (pos- o
r--.95 r =.88
sible) motifs. The onset-onset time was chosen to be
180 ms, and the tone duration was 100 ms again. The
method of controlled anticipation was applied, the
1
period of the meter being four tones. Anticipation could
be induced for each tone of the motif, resulting in four
possible sequences per motif. In a pair comparison ex-
periment these four sequences wouid yield 4x (4-1)= 12
o
pairs per motif. With 12 motifs and the need to present r--.93 r = .88 r =.88

each pair several times this leads to a number of stimu-


2 3 4 1 2 3 4
li that would take too much timeø It seemed more ef-
TONE •-
ficient to ask the subjects to judge separate tone se-
quences using a four-point scale to be interpreted as' FIG. 6. The accent strengths for the tones of 12 motifs. Each
the rhythm (meter) of the sequenceis (++) for surely motif is represented by the succession of frequency level
regular, (+) for regular, (-) for irregular, or (--) for changes. The correlation coefficient r shows the agreement
surely irregular. between theoretical and experimental scale values. connected
respectively by solid or dashed lines. The standard deviation
Further time saving was obtained by testing six sub- (six subjects) is indicated by vertical bars.
jects at a time together in a quiet room. All subjects
had participated in experiment I (group A). The stimuli
were presented diotically by headphones at a comfort-
able sound level. In each of three sessions the subjects calculated according to the model together with experi-
had to work through the material of four motifs. The mental scale values averaged over six subjects. The
sequences for a motif were presented successively, the corresponding standard deviations over the subjects
order being random with respect to the tone for which are indicated by vertical bars. The predicted values
anticipation was induced. Every sequence was presen- are connected by solid lines, the experimental values
ted eight times. Having treated a motif a pause was by dashed lines. The correlation coefficient is denoted
allowed before continuing with the next motif. A ses- by r.
sion lasted about 45 min.
C. Discussion
B. Results
In Fig. 6 mutually inverse motifs are arranged ver-
A presentation of the data in terms of median cate-
tically in pairs. The motifs in the first column were
gories and interquartile ranges would be appropriate
clearly the most easily judged, as implied by the rela-
but then comparisons between categorical data and pre-
tively small standard deviations. Obviously there is no
dicted scale values would not be straightforward. Hence
further interaction between two frequency changes if
the categorical responses were transformed into points
on a linear scale with boundaries 0 and 1 for the most
they are separated by a frequency repetition. In the
second column the accent strengths of the second and
irregular and most regular tone sequences. The two
the fourth motif tone are always the strongest. This
middle categories were assignedthe scale values « and
may express a fact of precedence: if the first frequen-
] althoughit is well recognizedthat responsecate-
cy level change has been effective, the second change
gories are not perceptually equidistant in generalø
cannot exert its influence whereas the third change can
Figure 6 shows the accent strengths for the 12 motifs again assert itself.

1603 J. Acoust.Soc. Am., Vol. 71, No. 6, June 1982 JosephM. Thomassen: Melodic accent 1603

Downloaded 07 Aug 2012 to 131.155.151.138. Redistribution subject to ASA license or copyright; see http://asadl.org/journals/doc/ASALIB-home/info/terms.jsp
In the last column not all mutually inverse motifs stated that the first of two opposite frequency changes
yield equal results, in particular the scale values for gives the stronger accentuation whereas two changes
the motifs (+4+4-4)and (-4-4 +4)differ. This may in the same direction are equally effective. in the
be due to the somewhat stronger accentuations of fre- case of clearly diverging relative magnitudes the larger
quency rises as compared to frequency falls. We in- change is the more powerful. Frequency rises have a
spected the categorical responses with respect to this somewhat stronger effect than frequency falls. Assum-
difference. There were 768 judgments to be made for ing the span of the window to be three tones, accent
sequences in which anticipation occurred immediately perception in motifs consisting of four tones could be
after a rise. The same number of inverted sequences described fairly well, although the difference in results
was presentedø This resulted in 569 positive (i.e., for some mutually inverse motifs could not be accoun-
+ + or +) judgments for rises against 534 positive ted for completely.
judgments in the case of falls: a small and insignifi-
For sinusoidal tones there is a straightforward rela-
cant difference. On the other hand, there was a sig-
tionship between frequency level and perceived pitch
nificant difference in the number of judgments that
and it seems reasonable to assume that the findings
were surely regular (++): 345 for rises against 271
apply to pitch changes in general. It is surprising then,
for falls, P< 5%.
that the accentuating role of frequency level changes in
Although the model contains a certain asymmetry music has not been determined before, although it is
with respect to rises and falls, this is not sufficient well known that such changes are powerful cues to the
to account for the difference in the results of some perception of accent in speech. It was found, for in-
mutually inverse motifs. Nevertheless the procedure stance, that a rapid rise early or halfway in the vowel
gives a reasonable description of the four-tone motifs' or a combination of the early rise and a rapid fall in
on the whole there is a fairly good agreement between the middle of the vowel largely determine accent per-
predicted values and outcomes; a calculation of the cor- ception in Dutch.•5'•6
relation r of the whole material (48 points) gives r
This shows that the timing of movement of fre-
=0.76. The correlation coefficients of the separate
quency level with respect to vowel onset and end is par-
motifs cannot be assigned much weight, since there are
ticularly important, as is its position in the overall
only a few degrees of freedom.
contour.•7 There seemsto be no a pt/or/ reasonwhy
frequency level changes should be less effective in
V. GENERAL DISCUSSION music than they are in speech.
The method of controlled anticipation proved to be It is obvious, for instance, that melodic accents
adequate for the study of accent perception in tone se- play an important role in the psalmody of Gregorian
quences. We believe that the subjects were able to chant. The main part of a psalm verse is recited in a
handle the criterion of judging regularity in a way that reciting tone or tenor, each half-verse beifig concluded
admits conclusions about accent perception. In the case by a melodicformula, called cadence[exampleFig.
of dynamic accentuation the tone sequences with coin- 7(a)]. This cadenceis built on the last or the two last
ciding accentuation and anticipation were preferred unan- word accents of the half-verse. Although there is some
imously. This was also the case with melodic ac- discussion about the character of the cadences,•8 in the
centuation, where the agreement between subjects was psalmody as it can be found in the Solesroes edition of
highly significant. A number of them had already par- the Antiphonale there should be a correspondence be-
ticipated in experiments with dynamic accentuation and tween word accents and melodic accents. Indeed, when
it is not plausible that they handled the criterion dif- the window model is applied on the melodic contours of
ferently for dynamic accentuation and melodic accentua- the psalmody the predicted melodic accents coincide
tion. Thus there seems to be no reason to doubt the
with word accents in the majority of cases. Of course
validity of the results. it is no surprise that the model can be applied success-
The results of the experiments led to the following fully on the one part melodies of psalmody. The experi-
mental data on which the model is based are obtained
concept. Accent perception in a tone sequence is de-
termined by the acoustical properties of the tone se- with stimuli resembling a tenor followed by a cadence
quence and the anticipation of the observer. Acoustical and the declamatory rhythm of psalmodyoften departs
factors will exert their influence mostly within a short not too much from the isochrony used in the experi-
ments.
time span, allowing for a description in terms of a
"window" sliding along the tone sequence. The frequen- This does not mean, however, that the results only
cy pattern within the window is a determinant of melo- apply to melodic accentuation in the absence of other
dic accent. It appears that in principle every change modes of accentuation. Usually, melodic accentuation
of frequency level between two successive tones can be is not recognized because of its different functional
interpreted as accentuation of the tone that ends the nature. The existence of a meter is often supported
change. Successive frequency changes interact; differ- mainly by dynamic, temporal, and harmonicaccentua-
ences in magnitude and direction determine their rela- tion. Although there are numerous examples in which
tive strengths of accentuation. It is useful to make a melodic accentuation(co)determinesthe meter [ex-
distinction between effects of melodic contour and ef-
ample in Fig. 7(b)], there are at least as manycasesin
fects of relative magnitude of frequency intervals. As which melodih accentuation is set off from the meter
far as the melodic contour is concerned, it can be makingfor "interestingrhythm" and"tension"[example

1604 J. Acoust.Soc.Am., Vol. 71, No. 6, June !982 JosephM. Thomassen'Melodicaccent 1604

Downloaded 07 Aug 2012 to 131.155.151.138. Redistribution subject to ASA license or copyright; see http://asadl.org/journals/doc/ASALIB-home/info/terms.jsp
(a) I wish to thank Ben Cardozo for the valuable discus-
TENOR MEDIAN FINAL sions we had, and Theo de Jong and Leo Vogten for
CADENCE CADENCE
their assistance during the experiments. Acknowledg-
,0 V V V
,-,, - • - • --= .... ; '-• ?,•-- ments also to Herman Bouma, Don Bouwhuis, Louis
Goldstein, Ab van Katwijk, and Gideon Keren, who
read earlier drafts of the manuscript and made help-
(b) ful comments.
V V V V V

V V V V V V
1G.Cooperand L. B. Meyer, The RhythmicStructureof Music
(Univ. Chicago Press, Chicago, 1960).
2E. Meumann,"ntersuchungenzur Psychologte
undAesthetik,"
Philos. Studten 10, 249-322, 393-430 (1894).
SC.R. Squire, '• geneticstudyof rhythm," Am. J. Psychol.
12, 492-589 (1901).
4H.Woodrow,"Therole of pitch tn rhythm," Psychol.Rev.
18, 54-72 (1911).
(c)
V v V v V v V v V v V v aS.Ehrlich, G. Oldton, andP. Fratsse, "La structurattonto-
nale des rhythmes,"Annie Psychol.56, 27-45 (1956).
•F. L. Royer andW. R. Garner, 'Responseuncertaintyand
perceptual difficulty of auditory temporal patterns," Per-
cept. Psychophys. 1, 41-47 (1966).
?J.Sundberg
andB. Lindbiota, "eneratire theoriestn lan-
guageand music descriptions,"Cognition 4, 99-122 (1976).
FIG. 7. Examples of occurrence of melodic accent. Melodic 8A. S. Bregman, 'The formationof auditorystreams," in At-
accents are indicated by the letter V. (a) Median and final tention a•d Performance VII, edited by J. Requin (Erlbaum,
cadencein Gregorian psalmody (fifth church tone). Coinci- Hillsdale, NJ, 1978), pp. 63-75.
dence of word accents and melodic accents. (b) Praeludium •L. P. A. S. van Noorden, "Temporal coherencetn the percep-
XV, BWV 860 of J. S. Bach's'Das WohltemperierteKlavier.' tion of tone sequences," Doctoral thesis, Eindhoven, Univ-
Metric structure codetermined by melodic accents. (c) ersity of Technology (1975).
Valse, Op. 64 Nr. 1 of F. Chopin. Melodic accents set off løO.Ortmann, "Onthe melodicrelativity of tones,"Psychol.
from the meter. Monogr. XXXV 162, 1-47 (1926).
llJ. SmitsvanWaesberghe,On Melody (AmericanInstituteof
Musicology, 1950).
in Fig. 7(c)]. In those casesthe melodicaccentuations x2j. Thomassen,%Vaarneming
vangeringedynamische
accen-
are not consciously perceived as such, in contrast with tuering in toonreeksen," I. P.O. Rep. 300 (in Dutch) (1976).
temporal accentuations which are usually associated lSG.J. J. Moohenand Th. A. de Jong, 'qVIARIE--Interfacebe-
with metric accents and are therefore denotedas "syn- tween computer and experiment," I.P.O. Annu. Prog. Rep. 8,
copes" when disturbing metric regularity. With this 54-56 (1973).

functional difference in mind, it is possible to develop


X4W.J. DowlingandD. S. Fujitani, "Contour,interval, and
pitch recognitionin memoryfor melodies,"J. Acoust.Soc.
an eye and ear for melodic accentuations whenever they Am. 49, 524-531 (1971).
occur, and to recognize the applicability of findings on l•A. Cohenand J. 't Hart, '•Onthe anatomyof intonation,"
melodic accentuation to music in general. Lingua 19, 177-192 (1967).
16j. 't Hart andA. Cohen,'•[ntonatton
by rule: a perceptual
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS quest," J. Phon. 1, 309-327 (1973).
l?A.F. V. vanKatwijk, '•ccentuatton
in D.utch,"Doctoral
This research was supported by a grant from the thesis, University of Utrecht (1974).
Netherlands Organization for the Advancement of Pure 18T.Bailey, "Accentua!
andcursire cadencesin Gregorian
Research. Psalmody," J. Am. Mustcolog. 29, 463-471 (1976).

1605 J. Acoust.Soc. Am., Vol. 71, No. 6, June 1982 JosephM. Thomassen'Melodicaccent 1605

Downloaded 07 Aug 2012 to 131.155.151.138. Redistribution subject to ASA license or copyright; see http://asadl.org/journals/doc/ASALIB-home/info/terms.jsp

Вам также может понравиться